Dev Reading Comprehension
Dev Reading Comprehension
Chapter 10
205
207
cover here. Hmm...I see a picture of an owl. It looks like heI think
it is a heis wearing pajamas, and he is carrying a candle. I predict
that this is going to be a make-believe story because owls do not
really wear pajamas and carry candles. I predict it is going to be
about this owl, and it is going to take place at nighttime.
The title will give me more clues about the book; the title is
Owl at Home. So this makes me think even more that this book is
going to be about the owl. He will probably be the main character.
And it will take place in his house.
Okay, I have made some predictions about the book based on
the cover. Now I am going to open up the book and begin reading.
3. Collaborative use of the strategy in action. I have made some good
predictions so far in the book. From this part on I want you to
make predictions with me. Each of us should stop and think about
what might happen next.... Okay, now lets hear what you think
and why....
4. Guided practice using the strategy with gradual release of responsibility.
Early on...
I have called the three of you together to work on making predictions while you read this and other books. After every few pages
I will ask each of you to stop and make a prediction. We will talk
about your predictions and then read on to see if they come true.
Later on...
Each of you has a chart that lists different pages in your book.
When you finish reading a page on the list, stop and make a prediction. Write the prediction in the column that says Prediction.
When you get to the next page on the list, check off whether your
prediction Happened, Will not happen, or Still might happen.
Then make another prediction and write it down. (This is based
on the Reading Forecaster Technique from Mason and Au [1986]
described and cited in Lipson and Wixson [1991].)
5. Independent use of the strategy. It is time for silent reading. As you
read today, remember what we have been working onmaking
predictions while we read. Be sure to make predictions every two
or three pages. Ask yourself why you made the prediction you
didwhat made you think that. Check as you read to see whether
209
your prediction came true. Jamal is passing out Predictions! bookmarks to remind you.
Throughout these five phases, it is important that neither the teacher
nor the students lose sight of the need to coordinate or orchestrate comprehension strategies. Strategies are not to be used singlygood readers
do not read a book and only make predictions. Rather, good readers use
multiple strategies constantly. Although the above model foregrounds a
particular strategy at a particular time, other strategies should also be referenced, modeled, and encouraged throughout the process. A way of conceptualizing the orchestration process is captured in a classic visual model
from Pearson and Gallaghers (1983) early work on comprehension instruction. In that model (see Figure 10.1), teachers move from a situation in which they assume all the responsibility for performing a task
Figure 10.1. Gradual release of responsibility
100
Percent of Task Responsibility Assumed by the Teacher
Primarily Teacher
Modeling*
Guided Practice
Direct
Instruction*
Scaffolding*
Participating*
Facilitating*
Primarily
Student
0
100
As one moves down the diagonal from upper left to lower right, students assume more, and
teachers less, responsibility for task completion. There are three regions of responsibility:
primarily teacher in the upper left corner, primarily student in the lower right, and shared
responsibility in the center. (This figure is adapted with permission from Pearson and Gallagher
[1983]; the asterisked terms are borrowed from Au & Raphael [1998].)
211
experience reading real texts for real reasons and creating an environment rich in high-quality talk about textwill undoubtedly help. Other
strategies can be found in books, articles, and chapters devoted specifically to the topic of motivation and engagement (e.g., Guthrie &
Wigfield, 1997).
Ongoing assessment. Finally, as with any good instruction, comprehension instruction should be accompanied by ongoing assessment.
Teachers should monitor students use of comprehension strategies and
their success at understanding what they read. Results of this monitoring
should, in turn, inform the teachers instruction. When a particular
strategy continues to be used ineffectively, or not at all, the teacher
should respond with additional instruction or a modified instructional
approach. At the same time, students should be monitoring their own
use of comprehension strategies, aware of their strengths as well as their
weaknesses as developing comprehenders.
213
discussion), and the SAIL program (see later discussion), all of which
have been shown to be effective at improving student comprehension. It
is also an important part of the early modeling stages of instruction in
many comprehension training routines, for example, the QAR work of
Raphael and her colleagues (Raphael, Wonnacott, & Pearson, 1983) and
the inference training work of Gordon and Pearson (1983). These studies suggest that teacher modeling is most effective when it is explicit, leaving the student to intuit or infer little about the strategy and its
application, and flexible, adjusting strategy use to the text rather than presenting it as governed by rigid rules. Teacher think-aloud with these attributes is most likely to improve students comprehension of text.
