Feld - Sound Structure As Social Structure
Feld - Sound Structure As Social Structure
$XWKRUV6WHYHQ)HOG
5HYLHZHGZRUNV
6RXUFH(WKQRPXVLFRORJ\9RO1R6HSSS
3XEOLVKHGE\University of Illinois PressRQEHKDOIRISociety for Ethnomusicology
6WDEOH85/http://www.jstor.org/stable/851232 .
$FFHVVHG
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
University of Illinois Press and Society for Ethnomusicology are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve
and extend access to Ethnomusicology.
http://www.jstor.org
383
384
SEPTEMBER1984
ETHNOMUSICOLOGY,
FELD:SOUNDSTRUCTURE
AS SOCIALSTRUCTURE
385
Lomax for these materials.Over the past few years I have listened to the tapes,
learned Lomax's core examples, and tried to apply his parametersto a society
and musical system I know well from intensive field research. I began with a
sample of about700 Kaluli songs and reducedthis numberto 500 afterremoving
single performerrenditionsor other reliabilityproblems. Whatcantometricshas
taught me about those 500 songs is that they display so much intracultural
variabilityand subtletythat it is virtuallyimpossibleto code a normalizedprofile
for them. To constructa typical cantometricprofile what I really need is onl'yten
songs, but the problem is, which ten?' How can I maintain the integrity of
patternsdiscoverablein largebodies of datawhen the cantometricssystem seems
to sacrifice so much significant data in order to objectify a "core pattern?"
After wrestling with the trainingtapes (an example of one of my specific
problems, with the social organizationof the vocal group, follows), I still feel
that Lomax is asking many of the right questions about music and social
institutions, but the mechanics of cantometrics crunches them in ways that
cannot satisfy the researcher accustomed to intensive field work, in-depth
analysis, and groundedethnographictheory. So I go back to the initial question:
compare what?
My suggestion is true heresy to many committedcomparativists,but I think
we need to pioneer a qualitative and intensive comparativesociomusicology,
without reified and objectified musical and social structuraltrait lists, without
unsituatedlaminationsof variouslycollected and historicallyungroundedmaterials. Comparativesociomusicology shouldtake the tough questionsand sort them
out with the best materials available for detailed comparison:the thorough,
long-term,historicallyand ethnographicallysituatedcase study. The meaningful
comparisonsare going to be the ones between the most radicallycontextualized
case examples, and not between decontextualizedtrait lists.
The data needed to begin this kind of comparativesociomusicology are
statementsof patternfor single societies, focusing on stylistic integrity, sociomusical coherence, and the role of music in role differentiation.Forthis the best
etic inputwill have to be the most thoroughemic data. By this I meanthatbroad
meaningfulcomparisonswill have to be basedon accurate,detailed, careful local
ethnographicmodels. So to start, the best way to answer Lomax's questions
about the systematicnatureof musical representationin social organizationis to
study them on the ground, in the field, up close, over long periodsof time, where
sound structuresare observably and undeniably socially structured.
A FRAMEWORK
While my firm belief is thatthe basis for comparingthe social life of sounds
must be qualitativeand derived from intensive local research,I also believe that
386
SEPTEMBER1984
ETHNOMUSICOLOGY,
AS SOCIALSTRUCTURE
FELD:SOUND STRUCTURE
387
388
SEPTEMBER
1984
ETHNOMUSICOLOGY,
6. How do musical materials or performancesmark or maintain social differences? How are such differences interpreted?How are they sustained?
Broken or ruptured?Accepted or resisted?
These questions, and the six domains that head them, are intended as an
approachto integratingthe microscopic, ethnographicallydetailed analyses of
musical lives, with an arenaof comparable,general, relevantissues thatwill help
us compare sociomusical realities and practices. After a brief orientationto the
Kaluli, I summarizethe most salient issues in the six namedareas. By providing
key Kaluli metaphorsand concepts for each of these areas, I hope to stay trueto
an emic Kaluli sociology of sound, to the Kaluli articulationand constructionof
a sociomusical coherence system. At the same time, use of this simple comparativegrid will, I hope, make the Kaluli patternmore available to contrast,
comparison, and question in terms of the larger issues surroundingclassless,
small-scale, relatively egalitarian societies.
