Dezv Managerilor de Criza
Dezv Managerilor de Criza
Resources
http://adh.sagepub.com
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
Additional services and information for Advances in Developing Human Resources can be found at:
The problem and the solution. Since the events of September 11,
2001, and Hurricane Katrina during the summer of 2005, the words
crisis and disaster evoke images of families stranded on causeways
turned islands, surrounded by what little property they could carry;
houses smashed by killing winds; and skyscrapers crumbling out of the
sky. Disastrous events, such as fires, tornadoes, hurricanes, terrorist
incidents, and chemical spills, cause the loss of resources, destruction
of property, financial hardship, and death. During these events, local,
state, and federal governments commit large numbers of resources,
time, and money to mitigate the consequences of the disaster.To manage the response to these events, leaders of public safety organizations and agencies such as fire and police departments, emergency
medical services, and health-care organizations, public works departments, private industry, and nongovernmental organizations (NGOs)
descend on the disaster site.These leaders are required to make high
consequence decisions with incomplete or inaccurate information, illdefined goals, and the pressures of time and a constantly changing situation by drawing on their training and experience. This article
positions scenario planning and scenario-based training as two
cutting-edge methods for organizational leaders to understand better
their environments so as to avoid disastrous events and to put in
place efficient and effective plans for coping if disaster should strike.
Keywords:
This article was subjected to a two-tier blind review process that did not involve the authors who
are currently members of the editorial board.
Advances in Developing Human Resources Vol. 10, No. 3 June 2008 397-424
DOI: 10.1177/1523422308316456
Copyright 2008 Sage Publications
398
June 2008
Therefore, planning for a disaster is not futile; it is also not the whole solution
(Chermack, 2004; Mitroff & Alpasian, 2003). Ultimately, planning provides
more experience to build the decision-making capabilities of the individual
and the organization (Klein, 1999, 2004).
In contrast to disaster, a crisis has been defined as a decisive or critical
moment (Merriam-Websters Dictionary, 2005). Other references define crisis as an unstable or crucial time or state of affairs in which a decisive change
is impending; especially one with the distinct possibility of a highly undesirable outcome (Encyclopedia Britannica Online, 2007). Crisis management
literature provides us with diverse meanings for crisis. Reilly (1998) defined
crisis as
A situation which is harmful and disruptive (versus a turning point or an opportunity); is of
high magnitude (versus a threat or a problem); is sudden, acute, and demands a timely
response (versus decline); and is outside the firms typical operating frameworks (versus routine, such as fire to firefighters). (p. 284)
Pearson and Clair (1998) provided a more appropriate definition: An organizational crisis is a low-probability, high-impact event that threatens the viability of the organization and is characterized by ambiguity of cause, effect,
and means of resolution, as well as by a belief that decisions must be made
swiftly (p. 60). Pearson and Clairs (1998) definition provides greater flexibility in how we think about crisis because Reilly (1998) asserted that the situation is harmful and disruptive as opposed to one that threatens the viability.
399
400
June 2008
In other words, not all crises are harmful and disruptive (Boin, Kofman-Bos,
& Overdijk, 2004; Borodzicz & van Haperen, 2002). We submit that only the
magnitude of the event separates disaster from crisis. Mitroff (2002) supports
our assertion by suggesting that crises represent the breakdown of complex
systems whereas disasters represent the breakup of those systems. Mitroff
(2002) infers that normal accidents are inadvertent and should be expected
because we operate exceedingly complex technologies and systems with
management and safety systems that are faulty or simply inadequate (p. 19).
These accidents represent a shift . . . from safe to unsafe conditions (Mitroff,
2002, p. 20), whereas incidents such as the World Trade Center and the
ENRON collapse were intentional (p. 19) and represented the shift from
good to evil (p. 20). Although the metaphysical approach is dramatic, it does
provide a distinct difference in magnitude.
Conditions are similar in both a crisis and a disaster. High consequence decisions are required to be made in spite of a constantly changing situation, incomplete or inaccurate information, ill-defined goals, and the pressures of time
(Klein, 1999, 2004; Mitroff, 2002; Reilly, 1998). Those making decisions are
organizational leaders who must draw on their training and experience (Klein,
1999, 2004). Unfortunately and all too often, these decision makers are ineffective and ultimately their efforts to manage the crisis are inefficient (Klein,
1999). In this article, we address why we believe this failure in decision making occurs by reviewing two cases from emergency response disciplines.
and consider a variety of responses, aiming for the most efficient and effective
of both. Scenario planning has been defined as
a process of positing several informed, plausible and imagined alternative future environments
in which decisions about the future may be played out, for the purpose of changing current
thinking, improving decision making, enhancing human and organization learning and
improving performance. (Chermack & Lynham, 2002, p. 376)
This definition has been used with considerable success, with widely documented cases (Kahane, 1992; Wack, 1985a). SBT (operating at a more detailed
level concerning what action to take given the unfolding of a specific scenario,
and then using that plan to train responders to execute the response efficiently)
has long been in use in varying emergency response professions including
wilderness search and rescue teams, firefighters, law enforcement teams, and
many others with success (Jenvald & Morin, 2004; Lynch, 2005; Mirabella &
Macpherson, 1995; OReilly & Brandenburg, 2006; Strohschneider & Gerdes,
2004; Whitcomb, 1999).
