Cognitive Science
Cognitive Science
1 of 14
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Cognitive_science
Cognitive science
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Contents
1 History
2 Principles
2.1 Levels of analysis
2.2 Interdisciplinary nature
2.3 Cognitive science: the term
3 Scope
3.1 Artificial intelligence
3.2 Attention
3.3 Knowledge, and Processing, of
Language
3.4 Learning and development
3.5 Memory
3.6 Perception and action
4 Research methods
4.1 Behavioral experiments
4.2 Brain imaging
4.3 Computational modeling
4.4 Neurobiological methods
5 Key findings
6 Criticism
7 Notable researchers
8 See also
9 References
10 Further reading
11 External links
1/27/2012 12:05 PM
2 of 14
History
Cognitive science has a pre-history traceable back to ancient
Greek philosophical texts (see Plato's Meno); and certainly must
include writers such as Descartes, David Hume, Immanuel Kant,
Benedict de Spinoza, Nicolas Malebranche, Pierre Cabanis,
Leibniz and John Locke. But, although these early writers
contributed greatly to the philosophical discovery of mind and
this would ultimately lead to the development of psychology,
they were working with an entirely different set of tools and core
concepts than those of the cognitive scientist.
The modern culture of cognitive science can be traced back to
the early cyberneticists in the 1930s and 1940s, such as Warren
McCulloch and Walter Pitts, who sought to understand the
organizing principles of the mind. McCulloch and Pitts
developed the first variants of what are now known as artificial
neural networks, models of computation inspired by the
structure of biological neural networks.
Another precursor was the early development of the theory of
computation and the digital computer in the 1940s and 1950s.
Alan Turing and John von Neumann were instrumental in these
developments. The modern computer, or Von Neumann machine,
would play a central role in cognitive science, both as a
metaphor for the mind, and as a tool for investigation.
In 1959, Noam Chomsky published a scathing review of B. F.
Skinner's book Verbal Behavior. At the time, Skinner's
behaviorist paradigm dominated psychology: Most psychologists
focused on functional relations between stimulus and response,
without positing internal representations. Chomsky argued that
in order to explain language, we needed a theory like generative
grammar, which not only attributed internal representations but
characterized their underlying order.
The term cognitive science was coined by Christopher LonguetHiggins in his 1973 commentary on the Lighthill report, which
concerned the then-current state of Artificial Intelligence
research.[2] In the same decade, the journal Cognitive Science
and the Cognitive Science Society were founded.[3] In 1982,
Vassar College became the first institution in the world to grant
an undergraduate degree in Cognitive Science.[4]
In the 1970s and early 1980s, much cognitive science research
focused on the possibility of artificial intelligence. Researchers
such as Marvin Minsky would write computer programs in
languages such as LISP to attempt to formally characterize the
steps that human beings went through, for instance, in making
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Cognitive_science
Computational physics
Condensed matter physics
Experimental physics Mechanics
Nuclear physics
Particle physics Plasma physics
Quantum mechanics (introduction)
Solid mechanics Theoretical physics
Thermodynamics Entropy
General relativity M-theory
Special relativity
Chemistry
Acid-base reaction theories Alchemy
Analytical chemistry Astrochemistry
Biochemistry Crystallography
Environmental chemistry Food science
Geochemistry Green chemistry
Inorganic chemistry Materials science
Molecular physics Nuclear chemistry
Organic chemistry Photochemistry
Physical chemistry Radiochemistry
Solid-state chemistry Stereochemistry
Supramolecular chemistry
Surface science Theoretical chemistry
Astronomy
Astrophysics Cosmology
Galactic astronomy Planetary geology
Planetary science Stellar astronomy
Earth sciences
Atmospheric sciences Ecology
Environmental science Geodesy
Geology Geomorphology
Geophysics Glaciology Hydrology
Limnology Mineralogy Oceanography
Paleoclimatology Palynology
Physical geography Soil science
Space science
Life sciences
Biology
Anatomy Astrobiology Biochemistry
Biogeography Biological engineering
Biophysics
Behavioral neuroscience Biotechnology
Botany Cell biology Conservation biology
Cryobiology
Developmental biology
Ecology Ethnobiology
Evolutionary biology (introduction)
Genetics (introduction)
Gerontology Immunology Limnology
1/27/2012 12:05 PM
3 of 14
Principles
Levels of analysis
A central tenet of cognitive science is that a complete
understanding of the mind/brain cannot be attained by studying
only a single level. An example would be the problem of
remembering a phone number and recalling it later. One
approach to understanding this process would be to study
behavior through direct observation. A person could be
presented with a phone number, asked to recall it after some
delay. Then the accuracy of the response could be measured.
