The Relationship of Achievement Motivation To Entrepreneurial Behavior: A Meta-Analysis
The Relationship of Achievement Motivation To Entrepreneurial Behavior: A Meta-Analysis
DigitalCommons@ILR
Articles and Chapters
ILR Collection
2004
Paul J. Hanges
University of Maryland
Edwin A. Locke
University of Maryland
Entrepreneurship is a major factor in the national economy; thus, it is important to understand the
motivational characteristics spurring people to become entrepreneurs and why some are more successful than
others. In this study, we conducted a meta-analysis of the relationship between achievement motivation and
variables associated with entrepreneurial behavior. We found that achievement motivation was significantly
correlated with both choice of an entrepreneurial career and entrepreneurial performance. Further, we found
that both projective and self-report measures of achievement motivation were valid. Finally, known group
studies yielded a higher validity coefficient than did individual difference studies.
Keywords
Entrepreneurial and Small Business Operations | Human Resources Management | Labor Relations
Comments
Suggested Citation
Collins, C. J., Hanges, P. J., & Locke, E. A. (2004). The relationship of achievement motivation to entrepreneurial
behavior: A meta-analysis [Electronic version]. Retrieved [insert date], from Cornell University, ILR School
site: http://digitalcommons.ilr.cornell.edu/articles/x
Required Publisher Statement
Taylor & Francis. Reprinted with permission. All rights reserved. Final version published as: Collins, C. J.,
Hanges, P. J., & Locke, E. A. (2004). The relationship of achievement motivation to entrepreneurial behavior:
A meta-analysis. Human Performance, 17(1), 95-117.
individual characteristics, there are still many unanswered questions regarding the role
that motivation and personal characteristics have on entrepreneurial activity (Shane &
Venkataraman, 2000). Therefore, in this article, we seek to better understand how one
important motivating factor, need for achievement, is related to both the choice of
entrepreneurship as a career and performance in entrepreneurial roles.
The concept of need for achievement (nAch) was formulated in the 1950s
(McClelland, Clark, Roby, & Atkinson 1958). McClelland and his colleagues argued that
high-nAch people are more likely than low-nAch people to engage in energetic and
innovative activities that require planning for the future and entail an individuals
responsibility for task outcomes. McClelland (1961) argued that high-nAch people should
also prefer tasks that involve skill and effort, provide clear performance feedback, and
were of moderate challenge or risk. He also argued that entrepreneurial positions have
more of these characteristics than other types of positions. Hollands (1985) vocation
choice model suggests that individuals will be most attracted to careers that offer the
environmental characteristics that match their personality. Holland (1985) also argued
that performance and career satisfaction are higher when there is a good fit between work
environment characteristics and personality. Therefore, as was suggested by McClelland
(1961), it seems likely that individuals high in nAch should be attracted to and perform
well in entrepreneurial jobs.
Although there is a considerable body of literature in the area of achievement,
motivation and entrepreneurship, there are important issues that have yet to be addressed.
For example, Johnson (1990) conducted a traditional review of this literature and
concluded that there is evidence of a relationship between achievement motivation and
entrepreneurial activity. However, a close examination of Johnsons review raises some
questions about the strength of this conclusion. First, because of the narrative nature of this
review, the magnitude of the relationship between achievement motivation and
entrepreneurial activity is unclear. In fact, many of the studies reviewed by Johnson
showed non-significant relationships between achievement motivation and
entrepreneurial activity. Johnsons review also indicated that there was considerable
variance in the results across studies, suggesting that there may be moderators of the
achievement-motivationentrepreneurial activity relationship. In contrast to traditional
reviews, meta-analysis has the potential to address some of these issues by quantitatively
combining the results of individual studies to estimate the magnitude of the relationship
(Hunter & Schmidt, 1990) and by allowing formal tests of potential moderators of these
relationships.