To clarify, scenario planning explores the potential realities whereas SBT
provides the opportunity to experience interaction within the potential reality
to develop and test possible solutions to the problems presented. Each of these
tools warrants further description and clarification.
Scenario Planning
Scenario planning evolved from Herman Kahns methods to think the
unthinkable (Kahn & Wiener, 1967) in the 1950s. The scenario approach recognizes the inherent weaknesses in forecasts, and single-outcome methods
that essentially aim to predict the future. Instead, scenario planning makes use
of multiple scenarios or stories of different futures to underscore the fact that
the future is unpredictable, unstable, and inherently filled with uncertainty.
Reframed as tools for learning, scenarios are intended to shift the thinking
inside the organization (Wack, 1985a, p. 34) and help managers and decision
makers reperceive the organizational situation and consider numerous ways in
which the future might unfold.
Pierre Wack translated Kahns ideas into a corporate setting in his years as
the head of long-range planning at Royal Dutch/Shell. Wack spent most of the
1970s experimenting with and refining his methods and he credited Shells
ability to anticipate the oil shocks of the mid-1970s and 1980s to this new
technique. Eventually, he published Shells successes with scenarios in the
Harvard Business Review (Wack, 1985a, 1985b). Wacks work at Shell served
as the foundation for the modern scenario planning methods used and sold
through the Global Business Network (www.gbn.org)the organization most
commonly sought for expertise in scenario practices today.
401
402
June 2008
SBT
SBT is a training method that provides a context in which individuals and
groups can experience and interact with a possible future (Funke, 1998;
Hermann, 1997; Jenvald & Morin, 2004; Kirkley & Kirkley, 2005; Mirabella
& Macpherson, 1995; Smith, 2004; Strohschneider & Gerdes, 2004;
Whitcomb, 1999; Yusko & Goldstein, 1997). SBT presents learners with an
interactive story and places them in a specific environment that resembles the
context in which the problem would typically be encountered (i.e., work environment). The story is reactive to the actions of the participants. Through the
story and the environment, learners are presented a problem or a series of
problems at specific points in the story that flow logically throughout, called
decision points (Whitcomb, 1999). The story and corresponding decision
points are developed to stimulate specific learning outcomes (Lynch, 2005).
The decision points are arranged in a specific order and frequency to increase
or decrease the complexity of the given problem.
SBT is a versatile training method that can be used to reinforce classroom
lectures on a single topic to providing an environment to hone command and
control skills and enhance decision-making skills (Moats, Hightower, Ware, &
Wall, 2004; Texas Engineering Extension Service [TEEX], 2005; Whitcomb,
1999) to providing integrated scenarios that can be used as a comprehensive
evaluation of skills (Babicz, 2003; Lebow & Wager, 1994; Lynch, 2005;
Whitcomb, 1999). The method has been in practice for many years
Downloaded from http://adh.sagepub.com by kimbao bao on April 13, 2009
TABLE 1:
Source: Wilson and Ralston (2006, p. 25). Copyright 2006 Southwestern Educational.
(Whitcomb, 1999) and combines techniques and processes from theater, storytelling, and training, often mixed with computer-based simulations (Boin
et al., 2004; Borodzicz & van Haperen, 2002; Diehl, 1991; Funke, 1998;
Hermann, 1997; Whitcomb, 1999). However, there has not been any significant empirical study of the topic, although it is widely assumed that this is an
effective method of training (OReilly & Brandenburg, 2006). SBT is a technique that is supported by experiential learning theory (Kolb, 1984; Kolb,
Rubin, & McIntyre, 1983), transformational learning theory (Cranton, 2002;
Mezirow, 1991, 1994, 1997; Taylor, 1997) and many principles of naturalistic
decision making (Alby & Zucchermaglio, 2006; Gore, Banks, Millward, &
Kyriakidou, 2006; Klein, 1999, 2004; Klein & Calderwood, 1990; Lipshitz,
Klein, & Carroll, 2006; Montgomery et al., 2005). In addition, some SBT
applications also use computer-based simulations to provide a level of fidelity
(realism) (Boin et al., 2004; Borodzicz, 2004; Borodzicz & van Haperen,
2002; Dugdale, Pallamin, & Pavard, 2006; Helsloot, 2005; Hermann, 1997;
Issenberg, Gordon, Gordon, & Safford, 2001; Jenvald & Morin, 2004;
Mirabella & Macpherson, 1995; OReilly & Brandenburg, 2006; Smith, 2004;
Strohschneider & Gerdes, 2004; Yusko & Goldstein, 1997).
