A Comparative Study of Creativity and Cognitive Problem-Solving Strategies of High-IQ and Average Students
A Comparative Study of Creativity and Cognitive Problem-Solving Strategies of High-IQ and Average Students
http://gcq.sagepub.com
A Comparative Study of Creativity and Cognitive Problem-Solving Strategies of High-IQ and Average
Students
Christine Fiorella Russo
Gifted Child Quarterly 2004; 48; 179
DOI: 10.1177/001698620404800303
The online version of this article can be found at:
http://gcq.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/48/3/179
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
Additional services and information for Gifted Child Quarterly can be found at:
Email Alerts: http://gcq.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts
Subscriptions: http://gcq.sagepub.com/subscriptions
Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav
Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
Citations http://gcq.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/48/3/179
C R E AT I V I T Y A N D C O G N I T I V E P R O B L E M - S O LV I N G S T R AT E G I E S
ABSTRACT
This study assessed the creative thinking abilities of
high-IQ and average students on f luency, f lexibility,
originality, and elaboration. Seventeen high-IQ fifth
and sixth graders and 20 average fifth and sixth
graders were given 4 batteries of Torrances Verbal
and Figural Tests (A and B) as pretests and posttests
and three Future Problem Solving tasks to assess
their creative thinking and problem-solving skills. A
significant interaction between performance and IQ
over time was demonstrated for verbal f luency. The
high-IQ students scored low and finished higher
over time. The average students scored high on the
pretest, but dropped over time. The interaction for
figural elaboration indicated little difference
between the groups at pretest, but there was an
increase for the average students and a decrease for
the high-IQ students at posttest. Variability in performances of both high-IQ and average students
suggests the need for training in creative thinking
skills.
G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A RT E R LY S U M M E R 2 0 0 4 VO L 4 8
Downloaded from http://gcq.sagepub.com by kimbao bao on April 14, 2009
NO 3
179
C R E AT I V I T Y A N D C O G N I T I V E P R O B L E M - S O LV I N G S T R AT E G I E S
gence is required for creativity, but intelligence and creativity are by no means synonymous. Wallach and Kogan
(1965) demonstrated that highly intelligent, but not particularly creative subjects have a disinclination, rather
than an inability, to use their imaginations. Getzels and
Jackson (1959) and Torrance (1959) pointed out that, if
an intelligence test were used to select top-level talent,
70% of the people with the highest 20% of scores on a
battery of creativity tests would be missed. The work of
Getzels and Jackson (1962) and Wallach and Kogan
(1965) has made it clear that an IQ score tells only a fraction of the story, as the full knowledge of a persons
potential can only be discovered when IQ scores are
paired with creativity scores. Terman and Oden (1947)
revealed in a follow-up study of high-IQ students that
these students showed greater productivity in arts and
sciences when compared to an average-ability group,
though few reached the highest f lights of creative ability.
This suggests that intelligence is of some relevance to
achievement.
A distinct definition and interpretation of both creativity and intelligence can establish a theoretical prediction of the relationship between these two variables. This
prediction can be realized when creativity is viewed as
performance on a specific measure of divergent production and intelligence is defined as performance on a specific IQ test (Treffinger & Poggio, 1972).
Houtz, Rosenfield, and Tetenbaum (1978) investigated the relationship between intelligence, achievement, creative thinking, and problem solving among
233 intellectually gifted second through sixth graders.
The results of the factor analysis suggested that intelligence, achievement, creative thinking, and problem
solving can be separated conceptually. Gifted students
demonstrated strengths and weaknesses across the range
of tasks, rather than consistently high performance.
There was great individual variation within the sets of
creative thinking and problem-solving tasks, suggesting
a need for creativity and problem-solving skills training
for the gifted.
Intelligence and creativity tests have been widely
used as a basis for assessing intellectual giftedness
(Sternberg, 1983). Accumulation of evidence reveals that
intelligence tests disclose only minor variations in creative performance, but do not directly involve the ability
to create ideas or things. Research indicates that the ability to sense problem areas, to be f lexible in each of several ways, and to produce original ideas tend to have little
relation to tests used to measure intelligence (French,
1951; Guilford, 1959).
180
M e a s u r e s o f D i ve r g e n t T h i n k i n g
Since ordinary IQ tests fail to identify the potential
creative problem solver, IQ tests should be expanded to
include creative aptitude tasks (Guilford, 1950).
