Modeling Driver Behavior in Automotive Environments
Modeling Driver Behavior in Automotive Environments
Environments
Carlo Cacciabue (Ed.)
Modelling Driver
Behaviourin Automotive
Environments
Critical Issues in Driver Interactions with Intelligent Transport Systems
~ Springer
P.Carlo Cacciabue,Ph.D.
EC IRC -IPSC, Italy
Whilst we have made considerable efforts to contact all holders of copyright material contained in this
book, we may have failed to locate some of them. Should holders wish to contact the Publisher, we will
be happy to come to some arrangement with them.
Apart from any fair dealing for the purposes of research or private study, or criticism or review, as
permitted under the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988, this publication may only be reproduced,
stored or transmitted, in any form or by any means, with the prior permission in writing of the
publishers, or in the case of reprographic reproduction in accordance with the terms of licences issued
by the Copyright Licensing Agency. Enquiries concerning reproduction outside those terms should be
sent to the publishers.
The use of registered names, trademarks, etc. in this publication does not imply, even in the absence of
a specific statement, that such names are exempt from the relevant laws and regulations and therefore
free for general use.
The publisher makes no representation, express or implied, with regard to the accuracy of the infor-
mation contained in this book and cannot accept any legal responsibility or liability for any errors or
omissions that may be made.
9 8 7 6 543 2 1
v
VI Contents
Index...................................................................................... 429
Editorial
viii
Editorial ix
behaviours, and it offers many different technological solutions for all different
control processes.
In reality, many automation controls currently applied to vehicles already
contain models of a certain complexity of cognition and behaviour, based mostly
on dynamic manifestation of control operations. The "automatic gearbox" of cer-
tain vehicles is a typical example of this type of control systems, which adapt
dynamically and independently to different "driving styles", measured through
intrinsic evaluation of behavioural variables, such as rate of accelerator pressure,
overall speed, etc. Another example is the system that manages the availability
of in vehicle information systems (IVIS), such as telephones or radios. In this
case, certain IVIS managers adapt to the environmental situations, by inhibiting
or discouraging the use of certain IVIS in risky situations.
The models of cognition and behaviour that are implemented in such types of
vehicle control systems are naturally elementary from the cognitive point of view.
However, this shows that this industrial field, both in terms of vehicle integrators
and original equipment manufacturers, needs to apply at design level and integrate
at implementation level adequate models of driver behaviour. These models are
equally important for academic and research purposes, where more complex and
varied solutions can be proposed and studied in relation to theoretical paradigms of
different nature and targets. Another area where modelling of driver behaviour is
essential is the transport safety authorities and regulators, where the consideration
of driver performance becomes essential in setting standards and rules governing
new and future regulations of vehicle control systems, road infrastructures and
traffic management. Similarly, models of drivers are necessary for the study of
accidents and investigation of root causes.
The availability of models and paradigms of driver behaviour at different levels
of complexity and development is therefore quite obvious according to the field of
application.
This book offers to the reader the possibility to assess different approaches and
considerations in relation to driver behaviour modelling, resulting from different
fields. Indeed, the authors of the different manuscripts come from the industrial
area, both car and original equipment manufactures and integrators, from the re-
search and academic fields and from national and international regulators and
automotive transport authorities.
More in detail, Chapter 1presents the ongoing activities in International Projects
and Actions on Driver Modelling. In particular, the European sustained research
Projects carried out over the last decades and presently under development are
reviewed in the paper by Panou, Bekiaris, and Papakostopoulos. Similarly, the US
research actions on driver models and a recently held workshop on these issues
are discussed in the paper by Cody and Gordon. The last paper of this Chapter,
by Inagaky, also revises the actions in Japan on driver modelling, focusing on
monitoring and modelling situation-adaptive driver assistance systems.
After this initial review, more specific subjects are dealt with, beginning with
the existing Conceptual Frameworks and Modelling Architectures (Chapter 2)
that sustain the development of specific models of driver behaviour. In all three
x Editorial
P. Carlo Cacciabue
List of Contributors
xii
List of Contributors Xl11
Thomas Jiirgensohn
HFC Human-Factors-Consult Vassilis Papakostopoulos
GmbH, Hellenic Institute of Transport,
Berlin, Germany. Athens, Greece.
DianneParker
JosefF.Krems University of Manchester,
Chemnitz University of Technology, School of Psychological
Department of Psychology, Sciences,
Chemnitz, Germany. Manchester M13 9PL, UK.
LuigiMacchi
BjornPeters
EC, Joint Research Centre,
VTI,
Institute for the Protection and
Linkoping, Sweden.
Security of the Citizen,
Ispra (VA), Italy.
Aris Polychronopoulos
Stefano Marzani I-SENSE Group,
University of Modena and Reggio Institute of Communications and
Emilia, Computer Systems,
Dipartimento di Scienze e Metodi Athens, Greece.
dell'lngegneria,
Reggio Emilia, Italy.
Cristina Re
EC, Joint Research Centre,
PierreMayenobe Institute for the Protection and
INRETS-LESCOT, Security of the Citizen,
Bron cedex, France. Ispra (VA), Italy.
xiv List of Contributors
1.1 Introduction
Human (or operator) modelling has been an extensive area of research in many
application areas, such as artificial intelligence, aviation, probabilistic risk as-
sessments, system safety analysis and human performances in working contexts
(Cacciabue et al., 1993; Baron et al., 1980). Still, human behaviour is fairly con-
textual and substantially different from one person to another. Thus, the initial
linear models have been gradually replaced by nonlinear and even probabilistic
models, based upon artificial intelligence (AI) principles, such as artificial neural
networks or genetic algorithms. This becomes even more intrigued if we consider
a complex behavioural task such as vehicle driving.
The traffic system as a whole can be seen as being composed of three interactive
parts: vehicles, road users and the road environment. Any traffic situation is the
result of the interaction between these three systems. Normally, the traffic situation
develops as planned, but, in certain circumstances the resulting interaction will
result in a critical situation or in a crash.
The driver is a critical component of the traffic system. Attempts have been
made to estimate the importance of the driver as an accident cause (Evans, 1985).
It has been estimated that road user factors are the sole or contributory factors in
a great majority of road crashes.
There is no generally accepted model of the complete driving task. There are
detailed descriptions focusing on perception and handling aspects and reporting
what drivers really do in every possible ('normal') situation from the beginning
to the end of a journey (see McKnight and Adams, 1970). There are also more
analytical approaches focusing on driver behaviour in relation to task demands,
with the purpose of trying to explain and understand the psychological mechanisms
underlying human behaviour (Rasmussen, 1984; Michon, 1985).
Usually, car driving is described as a task containing three different levels of
demands. At the strategic level, the general planning of a journey is handled. For ex-
ample, the driver chooses the route and transportation mode and evaluates resulting
costs and time consumption. At the tactical level, the driver has to exercise ma-
noeuvres, allowing him/her to negotiate the 'right now' prevailing circumstances,
3
4 Panou, Bekiaris and Papakostopoulos
Analysing the driving task requires consideration of the dynamic interaction be-
tween drivers and the traffic system. Driver-specific factors include performance
aspects, individual dispositions and transient driver states. Driver behaviour mod-
els attempt to formalise the complex relation between the driver and the traffic
system.
Environmental Controlled
input sees
action patterns
FIGURE 1.1. The hierarchical structure of the driving task (adapted from Michon, 1985).
with the traffic system. The control level, finally, refers to basal car control
processes. Although a dynamic relationship between the concurrent activities
is assumed, the different levels require different types of information: The
strategical level is mainly top-down (knowledge) controlled. The manoeuvring
and control levels require in addition bottom-up (data) input from the traffic
environment.
Closely associated to Michon's hierarchical model of driver behaviour is Ras-
mussen's division of operative behaviour into three levels: skill-based behaviour
refers to automatic procedures, rule-based behaviour to application of learned
rules and knowledge-based behaviour to conscious problem solving (Rasmussen,
1984). Skill-based behaviour is applied in Michon's model mainly at the control
level in the form of automatic action patterns. Ranney (1994) relates Michon's
control hierarchy to Rasmussen's taxonomy of operative behaviour. Skill-based
behaviour is applied in all familiar situations. Rule-based behaviour dominates
during standard interactions with other road users as well as in some rare situa-
tions like driving a new car, where automatic routines have to be transferred to a
new system. Knowledge is applied when driving in unfamiliar traffic networks,
in difficult environmental conditions or when skills are not fully developed as in
novice drivers.
(a) Goals for life and skills for living: An individual driver's attitudes, lifestyle,
social background, gender, age and other personal preconditions that might
influence driving behaviour and accident involvement.
(b) Driving goals and context: Strategical planning of a trip; the focus is on why,
where, when and with whom one is driving.
(c) Mastery of traffic situations: Actual driving in a given context, resembles
Michon's manoeuvring level.
(d) Vehicle manoeuvring: Overlaps despite a different terminology with Michon's
car control level. The focus is on the vehicle, its construction and how it is
operated.
A safe driver has, however, not only developed skills but also knowledge about
his/her own abilities, preconditions and limits. Experienced drivers have, in addi-
tion, cognitive driving skills, such as anticipation and risk perception. In order to
cover these higher-order aspects of driver behaviour, vertical columns are added to
the so far horizontal structure of hierarchical control models (see Table 1.1). The
columns of the GADGET-Matrix are as follows:
(a) Knowledge and skills: Routines and information required for driving under
normal circumstances.
(b) Risk-increasing factors: Aspects of traffic and life associated with higher risk.
(c) Self-assessment: How good the driver reflects his/her own driving skills and
motivations.
Levels and cells of the GADGET-Matrix are not mutually exclusive - there is
large vertical as well as horizontal overlap due to the complex and cyclic nature
of the driving task, where subtasks usually have to be carried out in parallel at
different levels (e.g., routing, turning left, gap acceptance, speed control, steering,
braking, etc.).
3. Environmental factors: This includes the vehicle status, the existence of traffic
hazards, the weather, road and traffic conditions. The combinations of these
may generate a risky situation , which certainly influences DRIVABILITY.
4. Two common denominators between driver resources and environmental status,
namely workload and riskawareness.
The two intermediate factors between driver resources and the environment,
namely workload and risk assessment, are among the key issues in order to under-
stand and analyse driving performance. Risk awareness depends on three major
contributors:
1. Risk perception, namely the ability to understand/recognise the specific risk at
the specific time moment.
2. Level of attention, the ability to spot the risk in time.
3. Possible external support so as to spot the risk in time, i.e., by advanced driver
assistance systems (ADAS).
In contrast to the risk awareness level, which is rather discrete and may change
arbitrarily, the other factor, workload, is continuous and evolves with time. Even
temporary input, i.e., use of mobile phone, may have high impact on workload
for limited time periods . The major contributors to DRIVABILITY are depicted
in Fig. 1.2.
TABLE 1.2. Driver behaviour issues when introducing ACC (Bekiaris et al., 2001).
Short term Long term
Mistrust: distrusting the ACC system Spare capacity: using spare capacity for other
in-vehicle tasks
Over-reliance: relying too much on the ACC system
Brake pedal forces: increasing brake pedal Fatigue: ACC could take over too many
forces driving tasks causing fatigue
Imitation: unequipped vehicles imitate Quick approach to vehicle in front: the
equipped vehicles development of new behaviour
Reliance on vehicle in front: vehicle in front Time-headway: driving with smaller
might have poor driving behaviour time-headways
Indication for overtaking: use ACC as an
indication of when to overtake
Overtaking: difficulties with overtaking and being overtaken
1.3.1 Automaticity
Automation refers to the mechanical or electrical accomplishment of work. Some
of the automatic components provided by ADAS act as a substitution for tasks that
humans would otherwise be capable of performing. In other cases, ADAS provides
automatic components, which carry out additional tasks that humans would not
have been capable of but will also assist in the overall driving task.
Introducing automation into the driving task offers several potential advantages
in aspects of both efficiency and safety. For example, the use of dynamic route
guidance systems will assist drivers in taking the most cost-effective route, in
terms of fuel and time, for current traffic conditions. Furthermore, automation can
reduce driver workload in areas of decision choice, information acquisition and
information analysis. This reduction in workload should then reduce driver error
and stress, thus increasing road safety.
Overtrust on a system also brings problems. Drivers may become complacent
and may not detect when a system fails. Drivers are left with a false sense of
security, thereby failing to monitor the system leading to the added disadvantage
of loosing system awareness. Drivers may also lose the opportunity to learn and
retain driving skills. Furthermore, the role of the driver may be reduced to such an
extent that their manual driving skills may degrade. This concept has been termed
'out-of-the-loop familiarity'.
Bainbridge (1987) discusses what she terms the' ironies of automation', which
occur with the changing role of the human in the human-machine relationship
when a system is automated. She points out that the more advanced a system is,
the more crucial the contributions of the human operator become. Automation aims
to eliminate the human factor; however, ironically, the human operator is required
to carry out those tasks that cannot be automated. The human operator is therefore
required to monitor the system and to take over and stabilise the system manually
in situations of system failure. However, as previously discussed manual control
skills deteriorate when using an automated system, leading an experienced user to
become inexperienced.
Bainbridge points out the loss of cognitive skills that an operator using an auto-
mated system, such as ADAS, is likely to suffer. As the retrieval of knowledge from
the long-term memory is dependent on the frequency of use, operators will loose
the benefit of long-term knowledge concerning processes. This practical knowl-
edge can be used to generate strategies in emergency or unusual situations. It is
difficult to teach practical knowledge without experience; it is thus of great concern
that when automating a system this practical experience and the reinforcement by
frequency of use will be taken away from the operator.
the impact of this change in locus of control both on driver behaviour and also in
terms of safety. The level of control left to the driver must be carefully considered
(e.g., will drivers be given the opportunity to override vehicle decisions and vice
versa?).
Once level of control is decided upon, it is still important to consider the likely
consequences of implementation. Several concerns exist, including those discussed
in the previous section. First, drivers may feel mistrust in the system and experience
problems in handing over control to the vehicle; this factor is dependent on the
confidence a driver has in the capabilities of the system. Secondly, drivers may
overtrust the system; drivers may become dependent on the system. This may be
problematic both in cases where the system fails; the driver may not detect failure
due to reduced monitoring. Also, in situations when the driver is handed back
control, driver's skills and learning may have been diminished due to out-of-the-
loop familiarity.
Finally, driver behaviour and safety during the handover of control between the
driver and the vehicle needs to be considered. De Vos et al. (1997) investigated
safety and performance when transferring control of the vehicle between the driver
and an Automatic Vehicle Guidance (AVG) system. Drivers were found to be able
to leave the automated lane even when high-speed differences and traffic-density
differences between lanes were present. Unsafe interactions were observed in the
scenario of a low-speed manual lane. As expected, increased trust in the reliability
of the system increased driver comfort. However, as headway decreased drivers
were observed to experience greater discomfort, implying that total trust in the
system did not exist.
The field of application of such models in ED projects is vast and is gaining pace
over the last decades. Rather than attempting to meticulously cover this extensive
area, we will provide an in-depth overview of characteristic examples, showing
the related difficulties as well as benefits in applying such models.
Starting 0 X
Shifting gears 0 X
Accelerating/decelerating 0 X
Steering/lane following 0 X
Speed control 0 X
Braking/stopping 0 X
Use of new cars control aids (ABS, ACC, etc) X X X
Insufficient skills and incomplete automation X
Realistic self-evaluation X
Following X 0 X
Overtaking/Passing X 0 X X
Entering and leaving the traffic X 0 X
Tailgating X X
Lane changing X 0 X X
Scanning the road (eye cues) X 0 X X
Reacting to other vehicles X OX X
Reacting to pedestrians X X X
Parking 0
Negotiating intersections X 0 X X
Negotiating hills/slopes X 0 X
Negotiating curves X 0 X X
Road surface (skid, obstacles) X OX X
Approach/exit of motorways X 0 X
Turning off/over 0
Railroad crossings, bridges, tunnels 0
Reacting to traffic signs and traffic lights X 0 X X
Reacting to direction signs (including in-car X
devices)
Emergency brake OX
Urban driving 0 X X
Rural driving 0
Convoy driving X
Motorway driving OX X
Weather conditions X OX X
Night driving X OX X
Insufficient skills and incompletely X X X
automation
Information overload X X X
Insufficient anticipating skills and wrong X X X
expectations
Risky driving style X X X
Realistic self-evaluation X X X
Awareness of personal driving style X X X
Determination of trip goals, route and modal
choice
Preparation and technical check 0 X
Safety issues X 0 X
Maintenance tasks 0 X
1. Modelling Driver Behaviour in ED and International Projects 17
Intemationallegislation X
First aid (}X X
Economic driving x o X
Driver's condition (stress, mood, fatigue) X X X
Motives for driving X
Awareness of personal planning skills
Awareness of typical driving goals and risky X
driving motives
Knowing about the general relations X
between lifestyle/age/gender and
driving style
Knowing the influence of personal values X
and social background
Knowing about the influence of passengers X X
High level of sensation seeking X
Consequences of social pressure, use of X X X
alcohol and drugs
Awareness of own personal tendencies X
(risky habits, safety-negative motives)
0: the task is trained in all or nearly all European countries as the analysis of the questionnaires
showed; 0: the task is trained only in few or at least one country; X: the driving authorities and
driving instructors questioned indicate that the task is not trained, but should be trained in the particular
country.
because of the generalisation of go-karting to real life driving, as well as due to the
differences in overall behavioural patterns of drivers using seat belts versus those
that do not.
the removal of helmets was not great enough to compensate for the loss of safety
benefits that the helmets provide. Overall, research into motorcycle helmet wearing
has been found to provide a safety benefit and offers little support to any of the
behavioural adaptation theories. However, as Grayson (1996) pointed out these
findings are not surprising and more relevant to the theories are those mechanisms
which protect one part of the anatomy and lead to disregard for safety of other
parts of the body.
TABLE 1.5. Correlation of AWAKE (driver vigilance monitoring and warning system) use
cases and warning strategy with overall DRIVABILITY Index (Bekiaris et aI., 2003).
Values of Overall
DRIVABILITY DRIVABILITY AWAKE warning
AWAKE use cases Indexes Index levels/strategy
multi-stage driver monitoring and driver warning system that takes such param-
eters into account. Table 1.5 correlates the overall DI and the indexes of the
DRIVABILITY contributors to the different AWAKE driver warning levels and
media. It should be noticed that the sensors included in the AWAKEsystem (such as
driver eyelid and steering grip force monitoring, frontal radar, lane recognition sys-
tem, etc.) allow for sufficient, real-time estimation of all DRIVABILITY indexes
(except KSI, which is however included in the system by the driver at its initiation
as the system adapts itself to the driver profile). This is done by storing driver's,
vehicle and environmental data on the system and automatically processing them.
Further processing off-line is also feasible.
22 Panou, Bekiaris and Papakostopoulos
This is indeed one of the very few cases where such a direct relation between
a driver behaviour model and the development of the HMI of an ADAS has been
attempted, and in fact with great success, as the final AWAKE HMI has been rated
as adequate and useful by over 90% of its users.
Finally, within AIDE (IST-1-507674), a new driver model is being developed, at-
tempting to model concurrently the driver, the vehicle and the environment (Panou
et al., 2005). The basic assumption made for the development of the model of the
driver is that the driver is essentially performing a set of actions that are familiar ac-
cording to his/her experience. As the driving process is very dynamic, these actions
are continuously selected from a vast repository of knowledge (knowledge base) by
a diagnostic process. Consequently, the processes of diagnosis and interpretation
of acquired information become crucial for the dynamic sequencing of driver's
activity. The model of the driver adopted is based on a very simple approach that
assumes that behaviour derives from a cyclical sequence of four cognitive func-
tions: perception, interpretation, planning and execution (PIPE). This model is
not sequential as the execution function, i.e., the manifested form of behaviour,
may result from several iterations (cyclical) of the other functions. Moreover, in
agreement with the initial hypothesis, the planning function, is usually bypassed
by the 'automatic' selection of familiar frames of knowledge that are associated
with procedures or sets of several actions aiming at the fulfilment of the goal of a
frame. This function is important as it becomes effective in unknown situations or
in the case of novice drivers, when 'simpler' frames, based on single actions or on
a limited sequence of very simple/familiar actions, are called into play to deal with
the situation. These four cognitive functions can be associated to either sensorial
or cognitive processes and are activated according to certain rules or conditions
(Table 1.6). This model will be utilised in personalising the multi-ADAS system
HMI, in accordance to a particular driver's needs and preferences, and will also
be used in the traffic environment.
1.5 Conclusions
Driving task modelling has started as simple task-layers representation for taxo-
nomic use in driver training and has gradually evolved to dynamic models, which
consider driver behaviour adaptation as well as the impact of the traffic environment
1. Modelling Driver Behaviour in ED and International Projects 23
and the driving context. The initial list of driver training and assessment projects
that used driver models as their theoretical basis (GADGET, DAN, TRAINER,
AGILE, CONSENSUS, etc.) have been followed by a new generation of projects
that use driver models to assess the impacts of driver support systems (i.e.,
ADVISORS, TRAVELGUIDE) and, more recently, by those that attempt to use the
model parameters for optimal HMI design (AWAKE, COMUNICAR, AIDE, etc.).
Preliminary results have proven that such a correlation is feasible and beneficiary,
but it is far from obvious. The model output has to be evaluated and even modi-
fied by empirical results. Thus, currently the model is being applied and tested in
AIDE, SENSATION and PREVENT Integrated Projects through short- and long-
term testing of drivers. Furthermore, the model can only be at the starting basis
of the design and development process and only influence the actual ADAS HMI
within predefined design boundaries. Nevertheless, we seem to be at the infancy
of a new design principle for driver support training and assessment systems - the
model-based modular and personalised design.
References
Adams, 1.G.U. (1985). Smeed's Law, seat belts and the emperor's new clothes. In
L. Evans and R.C. Schwing (Eds.). Human Behaviour and Traffic Safety. Plenum,
New York.
Aschenbrenner, K.M. and Biehl, B. (1994). Empirical studies regarding risk compensation
processes in relation to anti-lock braking systems. In R.M. Trimpop and GJ.S. Wilde
(Eds.). Challenges to Accident Prevention: The Issue of Risk Compensation Behaviour.
Styx Publ., Groningen, The Netherlands.
Bainbridge, L. (1987). Ironies of automation. In 1. Rasmussen, K. Duncan and 1. Leplat
(Eds.). New Technology and Human Error. Wiley, New York.
Baron, S., Zacharias, G., Muralidharan, R. and Lancraft , R. (1980). PROCRU: A Model
for Analyzing Flight Crew Procedures in Approach to Landing. NASA CR-152397.
Bekiaris, E., Papakonstantinou, C., Stevens, A., Parkes, A., Boverie, S., Nilsson, L.,
Brookhuis, K., Van Wees, K., Wiethoff, M., Damiani, S., Lilli, F., Ernst, A., Heino, A.,
Widlroither, H. and Heinrich, J. (2001). ADVISORS Deliverable 3/8.1v4: Compendium
of Existing Insurance Schemes and Laws, Risk Analysis ofADA Systems and Expected
Driver Behavioural Changes. User Awareness Enhancement, dissemination report and
market analysis and ADAS marketing strategy. ADVISORS Consortium, Athens, Greece.
Bekiaris, E., Amditis, A. and Panou, M. (2003). DRIVABILITY: A new concept for mod-
elling driving performance. International Journal ofCognition Technology & Work, 5(2),
152-161.
Blomquist, G. (1986). A utility maximization model of driver traffic safety behaviour.
Accident Analysis and Prevention, 18,371-375.
Boverie, S., Bolovinou, A., Polychronopoulos, A., Amditis, A., Bellet, T., Tattegrain-Veste,
H., Manzano, 1., Bekiaris, E., Panou, M., Portouli, E., Kutila, M., Markkula, G. and
Angvall A. (2005). AIDE Deliverable 3.3.1: AIDE DVE monitoring module - Design
and development. AIDE Consortium, Toulouse, France.
Breker, S., Henrikson, P., Falkmer, T., Bekiaris, E., Panou, M., Eeckhout, G., Siren, A.,
Hakamies-Blomqvist, L., Middleton, H. and Leue E. (2003). AGILE deliverable 1.1:
Problems of elderly in relation to the driving task and relevant critical scenarios.
24 Panou, Bekiaris and Papakostopoulos
Cacciabue, P.C., Mauri, C. and Owen, D. (1993). Development of a model and simulation
of aviation maintenance technician task performance, International Journal ofCognition
Technology & Work, 5(4),229-247.
Chenier, T.C. and Evans, L. (1987). Motorcyclist fatalities and the repeal of mandatory
helmet wearing laws. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 19, 133-139.
Christ et al. (2000). GADGET final report: Investigations on influences upon driver be-
haviour - Safety approaches in comparison and combination. GADGET Consortium,
Wien, Austria.
Cownie, A.R. and Calderwood, 1M. (1966). Feedback in accident control. Operational
Research Quarterly, 17, 253-262.
De Vos, A.P., Hoekstra, W. and Hogema, I.H. (1997). Acceptance of automated vehicle
guidance (AVG): System reliability and exit manoeuvres. Mobility for everyone. Pre-
sented at the 4th World Congress on Intelligent Transport Systems, Berlin.
Evans, L. (1985). Human behaviour feedback and traffic safety. Human Factors, 27 (5),
555-576.
Evans, L. and Schwing, R.C. (Eds.) (1982). Human Behaviour and Traffic Safety. Plenum,
New York.
Forward, S.E. (1993). Prospective methods applied to dynamic route guidance. In O.MJ.
Carsten (Ed.). Framework for Prospective Traffic Safety Analysis. HOPES Project De-
liverable 6.
Fuller, R. (1984). A conceptualization of driving behaviour as threat avoidance. Ergonomics
27, 1139-1155.
Grayson, G. (1996). Behavioural adaptation: A review of the literature. TRL Report 254.
Transport Research Laboratory, Crowthorne.
Hatakka, M., Keskinen, E., Gregersen, N.P., Glad, A and Hernetkoski, K. (1999). Results
of EU-project GADGET. In S. Siegrist (Ed.). Driver Training, Testing and Licensing-
Towards Theory Based Management ofYoung Drivers' Injury Risk in Road Traffic. BFU-
report 40, Berne.
HLDI. (1994). Collision and property damage liability losses ofpassenger cars with and
without antilock brakes. Highway Loss Data Institute Report A-41. HLDI, Arlington,
VA.
Howarth, I. (1993). Effective design: Ensuring human factors in design procedures. In
A. Parkes and S. Franzen (Eds.). Driving Future Vehicles. Taylor and Francis, London.
Kullgren, A., Lie, A. and Tingvall, C. (1994). The effectiveness of ABS in real life accidents.
Paper presented at the Fourteenth International Technical Conference on the Enhanced
Safety of Vehicles, Munich.
Lund, A.K. and O'Neill, B. (1986). Perceived risks and drinking behaviour. Accident Anal-
ysis and Prevention, 18, 367-370.
McKnight, AJ. and Adams B.D. (1970). Driver Education Task Analysis, Vol. 1: Task
Descriptions. Human Resources Research Organization, Alexandria, VA.
Michon, lA. (1985). A critical view of driver behaviour models: What do we know, what
should we do? In L. Evans and R.C. Schwing (Eds.). Human Behaviour and Traffic Safety.
Plenum, New York.
Naatanen, R. and Summala, H. (1973). A model for the role of motivational factors in
drivers' decision making. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 6, 243-261.
OECD. (1990). Behavioural Adaptations to Changes in the Road Transport System. OECD,
Paris.
O'Neill, B. (1977). A decision-theory model of danger compensation. Accident Analysis
and Prevention, 9, 157-165.
1. Modelling Driver Behaviour in EU and International Projects 25
Panou, M., Cacciabue, N., Cacciabue, P.C. and Bekiaris, E. (2005). From driver modelling
to human machine interface personalisation. Paper presented at the IFAC World Congress,
Prague.
Ranney, T. (1994). Models of driving behaviour: A review of their evolution. Accident
Analysis and Prevention, 26(6), 733-750.
Rasmussen, J. (1984). Information processing and human-machine interaction. In An ap-
proach to cognitive engineering. North Holland, New York.
Rompe, K., Schindler, A. and Wallrich, M. (1987). Advantages of an anti-wheel lock system
(ABS) for the average driver in difficult driving situations. Paper presented at the Eleventh
International Technical Conference on Experimental Safety Vehicles, Washington, DC.
Rumar, K., Berggrund, U., Jernberg, P. and Ytterbom, U. (1976). Driver reaction to a
technical safety measure - Studded tyres. Human Factors, 18, 443--454.
Stokes, A., Wickens, C. and Kite, K. (1990). Display Technology: Human Factors Concepts.
Society of Automotive Engineers, Inc., USA.
Streff, F.M. and Geller, E.S. (1988). An experimental test of risk compensation: Between-
subject versus within-subject analyses. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 20, 277-287.
Summala, H, Lamble, D. and Laakso, M. (1988). Driving experience and perception of
the lead cars braking when looking at in-car targets. Accident Analysis and Prevention,
30(4), 401--407.
Taylor, D.H. (1964). Drivers' galvanic skin response and the risk of accident. Ergonomics,
7, 439--451.
Van Winsum, W., Van Knippenberg, C. and Brookhuis, K. (1989). Effect of navigation
support on driver's metnal workload. In Current Issues in European Transport, Vol. I:
Guided Transport in 2040 in Europe. PTRC Education and Research Services, London.
2
TRB Workshop on Driver Models: A
Step Towards a Comprehensive Model
of Driving?
DELPHINE CODY AND TIMOTHY GORDON
2.1 Introduction
Various disciplines use the same or similar terminology for driver models - vehicle
and traffic engineering, psychology, human factors, artificial intelligence to men-
tion the most common; however, the definition of the term varies not only between
disciplines but even between different researchers within any given discipline. Re-
cent efforts in applied psychology and human factors have emphasised the need
of developing models that can be implemented and used in computer simulation,
hence representing a possible link between these disciplines, and also a chance
to consider the broader picture of driver model within a transportation/traffic sys-
tem. In order to discuss this link, the authors organised a workshop on driver
modelling during the 84th annual meeting of the Transportation Research Board,
Washington, DC. The workshop was attended by 25 researchers from the vari-
ous fields listed above and lead researchers from the United States, Europe and
Asia.
Three objectives were set for this workshop: (i) create a group of driver model
developers and users, (ii) share common experience and reach common definitions
and finally (iii) set a road map for the next generation of driver models. The topics
that were more specifically dealt with were the design and application of cognitive
and driver behaviour models and their integration within a broader simulation
framework. This chapter is divided into three sections. In the first section, we
present the workshop content in more detail and provide a summary of the speakers'
contribution. In the second section, we present a synthesis of these models with
the introduction of a set of dimensions allowing for visualisation of the different
models on a similar scale. The final section presents what the authors believe to
be the critical steps necessary to coordinate efforts towards a new generation of
driving model- a framework where researchers from different fields contribute to
create a comprehensive model of the driving activity.
26
2. TRB Workshop on Driver Models 27
This section comprises two parts. We will first detail each of the themes around
which the speakers organised their presentation and will present summaries of
each of the presentations in the second part.
/
Inputs Outputs
~
' Cognitive Processes
' driver assistance systems -Hurnan motor control
Vehicle
I
' other vehicles
'road geometry and signals -Decisions
Driver behavior
Vehicle control
\.
1
FactlJrs/ parameters
Measures of Effectiveness/Evaluation
Risk acceptance
safety
I--
Cras h risk
Efficiency
Workl oa d
Stress
etc., and so should any realistic driving model; resolving these apparently conflict-
ing viewpoints requires a somewhat deeper concept of what is required for model
validation. At the very least, validation of real-time models must take account for
a wide range of stochastic influences.
Out of the five models that were presented, four are proceeding with more or
less extensive validation and one is still in the process of calibration.
perform IVIS tasks compete with the resources needed to drive the vehicle. In order
to avoid reducing the driving performance to unacceptable levels, only limited
resources can be required by IVIS. The different resource components and/or
magnitudes can be used to perform the same task and are required for different
tasks. The combination of the demand of these specific components determines the
required resources to perform a task. The required resource demand can be used to
estimate the potential of a decrement in driving performance. The principle of IVIS
is that nominal values for measures are derived that can be modified to match a
task or design specification; for example, a subtask modifier is the message length
and a task modifier is the roadway complexity.
The scale of simulation is one driver within one of the three age groups. The
software allows visualising the behaviour change outcome and the modifiers that
can be selected to adjust. Finally, the source code and a user manual are available
from FHWA. The software is still in a proof of concept stage. The model is used and
designed to be used in the industry, which can make the feedback to the scientific
community a slower process. For a review of the validation carried out at VTTI,
the reader can consult Jackson and Bhise (2002).
Declarative Module
(TemporallHippocampus
External World
FIGURE 2.3. ACT-R architecture.
and motor dimensions (Salvucci et al., 2001) . The approach involves applying
a cognitive architecture, ACT-R associated to a computational framework . The
advantage of this approach is the possibility to re-use theories and mechanisms
already integrated within the cognitive architecture. This approach currently fo-
cuses on highway driving involving moderate traffic. The two current practical
applications are (i) the prediction of driver distraction, for which models of typical
secondary tasks (e.g., phone dialling) are integrated into the model in order to pre-
dict real-world observables measures and (ii) the recognition of driver intentions,
where many models are run simultaneously, each trying to accomplish a different
goal and the method consist of tracking which model best matches the observed
data.
Figures 2.3 and 2.4 depict two diagrams: one for ACT-R and the othe r for ACT-
R driver model. Figure 2.3 shows (Anderson et a\., 2004) ACT-R architecture
and details the overlap between brain structures and information processing steps.
Figure 2.4 describes (Salvucci et a\., 2001) the component involved in the ACT-R
driver model. ACT-R provides a framework and specific rules are developed for
the driving model. The model also integrates cognitive models for other tasks, such
as using a cell phone or a navigation system. The current scale of simulation for
the ACT-R driver model is focused on one driver in an environment that provides
32 Cody and Gordon
interactions with other vehicles. This effort also includes the modelling of younger
versus older drivers. The visualisation allows observing driver's eye view and
mirror and in a new system currently under development, graphs will provide
measure of behaviour. Regarding the ease in using a model, the current version
of the ACT-R driver model cannot be used easily by other researchers, but a new
version is under development that should be easy to use (Salvucci et al., 2005).
In terms of calibration and validation, the approach applied for the ACT-R driver
model is the comparison of model and human data sets recorded similarly in terms
of real-world measures. The data sets that have been collected so far are highway
driving, phone dialling and distraction, radio tuning and distraction and the age
effects. Examples of measures are lane change steering profiles, gaze distribution
on highway, lateral deviation during phone dialling.
f
FI GURE 2.5. Block diagram for driver/vehicle/system and driver/model.
Two figures support the model architecture. Figure 2.5 is a block diagram of
the driver vehicle system. In this figure , the concept of task covers elements such
as lane keeping , car following or speed control , while the behaviour is described
as measures of performance and workload . Figure 2.6 illustrates a 3 x 3 x 3
description of driver behaviour (Theeuwes, 2001) relative to the task hierarchy,
task performance and information processing . The shaded area denotes the part
of the behaviour that is currently modelled , i.e., the control of the vehicle at a
skilled-based level. For this part of the behaviour, all of the information proce ss
is integrated, from percept ion to action. Figure 2.6 provoked a lot of interest from
the audience, as it is commonly considered that strategic equal s knowled ge-based
level, manoeu vring equal s rule-b ased level and control equals skill-based level. A
question was raised about how to transition within this 3 x 3 x 3 representation.
For example , how can a driver be simulated at the skill-based control and then
simulated at the knowledge-based level? Is it only by learning? Can it also be
due to other factors? The answer pointed to situations such as degraded driving
co nditions. For exampl e, an experienced driver mobili ses skills (Task performance)
at the control level (Task hierarchy), but while driving another vehicle will move
the vehicle control one level up to the rule level. Another example given was the
case of a novice driver, for whom the control of the vehicle can be associated to a
kn owledge-level perform ance.
>-
/
:I: ,/
U ,/ / v
0:::
<
0:::
strateg ic ///
,./.
W maneuvering A /l.-(
:I:
~ control
s
In
World
IObjects I 4--------------------------------------------------------
vehicle dynam ics
---------1
r
Driver ~ - - -- , , --- - -- ., ,... ---
Sensors
I
Processor I
Body
Eyes, 1 s ensor-r H Action- Anns , Legs ,
Ears. filters filter Head
- - _... , I, , -- - -
Proprioception
---- ---,
I
.a.
,
I I ..., 0
Vehicle
--------- --- ----- ------ --- ---- ---- ----- ----- --- -- - Controls
Field of view Pedals ---
Steering whee l.. .
The outputs generated by the OCM are time simulation results , performance
and workload measures and a workload index. The example of validation provided
during the workshop was a comparison of data collected on a driving simulator and
of model prediction on the lane-keeping task . The agreement between the model
and experimental results indicates a useful predictive capability.
2.2.2.4 ACME
The purpose of the ACME driver model it to develop a combined view on car-
following and lane-changing as well as describe and evaluate the mental processes
needed for driving . It is a man in the loop simulation of a 'car-driver' unit composed
of three sub-models: model of human sensors, model for information processing
and a model for action execution. The simulation integrates models of the dynamics
of the vehicle and models of the simulated area.
The ACME driver model is a very modular architecture (Fig. 2.7) and one of the
most driver vehicle infrastructure system oriented among the presentations given.
The model can be divided into three major substructures: (i) senses, (ii) informa-
tion processing and (iii) actions execution. The senses that are implemented are
vision and hearing, and the perception of acceleration is used to influence speed
decision and the haptic input is not integrated. The information processor consists
of an internal world representation storing , a planning instance and an execution
instance. In order to carry out the execution of action, the extremities are simulated
to move in-vehicles devices to certain position. These devices in tum determine the
vehicle's dynamism. Regarding the scale of simulation, approximately 20 vehicles
can be simulated around an intersection. The simulation step ranges from 10 to
100 ms. The visualisation represents the states within the driver's cognition and
includes timelines of measures. Regarding the ease of using the model, it would
2. TRB Workshop on Driver Models 35
Watc hing
still require time for the user to become familiar with the model and its implemen-
tation. The ACME model is still in the process of development and hence has not
been validated yet.
Psycho-motor Auditory
cognition
Motor control
Vehicle response
Vehicle(s) model
Vehicle motions
level of details
Infrastructure model
Type
psycho-motor
driving tasks
control level
model capacities
simulated phenomena
simulation control
control variables
vehicle motions
vehicle subsystems
vehicle response
And, for the infrastructure component, the two main proposed categories were
As the workshop was limited in time, we focused the scope of the description to
the three main components and on the details of the psycho-motor dimensions.
The rating was as follows:
0: not represented
1: indirectly represented (there is a related parameter)
2: basic inclusion (there is a specific parameter in the model that indicates relevant
trends)
3: included (the parameter or state is directly represented via a simple sub-model)
4: modelled (a sub-model represents the process)
5: represented in detail (the process is a core aspect of the model and be related in
some detail to experiments or theories)
This rating was then integrated in a 'radar web' graph. Each speaker presented
a graph for each of the three components and then a more detailed graph of the
psycho-motor level. The ratings for all the models were then integrated on a same
graph (see Figs. 2.10 and 2.11).
The purpose of representing the models presented at the workshop via a common
graph was to identify synergies and limitations. In order to categorise synergistic
dimensions, it was proposed that when a dimension was ranked three or above
by at least three models, then a synergy between the three models is possible. As
shown in Fig. 2.10, the dimensions that fit this category are perception, cognition,
operational, rule, reaction and static input. A second category is one where di-
mensions could clearly be further developed, where at least one model ranks three
or above. The dimensions in this category are motor control, strategic, tactical,
skill, non-driving activities, knowledge, anticipation, distraction and driver char-
acteristics. Another interesting category is one where none of the models ranked
38 Codyand Gordon
Perception Ps cho-motor
Simulation
Control
Driving
driver characteristics tactical Tasks
reaction time~~~~~~~~~~~~~l;;;9~~~Joperational
Simulated
Control Levels
Model Capacities
non driving activities
three or above with the following three dimensions: learning , variable and dynamic
inputs.
The same method was applied for the description of the psycho-motor dimen-
sions (Fig. 2.11), and the areas for which we identified synergies are vision, decision
making , memories, steering control and velocity control. The areas that need to
be developed are haptic, auditory, recognition, anticipation, rules/knowledge and
attentional resources. Finally, the areas still to be covered are tactile and other
vehicle control.
This categorization of the dimensions led to a question about researchers imple-
mentation strategy and what dictates the choice of what to implement first. Are the
dimensions from the first category receiving so much attention because they are
easier to implement than the low ranking ones or because they are more important
to the concept of driver model and its current applications?
Vision
Perception
Motor Control
attentional resources
Cognition
I...... IVIS " ' - ACT-R ..... Workload ACME .... Flowsim I
and those parameters should be rich enough to encompass the interests of a very
wide class of transportation researchers - across the disciplines mentioned in the
Introduction (Section 2.1). Roughly speaking, relevant time-based responses (eye
glance, steering, emergency braking, decision latency, etc.) should be available to
predict trends as key parameters of interest change (external vehicle behaviour,
vehicle control system activity, driver experience, vehicle information and warning
systems, driver-vehicle interface, distracting activities, external signalling, etc.).
No such model exists at present; indeed, it is probably naive to expect that any
single model will ever be sufficiently full and complete to represent all possible
behavioural changes in response to all possible parametric changes. However it is
plausible to expect that a single modular formulation of the driving process - or
more precisely a single functional representation of driving - could be developed
to accelerate the progress on any particular model-based question of this type.
A common modular framework is also an essential starting point for any se-
rious coordinated effort in driver modelling. In the foregoing text (Fig. 2.9) we
have suggested the general scope of such a modular approach, but the framework
itself is undefined. The aim here is to start to define what such a framework might
look like, without necessarily proposing this in any final form. Figure 2.12 illus-
trates the point. Each rectangular shape depicts a process that is at the same time
stochastic and predictable. Those associated with the driver are open to learning
and adaptation. Note that in this representation, the driver appears as a distributed
set of processes! Indeed, the structure embodies a whole range of assumptions and
hypotheses. For example, it suggests that some form of 'vision-based' driving is
possible without the involvement of higher cognitive function, but that manoeu-
vring decisions are not possible without (at least occasional) strategic input. The
diagram is not intended to be complete (e.g., there is no auditory information chan-
nel) or even correct; indeed the correctness of this or other functional maps of its
kind ultimately depends on its ability to match experiments.
2.5 Conclusions
The models presented at the workshop varied based on the goal for which they
are designed and the methods applied to implement, calibrate and validate them.
However, they do share commonalities in the processes that they manipulate. The
authors' intention when convening the speakers was to illustrate the variety of
driver models and to convey the point that the aim of developing driver models is
not to create 'the' driver model or a driver model representing the right approach.
Actually, the level of detail to integrate in a driver model really depends on the goal
of the developer. For instance, in order to predict driver behaviour, how necessary
is it to describe the psycho-motor processes underlying the driving activity or is a
data analysis of data patterns and trends sufficient?
The intent of the workshop was to open up the discussion on modelling issues
and exchange, move from a 'researcher centric' to a community-directed approach
in order to take driver model development further. The other goal was to discuss the
2. TRB Workshop on Driver Models 41
strategic
DVI
vehicle vehicle
control
dynamics kinematics
:=
+-- other
vision vehicles
infrastructure
idea of extending the notion of driver model towards the concept of comprehensive
driving model, where the driver becomes one the component of a system and could
become a tool used in order to support ITS development or other safety application
for which it is necessary to know more about the driver behaviour or information-
processing characteristics. In this sense, this concept differs from classical traffic
simulation tool by the scale at which it is envisaged, although simplified versions
could eventually be coupled with more conventional traffic simulation tools. In this
regard, the aim is to take the concept of driver model out of the research community
and bring it to a wider range of users, who can be engineers designing in-vehicle
systems and needing to understand the impact of their systems on driving or traffic
engineers needing to use driver models when changing a roadway design without
systematically having to conduct lengthy data collection.
The next step to pursue the development of a comprehensive driving model will
be to continue to take advantage of conferences to bring together model developers
and users in order to discuss about the elements to include on a driving model and
methods for exchanging the results of their research.
42 Cody and Gordon
References
Anderson, 1.R., Bothell, D., Byrne, M.D., Douglass, S., Lebiere, C. and Qin, Y. (2004) An
integrated theory of the mind. Psychological Review, 111, 1036-1060.
Brackstone, M. (2000). An examination of the use of fuzzy sets to describe relative speed.
Perception Ergonomics, 43(4), 528-42.
Brackstone, M., McDonald, M. and Wu, 1. (1997). Development of a fuzzy logic based
microscopic motorway simulation model. In Proceedings of the IEEE Conference on
Intelligent Transportation Systems (lTSC97). Boston, MA.
Jackson, D.L. and Bhise, V.D. (2002). An evaluation of the IVIS-DEMAnD driver attention
demand model. Report No. SAE 2002-01-0092, UMTRI-95608 A08. Human Factors in
Seating and Automotive Telematics, Warrendale, SAE, pp. 61-70.
Salvucci, D.D., Boer, E.R., and Liu, A. (2001). Toward an integrated model of driver
behavior in a cognitive architecture. Transportation Research Record, 1779, 9-16.
Salvucci, D.D., Zuber, M., Beregovaia, E., and Markley, D. (2005). Distract-R: Rapid
prototyping and evaluation of in-vehicle interfaces. In Proceedings ofHuman Factors in
Computing Systems. Portland, OR.
Theeuwes, 1. (2001). The effects of road design on driving. In Pierre-Emmanuel Barjonet
(Ed.). Traffic Psychology Today (pp. 241-263). Kluwer, Boston, MA.
Wu, 1., Brackstone, M. and McDonald, M. (2000). Fuzzy sets and systems for a motorway
microscopic simulation model. Fuzzy Sets and Systems 116(1), 65-76.
Wu, 1., Brackstone, M. and McDonald, M. (2003). The validation of a microscopic simula-
tion model: A methodological case study. Transportation Research C, 11(6),463-479.
3
Towards Monitoring and Modelling
for Situation-Adaptive Driver
Assist Systems
TOSHIYUKIINAGAKI
3.1 Introduction
In the classic tri-level study of the causes of traffic accidents, Treat et al. (1979) as-
cribe 92.6% of car accidents to human error, where human errors include improper
lookout (known as 'looking but not seeing'), inattention, internal distraction and
external distraction. Green (2003) reports that other studies have found similar
results that a human error is involved in 90% of car accidents. Human errors, such
as those listed in the above, can happen for everybody and may not be eradicated.
However, if there were some technology to detect driver's possibly risky behaviour
or state in a real-time manner, car accidents may be reduced effectively. Proactive
safety technology that detects driver's non-normative behaviour or state and pro-
vides the driver with appropriate support functions plays a key role in automotive
safety improvement. Various research projects have been conducted worldwide to
develop such technologies (see, e.g. Witt, 2003; Panou et al., 2005; Saad, 2005;
Amditis et al., 2005; Tango and Montanari, 2005; Cacciabue and Hollnagel, 2005).
This paper gives an overview on two of research projects in Japan, which aim
to develop technologies to detect driver's behaviour or state that is inappropriate
to a given traffic environment so that the driver may be provided with support for
enhancing his or her situation awareness or for reducing risk in the environment.
The first project is the 'Behaviour-Based Human Environment Creation Technol-
ogy', which was conducted during the period of 1999 to 2003, with the support of
the Ministry of Economy, Trade and Industry (METI), Government of Japan, and
the New Energy and Industrial Technology Development Organization (NEDO).
The other project is the' Situation and Intention Recognition for Risk Finding and
Avoidance' which has been proceeding since 2004, with the support of the Ministry
of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology (MEXT), Government of
Japan.
This paper tries to focus on the modelling-related aspects of the projects, and
picks up a model from each project. From the first project, is the Bayesian network
model for detecting non-normative behaviour of the driver, in which the model
has been constructed based on driving behavioural data collected in the real traffic
environment. From the second project, is the discrete-event model of dynamical
43
44 Inagaki
changes of driver's psychological state, which has been developed to analyse and
determine how decision authority should be distributed between the driver and
automation under possibility of the driver's overtrust in 'smart and reliable' au-
tomation. The models are still in their early stages; however, they are expanding
description capabilities and applicabilities in the real world.
Crash
(Fail to perfonn task)
FiGURE 3.1. Driving performance and task demand (Akamatsu and Sakaguchi , 2003); orig-
inally from Fuller and Santos (2002) .
3. Towards Monitoring and Modelling for Situation-Adaptive Driver Assist Systems 45
FIG URE 3.2. Bayesian network model for the behavioural events when approaching an
intersection with the STOP sign (Akamatsu and Sakaguchi, 2003).
distributions for X and Y and the knowledge of probabilistic causal relation among
the nodes, one can derive the conditional probability distribution, P(ZIX , Y) for
each combination of values of X and Y. The conditional probability distribution
P(ZIX , Y) describes how Z can take different values when the driver behaves in
a normative manner.
Suppose an on-board sensor has observed that the random variable Z took a
value z at some time point. Hypothesis testing is then performed to determine
whether 'z may be regarded as a sample from the distribution P(Z IX, Y)'. If the
hypothesis was rejected at some level of significance, it is then concluded that
the driver's behaviour may be deviated from his or her normative performance.
Akamatsu et al. (2003) defined the ' level of normality' and have developed a
method to give a warning to the driver when the calculated level of normality
becomes less than a specified threshold value .
index of driver's mental strain (Sakakibara and Taguchi, 2003). The index was not
convenient for real-time sensing of driver's mental tension. They found, however,
that the steering operation and the head motion of a driver can replace salivary CgA
in estimating a level of driver's mental tension. Taguchi and his colleagues argued
that the steering operations at a frequency lower than 0.5 Hz may reflect a driver's
reduced activation state in a monotonous driving environment and that the driver's
intentional behaviour, such as a lane change, does not usually have influence on
operational performance at such low frequencies (Sakakibara and Taguchi, 2005;
Taguchi and Sakakibara, 2005). They also considered that while concentrating
on driving, a driver generally puts power into various muscles to control body
in response to vibrations from road surface, acceleration and/or deceleration of
vehicle, which may create stiffness in the shoulders. Experiments were conducted
to investigate relations between the two indices for tension. It has been observed
that the steering operation and the head motions are complementary to each other.
