Training ManualCIA Ion
Training ManualCIA Ion
INTERROGATION
July 1963
TABLE OF CONTENTS
I. INTRODUCTION 1-3
II . DEFINITIONS 4-5
A. Screening 30-33
B. Other Preliminary Procedures 33-37
C. Summary 37
A. Restrictions 82
B. The Theory of Coercion 82-85
C. Arrest 85-86
D. Detention 86-87
E. Deprivation of Sensory Stimuli 87-90
F. Threats and Fear 90-92
G. Debility 92-93
H. Pain 93-95
I. Heightened Suggestibility and Hypnosis 95-98
J. Narcosis 98-100
K. The Detection of Malingering 101-102
L. Conclusion 103-104
This manual cannot teach anyone how to be, or become, a good interrogator.
At best it can help readers to avoid the characteristic mistakes of poor
interrogators.
1 [page break]
2 [page break]
B. Explanation of Organization
This study moves from the general topic of interrogation per se (Parts I, II, III,
IV, V, and VI) to planning the counterintelligence interrogation (Part VII) to
the CI interrogation of resistant sources (Parts VIII, IX, and X). The
definitions, legal considerations, and discussions of interrogators and sources,
as well as Section VI on screening and other preliminaries, are relevant to all
kinds of interrogations. Once it is established that the source is probably a
counterintelligence target (in other words, is probably a member of a foreign
intelligence or security service, a Communist, or a part of any other group
engaged in clandestine activity directed against the national security), the
interrogation is planned and conducted accordingly. The CI interrogation
techniques are discussed in an order of increasing intensity as the focus on
source resistance grows sharper. The last section, on do's and dont's, is a return
to the broader view of the opening parts; as a check-list, it is placed last solely
for convenience.
3 [page break]
4 [page break]
5 [page break]
Detention poses the most common of the legal problems. KUBARK has no
independent legal authority to detain anyone against his will, [approx. 4 lines
deleted] The haste in which some KUBARK interrogations have been
conducted has not always been the product of impatience. Some security
services, especially those of the Sino-Soviet Bloc, may work at leisure,
depending upon time as well as their own methods to melt recalcitrance.
KUBARK usually
6 [page break]
The handling and questioning of defectors are subject to the provisions of [one
or two words deleted] Directive No. 4: to its related Chief/KUBARK
Directives, principally [approx. 1/2 line deleted] Book Dispatch [one or two
words deleted] and to pertinent [one or two words deleted]. Those concerned
with the interrogation of defectors, escapees, refugees, or repatriates should
know these references.
7 [page break]
[approx. 2/3 line deleted] but should not normally become directly involved.
Clandestine activity conducted abroad on behalf of a foreign power by a
private PBPRIME citizens does fall within KUBARK's investigative and
interrogative responsibilities. However, any investigation, interrogation, or
interview of a PBPRIME citizen which is conducted abroad because it be
known or suspected that he is engaged in clandestine activities directed against
PBPRIME security interests requires the prior and personal approval of
Chief/KUDESK or of his deputy.
8 [page break]
The CI interrogator dealing with an uncooperative interrogatee who has been
well-briefed by a hostile service on the legal restrictions under which
ODYOKE services operate must expect some effective delaying tactics. The
interrogatee has been told that KUBARK will not hold him long, that he need
only resist for a while. Nikolay KHOKHLOV, for example, reported that
before he left for Frankfurt am Main on his assassination mission, the
following thoughts coursed through his head: "If I should get into the hands of
Western authorities, I can become reticent, silent, and deny my voluntary visit
to Okolovich. I know I will not be tortured and that under the procedures of
western law I can conduct myself boldly." (17) [The footnote numerals in this
text are keyed to the numbered bibliography at the end.] The interrogator who
encounters expert resistance should not grow flurried and press; if he does, he
is likelier to commit illegal acts which the source can later use against him.
Remembering that time is on his side, the interrogator should arrange to get as
much of it as he needs.
9 [page break]
Perhaps the four qualifications of chief importance to the interrogator are (1)
enough operational training and experience to permit quack recognition of
leads; (2) real familiarity with the language to be used; (3) extensive
background knowledge about the interrogatee's native country (and intelligence
service, if employed by one); and (4) a genuine understanding of the source as
a person.
[approx. 1/2 line deleted] stations, and even a few bases can call upon one or
several interrogators to supply these prerequisites, individually or as a team.
Whenever a number of interrogators is available, the percentage of successes is
increased by careful matching of questioners and sources and by ensuring that
rigid prescheduling does not prevent such matching. Of the four traits listed, a
genuine insight into the source's character and motives is perhaps
10 [page break]
most important but least common. Later portions of this manual explore this
topic in more detail. One general observation is introduced now, however,
because it is considered basic to the establishment of rapport, upon which the
success of non-coercive interrogation depends.
The interrogator should remember that he and the interrogatee are often
working at cross-purposes not because the interrogates is malevolently
withholding or misleading but simply because what he wants front the situation
is not what the interrogator wants. The interrogator's goal is to obtain useful
information -- facts about which the interrogatee presumably have acquired
information. But at the outset of the interrogation, and perhaps for a long time
afterwards, the person being questioned is not greatly concerned with
communicating his body of specialized information to his questioner; he is
concerned with putting his best foot forward. The question uppermost in his
mind, at the beginning, is not likely to be "How can I help PBPRIME?" but
rather "What sort of impression am I making?" and, almost immediately
thereafter, "What is going to happen to me now?" (An exception is the
penetration agent or provocateur sent to a KUBARK field installation after
training in withstanding interrogation. Such an agent may feel confident
enough not to be gravely concerned about himself. His primary interest, from
the beginning, may be the acquisition of information about the interrogator and
his service.)
The skilled interrogator can save a great deal of time by understanding the
emotional needs of the interrogates. Most people confronted by an official --
and dimly powerful -- representative of a foreign power will get down to cases
much faster if made to feel, from the start, that they are being treated as
individuals. So simple a matter as greeting an interrogatee by his name at the
opening of the session establishes in his mind the comforting awareness that he
is considered as a person, not a squeezable sponge. This is not to say that
egotistic types should be allowed to bask at length in the warmth of individual
recognition. But it is important to assuage the fear of denigration which afflicts
many people when first interrogated by making it clear that the individuality of
the interrogatee is recognized. With this common understanding established,
the interrogation can move on to impersonal matters and will not later be
thwarted or interrupted --
11 [page break]
It is frequently said that the interrogator should be "a good judge of human
nature." In fact, [approx. 3 lines deleted] (3) This study states later (page
"Great attention has been given to the degree to which persons are able to
make judgements from casual observations regarding the personality
characteristics of another. The consensus of research is that with respect to
many kinds of judgments, at least some judges perform reliably better than
chance...." Nevertheless, "... the level
12 [page break]
There has been a good deal of discussion of interrogation experts vs. subject-
matter experts. Such facts as are available suggest that the latter have a slight
advantage. But for counterintelligence purposes the debate is academic.
[approx. 5 lines deleted]
It is sound practice to assign inexperienced interrogators to guard duty or to
other supplementary tasks directly related to interrogation, so that they can
view the process closely before taking charge. The use of beginning
interrogators as screeners (see part VI) is also recommended.
___________________
13 [page break]
variety of aids can be made available in the field or from Headquarters. (These
are discussed in Part VIII.) The intensely personal nature of the interrogation
situation makes it all the more necessary that the KUBARK questioner should
aim not for a personal triumph but for his true goal -- the acquisition of all
needed information by any authorized means.
14 [page break]
From the viewpoint of the intelligence service the categories of persons who
most frequently provide useful information in response to questioning are
travellers; repatriates; defectors, escapees, and refugees; transferred sources;
agents, including provocateurs, double agents, and penetration agents; and
swindlers and fabricators.
c. Determination of repatriate's kind and level of access while outside his own
country.
15 [page break]
All analyses of the defector-refugee flow have shown that the Orbit services
are well-aware of the advantages offered by this channel as a means of planting
their agents in target countries.
16 [page break]
17 [page break]
interrogatee.
18 [page break]
The number of systems devised for categorizing human beings is large, and
most of them are of dubious validity. Various categorical schemes are outlined
in treatises on interrogation. The two typologies most frequently advocated are
psychologic-emotional and geographic-cultural. Those who urge the former
argue that the basic emotional-psychological patterns do not vary significantly
with time, place, or culture. The latter school maintains the existence of a
national character and sub-national categories, and interrogation guides based
on this principle recommend approaches tailored to geographical cultures.
On the other hand, there are valid objections to the adoption of cultural-
geographic categories for interrogation purposes (however valid they may be
as KUCAGE concepts). The pitfalls of ignorance of the distinctive culture of
the source have "[approx. 4 lines deleted]
19 [page break]
The ideal solution would be to avoid all categorizing. Basically, all schemes
for labelling people are wrong per se; applied arbitrarily, they always produce
distortions. Every interrogator knows that a real understanding of the
individual is worth far more than a thorough knowledge of this or that pigeon-
hole to which he has been consigned. And for interrogation purposes the ways
in which he differs from the abstract type may be more significant than the
ways in which he conforms.
But KUBARK does not dispose of the time or personnel to probe the depths of
each source's individuality. In the opening phases of interrogation, or in a quick
interrogation, we are compelled to make some use of the shorthand of
categorizing, despite distortions. Like other interrogation aides, a scheme of
categories is useful only if recognized for what it is -- a set of labels that
facilitate communication but are not the same as the persons thus labelled. If an
interrogatee lies persistently, an interrogator may report and dismiss him as a
"pathological liar." Yet such persons may possess counterintelligence (or
other) information quite equal in value to that held by other sources, and the
interrogator likeliest to get at it is the man who is not content with labelling but
is as interested in why the subject lies as in what he lies about.
With all of these reservations, then, and with the further observation that those
who find these psychological-emotional categories pragmatically valuable
should use them and those who do not should let them alone, the following
nine types are described. The categories are based upon the fact that a person's
past is always reflected, however dimily, in his present ethics and behavior.
Old dogs can learn new tricks but not new ways of learning them. People do
change, but what appears to be new behavior or a new psychological pattern is
usually just a variant on the old theme.
20 [page break]
It is not claimed that the classification system presented here is complete; some
interrogatees will not fit into any one of the groupings. And like all other
typologies, the system is plagued by overlap, so that some interrogatees will
show characteristics of more than one group. Above all, the interrogator must
remember that finding some of the characteristics of the group in a single
source does not warrant an immediate conclusion that the source "belongs to"
the group, and that even correct labelling is not the equivalent of understanding
people but merely an aid to understanding.
21 [page break]
Usually the orderly-obstinate character has a history of active rebellion in
childhood, of persistently doing the exact opposite of what he is told to do. As
an adult he may have learned to cloak his resistance and become passive-
aggressive, but his determination to get his own way is unaltered. He has
merely learned how to proceed indirectly if necessary. The profound fear and
hatred of authority, persisting since childhood, is often well-concealed in
adulthood, For example, such a person may confess easily and quickly under
interrogation, even to acts that he did not commit, in order to throw the
interrogator off the trail of a significant discovery (or, more rarely, because of
feelings of guilt).
