BingRedish EpistFraming PDF
BingRedish EpistFraming PDF
, to a stu-
dent. You might remind him of the expression for the
gravitational force,
F
g
=
Gm
1
m
2
r
2
r
, and demonstrate
how ideas from this more familiar bit of math map to
Coulombs Law. Both forces depend on the relative
strengths (mass or charge) of the two objects in ques-
tion. Both forces fall off with respect to distance in the
same way, and both include a proportionality constant
(G or
1
4
o
) that must be experimentally measured.
Even dis-analogous observations can be illuminating.
Gravity is always attractive, hence the negative sign is
explicitly included in front of the always positive
masses. An electric force can be attractive or repul-
sive, so the implicit signs on the positive or negative
charges, q
1
and q
2
, will determine the direction of the
Coulomb force.
Implicit in your discussion with the student would
be the class of warrants indicative of a Math Consis-
tency framing: mathematics and mathematical ma-
nipulations have a regularity and reliability and are
consistent across different situations. Establishing a
common underlying mathematical structure allows one
to trust the relevant set of relations and inferences.
Correlates of the four framing clusters
Our four common framings are primarily identified
via the warrants physics students use in their mathe-
matical reasoning. Other indicators, however, have
been observed to cluster preferentially around certain
framings. The table below summarizes these primary
(i.e. warrants) and secondary framing indicators we
have observed in our data set.
Framing is a dynamic cognitive process. A per-
sons mind makes an initial judgment regarding the
nature of the situation at hand, but that judgment is
continually updated and reevaluated. New information
comes to the student all the time, whether in the form
of a classmates comment, an interviewers interjec-
tion, simply turning to a different page in a textbook,
or even spontaneous random associations within her
own brain. This new information can lead a student to
reframe her activity. As a result, the epistemological
framings observed in these students work can extend
over a range of time periods. We have found examples
in our data set ranging from ten seconds to ten min-
utes.
Table 1: Four common framings and their primary (i.e. warrants) and secondary indicators.
Calculation Physical Mapping Invoking Authority Math Consistency
Class of
Warrant
Used
Correctly follow-
ing algorithmic
steps gives trust-
able result
Goodness-of-fit between
math and physical ob-
servations or expecta-
tions attests to a result.
Authoritatively as-
serting a result or a
rule gives it cre-
dence.
Similarity or logical
connection to another
math idea offers vali-
dation.
Other
Common
Indicators
-focus on techni-
cal correctness
-math chaining:
need this to get
that
-often aided by a dia-
gram
-demonstrative gesturing
mechanistic chaining
-quoting a rule
-absence of mecha-
nistic chaining
-little acknowledg-
ment of substructure
-analogy with another
math idea
-categorization
Statement of this study's purpose
We now have laid a sufficient foundation upon which
to state this papers main premise in its full detail:
framing and epistemic resources are, by definition,
cognitive modeling elements. They aim to capture an
important aspect of students thinking (i.e. its tendency
to selective attention), but one can never directly ob-
serve, say, an epistemic resource. We suggest that
analyzing the warrants students rely on provides a way
to pin down evidence of their framing and the epistemic
resources activated in their minds.
Why have we put Class of Warrant Used on a ped-
estal in Table 1 while relegating various student actions
to Other Common Indicators? Isnt an action like
quoting a rule also excellent evidence of an Invoking
Authority framing? It certainly is, but the phase space of
possible student actions is immense. Consider one stu-
dent who confidently states and restates some canned
phrase (maybe work is path independent) and another
student who combs through fifty pages of his textbook
looking for the formula I = (2/5)MR
2
. Their actions are
different, but the class of warrant that underlies their
action tends to be the same: authoritatively asserting a
rule or result gives it credence. Both students are fram-
ing their work as Invoking Authority. The dimensional-
ity of the warrant phase space is much lower (on the or-
der of four) as the upcoming case study demonstrates.
