The Role of Practice in Chess - A Longitudinal Study
The Role of Practice in Chess - A Longitudinal Study
*Corresponding author
Email: [email protected]
Phone: +44 (1895) 265484
Fax:
Abstract
Keywords:
designed to improve performance, which are typically effortful and not enjoyable.
These activities cannot be extended throughout long periods and must therefore be
limited to a few hours a day. Moreover, Charness, Krampe and Mayr (1996),
Charness, Tuffiash, Krampe, Reingold and Vasyukova (2005), and Ericsson et al.
(1993) identified individual practice as critical to the development of expertise. On
the other hand, Gobet and Campitelli (2007) found that group practice is also relevant
to acquire chess expertise.
Assuming that practice is an essential aspect of the acquisition of high-level
cognitive skills, a second issue arises: do all individuals benefit to the same extent
from practice? This issue was investigated nearly 100 years ago by Thorndike (1908),
who suggested a relation between inter-individual variability in performance and the
nature of the task. He suggested that if inter-individual variability in performance
increases with increasing practice in a particular domain, then the task performed is
one that requires factors not related to practice (i.e., innate factors). On the other hand,
if inter-individual variability in performance decreases with increasing practice, then
this is a sign that practice is the main factor to perform the task. Ackermans (1987)
literature review showed that, in most studies, inter-individual variability in fact
decreased, a pattern of results that supports the role of practice.
A third issue revolves around the type of activities that are useful for acquiring
high-level cognitive skills. In particular, is it important first to engage in activities
leading to the creation of a vast internal knowledge base, and only then to participate
in performance situations? Or, alternatively, should the participation in performance
situations start early on in the learning process? Perhaps surprisingly, Ericsson et al.
(1993) excluded activities in a performance situation from their definition of
deliberate practice. In a study with chess players, Charness, Krampe and Mayr (1996)
followed Ericsson et al.s (1993) decision and suggested that individual practice is
more related to deliberate practice than group practice. (Tournament games were
considered as group practice in Charness et al.s definition.).
Research into expertise has enriched practice in education, for example
through the concepts of adaptive expertise, pattern recognition, chunking, and
selective search (Anderson, Reder & Simon, 2000; Baxter & Glaser, 1998; Gobet,
2005; Gobet & Wood, 1999; Hatano & Inagaki, 1986; Simon, 1980). Similarly,
understanding the three issues introduced above would have strong implications for
education. For example, it would help teachers and coaches to choose appropriate
teaching and training activities.
In this paper, we address these three issues by studying practice behaviour in
chessplayers. Chess has already considerably enriched our understanding of problem
solving (e.g., Campitelli & Gobet, 2004; Charness, 1981; Gobet, 1998a), imagery
(e.g., Campitelli & Gobet, 2005; Saariluoma, 1991), perception (Charness, Reingold,
Pomplun, & Stampe, 2001; De Groot & Gobet, 1996), memory (e.g., Chase & Simon,
1973; Cooke, Atlas, Lane & Berger, 1993; Gobet & Simon, 1996a, b) and other
psychological phenomena (see Gobet, de Voogt & Retschitzki, 2004, for a review).
Chess has several advantages that make it a powerful task for studying cognitive
processes and learning (see Gobet, 1998b). For the purpose of this study, three of
these advantages are essential. First, it is a complex game that requires many years of
practice to attain high levels. Second, the existence of an international rating scale
allows researchers to know the level of expertise of their participants with precision.
Third, the existence of archival data of chess players ratings makes it possible to
track their level of expertise throughout their careers.
had international rating, we used the national rating in order to measure chess skill in
some analyses. The two ratings are closely related: in our sample, for the 65 players
with both international and national ratings, the correlation between the two scales
was .89. The range of the sample was 983 points (from 1490 to 2473), with a mean of
1991 and a standard deviation of 221. Since the Elo rating has a normal distribution
with a theoretical SD of 200, our sample had a range of nearly 5 SD.
Materials
The questionnaire was divided into three sections. The first section (see
appendix 1 for an English translation) consisted of questions about date of birth, age,
profession, international rating, national rating, speed chess rating (rating of the
Crculo de Ajedrez Torre Blanca),2 chess title, chess category, age at starting to play
chess, age at starting to play chess seriously, age at joining a chess club (club age),
presence of a coach (0,1) at any time during the career, number of chess books owned,
playing speed chess (0,1), number of speed games played, and type of training
(blindfold chess, reading games without seeing the board [henceforth, blindfold
reading], use of chess databases, and use of chess programs). The second section
contained a grid in which participants had to fill out the number of hours per week
they spent studying chess alone at each age (henceforth, individual practice). They
also had to fill out a second row with the number of hours per week they spent
studying or practising with other chessplayers, including tournament games
(henceforth, group practice).3 In both cases the participants were asked to estimate the
mean number of hours per week they spent studying or practising each year. We
estimated the number of hours studied per year by multiplying the figures reported by
52, and then we calculated the sum of the total hours spent with individual and group
practice in their entire chess career. Not all players answered all questions, with the
result that the number of data points varies across our measures. The third section
contained the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971). As the results of the
latter were already presented in a previous paper (Gobet & Campitelli, 2007), we do
not present these data here.