Such a person has usually had a great deal of over-indulgence in early life. He
is sometimes the youngest member of a large family,
22 [page break]
The greedy, demanding character often suffered from very early deprivation of
affection or security. As an adult he continues to seek substitute parents who
will care for him as his own, he feels, did not.
23 [page break]
factory substitute because the demand arises not from a specific need but as an
expression of the subject's need for security. He is likely to find reassuring any
manifestation of concern for his well-being.
In dealing with this type -- and to a considerable extent in dealing with any of
the types herein listed -- the interrogator must be aware of the limits and
pitfalls of rational persuasion. If he seeks to induce cooperation by an appeal to
logic, he should first determine whether the source's resistance is based on
logic. The appeal will glance off ineffectually if the resistance is totally or
chiefly emotional rather than rational. Emotional resistance can be dissipated
only by emotional manipulation.
People who show these characteristics are actually unusually fearful. The
causes of intense concealed anxiety are too complex and subtle to permit
discussion of the subject in this paper.
24 [page break]
are likelier to divulge if made to feel that the truth will not be used to harm
them and if the interrogator also stresses the callousness and stupidity of the
adversary in sending so valiant a person upon so ill-prepared a mission. There
is little to be gained and much to be lost by exposing the nonrelevant lies of
this kind of source. Gross lies about deeds of daring, sexual prowess, or other
"proofs" of courage and manliness are best met with silence or with friendly
but noncommittal replies unless they consume an inordinate amount of time. If
operational use is contemplated, recruitment may sometimes be effected
through such queries as, "I wonder if you would be willing to undertake a
dangerous mission."
5. The guilt-ridden character. This kind of person has a strong cruel,
unrealistic conscience. His whole life seems devoted to reliving his feelings of
guilt. Sometimes he seems determined to atone; at other times he insists that
whatever went wrong is the fault of somebody else. In either event he seeks
constantly some proof or external indication that the guilt of others is greater
than his own. He is often caught up completely in efforts to prove that he has
been treated unjustly. In fact, he may provoke unjust treatment in order to
assuage his conscience through punishment. Compulsive gamblers who find no
real pleasure in winning but do find relief in losing belong to this class. So do
persons who falsely confess to crimes. Sometimes such people actually commit
crimes in order to confess and be punished. Masochists also belong in this
category.
The causes of most guilt complexes are real or fancied wrongs done to parents
or others whom the subject felt he ought to love and honor. As children such
people may have been frequently scolded or punished. Or they may have been
"model" children who repressed all natural hostilities.
25 [page break]
the best advice is that the interrogator, once alerted by information from the
screening process (see Part VI) or by the subject's excessive preoccupation
with moral judgements, should treat as suspect and subjective any information
provided by the interrogatee about any matter that is of moral concern to him.
Persons with intense guilt feelings may cease resistance and cooperate if
punished in some way, because of the gratification induced by punishment.
26 [page break]
8. The exception believes that the world owes him a great deal. He feels that
he suffered a gross injustice, usually early in life, and should be repaid.
Sometimes the injustice was meted out impersonally, by fate, as a physical
deformity, an extremely painful illness or operation in childhood, or the early
loss of one parent or both. Feeling that these misfortunes were undeserved, the
exceptions regard them as injustices that someone or something must rectify.
Therefore they claim as their right privileges not permitted others. When the
claim is ignored or denied, the exceptions become rebellious, as adolescents
often do. They are
27 [page break]
convinced that the justice of the claim is plain for all to see and that any refusal
to grant it is willfully malignant.
When interrogated, the exceptions are likely to make demands for money,
resettlement aid, and other favors -- demands that are completely out of
proportion to the value of their contributions. Any ambiguous replies to such
demands will be interpreted as acquiescence. Of all the types considered here,
the exception is likeliest to carry an alleged injustice dealt him by KUBARK to
the newspapers or the courts.
The best general line to follow in handling those who believe that they are
exceptions is to listen attentively (within reasonable timelimits) to their
grievances and to make no commitments that cannot be discharged fully.
Defectors from hostile intelligence services, doubles, provocateurs, and others
who have had more than passing contact with a Sino-Soviet service may, if
they belong to this category, prove unusually responsive to suggestions from
the interrogator that they have been treated unfairly by the other service. Any
planned operational use of such persons should take into account the fact that
they have no sense of loyalty to a common cause and are likely to turn
aggrievedly against superiors.
C. Other Clues
28 [page break]
The true defector (as distinguished from the hostile agent in defector's guise) is
likely to have a history of opposition to authority. The sad fact is that defectors
who left their homelands because they could not get along with their
immediate or ultimate superiors are also likely to rebel against authorities in
the new environment (a fact which usually plays an important part in
redefection). Therefore defectors are likely to be found in the ranks of the
orderly-obstinate, the greedy and deriding, the schizoids, and the exceptions.
29 [page break]
VI. Screening and Other Preliminaries
A. Screening
[approx. 2/3 line deleted] some large stations are able to conduct preliminary
psychological screening before interrogation starts. The purpose of screening is
to provide the interrogator, in advance, with a reading on the type and
characteristics of the interrogatee. It is recommended that screening be
conducted whenever personnel and facilities permit, unless it is reasonably
certain that the interrogation will be of minor importance or that the
interrogatee is fully cooperative.
30 [page break]
The task of screening is made easier by the fact that the screener is interested
in the subject, not in the information which he may possess. Most people --
even many provocation agents who have been trained to recite a legend -- will
speak with some freedom about childhood events and familial relationships.
And even the provocateur who substitutes a fictitious person for his real father
will disclose some of his feelings about his father in the course of detailing his
story about the imaginary substitute. If the screener
31 [page break]
has learned to put the potential source at ease, to feel his way along in each
case, the source is unlikely to consider that a casual conversation about himself
if dangerous .
The screener is interested in getting the subject to talk about himself. Once the
flow starts, the screener should try not to stop it by questions, gestures, or other
interruptions until sufficient information has been revealed to permit a rough
determination of type. The subject is likeliest to talk freely if the screener's
manner is friendly and patient. His facial expression should not reveal special
interest in any one statement; he should just seem sympathetic and
understanding. Within a short time most people who have begun talking about
themselves go back to early experiences, so that merely by listening and
occasionally making a quiet, encouraging remark the screener can learn a great
deal. Routine questions about school teachers, employers, and group leaders,
for example, will lead the subject to reveal a good deal of how he feels about
his parents, superiors, and others of emotional consequence to him because of
associative links in his mind.
It is very helpful if the screener can imaginatively place himself in the subject's
position. The more the screener knows about the subject's native area and
cultural background, the less likely is he to disturb the subject by an
incongruous remark. Such comments as, "That must have been a bad time for
you and your family," or "Yes, I can see why you were angry," or "It sounds
exciting" are sufficiently innocuous not to distract the subject, yet provide
adequate evidence of sympathetic interest. Tasking the subject's side against
his enemies serves the same purpose, and such comments as "That was unfair;
they had no right to treat you that way" will aid rapport and stimulate further
revelations.
32 [page break]
Unless a shock effect is desired, the transition from the screening interview to
the interrogation situation should not be abrupt. At the first meeting with the
interrogatee it is usually a good idea for the interrogator to spend some time in
the same kind of quiet, friendly exchange that characterized the screening
interview. Even though the interrogator now has the screening product, the
rough classification by type, he needs to understand the subject in his own
terms. If he is immediately aggressive, he imposes upon the first interrogation
session (and to a diminishing extent upon succeeding sessions) too arbitrary a
pattern. As one expert has said, "Anyone who proceeds without consideration
for the disjunctive power of anxiety in human relationships will never learn
interviewing." (34)
33 [page break]
The key points are repeated here for ease of reference. These preliminary tests
are designed to supplement the technical examination of a walk-in's
documents, substantive questions about claimed homeland or occupation, and
other standard inquiries. The following questions, if asked, should be posed as
soon as possible after the initial contact, while the walk-in is still under stress
and before he has adjusted to a routine.
a. The walk-in may be asked to identify all relatives and friends in the area, or
even the country, in which PBPRIME asylum is first requested. Traces should
be run speedily. Provocation agents are sometimes directed to "defect" in their
target areas, and friends or relatives already in place may be hostile assets.
b. At the first interview the questioner should be on the alert for phrases or
concepts characteristic of intelligence or CP activity and should record such
leads whether it is planned to follow them by interrogation on the spot [approx.
1 line deleted]
34 [page break]
35 [page break]
[entire page redacted, except for "4." about 3/4 of the way down the page]
36 [page break]
5. All documents that have a bearing on the planned interrogation merit study.
Documents from Bloc countries, or those which are in any respect unusual or
unfamiliar, are customarily sent to the proper field or headquarters component
for technical analysis.
C. Summary
Screening and the other preliminary procedures will help the interrogator - and
his base, station, [one or two words deleted] to decide whether the prospective
source (1) is likely to possess useful counterintelligence because of association
with a foreign service or Communist Party and (2) is likely to cooperate
voluntarily or not. Armed with these estimates and with whatever insights
screening has provided into the personality of the source, the interrogator is
ready to plan.
37 [page break]
The long-range purpose of CI interrogation is to get from the source all the
useful counterintelligence information that he has. The short-range purpose is
to enlist his cooperation toward this end or, if he is resistant, to destroy his
capacity for resistance and replace it with a cooperative attitude. The
techniques used in nullifying resistance, inducing compliance, and eventually
eliciting voluntary cooperation are discussed in Part VIII of this handbook.
38 [page break]
39 [page break]
7. The accused, finally, is pushed far enough along the path toward confession
that it is easier for him to keep going than to turn back. He perceives
confession as the only way out of his predicament and into freedom. (15)
40 [page break]
The interrogation process is a continuum, and everything that takes place in the
continuum influences all subsequent events. The continuing process, being
interpersonal, is not
41 [page break]
Planning for interrogation is more important than the specifics of the plan.
Because no two interrogations are alike, the interrogation cannot realistically
be planned from A to Z, in all its particulars, at the outset. But it can and must
be planned from A to F or A to M. The chances of failure in an unplanned CI
interrogation are unacceptably high. Even worse, a "dash-on-regardless"
approach can ruin the prospects of success even if sound methods are used
later.
The intelligence category to which the subject belongs, though not determinant
for planning purposes, is still of some significance. The plan for the
interrogation of a traveller differs from that for other types because the time
available for questioning is often brief. The examination of his bona fides ,
accordingly, is often less searching. He is usually regarded as reasonably
reliable if his identity and freedom from other intelligence associations have
been established, if records checks do not produce derogatory information, if
his account of his background is free of omissions or discrepancies suggesting
significant withholding, if he does not attempt to elicit information about the
questioner or his sponsor, and if he willingly provides detailed information
which appears reliable or is established as such.
43 [page break]
C. The Specifics
Before questioning starts, the interrogator has clearly in mind what he wants to
learn, why he thinks the source has the information, how important it is, and
how it can best be obtained. Any confusion here, or any questioning based on
the premise that the purpose will take shape after the interrogation is under
way, is almost certain to lead to aimlessness and final failure. If the specific
goals cannot be discerned clearly, further investigation is needed before
querying starts.