A Common SBT Process
There are many accepted ways to develop, execute, and revise SBT applications. Figure 1 shows the most common elements of the SBT methodology
Downloaded from http://adh.sagepub.com by kimbao bao on April 13, 2009
403
404
June 2008
grouped into three main stages: (a) scenario development, (b) delivery, and (c)
after action review(s) (Babicz, 2003; Borodzicz, 2004; Borodzicz & van
Haperen, 2002; Jenvald & Morin, 2004; Kirkley & Kirkley, 2005; Lynch,
2005; Mirabella & Macpherson, 1995; Whitcomb, 1999). The first stage, scenario development, begins with determining the scope and intent of the training. An ill-prepared or inappropriate scenario can result in huge transfer
distances and negative learning (Holton, Bates, & Ruona, 2000). The use of an
appropriate scenario can provide the participant a small transfer distance and
make it much easier to utilize the knowledge and skills acquired in a number
of applications. Fidelity plays a large role in lessening this distance (Holton
et al., 2000; OReilly & Brandenburg, 2006). During this first phase of scenario
development, the scope of the scenario and the most appropriate context is
determined (Noe, 2005). The learning objectives should be oriented to define
expected task performance, outline the conditions in which the outcomes
should take place, and define the criteria that describes to what level of accuracy and what level of proficiency the activity is to be performed (OReilly &
Brandenburg, 2006). The scenarios should reinforce the learning objectives
and ensure there is an opportunity for each objective to be met. To illustrate
this point, Smith (2004) wrote: Any scenario has to be grounded in the realities of the organization but sufficiently challenging to expose gaps in the
knowledge base of those managers in the various crisis teams (p. 356).
Once the objectives are determined, the second phase of scenario development incorporates a process such as scenario planning to develop the detailed
storyline and decision points of the scenario. This process is one of the most
critical in the development of SBT. The development of the scenario should be
done by a comprehensive committee that has the necessary knowledge to
develop a logical and appropriate storyline. Whitcomb (1999) stated, The
committee should be diverse enough to reflect disparate training interests and
broad enough to address all core competencies (p. 44). Whitcomb (1999)
added that the committee should be led by someone who fully understands the
training objectives and intent of the mission to make the appropriate decisions
concerning the details of the scenario and the logic that supports it.
Once the detailed storyline has been developed with the integrated decision
points, extensive and detailed care should be taken to develop scripts and
actions consistent with the storyline (Lynch, 2005). These scripts and actions
are essential to building the fidelity, or realism, of the scenarios. If these scripts
are unrealistic or flawed, they have the potential to inhibit or interrupt the
learning process (Lebow & Wager, 1994). In addition to the scenario, the context of the training is equally important to the fidelity of the training (Lynch,
2005; Whitcomb, 1999).
The aesthetic value, in essence, the look and feel of the training environment, including props, equipment, and the facility, has obvious value to the
context. However, safety and logistical concerns, such as the appropriate
number of instructors, actors, and coordination specialists, are important to
FIGURE 1:
405
406
FIGURE 2:
June 2008
2004), to bring learners to connect the dots from the old knowledge and skills
they have, and to provide learners opportunities to apply new knowledge and
skills. Carefully planned debriefings can help minimize the weaknesses caused
by problems with fidelity, whether they occur because of a poor scenario or
other unavoidable problems to manage the sense of reality (Diehl, 1991).
The debriefing is led by an instructor and provides participants the opportunity to review the actions of the course and ultimately change their worldview and individually held paradigms (Ellis, Mendel, & Nir, 2006; Lederman,
1992). These guided reflections, and ultimately SBT, rely heavily on what
Mezirow (1994, 2003; Mezirow & Associates, 1990) refers to as discourse.
Discourse refers to a special type of dialogue that involves the introspective
assessment of ones held beliefs, feelings and values (Mezirow, 2003, p. 59).
The after action review portion of SBT provides the opportunity to make good
use of dialogue as a mediating process. Lederman (1992) states that this
process is not ancillary to the educational process . . . [it] is an integral part
of any learning experience (p. 158).
407
408
June 2008
effects of the storm, the city of New Orleans and the surrounding parishes
were flooded when the levees, which were to protect the city, failed
(Townsend, 2006; U.S. Congress, 2006).