This study has three objectives. First, we assess the degree of support for
McClellands claim by conducting a meta-analytic review of the achievement motivation
and entrepreneurship research that is sensitive to level of analysis issues. Second, we
identify and test several potential moderators of the relation between achievement
motivation and entrepreneurial action. Third, we compare the validity of achievement
motivation measures as a function of measurement methodology (e.g., projective measure
vs. self-report questionnaire measures). We discuss the implications of our findings for
future research and for organizations and institutions interested entrepreneurship.
levels of analysis in their reviews. However, inappropriate conclusions can be drawn when
concepts developed at one level of analysis are tested at a different level of analysis (Ostroff
& Harrison, 1999). Thus, in our analysis we separated these two levels. 1
Since Spangler (1992) conducted a previous meta-analysis of achievement
motivation studies, some key differences between his study and ours need to be
mentioned. First, in Spanglers (1992) study, only 5 out of 115 or 4% of the studies he used
involved entrepreneurs or entrepreneurial activity. In addition, Spanglers studies included
a very heterogeneous array of dependent variables, including IQ test scores, attitudes,
sensation seeking, enrollment in an academic honors program, performance on a ring toss
game, GPA, and industrial output. This meta-analysis was restricted to only studies
involving entrepreneurs and entrepreneurial performance. When testing the predictive
validity of achievement motivation, at least as conceived by McClelland (1961), it seems
best to restrict validity analysis to the domain where it is asserted to be most directly
applicable. For example, McClelland (1965) explicitly argued that college performance was
not an appropriate domain for achievement motivation (1965). Finally, Spangler did not
test the homogeneity of effect size assumption (Hedges & Olkin, 1985). We included this
test, because failure to assess this assumption can lead to the aggregation of effect sizes
from multiple populations resulting in an average effect size estimate that does not
generalize to any particular population (Hedges & Olkin, 1985).
By sticking closely to the most appropriate theoretical domain, 2 using different
measures of achievement motivation, conducting a test of homogeneity of effects, and
controlling for differences between known group and individual studies, we believe that
this meta-analysis has the best chance of accurately identifying the relationship between
achievement motivation and performance.
Definitions of Entrepreneur
Ostroff and Harrison (1999) have recently argued that it is critical to consider the
levels of analysis of the original empirical studies when conducting meta-analyses. It is
inappropriate to pool studies from multiple levels of analysis because the meta-analytically
derived average correlation will not be representative of any population parameter
(Ostroff & Harrison, 1999). This is a particular problem in our review because, as
previously indicated, researchers have examined the effects of achievement motivation
differences between both individuals and known groups. Further, the complex nature of
achievement motivation makes it important to examine potential differences between
known group and individual studies. Achievement motivation has been conceived of as a
complex set of values associated with entrepreneurial activity (McClelland, 1961).
However, though values can affect action, they are too abstract by themselves to say what a
person would do in a specific situation. For example, Eisenstadt (1963) argued that the
social and organizational setting in which people exist will affect the extent to which people
exhibiting high levels of achievement motivation will act in an entrepreneurial manner.
Consistent with this perspective, Holland (1985) and Schneider (1987) suggested
that when individuals are free to select themselves into certain groups (e.g., jobs,
occupations, careers), their attraction and willingness to remain in certain jobs and careers
is a function of their personality, attitudes, and values. Further, the homogeneity among
people who remain in a particular job or career tend to increase as people with different
personality, attitudes, and values leave these jobs or careers (Schneider, 1987). Thus, effect
sizes for analyses conducted on known groups should be enhanced because the
homogenization that occurs as a result of individuals staying in their careers that fit their
personality and leaving careers that are a mismatch will increases the size of the difference
between groups over time (e.g., entrepreneurs vs. non-entrepreneurs). 3
H2. In relation to levels of analysis, achievement motivation will have a
stronger association with entrepreneurial activity in known group studies
than in individual difference studies. 4
Holland (1985), in his theory of vocational choice, also suggested that the
interaction of work environment and personality may affect performance in a career.