Student think-aloud. Instruction that entails students thinking aloud
themselves also has proven effective at improving comprehension (see
Kucan & Beck, 1997, for a review). A classic study by Bereiter and Bird
(1985) showed that students who were asked to think aloud while reading had better comprehension than students who were not taught to
think aloud, according to a question-and-answer comprehension test.
A compelling study by Silven and Vauras (1992) demonstrated that students who were prompted to think aloud as part of their comprehension
training were better at summarizing information in a text than students
whose training did not include think-aloud.
Several scholars have theorized about why student think-aloud is effective at improving comprehension. One popular theory is that getting
students to think aloud decreases their impulsiveness (Meichebaum &
Asnarow, 1979). Rather than jumping to conclusions about text meaning
or moving ahead in the text without having sufficiently understood what
had already been read, think-aloud may lead to more thoughtful, strategic reading. A study conducted with third-grade students provides some
empirical support for this theory. Baumann and his colleagues found
that training in think-aloud improved childrens ability to monitor their
comprehension while reading (Baumann, Seifert-Kessel, & Jones, 1992).
Third-grade children trained to think aloud as they used several comprehension strategies were better than a comparison group at detecting
errors in passages, responding to a questionnaire about comprehension
monitoring, and completing cloze items. One student trained in thinkaloud explained, When I read I think, is this making sense? I might...ask
questions about the story and reread or retell the story (Baumann et al.,
p. 159). This and other student comments suggested a thoughtful, strategic approach to reading through think-aloud.
215
not enough. Students must actually follow the texts structure in building their recall for the effect to be realized; not surprisingly, more good
than poor readers are inclined to do so (Bartlett, 1978; Taylor, 1980).
The approaches to teaching text structure have exhibited substantial
variability, beginning with general attempts to sensitize students to
structural elements (e.g., Bartlett, 1978; Davis, Lange, & Samuels, 1988;
Slater, Graves, & Piche, 1985) and extending to hierarchical summaries
of key ideas (e.g., Taylor & Beach, 1984) and to visual representations of
key ideas, such as conceptual maps, semantic networks, charts, and
graphs (e.g., Armbruster & Anderson, 1980; Armbruster, Anderson, &
Ostertag, 1987; Gallagher & Pearson, 1989; Geva, 1983; Holley &
Dansereau, 1984). In general, the research suggests that almost any approach to teaching the structure of informational text improves both
comprehension and recall of key text information. One plausible explanation is that systematic attention to the underlying organization,
whether intended by the authors of texts or not, helps students relate
ideas to one another in ways that make them more understandable and
more memorable. Another plausible explanation is that it is actually
knowledge of the content, not facility with text structure, that children
acquire when they attend to the structural features of text. In other
words, text structure is nothing more than an alias for the underlying
structure of knowledge in that domain.
Only a few of the studies in this area have evaluated these competing
hypotheses. The results of the Gallagher and Pearson (1989) work suggest
that both content and structural features contribute to the salutary effects
of text structure instruction. Over a series of several weeks, Gallagher
and Pearson taught fourth-grade students, mainly poor readers, to apply
a consistent structural framework, instantiated as a set of matrix charts
and flowcharts, to their reading and discussion of short books about different social insects (ants, bees, and termites). The outcome measures
included several independently read passages, each passage successively
more distant from the original social insect books. They read, in order, a
passage about a fourth social insect, the paper wasp, a passage about a human society, and a passage about geographic formations such as gulfs,
capes, peninsulas, and the like. As the conceptual distance between the
original set of books and the testing passages increased, the effect of the
intervention (compared with a group who read the same texts and
answered questions and with a group that only read the texts) decreased in
magnitude, but was still statistically significant, suggesting that students
217
were learning something about (a) insect societies, (b) social organization in general, and (c) how to unearth the structure of an informational
text. From a classroom teachers perspective, there is some comfort in
knowing that content knowledge and text structure are naturally intertwined; after all, either or both represent legitimate curricular goals.
Visual representations of text. There is an old saying that a picture is
worth a thousand words. When it comes to comprehension, this saying
might be paraphrased, a visual display helps readers understand, organize, and remember some of those thousand words. Compare the
short text on digestion to the flow chart in Figure 10.2. The text is verbal,
abstract, and eminently forgettable; by contrast, the flowchart is visual,
concrete, and arguably more memorable.