THE KALULI PEOPLE
Twelve hundredKaluli people live in the tropical rain forest of the Great
PapuanPlateauin the SouthernHighlandsProvinceof PapuaNew Guinea.2On
several hundredsquaremiles of rich land, at an altitudeof about two thousand
feet, they hunt, fish, gather, and tend land-intensiveswidden gardensthat yield
sweet potatoes, taro, pandanus,pumpkin, bananas, and many other fruits and
vegetables. Their staple food is sago, processed from wild palms that grow in
shallow swamps and creeks branching off of larger river arteries that flow
downwardfrom Mt. Bosavi, the collapsed cone of an extinct volcano reaching
eight thousand feet (E. L. Schieffelin 1976).
Kaluli live in about twenty distinct longhouse communities;in each, most
people reside in a single communal house, comprising some fifteen families
(sixty to eighty people). Social life for the village is centeredaroundthe house,
where primaryface to face interactionoccupies most time people are not in their
gardens,on the trails, visiting relativesin othercommunities,or stayingat small
garden homes or sago camps for major food processing activities.
This is a classless society. There are no social or occupationalspecializations, stratifications,or ranks.Thereare no professions, no ascribedor achieved
statuses that form the basis for social differentiation.All Kaluli are assumedby
their fellows to have equal social potential and endowment, which is theirs to
make of as best they can. Adults are responsiblefor getting what they want and
need out of daily affairs; assistance throughfriendshipand networksof social
relations is of primary importancefor all Kaluli.
This is also a generallyegalitariansociety in matterseconomic andpolitical.
There are no appointedor elected leaders, spokesmen, chiefs, bosses, control-
AS SOCIALSTRUCTURE
FELD:SOUNDSTRUCTURE
389
Competence
For Kaluli there is no "music," only sounds, arrangedin categoriesshared
to greateror lesser degrees by natural,animal, and humanagents. Knowledgeof
these categories is widespread,tacit backgroundfor everyday life in the forest.
No hierarchies of sound types are imposed, no rationales constructed for
differentiatinghuman-madesounds from those of other sources. It is broadly
assumed that every Kaluli must become a competent maker, recognizer, user,
and interpreterof naturaland cultural sound patterns. Not only does physical
adaptationto the rain forest demand and favor acute auditoryperceptualskills;
Kaluli have developed the kind of ideological and aesthetic scaffolds for these
skills that humanize them and provide a coherent culturalframeworkfor their
acquisition. This is precisely the point of co-evolution: physical imperatives
alone do not explain adaptation;societies invent mutually supportiveadaptive
strategies linking nature and culture.
Acquisition of skill in song, weeping, whooping, cheering, humming,
390
SEPTEMBER1984
ETHNOMUSICOLOGY,
AS SOCIALSTRUCTURE
FELD:SOUNDSTRUCTURE
391
or drum more than others were usually met by Kaluli with nonchalantreplies
citing the obvious: some garden more than others, some often make netbags,
some cook well, othersknow aboutbuildinghouses, and so forth. In sum, Kaluli
seem to have no investment in rationalizingdifferences in competence; they
simply assume that skills for interpretingand making sounds are naturally
acquiredand required,and that with instructionand encouragement,all children
will learn to sing and compose as part of their general socialization.
Form
Most all Kaluli sound expression is vocal, dependenton a strong interplay
of poetic and melodic elements. Five types of song exist, one of uniqueinvention
(gisalo) and four borrowedfrom neighbors(heyalo, ko:luba, sabio, iwo:). Each
of these is organizedby pentatonicprinciples;reductionsto three- and four-tone
variantsare common, and there is evidence of melodic convergence paralleling
the historyand diffusion of these four styles in the Bosavi area(Feld i.p.). Kaluli
compose in three of these five forms (gisalo, heyalo, ko.:luba);the other two
styles consist of closed sets (sabio, iwo:). Gisalo is limited to ceremonial and
seance occasions and is composed only by men. Heyalo and ko:luba are
composed for ceremonies but widely sung during everyday work and leisure
activities. Only in the case of heyalo do men and women participateequally as
composers.
Cutting across these song forms are other varieties and means of sound
expression:women's sung-texted-weeping(sa-ye:.lab),and cheering (uwo:lab),
men's whooping (ulab), and instrumentaldrumming(ilib) and bamboo jaw's
harp playing (uluna). Three other rattle instruments(sologa, seed-pod rattle;
degegado, crayfish claw rattle;sob, mussel shell rattle)are used for ceremonial
accompanimentto songs. Only the sologa is used more casually for accompaniment; additionally it is the only one of these instrumentsused by women.
