Projective Personality Tests
Projective Personality Tests
The most important things about an individual are what he cannot or will not say
- Lawrence K Frank (1939)
(The man who coined the term projective methods)
In a scene in Shakespeares play Hamlet, Polonius and Hamlet discuss what can be seen in clouds. In fact, one of the earliest projective measures was Wilhelm Sterns Cloud Picture Test, in which subjects were asked to tell what they saw in pictures of clouds.
Unlike self-report methods, projective tests are indirect methods of personality assessment. Test takers will have to talk about something or someone other
than themselves, and the examiner makes inferences based on their responses. Hence the ability and inclination of the examinees to fake is greatly minimized. Some of the other advantages of projective tests are No need for great proficiency in language in order to take the test The promise of cross-cultural utility These tests tap the unconscious as well as conscious material
Projective tests were born in the spirit of rebellion against normative data. This orientation is exemplified by Frank (1939), who reflected, It is interesting to see how the students of personality have attempted to meet the problem of individuality with methods and procedures designed for study of uniformities and norms that ignore or subordinate individuality, treating it as a troublesome deviation which derogates from the real, the superior, and the only important central tendency, mode , average, etc. Projective methods were once the technique of choice for focusing on the individual from a purely clinical perspective that examined the unique way an individual projects onto an ambiguous stimulus his way of seeing life, his meanings, significances, patterns, and especially his feelings (Frank, 1939).
To fill the need for a test manual, a number systems have been devised. The most widely used one is the comprehensive system devised by Exner. Procedure of administration The cards initially presented to the test taker one at a time in numbered order from 1 to 10. The test taker is instructed to tell what is on each of the cards with a question such as What might this be? Test takers have a great deal of freedom with the Rorschach. They may rotate the cards and vary the number and length of their responses to each card. The examiner records all relevant information, including the test takers verbatim responses, nonverbal gestures, the length of time before the first response to each card, the position of the card, and so forth. The examiner does not engage in any discussion concerning the test takers responses during the initial administration of the cards. Every effort is made to provide the test taker with the opportunity to project, free from any outside distractions. After the entire set of cards has been administered once, a second administration, referred to as the inquiry, is conducted. During the inquiry, the examiner attempts to determine what features of the inkblot played a role in formulating the test takers percept (perception of an image). Questions such as What made it look like ...? and How do you see ...? asked in an attempt to clarify what was seen and which aspects of the inkblot were most influential in forming the perception. The third component of the administration, referred to as testing the limits, may also be included. 1. This procedure enables the examiner to restructure the situation by asking specific questions that provide additional information concerning personality functioning. If the test taker has utilised the entire inkblot when forming percepts throughout the test, the examiner might want to determine if details within the inkblot could be elaborated. 2. Also, any confusion or misunderstanding concerning the task is identified. 3. The test taker's ability to refocus percepts given a new frame of reference is determined and it is also seen that if a test taker made anxious by the ambiguous nature of the task is better able to perform given this added structure. 4. At least one Rorschach researcher (Cerney, 1984) has advocated the technique of trying to elicit one last response from test takers who think they have already given as many responses as they are going to give. The rationale was that endings have many meanings, and the one last response may provide a source of questions and inferences applicable to treatment considerations. Scoring The various variables considered in scoring are:
1. Location - the part of the inkblot that was utilised in forming the percept entire inkblot, a large section, a small section, a minute detail or white spaces. The number of whole responses is typically associated with conceptual thought process. 2. Determinants - the qualities of the inkblot that determine what the individual perceives - form, colour, shading, movement. Human movement has been associated with creative imagination. Colour responses have been associated with emotional reactivity. 3. Content - the content category of the responses-human figures, animal figures, anatomical parts, blood, clouds, x-rays and sexual responses. 4. Popularity - the frequency with which a certain response has been found to correspond with a particular inkblot or a section of an inkblot in the general population. 5. Form - how accurately the individual's perception matches or fits the corresponding part of the inkblot. Form level is associated with reality testing. Other variables which are considered are patterns of responses, recurrent themes and the interrelationships among the different scoring categories. Reliability of the Rorschach 1. Split-half reliability - Because each inkblot is considered to have a unique stimulus quality, evaluation of reliability by split-half method would be appropriate. 2. Test-retest reliability - This is also inappropriate for use with the Rorschach, because of the effect of familiarity on response to the cards and because responses may reflect transient states as opposed to enduring traits. 3. Inter-scorer reliability -The widespread acceptance of Exners system has advanced the cause of Rorschach reliability. The Rorschach remains one of the most frequently used and frequently taught psychological tests. It is widely used in forensic work and generally accepted by the courts.
