Cognitive Linguistics: An Introduction
Cognitive Linguistics: An Introduction
Cognitive linguistics: An
introduction. NJ & London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. xxvi + 830. ISBN 0-
8058-6013-4 (case).
REVIEWER: Dinha T. Gorgis, Jadara University for Graduate Studies, Irbid, Jordan.
E-mail: [email protected]
This book, which is more of a portable library than a book to be stacked on a library
shelf, is a "comprehensive" introduction to cognitive linguistics which has grown
rapidly since the publication of Lakoff & Johnson's (1980) Metaphors We Live By.
It is recommended to be used as a text as well as a reference book. Reasonably
enough, Evans & Green suggest that the book as a text be used by tutors in three
different types of course (p.xxi). It comprises three main parts and a fourth which, as I
can see, need not to have been called a part because it is only a five-page chapter
compared with the length of each of the other 22 chapters which all end with a
summary, a section on further reading, and exercises. However, the book includes an
appendix in which tables and figures are presented, along with hundreds of references
and an index; a laborious task indeed.
SUMMARY
Part I: Overview of the cognitive linguistics (CL) enterprise contains four chapters
which tell the reader that CL is a "movement" (p.3) rather than a specific theory.
Therefore, this part provides an overview of what has been achieved over the years in
this revolutionary and interdisciplinary programme from different but complementary
perspectives while also drawing comparisons with the Chomskyan paradigm, in
particular, so often referred to throughout the entire book as formal grammar.
Chapter 1 explicitly states that CL attempts, just like any other linguistic enquiry, to
"describe and account for linguistic systematicity, structure and function" (p.20).
But to study language within the broadly conceived CL is to study patterns of thought,
i.e. conceptualizations, rather than the rules assumed generatively to underlie it. In
short,
CL is "a non-reductionist model" (p.701).
Part II, which includes eight chapters, is wholly devoted to cognitive semantics (CS),
an approach that is arguably set against truth-conditional semantics on one hand, and
the semantic/pragmatic division of labour on the other hand. Rather than using the
formal language of logic, meaning in CL is largely represented in terms of diagrams.
Chapter 5 starts with the characterization of CS and its guiding principles, viz. central
assumptions (principles) that read as follows:
"1. Conceptual structure is embodied (the 'embodied cognition thesis').
2. Semantic structure is conceptual structure.
3. Meaning representation is encyclopaedic.
4. Meaning construction is conceptualization" (p.157).
The first two assumptions are explored in more detail in chapter 6 where we find an
interesting account of image schema and its properties (cf. Johnson 1987). Image
schemas are contrasted with mental images. While the former are more abstract in
nature, emerging from ongoing embodied experience, the latter "are detailed and
result from an effortful and partly conscious cognitive process that involves recalling
visual memory" (p.185). The authors provide a partial list of image schemas that have
been proposed in the literature but rearranged according to the nature of their
experiential grounding (cf. p.190).
Two of these conceptual projections, viz. (3) & (4), are the concerns of chapter 9
which examines "the central claims associated with Conceptual Metaphor Theory,
including the recent challenges of primary metaphor theory. According to this
theory, the cognitive function of metaphor" is to foreground otherwise background
operations" (p.321). We also find in this chapter a brief introduction to the metaphor-
metonymy interface and, in particular, the metonymic basis of metaphor.
Building on insights gained from categorization and ICMs in the previous two
chapters, chapter 10 overviews what has come to be known as cognitive lexical
semantics, an approach which views lexical items as "conceptual categories,
structured with respect to a prototype" (p.356). But Evans is on the scene here
proposing with her research mate, Andrea Tyler, what they call "The Principled
Polysemy approach" (pp.342 ff.). This approach, they claim, differs markedly from
Lakoff's approach (see their illustration of a radical category, p. 347). In sum, "word
meaning involves a complex interaction between polysemy, context and
encyclopaedic knowledge" (p.356).
Recall now the fourth assumption which views meaning construction as "a dynamic
process whereby linguistic units serve as prompts for an array of conceptual
operations and the recruitment of background knowledge" (p.162). This process,
called conceptualisation (pp.363 & 396), is considered in chapter 11, primarily in
terms of Mental Spaces Theory which "accounts for a diverse range of linguistic
phenomena relating to meaning at the level of sentence and text, including referential
ambiguities and the role of tense and aspect in discourse management and
epistemic distance" (pp.396-7).
Part III, which contains nine chapters, overviews cognitive approaches to grammar
which, like CS, are said to be guided by the same theoretical assumptions despite the
inevitable differences. What unites them are the symbolic thesis which "entails that
sound, meaning and grammar are inextricably linked" (p.471) and the usage-based
thesis.
Chapter 14 outlines the above two theses which characterize a cognitive approach to
grammar and identifies four main types of theoretical approach, each of which being
followed by a brief overview. The relationship that holds between them is best
represented in a figure on (p. 483).