They have also found that a real-time estimation method can be implemented to
distinguish the four grades of driver's tension (viz. reduced activation, neutral,
moderate tension and hypertension).
traffic environment
infer "intent"
and Bye, 2000). In reality, however, drivers' situation recognition may not always
be perfect. Decisions and actions that follow poor or imperfect situation recogni-
tion can never be appropriate to given situations. It is not possible to 'see' directly
whether a driver's situation recognition is correct or not. However, monitoring the
driver 's action (or its precursor) and traffic environment may make it be possible
to estimate (a) whether the driver may have lost situation awarenes s, (b) whether
the driver's interpretation of the traffic environment is appropriate and (c) whether
the driver is inactive psychologically, e.g. due to complacency, or physiologically,
e.g . due to fatigue, (see Fig. 3.3).
Since 2005, the author has been conducting a 3-year research project , enti-
tled ' Situation and Intention Recognition for Risk Finding and Avoidance', with
the support of the MEXT. The aim of the project is to develop proactive safety
technologies to (a) monitor driver behaviour for assessing his or her intention, (b)
detect mismatches of traffic environment and driver's interpretation of it, (c) assess
the driver state and (d) provide the driver with appropriate assist functions in a
situation-adaptive and context-dependent manner (see Fig. 3.4).
The research topics in the project are categorised as follows : (1) estimation of
driver 's psychological and physiological state, (2) driver behaviour modelling, (3)
intelligent information processing for situation recognition and visual enhance-
ment and (4) adaptive function allocation between drivers and automation. In (I),
real-time methods are under development for detecting the driver's inattentive-
ness , hypo-vigilance, and complacency, through monitoring parameters, such as
body movement, dynamical changes of the LCP on the back, eye and head move-
ments , blinks , instep position of the right leg, operation of the steering wheel
and movements of gas and brake pedals (Itoh et al., 2006) . In (1), levels of
w
~~ .:_ =-~ ~ ~. ,
'.--:--. . . -::4-- _.., ~ -. Q3
:E
~
- ''' , . .,
- -. '"a.
'"
~
o
::l
s"
:::!.
Driver's behaviors ::l
oe
::l
'"0-
I ~
o
0-
~
s
{Jt>
..,0'
co
;:
;:;
o
::l
:i>
0-
-g
'"
<.
rt>
.o.,
Risk finding <.
rt>
..,
:>
'"
'"
~"
(/)
'<
o
n;
Human -centered accident-prevention technology for transportation safety 3
'"
.j:>.
FIGURE 3.4. Situation and intention recognition for risk finding and avoidance project. \Q
50 Inagaki
driver's fatigue and drowsiness are also estimated by applying a chaos theoretic
method (Shiomi and Hirose, 2000) to a driver's voice during verbal communi-
cation. Driver modelling in (2) adopts a Bayesian network approach, as in the
case of the Behaviour-Based Human Environment Creation Technology project.
Some mathematical and information processing methods are under development
in (3) for machine learning, recognition of traffic environments and human vision
enhancement.
The methods in (1) to (3) give messages (or warnings) to the driver, when
they determine that the driver's situation recognition and intention may not be
suitable to a given traffic condition. If the driver responds quickly to the messages,
the potential risk shall be diminished successfully. If the driver fails to accept
or respond to the messages in a timely manner, on the other hand, accidental or
incidental risks may grow. Research topics in (4) investigate such situations. They
aim to develop an adaptive automation that can support drivers at various levels of
automation, which shall be discussed in the next section.
A H
B c D
C is coming from behind on the left lane . Based on the understanding of driver's
intention and the approach of vehicle C, the computer puts its safety control
function into its armed position in preparation for a case when the driver chooses
a wrong timing to execute an action (viz. steering the wheel) due to improper
interpretation of the traffic environment. Now the driver, who has seen that a
very fast vehicle B almost passed him on the left, begins to steer the wheel to the
left, failing to notice vehicle C (Fig. 3.5). The computer immediately activates
the safety control function to make the wheel either slightly heavy to steer (soft
protection) or extremely heavy to steer (hard protection). The soft protection is
for correcting the driver 's interpretation of the traffic environment, and the hard
protection is for preventing a collision from occurring. The computer takes the
steering authority from the driver partially in cases of soft protection and fully
in cases of hard protection.
Example 2: Suppose that the driver of the host vehicle H wants to make a lane
change to the left, because the lead vehicle A drives rather slowly. When glancing
at the rear view mirror, the driver noticed that faster vehicles, C and D, are
coming from behind on the left lane (Fig . 3.5). By taking several looks at the
side mirror, the driver tries to find a precise timing to cut in. In the meantime,
based on the observation that the driver looked away many times in a short
period of time , the on-board computer determined that the driver has formed an
intention of changing lane and that he might not be able to pay full attention
to the lead vehicle A. The computer then puts its safety control function into
its armed position in preparation for a deceleration of the lead vehicle A. If the
lead vehicle A did not make any deceleration before the host vehicle's driver
completes a lane change, the computer will never activate the safety control
function and will put it back into a normal standby position. On the other hand,
if the computer detected a rapid deceleration of the lead vehicle A while the
driver is still looking for a timing to make a lane change, it immediately activates
its safety control function, such as an automatic emergency brake .
After Sheridan (1992), Inagaki et al. (1998) and Inagaki and Furukawa (2004).
level 6.5, was first introduced in Inagaki et al. (1998) with twofold objectives:
(1) to avoid automation surprises possibly induced by automatic actions and (2)
to implement actions indispensable to assure systems safety in emergency. When
the LOA is positioned at level 6 or higher, the human may not be in command.
Generally speaking, it would be desirable, philosophically and practically, that
human is maintained as the final authority over the automation. However, as can
be seen in Examples 1 and 2, there are cases in which automation may be given
decision authority (Inagaki, 2006).
t
State IV: Hyper-normal and excited state. Target vehicle ~
approaches
C~:::>
State III: Normal and vigilant state
(the best state for safe driving).
emerge.ncy
-brakmg
State II+: Normal and relaxed state with
moderate level of trust in the
t , ! alert
automation.
~ CJL:)
State II : Normal and relaxed state, with experiencing
complete faith in the automation.
in State II+ or III, he or she applies the emergency brake himself or herself either
in T2+ or T3 seconds, respectively. (4) In State IV, the driver panics and fails to
take any meaningful actions to attain car safety. T2, T2+ and T3 are treated as
random variables with different means.
compared to control theoretic models such as those discussed by Weir and Chao
(2005) and Juergensohn (2005). However, the models are expanding their descrip-
tion capabilities and applicability.
Drivers must be provided with necessary and sufficient supports by machine
or automation. Intention understanding and communication play important roles
in realising such meaningful support functions. If the driver fails to understand
the intention of machine intelligence, an automation surprise may happen. If the
machine does not recognise the driver's intention, its 'good support' may be an-
noying to the driver. The topic of intent inference attracts keen interests in Japan as
well as the rest of the world. Although no discussion could be made in this paper,
a fuzzy association system model with case-based reasoning has been developed
for recognising human intention through monitoring his or her behaviour in the
'Humatronics' project in Japan for the safety of drivers and pedestrians (Umeda
et al., 2005; Yamaguchi et al., 2004).
In spite of its usefulness, an intention understanding approach may have some
limitations, at least at the present time. The second project described in this pa-
per, for instance, sometimes found difficulty in understanding the intention of the
driver from his or her behaviour. Norman (1988) has distinguished seven stages
of action, in which the first-four stages are (1) forming the goal, (2) forming the
intention, (3) specifying an action and (4) executing the action. The project tries to
infer the intention of the driver by catching some 'precursor' to the action (recall,
Fig. 3.3) that may come between stages (3) and (4). Some of problems observed
in the project are as follows: (a) No useful precursor may exist for some intended
actions, which might also be individual-dependent. (b) The driver may form a goal
for a very immediate future in dynamically changing environment, which makes
it hard either to infer its associated intention or to identify in a timely manner a
driver support that is appropriate for the intention. (c) When the driver's action
was corrected by the machine, the driver is likely to perceive that the machine did
not understand his or her intention at all, although the action needed to be cor-
rected because its execution timing did not match the traffic environment. Proactive
safety technologies usually assume machine intelligence. Failure in mutual under-
standing of intentions between the driver and the machine intelligence may bring
various inconveniences, such as automation surprises, distrust and overtrust. The
second project is now trying to challenge these problems via a context-dependent
adjustment of LOA as well as designing human interface that enables the driver
to (a) understand the rationale why the automation thinks so, (2) recognise in-
tention of the automation, (3) share the situation recognition with the automa-
tion and (4) perceive limitations of automation's functional abilities (Inagaki,
2006).
References
Akamatsu, M. (2003). Measurement technologies for driver and driving behavior. In Pro-
ceedings oflEA 2003 (CD-ROM).
Akamatsu, M. and Sakaguchi, Y. (2003). Personal fitting driver assistance system based on
driving behavior model. In Proceedings of the lEA 2003 (CD-ROM).
Akamatsu, M., Sakaguchi, Y. and Okuwa, M. (2003). Modeling of driving behavior when
approaching an intersection based on measured behavioral data on an actual road. In
Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 47th Annual Meeting (CO-
RaM).
Amiditis, A., Lentziou, Z., Polychronopoulos, A., Bolovinou, A. and Bekiaris, E. (2005).
Real time traffic and environment monitoring for automotive applications. In L. Macchi,
C. Re and P.C.Cacciabue (Eds.). Proceedings ofthe International Workshop on Modelling
Driver Behaviour in Automotive Environments (pp. 125-131).
Cacciabue, P.C. and Hollnagel, E. (2005). Modelling driving performance: A review of
criteria, variables and parameters. In L. Macchi, C. Re and P.C. Cacciabue (Eds.). Pro-
ceedings of the International Workshop on Modelling Driver Behaviour in Automotive
Environments (pp. 185-196).
Fuller, R. and Santos, J. (2002). Psychology and the highway engineer. In R. Fuller
and J. Santos (Eds.). Human Factors for Highway Engineers (pp. 1-10). Pergamon,
Amsterdam.
Furugori, S., Yoshizawa, N., Iname, C. and Miura, Y. (2003). Measurement of driver's
fatigue based on driver's postural change. In Proceedings ofthe SICE Annual Conference
(pp. 1138-1143).
Furugori, S., Yoshizawa, N., Iname, C. and Miura, Y. (2005). Estimation of driver fatigue
by pressure distribution on seat in long term driving. Review ofAutomotive Engineering,
26(1), 53-58.
Green, M. (2003). What causes 90% of all automobile accidents? Available at
http://www.visualexpert.com/accidentcause.html.
Hashimoto, K. (1984). Safe Human Engineering. Chuo Rodo Saigai Boushi Kyokai (in
Japanese).
Hollnagel, E. and Bye, A. (2000). Principles for modeling function allocation. International
Journal ofHuman-Computer Studies, 52, 253-265.
Inagaki, T. (2003). Adaptive automation: Sharing and trading of control. In E. Hollnagel
(Ed.). Handbook of Cognitive Task Design (pp. 147-169). Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah,
NJ.
Inagaki, T. (2006). Design of human-machine interactions in light of domain-dependence
of human-centered automation. Cognition, Technology and Work, 8,161-167.
Inagaki, T. and Furukawa, H. (2004). Computer simulation for the design of authority in
the adaptive cruise control systems under possibility of driver's over-trust in automation.
In Proceedings of the IEEE SMC Conference (pp. 3932-3937).
Inagaki, T., Moray, N. and Itoh, M. (1998). Trust self-confidence and authority in human-
machine systems. In Proceedings of the IFAC Man-Machine Systems (pp. 431-436).
3. Towards Monitoring and Modelling for Situation-Adaptive Driver Assist Systems 57
Itoh, M., Akiyama, T. and Inagaki, T. (2006). Driver behavior modeling. Part II: Detec-
tion of driver's inattentiveness under distracting conditions. In Proceedings of the DSC-
Asia/Pacific (CD-ROM).
Juergensohn, T. (2005). Control theory models of the driver. In L. Macchi, C. Re and
P.C. Cacciabue (Eds.). Proceedings of the International Workshop on Modelling Driver
Behaviour in Automotive Environments (pp. 37-42).
Miura, Y., Yoshizawa, N. and Furugori, S. (2002). Individual variation analysis of body
pressure distribution for long term driving simulation task. The Japanese Journal of
Ergonomics, 38(Suppl), 70-72.
Norman, D.A. (1988). The Psychology of Everyday Things. Basic Books, New York.
Panou, M., Bekiaris, E. and Papakostopoulos, V.(2005). Modeling driver behavior in EU and
international projects. In L. Macchi, C. Re and P.C. Cacciabue (Eds.). In Proceedings of
the International Workshop on Modelling Driver Behaviour in Automotive Environments
(pp.5-21).
Saad, F. (2005). Studying behavioural adaptations to new driver support systems. In L.
Macchi, C. Re and P.C. Cacciabue (Eds.). Proceedings of the International Workshop on
Modelling Driver Behaviour in Automotive Environments (pp. 63-73).
Sakakibara, K. and Taguchi, T. (2003). Biochemical measurement for driver's state based
on salivary components. In Proceedings of the lEA 2003 (CD-ROM).
Sakakibara, K. and Taguchi, T. (2005). Evaluation of driver's state of tension. In Proceedings
of the HCI International (CD-ROM).
Scerbo, M.W. (1996). Theoretical perspectives on adaptive automation. In R. Parasuraman
and M. Mouloua (Eds.). Automation and Human Performance (pp. 37-63). Lawrence
Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ.
Sheridan, T.B. (1992). Telerobotics, Automation, and Human Supervisory Control. MIT
Press, Cambridge, MA.
Shiomi, K. and Hirose, S. (2000). Fatigue and drowsiness predictor for pilots and air traffic
controllers. In Proceedings of the 45th Annual ACTA Conference (pp. 1-4).
Taguchi, T. and Sakakibara, K. (2005). Evaluation of driver's state of tension. Review of
Automotive Engineering, 26(2), 201-206.
Tango, F. and Montanari, R. (2005). Modeling traffic and real situations. In L. Macchi, C.
Re and P.C. Cacciabue (Eds.). Proceedings of the International Workshop on Modelling
Driver Behaviour in Automotive Environments (pp. 133-147).
Treat, J.R., Tumbas, N.S., McDonald, S.T., Shinar, D., Hume, R.D., Mayer, R.E., Stansifer,
R.L. and Castellan, N.J. (1979). Tri-level study of the causes of traffic accidents: Final
report volume 1. Technical report, Federal Highway Administration, US DOT.
Umeda, M., Yamaguchi, T. and Ohashi, K. (2005). Intention recognition system using
case-based reasoning. In Proceedings of the RISP International Workshop on Nonlinear
Circuit and Signal Processing (pp. 243-246).
Weir, D.H. and Chao, K.C. (2005). Review of control theory models for directional and speed
control. In L. Macchi, C. Re and P.C. Cacciabue (Eds.). Proceedings ofthe International
Workshop on Modelling Driver Behaviour in Automotive Environments (pp. 25-36).
Witt, G.J. (2003). SAfety VEhicle(s) Using Adaptive Interface Technology (SAVE-IT) Pro-
gram, DTRS57-02-R-20003. U.S. Department of Transportation.
Yamaguchi, T., Matsuda, S., Ohashi, K., Ayama, M. and Harashima, F. (2004). Humane
automotive system using driver and pedestrian intention recognition. In Proceedings of
the World Congress on Intelligent Transport Systems (CD-ROM).
II
Conceptual Framework and
Modelling Architectures
4
A General Conceptual Framework for
Modelling Behavioural Effects of
Driver Support Functions
lORAN ENGSTROM AND ERIK HOLLNAGEL
4.1 Introduction
In recent years, the number of in-vehicle functions interacting with the driver
has increased rapidly. This includes both driving support functions (e.g. anti-lock
brakes, collision warning systems, adaptive cruise control) and functions support-
ing non-driving tasks, e.g. communication and entertainment functions. Today,
many of these functions are also featured on portable computing systems, com-
monly referred to as nomadic devices. Moreover, in order to handle this growth in
diversity and complexity of in-vehicle functionality, several types of meta func-
tions for human-machine interface integration and adaptation have been proposed.
Such functions, often referred to as workload managementfunctions, are intended
to resolve potential conflicts between individual functions with respect to their
interaction with the driver (see Engstrom et al., 2004; Brostrom et al., 2006).
The term driver support functions will henceforth be used to refer to in-vehicle
functions that support what drivers do, whether related to driving or not.
The proliferation of driver support functions naturally changes the nature of
driving and the different types of functions may induce a variety of behavioural
effects. One general reaction to new driver support functions is that drivers change
their behaviour in various ways to incorporate the functions into the driving task,
an effect commonly referred to as behavioural adaptation (Smiley, 2000; Saad
et al., 2004). Another issue that has received much recent attention is the effects of
multitasking while driving, e.g. driver distraction due to interaction with in-vehicle
functions, passengers or other objects in the vehicle.
While a wide range of driver models exists, addressing different aspects of driver
behaviour, a generally agreed conceptual framework for describing behavioural ef-
fects of driver support functions is still lacking. Technological and methodological
development in this area is therefore generally made without a common concep-
tual basis. The objective of this paper is to outline some basic requirements for
such a conceptual framework as well as to propose a specific candidate. The main
starting point will be models based on the cognitive systems engineering tradition,
specifically the COCOMIECOM framework (Hollnagel et al., 2003; Hollnagel and
Woods, 2005). It should be stressed that the aim here is not to present a validated
61
62 Engstrom and Hollnagel
In order to derive the basic requirements for the intended conceptual framework, it
is necessary to first consider how it is intended to be used. As mentioned above, the
main purpose of the framework is not a validated model of driver behaviour. Rather,
the idea is that the framework should provide a common language to describe key
issues related to behavioural effects of in-vehicle functions. More specifically, the
framework should be applicable to (at least) the following problems:
elements of feed forward control (McRuer et aI., 1977; Donges, 1978). While these
types of models are useful for modelling manual lateral and longitudinal vehicle
control in constrained situations, they do not capture higher level aspects of driving
such as decision making, planning or motivation.
In more recent model, the same author proposes that the driver attempts to maintain
a certain level of task difficulty rather than a level of risk (Fuller, 2005; see also
Chapter 10 in this book). Finally, based on Damasio's concept of somatic markers
(Damasio, 1994), Vaa (Chapter 12 in this book) proposes that driver behaviour is
largely driven by emotional responses to risky situations.
Motivational models have been criticised for being too unspecific regarding
internal mechanisms and, as a result, being unable to generate testable hypotheses
(Michon, 1985). It could also be argued that the need to include motivational or
emotional aspects partly is an artefact of the limitations of information processing
models. Since 'cold' cognition automatically excluded 'hot' cognition (Abelson,
1963), something important was missing from these models. This is, however, not
an issue in models that pre-dates information processing models, such as Gibson
and Crooks (1938, see below).
The key elements of the proposed framework, the COCOM and ECOM models,
are then presented.
There are two basic forms of control: In feedback (or compensatory) control,
the controller performs corrective actions based on the deviation between a desired
outcome (the goal) and the actual state. The prototypical example of a feedback
control system is the thermostat. Another type of control is feed forward (or an-
ticipatory) control. In this case, control actions are based on predictions of future
states and, hence, proactive rather than reactive. Driving behaviour is generally a
mixture of feedback and feed forward control.
In engineering control applications, such as a thermostat or an Adaptive Cruise
Control system, the target values (i.e. the desired temperature and time gap re-
spectively) are generally determined beforehand by the engineer or set by the user.
However, when analysing human controlled behaviour, identifying the target, or
controlled variable, is often viewed as the key issue (Powers, 1998; Marken, 1986).
The indentification of the controlled variable is complicated by the fact that the
human controller, as mentioned above, seldom operates as an optimiser but rather
acts as a satisfier (Simon, 1955), thus tolerating a certain deviation from the target
value. In driving, time based safety margins (e.g. van der Horst and Godthelp,
1989) or risk thresholds (Summala, 1988) could be thought of as reference values
for the vehicle control loop (although this idea has not yet, to our knowledge, been
exploited in extisting models).
As described in the previous section, control theory has been widely applied
to the modelling of vehicle handling, in automotive and other domains (e.g. Weir
and McRuer, 1968; Donges, 1978). Control theory has also been applied to the
modelling of higher-level aspects of driving. For instance, many of the moti-
vational models reviewed above, especially the risk homeostasis model (Wilde,
1982), are based on control theoretical concepts. However, few existing models
allow for a unified representation of controlled behaviour at different levels of
the driving task. One existing modelling framework able to provide such descrip-
tions is the COCOMIECOM model (Hollnagel and Woods, 2005), which thus
has been selected as the main starting point for the proposed conceptual frame-
work. The next two sections describe the COCOM and ECOM models in more
detail.
considered part of the JCS if (I) it is considered important for the ability of the
JCS to maintain control and (2) it can be controlled by the JCS. Moreover, objects
satisfying (2) but not (I) may be included in the JCS if this facilitates the purpose
of the analysis (Hollnagel and Woods, 2005). For present purposes, the JCS of
interest is in most cases the joint driver-vehicle system (JDVS).
COCOM is a general model of how control is maintained by a JCS and is appli-
cable across a range of different JCS types on different levels of description. This
type of analysis makes minimal assumptions about internal cognitive processes
and focuses on behaviour and the dynamical interactions between the compo-
nents in the JCS, in particular on how the JCS maintains, or loses, control of a
situation .
A central concept in COCOM is the construct-action-event cycle. The con-
struct refers to what the controller knows or assumes about the situation in which
the action takes place. The construct is the basis for selecting actions and in-
terpreting information. The selected actions affect the process/application to be
controlled. This generates events that provide feedback on the effects of the
action which, together with external disturbances, modifies the construct and,
hence, the future action selection . An important property of this model is thus
that it accounts for both the feedback and feedforward aspects of control, i.e. ac-
tion selection is a function of both direct feedback and the predictions of future
events.
The main factors determining the level of control maintained by a JCS is pre-
dictability and available time. A key property of COCOM is that it offers an
explicit account of time. Figure 4.1 gives an illustration of the three main temporal
parameters involved: (I) Time to evaluate events (T E) , (2) time to select an action
or response (T s) and (3) time to perform an action (T p ; see Hollnagel and Woods,
External event I
... ~ disturbance
Events l ...........
feedback
TE =time to
evaluate
events
Tp =time to
perform an
action
FiGURE 4.1. The contextual control model (Hollnagel and Woods, 2005) .
70 Engstrom and Hollnagel
Targeting ----
Anti cipatory
control
Tracking
A key property of the model is that during normal (controlled) task performance,
the goals/targets for the control processes on a given layer are determined by the
control processes one layer up. In the driving domain, the tracking control refers to
the momentary, automated, corrections to disturbances, e.g. wind gusts . Regulat-
ing refers to more conscious processes of keeping desired safety margins to other
traffic elements. This determines the target values for the tracking control loops .
Monitoring refers to the control of the state of the joint vehicle-driver system rela-
tive to the driving environment. It involves monitoring the location and condition
of the vehicle, as well as different properties of the traffic environment, e.g. speed
limits. This generates the situation assessment that determines the reference for
the regulating layer. Finally, the targeting control level sets the general goals of
the driving task , which determines the objectives for the monitoring layer. The
functional characteristics of the four layers , in the context of driving, are described
in Table 4.1.
As illustrated in Fig. 4.2, tracking control is typically based on feedback (com-
pensatory control) while monitoring and targeting are mainly of the feed-forward
(anticipatory) type . The regulating layer may involve a mix of feedback and feed
forward control. The ECOM model provides an account of how goals at different
layers interact and how higher goals propagate all the way down to moment-to-
moment vehicle handling. Control tasks on different layers may also interfere with
each other and disturbances on lower layers may propagate upwards. For exam-
ple, looking for directions (monitoring) may disrupt visual feedback, which may
affect regulating and tracking control ; a sudden experience of slipperiness due to
ice on the road , manifested on the tracking level may modify higher-level goals
(e.g. increasing safety margins on regulating level) and even change the targets for
driving (e.g . choosing a different route) .
72 Engstrom and Hollnagel
4.5 Application
This section gives some examples of how the proposed framework can be applied
in the three problem domains stated in Section 2: (1) Characterisation of driver
support functions, (2) description of their behavioural effects and (3) reasoning
about the relation between behaviour/performance and road safety.
Targeting
Worklo ad
management
automated driving, the driver's task would be limited to targeting, e.g. setting the
destination, perhaps supported by monitoring of non-instrumented information
sources.
These are functions with the purpose to support regulatory control, for instance
helping the driver maintain adequate safety margins. This can be done , e.g. by
providing warnings when safety margins are about to be violated or otherwise
enhancing the perception of safety margins. Examples include forward collision
warning (FeW), lane departure warning (LDW) and night vision systems.
Direct effects are those that are intended by the system designers and implied by the
system's functional specification. For driving support functions, direct effects are
normally the intended performance enhancements on one or more control layers
(e.g. increased lane keeping performance for LDW and increased route-finding
ability for navigation support). By contrast, indirect effects are not intended by
the designers (and thus not implied by the functional specification). It should be
noted that indirect effects are not necessarily bad. For example, a potential positive
indirect behavioural effect of a lane departure warning system could be an increased
use of the turn signal.
In the following sections, some of the main types of behavioural effects
found in the literature are discussed from the perspective of the proposed frame-
work.
how the dynamical interaction between high- and low level driving goals can be
conceptualised by the present framework.
An important issue that has attracted much recent attention is how the time sharing
between driving and other tasks affects driving performance and safety. A typical
case of such multitasking is the use of different in-vehicle information functions
while driving. These tasks are often referred to as secondary tasks while driving
is considered the primary task. The effects of secondary tasks are often concep-
tualised in terms of distraction, which has been defined as 'attention given to a
non-driving related activity, typically to the detriment of driving performance' (ISO
TC22/SCI3/WG8 CD 16673). However, this type of terminology creates concep-
tual difficulties because 'driving-related activity' and 'driving performance' are
not further defined. For instance, reading a map on the navigation system display
should clearly be regarded as a 'driving related activity'. On the other hand, it
could at the same time distract the driver and cause degraded driving performance.
Related problems have also been noted in analysis of naturalistic driving data in
the recent 100 car study. As pointed out by Neale et al. (2005):
'Historically, driver distraction has been typically discussed as a secondary
task engagement. Fatigue, has also been described as relating to driver inatten-
tion. In this study, it became clear that the definition of driver distraction needed
to be expanded to a more encompassing 'driver inattention' construct that in-
cludes secondary task engagement and fatigue as well as two new categories,
'Driving-related inattention to the forward roadway' and 'non-specific eye glance'.
'Driving-related inattention to the forward roadway' involves the driver checking
rear-view mirrors or their blind spots. This new category was added after viewing
multiple crashes, near crashes and incidents for which the driver was clearly pay-
ing attention to the driving task, but was not paying attention to the critical aspect
of the driving task (i.e. forward roadway) at an inopportune moment involving a
precipitating factor.' (p. 6).
The proposed framework reconciles these conceptual difficulties in a quite
straightforward way. As suggested above, the driving task should not be seen
as a single activity, but rather as a set of multiple simultaneous and layered control
tasks. Thus, driving performance could be defined with respect to any of these
control tasks. For example, driving performance on the tracking level is associated
with the ability to keep the vehicle within acceptable safety margins. Similarly,
performance on the regulating level would be related to the ability to select appro-
priate safety margins based on a general situation assessment at the monitoring
level. This situation assessment may induce inattention to the forward roadway
(e.g. when checking mirrors), and could hence be described as a distraction on the
tracking and/or regulating layers. In general, distraction with respect to a given
control process (e.g. tracking) could thus be viewed in terms of interference by
another (driving- or non-driving related) control process, typically resulting in
degraded performance on the given control task. Historically, 'driver distraction'
4. A General Conceptual Framework for Modelling Behavioural Effects 77
often implicitly refers to interference with the tracking and/or regulating control
tasks (and this is probably the intended meaning of the definition cited above).
However, the present framework enables a more precise conceptualisation and
makes it possible to describe in more detail which tasks/control processes that are
affected in a given distraction scenario.
Another commonly used concept is mental workload. While distraction is de-
fined on the basis of attention allocation, driver mental workload refers to the
amount of resources that the driver needs to perform one or more tasks, relative
to a limited subjectively defined resource pool. As mentioned in section 4.3.2,
the concept has strong roots in the information processing paradigm (e.g. Moray,
1967). It should be noted that mental workload is not a necessary precondition
for distraction, since inappropriate attention allocation may be caused by low-
workload tasks as well, e.g. daydreaming, checking mirrors and looking at road
signs. One limitation of the traditional workload concept, based on the limited
capacity metaphor, is that it does not account well for the dynamics of self-paced
driving. Based on the COCOMIECOM model, a more dynamic view of workload,
viewing the driver as an active agent, can be outlined where the spare resources
for a control process can be viewed in terms of the difference between total time
available and the total time needed to perform the control loop (see Fig. 4.1). Thus,
for example, if the available time for the tracking control loop is reduced, e.g. due
to time sharing with another visually demanding task (e.g. entering a mobile phone
number) the driver can gain time by reducing speed. The mechanisms that drive
this type of adaptive behaviour can, again, be understood in terms of the balance
between higher-level goals (e.g. the desire to arrive in time, i.e. targeting) and
lower level goals (e.g. to keep acceptable safety margins, i.e. regulating).
In order to further illustrate the potential benefits of the proposed framework
compared to traditional information processing models, it is useful to take a closer
look at some empirical data from the HASTE EO-funded project. As part of
the project, a set of parallel experiments were conducted in different sites across
Europe, with the specific objective to investigate systematically the effects of visual
and cognitive load on driving performance and state (see, e.g. Engstrom et aI.,
2005a; Victor et aI., 2005; Jamson and Merat, 2005; Markkula and Engstrom, 2006;
Ostlund et al., 2004). In short, the HASTE results showed that visual time sharing
induced increased lane position variation, increased number of large steering wheel
reversals, reduced speed and increased headway to lead vehicles. By contrast, time
sharing with purely cognitive tasks (i.e. tasks that require no visual interaction)
did not interfere with tracking control at all, a result consistent with the meta-
analysis of mobile-phone studies made by Horrey and Wickens (2004). Rather,
results from HASTE indicate that cognitively loading tasks lead to significantly
improved tracking control in terms of reduced lane keeping variation compared to
baseline driving (e.g. Engstrom et aI., 2005a; Jamson and Merat, 2005), an effect
that has also been documented in other studies (e.g. Brookhuis et aI., 1991). This
increased lane keeping performance for cognitive tasks was also accompanied by
a concentration of gaze towards the road centre (Victor et aI., 2005), an effect also
found in previous studies (e.g. Harbluk and Noy, 2002; Recarte and Nunes, 2003)
78 Engstrom and Hollnagel
4. The ability to adopt safety margins that are appropriate to (1) and (2), based on
(3) (determines performance on the regulation layer).
5. The effort spent on the control tasks on the different layers (2-4).
Based on this view, it is clear that performance degradation on one control layer
does not automatically increase accident risk. For example, reduced tracking per-
formance (e.g. due to visual distraction) is most risky if not properly compensated
for on the regulating layer (e.g. by reducing speed). Thus, risk (as a function of
performance) must be understood in terms of the relation between performance
on the different layers, where inadequate adaptation to the current driving condi-
tions and ones own driving skills could be hypothesised to be a critical factor. A
typical example of this is drunk driving, where alcohol is well known to induce
overestimation of the own performance driving capability. Thus, in this case, the
erroneous safety margin setting (on the regulation layer) is due to the cognitive
impairment induced by the drug. This line of reasoning also applies to individ-
ual differences with respect to risk taking. For example, the over-involvement of
young male drivers in accidents could be understood as in terms of overestimation
of driving performance combined with a higher propensity for sensation seeking
(i.e. different goals on the targeting level), leading to inadequate safety margin
settings. Yet another example is run-off-road accidents due to slippery roads. In
this case, the erroneous safety margin setting (reflected, e.g. in too high speed
in a curve) is due to an erroneous situation assessment on the monitoring layer,
which affects the regulatory level and finally induces instability and breakdown of
the tracking control. As discussed in the previous section, there is evidence that
cognitively loading tasks, such as phone conversation, impairs the ability to set ap-
propriate safety margins and adapt accordingly. However, the safety consequences
of this are still unknown.
It is very difficult to determine whether adequate adaptation has been achieved
in a particular situation, especially in terms of quantitative driving performance
metrics. One potential approach is to look for violations ofsafety margins. Possible
metrics of such violations include the amount of involuntary lane departures, min-
imum TLCITTC value, the amount of TLC/TTC values below some critical value
(van der Horst, 1990), or the total time spent below the critical value (time-exposed
TTC-TET; Minderhoud and Bovy, 2001). However, a basic problem with this
is that the accepted safety margins generally differ substantially between drivers
(and possibly also varies over time for an individual driver). Moreover, there is
yet no hard empirical data showing how these metrics relate to actual accidents.
Other key factors related to accident risk are expectancy and predictability (Victor,
2005). The ability to predict is central to remain in control and the occurrence of
unexpected events increases the risk for losing control. An issue of key impor-
tance is the ability of the driver to regain control. This is more difficult if the loss
of control also involves the loss of goals (e.g. on the regulating level).
While the COCOMIECOM framework is suitable for describing and reasoning
about these issues, it is clear that accident causation cannot be explained only in
terms of inadequate adaptation, or reduced predictability, but rather in terms of the
80 Engstrom and Hollnagel
to the traditional information processing models, which tend to view the human
as a passive receiver of information, subject to overload if the limited capacity is
exceeded. Another key property of the framework is the characterisation of driving
performance in terms of hierarchical control. The idea of hierarchical driver models
is certainly not new and the ECOM does not contradict existing models such as,
e.g. Alexander and Lunenfeld (1986) and Michon (1985). However, the added
value brought by ECOM is that it offers an account for how the different layers are
related, e.g. how a change in a high-level goal (such as realising that one is late to
the airport) can change performance on lower control layers (such as reducing the
accepted safety margins).
As stressed throughout this paper, COCOMIECOM are functional models that
make very few assumptions about internal cognitive structures and processes. For
present purposes, these models provide the starting point for a general conceptual
framework, based on which more specific models can be developed. Such models
should include both functional models as well as more detailed structural models
of the cognitive/neural mechanisms underlying behavioural effects of driver sup-
port functions (see, e.g. Victor, 2005 for a comprehensive review of models of the
latter type). Hence, functional and structural cognitive models should be viewed
as complementary and useful for different purposes. In addition to the further de-
velopment of more detailed driver models, the proposed conceptual framework is
a suitable starting point for framing hypotheses for empirical work on behavioural
effects of driving support functions. It is also useful as a common language in
industrial development of driver support functions, which helps maintaining the
focus on the purpose of the functions (e.g. how they should change driver be-
haviour), rather than their technological implementation (e.g. what type of sensors
are used).
References
Abelson, R.P. (1963). Computer simulation of "hot" cognition. In S.S. Tomkins & S Messick
(Eds.). Computer simulation ofpersonality. John Wiley and Sons, New York.
Alexander, G.J. and Lunenfeld, H. (1986). Driver expectancy in highway design and traffic
operations. US Department of Transportation. Report No: FHWA-TO-86-1.
Amditis, A., Polychronopoulos, A., Belotti, F. and Montanari, R. (2002). Strategy plan
definition for the management of the information flow through an HMI unit inside a car.
e-Safety Conference Proceedings, Lyon.
Bloch, H., Chemama, R., Gallo, A., Leconte, P., Le Ny, 1.-F. and Postel, 1. et aI., (Eds.)
(1999). Grand Dictionnaire de La Psychologie. Larousse, Paris.
Brookhuis, K.A., de Vries, G. and de Ward, D. (1991). The effects of mobile telephoning
on driving performance. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 23(4), 309-316.
Brostrom, R., Engstrom, 1., Agnvall, A. and Markkula, G. (2006). Towards the next gener-
ation intelligent driver information system (lDIS): The Volvo Cars Interaction Manager
concept. In Proceedings of the 2006 ITS World Congress, London.
Cacciabue, P.C. and Hollnagel, E. (2005). Modelling driving performance: A review of
criteria, variables and parameters. In L. Macchi, C. Re and P.C. Cacciabue (Eds.). Pro-
ceedings of the International Workshop on Modelling Driver Behaviour in Automotive
Environments. Ispra, Italy.
82 Engstrom and Hollnagel
Recarte, M.A.and Nunes, L.M. (2003). Mental workload while driving: Effects on visual
search, discrimination, and decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 9.
Saad, F., Hjalmdahl, M., Canas, 1., Alonso, M., Garayo, P. and Macchi, L. et aI., (2004).
Literature review of behavioural effects. Deliverable 1.2.1, AIDE Integrated Project,
Sub-project 1. IST-I-507674-IP.
SAVE-IT (2002). Safety vehicle( s) using adaptive interface technology (SAVE-IT) Program,
DTRS57-02-20003. US DOT, RSPSNolpe National Transportation Systems Center (Pub-
lic Release of Project Proposal), www.volpe.dot.gov/opsad/saveit/index.htmI.
Salvucci, D.D. (2001). Predicting the effects of in-car interface use on driver performance:
An integrated model approach. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 55,
85-107.
Shinar, D. (1993). Traffic safety and individual differences in drivers' attention and infor-
mation processing capacity. Alcohol, Drugs and Driving, 9, 219-237.
Smiley, A. (2000). Behavioural adaptation. Safety and Transportation Systems Research
Record, 1724, 47-51.
Simon, H.A. (1955). A behavioural model of rational choice. The Quarterly Journal of
Economics, LXIX, 99-118.
Summala, H. (1985). Modeling driver behavior: A pessimistic prediction? In Evans L,A"
Schwing R.C. (Eds.). Human Behaviour and Traffic Safety. Plenum Press, New York,
pp.487-525.
Summala, H. (1988). Risk control is not risk adjustment: The zero-risk theory of driver
behaviour and its implications. Ergonomics, 31(4),491-506.
van der Horst, R. (1990). A time based analysis of road user behaviour in normal and
critical encounters. PhD Thesis. Delft University of Technology, The Netherlands.
van der Horst, R., Godthelp, H. (1989). Measuring road user behaviour with an instrumented
car and an outside-the-vehicle video observation technique. Transportation Research
Record # 1213, Washington DC: Transportation Research Board, 72-81.
Weir, D.H., McRuer, T.M. (1968). A theory of driver steering control of motor vehicles.
Highway Research Record, 247, 7-39.
Wickens, C.D. (2002). Multiple resources and performance prediction. Theoretical Issues
in Ergonomics Science, 3(2), 159-177.
Victor, T.W. (2005). Keeping eye and mind on the road. PhD Thesis. Uppsala University,
Sweden.
Victor, T.W., Harbluk, 1.L. and Engstrom, 1. (2005). Sensitivity of eye-movement measures
to in-vehicle task difficulty. Transportation Research Part F, 8, 167-190.
Wilde, GJ.S. (1982). The theory of risk homeostasis: Implications for traffic safety and
health. Risk Analysis, 2, 209-225.
5
Modelling the Driver in Control
BJORN PETERS AND LENA NILSSON
5.1 Introduction
Modelling driver behaviour can be done with different purposes. One obvious aim
could be to provide a model of what drivers actually do and explain observed be-
haviour with or without support systems. Other objectives can be to discriminate
between safe and unsafe driving. Furthermore, a model can be used to imple-
ment a dynamic real-time control of driver support functions (see Chapter 6). The
following text is written in line with the first objective.
Safe driving requires that the driver is in control of the vehicle and the driving
situation. Driver support systems aim to help the driver and facilitate driving in
one way or the other. Support systems should be a response to identified existing
or potential problems related to the driving task. New technology like ADAS and
IVIS can facilitate driving but it could, on the other hand, make driving even
more complex and demanding. Solving one problem could be done at the cost of
introducing another. Thus, we need to understand driving task demands, driver
behaviour and how support systems can influence the driver's control and safety.
Here a broad overview of different theoretical approaches is given with focus
on control and safety. The use of a cognitive systems engineering approach is
advocated to investigate pros and cons of driver support systems. In the end, an
example is given on how findings from an experiment with a joystick controlled
car can be interpreted.
Driving is one of the most complex and safety critical everyday tasks in modern
society (Groeger, 2000). Driving a car is complex in the sense that it requires
the driver to employ a wide range of abilities in order to interact with a complex
environment and to manage the driving task demands. Driving is dynamic as the
demands can change back and forth from very low to extremely high, sometimes
within fractions of a second. When demands are high, driving is carried out in a
85
86 Peters, VTI and Nilsson, VTI
force-paced fashion; while as the demands are low, it can be performed in a more
self-paced manner. Normal driving can be considered as a cognitively motivated
and controlled task. Cognition is here used in the pragmatic sense as defined by
Neisser (1976), that is, cognition in context. A motive for this cognitive stance
can be found in Michon's (1985) visionary talk on driver behaviour modelling
' . .. the distinctly hierarchical cognitive structure of human behaviour in the traffic
environment . . . '. However, a cognitive approach does not imply that the driver's
perceptual and psychomotor abilities are to be neglected. Thus, driving can be
viewed as a cognitive task of control in a context perceived through the senses and
manipulated with control actions based on unconscious (automated) or conscious
decisions. This cognitive approach has been applied , for example , in adaptive
control models, control theory and cognitive systems engineering in order to model
driver behaviour.
Cognitive
* mem OlY
21'
* attent on
Perceptual * decision
* vision
* hearing
* touch ~
* proprioception ----v
Physical
* size
* reach
*force ~
* endurance ~
FIGURE 5.1. Humanabilities em-
ployed by the driver.
5. Modelling the Driver in Control 87
Skill based
Strategical
Driving
Task Tactical
Demands I - - - -I -- - - -+-____
Control
FiGURE 5.2. IVIS and ADAS can influence both driving task demands and driver behaviour
(from Nilsson et al., 2001) .
With the hierarchical structuring of both task and behaviour, it becomes evident
that time is an aspect of driving that should be considered. Time constraints are im-
plicitly different for the three task levels, even if time is not specifically addressed
in the hierarchical control model. The following approximate time frames apply:
lOs or more for tasks at strategic level, between I and lOs at tactical level and less
than I s at control level. Finally, it should be noted that more complicated driver
tasks, for example, performing route planning while driving or mobile phoning
during driving may require knowledge-, rule- and skill-based actions in combina-
tion . It is not simply the time requirements that distinguish the different levels of
tasks, but also the requirements of attention, workload and the consequences of
mistakes, etc. The hierarchical control models are functional models, which do not
specifically include motivational aspects and should be classified as adaptive con-
trol models . Furthermore, they do not explicitly consider the context, for example,
controller's influence on the system to be controlled (Hollnagel, 2000) .
Stimulus
Environment
1
I
Decision ~
1 1
Perception .I
~ J ~-------------..!
FIGURE 5.3. Open loop - closed system versus closed loop - open system adapted from
Jagacinski and Flach (2003).
By closing the loop, the cause - effect relation between stimulus and response
loses its meaning. In a closed-loop system the stimulus and response are tightly
linked and there is no clear distinction between what is cause and what is effect.
This restructuring was done to illustrate that the stimuli are as much determined
by the actions as the actions are determined by the stimuli (Jagacinski and Flach,
2003). In this way we have a closed-loop open system, as the environment is
included and the interdependencies between the controller and the environment
are considered. This view can be useful when modelling driver behaviour and, in
particular, driver behaviour adaptation and driver support systems.
Cognitive Systems Engineering (CSE) put cognition in context by applying an
overall systems view. CSE decomposes complex tasks along two dimensions ab-
stract - concrete and whole - part (Jagacinski and Flach, 2003). In this way the
focus is shifted from internal functions of either humans or machines to the ex-
ternal function making up a joint cognitive system including both the controller
and the controlled system (Hollnagel and Woods, 2005). The driver will be viewed
as a controlling system and the car as a technical system to be controlled. In this
perspective the car will constitute the primary context for the driver. As more of the
context is included, the system will expand as a multi -layered functional descrip-
tion. The system boundaries have to be defined according to the purpose of the
analysis. The two most important concepts in Control Theory are the open/closed
system and the open/closed loop control. Jagacinski and Flach (2003), among oth-
ers (e.g. Hollnagel and Woods, 2005; Hollnage12002), have further developed the
concepts of control theory. Jagacinski and Flach have also provided some quan-
titative tools based on control theory that can be used to capture and understand
also qualitative aspects of human performance, for example, driving behaviour and
driver support systems.
External _ _-----,
disturbances
Lateral
position
Heading
angle
Desired
path
FIGURE 5.4. A three-level servo-control model of steering (from McRuer et al., 1977).
described above cannot successfully cope with driver tasks other than follow-
ing straight and smoothly curved roads. The model needs to be better integrated
with the guiding visual environment as described by, for example, Gibson (1966).
Michon concluded that 'The two fields - perception and vehicle control - are
still lacking a theoretical integration. Combining them would constitute a major
breakthrough, ... ' .
Most of the models discussed so far are basically mechanistic, as they do not
recognise the need of higher order cognitive abilities. The interaction between the
driver and the environment is not explicitly included in the model but rather seems
to be an implicit presumption. A useful model needs to be better connected with
the context. The lack of context in cognition was addressed by Neisser. Neisser
(1976) criticised the concept of direct perception (Gibson, 1966) by stating that
to see is not just to perceive but also to interpret and understand on a conscious
level. Neisser (1976) proposed a cognitively driven model of perception called
the perceptual cycle, which includes the interaction between the observer and the
environment. He introduced what he called anticipatory schemata that prepare and
control our perception. Neisser (1976) further meant that human control works in a
way similar to the perceptual cycle. Thus, to control a system, the controller has to
have a model of the system to be controlled. The importance of a control-guiding
model can also be understood in the light of 'The law of requisite variety' (Ashby,
1956), which states in principle that the variety of the controller should match the
variety of the system to be controlled. Thus, the controller's understanding of the
system that is being controlled will determine the actual control actions. In other
words the driver's mental model of the vehicle, other drivers, road condition, etc.,
will determine the driver's control behaviour.
Hollnagel and Woods (2005) described a cyclical model of control, the basis
of on the principles of Neisser's perceptual cycle. Hollnagel's cyclical control
model was used as the basis for the Contextual Control Model, which describes in
general terms how performance depends on perceiving feedback events, interpret-
ing and modification of current understanding, selection and execution of actions.
Driver control behaviour can be described as shown in Fig. 5.5, which is based on
Hollnagel's cyclical model of control (Chapter 4).
The control cycle is divided into three phases: perception, decision and action.
The control cycle is cognitively initiated by the driver depicted with the arrow
coming out of the driver's head. The driver's mental model of the system to be
controlled and the environment will guide the search for information during the
perceptual phase. The perceived situation is compared to a reference value defined
by the driver. The comparison is followed by a cognitive phase. During this phase
a decision will be made on the basis of the difference between reference value and
current situation, that is, the error. The aim will usually be to minimise the error.
This cognitive phase is followed by an action phase during which an appropriate
94 Peters, VII and Nilsson, VTI
r'ITeA~
contextual control model.
action is selected and carried out. This action influences the environment depicted
by the outward arrow. Once the action is carried out, the driver searches and per-
ceives the effect of the action together with possible external events and the circle
is closed. The result of the action phase is also fed back to the driver and will
change the driver's mental model of the control loop and the system under con-
trol. In other words it is previous experience and outcome of the driver's actions
on the controlled system that will form and develop his or her mental models.
The three phases are described as three distinct entitie s but in reality the phases
might be overlapping and not separated as might appear from the figure. How-
ever, in principle the three phases are different in character. This model of driver
control provide s a foundation to capture the dynamics in driving, for example ,
compensatory closed-loop and anticipatory open-loop driving .
In relation to the discussed control model it can be interesting to consider the
dual control problem: The driver should both determin e the system status and at
the same time control it (Jagacinski and Flach, 2003). The driver occasionally has
to get out of control in order to maintain control. This was described by Weinberg
and Weinberg (1979) as the fundamental regulator paradox: The lesson is easiest
to see in terms ofan expe rience common to anyon e who has ever driven on an icy
road. The driver is trying to keep the carfrom skidding. Toknow how much steering
is required, she must have some inkling of the road' s slickness. But ifshe succeeds
in completely preventing skids, she has no idea how slippery the road really is.
Good drivers. expe rienced Oil icy roads, will intentionally test the steering from
time to time by 'jiggling ' to cause a small amount of skidding. By this technique
they intentionally sacrifice the perfect regulation they know they cannot attain in
any case. III return. they receive information that will enable them to do a more
reliable. though less pe rfect job. Unintentional skidding will, of course, provide
sufficient information to the driver on how to adapt the driving behaviour in order
to overcome the slippery road condition.
5. Modelling the Driver in Control 95
I
,I
I
I
I
,/
,./
,,/'.
.'
FIGURE 5.6. The concept of field of safe travel (from Gibson and Crooks [1938] published
with permission of The American Journal of Psychology).
96 Peters, VTI and Nilsson, VTI
adaptation. The driver can choose from several driving strategies. This is a view that
can help to better understand the mechanism behind the anticipatory behaviour. Van
der Hulst (1999) also meant that driving is a task that allows for pace adjustments
by means of adjustments in speed and other safety margin. The driver's choice
of speed and safety margins will determine the time available to react to relevant
changes in the environment. Van der Hulst thereby connected Gibson and Crooks's
'field of safe travel' with the Ashby's 'law of requisite variety'. Summala (1985,
1988) proposed that safety margins could be operationally defined as distance-
or time-related measures like Time-to-line-crossing (TLC) and Time-to-collision
(TTC). The concept of safety margins can also be used to explain, at least partly,
accident causation. In-depth accident studies have shown that late detection is a
very common explanation given for collisions (Rumar, 1988). Late detection can
be described as violation of safety margins. Thus, time is critical for safe driving,
which will be discussed later.