Results
We present the results according to the three issues developed in the
introduction. First, we consider the results related to the importance of practice in
achieving high levels of expertise in cognitive abilities. Second, we expound the
results related to inter-individual variability in performance. Third, we discuss data
related to the relative role of different types of practice.
The Role of Practice
We performed correlations between national rating and the total cumulative
number of hours of individual practice on the one hand, and of group practice on the
other hand. The correlation between current national rating and log cumulative
individual practice was .42 (p < .0001; calculated with 90 participants), and the
correlation between current national rating and log cumulative group practice was .54
(p < .0001; calculated with 89 participants).4 These statistically significant
correlations between the current skill level and two different measures of practice
show that the amount of previous practice has a very important role in the current skill
level.
Related interesting research questions include: How many hours per week do
the players spend practising, on average? Is there a difference between the stronger
players and the weaker players? And, do the players carry out more group practice or
individual practice? In order to investigate these issues, we performed a 2x3 ANOVA
with type of practice (group and individual) as a within-subjects independent variable,
10
With respect to the data of experts and intermediates presented in Figure 1 and
2, this hypothesis predicts a cubic function, with increase up to about 18 years old
where there should be a peak, then decrease to a local minimum, and then increase
again. We tested this hypothesis with linear (y = a + bx), quadratic (y = a + bx + cx2)
and cubic (y = a + bx + cx2 + dx3) regression analyses predicting hours of practice as
a function of age. Table 1 shows that, as predicted, the data of experts and
intermediates were fitted better by a cubic function than with a linear or quadratic
function. In particular, with the intermediates the cubic function explains at least an
additional 41% of the variance compared to the linear and quadratic functions. With
the masters, who were expected to be professionals, the cubic function does not
improve on the quadratic with individual practice, although, somewhat surprisingly, it
does with group practice.
11
seriously as the first year of serious practice and the figures depict the estimated
number of hours of practice in the first ten years of serious practice. These figures are
in agreement with the previous ones in that the three groups differ in the number of
hours of cumulative group and individual practice. They also show that the
differences in group practice (Figure 3) arise earlier that those in individual practice
(Figure 4).5 Not all the players in our sample achieved ten years of serious practice
(either because they became serious at chess late or because they were too young at
the point of measurement); therefore, the average hours of practice for each year was
calculated with a different number of players. This explains why in Figure 4 the
estimation of average cumulative number of hours of individual practice in
intermediate players at 9 and 10 years of serious practice is lower than that of 6 to 8
years of serious practice.
Inter-Individual Variability
Addressing the second issue, that of inter-individual variability, requires
correlating a measure of amount of practice with a measure of performance
variability. To do so, we ranked the players of our sample according to the amount of
cumulative individual practice, and then divided the sample into 9 groups of 10
players. Thus, groups were homogeneous with respect to players amount of practice,
with average practice increasing from group 1 to group 9. For each group, we
calculated the mean and standard deviation for national rating (see Figure 5). Then,
we obtained the correlation of the mean national rating of the groups with their mean
number of hours of individual practice. Also, we calculated the correlation between
the standard deviation of national rating and the mean number of hours of individual
practice. We divided the sample into 9 groups in order to have enough data points for
12
the correlation analysis and enough subjects within each group for the average ratings
and standard deviations of rating to be calculated reliably.
In Figure 5, the blip occurring in group 6 with the standard deviation of
national rating is due to an outlier, which was removed in all the following analyses.
(The pattern of correlations and significances is the same when this outlier is included
in the analyses.) There was a significant positive correlation between mean national
rating and mean number of hours of individual practice, r(7) = .79, p < .02, but we did
not find any significant correlation between standard deviation of national rating and
mean hours of individual practice, r(7) = .38, ns.
We followed the same procedure with group practice (see Figure 6). We found
a significant positive correlation between mean national rating and mean number of
hours of group practice, r(7) = .94, p < .001, and we also found a significant negative
correlation between standard deviation of national rating and mean hours of group
practice, r(7) = -.82, p < .01.
Given that age is positively correlated with amount of practice in this sample
(see Gobet & Campitelli, 2007, p. 164), it is possible that the significant correlations
presented in the previous analyses are accounted for by this variable. In order to
investigate this issue we calculated partial correlations, controlling for the mean age
of each group. There was no significant partial correlation between mean national
13
rating and mean number of hours of individual practice, r(6) = .61, ns., nor between
standard deviation of national rating and mean hours of individual practice, r(6) = .30,
ns. However, there was a significant positive partial correlation between mean
national rating and mean number of hours of group practice, r(6) = .89, p < .005, and
a significant negative partial correlation between standard deviation of national rating
and mean hours of group practice, r(6) = -.87, p < .005. Given the relatively low
power of these analyses, some of the medium-sized but non-significant correlations
should be interpreted with caution.