Divergent production tests, which require students to
produce their own answers, are absent in modern group
tests of intelligence (Guilford, 1972). It isnt necessary to
include IQ within creativity tests. Creativity tests are in
some ways an IQ test because both tests measure cognitive skills. However, creativity tests evaluate the students
higher mental functions as insight and creative imagination (Guilford, 1950). Guilford (1967) isolated a large
number of intellectual factors, among them convergent
and divergent thinking, and he believed that creativity
tests are tapping something that is ignored by conventional IQ tests.
An examination of the content of intelligence tests
reveals very little coverage of creative abilities (Guilford,
1950). IQ tests ref lect a conformist type of knowledge,
which is obtained from living in a specific social and educational environment; they rarely assess a students skill in
assimilating new information or in solving new problems
(Vernon, 1964). In addition, they reveal very little about
an individuals potential for further development
(Gardner, 1983). In contrast, tests of creative ability assess
those skills that are independent of academic achievement as measured by intelligence tests (Guilford, 1984).
Creativity tests measure creative behavior and reveal the
students creative talents in the areas of f luency, f lexibility, originality, and elaboration (Guilford, 1975). While
divergent thinking tests are not perfect measures of creativity, they are useful estimates of the individuals potential for creative thought (Runco, 1991). According to
Sternberg (1984), intelligence and creativity can be separated conceptually, and there are underlying processes
and strategies that can be used to improve mental processing.
C r e a t i v i t y a n d D i ve r g e n t
Thinking
For the past 50 years, there has been no general consensus among psychologists and educators about the
nature of creativity and the reliability of criteria that
could be used in measuring this psychological construct.
It is thought that the lack of agreement on this issue
among psychologists and educators has been due to the
multidimensional nature of creativity, which has brought
G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A RT E R LY S U M M E R 2 0 0 4 VO L 4 8
NO 3
C R E AT I V I T Y A N D C O G N I T I V E P R O B L E M - S O LV I N G S T R AT E G I E S
G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A RT E R LY S U M M E R 2 0 0 4 VO L 4 8
Downloaded from http://gcq.sagepub.com by kimbao bao on April 14, 2009
NO 3
181
C R E AT I V I T Y A N D C O G N I T I V E P R O B L E M - S O LV I N G S T R AT E G I E S
Method
C r e a t i ve I n t e l l i g e n c e
Participants
182
G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A RT E R LY S U M M E R 2 0 0 4 VO L 4 8
NO 3
C R E AT I V I T Y A N D C O G N I T I V E P R O B L E M - S O LV I N G S T R AT E G I E S
two standard deviations in size, suggesting that the highIQ group was functioning in the very superior to superior range and the average group was functioning in the
average range. The fifth-grade high-IQ group consisted of 4 boys and 4 girls, and the sixth-grade high-IQ
group was made up of 5 boys and 4 girls. The participants
in the fifth-grade average group included 5 boys and 7
girls, and the sixth-grade average group was comprised of
4 boys and 4 girls. There were several ethnic groups represented in this study: 34 White students (91.9%), two
Asian students (5.4%), and one Hispanic (2.7%) student.
Materials
Creativity. The Verbal Torrance Test of Creative
Thinking With Words (TTCT), Form A (Torrance,
1990b) was administered as a pretest at the beginning of
the program in October. The Verbal TTCT uses six
word-based tasks to assess the three mental functions of
f luency (total number of relevant responses), f lexibility
(number of divergent categories of relevant responses),
and originality (the statistical rarity of responses). These
exercises are models of important kinds of creative thinking required in daily life (Torrance, 1990b). They give
students opportunities to look at pictures and then
respond by asking questions, improving products, and
supposing. The Figural Torrance Test of Creative
Thinking With Pictures, Form A (Torrance, 1990a) was
also given as a pretest. The Figural TTCT uses three picture-based exercises to assess the mental characteristics of
f luency, f lexibility, originality, elaboration (adding details
to the original concept), abstractness of titles, and resistance to closure (keeping open to new ideas; Torrance,
1966; Torrance & Ball, 1992).