However, driver behaviour is most likely determined by a set of concurrent
goals. The goals might also shift during a drive. Given the hierarchical structure
of the driving task and driver behaviour as described above, it seems likely that
the driver applies different goals for different levels of control. Thus, the driver
can at the same time drive to reach a destination in time, stick to the traffic rules
and avoid accidents. Goals for driving behaviour can also differ between drivers
and situations. Furthermore, goals for driving can be extended to incorporate, for
example, goals for life, skill for living, sensation seeking, pleasure, etc. (Hatakka
et aI., 2002). Thus, it seems likely that the driver has to find a balance between
different goals. The idea of balancing between different goals was applied by
Wilde (1982) in the risk homeostasis theory. Homeostasis is originally a term
used to describe a complex mechanism for maintaining metabolic equilibrium in
biological systems. Wilde meant that driver behaviour is guided by a target risk
level that is determined by a combination of subjective and objective risk. An
implication of Wilde's approach is that all actions taken to improve safety will
be neutralised by the driver - at least on an aggregated level. The consequences
of Wilde's theory led to considerable controversy and the theory has been re-
jected by several researchers (e.g. McKenna, 1982; Michon, 1985; Sanders and
McCormick, 1993). According to Fuller (2005, Paper 10), Wilde based his the-
ory on a misinterpretation of empirical findings made by Taylor (1964). Fuller
proposed instead that task difficulty homeostasis, which describes the dynamic
interaction between driving task demands and driver capability, could be used
as key-sub goals to describe driver behaviour. Fuller argues that the task diffi-
culty homeostasis overcomes some problems with the safety margin concept and
conforms to the hierarchical structuring of the driving task and driver behaviour
and risk homeostasis theory can be viewed as a special case. Other human be-
haviour researchers has shown how homeostasis can be used to understand how,
for example, emotions can influence behaviour (Damasio, 1999; see Vaa, 2005,
Paper 12). Thus, it seems likely that homeostasis is a mechanism that could be
explored further to understand how different goals interact and determine driver
behaviour.
5. Modelling the Driver in Control 97
Time considerations are crucial for the cognitive control cycle described above.
Hollnagel and Woods (2005) divided the control cycle in three different phases:
perception, decision and action. Time constraints can be incorporated into the
cognitive control cycle (see Fig. 5.7), slightly adapted from Hollnagel. Thus, speed,
road geometry, obstacles in the field of travel, sight conditions among a range of
other factors determine the time available for the control cycle. This time is labelled
Tu (usable time). The times needed to carry out the three phases (perceive, decide
and act) are labelled Tp , Td and Ta , respectively. In 'normal' driving Tp + Td + Ta
is less than Tu .
Depending on the situation, normal driving constitutes a combination of com-
pensatory and anticipatory control. The driver usually strives to balance between
compensatory and anticipatory driving. Anticipatory driving requires Tu to be
longer than (Tp + Td + Ta ) but as the time required comes close to what is avail-
able driving becomes more compensatory. If the total usable time is not sufficient,
performance will start to degrade. The control becomes more erroneous or slug-
gish and oscillatory (Jagacinski, 1977). Reducing speed is one way to gain time
and control. The model also depicts that the reason for deteriorated performance
can be attributed to prolonged perception, prolonged decision, prolonged action
or some combination of the three. In any case, the result will be that the used
time will be more than the usable time. All three phases are connected, meaning
that if one part requires less time than expected then there will be more time for
the remaining two phases. When traffic demands are low and the driver is ex-
perienced, evaluation, selection and even action require little time and there will
be plenty of time available. Time pressure is in this view a critical component
of driving behaviour when determining the driver's safety margins. Closely re-
lated to this is the concept of uncertainty. The driver will try to keep uncertainty
at an acceptable level to maintain pace control and safety margins (Godthelp,
external
events
FIGURE 5.7. Time and control in the cyclic control model (adapted from Hollnagel, 2002).
98 Peters, VTI and Nilsson, VTI
Milgram and Blauw, 1984; Wierwille, 1993). The driver can apply an intermittent
sampling strategy to cope with lack of time for the control loop. However, this
strategy can, if maintained, increase the level of uncertainty, depending on the
situation (Lee, 2006). Increased uncertainty and lack of anticipation will make
drivers vulnerable to accidents. The time concept in the control cycle described
above can also be applied to driver support systems. If the driver's mental model
of a support system is incorrect or incomplete, this misunderstanding can pro-
long the time needed to complete the control loop. If the support system provides
delayed or contradictory feedback and the driver is not guided to the right ac-
tion, then the driver might eventually lose control. Lost control can also be due
to a situation when the driver requires a long time to perceive what the system is
doing.
External events
and targeting loops as concentric circles with increasing diameters (see Fig. 5.8).
Thus, what Hollnagel described as the lowest level of control tracking will be the
outer circle representing the physical interaction with the interface to the vehicle 's
physical controls (e.g. steering wheel, pedals or various driver support systems).
Control goals are determined in the inner control loops and applied in the outer
control loops. That is to say that, for example in the targeting control loop the
goals are determined for the monitoring control loop. This flow is represented by
the outward-bound arrow-labelled goals at the top of Fig. 5.8. Feedback used to
modify and supervise the control is fed back from outer circles to inner control
circles represented by the in-bound arrow-labelled feedback at the top of Fig. 5.8.
The interaction between the driver and the physical environment is represented by
the two arrows-labelled external events and actions at the bottom of Fig. 5.8.
The driver can conduct an imaginary drive without physical interaction on the
targeting level. The driver can also, while driving , anticipate the possible outcome
of possible actions and let that determine the choice of action . However, even if the
cognitive skills are important, it is the lower level psychomotor skills that are the
foundation for safe driving (Ranney, 1997). Thus both cognitive and psychomotor
skills are needed for safe driving and furthermore the integrations and interaction
between the skills. The driver's mental model is, to a large extent, formed by the
experiences from driving - ' learning by doing' . Driver support can be provided
at different layers or levels of control and it seems likely that the ECOM can be
used to analyse and better under stand how different driver support systems can
influence driving behaviour.
100 Peters, VTI and Nilsson, VTI
(i.e., without hitting any cones). The joystick drivers performed worse than a group
of drivers matched with respect to age and driving experience. These drivers drove
a standard car. The joystick drivers hit more cones and produced higher lateral
acceleration forces despite driving their own individually adapted cars. All of the
identified problems with these joystick systems can be described and analysed in
terms of time-based safety margins.
Practical experiences have shown that it is a tedious task to learn to drive with this
type of control system (often implemented as a electro-hydraulic system). Thus, in
a follow-up simulator experiment an alternative joystick design was tested (Peters
and Ostlund, 2005). Time lags had been made similar to what is found in ordinary
car controls (steering wheel and pedals), steering and speed control had been made
more independent and active feedback was provided in the joystick lever. It was
found that the reduced time lag contributed substantially to make it easy to learn
to drive with this joystick. The separation of steering and speed control did not
as clearly improve performance but contributed somewhat to improved control.
The active feedback was not sufficiently tuned according to the individual drivers.
Thus, it was only drivers with unimpaired upper limb functions who could benefit
from the feedback.
In the example above it is obvious that the drivers were in need of support as
they could not drive a standard production car due to their physical impairment
(i.e., they were paralysed in their lower limbs and had impaired function in their
upper limbs). However, it can well serve as an example on how to understand
potential problems related to specifically ADAS and guide on how to resolve
them. What becomes obvious is that a driver in control depends on timely, sufficient
and intuitive feedback. Furthermore, integrated control functions with a high risk
of interference should be avoided. The human controller can often learn how to
compensate even badly designed support systems but a well-designed system will
both facilitate learning and ensure that the driver stays in control even in a critical
situation.
basic human abilities needed for the driving task was proposed as a useful model
of driver behaviour. Driver behaviour is also guided by a set of concurrent goals.
The homeostasis principle seems to be useful to understand the interrelationship
between different goals and driver behaviour. Human control seems to be very
sensitive to time. Time determines the margins for action. The cognitive control
cycle was described in terms of time. Finally the control cycle was expanded to
also consider hierarchical layers of control and the interaction with the system to
be controlled. The overall aim should be to ensure that the driver is in full control
even when driver support systems are installed. Well-designed support systems
are easier to learn and safer to use.
References
Allen, T.M., Lunenfeld, H. and Alexander, G.1. (1971). Driver information needs. Highway
Research Record, 366, 102-115.
Ashby, W.R. (1956). An introduction to cybernetics. Chapman and Hall Ltd., London.
Carsten, O. (2005). From driver models to modelling the driver: What do we really know
about the driver? In L. Macchi, C. Re and P.C. Cacciabue (Eds.). Modelling driver
behaviour in automotive environments. Office of Official Publiations of the European
Communities, Ispra, Italy.
Carver, C.S. and Schreier, M.F. (1982). Control theory: A useful conceptual framework for
personality - social, clinical and health psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 111-135.
Damasio, A.R. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of
consciousness. Harvest Book Harcourt Inc., San Diego, CA.
Engstrom, 1. and Hollnagel, E. (2005). Towards a conceptual framework for modelling
driver' interaction with in-vehicle systems. In L. Macchi, C. Re and P.C.Cacciabue (Eds.).
Modelling driver behaviour in automotive environments. Office of Official Publications
of the European Communities, Ispra, Italy.
Flach, 1.M. (1999). Beyond error: The language of coordination and stability. In P.A.
Hancock (Ed.). Handbook of perception and cognition: Human performance and er-
gonomics. Academic Press, New York.
Fuller, R.G.C. (1984). A conceptualisation of driving behaviour as threat avoidance. Er-
gonomics, 27, 1139-1155.
Fuller, R. (2005a). Control and affect: Motivational aspects of driver decision-making. In
L. Macchi, C. Re and P.C. Cacciabue (Eds.). Modelling driver behaviour in automotive
environments. Office of Official Publications of the European Communities, Ispra, Italy.
Fuller, R. (2005b). Towards a general theory of driver behaviour. Accident Analysis and
Prevention, 37,461-472.
Gibson, 1J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Houghton Mifflin, Bostan.
Gibson, 1.1. and Crooks, L.E. (1938). A theoretical field-analysis of automobile-driving.
The American Journal of Psychology, 51,453-471.
Godthelp, H., Milgram, P. and Blaauw, GJ. (1984). The development of a time-related
measure to describe driving strategy. Human Factors, 26, 257-268.
Groeger, 1.A. (2000). Understanding driving -Applying cognitive psychology to a complex
everyday task. Psychology Press, Taylor and Francis, Hove, UK.
Hatakka, M., Kesikinen, E., Gregersen, N.P., Glad, A. and Hernetkoski, K. (2002). From con-
trol of vehicle to personal self-control; broadening the perspectives to driver education.
Transport Research Part F, Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 201-215.
5. Modelling the Driver in Control 103
Hollnage, E. and Woods, D.D. (2005). Joint cognitive systems: Foundations of cognitive
systems engineering. Taylor and Francis, Boca Raton, FL.
Hollnagel, E. (2002). Time and time again. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 3,
143-158.
Jagacinski, R.I. (1977). A qualitative look at feedback control theory as a style of describing
behavior. Human Factors, 19(4),331-347.
Jagacinski, R.I. and Flach, 1.M. (2003). Control theory for humans-Quantitative ap-
proaches to modelling performance. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ.
Janssen, W.H. (1979). Routeplanning en geleiding: Een litteratuurstudie. Institute for per-
ception TNO, Soesterberg.
Lee, 1.D. (2006). Driving safety. In R.S. Nickerson (Ed.). Reviews of human factors and
ergonomics. Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, St. Monica, CA.
Mckenna, F.P. (1982). The human factor in driving accidents. An overview of approaches
and problems. Ergonomics, 25, 867-877.
Mcruer, D.T., Allen, R.W., Weir, D.H. and Klein, R.H. (1977). New results in driver steering
control models. Human Factors, 19,381-397.
Michon, J .A. (1985). A critical view of driver behavior models: What do we know, what
should we do? In L.A. Evans and R.C. Schwing (Eds.). Human behavior and traffic safety
(pp. 487-525). New York: Plenum, New York.
Michon, 1.A. (Ed.) (1993). Generic intelligent driver support-A comprehensive report on
GIDS. Taylor and Francis, London.
Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality: Principles and implications ofcognitive psychol-
ogy. W.H. Freeman and Company, New York.
Nilsson, L., Harms, L. and Peters, B. (2001). The effect of road transport telematics. In P-E.
Barjonet (Ed.). Traffic psychology today (1st ed., Vol. 1, pp. 265-285). Norwell: Kluwer
Academic Publishers.
Naatanen, R. and Summala, H. (1974). A model for the role of motivational factors in
driver' decision making. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 6, 243-261.
Ostlund, 1. and Peters, B. (1999). Joystick Equipped Cars' Maneuverability. VTI Medde-
lande No. 860A.
Peters, B. and Ostlund, J. (2005). Joystick controlled driving for drivers with disabil-
ities - A driving simulator experiment. Statens Vag och transportforskningsinstitut,
Linkoping,
Powers, W.T. (1998). Making sense ofbehavior - The meaning ofcontrol. Benchmark, New
Canaan, CT.
Ranney, T.A. (1994). Models of driving behaviour: A review of their evolution. Accident
Analysis and Prevention, 26, 733-750.
Ranney, T.A. (1997). Good driving skills: Implications for assessment and training. Work,
8,253-259.
Rasmussen, 1. (1986). Information processing and human-machine interaction: An ap-
proach to cognitive engineering. North-Holland, New York.
Reid, L.D. (1983). Survey of recent driving steering behaviour models suited to accident
investigations. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 15, 23-40.
Rumar, K. (1988). Collective risk but individual safety. Ergonomics, 31, 507-518.
Sanders, M.S. and Mccormick, EJ. (1993). Human factors and the automobile. In M.S.
Sanders and EJ. Mccormick (Eds.). Human Factors in engineering and design (7th ed.).
McGraw-Hill, New York.
Summala, H. (1985). Modeling driver behavior: A pessimistic prediction? In L. Evans and
R.C. Schwing (Eds.). Human behavior and traffic safety (pp. 43-65). New York: Plenum.
104 Peters, VTI and Nilsson, VTI
Summala, H. (1988). Risk control is not risk adjustment: The zero-risk theory of driver
behaviour and its implications. Ergonomics, 31, 491-506.
Taylor, D.H. (1964). Drivers' galvanic skin response and the risk of accident. Ergonomics,
7,439-451.
Vaa, T. (2005). Modelling driver behaviour on basis of emotions and feelings: Intelligent
transport systems and behavioural adaptations. In L. Macchi, C. Re and P.C. Cacciabue
(Eds.). Modelling driver behaviour in automotive environments. Office of Official Pub-
lications of the European Communities, Ispra, Italy.
Van Der Hulst, M. (1999). Adaptive control of safety margins in driving. Department of
Psychology. University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands.
Weinberg, G.M. and Weinberg, D. (1979). On the design of stable systems. Wiley, New
York.
Weir, D.H. and Chao, K.C. (2005). Review of control theory models for directional and speed
control. In L. Macchi, C. Re and PC. Cacciabue (Eds.). Modelling driver behaviour in
automotive environments. Office of Official Publications of the European Communities,
Ispra, Italy.
Wickens, C.D. (1992). Engineering psychology and human performance. New York, Harper
Collins.
Wiener, N. (1954). The human use ofhuman beings. Da Capo Series in Science. Pergamon
Press, Oxford.
Wierwille, W.W. (1993). Visual and manual demands of in-car controls and displays. In
B. Peacock and W. Karwowski (Eds.). Automotive ergonomics (pp. 229-320). London:
Taylor and Francis.
Wilde, GJ.S. (1982). The theory of risk homeostasis: Implications for safety and health.
Risk Analysis, 2, 209-225.
6
From Driver Models to Modelling
the Driver: What Do We Really Need
to Know About the Driver?
OLIVER CARSTEN
6.1 Introduction
The variety of models of the driving task is almost as numerous as the num-
ber of authors who have contributed the models. Part of this variety is due
to the different applications for which the models are intended and another
part is due to the part of the driving task they are intended to describe. Since
driving encompasses so many tasks and subtasks at different levels, often per-
formed by the driver simultaneously, it is perhaps no surprise that it is hard
to find any consensus in the literature on how the process of driving should be
modelled.
Here the focus is on creating a structured model that can be used in real time, in
particular by a driver assistance system to monitor driver state and performance,
predict how momentary risk is changing, anticipate problem situations and in
response to adjust the behaviour of in-vehicle information systems and driver
assistance systems and also adjust feedback to the driver. The driver model would
therefore be the major component of a larger model supervising the interaction
among driver, vehicle and the traffic and road environment. The starting point
is a review of existing models to identify elements that can be used to predict
momentary risk.
The literature on models of the driving task is very extensive, going back at least
as far as Gibson and Crooks' Field of Safe Travel Model (Gibson and Crooks,
1938). Yet, in spite of the considerable effort put into producing such models, few
of them have been validated as predictive tools, apart from specific and limited
aspects of the driving task, where for example mathematical representations of
car following are used in microsimulation models of traffic. Still less is it the
case that any generalised model has been used in a driving assistance system to
guide the operation of the system as it supports the driver. Yet the potential of an
assistance system that 'understood' the driver is huge: it could give feedback to
novices, assist elderly drivers in difficult situations, inform a driver when he or she
is fatigued and adapt the operation of the vehicle to the needs of each individual
driver.
105
106 Carsten
Two broad types of driver model can be distinguished in the literature. The first
type is descriptive models. These models attempt to describe parts or the whole
of the driving task in terms of what the driver has to do. The second major type is
motivational models, which aim to describe how the driver manages risk or task
difficulty. The first type can be further subdivided into a number of categories -
there are task models, adaptive control models and production models.
Time Constant
General
Long
Plans
Controlled
Environmental secs
----+I Action Patterns
Input
Automatic
Environmental msec
----+I Action Patterns
Input
d
External
Driver Disturbances
Remnant
Lateral
lateral Lateral Driver Steering - - - - - . ::~:I ....--.., Position, y
Driver Wheel Steering Steer
Position Position Operations
Operations Angle linkage Angle
Command on Functions
----+-.@~~ of Position
on Functions and Vehicle Heading
A ~y Error
of Heading Os Actuator 6 Angle.ljJ
YljJ
I Yy G
I
I
I
I
t
describe what the driver has to do in order to drive but they do not state ex-
plicitly how the quality of the driving performance is affected by the nature of
driver performing the tasks or by the nature of the driving being carried out. Risk
is only addressed implicitly in that a failure in performing a task is likely to lead
to a problem, but no guidance is afforded on what might cause such failure.
Maintain
Speed
Brake
(Stop)
FIGURE 6.3. Driver decision making on approach to an intersection (Kidd and Laughery,
1964).
Anticipatory
control
@
G=2
@
G=3
(J)
G=4
four-speed car. The action goals structure for changing gear is shown in Fig. 6.5.
Here Action Goal 3, for example, shows the process of shifting into neutral on
stopping.
But once again such a model, while perhaps serving a purpose as a detailed
description of the purposes for which a driver changes gear, does not help to ex-
plain why one driver may choose a higher gear than another in identical traffic
circumstances. So it does not tell us in full why a driver changes gear at a partic-
ular moment. Car manufacturers know that motivation affects gear selection and
therefore have produced cars that feature automatic gearboxes that can change
style from sedate and economical to aggressive and sporty.
a
Expected Utilities of
Action Alternatives
4
Perceptual Skills
e
Resulting Accident
Rate
Lagged
Feedback
on the interaction between task demands and driver capability, arguing that speed
choice in one major mechanism for the adjustment of task demands so that they
remain within capability. This latter model does not seek, however, to develop an
explanation of the major factors that determine capability; rather they are identified
in terms of broad groupings such as 'constitutional features' , that is, biological fac-
tors and 'human factors'. Focusing in particular on human factors, Rumar (1985)
describes the driving task in terms of information processing and introduces a
number of filters (physical, perceptual and cognitive) that can introduce errors. He
also incorporates such factors as motivation, experience, attention and expectation
in his model (see Fig. 6.9).
Personality Stimulus
Experiences Situation
Subj
Risk
Monitor
Reaction
Behavior
..., I
From an overall perspective, it can be seen that the motivational models introduce
more factors that permit prediction than the descriptive models. Thus, in theory,
they should be more subject to parameterisation and verification. However, such
empirical testing of the models has not generally taken place (see, e.g. Ranney
(1994), who argues that motivational models have normally not even been fully
specified let alone tested), and thus most of them remain as constructs rather than
as entities leading to the generation of rules and mathematical relationships.
o Experiences
o Motivation
o Perception
o Vigilance
o Desired action
o Arousal
o Experience
o Attention
o Expectation
Situation assessment
o
o Experience
Attitudes/ personality
o Naatanen and Summala: Personality
o Rumar: Motivation
Experience
o Naatanen and Summala: Experiences
o Rumar: Experience
o Fuller: Capability
Task demand
o Fuller (1984): Arousal
Situation awareness
o Naatanen and Summala: Perception
o Naatanen and Summala: Vigilance
o Rumar: Attention
o Rumar: Expectation
Intention
o Hollnagel: Goals/targets/plans
We thus end up with five major categories of driver capability, performance and
behaviour that are related to risk. They are as follows:
1. Attitudes/personality
2. Experience
3. Driver state (impairment level)
4. Task demand (workload)
5. Situation awareness
Task demand
(workload)
FIGURE 6.10. A causal structure for the categories of the driver model.
116 Carsten
decisions. In addition, given sensor technology, a model running in real time cannot
be omniscient about the environmental situation and may indeed make errors in
interpreting driver actions and capabilities. So it is sensible in using some kind
of risk factor, that is, the risk of a serious error or the risk of crashing as the
dependent variable. The risk factor could be used in real time to identify when a
driver assistance system might need to warn the driver or to intervene in order to
keep prevent performance from deteriorating drastically with consequent impact
on risk. A relationship with performance and risk is suggested in Fig. 6.11.
amount of driving. But the payoff would be the delivery of a truly intelligent
co-driver.
A major step in model development and verification is identifying candidate
parameters to be included in each of the categories. Here one can conceive of a
two-stage process in which first of all the model is developed as an offline tool and
then at a second stage an online, real-time version of the model is created to run on
board the vehicle as an intelligent co-driver, supervisor of interaction with IVIS
and ADAS systems and manager of those systems to adapt to the current driver
state and current situation.
The second stage is clearly quite ambitious: care would have to be taken to
ensure that such a model did not create safety risks by going into unanticipated
states or unstable loops. One approach to minimise this would be setting boundaries
on system flexibility. Another, more straightforward alternative for an on-board
version is the creation of a simple rule-based structure that echoes the 'full' model
but does not emulate it.
However, the potential ambition of the second stage does not negate the use-
fulness of the first stage. It could have wide application and utility as an offline
design tool for new in-vehicle systems (creating 'what if?' scenarios, i.e. a kind
of failure mode and effects analysis), as a tool for evaluating road designs (e.g. in
safety audit) and even as a component in micro simulation models. In the last role,
it could replace the current rather crude practice of representing driver behaviours
by means of sampling from a set of built-in distributions, so as to represent for
example desired speed, typical headway or reaction time.
The model can initially be populated with relationships derived from the lit-
erature. Thus, since Maycock et al. (1991) have quantified the relationship be-
tween, on the one hand, experience and age and, on the other hand, risk, it may be
feasible to introduce some rules derived from that study into the model. However,
what is really required is the confirmation of parameters, conditional relation-
ships and interactions from empirical studies designed to test the hypothesised
relationships.
likely to overtake than to detect the overtaking manoeuvre once it has started and
is too late to prevent. Here a system forearmed may mean a driver forewarned.
6.7 Conclusions
There are many driver models that are purely descriptive as opposed to being
predictive. Even the motivational models tend to be incomplete, addressing only
some of the driver factors that can elevate risk. A new structure has been proposed
here for a model that can be both verified and, if confirmed, applied in the long run to
monitor in real time the risk associated with the behaviour and performance of the
driver and to adjust feedback to the driver and tune the response of driver assistance
systems accordingly. It can be argued that, in order to produce a well-designed
advanced driver assistance system, particularly a complex multi-functional one,
such modelling is not only feasible but maybe even necessary. Otherwise, users
may reject such systems because they are not adapted to their needs.
References
Amditis, A., Polychronopoulos, A., Belotti, F. and Montanari, R. (2002). Strategy plan
definition for the management of the information flow through an HMI unit inside a car.
In Proceedings of the e-Safety Conference. Lyon.
Bellet, T., Bruyas, M.P., Tattegrain- Veste, H., Forzy, J.F., Simoes, A., Carvalhais, 1., Lock-
wood, P., Boudy, J., Baligand, B., Damiani, S. and Opitz, M. (2002). "Real-time" analysis
of the driving situation in order to manage on-board information. In Proceedings of the
e-Safety Conference. Lyon.
Brookhuis, K.A. and De Waard, D. (2003). On the assessment of criteria for driver im-
pairment: In search of the golden yardstick for driving performance. In Proceedings of
Driving Assessment 2003, the 2nd International Driving Symposium on Human Factors
in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design. Park City, Utah.
Boer, E. (2001). Behavioral entropy as a measure of driving performance. In Proceedings
of Driving Assessment 2001, International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in
Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design. Aspen, Colorado.
Bums., P.C., Parkes, A., Burton, S., Smith, R.K. and Burch, D. (2002). How dangerous is
driving with a mobile phone? Benchmarking the impairment to alcohol. TRL Report 547.
TRL, Crowthome, UK.
Carsten, O. (2004). Implications of the first set of HASTE results on driver distraction. In
Behavioural Research in Road Safety 2004: Fourteenth Seminar (pp. 100-109). Depart-
ment for Transport, London.
Department for Transport. (2005). Road casualties Great Britain 2004. The Stationery
Office, London.
Fuller, R. (1984). A conceptualization of driving behaviour as threat avoidance. Ergonomics,
27, 1139-1155.
Fuller, R. (2004). Towards a general theory of driver behaviour. Accident Analysis and
Prevention, 37,461-472.
120 Carsten
Gibson, J.1. and Crooks, L.E. (1938). A theoretical field-analysis of automobile driving.
American Journal of Psychology, 51,453-471.
Grayson, G.B. (1997). Theories and models in traffic psychology - A contrary view. In
T. Rothengatter and E. Carbonell Vaya (Eds.). Traffic and transport psychology: Theory
and application. Pergamon, Amsterdam.
Hollnagel, E., Ndbo, A. and Lau, LV. (2003). A systemic model for driver-in-control. In
Proceedings of Driving Assessment 2003, the 2nd International Driving Symposium on
Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design (pp. 87-91). Park
City, Utah.
Hurst, P.M., Harte, D. and Frith, W.1. (1994). The Grand Rapids dip revisited. Accident
Analysis and Prevention, 26, 647-654.
Jamson, A.H. (1999). Curve negotiation in the Leeds driving simulator: The role of driver
experience. In D. Harris (Ed.). Engineering psychology and cognitive engineering (Vol.
3, pp. 351-358). Ashgate, London.
Kidd, E.A. and Laughery, K.R. (1964). A computer model of driving behaviour: The
Highway Intersection Situation. Report VI-1843-V-I, Cornell Aeronautical Laboratories,
Buffalo, New York.
Maycock, G., Lockwood, C.R. and Lester, IF. (1991). The accident liability of car drivers.
Research Report 315. Transport and Road Research Laboratory, Crowthorne, UK.
Michon, lA. (1985). A critical review of driver behaviour models. In L. Evans and R.G.
Schwing (Eds.). Human behavior and traffic safety (pp. 485-520). Plenum Press, New
York.
McKnight, AJ. and Adams, B.B. (1970). Driver education task analysis. Vol. I: Task de-
scriptions. Human Resources Research Organization, Alexandria, Virginia. Final Report,
Contract No. FH 11-7336.
McMillen, D.L., Smith, S.M. and Wells-Parker, E. (1989). Brief report: The effects of
alcohol, expectancy, and sensation on risk-taking. Addictive Behaviors, 14, 477-483.
McRuer, D.T., Allen, R.W., Weir, D.R. and Klein, R.R. (1977). New results in driver steering
control models. Human Factors, 19,381-397.
Naatanen, R. and Summala, H. (1974). A model for the role of motivational factors in
drivers' decision-making. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 6, 243-261.
Ranney, T.A. (1994). Models of driving behaviour: A review of their evolution. Accident
Analysis and Prevention, 26, 733-750.
Rumar, K. (1985). The role of perceptual and cognitive filters in observed behavior. In
L. Evans and R.G. Schwing (Eds.). Human behavior and traffic safety (pp. 151-165).
Plenum Press, New York.
Rumar, K. (1999). Transport safety visions, strategies and targets: Beyond 2000. European
Transport Safety Council, Brussels.
Salvucci, D.D. and Liu, A. (2002). The time course of a lane change: Driver control and
eye-movement behaviour. Transportation Research Part F, 5, 123-132.
Stradling, S.G. and Meadows, M.L. (2000). Highway code and aggressive violations in UK
drivers. In Global Web Conference on Aggressive Driving. http://www.aggressive.drivers.
com/papers/stradling-meadows/stradling-meadows. pdf.
Wilde, G.1.S (1982). The theory of risk homeostasis: Implications for safety and health.
Risk Analysis, 2, 209-225.
Yu,1. and Williford, W.R. (1993). Alcohol and risk/sensation seeking: Specifying a causal
model on high-risk driving. Journal ofAddictive Diseases, 12(1),79-96.
III
Learning and Behavioural Adaptation
7
Subject Testing for Evaluation of Driver
Information Systems and Driver
Assistance Systems - Learning Effects
and Methodological Solutions
KLAUS BENGLER
SUMMARY
Nowadays, subject testing represents a well-established methodology to evaluate
different properties of driver information systems and driver assistance systems.
Among several criteria, learnability is one important system property. User and
usage strategies are dependent on the subject's learning state, for example, to
switch attendance between driving task and operation of a driver information
system. Therefore it is wishful that the user acquires a model of the system, for
example learns as quickly as possible. Also, the intended usage of driver assistance
systems in given driving situations is influenced by the user's experience. A suitable
way to investigate related questions is to conduct a typical learning experiment
and to analyse data with the given methodology. In this type of experiment, the
familiarity and training state of the subject are set as independent variable. Beside
learnablity, other properties of human-machine interaction are to be investigated
and evaluated. In this case, however, the learning effectuated by a subject is an
important dependent variable or even noise in sense of measurement theory that
might cover a given main effect. After some empirical examples, possible solutions
will be discussed that help to manage this problem with justifiable expense.
7.1 Introduction
Following the idea of user-centered system design, subject testing is an estab-
lished and frequently used procedure to evaluate and ensure quality of human-
machine interface (HMI) concepts. Appropriate methods are applied in areas of
In-vehicle information systems (IVIS)l and advanced driver assistance systems
(ADAS 2 ) (Mayser, 2002). A good overview of different methodologies and of
123
124 BENGLER
their interaction with possible product development processes is given in the pro-
ceedings of Bundesanstalt fur StraBenwesen (2000).
Documents like DIN 66234 Part 8 (DIN66234 1986) and the European Statement
of Principles (2000/53/EC 2000) make clear that beneath properties like 'error
robustness', 'interruptability' or 'visual demand', learnability of a system is also
an important system property. More and more questions are raised concerning the
quality of subject testing in the sense of test theory and methodology (Kanis, 2000;
Haigney 2001). This paper discusses the conditions under which an independent
evaluation of dialogue quality and assistance characteristics can be conducted. An
evaluation that accounts for the fact that learnabilty and learning state should not
influence the results erroneously.
Frank and Reichelt (2001) also mention that learnability is one criterion that
can be tested during the development of an ADAS by expert judgement and ex-
perimental testing to ensure high product quality and system acceptance.
In general, learning is defined as a 'permanent change of behaviour based on
experience' (Hilgard and Bower, 1966) and therefore parameters for the learning
process were modelled for given tasks. More specifically, Woods (1999) states that
the user is building up heuristics and simplified models to structure the interaction
with a given system.
In addition, Reeves (1999) introduces the concept of 'cognitive complexity'
as quality for an HMI based on models for cognitive processes. Cognitive com-
plexity describes the property of a system that enhances or prohibits the user's
learning process. This process includes elements of perception, model building
and categorisation. Following this, Reeves (1999) gives recommendations for sys-
tem interaction concepts and information presentation to increase the usability
of an HMI concept. These include guidelines for a leamer-oriented design pro-
cess that targets interaction concepts that support perception, visualisation, model
building, categorisation and problem solving (cf. also Groeger, 1991).
This emphasises that learnability of a system is a predominant feature of an
interaction concept besides other properties that contribute to usability and utility
of the system. Therefore, in the following, learning experiments as one type of
experiment will be distinguished from system evaluations.
A learning experiment typically focuses on questions about acquisition of
knowledge on the system and users' mental models. Questionnaires and inter-
views are used as dependent variables as well as performance measures (e.g.
driving performance, user errors). In case of a system evaluation, properties like
visual or motoric demand are analysed as well as additional performance measures
and user errors - that is observable behaviour (Woods 1999) describes the idea
of so-called built in or designed system diseases as one of the most important
source for human errors in contrast to the mostly stated human error and give more
information on error analysis as a further evaluation method. Thus, in both types
of experiments, the analysis is based on similar measurements. If a subject was
learning how to operate the system during a system evaluation, the learning pro-
cess will produce variability and cover the main effect and item of interest of this
investigation.
7. Subject Testing for Evaluation of Driver Information Systems 125
This makes clear that learning processes expand the range of the driver especially
by the establishment of highly automatised expert behaviour. Vehicle stabilisation
(distance and lane keeping) and the more efficient usage of in-car devices based on
usage strategies are an example. An example for such a strategy is to use a given
device not in any but a suitable traffic situation or to interrupt the operation and
continue afterwards (Sayer et al., 2005).
Learnability is therefore a beneficial system property that is to be tested
('Learnability' DIN 66234/8) like other ergonomic qualities during product de-
velopment. A problematic fact in this context is that only few models ex-
ist' that describe the learning behaviour described in the section above. This
fact makes it difficult to plan and conduct learning experiments during system
development.
On the other hand, learning is also a potential source of 'noise' in empiri-
cal testing to other values describing HMI qualities like visual demand or in-
terruptability. Therefore, learning processes have to be taken into account ei-
ther as a main effect or as a side effect at the different steps of the evaluation
process:
This is now the point to stress the lack of detailed learning models that could
help the experimenter to decide on the above questions.
Especially the model of Rasmussen (1983) is suited and used to describe learning
in the context of the driving task.
In relation to the driving task, learning is described as a staged process
Rasmussen (1983), however, the transition between skill-based - rule-based -
knowledge-based stages are not described very concisely. Therefore, the stages
are difficult to handle for planning and experimental procedures. But they can be
used post hoc to reduce variability, in most cases, as a covariate.
The learning processes in relation to in-car HMI are mostly modelled using
the power law. This shall also help to deal with these effects within experimental
procedures. As different system types and usage scenarios might require a very
differentiated discussion, mostly the necessary parameters to describe the power
law function are missing.
40
35
30
25
20
15
10
o-J,.-"
Timing Vocabulary Dialog Recognition
Errors Errors Errors Errors
FiGURE 7.1. Frequency of different error types during first and last phases of experiment
(all subjects and traffic situations).
TABLE 7.1. Totaltask times (mean and standarddeviation) telephone and navigationtasks
in four subsequentruns during the experiment (N == 11).
Telephone tasks Navigation tasks
2 3 4 2 3 4
Twelve subjects participated in the experiment (aging 35-55 years) having more
than 100,000-km driving experience.
In sum, 100 destination entries were negotiated at speeds of 40-50 km/h.
Two route complexities are distinguished.
Easy: 1.5 km, straight
Winding: 1.2 km; turning into narrow roads
The resulting data set gives insight into the learning process of this very specific
task.
At first, a learning process can be seen with a clear performance improvement
between Blocks 1 and 2 for the winding route. In addition, there is a significant
influence of route complexity on mean duration for destination entry and the level
of the learning process in general (see Fig. 7.2).
Comparison of different subjects shows two further effects. Learning processes
of subjects 1 to 5 start at significant different levels and are hardly comparable in
their shape. This means that either the mean value computed of this sample would
go along with a high standard deviation. This is especially problematic if the data
would be used for comparison with another sample using a different system. Or,
the experimenter would have to decide to extract subgroups of subjects due to their
difference in learning behaviour.
130 BENGLER
2 345 6
Block
Figures 7.2 and 7.3 show that both effects -learning process and inter-individual
differences - result in a high level of variance of the data set. Therefore, they should
be treated as error variance in system evaluation experiment.
130
120
~ 110
e-
C 100
<D
90
en<D 80
"0
70
'0
c 60
0
~ 50
:; P1
"0
40 P2 ---------.
C 30 P3 -----------
ctj
<D 20 P4 FIGURE 7.3. Mean duration of a destination
~ P5 _._._._._--
10 entry depending on experimental run per
0 subject (PI to P5). Block I = 20 destination
2 3 4 5 6 entries. Block 2-6 = at times 16 destination
Block entries.
7. Subject Testing for Evaluation of Driver Information Systems 131
The authors distinguish 'eager testers' and 'careful approachers'. Obviously, dif-
ferent usage strategies again lead to a remarkable variance within this small sample.
This variance can be used to describe different learning strategies. But driving per-
formance and other effects - especially at the beginning of the experiment - have
to be interpreted by taking account of these individual learning strategies.
An attempt to present a model on skill acquisition of an ACC system is given by
Hoedemaeker (1999). She proposes the idea that the adaptation of the ACC mental
model results of two processes that operate at different abstraction levels within
the mental model. The first process is based on the difference of expected system
behaviour provided by the user's mental model of the ACC and the observed system
behaviour. Depending on the degree of wrong predictions, the ACC mental model
is updated to arrive at a more accurate account of the ACC's operational domain.
The second process dealing with the adaptation of the mental model takes place
at a higher abstraction level. The intensity of ACC usage then is highly correlated
with the degree of perceived inconsistencies between ACC behaviour and user
expectation based on his or her mental model. The less inconsistencies the more
system usage and vice versa.
A very sophisticated long-term field operational test using an ADAS was con-
ducted by Weinberger (2001). The experiment gathered more information about
the learning process for the usage of controls and display and the judgement of
take-over situations. Participants used an ACC-equipped car for a period of 4
weeks per person. Data were analysed with respect to the duration of the learning
phase. The change in behaviour was investigated using drivers' self-assessment of
the length of learning and observed driving behaviour during take-over situations.
The results suggest that 2 or 3 weeks are needed to learn the operation of ACC and
the assessment of take-over situations for a goal-directed system usage. Interesting
is the methodological advice given by the authors that other ACC users might need
a different learning time as the participants in the study drove much more than the
average driver.
Kostka et al. (2004) recommend the analysis of driver errors by expert observa-
tion as a practical empirical method to evaluate workload and distraction but might
serve as well as a tool to investigate the learnability of the concept and especially
erroneous user expectations on system functionality.
Results from this study and that of Weisse (2002) give the impression that the
investigation of learning processes and individual behaviour in the context of driver
assistance systems demands considerably more effort than driver information sys-
tems.
7.4 Solutions
The discussion of the 'learning problem' and the selected experiments show that on
the one hand learning processes and learnability in the in-car domain can be inves-
tigated using classical methodologies. Due to this method, the effort is remarkable.
132 BENGLER
On the other hand it is also true that learning processes can lead to considerable
data variablity in evaluation experiments:
7.5 Conclusions
The examples give an impression of learning effects that have to be expected
in connection with subject tests evaluating driver information systems and driver
assistance systems. It turns out that learning experiments that investigate technolo-
gies such as voice recognition and future driver assistance systems would ease the
planning and conduction of system evaluation and raise their quality.
In addition, one can state that the investigation of total task time and other
performance measures is only reliable and makes sense if learning is finished and
subjects are in a stable state.
7. Subject Testing for Evaluation of Driver Information Systems 133
Suitable models are still missing that would describe learning of driver informa-
tion systems and driver assistance systems in detail. Therefore, the experimenter
has to reduce high variances that are based on learning effects by carefully selecting
experimental procedures that do not exceed manageable effort.
References
2000/53/EC (2000). 2000/53/EC: Commission recommendation of 21 December 1999 on
safe and efficient in-vehicle information and communication systems: A European state-
ment of principles on human machine interface. Official Journal of the European Com-
munities L19 43 (25.01.2000), 64-68.
Bengler, K., Noszko, Th. and Neuss, R. (2002). Usability of multimodal human-machine-
interaction while driving. In Proceedings of the 9-th World Congress on ITS, CD-Rom,
9th World Congress on Intelligent Transport Systems, ITS America, October 2002.
Bortz, J. (1999). Statistik fur Sozialwissenschaftler (5th ed.). Springer-Verlag, Berlin-
Heidelberg.
Bundesanstalt fur StraBenwesen (2000). Informations- und Assistenzsysteme im Auto be-
nutzergerecht gestalten. Methoden fur den EntwicklungsprozeB. Bd. Heft 152. Bergisch
Gladbach: Bundesanstalt fur StraBenwesen, 2000.
Norm DIN66234 8 8 (1986). Bildschirmarbeitsplatze: Grundsatze der Dialoggestaltung.
Frank, P. and Reichelt, W. (2001). Fahrerassistenzsysteme im Entwicklungsprozess. In Th.
Jurgensohn and K.-P. Timpe (Hrsg.) (2001). Kraftfahrzeugfuhrung. Springer, Berlin,
Heidelberg, New York, pp. 71-80.
Groeger, J.A. (1991). Learning from learning: Principles for supporting drivers. In Inter-
national Symposium on Automotive Technology & Automation (24th: 1991: Florence,
Italy). Croydon, UK: Automotive Automation, pp. 703-709.
Haigney, D. and Westerman, SJ. (2001). Mobile (cellular) phone use and driving: A critical
review of research methodology. Ergonomics, 44(2),132-143.
Hilgard, E.R. and Bower, G.R. (1966). Theories of learning (3rd ed.). Appleton-Century-
Crofts, New York.
Hoedemaeker, M. (1999). Driving Behaviour with Adaptative Cruise Control and the Ac-
ceptance by Individual Drivers. Delft University Press, Delf.
Jahn, G., Krems, J.F. and Gelau, C. (2002). Skill-development when interacting with in-
vehicle information systems: A training study on the learnability of different MMI con-
cepts. In D. de Waard, K.A. Brookhuis, J. Moraal and A. Toffetti (Eds.). Human fac-
tors in transportation, communication, health, and the workplace. Shaker Publishing,
Maastricht, The Netherlands.
Kanis, H. (2000). Questioning validity in the area of ergonomics/human factors. Er-
gonomics, 43(12), 1947-1965.
Kostka, M., Dahmen-Zimmer, K., Scheufler, I., Piechulla, W. and Zimmer, A. (2004).
Fahrfehlerbeobachtung durch Experten. Eine Methode zur Evaluierung von Belastung
und Ablenkung des Fahrers. In A.C. Zimmer, K. Lange, et al. (Hrsg.). Experimentelle
Psychologie im Spannungsfeld von Grundlagenforschung und Anwendung. Proceedings
43. Tagung experimentell arbeitender Psychologen (CD-ROM). Regensburg.
Manstetten (2005). Evaluating the traffic safety effects of driver assistance systems. AAET
2005, Automation, Assistance and Embedded Real Time Platforms for Transportation:
Airplanes, Vehicles, Trains, 6th Braunschweig Conference. (Vol. 1). Braunschweig, DE.
134 BENGLER
8.1 Introduction
A realistic estimate of the risk-reducing effects that will be gained by the intro-
duction of advanced driver supports (ADAS) requires knowledge of a number of
elements, of which user behaviour is the least understood. This paper focuses on
some of the most essential knowledge that is already available, in particular on the
mechanisms by which users could possibly change their behaviour once they start
using the support.
135
136 Janssen
3. Effects on and changes in the user's behaviour that may be brought about by
the device, in particular the so-called behavioural adaptation processes.
4. The functional relationships linking behavioural parameters (e.g. driving speed
and its variability) to resultant accident probability and severity.
We need to know more on all of these, but this paper focuses on the last two
and on behavioural adaptation in particular. From the behavioural point of view,
selective recruitment is an almost equally important issue, but it deserves more
space than could be devoted here.
Increase in driving speed (km/h; baseline: 112.3 kmlh) +2.2 +2.6 + 2.8
Increase in occurrence of following headways < 0.5 s +0.5 + 6.3 +7.9
(%; baseline: 5.5 %)
who originally were non-wearers of the belt and who became wearers for the pur-
pose of the study. This long-term study could apply an almost ideal experimental
design in that it had an alternating own vehicle/instrumented vehicle design; that
is, subjects started wearing the belt all the time in their own vehicle and came to
the laboratory for measurements in an instrumented vehicle at regular (4-months)
intervals after an initial beltless baseline measurement. Their driving performance
was compared to a control group of non-wearers who remained so for at least the
duration of the study.
While ABS and seat belts are apparently demonstrations of the existence of
the phenomenon, it is not yet clear (a) whether this will always happens or what
would distinguish cases in which they do from cases in which they do not and (b)
whether the compensation is complete and will totally eliminate the safety effect
that should follow from the engineering estimate.
To come to terms with these questions, we would need valid and quantitative
models of road user decision making. Elementary utility models (O'Neill, 1977;
Janssen and Tenkink, 1988) have already paid some services in this respect. In the
Janssen and Tenkink model (see Fig. 8.1), the road user is assumed to balance the
(dis)utilities of time loss during the trip, plus the possible accident risk, against
the utility of being at the destination. From this a choice of optimal speed, and
possibly of other driving behaviour parameters, then follows so as to be at the
optimum of that balance.
It has been derived, for example, from this type of consideration that a device
having an expected effectiveness (i.e. an engineering estimate) 8 will not reduce
accident risk with that same factor but with a factor that happens to be
== 1 - (1 - 8)-1/(c+l) , (1)
time loss
loss accident loss, befor~
per km
o,....-~-------+---+------v
FIGURE 8.1. Utility model (Janssen and Tenkink, 1988) shows how drivers select optimum
speed as a function of time (opportunity) losses and accident risk so as to make the resulting
total expected loss minimal. It appears to be generally true that whenever accident risk is
objectively reduced ('after' situation) the optimum speed that is selected will move towards
the higher end of the scale.
0
~
Q) 0.4
ii
:1
.....
U
co
0.2
rate multiplied by the engineering estimate). On the other hand the utility model,
on the basis of Equation (1), would have predicted a reduction of between 3% and
4%. The actual reduction was 6.7%. The readers may draw their own conclusions
as to which of the two was the best prediction (or rather, postdiction): Janssen,
(1994b), has further discussion 1.
The generation of extra mobility (VMT). For example, navigation systems may
not so much reduce excess mileage as generate extra mileage into areas that
were formerly avoided. Or entrepreneurs who formerly 'lost' 5% or 6% of the
mileage driven by their fleet because drivers selected non-optimal routes to their
destination may now plan an extra trip a day because navigation performance
has become flawless.
Road use by less qualified segments ofthe driving population. It is to be expected
that some categories of users that did not dare to venture out in traffic, realis-
ing their own imperfections, will do so if offered an extra amount of 'built-in'
safety.
Driving under more difficult conditions. Similarly, the extra safety offered will
tempt road users to move to places they formerly avoided.
rates and same-year per capita traffic death rates in seven Western countries. Taken
together, the data do appear to confirm the basic assumption that safety and utility
are related to each other, i.e. safety is a factor in the utility considerations associated
with undertaking a trip.
For a given headway it is calculated whether, for a given range of reaction times,
a collision would follow if the preceding vehicle were to brake vehemently, i.e.
at full braking power.
The total probability of a collision is then computed by integration over a log-
normal distribution (which has a tail towards the longer reaction times) of driver
reaction times.
The mean and the standard deviation of the distribution are, moreover, adapted
to headway itself. This procedure was introduced by Farber so as to indicate that
drivers follow more attentively at shorter headways.
In case of a rear-end collision, the speed difference at the moment of impact is
computed. The overall risk of the car-following situation is then computed by
multiplying accident probability by the squared speed difference at impact.
Fig. 8.3 illustrates results for a few everyday car-following situations. As has
been observed by other authors, there is a 'worst' headway to follow, which is not
at the shortest range. This is intuitively clear when it is realised that although the
probability of the collision itself happening becomes higher at shorter headways,
its severity will be less because at a short headway the speed difference between
the two vehicles at the moment of impact will be less.
200
~ 150
a:
0
100
50
0
0.25 0.5 0.75 1 1.25 1.5 1.75 2
Headway
FIGURE 8.3. Rear-end collision risk when following a vehicle at a certain headway that
drives at 20 m/s and brakes suddenly (for two speeds of the following vehicle). Risk units
are arbitrary, i.e. defined as 100 at one of the configurations.
144 Janssen
8.7 Conclusions
There is now at least some useful knowledge available on the behavioural effects of
driver supports, which permits more than an educated guess on what safety effects
will follow from the provision of these supports. This derives both from a more
advanced insight into the behaviour itself and from the availability of procedures
to translate behavioural effects into safety effects. Thus
8.8 An Afterthought
When driver support systems that offer a safety benefit are introduced on the road
their benefits will, in all likelihood, be less than what originally could be expected.
This is, by itself, an important fact of life. However, as some authors have surmised,
the introduction of any specific safety measure could well be no more than a tiny
bubble on top of a continuously ongoing societalleaming curve of a much broader
nature. Although Smeed's ideas (Smeed, 1949, 1968, 1972; Smeed and Jeffcoate,
1970) about the learning process that comes along with a society's increasing level
of motorisation are no longer popular today, they may well reflect what is the more
significant permanent background against which all incidental safety measures
8. Modelling Driver's Risk Taking Behaviour 145
become relatively minor. Modelling that background process is maybe really what
we should aim for in the long term.
References
Aschenbrenner, K., Biehl, B. and Wurm, G. (1994). Mehr Verkehrssicherheit durch bessere
Technik? Felduntersuchungen zur Risikokompensation am Beispiel des Antiblockiersys-
terns (ABS). Bast, Bergisch Gladbach, Bericht 8323.