2. Resistance
The kind and intensity of anticipated resistance is estimated. It is useful to
recognize in advance whether the information desired would be threatening or
damaging in any way to the interests of the interrogates. If so, the interrogator
should consider whether the same information, or confirmation of it, can be
gained from another source. Questioning suspects immediately, on a flimsy
factual basis, will usually cause waste of time, not save it. On the other hand, if
the needed information is not sensitive from the subject's viewpoint,
44 [page break]
merely asking for it is usually preferable to trying to trick him into admissions
and thus creating an unnecessary battle of wits.
Good planning will prevent interruptions. If the room is also used for purposes
other than interrogation, a "Do Not Disturb" sign or its equivalent should hang
on the door when questioning is under way. The effect of someone wandering
in because he forgot his pen or wants to invite the
45 [page break]
interrogator to lunch can be devastating. For the same reason there should not
be a telephone in the room; it is certain to ring at precisely the wrong moment.
Moreover, it is a visible link to the outside; its presence makes a subject feel
less cut-off, better able to resist.
46 [page break]
means can study their mistakes and their most effective techniques.
Exceptionally instructuve interrogations, or selected portions thereof, can also
be used in the training of others.
4. The Participants
The number of interrogators used for a single interrogation case varies from
one man to a large team. The size of the team depends on several
considerations, chiefly the importance of the case and the intensity of source
resistance. Although most sessions consist of one interrogator and one
interrogatee, some of the techniques described later call for the presence of
two, three, or four interrogators. The two-man team, in particular, is subject to
unintended antipathies and conflicts not called for by assigned roles. Planning
and
47 [page break]
Team members who are not otherwise engaged can be employed to best
advantage at the listening post. Inexperienced interrogators find that listening
to the interrogation while it is in progress can be highly educational.
48 [page break]
5. The Timing
Before interrogation starts, the amount of time probably required and probably
available to both interrogator and interrogatee should be calculated. If the
subject is not to be under detention, his normal schedule is ascertained in
advance, so that he will not have to be released at a critical point because he
has an appointment or has to go to work.
Interrogation sessions with a resistant source who is under detention should not
be held on an unvarying schedule. The capacity for resistance is diminished by
disorientation. The subject may be left alone for days; and he may be returned
to his cell, allowed to sleep for five minutes, and brought back
49 [page break]
6. The Termination
50 [page break]
contemplated, conversion is imperative. But even if the source has no further
value after his fund of information has been mined, spending some extra time
with him in order to replace his new sense of emptiness with new values can be
good insurance. All non-Communist services are bothered at times by
disgruntled exinterrogatees who press demands and threaten or take hostile
action if the demands are not satisfied. Defectors in particular, because they are
often hostile toward any kind of authority, cause trouble by threatening or
bringing suits in local courts, arranging publication of vengeful stories, or
going to the local police. The former interrogatee is especially likely to be a
future trouble-maker if during interrogation he was subjected to a form of
compulsion imposed from outside himself. Time spent, after the interrogation
ends, in fortifying the source's sense of acceptance in the interrogator's world
may be only a fraction of the time required to bottle up his attempts to gain
revenge. Moreover, conversion may create a useful and enduring asset. (See
also remarks in VIII B 4.)
51 [page break]
If the interrogatee is under detention, the interrogator can also manipulate his
environment. Merely by cutting off all other human contacts, "the interrogator
monopolizes the social environment of the source."(3) He exercises the powers
of an all-powerful parent, determining when the source will be sent to bed,
when and what he will eat, whether he will be rewarded for good behavior or
punished for being bad. The interrogator can and does make the
52 [page break]
subject's world not only unlike the world to which he had been accustomed but
also strange in itself - a world in which familiar patterns of time, space, and
sensory perception are overthrown. He can shift the environment abruptly. For
example, a source who refuses to talk at all can be placed in unpleasant solitary
confinement for a time. Then a friendly soul treats him to an unexpected walk
in the woods. Experiencing relief and exhilaration, the subject will usually find
it impossible not to respond to innocuous comments on the weather and the
flowers. These are expanded to include reminiscences, and soon a precedent of
verbal exchange has been established. Both the Germans and the Chinese have
used this trick effectively.
The interrogator also chooses the emotional key or keys in which the
interrogation or any part of it will be played.
Because of these and other advantages, " [approx. 6 lines deleted] ."(3)
1. The Opening
53 [page break]
There is another advantage in letting the subject talk freely and even
ramblingly in the first stage of interrogation. The interrogator is free to
observe. Human beings communicate a great deal by non-verbal means.
Skilled interrogators, for example, listen closely to voices and learn a great
deal from them. An interrogation is not merely a
54 [page break]
(3) A pale face indicates fear and usually shows that the interrogator is hitting
close to the mark.
(4) A dry mouth denotes nervousness.
(6) Emotional strain or tension may cause a pumping of the heart which
becomes visible in the pulse and throat.
(7) A slight gasp, holding the breath, or an unsteady voice may betray the
subject.
(8) Fidgeting may take many forms, all of which are good indications of
nervousness.
55 [page break]
(9) A man under emotional strain or nervous tension will involuntarily draw
his elbows to his sides. It is a protective defense mechanism.
(10) The movement of the foot when one leg is crossed over the knee of the
other can serve as an indicator. The circulation of the blood to the lower leg is
partially cut off, thereby causing a slight lift or movement of the free foot with
each heart beat. This becomes more pronounced and observable as the pulse
rate increases.
56 [page break]
How long the opening phase continues depends upon how long it takes to
establish rapport or to determine that voluntary cooperation is unobtainable. It
may be literally a matter of seconds, or it may be a drawn-out, up-hill battle.
Even though the cost in time and patience is sometimes high, the effort to
make the subject feel that his questioner is a sympathetic figure should not be
abandoned until all reasonable resources have been exhausted (unless, of
course, the interrogation does not merit much time). Otherwise, the chances are
that the interrogation will not produce optimum results. In fact, it is likely to be
a failure, and the interrogator should not be dissuaded from the effort to
establish rapport by an inward conviction that no man in his right mind would
incriminate himself by providing the kind of information that is sought. The
history of interrogation is full of confessions and other self-incriminations that
were in essence the result of a substitution of the interrogation world for the
world outside. In
57 [page break]
other words, as the sights and sounds of an outside world fade away, its
significance for the interrogatee tends to do likewise. That world is replaced by
the interrogation room, its two occupants, and the dynamic relationship
between them. As interrogation goes on, the subject tends increasingly to
divulge or withhold in accordance with the values of the interrogation world
rather than those of the outside world (unless the periods of questioning are
only brief interruptions in his normal life). In this small world of two
inhabitants a clash of personalities -- as distinct from a conflict of purposes --
assumes exaggerated force, like a tornado in a wind-tunnel. The self-esteem of
the interrogatee and of the interrogator becomes involved, and the interrogatee
fights to keep his secrets from his opponent for subjective reasons, because he
is grimly determined not to be the loser, the inferior. If on the other hand the
interrogator establishes rapport, the subject may withhold because of other
reasons, but his resistance often lacks the bitter, last-ditch intensity that results
if the contest becomes personalized.
The interrogator who senses or determines in the opening phase that what he is
hearing is a legend should resist the first, natural impulse to demonstrate its
falsity. In some interrogatees the ego-demands, the need to save face, are so
intertwined with preservation of the cover story that calling the man a liar will
merely intensify resistance. It is better to leave an avenue of escape, a loophole
which permits the source to correct his story without looking foolish.
"Much depends upon the sequence in which one conducts the cross-
examination of a dishonest witness. You should never hazard the important
question until you have laid the foundation for it in such a way that, when
confronted with the fact, the witness can neither deny nor explain it. One often
58 [page break]
2. The Reconnaissance
During the opening phase the interrogator will have charted the probable areas
of resistance by noting those topics which caused emotional or physical
reactions, speech blocks, or other indicators. He now begins to probe these
areas. Every experienced interrogator has noted that if an interrogatee
59 [page break]
is withholding, his anxiety increases as the questioning nears the mark. The
safer the topic, the more voluble the source. But as the questions make him
increasingly uncomfortable, the interrogatee becomes less communicative or
perhaps even hostile. During the opening phase the interrogator has gone along
with this protective mechanism. Now, however, he keeps coming back to each
area of sensitivity until he has determined the location of each and the intensity
of the defenses. If resistance is slight, mere persistence may overcome it; and
detailed questioning may follow immediately. But if resistance is strong, a new
topic should be introduced, and detailed questioning reserved for the third
stage.
The interrogator must know exactly what he wants to know. He should have on
paper or firmly in mind all the questions to which he seeks answers. It usually
60 [page break]
happens that the source has a relatively large body of information that has little
or no intelligence value and only a small collection of nuggets. He will
naturally tend to talk about what he knows best. The interrogator should not
show quick impatience, but neither should he allow the results to get out of
focus. The determinant remains what we need, not what the interrogatee can
most readily provide.
At the same time it is necessary to make every effort to keep the subject from
learning through the interrogation process precisely where our informational
gaps lie. This principle is especially important if the interrogatee is following
his normal life, going home each evening and appearing only once or twice a
week for questioning, or if his bona fides remains in doubt. Under almost all
circumstances, however, a clear revelation of our interests and knowledge
should be avoided. It is usually a poor practice to hand to even the most
cooperative interrogatee an orderly list of questions and ask him to write the
answers. (This stricture does not apply to the writing of autobiographies or on
informational matters not a subject of controversy with the source.) Some time
is normally spent on matters of little or no intelligence interest for purposes of
concealment. The interrogator can abet the process by making occasional notes
-- or pretending to do so -- on items that seem important to the interrogatee but
are not of intelligence value. From this point of view an interrogation can be
deemed successful if a source who is actually a hostile agent can report to the
opposition only the general fields of our interest but cannot pinpoint specifics
without including misleading information.
61 [page break]
It is also a good expedient to have the interrogatee make notes of topics that
should be covered, which occur to him while discussing the immediate matters
at issue. The act of recording the stray item or thought on paper fixes it in the
interrogatee's mind. Usually topics popping up in the course of an interrogation
are forgotten if not noted; they tend to disrupt the interrogation plan if covered
by way of digression on the spot.
62 [page break]
and more abruptly with reminiscences or digressions. His interest has shifted
from the interrogatee himself, who jut a while ago was an interesting person, to
the atsk of getting at what he knows. But if rapport has been established, the
interrogatee will be quick to sense and resent this change of attitude. This point
is particularly important if the interrogatee is a defector faced with bewildering
changes and in a highly emotional state. Any interrogatee has his ups and
downs, times when he is tired or half-ill, times when his personal problems
have left his nerves frayed. The peculiar intimacy of the interrogation situation
and the very fact that the interrogator has deliberately fostered rapport will
often lead the subject to talk about his doubts, fears, and other personal
reactions. The interrogator should neither cut off this flow abruptly nor show
impatience unless it takes up an inordinate amount of time or unless it seems
likely that all the talking about personal matters is being used deliberately as a
smoke screen to keep the interrogator from doing his job. If the interrogatee is
believed cooperative, then from the beginning to the end of the process he
should feel that the interrogator's interest in him has remained constant. Unless
the interrogation is soon over, the interrogatee's attitude toward his questioner
is not likely to remain constant. He will feel more and more drawn to the
questioner or increasingly antagonistic. As a rule, the best way for the
interrogator to keep the relationship on an even keel is to maintain the same
quiet, relaxed, and open-minded attitude from start to finish.
