Critique of Claims of Improved Visual Acuity After Hypnotic Suggestion
Critique of Claims of Improved Visual Acuity After Hypnotic Suggestion
872879
OPTOMETRY AND VISION SCIENCE
Copyright 2004 American Academy of Optometry
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
ABSTRACT: Psychological approaches to improving vision present an enticing alternative to invasive procedures and
corrective lenses; hypnotic suggestion is one such technique. During the past 60 years, multiple studies have
documented improvements in the vision of myopic individuals after hypnotic interventions. Given the increasing
interest in behavioral and alternative approaches, we have reviewed the pertinent studies to evaluate their validity. We
delineate various shortcomings in these reports, including potential methodological caveats, problems with experimental controls, and controversial data interpretation. Overall, the data do not seem to support hypnosis as a viable
option for significant long-term improvement of myopia. However, hypnosis can increase ones subjective feeling of
enhanced visual acuity by affecting higher cognitive functions, such as attention, memorization, and perceptual
learning, which could influence performance on visual tasks. (Optom Vis Sci 2004;81:872879)
Key Words: myopia, suggestion, hypnosis, attention, visual acuity
Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 81, No. 11, November 2004
mance after prolonged wakefulness and sleep deprivation correlates with a cumulative strain on many muscles, including perhaps
the ciliary body. Visual fatigue induced by continuous engagement
in visual tasks also causes a temporary decrease in visual acuity.25
Given the evidence for psychological effects on vision, investigators have attempted to use a variety of psychological strategies to
improve myopia. Researchers have claimed success using biofeedback (enabling voluntary control by using system monitoring),
fading mechanisms (i.e., a variant of reinforcement), and training.26 31 Other scientists have disputed these findings. For example, some claim that although training may have a positive effect on
psychological conditions and subjective visual acuity, it does not
measurably reduce myopia.26, 3234 After reviewing the literature,
prominent researchers argued that more clinical data were needed
before operant conditioning and biofeedback qualified as effective
treatments of myopia.35
Precedents do exist for using operant conditioning to improve
myopia. However, they are based on unpublished observations36
and sparse data.37, 38 One study reported a significant decrease in
refractive error using operant conditioning.39 Several earlier studies claimed that optometric training could significantly improve
visual acuity without using corrective lenses.40, 41 However, investigators have concluded that these training techniques do not alter
the refractive power of the eye.42 Instead, visual acuity improves
when subjects learn to maximize their use of available perceptual
cues. It is likely that such perceptual changes form the basis of the
effects caused by hypnosis.
873
56 61
Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 81, No. 11, November 2004
A CRITIQUE
Hypnotic suggestion and other behavioral techniques may improve visual acuity in myopic individuals. However, the published
reports on the subject have many shortcomings that bring into
question the validity of such claims. These include questionable
background data, experimental problems, and controversial interpretation of results.
Graham and Leibowitzs influential study60 based its claim on
largely anecdotal findings. Not only did it reference preliminary
case studies67, 71 but also it cited data using scenarios devoid of
suggestion.65 67, 70 The references included an unrelated report,69
an unpublished case report using an authors wife as subject,68 and
a reference-free paper based on a brief unpublished thesis66 with an
unclear number of subjects (once reported as 8 and once as 9).
Methodological shortcomings within the experiments also obscure the validity of the results. Memorization effects occur when
the same chart is used between left and right eyes or test-retest
assessments of the same eye.78 Increased tolerance of blur can also
contribute to an apparent improvement in myopia after a period
Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 81, No. 11, November 2004
ALTERNATIVE ACCOUNTS
Although psychological approaches may not improve myopia at
the level of the eye, differences in visual information processing in
875
the brain can affect vision. When we train the brain to interpret
retinal blotches, the cognitive phase of seeing becomes more responsive, causing an improvement in vision. An illustration of this
phenomenon is a smudge on an X-ray chart that may be a blur to
a novice but highlights a serious medical indication to an experienced radiologist. The retinal images in the eyes of the two observers may be identical in clarity, size, and shape, but the cognitive
interpretation and mental contribution of this retinal image are
vastly different. In another example, individuals who failed military entrance tests because of color blindness managed to pass the
required test after visual training. There was consensus within
medical circles that the visual exercises did not cure their color
blindness but instead educated the individuals in how to better
discern colors. Regardless of whether these individuals were cured
or educated, they could distinguish the colored patterns of the
test92, 93 after completing the training exercises. Despite current
knowledge of the substrates of color vision, some practitioners still
mistakenly interpret these data to mean that those persons had
acquired a greater degree of color perception and color discrimination than they had before doing the exercises.