Several events surrounding Hurricane Katrina provide powerful and tragic
examples of failures in the management of the crisis. However, a lack of willingness to entertain scenarios that were circulated more than a year before
Hurricane Katrinas landfall provides a clear example of failure to consider the
possible over the likely. In July 2004, federal, state, and local emergency planners and responders were confronted with a planning exercise that used a hurricane scenario called Hurricane PAM. The scenario had an eerie likeness to
Hurricane Katrina which followed some 13 months later (U.S. Congress,
2006). The report submitted to Congress detailing the events before, during,
and after Hurricane Katrina provide a concise description of the Hurricane
PAM scenario (U.S. Congress, 2006).
The Hurricane Pam scenario focused on 13 parishes in southeast LouisianaAscension,
Assumption, Jefferson, Lafourche, Orleans, Plaquemines, St. Bernard, St. Charles, St. James,
St. John, St. Tammany, Tangipahoa, and Terrebonne. Representatives from outside the primary
parishes, including officials from Mississippis Emergency Management Agency (EMA), participated because hurricane evacuation and sheltering involve communities throughout
Louisiana and into Arkansas, Mississippi, and Texas. The Hurricane Pam exercise scenario
was prescient. The virtual storm brought sustained winds of 120 mph, up to 20 inches of rain
in parts of Southeast Louisiana, and storm surges that topped the levees and flooded the New
Orleans area. The exercise assumed that:
300,000 people would not evacuate in advance;
500,000 to 600,000 buildings would be destroyed;
Phone and sewer services would be knocked out and chemical plants would be flooded;
97 percent of all communications would be down;
About 175,000 people would be injured, 200,000 would become sick, and more than 60,000
would be killed;
About 1,000 shelters would be needed for evacuees;
Boats and helicopters would be needed for thousands of rescues because many residents would
be stranded by floodwaters;
A catastrophic flood would leave swaths of southeast Louisiana uninhabitable for more than a
year. (U.S. Congress, 2006, p. 81)
When confronted with this scenario, emergency planners denied the vision
and thought it too extreme and then failed to implement many of the recommendations and lessons learned as a result of the exercise (Mitroff, 2005a;
Phillips, 2006; U.S. Congress, 2006). Scenario planning was used in this situation however; the reluctance of decision makers to seriously entertain the
409
410
June 2008
possibility of a disaster in New Orleans prevented them from taking the proper
precautionary measures. This was a case of scenario planning in which decision makers were not compelled by the content of PAM scenario and represents both a lack of vision (in terms of a willingness to explore catastrophe as
a real possibility) and denial.
However, these facts also prompt us to ask whether learning actually
occurred (Phillips, 2006). Perhaps, this is an example in which no learning
occurred. Or, was the knowledge gained, the learning, forgotten? We propose
that the weakness in crisis management planning is a lack of vision that is adequate for the unexpected, unanticipated, and abnormal. Mitroff (2005a) supported our assertion:
Most natural disasters, such as earthquakes, are neither predictable nor preventable. However,
many, such as hurricanes and tsunamis, are somewhat predictable even if they are not fully
preventable. . . . In the case of Katrina, it was thought that the probability of a hurricane of its
magnitude occurring was only 0.03. . . . To his credit, Homeland Security Secretary Michael
Chertoff wants to replace traditional RM [risk management] by approaches that focus on the
magnitude of the consequences only. If we dont, then we will continue to downplay high consequence-low probability crises like Katrina, not to mention atrocities like 9/11. (Pp. 2-3)
thundercloud to the ground (p. 297). In other words, had the vision of such
an attack been understood, the attacks could have possibly been avoided.
Unfortunately, the planning failures that would have prevented or lessened
the impact of September 11 were not limited to the intelligence community. The
engineering and emergency response communities failed to envision and consequently plan for the possibility that the towers of the World Trade Center would
collapse (Greenhom & OMara, Incorporated, 2002; 9/11 Commission, 2004).
Emergency response planners failed to imagine the consequences if the towers,
each more than 1,000 ft into the New York skyline, were to collapse. The consequences of this lack of vision allowed emergency response vehicles and personnel as well as command and control centers to locate in the collapse zone. When
the buildings did fail, they literally crushed the incident response efforts.
A second failure of the planning effort existed in the inability to conceive of
an event that would overwhelm the considerable resources of New York City
(Hoffman, 2002; 9/11 Commission, 2004). Prior to September 11, 2001, there
had been multiple large-scale incidents, such as the 1993 World Trade Center
bombing; however, the complexity and magnitude of the 2001 attack made all
previous incidents pale in comparison. Because the emergency response planners failed to envision any event that would overwhelm New York, the emergency responders and city government officials were lulled into a false sense of
security (Hoffman, 2002) allowing the various major response agencies to
remain virtually autonomous (9/11 Commission, 2004). In addition, many policy decisions were made in an attempt to resolve conflicts instead of improve
the functionality of the holistic response effort (9/11 Commission, 2004). The
result of this inability to vision this event was an uncoordinated, inefficient
response effort (McKinsey & Company, 2002; 9/11 Commission, 2004).