Specifically, he argued that higher levels of fit between the personality and work
Though achievement motivation may significantly predict both career choice and
performance, it is not necessarily the case that the effect sizes for the two distinct outcomes
would be the same. McClelland (1965, 1972) suggested that nAch would be more likely to
predict career choice than performance in an entrepreneurial position. This argument
seems logical on statistical grounds alone. If it is true that those high in achievement
motivation will be more likely to choose an entrepreneurial career, then there should be a
restriction in the range of achievement motivation for those in entrepreneurial jobs. Thus,
achievement motivation should have less power to predict which entrepreneurs would be
successful and which would not be successful. In a review of achievement motivation,
Johnson (1990) found that 20 of 23 studies showed a significant relationship between
achievement motivation and entry into entrepreneurial positions. However, none of the 23
studies found significant ties between achievement motivation and performance of
entrepreneurs. As discussed earlier, Johnson did not use any meta-analytic technique to
statistically test the combined results of these studies, and we are still not clear on whether
achievement motivation is related to both career choice and performance. Thus, we predict
that
H5. Achievement motivation will predict entrepreneurial choice better than
entrepreneurial performance.
As we noted earlier, the achievement motivation literature suggests that there are
distinct differences between the types of tests used to measure achievement motivation. In
his formulation of the concept of nAch, McClelland (1961, 1972) contended that the
underlying causes of nAch are rooted in the subconscious and were developed during early
childhood. Projective tests are the only method to accurately assess nAch, because it is a
subconscious trait (McClelland, 1972). According to McClelland, questionnaire methods are
inappropriate for measuring nAch, because they measure an individuals conscious value
for achievement (McClelland, 1972). Therefore, McClelland (1980) argued that the TAT will
be a better predictor of entrepreneurial behavior than questionnaire measures.
Though it might be expected that the TAT would perform worse because projective
tests tend to have much lower reliability than questionnaire measures, there is some
support for McClellands hypothesis. For example, Spangler (1992) found that the TAT was
a better predictor of outcome criterion than were questionnaires. However, as discussed
earlier, Spangler did not address the issue of achievement motivation and
entrepreneurship. Thus, it is still a question whether the TAT predicts performance or
distinguishes between entrepreneurs and other types of individuals (i.e., predicts career
choice) better than other types of measures.
This is an especially important question to answer when considering questions
regarding the validity and reliability of the TAT that we raised earlier. For example,
researchers and practitioners might consider using questionnaire tests that have
demonstrated higher reliability if such tests are valid predictors and the TAT is not.
Similarly, researchers might consider other projective tests (besides the TAT) if those tests
have shown equal validity and/or higher reliability. The Miner Sentence Completion Scale
is one projective test that has been used to predict entrepreneurial performance and has
shown higher reliability than the TAT (Smith & Miner, 1984). To see if the TAT is a better
predictor than the other types of measures of achievement motivation, we explored the
difference in predictive power of the different measures of achievement motivation (i.e.,
TAT, questionnaire measures, and the Miner Sentence Completion Scale) in our metaanalysis. Based on the arguments of McClelland and prior reviews of this literature, we
predict that
Method
H6. The strength of the relationship between the TAT measure of nAch and
(a) occupational choice and (b) entrepreneurial performance will be
significantly higher than for other projective measures or questionnaire
measures of achievement motivation.
types of questionnaire-based measures. We kept only those studies that conformed to the
following three criteria. First, the study had to involve findings linking entrepreneurs and
achievement motivation. Second, the study needed to provide either an effect size or
sufficient statistical information to calculate an effect size. Finally, the study needed to
provide sufficient background information and description of the variables of interest so
that the study could be coded according to one or more of the potential moderators
identified earlier.
Based on these criteria a total of six studies were dropped from further analysis.
Four of these studies were dropped because the authors failed to provide standard
deviations with reported mean scores (i.e., failed to meet the second criterion). Two other
studies were dropped when we realized that the author had reported the same findings in
multiple journals. For example, Singh reported the same findings in two studies (Ray &
Singh, 1980; Singh, 1979) so we retained only one of the studies in our sample. All six of the
dropped studies were from the TAT set of studies. The final set used for analysis comprised
41 studies.