Until now, we have used current chess level (measured by current national
rating) as dependent variable. An interesting additional question is how ratings change
as a function of yearly practice. Given that we did not have complete archives for the
national rating, we used archives of international rating. Using international rating had
the disadvantage that not all players were included, because the entry requirements
are higher than those of national rating. Therefore, only a sub-sample could be
included (17 masters and 35 experts); in addition, information is lacking about what
happened at young ages or during the first years of serious practice. Nonetheless, our
data are useful to show different trends in performance change in masters and experts
from the time they achieved the level of experts.
Figures 7 and 8 show the international rating for the current masters and
experts, as a function of years of serious practice and age respectively. Figure 7 was
constructed as follows. First, we determined the starting point for each player; for that
14
purpose, we used the year in which the players reported they started playing seriously.
Therefore age in this figure does not matter. For example, a player who reported the
age of 13 would have 1 year of serious playing at the age of 14, 2 years of serious
playing at the age of 15, and so forth. Another player that started playing seriously at
the age of 6 would have 1 year of serious playing at the age of 7, and 8 years of
serious playing at the age of 14. Then, we calculated the average international rating
of both groups at each number of years of serious playing. The number of players at
each of the x-axis values changes. For example, there were many players younger
than 20 years, so they could not possibly have data for 20 years of serious playing.
Note also that we started the analysis at 3 years of serious playing because there was a
negligible number of players with international rating after 1 or 2 years of serious
playing.
We performed a regression analysis to predict international rating in masters
and experts as a function of the number of years of practising chess seriously (the
range of years of serious practice was 3 to 20). The correlation between international
rating and the number of years of serious practice was significant both for masters
(r(16) = .95, p < .0001) and experts (r(16) = .40, p = .05). The regression analyses
showed the following results:
international rating of masters = 2257 + 7.0 * years of serious practice;
international rating of experts = 2174 + 1.03 * years of serious practice.
These results show that, after three years of practice, masters have on average
83 international rating points more than experts (i.e., 2257 vs. 2174). Moreover, from
this point masters increase at the rate of 7 international rating points per year of
serious practice, whereas experts only increase at the rate of 1 Elo point per year of
15
serious practice. The difference between the two slopes was statistically significant:
Hotellings t(15) = 5.5, p < .01.
For Figure 8, we calculated the average international rating of players at each
age for both groups. We started the analysis at the age of 14 because there were too
few players with international rating before this age. We performed the same analysis
as above with age as a predictor variable (the age range was from 14 to 33). The
correlation between international rating and age was significant with the masters,
r(18) = .85, p < .0001), and non-significant with the experts, r(18) = .02, ns. The
regression analyses provided the following equations:
international rating of masters = 2169 + 7.2 * age;
international rating of experts = 2189 + 0.05 * age.
This result is similar to the previous one for the masters but not for the experts.
The experts seem to benefit somehow from the first years of serious practice and only
if they start before the age of 14; after this age they do not seem to improve at all. The
difference between the two slopes was statistically significant: Hotellings t(17) = 4.5,
p < .01.
The analyses of the data presented in Figures 7 and 8 should be taken with
caution. The means at each value of the x-axes were calculated only with the players
that had an international rating. It was not appropriate to follow the same procedure as
with Figures 1 and 2 (i.e., to calculate the mean value including the players with 0
hours of practice) because lack of international rating is not lack of chess skill. Hence,
the absolute values of international rating at each data point do not reflect the true
chess skill of the group perfectly. On the other hand, the differences between the
slopes show a reliable pattern: experts increase their skill level very little with the pass
of time, whereas masters keep increasing theirs.
16
We performed a different analysis with the same data in order to show that the
pattern is reliable. We calculated the percentage of players with international rating
and the percentage of players with more than 2200 Elo points at each year of serious
practice (see Figure 9) and age (see Figure 10). The data show a clear difference
between experts and masters.
17
number of books, .35 for the presence of coaching (0,1), .32 for the use of databases
(0,1), .27 for playing speed chess (0,1), and .27 for log speed games (all p < .05).
There were no significant correlations between skill and blindfold chess, blindfold
reading of games, use of chess programs.
The activities that are most practised by the players in this samplespeed
games and coaching (0,1)were correlated with skill, and thus seem to be useful as
well. Use of databases and computer programs are activities that were performed by
the same number of players, but the mean national rating of players using databases
was higher than that of players using programs. Playing blindfold games was not an
activity performed by many players and it did not have a high correlation with skill
level either. With speed chess rating used as dependent variable, the correlations were
.38 for log number of books, and .35 for coaching (both p < .05). There were no
significant correlations with the other variables.