Teeth
break
food into
small bits
Swallow and
travels
. food
through tube
into stomach
Food is broken
down even
more in the
stomach, creating
nutrients and
waste
That said, we readily admit that when it comes to the use of visual
representations of text, it is difficult, perhaps impossible, to specify exactly what it is that students attend to and learn when teachers use them
as heuristic devices to aid in comprehension and recall. The ubiquitous
use of semantic maps and webs reveals this ambiguity. Consider, for example, the web in Figure 10.3.
This could be a graphic summary of an article about coyotes. Or, it
could be a map of an individuals (or a whole classs collective) knowledge
about coyotes. Or, it could be a heuristic device used by a teacher to teach
key vocabulary in a unit on scavenging animals. In a practical sense, as we
pointed out in discussing text structure instruction, it does not really
matter. To the contrary, we would expect tools and activities that improve
comprehension to also enhance knowledge of text structure and vocabulary acquisition. The point about visual representations is that they are
re-presentations; literally, they allow us to present information again. It
is through that active, transformative process that knowledge, comprehension, and memory form a synergistic relationshipwhatever improves one of these elements also improves the others.
Much of the research cited in the previous section on text structure
applies to the use of visual displays. Most notable, because of their
Garbage g
Food
Habitats
ests
For
Deserts
Cities
Famous
Coyotes
Relatives
Coyotes
Humans
lves
Wo
Dogs
Hyenas
Ways to
Harm Humans
Natural
Enemies
Ways to
Help Humans
219
the original. This sounds difficult, and the research demonstrates that,
in fact, it is. (p. 244)
Indeed, most people with relevant experience will agree that summarizing is a difficult task for many children. Many children require instruction and practice in summarizing before they are able to produce good
oral and written summaries of text. Interestingly, research suggests that
instruction and practice in summarizing not only improves students
ability to summarize text, but also their overall comprehension of text
content. Thus, instruction in summarization can be considered to meet
dual purposes: to improve students ability to summarize text and to improve their ability to comprehend text and recall.
There are at least two major approaches to the teaching of summarization. In rule-governed approaches, students are taught to follow a set
of step-by-step procedures to develop summaries. For example, McNeil
and Donant (1982) teach the following rules, which draw from the work
of Brown, Campione, and Day (1981) and Kintsch and Van Dijk (1978):
Rule 1: Delete unnecessary material.
Rule 2: Delete redundant material.
Rule 3: Compose a word to replace a list of items.
Rule 4: Compose a word to replace individual parts of an action.
Rule 5: Select a topic sentence.
Rule 6: Invent a topic sentence if one is not available.
Through teacher modeling, group practice, and individual practice, students learn to apply these rules to create brief summaries of text.
Other approaches to summarizing text are more holistic. One that
has been the subject of research is the GIST procedure (Cunningham,
1982). In GIST, students create summaries of 15 or fewer words for increasingly large amounts of text, beginning with single sentences and
working incrementally to an entire paragraph. As Cunningham describes it, GIST is conducted first as a whole class, then in small groups,
and finally on an individual basis.
Working with sixth-grade students, Bean and Steenwyk (1984) studied the effectiveness of McNeil and Donants set of rules procedure and
Cunninghams GIST procedure. They found that versions of both approaches were effective not only in improving students written summaries of text, but also in improving their comprehension of text as
221
questions about the texts they read, although not definitive, is generally
positive and encouraging (see Rosenshine, Meister, & Chapman, 1996,
for a review). Raphael and her colleagues (Raphael & McKinney, 1983;
Raphael & Pearson, 1985; Raphael & Wonnacott, 1985) carried out
perhaps the most elaborate line of work on question generation in
the mid-1980s. Using a technique called QARs (Question-AnswerRelationships), Raphael and her colleagues modeled and engaged students in the process of differentiating the types of questions they could
ask of text. Students learned to distinguish among three types of questions: (1) Right There QARs were those in which the question and the answer were explicitly stated in the text, (2) Think and Search QARs had
questions and answers in the text, but some searching and inferential text
connections were required to make the link, and (3) On My Own QARs
were those in which the question was motivated by some text element
or item of information, but the answer had to be generated from the students prior knowledge. Through a model of giving students everincreasing responsibility for the question generation, Raphael and her
colleagues were able to help students develop a sense of efficacy and confidence in their ability to differentiate strategies in both responding to
and generating their own questions for text.