Beyond a taxonomyof expressive forms, and the distributionof expressive
means in song, poetics, instruments, and performance, one very significant
generalizationcan be made about all Kaluli sound forms: no Kaluli sounds are
performedunison. Kaluli know quite well what unison is because missionaries
have tried to get them to sing this way for twelve years and churchleadershave
been taughtto count "one-two-three" before each song. Aside from church
activities (where only the most committed Christianscan actually manage this
new form of vocal organization)it is rare to hear anythingapproachingunison
sung by Kaluli or emitted from any sound sources in their environment.
Kaluli sound preferences,modeled consciously on bird sounds and the rest
of the forest environment,involve extensive overlappingand alternation,layering parts and sounds in coordinatednondiscretetextures. In Western musical
terms there is much canon and hocket. In cantometricterms, from Lomax's
392
SEPTEMBER
1984
ETHNOMUSICOLOGY,
characterizationof the social organizationof the vocal group (1976:86,177180), threepatternsare found:interlock,overlap, andalternation.Interlockis the
term Lomax uses for the social organizationof musical groups with an equality
of parts, common among acephalousbands of hunter-gatherersor noncomplex
forest societies. Lomax finds thatoverlap is most typical of largersocieties with
herd animals and a somewhat more complex production system. He cites
alternationof partsas morecharacteristicof societies with clear divisions of parts
and productive systems (Lomax 1976:86).
Listening to Kaluli songs makes it clear that no one of these three
characterizationspredominates;there seems to be an equal mix of all three. At
the same time, the Kaluli techno-economicpicture does not follow any of the
threeLomax cites as typically matchingthese sound types. The maingeneralization for Kaluli social organizationof the vocal group is that interlock, overlap,
and alternationare equally salient, precisely to the exclusion of unison forms.
Lomax (1976:86) cites unison as "the simplest technique of coordinating
effort"; he finds it "resorted to everywhere but most prominent in the performancesof small tribalsocieties, especially among planterswithoutlarge herd
animals." While this techno-economic characterizationis closer to Kaluli
society than any of the others cited above, Kaluli sound organizationsimply
lacks a unison principle altogether.
Like the use of "hardness" to characterize a Kaluli ideal of social
competence, there is also a metaphorthat draws together the dimensions of
interlock,overlap, and alternationwhich are so importantin Kalulisoundmaking
style. This is duluguganalan, "lift-up-oversound." Parts, sounds, whetherfew
or many, must constantly "lift-up-over" one another; one cannot speak of
sounds "leading" or "following" or "starting" or "finishing." Human
soundmakingmust stagger in layers, like bird calls, or arch up and over, like
waterfalls.The idea is more spatio-acousticthanvisual; Kaluli like all sounds to
be dense, compacted,withoutbreaks,pauses, or silences (Feld 1983). Whentwo
people sing together, the subtletiesof the shifting lengths of overlap (or, in the
case of a leader and a group, the nuances in the alternation)are the locus of
aesthetic play and tension. In the forest, sounds constantly shift figure and
ground;examples of continually staggered alternationsand overlaps, at times
sounding completely interlockedand seamless, are abundant.For Kaluli this is
the naturally coherent organizing model for soundmaking, whether human,
animal, or environmental:a constant textural densification constructed from
"lift-up-over sounds."
Performance
The performanceof all Kaluli sound expression focuses upon collective
texture and coordinationof layered parts. No competitive agendas play out
AS SOCIALSTRUCTURE
FELD:SOUNDSTRUCTURE
393
throughsong performance;the value of layering,juxtaposing, arching, "liftingup-over," and densifying are conceived as social activity. Even when the
situationinvolves a single voice, the sound is coordinatedwith the surrounding
acoustic featuresof the environment;this is particularlyso when Kaluli sing at
work.
At Kaluli ceremonies composed songs are sung all night long by members
of a visiting communityat the longhouse of their hosts. The hosts find the song
texts in particularto be sad and evocative because they concentrateon maps and
images of the places in the immediate, surroundingforest, places to which the
hosts have a sentimentalattachment.The performanceusuallyprovokesthe hosts
to tears, and to a sung-melodic-weeping performed in polyphony with the
ongoing song to which they are responding. The intense grief and sadness
experiencedby the hosts resultsfrom theirbeing remindedof the dead who have
lived, worked, and sharedmany experienceswith them at the places sequentially
mentioned in the song (E. L. Schieffelin 1976, 1979; Feld 1982).