We entered the eight variables corresponding to either group practice or
individual practice into a stepwise regression analysis, which removed all variables
except Log number of books, Presence of coach (0,1), and Log number of speed
games. A multiple regression with these three independent variables showed that they
all made a significant contribution (see Table 2). As Charness et al.s (2005) showed
that age was a moderator of the link between practice activities and skill level, we
carried out additional analyses with these three independent variables, splitting our
sample in players below 40 years of age and above (or equal) 40 years of age. The
results, shown in Tables 3 and 4, showed that with younger players, only Log number
of books was a significant predictor of skill, while, with older players, Log number of
books and Log number of speed games were predictors, the latter only marginally so.
18
Discussion
In the introduction we mentioned three issues: (a) to what extent is the amount
of practice important to achieving high levels of expertise? (b) does everybody benefit
from practice in the same way? and (c) what types of chess activities are most useful,
and in what order should they be practised? To address these three issues, we
considered both individual and group practice. We first discuss how our data stand
regarding these issues. Then, we will address the topics of generalisation and transfer.
Finally, we will consider the implications of our study for learning sciences in
general.
Before tackling these issues, we address the question of the reliability of the
questionnaire used in this study. It has been argued that individuals might not reliably
report their behaviour in the past (e.g., Davids, 2000; Starkes, Weir, & Young, 2003).
However, previous research has shown that this kind of questionnaire correlates fairly
well with independent measures (Ericsson et al., 1993). In addition, this methodology
produces results that are replicable. We used the same method as two previous studies
on chess (Charness, Krampe & Mayr, 1996; Charness et al., 2005), and, for example,
one key resultthe correlation between cumulative hours of individual practice and
19
skill level is fairly similar in the three studies, considering the fact that the players
participating in these studies come from three continents. We found a correlation of
.42, Charness et al. (1996) found a correlation of .60, and Charness et al. (2005) found
a correlation of .54 for the enlarged sample from their 1996 study, and .48 for a new
sample.
Regarding the role of practice in the acquisition of high-level cognitive skills,
our data partly support Ericsson et al.s (1993) framework of deliberate practice but
are against an extreme view of the role of practice (e.g., Howe, Davidson, & Sloboda,
1998). Moreover, our data are against an extreme view of the role of talent in the
acquisition of high-cognitive skills (e.g., Galton, 1869/1979). We found a significant
correlation between individual practice and national rating and also a significant
correlation between group practice and national rating. This result is also consistent
with previous similar studies in music (Ericsson et al., 1993), chess (Charness et al.,
1996; Charness et al., 2005) and sports (Janelle & Hillman, 2003). On the other hand,
none of these studies showed a correlation that accounts for all or even most of the
variability in skill level. There are several reasons why not all the variability is
explained by the number of hours of practice: measurement error, lack of sensitivity
of the method used, differences in quality of the activities performed (e.g., some
players may have had better coaches than others), age at which players started playing
chess or studying seriously, or non-chess related factors.
In a separate paper (Gobet & Campitelli, 2007), we explored other factors not
related to practice that may influence skill. We showed that a non-chess related factor,
handedness, differentiated chess players from the general population (i.e., there were
more non-right handed persons in a chess sample than in a non-chess sample). We
also found that the age at which players started playing seriously or enrolled in a
20
chess club was an important factor (i.e., there is a critical or sensitive period to start
practising chess seriously in order to achieve high levels of skill). This finding was
strengthened by the analyses presented in the current paper.
The novelty of the present article is that we present longitudinal data, which
affords the possibility of showing increases in rating and in amount of practice as a
function of age or as a function of years of serious practice. This new analysis shows
a difference in the number of hours of chess practice at early ages between masters,
experts and intermediate players (see Figures 1 and 2). Masters also showed an
advantage over experts in chess rating after only 3 years of serious practice (see
Figure 7), although there was no difference in amount of practice between masters
and experts after 3 years of serious practice (see Figures 3 and 4). This is a clear
indication that the first 3 years of serious practice at early ages are much more
profitable than the first 3 years of serious practice at later ages. Moreover, the present
data indicate that there was a considerable amount of dedication to chess before the
period players reported becoming serious about chess (between 10 and 12 years old in
masters and around 14 years old in experts; see Gobet & Campitelli, 2007, p. 163) and
that differences between masters and the rest of the players were already noticeable
by then (see Figures 1 and 2). This indicates that recreational exposure to chess at
early ages is also important, in contradiction to the deliberate practice framework.
Therefore, an extreme view that considers practice as a necessary and sufficient
condition to acquire high levels of expertise is not supported by our data. These new
findings highlight the advantages of using longitudinal data.
In order to address the second issue, we analysed our data from a different
perspective. Thorndike (1908) and Ackerman (1987) argued that with increased
practice the inter-individual variability in performance should diminish if the main
21
22
activity needed to acquire a vast database of chess knowledge. The more individual
practice, the higher the chess level. However, differences in individual practice alone
cannot explain the inter-individual variability in performance that is observed early
on, when players have little time of practice behind them (cf. Figures 7 and 8). Thus,
third, group practice plus individual practice plus factors unrelated to practice are the
necessary and sufficient conditions to achieve high-level performance in cognitive
skills such as chess. What are these factors? As explained above, in a separate paper
(Gobet & Campitelli, 2007), we showed that the age at which players start to play
seriously, controlling for number of hours of practice, is a good predictor of chess
skill and that an innate factor (i.e., handedness) differentiated chess players from nonchess players. Therefore, we suggest that large amounts of individual and group
practice, starting serious practice as early as possible, and some innate factors
(perhaps the ones responsible for determining handedness) are important factors for
acquiring expert performance.