Chapter 15 is a detailed account of the first type, namely 'The Conceptual Structuring
System Model' developed by Leonard Talmy, and partly of the second type, viz.
Roland Langacker's 'Cognitive Grammar' which, viewed as complementing the
former (p.549), is further explored in three more chapters. Rather than drawing a
sharp boundary between grammatical knowledge systems, CL holds that "the
grammatical sub-system can semantically be characterised along the same lines as the
open-class sub-system" (p.512) because "language is represented in the mind of the
speaker in terms of a structured inventory. This inventory is structured in terms of a
network of links between symbolic units. These symbolic units may be either lexical
(open-class) or grammatical (closed-class) elements. This structured inventory
represents semantic structure" (p.513).
Three more construction grammars are overviewed and compared in chapter 20. More
detailed is Goldberg's model, which is said to depart from Kay & Fillmore's grammar,
though being influenced by their work and by the early work of George Lakoff (cf. p.
667), because "it is fully usage-based" (p.666). Goldberg's approach to verb argument
structure is claimed to have four advantages: (1) it "avoids the necessity of positing
several distinct senses for one verb" (p.669); (2) it "avoids circularity" (p.670); (3) it
"enables semantic parsimony" (p.670); and (4) it "preserve compositionality" (p.671).
While focusing on sentence-level constructions, "verbs are associated with rich
Frame Semantics which gives rise to participant roles that are mapped onto the
argument roles provided by the construction" (p.701). Croft's (2001) Radical
Construction Grammar (pp.692-697) focuses on cross-linguistic typological variation
and hence linguistic generalizations (or universals). In this model, grammatical
relations, such as subject/object, are not recognized. Rather meaning is linked by
symbolic relations as gestalts (cf. p. 695). Embodied Construction Grammar,
developed recently by Bergen and Chang (2005), is a model on on-line language
processing. It "assumes that all linguistic units are constructions, including
morphemes, words, phrases and sentences" (p.697). However, future research is still
required for the model to provide explanations for the inferences that "arise in
utterance comprehension" (p.699).
In chapter 21, Evans & Green provide "a descriptive overview of the
grammaticalisation process" (p.708) which is said to involve form-meaning change in
the history of a language. Three cognitively oriented theories of grammaticalisation,
which view change as a usage-based phenomenon, are presented and compared.
These theories, supported by case studies carried out on different languages, include:
(1) metaphorical extension approaches (pp.714-721); (2) Invited Inferencing Theory
(pp.720-727); and (3) the subjectification approach (pp.728-732).
Part IV, which is made of one short chapter, takes the reader to the close of a long
intellectual journey. While the achievements of CL are highly appreciated, challenges
are said to remain. As such, the authors not only call for the continuation of
developing "a stronger empirical basis" (p.782), but also for the integration of
approaches.
EVALUATION
Although the book is primarily addressed to students, and hence the too many
reminders and reiterations appearing here and there, some readers, like me, may find
repetitions rather boring. One is made aware though that this technique in writing a
textbook on a relatively new discipline is used as a 'reinforcing strategy'. However,
this minor remark should in no way reduce the strength of the book; for it is after all
an indispensable reading. But my two major remarks, which I suggest to be
considered in a future edition, relate to "Lack of empirical rigour" (p.780). The first
concerns corpus-based approaches, e.g. Stefanowitsch & Gies (2006) whose earlier
work is only acknowledged. I reckon that a small section at least would have thrown
some light on ongoing quantitative and empirical research efforts. Unfortunately, too,
the reader finds nowhere in the book any mention of work done on 'mental models'
(see, for example, the excellent collection of papers in Rickheit & Habel 1999).
Although mental models have largely been researched by cognitive psychologists who
hold similar assumptions to those of cognitive linguists, yet they ought to have been
considered along with image schemas, mental image, semantic frames, domains, and
context.
Out of the ten trivial typos, starting from page 189 and ending with page 769, one
seems to be a slip of the pen. This is the word "excluding" (p.495, line 4 before last)
which must be read as 'including'. Despite all this, I whole-heartedly congratulate
Evans & Green for their laborious work and, accordingly, recommend it to be used as
a textbook.
REFERENCES
Dinha T. Gorgis is currently professor of linguistics at Jadara University for Graduate Studies,
Jordan. He has been teaching English phonology (with occasional reference to Arabic) at a
number of Arab universities since 1975. During 1984-1999, he was mainly involved in
teaching graduate courses, e.g. syntax, semantics, sociolinguistics, pragmatics, discourse
analysis, and translation, and in supervising M.A. and Ph.D. research work pertinent to these
fields. He is co-editor of the international journal Linguistik online, and is member of IPrA. His
most recent publication (2005) is "Binomials in Iraqi and Jordanian Arabic", which can be
accessed freely in: Journal of Language and Linguistics, Vol. 4, No. 2, 135-151. He reviewed
Yavas, Mehmet (2006): Applied English Phonology, which appeared on LINGUIST List: Vol-
16-3630, and has recently written two book notices for eLanguage.