Lack of abilityproblem-solving failures. In the current business environment,
executives and crises planners are subject to the same failures as those in emergency response organizations (Klein, 1999; Reilly, 1998). As Smith (2004)
wrote, The manager was, after all, employed because of his or her demonstrable skills in steady-state management (p. 347). The events surrounding
Hurricane Katrina provide another example of a failure of crisis management.
However, this is not a breakdown of imagination; it is a collapse of action
decision making. The failure to make timely decisions about a myriad of topics
resulted in exacerbating the Hurricane Katrina disaster to a magnitude of the
highest proportions (Townsend, 2006; U.S. Congress, 2006). The most agonizing example of this breakdown concerns the decision, or rather lack thereof, to
evacuate New Orleans and the surrounding parishes (U.S. Congress, 2006).
More than 56 hr before landfall, the National Weather Service (NWS) and
National Hurricane Center had provided accurate and timely predictions of the
storms path (NWS, 2006; Reston, 2005; U.S. Congress, 2006). Emergency
plans, recently revised since Hurricane Ivan in 2005, also provided prompts for
evacuation decisions (U.S. Congress, 2006). The information proved useful for
411
412
June 2008
413
414
June 2008
the scenarios once they have been developed. In fact, so much focus has been
placed on scenario development that no documented process for using scenarios to examine strategy is available. Some reference is made to a process called
windtunnelling, but again a detailed description is missing. We propose in
this article that once a set of scenarios are developed, SBT may be a valuable
method for translating new thinking gleaned from scenario development into
action plans for responding as situations unfold.
SBT Strategies
There is no doubt that the recent events, such as September 11, 2001, and
Hurricane Katrina, have heightened the organizations awareness to increase
their crisis management capability (Boin et al., 2004). Unfortunately, crisis
management planning and training are happenstance and not institutionalized
(Mitroff, 2005a). Certain federal initiatives, such as the National Incident
Management System (U.S. Department of Homeland Security [DHS], 2004)
require that some organizations included in critical infrastructure or business
sectors must be trained in the incident command system (Homeland Security
Act of 2002, 2002). In addition, some federal regulations, such as the EPCRA
require organizations to have emergency operations plans and train select
employees in emergency response procedures if they meet certain criteria,
such as storing or manufacturing a specified quantity of hazardous substances.
However, this requirement affects a relatively small number of organizations.
In most organizations that are not subject to federal requirements, crisis
management training is typically relegated to annual employee in-service sessions or is not conducted. One reason may be that crisis management is often
viewed as a drain on profits (Klein, 1999, p. 238). A search of the Internet
shows that a significant amount of the available crisis management training is
typically oriented toward emergency situations (i.e., fire, bomb threats, active
shooters, etc.). Many consulting firms do provide customized crisis management training designed to meet the customers needs. However, these programs
are usually expensive (Sachs, 2007).
Emergency response organizations have a greater selection of training strategies; however, it is not optimal. Larger cities, such as New York, Dallas, Los
Angeles, and Houston have their own public safety training institutions.
Typically, the training offered by these institutions typically meets or exceeds
national and industry standards. However, training is developed based on the
needs of the organization and is typically limited to members of the organization.
State agencies, such as the TEEX, the Maryland Fire Rescue Institute (MFRI),
and the Kentucky Community and Technical College System (KCTCS), are designated by state legislation as state public safetyoriented training agencies. Federal departments, such as the U.S. DHS and the Department of
Transportation provide training and training funds. The training may be delivered
415
416
June 2008
417
418
June 2008
Conclusions
This article has described two cutting-edge methods for managing and
avoiding crisis. The thesis of this article was that these methods were not properly used in two recent cases of crisis. Our analysis has suggested where the
use of these methods went wrong and has provided suggestions and key tips
for improving the use of these methods in the future. Two key pitfalls were
identified in both cases: (a) lack of vision and (b) lack of action once crisis
began to unfold. The implications of the suggested solutions to these key pitfalls have been discussed in the context of HRD research, theory, and practice.
Planning can be considered one of the most difficult and ambiguous organizational activities (Mintzberg et al., 1998), and the level of uncertainty for
decision makers to account for has never been so high. There will never be a
method that is 100% reliable in terms of its ability to steer organizations away
from crisis (Mitroff, 2005a). However, scenario planning and SBT are the best
of what can be offered to address these serious deficiencies. Study is required
to understand how and why these practice-born techniques work. In addition,
study should also look for ways to transfer these techniques to organizations.