63 [page break]
was exhausted. KUBARK must keep track of such persons, because people
and circumstances change. Until the source dies or tells us everything that he
knows that is pertinent to our purposes, his interrogation may be interrupted,
perhaps for years -- but it has not been completed.
4. The Conclusion
64 [page break]
This principle also affects the decision to employ coercive techniques and
governs the choice of these methods. If in the opinion of the interrogator a
totally resistant source has the skill and determination to withstand any con-
coercive method or combination of methods, it is better to avoid them
completely.
65 [page break]
In brief, the techniques that follow should match the personality of the
individual interrogatee, and their effectiveness is intensified by good timing
and rapid exploitation of the moment of shock. (A few of the following items
are drawn from Sheehan.) (32)
66 [page break]
2. Nobody Loves You
The interrogator who already knows part of the story explains to the source
that the purpose of the questioning is not to gain information; the interrogator
knows everything already. His real purpose is to test the sincerity (reliability,
honor, etc.) of the source. The interrogator then asks a few questions to which
he knows the answers. If the subject lies, he is informed firmly and
dispassionately that he has lied. By skilled manipulation of the known, the
questioner can convince a naive subject that all his secrets are out and that
further resistance would be not only pointless but dangerous. If this technique
does not work very quickly, it must be dropped before the interrogatee learns
the true limits of the questioner's knowledge.
4. The Informer
67 [page break]
6. The Witness
One of these is to place the interrogatee in an outer office and escort past him,
and into the inner office, an accuser whom he knows personally or, in fact, any
person -- even one who is friendly to the source and uncooperative with the
interrogators -- who is believed to know something about whatever the
interrogatee is concealing. It is also essential that the interrogatee know or
suspect that the witness may be in possession of the incriminating information.
The witness is whisked past the interrogatee; the two are not allowed to speak
to each other. A guard and a stenographer remain in the outer office with the
interrogatee. After about an hour the interrogator who has been questioning the
interrogatee in past sessions opens the door and asks the stenographer to come
in, with steno pad and pencils. After a time she re-emerges and types material
from her pad, making several carbons. She pauses, points at the interrogatee,
and asks the guard how
68 [page break]
his name is spelled. She may also ask the interrogatee directly for the proper
spelling of a street, a prison, the name of a Communist intelligence officer, or
any other factor closely linked to the activity of which he is accused. She takes
her completed work into the inner office, comes back out, and telephones a
request that someone come up to act as legal witness. Another man appears and
enters the inner office. The person cast in the informer's role may have been let
out a back door at the beginning of these proceedings; or if cooperative, he
may continue his role. In either event, a couple of interrogators, with or
without the "informer", now emerge from the inner office. In contrast to their
earlier demeanor, they are now relaxed and smiling. The interrogator in charge
says to the guard, "O.K., Tom, take him back. We don't need him any more."
Even if the interrogatee now insists on telling his side of the story, he is told to
relax, because the interrogator will get around to him tomorrow or the next
day.
A session with the witness may be recorded. If the witness denounces the
interrogatee there is no problem. If he does not, the interrogator makes an
effort to draw him out about a hostile agent recently convicted in court or
otherwise known to the witness. During the next interrogation session with the
source, a part of the taped denunciation can be played back to him if necessary.
Or the witnesses' remarks about the known spy, edited as necessary, can be so
played back that the interrogatee is persuaded that he is the subject of the
remarks.
69 [page break]
like these I'll have to hand him over to the local police for trial." On hearing
these remarks, the interrogatee may confess the truth about the lesser guilt in
order to avoid heavier punishment. If he continues to withhold, the interrogator
may take his side by stating, "You know, I'm not at all convinced that so-and-
so told a straight story. I feel, personally, that he was exaggerating a great deal.
Wasn't he? What's the true story?"
7. Joint Suspects
If the interrogator is quite certain of the facts in the case but cannot secure an
admission from either A or B, a written confession may be prepared and A's
signature may be reproduced on it. (It is helpful if B can recognize A's
signature, but not essential.) The confession contains the salient facts, but they
are distorted; the confession shows that A is attempting to throw the entire
responsibility upon B. Edited tape recordings which sound as though A had
denounced B may also be used for the purpose, separately or in conjunction
with the written "confession." If A is feeling a little ill or dispirited, he can also
be led past a window or otherwise shown to B without creating a chance for
conversation; B is likely to interpret A's hang-dog look as evidence of
confession and denunciation. (It is important that in all such gambits, A be the
weaker of the two, emotionally and psychologically.) B then reads (or hears)
A's "confession." If B persists in withholding, the
70 [page break]
interrogator should dismiss him promptly, saying that A's signed confession is
sufficient for the purpose and that it does not matter whether B corroborates it
or not. At the following session with B, the interrogator selects some minor
matter, not substantively damaging to B but nevertheless exaggerated, and
says, "I'm not sure A was really fair to you here. Would you care to tell me
your side of the story?" If B rises to this bait, the interrogator moves on to
areas of greater significance.
When, in the judgment of the interrogator, B is fairly well convinced that A has
broken down and told his story, the interrogator may elect to say to B, "Now
that A has come clean with us, I'd like to let him go. But I hate to release one
of you before the other; you ought to get out at the same time. A seems to be
pretty angry with you -- feels that you got him into this jam. He might even go
back to your Soviet case officer and say
71 [page break]
that you haven't returned because you agreed to stay here and work for us.
Wouldn't it be better for you if I set you both free together? Wouldn't it be
better to tell me your side of the story?"
8. Ivan Is a Dope
It may be useful to point out to a hostile agent that the cover story was ill-
contrived, that the other service botched the job, that it is typical of the other
service to ignore the welfare of its agents. The interrogator may personalize
this pitch by explaining that he has been impressed by the agent's courage and
intelligence. He sells the agent the idea that the interrogator, not his old
service, represents a true friend, who understands him and will look after his
welfare.
9. Joint Interrogators
the floor or holding his nose or any gross gesture. Finally, red-faced and
furious, he says, "I'm going to take a break, have a couple of stiff drinks. But
I'll be back at two -- and you, you bum, you better be ready to talk." When the
door slams behind him, the second interrogator tells the subject how sorry he
is, how he hates to work with a man like that but has no choice, how if maybe
brutes like that would keep quiet and give a man a fair chance to tell his side of
the story, etc., etc.
An interrogator working alone can also use the Mutt-and-Jeff technique. After
a number of tense and hostile sessions the interrogatee is ushered into a
different or refurnished room with comfortable furniture, cigarettes, etc. The
interrogator invites him to sit down and explains his regret that the source's
former stubbornness forced the interrogator to use such tactics. Now
everything will be different. The interrogator talks man-to-man. An American
POW, debriefed on his interrogation by a hostile service that used this
approach, has described the result: "Well, I went in and there was a man, an
officer he was... -- he asked me to sit down and was very friendly.... It was
very terrific. I, well, I almost felt like I had a friend sitting there. I had to stop
every now and then and realize that this man wasn't a friend of mine.... I also
felt as though I couldn't be rude to him.... It was much more difficult for me to -
- well, I almost felt I had as much responsibility to talk to him and reason and
justification as I have to talk to you right now."(18)
Another joint technique casts both interrogators in friendly roles. But whereas
the interrogator in charge is sincere, the second interrogator's manner and voice
convey the impression that he is merely pretending sympathy in order to trap
the interrogated. He slips in a few trick questions of the "When-did-you-stop-
beating-your-wife?" category. The interrogator in charge warns his colleague
to desist. When he repeats the tactics, the interrogator in charge says, with a
slight show of anger, "We're not here to trap people but to get at the truth. I
suggest that you leave now. I'll handle this."
73 [page break]
Language
If the recalcitrant subject speaks more than one language, it is better to
question him in the tongue with which he is least familiar as long as the
purpose of interrogation is to obtain a confession. After the interrogatee admits
hostile intent or activity, a switch to the better-known language will facilitate
follow-up.
74 [page break]
75 [page break]
Alice in Wonderland
The confusion technique is designed not only to obliterate the familiar but to
replace it with the weird. Although this method can be employed by a single
interrogator, it is better adapted to use by two or three. When the subject enters
the room, the first interrogator asks a doubletalk question -- one which seems
straightforward but is essentially nonsensical. Whether the interrogatee tries to
answer or not, the second interrogator follows up (interrupting any attempted
response) with a wholly unrelated and equally illogical query. Sometimes two
or more questions are asked simultaneously. Pitch, tone, and volume of the
interrogators' voices are unrelated to the import of the questions. No pattern of
questions and answers is permitted to develop, nor do the questions themselves
relate logically to each other. In this strange atmosphere the subject finds that
the pattern of speech and thought which he has learned to consider normal
have been replaced by an eerie meaninglessness. The interrogatee may start
laughing or refuse to take the situation seriously. But as the process continues,
day after day if necessary, the subject begins to try to make sense of the
situation, which becomes mentally intolerable. Now he is likely to make
significant admissions, or even to pour out his story, just to stop the flow of
babble which assails him. This technique may be especially effective with the
orderly, obstinate type.
Regression
76 [page break]
77 [page break]
in this instance, the prisoner's hand actually does become warm, a problem
easily resolved by the use of a concealed diathermy machine. Or it might be
suggested... that... a cigarette will taste bitter. Here again, he could be given a
cigarette prepared to have a slight but noticeably bitter taste." In discussing
states of heightened suggestibility (which are not, however, states of trance)
Orne says, "Both hypnosis and some of the drugs inducing hypnoidal states are
popularly viewed as situations where the individual is no longer master of his
own fate and therefore not responsible for his actions. It seems possible then
that the hypnotic situation, as distinguished from hypnosis itself, might be used
to relieve the individual of a feeling of responsibility for his own actions and
thus lead him to reveal information."(7)
78 [page break]
The Polygraph
The polygraph can be used for purposes other than the evaluation of veracity.
For example, it may be used as an adjunct in testing the range of languages
spoken by an interrogatee or his sophistication in intelligence matters, for rapid
screening to determine broad areas of knowledgeability, and as an aid in the
psychological assessment of sources. Its primary function in a
counterintelligence interrogation, however, is to provide a further means of
testing for deception or withholding.
Although the polygraph has been a valuable aid, no interrogator should feel
that it can carry his responsibility for him. [approx. 7 lines deleted] (9)
The best results are obtained when the CI interrogator and the polygraph
operator work closely together in laying the groundwork for technical
examination. The operator needs all available information about the personality
of the source, as well as the operational background and reasons for suspicion.
The CI interrogator in turn can cooperate more effectively and can fit the
results of technical examination more accurately into
79 [page break]
The galvanic skin response is one of the most easily triggered reactions, but
recovery after the reaction is slow, and "... in a routine examination the next
question is likely to be introduced before recovery is complete. Partly because
of this fact there is an adapting trend in the GSR with stimuli repeated every
few minutes the response gets smaller, other things being equal."