The third issue we tackled in our study was that of the types of activities that
are useful for acquiring high-level cognitive skills. We examined this issue in two
different ways. First, when considering the role of practice, we investigated the
differential roles of individual and group practice and, second, we enquired about
more specific activities. This is an important issue because, although we agree with
Ericsson et al.s (1993) thesis that deliberate practice is a crucial factor for the
achievement of expert performance, we consider that their description of deliberate
practice in chess is simplistic. For example, they explicitly rule out tournament games
as part of deliberate practice. However, Charness et al. (1996, p. 75) presented data
which showed that active participation in chess tournaments was considered the most
relevant activity by chess players. Given that deliberate practice is the intentional
23
24
whole chess community), there are other factors that may be specific of our sample.
For example, the former studies were carried out with participants of Russia,
Germany, Canada and United States, whereas our study was carried out with
Argentine players. There may be cultural differences in the way Argentine players
approach chess in comparison to European and American players. Another reason that
may explain this difference is the wording of the questions. Charness et al.s (2005)
question about group practice is only restricted to serious competition, whereas our
question about group practice included both serious competition and study with other
players.
Another interesting pattern is that individual practice drastically diminishes at
the age of 18 in experts and intermediate players (see Figure 2). This is the age at
which most people in Buenos Aires either start university or start working; therefore,
these data may be showing that only masters that are having good results keep
practising at home, whereas experts and intermediates only continue playing chess for
fun at their leisure time.
Regarding the specific activities, we found that reading books, playing speed
chess, using databases, and having a coach are the most important ones inasmuch as
they showed the highest correlations with national rating, although the impact of these
activities on rating was moderated by age. This result suggests that the acquisition of
high levels of chess skill requires both individual practice (i.e., reading books and
studying with databases of chess games) and group practice (playing games and
receiving chess classes or coaching). These findings can be explained in terms of a
general theory of expertise: the template theory (Gobet & Simon, 1996a). We first
briefly explain the theory and then we relate these findings with the theory.
25
26
models that simulated human data in detail (Freudenthal et al., 2006; Gobet & Simon,
2000).
Within the framework of the template theory, playing numerous games
facilitates the acquisition of a large knowledge base of domain specific patterns in
long-term memory. (Note that playing rapid chess allows for the playing of many
more games than normal chess in the same amount of time.) However, it is necessary
to complement playing games with reading books, receiving formal coaching, and
using chess databases. Without the learning of proper actions to carry out in a given
situation, the large database of domain-specific patterns held in long-term memory
(chunks and templates) would be useless: a specific situation would be recognized,
but the player would not have knowledge as to what to do given this situation.
Activities such as coaching, studying databases with games of grandmasters or
reading books with grandmasters comments make it possible to learn specific chess
methods and sequences of moves and to link this procedural knowledge to perceptual
patterns, thus giving quality to the knowledge base. Playing rapid games may also
contribute to an ability important in the development of chess skill: the aptitude for
making decisions rapidly under time pressure, with only a minimal exploration of the
search tree and thus far less information than could be obtained after several minutes
of deliberation. This is partly made possible by the presence of chunks and templates,
which speed up the rate at which strong players can generate moves as they can rely
on automatic, pattern-recognition based processing rather than using slower heuristics
(Gobet, 1997).
Since the results reported in this article are consistent with a general theory of
expertise we consider that they can be generalised to other high-level cognitive
domains. However, we are negative about the possibility of transferring skills from
27
chess to other domains, and we do not recommend studying chess in order to improve
performance in other domains. Although there are strong claims in this respect, we
have showed elsewhere that most studies that claimed transfer from chess to other
cognitive skills had severe methodological problems (see Gobet & Campitelli, 2006,
for a review of research into chess and education). The explanation for the lack of
transfer is simple: our cognitive system operates with general mechanisms; what
differs is the type of information that we acquire from the environment. Acquiring a
vast knowledge base in one domain requires investing time and effort, and therefore
leaves less time to devote to the domain to which one wants to transfer skills. We
would predict transfer between domains that have similar environments but not
between domains that deal with different environmental information. For example, in
our sample, the correlation between national rating and speed chess rating is high (i.e.,
.83). Both skills share the same environment and follow the same rules, the only
difference being that the national rating measures chess performance in games lasting
4 hours on average and the speed chess rating measures performance in games lasting
10 minutes on average.