Mitroff (2005a) offered this warning to all organizations:
These techniques can initiate change and equip crisis managers to prepare
and respond to the next crisis.
References
Alby, F., & Zucchermaglio, C. (2006). Afterwards we can understand what went
wrong, but now lets fix it: How situated work practices shape group decision making. Organization Studies, 27(7), 943-966.
Babicz, G. (2003). Simulation: Making it real. Quality, 42(4), 90-91.
Bartlett, C., & Ghoshal, S. (2002). Building competitive advantage through people.
MIT Sloan Management Review, 43(2), 34-41.
Bloom, M. J., & Menefee, M. K. (1994). Scenario planning and contingency planning.
Productivity and Management Review, 17(2), 223-230.
Boin, A., Kofman-Bos, C., & Overdijk, W. (2004). Crisis simulations: Exploring
tomorrows vulnerabilities and threats. Simulation & Gaming, 35(3), 378-393.
Borodzicz, E. (2004). The missing ingredient is the value of flexibility. Simulation &
Gaming, 35(5), 414-426.
Borodzicz, E., & van Haperen, K. (2002). Individual and group learning in crisis simulations. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management, 10(3), 139-147.
Chermack, T. J. (2004). Improving decision-making with scenario planning. Futures,
36(3), 295-309.
Chermack, T. J. (2008). Exploring the impact of scenario planning on participant decision-making styles. In T. J. Chermack, J. Storberg-Walker, & C. Graham (Eds.),
Academy of Human Resource Development Conference Proceedings (pp. 799-807).
Bowling Green, OH: Academy of Human Resource Development.
Chermack, T. J., & Lynham, S. A. (2002). Definitions and outcome variables of scenario planning. Human Resource Development Review, 1(3), 366-383.
Chermack, T. J., Lynham, S. A., & Ruona, W. E. A. (2001, Summer). A review of scenario planning literature. Futures Research Quarterly, 7-31.
Chermack, T. J., Lynham, S. A., & van der Merwe, L. (2006). Exploring the relationship between scenario planning and perceptions of learning organization characteristics. Futures, 38(7), 767-777.
Chermack, T. J., & van der Merwe, L. (2003). The role of constructivist learning in scenario planning. Futures, 35(5), 445-460.
Chermack, T. J., van der Merwe, L., & Lynham, S. A. (2007). Exploring the relationship between scenario planning and perceptions of strategic conversation quality.
Technological Forecasting and Social Change 74(3), 379-390.
Cranton, P. (2002). Teaching for transformation. New Directions for Adult and
Continuing Education, 93, 63-71.
419
420
June 2008
Darcy, M., OHanlong, M., Orszag, P., Shapiro, J., & Steinberg, J. (2006). Protecting
the homeland. New York: Brookings Institution Press.
Davis, P. (2002, July 24, Wednesday). Report calls response at Pentagon successful.
Washington Post, p. B.01.
de Geus, A. (1988). Planning as learning. Harvard Business Review, 66(2), 70-74.
de Geus, A. P. (1999). The living company. New York: John Wiley.
Diehl, B. J. (1991). CRISIS: A process evaluation. Simulation & Gaming, 22(3), 293-307.
Dooley, L. M. (2002). Case study research and theory building. Advances in
Developing Human Resources, 4(3), 335-354.
Dugdale, J., Pallamin, N., & Pavard, B. (2006). An assessment of a mixed reality environment: Toward an ethnomethodological approach. Simulation & Gaming, 37(2),
226-244.
Ellis, S., Mendel, R., & Nir, M. (2006). Learning from successful and failed experience: The moderating role of kind of after-event review. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 91(3), 669-680.
Emergency Planning and Community Right-to-Know Act (EPCRA), 42 U.S.C. 9601
et seq. (1986).
Encyclopedia Britannica online. (2007). Retrieved February 3, 2007, from http://www
.britannica.com/dictionary?hdwd=crisis&book=Dictionary&jump=crisis&list=crisis%3D240388%3Bidentity+crisis%3D524377%3Bmidlife+crisis%3D673836a
Freidman, T. (2005). The world is flat. New York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux.
Funke, J. (1998). Computer-based testing and training with scenarios from complex
problem-solving research: Advantages and disadvantages. International Journal of
Selection and Assessment, 6(2), 90-96.
Gore, J., Banks, A., Millward, L., & Kyriakidou, O. (2006). Naturalistic decision making
and organizations: Reviewing pragmatic science. Organization Studies, 27(7), 925-942.
Greenhom & OMara, Incorporated. (2002). World Trade Center building performance
study: Data collection, preliminary observations and recommendations. Washington,
DC: FEMA.
Helsloot, I. (2005). Bordering on reality: Findings on the Bonfire crisis management
simulation. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management, 13(4), 159-169.