We do not wish to disparage perceptual training to develop a
keener ability to interpret blurred images. The improvement of
some aspects of visual performance through perceptual learning
has been verified.42 Instead, we stress that there is admissible evidence of improvement in visual acuity not explained by refractive
changes.
Graham and Leibowitz60 showed a slight increase in visual acuity for some subjects under suggestion. The effect was the same for
low to moderate myopes and was not related to relaxation of accommodation as would be present in pseudomyopia. Eye practitioners acknowledge that ones vision fares differently on different
examination days, either within multiple tests with the same examiner or among independent examiners. The degree to which an
examiner pushes the patient to discern the visual objects, colloquially called whipping the patient, can affect the testing, producing
an increase on the order of magnitude equal to that seen in some of
the experimental subjects studied under hypnosis. This outcome
probably results from increased attentional effort, concentration,
motivation, or a willingness to use visual (e.g., contrast) and cognitive (e.g., elimination) clues other than enhanced resolution. To
implicate suggestion or hypnosis as the cause of this slight increase
in visual acuity may be overreaching.
Negative accommodation could also possibly explain this occasional improvement in visual acuity in uncorrected myopes.94
Sparsely documented in rare individuals, the baffling phenomenon
of negative accommodation involves evanescent increases in vision
(i.e., flashes of clear vision) accompanied by a decrease in the overall
plus power of the eye. It is thought that there is a base tonus for the
accommodative mechanism9597 that, combined with the dioptric
power of the lens/cornea and the axial length of the eye, produces
the total refractive state. Actively reducing the base accommodative
tonus would reduce the plus power of the eye, enabling myopes to
see better.98 However, negative accommodation, if it does exist, is
extremely rare. In our search, we have found only a few personal
communications from prominent clinicians who report having
assessed negative accommodation objectively (e.g., by retinoscopy).94 However, these accounts are not consistent with the descrip-
Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 81, No. 11, November 2004
TABLE 1.
Chronological summary of the primary evidence (since 1950) typically cited in favor of the effects of hypnosis and
suggestion on visual acuity
Author(s)
Sample Size
Method
Weitzenhoffer69
N6
LeCron68
N1
Kline70
N1
Browning and
Crasilneck71
N9
Kelley62,63
N 414
Kliman and
Goldberg100
N 10
Copeland65,66
N 8 or 9
Davison and
Singleton67
N1
Graham59
N5
Graham and
Leibowitz60
N9
Sheehan et al.61
N 16
Kay101
N 75
A test of visual
discrimination in both the
waking and hypnotic states
Exploring the effects of
positive hypnotic
suggestion on visual acuity
in patients with
suppression amblyopia
(amblyopia ex anopsia)
Hypnotic suggestion
accompanied by
optometric assessments
using such manipulations
as cycloplegia and a
haploscope.
Studying visual recognition
thresholds of words seen
in hypnotic and control
waking states, compared
with a baseline waking
state.
Hypnosis without suggestion
A glasses-wearing subject
induced to have positive
and negative
hallucinations under
hypnosis with and without
cycloplegia
Hypnotic suggestion to
improve vision.
Three experiments to explore
whether hypnotic
suggestion could improve
vision in myopes while
refraction and acuity were
measured simultaneously.
Signal detection method to
assess monocular spatial
discrimination while
listening to either taped
hypnotic suggestion or
taped music.
Compares hypnosis with
suggestion for improved
vision, neutral hypnosis,
progressive relaxation, and
control conditions across
myopes
Effect Reported
Comments
Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 81, No. 11, November 2004
CONCLUSION
We have presented thorough evidence challenging the original
premise that hypnotic suggestion improves visual acuity in
myopes. Early studies supporting this use of hypnosis have many
shortcomings, including small sample sizes, weak procedures, and
disputable interpretation of results. Based on results from these
studies, the effect of suggestion on myopes visual acuity is not
likely to be significant or long lived.