Later research by Yopp (1988) indicated that when students learn to
generate questions for text, their overall comprehension improves. In a
variation that wedded the logic of QARs with the work on story schemas
(e.g., Singer & Donlan, 1982), Yopp studied three different groups that
varied in terms of who was taking the responsibility for question generation. In the first group, the teacher asked the questions; in the second,
the students generated their own; in the third, the students generated
their own and were provided with a metacognitive routine (in the manner of QAR) for answering their own questions. The second and third
groups performed better on posttests given during instruction and after the instruction had ended, suggesting that student control of the
questioning process is a desirable instructional goal. Furthermore, although it did not translate into higher performance on the comprehension assessments, the third group, those who received the additional
metacognitive routine, were better at explaining the processes they used
to answer questions.
Perhaps the most compelling evidence for the efficacy of teaching
students to generate their own questions while reading comes from the
research cited in the subsequent section in which we move from
223
225
readers. The following dialogues come from reciprocal teaching sessions with students struggling with the technique:
T: What would be a good question about pit vipers that starts with the
word why?
S: (No response)
T: How about, Why are the snakes called pit vipers?
T: That was a fine job, Ken, but I think there might be something to
add to our summary. There is more information that I think we
need to include. This paragraph is mostly about what?
S: The third method of artificial evaporation. (Palincsar & Brown,
1984, p. 138)
This next dialogue comes from a first-grade class employing reciprocal teaching.
S1: My question is, what does the aquanaut need when he goes under
water?
S2: A watch.
S3: Flippers.
S4: A belt.
S1: Those are all good answers.
T:` Nice job! I have a question too. Why does the aquanaut wear a belt?
What is so special about it?
S3: Its a heavy belt and keeps him from floating up to the top again.
T: Good for you.
S1: For my summary now: This paragraph was about what aquanauts
need to take when they go under the water.
S5: And also about why they need those things.
227
Students Achieving Independent Learning (SAIL) and other transactional strategies approaches. The Students Achieving Independent
Learning, or SAIL, program also teaches a package of comprehension
strategies. Used in Montgomery County, Maryland, USA, strategies emphasized in SAIL include predicting, visualizing, questioning, clarifying,
making associations (e.g., between the text and the students experiences),
and summarizing (Pressley et al., 1994). Use of these strategies is taught
through teacher think-aloud and explicit instruction. Students practice
the strategies in various settings, with an emphasis on student interpretation of text. Indeed, SAIL and a similar program used at the Benchmark
School in Media, Pennsylvania, USA, have been characterized as transactional strategies instruction because of their emphasis on transactions
among teacher, student, and text (Pressley et al., 1992).
In SAIL, the emphasis is on helping students learn when to use which
comprehension strategies. The program uses a range of different kinds of
texts that are often quite challenging for students because they are at or
above grade level. Consider this summary of a SAIL lesson from a
fourth-grade classroom:
Teacher asks students to write a prediction about what the book will
be about based on its cover.
Teacher begins reading the book, thinking aloud as she reads (e.g.,
I wonder if that is the Georgetown in Washington, D.C.; August
must be the name of a person).
Students take turns reading aloud. As students read, the teacher cues
students to apply strategies as appropriate (e.g., Tell us what has
been going on here).
Students spontaneously employ strategies they have learned in previous work, including seeking clarification, relating the text to their
lives, and visualizing (e.g., I can see a...).
Students return to their written predictions to assess their accuracy.
As this summary suggests, there is not a predetermined sequence of
strategies to use in SAIL lessons. Rather, strategy use depends on the
situation; students must coordinate their repertoire of comprehension
strategies. Also, more attention is given to individual interpretation of
text than to right answers. Figure 10.4 lists the menu of strategies that
can be used in transactional strategies instruction. Two features of the
list are worth noting: First, it incorporates all the strategies within
Interpretive Strategies
Thinking aloud
Character development
Imagining how a character might feel
Identifying with a character
Constructing images
Creating themes
Summarizing
Questioning
Clarifying
reciprocal teaching (on the cognitive side of the ledger). Second, the list
is long enough to guarantee selective application (based on the text and
the learning context) to any given text. There is no way that a teacher
could ensure that each strategy was applied to every text encountered
by a group of students.