Finally, we draw conclusions for learning and teaching. Our data support
constructivist methods where the activity of the learner is emphasised and oppose
traditional educational methods where the students role is mostly passive. This study
showed that chess masters start engaging in group practice early on and to a larger
extent than experts and intermediate players. For example, at the age of 8 the average
number of cumulative hours of individual practice is 202, 51, and 5 in masters,
experts and intermediates, respectively; by contrast, the average number of
cumulative hours of group practice at the same age is higher in all the groups (547,
279 and 82, respectively). Given the correlational nature of our analysis, we cannot
28
decide whether the masters of our study practised in group more than the experts and
intermediates because they were winning more or that practising more led to
improved performance. In any case, from a learning standpoint our data suggest that it
is advisable to start participating in group activities from the beginning. Children
should have a strong interaction with the environment by playing against peers,
receiving qualified advice from a teacher or coach as a complement of reading books
individually. Moreover, the use of new technologies (in our case chess databases but
not chess playing programs) favours the acquisition of high-level cognitive skills (see
also Gobet, Campitelli & Waters, 2002).
An important finding of this article is that practice seems to be more profitable
at early ages than at older ages. This can be explained by the importance that the
template theory gives to the order in which knowledge is acquired (see Gobet, 2005).
Younger children tend to pay attention to concrete things; therefore it is likely that
they will prefer the perceptual configuration of chess pieces rather than conceptual
knowledge. Gobet (2005) argued that conceptual schemata (or templates) are better
acquired on the basis of perceptual chunks, because these chunks provide a cue that
would improve the efficiency of memory retrieval in the future. It is probable that
older teenagers, who have been already exposed to logical thinking at school, pay
relatively more attention to conceptual aspects of chess games before acquiring the
domain-specific perceptual chunks. As suggested by Gobet (2005) this would impact
in the speed of information retrieval in future chess games.
To conclude, our longitudinal data raise important questions with respect to
the deliberate practice framework. Contrary to its assumptions, practice is more
beneficial at early than later ages; group practice is more important than individual
practice; in spite of the absence of difference in amount of serious practice between
29
masters and experts in the first three years, the former had higher ratings than the
latter; and the experts did not improve much after three years in spite of considerable
amounts of deliberate practice. Together, these results raise serious doubts as to
whether practice is sufficient for reaching high levels of expertise. Similarly, our data
do not support traditional educational methods of acquiring a vast knowledge base by
long hours of isolation and little social interaction. Rather, reading books is still
advisable only if it is complemented with rich interaction with the environment. In
this way, the knowledge base the essential aspect of high-level skill acquisition
would not only be sizeable but it would also be of high quality.
30
References
Ackerman, P. L. (1987). Individual differences in skill learning: An integration of
psychometric and information processing perspectives. Psychological Bulletin,
102, 3-27.
Anderson, J. R., Reder, L. M., & Simon, H. A. (2000, Summer). Applications and
misapplications of cognitive psychology to mathematics education. Texas
Education Review.
Baxter, G. P., & Glaser, R. (1998). Investigating the cognitive complexity of science
assessments. Educational measurements: Issues and practices, 17, 37-45.
Campitelli, G., & Gobet, F. (2004). Adaptive expert decision making: Skilled chess
players search more and deeper. Journal of the International Computer Games
Association, 27, 209-216.
Campitelli, G., & Gobet, F. The mind's eye in blindfold chess. European Journal of
Cognitive Psychology, 17, 23-45.
Charness, N. (1981). Aging and skilled problem solving. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 110, 21-38.
Charness, N., Krampe, R. Th., & Mayr, U. (1996). The role of practice and coaching
in entrepreneurial skill domains: An international comparison of life-span chess
skill acquisition. In K. A. Ericsson (Ed.), The road to excellence (pp. 51-80).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Charness, N., Reingold, E. M., Pomplun, M., & Stampe, D. M. (2001). The perceptual
aspect of skilled performance in chess: Evidence from eye movements. Memory
& Cognition, 29, 1146-1152.
31
Charness, N., Tuffiash, M., Krampe, R., Reingold, E., & Vasyukova, E. (2005). The
role of deliberate practice in chess expertise. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19,
151-165.
Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). The mind's eye in chess. In W. G. Chase (Ed.),
Visual information processing. New York: Academic Press.
Cooke, N. J., Atlas, R. S., Lane, D. M., & Berger, R. C. (1993). Role of high-level
knowledge in memory for chess positions. American Journal of Psychology,
106, 321-351.
Davids, K. (2000). Skill acquisition and the theory of deliberate practice: It aint what
you do its the way that you do it! International Journal of Sport Psychology,
31, 461-466.
De Groot, A. D., & Gobet, F. (1996). Perception and memory in chess. Assen: Van
Gorcum.
Dextreit, J., & Engel, N. (1981). Jeu d'checs et sciences humaines [Chess and human
sciences]. Paris: Payot.
Elo, A. E. (1978). The rating of chessplayers. Past and present. New York: Arco.
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. Th., & Tesch-Romer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate
practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100,
363-406.
Ferrari, V., Didierjean, A., & Marmche, E. (2006). Dynamic perception in chess. The
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 397-410.
Freudenthal, D., Pine, J. M., & Gobet, F. (2006). Modelling the development of
childrens use of optional infinitives in English and Dutch using MOSAIC.