History
One of the earliest use of pictures as projective stimuli came at the beginning of the 20th century. Sex differences were reported in the stories that children gave in response to 9 pictures (Brittain, 1907). The author reported that the girls were more interested in religious and moral themes than boys were. In 1932, a psychiatrist working at the Clinic for Juvenile Research in Detroit developed the Social Situation Picture Test (Schwartz, 1932), a projective instrument that contained pictures relevant to juvenile delinquents.
The Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) [Apperceive = perceive in terms of past perceptions] Originally designed by Christiana D Morgan and Henry A Murray in 1935, as an aid in eliciting fantasy material from patients in psychoanalysis, the TAT is the most widely used of all the picture/storytelling tests. The stimulus materials consist of 30 black-and-white picture cards and 1 blank card. 20 cards is the recommended number for presentation. The cards depict various scenes containing certain classical human situations. Some pictures contain a lone individual, some a group of people, and yet others contain no people. Some are as real as a photo, while others are surrealistic drawings. Examinees are instructed to make up a story which covers the events which led up to the scene, what is happening at the moment and what follows. They are also asked to tell what the people in the scene are thinking and feeling. For the blank card, test takers are asked to imagine that there is a picture on the card and tell a story about it. Examiners must attempt to find out the source of the examinees story. The source could be a personal experience, a dream, an imagined event, a book or movie, etc. Some cards are suggested for use with adult males, adult females, or both, and some are suggested for use with children. This is so because; certain pictures lend themselves more than others to identification and projection by members of these groups. Analysis is based on: 1. Stories as told by the examinee 2. The clinicians notes about the way in which the examinee responded to the cards 3. The clinicians notes about extra-test behavior and verbalizations.
Example of how a test takers behavior during testing may influence the examiners interpretations of the findings was provided by Sugarman (1991). He tells about a highly narcissistic patient who demonstrated contempt and devaluation of the examiner (and presumably others) by dictating TAT stories complete with spelling and punctuation as though the examiner was a stenographer. Henry Murray has mentioned some concepts which are useful in interpreting TAT data. They are 1. Need determinants of behavior arising from within the individual 2. Press - determinants of behavior arising from within the environment 3. Thema a unit of interaction between needs and press In general, the guiding principle in interpreting TAT stories is that the examinee is identifying with someone (the protagonist) in the story and that the needs, environmental demands, and conflicts of the protagonist in the story are in some way related to the concerns, hopes, fears, or desires of the test taker.
Courtesy: PSYCHOLOGICAL TESTING AND ASSESSMENT, 6TH EDITION COHEN AND SWERDLIK
Description- A male and a female are seated in close proximity on a sofa. The female is talking on the phone. There is an end table with a magazine on it next to the sofa. Manifest stimulus demand- Some explanation of the nature of the relationship between these two persons and some reason the woman is on the phone are required. Less frequently noted is the magazine on the table and its role in this scene. Form demand- Two large details, the woman and the man, must be integrated. Small details include the magazine and the telephone. Latent stimulus demand- This picture is likely to elicit attitudes towards heterosexuality and, within that context, material relevant to the examinees place on optimism-pessimism, security-insecurity, dependence-independence, passivity-assertiveness, and related continuums. Alternatively, attitudes toward family and friends may be elicited, with the two persons being viewed as brother and sister, the female talking on the phone to a family member, and so on. Frequent plots- If this card is administered to adolescents, the frequent plot may be based on a heterosexual relationship between the two persons.
Significant variations- In this example, any plot in which the characters are not involved in a heterosexual relationship (eg: employer/employee) may be a significant variation. Introduced figures- Persons not pictured in the card but introduced into the story by the examinee. With whom is the woman speaking on the phone?
Finally, although the relationship between expression of fantasy stories and real-life behavior is tentative at best, and although it is highly susceptible to faking, the TAT continues to be widely used by practitioners.
This entails the presentation of a list of stimulus words, to each which, the assessee responds verbally or in writing with whatever comes to mind first upon hearing that word. The first method was given by Galton (1897). The associations individuals make to words are not chance happenings but rather the result of the interplay between ones life experiences, ones attitudes, and ones unique personality characteristics. Word Association Test Developed by Rapaport, Gill and Schafer (1946) Inspired by Jungs idea that by selecting certain key words that represented possible areas of conflict, word association techniques could be employed for psychodiagnostic purposes. This test consists of 3 parts: I. Each stimulus word is administered to the examinee, who has been instructed to respond quickly with the first word that came to mind and the length of time taken for each response is noted. Each stimulus word is again presented, and the examinee is instructed to give the same responses as he had given in the first part. Any deviation from the original, and the time taken for response is noted. The Inquiry Examiner asks questions to clarify the relationship that exists between the stimulus words and their respective responses.