Another approach would be to study the firings of individual
neurons while a person is trying to remember the phone number.
Neither of these experiments on their own would fully explain
how the process of remembering a phone number works. Even if
the technology to map out every neuron in the brain in real-time
were available, and it were known when each neuron was firing,
it would still be impossible to know how a particular firing of
neurons translates into the observed behavior. Thus an
understanding of how these two levels relate to each other is
needed. This can be provided by a functional level account of
the process. Studying a particular phenomenon from multiple
levels creates a better understanding of the processes that occur
in the brain to give rise to a particular behavior. Marr[6] gave a
famous description of three levels of analysis:
1. the computational theory, specifying the goals of the
computation;
2. representation and algorithm, giving a representation of
the input and output and the algorithm which transforms
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Cognitive_science
1/27/2012 12:05 PM
4 of 14
Interdisciplinary nature
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Cognitive_science
Scientific method
History of science
Philosophy of science
Science policy
Humanities
Fringe science
Pseudoscience
Scope
Cognitive science is a large field, and covers a wide array of topics on cognition. However, it should be
recognized that cognitive science is not equally concerned with every topic that might bear on the nature and
operation of the mind or intelligence. Social and cultural factors, emotion, consciousness, animal cognition,
comparative and evolutionary approaches are frequently de-emphasized or excluded outright, often based on
key philosophical conflicts. Another important mind-related subject that the cognitive sciences tend to avoid is
the existence of qualia, with discussions over this issue being sometimes limited to only mentioning qualia as a
philosophically-open matter. Some within the cognitive science community, however, consider these to be vital
topics, and advocate the importance of investigating them.[8]
Below are some of the main topics that cognitive science is concerned with. This is not an exhaustive list, but is
meant to cover the wide range of intelligent behaviors. See List of cognitive science topics for a list of various
aspects of the field.
1/27/2012 12:05 PM
5 of 14
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Cognitive_science
Artificial intelligence
Main article: Artificial intelligence
"... One major contribution of AI and cognitive science to psychology has been the information processing
model of human thinking in which the metaphor of brain-as-computer is taken quite literally. ." AAAI Web
pages (http://www.aaai.org/AITopics/html/cogsci.html#simon) .
Artificial intelligence (AI) involves the study of cognitive phenomena in machines. One of the practical goals of
AI is to implement aspects of human intelligence in computers. Computers are also widely used as a tool with
which to study cognitive phenomena. Computational modeling uses simulations to study how human intelligence
may be structured.[9] (See the section on computational modeling in the Research Methods section.)
There is some debate in the field as to whether the mind is best viewed as a huge array of small but individually
feeble elements (i.e. neurons), or as a collection of higher-level structures such as symbols, schemas, plans, and
rules. The former view uses connectionism to study the mind, whereas the latter emphasizes symbolic
computations. One way to view the issue is whether it is possible to accurately simulate a human brain on a
computer without accurately simulating the neurons that make up the human brain.
Attention
Main article: Attention
Attention is the selection of important information. The human mind is bombarded with millions of stimuli and it
must have a way of deciding which of this information to process. Attention is sometimes seen as a spotlight,
meaning one can only shine the light on a particular set of information. Experiments that support this metaphor
include the dichotic listening task (Cherry, 1957) and studies of inattentional blindness (Mack and Rock, 1998).
In the dichotic listening task, subjects are bombarded with two different messages, one in each ear, and told to
focus on only one of the messages. At the end of the experiment, when asked about the content of the
unattended message, subjects cannot report it.
The study of language processing ranges from the investigation of the sound patterns of speech to the meaning
of words and whole sentences. Linguistics often divides language processing into orthography, phonology and
1/27/2012 12:05 PM
6 of 14
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Cognitive_science
phonetics, morphology, syntax, semantics, and pragmatics. Many aspects of language can be studied from each
of these components and from their interaction.
The study of language processing in cognitive science is closely tied to the field of linguistics. Linguistics was
traditionally studied as a part of the humanities, including studies of history, art and literature. In the last fifty
years or so, more and more researchers have studied knowledge and use of language as a cognitive
phenomenon, the main problems being how knowledge of language can be acquired and used, and what
precisely it consists of. Linguists have found that, while humans form sentences in ways apparently governed by
very complex systems, they are remarkably unaware of the rules that govern their own speech. Thus linguists
must resort to indirect methods to determine what those rules might be, if indeed rules as such exist. In any
event, if speech is indeed governed by rules, they appear to be opaque to any conscious consideration.