Validity and Reliability of the Torrance Tests of Creative
Thinking. Data on the Torrance Tests of Creative
Thinking (TTCT) suggests that a different kind of ability is being measured than that assessed by traditional
achievement and ability tests. Torrance and others have
conducted follow-up studies showing that high-scoring
individuals on the TTCT continue to be highly creative
5 to 40 years later (Millar, 2002; Torrance, 2000;
Torrance & Wu, 1981). The raw scores for the tests are
determined through the use of standard rubrics by
trained scorers with reported interrater reliabilities of .95
or higher. The norm-referenced scores are national percentiles and standard scores (40180), with a mean of
100 and a standard deviation of 15. The scores are
reported for different grade and age groups (Torrance,
1998).
G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A RT E R LY S U M M E R 2 0 0 4 VO L 4 8
Downloaded from http://gcq.sagepub.com by kimbao bao on April 14, 2009
NO 3
183
C R E AT I V I T Y A N D C O G N I T I V E P R O B L E M - S O LV I N G S T R AT E G I E S
Procedure
The school system allowed accessibility to the students Cognitive Skills Index (CSI) scores, but precluded
any further testing of IQ by the examiner. Based upon
the districts group IQ test, the CSI, 17 students were
placed in the high-IQ group and 20 students in the average group. Torrances Figural and Verbal Tests A of
Creative Thinking, used as pretests, were administered to
all participants as a group in a regular classroom setting
during the first session. At the end of 6 months,
Torrances Figural and Verbal Tests B of Creative
Thinking were given to all the students during the last
session.
For 6 months, all the students attended one 90minute session per week and worked on three problems
developed by the Future Problem Solving program using
the six-step creative problem-solving process. For
approximately 7 weeks, the participants researched and
solved Practice Problem #1 using only two steps of the
G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A RT E R LY S U M M E R 2 0 0 4 VO L 4 8
NO 3
C R E AT I V I T Y A N D C O G N I T I V E P R O B L E M - S O LV I N G S T R AT E G I E S
Table 1
The Verbal Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (TTCT) Scores of High-IQ and Average Students
Pretest
Posttest
Average IQ
TTCT Scores
Fluency
Flexibility
Originality
High IQ
Average IQ
High IQ
SD
SD
SD
SD
83.0
37.1
47.8
27.8
11.8
21.4
74.3
33.2
46.1
40.6
15.9
25.2
74.6
34.6
45.7
26.8
13.8
16.4
84.9
32.4
43.4
45.6
17.4
24.5
Table 2
The Figural Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (TTCT) Scores of High-IQ and Average Students.
Pretest
Posttest
Average IQ
TTCT Scores
Fluency
Flexibility
Originality
Elaboration
High IQ
Average IQ
High IQ
SD
SD
SD
SD
29.9
21.9
35.7
38.9
7.0
4.2
11.2
15.3
29.1
21.6
34.9
40.6
10.1
6.8
13.9
15.4
16.1
13.3
36.6
44.2
10.0
6.0
14.7
22.7
13.1
11.8
33.6
34.3
6.2
4.5
15.1
17.4
Results
The first research question asked whether there were
any significant differences in problem identification and
problem solution for the variables of f luency, f lexibility,
originality, and elaboration between high-IQ and average
students. The data collected for problem identification
and problem solutions consisted of the eight teams
aggregate numerical scores in each area. Results of t-tests
indicated no significant differences for problem identification (t [10] = .87, p = .40) or problem solution (t [10] =
-.54, p = .60). Although there was no significant difference between the two groups, the high-IQ groups moti-
G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A RT E R LY S U M M E R 2 0 0 4 VO L 4 8
Downloaded from http://gcq.sagepub.com by kimbao bao on April 14, 2009
NO 3
185
C R E AT I V I T Y A N D C O G N I T I V E P R O B L E M - S O LV I N G S T R AT E G I E S
Figure 1. Mean pretest and posttest verbal fluency scores on Torrance Test of Creative Thinking for high-IQ
and average students.
formance and IQ over time was demonstrated for verbal
fluency (F [1, 35] = 5.37, p = .026). The average students
scored high on the pretest (M = 83.0, 67th national percentile) and dropped over time (M = 74.6, 57th percentile),
while the high-IQ students started low (M = 74.3, 57th
national percentile) and finished higher (M = 84.9, 69th
national percentile). A significant interaction was also
obtained on figural elaboration (F [1, 34] = 3.01, p = .092).