Much of the research on SAIL and its intellectual cousin, transactional
strategies instruction, has been qualitative, looking in detail at the ways
that strategies are taught and learned. These studies suggest that SAIL and
similar programs offer a promising approach to comprehension instruction, with rich, motivating interactions around text and increasing sophistication of student strategy use over time. One quasi-experimental
study of SAIL has confirmed the effectiveness of the approach at improving student comprehension (Brown, Pressley, Van Meter, & Schuder,
1996). In the study, second-grade students in SAIL classrooms outperformed students in comparable non-SAIL classrooms on standardized
measures of both reading comprehension and word attack. Students in
SAIL classrooms also remembered more content from their daily lessons
than students in non-SAIL classrooms. Additional evidence for the efficacy of this family of transactional strategy instruction routines can be
found in Pressleys (1998) recent review.
229
Questioning the Author. Beginning in the early 1990s, Isabel Beck and
Margaret McKeown, along with a group of colleagues at the University
of Pittsburgh and in the surrounding schools, began work on a comprehension routine called Questioning the Author (QtA). Inspired by
their own insights (see Beck, McKeown, Sandora, & Worthy, 1996,
p. 386) in revising text to make it more considerate (Beck, McKeown, &
Gromoll, 1989), Beck and her colleagues bootstrapped this approach to
engaging students with text. The idea was that if they, as knowledgeable
adult readers, found the process of trying to figure out what authors
had in mind in writing a text in a certain way helpful, perhaps students
would benefit from querying the author in a similar spirit. Hence, they
developed a set of generic questions that could be asked as a teacher
and group of students made their way through a text. The essential approach is to query a text collaboratively, section by section, with questions like those listed in Figure 10.5 as a guide.
Candidate Questions
231
233
Summary
In this chapter, we have described effective individual and collective
strategies for teaching comprehension of text and discussed characteristics of a balanced comprehension program into which such strategies
could be embedded. In Figure 10.6, we offer a tool for assessing the comprehension instruction environment in your own classroom. We hope
that this will aid readers in identifying both strengths and weaknesses
in comprehension instruction as well as serving as a summary of the material presented in this chapter. We hope it will not prove overwhelming, even to those who are novices at comprehension instruction. Realize
that the use of even one of the techniques described in this chapter has
235
Armbruster, B.B., Anderson, T.H., & Ostertag, J. (1987). Does text structure/summarization instruction facilitate learning from expository text? Reading Research
Quarterly, 22, 331346.
Au, K.H., & Raphael, T.E. (1998). Curriculum and teaching in literature-based programs. In T.E. Raphael & K.H. Au (Eds.), Literature-based instruction: Reshaping the
curriculum (pp. 123148). Norwood, MA: Christopher-Gordon Publishers.
Ausabel, D.P. (1968). Educational psychology: A cognitive view. New York: Holt, Rinehart
and Winston.
Bartlett, B.J. (1978).Top-level structure as an organizational strategy for recall of classroom text. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Arizona State University, Tempe.
Baumann, J.F., Seifert-Kessel, N., & Jones, L.A. (1992). Effect of think-aloud instruction on elementary students comprehension monitoring abilities. Journal of Reading
Behavior, 24, 143172.
Bean, T.W., & Steenwyk, F.L. (1984). The effect of three forms of summarization instruction on sixth graders summary writing and comprehension. Journal of Reading
Behavior, 16, 297306.
Beck, I.L., McKeown, M.G., & Gromoll, E.W. (1989). Learning from social studies texts.
Cognition and Instruction, 6, 99158.
Beck, I.L., McKeown, M.G., Hamilton, R.L., & Kucan, L. (1997). Questioning the author: An approach for enhancing student engagement with text. Newark, DE:
International Reading Association.
Beck, I.L., McKeown, M.G., Sandora, C., & Worthy, J. (1996). Questioning the author:
A yearlong classroom implementation to engage students with text. The Elementary
School Journal, 96, 385414.
Bereiter, C., & Bird, M. (1985). Use of thinking aloud in identification and teaching of
reading comprehension strategies. Cognition and Instruction, 2, 131156.
Block, C.C., & Pressley, M. (2001). Comprehension instruction: Research-based best practices. New York: Guilford.