Memory
Main article: Memory
Memory allows us to store information for later retrieval. Memory is often thought of consisting of both a
long-term and short-term store. Long-term memory allows us to store information over prolonged periods (days,
weeks, years). We do not yet know the practical limit of long-term memory capacity. Short-term memory allows
us to store information over short time scales (seconds or minutes).
Memory is also often grouped into declarative and procedural forms. Declarative memory--grouped into subsets
of semantic and episodic forms of memory--refers to our memory for facts and specific knowledge, specific
meanings, and specific experiences (e.g., Who was the first president of the U.S.A.?, or "What did I eat for
breakfast four days ago?). Procedural memory allows us to remember actions and motor sequences (e.g. how to
ride a bicycle) and is often dubbed implicit knowledge or memory .
Cognitive scientists study memory just as psychologists do, but tend to focus in more on how memory bears on
cognitive processes, and the interrelationship between cognition and memory. One example of this could be,
1/27/2012 12:05 PM
7 of 14
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Cognitive_science
what mental processes does a person go through to retrieve a long-lost memory? Or, what differentiates between
the cognitive process of recognition (seeing hints of something before remembering it, or memory in context)
and recall (retrieving a memory, as in "fill-in-the-blank")?
The study of haptic (tactile), olfactory, and gustatory stimuli also fall into the
domain of perception.
Action is taken to refer to the output of a system. In humans, this is accomplished through motor responses.
Spatial planning and movement, speech production, and complex motor movements are all aspects of action.
Research methods
Many different methodologies are used to study cognitive science. As the field is highly interdisciplinary,
research often cuts across multiple areas of study, drawing on research methods from psychology, neuroscience,
computer science and systems theory.
Behavioral experiments
In order to have a description of what constitutes intelligent behavior, one must study behavior itself. This type
of research is closely tied to that in cognitive psychology and psychophysics. By measuring behavioral responses
to different stimuli, one can understand something about how those stimuli are processed. Lewandowski and
Strohmetz (2009) review a collection of innovative uses of behavioral measurement in psychology including
behavioral traces, behavioral observations, and behavioral choice.[11] Behavioral traces are pieces of evidence
that indicate behavior occurred, but the actor is not present (e.g., litter in a parking lot or readings on an electric
meter). Behavioral observations involve the direct witnessing of the actor engaging in the behavior (e.g.,
watching how close a person sits next to another person). Behavioral choices are when a person selects between
two or more options (e.g., voting behavior, choice of a punishment for another participant).
Reaction time. The time between the presentation of a stimulus and an appropriate response can
indicate differences between two cognitive processes, and can indicate some things about their nature.
For example, if in a search task the reaction times vary proportionally with the number of elements,
then it is evident that this cognitive process of searching involves serial instead of parallel processing.
Psychophysical responses. Psychophysical experiments are an old psychological technique, which has
been adopted by cognitive psychology. They typically involve making judgments of some physical
property, e.g. the loudness of a sound. Correlation of subjective scales between individuals can show
1/27/2012 12:05 PM
8 of 14
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Cognitive_science
cognitive or sensory biases as compared to actual physical measurements. Some examples include:
sameness judgments for colors, tones, textures, etc.
threshold differences for colors, tones, textures, etc.
Eye tracking. This methodology is used to study a variety of cognitive processes, most notably visual
perception and language processing. The fixation point of the eyes is linked to an individual's focus of
attention. Thus, by monitoring eye movements, we can study what information is being processed at a
given time. Eye tracking allows us to study cognitive processes on extremely short time scales. Eye
movements reflect online decision making during a task, and they provide us with some insight into
the ways in which those decisions may be processed.
Brain imaging
Main article: Neuroimaging
Brain imaging involves analyzing activity within the brain while performing various
cognitive tasks. This allows us to link behavior and brain function to help
understand how information is processed. Different types of imaging techniques
vary in their temporal (time-based) and spatial (location-based) resolution. Brain
imaging is often used in cognitive neuroscience.
Single photon emission computed tomography and Positron emission
tomography. SPECT and PET use radioactive isotopes, which are
injected into the subject's bloodstream and taken up by the brain. By
observing which areas of the brain take up the radioactive isotope, we
can see which areas of the brain are more active than other areas. PET
has similar spatial resolution to fMRI, but it has extremely poor temporal
resolution.