80 [page break]
stantiated this theory. The theory of conflict presumes that a large physiologic
disturbance occurs when the subject is caught between his habitual inclination
to tell the truth and his strong desire not to divulge a certain set of facts. Davis
suggests that if this concept is valid, it holds only if the conflict is intense. The
threat-of-punishment theory maintains that a large physiologic response
accompanies lying because the subject fears the consequence of failing to
deceive. "In common language it might be said that he fails to deceive the
machine operator for the very reason that he fears he will fail. The 'fear' would
be the very reaction detected." This third theory is more widely held than the
other two. Interrogators should note the inference that a resistant source who
does not fear that detection of lying will result in a punishment of which he is
afraid would not, according to this theory, produce significant responses.
Graphology
81 [page break]
The purpose of this part of the handbook is to present basic information about
coercive techniques available for use in the interrogation situation. It is vital
that this discussion not be misconstrued as constituting authorization for the
use of coercion at field discretion . As was noted earlier, there is no such
blanket authorization.
For both ethical and pragmatic reasons no interrogator may take upon himself
the unilateral responsibility for using coercive methods. Concealing from the
interrogator's superiors an intent to resort to coercion, or its unapproved
employment, does not protect them. It places them, and KUBARK, in
unconsidered jeopardy.
Coercive procedures are designed not only to exploit the resistant source's
internal conflicts and induce him to wrestle with himself but also to bring a
superior outside force to bear upon the subject's resistance. Non-coercive
methods are not
82 [page break]
likely to succeed if their selection and use is not predicated upon an accurate
psychological assessment of the source. In contrast, the same coercive method
may succeed against persons who are very unlike each other. The changes of
success rise steeply, nevertheless, if the coercive technique is matched to the
source's personality. Individuals react differently even to such seemingly non-
discriminatory stimuli as drugs. Moreover, it is a waste of time and energy to
apply strong pressures on a hit-or-miss basis if a tap on the psychological
jugular will produce compliance.
Farber says that the response to coercion typically contains "... at least three
important elements: debility, dependency, and dread." Prisoners "... have
reduced viability, are helplessly dependent on their captors for the
83 [page break]
satisfaction of their many basic needs, and experience the emotional and
motivational reactions of intense fear and anxiety.... Among the [American]
POW's pressured by the Chinese Communists, the DDD syndrome in its full-
blown form constituted a state of discomfort that was well-nigh intolerable."
(11). If the debility-dependency-dread state is unduly prolonged, however, the
arrestee may sink into a defensive apathy from which it is hard to arouse him.
The profound moral objection to applying duress past the point of irreversible
psychological damage has been stated. Judging the validity of other ethical
arguments about coercion exceeds the scope of this paper. What is fully clear,
however, is that controlled coercive manipulation of an interrogatee may
impair his ability to make fine distinctions but will not alter his ability to
answer correctly such gross questions as "Are you a Soviet agent? What is
your assignment now? Who is your present case officer?"
84 [page break]
When an interrogator senses that the subject's resistance is wavering, that his
desire to yield is growing stronger than his wish to continue his resistance, the
time has come to provide him with the acceptable rationalization: a face-saving
reason or excuse for compliance. Novice interrogators may be tempted to seize
upon the initial yielding triumphantly and to personalize the victory. Such a
temptation must be rejected immediately. An interrogation is not a game
played by two people, one to become the winner and the other the loser. It is
simply a method of obtaining correct and useful information. Therefore the
interrogator should intensify the subject's desire to cease struggling by showing
him how he can do so without seeming to abandon principle, self-protection, or
other initial causes of resistance. If, instead of providing the right
rationalization at the right time, the interrogator seizes gloatingly upon the
subject's wavering, opposition will stiffen again.
C. Arrest
85 [page break]
D. Detention
The point is that man's sense of identity depends upon a continuity in his
surroundings, habits, appearance, actions, relations with others, etc. Detention
permits the interrogator to cut through these links and throw the interrogatee
back upon his own unaided internal resources.
86 [page break]
determine his diet, sleep pattern, and other fundamentals. Manipulating these
into irregularities, so that the subject becomes disorientated, is very likely to
create feelings of fear and helplessness. Hinkle points out, "People who enter
prison with attitudes of foreboding, apprehension, and helplessness generally
do less well than those who enter with assurance and a conviction that they can
deal with anything that they may encounter.... Some people who are afraid of
losing sleep, or who do not wish to lose sleep, soon succumb to sleep loss...."
(7)
In short, the prisoner should not be provided a routine to which he can adapt
and from which he can draw some comfort -- or at least a sense of his own
identity. Everyone has read of prisoners who were reluctant to leave their cells
after prolonged incarceration. Little is known about the duration of
confinement calculated to make a subject shift from anxiety, coupled with a
desire for sensory stimuli and human companionship, to a passive, apathetic
acceptance of isolation and an ultimate pleasure in this negative state.
Undoubtedly the rate of change is determined almost entirely by the
psychological characteristics of the individual. In any event, it is advisable to
keep the subject upset by constant disruptions of patterns.
For this reason, it is useful to determine whether the interrogattee has been
jailed before, how often, under what circumstances, for how long, and whether
he was subjected to earlier interrogation. Familiarity with confinement and
even with isolation reduces the effect.
87 [page break]
the greatest fears and hence the greatest danger of giving way to symptoms;
previous experience is a powerful aid in going ahead, despite the symptoms.
"The symptoms most commonly produced by isolation are superstition, intense
love of any other living thing, perceiving inanimate objects as alive,
hallucinations, and delusions." (26)
The apparent reason for these effects is that a person cut off from external
stimuli turns his awareness inward, upon himself, and then projects the
contents of his own unconscious outwards, so that he endows his faceless
environment with his own attributes, fears, and forgotten memories. Lilly
notes, "It is obvious that inner factors in the mind tend to be projected outward,
that some of the mind's activity which is usually reality-bound now becomes
free to turn to phantasy and ultimately to hallucination and delusion."
At the National Institute of Mental Health two subjects were "... suspended
with the body and all but the top of the head immersed in a tank containing
slowly flowing water at 34.5 [degrees] C (94.5 [degrees] F)...." Both subjects
wore black-out masks, which enclosed the whole head but allowed breathing
and nothing else. The sound level was extremely low; the subject heard only
his own breathing and some faint sounds of water from the piping. Neither
subject stayed in the tank longer than three hours. Both passed quickly from
normally directed thinking through a tension resulting from unsatisfied hunger
for sensory stimuli and concentration upon the few available sensations to
private reveries and fantasies and eventually to visual imagery somewhat
resembling hallucinations.
88 [page break]
"In our experiments, we notice that after immersion the day apparently is
started over, i. e., the subject feels as if he has risen from bed afresh; this effect
persists, and the subject finds he is out of step with the clock for the rest of the
day."
"Three studies suggest that the more well-adjusted or 'normal' the subject is,
the more he is affected by deprivation of sensory stimuli. Neurotic and
psychotic subjects are either comparatively unaffected or show decreases in
anxiety, hallucinations, etc." (7)
89 [page break]
These findings suggest - but by no means prove - the following theories about
solitary confinement and isolation:
3. The interrogator can benefit from the subject's anxiety. As the interrogator
becomes linked in the subject's mind with the reward of lessened anxiety,
human contact, and meaningful activity, and thus with providing relief for
growing discomfort, the questioner assumes a benevolent role. (7)
90 [page break]
The effectiveness of a threat depends not only on what sort of person the
interrogatee is and whether he believes that his questioner can and will carry
the threat out but also on the interrogator's reasons for threatening. If the
interrogator threatens because he is angry, the subject frequently senses the
fear of failure underlying the anger and is strengthened in his own resolve to
resist. Threats delivered coldly are more effective than those shouted in rage. It
is especially important that a threat not be uttered in response to the
interrogatee's own expressions of hostility. These, if ignored, can induce
feelings of guilt, whereas retorts in kind relieve the subject's feelings.
Another reason why threats induce compliance not evoked by the inflection of
duress is that the threat grants the interrogatee time for compliance. It is not
enough that a resistant source should placed under the tension of fear; he must
also discern an acceptable escape route. Biderman observes, "Not only can the
shame or guilt of defeat in the encounter with the interrogator be involved, but
also the more fundamental injunction to protect one's self-autonomy or 'will'....
A simple defense against threats to the self from the anticipation of being
forced to comply is, of course, to comply 'deliberately' or 'voluntarily'.... To the
extent that the foregoing interpretation holds, the more intensely motivated the
[interrogatee] is to resist, the more intense is the pressure toward early
compliance from such anxieties, for the greater is the threat to self-esteem
which is involved in contemplating the possibility of being 'forced to'
comply...." (6) In brief, the threat is like all other coercive techniques in being
most effective when so used as to foster regression and when joined with a
suggested way out of the dilemma, a rationalization acceptable to the
interrogatee.
91 [page break]
The threat of death has often been found to be worse than useless. It "has the
highest position in law as a defense, but in many interrogation situations it is a
highly ineffective threat. Many prisoners, in fact, have refused to yield in the
face of such threats who have subsequently been 'broken' by other procedures."
(3) The principal reason is that the ultimate threat is likely to induce sheer
hopelessness if the interrogatee does not believe that it is a trick; he feels that
he is as likely to be condemned after compliance as before. The threat of death
is also ineffective when used against hard-headed types who realize that
silencing them forever would defeat the interrogator's purpose. If the threat is
recognized as a bluff, it will not only fail but also pave the way to failure for
later coercive ruses used by the interrogator.
G. Debility
92 [page break]
H. Pain
Everyone is aware that people react very differently to pain. The reason,
apparently, is not a physical difference in the intensity of the sensation itself.
Lawrence E. Hinkle observes, "The sensation of pain seems to be roughly
equal in all men, that is to say, all people have approximately the same
threshold at which they begin to feel pain, and when carefully graded stimuli
are applied to them, their estimates of severity are approximately the same....
Yet... when men are very highly motivated... they have been known to carry
out rather complex tasks while enduring the most intense pain." He also states,
"In general, it appears that whatever may be the role of the constitutional
endowment in determining the reaction to pain, it is a much less important
determinant than is the attitude of the man who experiences the pain." (7)
93 [page break]
it less, and react less, than one whose distress is heightened by fear of the
unknown. The individual remains the determinant.
It has been plausibly suggested that, whereas pain inflicted on a person from
outside himself may actually focus or intensify his will to resist, his resistance
is likelier to be sapped by pain which he seems to inflict upon himself. "In the
simple torture situation the contest is one between the individual and his
tormentor (.... and he can frequently endure). When the individual is told to
stand at attention for long periods, an intervening factor is introduced. The
immediate source of pain is not the interrogator but the victim himself. The
motivational strength of the individual is likely to exhaust itself in this internal
encounter.... As long as the subject remains standing, he is attributing to his
captor the power to do something worse to him, but there is actually no
showdown of the ability of the interrogator to do so." (4)
Interrogatees who are withholding but who feel qualms of guilt and a secret
desire to yield are likely to become intractable if made to endure pain. The
reason is that they can then interpret the pain as punishment and hence as
expiation. There are also persons who enjoy pain and its anticipation and who
will keep back information that they might otherwise divulge if they are given
reason to expect that withholding will result in the punishment that they want.