It would appearthat this sort of evocation in performancemight create a
tremendousfocus on the composer and performerof the song as an individual
creator. Indeed, direct social manipulationis involved in composing a song that
might move a specific individualto tears. At the same time, these are not the
featuresof a song that Kaluli stress;they preferto cast the whole activity into an
explicit social and community-widemessage framework,largely throughimagery of land, the central Kaluli metaphorfor the accumulationand meaning of
social experience relating, sharing, and being with others in the forest.
When Kaluli men sat down with me aftera ceremonyto talk aboutthe songs
and their meanings, they always stressed the social rather than individual
motivationof song poetics. There is always collective sorrow it seems, because
of the underlying assumption that audience members will empathize with
whomever is moved to tears. This derives from the feeling of common experience among those people who sharea longhouseand surroundingforest community. Over and beyond ties of direct close kin, Kaluli feel deeply connected by
the places they have lived, gardened,worked,exchanged, and travelledtogether.
Soundmakingprovidesno formatfor the assertionof power, dominance,or
personalexcellence at the cost of others. The recognitionof skill in composition
and performanceis clear, and its pragmaticoutcome is the weeping of the hosts.
Despite all this, competition is not a major agenda in the ceremonies, and the
provocation involved is not a manipulationfor the sake of power. The Kaluli
constructionof a performanceis really activity in concert ratherthan activity
involving sequentialsolo performances.Individualityis not played up the way it
might be; costumes in fact conceal identities, so only the voice singles out the
singer.
If "lift-up-over sound" and "hardness" are the central metaphorsfor the
form of and competence in soundmaking,then its centralmetaphorfor perform-
394
SEPTEMBER1984
ETHNOMUSICOLOGY,
395
reflections" (ane mama) to the visible, usually in the form of birds in the forest
treetops. Thus the immediate village area is surroundedwith the presence,
throughvoices and sounds, of friendsand relatives. Because birds sing, whistle,
say their names, make a lot of noise, weep, or speak, they provide a simultaneous index of the environmentas well as a deepersymbolic understandingabout
self, place, and time.
Beyond these notions-tuning fork, model, mystery-there is also a deeply
pleasurable aspect to the way Kaluli approach the forest, which couples a
sentimentalitybased on land as mediatorof identity(E. L. Schieffelin 1976:2945) and an outrightenjoymentof the soundscape. Kaluli find the forest good to
listen to, and good to sing with as well. Improvisedhuman duets with birds,
cicadas, or other forest sounds are not uncommoneveryday events. Sometimes
people will find themselves a waterfalljust for the pleasure of singing with a
shimmeringaccompaniment.Again, the co-evolutionarytendencies for ecology
and aesthetics: Kaluli not only take inspiration from, listen to, and enjoy
the forest, but become part of it, which ultimately intensifies their sentiments
about it.
In sum, the Kaluli relationshipwith the forest is neither antagonisticnor
destructive,patternsthatare typical among some swiddenhorticulturalists.Little
ecological pressure, extremely low population density, no competition for
resources, and constantlyavailablefood (fish, fowl, vegetables) all contributeto
easy materialextractionand exploitationof the environment.At the same time,
the mystical, pleasurable, and tuning-fork dimensions of the forest-Kaluli
relationship reinforce this materialist basis. In all, the forest is a mama, a
"reflection," or mirrorfor social relationships,particularlyas mediatedthrough
the poetic imagery of songs that concern maps, lands and identities (Feld
1982:150-156), as well as through formal structureand singing style.
Theory
For Kaluli, the theory and concepts of where sounds come from and how
they can be organized, and, particularly,what they mean, is not contained in
esoteric knowledge or in a body of private lore controlled or circulated by
specialists. Myths about human-birdtransformationsexplain the origins of
categories of sounds that humans share with each other and with the natural
world, namely, weeping, song, poetics, whistling, talking, noise, mimicry.