Inter-rater reliability of
epistemological framing analysis
The value of this studys epistemological framing
analysis depends in part on how readily other researchers
can apply it consistently. An inter-rater reliability study
was carried out by giving this papers methodology dis-
cussion to another researcher and that researcher was
then asked to parse a new transcript for epistemological
framing (i.e. identify the class of warrants underlying the
students arguments). Details of this inter-rater reliabil-
ity test are given in Chapter Four of the first authors
dissertation.
1b
Different researchers agreed on their war-
rant codes (and hence their framing codes) 70% of the
time for a novel transcript before any consultation or
discussion. This figure improved to 80% after discus-
sion. As our case study demonstrates, this warrant
analysis does indeed parse students thought into frames
in a natural, reasonable way.
We do not expect our warrant coding scheme to yield
a 100% consistent coding of a random transcript. Stu-
dents thinking is simply not that cleanly compartmental-
ized. Indeed, we argue (see ref. 1b, Chapter 7) that one
characteristic of expertise in physics problem solving is
the ability to effectively blend these four framings dy-
namically.
Two issues are relevant here. First, there is the ques-
tion of how often students are observed to spend an ap-
preciable time, say a minute or more, uniquely in one of
this papers four common framings. Of all the data ana-
lyzed for this study, perhaps less than 50% can be
cleanly coded in minute-or-longer chunks under one of
these general framings.
The second notion of clean coding of framing that
is relevant concerns not these minute-long pure state
framings but rather our ability to identify smaller chunks
in hybrid framings. Calculation, physical mapping, in-
voking authority, and math consistency do a reasonable
job of spanning the space of these students mathemati-
cal arguments. We observed that about 90% of a random
episode or more can be seen as made up of behavior in-
dicative of those four landmark framings. But at this
stage of student development (upper division physics
majors) hybrids are common. Perhaps a student quotes a
few computational rules as he performs a long calcula-
tion. Maybe a student makes an analogy to both a similar
physical situation and a similar math structure. As the
inter-rater reliability test shows, researchers can still use
this studys analysis scheme to identify evidence of these
elemental framings in a piece of transcript that is, in gen-
eral, a more complicated hybrid framing.
Bing & Redish Epistemological Framing
11
V. CASE STUDY
We now turn to our case study. This detailed exam-
ple illustrates how this papers warrant-based framing
analysis can be applied to parse an authentic conversa-
tion among physics students.
The students framing of their math use plays a sig-
nificant role in the episode. The principal dynamic con-
cerns how to interpret the math at hand. A significant
amount of these students energy goes into trying to es-
tablish the epistemological framing they see as appropri-
ate.
Their thinking is dynamic. Different bits of their
mathematical knowledge are activated and deactivated as
they frame and reframe their activity. Sometimes fram-
ing differences have marked effects. The students some-
times talk past each other, neither one seeming to hear
what the other is saying, because they are framing their
work differently. Sometimes a students framing can
exhibit considerable resistance to change. As students
become more sophisticated and expert-like, we conjec-
ture, students become more flexible in their framing and
increasingly create hybrid frames.
49
Our case study comes from a group of three students
enrolled in the class Intermediate Theoretical Methods
(PHYS 374). One is a junior (S2) and the other two are
sophomores (S1 and S3). These three students met regu-
larly outside of class to work on their homework to-
gether, and this episode was taped during one such
homework session.
The question
Our episode starts in the middle of their work on one
of that weeks homework problems. The problem they
were considering reads:
A rocket (mass m) is taken from a point A near
an asteroid (mass M) to another point B. We
will consider two (unrealistic) paths as shown
in figure 1.
Calculate the work done by the asteroid on the
rocket along each path. Use the full form of
Newtons Universal Law of Gravitation (not the
flat earth approximation mg). Calculate the
work done by using the fundamental definition
of work:
W
AB
= F
dr
A
B
.
The reader familiar with the physics of this example
will recognize an attempt to get the student to see how
the theorem that says potential energy is path independ-
ent arises out of explicit calculation. Not all of the stu-
dents in this discussion recognized the point of the prob-
lem from the beginning.