Cognitive Science, 30, 277-310.
32
Galton, F. (1979). Hereditary genius. An inquiry into its laws and consequences.
London: Julian Friedman. (Originally published in 1869).
Gobet, F. (1997). A pattern-recognition theory of search in expert problem solving.
Thinking & Reasoning, 3, 291-313.
Gobet, F. (1998a). Chess players' thinking revisited. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 57,
18-32.
Gobet, F. (1998b). Expert memory: A comparison of four theories. Cognition, 66,
115-152.
Gobet, F. (2005). Chunking models of expertise: Implications for education. Applied
Cognitive Psychology, 19, 183-204.
Gobet, F., & Campitelli, G. (2007). The role of domain-specific practice, handedness
and starting age in chess. Developmental Psychology, 43, 159-172.
Gobet, F., & Campitelli, G. (2006). Educational benefits of chess instruction: A
critical review. In T. Redman (Ed), Chess and education: Selected essays from
the Koltanowski conference (pp. 124-143). Dallas, TX: Chess Program at the
University of Texas at Dallas.
Gobet, F., Campitelli. G., & Waters, A. J. (2002). Rise of human intelligence:
Comments on Howard (1999). Intelligence, 30, 303-311.
Gobet, F., de Voogt, A. J., & Retschitzki, J. (2004). Moves in mind: The psychology
of board games. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Gobet, F., Lane, P. C. R., Croker, S., Cheng, P. C-H., Jones, G., Oliver, I. & Pine, J.
M. (2001). Chunking mechanisms in human learning. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 5, 236-243.
Gobet, F., & Simon, H. A. (1996 a). Templates in chess memory: A mechanism for
recalling several boards. Cognitive Psychology, 31, 1-40.
Gobet, F., & Simon, H. A. (1996 b). The roles of recognition processes and lookahead search in time-constrained expert problem solving: Evidence from grandmaster-level chess. Psychological Science, 7, 52-55.
Gobet, F., & Simon, H. A. (2000). Five seconds or sixty? Presentation time in expert
memory. Cognitive Science, 24, 651-682.
Gobet, F., & Wood, D. J. (1999). Expertise models of learning and computer-based
tutoring. Computers and Education, 33, 189-207.
Hatano, G., & Inagaki, K. (1986). Two courses of expertise. In H. Stevenson, H.
Azuma & K. Hakuta (Eds.), Child development and education in Japan (pp.
262-272). New York: Freeman.
Howe, M. J. A., Davidson, J. W., & Sloboda, J. A. (1998). Innate talents: Reality or
myth? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 399-442.
Janelle, C. M., & Hillman, C. H. (2003). Expert performance in sport: Current
perspectives and critical issues. In J. L. Starkes & K. A. Ericsson (Eds.), Expert
performance in sports: Advances in research on sport expertise (pp. 19-45).
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Lane, P. C. R., Cheng, P. C. H., & Gobet, F. (2000). CHREST+: Investigating how
humans learn to solve problems using diagrams. AISB Quarterly, 103, 24-30.
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh
Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97-113.
Saariluoma, P. (1991). Aspects of skilled imagery in blindfold chess. Acta
Psychologica, 77, 65-89.
Simon, H. A. (1980). Problem solving and education. In D. Tuma & F. Reif (Eds.),
Problem solving and education (pp. 81-96). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Do
you
reproduce
chess
games
from
journals
without
using
the
chessboard?__________________
17) Do you use any computer database to study chess?_________________________
18) Do you play games against chess software?_______________________________
19) Do you play speed chess games?_______________________________________
How many per week?________________________________________________
Table 1
Amount of Variance (r2) Explained by Linear, Quadratic, and Cubic Regressions, as
a Function of Skill Level and Type of Practice.
Linear
Skill
r2
Quadratic
r2
Cubic
r2
Group practice
Masters
.25
10.6**
.59
22.0***
.80
41.1***
Experts
.41
22.0***
.45
12.7***
.78
35.7***
Intermediates
.14
5.3*
.19
3.7*
.66
19.3***
Individual practice
Masters
.14
5.4*
.83
75.7***
.83
50.5***
Experts
.05
1.8
.51
15.9***
.63
17.0***
Intermediates
.09
3.2
.15
2.6
.56
12.8***
Note: The degrees of freedom are 32, 31, and 30 for the linear, quadratic, and cubic
regression, respectively.
*p < .05. **p < .01. *** p < .001.
Variable
Constant
SE
Beta
95% CI
1597.0
65.6
158.8
34.7
.403
89.9, 227.8
136.4
52.4
.233
32.4, 240.4
62.0
29.2
.190
4.1, 120.0
Note. R = .557, R2 = .310, Adjusted R2 = .288, F(3, 91) = 13.64, p < .001.
Variable
Constant
SE
Beta
95% CI
1646.8
97.3
165.2
42.2
.421
108.5
86.0
.135
1.26
ns
-63.0, 280.0
37.6
37.9
.107
0.99
ns
-38.1, 113.3
Note. R = .487, R2 = .237, Adjusted R2 = .203, F(3, 68) = 7.035, p < .001.