Coding of Studies
Coding of studies was based on the potential moderators discussed earlier. Because
the studies were all field studies and did not involve any direct manipulations, the
moderators clearly fell into these very broad categories (see Table 1 for examples of
coding). The first step in coding the studies was to develop a coding manual based on
several potential mediating variables (discussed later). Next, the first author and three
research assistants independently coded the research articles. The first author and two of
the three research assistants agreed on the coding for all of the studies included in the
meta-analysis. The first author and the third research assistant disagreed on coding for two
studies, but these discrepancies were resolved after further discussion.
A sample of coded studies can be found in Table 1. First, we coded studies based on
grouping of participants (individual or known group). Studies were coded as individual
level studies when researchers examined mean differences based on individuals responses
and coded as known group studies when researchers examined mean differences on some
dependent variable among these groups. Second, we coded studies based on dependent
variable (performance or career pattern). Studies were coded as performance when the
dependent variable included some form of entrepreneurial performance and as career
choice when the studied compared mean differences in achievement motivation scores
between entrepreneurs and some other career (e.g., scientist, manager). Note that
performance measures varied across studies from secondary source measures (e.g., sales
growth, stock growth) to scaled measures of performance. Type of performance measure
was not a statistically significant moderator; therefore, all studies with performance as a
dependent variable were included together in tests of hypotheses.
Third, we coded studies based on type of career pattern comparison (entrepreneur vs.
manager, non-founder or entrepreneur vs. all other types of professions). Fourth, we coded
studies based on the type of test used (TAT, MSCSt [Miner Sentence Completion ScaleForm t], or questionnaire). Finally, we coded studies based on the definition of
entrepreneurship. We coded studies as following a broad definition if they included both
founders and non-founding managers and coded them as following a narrow definition
when they included only founders.
Several studies provided tests on multiple independent groups, used more than one
type of achievement motivation test, or looked at more than one outcome. These
independent sets of data were treated as individual data points if they could not be
meaningfully collapsed. For example, Tracy, Locke, and Renard (1997) used both the TAT
and the MSCSt to measure achievement motivation and provided effect sizes for both
tests. We felt that it was legitimate to treat these data points individually to test hypotheses
about potential moderators. The consequence of including the multiple coefficients from a
single study is that we decrease our ability to find moderators (i.e., it is a conservative bias
for the test of moderators). However, including multiple correlations from the same single
study may result in a liberal bias in the significance test for the general achievement
motivationactivity relationship. 5 To guard against the liberal bias, we replicated our
results for the overall achievement motivationactivity relationship using only one average
correlation per study. The results of this analysis did not significantly differ from our
analyses with multiple correlations per study in terms of effect sizes or conclusions
regarding hypotheses. Therefore, we have only reported the findings from the larger
sample in which we included multiple coefficients from the single studies.
When accounting for studies with multiple predictors or multiple criteria, we had 69
individual data points (observed relationships between one achievement motivation
measure and one outcome variable) and a total sample size of 9,032 participants. The TAT
data included 24 data points and a total sample size of 2,711 participants. The MSCSt data
were made up of 15 data points and a total sample size of 1,166. For the questionnaire data,
there was a total of 30 data points and a total sample size of 5,155 participants.
Transformation of Raw Data
The studies in our sample reported results with a wide range of statistics, including
F tests, t tests, means and standard deviations, and chi-square statistics. The raw statistics
were all converted to correlations using the formulas provided by Hunter and Schmidt
(1990) and Wolf (1986). Several studies reported their findings in multiple ways. When
this occurred, both sets of statistics were converted and compared as a means of testing the
accuracy of our conversions. In all cases, the correlations generated were statistically the
same.
Analyses
We used the random effects meta-analysis method developed by Hedges and Olkin
(1985) to test our hypotheses. This method treats studies as heterogeneous instead of
assuming generalizability from the start as fixed effects models of meta-analysis assume.
The Hedges and Olkin method first tests the overall sample for homogeneity of variance. If
the homogeneity of variance hypothesis is rejected, then the sample is believed to contain
true between study differences, and a weighted least-squares regression is used to test
potential moderators that are identified through theory. Differences between studies that
significantly explain between study variance in a regression equation are significant
moderators of the relationship in question.