Bruhning, E., Ernst, R., Glaeser, H.P., Hundhausen, G., Klockner, J.H. and Pfafferott, I.
(1986). Zum Riickgang der Getotetenzahlen im StraBenverkehr-Entwicklung in der
Bundesrepublik Deutschland von 1970 bis 1984. Zeitschrift fiir verkehrssicherheit, 32,
154-163.
Evans, L. (1985). Human behaviour feedback and traffic safety. Human Factors, 27, 555-
576.
Farber, E. (1993). Using freeway traffic data to estimate the effectiveness of rear-end colli-
sion countermeasures. Proceedings ofthe Third Annual Meeting ofthe Intelligent Vehicle
Society ofAmerica, Washington, DC.
Farber, E. (1994). Using the Reamacs model to compare the effectiveness of alternative rear
end collision warning algorithms. XIVth International Technical Conference on Enhanced
Safety of Vehicles, Miinchen. Paper Nr. 94 S3 0 03.
Janssen, W.H. (1988). Gurtanlegequoten und Kfz-Insassen-Sicherheit: eine Anmerkung zu
jiingsten deutschen Erkentnissen. Ze itsch rift fiir verkehrssicherheit. 34, 65-67.
Janssen, W.H. (1990). The economy of risk; or, what do I get for my error? Ergonomics,
33,1333-1348.
Janssen, W.H. (1994a). Seat-belt wearing and driving behaviour: An instrumented-vehicle
study. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 26, 249-261.
Janssen, W.H. (1994b). Behavioural adaptation to road safety measures: A framework and an
illustration. In R.M. Trimpop and GJ.S. Wilde (Eds.). Challenges to Accident Prevention.
Styx, Groningen, pp. 91-100.
Janssen, W.H. (2000). Functions relating driver behaviour and accident risk. Report TM-
00-D004. TNO Human Factors, Soesterberg, The Netherlands.
Janssen, W.H. (2001). Advanced driver supports, driver behaviour, and road safety. Pro-
ceedings ofthe 8th World Congress on Intelligent Transport Systems, Sydney. (CD-ROM)
Janssen, W.H., Kaptein, N. and Claessens, M. (1999). Behaviour and safety when driving
with in-vehicle devices that provide real-time traffic information. Proceedings of the 6th
World Congress on Intelligent Transport Systems, Toronto. (CD-ROM)
Janssen, W.H. and Tenkink, E. (1988). Considerations on speed selection and risk home-
ostasis in driving. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 20, 137-143.
Joksch, H.C. (1984). The relation between motor vehicle accidents deaths and economic
activity. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 16,207-210.
Nilsson ,G. (1984). Speeds, accident rates and personal injury consequences for different
road types. Report number 277VTI, Linkoping, Sweden.
O'Neill, B. (1977). A decision-theory model of danger compensation. Accident Analysis
and Prevention, 9, 157-165.
Partyka, S.C. (1984). Simple models of fatality trends using employment and population
data. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 16, 211-222.
Salusjarvi, M. (1990). Finland. In G. Nilsson (Ed.). Speed and Safety: Research Results
from the Nordic Countries. Linkoping.
146 Janssen
Shannon, H.S. (1986). Road-accident data: Interpreting the British experience with partic-
ular reference to the risk homeostasis theory. Ergonomics, 29(8), 1005-1015.
Smeed, R.I. (1949). Some statistical aspects of road safety research. Journal of the Royal
Statistical Society, Series A, Part I, 1-34.
Smeed, R.I. (1968). Variation in the pattern of accident rates in different countries and their
causes. Traffic Engineering and Control, 364-371.
Smeed, R.I. (1972). The usefulness of formulae in traffic engineering and road safety.
Accident Analysis and Prevention, 4, 303-312.
Smeed, R.I. and Jeffcoate, G.O. (1970). Effects of changes in motorisation in various
countries on the number of road fatalities. Traffic Engineering and Control, 150-151.
Wagenaar, A.C. (1984). Effects of macro-economic conditions on the incidence of motor
vehicle accidents.Accident Analysis and Prevention, 16, 191-205.
Wilde,G.I.S. (1991). Economics and accidents: A commentary. Journal of Applied Be-
haviour Analysis, 24, 81-84.
9
Dealing with Behavioural Adaptations
to Advanced Driver Support Systems
FARIDA SAAD
9.1 Introduction
Over the past 15 years major technological advances have been made in the field of
automotive technology. Many research and development programmes (in Europe,
Japan and the United States) have been devoted to the design and implementation
of new driver support systems and information management systems (for route
planning, obstacle detection, car-following situations, speed control and so on).
The development of these systems raises crucial issues at a technical level as
well as in terms of their consequences on driver activity (for an overview, see,
e.g., Michon, 1993; Parkes and Franzen, 1993; Noy, 1997). Some of these issues
deal, in particular, with the conditions of use of the systems, their effects on driver
behaviour and strategies and their impact on the operation and safety of the traffic
system. A major concern is about the 'behavioural adaptations' that may occur in
response to the introduction of these systems in the driving task and their impacts
on road safety (Smiley, 2000).
These new support systems will mediate drivers' interactions with their driving
environment (vehicle, road infrastructure, traffic rules, other road users) by creating
new sources of information and/or offering new modes of vehicle control. They
will thus alter the conditions in which the driving task is currently performed and
can thus be expected to engender changes in drivers' activity. Changes may occur
(1) within the very activity of 'supported' drivers (in terms of divided attention
between the new internal sources of information and direct monitoring of the road
environment, changes of driving strategies, delegation of control to the driving
support system and so on); (2) within their interactions with other road users (effect
on the behaviour of other road users, 'readability' of assisted drivers' behaviour for
other drivers, etc.). It is then important to specify the nature, direction and extent
of the changes likely to occur at these different levels, since they will determine
the ultimate impact on road safety (Evans, 1985; OCDE, 1990).
The changes associated with the use of these new support systems and their
acceptance by drivers will depend (1) on the types of task they are designed
to support (navigation, guidance or control tasks; Allen et al., 1971); (2) their
functions and the type of mediation they provide ('description' as regards the state
147
148 Saad
of the driving environment, 'prescription' as regards the regulating action the driver
has to take; 'intervention' or 'taking over' part of the driving task in the event of
deliberate driver delegation or of driver failure).
Up to now, most support systems are dedicated to specific driving tasks. Their
competence is by definition limited to the area of that task (or a subset of conditions
in which that task is performed, such as conditions of good visibility for instance).
The mediation offered is thus only partial, the driver's direct control over the
driving environment is always necessary and he remains responsible for the overall
management of his journey.
Studying the integration of the systems into the overall driving task and iden-
tifying behavioural changes when using them are thus critical aspects that need
to be carefully studied and analysed. This entails (Saad and Villame, 1999) the
following:
Taking account of the essential dimensions of the road environment in which
that activity takes place (nature of the interactions at work, regulatory, structural
and dynamic constraints, etc.). This reference to the context (Suchman, 1987)
is particularly important in view of the diversity and variability of the road
situations that drivers may encounter during a journey.
Choosing functional units of analysis making it possible to examine not only
the impact on the performance of the specific task to which the support system
is dedicated (compliance with safety margins or speed limits, for instance), but
also its compatibility with the performance of other driving tasks (overtaking,
interacting with other users, etc.).
Selecting the relevant indicators for revealing the changes likely to take place in
driver's activity.
These issues make direct demands on our knowledge of the driving task and of the
psychological processes (cognitive and motivational) that govern drivers' activity.
to influence the occurrence of behavioural adaptation such as the nature and the
'perceptibility' of the changes introduced in the traffic system (changes that directly
influence the way the driving task is performed or changes that alter the driver's
subjective safety, for instance), the degree of freedom that the change allows drivers
(changes that give the driver an opportunity for adapting his behaviour) or the
presence of competitive motives (safety versus mobility or productivity motives,
for instance).
Although behavioural adaptation is a widely acknowledged phenomenon, the
factors likely to explain it and the processes underlying its occurrence are not
clearly established. Numerous processes may in fact come into play between the
introduction of an 'innovation' in the traffic system and its 'adoption' by drivers,
its 'translation' into behaviour (whether 'safe' or 'risky') and its longer term con-
sequences on the operation and safety of the traffic system (Brown, 1985).
These processes should be analysed in greater depth, in particular those in-
fluencing the way drivers interact with their driving environment (vehicle, road
infrastructure, traffic rules, other road users, etc.). Such analyses should help to
formulate hypotheses about the changes in behaviour that may occur, identify the
conditions in which a 'negative' compensation for safety might appear and direct
thoughts on the means of minimising the extent of such negative changes.
Within the European project adaptive integrated driver-vehicle interface
(AIDE 1) , which aims to develop an harmonised interface that integrates all in-
vehicle support and information systems, a research activity is devoted specifi-
cally to identifying crucial behavioural adaptation issues associated with the use
of new support systems and determining the most relevant parameters that can be
implemented in models for supporting design and safety assessment processes.
The aim of this paper is to present the main phases of this research activity and
the results obtained so far. We begin by outlining the main results of a literature
review on 'behavioural adaptation' to new driver support systems (Saad et al.,
2004), especially advanced driver assistance systems (ADAS), which intervene
more or less directly in the performance of the driving task, such as adaptive cruise
control (ACC) or intelligent speed adaptation (ISA). We then describe the ongoing
activity associated with the conduct of experimental studies in order to improve
our knowledge on short and long term behavioural adaptation (Saad et aI., 2005)
and to develop models that can act as a reference for the design of an adaptive,
integrated in-vehicle interface supporting the multiple tasks drivers have to perform
in modem vehicles (Cacciabue and Hollnagel, 2005).
1996) or negative side effects associated with their use and which may reduce the
expected (safety) benefits of the assistance provided to the driver. In many research
studies, it is assumed that driving with systems which take over some elements
of the driving task (such as speed and time headway control) may reduce drivers'
workload and provide them with an opportunity for devoting less attention to the
(primary) driving task. Another concern relates to the drivers' ability to cope with
the limitation of support systems and to resume control in 'safety critical' traffic
scenarios, either because of drivers' misconceptions about the functioning of the
system and over-reliance on the system or as a consequence of drivers' reduced
attention to the driving task. In some studies, particular emphasis is placed on the
possible deterioration of drivers' interactions with other road users.
These research orientations guide the choice of the driving performance indica-
tors taken into account for assessing behavioural changes (such as driving speed,
safety margins in car-following situations and lateral control of the vehicle, per-
formance to a secondary task or subjective assessment of workload). They also
guide the choice of the driving situations or scenarios examined ('normal' driving
situations, 'safety critical' driving situations such as take-over situations in which
the driver has to regain control of his vehicle because of the limitations themselves
of the systems or a technical failure).
The second major concern relates to drivers' opinions and their acceptance of
the assistance provided. In most research studies, perceived usefulness of and sat-
isfaction about the systems are assessed either through standardised questionnaires
(see, e.g., Van der Laan et al., 1997) or through drivers' verbal reports and in-depth
interviews. In some studies, drivers' acceptance of the support system is more pre-
cisely assessed through the very usage of the system (in terms of drivers' decision
to engage the systems in various situational contexts and the overall duration of
system engagement, for instance).
Several empirical studies have been carried out, focusing mainly on the impact
of individual support systems, such as collision avoidance systems (CAS), speed
limiters, ISA or ACC systems, either in the' controlled' context of driving simulator
or in the complexity of real driving situations. Most of these studies have been
short-term studies and 'the effects (of support systems) on traffic safety and driver
behaviour are still uncertain in many respects' (Nilsson et al., 2002), especially their
long-term effects. Nevertheless, some critical issues have already been identified.
These issues are presented and discussed below.
More generally, the diversity of the results obtained raises questions about the
methods used, the type and number of variables selected for assessing the im-
pact of the system, and finally the (implicit or explicit) models governing their
choice. When examining the results, attention should be paid to these theoretical
and methodological issues. In particular, the context in which the studies have
been carried out (driving simulator, closed tracks or real driving situations) should
be specified as well as the various scenarios and driving tasks in which the be-
havioural changes have been identified. The diversity of the results obtained could
then be examined and discussed in the light of the characteristics of these vari-
ous contexts and scenarios. Such analysis is particularly relevant insofar as the
situational context plays an important role in the behavioural changes observed
when driving with a support system and more generally in drivers' activity (Saad,
2002).
significant deviation from their usual strategies. For example, in a field trial with
a 'Driver Select' ISA (one which the driver can choose to engage or not), Carsten
and Fowkes (2000) observed that drivers were prone to disengage the system in
areas where speeding was the norm for the surrounding traffic. In such traffic
conditions, drivers prefer to be in control of their speed and tum the system off
when they feel under pressure from other drivers. Furthermore, drivers are con-
cerned about the way other drivers might interpret their own behaviour (Saad and
Malaterre, 1982). Certain aggressive reactions on the part of other drivers (close-
following behind, cutting-in manoeuvres, flashing headlights, etc.) are perceived
as negative social feedback and often lead them to give up the use of the support
system.
Finally, some studies have shown that driving with ISA may also influence the
driver's interaction with other road-users (at junctions or at pedestrian crossings in
urban areas), either negatively (Persson et aI., 1993) or positively (Almqvist and
Nygard. 1997) in the short term, but with the probability of improvement after
longer experience with the system (Hjalmdahl and Varhelyi, 2004).
These studies highlight the circumstantial requirements of driving assistance
according to the dynamics of various environmental conditions and to the drivers'
motives, objectives and intentions in these conditions. They also confirm the need
to adopt a multi-level approach when assessing behavioural adaptation to new
driver support system, which is to say to study possible changes within the activity
of 'assisted' drivers as well as within their interactions with other road users.
most frequently adopted, the level of attention devoted to the driving task and so
on; French et al., 1993). Several studies have taken into account this variable when
studying the impact of ACC, whether by design (Hoedemaeker and Brookhuis,
1998, who selected the participants on the basis of two dimensions of the driving
style questionnaire established by French et al. op.cit., namely 'speed' and 'focus ')
or a posteriori, on the basis of the identification of some manifest behaviour patterns
(such as the driver's propensity to change lane frequently on the motorway - Saad
and Villame, 1996 - or the driver's tendency to drive faster or slower than the
surrounding traffic and to adopt short-time headways in car-following situations;
Fancher et al., 1998). The results suggest that the various dimensions of 'driving
style' taken into account in these studies may play an important role in the use and
acceptance of new driver support systems and in the occurrence of behavioural
changes when using them. For example, some behavioural changes observed in
ACC driving, such as a reduction in the number of lane change manoeuvres and
a higher rate of left-lane occupancy, were primarily observed within the group of
drivers who usually tend to change lane frequently when driving on motorways,
while no significant changes were observed for the other" rather less mobile" group
(Saad and Villame, Ope cit.). Fancher et al. (1998) also suggest that driving style
may account for the differences in the overall use of ACC. A particular finding was
that drivers who are described as " hunter/Tailgater " because they drive fast and are
inclined to adopt short-time headways used ACC less often than the other groups.
Hoedemaeker and Brookhuis (1998) also identified some differences as regards
(self-reported) driving styles, depending on the variable taken into consideration.
For instance, while all drivers increase their driving speed with ACC, irrespective
of their driving style, in a critical scenario (in this case a situation in which the
driver has to brake in response to the full stop of a lead vehicle) 'low speed' drivers
braked harder when driving with ACC while 'high speed' drivers braked the same
as when driving without ACC. Differences in driving styles have also an effect
on drivers' acceptance of the assistance provided: 'high speed' drivers are less
positive about ACC than 'low speed' drivers.
Other authors (Rudin-Brown and Parker, 2004; Ward et al., 1995) took into
account some general personality traits, namely 'Sensation Seeking' and/or 'Locus
of Control' (LOC) when studying the effect of ACC on driver behaviour. These
personality traits are assumed to influence, more or less directly, the occurrence of
behavioural adaptation either through a general propensity for 'risk compensation'
(for 'high sensation seekers ') or a tendency to manifest over-reliance on automation
(for'external LOC'). Their results suggest that these individual characteristics tend
to amplify some behavioural changes observed when driving with ACC, such as
impaired lane keeping, slower reaction to the activation of the brake lights of a
lead vehicle (more pronounced for 'high sensation seekers' than for 'low sensation
seekers') or slower reaction to a (simulated) failure of ACC (more pronounced
for drivers with external LOC than for drivers with internal LOC). Individual
characteristics also influence drivers' subjective assessments of the impact of the
system on their driving (for instance, 'high sensation seekers' reported lower level
of arousal and effort when driving with ACC than 'low sensation seekers ').
9. Dealing with Behavioural Adaptations to Advanced Driver Support Systems 155
The results suggest that individual driver characteristics, such as 'driving style' ,
'sensation seeking' or 'locus of control' , seem to playa role in the overall frequency
of ACC usage, in the occurrence and/or the magnitude of some behavioural changes
when using it and in the acceptance of the assistance provided. It should be noted,
however, that the relationship between general personality traits and other individ-
ual characteristics such as 'driving style' are not quite clear and should be more
precisely established.
through different stages: a preliminary stage of getting to know the system (learn-
ing to operate it); a testing stage (learning the system limits); and a familiarisation
stage (learning to use the system appropriately according to the situational context).
An analysis of changes over time in drivers intervention in 'take-over' situations
suggests that there was a trend towards testing the limits of the system at the be-
ginning, followed by a certain apprehension of the system's capabilities and then a
more personalised 'steady use' of the system. Weinberger et al. (2001) conducted
a long-term operational field test to obtain more information about the learning
process as regards both the usage of controls and display and the judgement ap-
plied in take-over situations. The participants used an ACC-equipped car over a
period of 4 weeks each. Both drivers' self-assessment of the length of the learn-
ing process and drivers' behaviour during 'take-over' situations suggest that 2 or
3 weeks are needed to learn the operation of ACC and the assessment of take-over
situations. However, as the participants in the study drove much more than the av-
erage driver, the authors suggest that other ACC users might need a longer learning
time.
These studies provide some useful insights into the duration of the learning
process of ACC and its different stages and into the way in which drivers' in-
teractions with the system change and develop over time. They also suggest that
it is important to find means to accelerate the learning process, by supporting
drivers' predictive activities about the behaviour of ACC and more generally by
helping drivers develop appropriate conceptions of the systems' behaviour and
limits. More generally, these studies highlight an important dimension to take
into account when dealing with behavioural adaptation, the temporal dimension
of behavioural changes.
It should be noted that the issue of learning to use new support systems is
attracting more attention, as indicated by the development of projects such as
the INVENT FVM project (Manstetten et al., 2003) or the TAC Safecar project
(Regan et al., 2001) and more recently by the research activity planned within the
HUMANIST Noe (EC funded network of excellence within the sixth framework
programme). The research planned in the AIDE project will also contribute to this
effort of understanding learning processes and their impact on short-and long-term
behavioural adaptations.
Some interesting concepts have been introduced, such as the concept of 'learn-
ability' and of 'self-explanatory' support systems. With reference to the RE-
SPONSE project (Beker et al., 2001), 'a system is learnable, if accurate assimila-
tion of information by the driver occurs, evidenced in the driver's understanding
of system function, system handling and situational limits'. A 'self-explanatory
ADAS' is defined as a 'driver assistance system leaving a minimal amount of
learning demand to the driver and eliminating learnability issues which can result
in safety-critical traffic situations'.
These concepts are interesting inasmuch as they extend the number of criteria
for assessing the usability of support systems and emphasise the need to take
account of learning issues in the design of driver support systems. These issues
are likely to be particularly crucial in the future development of support systems,
9. Dealing with Behavioural Adaptations to Advanced Driver Support Systems 157
as the systems will become more complex and integrate multiple driver support
and information functions.
With respect to the temporal factors affecting behavioural adaptation, two main
phases are considered, namely:
Learning and appropriation phase. During this phase, drivers discover the sys-
tems, learn how they operate, identify the precise limits of their competence and
delimit their domains of utility. This learning phase is assumed to be crucial for
drivers to build an appropriate model of the operation of the systems and for
'calibrating' their trust in them. It is also assumed that the learning process is
oriented by the way the systems are presented to the driver (instruction for use
in the manuals, for instance) as well as by the information and feedbacks they
received on-line when interacting with them.
Integration phase. It is assumed that, through experience gained with the systems
in various driving situations, the drivers are able to assess whether or not, how and
in which situational context, it is possible to integrate them in the management
of the overall driving task.
This involves examining whether, when and how behaviour associated with the use
of support systems changes with training and experience. It has to be pointed out
that, because of the scarcity of research carried out into the learning process and the
long -term effects, it is hard to determine the temporal span of the different phases
distinguished above. Different support systems will probably require different
learning and integration times. The research planned in the AIDE project has been
developed in such a way as to optimise the opportunities for identifying the main
'stabilisation phases' of the learning and integration process for different support
systems.
This research activity will lead to the identification of the relevant variables
to be used to assess behavioural adaptation effects. The correlation between these
variables and adequate taxonomies and classification of road situations and driving
tasks (scenarios of dynamic situation) will be devised to associate the variables
with realistic conditions. On this basis, it will be possible to develop a model of
driver behaviour that can act as reference for the design of an adaptive-integrated
in vehicle interface supporting the multiple tasks of drivers in modem vehicles
(Cacciabue and Hollnagel, 2005).
References
Allen, T., Lunenfeld, H. and Alexander, G. (1971). Driver information needs. Highway
Research Record, 366, 102-115.
Almqvist, S. and Nygard, M. (1997). Dynamic speed adaptation: A field trial with automatic
speed adaptation in an urban area. Bulletin 154. Lund Institute of Technology, Department
of Traffic Planning and Engineering, University of Lund, Sweden.
Amalberti, R. (1996). La conduite des systemes if risques. Presses Universitaires de France,
Paris.
Becker, S., Johanning, T., Feldges, 1. and Kopf, M. (2001). RESPONSE. The integrated
approach of user, system, and legal perspective. Final report on recommendations for
9. Dealing with Behavioural Adaptations to Advanced Driver Support Systems 159
Michon, lA. (1993). Generic intelligent driver support, a comprehensive report on GIDS.
Taylor and Francis, London.
Muir, B.M. (1987). Trust between humans and machines, and the design of decision aids.
International Journal ofAviation Psychology, 8, 47-63.
Nilsson, L. (1995). Safety effects of adaptive cruise controls in critical traffic situations.
In Proceedings of the 2nd World Congress on Intelligent Transport Systems. Yokohama,
Japan,pp.1254-1259.
Nilsson, L., Harms, L. and Peters, B. (2002). The effect of road transport telematics. In
P Barjonnet (Ed.). Traffic psychology today. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Boston, pp.
265-285.
Noy, I. (1997). Ergonomics and safety of intelligent driver interfaces. Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, Mahwah.
OECD (1990). Behavioural adaptations to changes in the road transport system. Organi-
zation for Economic Co-operation and Development, Paris.
Parkes, A.M. and Franzen, S.F. (1993). Drivingfuture vehicles. Taylor and Francis, London.
Regan, M.A., Mitsopoulos, E., Tomasevic, N., Healy, D., Connelly, K. and Williams, L.
(2001). Behavioural adaptation to in-car ITS technologies: Update on the Australian
TAC safecar project. In Proceedings of the 8th World Congress on Intelligent Transport
Systems. Sydney, Australia.
Persson, H., Towliat, M., Almqvist, S., Risser, R. and Magdeburg (1993). Speed limiters for
cars. In A field study of driving speeds, driver behaviour, traffic conflicts and comments
drivers in town and city traffic. Report of Department of Traffic Planning and Engineering,
University of Lund, Sweden.
Rudin-Brown, C.M. and Parker, H.A. (2004). Behavioural adaptation to adaptive cruise
control (ACC): Implications for preventive strategies. Transport Research, Part F, 7,
59-76.
Saad, F. and Malaterre, G. (1982). Regulation de la vitesse: aide au controle de la vitesse.
Synthese. Rapport ONSER.
Saad, F. and Villame, T. (1996). Assessing new driving support systems - Contribution
of an analysis of drivers' activity in real situations. In Proceedings of the Third Annual
World Congress on Intelligent Transport Systems. Orlando, USA.
Saad, F. and Villame, T. (1999). Integration d'un nouveau systeme d'assistance dans
l'activite des conducteurs d'automobile. In l-G. Ganascia (Ed.). Securite et cognition.
Editions Hermes. Paris, pp. 105-114.
Saad, F., Munduteguy, C. and Darses, F. (1999). Managing interactions between car drivers:
An essential dimension of reliable driving. In Proceedings of Seventh European Con-
ference on Cognitive Science Approaches to Process Control (CSAPC'99). Villeneuve
d' Ascq, France. Presses Universitaires de Valenciennes, pp. 99-104.
Saad, F. (2002). Ergonomics of the driver's interface with the road environment - The
contribution of psychological research. In R. Fuller, and lA. Santos, (Eds.). Human
factors for highway engineers. Pergamon, Oxford, pp. 23--41.
Saad, F., Hjalmdahl, M., Canas, r, Alonso, M., Garayo, P., Macchi, L., Nathan, F., Ojeda,
L., Papakostopoulos, V., Panou, M. and Bekiaris, A. (2004). Literature review -Analysis
of behavioural changes induced by ADAS and IVIS. AIDE Project, Deliverable D1_2_1.
Saad, F., Bekiaris, A., Brouwer, R., Carsten, 0., Hjalmdahl, Hoedemaker, M., Ojeda, L.,
Papakostopoulos, V., Nathan, F. and Vezier, B.(2005). General experimental plan for
short and long term behavioural assessment. AIDE project IST-I-507674-IP, Deliverable
Dl_2_2.
9. Dealing with Behavioural Adaptations to Advanced Driver Support Systems 161
10.1 Introduction
Road transport is mainly about moving people and goods in motorised vehicles.
Since the introduction of such vehicles in the nineteenth century, people have
had the task of controlling them, although developments in technology today are
moving rapidly towards the possibility of displacing the human element. For the
time being, however, a human driver is in control and is faced at each moment
on a journey with two fundamental choices, the direction in which to steer the
vehicle (i.e., path choice) and the speed at which to move. In trying to understand
how drivers make these choices, this paper will focus primarily on the choice of
speed. Choice of speed is not only a much less constrained choice than choice of
path (except in congested traffic) but it is choice of an inappropriate speed which
contributes significantly to a very large proportion of collisions and road run-offs.
As has frequently been written and perhaps all too frequently forgotten, driving is
a self-paced task and it is the freedom of this self-pacing that underlies so much
of what can go wrong. Despite the focus on speed choice here, however, choice
of path or direction is not entirely neglected: The emerging understanding of the
fundamental determinant of speed choice will be seen to apply equally well to
choice of direction.
165
166 Fuller
a clear link with the behavioural approach discussed above, because its implicit
fundamental feature is an internalisation of the behavioural paradigm as a set of
beliefs about each specific behaviour that might be emitted.
In the theory of planned behaviour (TPB), any specific behaviour is determined
by intention. This term does not appear to mean intention in the conventional
sense (i.e., a conscious representation of a plan or purpose) but rather is assumed
'to capture the motivational factors that influence a behaviour; they (intentions)
are indications of how hard people are willing to try, of how much effort they are
planning to exert, in order to perform the behavior' (Ajzen, 1991, p. 181). Thus
the stronger the intention, the more likely the behaviour (assuming it is under
volitional control). Intention is in tum determined by three other constructs or
variables, respectively labelled 'attitude toward the behaviour', 'subjective norm'
and 'perceived behavioural control' (see Fig. 10.1).
The variable 'attitude toward the behaviour' is influenced by beliefs which link
the behaviour to certain outcomes or some other attribute such as the cost incurred
in performing the behaviour (both are types of consequence). Thus beliefs about
behaviours internalise consequences as important determinants of what behaviour
is learned: 'We learn to favour behaviours we believe have largely desirable conse-
quences and we form unfavourable attitudes toward behaviours we associate with
mostly undesirable consequences (op. cit. p. 191).'
The variable' subjective norm' represents a specific kind of social consequence,
expressed as perceived social pressure to perform or not to perform the behaviour
in question. This social pressure is determined by the degree of approval or dis-
approval (i.e., rewarding or punishing social consequences) of the behaviour by
important referent individuals or groups.
Finally in Ajzen's theory, 'perceived behavioural control' is based on beliefs
about the availability of opportunities and resources for performing the behaviour
of interest. This aspect of the belief structure in the theory may be regarded as an
internal representation of the discriminative stimulus which, as described earlier,
plays a key role in behaviour analysis - specifying the conditions which signal
the behaviour-consequences contingency. Interestingly in behaviour analysis it is
argued that over time a discriminative stimulus can come to exert direct control
over behaviour, the concept of stimulus control. In the same way, this notion is
reflected in TPB by the assertion that there can be a direct determining link between
'perceived behavioural control' and the behaviour itself (see Fig. 10.1).
So the step in the direction of a cognitive explanation characterised by TPB
essentially provides for a mental representation of the contingencies of the be-
haviour analysis paradigm, but with an added emphasis on social consequences of
behaviour. How does it fare? According to Ajzen (1991), the theory is designed to
predict human behaviour in specific contexts. If the theory successfully includes all
of the determinants of behaviour, then all variance in behavioural outcome should
be predicted, except for any residual arising out of measurement error. Each in-
dividual behaviour should have associated with it a particular pattern showing
the relative contribution of each variable in the theory in the determination of
that particular behaviour and it may be noted that in the individual case it is not
10. Motivational Determinants of Control in the Driving Task 169
attitude
toward the
behaviour
subjective
intention behaviour
norm
perceived
behavioural
control
FIGURE 10.1. A simplified representation of the constructs of the theory of planned be-
haviour (arrows identify potential determining relationships).
necessary that all variables should have an equal or indeed any influence. In a
relatively recent meta-analysis performed by Armitage and Connor (2001), it was
found that the TPB model accounted for only about 27% of the variance in actual
behaviour. Furthermore, if reinforcement history is included as an additional pre-
dictor, operationalised as frequency of the behaviour in the past, the increase in
explained variance can be anything from 5% to 32% (Ajzen, 1991). Thus, although
one published study has demonstrated that the TPB can provide some account of
actual speeding behaviour (Elliott et al., 2003), the outlook for the theory in this
respect does not look very promising. Perhaps its real strength for the moment is
in its potential for separately identifying key beliefs which are strongly related to a
particular behaviour, beliefs which might then provide the focus for interventions
to change that behaviour.
edge-lines to two-lane rural roads increases average speeds, but decreases accident
frequency and severity (OECD, 1990). The report concluded that although there
was some evidence of risk compensation under certain conditions - there was little
to support the concept of risk homeostasis. Drivers are clearly able to adapt their
behaviour in varying ways, to varying extents and to varying conditions. However
until relatively recently, there has been no account to explain the conditions under
which behavioural adaptation may occur. An attempt to do this and to describe the
components that influence driver decision making in a comprehensive way is the
task-capability interface model (Fuller, 2000; Fuller and Santos, 2002) .
: LUC KY ESCA PE :
,
I
'
'
I '
- -- --.-- - -- --- - - - ~ -_.
, I
, I
I r--------'------- :- - - - - - - - - - - -
: I :
: : co mpe nsatory: : CO LLIS ION
I : ac tion by others ' .. ' !
:I ~ ---- -- - -r---
I
--""-~ ~ ; ; :1- - - - - - - - - - - -
.------1- -- --. t.. . . . .... ..
I
I I .......
: LOSS OF :, /
C A PA BI LI T Y (C) : CONT ROL:
~---------------------
<. .>
~ ......
>"
:_ -- ---------~
C<D
C>D
, ,k """ T ASK
,, DEM AND S (D )
,,
,,: CONTRO L ,:
1
, --- - -- -
,,
FIG URE 10.3. The basic conditions for control of the driving task.
172 Fuller
being the current preferred term. In principle, however, although collisions are
frequently the outcome of failed driver performance and are the significant events
from a safety perspective, they are nevertheless not a necessary outcome.
We can explore the Tel model further by unpacking the elements of driver ca-
pability on the one hand and driving task demands on the other. Driver capability is
bounded by the constraints imposed by the biological characteristics of the driver,
constraints associated with for example the effectiveness and efficiency of sensory
and perceptual processes, information processing capacity and speed, speed of mo-
tor response, motor coordination, flexibility, strength and physical reach. Starting
with these constitutional biological characteristics, education, training and experi-
ence each contribute to the development of knowledge and skills. Such knowledge
includes formal elements such as rules of the road, procedural knowledge defining
what to do under what circumstances (conditional rules) and a representation of
the dynamics of road and traffic scenarios which enable prediction of how those
scenarios will develop (like an internalised mental video which runs on ahead of the
immediately-observed situation). Skills include basic vehicle control skills as well
as handling skills in challenging circumstances (such as skidding). Together these
biological characteristics and acquired characteristics through education, training
and experience determine the upper limit of competence of the driver. However,
this competence is not necessarily available at every moment. Performance is vul-
nerable to a host of variables which include motivation, fatigue, drowsiness, time
of day, drugs, distraction, emotion (such as fear, anger and aggression), stress and
level of effort. Any of these can undermine driver competence to yield a level of
capability at a somewhat lower level (see Fig. 10.4). We might label these variables
collectively as human factor variables. A further set of human factor variables re-
late to motivation for speed and therefore have an effect not so much on driver
capability but rather on the demand level of the driving task.
Driving task demands are determined by a range of interacting elements (see
Fig. 10.4). These include first the physical environmental factors such as visibility,
road alignment, road marking, road signs and signals, road surfaces and curve
camber angles and so on. Secondly, there are other road users with various prop-
erties including that of occupying or the imminent potential of occupying, critical
space in the projected path of the driver. Thirdly, there are the operational features
of the vehicle being driven, such as its information display characteristics, control
characteristics of steering, braking and accelerating and its capability to provide
roadway illumination in dark conditions. Finally and perhaps most important of
all, there are elements of task difficulty over which the driver has immediate and
direct control, namely the vehicle's trajectory and speed. Of these speed is clearly
the most significant factor: It is self-evident that the faster a driver travels, the
less time is available to take information in, process it and respond to it and the
less time there is to correct any emergent error. As mentioned earlier, the driving
task is a self-paced task and this means that in the last analysis driving task de-
mand is under the control of the driver (exceptions to this rule are where a driver
complies with a speed limit which is slower than the driver's preferred speed;
where a driver is under pressure to comply with a schedule or where a minimum
10. Motivational Determinants of Control in the Driving Task 173
biological
characteristics
ed ucation/tra ining!
experience ,,r- - - - - - - - - - - - - - -- - --- -''
, '
: LUC KY ESCA PE :
,, '
...
'- - - - - - - - - - - --- - - :.: - - ~
'
,
,
,I
: i- -- - - - - - '- -- --- j :- - - - - - - - - - - -
i : co.mpe nsatory i : CO LLIS ION
: i ac no n by others : ~.. : !
, 1 -_-_-,----- -[~~~-::.::;'/ :------------
I CA PA BILITY (C)
L - -_
...............
LOSS OF
CONT RO L
--
:--/~
i
_,
......y ....
t
11'---------
vehicle properti es
I
speed is required over a road section). Each of these task demand variables may
independently contribute to the level of task demand and they may also interact in
generating that demand. Furthermore, human factor variables may influence speed
choice, which in tum may influence other human factor variables such as arousal
level (see Fuller, 200Sa). These determinants of task demand and the determinants
of driver capability described above are brought together in the representation of
the task-capability interface model presented in Fig . lOA.
Thus far, what we have is a descriptive model of classes of variable which interact
at the interface between capability and task demand to determine the outcome for
the driver in terms of maintenance of control. We can now conceptualise and define
the difficulty of the driving task in terms of the degree of separation of task demand
and capability, with high difficulty where there is little separation and low difficulty
where there is large separation. Difficulty level as here defined is proportional to
the inverse of spare capacity, with spare capacity shrinking as difficulty level rises
174 Fuller
biological characteristics
education, training
experience
I
human factor variables
t
~ effort
1--------------
I
I
CA PA BILITY
(see Fig. 10.5). It may also be considered to be equivalent to the concept of mental
workload (de Waard, 2002), although a more comprehensive equivalence would
need to include physical workload as well.
As a model, the TCI model provides a conceptual framework for organizing
the critical variables which generate potential hazard scenarios and the conditions
under which the driver will lose control of the driving task and become vulnerable
to the range of possible disastrous consequences which might ensue . At this level of
description the model is mainly behavioural in the sense that it is largely confined
to describing observable phenomena (driver performance characteristics, history
of learning, training and experience and human factor variables on one side of
the key interface between capability and task demand and the characteristics of
the road and road-user environment, the vehicle, vehicle trajectory and speed
on the other). Note that this approach avoids a difficulty which is not addressed in
some other formulations (e.g., Deery, 1999) namely the important question of how
to define a hazard : This is determined in the model as the outcome of the interface
or transaction between capability and task demand.
perceived
capability; range of
journey goals; acceptable
effort task difficulty
motivation;
characteristic
difficulty level
preference decision and
perceived response
r----- task difficulty
I
I
I
I
I
I objective task
~
I difficulty effects on vehicle speed
I
I and position
I
~------------------------------------------ and
on other road users
in the complex dynamic of a driver engaging with the road and traffic system. One
important question then arises from a safety perspective and that is the question
of what determines the level of task difficulty that pertains at any moment of time.
It is self-evident that this is clearly not a random state that emerges quite inde-
pendently of driver determination. As stated earlier, driving is a self-paced task
and by manipulating the speed of the vehicle the driver has direct control over the
most important single determinant of task demand. The Tel model thus specifies
one key hypothesis, namely that drivers drive in such a way as to maintain level
of driving task difficulty within a preferred range. This hypothesis amounts to one
of task-difficulty (or workload) homeostasis, as represented in Fig. 10.6. Based
on the goals of a particular journey, self-appraisal of capability, effort motivation
and perhaps a reasonably stable preferred level of difficulty characteristic of the
individual, a driver' selects' a range of difficulty within which she or he is prepared
to operate and drives in such a way as to maintain experienced difficulty within
that range. Manipulation of speed is the primary mechanism for achieving this,
although undertaking or dumping other tasks secondary to the primary driving task
may be used on occasion (see, e.g., Hart and Wickens, 1990). This hypothesis of
task-difficulty homeostasis provides an intriguing explanation for why inexperi-
enced drivers are so vulnerable to loss of control and collision: They are liable to
underestimate task difficulty by (a) overestimating capability on the one hand and
(b) underestimating task demand on the other (some relevant evidence is reviewed
in Fuller, 2000). The hypothesis also provides a theoretical basis for the design of
safety interventions, for example by making the driving task appear more difficult
than it really is in situations where a reduction in driving speed is warranted. How-
ever what is more important for this discussion is to examine next the evidence for
the hypothesis of task-difficulty homeostasis.
176 Fuller
s:
Hpreferred speed i ;.1
Cl
.E ~r
.-
.r
I ---+-
Task difficulty
___ Statistical risk
" ./ ~ j Feelings of risk
f - - -. /
r:
,/,
/
/
10 speed hi
FIGURE 10.7. Stylised representation of driver ratings of task difficulty, statistical risk,
feelings of riskand preferred speedforroadsegments viewed at systematically incremented
speeds.
One strong prediction from the hypothesis is that, if all else is held constant,
task difficulty should correlate with speed. A second prediction is that estimates of
statistical risk should be zero until task demand begins to approach capability, that
is when difficulty level exceeds some criterion value. This point would correspond
with the driver 's perception of a real hazard emerging in the road and traffic
situation. A third prediction is that a driver's preferred level of difficulty should
be at a speed lower than that at which estimates of the statistical risk of loss-of-
control and crashing rise above zero. This last provides a further test of Wilde 's
target risk hypothesis. We have examined these predictions by soliciting drivers'
ratings of task difficulty and preferred speed as they viewed video-tapes of the
same segment of roadway travelled at different speeds . We also got the drivers
to provide an estimate of statistical risk by imagining travelling each segment at
a particular speed on 30 separate occasions and indicating how many times they
thought they would lose control. This technique was devised to enable drivers to
avoid the difficulty associated with providing a probability estimate of a single
rare event, at the same time enabling use of a meaningful estimate (e.g., 2 times
out of 30; 0.1 times out of 30, etc .).
The results confirmed both predictions one and two (see Fig. 10.7). Task dif-
ficulty correlated very highly with speed (speed accounted for over 98% of the
variance in ratings of difficulty) and over a range of slow to moderate speeds,
statistical risk ratings remained at zero and did not begin to rise until speeds were
very much faster. We obtained the same pattern for different types of roadway and
repeated the results in a subsequent replication (see Fuller et aI., 2006, in press).
In relation to the test of Wilde's Target risk hypothesis, as can be seen from the
vertical line in Fig. 10.7, the driver 's preferred speed was lower than the point at
which statistical risk exceeded zero, providing further evidence against the Target
risk concept. However the simulation studies threw up an unexpected discovery.
Along with ratings of task difficulty and ratings of statistical risk , we also asked
drivers to rate their feelings of risk, fully expecting those ratings to track their
statistical risk ratings . There seemed to be no a priori reason why feelings of
risk should be dissociated from ratings of statistical risk and there seemed to
10. Motivational Determinants of Control in the Driving Task 177
required level and the accompanying oxygen debt leaves the individual winded and
with pounding heart. Immediate cessation of the level of activity is then the only
option.
The selection by an individual of a particular level of sustainable workload for
a physical task will be determined by the goals of the task, perceived level of
capability (possibly modified by human factor variables such as level of activation
and level of fatigue), the level of effort the person is prepared to make and possibly
a level of preferred workload that is characteristic of the individual (as represented
in the distinction: She or he is or is not a 'hard worker').
Is there a similar system of sensitivity to mental workload (as one possible
representation of driving task difficulty) that parallels that for physical workload?
There is no question that one can be aware that different cognitive tasks demand
different degrees of cognitive effort (see, e.g., Shugan, 1980; Payne et al., 1993) but
there do not appear to be reliable and, more importantly, detectable physiological
changes which underpin that effort which can provide additional feedback to the
individual in the same manner as increased respiration, heart rate, temperature and
sweating as in the case of muscular effort. Nevertheless, of course, there are rate
and/or capacity limits in the cognitive processes of information uptake, working
memory storage and processing. As mental workload or task difficulty increase,
there comes a point where the cognitive requirement approaches these upper limits.
At this stage, just as with dynamic physical work, the sense of effort required will
increase significantly, there may also be a sense of mental fatigue, the condition
may even feel aversive, but there does not appear to be an equivalent sense of pain
(although this possibility was once amusingly portrayed in a Monty Python sketch
in which the intellectually challenged Mr. Gumby complained 'my brain hurts').
As indicated in Fig. 10.6 and just as in the case for a physical task, the selection
of a sustainable workload for a cognitive task (i.e., task difficulty) will be deter-
mined by the goals of the task, perceived level of capability, the level of effort the
person is prepared to make and again possibly a level of preferred workload that
is characteristic of the individual. But without the physical changes which feed
information back to the individual in physical work, how is a particular level of
work or task difficulty detected and controlled? One possibility relates to the goals
of the driving task.
The immediate goals of the driving task are twofold: To achieve a journey and
to maintain control. Control is the primary element here because without control
the journey cannot be completed. So perhaps we can rephrase the question as
'how is control represented in the system and what information provides feedback
regarding the status of control?'
We might speculate that the representation of being in a state of control includes
the ability to make progress, the ability to make avoidance responses where nec-
essary (e.g., of objects or other road users), the maintenance of adhesion to the
road surface, having access to critical information, such as the requirement for
an avoidance response and a rate of flow of information and rate of response re-
quirement that are within the individual's capability. All of these seem self-evident
because control would be lost if any of them were absent. Imagine a vehicle with
10. Motivational Determinants of Control in the Driving Task 179
but is not necessarily a substitute for it. However, when we immediatel y reject an
option that would lead to certain disaster, reasoning may be 'almost superfluous'.
Because emotional signals are body-related, Damasio labelled this set of ideas ' the
somatic marker hypothe sis'. Siovic et al. (2002) refer to a similar set of ideas as
' the affect heuristic ' . Throu gh learning, somatic markers become linked to stimuli
and patterns of stimuli. When a negative somatic marker is linked to an image of
a future outcome , it sounds an alarm (see an exampl e in Fig. 10.8).
What is compelling about the somatic marker hypothe sis and the affect heuris-
tic is the evidence cited in their support. Certain types of brain lesion specifically
exclude access to feelin gs associated with objects, events and scenarios. At the
same time they degrade decision performance: 'The powers of reason and the
experi ence of emotion decline together and their impairment stands out in a neu-
ropsychological profile within which basic attention , memory, intelligence and
language appear so intact that they could never be invoked to explain the patients'
failures in j udgement' (Damasio, 1994, pp. 53-54). Damasio (2003) has also out-
lined a plau sible and coherent neurological model which could sustain this entire
process. In addition, Siovic et al. (2002) and Loewenstein et al. (200 1) cite a num-
ber of experimental studies of decision making in normal individuals which clearl y
demon strate the interplay between emotion and reason, with the clear conclusion
that affect is essential to rational action.
In one such study cited by Siovic et aI., participants were asked to rate the
attractiveness of purchasing new equipment for use in the event of an airliner
crash-landing. It was hypothesised that saving 150 lives was a somewhat diffuse
10. Motivational Determinants of Control in the Driving Task 181
positive outcome and would have a relatively weak positive effect. On the other
hand, saving 98% of something would be more convincingly good and would have
a much stronger positive effect. In one condition, participants were told that the
equipment would save 150 lives, which would otherwise be at risk. In a second
condition, they were told that the equipment would make it possible to save 98%
of the 150 lives, which would otherwise be at risk. It was found that support for
the purchase of the life-saving equipment was significantly higher in the 98%
of 150 condition than in the 150 condition. Support for the purchase was also
higher than in the 150 condition in a third situation in which participants were told
the equipment would make it possible to save 85% of the 150 lives that would
otherwise be at risk.
Now it might be suggested that explanation in terms of the somatic marker
hypothesis simply brings us right back to the threat-avoidance model, as pro-
posed in 1984 (Fuller, 1984), but with the concept of 'threat' being unpacked in
terms of its associated negative, punishing feelings. Damasio (1994) argues that
somatic markers are acquired through experience, under the control of an internal
preference system and under the influence of an external set of circumstances.
The internal preference system consists of 'mostly innate regulatory dispositions,
posed (poised?) to ensure survival of the organism' (p. 179). The external set of
circumstances includes
'events relative to which individuals must act; possible options for action; possible future
outcomes for those actions; and the punishment or reward that accompanies a certain option,
both immediately and in deferred time, as outcomes of the opted action unfold ... The
interaction between an internal preference system and sets of external circumstances extends
the repertory of stimuli that will become automatically marked' (p. 179).
fixations by novice and experienced drivers, Underwood et aI. (2003) have shown
that on rural roads, two-fixation transitions by novices typically terminated in just
one zone, the road far ahead, whereas fixations by experienced drivers terminated
in five different parts of the scene. On a dual-carriageway, experienced drivers also
showed more extensive scanning, particularly in the horizontal plane. Underwood
et aI. interpreted this as evidence for greater sensitivity of experienced drivers to
prevailing traffic conditions. They concluded that the monitoring of other road
users is learned through experience and thus novices have relatively little ability to
switch the focus of their attention as potential hazards appear. The somatic marker
hypothesis offers the possibility that these learned differences in visual scanning
between novice and experienced drivers may be the result of learned affective
responses to events on the roadway, such as fast-moving vehicles merging from
both left and right in the dual-carriageway situation (Fuller, 2005b). Consistent with
this is the speculation by Loewenstein et aI. (2001), who suggest that age-related
differences in risk-taking may be 'affectively mediated', in particular, perhaps, by
differences in the vividness of mental simulations of consequences at the moment
of decision making.
Although linking of somatic markers to images is suggested to arise from a
process of learning, it is possible that affective responses to some events may be
unlearned reflex responses or that they are associated with a 'learning readiness' .
Put another way, it may be that there are universal somatically-marked stimuli.
Recognition of impending loss of control of a threatening situation might be one
such event that is, of course, highly pertinent to driver behaviour. Information
overload or an impossible response requirement may contribute substantially to
this recognition. Other unlearned but relevant affective responses, which might
at the same time also contribute to a feeling of loss of control, include the re-
sponses to a looming stimulus (rapid expansion of the retinal image of an object,
approaching collision; see, e.g., Schiff et aI., 1962; Bottomore, 1999; Franconeri
and Simons, 2003), to intense vestibular stimulation or g forces (e.g., Moro re-
flex; see, e.g., Goddard-Blythe, 1995) and to unexpected events (e.g., orienting
reflex). There may even be the equivalent of an inverse square law of affect inten-
sity in driving, with feeling intensity growing in proportion to the inverse square
root (or some other expression) of the time-to-line crossing or to collision, for
example.
A further prediction from the somatic marker hypothesis is that individual dif-
ferences in the affective response to particular scenarios should be associated with
different decisions in relation to those scenarios. Thus if we take speed choice
as an operationalisation of decision making, drivers who are more emotionally
reactive to road scenarios representing various degrees of threat (or impending
loss of control) should opt for lower speeds than drivers who are less reactive. If
there are stable individual differences in emotional reactivity (Larsen and Diener,
1987), this could mean that the same situation would ring alarm bells somewhat
differently for different individuals. Some may be relatively so 'deaf' that an im-
pending hazard has to be right on top of them before they are able to hear it, so
to speak. These persons would unwittingly be in a condition of delayed avoidance
10. Motivational Determinants of Control in the Driving Task 183
(Fuller, 1984). Fujita et al. (1991) have shown that women experience negative
and positive affect more intensely than men. They asked 100 students to complete
the affect intensity measure (Larsen and Diener, 1987), which consists of 40 items
that measure how intensely participants feel emotions, yielding both a positive
and negative affect intensity score. Females scored higher on both positive and
negative affect intensity. Is it the case, then, that male-female differences in risk
taking are mediated by differences in emotional reactions to potentially hazardous
situations? Perhaps males crash more because they feel less.
