Task-Specific Training
Task-Specific Training
176
Hubbard et al.
Task-specific training is a term that has evolved from the movement science
and motor skill learning literature (Schmidt and Lee, 2005) and is defined as
training or therapy where patients practice context-specific motor tasks and
receive some form of feedback (Teasell et al., 2008, p. 576). In the field of skill
learning, it may be associated with different practice conditions, feedback and
conditions of transfer (Schmidt and Lee, 2005; Winstein et al., 2006). Taskspecific training in rehabilitation focuses on improvement of performance in
functional tasks through goal-directed practice and repetition. The focus is on
training of functional tasks rather than impairment, such as with muscle
strengthening. Other terms used that reflect these elements are repetitive functional task practice, repetitive task practice (French et al., 2008), task-related
training (Carr and Shepherd, 1982) and task-orientated therapy (Bayona
et al., 2005).
A strength of the task-specific training approach is in its scientific origins.
The evidence informing task-specific training is based on animal (basic science)
research (Knapp et al., 1963; Nudo and Milliken, 1996; Nudo et al., 1996), has
been developed within the psychology literature of motor control and learning
(Schmidt and Lee, 2005), and has since been applied in human studies with
healthy participants (Schmidt and Lee, 2005) and following injury (Nelson et
al., 1996; Winstein et al., 2004; Michaelsen et al., 2006). Further, there is
increasing evidence of neural plastic changes associated with training (Richards
et al., 2008). Learning is reported to be maximal for the specific task trained
(Schmidt, 1991; Goldstone, 1998). Importantly, repetitive use alone may not be
sufficient to effect changes in cortical representation. Rather, changes are associated with specific skill learning, consistent with a learning-dependent model
of neural plasticity (Karni et al., 1995; Plautz et al., 2000). Neurophysiological
evidence also supports the value of the object used or task undertaken in the
organization of movement (Lemon et al., 1991; Turton et al., 1993). The evidence indicates that cortico-motor neuron pools are organized relative to specific tasks rather than specific muscles. Importantly, evidence suggests that
motor skill learning capability may be retained in stroke survivors under similar
conditions to healthy volunteers (Platz, 2004; Winstein et al., 2006).
of the brain are either partially or completely shut down following injury.
Researchers have suggested that in time, therapists should be able to decide on
the most optimal intervention for an individual based on evidence of residual
brain circuits (Guadagno et al., 2003; Dobkin and Carmichael, 2005; Johansson,
2005; Teasell et al., 2005; Duffau, 2006; Carey, 2007; Carey and Seitz, 2007;
Stinear et al., 2007).
Animal studies have demonstrated that task-specific training (e.g. skilled
reaching task) can restore function by using spared (non-affected) parts of
the brain which are generally adjacent to the lesion (Nudo et al., 1996)
and/or recruiting supplementary parts of the brain (Nudo et al., 2000). Many
authors, including Rossi et al. (2007), have detailed the neurobiological changes
underlying the brains reorganization in response to task-specific training,
concluding:
Regardless of concomitant interventions, the extent of functional improvement is strongly dependent on the specific external stimulation that the
rewiring circuits experience. Adaptive cortical reorganization in both intact
and injured CNS is not induced by generic use or activation, but requires
the application of task-specific training protocols. (Rossi et al., 2007, p. 19)
Neural plastic changes have also been demonstrated in the human brain
(Calautti et al., 2001; Carey and Seitz, 2007; Richards et al., 2008) following an
ischaemic stroke and neuromotor interventions. For example, the effect of taskoriented arm training on motor function and brain reorganization has been
investigated in randomized controlled trials with a small number of patients
(Nelles et al., 2001; Carey et al., 2002). Using a task-oriented training regime of
intensive finger movement tracking, improvement in finger control was found
in association with evidence of brain reorganization in chronic stroke patients
(Carey et al., 2002). There are now an increasing number of such studies measuring changes in brain activation patterns following task-specific training
which, although still relatively small in participant numbers, provided enough
data for meta-analysis (Richards et al., 2008). Findings from this analysis suggest
that task-specific training can influence functional outcomes and brain activation patterns.