These myths frame the meanings of sounds in terms of the range of social
sentiments associated with categories of bird-spirit"reflections." No special
occasions are requiredfor their telling, and no constraintsexist on who may tell
or hear them. All in fact are quite short and even Kaluli who don't volunteerto
narratethem can certainlyrecountthe punchlineor generalpoint to each. These
myths are central to the meaning and theory of sounds for Kaluli; the facts of
396
SEPTEMBER1984
ETHNOMUSICOLOGY,
AS SOCIALSTRUCTURE
FELD:SOUNDSTRUCTURE
397
398
ETHNOMUSICOLOGY,SEPTEMBER 1984
few compositionsby women, althoughsome women with family ties to the area
from which ko:lubaderives know quite a numberof the songs and sing them for
work. Iwo.:is a fixed cycle of songs sung the night before killing pigs; it has an
explicit women's counterpartcalled kelekeliyoba, sung the morning after the
ceremony. These songs also use fixed formulaewith only new placenamesand
pig names added by the singers.
In terms of instruments,handdrumsand bamboojaw's harpsare relatively
recent introductionsto the Bosavi area, althoughthe latter is said to replace a
monochordicmouthbow, whose history is uncertain.Both instrumentsare only
availableto men;drummingtakes place beforeor duringceremonialactivity, and
the jaw's harpis associatedwith the personalrecreationof young men. The drum
involves magical constructionsecrets which are kept from women; even though
it is not hiddenfrom their sight, women do not touchthe instrument(Feld 1983).
Even so, the dimensions of drum secrets seem to be nothing like the secret
character of flutes and garamuts reported elsewhere in Papua New Guinea
(Gourlay 1975). Of the three rattleinstrumentsused for ceremonialaccompaniment, only one is sharedwith women (sologa), while the one used for gisalo, the
sob, is clearly consideredsecret, and should not be touched by women. Women
are not aware of the mystical events through which the cane handles (olo:
se:se:lo:) of these instrumentsare passed on to Kaluli men from spirit mediums
(E. L. Schieffelin 1976:214).
One other major soundmakingresource does return us to a very basic
male-femalecomplementarity;these are the matchedpair of demonstrativeand
assertivecollective soundscalled ulab, ("says u") and uwo:lab, ("says uwo.:").
For men ulab is a loud group whoop, celebratingthe call of the eagle usulage, a
booming UUU! For women, uwo.lab is a raucousgroup cheer, celebratingthe
call of the SuperbBird of Paradiseuwo:lo, a screeching U-WO:O:!These two
birds are prominentspirit representationsof Kaluli men and women, and the
group soundmakingusually takes place when men collectively work or prepare
for a ceremony, or when women cheer during a ceremony.
The complementarydistributionof gisalo and weeping, whooping and
cheering speak for the way Kaluli men and women have coordinatedseparate
expressive spheres that are relatedand mutuallysignificant in modes of appeal
and assertion. At the same time the distributionof instrumentaland ceremonial
song resources clearly points to the way Kaluli men have appropriatedand
control new expressive resources.
Finally, it is importantto note the clear inequalitiesin what it is that these
expressive resources allow men and women to achieve, symbolically and
pragmatically.For men, the compositionand performanceof ceremonialsongs
creates a grand social focus around them and their powers of evocation.
Ceremonial action of this sort is the height of Kaluli stagecraft, drama, and
collective celebration (E. L. Schieffelin 1976:172-196, Feld 1982:163-216).
AS SOCIALSTRUCTURE
FELD:SOUNDSTRUCTURE
399
Gisalo is the most powerful of these, but hevalo and ko:luba achieve the same
effect for men. In all threethe performancemay be so moving thata woman will
lose her heartto the dancer, wish to elope, and follow him home. Drummingcan
contribute to the same social power (Feld 1983).
What weeping achieves for women is far less sweeping. It certainlycreates
a social focus on women as performers,and thatfocus is significantin validating
the role weepers play in articulatingcommunity sentiments. But no persuasive
social ends and no long-term changes in social life are effected by weeping;
largely it is an intensely aesthetic public display of personal grief.
If there is a key Kaluli metaphor that sums up male-female relations
generally, and male-female expressive means and ends specifically, it is the
rather ambiguous term ko.li, "different." While this was about the most
common term I heard men use when speaking of women, or women use when
speaking of men, it certainly means different things from the relative vantage
points, and can have positive ("different" in the sense of new, exciting,
valuable), flat ("different" in the sense of bland, not quite right) or neutral
connotations.Kaluli recognize real social difference, but there is often little that
is negative or antagonisticin that recognition. At the same time, male ideology
clearly casts women's differences in a dangerous light: aversion to menstrual
blood and belief that women drain male energy are rationalesfor taboos and
other social practices which reproduce beliefs in and actions against female
contamination.The tensions and contradictionsfound here parallel the duality
of expressive domains, which have both a complementarydimension (songweeping, whooping-cheering) and an overlay of male-appropriatednew resources.