Hermann, M. G. (1997). In conclusion: The multiple pay-offs of crisis simulations.
Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management, 5(4), 241-243.
Hoffman, B. (2002). Lessons of 9/11: Testimony before the United States Joint
September 11, 2001 Inquiry Staff of the House and Senate Select Committees on
Intelligence on October 8, 2002. Arlington, VA: Rand Corporation. (Rand
Corporation Document No. CT-201)
Holton, E. F., Bates, R. A., & Ruona, W. E. A. (2000). Development of a generalized
learning transfer system inventory. Human Resource Development Quarterly, 11(4),
333-360.
The Homeland Security Act of 2002, 107-296 6 U.S.C. 101 (2002).
Issenberg, S. B., Gordon, M. S., Gordon, D. L., & Safford, R. E. (2001). Simulation
and new learning technology. Medical Teacher, 23(1), 16-23.
Jenvald, J., & Morin, M. (2004). Simulation-supported live training for emergency
response in hazardous environment. Simulation & Gaming, 35(3), 363-377.
Kahane, A. (1992). The Mont Fleur scenarios: What will South Africa be like in the
year 2002? (Supplement to the Weekly Mail and The Guardian Weekly). Bellville,
South Africa: Global Business Network.
Kahn, H., & Wiener, A. J. (1967). The next thirty-three years: A framework for speculation. Daedalus, 96(3), 705-732.
Kirkley, S. E., & Kirkley, J. R. (2005). Creating next generation blended learning environments using mixed reality, video games and simulations. TechTrends: Linking
Research and Practice to Improve Learning, 49(3), 42-89.
Klein, G. A. (1999). Sources of power: How people make decisions. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Klein, G. A. (2004). The power of intuition: How to use your gut feelings to make
better decisions at work. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.
Klein, G. A., & Calderwood, R. (1990). Investigations of naturalistic decision making
and the recognition-primed decision model (No. KATR8802Z, ARIRN9059,
ADA2260065). Alexandria, VA: Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and
Social Sciences.
Kolb, D. A. (1984). Experiential learning: Experience as the source of learning and
development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Kolb, D. A., Rubin, I. M., & McIntyre, J. M. (1983). Organizational psychology: An
experiential approach to organizational behavior (4th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice Hall.
Korte, R. F., & Chermack, T. J. (2007). Changing organizational culture: Using scenario
planning to change the collective mind of the organization. Futures, 39(2), 645-656.
Lebow, D. G., & Wager, W. W. (1994). Authentic activity as a model for appropriate
learning activity: Implications for design of computer-based simulations. Paper presented at the 1994 National Convention of the Association for Educational
Communications and Technology, Nashville, TN.
Lederman, L. C. (1992). Debriefing: Toward a systematic assessment of theory and
practice. Simulation & Gaming, 23(2),145-160.
Lindell, M. K., & Perry, R. W. (1992). Behavioral foundations of community emergency planning. Washington, DC: Hemisphere.
Lindell, M. K., Prater, C. S., & Perry, R. W. (2007). Introduction to emergency management. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.
Lipshitz, R., Klein, G., & Carroll, J. S. (2006). Introduction to the special issue.
Naturalistic decision making and organizational decision making: Exploring the
intersections. Organization Studies, 27(7), 917-923.
Lynch, M. D. (2005). Developing a scenario-based training program. FBI Law
Enforcement Bulletin, 74(10), 1-8.
Lynham, S. A., & Cunningham, P. W. (2006). HRD in transitioning societies in the
developing world: A model and synthesis of perspectives. Advances in Developing
Human Resources, 8(1), 116-135.
McKinsey & Company. (2002). Increasing FDNYs preparedness. New York: New
York City Fire Department.
McLean, G. N. (2004). National human resource development: What in the world is it?
Advances in Developing Human Resources, 6(3), 269-275.
Merriam-Websters dictionary (Vol. 1). (2005). Springfield, MA: Merriam-Webster.
Mezirow, J. (1991). Transformative dimensions of adult learning. San Francisco:
Jossey-Bass.
Mezirow, J. (1994). Understanding transformation theory. Adult Education Quarterly,
44(4), 222-232.
421
422
June 2008
Robert T. Stafford Disaster Relief and Emergency Assistance Act, 100-707 42 U.S.C.
5121-5207 (2007).
Roberts, P. S. (2006). FEMA after Katrina. Policy Review, 137, 15-33.
Sachs, A. (2007, April 30). The crisis doctors. Time Pacific South, 16, pp. 53-54.
Schoemaker, P. (1995). Scenario planning: A tool for strategic thinking. Sloan
Management Review, 36(2), 25-40.
Schwartz, P. (1991). The art of the long view. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.
Simon, H. A. (1990). Invariants of human behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 41,
1-19.