As summarized by Bayona et al. (2005):
Task-oriented therapy is important. It makes intuitive sense that the best
way to relearn a given task is to train specifically for that task. In animals,
functional reorganization is greater for tasks that are meaningful to the
animal. Repetition alone, without usefulness or meaning in terms of function, is not enough to produce increased motor cortical representations. In
humans, less intense but task-specific training regimens with the more
affected limb can produce cortical reorganization and associated, meaningful
functional improvements. (p. 58)
Occup. Ther. Int. 16(34): 175189 (2009)
Copyright 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd
DOI: 10.1002/oti
177
178
Hubbard et al.
179
180
Hubbard et al.
motor control in everyday activities and represents a shift away from facilitation
of movement and exercise therapy. This approach formally identifies the task
as integral to effective motor relearning. The repetitive task training is combined with techniques to enhance cognitive involvement (e.g. through functional relevance of tasks used and knowledge of performance). Task-specific
training is most closely related to the motor relearning approach, but the two
are not synonymous.
An approach that has gained interest more recently and is supported by
evidence from animal studies (Knapp et al., 1958, 1963; Taub et al., 1993; Nudo
et al., 1996) and systematic review (Hakkennes and Keating, 2005) is CIMT.
CIMT is primarily designed to reverse the conditioning that leads to learned
non-use and aims to promote spontaneous use of the hand through using of
shaping procedures (Taub et al., 1993, 2002). The approach involves a constraint applied to the less affected limb and intensive upper limb training of the
more affected limb. The shaping procedure is based on operant conditioning,
with the aim of eliciting a behaviour (task goal) and reinforcing it (positive
feedback). This involves intensive periods of task practice using shaping and
progressive increments in task difficulty, feedback and encouragement (Wolf
et al., 2002). Although the approach is task based and involves practice of
graded activities, the focus is not on the acquisition of a voluntary skill nor the
optimization of motor skill learning (Winstein et al., 2006).
181
182
Hubbard et al.
activities which, although appearing important to the therapist and/or institution, have no such value for the patient, could be counterproductive.
Secondly, evidence indicates that where possible, the task trained should be
real world or context specific (Shumway-Cook and Woollacott, 1995). For
example, if a patient/client is relearning to use a knife and fork, then they should
be doing this in sitting and if possible, with real food and using ordinary cutlery
and crockery. This evidence supports the move in many neurorehabilitation
settings to set up the treatment environment to reflect the usual home and/or
community environment. Some may also refer to this as enriching the environment (Johansson, 2005; Davis, 2006).
Strategy 2: task-specific training practice sequences should be randomly ordered
Task variability has been identified as important to increasing generalisation of
learning to new tasks (Krakauer, 2006, p. 85). Further, evidence indicates that
utilizing randomly ordered practice facilitates retention and transfer, thus
increasing the tasks generalizability (Schmidt and Lee, 2005). Task-specific
therapy, therefore, should be random in its application using differing contexts
and settings, and differing occupational demands and sequences (Bayona et al.,
2005; Dobkin and Carmichael, 2005; Teasell et al., 2005; Davis, 2006). If taskspecific training is too task or movement specific, and applied in only one
context or sequence, then potentially the skills re-learned or learned are not as
readily applied across similar tasks and alternate settings. Clearly, there are times
when this is neither practical nor feasible (e.g. showering), but for the most part,
where possible, therapists should randomly schedule therapy routines and task
selection.
Strategy 3: task-specific training should be repetitive
Task-specific training should be repetitive and involve massed practice (Schmidt
and Lee, 2005; Winstein et al., 2006). The old adage of practice makes perfect
applies in this context as it is practice which assists the healthy and injured
brain alike to master skills and to reorganize to accommodate the new learning. Most researchers (Blennerhassett and Dite, 2004; Mathiowetz, 2004; Bayona
et al., 2005) recommend that the more a task is practiced, the better the overall
performance. However, Page (2003) suggested that task specificity is clinically
more significant than intensity, and recommends that task-specific training is
still worth considering even if patients are not able to manage high-intensity
treatment regimes.
Bearing in mind that for much of the day, patients in hospital are frequently
doing very little (Bernhardt et al., 2007; Hubbard and Parsons, 2007), therapists
should assume that more is better and that most patients are not practicing
enough. It is recommended that the maximum amount of repetition feasible
should be prescribed in task-specific, neuromotor interventions and that the
Occup. Ther. Int. 16(34): 175189 (2009)
Copyright 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd
DOI: 10.1002/oti
183
184
Hubbard et al.