Brown, A.L., Campione, J.C., & Day, J.D. (1981). Learning to learn: On training students
to learn from texts. Educational Researcher, 10, 1421.
Brown, A.L., & Palinscar, A.S. (1985). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension strategies: A
natural history of one programme for enhancing learning. (Tech. Rep. No. 334).
Urbana, IL: University of Illinois, Center for the Study of Reading.
Brown, R., Pressley, M., Van Meter, P., & Schuder, T. (1996). A quasi-experimental
validation of transactional strategies instruction with low-achieving second-grade
readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 1837.
Cunningham, J.W. (1982). Generating interactions between schemata and text. In J.A.
Niles & L.A. Harris (Eds.), New inquiries in reading research and instruction (pp.
4247). Rochester, NY: National Reading Conference.
Davis, J.N., Lange, D.L., & Samuels, S.J. (1988). Effects of text structure instruction on
foreign language readers recall of a scientific journal article. Journal of Reading
Behavior, 20, 203214.
Dole, J.A., Brown, K.J., & Trathen, W. (1996). The effects of strategy instruction on the
comprehension performance of at-risk students. Reading Research Quarterly, 31,
6288.
237
Dole, J.A., Duffy, G.G., Roehler, L.R., & Pearson, P.D. (1991). Moving from the old to
the new: Research on reading comprehension instruction. Review of Educational
Research, 61, 239264.
Durkin, D. (1978). What classroom observations reveal about reading comprehension
instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 14, 481533.
Fielding, L.G., Anderson, R.C., & Pearson, P.D. (1990). How discussion questions influence childrens story understanding (Tech. Rep. No. 490). Urbana, IL: University of
Illinois, Center for the Study of Reading.
Fitzgerald, J., & Spiegel, D.L. (1983). Enhancing childrens reading comprehension
through instruction in narrative structure. Journal of Reading Behavior, 15, 117.
Gallagher, M., & Pearson, P.D. (1989). Discussion, comprehension, and knowledge acquisition in content area classrooms (Tech. Rep. No. 480). Urbana, IL: University of
Illinois, Center for the Study of Reading.
Geva, E. (1983). Facilitating reading comprehension through flowcharting. Reading
Research Quarterly, 18, 384405.
Gordon, C.J., & Pearson, P.D. (1983). The effects of instruction in metacomprehension and
inferencing on childrens comprehension abilities (Tech. Rep. No. 277). Urbana, IL:
University of Illinois, Center for the Study of Reading.
Greenewald, M.J., & Rossing, R.L. (1986). Short-term and long-term effects of story
grammar and self-monitoring training on childrens story comprehension. In J.A.
Niles & R.V. Lalik (Eds.), Solving problems in literacy: Learners, teachers, and researchers (35th Yearbook of the National Reading Conference, pp. 210213).
Rochester, NY: National Reading Conference.
Guthrie, J.T., Van Meter, P., McCann, A.D., Wigfield, A., Bennett, L., Poundstone, C.C.,
et al. (1996). Growth of literacy engagement: Changes in motivations and strategies
during concept-oriented reading instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 31,
306332.
Guthrie, J.T., & Wigfield, A. (Eds.). (1997). Reading engagement: Motivating readers
through integrated instruction. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
Guzzetti, B.J., Snyder, T.E., Glass, G.V., & Gamas, W.S. (1993). Promoting conceptual
change in science: A comparative meta-analysis of instructional interventions from
reading education and science education. Reading Research Quarterly, 28, 116159.
Hansen, J. (1981). The effects of inference training and practice on young childrens
reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 391417.
Hansen, J., & Pearson, P.D. (1983). An instructional study: Improving the inferential
comprehension of good and poor fourth-grade readers. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 75, 821829.
Holley, C.D., & Dansereau, D.F. (1984). Spatial learning strategies: Techniques, applications, and related issues. New York: Academic Press.
Idol, L. (1987). Group story mapping: A comprehension strategy for both skilled and unskilled readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 20, 196205.
Kintsch, W., & Van Dijk, T.A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85, 363394.
Klinger, J.K., & Vaughn, S. (1999). Promoting reading comprehension, content learning,
and English acquisition through Collaborative Strategic Reading (CSR). The Reading
Teacher, 52, 738747.
Kucan, L., & Beck, I.L. (1997). Thinking aloud and reading comprehension research:
Inquiry, instruction and social interaction. Review of Educational Research, 67,
271299.