Results
The correlations for all samples and measures are shown in Table 2. The test for
homogeneity of variance (Hedges & Olkin, 1985) indicated that there was significant
between study variability (Q = 22.08, p < .001). Thus, the results indicated that moderators
are operating in the relationship between achievement motivation and overall
entrepreneurial activity.
H1 predicted that the relationship between achievement motivation and
entrepreneurial behavior will be moderated by the definition of entrepreneurship. As
shown in Table 2, the mean correlation for studies defining entrepreneurs as founders was
.21 and the mean correlation for studies defining entrepreneurs as business leaders was
.19. Further, as shown in Table 3, the weighted least squares analysis did not show support
for our hypothesis, F(1, 58)=.41, ns. Therefore, it does not appear that the relationship
between achievement motivation and entrepreneurial activity varies depending on how
researchers have defined entrepreneurs in their study.
H2 predicted that achievement motivation will have a have a stronger association
with entrepreneurial activity in known group studies than in individual studies. When all
need for achievement studies were included (see Table 2), the respective mean
correlations were .18 in individual studies and .25 in known group studies. As shown in
Table 3, the weighted least squares analysis showed support for this hypothesis, F(1, 67) =
3.42, p < .05. When we examined this relationship with only the performance studies, we
again found a large difference in effect sizes across levels of analysis. The mean correlation
between achievement motivation and performance was .46 in known group studies and .18
in individual studies. As shown in Table 3, these mean correlations were significantly
different, F(1, 26) = 24.60, p < .01. Thus, H2 is supported; it appears that the relationship
between achievement motivation and entrepreneurial activity is stronger in known group
studies than in individual studies.
H3 predicted that achievement motivation would be significantly related to choice
of an entrepreneurial occupation. Note that career choice has only been studied using the
known group design. The analyses showed significant correlations between achievement
motivation and career choice (r = .21, p < .001). The lower bound of the 95% confidence
interval was .18 and the upper bound was .24. Because the confidence interval did not
contain zero, H3 was supported. It appears that individuals that pursue entrepreneurial
careers are significantly higher in achievement motivation than individuals that pursue
other types of careers.
H3a predicted that the relationship between achievement motivation and career
choice will be moderated by the comparison sample that is used. Specifically, we predicted
that achievement motivation will have a weaker relationship with career choice when the
sample compared entrepreneurs and managers than when the sample compared
entrepreneurs and non-managers. The correlation between achievement motivation and
career choice was lower when the sample included only managers (mean r = .14) than
when it included all other professions (mean r = .35). The regression equation showed a
significant difference between the two types of studies, F(1, 39) = 14.78, p < .01. Therefore,
it appears that the sample of participants involved in the study moderates the achievement
motivationcareer choice relationship.
H4 predicted that achievement motivation would be significantly related to
performance. As noted before, the analyses for known group studies showed a significant
mean correlation between achievement motivation and performance (r = .46, p < .01) with
a lower bound of .42 and an upper bound of .49. The analyses of individual studies also
showed a significant correlation between achievement motivation and performance (r =
.18, p < .01) with a lower bound of .15 and an upper bound of .22.Becauseneither of these
confidence intervals included zero,H4is supported. 6
Discussion
Overall, our results support McClellands theory that achievement motivation is
significantly related to both occupational choice and performance in an entrepreneurial
role. Although these results superficially agree with prior reviews (e.g., Spangler, 1992) in
showing that need for achievement is related to action, our results were based on
entrepreneurship studiesthe domain that is directly relevant to achievement motivation.
Further, we found a stronger relationship between need for achievement and
entrepreneurial activity in known group studies (mean r = .25) than in individual level
studies (mean r = .19). Consistent with Holland and Schneider, achievement motivation
appears to be an important characteristic for entrepreneurs; thus, the correlations from
known group studies were stronger because of the increasing similarity among these
entrepreneurs over time.
We were surprised that we failed to find support for McClellands hypothesis that
achievement motivation strength would predict career choice better than it predicted
performance. For the known group studies, we found the exact opposite to hold. The mean
r for performance was significantly higher than the mean r for career choice (.46 vs. .21).