The five strategies arise out of the task-specific evidence and expert commentary. Again, much of this may not be new to readers, but may provide evidence to support practices already applied. However, there is some evidence that
the inpatient setting particularly can raise competing agendas for therapists
(Danils et al., 2002), and these strategies may serve to refocus the efforts of
therapists in their prescription of neuromotor interventions.
Implications of task-specific training for occupational therapists and future
practice
The evidence relating to task-specific training has direct application to occupational therapy practice and resonates with the theory and ideology associated
with occupational science (Christiansen and Townsend, 2004) and occupational therapy. The strategies are steeped in long-held, professional values concerning client-centred practice and the importance of involving the patient/client
in goal setting and rehabilitation agendas (Armstrong, 2008). However, while
the premise of occupation and our approach to learning new motor skills are
highly consistent with task-specific training, it is not a term that is commonly
used by us. Further, despite the growing evidence for task-specific training,
rehabilitation is commonly based instead on accepted practice or custom. The
theoretical and empirical foundations for task-specific training, derived from
research on brain plasticity and motor learning, provide a strong, evidencebased platform for occupational therapists to confidently select neuromotor
interventions which involve task-specific training and everyday tasks and
activities.
References
Armstrong J (2008). The benefits and challenges of interdisciplinary, client-centred, goal setting
in rehabilitation. New Zealand Journal of Occupational Therapy 55: 2025.
Ayres AJ (1965). Patterns of perceptualmotor dysfunction in children: a factor analytic study.
Perceptual and Motor Skills 20: 335368.
Bayona NA, Bitensky J, Salter K, Teasell R (2005). The role of task-specific
training in rehabilitation therapies. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation 12: 5865. DOI:
10.1310/BQM5-6YGB-MVJ5-WVCR
Bernhardt J, Chan J, Nicola I, Collier JM (2007). Little therapy, little physical activity: rehabilitation within the first 14 days of organized stroke unit care. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine 39: 4348. DOI: 10.2340/16501977-0013
Betker AL, Desai A, Nett C, Kapadia N, Szturm T (2007). Game-based exercises for dynamic
short-sitting balance rehabilitation of people with chronic spinal cord and traumatic brain
injuries. Physical Therapy 87: 13891398. DOI:10.2522/ptj.20060229
Blennerhassett J, Dite W (2004). Additional task-related practice improves mobility and upper
limb function early after stroke: a randomised control trial. Australian Journal of Physiotherapy 50: 219224.
Bobath K (1980). A Neurophysiological Basis for the Treatment of Cerebral Palsy. Clinics in
Developmental Medicine No. 75. London: Spastics International Medical Publications (Mac
Keith Press).
185
186
Hubbard et al.
Duffau H (2006). Brain plasticity: from pathophysiological mechanisms to therapeutic applications. Journal of Clinical Neuroscience 13: 885897. DOI: 10.1016/j.jocn.2005.11.045
Feys HM, De Weerdt WJ, Selz BE, Cox Steck GA, Spichiger R, Vereeck LE, Putman KD, Van
Hoydonck GA (1998). Effect of a therapeutic intervention for the hemiplegic upper limb in
the acute phase after stroke: a single-blind, randomized, controlled multicenter trial. Stroke
29: 785792.
French B, Leathley M, Sutton C, McAdam J, Thomas L, Forster A, Langhorne P, Price C,
Walker A, Watkins C (2008). A systematic review of repetitive functional task practice with
modelling of resource use, costs and effectiveness. Health Technology Assessment 12:
1117.
Goldstone RL (1998). Perceptual learning. Annual Reviews in Psychology 49: 585612.
Guadagno JV, Calautti C, Baron, JC (2003). Progress in imaging stroke: emerging clinical applications. British Medical Bulletin 65: 145157.
Hakkennes S, Keating JL (2005). Constraint-induced movement therapy following stroke: a
systematic review of randomised controlled trials. Australian Journal of Physiotherapy 51:
221231.