Computational modeling
1/27/2012 12:05 PM
9 of 14
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Cognitive_science
Symbolic modeling evolved from the computer science paradigms using the technologies of
Knowledge-based systems, as well as a philosophical perspective, see for example "Good
Old-Fashioned Artificial Intelligence" (GOFAI). They are developed by the first cognitive researchers
and later used in information engineering for expert systems . Since the early 1990s it was generalized
in systemics for the investigation of functional human-like intelligence models, such as personoids,
and, in parallel, developed as the SOAR environment. Recently, especially in the context of cognitive
decision making, symbolic cognitive modeling is extended to socio-cognitive approach including
social and organization cognition interrelated with a sub-symbolic not conscious layer.
Subsymbolic modeling includes Connectionist/neural network models. Connectionism relies on the
idea that the mind/brain is composed of simple nodes and that the power of the system comes
primarily from the existence and manner of connections between the simple nodes. Neural nets are
textbook implementations of this approach. Some critics of this approach feel that while these models
approach biological reality as a representation of how the system works, they lack explanatory powers
because complicated systems of connections with even simple rules are extremely complex and often
less interpretable than the system they model.
Other approaches gaining in popularity include the use of Dynamical systems theory and also techniques putting
symbolic models and connectionist models into correspondence (Neural-symbolic integration). Bayesian models,
often drawn from machine learning, are also gaining popularity.
All the above approaches tend to be generalized to the form of integrated computational models of a
synthetic/abstract intelligence, in order to be applied to the explanation and improvement of individual and
social/organizational decision-making and reasoning.
Neurobiological methods
Research methods borrowed directly from neuroscience and neuropsychology can also help us to understand
aspects of intelligence. These methods allow us to understand how intelligent behavior is implemented in a
physical system.
Single-unit recording
Direct brain stimulation
Animal models
Postmortem studies
Key findings
Cognitive science has much to its credit. Among other accomplishments, it has given rise to models of human
cognitive bias and risk perception, and has been influential in the development of behavioral finance, part of
economics. It has also given rise to a new theory of the philosophy of mathematics, and many theories of
1/27/2012 12:05 PM
10 of 14
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Cognitive_science
artificial intelligence, persuasion and coercion. It has made its presence firmly known in the philosophy of
language and epistemology - a modern revival of rationalism - as well as constituting a substantial wing of
modern linguistics. Fields of cognitive science have been influential in understanding the brain's particular
functional systems (and functional deficits) ranging from speech production to auditory processing and visual
perception. It has made progress in understanding how damage to particular areas of the brain affect cognition,
and it has helped to uncover the root causes and results of specific disfunction, such as dyslexia, anopia, and
hemispatial neglect.
Criticism
See also: Functionalism (philosophy of mind)#Criticism
In a paper written shortly before his death, B.F. Skinner stated that "cognitive science is the creation science of
psychology."[12]
Notable researchers
See also: List of cognitive scientists
Some of the more recognized names in cognitive science are usually either the most controversial or the most
cited. Within philosophy familiar names include Daniel Dennett who writes from a computational systems
perspective, John Searle known for his controversial Chinese Room, Jerry Fodor who advocates functionalism,
and Douglas Hofstadter, famous for writing Gdel, Escher, Bach, which questions the nature of words and
thought. In the realm of linguistics, Noam Chomsky and George Lakoff have been influential (both have also
become notable as political commentators). In Artificial intelligence Marvin Minsky, Herbert Simon, Allen
Newell, and Kevin Warwick are prominent. Popular names in the discipline of psychology include James
McClelland and Steven Pinker. Anthropologists Dan Sperber, Edwin Hutchins, Scott Atran, Pascal Boyer and
Joseph Henrich have been involved in collaborative projects with cognitive and social psychologists, political
scientists and evolutionary biologists in attempts to develop general theories of culture formation, religion and
political association.
See also
Affective science
Cognitive science of religion
Cognitive bias
Cognitive linguistics
Cognitive neuropsychology
Cognitive neuroscience
Cognitive psychology
Computational neuroscience
Decision theory
Decision field theory
Dynamicism
Educational psychology
Educational neuroscience
Heterophenomenology
Human Cognome Project
1/27/2012 12:05 PM
11 of 14
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Cognitive_science
References
1. ^ Thagard, Paul, Cognitive Science (http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2008/entries/cognitive-science/) , The
Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2008 Edition), Edward N. Zalta (ed.).