Levin, J.R., & Pressley, M. (1981). Improving childrens prose comprehension: Selected
strategies that seem to succeed. In C.M. Santa & B.L. Hayes (Eds.), Childrens prose
comprehension: Research and practice (pp. 4471). Newark, DE: International
Reading Association.
Lipson, M.Y., & Wixson, K.K. (1991). Assessment and instruction of reading disability: An
interactive approach. New York: HarperCollins.
Mandler, J.M. (1978). A code in the mode: The use of a story schema in retrieval.
Discourse Processes, 1, 1435.
McGinley, W.J., & Denner, P.R. (1987). Story impressions: A prereading/writing activity. Journal of Reading, 31, 248253.
McNeil, J., & Donant, L. (1982). Summarization strategy for improving reading comprehension. In J.A. Niles & L.A. Harris (Eds.), New inquiries in reading research and
instruction (pp. 215219). Rochester, NY: National Reading Conference.
Meichebaum, D., & Asnarow, J. (1979). Cognitive behavior modification and metacognitive development: Implications for the classroom. In P. Kendall & S. Hollon (Eds.),
Cognitive behavioral interventions: Theory research and procedures (pp. 1135). New
York: Academic Press.
Meyer, B.J.F. (1975). The organization of prose and its effect on memory. Amsterdam,
Netherlands: North-Holland Publishing.
Meyer, B.J.F., Brandt, D.M., & Bluth, G.J. (1980). Use of top-level structure in text: Key
for reading comprehension of ninth-grade students. Reading Research Quarterly, 16,
72103.
Meyer, B.J.F., & Rice, G.E. (1984). The structure of text. In P.D. Pearson, R. Barr, M.L.
Kamil, & P. Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 319351). New
York: Longman.
Moore, P.J. (1988). Reciprocal teaching and reading comprehension: A review. Journal
of Research in Reading, 11, 314.
Morrow, L.M. (1984a). Effects of story retelling on young childrens comprehension and
sense of story structure. In J.A. Niles & L.A. Harris (Eds.), Changing perspectives on research in reading language processing and instruction (33rd Yearbook of the National
Reading Conference, pp. 95100). Rochester, NY: National Reading Conference.
Morrow, L.M. (1984b). Reading stories to young children: Effects of story structure
and traditional questioning strategies on comprehension. Journal of Reading
Behavior, 16, 273288.
National Reading Panel (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment
of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction
(National Institute of Health Pub. No. 00-4769). Washington, DC: National Institute
of Child Health and Human Development.
Neuman, S. (1988). Enhancing childrens comprehension through previewing. In J.
Readence & R.S. Baldwin (Eds.), Dialogues in literacy research (37th Yearbook of the
National Reading Conference, pp. 219224). Chicago: National Reading Conference.
Nolte, R., & Singer, H. (1985). Active comprehension: Teaching a process of reading
comprehension and its effects on achievement. The Reading Teacher, 39, 2431.
239
Palincsar, A.S. (1982). Improving the reading comprehension of junior high students
through the reciprocal teaching of comprehension-monitoring strategies. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.
Palincsar, A.S., & Brown, A.L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension fostering
and monitoring activities. Cognition and Instruction, 1, 117175.
Palincsar, A.S., & Brown, A.L. (1986). Interactive teaching to promote independent
learning from text. The Reading Teacher, 39, 771777.
Palincsar, A.S., Brown, A.L., & Martin, S.M. (1987). Peer interaction in reading comprehension instruction. Educational Psychologist, 22, 231253.
Paris, S.G., Cross, D.R., & Lipson, M.Y. (1984). Informed strategies for learning: A program to improve childrens reading awareness and comprehension. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 76, 12391252.
Pearson, P.D. (1981). Asking questions about stories. In Ginn Occasional Papers:
Writings in reading and language arts (Monograph No. 15). Lexington, MA: Ginn &
Co. Reprinted in A.J. Harris & E.R. Sipay (Eds.), Readings in reading instruction (3rd
ed.). New York: Longman, 1984.
Pearson P.D., & Fielding, L. (1991). Comprehension instruction. In R. Barr, M.L. Kamil,
P. Mosenthal, & P.D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. II, pp.
815860). White Plains, NY: Longman.
Pearson, P.D., & Gallagher, M.C. (1983). The instruction of reading comprehension.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 8, 317344.