Discussion
To roundout the shape of the Kaluli pattern,I organize a summaryaround
three questions:
(1) How are the obvious egalitarianfeaturesof Kalulilife markedwithin
the organizing schemes of Kaluli sound structuresand soundmaking?
(2) How is the most obvious form of Kaluli inequality (men over
women) markedand resolved within the organizingschemes of Kaluli
sound structuresand soundmaking?
(3) How does the Kaluli pattern fit into the broader comparative
framework of Papua New Guinea societies?
. in egalitarian societies the division of labor by sex has led to
complementarity and not female subservience;
. . .
status when they lost control over the products of their work" (Leacock
400
SEPTEMBER1984
ETHNOMUSICOLOGY,
1978a:79, see also Leacock 1978b, 1977). Leacock's examinationof huntergathererand horticulturaleconomics shows thatwomen foragedand furnishedas
much if not more basic resourcesto hunter-gatherer
life thanmen did. Moreover
they distributedthe food to networksof kin as well as nuclearfamilies. Societies
which typically mix hunting and gatheringsubstratawith a dominanthorticulturalpracticepartiallysharein the patternLeacock describes, and presenta wide
range of egalitarian tendencies as well as unequal or contradictoryones.
Schlegel's (1977) notion that theories of sexual stratificationmust account for
person-to-personrelations and ideology as well as person-to-goodsrelations is
clearly essential if we are to understandsocieties like the Kaluli that are more
politically and economically egalitarianthan sexually so. This is also important
because Kaluli have the economic resources to produce a surplus far beyond
subsistence needs. The theory that stratificationsystematically co-varies with
surplushas been recentlydismantledby both Marxistsand non-Marxists,largely
in favor of theories that see stratificationmore closely related to population
dynamics (Cancian 1976).
Given all this, one thing we can clearly predict is that a society like the
Kaluli with such a combinationof demographiccharacteristicsas low density,
easy subsistence, ecological diversity, and economic resources like combined
hunting,fishing, sago production,and gardening,will not neatly matchidealized
social-economic-technologicalcharacterizationslike "hunter-gatherer,""horticultural," "peasant," and so forth. Our expectation therefore should be for
complex, and ambiguousor contradictorytendenciesin sociomusicalpracticeas
well as social organization.This seems to be the case in several regards.On the
one hand, the image of deep mutualismand co-aesthetic relationshipwith the
forest ecology is reminiscentof hunter-gathererslike the pygmies of the Ituri
(summarized,from Turnbull, in Lomax 1962). The general situationfor competence, musical form and performance, environment and theory depicts an
emphasison cooperation,complementarity,autonomy,valuingself in relationto
othersand to ecology. It is easy to imaginehow these musicalbeliefs, structures,
and actions can be coherent within an egalitariansocietal framework,and how
they can be locally perceived and sustained within that framework.
At the same time there are imbalances in the distributionof expressive
resourcesbetween men and women; more significantly, there are differences in
the pragmatic ends served by these resources. Men can mystify, impress,
persuade, and even win women, but what are women getting from weeping at
funerals and singing at work besides personal enjoyment, fulfillment, and
general social solidarity?Male-femaleinequalitiesthen are definitely markedin
the sphere of musical organization,but to push the qualitativepoint, it also is
importantto realize that the form in which they are markeddoes not involve
secret instruments stolen from women in mythic times, does not involve
deception maintenance, does not involve daily antagonisms, and does not
FELD:SOUNDSTRUCTURE
AS SOCIALSTRUCTURE
401
influence on men. A man who has had too much to do with women is likely to
lose his stamina, become fatigued on the trail" (E. L. Schieffelin 1982:178).
Moreover,
402
ETHNOMUSICOLOGY,SEPTEMBER 1984
To non-participants,and in particularwomen, the bau a was presentedas a mystically
powerful and dangerous institution (ibid:163).
In other words, they emerge as controlled, energetic, good hunters, knowledgable about the forest, including the spirits who reside there. They also emerge
supplying a large amount of cooked meat to members of their own and the
surroundingcommunities, thereby placing them all in their debt.