Hamzei F, Liepert J, Dettmers C, Weiller C, Rijntjes M (2006). Two different reorganization
patterns after rehabilitative therapy: an exploratory study with fMRI and TMS. NeuroImage
31: 710720. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.035
Hesse S (1999). Treadmill training with partial body weight support in hemiparetic patients
further research needed. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 13: 179181.
Hubbard I, Parsons M (2007). The conventional care of therapists as acute stroke specialists: a
case study. International Journal of Therapy and Rehabilitation 14: 357362.
Johansson BB (2005). Regenerative ability in the central nervous system. In: Barnes M,
Dobkin BH, Bogouslavsky J (eds). Recovery After Stroke (pp. 88123). New York, NY:
Cambridge.
Karni A, Meyer G, Jezzard P, Adams MM, Turner R, Ungerleider LG (1995). Functional MRI
evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill learning. Nature 377:
155158.
Kelly C, Foxe JJ, Garavan H (2006). Patterns of normal human brain plasticity after practice
and their implications for neurorehabilitation. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 87: S2029. DOI: 10.1016/j.apmr.2006.08.333
Kim Y-H, Park J-W, Ko M-H, Jang S-H, Lee PKW (2004). Plastic changes in motor network after
constraint-induced movement therapy. Yonsei Medical Journal 45: 241248.
Knapp H, Taub E, Berman A (1958). Effect of deafferentation on a conditioned avoidance
response. Science 128: 842843.
Knapp H, Taub E, Berman A (1963). Movements in monkeys with deafferented forelimbs.
Experimental Neurology 7: 305315.
Krakauer JW (2006). Motor learning: its relevance to stroke recovery and neurorehabilitation.
Current Opinion in Neurology 19: 8490. DOI: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32831997af
Kubasak MD, Jindrich DL, Zhong H, Takeoka A, McFarland KC, Munoz-Quiles C, Roy RR,
Edgerton VR, Ramn-Cueto A, Phelps PE (2008). OEG implantation and step training
enhance hindlimb-stepping ability in adult spinal transected rats. Brain 131: 264267.
DOI:10.1093/brain/awm267
Langhammer B, Stanghelle JK (2000). Bobath or motor relearning programme? A comparison
of two different approaches of physiotherapy in stroke rehabilitation: a randomized controlled study. Clinical Rehabilitation 14: 361369. DOI: 10.1191/026921500cr338oa
Law M, Baptiste S, Carswell A, Mccoll MA, Polatajko H, Pollock N (2005). Canadian Occupational Performance Measure (4th edn). Ottawa: CAOT Publications ACE.
Lemon RN, Bennett KM, Werner W (1991). The cortico-motor substrate for skilled movements
of the primate hand. In: Requin SG (ed.). Tutorials in Motor Neurosciences (pp. 477495).
Holland: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Mak MKY, Hui-Chan CWY (2008). Cued task-specific training is better than exercise in
improving sit-to-stand in patients with Parkinsons disease: a randomized controlled trial.
Movement Disorders 23: 501509. DOI: 10.1002/mds.21509
Mathiowetz V (2004). Task-orientated approach to stroke rehabilitation. In: Gillen G, Burkhardt
A (eds). Stroke Rehabilitation: A Function-based Approach (2nd edn, pp. 5974).
St Louis, MO: Mosby Elsevier.
McCannon R, Casey S, Elfessi ANA, Tiry S (2005). A longitudinal study of the learning and
retention of task-specific training. Work 24: 139144.
Michaelsen SM, Dannenbaum R, Levin MF (2006). Task-specific training with trunk restraint
on arm recovery in stroke: randomized control trial. Stroke 37: 186192. DOI: 10.1161/01.
STR.0000196940.20446.c9
Moulton C (1997). Current trends in the practice of home health care of occupational therapists
treating patients who have had a stroke. Occupational Therapy International 4: 3151. DOI.
wiley.com/10.1002/oti.46
National Stroke Foundation (2005). Clinical Guidelines for Stroke Rehabilitation and Recovery.
Melbourne: National Stroke Foundation.
Nelles G, Jentzen W, Jueptner M, Muller S, Diener HC (2001). Arm training induced brain
plasticity in stroke studied with serial positron emission tomography. Neuroimage 13: 1146
1154. DOI: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0757
Nelson DL, Konosky K, Fleharty K, Webb R, Newer K, Hazboun VP, Fontaine C, Licht B (1996).