Reports of temporary changes in subjective acuity and refractive
error as a function of behavioral interventions seem to support a
psychological component to vision. Psychological factors may play
a progressively more important role in our understanding of myopia. Whereas the correlation between suggestion and myopic visual
improvement remains uncertain, evidence relating hypnotic suggestion to attentional mechanisms is mounting.53 Hypnotic suggestion can affect visual attention, which in turn could influence
performance on visual tasks. These findings, together with data
illuminating visual attention and acuity,99 provide the likely mechanism of how suggestion can influence visual acuity.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank Mindy Tanzola and Karla Zadnik for meticulous editing and
comments on an early version of this manuscript, respectively.
Received March 2, 2003; accepted August 11, 2004.
REFERENCES
1. Safir A. Symposium: Clinical management of physiologic myopia.
Introduction. Ophthalmology 1979;86:67980.
2. Kleinstein RN, Jones LA, Hullett S, Kwon S, Lee RJ, Friedman NE,
Manny RE, Mutti DO, Yu JA, Zadnik K. Refractive error and
ethnicity in children. Arch Ophthalmol 2003;121:11417.
3. Zadnik K, Manny RE, Yu JA, Mitchell GL, Cotter SA, Quiralte JC,
Shipp M, Friedman NE, Kleinstein RN, Walker TW, Jones LA,
Moeschberger ML, Mutti DO. Ocular component data in schoolchildren as a function of age and gender. Optom Vis Sci 2003;80:
22636.
4. Gilmartin B. Myopia: pathways to therapy. Optom Vis Sci 2004;
81:13.
5. Kaplan RM. Seeing Without Glasses: Improving Your Vision Naturally. Hillsboro, OR: Beyond Words Publishing, 1994.
6. Kemery WE. Hypnosis may help visual problems. Hypnotherapy
Rev 2000;October:23.
7. Eyesight protection massage in Japan. Peking Rev 1977;15:278.
8. Bates WH. Eye training for the cure of functional myopia. NY Med
J 1912;78:102932.
9. Pollack P. The Truth about Eye Exercises. Philadelphia: Chilton
Co., 1956.
10. Worrall RS, Nevyas J, Barrett S. Eye-Related Quackery, February
2002.
Available
at:
http://www.quackwatch.com/
01QuackeryRelatedTopics/eyequack.html. Accessed August 13,
2004.
11. Friedman E. Vision training program for myopia management.
Am J Optom Physiol Opt 1981;58:54653.
877
Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 81, No. 11, November 2004
60.
61.
62.
63.
64.
65.
66.
67.
68.
69.
70.
71.
72.
73.
74.
75.
76.
77.
78.
79.
80.
81.
82.
83.
84.
Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 81, No. 11, November 2004
879
96. Gilmartin B, Hogan RE. The role of the sympathetic nervous system in ocular accommodation and ametropia. Ophthalmic Physiol
Opt 1985;5:913.
97. Gilmartin B, Mallen EA, Wolffsohn JS. Sympathetic control of
accommodation: evidence for inter-subject variation. Ophthalmic
Physiol Opt 2002;22:36671.
98. Randle RJ. Responses of myopes to volitional control training of
accommodation. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 1988;8:33340.
99. Yeshurun Y, Carrasco M. Spatial attention improves performance in
spatial resolution tasks. Vision Res 1999;39:293306.
100. Kliman G, Goldberg EL. Improved visual recognition during hypnosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1962;7:15562.
101. Kay LM. The effects of hypnosis, relaxation, and suggestion on
visual acuity. PhD dissertion. California School of Professional Psychology, 1992.
Amir Raz
Magnetic Resonance Imaging Unit in the Department of Psychiatry
Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
Columbia University College of Physicians & Surgeons
New York State Psychiatric Institute
1051 Riverside Drive, Unit 74
New York, NY 10032
e-mail: [email protected]
Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 81, No. 11, November 2004