Although both correlations were significant, this finding suggests that achievement
motivation may be particularly potent at differentiating between successful and
unsuccessful groups of entrepreneurs. Thus, achievement motivation may be particularly
useful for selecting entrepreneurs that may be more likely to successfully take advantage of
entrepreneurial financing and other supporting activities. However, we note that the mean
r for career choice may be lower than that for performance because individuals high in
achievement motivation may be attracted to several different types of careers. For
example, our results suggested that managers are much more similar to entrepreneurs in
terms of their achievement motivation than are individuals attracted to other, less similar
careers such as engineers or scientists. Thus, achievement motivation may be a useful
predictor of career choice for a number of types of positions that have job characteristics
and responsibilities similar to entrepreneurial positions.
Also contrary to McClellands arguments, we found that the TAT showed no greater
validity than other projective tests or than self-report questionnaire measures in predicting
performance or career choice. When we examined the results for performance at the
individual level, the mean correlation for the TAT studies is .16compared to .20 for MSCSt
studies and .20 for the questionnaire studies. For predicting career choice at the group
level, the mean correlations are again very similar (.21 for the TAT, .23 for the MSCSt, and
.22 for the questionnaire). Our failure to find a higher mean validity r for the TAT
contradicts the findings of Spanglers (1992) meta-analysis and those of more traditional
reviews (e.g., Johnson, 1990). In a review of the raw data, it appears that recent studies
using the TAT have reported much lower correlations with entrepreneurial activity than
did the earlier studies conducted by McClelland. Thus, it is possible that the mean r for TAT
studies may have been affected by undertraining on the intricacies of evaluating the TAT or
other errors that may have led to incorrect scoring by later researchers who did not have
the same level of experience with the test as McClelland. We were unable to test this post
hoc speculation because we could not find data to test for reliability differences between
McClelland and other researchers. However, if our speculation is true, it suggests that
*Babb, E. M., & Babb, S. V. (1992). Psychological traits of rural entrepreneurs. The Journal of
Socio-Economics, 21, 353362.
Baum, R., Locke, E. A., & Smith, K. G. (2000). A longitudinal model of a multi-dimensional
model of venture growth. Academy of Management Journal, 44, 292303.
*Begley, T. M. (1995). Using founder status, age of firm, and company growth rate as the
basis for distinguishing entrepreneurs form managers of smaller businesses. Journal of
Business Venturing, 10, 249263.
*Begley, T. M., & Boyd, D. P. (1987). A comparison of entrepreneurs and managers of small
business firms. Journal of Management, 13, 99108.
*Cromie, S., & Johns, S. (1983). Irish entrepreneurs: Some personal characteristics. Journal
of Occupational Behaviour, 4, 317324.
*DeCarlo, J. F., & Lyons, P. R. (1979). A comparison of selected personal characteristics of
minority and non-minority female entrepreneurs. Journal of Small Business Management,
12, 2229.
Eisenstadt, S. N. (1963). The need for achievement. Economic Development and Cultural
Change, 11, 420431.
Fineman, S. (1977). The achievement motive construct and its measurement: Where are we
now? The British Journal of Psychology, 68, 122.
Finison, L. J. (1976). The application of McClellands national development model to recent
data. The Journal of Social Psychology, 98, 5559.
Frey, R. S. (1984). Need for achievement, entrepreneurship, and economic growth: A
critique of the McClelland thesis. The Social Science Journal, 21, 125134.
*Hull, D. L., Bosley, J. J., & Udell, G. G. (1980). Renewing the hunt for heffalump: Identifying
potential entrepreneurs by personality characteristics. Journal of Small Business, 18, 1118.
Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (1990). Methods of meta-analysis. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
*Lee, D. Y., & Tsang, E.W. K. (2001). The effects of entrepreneurial personality, background
and network activities on venture growth. Journal of Management Studies, 38, 584602.
Mazur, A., & Rosa, E. (1977). An empirical test of McClellands achieving society theory.
Social Forces, 55, 769774.
*McClelland, D. C. (1961). The achieving society. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.