Persons of considerable moral or intellectual stature often find in pain inflicted
by others a confirmation of the belief that they are in the hands of inferiors, and
their resolve not to submit is strengthened.
94 [page break]
But despite the fact that hypnosis has been an object of scientific inquiry for a
very long time, none of these theories has yet been tested adequately. Each of
them is in conflict with some observations of fact. In any event, an
interrogation handbook cannot and need not include a lengthy discussion of
hypnosis. The case officer or interrogator needs to know enough about the
subject to understand the circumstances under which hypnosis can be a useful
tool, so that he can request expert assistance appropriately.
95 [page break]
lay practitioner does not know enough to use the technique safely. The second
reason is that an unsuccessful attempt to hypnotize a subject for purposes of
interrogation, or a successful attempt not adequately covered by post-hypnotic
amnesia or other protection, can easily lead to lurid and embarrassing publicity
or legal charges.
Martin T. Orne has written at some length about hypnosis and interrogation.
Almost all of his conclusions are tentatively negative. Concerning the role
played by the will or attitude of the interrogates, Orne says, "Although the
crucial experiment has not yet been done, there is little or no evidence to
indicate that trance can be induced against a person's wishes." He adds, "...the
actual occurrence of the trance state is related to the wish of the subject to enter
hypnosis." And he also observes, "...whether a subject will or will not enter
trance depends upon his relationship with the hyponotist rather than upon the
technical procedure of trance induction." These views are probably
representative of those of many psychologists, but they are not definitive. As
Orne himself later points out, the interrogatee "... could be given a hypnotic
drug with appropriate verbal suggestions to talk about a given topic. Eventually
enough of the drug
96 [page break]
Orne also holds that even if a resister can be hypnotized, his resistance does
not cease. He postulates "... that only in rare interrogation subjects would a
sufficiently deep trance be obtainable to even attempt to induce the subject to
discuss material which he is unwilling to discuss in the waking state. The kind
of information which can be obtained in these rare instances is still an
unanswered question." He adds that it is doubtful that a subject in trance could
be made to reveal information which he wished to safeguard. But here too
Orne seems somewhat too cautious or pessimistic. Once an interrogatee is in a
hypnotic trance, his understanding of reality becomes subject to manipulation.
For example, a KUBARK interrogator could tell a suspect double agent in
trance that the KGB is conducting the questioning, and thus invert the whole
frame of reference. In other words, Orne is probably right in holding that most
recalcitrant subjects will continue effective resistance as long as the frame of
reference is undisturbed. But once the subject is tricked into believing that he is
talking to friend rather than foe, or that divulging the truth is the best way to
serve his own purposes, his resistance will be replaced by cooperation. The
value of hypnotic trance is not that it permits the interrogator to impose his will
but rather that it can be used to convince the interrogatee that there is no valid
reason not to be forthcoming.
97 [page break]
A third objection raised by Orne and others is that material elicited during
trance is not reliable. Orne says, "... it has been shown that the accuracy of
such information... would not be guaranteed since subjects in hypnosis are
fully capable of lying." Again, the observation is correct; no known
manipulative method guarantees veracity. But if hypnosis is employed not as
an immediate instrument for digging out the truth but rather as a way of
making the subject want to align himself with his interrogators, the objection
evaporates.
J. Narcosis
Just as the threat of pain may more effectively induce compliance than its
infliction, so an interrogatee's mistaken belief that he has been drugged may
make him a more useful interrogation subject than he would be under narcosis.
Louis A. Gottschalk cites a group of studies as indicating "that 30 to 50 per
cent of individuals are placebo reactors, that is, respond
98 [page break]
Drugs are no more the answer to the interrogator's prayer than the polygraph,
hypnosis, or other aids. Studies and reports "dealing with the validity of
material extracted from reluctant informants... indicate that there is no drug
which can force every informant to report all the information he has. Not only
may the inveterate criminal psychopath lie under the influence of drugs which
have been tested, but the relatively normal and well-adjusted individual may
also successfully disguise factual data." (3) Gottschalk reinforces the latter
observation in mentioning an experiment involving drugs which indicated that
"the more normal, well-integrated individuals could lie better than the guilt-
ridden, neurotic subjects." (7)
99 [page break]
Particularly important is the reference to matching the drug to the personality
of the interrogatee. The effect of most drugs depends more upon the
personality of the subject than upon the physical characteristics of the drugs
themselves. If the approval of Headquarters has been obtained and if a doctor
is at hand for administration, one of the most important of the interrogator's
functions is providing the doctor with a full and accurate description of the
psychological make-up of the interrogatee, to facilitate the best possible choice
of a drug.
Like other coercive media, drugs may affect the content of what an
interrogatee divulges. Gottschalk notes that certain drugs "may give rise to
psychotic manifestations such as hallucinations, illusions, delusions, or
disorientation", so that "the verbal material obtained cannot always be
considered valid." (7) For this reason drugs (and the other aids discussed in this
section) should not be used persistently to facilitate the interrogative debriefing
that follows capitulation. Their function is to cause capitulation, to aid in the
shift from resistance to cooperation. Once this shift has been accomplished,
coercive techniques should be abandoned both for moral reasons and because
they are unnecessary and even counter-productive.
This discussion does not include a list of drugs that have been employed for
interrogation purposes or a discussion of their properties because these are
medical considerations within the province of a doctor rather than an
interogator.
Most persons who feign a mental or physical illness do not know enough about
it to deceive the well-informed. Malcolm L. Meltzer says, "The detection of
malingering depends to a great extent on the simulator's failure to understand
adequately the characteristics of the role he is feigning.... Often he presents
symptoms which are exceedingly rare, existing mainly in the fancy of the
layman. One such symptom is the delusion of misidentification, characterized
by the... belief that he is some powerful or historic personage. This symptom is
very unusual in true psychosis, but is used by a number of simulators. In
schizophrenia, the onset tends to be gradual, delusions do not spring up full-
blown over night; in simulated disorders, the onset is usually fast and delusions
may be readily available. The feigned psychosis often contains many
contradictory and inconsistent symptoms, rarely existing together. The
malingerer tends to go to extremes in his portrayal of his symptoms; he
exaggerates, overdramatizes, grimaces, shouts, is overly bizarre, and calls
attention to himself in other ways....
of the behavior of lie-detector subjects, for example, showed that persons later
'proven guilty' showed certain similarities of behavior. The guilty persons were
reluctant to take the test, and they tried in various ways to postpone or delay it.
They often appeared highly anxious and sometimes took a hostile attitude
toward the test and the examiner. Evasive tactics sometimes appeared, such as
sighing, yawning, moving about, all of which foil the examiner by obscuring
the recording. Before the examination, they felt it necessary to explain why
their responses might mislead the examiner into thinking they were lying. Thus
the procedure of subjecting a suspected malingerer to a lie-detector test might
evoke behavior which would reinforce the suspicion of fraud." (7)
Meltzer also notes that malingerers who are not professional psychologists can
usually be exposed through Rorschach tests.
L. Conclusion
A brief summary of the foregoing may help to pull the major concepts of
coercive interrogation together:
4. When regression has proceeded far enough so that the subject's desire to
yield begins to overbalance his resistance, the interrogator should supply a face-
saving rationalization. Like the coercive technique, the rationalization must be
carefully chosen to fit the subject's personality.
The purpose of this part of the handbook is to present basic information about
coercive techniques available for use in the interrogation situation. It is vital
that this discussion not be misconstrued as constituting authorization for the
use of coercion at field discretion . As was noted earlier, there is no such
blanket authorization.
For both ethical and pragmatic reasons no interrogator may take upon himself
the unilateral responsibility for using coercive methods. Concealing from the
interrogator's superiors an intent to resort to coercion, or its unapproved
employment, does not protect them. It places them, and KUBARK, in
unconsidered jeopardy.
Coercive procedures are designed not only to exploit the resistant source's
internal conflicts and induce him to wrestle with himself but also to bring a
superior outside force to bear upon the subject's resistance. Non-coercive
methods are not
82 [page break]
likely to succeed if their selection and use is not predicated upon an accurate
psychological assessment of the source. In contrast, the same coercive method
may succeed against persons who are very unlike each other. The changes of
success rise steeply, nevertheless, if the coercive technique is matched to the
source's personality. Individuals react differently even to such seemingly non-
discriminatory stimuli as drugs. Moreover, it is a waste of time and energy to
apply strong pressures on a hit-or-miss basis if a tap on the psychological
jugular will produce compliance.
Farber says that the response to coercion typically contains "... at least three
important elements: debility, dependency, and dread." Prisoners "... have
reduced viability, are helplessly dependent on their captors for the
83 [page break]
satisfaction of their many basic needs, and experience the emotional and
motivational reactions of intense fear and anxiety.... Among the [American]
POW's pressured by the Chinese Communists, the DDD syndrome in its full-
blown form constituted a state of discomfort that was well-nigh intolerable."
(11). If the debility-dependency-dread state is unduly prolonged, however, the
arrestee may sink into a defensive apathy from which it is hard to arouse him.
The profound moral objection to applying duress past the point of irreversible
psychological damage has been stated. Judging the validity of other ethical
arguments about coercion exceeds the scope of this paper. What is fully clear,
however, is that controlled coercive manipulation of an interrogatee may
impair his ability to make fine distinctions but will not alter his ability to
answer correctly such gross questions as "Are you a Soviet agent? What is
your assignment now? Who is your present case officer?"
84 [page break]
When an interrogator senses that the subject's resistance is wavering, that his
desire to yield is growing stronger than his wish to continue his resistance, the
time has come to provide him with the acceptable rationalization: a face-saving
reason or excuse for compliance. Novice interrogators may be tempted to seize
upon the initial yielding triumphantly and to personalize the victory. Such a
temptation must be rejected immediately. An interrogation is not a game
played by two people, one to become the winner and the other the loser. It is
simply a method of obtaining correct and useful information. Therefore the
interrogator should intensify the subject's desire to cease struggling by showing
him how he can do so without seeming to abandon principle, self-protection, or
other initial causes of resistance. If, instead of providing the right
rationalization at the right time, the interrogator seizes gloatingly upon the
subject's wavering, opposition will stiffen again.
C. Arrest
85 [page break]
D. Detention
The point is that man's sense of identity depends upon a continuity in his
surroundings, habits, appearance, actions, relations with others, etc. Detention
permits the interrogator to cut through these links and throw the interrogatee
back upon his own unaided internal resources.
86 [page break]
determine his diet, sleep pattern, and other fundamentals. Manipulating these
into irregularities, so that the subject becomes disorientated, is very likely to
create feelings of fear and helplessness. Hinkle points out, "People who enter
prison with attitudes of foreboding, apprehension, and helplessness generally
do less well than those who enter with assurance and a conviction that they can
deal with anything that they may encounter.... Some people who are afraid of
losing sleep, or who do not wish to lose sleep, soon succumb to sleep loss...."