Another individual difference factor that may be relevant in this discussion is the
tendency to seek enhanced external stimulation. Personality traits of extraversion
and sensation seeking are both considered to be constitutional characteristics of
the individual (Zuckerman, 1979) and both are associated with a preference for
enhanced levels of stimulation. Does this then mean that individuals with such
traits may be more likely to accept higher levels of somatic arousal and tolerate
more readily a driving situation where task demand is very close to capability, in
other words tolerate smaller safety margins? Research on individual differences
and accident involvement tends to support this prediction (Loo, 1979). Individu-
als high in sensation seeking are more likely to speed, overtake and adopt shorter
headways. They are also over-represented in traffic crashes (Jonah, 1997). Further-
more, Dahlen et al. (2005) have reported that degree of sensation seeking predicts
dangerous driving.
If the somatic marker hypothesis is correct in its implications for attention
capture by competent stimuli, the possibility presents itself that emotional re-
sponses arising from the unfolding road and traffic scenario may be drowned
out by or misattributed to other emotions or indeed, may even be suppressed or
extinguished (see concept of 'desensitisation' in Summala, 1986). For example,
feelings of anger may overwhelm the somatic marker indications, which would
otherwise inform decision making. Indeed is this the effect we are referring to
when we talk about rage being 'blind'? Deffenbacher et al. (2003) have shown
that high anger drivers are between 1.5 and 2.0 times more likely to engage in
risky driving, such as exceeding the speed limit and not wearing a seatbelt. Dahlen
et al. (2005), in a questionnaire study of 224 undergraduate drivers, found that the
propensity to become angry while driving predicted risky driving, minor losses of
vehicle control and loss of concentration. Levelt (2001) has reported that drivers
who say they are often irritated when driving also say that speeding is often the
consequence and Carbonell et al. (1997) found that anxiety combined with time
pressures can lead to engaging in dangerous manoeuvres (see review by Mesken,
2003).
10.3 Conclusions
Thus it is suggested here, albeit speculatively, that feelings of risk in driving may
arise from a sensitivity to changes which signal an impending loss of control and
that these feelings inform the driver's experience of task difficulty or workload.
184 Fuller
They (or in Damasio's sense, their underlying emotional substrates) may arise
naturally and spontaneously or through a process of learned association. They
have the power to direct attention to pertinent stimuli and to determine priorities
amongst response options. In short, they are integral to driver decision making,
even where they are so weak they are equivalent to what Slovic et al. (2002) call
'whispers of affect' . If we reinterpret the hypothesis of task-difficulty homeostasis
in terms of feelings of risk, the implication would be that drivers drive in such a way
as to keep feelings of risk below some threshold level (which may even be zero). If
the behaviour of other road users or the driver's own behaviour (such as an increase
in speed) should stimulate an increase in feelings of risk above this threshold level,
then the driver will take action to bring the level of felt risk back down, such as
by reducing speed. Only if the rewards of any supra-threshold feelings of risk are
compensated for by rewarding consequences of one sort or another will the driver
intentionally tolerate any increase above threshold. Nevertheless, increases in risk
may not be felt because of the swamping effect of other feelings or if felt they may
be attributed to events other than those related to the driving task. And experience
may not have provided sufficient learning opportunities to link particular potential
hazard scenarios to feelings of risk, that is, to provide the link to a somatic marker.
If we can accept all this as a working hypothesis and the evidence continues to
support it, then a whole new agenda for driver behaviour research emerges (see
Fuller, 2005b).
From the forgoing discussion it can be seen that the somatic marker hypothesis
has the potential to provide a unifying explanation for a diverse set of empirical
findings in the domain of driver behaviour, including our finding that feelings of
risk track ratings of driving task difficulty and speed almost perfectly. It also raises
a number of new questions regarding the role of affect and emotional condition-
ing in attention and decision making. This makes the experimental evaluation of
the somatic marker hypothesis of some importance in the contemporary research
agenda. It may be noted that one author has already begun to develop a model
of driver decision making based fundamentally on the somatic marker hypothesis
(Vaa, 2004, and this volume) and that Summala has proposed a thesis along simi-
lar lines (Summala, 2005, and this volume). The implication is that if we want to
understand driver decision making more clearly, we need to take into account not
just thinking but also feeling. Perhaps this is in part why decision making in safety-
sensitive industries, such as commercial aviation, has moved away from individual
decision making (reliance on somatic markers?) towards standard operating pro-
cedures (SOPs): Prescriptive rules for dealing with each contingency experienced.
In areas such as aircraft maintenance, where the affective consequences of inap-
propriate actions must be relatively weak, a reliance on SOPs must be especially
important for maintaining system safety. In this context any deviation from SOPs in
decision making is a matter of very serious concern (McDonald et al., 1999). Work
in this area also highlights the importance of avoiding conflict between SOPs and
what is perceived by the operative to be a better procedure - avoiding the tension
between formal and normal ways of working (Ward, 2005).
10. Motivational Determinants of Control in the Driving Task 185
References
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human
Decision Processes, 50, 179-211.
Armitage, CJ. and Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behavior: A meta-
analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 471-499.
Bern, S. and de long, H.L. (2006). Theoretical issues in psychology (2nd ed.). Sage Publi-
cations, London.
Bottomore, S. (1999). The panicking audience? Early cinema and the 'train effect'. Histor-
ical Journal of Film, Radio and Television, 19(2), 177-216.
Carbonell, EJ., Banuls, R., Chisvert, M., Monteagudo, MJ. and Pastor, G. (1997). A com-
parative study of anxiety responses in traffic situations as predictors of accident rates
in professional drivers. Proceedings of the Second Seminar in Human Factors in Road
Traffic, Universidade do Minho, Braga.
Dahlen, E.R., Martin, R.C., Ragan, K. and Kuhlman, M.M. (2005). Driving anger, sensa-
tion seeking, impulsiveness, and boredom proneness in the prediction of unsafe driving.
Accident Analysis and Prevention, 37, 341-348.
Damasio, A.R. (1994). Descartes' error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. Putnam,
New York.
Damasio, A.R. (2003). Looking for spinoza: Joy, sorrow and the feeling brain. Heinemann,
London.
Deery, H.A. (1999). Hazard and risk perception among young novice drivers. Journal of
Safety Research, 30(4), 225-236.
Deffenbacher, lL., Lynch, R.S., Filetti, L.B., Dahlen, E.R. and Oetting, E.R. (2003). Anger,
aggression, risky behavior, and crash-related outcomes in three groups of drivers. Be-
haviour Research and Therapy, 41,333-349.
de Waard, D. (2002). Mental workload. In R. Fuller and lA. Santos (Eds.), Humanfactors
for highway engineers. Pergamon, Oxford, pp. 161-176.
Elliott, M.A., Armitage, CJ. and Baughan, CJ. (2003). Drivers' compliance with speed
limits: An application of the theory of planned behavior. Journal ofApplied Psychology,
88(5), 964-972.
Evans, L. (1991) Traffic safety and the driver. Van Nostrand Reinhold, New York.
Franconeri, S.L. and Simons, DJ. (2003). Moving and looming stimuli capture attention.
Perception and Psychophysics, 65(7), 999-1010.
Fujita, F., Diener, E. and Sandvik, E. (1991). Gender differences in negative affect and well-
being: The case for emotional intensity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
61,427-434.
Fuller, R. (1984). A conceptualisation of driving behaviour as threat avoidance, Ergonomics,
27, 1139-1155.
Fuller, R. (1991a). The modification of individual road user behaviour. In MJ. Koomstra
and J. Christensen (Eds.). Enforcement and rewarding: Strategies and effects. SWOV
Institute for Road Safety Research, Leidschendam, pp. 33-40.
Fuller, R. (1991b). Behaviour analysis and unsafe driving: Warning - Learning trap ahead!
Journal ofApplied Behaviour Analysis, 24, 73-75.
Fuller, R. (2000). The task-capability interface model of the driving process. Recherche
Transports Securite, 66,47-59.
Fuller, R. (2002). Learning to drive. In P. Barjonet (Ed.), Traffic psychology today. Kluwer
Academic Publishers, Dordrecht, pp. 105-118.
186 Fuller
Fuller, R. (2005a). Towards a general theory of driver behaviour. Accident Analysis and
Prevention, 37,461-472.
Fuller, R. (2005b). Driving by the seat of your pants: A new agenda for research.
In Behavioural research in road safety 2005. Department for Transport, London,
pp.85-93.
Fuller, R. and Farrell, E. (2001). Operation lifesaver assessment. RS 459. Project aLA,
NRA, Dublin.
Fuller, R. and Santos, 1.A. (2002). Psychology and the highway engineer. In R. Fuller and
J.A. Santos (Eds.), Human factors for highway engineers. Pergamon, Oxford, pp. 1-10.
Fuller, R., McHugh, C. and Pender, S. (2006). Task difficulty and risk in the determination
of driver behaviour. European Review ofApplied Psychology. In press.
Geller, E.S. (1998). Applications of behavior analysis to prevent injuries from vehicle
crashes. Behavior monographs: Cambridge Center for Behavioral Studies, Cambridge.
Goddard-Blythe, S. (1995). The role of primitive survival reflexes in the development of
the visual system. Journal ofBehavioural Optometry, 6, 31-35.
Harrison, W.A. (2005). A demonstration of avoidance learning in turning decisions at
intersections. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 8F,
4-5, pp. 341-354.
Hart, S.G. and Wickens, C.D. (1990). Workload assessment and prediction. In H.R.
Booher (Ed.). MANPRINT: An emerging technology. Advanced concepts for integrat-
ing people, machines and organizations. Van Nostrand Reinhold, New York, pp. 257-
300.
Hutton, K.A., Sibley, C.G., Harper, D.N. and Hunt, M. (2001). Modifying driver behaviour
with passenger feedback. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Be-
haviour, 4F, 4, 271-278.
Janssen, W.H. and Tenkink, E. (1988). Considerations on speed selection and risk home-
ostasis in driving. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 20, 137-142.
Jonah B.A. (1997). Sensation seeking and risky driving: A review and synthesis of the
literature. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 29, 651-665.
Larsen, R.J. and Diener, E. (1987). Affect intensity as an individual difference characteristic:
A review. Journal ofResearch in Personality, 21, 1-39.
Levelt, P.B.M. (2001). Emoties bij vrachtautochauffers. R-2001-14, SWay Institute for
Road Safety Research, Leidschendam, The Netherlands.
Loewenstein, G.F., Weber, E.U., Hsee, C.K. and Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psy-
chological Bulletin, 127(2), 267-286.
Loo, R. (1979). Role of primary personality factors in the perception of traffic signs and
driver violations and accidents. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 11, 125-127.
Ludwig, T.D. and Geller, E.S. (2000). Intervening to improve the safety of delivery drivers:
A systematic behavioral approach. Journal of Organizational Behavior Management,
19(4),1-124.
MacCorquodale, K. and Meehl, P.E. (1948). On a distinction between hypothetical con-
structs and intervening variables. Psychological Review, 55, 95-107.
Makinen, T., Biecheler-Fretel, M.M., Cardoso, 1., Fuller, R., Goldenbeld, C., Hakkert, S.,
Sanchez Martin, M.C., Skladana, P., Vaa T. and Zaidel D. (1999). Legal measures and
enforcement. GADGET WP-5-Report, VTI, Espoo.
McDonald, N., Daly, C., Corrigan, S., Cromie, S. and Ward, M. (1999). Human-centred
management guide for aircraft maintenance. APRG, Trinity College, Dublin.
Mesken, 1. (2003). The role of emotions and moods in traffic. D-2003-8, SWOV Institute
for Road Safety Research, Leidschendam, The Netherlands.
10. Motivational Determinants of Control in the Driving Task 187
Michon, J.A. (1989). Explanatory pitfalls and rule-based driver models. Accident Analysis
and Prevention, 21(4), 341-353.
Miller, G.A., Galanter, E. and Pribram, K.H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior.
Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York.
Naatanen, R. and Summala, H. (1976). Road user behaviour and traffic accidents. North
Holland/Elsevier, Amsterdam and New York.
Oborne, D.1. (1995). Ergonomics at work (3rd ed.). Wiley, Chichester.
OECD - Road Transport Research (1990). Behavioural adaptation to changes in the road
transport system. OECD, Paris.
Payne, J.W., Bettman, J.R. and Johnson, EJ. (1993). The adaptive decision maker. Cam-
bridge University Press, Cambridge.
Ranney, T.A. (1994). Models of driving behavior: A review of their evolution. Accident
Analysis and Prevention, 26(6), pp. 733-750.
Reinhardt-Rutland, A.H. (2001). Seat-belts and behavioural adaptation: The loss of looming
as a negative reinforcer. Safety Science, 39, 3, 145-156.
Rumar, K., Berggrund, U., Jernberg, P. and Ytterbom, U. (1976). Driver reaction to a
technical measure: Studded tyres. Human Factors, 18,433--454.
Schiff, W., Caviness, J.A. and Gibson, J.1. (1962). Persistent fear responses in rhesus mon-
keys to the optical stimulus of "looming." Science, 136, 982-983.
Shugan, S.M. (1980). The cost of thinking. Journal of Consumer Research, 7, 99-111.
Slovic, P., Finucane, M.L., Peters, E. and MacGregor, D.G. (2002). Risk as analysis and risk
as feelings. Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk and rationality. Paper presented at the
Annual Meeting ofthe Society for Risk Analysis. New Orleans, Louisiana, 10 December,
2002.
Slovic, P., Fischoff, B., Lichtenstein, S., Corrigan, B. and Coombs, B. (1977). Preference
for insuring against probably small losses: Insurance implications. Journal of Risk and
Insurance, 44, 237-258.
Summala, H. (1986). Risk control is not risk adjustment: The zero-risk theory of driver
behavior and its implications. Reports 11: 1986. University of Helsinki Traffic Research
Unit, Helsinki.
Summala, H. (1997). Hierarchical model of behavioural adaptation and traffic accidents. In
T. Rothengatter and E. Carbonell Vaya (Eds.). Traffic and transport psychology: Theory
and application. Elsevier Science, Oxford, pp. 41-52.
Summala, H. (2005). Towards understanding driving behaviour and safety efforts. In L.
Macchi, C. Re and P.C. Cacciabue (Eds.), Proceedings ofthe International Workshop on
Modelling Driver Behaviour in Automotive Environments. European Commission, Joint
Research Centre, Ispra, Italy, 25-27 May, 2005. Office for Official Publication of the
European Communities, Luxembourg, pp. 205-214.
Taylor, D.H. (1964). Drivers' galvanic skin response and the risk of accident. Ergonomics,
7, 439--451.
Underwood, G., Chapman, P., Brocklehurst, N., Underwood, J. and Crundall, D. (2003).
Visual attention while driving: Sequences of eye fixations made by experienced and
novice drivers. Ergonomics, 46, pp. 629-646.
Vaa, T. (2003). Survival or deviance? A modelfor driver behaviour. TOI Report 666/2003,
Institute of Transport Economics, Oslo.
Ward, M. (2005). Contributions to human factors from three case studies in aircraft main-
tenance. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Dublin, Trinity College.
Wilde, G.1.S. (2001). Target risk 2: A new psychology of safety and health: What works?
What doesn't? and why? PDE Publications, Toronto.
188 Fuller
Young, P.M., Clegg, B.A. and Smith, C.A.P. (2004). Dynamic models of augmented cog-
nition. International Journal ofHuman-Computer Interaction, 17(2),259-273.
Zajonc, R.B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psy-
chologist, 35, 151-175.
Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking: Beyond the optimal level of arousal. Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, New Jersey.
11
Towards Understanding
Motivational and Emotional
Factors in Driver Behaviour:
Comfort Through Satisficing!
HEIKKI SUMMALA
11.1 Introduction
The early 'skill models' of driver behaviour and safety posited that the safety
of a driver is mainly determined by the level of his or her perceptual and motor
skills in relation to the task demands: a crash - a failure in driver performance -
occurs when task demands exceed driver capabilities (e.g., Blumenthal, 1968).
Consequently, improving driver skills and reducing task demands would make
traffic safer. Obviously, however, this early concept was too simple. A good piece
of counterevidence, among others, came from Williams and O'Neill (1974), who
showed that classified U.S. race drivers have more crashes per exposure than
average drivers - and also more speeding tickets. At least the advanced skills which
make those drivers competitive in race track did not save them from crashes, as
they obviously traded skills for speed on ordinary roads, too. Theorists and road
safety people indeed forgot that driving is a self-paced task and drivers themselves
do determine their task demands to a large extent (Naatanen and Summala, 1974).
The behavioural adaptation concept is now one of basic tenets in traffic psychology,
here defined by Summala (1996): 'the driver is inclined to react to changes in the
traffic system, whether they be in the vehicle, in the road environment, in road and
weather conditions, or in hislher own skills or states, and that this reaction occurs
in accordance with hislher motives' .
The safe completion of the trip is usually taken for granted by drivers. Therefore,
time goals, conservation of effort, maintenance of speed and progress, pleasure of
driving, all what Naatanen and Summala (1976) called extra motives, gain ground
in driver behaviour and cannot be forgotten anymore: they tend to push drivers
towards hazards primarily in terms of higher speed and shorter safety margins.
But what are the mechanisms which shelter us of crashes, of going too close to
hazards, and of exposing us to hazards?
IThis is an extension of the author's earlier paper presented in the symposium on traffic
psychology theories in the 3rd International Congress of Traffic and Transport Psychology
(see Summala, 2005).
189
190 Summala
One line of thinking attempted to find one single motivational measure which
could explain driver behaviour, even including the moment-to-moment speed con-
trol. It started from the work of Taylor (1964). In two on-road studies he measured
galvanic skin responses in drivers who were driving in a wide range of roads and
road conditions. He found that GSR activity varied substantially by conditions but,
when controlled for speed, it was quite evenly distributed in time. He concluded
that it is the level of emotional tension or anxiety which guides the driver: 'Driving
is a self-paced task governed by the level of emotional tension or anxiety which
the driver wishes to tolerate'. Interestingly, Taylor probably interpreted his results
at least partly wrongly. Variation in speed and GSR activity in Taylor's data cor-
related with road type and road conditions such that lower speed and higher GSR
activity occurred on road sections with more side turnings and more other traffic
(Taylor, 1964). It can be assumed therefore that an important source of GSR was
motor activity rather than anxiety (see also Naatanen and Summala, 1976, p. 191).
However, Taylor's conclusion was quite influential on later theoretical work.
Wilde (1982), in his well-known and much debated model, proposed that drivers
tend to target a certain level of risk. The actual model is a simple homeostatic
(thermostatic) system with a traditional optimising decision model included in it,
but the model is best known from its safety prediction: Should any changes be made
in the traffic system, road users tend to maintain a certain target level of risk and,
therefore, safety level keeps approximately constant. It is obvious that this is not the
case, as already predicted by Smeed (1949) and confirmed by many big successes
in safety developments (see, e.g., Evans, 1991; Robertson and Pless, 2002). The
feedback loop from accident statistics (the knowledge of crash risk in a given
jurisdiction) in Wilde's model also seems quite inefficient to guide drivers' daily
task control and choices. Fuller (2000, 2004, 2005, see also Chapter lOin this book)
incorporates the concept of the target into his task-capability interface model.
He claims that drivers are sensitive to task difficulty and try to keep experienced
difficulty within a certain margin in a homeostatic loop. Vaa (2004, see also Chapter
12 in this book) lists several candidates for drivers' targets. Drivers may target for
a certain arousal level, in all of its varieties, and sensation, pleasure, security,
workload, avoiding violations or even non-compliance of the rules. Finally, while
applying Damasio's model of emotions and feelings, he proposes that the pacing
factor is target feeling or best feeling, and the body is the risk monitor (Damasio,
1994).
But to what degree dynamic driver behaviour can be explained with one 'mo-
tivational', targeted control measure which drivers tend to adjust? And, could it
be possible to operationalise such a measure to test it adequately, and to apply in
automotive and transportation research and development?
Naatanen and Summala (1974, 1976) adopted quite a different position. In line
with Taylor (1964), they claimed that driving is a self-paced task. They proposed
that the task difficulty level is determined by the drivers according to their motives.
Drivers are not only able to compensate for changes in the degree of difficulty of
11. Towards Understanding Motivational and Emotional Factors 191
(a) (b)
FIGURE 11.1. (a) Humans can be seen as having different zones (intimate, personal, social)
around them (Hall, 1966), with thresholds which trigger approach or avoidance response,
depending on the approaching person (or animal or object). (b) When in motion, more space
is needed in front to avoid collisions with objects. The safety zone (Gibson and Crooks,
1938), whether in motion or not, can also be seen as a comfort zone, with no threat, risk, or
discomfort felt (cf. 'zero-risk model', Naatanen and Summala, 1976; Summala, 1988).
traffic situations by modifying their efforts (attention, vigilance), but they also
determine the nature and degree of the difficulty of these various situations and
their current task (Naatanen and Summala, 1976, p. 35). This is the prerequisite
for the fact that many safety measures have not led to expected results.
In sharp contrast to Taylor's and Wilde's models, they proposed a threshold
model with three major starting points:
(1) 'Subjective risk monitor' for present or anticipated fear is the major inhibitory,
limiting mechanism in driver behaviour.
(2) Drivers' goals and excitatory motives push them towards the limits, e.g., to-
wards higher speed if not otherwise restricted.
(3) Safety margins are in a key position in driver's task control (see the next
section).
In their control flow model, Naatanen and Summala (1974) postulated a moti-
vation module which produces desired actions or tends to keep pace (to maintain
progress). Action is continuously monitored by the subjective risk monitor which,
given a certain threshold is exceeded or anticipated, alerts and takes a role in deci-
sions. Drivers' goals and so-called extra motives, either arisen in traffic or brought
from outside it, got an important role in Naatanen and Summala model.
Phenomenally, it is a sort of tongue protruding forward along the road. Its boundaries are
chiefly determined by objects or features of the terrain with a negative 'valence' in percep-
tion. It is not, however, merely a subjective experience of the driver. It exists objectively as
the actual field within which the car can safely operate, whether or not the driver is aware of
it. It shifts and changes continually, bending and twisting with the road, and also elongating
and contracting, widening and narrowing, according as obstacles encroach on it and limits
its boundaries. (Gibson and Crooks, 1938, p. 121)
The safety zone, or safety margins which road users must keep around them,
can be expressed, measured, and are functional in both space and time. We have
to drive a car through gaps, in space or time, and to keep distance to other ve-
hicles and to pedestrians, bicyclists and road-side obstacles. This distance from
crash is essential metrics in everyday control of safety. On a two-lane road, for
example, an almost certain death lurks at a distance of 2 to 3 m when one meets
a heavy vehicle (even a car) and every driver must take care of keeping a suf-
ficient margin. If the distance - safety margin - is not wide enough, we feel
uncomfortable.
However, car control is also extensively based on time margins such as time-
to-collision and time-to-line-crossing. Therefore, keeping the car in a lane is not
a simple tracking task nulling the error from the mid of the lane or the intended
trajectory (cf. Donges, 1978). The latter model might be relevant in 'active driving' ,
with all attention focussed on driving and optimal lane control. In normal everyday
driving, instead, a lane should rather be conceived as a tube with a lot of tolerance
and time to correct path within it. As proposed by Godthelp et al. (1984), time-
to-line-crossing is a relevant measure, referring to the time until the car drifts out
of the lane if not corrected through steering. It is the measure which sets the time
limit - the threshold - for drivers' path correcting.
Time-to-collision or time-to-contact is a central control measure when we con-
tinuously adjust distance to a braking vehicle in front of us (Lee, 1976), or start
braking when approaching an obstacle or a stop line at a crossing (van der Horst,
1990). It defines time distance to a crash, or to the moment that braking must be
11. Towards Understanding Motivational and Emotional Factors 193
started to avoid the crash. As proposed by Lee (1976), the optical variable r alone
gives a direct approximation of the time to collision, and its first-time derivative
(tau dot) provides a feasible strategy to control braking when kept within a certain
range. There is indeed a lot of evidence that the human perceptual system is well
equipped to accurately estimate time to collision, at short distances at least when
optic resolution of the looming object is sufficient. This means that at any moment
we know when we will crash with a car or an obstacle we are approaching to.
However, this information does not yet reveal whether we are able to stop before
the obstacle, or when we should start braking to avoid the crash.
Quite recently, Fajen (2005) showed that performance in stopping in front of an
obstacle also depends on global optic flow and edge rate (providing speed infor-
mation)' and that information is needed (along with tau) to tell whether stopping
is possible with available braking force. In other words, this information - open
to continuous calibration of maximum deceleration level - provides us with an
estimate of the action boundary, that is, the limit above which stopping is no more
possible. Fajen (2005) also returns to Gibson and Crooks (1938): it was already
in their paper that, in defining the safety zone, they made a distinction between
trajectories which are available and the ones which not. It was a beginning of
the later development of Gibson's concept of affordance - what the environment
affords to the human.
It should be added that (mainly vision-based) space and time margins are not
of course the only ones to determine and to set limits for driver behaviour. For
example, while time-to-line-crossing has a role when approaching a bend, lateral
acceleration is a marked factor and a marked source of proprioceptive and kines-
thetic information in curves and steering manoeuvres (Reymond et al., 2001).
Available friction in a curve can also be seen as a safety margin measure (Rumar
et al., 1976; Summala and Merisalo, 1980; Wong and Nicholson, 1992), indicating
a distance to a loss of grip and control. At low friction and low speed in a steep
off-ramp drivers typically drive close to the threshold of loss of control, in terms
of available friction, while safety margins grow with friction (when slipperiness
decreases) and speed (Summala and Merisalo, 1980). It suggests that available
friction margin is far from a simple distance measure. Drivers may misperceive
the slipperiness at approach phase - in wintertime lower volume ramps are often
more slippery than motorway lanes - but they may also accept a smaller margin to
grip loss at slow speed when the chances to regain control are bigger (cf. Brown,
1980) and consequences of loss of control and running off the road smaller. Some
drivers - young men especially - may also intentionally make their car skidding
in more or less good control.
It is essential to note that time safety margins have an important feature: they imply
a concept of available time. Available time determines brake reaction latencies
as well as time sharing while driving, among other things. The timing of brake
194 Summala
L MOBILITY NEEDS
and possibilities
strategic level
~
TRIP DECISIONS
~ SHORT
OF
TIME:
TARGET SPEED increased
tactical level
MANOEUVRING
/" mental
load,
modifies
overtaking higher-
~~~~sing management ~ level
goals
SAFETY
operational level
MARGINS
time margins
PLENTY OF
TIME:
highly
automated,
effortless
easy
monotonous
task
FIGURE 11.2. The role of time is essential when a human is in motion and making travel
decisions. Time margins and available time for action, through mental load, also mediate
between different levels of driving (adapted from Summala, 1997).
of car and traffic) at this speed, within available time margins, he or she feels high
mental load and
either slows down to get more time, to reach the feeling of control and an acceptable
(or comfortable) level of workload; or
continues in the unstable zone, feels increased mental load, and may experience
sudden overload problems.
However, if a driver repeatedly or continuously experiences certain conditions
too loading, pressing, or difficult, he or she starts to avoid them. For example,
196 Summala
elderly drivers with slower performance (and lower contrast sensitivity and in-
creased glare discomfort) start to avoid driving in the dark, in heavy traffic and in
winter time, etc.
On the other side, if a driver has plenty of time, like on a high-standard road
with a fairly low speed limit, he/she is inclined to perform secondary tasks; or
is at risk of getting bored and tired, both of which mean unstable performance
of the driver/vehicle system. Secondary tasks, while driving, require reliable time
and attention sharing and easily lead to delayed detection of relevant information,
overload, and increased risk. Underload and lowered arousal level also result in
delayed responses and attention lapses, if not falling asleep.
Available time incorporates many essential features like task (environment)
complexity, mental load and stress. It is, by definition, always a factor when humans
are in motion. It is also a factor when road users are doing decisions while stopped
in traffic, in gap acceptance for example, when drivers are crossing or entering a
priority road. In that case the traffic flow on a main road actually means making
a selection from a number of closing time margins, and the situation may be very
loading.
The key issue in the model is that time margins (in the meaning of distance from
crash or from the normal or 'last' response threshold) are very basic measures for
humans, with a strong affective component if certain limits are violated. Available
time in tum defines what we can do in each situation, how loading we feel a
situation, and therefore it provides both cognitive and affective mechanism for
control. Hollnagel (2002) and Hollnagel and Woods (2005) similarly gave the
concept of available time a marked position in their Extended Control Model,
such that time and control are closely intertwined and mediate between different
hierarchical control levels (see also Chapter 4 in this volume).
directly dependent on the length of the knee, and Warren and Whang (1987)
showed that an affordable aperture for walking through without body rotation de-
pends on shoulder width. Available space in relation to the shoulder width there-
fore determines the action which is most comfortable and efficient, as well as
the transition point between two choices, unrotated and rotated pass. The affor-
dances of the car/driver system are similarly related to the 'body size' of the
vehicle, but also on the driver's ability to estimate car dimensions relative to the
available gap and to steer through it. Similarly, a gap between vehicles in the
oncoming flow is sufficient or not for overtaking a slower vehicle in front, de-
pending on its size, vehicles' speeds, the own car's acceleration and the driver's
estimation skills. Therefore, the skills acquired during the driving career essen-
tially affect on what is affordable. This is also the essence of the process where
novice drivers extend the realm of the possible operations they will attempt while
learning to know dynamic, spatiotemporal limits of the driver-car unit amid of
traffic:
Every decision made, every action taken, every traffic situation one is exposed to, provides
some kind of feedback to the driver. It is presumably this feedback that is the really efficient
driving teacher. As to the aspect of the safety of different actions, decisions and situations, the
related feedback is presumably received in the form of subjective time and space separating
the driver from an accident; hence the driver can test different kinds of operations, driving
manners, etc. against these subjective dimensions and thus finally develop rather permanent
criteria for different kinds of actions, decisions and traffic situations. It is proposed that
these criteria are subject to a continuous back-and-forth change, at least to a minor extent,
through the whole driving career. (Naatanen and Summala, 1976, pp. 87-88)
we could say that in the potentially hostile road environment the options on the
road obviously are tagged with either negative (risky) or positive (goal-directed)
somatic markers. These markers then precede rational evaluation and facilitate and
speed up the choice. In dynamic time-limited situations like driving, fast affective
heuristics must have a big role (Finucane et aI., 2000). We could even say that safety
margins have such a role (see already in Summala, 1988), telling what choices are
affordable and what are not, and when the situation is going out of the control and
needs an appropriate reaction. In line with Naatanen and Summala's model, given
certain environmental information through perception and expectancies it triggers,
motives feed desired actions while 'risk monitor' blocks implementation if fired
through perceived or anticipated threat.
output of many cognitively definable processes all of which can be included under
the shared umbrella concept of comfort.
The factors to be kept within the 'comfort zone', to exemplify normal driving
situations, are proposed below, both including inhibitory and excitatory ones. Note
that for each main factor, there are specific models to explain cognitive processes
involved.
frightens: this is the affective 'subjective risk' monitor proposed by Naatanen and
Summala (1974, 1976).
Urgenthurry Reactive
Beginner
ACTIVATION
tense Competitive
alert
Frustrated, nervous excited
aggressive
stressed
upset
sad
depressed
relaxed
lethargic Comfortable,
fatigued
calm routine
DEACTIVATION
Drowsy
FIGURE 11.3. Driving moods as projected on the two-dimensional schematic map of core
affects (Russell and Feldman Barrett, 1999). Routine, daily, comfortable driving ranges
from contended to relaxed or calm.
mobility goals, such as hurry, social pressure (in car and by other drivers), self-
enhancement, competition, thrill seeking, may also take control. Stronger emo-
tions arise when driving becomes competitive, or reactive in heavy traffic, while
hurry and frustrated aggressive driving exemplifies aroused (activated) emotions
on the side of displeasure (Fig. 11.3). Even a thought that the roadway might
be slippery due to black ice makes driving tense and unpleasant, at least for a
while.
However, it is claimed here that normal everyday driving is largely habitual
activity, aiming at keeping within comfortable limits, with no feelings of risk, or
anxiety, or discomfort. The output is, then, a fairly stable emotional state (which,
unfortunately, every now and then is disturbed by hurry, frustration, aggression
and other strong effects). Our cognitive system provides fairly good information
on where the limits of 'safety zone' are (even if it often tends to be biased) while
the emotional system essentially contributes and warns about 'wrong' choices or
when limits are being approached. This is well in line with weak emotional signals
proposed by Damasio (1994; for more detail, see also Chapters 10 and 12 in this
volume).
It is interesting to see how strongly emotions have now intruded into modelling
human (risky) choices. Damasio's (1994) book and subsequent experimental work
(e.g., Bechara et aI., 1997) has attracted much enthusiasm. Also the long tradition of
risk perception research, in the meaning of evaluating risks of different activities
11. Towards Understanding Motivational and Emotional Factors 203
(e.g., Fischhoff et al., 1978) - has now more explicitly welcome the emotional
component in risky decisions, 'risk as feeling' (e.g., Slovic et al., 2004). However,
early 'risk theories' in driver behaviour already assumed a marked emotional
component in driver task control. Taylor (1964) indeed launched the 'emotional
tension' as a critical measure, and Naatanen and Summala (1976), while describing
the function of 'risk monitor' , considered risk as feeling:
Hence, the general view of the road users' behavioral dynamics was advanced that his
behavior is continuously pushed by his (other-than-safety) motives in their direction. For
example, a driver in a hurry wants more and more speed, but that somewhere along this shift
toward those behavioral forms which give more and more satisfaction to his (excitatory)
motives (which, simultaneously, are also in general more dangerous), the' Subjective Risk
Monitor' becomes activated and this development is usually soon stopped. It was suggested
that under 'normal', 'relaxed' driving motivation, only those decisions are made which are
not associated with subjective risk (at the moment of decision) and that when subjective
risk is felt in some actual driving situation, the behavior is changed so as to eliminate the
source of this feeling. (Naatanen and Summala, 1976, p. 221)
References
Ajzen, L (1991). The theory of planned action. Organizational Behavior and Human Deision
Processes, 50, 179-211.
Baguley, C.1. (1988). 'Running the red' at signals on high-speed roads. Traffic Engineering
and Control, 29, 415-420.
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. and Damasio, A.R. (1997). Deciding advantageously
before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275(5304), 1293-1295.
Black, S. (1966). Man and motor cars. Seeker & Warburg, London.
Blumenthal, M. (1968). Dimensions of the traffic safety problem. Traffic Safety Research
Review, 12,7-12.
Brown, LD. (1980). Error-correction probability as a determinant of drivers' subjective risk.
In D.1. Oborne and lA. Levis (Eds.). Human factors in transport research (Vol. 2, pp.
311-319). Academic Press, London.
Canudas-de-Wit, C., Bechart, H., Claeys, X., Dolcini, P. and Martinez, J.1. (2005). Fun-to-
drive by feedback. European Journal of Control, 11(4/5),353-383.
Connolly, T. and Aberg, L. (1993). Some contagion models of speeding. Accident Analysis
and Prevention, 25(1), 57-66.
Damasio, A.R. (1994). Descartes' error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. G. P.
Putnam's Sons, New York.
De Vos, A.P., Theeuwes, J., Hoekstra, W. and Coemet, M.1. (1997). Behavioral aspects of
automatic vehicle guidance. Relationship between headway and driver comfort. Trans-
portation Research Record, 1573, 17-22.
Donges, E. (1978). A two-level model of driver steering behavior. Human Factors, 20,
691-707.
Evans, L. (1991). Traffic safety and the driver. Van Nostrand Reinhold, New York.
Fajen, B.R. (2005). Calibration, information, and control strategies for braking to avoid a
collision. Journal of Experimental Psychology - Human Perception and Performance,
31(3), 480-501.
Finucane, M.L., Alhakami, A., Slavic, P. and Johnson, S.M. (2000). The affect heuristic in
judgments of risks and benefits. Journal ofBehavioral Decision Making, 13(1), 1-17.
Fischhoff, B., Slavic, P., Lichtenstein, S., Read, S. and Combs, B. (1978). How safe is
safe enough? A psychometric study of attitudes towards technological risks and benefits.
Policy Sciences, 9, 127-152.
Fuller, R. (2000). The task-capability interface model of the driving process. Journal
Recherche-Transport-Securite (RTS), 66,47-59.
Fuller, R. (2004). Zero risk versus target risk: Can they both be right? Paper presented at
the 3rd International Conference on Traffic and Transport Psychology 2004, Symposium
on Traffic Psychology Theories. Nottingham, UK.
Fuller, R. (2005). Towards a general theory of driver behaviour. Accident Analysis and
Prevention, 37,461-472.
Gameiro da Silva, M.C. (2002). Measurements of comfort in vehicles. Measurement Science
and Technology, 13, R41-R60.
Gibson, J.1. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin,
Boston.
Gibson, J.1. and Crooks, L.E. (1938). A theoretical field-analysis of automobile driving.
American Journal of Psychology, 51,453-471.
Godthelp, H. (1988). The limits of path error-neglecting in straight lane driving. Ergonomics,
31,609-619.
11. Towards Understanding Motivational and Emotional Factors 205
Godthelp, H., Milgram, P. and Blaauw, GJ. (1984). The development of a time-related
measure to describe driving strategy. Human Factors, 26, 257-268.
Goodrich, M.A., Stirling, W.C. and Boer, E.R. (2000). Satisficing revisited. Minds and
Machines, 10(1),79-110.
Greeno, J.G. (1994). Gibson's Affordances. Psychological Review, 101(2),336-342.
Groeger, J. (2000). Understanding Driving. Psychology Press, Hove.
Haglund, M. and Aberg, L. (2000). Speed choice in relation to speed limit and influences
from other drivers. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour,
3(1), 39-51.
Hall, E.T. (1966). The hidden dimension. Doubleday, Garden City.
Hancock, P.A. and Caird, J.K. (1993). Experimental evaluation of a model of mental work-
load. Human Factors, 35,413-429.
Hancock, P.A. and Warm, lS. (1989). A dynamic model of stress and sustained attention.
Human Factors, 31,519-537.
Harms, L. (1991). Variation in drivers' cognitive load: Effects of driving through village
areas and rural junctions. Ergonomics, 34, 151-160.
Hart, S.G. and Staveland, L.E. (1988). Development of the NASA-TLX (Task Load Index):
Results of empirical and theoretical research. In P.A. Hancock and N. Meshkati (Eds.).
Human mental workload (pp. 139-183). Elsevier, Amsterdam.
Hollnagel, E. (2002). Time and time again. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 3(2),
143-158.
Hollnagel, E. and Woods, D.D. (2005). Joint cognitive systems. CRC Press, Boca Raton,
FL.
Jiang, Z.Y., Streit, D.A. and El-Gindy, M. (2001). Heavy vehicle ride comfort: Literature
survey. Heavy Vehicle Systems - International Journal of Vehicle Design, 8(3/4), 258-
284.
Lawton, R., Parker, D., Stradling, S.G. and Manstead, A.S.R. (1997). Predicting road traffic
accidents: The role of social deviance and violations. British Journal ofPsychology, 88,
249-262.
Lee, D.N. (1976). A theory of visual control of braking based on information about time-
to-collision. Perception, 5, 437-459.
McKenna, F.P. (1982). The human factor in driving accidents: An overview of approaches
and problems. Ergonomics, 25, 867-877.
Naatanen, R. and Summala, H. (1974). A model for the role of motivational factors in
drivers' decision-making. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 6, 243-261.
Naatanen, R. and Summala, H. (1976). Road-user behavior and traffic accidents. North-
Holland/American Elsevier, AmsterdamlNew York.
Niittyrnaki, J. and Pursula, M. (1994). Valo-ohjattujen liittymien simulointi ja ajodynami-
ikka (Simulation ofsignalized intersections and driving dynamics) (Publication No. 81).
Helsinki University of Technology, Transportation Engineering.
Ohta, H. (1993). Individual differences in driving distance headway. In A.G. Gale et al.
(Eds.). Vision in vehicles (Vol. 4, pp. 91-100). Elsevier, Amsterdam.
Olson, P.L. and Sivak, M. (1984). Discomfort glare from automobile headlights. Journal of
the Illuminating Engineering Society, 13, 296-303.
Ouellette, lA. and Wood, W. (1998). Habit and intention in everyday life: The multiple
processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 124,
54-74.
Rasanen, M. and Summala, H. (2000). Car drivers' adjustments to cyclists at roundabouts.
Transportation Human Factors, 2, 1-17.
206 Summala
Reymond, G., Kemeny, A., Droulez, 1. and Berthoz, A. (2001). Role of lateral acceleration
in curve driving: Driver model and experiments on a real vehicle and a driving simulator.
Human Factors, 43(3), 483-495.
Robertson, L.S. and Pless, LB. (2002). For and against: Does risk homoeostasis theory have
implications for road safety. British Medical Journal, 324, 1151-1152.
Rothengatter, T. (1988). Risk and the absence of pleasure: A motivational approach to
modelling road user behaviour. Ergonomics, 31, 599-607.
Roufs, J.AJ. and Boschman, M.C. (1997). Text quality metrics for visual display units .1.
Methodological aspects. Displays, 18(1), 37-43.
Rumar, K. (1988). Collective risk but individual safety. Ergonomics, 31, 507-518.
Rumar, K., Berggrund, U., Jernberg, P. and Ytterbom, U. (1976). Driver reaction to a
technical safety measure: Studded tires. Human Factors, 18, 443-454.
Russell, J.A. and Barrett, L.F. (1999). Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and
other things called emotion: Dissecting the elephant. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 76(5),805-819.
Simon, H. (1955). A behavioral model of rational choice. Quarterly Journal ofEconomics,
69,99-118.
Sivak, M., Flannagan, M., Ensing, M. and Simmons, CJ. (1991). Discomfort glare is task
dependent. International Journal of Vehicle Design, 12, 152-159.
Slovic, P., Finucane, M.L., Peters, E. and MacGregor, D.G. (2004). Risk as analysis and
risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Analysis,
24(2), 311-322.
Smeed, R.J. (1949). Some statistical aspects of road safety research. Journal of the Royal
Statistical Society, Series A (General), 112, 1-34.
Summala, H. (1985). Modeling driver task: A pessimistic prediction? In L. Evans and R.C.
Schwing (Eds.). Human behavior and traffic safety (pp. 43-65). Plenum, New York.
Summala, H. (1988). Risk control is not risk adjustment: The zero-risk theory of driver
behaviour and its implications. Ergonomics, 31,491-506.
Summala, H. (1996). Accident risk and driver behaviour. Safety Science, 22,103-117.
Summala, H. (1997). Hierarchical model of behavioural adaptation and traffic accidents.
In T. Rothengatter and E. Carbonell Vaya (Eds.). Traffic and transport psychology (pp.
41-52). Pergamon, Amsterdam.
Summala, H. (2000). Brake reaction times and driver behavior analysis. Transportation
Human Factors, 2, 217-226.
Summala, H. (2005). Traffic psychology theories: Towards understanding driving behaviour
and safety efforts. In G. Underwood (Ed.). Traffic and transport psychology (pp. 383-
394). Elsevier, Oxford.
Summala, H. and Koivisto, I. (1990). Unalerted drivers' brake reaction times: Older drivers
compensate their slower reactions by driving more slowly. In T. Benjamin (Ed.). Driving
behaviour in a social context (pp. 680-683). Paradigme, Caen.
Summala, H. and Merisalo, A. (1980). A psychophysical method for determining the effect
of studded tires on safety. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 21, 193-199.
Summala, H. and Rasanen, M. (2000). Top-down and bottom-up processes in driver behavior
at roundabouts and crossroads. Transportation Human Factors, 2, 29-37.
Sylwan, C. (1919). Trafiksakerheten pa gata och landsvag (Traffic safety on streets and
highways). Industritidningen i Norden (No. 50), 357-364.
Taieb-Maimon, M. and Shinar, D. (2001). Minimum and comfortable driving headways:
Reality versus perception. Human Factors, 43(1), 159-172.
11. Towards Understanding Motivational and Emotional Factors 207
Taylor, D.H. (1964). Drivers' galvanic skin response and the risk of accident. Ergonomics,
7,253-262.
Theeuwes, 1., Alferdinck, J. and Perel, M. (2002). Relation between glare and driving
performance. Human Factors, 44(1),95-107.
Vaa, T. (2004). Developing a driver behaviour model based on emotions and feelings:
Proposing building blocks and interrelationships. Paper presented at the 3rd International
Conference on Traffic and Transport Psychology 2004, Symposium on Traffic Psychology
Theories. Nottingham, UK.
Van der Horst, R. and Wilmink, A. (1986). Drivers' decision making at signalized intersec-
tions: An optimization of the yellow timing. Traffic Engineering & Control, 27, 615-622.
Van der Horst, A.R.A. (1990). A Time-Based Analysis of Road User Behaviour in Normal
and Critical Encounters. TNO Institute for Perception, Soesterberg.
Verplanken, B., Aarts, H., van Knippenberg, A. and Moonen, A. (1998). Habit versus
planned behaviour: A field experiment. British Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 111-
128.
Warren, W.H. (1984). Perceiving affordances - Visual guidance of stair climbing. Journal
of Experimental Psychology - Human Perception and Performance, 10(5),683-703.
Warren, W.H. and Whang, S. (1987). Visual guidance of walking through Apertures -
Body-scaled information for affordances. Journal ofExperimental Psychology - Human
Perception and Performance, 13(3),371-383.
Wikman, A.-S., Laakso, M. and Summala, H. (1997). Time sharing of car drivers as a func-
tion of driving speed and task eccentricity. In The 13th Triennial Congress ofInternational
Ergonomics Association. Tampere, Finland, June 29-July 4, 1997.
Wikman, A.S., Nieminen, T. and Summala, H. (1998). Driving experience and time-sharing
during in-car tasks on roads of different width. Ergonomics, 41(3), 358-372.
Wikman, A.S. and Summala, H. (2005). Aging and time-sharing in highway driving. Op-
tometry and Vision Science, 82(8), 716-723.
Wilde, GJ.S. (1982). The theory of risk homeostasis: Implications for safety and health.
Risk Analysis, 2, 209-225.
van Winsum, W. and Heino, A. (1996). Choice of time-headway in car-following and the
role of time-to-collision information in braking. Ergonomics, 39(4), 579-592.
Williams, A.F. and O'Neill, B. (1974). On-the-road driving records of licensed race drivers.
Accident Analysis and Prevention, 6, 263-270.
Wong, Y.D. and Nicholson, A. (1992). Driver behavior at horizontal curves - risk compen-
sation and the margin of safety. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 24(4), 425-436.
12
Modelling Driver Behaviour
on Basis of Emotions and Feelings:
Intelligent Transport Systems and
Behavioural Adaptations
TRULS VAA
12.1 Introduction
Intelligent transport system (ITS) is a generic concept, which covers a wide range of
systems. In this context the concept is applied on automotive systems and comprises
systems generally defined as (advanced) driver assistance systems (ADASIDAS),
in-vehicle information systems (IVIS) and roadside telematics (RT). The present
text focuses on anti-locking brake systems (ABS), which is used as an illustrative
example of an ITS, mainly because evaluation studies have shown unintended ef-
fects that call for explanations. ABS, which aims to maintain the steering capacity
during (heavy) braking by preventing the wheels from locking, is considered a
driver assistance system (DAS). ABS has become an increasingly standard equip-
ment of new car makes and has been around for more than 20 years. Several studies
have evaluated the effects of ABS on behaviour and accidents and the system is a
case of special interest for several reasons: One is the demonstration of risk com-
pensation associated with ABS and other reasons are contra-intuitive and even
detrimental effects on traffic safety. With ABS as an illustrative example, several
key issues can be discussed when considering ITS in a more generic sense. Fur-
ther, to better understand and predict effects of ITS, a theoretical driver behaviour
model based on emotions and feelings is presented. Behavioural adaptation and
risk compensation are regarded as core problems, which have to be addressed
in terms of traffic safety. One of the very aims of the proposed driver behaviour
model is to explain and predict risk compensation that might be associated with a
given ITS.
208
12. Modelling Driver Behaviour on Basis of Emotions and Feelings 209
implies that the repulsive or attractive object has to be loaded with some emotional
quality. Otherwise, there would be no energy and no direction (Overskeid, 2000).
A neutral object is neither repulsive nor attractive. Hence, the emotional dimension
of motives is then a core aspect of motivation.
A second feature of the definition is that it does not say anything about cognition,
that is, whether motives are rooted in consciousness or in the unconscious. It may
seem self-evident as the role of the unconscious in psychoanalysis and psychody-
namic theory is well known, but I nevertheless regard this issue as a significant
point, for two reasons:
1. the role of the unconscious has seldom been made explicit in prevailing driver
behaviour models, and
2. the phenomenon of risk compensation cannot reach any satisfactory explana-
tion, without including and addressing unconscious processes.
One can hardly say that the task of modelling driver behaviour has reached con-
sensus. There is no breakthrough or 'great unified theory' within the field of traffic
safety research regarding modelling of driver behaviour (Vaa, 200 1a). Models ad-
dress diverging aspects, several 'favourite' issues and/or concepts are pursued,
discussions and disagreement prevail. The listing below includes some of the most
predominant theories and models that have been applied to explain and predict
driver behaviour and which have motivational aspects as a key factor. The history
of models starts in 1938 when Gibson and Crooks' presented their theoretical field-
analysis of automobile driving (motivational aspects or motivational processes are
stated in short for each of the models):
The above listing is by no means a complete list of theories and driver behaviour
models, the purpose of presenting it is to focus on the main motivational aspects
which have been proposed, applied and discussed within traffic safety research.
It can be argued that a common denominator for most of the models is emo-
tion: 'Safe travel' (Gibson and Crooks, 1938), 'tension/anxiety' (Taylor, 1964),
'zero risk' (Naatanen and Summala, 1974), 'target risk' (Wilde, 1982), 'threat-
avoidance' (Fuller, 1984), 'pleasure' (Rothengatter, 1988) and 'difficulty' (Fuller,
2000). Wilde's RHT, which has been heavily debated since it was launched, repre-
sents something different, because the target level of risk is understood and defined
as a number, that is, as a number> 0 (Vaa, 2001a), not as an emotion or a feeling.