2. ^ Longuet-Higgins, H. C. (1973). "Comments on the Lighthill Report and the Sutherland Reply", in Artificial
Intelligence: a paper symposium, Science Research Council, 35-37
3. ^ Cognitive Science Society (http://www.cognitivesciencesociety.org/about_description.html)
4. ^ http://cogsci.vassar.edu/about/index.html
5. ^ Artur S. d'Avila Garcez, Luis C. Lamb and Dov M. Gabbay. Neural-Symbolic Cognitive Reasoning. Cognitive
Technologies. Springer, 2008, ISBN 978-3-540-73245-7, 2008.
6. ^ Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual
Information. W. H. Freeman.
7. ^ Miller, G. A. (2003). The cognitive revolution: a historical perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7,
141-144.
8. ^ A number of authors consider the qualia problem to be part of the cognitive science field, e.g. Some philosophical
issues in cognitive science: qualia, intentionality, and the mind-body problem (http://portal.acm.org
/citation.cfm?id=166791.166844) , Qualia: The Hard Problem (http://chil.rice.edu/byrne/Pubs/cogsci96.pdf) , and
indeed the entire discipline of philosophy as being part of the cog sci field, e.g. What is Cognitive Science?
(http://ls.berkeley.edu/ugis/cogsci/major/about.php) , while other reputable sources that cover both qualia and cog
sci do not draw any obvious connection between them, e.g. the Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy
(http://plato.stanford.edu) (Jan 2008 online edition) does have full-size articles on both qualia
(http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/qualia/) and cog sci (http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/cognitive-science/) , but
qualia are not even mentioned in the cog sci article while cog sci is not mentioned in the qualia article.
9. ^ Sun, Ron (ed.) (2008). The Cambridge Handbook of Computational Psychology. Cambridge University Press,
New York.
10. ^ Pinker S., Bloom P. (1990). "Natural language and natural selection". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):
707784. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00081061 (http://dx.doi.org/10.1017%2FS0140525X00081061) .
11. ^ Lewandowski, Gary; Strohmetz, David (2009). "Actions can speak as loud as words: Measuring behavior in
psychological science". Social and Personality Psychology Compass 3 (6): 9921002.
doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00229 (http://dx.doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.1751-9004.2009.00229) .
12. ^ B. F. Skinner, "Can Psychology be a Science of Mind?", American Psychologist, November 1990, page 1209, At
the APA Web Site (http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/amp/45/11/1206.html) Successfully accessed 29 December 2009
Further reading
Introductory literature
Eckardt, Barbara Von (2003): Cognitive Science: Philosophical Issues. In: Lynn Nadel (Ed.):
Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science, Vol. 1, London: Nature Publishing Group, pp. 552559.
Thagard, Paul (2nd, 2005). Mind : Introduction to Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT
Press.
General
Bechtel, W. et al. Ed. (1999). A Companion to Cognitive Science. Blackwell Companions to
1/27/2012 12:05 PM
12 of 14
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Cognitive_science
1/27/2012 12:05 PM
13 of 14
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Cognitive_science
External links
Cognitive Science Society (http://www.cognitivesciencesociety.org)
University of California San Diego, Department of Cognitive Science (http://www.cogsci.ucsd.edu)
iCogSci: An online information portal of everything Cognitive Science (http://www.cogs.indiana.edu
/iacs/)
Cognitive Science Movie Index: A broad list of movies showcasing themes in the Cognitive Sciences
(https://www.indiana.edu/~cogfilms)
Piero Scaruffi's annotated bibliography on the mind (http://www.scaruffi.com/mind.html)
List of leading thinkers in cognitive science (http://carbon.ucdenver.edu/~mryder/itc/cogsci.html)
Dr. Roy Ruddle's history page at the University of Leeds (http://www.comp.leeds.ac.uk
/ai12/history.html)
Dr. Carl Stahmer's history page at the University of Santa Barbara (http://www.carlstahmer.com
/cogsci/index.php)
Department of Cognitive Science at the Hebrew University of Jerusalem (http://www.hum.huji.ac.il
/english/units.php?cat=3046&incat=3045)
Cognitive Science Center Amsterdam (http://www.csca.nl)
Retrieved from "http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Cognitive_science&oldid=473419735"
1/27/2012 12:05 PM
14 of 14
Categories:
Cognitive science
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Cognitive_science
Interdisciplinary fields
1/27/2012 12:05 PM