Pearson, P.D., & Johnson, D. (1978). Teaching reading comprehension. New York: Holt,
Rinehart & Winston.
Pearson, P.D., Roehler, L., Dole, J., & Duffy, G. (1992). Developing expertise in reading
comprehension. In S.J. Samuels & A.E. Farstrup (Eds.), What research has to say about
reading instruction (2nd ed., pp. 145199). Newark, DE: International Reading
Association.
Pressey, S.L.A. (1926). A simple apparatus which gives tests and scoresand teaches.
School and Society, 23, 373376.
Pressley, M. (1998). Comprehension strategies instruction. In J. Osborn & F. Lehr (Eds.),
Literacy for all: Issues in teaching and learning (pp. 113133). New York: Guilford.
Pressley, M. (2000). What should comprehension instruction be the instruction of? In
M.L. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, P.D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 3, pp. 545561). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Pressley, M., & Afflerbach, P. (1995). Verbal protocols of reading: The nature of constructively responsive reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Pressley, M., Almasi, J., Schuder, T., Bergman, J., Hite, S., El-Dinary, P.B., et al. (1994).
Transactional instruction of comprehension strategies: The Montgomery County,
Maryland, SAIL Program. Reading and Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning
Difficulties, 10, 519.
Pressley, M., El-Dinary, P.B., Gaskins, I., Schuder, T., Bergman, J.L., Almasi, J., et al.
(1992). Beyond direct explanation: Transactional instruction of reading comprehension strategies. The Elementary School Journal, 92, 513555.
Pressley, M., & Wharton-McDonald, R. (1998). The development of literacy, part 4: The
need for increased comprehension in upper-elementary grades. In M. Pressley (Ed.),
Reading instruction that works: The case for balanced teaching (pp. 192227). New
York: Guilford.
Raphael, T.E., & McKinney, J. (1983). An examination of fifth- and eighth-grade childrens question answering behavior: An instructional study in metacognition. Journal
of Reading Behavior, 15, 6786.
Raphael, T.E., & Pearson, P.D. (1985). Increasing students awareness of sources of information for answering questions. American Educational Research Journal, 22,
217236.
Raphael, T.E., & Wonnacott, C.A. (1985). Heightening fourth-grade students sensitivity to sources of information for answering comprehension questions. Reading
Research Quarterly, 20, 282296.
Raphael, T.E., Wonnacott, C.A., & Pearson, P.D. (1983). Increasing students sensitivity
to sources of information: An instructional study in question-answer relationships (Tech.
Rep. No. 284). Urbana, IL: University of Illinois, Center for the Study of Reading.
Rickards, J.P. (1976). Type of verbatim question interspersed in text: A new look at the
position effect. Journal of Reading Behavior, 8, 3745.
Rosenshine, B., & Meister, C. (1994). Reciprocal teaching: A review of the research.
Review of Educational Research, 64, 479530.
Rosenshine, B., Meister, C., & Chapman, S. (1996). Teaching students to generate questions: A review of the intervention studies. Review of Educational Research, 66,
181221.
Silven, M., & Vauras, M. (1992). Improving reading through thinking aloud. Learning
and Instruction, 2, 6988.
Singer, H., & Donlan, D. (1982). Active comprehension: Problem-solving schema with
question generation for comprehension of complex short stories. Reading Research
Quarterly, 17, 166186.
Slater, W.H., Graves, M.H., & Piche, G.H. (1985). Effects of structural organizers on
ninth-grade students comprehension and recall of four patterns of expository test.
Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 189202.
Stauffer, R.B. (1976). Teaching reading as a thinking process. New York: Harper.
Stauffer, R.B. (1980). Directing the reading-thinking process. New York: Harper & Row.
Stein, N.I., & Glenn, C. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school
children. In R.O. Freedle (Ed.), New directions in discourse processing (Vol. 2, pp.
53120). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Taylor, B.M. (1980). Childrens memory for expository text after reading. Reading
Research Quarterly, 15, 399411.
Taylor, B.M., & Beach, R.W. (1984). The effects of text structure instruction on middlegrade students comprehension and production of expository text. Reading Research
Quarterly, 19, 134146.
Yopp, R.E. (1988). Questioning and active comprehension. Questioning Exchange, 2,
231238.
OTHER RESOURCES
241