On the other hand, Wilde's RHT is inescapable regarding the discussion of models
because of its reliance on central concepts as homeostasis and risk compensation.
There is a need, however, for a reorientation of RHT, because the debate somehow
has resulted in a deadlock for the development of driver behaviour models. Such
a reorientation is also strongly needed because risk compensation is still not fully
understood and accounted for in a satisfactory way.
One may get the impression that models have been too focused on cognitive
aspects as determinants of driver behaviour, as in theory of reasoned action (TRA)
(Aijzen and Fishbein, 1980) and theory of planned behaviour (TPB) (Aijzen, 1985).
One could even say that the focus on cognitive models has been predominant to
such an extent that the role of the unconscious has more or less been neglected.
Extending this assertion, I would argue that there is no common understanding of
driver behaviour that is based on recent achievements in cognitive psychology and
neurobiology. In fact, Taylor's early work of 1964 may be more in line with recent
achievements in neurobiology than any other of the models listed above (Damasio,
1994; Bechara et al., 1997). No deep understanding of risk compensation will
emerge unless recent developments in cognitive psychology and neurobiology are
integrated in the modelling of driver behaviour (Vaa, 2001a).
The hypothesis which is proposed is that the role of the unconscious is significant
as a motivating force also when it comes to driver behaviour. Hence, there is a need
for a deeper and more considerate elaboration of the role of the unconscious. This
is made a predominant point in the present discussion, which aims at a deeper
understanding of the phenomenon of risk compensation.
especially for drivers, which call for specific definitions. Behavioural adaptation
and risk compensation are concepts which sometimes are used interchangeably,
but here a distinction between them is proposed. Behavioural adaptation is nat-
urally the widely used generic concept and this meaning remains. However, as
our perspective is road traffic and especially driver behaviour, I will limit be-
havioural adaptation to strategic decisions, that is, to conscious decision mak-
ing. Strategic decision making may take place outside as well as inside the road
traffic system. One example is driving in darkness, when the number of elderly
drivers and women (all ages), increases as a function of road lighting on a given
stretch of road, because these specific driver groups feel more secure when a road
is lit up by road lighting (Assum et al., 1999). A second example is when you
decide to drive faster because you are out of time or you decide to take an al-
ternative route because you are stuck in traffic, these are, likewise, also strategic
decisions.
Risk compensation also represents behavioural adaptation, but I regard it as a
special case of adaptation, that is, adaptations which predominantly occur without
involving consciousness. Hence, this concept is used only for decisions made on
an unconscious level, as when the driving speeds are increased (Aschenbrenner et
al., 1987) or when time headways are reduced (Sagberg et al., 1997), for drivers
driving cars equipped with ABS compared to a control group of drivers with cars
without ABS. This distinction is deliberately made because these kinds of decisions
origin in bodily reactions, that is the hypothesis, which drivers do not necessarily
experience at a conscious level. This type of process is named risk compensation,
because, when a given, supposed risk-reducing measure is introduced in the road
traffic system (here: Cars with ABS), the risk-reducing effects, which are expected,
are compensated by certain behaviour changes, most predominantly by increased
driving speeds or by changes of levels of attention (Elvik and Vaa, 2004).
It should be added that the 'hierarchical' categorisation strategic-tactic-
operational introduced by Michon (1985) is not adopted here, behaviour is rather
seen as belonging to a continuum ranging from highly conscious to completely
unconscious as end points, i.e. not as separate and distinct categories in itself (Vaa,
2003a). The degree of conscious/unconscious information processing and deci-
sion making is then understood as going back and forth along the continuum, thus
illustrating the dynamics and integration of cognitive processes, bodily reactions
and emotions and feelings.
a
I. Benefits expected from risky behaviours (.)
2.Costs expected from cautious behaviours (+)
3.Benefi1s expected from cautious behaviours (-)
4. Costs expected from risky behaviours (-)
c
Individual estimates 01 the
intrinsic effect of 0 new t - - -_ _. .
non - mofivationo1 occident
countermeasure
d
Individual levels of
perceived. rood-accident
risk
FIGURE 12.1. Wilde's model of risk homeostasis (after Wilde, 1982, 1988).
and finally (4) RHT is inescapable, it addresses a core problem in driver decision
making, there is an essence in it, which I will try to extract by contrasting Naatanen
and Summala's 'zero-risk' -model with Wilde's RHT.
While Naatanen and Summala (1974) postulate that drivers try to avoid risk by
regulating their behaviour according to a perception of zero risk, Wilde postulates
the opposite by stating that drivers seek a certain risk level- 'a target risk level' -
a risk level that must be perceived as a number > 0 presumed to be defined by
a measure of exposure, that is, as number of accidents per kilometres driven, a
certain unit of time or the like (Vaa, 2001a). This target risk level varies between
drivers, it seems partly to have idiosyncratic origins, partly to be a regulator in
a homeostatic system: When the driver is confronted with certain changes in the
road environment, he or she will meet these changes with adaptations that secures
that the level of target risk is sustained. Wilde postulates further that the target level
of risk can be increased when expected benefits from risky behaviour or expected
costs from cautious behaviour, increases. And finally, it can be reduced when
expected benefit from cautious behaviour or expected costs from risky behaviour,
increases.
Wilde's RHT model contains one explicit element called a comparator. This
is a place, a function, a process where three input factors are put together and
compared: b, c and d (Fig. 12.1) resulting in one output factor e, where:
b == Individual levels of target risk
c == Individual estimate of the intrinsic effect of a new, non-motivational road
safety measure
d == Individual levels of perceived road-accident risk
e == Desired adaptation satisfying the formula: b - c - d == 0
12. Modelling Driver Behaviour on Basis of Emotions and Feelings 213
According to Wilde, the three input factors are 'weighed together' in the com-
parator. This is a bound weighing: RHT predicts that the end result should be zero.
All three input factors must be comprehended as numbers where the values of b,
c and d have the property that
b-c-d==O (I)
Translated into words, it means that the output from the comparator, the result
of the weighing procedure, must be chosen in such a way that the property (I) is
fulfilled. Translated to behaviour, it means that the output factor must be regarded
as the desired adaptation of the individual driver, which is such that the risk
homeostasis is sustained on an individual basis.
A problem with RHT, which in my opinion must be read according to the
above, is that all input factors and the predicted output factor, are comprehended
as numbers. And, as such, they should be confirmed to exist by individual drivers.
Are they? I have never seen any such numbers or calculating procedures being
confirmed by drivers and I have no such numbers or comparing in myself. If they
exist, the prediction must take place unconsciously. The prediction would hence
be impossible to test, as the entities are impossible to observe and impossible to
measure.
Looking more closely at the factor d, individual levels of perceived road-accident
risk, d seem to be the time-lagged feedback product (h) of a preceding aggregate
accident loss in the population (g). I guess we all have a perception of an 'aggregate
accident loss', but it is not very accurate. It may be uttered in terms of 'speeds
are increasing', 'traffic is getting worse' or 'traffic seem to improve now', but
such terms are very crude and not based on any kind of calculation or unbiased
knowledge of the situation. We may remember certain accidents, because of their
magnitude or some other characteristic, but we may have forgot all the ones with
minor injuries or damage. Tversky and Kahneman (1974) show that people put
much weight into their own experiences. The properties of one small sample may,
independent of its size, be considered as being representative of a much larger
population. Tversky and Kahneman are, however, not referred to in Wilde (1982,
1988). There seem to be no trace in the literature of the asserted weighing procedure
between b, c and d. that is, between target level of risk (b), individual estimates
of effects (c) and individual levels of perceived risk (d) constituting the desired
adaptations b - c - d == O. Not even on a strategic level of driver behaviour, that
is, thinking supposed to take place in a highly conscious and rational manner, does
it seem possible to observe these kinds of cognitive operations.
As a conclusion, a target level of risk cannot be a number, a thought or an
imagination that I bring with me consciously and which I put into some weighing
procedure when I decide what speed I should choose or what kind of acts I should
perform as was it a constant, predominant thought or imagination in the dynamics
of my thinking. And that is exactly my critique against Wilde: The RHT model does
not grasp or mimic the varied dynamics of thinking and feeling, 'the streams of
consciousness', the fluctuations of automated states mixed with thoughts coming
and going so characteristic of everyday driving. The RHT model somehow assumes
214 Vaa
Perceived risk V
X Speed
a powerful, hidden, unconscious force that forces us to act in such a way that the
target level of risk is sustained individually for everyone as well as for everybody
else. Such a powerful force somehow resembles the cosmological anti-gravity
force, 'dark matter', 'dark energy' or whatever: The force is there, it makes the
universe accelerate in its expansion, but we cannot observe it (Vaa, 2001a).
Joy/pleasure
Relaxed, secure
Threat-avoidance
Avoiding/reducing difficulties
Compliance/rule-based driving: Avoid violations, no errors, 'always behave cor-
rectly'
Non-compliance
It is worth noting that several of the driver behaviour models mentioned previ-
ously do have inherent emotional aspects, but, with the exception of Taylor (1964)
and Naatanen and Summala (1974) none of the models view emotions and feel-
ings as a governing principle in a general way i.e. only in a specific way, they
somehow isolate single feeling dimensions as their key variable as with threat-
avoidance (Fuller, 1984), joy/pleasure (Rothengatter, 1988) and sensation seeking
(Zuckerman, 1994). Three other feeling dimensions as motivating forces are also
suggested: 'Arousal' (being vigilant, attentive), compliance/rule-based driving and
non-compliance. The idea is to make the picture complete by not singling out one
specific feeling, but rather try to grasp a more complete variety of feelings that
may govern driver behaviour. Drivers are different, some feelings might be more
predominant than others. The predominance of certain feelings are likely to be
associated with personality traits, as suggested by Ulleberg (2002).
Not all drivers enjoy driving, so the 'best feeling' that can be achieved or sus-
tained may be negatively defined, as an optimal choice where unpleasantness, dif-
ficulties, etc., are reduced to their minimum in any given situation. It is proposed
that such choices are at least two-dimensional. Avoiding accidents, 'zero risk', is
not the full answer. A certain emotional experience has to be added. Car driving
is characterised by constantly solving problems, problems that involve thinking,
choosing and deciding between different alternatives. All alternatives, scenarios,
acts, can be characterised by an outcome that has an emotional dimension attached
to it. In fact, that emotional dimension is the very variable that enables drivers or
any other in any other situation, to evaluate and choose between alternatives. If
there is no feeling, there is no possibility for evaluating the outcomes (Damasio,
1994; Overskeid, 2000). There is no such thing as thinking and reasoning without
an emotional dimension.
taxi company had some of their cars equipped with ABS, while others were with-
out ABS. The two groups were similar; the only difference was the fitting with and
without ABS. The drivers were randomly assigned to the groups and they were
all told, which kind of brakes their taxi had. Driver behaviour was recorded by
observers camouflaged as passengers. They all asked the taxi drivers to drive ex-
actly the same trip. Data from a total of 113 trips were recorded, evenly distributed
between taxis with and without ABS.
Driver behaviour data were collected for 18 variables. Of these, statistically
significant differences were observed for four variables (Aschenbrenner et aI.,
1987). These were as follows:
Drivers of taxis equipped with ABS were more often outside their lane than
drivers of taxis without ABS.
Drivers with ABS 'cut corners' more often than drivers without ABS.
Drivers with ABS predicted the traffic ahead to a lesser degree than drivers
without ABS.
Drivers with ABS were more often involved in conflicts with other road users
than drivers without ABS.
Driving speeds were measured at four sites along the fixed route. By one of
these, in a 60 km/h speed zone, driving speeds were measured. The driving speeds
of ABS drivers were significantly higher than among drivers without ABS.
Accidents were also recorded and analysed. The number of accidents was con-
trolled for mileage and also for seasonal variations. It turned out that cars with
ABS were involved in as many accidents as cars without ABS. Aschenbrenner
et al. conclude that driving behaviour of the ABS-taxis has been less cautious as
no effect of ABS on the number of accidents was recorded (Aschenbrenner et aI.,
1987).
What happens to drivers who drive vehicles equipped with ABS? What lie
behind the behavioural differences? Is it a matter of thinking differently than
drivers of vehicles without ABS? Do they feel differently? Do the differences in
driver behaviour have their origin in conscious processes, in unconscious processes
or both? Is it to be explained by vehicle characteristics, driver characteristics or
both? Aschenbrenner et aI. The study of is not the only one confirming behavioural
differences between drivers of ABS-vehicles and drivers of vehicles without ABS.
Sagberg et aI. (1997) found that taxis with ABS had significantly shorter headways
than taxis without ABS, but they found no relationships with driving speeds,
possibly because dense traffic at the observation site may have prevented drivers
from driving at their preferred speeds. No other behavioural differences for drivers
with cars with and without airbags were found.
Broughton and Baugha (2002) found in a postal survey that ABS does have the
potential of reducing the number of accidents, but also that many drivers have little
or no knowledge of ABS and its effects. They found an overall accident reduction
tendency of 3% (insignificant at confidence level of 90%), a tendency of increased
number of accidents by 10% (insignificant) among men aged 56+, a 16% accident
reduction among men aged 17-55 (significant) and a tendency among women of
12. Modelling Driver Behaviour on Basis of Emotions and Feelings 217
The meta-analysis is based on the following evaluation studies: Aschenbrenner et al. (1987),
Kahane (1993 and 1994), HLDI (1995), Hertz et al. (1995A and 1995B), and Evans and
Gerrish (1996).
by 20% at speeds over 80 kmh and with maintenance of stability. But doubts
have been raised that ABS may have failed or lost its effect in critical situations.
American traffic police have addressed this question (Brandt, 1994). The situation
they wanted to investigate would probably be rare for the common driver, but
more prevalent for police patrols during an alarm, chasing a criminal or the like.
Suspicions arose among traffic police forces to the extent that they decided to inves-
tigate and measure stop lengths in critical avoidance manoeuvres. The suspicions
were confirmed, stop lengths did increase when police drivers were braking in
avoidance manoeuvres. The explanation to this phenomenon is uncertain, but con-
sider the difference between braking with ABS and with ordinary brakes: Heavy
braking with ordinary brakes in a critical situation would lock the wheels; that is,
all friction will be used to reduce speed to the disadvantage of loosing steering
control, while the braking forces with ABS would be split between reducing speed
and maintaing steering capacity. In other words, some of the friction forces be-
tween wheel and road surface is utilised for steering, resulting in less friction for
reducing the speed of the car (Brandt, 1994).
(a) The suggested effects of ITS may be counteracted and compensated by be-
havioural changes among drives. If so: Why do (some) drivers change their
behaviours as a function of a given ITS?
(b) What kind of properties elicit behaviour changes among drivers?
(c) Improper or insufficient knowledge of a given system may lead to a reduction
of the potential effect of ITS or even to detrimental effects.
(d) The scenarios incorporated in the risk analysis preceding the development of
a given ITS may be incomplete; that is, some outcomes of an ITS may be
unforeseen. Given the large variation of driving situations, driving tasks and
drivers, (some) drivers will hit the occasions where the effects are reduced,
unforeseen and/or detrimental.
(e) Technology can be defined, at least in some cases, as an extension of the
human organism, which can bring humans to situations in which the organism
is poorly suited for mastering. In principle, any ITS could have the potential
of 'transporting' a driver to situations which are difficult to master.
being killed by a predator some 30,000 years ago has been estimated to have been
1 in 10.
It follows that the inherent propensities and the behavioural repertoire of the
human organism may have limitations regarding the ability to cope with situations
created and provided by today's technology, which also means that we might be
misguided, misled and not adequately warned, when we enter situations brought
to us by technology, because the ability to monitor and judge risk is not adapted to
situations into which technology has 'transported' us. The expansion of the window
of opportunities is not fully accompanied with the tools the organism needs for
coping adequately with the dangers, which are provided by the technology of the
car. Given the enormous death tolls and personal injuries, the car is probably the
most unprecedented example of the mismatch and maladjustment between humans
and technology of any time.
Who is responsible for the mismatch between man and technology? Should we
demand that human beings, by their ability to think, of being conscious of what
he or she is doing, should detect, stop and refrain from situations, which he or she
is at danger? Should we demand that the human organism, with its inherent, but
limited ability to monitor and assess risk, should detect any danger in situations
created by the technological expansion of the window of opportunities?
The scope here is not to answer all these issues, but rather to present a model
of driver behaviour that may have a potential of understanding and predicting
behavioural effects of a given ITS.
It follows axiomatically from the assumption that man's deepest motive is sur-
vival, that the organism must have an instrument, an organ, enabling it to monitor
its surroundings and the situations in which it acts. This organ is the organism itself,
the complete body and its inherent physiology developed by evolution through the
history of man where observation and identification of dangers have been of vital
importance. The organism taken as a whole is considered as a monitor, an organ
for surveillance whose prime task is to monitor the interior, that is, the state of
the body and the exterior, that is, the environment and other actors with which the
organism interact.
Damasio postulates a relationship between internal states and external behaviour
when the human organism is exposed to certain strain and emotional stress, which
forms:
.... a set of alterations [which] defines a profile of departures from a range of average states
corresponding to a functional balance or homeostasis, within which the organism's economy
probably operates at its best, with lesser expenditure and simpler and faster adjustments.
(Damasio, 1994)
A central concept in the above citation is the functional balance. This functional
balance is defined as the target feeling, which was discussed previously. This
target feeling is a kind of state that drivers are seeking to achieve and/or maintain
while driving. The drive to achieve a functional balance is regarded as a central,
predominantly unconscious knowledge, which the organism possesses about itself
and which the organism is actively seeking to maintain or to restore.
Damasio states his model by saying that something important happens before
thinking and reasoning. If, for example, a situation seems to develop into some-
thing threatening or dangerous, a feeling of unpleasantness will enter the body,
an unpleasant 'gut feeling' may be under way. Because this emotion is knit to the
body, Damasio labels it somatic ('soma' is Greek for 'body') and marker because
the emotion marks the picture or the scenario. Damasio describes the consequence
of this somatic-marker in the following way:
[A somatic marker] ... .forces attention on the negative outcome to which a given action
may lead and functions as an automated alarm signal which says: Beware of danger ahead
if you choose the option which leads to this outcome....
. . . . The automated signal protects you against future losses, without further ado and then
allows you to choose from among fewer alternatives. (Damasio 1994, p. 173)
12. Modelling Driver Behaviour on Basis of Emotions and Feelings 223
Damasio separates between emotion and feeling and limits the concept of emo-
tion to what goes on in the body of the organism, that is, the myriads of changes in
the state of the body that is induced autonomously in all its parts and organs when
the organism is exposed to a given external event. Damasio points out that a lot
of the changes in the body state, as changes in skin colour, body position, facial
expressions etc., are also visible to others. The etymological meaning of the word
emotion relates to the direction of the changes in body state as e-motion means
'movement out' (Vaa,2001b).
Damasio distinguishes specifically between emotions and feelings and limits
feeling to processes of consciously experience, consciously sensing, the changes
of the body and the mental states. Damasio distinguishes between several levels
and defines emotions and feeling as follows:
Primary emotions: Emotions that are innate and unconscious, corresponds to the
neurobiological apparatus of the newborn infant.
Secondary emotions: Emotions that are learnt and based on individual experi-
ences, accumulated by the individual- that is, as they develop into 'the emotions
of the adult'. Predominantly unconscious or pre-conscious.
Feelings: The process of 'feeling an emotion', the process of 'making an emo-
tion conscious', to feel and transform changes in body states into conscious
experiences.
This is, in short, what I will label as 'The Damasio model' . Damasio is explicitly
aware that his definitions are 'unorthodox' (Damasio, 1994). Personally I adopt his
definitions as I consider them as fruitful definitions that facilitate an understanding
of - say - driver behaviour and also fruitful for an elaboration of a model for driver
behaviour.
While primary emotions are exclusively sub-cortical and directed towards the
body, secondary emotions also include activation of numerous prefrontal cortices,
which means that secondary emotions, in addition to the sub-cortical responses of
primary emotions, also include cortical, but still unconscious responses activated
by the external stimuli. It is assumed that the cortical loop in prefrontal cortices
that is involved in secondary emotions may give access to schemas formed and
accumulated by the learning history of the individual and that this loop enables
the body to react without involving conscious processes. And furthermore, it is
this 'loop of secondary emotions' that enables the organism to act automatically in
behaviours that are 'over-learnt' - as often experienced by drivers in driving tasks
(Vaa,200lb).
Finally, to feel an emotion, it is necessary, but not sufficient, that neural sig-
nals from the viscera, muscles, joints, neurotransmitter nuclei, that is, all body
organs that are emotionally activated, are redirected towards the neo-cortex and
certain sub-cortical nuclei. The signals from the body back to cortex go through
endocrine and other chemical routes and reach the central nervous system via
the bloodstream. The feelings, that is, the conscious experience of body states
impinged by external stimuli, then establish an association between an external
object, say a given situation in traffic and an emotional body state. Hence, by the
224 Vaa
processes of feeling and emotion, the individual is able to evaluate, consider and
choose between alternative acts in a situation that demands action. The conscious-
ness needs a continuous update of 'here-and-now', of what the body does and what
it experiences. Feelings are then the conscious experience of what the body does -
by representations of emotional body states or, as Damasio puts it,
That process of continuous monitoring, that experience of what your body is doing while
thoughts about specific contents roll by, is the essence of what I call a feeling. (Damasio
1994,p. 145).
The Monitor
Otherroadusers ~ ~ ~ ~
f! ~ .. ~ ~ Personality ~ ~
~ ~ ~ Somatl~ ~arklng ~ 1 traits ~ ..
eo
U)
cQ)
en
Act/Target feeling
'Best feeling'
FIGURE 12.3. Basic structure of the monitor model (from Vaa 2003).
12. Modelling Driver Behaviour on Basis of Emotions and Feelings 225
The monitor is nothing less than the whole of the body, the whole organism.
The boundaries of the monitor (solid line) correspond to the boundary of the
body. The internal components are all elements and processes surrounded by the
solid line: Somatic marking, personality traits, motives, interaction patterns and
a residual of other factors. Deep motivation serves as a base and will influence
other components through personality traits and motives. Personality traits influ-
ence motives and dispose for idiosyncratic interaction patterns. The interaction
patterns of the individual driver can in tum elicit new, latent motives as a con-
sequence of other road users' responses on the initial act(s) of the driver. Feed-
back loops through characteristics of the vehicle, of the road environment and
of the interactions with other road users are indicated; that is, the figure of the
model is an excerpt of a certain, instantaneous time window, which is constantly
changing by new, upcoming information fed back to the organism by feedback-
loops.
A second central concept of the model, that is, in addition to target feeling, is
the account of feelings, a concept which is used to describe an internal, con-
scious process in which one is imagining two or more alternative actions or
'inner scenarios', as when considering what to do in a given situation. It is a
process of cognitive 'weighting' or 'cost-benefit analysis' of conscious, inter-
nal scenarios against each other (Damasio, 1994; Overskeid, 2000). The point is
that given alternatives/scenarios must have a dimension of feeling, say of being
attractive or repulsive, pleasant or unpleasant or the like, that are weighed to-
gether in order to come up with a decision of what would be the best solution
in a given situation. It is this process of weighing the alternatives together that
is proposed to result in some 'account of feelings' on which a given decision is
based.
In the monitor model (Fig. 12.3) the organism is, in principle, by adopting
Damasio's concept, always 'somatically marked' as it always will be filled with
sensory stimuli from the external world (sensory storage) and with internal stimuli
from somatic marking and the knowledge storage (a concept used here equiva-
lently with Damasio's 'secondary emotions'). Two routes or modes of information
processing and decision making are suggested.
1. One predominantly conscious: From the 'marking of the body' in a given situa-
tion through feelings to the account of feelings (if more than one alternative has to
be considered) and finally resulting in a certain targetfeeling (a 'best feeling' may
also be used as a proper label of the target of the act). The result of the decision,
that is, the act itself, is fed back to the organism, through and by the effects it might
have on the vehicle, on other road users and the road environment.
2. The other route or mode is predominantly unconscious: The organism is (uncon-
sciously) seeking a functional balance, which is achieved by restoring or maintain-
ing a target feeling. The organism will constantly seek to maintain this functional
balance by appropriate behaviours. As long as the functional balance is maintained
in an automated mode, there is no need for involving consciousness. Automated
mode may prevail as long as the functional balance is maintained.
226 Vaa
The two modes of information processing and decision making described above
are naturally stylised and simplified. In real driving, there is no deliberate deci-
sion to 'drive in automated mode', this should be looked upon as an unconscious
decision of the organism itself and understood as a decision which is adequate
because the driving environment is recognised as so simple and familiar that no
conscious appraisals seem necessary. Both modes aim at maintaining or restoring
a functional balance of the organism, which is achieved by the act that realises
the target feeling, either by the conscious route or by the automated route. Two
connections or 'bridges' between the unconscious and the conscious modes of in-
formation processing are indicated. One is represented by the orienting reflex; that
is, the ability to be oriented towards certain objects by a light, a sound or a smell,
which bridges the gap from automated mode to conscious mode and further to
account of feelings if necessary. The second connection is the bridge between the
boxes of 'functional balance' and 'account of feelings'. The latter bridge is sug-
gested for describing an upcoming situation in which the driver is confronted with
a conflict, a consideration of overtaking, of choosing between certain routes, of
changing driving speeds, etc. Simply stated, that is what consciousness or working
memory is there for, as an instrument for conscious appraisals when needed. The
organism will prefer and seek to be governed by an automated mode if possible, as
this mode is less costly, that is, the organism wants to economise with its cognitive
(mental) resources (Reason, 1990; Damasio, 1994). However, the organism does
not decide (consciously) to go to automated mode, this 'decision' should rather be
regarded as a property of the organism itself, that is, a 'decision' to be governed
by automated mode whenever possible.
The direction of the links or bridges between the unconscious and the conscious
is from the former to the latter, not the other way around. One predominant logic
of the model is to maintain or restore functional balance by seeking a target or
best feeling, also indicated by the arrow directly from 'motives' to 'functional bal-
ance' and finally to 'target feeling'. During automated behaviour, there is identity
between target feeling and functional balance (Vaa, 2003a).
It is an assertion that this unconscious quest for functional balance becomes
the steering principle in the model, which also may constitute a base for a deeper
understanding of risk compensation. This view is presented as an alternative to
Wilde's RHT and especially also to his concept 'target risk' (Wilde 1982); that
is, drivers are not seeking a certain risk level other than zero, as in Naatanen and
Summala's 'Zero-Risk Model'(1974). In conclusion, drivers are seeking a target
feeling rather than a target risk.
Finally, some limitations of the monitor model should be mentioned. Naturally,
there are a lot of states that may jeopardise risk monitoring, some might be covered
up under'deep motivation' , other in the residual of 'other factors' . What the model
considers is basically an average driver, of average age, average health and with
an average composition of personality traits, etc., that is, general states where the
monitoring of risk should operate optimally. We know, however, that a variety
of states increase risk. Drink driving is by far the most dangerous of conditions:
A blood alcohol concentration (BAC) of 0.05%-0.1 % has a relative risk of 10
12. Modelling Driver Behaviour on Basis of Emotions and Feelings 227
compared to sober drivers (Vaa, 2003). The risk monitoring function is definitely
distorted in a drunken driver. To be affected by other substances also increase risk
levels, but not in the same magnitude as alcohol: Benzodiazepines, cannabis and
opiates all increase the risk of accidents as they have relative risks of 1.54, 1.70
and 1.83, respectively. Other examples are mental disorders, neurological disorders
and sleep apnoea with relative risks of 1.72, 1.75 and 3.71. Some, but not all, of
these states may be addressed and counteracted by adequate ITS solutions, as with
AlcoLock in the case of drink driving.
The above group of IT-system ideas would generally, as they all address the
primary task of driving, be labelled driver assistance systems (DAS) or advanced
12. Modelling Driver Behaviour on Basis of Emotions and Feelings 229
driver assistance systems (ADAS), although the difference between these two
groups of systems is not well defined. The examples address situations and con-
flicts where drivers may have difficulties with inattention and/or in perceiving the
presence of other road users. The main objective is then to provide the driver with
devices that increase attention by warning him or her of upcoming dangers. The
general issue considered here regarding (A)DAS is the one of risk compensation,
that is, whether or not a given ITS will enable the driver to seek a better feeling
by increasing the speed, changing the time headway and/or the level of atten-
tion. Risk compensation has been documented to be present with ABS, but not
with ESC. For other ITS in this group of driver assistance systems the question
remains if or how they will influence the monitoring of risk by compensatory
mechanisms.
As documented previously, the average and experienced driver perform quite well
regarding risk monitoring as it must be regarded as normal to have a lifelong
career of driving without a single personal injury accident. The introduction of
new ITS in cars would then face an issue of trust, that is, whether or not the
driver would perceive a given system as reliable. Consequently, it is suggested
that a given ITS must perform better than the driver, because the driver gener-
ally will regard him or herself as competent and skilled in handling the dangers
of everyday driving. It is a matter of driver confidence and reliance of a sys-
tem. ITS must prove its relevance and its reliability. If false alarms occur too
often, it will create mistrust and subsequently the driver will probably abandon the
system.
Initially, an attempt to categorise and define different subgroups of ITS was
made. In the present discussion only (A)DAS have been considered, while IVIS
(in-vehicle-information-systems) deliberately have been left out of consideration.
The main reason for this deliberate exclusion has been that the IVIS group is
more obscure than the (A)DAS-group. While (A)DAS generally could be said
to address primary driving tasks, especially by providing information about and
warnings of dangers, IVIS may address primary as well as secondary driving tasks.
Nomadic systems, as mobile phones and portable route guidance systems belong
to the IVIS group. Hence, IVIS may represent distractions and increased workload
and thereby threats to risk monitoring, more than provisions of information about
upcoming dangers. One example is the use of mobile phones, which increases
the risk of accidents. In a study conducted by Sagberg, the relative risk of using
a mobile phone while driving was estimated to 1.72; that is, mobile phone use
increases the risk of accidents by some 72% (Sagberg, 2001). It follows that drivers
may misjudge their ability to monitor risk while operating systems which do not
address primary driving tasks. By incorporating systems that impose distractions
and increased workload, the monitoring of risk may be hampered to such an extent
that the safety of driving is jeopardised.
230 Vaa
References
Aijzen, I. and Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behaviour.
Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.
Aijzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behaviour. In 1 Kuhl and
Beckmann (Eds.). Action control. From cognition to behaviour. Springer Verlag, Berlin,
pp. 11-40.
Aschenbrenner, K.M., Biehl, B. and Wurrn, G.W. (1987). EinfluB Der Risikokompensation
auf die Wirkung von Verkehrsicherheitsmassnahmen am Beispiel ABS. Schriftenreihe
Unfall- und Sicherheitsforschung Straj3enverkehr. Heft 63 Bergisch Gladbach, Bunde-
sanstalt fur Strassenwesen (BASt), pp. 65-70.
Assum, T., Bjernskau, T., Fosser, S. and Sagberg, F. (1999). Risk compenasation - The case
of road lighting. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 31, 545-533.
Atkinson, R.L., Atkinson, R.C., Smith, E.E., Bern, D.J. and Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1996).
Hilgard's introduction to psychology (12th ed.). Hartcourt Brace College Publishers,
Fortworth, TH.
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. and Damasio, A.R. (1997). Deciding advantageously
before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275, 1293-1295.
Bjernskau, T. (2003). Risk in road traffic 2001/2002. Oslo, Institute of Transport Economics.
T0I report no 694/2003. (In Norwegian, with summary in English).
Brandt, B. (1994). ABS increases stopping distances in braking/evasive manoeuvre. Acci-
dent Reconstruction Journal, 41-42.
Broughton, J. and Baugha, C. (2002). The effectiveness of antilock braking systems in
reducing accidents in Great Britain. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 34, 347-355.
Damasio, A.R. (1994). Descartes' error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. G.P.
Putnam's and Sons, New York.
Dang, IN. (2004). Preliminary results analyzing the effectiveness of electronic stability
control (ESC) systems. Report no. DOT-HS-809-790. U.S. Department of Transportation,
Washington, DC.
Elvik, R. and Vaa, T. (2004). The handbook of road safety measures. Elsevier, Oxford.
12. Modelling Driver Behaviour on Basis of Emotions and Feelings 231
Evans, L. and Gerrish, P.H. (1996). Antilock brakes and risk of front and rear impact in
two-vehicle crashes. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 28, 315-323.
Farmer, C. (2004). Effect ofelectronic stability control on automobile crash risk. Insurance
Institute for Highway Safety, USA Traffic Injury Prevention.
Fuller, R. (1984). A conceptualization of driving behaviour as threat avoidance. Ergonomics,
27, 1139-1155.
Fuller, F. (2000). The task-capability interface model of the driving process, Recherche
Transports Securite, 66, 47-59.
Gibson, J.1. and Crooks, L.E. (1938). A theoretical field-analysis of automobile-driving.
The American Journal of Psychology, 51(3),453-471.
Glad, A. (2001). Glare effects of high beam on motorcycles in daylight. Oslo, Insti-
tute of Transport Economics. T0I-report 521/2001. (In Norwegian with summary in
English).
Hertz, E., Hilton, 1. and Johnson, D.M. (1995A). An analysis ofthe crash experience oflight
trucks equipped with antilock braking systems. Report DOT HS 808278. U.S. Department
of Transportation, National Highway traffic Safety Administration, Washington, DC.
Hertz, E., Hilton, 1. and Johnson, D.M. (1995B). An analysis ofthe crash experience ofpas-
senger cars equipped with antilock braking systems. Report DOT HS 808279. U.S. De-
partment of Transportation, National Highway traffic Safety Administration Washington,
DC. (USA, smaller lorries and multi-purpose vehicles).
HLDI - Highway Loss Data Institute (1995). Three years on-the-road experience witn
antilock brakes. HLDI Special Report A-47. Highway Loss Data Institute Arlington, Va,
(USA, cars).
Hoff, T. (2002). Mind design: Steps into an ecology ofhuman-machine systems. Dr. Polit.
Dissertation. Trondheim, Department of Psychology and Department of Product Design
Engineering, Norwegian University of Science and Technology.
Kahane, C.1. (1993). Preliminary evaluation ofthe effectiveness ofrear-wheel antilock brake
systemsfor light trucks. Draft Report December 1993. US Department of Transportation,
National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, Washington, DC.
Kahane, C.1. (1994). Preliminary evaluation of the effectiveness of antilock brake systems
for passenger cars. Report DOT HS 808 206. US Department of Transportation, National
Highway Traffic Safety Administration, Washington, DC.
Lie, A., Tingvall, C., Krafft, M. and Kullgren, A. (2004). The effectiveness of ESP (electronic
stability programme) in reducing real life accidents. Traffic Injury Prevention, 5, 37-41.
Michon, J.A. (1985). A critical view of driver behavior models: What do we know, what
should we do? In L. Evans and R.C. Schwing (Eds.). Human behaviour and traffic safety.
Plenum Press, New York.
Naatanen, R. and Summala, H. (1974). A model for the role of motivational factors in
drivers' decision-making. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 6, 243-261.
Overskeid, G. (2000). The slave of the passions: Experiencing problems and selecting
solutions. Review of General Psychology, 4, 284-309.
Reason, J. (1990). Human error. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Robinson, B.1. and Duffin, A.R. (1993). The performance and reliability of anti-lock braking
systems. Braking of road vehicles. Proceedings of the Institution of Mechanical Engi-
neers, 23-24 March 1993. Institution of Mechanical Engineers (IMechE), Birdcage Walk,
London. Published by Mechanical Engineers Publications Limited, pp. 115-126.
Rothengatter, T. (1988). Risk and the absence of pleasure: A motivational approach to
modelling road user behaviour. Ergonomics, 31, 599-607.
232 Vaa
Sagberg, F., Fosser, S. and Saetermo, LA.F. (1997). An investigation of behavioural adapta-
tion to airbags and antilock brakes among taxi drivers. Accident Analysis and Prevention,
29, 293-302.
Sagberg, F. (2001). Accident risk of car drivers during mobile telephone use. International
Journal of Vehicle Design, 26(1), 57-69.
Taylor, D.H. (1964). Driver's galvanic skin response and the risk of accidents. Ergonomics,
7,439-451.
Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgement under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases.
Science, 185, 1124-1131.
Ulleberg, P. (2002). Personality subtypes of young drivers. Relationship to risk-taking pref-
erences, accident involvement and response to a traffic safety campaign. Transportation
Research Part F, 4, 279-297.
Vaa, T. (2000). A comment on the definition of aggression and aggressive driving behaviour.
Proceedings ofthe Conference 'Road Safety on three Continents' Pretoria. South Africa,
September 2000, pp. 20-22.
Vaa, T. (2001a). Cognition and emotion in driver behaviour models: Some critical view-
points. Proceedings of the 14th ICTCT Workshop. Caserta 2001 (www.ictct.org).
Vaa, T. (2001b). Driver behaviour models and monitoring of risk: Damasio and the role of
emotions. Proceedingsfrom VTI-Conference Traffic Safety on Three Continents. Moscow
19-21 September 2001.
Vaa, T. (2003a). Survival or deviance? A Model for Driver Behaviour. Final report. Oslo,
Institute of Transport Economics. T0I-report no. 666/2003 (In Norwegian with summary
in English).
Vaa, T. (2003b). Impairments, diseases, age and their relative risks ofaccident involvement:
Resultsfrom meta-analysis. Deliverable Rl.l of EU-project IMMORTAL. Oslo, Institute
of Transport Economics, T0I report no. 690/2003.
Varhelyi, A. (1996). Dynamic speed adaptation based on information technology: A theo-
retical background. Department of Traffic Planning and Engineering, Lund Institute of
Technology, University of Lund, Bulletin 142.
Wilde, G.J.S. (1982). The theory of risk homeostasis: Implications for safety and health.
Risk Analysis, 2, 209-225.
Wilde, G.J.S. (1988). Risk homeostasis theory and traffic accidents: Propositions, deduc-
tions and discussion of dissension in recent reactions. Ergonomics, 31(4),441-468.
Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavioural expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking.
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
V
Modelling Risk and Errors
13
Time-Related Measures for Modelling
Risk in Driver Behaviour
RICHARD VAN DER HORST
13.1 Introduction
Accident statistics have an important general safety monitoring function and form a
basis for detecting specific traffic safety problems. However, the resulting informa-
tion is inadequate for analysing and diagnosing, defining remedial measures and
evaluating their effects. Systematic observations of driver behaviour, combined
with knowledge of human information-processing capabilities and limitations,
offer wider perspectives in understanding the causes of safety problems and mod-
elling driver behaviour in both normal and critical situations. Renewed interest
results from the need to develop, test, assess and evaluate driver support systems
in terms of drivers' behaviour, performance and acceptance.
The processes that result in near-accidents or traffic conflicts have much in
common with the processes preceding actual collisions (Hyden, 1987); only the
final outcome is different. The frequency of traffic conflicts is relatively high, and
they offer a rich information source on causal relationships since the preceding
process can be systematically observed. In this approach, traffic situations are
ranked along a continuum ranging from normal situations, via conflicts to ac-
tual collisions. A pyramidal representation of this continuum was introduced by
Hyden (1987), clearly visualising the relative rate of occurrence of the different
events (Fig. 13.1). The analysis of driver behaviour in critical encounters may
not only offer a better understanding of the processes that ultimately result in
accidents, but, perhaps even more efficient in the long run, also provide us with
knowledge on drivers' abilities of turning a critical situation into a controllable
one.
A general conceptual description of the driving task as commonly used in traf-
fic psychonomics, with time-to-line crossing (TLC) and time-to-collision (TTC)
as a measure for describing the lateral and longitudinal driving task will be used
to distinguish normal from critical behaviour. That may serve as realistic crite-
rion settings for in-car warning systems such as forward collision warning and
intersection collision avoidance warning systems.
235
236 van der Horst
undisturbed passag es
FIGUR E 13.1. The continuum of traffic events from undisturbed passages to fatal accidents
(from Hyden. 1987).
J]
a:w manoeuvnng I - - - - - + - - - - + - - - - - r
I control
~ perception processing action
CIJ
(
~
\ _Age..based
~f\O'fJ~ ad
ru\e..b3S
s\<\\\-b3Sed
the navigation level and put all attention to the control level, since getting lost has
less severe consequences than running off the road.
At each level of the driving task successive steps of information processing; that
is, perception, processing and decision making, and action take place. Moreover,
the way a driver performs these steps strongly depends on the routine one has
developed in task performance. Rasmussen (1985) distinguishes three levels of
task performance: knowledge-based, rule-based and skill-based. The highest level
(knowledge-based) refers frequently to new situations (e.g., finding the best route
to a new destination) or situations that occur frequently in itself, but in which the
driver still has little experience. The choice of behaviour depends on interpretation
and deductive reasoning. When a situation occurs frequently, then after some time
a rule develops how to deal with that situation and recognising that situation leads
to appropriate behaviour without a 'need' to understand exactly what is going on.
Skill-based tasks are conducted automatically, incoming information automatically
results in behaviour without any cognitive control. Theeuwes (1993) introduced a
nice three-dimensional representation of the driving task as is given in Fig. 13.3.
It is obvious that also other aspects of the driver such as his intentions, attitudes,
emotions and subjective norms play a role in modelling driver behaviour. One
possible representation of this is given in Fig. 13.4 (van der Horst, 1998).
:- -
D~~~ -----------------------_. ----------------------------
Road
environment Vehicle
speed,
heading, etc.
FIGURE 13.4. A driver behaviour model according to van der Horst (1998).
238 van der Horst
"~
"00 I
o
a. ~""""'-----"'----------"'-~--'---~---
~
Q)
.I
o
~I---+----+---++----+----+--+----+l----+-+---
time
FIGURE 13.5. Example of time history for time-to-line crossing (TLC) to the left and right
lane marking at the moment of looking (from Godthelp 1984).
13. Time-Related Measures for Modelling Risk in Driver Behaviour 239
12
:e 10
CD
.~ 8
,,,
,,
,,
6 '~ ......
.. ....
............... ....
~
4 ......
2
20 40 60 80 100 120
speed (km/h)
rather consistent internal representation of the time available to neglect path errors
dependent of speed with a Tocc at about 40% of the totally available time.
Since then, many studies illustrated the value of the TLC approach for describing
drivers' steering strategy and performance (a.o. Godthelp, 1988; Godthelp and
Kaeppler, 1988; Van Winsum et al., 2000).
(van der Horst, 1996) (see Fig. 13.8). The analysis of cumulative distributions of
the lateral placement of free-driving vehicles (both passenger cars and trucks) at
both sites reveals that the package of speed-reducing measures does not result in
a lateral placement more to the middle of the road. On the contrary, the 0.30-m
central chipping strip marking (instead of the O.IO-mconventional central mark-
ing) even results in a lateral placement of minimally 0.10 m further away from
the centre of the road (see Fig. 13.9). Drivers mainly seem to focus on the most
nearby side of the central marking . At the moment oncoming traffic is passing ,
drivers seem to focus on the other vehicle. The mutual distance between two pass-
ing vehicles does not differ at all between the experimental and control location
(see Fig. 13.10).
FIGURE 13.8. Quantification of lateral placement from video at a control location with
conventional markings (top) and at an experimental location in Drenthe (bottom) .
in more complex interactions between two moving road users the collision course
is often ended before point B is reached. But even then, TIC min indicates how
imminent an actual collision has been . Details of the calculation of TTC can be
found in van der Horst (1990) . He evaluated the TTC measure in normal and more
critical encounters between road users in several empirical observation studies . In
one study he analysed all encounters with a collision course between intersecting
242 van der Horst
"'
's
E
~ 20
FIGURE 13.9. Cumulative distributions of the left (LW) and right (RW) wheel of free-driving
passenger cars at experimental and control location relative to the middle of the road (0 em).
passenger cars
100
- = control location
....... = experimental location
80
?ft-
c
0
'.j:i
::J
.lJ 60
~
+oJ
en
=u
Q)
>
',t:j
40
co
S
E
:J
CJ 20
0
0 50 100 150 200 250 300
mutual lateral distance when passing (em)
FIGURE 13.10. Cumulative distributions of the mutual distance between passenger cars at
the moment of passing at experimental and control locations.
road users (at a priority intersection and a general rule intersection) for a given
time period. Figure 13.12 gives the distribution of all encounters with a collision
course (van der Horst, 1991a). Only 1.6% of the 373 encounters with a collision
course displayed a TTC min of less than 1.5 s. Figure 13.13 combines these results
13. Time-Related Measures for Modelling Risk in Driver Behaviour 243
I
I
I
1
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
c
20 21 22 23 24 25 26
time Is)
t------~-----------
:....... ~
/e n= 373 80 c
/- o
<I) 15 /.
./
L- +'"
:J
~ .D
C
:J 60 L-
o UI
gOJ 10 '0
Qt
'0 40 ~
.... ~
QI
J::J ~
E 5 E
20 ~
:J
c
2 5 6
. _ '.::-- ."7"::':"':'.
c
o
..,
~ 0.8 /
-
.:
-i: /
.!!! i
~ 0.6 /
.~
C
~
e
::J
0.4
u
:" --Malmo (n= 101)
0.2 - - - TrautenfeLs In- 621
- . - bicycle route (n= 1477)
....... priori ty regulation (n= 373)
2 3 4 5 6
TrCmin(S)
FIGURE 13.13. TTC min distributions of conflicts from the Malmo and Trautenfels calibration
studies, of encounters at intersections with bicycle tracks, and of the study from Fig. 13.12.
with the outcome of two calibration studies on traffic conflicts techniques under
the auspices of the ICTCT (now International Co-operation on Theories and Con-
cepts in Traffic Safety). In these studies, one in Malmo and one in Trautenfels,
the conflicts as scored by traffic conflict observation teams from eight different
countries have been analysed quantitatively in terms of TTC. Obviously, the inter-
actions scored as conflicts by conflict observer teams in the field, have much lower
TTC min values than all encounters with a collision course that occur at a yield and
general rule intersection. A bicycle route study gives a distribution somewhere in
between, mainly due to the (conservative) selection procedure as applied for that
study (van der Horst, 1990).
Based on these empirical findings, the question arises whether time measures
such as TTC are used directly by the road user as a cue for decision making. Lee
(1976) suggested that drivers are able to control braking based on TTC information
as directly available from the optic flow field. He states that information from the
optic flow field is likely to be used directly rather than information on distance
and speed explicitly. In a study on drivers' strategies of braking, it was found
that both the decision to start braking and the control of the braking process itself
may well be based on TTC information as directly available from the optic flow
field (van der Horst, 1990, 1991b). In a field experiment, subjects approaching a
stationary object (simulated rear end of a small passenger car) with a given speed,
were instructed to start braking at the latest moment they thought they could stop
in front of the object. Figure 13.14 (top) reveals that TTC br increases with speed,
but less than expected on the basis of a constant deceleration model. The effect of
braking instruction (start normal braking at the latest moment you think you can
stop safely versus hard braking at the latest moment you think you are able to stop
in front of the object) indicates that subjects are able to apply the given instruction
well, independent of approach speed. The minimum TTC as reached during the
13. Time-Related Measures for Modelling Risk in Driver Behaviour 245
o normal braking
hard braking
30 50 70
speed (km/h)
o normal braking
3 hard braking
--~
30 50 70
speed (km/h)
Tr=2 s
150 8= ..3m/s 2
J: +
E 100 +
~
-c
Q)
Cl)
o,
en
+right lane
50 )Cleft lane
FIGURE 13.15. Mean free driving speed and visibility on the A59 motorway and initial speed
possible as a function of stopping distance given reaction time T, and required deceleration
level a.
stopping. Even in the more extreme case, the speeds of the free driving vehicles
with a visibility range between 40 and 120 m are too high to avoid a collision when
the driver is suddenly being confronted with a stationary object (e.g., a stopped
vehicle). Hogema and van der Horst (1994) computed other scenarios as well and
concluded that drivers at mean speed might be able to slow down in time for lead
vehicles that drive with a speed of at least 38 and 53 kmlh in the right and left lane,
respectively.
The results of the inductive loop data analysis only refer to the behaviour at
one cross section of the motorway. Although this approach has the advantage
that a huge amount of data is available on road user behaviour in real traffic,
the dynamics of car-following behaviour cannot be studied this way. To study
car-following behaviour of drivers over time and to have full control over the
experimental conditions, a driving simulator study has been conducted on car-
following behaviour in both good and adverse visibility conditions (day/night,
fog) (Hogema and van der Horst, 1994). Visibility distances were 40,80,120 and
600 m, according to the standard definition for the meteorological visual range
(MVR) (White and Jeffery, 1980). Since at night, the rear lights of a vehicle are
visible over a larger distance than the contour or the road outline following the
definition of MVR, the rear lights of the lead vehicle were made visible over a
larger distance according to the results of Heiss (1976). In each run, subjects were
partly free driving (i.e., no lead cars) and partly in car-following situations with
varying speeds. To prevent overtaking, the left lane of the freeway was closed by
means of diagonally striped work-zone panels.
The free driving speeds as found in the simulator experiment reveal a good
resemblance with real-world data in good and very poor visibility. In moderate
13. Time-Related Measures for Modelling Risk in Driver Behaviour 247
25 + = night,
[] = day, -40 .:,+
)( = night," .
~20
u
~ 15 x
.X
10
30
en
ctS
8' 20
l-
I-
10
10 20 30 40
TTCstim
~
....
.....
.....
-14 - 12 - 10 -8 -6 -4 -2 o
time (5)
FIGURE 13.18. Individual TTl curves of straight-going cars from theminor road at a yield
intersection involved in an encounter with traffic from theleft.
1.0t----------t~~~ ...-------I
0\
c
'0. 0.8
c-
o
.....
: 0.6
o
>. AKI railway grade
~ 0.4 crossings
J:J signaUsed intersections
.8 vehicle-actuated control
2 0.2 - - - fixed time
0. -log linear model fit
O......----....-L--------------~
o 2 345 6 7
TTS (5)
FIGURE 13.19. Probability of stopping for AKI railway grade crossings (Tenkink and van
der Horst, 1990) versus vehicle-actuated controlled and fixed-time control intersections
(van der Horst and Wilmink, 1986, and Williams, 1977, respectively).