(7)
In short, the prisoner should not be provided a routine to which he can adapt
and from which he can draw some comfort -- or at least a sense of his own
identity. Everyone has read of prisoners who were reluctant to leave their cells
after prolonged incarceration. Little is known about the duration of
confinement calculated to make a subject shift from anxiety, coupled with a
desire for sensory stimuli and human companionship, to a passive, apathetic
acceptance of isolation and an ultimate pleasure in this negative state.
Undoubtedly the rate of change is determined almost entirely by the
psychological characteristics of the individual. In any event, it is advisable to
keep the subject upset by constant disruptions of patterns.
For this reason, it is useful to determine whether the interrogattee has been
jailed before, how often, under what circumstances, for how long, and whether
he was subjected to earlier interrogation. Familiarity with confinement and
even with isolation reduces the effect.
87 [page break]
the greatest fears and hence the greatest danger of giving way to symptoms;
previous experience is a powerful aid in going ahead, despite the symptoms.
"The symptoms most commonly produced by isolation are superstition, intense
love of any other living thing, perceiving inanimate objects as alive,
hallucinations, and delusions." (26)
The apparent reason for these effects is that a person cut off from external
stimuli turns his awareness inward, upon himself, and then projects the
contents of his own unconscious outwards, so that he endows his faceless
environment with his own attributes, fears, and forgotten memories. Lilly
notes, "It is obvious that inner factors in the mind tend to be projected outward,
that some of the mind's activity which is usually reality-bound now becomes
free to turn to phantasy and ultimately to hallucination and delusion."
At the National Institute of Mental Health two subjects were "... suspended
with the body and all but the top of the head immersed in a tank containing
slowly flowing water at 34.5 [degrees] C (94.5 [degrees] F)...." Both subjects
wore black-out masks, which enclosed the whole head but allowed breathing
and nothing else. The sound level was extremely low; the subject heard only
his own breathing and some faint sounds of water from the piping. Neither
subject stayed in the tank longer than three hours. Both passed quickly from
normally directed thinking through a tension resulting from unsatisfied hunger
for sensory stimuli and concentration upon the few available sensations to
private reveries and fantasies and eventually to visual imagery somewhat
resembling hallucinations.
88 [page break]
"In our experiments, we notice that after immersion the day apparently is
started over, i. e., the subject feels as if he has risen from bed afresh; this effect
persists, and the subject finds he is out of step with the clock for the rest of the
day."
"Three studies suggest that the more well-adjusted or 'normal' the subject is,
the more he is affected by deprivation of sensory stimuli. Neurotic and
psychotic subjects are either comparatively unaffected or show decreases in
anxiety, hallucinations, etc." (7)
89 [page break]
These findings suggest - but by no means prove - the following theories about
solitary confinement and isolation:
3. The interrogator can benefit from the subject's anxiety. As the interrogator
becomes linked in the subject's mind with the reward of lessened anxiety,
human contact, and meaningful activity, and thus with providing relief for
growing discomfort, the questioner assumes a benevolent role. (7)
90 [page break]
The effectiveness of a threat depends not only on what sort of person the
interrogatee is and whether he believes that his questioner can and will carry
the threat out but also on the interrogator's reasons for threatening. If the
interrogator threatens because he is angry, the subject frequently senses the
fear of failure underlying the anger and is strengthened in his own resolve to
resist. Threats delivered coldly are more effective than those shouted in rage. It
is especially important that a threat not be uttered in response to the
interrogatee's own expressions of hostility. These, if ignored, can induce
feelings of guilt, whereas retorts in kind relieve the subject's feelings.
Another reason why threats induce compliance not evoked by the inflection of
duress is that the threat grants the interrogatee time for compliance. It is not
enough that a resistant source should placed under the tension of fear; he must
also discern an acceptable escape route. Biderman observes, "Not only can the
shame or guilt of defeat in the encounter with the interrogator be involved, but
also the more fundamental injunction to protect one's self-autonomy or 'will'....
A simple defense against threats to the self from the anticipation of being
forced to comply is, of course, to comply 'deliberately' or 'voluntarily'.... To the
extent that the foregoing interpretation holds, the more intensely motivated the
[interrogatee] is to resist, the more intense is the pressure toward early
compliance from such anxieties, for the greater is the threat to self-esteem
which is involved in contemplating the possibility of being 'forced to'
comply...." (6) In brief, the threat is like all other coercive techniques in being
most effective when so used as to foster regression and when joined with a
suggested way out of the dilemma, a rationalization acceptable to the
interrogatee.
91 [page break]
The threat of death has often been found to be worse than useless. It "has the
highest position in law as a defense, but in many interrogation situations it is a
highly ineffective threat. Many prisoners, in fact, have refused to yield in the
face of such threats who have subsequently been 'broken' by other procedures."
(3) The principal reason is that the ultimate threat is likely to induce sheer
hopelessness if the interrogatee does not believe that it is a trick; he feels that
he is as likely to be condemned after compliance as before. The threat of death
is also ineffective when used against hard-headed types who realize that
silencing them forever would defeat the interrogator's purpose. If the threat is
recognized as a bluff, it will not only fail but also pave the way to failure for
later coercive ruses used by the interrogator.
G. Debility
92 [page break]
H. Pain
Everyone is aware that people react very differently to pain. The reason,
apparently, is not a physical difference in the intensity of the sensation itself.
Lawrence E. Hinkle observes, "The sensation of pain seems to be roughly
equal in all men, that is to say, all people have approximately the same
threshold at which they begin to feel pain, and when carefully graded stimuli
are applied to them, their estimates of severity are approximately the same....
Yet... when men are very highly motivated... they have been known to carry
out rather complex tasks while enduring the most intense pain." He also states,
"In general, it appears that whatever may be the role of the constitutional
endowment in determining the reaction to pain, it is a much less important
determinant than is the attitude of the man who experiences the pain." (7)
93 [page break]
it less, and react less, than one whose distress is heightened by fear of the
unknown. The individual remains the determinant.
It has been plausibly suggested that, whereas pain inflicted on a person from
outside himself may actually focus or intensify his will to resist, his resistance
is likelier to be sapped by pain which he seems to inflict upon himself. "In the
simple torture situation the contest is one between the individual and his
tormentor (.... and he can frequently endure). When the individual is told to
stand at attention for long periods, an intervening factor is introduced. The
immediate source of pain is not the interrogator but the victim himself. The
motivational strength of the individual is likely to exhaust itself in this internal
encounter.... As long as the subject remains standing, he is attributing to his
captor the power to do something worse to him, but there is actually no
showdown of the ability of the interrogator to do so." (4)
Interrogatees who are withholding but who feel qualms of guilt and a secret
desire to yield are likely to become intractable if made to endure pain. The
reason is that they can then interpret the pain as punishment and hence as
expiation. There are also persons who enjoy pain and its anticipation and who
will keep back information that they might otherwise divulge if they are given
reason to expect that withholding will result in the punishment that they want.
Persons of considerable moral or intellectual stature often find in pain inflicted
by others a confirmation of the belief that they are in the hands of inferiors, and
their resolve not to submit is strengthened.
94 [page break]
But despite the fact that hypnosis has been an object of scientific inquiry for a
very long time, none of these theories has yet been tested adequately. Each of
them is in conflict with some observations of fact. In any event, an
interrogation handbook cannot and need not include a lengthy discussion of
hypnosis. The case officer or interrogator needs to know enough about the
subject to understand the circumstances under which hypnosis can be a useful
tool, so that he can request expert assistance appropriately.
95 [page break]
lay practitioner does not know enough to use the technique safely. The second
reason is that an unsuccessful attempt to hypnotize a subject for purposes of
interrogation, or a successful attempt not adequately covered by post-hypnotic
amnesia or other protection, can easily lead to lurid and embarrassing publicity
or legal charges.
Martin T. Orne has written at some length about hypnosis and interrogation.
Almost all of his conclusions are tentatively negative. Concerning the role
played by the will or attitude of the interrogates, Orne says, "Although the
crucial experiment has not yet been done, there is little or no evidence to
indicate that trance can be induced against a person's wishes." He adds, "...the
actual occurrence of the trance state is related to the wish of the subject to enter
hypnosis." And he also observes, "...whether a subject will or will not enter
trance depends upon his relationship with the hyponotist rather than upon the
technical procedure of trance induction." These views are probably
representative of those of many psychologists, but they are not definitive. As
Orne himself later points out, the interrogatee "... could be given a hypnotic
drug with appropriate verbal suggestions to talk about a given topic. Eventually
enough of the drug
96 [page break]
Orne also holds that even if a resister can be hypnotized, his resistance does
not cease. He postulates "... that only in rare interrogation subjects would a
sufficiently deep trance be obtainable to even attempt to induce the subject to
discuss material which he is unwilling to discuss in the waking state. The kind
of information which can be obtained in these rare instances is still an
unanswered question." He adds that it is doubtful that a subject in trance could
be made to reveal information which he wished to safeguard. But here too
Orne seems somewhat too cautious or pessimistic. Once an interrogatee is in a
hypnotic trance, his understanding of reality becomes subject to manipulation.
For example, a KUBARK interrogator could tell a suspect double agent in
trance that the KGB is conducting the questioning, and thus invert the whole
frame of reference. In other words, Orne is probably right in holding that most
recalcitrant subjects will continue effective resistance as long as the frame of
reference is undisturbed. But once the subject is tricked into believing that he is
talking to friend rather than foe, or that divulging the truth is the best way to
serve his own purposes, his resistance will be replaced by cooperation. The
value of hypnotic trance is not that it permits the interrogator to impose his will
but rather that it can be used to convince the interrogatee that there is no valid
reason not to be forthcoming.
97 [page break]
A third objection raised by Orne and others is that material elicited during
trance is not reliable. Orne says, "... it has been shown that the accuracy of
such information... would not be guaranteed since subjects in hypnosis are
fully capable of lying." Again, the observation is correct; no known
manipulative method guarantees veracity. But if hypnosis is employed not as
an immediate instrument for digging out the truth but rather as a way of
making the subject want to align himself with his interrogators, the objection
evaporates.
J. Narcosis
Just as the threat of pain may more effectively induce compliance than its
infliction, so an interrogatee's mistaken belief that he has been drugged may
make him a more useful interrogation subject than he would be under narcosis.
Louis A. Gottschalk cites a group of studies as indicating "that 30 to 50 per
cent of individuals are placebo reactors, that is, respond
98 [page break]
Drugs are no more the answer to the interrogator's prayer than the polygraph,
hypnosis, or other aids. Studies and reports "dealing with the validity of
material extracted from reluctant informants... indicate that there is no drug
which can force every informant to report all the information he has. Not only
may the inveterate criminal psychopath lie under the influence of drugs which
have been tested, but the relatively normal and well-adjusted individual may
also successfully disguise factual data." (3) Gottschalk reinforces the latter
observation in mentioning an experiment involving drugs which indicated that
"the more normal, well-integrated individuals could lie better than the guilt-
ridden, neurotic subjects." (7)
99 [page break]
Particularly important is the reference to matching the drug to the personality
of the interrogatee. The effect of most drugs depends more upon the
personality of the subject than upon the physical characteristics of the drugs
themselves. If the approval of Headquarters has been obtained and if a doctor
is at hand for administration, one of the most important of the interrogator's
functions is providing the doctor with a full and accurate description of the
psychological make-up of the interrogatee, to facilitate the best possible choice
of a drug.