The differences between the bau a and some of the other traumatic
separationrites practiced in the Papua New Guinea Highlands is clearly underscoredby societal differences.Complex initiationswith grades, alliances, and
the like require large populations. Although the bau a was symbolically and
ritually elaborate and complex, it had none of the stratified complexities of
graded initiations common for many of the other societies summarized by
Keesing (1982) and discussed in the literature.
Ecological pressures, root crop intensification, and increasing population
density are common in many Papua New Guinea Highlandssocieties. A major
shift towardintensivecultivationincreasesthe burdenson women, and seems to
be accompaniedin many societies by increasedmale control throughappropriation of the products of women's labor. The Kaluli, by contrast, are low in
production,lackingin ecological pressuresand crop intensification.They are not
competing for food or territory, and their population size is stable. Men's
hunting, clearing and gardening activities seem complementaryto women's
gardening and sago-making work. This produces none of the competition
necessary for the organizationand maintenanceof a "big man" social system
exchanging women and their products.
While the bau a shares many surfacefeatureswith initiationcults in Papua
New Guinea, it appears to have been a more subdued variantof most of the
institutionsdiscussed in the literaturesurroundingissues of male-femalerelations
(Keesing 1982, Allen 1967, Gourlay 1975, Langness 1974, Murphy 1959, A.
Strathern1970). Kaluli men seem far more interestedin impressingwomen than
in maintaininghostilities with them. Ceremonialsong, costuming and performance, hunting and providing meat for exchange are the activities males most
cultivate and use for impressingwomen. These activities are not filled with the
AS SOCIALSTRUCTURE
FELD:SOUNDSTRUCTURE
403
CONCLUSION
404
SEPTEMBER1984
ETHNOMUSICOLOGY,
Keil's final statement also makes a lot of sense for the Kaluli: the
interpenetrationof the environment with all Kaluli sound recognition and
expression really does symbolize everythingin some way: competence ("hardness"), form ("lift-up-oversound"), performance("flow") are all tied together
by theory (in myths of human/birdtransformation).The success of Kaluli
soundmakingas a deeply affective and emotional mediumof communicationis
grounded in this invented coherence.
Moving to the generalfrom the specific, my concernswith comparisonhave
in great part been stimulatedby reading Alan Lomax's work and by trying to
apply his training tape examples and coding procedures to Kaluli data. I
obviously differ from Lomax at the outset in a basic philosophicalmatter:I do
not equateexplanationwith normativestatisticalcorrelationsor causal analysis. I
am more concernedwith explaining the situatedmeanings of sound patternsin
the intersubjectivelycreated world of actors and actions, and I am concerned
with the role local ideologies play in constitutingand maintainingthose local,
specific sociological models of and for musical realities. I am thereforebiased
towardwhat Lomax calls the "narrowfeatures" of both the streamand content
of music, while his own focus is on grosser, objectifiableand redundantfeatures
of the behavioralstream. At the same time, I have arguedthat it is possible to
conceive a set of comparativeresearchquestions that make use of local models
and metaphors while at the same time identifying issues that have broader
comparativevalue, concerning both the conceptual and materialdimensions of
musicality. While these six domains may look more like artifacts of the
cultural-ideationalsphere rather than the sociological, I have indicated that
culturalconstructsare essential precisely because of the way they lead to and
provide local models for social structure (social roles, division of labor,
stratification, and differentiation).
I have also stressed that there is not a yes/no issue about correlatingsong
structureand social structure.Obviously such correlationsare possible, whether
undertakenin world sample quantitativetermsor small-scale ethnographicones.
The issue is: how do we interpretsuch relationships,and how do we argue for
whatthey mean?Lomax's tendencyhas been towardcausalconnectionsbetween
social evolution and singing style. He arguesfor causal covariancefrom societal
complexityto melodic intervalcomplexity, text precisionand rhythmicfreedom.
He argues for causal covariancefrom societal sexual restrictivenessto singing
voice nasality, and social cohesion to choral cohesion.