Smith, D. (2004). For whom the bell tolls: Imagining accidents and the development of
crisis simulation in organizations. Simulation & Gaming, 35(3), 347-362.
Soy, S. K. (1996). The case study as a research method [Electronic version]. University
of Texas, Graduate School of Library and Information Science web site. Retrieved
December 1, 2001, from http://www.gslis.utexas.edu/~ssoy/usesusers/l391d1b.htm
Stake, R. E. (1994). Case studies. In N. K. Denzin & Y. E. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook
of qualitative research (pp. 236-246). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Strohschneider, S., & Gerdes, J. (2004). MS ANTWERPEN: Emergency management
training for low-risk environments. Simulation & Gaming, 36(3), 394-413.
Swanson, R. A. (2007). Analysis for improving performance: Tools for diagnosing
organizations and documenting workplace expertise (2nd ed.). San Francisco:
Berrett-Koehler.
Taylor, E. W. (1997). Building upon the theoretical debate: A critical review of the
empirical studies of Mezirows transformative learning theory. Adult Education
Quarterly, 48(1), 34-59.
Texas Engineering Extension Service. (2005). E IMUC course information. Retrieved
March 4, 2008, from http://www.teex.com/teex-third.cfm?area=NERRTC&templateid=176
Titan Systems. (2002). Arlington County after action report on the response to the
September 11 terrorist attack on the Pentagon. Washington, DC: Department of Justice.
Townsend, F. F. (2006). The federal response to Hurricane Katrina: Lessons learned.
Washington, DC: The White House.
U.S. Congress. (2006). A failure of initiative: Final report of the select bipartisan committee to investigate the preparation for and response to Hurricane Katrina.
Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
U.S. Department of Homeland Security. (2004). National incident management system.
Washington, DC: Author.
van der Heijden, K. (2002). Sixth sense: Accelerating organizational learning with scenarios. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Wack, P. (1985a). Scenarios: Shooting the rapids. Harvard Business Review, 63(5),
139-150.
Wack, P. (1985b). Scenarios: Uncharted waters ahead. Harvard Business Review, 63(5),
73-89.
Wagner, C. G. (2005). Proactive crisis management. The Futurist, 39(2), 6-6.
Walton, J. (1999). Strategic human resource development. London: Financial Times Hall.
Whitcomb, C. (1999). Scenario-based training at the F.B.I. Training and Development,
53(6), 42-46.
Wilson, I., & Ralston, W. (2006). Scenario planning handbook: Developing strategies
in uncertain times. Belmont, CA: South-Western Educational.
423
424
June 2008
Yin, R. K. (1981). The case study crisis: Some answers. Administrative Science
Quarterly, 26, 58-65.
Yin, R. K. (1994). Case study research: Design and methods (2nd ed., Vol. 5).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Yorks, L. (2004). Toward a political economy model for comparative analysis of the
role of strategic human resource development leadership. Human Resource
Development Review, 3(3), 198-208.
Yusko, K. P., & Goldstein, H. W. (1997). Selecting and developing crisis leaders using
competency-based simulations. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management,
5(4), 216-223.
Jason B. Moats is a training manager with the Texas Engineering Extension Service
(TEEX). He manages, designs, and develops scenarios and training programs to train
emergency responders in incident management. He is also a student in Texas A&Ms
Human Resources Development doctoral program. His areas of research interest
include scenario-based training, leadership development, and workforce development.
His first book Agroterrorism: A Guide for First Responders was released in April 2007.
Thomas J. Chermack is an assistant professor at the Colorado State University in the
Organizational Performance and Change Program. His research focuses on the use of
scenario planning in organizations and theory-building methods in applied disciplines.
He is the founder and managing partner of Chermack Scenarios, a consulting organization affiliated with the Centre for Innovative Leadership. His research has appeared
in scholarly publications such as Futures, Futures Research Quarterly, and The
Academy of Strategic Management Journal, among others.
Larry M. Dooley is an associate professor in the Human Resource Development
Program in the Department of Educational Administration and Human Resource
Development at Texas A&M University. He is also a visiting professor both in the School
of Business and Law at Napier University, Edinburgh, Scotland, and in the Center for
Human Resource Management Research at Peking University, Beijing, China. He is past
president of the board of the Academy of Human Resource Development. In addition he
serves on the board of the Academy of Human Resource Development Foundation and
as the International Chief Operating Officer for the Academy of Human Resource
Development. His research interests include e-learning and international human resource
development.
This refereed journal article is part of an entire issue on organizational crisis management. For more information or to read other articles in the issue, see Hutchins, H. M., &
Wang, J. (2008). Crisis management in organizations: The role of human resource development [Special issue]. Advances in Developing Human Resources, 10(3).