Figure 1: Case Study problem.
The first framing clash
During this episode, the students are trying to decide
if the work done should be the same along the two paths
from A to B. They had previously suppressed the G, m,
and M constants and written the (incorrect)
50
equation
1
r
2
dr =
1
y
2
+ 9
dy
1
3
2
3 2
+
1
x
2
+1
1
3
dx
on the blackboard to express the work done along the
direct and two-part paths, respectively. They have also
copied the diagram of the situation from the problem
statement.
The students are standing at the blackboard where all
the relevant equations and diagrams appear. We focus on
the type of justification each student offers for his math
arguments:
1. S1: whats the problem?
2. You should get a different answer
3. from here for this. [Points to each path on two-path
diagram. ]
4. S2: No no no
5. S1: They should be equal?
6. S2: They should be equal
7. S1: Why should they be equal?
8. This path is longer if you think about it. [Points to
two-part path again]
9. S2: Because force, err, because
10. work is path independent.
11. S1: This path is longer, so it should have, [Points to
two-part path again]
12. this number should be bigger than
13. S2: Work is path independent. If you
14. go from point A to point B,
15. doesnt matter how you get there,
16. it should take the same amount of work.
Lines 1 to 6 contain the main issue of this episode.
While S1 thinks there should be different amounts of
work done on the small mass along the two different
paths, S2 believes the work done should be the same.
Bing & Redish Epistemological Framing
12
In the language of formal argumentation theory,
34
S1
makes the claim that more work is done along the two-
part path, and he offers the data that the two-part path is
longer. An unspoken warrant exists that connects his
data to his claim: the particular mathematics being used
should align with the physical systems under study. The
goodness-of-fit between the math at hand and the physi-
cal system attests to the validity of ones conclusions.
The work formula,
,
seems to say force times distance to S1. The two-part
path has more distance, and S1 thus draws justification
for his answer.
S1s warrant thus suggests he is framing his activity
as physical mapping. His use of a diagram in lines 1 to 3
and 7 to 8 supports this characterization. He gestures to
the different paths as he points out that the two-part path
is physically longer. Use of a diagram as intermediary
between the physical situation and the mathematics is a
commonly observed indicator of a physical mapping
framing. A diagram can often help a student entwine
physical and mathematical ideas as he examines how the
two fit together.
S2 not only has a different answer than S1, but he is
also framing his use of mathematics in a different way.
S2 claims that the work done on the small mass should
be the same along the two paths because work is path
independent (lines 9 and 10). His data is a familiar
mantra (though he omits mentioning how this statement
is only valid for conservative forces like gravity). The
unspoken warrant that S2 is relying on concerns the
common use of rules and definitions in math and phys-
ics: sometimes previous results are simply taken as giv-
ens for speed and convenience. S2 is framing his math
use as invoking authority.
After hearing S2s counterargument, S1 repeats him-
self. In lines 11 and 12, he restates his longer-path justi-
fication and again points to the relevant features of the
diagram they had previously drawn on the board. S2
responds by restating work is path independent in line
13 and again, slightly differently, in lines 14 to 16.
The most important observation in this first clip is
that S1 and S2 are disagreeing over much more than
merely the answer itself. Explicitly, they are debating
whether or not more work is done along the longer path.
Implicitly, they are arguing over the most useful way to
frame their present use of mathematics. S1 never explic-
itly says Please respond to my claim in a way that maps
our math to some detail of the physical situation I may
have overlooked. His phrasing and gesturing in his ini-
tial argument (lines 7 and 8) and beyond (lines 11 and
12) implies this framing request, though.
When S2 responds with his rule citation, he is not
merely arguing for a different answer. He is pushing for
a different type of warrant for judging the validity of a
given answer. S2s invoking an authority framing may
have even prevented him from really hearing what S1
was saying. S1s framing request may have passed by S2
unnoticed because he was too caught up in the subset of
all his math resources that his invoking authority framing
had activated within his mind. At any rate, S2 responds
in lines 9 and 10 with a different type of justification
than what S1 was expecting.