Like other coercive media, drugs may affect the content of what an
interrogatee divulges. Gottschalk notes that certain drugs "may give rise to
psychotic manifestations such as hallucinations, illusions, delusions, or
disorientation", so that "the verbal material obtained cannot always be
considered valid." (7) For this reason drugs (and the other aids discussed in this
section) should not be used persistently to facilitate the interrogative debriefing
that follows capitulation. Their function is to cause capitulation, to aid in the
shift from resistance to cooperation. Once this shift has been accomplished,
coercive techniques should be abandoned both for moral reasons and because
they are unnecessary and even counter-productive.
This discussion does not include a list of drugs that have been employed for
interrogation purposes or a discussion of their properties because these are
medical considerations within the province of a doctor rather than an
interogator.
Most persons who feign a mental or physical illness do not know enough about
it to deceive the well-informed. Malcolm L. Meltzer says, "The detection of
malingering depends to a great extent on the simulator's failure to understand
adequately the characteristics of the role he is feigning.... Often he presents
symptoms which are exceedingly rare, existing mainly in the fancy of the
layman. One such symptom is the delusion of misidentification, characterized
by the... belief that he is some powerful or historic personage. This symptom is
very unusual in true psychosis, but is used by a number of simulators. In
schizophrenia, the onset tends to be gradual, delusions do not spring up full-
blown over night; in simulated disorders, the onset is usually fast and delusions
may be readily available. The feigned psychosis often contains many
contradictory and inconsistent symptoms, rarely existing together. The
malingerer tends to go to extremes in his portrayal of his symptoms; he
exaggerates, overdramatizes, grimaces, shouts, is overly bizarre, and calls
attention to himself in other ways....
of the behavior of lie-detector subjects, for example, showed that persons later
'proven guilty' showed certain similarities of behavior. The guilty persons were
reluctant to take the test, and they tried in various ways to postpone or delay it.
They often appeared highly anxious and sometimes took a hostile attitude
toward the test and the examiner. Evasive tactics sometimes appeared, such as
sighing, yawning, moving about, all of which foil the examiner by obscuring
the recording. Before the examination, they felt it necessary to explain why
their responses might mislead the examiner into thinking they were lying. Thus
the procedure of subjecting a suspected malingerer to a lie-detector test might
evoke behavior which would reinforce the suspicion of fraud." (7)
Meltzer also notes that malingerers who are not professional psychologists can
usually be exposed through Rorschach tests.
L. Conclusion
A brief summary of the foregoing may help to pull the major concepts of
coercive interrogation together:
4. When regression has proceeded far enough so that the subject's desire to
yield begins to overbalance his resistance, the interrogator should supply a face-
saving rationalization. Like the coercive technique, the rationalization must be
carefully chosen to fit the subject's personality.
6. Does the interrogators selected for the task meet the four criteria of (a)
adequate training and experience, (b) genuine familiarity with the language to
be used, (c) knowledge of the geographical/cultural area concerned, and (d)
psychological comprehension of the interrogatee?
7. Has the prospective interrogatee been screened? What are his major
psychological characteristics? Does he belong to one of the nine major
categories listed in pp. 19-28? Which?
8. Has all available and pertinent information about the subject been assembled
and studied?
11. Has a check of logical overt sources been conducted? Is the interrogation
necessary?
12. Have field and headquarters traces been run on the potential interrogatee
and persons closely associated with him by emotional, family, or business ties?
13. Has a preliminary assessment of bona fides been carried out? With what
results?
22. Have arrangements been made to feed, bed, and guard the subject as
necessary?
23. Does the interrogation plan call for more than one interrogator? If so, have
roles been assigned and schedules prepared?
25. What disposition is planned for the interrogatee after the questioning ends?
30. During the opening phase, have the subject's voice, eyes, mouth, gestures,
silences, or other visible clues suggested areas of sensitivity? If so, on what
topics?
32. Has the opening phase been followed by a reconnaissance? What are the
key areas of resistance? What tactics and how much pressure will be required
to overcome the resistance? Should the estimated duration of interrogation be
revised? If so, are further arrangements necessary for continued detention,
liaison support, guarding, or other purposes?
33. In the view of the interrogator, what is the emotional reaction of the subject
to the interrogator? Why?
34. Are interrogation reports being prepared after each session, from notes or
tapes?
36. Are any promises made to the interrogatee unfulfilled when questioning
ends? Is the subject vengeful? Likely to try to strike back? How?
37. If one or more of the non-coercive techniques discussed on pp. 52-81 have
been selected for use, how do they match the subject's personality?
38. Are coercive techniques to be employed? If so, have all field personnel in
the interrogator's direct chain of command
43. Are threats to be employed? As part of a plan? Has the nature of the threat
been matched to that of the interrogatee?
44. If hypnosis or drugs are thought necessary, has Headquarters been given
enough advance notice? Has adequate allowance been made for travel time and
other preliminaries?
45. Is the interrogatee suspected of malingering? If the interrogator is
uncertain, are the services of an expert available?
2. Barioux, Max, "A Method for the Selection, Training, and Evaluation of
Interviewers," Public Opinion Quarterly , Spring 1952, Vol. 16, No. 1. This
article deals with the problems of interviewers conducting public opinion polls.
It is of only slight value for interrogators, although it does suggest pitfalls
produced by asking questions that suggest their own answers.
13. Gill, Merton, Inc., and Margaret Brenman, Hypnosis and Related States:
Psychoanalytic Studies in Regression , International Universities Press Inc.,
New York, 1959. This book is a scholarly and comprehensive examination of
hypnosis. The approach is basically Freudian but the authors are neither narrow
nor doctrinaire. The book discusses the induction of hypnosis, the hypnotic
state, theories of induction and of the hypnotic condition, the concept of
regression as a basic element in hypnosis, relationships between hypnosis and
drugs, sleep, fugue, etc., and the use of hypnosis in psychotherapy.
Interrogators may find the comparison between hypnosis and "brainwashing"
in chapter 9 more relevant than other parts. The book is recommended,
however, not because it contains any discussion of the employment of hypnosis
in interrogation (it does not) but because it provides the interrogator with sound
information about what hypnosis can and cannot do.
16. Inbau, Fred E. and John E. Reid, Lie Detection and Criminal Investigation ,
Williams and Wilkin Co., 1953. The first part of this book consists of a
discussion of the polygraph. It will be more useful to the KUBARK
interrogator than the second, which deals with the elements of criminal
interrogation.
conflict) with religious experience generally and some ten Buddhistic practices
particularly. The interrogator will find little here that is not more helpfully
discussed in other sources, including Gill and Brenman's Hypnosis and Related
States . Marginal.
29. Oatis, William N. "Why I Confessed," Life , 21 September 1953, Vol. 35.
Of some marginal value because it combines the writer's profession of
innocence ("I am not a spy and never was") with an account of how he was
brought to "confess" to espionage within three days of his arrest. Although
Oatis was periodically deprived of sleep (once for 42 hours) and forced to
stand until weary, the Czechs obtained the "confessions" without torture or
starvation and without sophisticated techniques.
b. Anxiety increases the desire to be with others who share the same fear.
c. Persons who are first-born or only children are typically more nervous or
afraid than those born later. Firstborns and onlies are also "considerably less
willing or able to withstand pain than are later-born children." (p. 49.)
32. Sheehan, Robert, Police Interview and Interrogations and the Preparation
and Signing of Statements . A 23-page pamphlet, unclassified and undated, that
discusses some techniques and tricks that can be used in counterintelligence
interrogation. The style is sprightly, but most of the material is only slightly
related to KUBARK's interrogation problems. Recommended as background
reading.
33. Singer, Margaret Thaler and Edgar H. Schein, "Projective Test Responses
of Prisoners of War Following Repatriation." Psychiatry , 1958, Vol. 21. Tests
conducted on American ex-POW's returned during the Big and Little Switches
in Korea showed differences in characteristics between non-collaborators and
corroborators. The latter showed more typical and humanly responsive
reactions to psychological testing than the former, who tended to be more
apathetic and emotionally barren or withdrawn. Active resisters, however,
often showed a pattern of reaction or responsiveness like that of collaborators.
Rorschach tests provided clues, with a good statistical incidence of reliability,
for differentiation between collaborators and non-collaborators. The tests and
results described are worth noting in conjunction with the screening procedures
recommended in this paper.
34. Sullivan, Harry Stack, The Psychiatric Interview , W. W. Norton and Co.,
New York, 1954. Any interrogator reading this book will be struck by parallels
between the psychiatric interview and the interrogation. The book is also
valuable because the author, a psychiatrist of considerable repute, obviously
had a deep understanding of the nature of the inter-personal relationship and of
resistance.
35. U.S. Army, Office of the Chief of Military History, Russian Methods of
Interrogating Captured Personnel in World War II , Secret, Washington, 1951.
A comprehensive treatise on Russian intelligence and police systems and on
the history of Russian treatment of captives, military and civilian, during and
following World War II. The appendix contains some specific case summaries
of physical torture by the secret police. Only a small part of the book deals
with interrogation. Background reading.
36. U.S. Army, 7707 European Command Intelligence Center, Guide for
Intelligence Interrogators of Eastern Cases , Secret, April 1958. This
specialized study is of some marginal value for KUBARK interrogators
dealing with Russians and other Slavs.
42. Wexler, Donald, Jack Mendelson, Herbert Leiderman, and Philip Solomon,
"Sensory Deprivation," A.M.A. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry , 1958,
79, pp. 225-233. This article reports an experiment designed to test the results
of eliminating most sensory stimuli and masking others. Paid volunteers spent
periods from 1 hour and 38 minutes to 36 hours in a tank-respirator. The
results included inability to concentrate effectively, daydreaming and fantasy,
illusions, delusions, and hallucinations. The suitability of this procedure as a
means of speeding up the effects of solitary confinement upon recalcitrant
subjects has not been considered.
OTHER BIBLIOGRAPHIES
(except sub-titles) are included. The range is broad, so that a number of nearly-
irrelevant titles are included (e.g., Employment psychology : the Interview ,
Interviewing in social research , and "Phrasing questions; the question of bias
in interviewing", from Journal of Marketing ).
Abnormalities, spotting of 32
Agents 17
Alice in Wonderland technique 76
All-Seeing Eye technique 67
Eliciting, definition of 5
Environment, manipulation of 45-46, 52-53
Escapees 16
Espionage Act 8
Exception, the, as psychological type 27-28
Fabricators 18-19
False confessions 94
First children 29
Joint Interrogations 4, 43
Joint interrogators, techniques suitable for 47-48, 72-73
Joint suspects 47, 70-72
Judging human nature, fallacies about 12-13
Khokhlov, Nikolai 9
Language considerations 74
LCFLUTTER 43
Legal considerations affecting KUBARK CI interrogations 6-9
Listening post for interrogations 47
Local laws, importance of 6
Narcosis 98-100
News from Home technique 68
Nobody Loves You technique 67
Non-coercive interrogation 52-81
Walk-ins 34-36
Witness techniques 68-70
Wolf in Sheep's Clothing technique 75
document ends