13.5 Conclusions
On the basis of the described studies, it can be concluded that one can learn a lot
from systematic behavioural observations in real traffic to come up with measures
for improving both safety and efficiency. These studies also point to the direct use
of time-related measures such as TTC and TLC as a direct cue for decision making
in longitudinal and lateral control of the vehicle. Time-related measures such as
TTl and TTS serve as appropriate measures for modelling driver behaviour when
negotiating intersections.
These time-related measures provide a framework for modelling driving as a
supervisory control task. Driving is considered to be a time-management task in
which two related measures at the different levels of the driving task are compa-
rable and may serve as a uniform decision criterion when to switch among sub-
tasks.
One example for modelling driver behaviour for automotive applications is an
appropriate criterion for activating a CAS (collision avoidance (warning) system)
based on the TTC approach, together with results of studies on driving behaviour
in adverse visibility conditions resulting in TTC criterion settings in the range
of 4.5-5 s. For driving in fog as an important application area of CASs, special
attention should be given to a distance range of 40-120 m.
References
Alexander, G.J. and Lunenfeld, H. (1986). Driver expectancy in highway design and
traffic operations (Report No. FHWA-TO-86). Federal Highway Administration,
Washington, DC.
13. Time-Related Measures for Modelling Risk in Driver Behaviour 251
Allen, T.M., Lunenfeld, H. and Alexander, G.J. (1971). Driver information needs. Highway
Research Record, 366, 102-115.
Godthelp, 1. (1984). Studies on human vehicle control. Ph.D. Thesis, Delft University of
Technology, Delft.
Godthelp, J. (1988). The limits of path error neglecting in straight lane driving. Ergonomics,
31,609-619.
Godthelp, 1. and Kaeppler, W.D. (1988). Effects of vehicle handling characteristics on
driving strategy. Human Factors, 30, 219-229.
Godthelp, 1., Milgram. P. and Blaauw, G.J. (1985). The development of a time-related
measure to describe driving strategy. Human Factors, 26, 257-268.
Hayward, 1.C. (1972). Near miss determination through use of a scale of danger (Report
no. TTSC 7115). The Pennsylvania State University, Pennsylvania.
Heiss, W.H. (1976). Highway fog visibility measures and guidance systems (NCHRP Report
171). Transportation Research Board, Washington, DC.
Hogema, 1.H. and Horst, A.R.A. van der (1994). Driving behaviour under adverse visibility
conditions. In ERTICO (Ed.). Towards an intelligent transport system: Proceedings ofthe
First World Congress on Applications of Transport Telematics and Intelligent Vehicle-
Highway Systems (pp. 1623-1630). Artech House, Boston London.
Horst, A.R.A. van der (1990). A time-based analysis of road user behaviour in normal and
critical encounters. Ph.D. Thesis, Delft University of Technology, Delft.
Horst, A.R.A. van der (1991a). Video analysis of road user behaviour at intersections. In
T.W. van der Schaaf, D.A. Lucas and A.R. Hale (Eds.). Near miss reporting as a safety
tool (pp. 93-109). Butterworth-Heinemann Ltd, Oxford.
Horst, A.R.A. van der (1991b). Time-to-collision as a cue for decision-making in braking ..
In A.G. Gale et al. (Eds.). Vision in vehicles III (pp. 19-26). Elsevier, Amsterdam.
Horst, A.R.A. van der (1996). Speed-reducing measures for 80 kmlh roads. In Proceedings
of the 9th ICTCT Workshop in Zagreb (pp. 84-95). University of Lund, Lund, October
1996.
Horst, A.R.A. van der (1998). Factors influencing drivers' speed behaviour and adaptation
(TNO-Report TM-98-D006). TNO Human Factors, Soesterberg.
Horst, R. van der (2004). Occlusion as a measure for visual workload: An overview of TNO
occlusion research in car driving. Applied Ergonomics, 35, 189-196.
Horst, A.R.A. van der and Hoekstra, W. (1994). Testing speed reduction designs for 80
kilometer per hour roads with simulator. In Transportation Research Record 1464 (pp. 63-
68). Transportation Research Board, Washington, DC.
Horst, A.R.A. van der and Hogema, J.H. (1994). Time-to-collision and collision avoidance
systems. In Proceedings 6th ICTCT Workshop (pp. 109-121). Salzburg. Kuratorium fur
Verkehrssicherheit, Salzburg, October 27-29, 1993.
Horst, A.R.A. van der and Wilmink, A. (1986). Drivers' decision-making at signalised
intersections: An optimisation of the yellow timing. Traffic Engineering and Control,
27(12), 615-622.
Hyden, C. (1987). The development of a method for traffic safety evaluation: The Swedish
Traffic Conflicts Technique (Bulletin 70). University of Lund, Lund Institute of Technol-
ogy, Department of Traffic Planning and Engineering, Lund.
Janssen, W.H. and Thomas, H. (1997). In-vehicle collision avoidance support under adverse
visibility conditions. In I. Noy (Ed.). Ergonomics and safety ofintelligent driver interfaces
(pp. 221-229). Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, N1.
Lee, D.N. (1976). A theory of visual control of braking based on information about time-
to-collision. Perception, 5, 437-459.
252 van der Horst
Milgram, P. and Horst, R. van der (1984). Field-sequential colour stereoscopy with liquid
crystal spectacles. In Proceedings Fourth International Display Research Conference.
Societe des electriciens, des electroniciens et des radio-electroniciens, Paris.
Rasmussen,1. (1985). Trends in human reliability analysis. Ergonomics, 28(8),1185-1195.
Tenkink, E. and Horst, A.R.A. van der (1990). Car driver behavior at flashing light railroad
grade crossings. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 22(3), 229-239.
Theeuwes,1. (1993). Visual attention and driving behaviour. In Proceedings ofthe Interna-
tional Seminar Human Factors in Road Traffic. 5-6 April 1993. Universidade do Minho,
Braga, Portugal.
White, M.E. and Jeffery, DJ. (1980). Some aspects ofmotorway traffic behaviour in fog
(TRRL laboratory report 958). Transport and Road Research Laboratory, Crowthorne.
Williams, W.L. (1977). Driver behavior during the yellow interval. In Transportation Re-
search Record 644 (pp. 75-78). Transportation Research Board, Washington, DC.
Winsum, W. van, Brookhuis, K.A. and Waard, D. de (2000). A comparison of different
ways to approximate time-to-line crossing (TLC) during car driving. Accident Analysis
and Prevention, 32,47-56.
14
Situation Awareness and Driving:
A Cognitive Model
M. BAUMANN AND J. F. KREMS
14.1 Introduction
One of the major preconditions of safe driving is that drivers correctly perceive and
interpret the relevant objects and elements of the current traffic situation and that
they consider these elements in planning and controlling their behaviour. Such
elements may be other drivers, the condition of the street or traffic signs. For
each of these elements drivers do not just have to perceive them but they must
understand them according to their relevance to their goals. In addition, drivers
must also make assumptions about the future actions or states of these elements.
For example, perceiving a car coming from the right when entering a crossroads is
far from being enough in order to react accordingly. The driver must interpret this
car according to its relevance to his own goal, that is, safely passing the crossroads.
He has to take into account whether he or the car from the right has to give way.
But even this is not enough to select the appropriate action. If the other car has to
give way, the driver will try to assess from the speed of the car whether the other
car will indeed stop. A concept that has recently become rather popular in aviation
psychology and that aims at describing and integrating these different cognitive
processes is called situation awareness.
253
254 Baumann and Krems
involves the generation of assumptions about the future behaviour of the elements
on the basis of the comprehension of the situation. Situation assessment encom-
passes the processes of achieving, acquiring and maintaining situation awareness
and is distinct from it.
Situation awareness as a state of knowledge and situation assessment as the
processes that lead to this state of knowledge are highly interdependent. Adams
et al. (1995) describe this interdependence, using Neisser's (1976) perception-
action cycle. Perceived changes in the task environment may lead to a change
in the comprehension of the current situation, that is, to a change in situation
awareness. This updated situation awareness will trigger certain actions that might
lead to the perception of new pieces of information, that is, because these actions
change the task environment or because they comprise the sampling of new pieces
of information. In either case the perception of these new pieces of information
will again change the current situation model. This will again trigger new actions
and so on. This is in line with Klein's (2000) proposal that constructing situation
awareness is an active process of guided information seeking rather than a passive
receipt and storage of information. Klein points out that situation awareness is
determined not only by the current situation but also by the person's actions.
each other. At the same time, these two interpretations are also activated or inhib-
ited by other knowledge. For example, if the driver knows that the police monitor
this crossroads, that knowledge will inhibit the acceleration interpretation. How-
ever, being a short distance from the traffic light will activate the acceleration
interpretation. In the end, the 'winning' interpretation will be the one that receives
the most activation from other activated knowledge.
The result of these two phases is an episodic memory representation. Despite
being a unitary representation, Kintsch (1998) distinguishes between two com-
ponents of the episodic memory representation: The text base and the situation
model. The text base consists of information that is directly derived from the
text. The situation model results from the connection between the text represen-
tation and the comprehender's world knowledge. Translating into the domain of
driving and situation awareness, we propose that the episodic memory represen-
tation similarly consists of two components: A situation-specific representation
(analogous to the text base model) and the situation model. The situation-specific
representation consists of information that is directly perceived from the envi-
ronment. This mainly includes the situation elements the driver perceives and
information about the status of these elements that can be directly perceived,
such as one's own speed. It also might include inferences that can be directly
made from the perception of the situation elements and their status, such as that
one's own speed is greater than the speed of the lead vehicle. This representa-
tion might reflect what Endsley (1995b) terms Level I situation awareness. The
situation model then results from the connection between this situation-specific
representation and the driver's world knowledge. Through this connection the per-
ceived configuration of situation elements becomes connected to prior experienced
situations with similar configurations of elements. This connection provides an in-
terpretation of the current situation, partly depending on the outcome of similar
prior situations. This mechanism might at least in part explain the advantage of
experienced drivers compared to novice drivers in hazard perception (Chapman
and Underwood, 1998; Groeger, 2000; McKenna and Crick, 1994). Experienced
drivers have a much greater chance of having stored in long-term memory the rel-
evant information that identifies the current situation as dangerous because their
database of experienced traffic situations is much greater than that of novices. But
the situation model does not only provide interpretations of the current situation,
such as whether it is dangerous or not. It also contains expectations about the
future development of the situation and the future behaviour of the situation ele-
ments. These expectations are also derived from prior experienced situations that
get connected to the situation-specific representation. For example, an experienced
driver will know how long it will take a yellow light to tum red and perceiving
a traffic light turning yellow will automatically activate this expectation. The sit-
uation model therefore might reflect Endsley's Level II and Level III situation
awareness.
To summarise, this model proposes that the different levels of situation aware-
ness are highly interconnected as they are part of an unitary episodic memory
representation. By using Kintsch's (1998) construction-integration theory of text
14. Situation Awareness and Driving: A Cognitive Model 257
activate the deceleration schema further. Because of the inhibitory link between
the deceleration and acceleration schema, both schemata inhibit each other. As the
deceleration schema becomes more activated than the acceleration schema, the
acceleration schema will be more strongly inhibited. As a result, the deceleration
schema is more highly activated than the acceleration schema and the deceleration
schema takes over the control of action. In sum, contention scheduling is sufficient
to control well-learned, simple action sequences. It resolves competition for se-
lection, prevents competitive use of common resources and negotiates cooperative
use of common resources when possible.
In cases where a schema is not available or when the task is novel or com-
plex, another control structure is necessary. Norman and Shallice (1986) call this
structure the Supervisory AttentionalSystem(SAS). The SAS allows for voluntary,
attentional control of performance. The SAS is basically an attentional system
that influences the selection of schemata in the contention scheduling process by
providing additional activation and/or inhibition to schemata in order to bias their
selection. The selection process itself is always based on the highest activation
values of the competing schemata. This system allows for top-down influences in
the action selection process. For example, if a physician on the way to an emer-
gency call sees the traffic light turning yellow, he will not stop. Instead he will try
to proceed even if the light is red because of his goal to get to the patient as quickly
as possible. In this case the deceleration schema receives greater inhibition from
the output of the SAS and is unlikely to be selected by the contention scheduling
process.
To summarise, we described two well-established theories, Kintsch's (1998)
construction-integration theory of text comprehension and Norman and Shallice's
(1986) theory of action selection. Both theories possess a firm empirical foundation
and describe their assumptions in detail. In addition, Norman and Shallice's theory
was already applied to the driving context as a framework to describe the selection
and execution of driving manoeuvres (Groeger, 2000). What we believe is new
and fruitful is the combination of these two theories as a framework for situation
awareness. We proposed to combine these two theories into a comprehension-
based model of situation awareness. By this it is possible to make much more
detailed assumptions about the processes that are involved in the construction and
maintenance of situation awareness and its use as a basis for action selection in
driving.
signals from other vehicles; or cognitive, for example, when the driver's mind is
preoccupied with a conversation. The comprehension-based model of situation
awareness addresses especially the last kind of distraction, cognitive distraction.
Next we will demonstrate how certain errors in driving can be explained within
the framework of this model.
An error that is called 'inattention blindness' or 'looked-but-did-not-see' de-
scribes the phenomenon that the distracted driver was actually looking at the rele-
vant situation element (e.g., the red traffic light), but did not react accordingly. How
can this be explained in terms of our model? The comprehension-based model of
situation awareness implicitly assigns working memory a key role in the processes
of situation awareness construction, maintenance, updating and action selection.
Working memory resources are necessary for associating perceived elements in
the environment with knowledge stored in long-term memory, for integrating these
new elements in the current situation model, for removing irrelevant elements from
the situation model, for keeping the information in the situation model available
for the selection of appropriate actions, for monitoring the selection and execution
of actions and so on. In the case of cognitive distraction, some of these resources
may not be available because they are assigned to other tasks, such as entering the
destination into a navigation system or talking to the passenger. The remaining
working memory capacity might be too reduced to ensure that the perceived situa-
tion elements get fully connected to the relevant knowledge in long-term memory.
The comprehension process stops prematurely and some relevant implications of
the perceived situation element do not become activated and therefore are not
available in the situation representation to trigger the relevant action schemata. In
addition, the impoverished processing of a perceived situation element may lead
to a shallow memory trace for this element, which then rapidly decays and is lost
from the situation representation. Again, this situation element is not considered
in the action selection process.
Cognitive distraction may not only impair the comprehension of situation ele-
ments, but it can also impair action selection directly. As explained above, actions
are controlled by action schemata that compete for the highest activation value.
The activation of each schema is partly determined by the match between its trigger
conditions and the current situation and by the SASe The SAS preactivates those
schemata that are compatible with the current task goal and inhibits those that are
incompatible. Yet as an attentional system the SAS depends on the availability of
the respective cognitive resources. If these are not available because the driver is
preoccupied with a demanding in-vehicle task or engaged in a conversation, action
selection may be primarily determined by the match between the schemata's trig-
ger conditions and the current situation. In such a case the driver often exhibits the
behaviour that is most strongly connected to the current situation even if it is not
compatible with the current task goal. As an example, imagine someone driving
along a very familiar road. At a certain crossroads the driver is used to turning
right, in the direction of the workplace. But on this day the driver wants to go to the
supermarket, requiring a left tum. The driver is very busy with thinking about what
to buy for dinner and automatically turns right, towards the workplace, instead of
260 Baumann and Krems
to the supermarket. In this case the resources of the SAS were so occupied with
reasoning about what to buy that it could not no longer control behaviour. That
is, preactivation of the less common action schema for this situation ('tum left')
failed in order to prevent the most highly connected action schema to this situation
('tum right') from taking over control.
One of the major concerns connected to the on-board use of information and
driver assistance systems is related to this kind of driver distraction and the errors
that follow from impaired situation awareness. Safe driving requires uninterrupted
surveillance of one's vehicle and awareness of the traffic situation and changes in
environmental conditions. Interacting with an in-vehicle information system can
lead to distraction, increased cognitive workload and a rise in the probability of
accidents. Therefore, within the ITS domain there is a need for a procedure to
measure the effects of IVIS tasks on the driver's situation awareness (European
Commision, 2000).
The theory outlined above emphasises the importance of working memory pro-
cesses for the construction and maintenance of the situation representation while
driving. It immediately follows that IVIS tasks that are associated with high work-
ing memory load should clearly impair the construction and maintenance of a
proper situation representation.
on the stimulus presented. But the correct response depends not only from the
presented stimulus but also from the current context. For example in context A,
the participant presses the right button if stimulus 1 appears and the left button
if stimulus 2 appears. In context B the stimulus-button pairing is reversed. The
current context is signalled by acoustic signals, a high- and a low-frequency tone.
Every three to five visual stimuli a new context signal is presented that can indicate
either a new context or the same as before. Remembering the current context and
updating the context define the memory part of this task.
This task yields two measures that are intended to allow the independent assess-
ment of visual and working memory demands of the primary task. First, one can
look at the proportion of visual stimuli responded to, not differentiating whether
the response was correct or not according to the respective context. This detection
rate is used as a measure of the visual demand of the primary task. If the primary
task is highly visually demanding, the participant will simply miss more of the
visual stimuli than if the primary task is of low visual demand. The proportion of
correct reactions from all given reactions is used as a measure of working memory
demands of the primary task. If the task is highly demanding in terms of working
memory, the participants may often forget the current context or may frequently
fail to update the context after a new context signal was presented. This leads to
more erroneous responses to the visual stimuli.
Mean SD Mean SD
than when the working memory task was the primary task. The reverse pattern
was expected for the correct response rate. Table 14.1 presents the mean detection
and correct response rates in the baseline, visual and working memory condi-
tion. The rates are based on the responses to the first stimulus after a context
change.
As predicted, the results show that the detection rate is clearly lower when
the visual search task is the primary task than for the working memory task,
Z == - 2.516, p == 0.012. This indicates that the detection rate is sensitive to the
visual demand of the primary task, confirming the results of Baumann et al. (2003).
The correct response rate was only slightly lower for the working memory task
than with the visual search task, indicating that the correct response rate is not very
sensitive to the working memory demands of the primary task. The difference was
not significant, Z == -0.608, p == 0.543. We assume that the failure to find a
significant difference in the correct response rate between the visual and working
memory condition is due to the too small working memory demands of the context-
dependent choice reaction task. Therefore, in a second experiment we will increase
the working memory demand of the task be increasing the frequency of the context
changes.
14.5 Conclusions
The goal of our approach is to establish the cognitive basis of situation awareness
in order to be able to apply it to the driving task. We assume, similar to Adams et al.
(1995), that the construction of situation awareness is basically a comprehension
process that yields a mental representation of the meaning of the different elements
of a traffic situation and the situation as whole, that is, the situation representation.
This situation representation serves as a basis for planning future behaviour that in
tum alters the situation representation again. According to this perspective, driver
knowledge plays a key role in determining the significance of events and elements
in a traffic situation. Viewing situation awareness as a comprehension process
also highlights the role of working memory in the process of constructing and
maintaining situation awareness. The processes necessary to interpret new pieces
of information, to determine their consequences for the current situation model,
to integrate new pieces of information into the situation model and to remove
264 Baumann and Krems
irrelevant information from the situation model must take place in working mem-
ory. If the resources of working memory are occupied by other tasks, for example
IVIS tasks, these processes will be impaired leading to a degraded situation aware-
ness. This process model of situation awareness is used to develop a procedure
that should allow the evaluation of visual and working memory demands of IVIS
tasks. The idea is that knowing the visual and working memory demands of IVIS
tasks allows to make predictions about the effects of these IVIS tasks on situation
awareness.
References
Adams, MJ., Tenney, YJ. and Pew, R.W. (1995). Situation awareness and the cognitive
management of complex systems. Human Factors, 37(1), 85-104.
Bailly, B., Bellet, T. and Goupil, C. (2003). Driver's mental representations: Experimental
study and training perspectives. In L. Dom (Ed.). Driver behavior and training. Aldershot,
UK, Ashgate.
Baumann, M., Rosler, D., Jahn, G. and Krems, J.F. (2003). Assessing driver distraction using
occlusion method and peripheral detection task. In H. Strasser, K. Kluth, H. Rausch and
H. Bubb (Eds.) Quality of work and products in enterprises of the future. Ergonomia
Verlag, Stuttgart, pp. 53-56.
Breuer, 1., Bengler, K., Heinrich, C. and Reichelt, W. (2003). Development of advanced
driver attention metrics (ADAM). In H. Strasser, K. Kluth, H. Rausch and H. Bubb (Eds.).
Quality of work and products in enterprises of the future. Ergonomia Verlag, Stuttgart,
pp.37-39.
Chapman, P.R. and Underwood, G. (1998). Visual search of driving situations: Danger and
experience. Perception, 27(8), 951-964.
Dawson, M.R.W. (1998). Understanding cognitive science. Blackwell, Malden.
Endsley, M.R. (1995a). Measurement of situation awareness in dynamic systems. Human
Factors, 37(1), 65-84.
Endsley, M.R. (1995b). Towards a theory of situation awareness in dynamic systems. Human
Factors, 37(1), 32-64.
European Commission (2000). Recommendation of 21st December 1999 on safe and ef-
ficient in-vehicle information and communication systems: A European statement of
principles on human machine interface. Official Journal, 2000/53/EC.
Groeger, J. (2000). Understanding driving. Psychology Press, Hove.
HauB, Y. and Eyferth, K. (2001). The evaluation of a multi-sector-planner concept:
SALSA - A new approach to measure situation awareness in ATC. Fourth Interna-
tionalAir TrafficManagement R&D Seminar ATM-2001. (http://atm2001.eurocontrol.fr'/
finalpapers/pap 136.pdf).
Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge University
Press, New York.
Klein, G. (2000). Cognitive task analysis of teams. In 1.M. Schraagen, S.F. Chipman and
T.D. Shalin (Eds.). Cognitive task analysis. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah,
pp. 417-430.
Marr, D. (1982). Vision. Freeman, San Francisco.
McKenna, F.P. and Crick, J.L. (1994). Hazard perception in drivers: A methodology for
testing and training. Transport Research Laboratory (Contractor Report 313), pp. 1-29.
14. Situation Awareness and Driving: A Cognitive Model 265
Miura, T. (1986). Coping with situational demands: A study of eye movements and periph-
erial vision performance. In A.G. Gale, J.D. Brown, C.M. Haselgrave, P. Smith and S.H.
Taylor (Eds.). Vision in vehicles-II. Elsevier, Amsterdam.
Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality: Principles and implications ofcognitive psychol-
ogy. Freeman, San Francisco.
Norman, D.A. (1981). Categorization of action slips. Psychological Review, 88(1), 1-15.
Norman, D.A. and Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control
of behavior. In RJ. Davidson, G.E. Schwartz and D. Shapiro (Eds.). Consciousness and
self-regulation (Vol. 4). Plenum Press, New York, pp. 1-18.
Rousseau, R., Tremblay, S. and Breton, R. (2005). Defining and modeling situation aware-
ness: A critical review. In S. Banbury and S. Tremblay (Eds.). A cognitive approach to
situation awareness: Theory and application. Ashgate, Hampshire Burlington, pp. 3-21.
Williams, LJ. (1985). Tunnel vision induced by a foveal load manipulation. Human Factors,
27,221-227.
Williams, LJ. (1995). Peripheral target recognition and visual field narrowing in aviators
and nonaviators. International Journal ofAviation Psychology, 5(2), 215-232.
Winsum, W. van, Martens, M.H. and Herland, L. (1999). The effects ofspeech versus tactile
driver support messages on workload, driver behaviour and user acceptance (Report
No. TM-99-C043). TNO Human Factors. Soesterberg, the Netherlands.
15
Driver Error and Crashes
DIANNE PARKER
266
15. Driver Error and Crashes 267
using the brakes and are able to combine these tasks with others such as talking
to a passenger or monitoring hazards on the road ahead. At this level of perfor-
mance, errors come in the form of slips and lapses. For example, driving towards
work when you intend to go to the supermarket on a Saturday because you were
distracted by the children at the point on the route where a different turning was
required.
to existing rules. For example, a safety helmet may restrict movement and so
become a safety hazard in a confined area.
Our programme of work has identified three basic types of bad driving, initially
using a 50-item self-report questionnaire, the Manchester Driver Behaviour Ques-
tionnaire (DBQ) (Reason et aI., 1990). The DBQ was administered to a national
sample of 520 drivers, who were asked how often each of the behaviours happened
to them in the course of their normal driving, using a simple frequency response
scale, where 0 == never and 5 == nearly all the time. Factor analysis of their re-
sponses revealed three strong underlying dimensions, reflecting distinct types of
bad driving that were statistically as well as conceptually, different. The question-
naire was streamlined to 24 items, to include the eight top loading errors, lapses
and violations. In a further study, the revised measure was completed by almost
1600 drivers, and the three factors were confirmed.
The first type, true errors, are mistakes and slips, such as misjudging the speed
of oncoming traffic when attempting to overtake, attempting to overtake a vehicle
you had not noticed was signalling a right turn, and failing to notice a Give Way
sign at a junction. This type of behaviour is potentially dangerous, for the driver
and other road users. The second group of behaviours, which we called lapses,
are usually harmless but irritating. They include turning on the windscreen wipers
when you mean to turn on the indicator, forgetting where you have left your car in
a car park, and realising you have no clear recollection of the road you have just
driven along. The focus of this paper is on the third group of behaviours, which we
called violations, includes speeding, tail gating and overtaking on the inside. These
behaviours differ from mistakes, slips and lapses in that they are committed, at
least in part, intentionally and in the knowledge that one is engaging in potentially
dangerous and often illegal behaviour.
Incidentally, this tripartite typology, of violation, error, and lapse has been repli-
cated in studies carried out by other researchers in Britain, and in the Netherlands,
Finland, Sweden, Brazil, Spain, Germany, Australia and China. In particular, Lars
Aberg in Sweden and Timo Lajunen in Turkey have worked with the DBQ ex-
tensively (e.g., Lajunen et al., 2004). While the factor structure uncovered varies
slightly from time to time, there is now clear evidence that this is a robust and
meaningful instrument.
i= m iii
EEl I!J em
II(
..J
0 lEI
>
0
E1
m Ii
1 EI ~ r:J E:J
EI EI &I
2
-3 -2 -1 0 2 3
ERRORS
FIGURE 15.1. The error and violation factor scores of 185 crash-involved drivers (from
Reason et al., 1989).
DBQ factor scores, and demographic information about the drivers, including their
annual mileage, and age and sex, as these are all known to be associated with crash
involvement.
Annual mileage driven, age and sex were all predictive of crash involvement,
in predictable ways. Those who drove more had more crashes, older drivers had
fewer crashes, and males had more crashes than females. However, even after
statistically controlling for the effects of mileage, age and sex, adding DBQ factor
scores to the equation resulted in a significant improvement in explained variance.
The violations factor score was significantly predictive of crash rates. Equally
significantly, scores on the error factor were not predictive of crash rate. Moreover,
this predictive relationship was found in a subsequent prospective study (Parker et
aI., 1995b) to hold good for crash involvement in the 3-year period after the DBQ
measure was used, showing that the measure is truly predictive.
The relationship is shown in Fig. 15.1, which plots almost 200 high crash-
involvement survey respondents (i.e., those who had had two or more crashes in
the previous 3 years) in terms of their scores on the DBQ violation and error
factors. Factor scores, by their nature, have a mean value of a and a standard
deviation of 1. Therefore, a negative score indicates a higher than average number
of reported errors or violations, and a negative score indicates a lower than average
number. It is evident from Fig. 15.1 that these respondents were characterised by
high mean scores on the violation factor, but not by high mean scores on the
error factor. In the equivalent plot for those respondents who had had only one
270 Parker
crash, error and violation scores are equally distributed around the means on both
factors.
The practical importance of this distinction between errors, on the one hand,
and violations, on the other, is that they have very different psychological origins
and they therefore have very different remedial implications. Errors are based on
perceptual, attentional, or judgmental processes, so if crashes are caused primarily
by errors, we should try to improve the situation by training people to use their
cognitive resources more carefully or efficiently. So errors can be tackled by the
type of driver training most often offered. Violations, on the other hand, would
seem to be based on attitudinal and/or motivational factors. So, if there were
evidence that crashes are caused primarily by violations, it would make sense to
try to improve the situation by changing people's beliefs and/or motives.
It is well established that young males are the highest violators. However, that
piece of information is not all that helpful to road safety professionals. There
will always be young male drivers on the roads, unless some fairly radical policy
decisions are taken. Moreover, no one could seriously argue that there is something
about being young or about being male that directly causes risk-taking behaviour
behind the wheel. It is far more plausible to suggest that there are psychological
factors, which are correlated with youth and maleness, that predispose some drivers
to commit violations. This suggestion is supported by the fact that not all young
males are high violators, and not all high violators are young males.
The crucial characteristic of high violators is that they are choosing to take risks.
The commission of a driving violation is a matter of choice. We do not simply find
ourselves overtaking on the inside, or gesturing to another road user, or shooting
through traffic lights as they tum red. While many drivers stopped by the police
plead that they were unaware that they were breaking the speed limit, it can be
argued that at some level we are well aware of the speed we are travelling. If
that were not the case the presence of a police vehicle would not have such an
immediate impact on traffic speeds. High violators are choosing to drive in a way
that is unacceptable to society, and the way to reduce the level of violating on
the roads is to change the attitudes of the violators. Put simply, we need to find
ways to persuade them not to do it. The threat of sanctions, in the form of penalty
points, fines or a driving ban, are powerful persuaders. Monitoring in the form of
increased police presence and speed cameras also play an important part. However,
the level of enforcement that would be necessary to eliminate the commission of
driving violations would be practically impossible as well as politically unpopular.
It is my contention that stable and enduring reductions in the level of violating
are more likely to be realised through attitude change. While attempting to change
the attitudes of existing drivers is a difficult process, fostering desirable attitudes
among young people who do not yet have a driving licence may be more fruitful
in the long term. Unfortunately, at least in England, to my knowledge little or no
effort is made to impress upon young people that being a good driver is about
being a responsible and courteous driver as well as having the right skills.
Emphasising the predictive link between violations and crashes does not imply
that errors are not important in crash causation. It is obviously necessary for all
15. Driver Error and Crashes 271
drivers to have the basic level of driving skill that training of novices provides. But it
is a truism that to err is human. Every driver makes mistakes behind the wheel, and
usually, thanks to good fortune and the efforts of engineers in designing primary
and secondary safety features into vehicles and road environments our errors escape
punishment. However, if a driver makes an error while committing a violation, or
if any other road user in the vicinity does, the consequences are more likely to be
disastrous.
The commission of violations takes the driver closer to the limits of his or her
abilities, into a situation where an error of their own, or of anyone else, is far more
likely to be punished, with dreadful results. Therefore, alongside the continuing
efforts to prevent, and recover from, driver error, there must be continuing research
to understand and explain driver violations.
Having made the initial distinction, we have continued to refine the Manchester
DBQ. For example, in a study focusing on the behaviour of drivers under 40
(Lawton et al., 1997), factor analysis of the DBQ produced a split in the violations
factor. Inspection of the items loading onto each factor showed that the interperson-
ally aggressive violations included in the scale had separated from what we called
the 'normal' or Highway Code violations. In other words, those violations that
in some sense were committed 'against' a specific other road user (racing, chas-
ing, etc.) appeared to be different from those with no immediately obvious victim
(speeding, drink-driving, etc.). To investigate this distinction further, we produced
a 12-item version of the violations scale, that included six interpersonally aggres-
sive, and six normal violations. Table 15.1 shows the wording of these violation
items.
This scale has since been used in a number of DBQ studies (e.g., Parker et aI.,
2002; Lajunen et al., 2004) and the distinction between the two scales broadly
supported. There is also some suggestion that the interpersonally aggressive vio-
lations consist of two further sub-types. The first sub-type of aggressive violation
is related to anger/hostility and may reflect a general personality characteristic.
Lajunen and Parker's (2001) study of personality aggression, anger while driv-
ing and propensity to commit violations provided some support for the idea that
aggressive driving may be seen as a learned problem-solving strategy.
The second sub-type of aggressive violations is to do with gaining advantage
over other road users and maintaining progress. These violations are more likely to
occur in areas of high traffic density, where the motorist comes across unexpected
hold-ups and delays. In our research (Lajunen et al., 1999), traffic obstructions like
traffic jams or road constructions did not seem to provoke anger among British
drivers, a finding supported by Underwood et aI.'s (1999) diary study. These kinds
of impediments may be so common in today's traffic in Britain that drivers can
expect them to occur and therefore do not become unduly frustrated. Hence, the
frustration-aggression hypothesis (Dollard et al., 1939) might be more applicable
272 Parker
in situations where drivers' goals are dramatically blocked by a sudden and unex-
pected event.
by the situation when someone nips in and takes the parking space you had been
waiting for.
15.7 Conclusions
Several aspects of human error have been outlined in this paper. It is clear that
intelligent systems can help in some of types of error, for example by reminding
drivers not to dazzle with their lights, or by warning them in advance about unex-
pected problems on their route and suggesting alternatives to prevent or minimise
delay. However, our studies suggest that the reduction of other types of aberrant
driving, that is, driving violations, will also require attention to the social and mo-
tivational aspects of driving. It is crucial to acknowledge that driving is a motivated
behaviour that involves emotions as well as rational decisions.
When drivers know what to expect, they usually cope relatively well. When
something unexpected crops up, that is going to impede their progress, they are
likely to become stressed, react with anger, and begin to take risks, committing
aggressive violations in order to gain advantage over other road users and main-
tain progress. The provision of information may help to prevent these aggressive
violations. At first it may appear that intelligent systems can have their greatest
impact in error reduction. However, it may be that, provided the social aspects of
driver behaviour are taken into account, such systems have an important role to
play in preventing the commission of at least some types of driving violation.
References
Dollard, 1., Miller, N.E., Doob, L.W., Mowrer, a.H. and Sears, R.R. (1939). Frustration
and aggression. Yale University Press, New Haven, CT.
Johnson, P. and Gill, J. (1993). Management control and organizational behaviour. Paul
Chapman, London.
Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgements under uncertainty. Science 185, 1124-
1131.
Lajunen, T. and Parker, D. (2001). Are aggressive people aggressive drivers? A study of the
relationship between self-reported general aggressiveness, driver anger and aggressive
driving. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 33, 243-255.
Lajunen, T., Parker, D. and Summala, H. (1999). Does traffic congestion increase driver
aggression? Transportation Research Part F, 2, 225-236.
Lawton, R. (1998). Not working to rule: Understanding procedural violations at work. Safety
Science, 28, 77-95.
Lawton, R.L., Parker, D., Stradling, S.G. and Manstead, A.S.R. (1997). The role of affect in
predicting social behaviors: The case of road traffic violations. Journal ofApplied Social
Psychology, 27, 1258-1276.
Lajunen, T., Parker, D. and Summala, H. (2004). The Manchester Driver Behaviour Ques-
tionnaire: A cross-cultural study. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 36(2), 231-238.
Parker, D. and Lawton, R. (2003). A psychological contribution to the understanding of
adverse events in healthcare. Quality and Safety in Healthcare, 12,453-457.
274 Parker
Parker, D., Reason, J.T., Manstead, A.S.R. and Stradling, S.G. (1995a). Driving errors,
driving violations and accident involvement. Ergonomics, 38, 1036-1048.
Parker, D., West, R.W., Stradling, S.G. and Manstead, A.S.R. (1995b). Behavioural char-
acteristics and involvement in different types of road traffic accident. Accident Analysis
and Prevention, 27, 571-581.
Parker, D., Lajunen, T. and Summala H. (2002). Anger and aggression among drivers in
three European countries. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 34(2), 229-235.
Rasmussen, J. (1990). Human error and the problem of causality in analysis of accidents.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 327,449-460.
Rasmussen, J. and Jensen, A. (1974). Mental procedures in real-life tasks: A case study of
electronic troubleshooting. Ergonomics, 17, 293-307.
Reason, J. (1990). Human error. Open University Press, Cambridge.
Reason, J., Parker, D. and Lawton, R. (1998). The varieties of rule-related behaviour. Journal
of Organisational and Occupational Psychology, 71, 289-304.
Reason, J.T., Manstead, A., Stradling, S., Baxter, J.S., Parker, D. and Kelemen, D. (1989). A
report to the TRRL on Research Contract 9885/35. The social and cognitive determinants
of aberrant driver behaviour. Unpublished report.
Reason, J.T., Manstead, A., Stradling, S., Baxter, James, S. and Campbell, K. (1990). Errors
and violations on the roads: A real distinction? Ergonomics, 33(10/11),1315-1332.
Underwood, G., Chapman, P., Wright, S. and Crundall, D. (1999). Anger while driving.
Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 2F( 1), 55-68.
VI
Control Theory Models of
Driver Behaviour
16
Control Theory Models of the Driver
THOMAS JURGENSOHN
16.1 Introduction
277
278 Jtirgensohn
u
Operator behaviour
y Target
FIGURE 16.1. Behaviour of an operator controlling the rotational speed of a flight defence
cannon via a hand gear (modified from Tustin, 1947).
defence cannon via a hand gear. This is a typical tracking task where the human
has to compensate a deviation. Tustin observed the behaviour of the controllers
(Fig. 16.1), which is fundamentally different from a technical controller: a jerky
curve with 'flats' (Tustin, 1947, p. 119).
Tustin tries to approximate the human controller behaviour by means of a linear
system. The remaining difference U r between approximation u, and real controller
behaviour U he denotes as the remnant (Fig. 16.2).
Both terms of the operator model together, the linear part H(s) and the remnant
Ur , are not an approximation of human behaviour, but describe it completely.
The idea of Tustin was to find a description of the remnant properties which is
independent from the experiment set-up, for example, in a stochastic sense. For
special cases, this is indeed possible (Elkind and Dorlex, 1963; McRuer and
Krendel, 1974). But mostly the remnant has to be considered as an error signal,
which has no special traits, and which cannot be described by means of simple
mathematical methods. However, the remnant part of the operator's output is often
Human Operator
: Controlled Process
y
FIGURE 16.2. Control theoretic model of the controlling human in the Laplace domain (from
Tustin, 1947).
16. Control Theory Models of the Driver 279
small, so the linear part characterises most of the control activity. The models of the
human as a controller, which were set up in the following, still contain the remnant,
but this item is usually neglected. In some applications, remnant models can be
useful. But in general, modelling non-linear parts of human behaviour by assuming
a remnant in terms of stochastics is not appropriate for many applications.
The existence of the remnant in the controller models of the human is an example
of an adoption of paradigms from the technical-physical modelling, which is
not appropriate. The successful method for improving the model quality applied
in the technical sector, that is, adding nonlinear parts to the model modelling
approach (e.g., Boeker et aI., 1986) does not resolve the problem of modelling
human behaviour. Human behaviour is variable to such an extent that the refinement
using nonlinear terms only seems to produce a more accurate model.
Reference Input
Operator
FIGURE 16.3. Tracking behaviour of an operator following reference signals with a pattern
(modified from Vossius, 1964).
280 Jtirgensohn
studies which were conducted by Elkind (1956) and Ekind and Forgie (1959)
dependencies between the parameters could be noticed. The operator adapts his
behaviour in such a way that the overall system behaviour remains approximately
constant in spite of different nominal values. A similar assertion is formulated by
McRuer und Krendel (1962) in their crossover-model. Experimentally measured
frequency responses of the total system exhibit a common property under different
conditions. If the open-loop linear transfer function L(s), which describes human
and machine together, is calculated from the observed closed-loop behaviour, then
the behaviour can be very well approximated by an integrator and an additional
phase correction by means of a dead time for all task environments in the region
of the crossover frequency, We (L(jw) == 0 dB). The crossover model requires only
two parameters, that is, the crossover frequency We and the dead time t , which is
not identical with the dead time of the quasi -linear model.
We
Crossover model: L(s) == _e- rs (2)
s
The interesting point of this model is that human and machine are not modelled
separately as successive connected structures, but as one system. The focus is not
on how humans manage to adapt to different environments, but this is assumed to
be the case. Only total system behaviour is considered. It should be noted that the
crossover model is not a rough approximation with very few parameters, but in
many cases a very precise description.
In a certain way the crossover model already contains the element of goal-
oriented action which dominated in recent approaches. The great similarities in
the frequency responses of the man-machine system for various types of control
plants are manifestations of the operator will. The operator wants to minimise the
control error and achieve a certain overall system behaviour, a goal that is indeed
accomplished for very different types of control plants. The behaviour of the
operator is different, but the overall system behaviour is approximately identical.
Although the crossover model contains internal goals of the operator implicitly
and is therefore more suitable to predict human control behaviour within unknown
control plants than the quasi -linear model, the parameters of the crossover model
depend on the type of the input function, too. Therefore, the description of the
influence of boundary conditions on goal achievement is still only possible a
posteriori or via a catalogue. McRuer and Krendel (1974) and other authors es-
tablished comprehensive collections of crossover model parameters for different
system environments and nominal signal types. As these parameters are not re-
lated to other invariant human properties, the validity of the crossover model for
predicting operator behaviour in unknown environments is also limited.
t [sec]
~
FIGURE 16.4. (Top) Control action of a double integrating control plant by a very well-
trained pilot (from McRuer et aI., 1968). (Bottom) Steering motion of a helmsman on a ship
(from Veldhuyzen and Stassen, 1976).
differential equations. On the other hand, the choice of the method is often also
due to the attraction of a 'modern' tool. Conceptional reasons are often given
only ex post. An example is the BBN model by Baron and Kleinman (1969),
which is a linear model based on an optimal filter approach. Retrospectively, this
model was often praised as especially appropriate, because it contains elements,
which can be interpreted as 'internal model'. In another example the advantage of
models containing adaptive control approaches is deduced from the adaptivity of
human behaviour. The suitability of modelling approaches thus is justified with
resemblances of the model to observed characteristics of human behaviour.
Driver
Vehicle
Set/Desired Value Controlled Value
y
L , J _---/
V
L(s)
FIGURE 16.6. 'Shaft model' of driver's control strategy. Copyright, Jurgensohn (2000).
The lateral control driver models were initiated in Japan. In 1953, Kondo (1953)
developed two different models for driver behaviour due to side wind disturbance
and calculated the behaviour of the whole system on a straight path with constant
speed by means of a single-track model. Both models already show the general
structure of the majority of models to be published for the following 35 years.
In the first model, Kondo assumes that the driver always steers in a way that an
imaginary point within a certain preview distance L, the sight point or aim point,
is on a predetermined course (Fig. 16.6). In terms of control theory this would
mean that the driver minimises the lateral offset ~ y L of the vehicle's projected
centreline from the desired course within the preview distance L in front of the
vehicle.
His assumption was inspired by a situation after an accident in which his car was
towed with a very short rope. For this reason the model is often called the shaft
model. He was also inspired by an experience by driving bicycle in the narrow
streets of Japanese cities: In a narrow lane it is easier to hold the bicycle steady
looking to the exit of the lane. This linear prediction model has been adopted by
many authors, especially in Japan and later in Germany (Fiala, 1967).
The model depicted in Fig. 16.6 can be described in control theoretic terms in
three different ways. First of all ~YL can be taken as a prediction of ~Yawith a
prediction time Tp For a simple P -controller it follows that
(3)
where Tp == L/va are the preview time and Va the longitudinal speed.
Another control theoretic formulation of the model of Fig. 16.6 can be found
for small values of the sight angle <t>.
Driver Vehicle
,----11-----------;-----1
I
I
s, II bT(sE-At lT
X
C ~-~
I Llyo
1++ I
I ______ 1
I
I c=[l,O,O]T
I x T=[LlyO,Llv,Ll'V]
I
L=[k 1,kbk3]T
I I
I I
~ J
FIGURE 16.7. Model of driver's control strategy as state-variable controller (Kondo, 1953).
With this transformation the model is reduced to a control of the centre of gravity.
For small steering angles, we can write:
(5)
(6)
The latter equation is in fact a linearisation ofEq. (3): A lead term as an approx-
imation of a time-preview term.
Kondo's second model from 1953 assumes that the steering angle is a linear
combination of yaw angle error (~ 1/1), heading angle error (~ v) and lateral position
error in the gravity centre (~Yo). In control theoretic manner this can be described
as a linear state variable controller (Fig. 16.7). This second model is modified and
used in many other models, too (e.g., Donges, 1978; Reid et al., 1981).
In another kind of graphical representation, this model has the following form
(Fig. 16.8).
Another well-known model is the so-called 'STI' -model. Systems Technology
Inc. dominated the research in the USA with already mentioned scientists such
as McRuer, Weir, Klein and others. They developed a model (Fig. 16.9), which
was derived from tracking models and pilot models. Besides the model class of
preview predictor models and linear state control models invented by Kondo, the
STI model can be seen as another model class.
A 'translation' of this cybernetic view of the model into a control theoretic
version (Fig. 16.10) shows the similarity to the model of Kondo.
i
DISTURBANCES
~RIVER - - - - - - - DR~E;j ( ~IND8ROAD ) ~AR/ROAD -;;OTlO;KINEM;:I~- ;
1 REMNANT I d I I
,..- ..," I V t:.y
I r I
I r5 8$ CAR LANE I
(TL S + l)e- I DYNAMICS I DEVIATION
I rI I
I : I HEADI ~G I
I I ROAD I U ANGL E :
IL DRIVER PERCEPTION
- J
I CURVAl\JRE p'-I
r L DEVIATION I
..J
VISUAL SCENE I
SIMULAT ION DISPLAY REL ATIVE HEADING ANGLE ~o/
C Jurgensohn,1999
desired
path curvature .... V002eTA
(82+2<;00'8+002)
I11III""""
street variables, too. This type of driver model was published first by Fiala (1967)
and Ohno (1966). It assumes that part of the steering angle is determined from
lane curvature K (see Fig. 16.11) at a particular heading distance. This is open-loop
control, because the lane curvature is independent from vehicle motion. Fiala called
this 'Scheinwerferorientierung' (head light orientation), whereas Ohno talks about
programmed action and intuitive steering. In Fig. 16.10, the structure of one of the
most renowned models with an anticipatory open-loop part, the 'two-level model'
by Donges (1978) is shown. It is a combination of the state variable controller in
the closed-loop as shown in Fig. 16.7 and an open-loop term, assumed as a linear
system with time prediction having 2 df. The reaction time i H of the driver is
modelled by a time delay term. The remnant is an additional source of unknown
influences (driver noise) - also adopted from the old pilot models.
The idea of a compensatory closed-loop control model with an additional open
loop part has found many imitators (Horn, 1986; Mitschke, 1977; Reid et al., 1980).
Yet another step further ahead are the models by Bosch (1991) and Plochl and
Lugner (1994), who introduce a third additive scope of control, the so-called 'local
control' (correction of the instantaneous path deviation) besides compensatory
control and anticipatory control.
Moreover, there are models, which are capable to change their structure during
simulation, for example the dual mode model (Me Ruer et al., 1977) which can
switch between open-loop and closed-loop behaviour. This model has been realised
in DRIVEM (Lieberman and Goldblatt, 1981). This kind of modelling can also
be found in adaptive models which link sudden changes in the environment, for
example, sudden appearance of black ice (Reichelt, 1990; Nagai, 1983; Nagai and
Mitschke, 1987), with changes in the model.
Besides these four types of driver models there are some variations, which are
distinguished by dead times, the kind of input or output parameters or by non-
linear elements. For example Baxter and Harrison (1979) include non-linearities
288 Jtirgensohn
Maneuver
Definition Vehicle State Vehicle
Dynamics
target Path
.... ... Stabilisation Level ...._ .....
(Position Control)
Feedback to Guidance
(e.g. adaptation to enginepower)
The distance between target point and vehicle is not fixed. One could imagine an
elastic linkage between the vehicle and the moving target point towing the vehicle:
The more the vehicle deviates from the target, the harder it will be pulled towards
the target. In the ideal case the driver will keep the vehicle just atop the target at
any time.
Individual driving behaviour like differences in risk-taking and driving expertise,
as well as typical driver errors can be reproduced by means of parameters of
the driver controller, for example, the preview distance, delay times for steering
response, etc., and by influencing the target course (Irmscher and Ehmann, 2004).
This short presentation of examples for models of the car driver shows that the
control theoretic core of the models has not changed much from the beginnings in
the 1950s up to now. Although much time was spent for iterative improvements,
290 Jtirgensohn
there was just a small, 'model evolution' as in many other fields of modelling.
The reason can be found in the large inter- und intra-individual differences and
deviations of human dynamic behaviour. A precise and differentiated modelling
of dynamic human behaviour is not possible or not meaningful. In general, it does
not make sense to describe driver behaviour by means of differential equations of
higher than second order. In a few applications, well-established neuromuscular
models for arm and hand actuation can add an additional third order. Such models
can be useful, for example, in studying steering characteristics.
Therefore, the focus of driver model research shifted from control theory to
other modelling methods, which are better suited to describe non-dynamic aspects
of behaviour, for example, decision making or planning. However, the last example
(cf. Fig. 16.13) illustrated that for the description of dynamic steering characteris-
tics of the driver control theory is still the most appropriate means. However, the
incorporation into an algorithmic environment is the central point here. Summing
up, it can be said sufficient research on driver models based on control theory has
been done, but that there are still open issues on the problem of its integration into
a 'hybrid modelling' (Jiirgensohn, 1997).
References
Allen, R.W. (1983). Defining the design driver from a vehicle control point-of-view. 62nd
Annual TRB Meeting, Washington, USA, STI-Report, No. 83327, S. 1-11.
Baron, S. and Kleinman, D.L. (1969). The human as an optimal controller and information
processor. IEEE Transactions on Man-Machine Systems, Vol. MMS-I0(1), 9-17.
Baxter, 1. and Harrison, 1.Y. (1979). A nonlinear model describing driver behaviour on
straight roads. Human Factors, 21(1), 87-97.
Billing, A.M. (1977). Modelling driver steering in normal and severe manoeuvres. In J.E.
Bernard (Ed.), An overview of simulation in highway transportation, Part II. Bosten:
Simulation Councels, Inc. (S. 151-165).