The average students scored low on the pretest (M = 39.0,
32nd national percentile), but finished higher over time on
the posttest (M = 44.0, 48th national percentile). The
high-IQ students scored high on the pretests (M = 41.0,
40th national percentile), but dropped to lower levels on
the posttest (M = 34.0, 28th national percentile). Post hoc
comparisons for simple main effects revealed that, at
posttest, high-IQ students scored higher than average students did on verbal fluency (see Figure 1) whereas the
average students did better than the high-IQ students on
figural elaboration (see Figure 2). Performance in the other
areas of flexibility and originality from pretest to posttest
appeared to be unrelated to IQ.
Discussion
In testing for the relationship among IQ, creativity,
and problem solving, two student groups were assessed:
the average group with a mean IQ of 104 and the high186
IQ group with a mean IQ of 130. Results provide statistical support for gains in f luency, f lexibility, originality,
and elaboration as a result of participation in a creative
problem solving program for both groups.
At the inception and end of the 6-month Future
Problem Solving program, the Verbal and Figural Forms
A (pretest) and the Forms B (posttest) of the Torrance
Tests of Creative Thinking were administered, and significant interactions were demonstrated between performance and IQ for verbal f luency and figural elaboration. Posttest results showed that, while the gifted students in the present study appear to perform at a higher
level than students of less superior intelligence on verbal
f luency, their performance was by no means consistent
across all types of creative thinking skills. This significant
difference in performance by the high-IQ group could be
due to the fact that both the IQ tests used to identify the
students and the verbal f luency measure are loaded with
the same verbal factors. While both the IQ tests and
Verbal Torrance Tests are dependent upon students verbal capabilities and therefore may have similar factor loadings, there are significant differences in the way in which
the abilities affect the scores. The verbal IQ tests have
correct/prescribed answers, while the TTCT Verbal tests
require responses in the form of questions, statements,
suppositions, imaginative situations, product improvement, and multiple uses of materials (Torrance, 1990b). It
is also possible that the high-IQ groups were more f luent
G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A RT E R LY S U M M E R 2 0 0 4 VO L 4 8
NO 3
C R E AT I V I T Y A N D C O G N I T I V E P R O B L E M - S O LV I N G S T R AT E G I E S
Figure 2. Mean pretest and posttest figural elaboration scores on Torrance Test of Creative Thinking for
high-IQ and average students.
or more adept in structuring their mental apparatuses,
which helped them to apply their verbal skills (Hunt,
1978). Sternberg (1981) has maintained that higher order
thinking processes could possibly allow high-IQ students
to excel in the access and implementation of information.
The high-IQ students were able to reason in a logical
manner, making connections among ideas and perceiving
more aspects of a problem.
The average subjects scored higher on figural elaboration than the high-IQ students. This might be attributed to a relative strength on nonverbal tasks, which
allowed them to elaborate on their drawings over an
extended period of time. As suggested by Gardner (1983,
1995), both groups appeared to have relative strengths
and weaknesses in different areas. The findings in this
empirical study are of significance because they indicate
that IQ may be limited in its importance as a predictor of
creativity. There was no indication that the high-IQ students were more creative overall than the average students. Therefore, IQ does not correlate with creativity
measures, which is consistent with previous research
studies.
The findings, moreover, may provide some corraborative evidence for the support of Houtz, Denmark,
Rosenfield, and Tetenbaums (1980) study, which
showed that the effects of intelligence on tolerance for
ambiguity, locus of control, and self-esteem may be less
important than the effects of affective characteristics.
Previous research has indicated that intelligence, creativity, and problem solving can be separated conceptually
(Guilford, 1975; Sternberg, 1981, 1984; Taylor &
Holland, 1962) and that other factors such as motivation
and determination impact upon creativity. Many others
have also indicated that IQ alone is not the sole predictor
of creative achievement and can only be one method for
the identification of divergent thinkers (Houtz,
Rosenfield, & Tetenbaum, 1978; Parnes & Noller, 1973;
Rose & Lin, 1984). Investigations by Amabile (1983,
1996), Gardner (1993), and Sternberg and Lubart (1991,
1995) have supported the importance of multiple components that are necessary for creativity to occur. While
this study has aimed to shed more light upon the relationship among IQ, creativity, and cognitive problemsolving strategies, future research needs to address
numerous significant and relevant issues surrounding
creativity.
Creativity must be viewed from a wide perspective.