II.
III.
The test consists of 60 words; some of which are considered to be neutral (eg: chair, book, water, dance, taxi) whereas others are considered to be traumatic (eg: love, girlfriend, boyfriend, fire, breast) . Responses on the test are evaluated with respect to variables such as popularity, reaction time, content and test-retest responses. Sentence completion tests I like to __________________________ Someday, I will ______________________ I worry about ____________________ I am most frightened when _______________ Sentence completion tests may contain items that are quite general and appropriate for administration in a wide variety of settings. Alternatively, sentence completion stems (the first part of the item) may be developed for use in specific settings (like school or business) or for specific purposes. The most popular one is the Rotter Incomplete Sentences Blank. It is available in three levels: high school, college and adult. Testtakers are instructed to respond to each of the 40 incomplete sentence items in a way that expresses their real feelings. The responses are interpreted according to several categories like family attitudes, social and sexual attitudes, general attitudes, and character traits. Each response is evaluated on a 7-point scale that ranges from need for therapy to extremely good adjustment. In general, a sentence completion test may be useful for obtaining diverse information about an individuals interests, educational aspirations, future goals, fears, conflicts, needs, and so forth. The tests have a high degree of face validity. However, with this high degree of face validity comes a certain degree of transparency about the objective of the test. For this reason, sentence completion tests are perhaps the most vulnerable of all the projective methods to faking on the part of an examinee, intent on making a good or a bad impression.
The tests proved not to differentiate between different groups of subjects who took it. Responses to the auditory stimuli lacked the complexity and richness of responses to inkblots, pictures, and other projective stimuli. None of the available scoring systems was very satisfactory.
the future. Placement to the lower left suggests depression with a desire to flee into the past. Another variable of interest to those who analyze figure drawings is the characteristics of the individual drawn. For example, unusually large eyes or large ears suggest suspiciousness, ideas of reference or other paranoid characteristics. Unusually large breasts drawn by a male may be interpreted as unresolved oedipal problems with maternal dependence. Long and conspicuous ties suggest sexual aggressiveness, perhaps overcompensating for fear of impotence. Button emphasis suggests dependent, infantile, inadequate personality. When instructed simply to draw a person, most people will draw a person of the same sex. It is deemed clinically significant if the person draws a person of the opposite sex when given these instructions. It has been found in some cases that children are uncertain of the sex of the figure drawn. Such cases are hypothesized as an indefinite or ill-defined notion of sexual identity. Other figure drawing tests are the House-Tree-Person Test (Buck, 1948) and the Kinetic Family Drawing (KFD) Test (Burns & Kaufman, 1970, 1972). In the KFD, the examinee is instructed to draw a picture of everyone in their family, including themselves, and everyone is to be depicted as DOING something.
Situational variables
Situational variables such as the examiners presence or absence have significantly affected the responses of examinees. TAT stories written in private are likely to be less guarded, less optimistic, and more affectively involved than those written in the presence of the examiner. Other such variables are the age of the examiner, specific instructions given. Examiners also rely on situational cues. They appear to interpret projective data with regard to their own needs and expectations, their own subjective feelings about the person being tested, and their own constructions regarding the total test situation. Masling (1965) demonstrated that through postural, gestural and facial cues, Rorschach examiners are capable of unwittingly eliciting the responses they expect.
The debate between academicians who argue that projective tests are not technically sound instruments and clinicians who find such tests useful has been raging ever since projectives came into widespread use. Frank (1939) responded to those who would reject projective methods because of their lack of technical rigor: These leads to the study of personality have been rejected by many psychologists because they do not meet psychometric requirements for validity and reliability, but they are being employed in association with clinical and other studies of personality where they are finding increasing validation in the consistency of results for the same subject when independently assayed by each of these procedures. . . If we face the problem of personality, in its full complexity, as an active dynamic process to be studied as a process rather than as entity or aggregate of traits, factors, or as static organization, then these projective methods offer many advantages for obtaining data on the process of organizing experience which is peculiar to each personality and has a life career.
Reference Psychological Testing and Assessment An Introduction to Tests and Measurement 6th Ed. Ronald Jay Cohen and Mark E Swerdlik