McClelland, D. C., Clark, R. A., Roby, T. B., & Atkinson, J. W. (1958). The effect of the need for
achievement on thematic apperception. In J.W. Atkinson (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action and
society (pp. 6482). Princeton, NJ: Van Norstrand.
Mill, J. S. (1848). Principles of political economy with some of their applications to social
philosophy. Boston: C. C. Little and Brown.
*Miner, J. B., Smith, N. R., & Bracker, J. S. (1989). Role of entrepreneurial task motivation in
the growth of technologically innovative firms. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 554560.
*Miner, J. B., Smith, N. R., & Bracker, J. S. (1994). Role of entrepreneurial task motivation in
the growth of technologically innovative firms: Interpretations from follow-up data. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 79, 627630.
*Morris, J. L., & Fargher, K. (1974). Achievement drive and creativity as correlates of
success in small business. Australian Journal of Psychology, 26, 217222.
Ostroff, C., & Harrison, D. A. (1999). Meta-analysis, levels of analysis, and best estimates of
population correlations: Cautions for interpreting meta-analytic results in organizational
behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 260270.
*Ray, J. J., & Singh, S. (1980). Effects of individual differences on productivity among
farmers in India. The Journal of Social Psychology, 112, 1117.
Rosenthal, R. (1979). The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychological
Bulletin, 86, 638641.
Schneider, B. (1987). The people make the place. Personnel Psychology, 40, 437454.
Siehl, C., & Martin, J. (1990). Organizational culture: A key to financial performance? In B.
Schneider (Ed.), Organizational climate and culture (pp. 241281). San Francisco: JosseyBass.
*Singh, S. (1969). nAch among successful-unsuccessful and traditional progressive
agricultural entrepreneurs of Delhi. Journal of Social Psychology, 79, 145149.
*Singh, S. (1970). nAch among agricultural and business entrepreneurs of Delhi. The
Journal of Social Psychology, 81, 145149.
*Singh, S. (1979). Relationships among projective and direct verbal measures of
achievement motivation. Journal of Personality Assessment, 43, 4549.
*Singh, S., & Ray, J. J. (1980). Modernization and development among Indian farmers: A
modern proof of some old theories. Economic Development and Cultural Change, 80, 509
521.
*Stewart, W. H., Watson, W. E., Carland, J. C., & Carland, J.W. (1995). A proclivity for
entrepreneurship: A comparison of entrepreneurs, small business owners, and corporate
managers. Journal of Business Venturing, 14, 189214.
Stewart, W. H., & Roth, P. L. (2001). Risk propensity differences between entrepreneurs and
managers: A meta-analysis review. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 145153.
*Waddell, F. T. (1983). Factors affecting choice, satisfaction, and success in the female selfemployed. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 23, 294304.
For a further discussion of the problems of ecological fallacies, see Judd, Smith, and Kidder (1991).
McClelland does not consider nAch to be a predictor of performance in a traditional, bureaucratic firm; rather he uses
his leadership motive pattern (which involves a certain relationship between nPower, power inhibition, and nAffiliation).
3 It should be noted that our expected enhanced effect of nAch and performance at the group level of analysis is different
from McClellands societial level hypotheses because our hypothesis depends on people freely selecting themselves into
and out of groups. Clearly, society level of analyses include more people than just those who freely select themselves into
that society.
4 As suggested by Ostroff and Harrison (1999), we began by examining whether need for achievement studies can be
examined across levels of analysis. If this hypothesis is supported, then all of the following hypotheses will be tested at
both the group and individual levels of analysis where appropriate.
5 Though inclusion of multiple correlations from the same sample will not affect the average correlation estimate, it will
affect the variance in correlations across studies.
6 We performed Rosenthals (1979) file-drawer analysis and found that 86 studies would be needed to change our results.
Given the extensive nature of our search to find studies focusing on entrepreneurs, these results appear to be stable.
7 We corrected our average correlations for measurement error by applying the correction for attenuation formula. Using
Fineman (1977), we estimated the reliability of the TAT at .54, the reliability of questionnaire methods to be .62, and the
reliability of the MSCS to be .65. After averaging the meta-analysis correlations for each measure, the results did not
significantly differ.
1
2