Divergent thinking and divergent thinking tests are not
the sole predictors of creative performance. According to
Sternberg and Lubart (1996),
Researchers have criticized brief paper and pencil tests as trivial, inadequate measures of creativity (see essays in Sternberg, 1986); they suggested that more significant productionssuch as
drawings, writing samples, or vocational-avoca-
G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A RT E R LY S U M M E R 2 0 0 4 VO L 4 8
Downloaded from http://gcq.sagepub.com by kimbao bao on April 14, 2009
NO 3
187
C R E AT I V I T Y A N D C O G N I T I V E P R O B L E M - S O LV I N G S T R AT E G I E S
Conclusion
References
In conclusion, evidence from the results of the present study suggests the need for creativity and problemsolving skills training for both average and gifted students.
The controversy surrounding the nature of creativity and
the applicable criterion used in measuring this psychological construct still remains a viable challenge to educators, psychologists, and laypeople. Through various
teaching methods, creative thinking can be developed
regardless of the individuals innate abilities. By means of
188
G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A RT E R LY S U M M E R 2 0 0 4 VO L 4 8
NO 3
C R E AT I V I T Y A N D C O G N I T I V E P R O B L E M - S O LV I N G S T R AT E G I E S
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). The domain of creativity. In M.
A. Runco & R. S. Albert (Eds.), Theories of creativity (pp
190212). Newburg Park, CA: Sage.
CTB Macmillan/McGraw-Hill. (1993). Test of Cognitive
Skills/2: Technical report. Monterey, CA: Author.
Davidson, J. E., & Sternberg, R. J. (1984). The role of insight
in intellectual giftedness. Gifted Child Quarterly, 28, 5864.
Finke, R. A., Ward, T. B., & Smith, S. M. (1992). Creative cognition: Theory, research, and applications. Cambridge:
Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring:
A new area of cognitive developmental inquiry. American
Psychologist, 34, 906911.
French, J. W. (1951). The description of aptitude and achievement
tests in terms of rotated factors. Psychometric Monographs, 5
(pp. 113; Part 2). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: BasicBooks.
Gardner, H. (1988a). Creative lives and creative works: A
synthetic scientific approach. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),
The nature of creativity: Contemporary psychological perspectives (pp. 298321). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge
University Press.
Gardner, H. (1988b). Creativity: An interdisciplinary perspective. Creativity Research Journal, 1, 826.
Gardner, H. (1993). Creating minds: An anatomy of creativity seen
through the lives of Freud, Einstein, Picasso, Stravinsky, Eliot,
Graham, and Gandhi. New York: BasicBooks.
Gardner, H. (1995). Multiple intelligences as a catalyst. English
Journal, 84, 1618.
Getzels, J. W., & Jackson, P. W. (1959). The highly intelligent
and the highly creative adolescent: A summary of some
research findings. In C. W. Taylor (Ed.), The third University
of Utah research conference on the identification of creative scientific talent (pp. 4657). Salt Lake City: University of Utah.
Getzels, J. W., & Jackson, P. W. (1962). Creativity and intelligence.
New York: Wiley.
Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5,
444454.
Guilford, J. P. (1959). Three faces of intellect. American
Psychologist, 14, 469479.
Guilford, J. P. (1964). Creative thinking and problem solving.
Education Digest, 29, 2931.
Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Guilford, J. P. (1972). Intellect and the gifted. Gifted Child
Quarterly, 16, 175187.
Guilford, J. P. (1975). Varieties of creative giftedness, their
measurement and development. Gifted Child Quarterly, 19,
107121.
Guilford, J. P. (1977). Way beyond the IQ. Buffalo, NY: Creative
Education Foundation.
Guilford, J. P. (1984). Varieties of divergent-production abilities
or functions. Journal of Creative Behavior, 18, 110.
Guilford, J. P., & Christensen, P. R. (1973). The one-way relation between creative potential and IQ. Journal of Creative
Behavior, 7, 247252.
Guilford, J. P., & Hoepfner, R. (1966). Creative potential as
related to measures of IQ and verbal comprehension.
Indian Journal of Psychology, 41, 716.
Houtz, J. C., Rosenfield, S., & Tetenbaum, T. J. (1978).
Creative thinking in gifted elementary schoolchildren.
Gifted Child Quarterly, 22, 513519.
Houtz, J. C., Denmark, R., Rosenfield, S. & Tetenbaum, T.
J. (1980). Problem solving and personality characteristics related to differing levels of intelligence and
ideational f luency. Contemporary Educational Psychology,
5, 118123.
Hunt, E. B. (1978). Mechanics of verbal ability. Psychological
Review, 85, 109130.