When S1 repeats himself in lines 11 and 12, he is im-
plicitly repeating his bid for a physical mapping framing.
One can imagine a situation when S2s invoking author-
ity justification would simply be accepted without inci-
dent, but here it did not align with S1s present framing.
S2 does not respond to this reframing request and repeats
his answer as he remains in invoking authority.
There is thus an intense framing argument going on
under the surface of this debate. Sensing that he is not
making any headway in the framing battle, S1 now
moves to shift both himself and S2 into a third framing.
A temporary agreement on a third framing
S1 now makes a move toward a third way of address-
ing the mathematics at hand. He suggests they churn
away and calculate what numeric answers their expres-
sions actually give (which happens during the lines 31 to
41). S2 accepts for a time.
17. S1: OK, thats assuming Pythagorean
18. Theorem and everything else add[s].
19. Well, OK, well is this what was the
20. answer to this right here? [Points to
.]
21. What was that answer?
22. S2: Yeah, solve each integral numerically.
23. S1: Yeah, what was that answer?
24. S3: Each individual one?
25. S1: Yeah, what was
26. S3: OK, let me, uhh [S3 starts typing into Mathe-
matica]
27. S1: Cause path two is longer than path one, so
28. S2: May I, for a minute? [S2 writes on a small cor-
ner of the blackboard, but never speaks about what
he writes.]
29. S1: and path one was this.
30. S2: Gimme this, I wanna think about something.
31. S1: J ust add those up, tell me the number for this
[Points to integrals again]
32. and Ill compare it to the number of
33. S3: OK, the y-one is point one five.
34. S1: I, just give me the, just sum those up.
35. I just want the whole total.
36. I just want this total quantity there,
37. just the total answer. [Points to integrals again]
Bing & Redish Epistemological Framing
13
38. S2: Oh, it was point four-
39. S3: No, thats the other one [direct path].
40. S1: you gave it to me before, I just didnt write it
down.
41. S3: Oh I see, point, what, point six one eight
42. S1: See, point six one eight, which is what I said,
43. the work done here should be larger
44. than the work done here cause the path [Points to
two-path diagram]
45. S2: No, no no, no no no
46. S3: the path where the x is changing
47. S2: Work is path independent.
48. S1: How is it path independent?
49. S2: by definition
50. S3: Somebody apparently proved this before we did.
S1 attempts to reframe the discussion in lines 19 to
21. He points to the integrals theyve written and asks,
Well, OKwhat was the answer to this right here?
What was that answer? He is calling for someone to
evaluate each of their expressions for the work so that he
can compare the numeric results. This argument relies on
another kind of warrant. Mathematics provides one with
a standardized, self-consistent set of manipulations and
transformations. Performing a calculation or having a
computer do it for you according to these rules will give
a valid, trustable result. S1 is moving to reframe their
math use as calculation.
Even though S1 doesnt explicitly detail the new war-
rant he is proposing, S2 quickly zeroes in on it. He im-
mediately responds, yeah, solve each integral numeri-
cally (line 22). Compare this successful, fluid epistemic
frame negotiation with the struggle of the previous snip-
pet. Lines 1 to 16 had S1 pushing for physical mapping
while S2 lobbied for invoking authority. Both stuck to
their positions, resulting in an inefficient conversation.
Neither was accepting what the other was trying to say.
Lines 19 to 22 have S1 and S2 agreeing, for the moment,
on what type of mathematical justification should count.
The calculation framing negotiated, lines 23 to 41 are
mostly about S1 directing S3 to input the proper expres-
sions into Mathematica, a common software calculator
package. They finish with Mathematica in line 41. It
turns out that the radial path integral,
1
r
2
2
3 2
dr
, is equal to
0.47 while the two-part path integrals,
1
y
2
+ 9
dy
1
3
+
1
x
2
+1
dx
1
3
dr