Variable
Constant
SE
Beta
95% CI
1525.0
104.9
185.1
67.3
.474
94.3
75.7
.214
1.2
ns
-64.2, 252.7
93.1
44.7
.334
2.08
.051
-0.44, 186.6
Note. R = .725, R2 = .526, Adjusted R2 = .451, F(3, 19) = 7.036, p < .005.
Figure captions
Figure 1: Group practice as a function of age and chess level. The mean and standard
deviations for each group calculated since 4 years of age were: Masters ( M = 10.6,
SD = 5.2); Experts ( M = 6.6, SD = 3.9); Intermediates ( M = 3.4, SD = 2.9). The mean
and standard deviations for each group calculated since starting playing were: Masters
( M = 13.0, SD = 5.7); Experts ( M = 9.4, SD = 5.6); Intermediates ( M = 6.2, SD =
4.8).
Figure 2: Individual practice as a function of age and chess level. The mean and
standard deviations for each group calculated since 4 years of age were: Masters ( M
= 7.3, SD = 4.6); Experts ( M = 3.7, SD = 3.6); Intermediates ( M = 2.6, SD = 3.5).
The mean and standard deviations for each group calculated since starting playing
were: Masters ( M = 8.7, SD = 4.6); Experts ( M = 6.3, SD = 6.9); Intermediates ( M =
4.4, SD = 6.4).
Figure 3: Cumulative group practice as a function of years of serious practice, for the
first ten years of serious practice. Note that the number of participants varies from
year to year. The first year of practice was calculated over 17 masters, 35 experts, and
31 intermediates and the tenth year of practice was calculated over 15, 22 and 17
participants, respectively. Bars represent the standard errors of the mean.
Figure 4: Cumulative individual practice as a function of years of serious practice, for
the first ten years of serious practice. Note that the number of participants varies from
year to year. The first year of practice was calculated over 17 masters, 35 experts, and
31 intermediates and the tenth year of practice was calculated over 15, 22 and 17
participants, respectively. Bars represent the standard errors of the mean.
Figure 5: Variability in national rating as a function of cumulated individual practice.
The mean and standard deviation of number of hours of individual practice in each
Figure 1.
Masters
Experts
Intermediates
20
15
10
5
0
4
10
13
16
19
22
Age
25
28
31
34
37
Figure 2.
Masters
Experts
20
Intermediates
15
10
5
0
4
10
13
16
19
22
Age
25
28
31
34
37
Figure 3.
9000
8000
Masters
7000
Experts
6000
Intermediates
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
0
1
10
Figure 4.
9000
8000
Masters
7000
Experts
6000
Intermediates
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
0
1
10
Figure 5.
300
250
2200
200
2000
150
100
1800
50
1600
Mean
SD
0
1
Individual practice
2400
Figure 6.
300
250
2200
200
150
2000
100
1800
50
1600
Mean
SD
0
1
Group practice
2400
Figure 7.
Masters
2500
Experts
Mean Elo
2400
2300
2200
2100
2000
1900
3
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
Figure 8.
2500
2400
Mean Elo
2300
2200
2100
Masters
2000
Experts
1900
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21 22
23 24
Age
25 26
27 28
29
30
31 32
33
Figure 9.
Experts_Elo
100
Masters_Elo
Percentage of players
90
Experts>2200
80
Masters>2200
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
0
3
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
Figure 10.
100
Percentage of players
90
80
70
Experts_Elo
60
Masters_Elo
50
40
Expert>2200
30
Masters>2200
20
10
0
14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33
Age
Authors note
We thank Merim Bilali, Neil Charness, Richard Smith, and anonymous
reviewers for comments on this paper.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Fernand Gobet,
Centre for the Study of Expertise, Brunel University, Uxbridge, Middlesex, UB8 3PH,
United Kingdom. Email: [email protected]
Footnotes
1
Federation).
2
The speed chess rating is computed independently from the national rating. In some
cases, the calculation for the former rating is based on more than one thousand games.
3
Charness et al.s study (1996, Table 2.4) considered active participation in chess
tournaments as the most relevant activity.
4
Charness et al. (2005) found that the relation between skill level and practice
activities was moderated by age. This was the case in our sample as well with respect
to the relation between rating and individual or group practice, as the correlations
were stronger for older players, although, critically, they were always higher for
group practice than individual practice. For the players below 40 years of age, the
correlations were r (69) = .36, p < .005, for individual practice, and r (68) = .55, p <
.001, for group practice. The respective correlations for players of 40 years of age and
above were: r(21) = .59, p = .005, and r ( 21) = .72, p < .001.
5
Note also that, although the skill levels are broken down in slightly different ways,
the data in Figure 4 are in close agreement with those presented in Figure 1 in
Charness et al. (2005). This common pattern suggests that the data of our study are
generalisable (see also the Discussion section).