Isaksen, S. G., & Parnes, S. J. (1985). Curriculum planning for
creative thinking and problem solving. Journal of Creative
Behavior, 19, 118.
Lowenfeld, V. (1959). What is creative teaching? In Proceedings of
second Minnesota conference on gifted children. Minneapolis:
University of Minnesota.
Lubart, T. I., & Sternberg, R. J. (1995). An investment
approach to creativity: Theory and data. In S. M. Smith, T.
B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.), The creative cognition approach
(pp. 271302). Cambridge: Massachusetts Institute of
Technology.
MacKinnon, D. W. (1962). The nature and nurture of creative
talent. American Psychologist, 17, 484495.
McNemar, O. (1964). Lost: Our intelligence? Why? American
Psychologist, 19, 871882.
Millar, G. W. (2002). Torrance kids at M-Life. Westport, CT:
Ablex.
Osborn, A. F. (1963). Creative imagination (3rd ed). New York:
Scribners.
Parnes, S. J. (1967). Creative behavior guide book. New York:
Scribners.
Parnes, S. J. (1972). Creativity: Unlocking human potential.
Buffalo, NY: D.O.K.
Parnes, S. J., & Noller, R. B. (1973). Toward supersanity:
Channeled freedom. Buffalo, NY: D.O.K.
Patton, C. (Ed.). (2002). Guidelines for evaluation 20022003.
Lexington, KY: Future Problem Solving Program 117.
Plucker, J. A., & Renzulli, J. S. (1999). Psychometric approaches to the study of human creativity. In R. J. Sternberg
(Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 3561). Cambridge,
England: Cambridge University Press.
Reisman, F., & Bach, C. N. (2002, April). Applying creativity to
uncover hidden talent in high school students. Paper presented at
the meeting of the American Creativity Association,
Drexel University, Philadelphia, PA.
Rose, L. H., & Lin, H. T. (1984). A meta-analysis of long-term
creativity training programs. Journal of Creative Behavior, 18,
1122.
G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A RT E R LY S U M M E R 2 0 0 4 VO L 4 8
Downloaded from http://gcq.sagepub.com by kimbao bao on April 14, 2009
NO 3
189
C R E AT I V I T Y A N D C O G N I T I V E P R O B L E M - S O LV I N G S T R AT E G I E S
Runco, M. A. (1986). Maximal performance on divergent
thinking tests by gifted, talented, and nongifted children
Psychological Schools, 23, 308 315.
Runco, M. A. (1991). Divergent thinking. Norwood, NJ; Ablex.
Runco, M. A., & Okuda, S. M. (1988). Problem discovery,
divergent thinking, and the creative process. Journal of
Youth and Adolescence, 17, 211220.
Smith, S. M., Ward, T. B., & Finke, R. A. (1995). Paradoxes,
principles, and prospects for the future of creative cognition. In S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.),
The creative cognition approach (pp. 327335). Cambridge:
Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
Sternberg, R. J. (1981). A componential theory of intellectual
giftedness. Gifted Child Quarterly, 25, 8693.
Sternberg, R. J. (1983). Criteria for intellectual skills training.
Educational Researcher, 12, 612.
Sternberg, R. J. (1984). What should intelligence tests test?
Implications of a triarchic theory of intelligence for intelligence testing. Educational Researcher, 13, 515.
Sternberg, R. J. (1985). A triarchic theory of intellectual giftedness. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.), Conceptions
of giftedness (pp. 223243). Cambridge, England:
Cambridge University Press.
Sternberg, R. J., Conway, B., Ketron, J. L., & Bernstein, M.
(1981). Peoples conceptions of intelligence. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 3755.
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1991). An investment theory of
creativity and its development. Human Development, 34, 131.
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1996). Investing in creativity.
American Psychologist, 51, 677688.
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1999). The concept of creativity: Prospects and paradigms. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),
Handbook of creativity (pp. 315). Cambridge, England:
Cambridge University Press.
Taylor, C. W., & Holland, J. L. (1962). Development and application of tests of creativity. Review of Educational Research,
32, 91102.
Terman, L. M., Oden, M. H., & Bailey, M. (1947). The gifted
child grows up: Twenty-five years follow-up of a superior group.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Torrance, E. P. (1959). Explorations in creative thinking in the
190
G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A RT E R LY S U M M E R 2 0 0 4 VO L 4 8
NO 3