The effects of occupationally embedded exercise on bilaterally assisted supination in persons
with hemiplegia. American Journal of Occupational Therapy 50: 639646.
Nudo RJ, Milliken GW (1996). Reorganization of movement representations in primary motor
cortex following focal ischemic infarcts in adult squirrel monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology 75: 21442149.
Nudo RJ, Wise BM, SiFuentes F, Milliken GW (1996). Neural substrates for the effects
of rehabilitation training on motor recovery after ischemic stroke. Science 39: 733
742.
Nudo RJ, Friel KM, Delia SW (2000). Role of sensory deficits in motor impairments
after injury to the primary motor cortex. Neuropharmacology 39: 733742. DOI: 10.1016/
S0028-3908(99)00254-3
Page SJ (2003). Intensity versus task-specificity after stroke: how important is intensity? American Journal of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation 82: 730732.
Page SJ, Sisto S-A, Levine P, McGrath RE (2004). Efficacy of modified constraint-induced movement therapy in chronic stroke: a single-blinded randomized controlled trial. Archives of
Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 85: 1418.
Page SJ, Levine P, Leonard AC (2005). Modified constraint-induced movement therapy in acute
stroke: a randomised controlled pilot study. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 19: 27
32. DOI: 10.1177/1545968304272701
Peurala SH, Pitkanen K, Sivenius J, Tarkka IM (2004). How much exercise does the enhanced
gait-oriented physiotherapy provide for chronic stroke patients? Journal of Neurology 251:
449453.
Platz T (2004). Impairment-oriented Training (IOT) scientific concept and evidence-based
treatment strategies. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience 22: 301315. Cote INIST:
22153, 35400012053865.0130
Plautz EJ, Milliken GW, Nudo RJ (2000). Effects of repetitive motor training on movement
representations in adult squirrel monkeys: role of use versus learning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 74: 2755.
Richards LG, Stewart KC, Woodbury ML, Senesac C, Cauraugh JH (2008). Movementdependent stroke recovery: a systematic review and meta-analysis of TMS and
fMRI evidence. Neuropsychologia 46: 311. DOI: 10.1016/j.neurophsychologia.2007.08.
013
187
188
Hubbard et al.
Winstein CJ, Campbell Stewart J (2006). Conditions of task practice for individuals with neurologic impairments. In: Selzer ME, Clarke S, Cohen LG, Duncan PW, Gage FH (eds).
Textbook of Neural Repair and Rehabilitation: Volume II Medical Neurorehabilitation (pp.
89102). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Winstein CJ, Rose DK, Tan SM, Lewthwaite R, Chui HC, Azen SP (2004). A randomized controlled comparison of upper-extremity rehabilitation strategies in acute stroke: a pilot study
of immediate and long-term outcomes. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
85: 620628. DOI: 10.1016/j.apmr.2003.06.027
Wolf SL, Blanton S, Baer H, Breshears J, Butler AJ (2002). Repetitive task practice: a critical
review of constraint-induced movement therapy in stroke. Neurologist 8: 325338.
Wolf SL, Winstein CJ, Miller JP, Taub E, Uswatte G, Morris D, Giulliani C, Light KE,
Nichols-Larsen D (2006). Effect of constraint-induced movement therapy on upper extremity
function 3 to 9 months after stroke: the EXCITE randomised clinical trial. Journal of the
American Medical Association 296: 20952104.
Wood-Dauphinee S, Kwakkel G (2005). The impact of rehabilitation on stroke outcomes. What
is the evidence? In: Barnes M, Dobkin BH, Bogouslavsky J (eds). Recovery After Stroke (pp.
161188). New York, NY: Cambridge.
Wu C-Y, Trombly CA, Lin K-C, Tickle-Degnen L (2000). A kinematic study of contextual
effects on reaching performance in persons with and without stroke: influences of
object availability. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 81: 95101. DOI:
10.1016/S0003-9993(00)90228-4
Address correspondence to Leeanne M. Carey, Level 2, Neurosciences Building, Austin Health,
Repatriation Campus, 300 Waterdale Road, Heidelberg Heights, Victoria, Australia, 3081
(E-mail: [email protected]).
189