The most complete and convincing quantitativereanalysis of cantometric
data thus far, by one of Lomax's early statisticalco-workers, uses multivariate
techniques to argue for historical diffusionist rather than evolutionary interpretationsof cantometricdata. Manysong featuresinterpretedby Lomaxto be
correlates of evolutionary process can be explained almost exclusively by
regional location(Erickson 1976). If culturehistoryis enough to explain the kind
of taxonomic variance Lomax originally found, Erickson insists that:
FELD:SOUNDSTRUCTURE
AS SOCIALSTRUCTURE
405
canbe shown
Fornearlyeverymajorstylein thecantometric
taxonomy,a widevariation
correlates.By contrast,such styles do not often
on the positedcultural/institutional
homes.Whentheydo, it appears
to be inthe
of theirethnohistoric
oversteptheboundaries
contextof an important
eventin history.The attemptto explainsongin termsof some
universalunilinearprocessof social evolution-for all thatsuch a dimensioncan be
shown to exist-is to over-simplify our understandingof this most humanof behaviors
(1976:307).
listener a feeling of security, for it symbolizes the place where he was born, his earliest
childhood satisfactions, his religious experience, his pleasurein communitydoings, his
courtshipand his work-any or all of these personalityshapingexperiences (1959:929).
406
SEPTEMBER1984
ETHNOMUSICOLOGY,
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Revision of a paper read at the Society for Ethnomusicologyannual meetings, Tallahassee,
October 1983 at a session co-organizedby CharlesKeil and myself on ComparativeSociomusicology
of Classless and EgalitarianSocieties. Many of the issues discussed here have emerged through
conversation with Charlie and been enhanced by our ongoing materialist-idealistdialogue. Comments at the SEM panel from MarinaRoseman, John Blacking, and Tony Seeger were helpful in
reformulatingmy remarks,as were commentsfromSimon Frith,GeorginaBorn, andJody Berlandat
the Sociology of Music Conferenceat Trent University in August 1983. 1 am also gratefulto John
Shepherdfor inviting me to presenta version of this materialthereamongstsociologists and popular
music researchers. For research support 1976-1977 and 1982 1 am grateful to the National
Endowmentfor the Arts, the Universityof PennsylvaniaResearchFoundation,the Instituteof Papua
New Guinea Studies, the Archives of TraditionalMusic, and the AnthropologyFilm Center. In
Kaluli words, the typed symbols /e:/ and /o:/ equal c and 3. Otherletterscarrytheirphoneticvalues;
for orthographysee Feld 1982:17-19.
NOTES
1. Lomax (1976:16) maintainsthat "Generally, the more songs per culture we analyzed, the
clearerthe core style became." This is a difficult statementto evaluate. While it should be obvious
407
REFERENCESCITED
Allen, M. R.
1967 Male Cults and Secret Initiations in Melanesia. Melbourne:MelbourneUniversityPress.
Becker, Alton
1979 "CommunicationAcross Diversity," in A. L. Becker and Aram Yengoyan, eds., The
Imagination of Reality (Norwood: Ablex), pp. 1-5.
Becker, Judith
1979 "Time andTune in Java," in A. L. Becker and Aram Yengoyan, eds., TheImaginationof
Reality (Norwood: Ablex), pp. 197-210.
Becker, Judith and Alton
1981 "A Musical Icon: Power and Meaning in Javanese Gamelan Music," in Wendy Steiner,
ed., The Sign in Music and Literature (Austin: University of Texas Press), pp. 203-215.
Man and Woman in the New Guinea Highlands. Washington: American Anthropological
Association.
Cancian, Frank
1976
"Social Stratification,"
Erickson, Edwin
1976 "Traditionand Evolution in Song Style: A Reanalysis of CantometricData," Behavior
Science Research
11(1):227-308.
Fallers, Lloyd
1977 "Equality and Inequalityin HumanSocieties," in S. Tax, ed., Horizons in Anthropology
(Chicago: Aldine, 2nd. edition), pp. 257-268.
Steven
Feld,
1981 " 'Flow like a Waterfall': The Metaphors of Kaluli Musical Theory," Yearbookfor
1982
408
ETHNOMUSICOLOGY,SEPTEMBER 1984
Alan
"Folk Song Style," American Anthropoologist61:927-954.
"Song Structureand Social Structure," Ethnology 1(1):425-451.
Cantometrics: an Approach to the Anthrolpologyof Music. Berkeley: University of
California, Extension Media Center.
409
Schlegel, Alice
1977 "Toward a Theory of Sexual Stratification,"in Alice Schlegel, ed., Sexual Stratification
(New York: Columbia University Press), pp. 1-40.
Strathern, Andrew
1970 "Male Initiation in New Guinea Highlands Societies," Ethnology 9(4):373-379.
Strathern, Marilyn
1972 Women In Between. London: Seminar Press.