Blaauw, GJ., Godthelp, H. and Milgram, P. (1984). Optimal control model applications and
field measurements with respect to car driving. Vehicle System Dynamics, 13,93-111.
Boeker, J., Hartmann, I. and Zwanzig, Ch. (1986). Nichtlineare und adaptive Regelsysteme.
Springer-Verlag, Berlin.
Bosch, P. (1991). Der Fahrer als Regler (the driver considered as a control element). Dis-
sertation, TV, Wien.
Braess, H.-H. (1970). Untersuchung des Seitenwindverhaltens des Systems Fahrzeug -
Fahrer (investigation of side gust behaviour of the driver-vehicle system). Deutsche
Kraftfahrtforschung und Straj3enverkehrstechnik, Heft. 206, 5-32.
Carson, 1.M., Wierwille, W.W. and Eastman, C. (1978). Development of a strategy model
of the driver in lane keeping. Vehicle System Dynamics, 7(4),233-253.
Crossman, E.R.F.W., Szostak, H. and Cesa, T.L. (1966). Steering performance of automobile
drivers in real and contact-analog simulated tasks. Human Factors Society 10th Annual
Meeting. Anaheim, California.
Donges, E. (1978). A two-level model of driver steering behaviour. Human Factors, 20(6),
691-707.
Elkind, J.1. (1956). Characteristics ofsimple manual control systems. M.I.T. Lincoln Lab-
oratories, Lexington, Massachusetts., Technical Report, No. 111.
16. Control Theory Models of the Driver 291
Elkind, 1.1. and Forgie, C.D. (1959). Characteristics of the human operator in simple manual
control systems. IRE Transactions on Automatic Control, 4(1), 44-55.
Elkind, 1.1. and Dorlex, D.L. (1963). The normality of signals and describing function
measurement of simple manual control systems. IEEE Transactions on Human Factors
in Electronics, 52-55.
Fiala, E. (1967). Die Wechselwirkung zwischen Fahrzeug und Fahrer (Interaction between
vehicle and driver). AutomobiltechnischeZeitschrift, 69(10), 345-348.
Guo, K. and Guan, H. (1993). Modelling of driver/vehicle directional control system. Vehicle
System Dynamics, 22, 141-184.
Hayhoe, G.E. (1979). A driver model based on the cerebellar model articulation controller.
Vehicle System Dynamics, 8, 49-72.
Hoffmann, E.R. (1975). Human control of road vehicles. Vehicle System Dynamics, 5,
105-126.
Horn, A. (1986). Fahrer-Fahrzeug-Kurvenfahrt auftrockener Strafie (driver-vehicle curve
driving on dry road). Dissertation, TV, Braunschweig.
Irmscher, M. and Ehmann, M. (2004). Driver Classification using ve-DYNA Advanced
Driver, SAE paper 2004-1-0451.
Johannsen, G. (1980). Manuelle Regelung in Mensch-Maschine-Systemen. Habilitation
RWTH Aachen.
Jurgensohn, T. (1997). Hybride Fahrermodelle (Hybrid driver models, ZMMS-Spektrum,
Band 4). Sinzheim: Pro Universitate Verlag.
Kondo, M. (1953). Directional stability (when steering is added). Journal of the Society
ofAutomotive Engineers ofJapan (JSAE), Jidosha-gijutsu, 7(5/6) (in Japanese). Tokyo,
104-106, 109, 123, 136-140.
Krendel, E.S. (1951). A preliminarystudy ofthepower-spectrumapproachto the analysis of
perceptual-motor performance. Wright Air Development Center, Wright-Patterson Air
Force Base, Dayton, Ohio, Technical Report, No. 6723.
Lieberman, E.B. and Goldblatt, R.A. (1981). A review of the driver-vehicle effective-
ness model. Final Report under NHTSA Contract,DTNH22-80-C-07082 (DOT-HS
806110).
MacAdam, C.C. (1980). An optimal preview control for linear systems. Journal ofDynamic
Systems, Measurement, and Control, 188-190.
McRuer, D.T. and Krendel, E.S. (1959). The human operator as a servo system element
Part I + II. Journal of The Franklin Institute, 267(5/6), 381-403, 511-536.
McRuer, D.T. and Krendel, E.S. (1962). The man-machine concept. Proceedings of the
IRE, 50, 1117-1123.
McRuer, D.T., Hofmann, L.G., Jex, H.R., Moore, G.P., Phatak, A.V., Weir, D.H. and
Wolkovitch,1. (1968). New Approaches to Human-Pilot/vehicle Dynamic Analysis. Air
Force Flight Dynamics Laboratory, Technical Report, AFFDL-TR-67-150.
McRuer, D.T. and Krendel, E.S. (1974). Mathematical models of human pilot behavior.
Agardograph, No. 188. Technical Editing and Reproduction Ltd., Harford House, Lon-
don.
McRuer, D.T., Allen, W.R., Weir, D.H. and Klein, R.H. (1977). New results in driver steering
control models. Human Factors, 19(4),381-397.
Mitschke, M. (1977). Kraftfahrzeug-Fahrer-Aktive Sicherheit (Vehicle-driver-active
safety). Automobil-Industrie, 3, 29-32.
Nagai, M. (1983). Adaptive behaviours of driver-car systems in critical situations. Pro-
ceedings of the International AMSE Summer Conference "Modelling and Simulation".
Nice, France.
292 Jurgensohn
Nagai, M. and Mitschke, M. (1987). An adaptive control model of a car-driver and computer
simulation of the closed-loop system. In M. Apetaur (Ed.), The Dynamics of Vehicles on
Roads and on Tracks. pp. 275-286.
Niemann, K. (1972). Messungen und Berechnungen iiber das Regelverhalten von Auto-
fahrern (measurements and calculations of the control behaviour of drivers). Dissertation,
TU, Braunschweig.
Ohno, T. (1966). Steering control on a curved course. Journal of the Society ofAutomotive
Engineers of Japan (JSAE), Jidosha-gijutsu, 20(5) (in Japanese), 413-419.
Plochl, M. and Lugner, P. (1994). Theoretical investigations of the interaction driver-
Feedback-Controlled automobile. Proceedings of the International Symposium on Ad-
vanced Vehicle Control 1994., SAE Paper, No. 9438006, pp. 42-48.
Reichelt, W. (1990). Ein adaptives Fahrermodell zur Bewertung der Fahrdynamik von
PKW in kritischen Situationen (An adaptive driver model to evaluate vehicle dynamics
in critical situations). Dissertation, TU, Braunschweig.
Reid, L.D. (1983). A survey of recent driver steering behaviour models suited to accident
studies. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 15(1), 23-40.
Reid, L.D., Graf, W.O. and Billing, A.M. (1980). The validation of a linear driver model.
University of Toronto, UTIAS Report, No. 245.
Reid, L.D., Solowka, E.N. and Billing, A.M. (1981). A systematic study of driver steering
behaviour. Ergonomics, 24(6),447-462.
Tustin, A. (1947). The nature of the operator's response in manual control and its implica-
tions for controller design. IEEE Journal, 94, Part II A2, 190-202.
Veldhuyzen, W. and Stassen, H.G. (1976). The internal model. What does it mean in human
control? In T.B. Sheridan and G. Johannsen (Eds.), Proceedings ofMonitoring Behavior
and Supervisory Control., Berchtesgaden, Germany, pp. 157-170.
Vossius, G. (1964). Die Vorhersageeigenschaften des Systems der Willkurbewegung,
Neuere Ergebnisse der Kybernetik. Munchen, Wien: Oldenbourg Verlag, pp. 196-209.
Willumeit, H.-P. and Jurgensohn, T. (1997). Driver-models: A critical survey. ATZ
Worldwide, Vol. 99, Part 1 + 2 in No.7 + 8. Wiesbaden: Vieweg, pp. 19-22/19-25.
17
Review of Control Theory Models for
Directional and Speed Control
DAVID H. WEIR, PhD AND KEVIN C. CHAO, MS
17.1 Introduction
Models of driver steering control in regulation tasks are well established and have
been used in a number of studies of driver/vehicle response and performance, e.g.
Weir (1973), Weir and McRuer (1968, 1973), Weir and DiMarco (1978), Weir et al.
(1982) and McRuer et al. (1975, 1977). There is a large literature on this general
topic, including the work of Hoffman (1975), Donges (1978), Sheridan and Ferrel
(1981), Godthelp (1985), Levison and Cramer (1995), Allen et al. (1996) and the
recent survey article (MacAdam, 2003), among many others.
The directional control models of interest here apply to straight or curving
roadways, with approximately constant speed operation and steering to stay in
the centre of the lane in the presence of a random (directional) yaw disturbance.
The models can involve multiple loops or perceptual feedbacks. They are typically
expressed in Laplace transform (transfer function) or differential equation form,
in a classical control theory manner.
The driver model for speed control is similar in form to the model for direc-
tional control, but simpler. The model of interest here is for speed regulation,
where the driver uses the accelerator pedal to maintain a specified constant speed
in the presence of a perceived speed error. The speed error could result from a dis-
turbance such as a headwind gust or could arise in a car-following situation. The
more general topic of car following can involve a number of additional factors not
addressed here, including nonlinear and deterministic behaviour, e.g. Brackstone
and McDonald (1999), Brown et al. (2001) and Fancher and Baraket (1998). The
speed regulation model described herein is a single-loop model. An additional
driver output, and perhaps additional feedback loops, can be added for braking
control.
Useful and simple forms of the driver control models are based on the'crossover
model' of the human operator, e.g. McRuer and Weir (1969) and McRuer and
Krendel (1974). This model applies to a wide variety of manual control tasks,
including driving a car or piloting an aircraft. It can be used to model single-
loop activity such as speed control. It can also model the inner and outer loop
293
294 Weir and Chao
y; == K e
-TS (TLs + 1) (1)
P P (TIS + 1)
where K p is the gain, adopted by the driver, T is the driver's time delay, TL is a
lead element reflecting anticipation, TI is a lag element reflecting smoothing and
S is the Laplace transform variable.
These parameters are adjusted according to the model, and the values of TL
and TI depend on the vehicle dynamics. The quasilinear model, Yp is called a
describing function, because it characterises only the linear part of the driver's
e C m
.
~-
Yp ---
....- Yc .....
OPERATOR CONTROLLED
ELEMENT FIGURE 17.1. Single-loop
manual control system.
17. Review of Control Theory Models for Directional and Speed Control 295
input/output or control activity. The nonlinear part is called remnant, and it can
be modelled as an additive noise (McRuer and Krendel, 1974). The linear part
is the main topic here, and it typically accounts for most of the driver's control
activity.
The model is applied by considering the open loop-properties of the
driver/vehicle system (YpYe ) . The crossover model says that the driver adjusts
the lead and lag terms (TL and TI ) as necessary to achieve a K/ s-like amplitude ra-
tio characteristic in the frequency region of closed-loop control. This is the region
of the crossover frequency (we), where the closed-loop 0 dB value occurs, based
on the driver's gain (K p ) . For example, if the vehicle dynamics are Ke/s, then no
lead or lag equalisation is needed, and the driver describing function is simply a
gain plus a time delay. This is illustrated in the Bode (frequency response) plot of
Fig. 17.2, which shows the amplitude and phase properties of YpYe for Ye = K e / S,
TL = TI = 0, and a time delay of 0.3 s. The amplitude ratio in Fig. 17.2 is seen
to represent a K/s transfer characteristic (a slope of -20 dB/decade). The phase
20
~
C 10
"(ij
C> odB (closedloop)
-10
-90r- _
-180
-270'-----_'----'----''___'__~....L..L__----'----'---.L...-..L-..I.-...............'''''---_'''---_'__'___'_...............~
10.1
Frequency (radlsec)
An inner feedback loop of heading angle (1/1). This loop equalises the outer loop,
and makes it easy for the driver to control lane position. It also provides path
damping.
An outer feedback loop of lateral lane position (y). This provides for lateral
deviation control and causes the driver/vehicle system to follow the desired path
input (Yc).
A random appearing yaw rate disturbance input (rd) that can be used to help
define the driving task and as a basis for identifying the driver describing function
components.
17. Review of Control Theory Models for Directional and Speed Control 297
Yaw
Disturbance
rd
!DRivER------------- ---- --- ---------[ :
Roadwaypath : Yc Ye :0: Actual
~~ :: roadway path
'-------'
~ ___ _ ~ ~ J
Headingangle
Lane position
An alternative and nearly equivalent inner loop candidate is path angle, which
can be used if that is preferred. Path angle involves perceiving the total velocity
vector or focus of expansion relative to the surround, whereas heading angle relates
a position vector (line) in the vehicle to a geometric reference in the surround (e.g. a
lane centreline or edge line). Other possible feedbacks such as sideslip and lateral
acceleration are less good from a perceptual standpoint and less good in terms
of desirable manual control qualities (path following, path damping, etc.). These
feedbacks may playa supplementary role in some manoeuvres, such as near limit
turns, which the model in Fig. 17.3 does not address.
Referring to Fig. 17.3, the dynamic model of the vehicle (YI) is known or can be
measured. The driver describing functions Yy and Y1/1 can be predicted or estimated
using a combination of the crossover model, control principles and empirical results
from driving simulator or instrumented vehicle studies. The describing functions
can also be measured using a driving simulator or test vehicle, and correlation
or spectral analysis methods. The main driver model parameters are gains and
a time delay (r). Equalisation (lead or lag, anticipation or smoothing) can also
be included, but this refinement is often not needed for contemporary passenger
vehicles.
The specific forms of the driver describing function Y1/1 and Yy can be estimated
for an example typical passenger car by successive application of the crossover
model. Consider the (inner) heading angle loop. The vehicle dynamics can be given
by the transfer function relating yaw rate to steer angle which can be approximated
by
yr _ Kr Uo/(a+b)
(2)
8 - (T; + 1) (T, + 1)
Here, T, is the vehicle's yaw rate time constant, which typically has a value of
about 0.1 s for modern passenger cars at a typical highway speed. U0 is the speed
and a + b is the wheelbase. So, in the region of crossover for the inner loop in
Fig. 17.3 (about 1 to 2 rad/s), YI is approximately a gain and y81/1 is approximately
K/s (heading angle is the integral of yaw rate). Therefore, applying the crossover
model, it follows that the inner loop driver model in this case is
(3)
298 Weir and Chao
1/! 1 r
Yo = -Yo (4)
s
Choosing a typical value of time delay, t = 0.3 s, and a driver gain (K1/!) of about
0.25 front wheel angle/" heading error, gives an estimated crossover frequency
of about 2 rad/s and a phase margin of about 40 for good closed-loop damping.
The resulting open outer loop driver/vehicle transfer function is approximately
Y Yy u, (5)
Ye (~s + 1) (T,s + 1) s
~ _ y 80
(6)
Ye - Ys(.5s+1)(.ls+1)
This open outer-loop driver/vehicle system has considerable low to mid frequency
lag. Applying the crossover model, it is apparent that a considerable amount of
low frequency driver lead in responding to the lateral position error would be
needed or else the driver would use a relatively low outer loop gain (K y) in order
to achieve an outer loop closure in accordance with the model. Data from simulator
and actual vehicle experiments show that drivers do the latter, i.e. use a relatively
low outer loop gain and rely on the inner loop function to provide the needed
lead equalisation or anticipation. Therefore, the outer loop describing function is
simply a gain, that is
(7)
An example value of K y for the assumed vehicle is about 0.2 heading com-
mand/ft of path error (1 rad/300 ft), or an outer loop crossover frequency KyVo
of about 0.27 rad/s. If the vehicle dynamic properties are other than these typical
passenger car examples, driver lead or lag equalisation can be used in the inner
loop to obtain the desired characteristics according to the crossover model.
where the terms are defined above and in Fig. 17.3. This expression was originally
published in (McRuer et al., 1975).
17. Review of Control Theory Models for Directional and Speed Control 299
YljrYI ( )
G == - 2 - YyUo +s (10)
s
and for s or W small
(11)
The outer loop driver gain (K y) defines a low frequency break point in the
amplitude ratio. This also results in an increasing phase lag at low frequency.
The crossover frequency (we) defines the closed-loop 0 dB line, and this becomes
the closed-loop bandwidth of the heading control loop. The crossover frequency
is equivalent to KljrK8.
The time delay term (e- r s ) results in a progressively increasing phase lag.
The vehicle yaw time constant (Tr ) appears as an additional high frequency lag
term.
These features of this example model application are shown subsequently in the
data for actual drivers.
The driver model for speed control is similar to the model for directional control,
above, but simpler. The model of interest is for speed regulation, where the driver
uses the accelerator pedal to maintain a specified constant speed in the presence of a
speed disturbance. This model assumes that the driver perceives and operates only
on forward velocity. A possible multiple loop alternative could include an inner
loop of longitudinal acceleration, for situations where that cue can be usefully
300 Weir and Chao
Driver:
K =0.25
K;= 11300
40
30
20 20 dB/dec
~
'U
C 10
xII
'ii
(9
0
-10
-20 1
10. 10
<. KlJIK&
-90
-180
a
-8
rn
tn
if -270
-360 '---_'---...a...-'--'-....I.....lo-.........._--'---'---'-....&.--I.~L....l...___
_ _ L . _ _ _ ' _ _ . . . . L _ _ I........................
10.
' Freq(radlsec)
FIGURE 17.5. Open-loop combined driver/vehicle system bode plot for steering control.
perceived. The additional model for speed control of braking is beyond the scope
of this paper.
The speed control model is shown in Fig. 17.6. It has only a single loop with one
driver describing function element (Y u ) . The speed disturbance (Ud ) helps define
the driving task, and can be used to experimentally identify describing function
parameters. The dynamic model of the vehicle (YOU) is assumed to be known. The
driver describing function can be estimated as discussed below.
The first step in applying the crossover model is to examine the controlled
element (vehicle) transfer function. The transfer function relating forward speed
17. Review of Control Theory Models for Directional and Speed Control 301
Speed Disturbance
Ud
r-------------------------
DRIVER :r------------------i----
VEHICLE
+
[IJU
I
8r : 10\ U
Desired r-r-r-r--:....... ~ 8 \(Y-"'-----. Actual
Speed :
I
+ Speed
I
I
I
I
I
~------------------------
yu _ Kf (13)
8 - (Tus + 1)'
where T; is the time constant associated with a change in vehicle speed.
This linear representation assumes (for simplicity) that positive and negative
changes in speed result from corresponding equivalent changes in pedal position.
Representative values in Eq. (13) for a passenger car at 50 mph are
K; = 1 ft/s per % pedal travel (14)
't; = lOs. (15)
So, Eq. (13) for this example is
u 1
(16)
Y8 = (lOs + 1)
Applying the crossover model and related manual control principles, a correspond-
ing form of the driver model is
1
Yu = x, (~ + TL ) e
-is
(TLs + 1) -is
= K; e. (17)
s
This involves proportional plus integral (trim) response, which reflects the lack of
very low frequency gain in the open-loop vehicle characteristic in Eq. (16) (it is not
K/s-like at very low frequency). A representative set of driver model parameters
for this speed control task is
K; = 0.3 % pedal travel/ft/s
TL = 12 s
i = 1.7 s
The drivers lead TL approximately cancels the vehicle lag Tu . The result is a rela-
tively low bandwidth driver/vehicle system for speed control, with a relatively
302 Weir and Chao
C -10
"(ij
~
-20
-30
-40
10-2
C>
(1)
"C
-90
CD
en
ca
..c:
CL
-180
-270
10-2
Frequency (rad/sec)
large time delay and a crossover frequency of about 0.3 rad/s, as shown in
Fig. 17.7.
Interestingly, the earlier literature and data involving the crossover model do
not address the low frequency integration form of Eq. (17). Therefore, the values
shown above are taken from more recent driving simulator studies involving this
particular control task, the large time delay in particular. More data examples are
given subsequently.
DRI software . Motion cues are provided by a large hexapod motion system, with
a secondary 4DF seat motion system to provide road vibration and other ride cues
(see Fig. 17.8).
A driving simulator is useful for driver behaviour studies because it is easy to
study a range of conditions, the conditions are well defined and repeatable, it is
easy to change tasks and parameters, experimental measures are easy to obtain, and
a simulator is efficient and has low risk compared to studies using actual vehicles.
the 20 runs are in the bottom row. The data were analysed using the equivalent
single-loop method described in Fig. 17.5. The first data column is the crossover
frequency (we). The second column (WI80) is the frequency where the open-loop
driver/vehicle phase angle is -180. The next column is the phase margin (<Pm),
which is a measure of closed-loop stability and damping. Combining We and
<Pm gives an estimate of the driver time delay (r) and this is shown in the next
column. Note that the driver time delay shown is the result of subtracting the
0.1 s yaw time constant, and an additional 0.07 s simulator graphics delay,
from the total measured driver/vehicle system delay. The final column is the
linear coherence (r02), which is the ratio of the driver's control action which is
linearly correlated with the disturbance input to the driver's total steering control
activity. The parameter values in the table illustrate both the within-subject and
between-subject variability for typical driver subjects.
The previous literature on the crossover model, and its occasional application
to driving, typically show values that differ somewhat from those in Table 17.2. In
particular, the example data for mature (age 44 to 57) typical drivers in Table 17.2
show somewhat lower crossover frequencies (gains) and larger time delays. In ad-
dition to possible age effects, these differences may reflect the effect of such things
as multiple tasking (speed regulation at the same time), use of a steering wheel
(increasing the arm-hand closed-loop neuromuscular delay), and lower levels of
skill (compared to pilot subjects, for example). Note that the main difference is a
difference in the time delay (r). To the extent that the subjects use the same phase
margin, the crossover frequency is a consequence of the time delay, as expressed in
the crossover model. Note also that Subject 1, who is an engineer and a pilot, had
larger We and lower t . These contemporary data and others like it, reflecting more
mature drivers and typical driving tasks, may be pertinent to future applications of
the driver models in addition to the values used in the past.
Subject 1 are in Table 17.1. Subject 11 was a 33-year-old female business admin-
istrator. The task was similar to that described above for the steering control data.
A random appearing longitudinal disturbance consisting of a sum of sinewaves
was used to define the task and facilitate the describing function measures. The
describing function calculations were made over an interval of 48 s imbedded in a
2-min run.
The columns in Table 17.3 are similar to those of Table 17.2. Analysed in
terms of the crossover model, and compared to typical results for other tasks in the
manual control literature, the example results for speed control show relatively low
crossover frequencies, large time delays, and large stability margins, for reasons
previously noted. Note that data for Subject 1 appear in both Tables 17.2 and
17.3, which provides an interesting direct comparison of control activity in the
two simultaneous control tasks. Although more data would be useful, the data of
Table 17.3, representing typical drivers in this task, may provide guidance in future
applications of the model.
a = 3.5 ft (1.1 m)
b = 5.5 ft (1.7 m)
m = 185 slugs (2700 kg)
I = 4000 slug/ft" (5400 kgm/)
YI = 4000 lb/rad (18 kN/rad)
Y2 = 5000 lb/rad (22.4 kN/rad)
A simple set of 2 of freedom lateral-directional equations for the vehicle is given
by
s - Yv
(18)
[ -Nv
where v is the lateral velocity, r is the yaw rate, 8 is the steer angle, V 0 is the
forward velocity, s is the Laplace transfer variable and the coefficients are
-2
Yv =- (YI + Y2 )
u,
2
Yr =- (bY2 - aYt)
u;
2
N; =- (bY2 - aYI )
IVo
NT = -2 (a 2 Y1 + b2 y 2)
IVo
2
Y8 = -Y1
m
2a
N8 = -YI.
I
Assume a speed of 120 ft/s (130 kmlh). The result is a relatively poor handling
example vehicle, with the assumed excessive load and inadequate tires, but it can
serve to illustrate the analytical use of the driver model. Entering the numerical
parameters listed into the equations above gives the following vehicle transfer
function for heading control (from Eq. 4):
1/1 7 (s + 1.16)
(19)
Y8 = S [s2 + 2 (.3)(2.7) s + (2.7)2]'
The damping ratio is 0.3 and the undamped natural frequency is 2.7 rad/s in
the characteristic equation, represented by the complex conjugate pair. While the
Eq. (2) vehicle approximation for yaw rate was a simple high frequency lag, the
slightly more complete form of Eq. (19) replaces that with a lead-double lag
combination. The equivalent yaw time constant, Ti., and phase lag contribution for
frequencies less than 2.7, would now be about 0.22 s, instead of the 0.1 s example
used with Eq. (2). The driver/vehicle open-loop response properties (see Fig. 17.3)
for this example become
(20)
308 Weir and Chao
20 .------.--...---.--~.........____r___,___,~....,...,._r_-.....___T'""____.__._......_r_r_r_l
FIGURE 17.9. Example
driver/vehicle system bode
10
plot for heading control.
:c
"0
0
~ -10
CJ -20
-30
-40 '------'----'--'---'--'"~O.__"______'__~~O.__"______'__..............................
10-2
0)
Q)
-90
~
"0
Q) --------------------"""\\ r = O
CJ)
ct1
..c:
Q. -180
\
\ 't= 05
.
-270 L-.._"______'__.&......J.-,.i~O'__"______'__iL_...L......l ~
..............." ' _ _ _ _ _ _ ' _ _ _ _ ' _ _...............
10-2
Frequency (rad/sec)
where Y81/1 is given by Eq. (19). The corresponding frequency response plot for
Eq. (20) is shown in Fig. 17.9, in the manner of Fig. 17.2. A driver time delay
of 0.5 s is used in this example, and its effect is shown by the lower phase angle
curve.
The next step is to estimate the driver gain K 1/1. Fig. 17.9 shows that the driver gain
is limited by the lightly damped second order made at 2.7 rad/s. If the driver's gain
is increased so that the closed-loop OdB line intersects the peak in the amplitude
ratio plot at to == 2.7 rad/s, the closed-loop driver/vehicle system becomes unstable.
Therefore, a lower driver gain has to be used resulting in relatively low values of
closed-loop bandwidth and directional stability. This is illustrated, alternatively,
in the corresponding root locus plot of Fig. 17.10. The branch of the locus from
the quadratic pole moves into the right half plane and becomes neutrally stable
or unstable for a relatively low level of driver gain K 1/1. This value of gain would
cause the vehicle to move back and forth in the lane. Figs. 17.9 and 17.10 both
show that this oscillatory behaviour for this hypothetical vehicle configuration is
due to the driver's time delay. The resulting phase lag causes the closed-loop poles
to move towards a region of instability as the driver's gain is increased (as the
driver tries harder and makes larger control inputs). This type of system response
has sometimes been characterised as a 'driver induced oscillation'. Note that there
is little point in analysing the outer path loop, since adding that feedback would
17. Review of Control Theory Models for Directional and Speed Control 309
4 r= 0
Cf)
x
3
1.. ,.,..-.-..-"
t > 0.5
0> 2
ctl
E
o
-1
-2
-6 -4 -2 o 2 4 6
RealAxis
have little effect on the vehicle's behaviour and lane-keeping performance in this
example.
A more detailed analysis would show that lag equalisation might improve the
closed-loop bandwidth in Fig. 17.9 a small amount. But, for the purposes of this
example, it assumed that the driver does not provide such smoothing. For this hypo-
thetical poor vehicle configuration, corrections of heading errors by the modelled
driver can only be done using small steering inputs. Aggressive or large amplitude
steer corrections would tend to result in oscillatory directional behaviour, as noted
above. In other words, the quasilinear model says that if the driver tries harder it
only worsens the stability, in this case. The driver could perhaps adapt a nonlinear
control behaviour, wherein the frequency of the oscillatory response of the vehicle
is recognised and the driver makes a properly timed and predicted discrete steering
correction to reduce the oscillation, in effect reducing or eliminating the effect of
his or her time delay. Another possibility is to hold the steering wheel approxi-
mately fixed and let the directional oscillations damp out if the vehicle alone has
positive path damping.
It should be emphasised that this hypothetical high speed vehicle example is
only to illustrate a possible use of the model. If the vehicle were to slow down
the damping would improve. In addition, or alternatively, the example vehicle
configuration could be readily changed to eliminate the undesirable effect shown,
by reducing the mass and inertia (the load), or by increasing the tire cornering
stiffness characteristics (more suitable, higher performance tires).
This example shows how driver/vehicle response properties can be predicted
analytically. This type of comparatively simple and robust model and analysis
can assist the understanding of a problem or issue. Such analysis also shows the
contribution of the several driver and vehicle parameters to the resulting response,
and it suggests how sensitivity analyses can be performed to improve or optimise
310 Weir and Chao
driver/vehicle response properties. For instance, one could analyse the effect of
varying the driver time delay in this example.
17.7 Discussion
Models for driver directional and speed control have been presented, and their
application has been illustrated. These are based on models in the classic literature
and manual control principles. Data have been presented for typical values of the
model parameters and to show how these parameters vary depending on the task,
the experimental venue and across driver subjects.
The quasilinear models described herein are best suited to regulation tasks where
the driver is compensating and minimising errors on straight and gradually curving
roads at an approximately constant speed, and these conditions represent a large
percentage of driving. Other models that are available for other driving tasks
such as rapid manoeuvres, obstacle avoidance, near limit turning and/or braking
manoeuvres, etc., are beyond the scope of this paper.
Driver models of the sort described herein have a wide variety of applications.
For example, they can be used to characterise the driver element in R&D-related
studies of vehicles, of devices-in-the-vehicle, or vehicles augmented by devices.
The models can be used to interpret driver behaviour or driver/vehicle response
and performance data. They can be used to represent the driver or driver/vehicle
system in simulations involving one vehicle or multiple vehicles. Perhaps most
usefully, they serve to quantify driver behaviour and driver/vehicle response and
performance in engineering terms, and in a way that both predicts what the driver
will do and quantifies data resulting from what the driver has done.
References
Allen, R.W., Rosenthal, TJ. and Hogue, lR. (1996). Modelling and Simulation of
Driver/Vehicle Interaction. SAE Paper 960177.
Brackstone, M. and McDonald, M. (1999). Car following: A historical overview. Trans-
portation Research Part F, 2, 181-196.
Brown, T.L., Lee, J.D. and McGehee, D.V.(2001). Human performance models and rear-end
collision avoidance algorithms. Human Factors, 43, 462-482.
Donges, E. (1978). A two-level model of driver steering behaviour. Human Factors, 20,
691-707.
Fancher, P.S. and Baraket, Z. (1998). Evolving model for studying driver-vehicle system
performance in longitudinal control of headway. Transportation Research Record No.
1631 (pp. 13-19).
Godthelp, J. (1985). Precognitive control: Open- and closed-loop steering in a lane change
maneuver. Ergonomics, 28,1419-1438.
Hoffman, E.R. (1975). Manual control of road vehicles. Vehicle System Dynamics, 5, 1-2.
Levison, W. and Cramer, N. (1995). Description of the integrated driver model. Report
FHWA-RD-94-092. FHWA, US DOT.
17. Review of Control Theory Models for Directional and Speed Control 311
MacAdam, C.C. (2003). Understanding and modelling the human driver. Vehicle System
Dynamics, 40, 101-134.
McRuer, D.T. and Weir, D.H. (1969). Theory of manual vehicular control. IEEE Transac-
tions, MMS-I0(4). (Also Ergonomics, 12 (5), 1969.)
McRuer, D.T. and Krendel, E.S. (1974). Mathematical models of human pilot behaviour.
AGARDograph No. 188. NATO Advisory Group for Aerospace Research and Develop-
ment.
McRuer, D.T., Weir, D.H., lex, H.R., Magdaleno, R.E. and Allen, R.W. (1975). Measure-
ment of driver/vehicle multiloop response properties with a single disturbance input.
IEEE Transactions, SMC-5(5).
McRuer, D.T., Allen, R.W., Weir, D.H. and Klein, R.H. (1977) . New results in driver steering
control models. Human Factors, 19(4),381-397.
Sheridan, T.B. and Ferrell, W.R. (1981). Man-Machine Systems, Information, Control, and
Decision Models ofHuman Performance. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Stapleford, R.L., Peters, R.A. and Alex, F.R. (1969). Experiments and a model for pilot
dynamics with visual and motion inputs. NASA CR-1325.
Weir, D.H. and McRuer, D.T. (1968). A theory for driver steering control of motor vehicles.
Highway Research Record, 247, 7-39.
Weir, D.H. and McRuer, D.T. (1973). Measurement and interpretation of driver/vehicle
system dynamic response. Human Factors, 15(4),367-378.
Weir, D.H. (1973). Driver/vehicle response and performance in the presence of aerodynamic
disturbances from large commercial vehicles. In First International Conference on Driver
Behaviour, Zurich, Switzerland.
Weir, D.H. and DiMarco, R.I. (1978). Correlation and evaluation of driver/vehicle direc-
tional handling data. SAE Paper 780010.
Weir, D.H., Klein, R.H. and Zellner, J.W. (1982). Crosswind response and stability of car
plus utility trailer combinations. SAE Paper 820137.
VII
Simulation of Driver Behaviour
18
Cognitive Modelling and
Computational Simulation of
Drivers Mental Activities
THIERRY BELLET, BEATRICE BAILLY, PIERRE MAYENOBE AND OLIVIER
GEORGEON
Several car driver models are available in the literature. From an historical point
of view, it is possible to identify three main phases, for the last 40 years, in this
research area. The works of Me Knight and Adams (1970), centred on task anal-
ysis, are typically representative of the studies carried out during the 1970s. The
authors proposed a taxonomy of the main driving tasks (e.g. accelerating, steering,
overtaking, lane changing) organised in nine categories (e.g. basic control tasks,
tasks related to traffic condition). This work closely parallels the research of Allen
et al. (1970), who divided the driving task in three levels: the microperformance,
the situational performance and the macroperformance. These levels differ both
according to their time scale and with regard to the kind of cognitive activity re-
quired. At the microperformance level, most of the actions are automated skills.
Steering and speed control are the main subtasks. Feedback loops, concerning
driving action implemented at this level, are very short (on the order of seconds).
The macroperformance concerns the trip planning and the route finding during the
trip. It corresponds to slow conscious processes requiring cognitive resources. The
time scale can be hours at this level. Between these two levels, situational perfor-
mance corresponds to the analysis of the road environment and to the selection of
relevant behaviour in the current situation and traffic conditions. Performance at
this level is determined by the driver's perception and understanding of the driving
context.
These types of studies are descriptive in nature and aim to propose an exhaustive
taxonomy of the different kinds of driving subtasks. This approach provides an
interesting framework to analyse driving behaviours, but is poor in terms of men-
tal activities study. It provides models of the driving task more than model of the
drivers. During the 1980s, the main paradigm was the human information process-
ing approach. Several models were developed in order to explain accidents. Most
of them are focused on the notion of risk: the risk threshold model of Naatanen
and Summala (1976), Wilde's risk homeostatis model (1982) and Fuller's threat
315
316 Bellet et al.
system. Only the matching of the model's prediction (predicted behaviour) and
the facts observed in the real world (effective behaviour) is considered. This kind
of model is more appropriate in the framework of a behavioural approach. On the
other hand, cognitive simulation models are not just focused on the probability
of occurrence between a situational context (the characteristics of a traffic situa-
tion) and driving behaviour. The way the processes are implemented leads to an
homomorphism with the human cognitive processes. This approach is heavier in
terms of human modelling, but allows a better understanding of the cause-effect
relationships between behaviour and the driving conditions.
The purpose of this paper is not to judge these two approaches in terms of
correctness. Both of them are relevant, but can differ in their goals. Performance
models are more appropriate if the aim is only to predict behaviour. In our case,
the goal is to evaluate theoretical approach and experimental results concerning
cognitive processing in the driving context, with the aim to provide a deep under-
standing of driver's cognition. From this point of view, the explicative modelling
approach was needed.
t it
l j"fex2tl
v ehi cle? ~ - 1~ III ~ ' D
\- -"' [ 'P i ' ~
Ii {J
ex? : rr-rr--:-:
ped estr ianf ,--~i I
:--; ex
ZJb2
=
=
c=J ~
=
=
= LI
--,
-~
- - -
-'
=
=
=
= -
:
ex21 color of the
1l traffic lights ?
L. _ ._jJ
c::J Initial State ex.!i : ped estri an ?
~ Local States
_ Final State
the generic driving frame (i.e. driving knowledge) of an urban crossroad with the
goal 'turn on the left' (this frame must be instantiated with event occurrences to
provide a mental model of a particular driving situation). A frame is defined by a
global initial state, a global final state, a set of perceptive exploration zones and
a driving path . A state corresponds to the vehicle position and speed at a given
time . The global initial state corresponds to the initial position and speed of the
car when the driver approaches the intersection. The global final state corresponds
to the final position of the driver when he or she leaves the junction. Reaching
this last position constitutes the global goal of the driver in this crossroad. The
driving path is composed of a sequence of driving zones. It allows the driver to
pass from the initial to the final state . Each driving zone is described by three
elements: a local initial state, and a set of localfinal states and a set of actions. An
action allows the driver to pass from the local initial state to one of the local final
states. In concrete terms , if we consider the Fig. 18.2, the road infrastructure is
described as a set of two kinds of zones : the driving zones (Zi) and the perceptive
exploration zones (Exi). The sequences of driving zones constitute the driving
paths. In this particular case, two driving paths are possible: Z I, Z2, Z3a, Z4 and
Z I, Z2, Z3b 1, Z3b2, Z4. The perceptive exploration zones correspond to the part
322 Belletet al.
of the environment that the driver observes in order to detect events. An event
corresponds to an object occurrence in a specific zone (e.g. a vehicle in ex3) or
to identify a particular characteristic of this object (e.g. the colour of the traffic
lights in ex l ), In order to tum left in this infrastructure, the driver realises several
consecutive steps: at the beginning of ZI (global initial state), he observes the
colour of the traffic lights . If the traffic lights are green , he then proceeds to Z2
with the same speed . If not, he stops the car at the end of Zl. In Z2, the driver
checks for vehicle presence in the opposite lanes near the crossroads (ex3, ex4 or
ex5 zones), and the presence of any high- speed vehicle in ex6 zone . If there is no
vehicle, the driver goes to Z3a. Else, he stops the car at the end of Z3b 1, and waits
until there is no vehicle in the ex4 and ex5 zones . In Z3a or Z3b2, the driver looks
for any pedestrian in the ex7 zone. If a pedestrian is present, he stops at the end of
Z3a or Z3b2 while the pedestrian crosses the street. And then, he continues his path
in Z4, towards the global final state of the frame. From a computational point of
view (Bellet and Tattegrain-Veste, 1999,2003), driving frames has been modelled
in COSMODRIVE with the object-oriented modelling technique of Rumbaugh
et al. (1991) and have been implemented in Small Talk language. In a more recent
work (Mayenobe et aI., 2002, 2003 ; Mayenobe, 2004) , a three-dimensional (3-D)
version of driving frames has been also developed, allowing dynamic simulation
of drivers' mental models (Fig. 18.3).
In terms of human cognition, these 3-D models correspond to visuo-spatial rep-
resentations of the road scene , mentally used by the driver for decision taking and
action planning at the tactical level. Content of these mental models depends on
several things, like the current driver's goal, driving experience, attention sharing
problem, age and so on. Experimental results will be presented in the next part
of this chapter to illustrate the potential impact of a secondary task or driving
18. Cognitive Modelling and Computational Simulation 323
experience effect on driver's situation awareness. In any case, these 3-D models
provide simulation of the driver's understanding of the road environment, including
infrastructure and events perceived from the pilot point of view. They also integrate
relative zones around the driver 's own car (Fig. 18.4). These relative zones corre-
spond to the part of the space that drivers must to keep around their car to avoid
collision or to comfortably interact with other road user. From a theoretical point
of view, the relative zone approach is partly based on Hall's concept of proximity
bubbles (Hall, 1966), which concerns social interactions between human s. In driv-
ing context, a same idea has been also proposed with the concept of safety margin
(Gibson and Crooks , 1938). Nevertheless, COSMODRIVE three-relative-zones
model is more particularly based on Kontaratos' work (Kontaratos, 1974) and
distinguishes the danger, the threat and the safety zones . These zones are called
'relative' because they are dependent on the car speed and position. They corre-
spond to the portion of space to be occupied by the future trajectory of the vehicle.
The lengths of these "relative zones" depend of the car speed, and correspond to
the distance covered by the vehicle during this period of time. According to Otha's
results (1993), the reserved values of the coefficients are, respectively, 0.6, 1.1 and
1.7 s for the zones of danger, threat and safety. However, inter-vehicular distance
regulation may vary according to the context, the driving strategies and the traffic
density .
Moreover, 3-D mental models also integrate a set ofremarkable points (i.e.
restricted set of points corresponding to specific landmarks, like comers of the
roads or intersection centre; cf. see Fig. 18.3) needed to geometrically match the
mental model and the road infrastructure (i.e. objective reality as perceived by
the driver). Once a driving frame is matched with reality (Fig. 18.5), the driver
324 Belletet al.
can mentally use the driving path of the frame to mentally simulate trajectories
of vehicles , according to its own trajectory. This mental simulation mechanism
provides anticipations, which correspond to drivers ' expectations.
Lastly, at a lower level (which corresponds to the operational and the execution
modules of COSMODRIVE) driving frames are implemented through the method
of pure-pursuit point developed for automatic car driving (Amidi, 1990) and used
by Sukthankar (1997) for drivers' situation awareness simulation (Fig. 18.6). A
pure-pursuit point is defined to be the intersection of the desired vehicle trajectory
and a circle of radius (I), centred at the vehicle's rear axle midpoint (assuming front
wheel steer) . Intuitively, this point describes the steering curvature that would bring
the vehicle to the desired lateral offset after travelling a distance of approximately 1.
Thus the position of the pure-pursuit point maps directly onto a recommended
steering curvature: k = -2xll , where k is the curvature (reciprocal of steering
radius), x is the relative lateral offset to the pure-pursuit point in vehicle coordinates
and I is a parameter known as the look-ahead distance.
!
-_._---_._-;-_.._.__.__._-
iI relative lateral
offset (x)
_.._r
f pure-pursuit point(P) i
f i
; desired trajectory
i
!
Isteeringradius(r)
f
i
representation (CTR). The goals list contains the 'local' goals to be attained in the
current infrastructure (e.g. turn left) and 'global' (or latent) goals to be reached
in the more or less long term (e.g. save time, because I am late). These goals
essentially come from the strategic module. The facts list contains all information
extracted from the road scene. This data comes from the perception module. The
CTR corresponds to the driver's mental model of the driving situation. It is built
from a generic driving frame activated in LTM, instantiated with data available
in the facts list and in the goals list. In contrast to the facts list, which only lists
events individually, the CTR specifies the spatial and dynamic relations existing
between these objects in the road environment. It provides a visuo-spatial mental
model of the road scene. Based on operative knowledge (i.e. driving frame), CTR
provides a functional, deformed, simplified and finalised representation of the
reality. On the one hand, only relevant pieces of information for the driver at
moment t, according to the pursued goal, are considered. On the other hand, some
data contained in the CTR could be not directly available in the environment at
moment t (e.g. data produced by inference, memorised previously, default values
of the driving frame). Once generated, the CTR provides a plan of the driving
activity. It orients new perceptive information collection and processing as well as
the decision making.
The AB holds ARs derived from the CTR by the anticipation agent. Each AR
constitutes a possible evolution of the current driving situation. ARs are organised
in a tree, whose root corresponds to the CTR, nodes correspond to the potential
future state of this situation (AR) and links between AR correspond to the actions
to be carried out and/or the conditions to be complied with to go from one AR to the
next one. Each AR is associated with two parameters indicating the potential risk
and the time saving to pass from the previous AR to this new one. In some cases,
these parameters come from default values (e.g. starting to cross the junction when
the lights are amber presents systematically more risk than stopping, but allow the
driver to save time). But generally, they depend on the current driving conditions
(e.g. according to visibility, the configuration of the crossroads and the traffic
conditions, the risk will be more or less important). Anticipation agent is in charge
to assess the value of these parameters for each new temporal derivation (i.e. AR
generation). This assessment is partly based on the relative zone conflicts between
the own driver's car, on the one hand, and other road users, on the other (see
Fig. 18.4). Furthermore, as the elaboration of the AR-tree progresses, anticipation
computes the overall value of these two parameters for the whole branch that
integrate the new derived AR (the whole sequence of AR starting from the RTC-
root). These 'local' (from one AR to another one) or 'global' values (for the whole
branch) will be then used by the decision agent to select the best action (or sequence
of elementary actions) in the current context.
major types of driving knowledge: declarative knowledge describing the road con-
text and operative schemas including procedural knowledge (i.e. practical know-
how) required to drive the car in the road environment. Regarding COSMODRIVE,
three specific KB are implemented at the tactical level: the road environment cat-
egories KB, the familiar place KB and the driving frame KB. These different
knowledge bases are not independent from each other. Strongly interconnected,
they constitute a global network handled by two cognitive agents specialised in
information retrieval: categorisation and place recognition. The road environment
categories KB is a typology of the different types of driving environment likely to
be encountered by the driver. This KB is hierarchically structured (Bellet, 1998).
At the top of the hierarchy can be found very generic categories corresponding to
classes of driving contexts (urban, country, highway). On descending the hierarchy,
the knowledge becomes more and more specialised (e.g. urban junction, access
lane), until reaching precise categorisations of the driving context (e.g. 'crossroads
with traffic lights in city centre'). The familiar places KB holds specific knowl-
edge of the road sites familiar to the driver. This KB is a direct extension of the
environment categories base: each familiar place constitutes a specific instance of
one of the classes represented by the road categories. Lastly, the driving frame KB
holds the driver's operative knowledge, as described earlier in this paper. Once
activated and instantiated with reality (matching with objective characteristics of
the infrastructure and integration of dynamic events), the driving frame becomes
the CTR, and this mental model will be used for driving the car.
18.2.2.5.1 Categorisation
The main function of this process is to provide a more or less precise categorisation
of the road environment into which the driver is going. Furthermore, this agent
simulates the retrieval processing of a relevant driving frame for car driving. To do
this, categorisation attempts to match, on the one hand, the information contained
in the hierarchy of road environment categories KB and, on the other, the data
available in the fact list of the CSB. If the facts list does not contain any of the
discriminatory criteria permitting the selection of a category, categorisation sends a
queries to the perceptive exploration agent (perception module) in order to extract
new data from the road scene. As soon as a category of road environments is
identified, categorisation considers the list of goals to be attained, then it activates
one of the frames (permitting to reach this goal) linked with this category (driving
frame KB). The chosen frame is then transferred in the CSB, in place of the
previous CTR.
, - - - - - -Goa ls L 1ST
Global Goal : saY. a_
Local Goal (current) : TurnLft (lIJ
FtIcls L 1ST
fact(obj eet. p osition . dynamic . chara cteristics)
foct(tnffic lights. tollC _ <>< 1. type_, tatic. color : 7)
foct(objeet: W. position: X. dynamic: Y. chlU1lCt : Z)
I
CURRENT T ACl7CAJ. R EPR~NTmON (CTR)
1) Frame : ~ban Cf'0661'O(Jds ""tir tniffi c lights: 1I."
2) Validity limit s : - facl( traflic li.ghts. static. exl . _)
- facl( W. X. Y. Z)
3) Infrastruc OJre :
KNOWLEDGE
BASCS
exl
traffic lights Current Position : Final Position :
color ? entry of Z 1. speed = 40 exit ofZ l .
kmlb. not any vehicle ahead speed = 25 kmIh
CurrentPosition :
lWHoo---EnJry into Zl,
~ei=40~. ::::/'::::::
,:! om
. : :::::::;:
:::::::
i ioNAL M ODULE,:"'::::'t,,,
:::: .:::;.;: .
: ::: : ::: :: : : :::: : :: :.: :: : : : : : :: : : :::: : :: ~ : :. : .' .'
CVIlRENT S TAn: BUCKBOARD
action should be taken, (5) sending this action to the operational module for its
effective implementation and (6) checking that the chosen action provides the
expected effects. Figure 18.8 illustrates the general activity of TRG in the tum
left (TL) situation at an urban crossroads as observed on a real site. At moment
to, the driver enters the junction (initial state of Z 1, speed 40 km/h). During the
seconds preceding to, the categorisation agent has selected a generic drivingframe
'crossroads with traffic lights: turn-on-the-left', then it transfers this frame to the
CS Blackboard. From this time, TRG task is to instantiate this frame with reality.
At this level, TRG must match the generic infrastructure contained in the frame
with the real road environment. Dynamic objects present in the road scene are then
integrated in the frame. At the end of this instantiation procedure, COSMODRIVE
has a CTR corresponding to its own mental model of the world at this time to. If
nothing contradicts it, this CTR will guide the driver 's actions until the frame is
totally performed (exit of crossroads at Z4), according to some updating in order
(a) to integrate new facts (TRG constantly consults the facts list for this purpose)
and (b) take into account the progression of the vehicle's current position as it
moves. Furthermore, a TRG verification procedure is in charge to assess the CRT
validity. To carry this out, TRG verify if the changes observed in the facts list do
not contradict the frame's validity limits (e.g. what has initially been identified as
a traffic light is in fact a shop sign) . It will also ensure that the goal pursued can
330 Bellet et al.
actually be reached (e.g. diversion, prohibiting a left tum). If this occurs, TRG
sends a message to categorisation in order to select a new frame, more adequate
with the new characteristics of the situation.
Now, let us return to moment to, and examine in more detail the TRG com-
putations in the seconds that follow (Fig. 18.8). At this time, TRG has a mental
model of the environment represented by the frame 'tum left at crossroads with
traffic lights' instantiated with the real situation. Taking into account knowledge
stored in this frame (perceptive exploration zones linked with the entry into Z 1),
the first COSMODRIVE reasoning will concern the traffic lights colour in exl.
As the information is not available in the facts list, TRG generates a query to per-
ceptive exploration (writing in the blackboard associated with this process). The
query taken into account, perceptive exploration positions the glance in ex1. Once
the road scene has been explored, the information gathered is transmitted to TRG
(message-response indicating that the light is green). TRG can then instantiate the
frame (light colour ex 1 == green) and choose one of two final positions associated
with Z 1: arrive at the exit of Z 1 with a speed of 25 km/h if the light is green and
at 0 kmIh if it is red (amber light although has also been taken into account). TRG
is thus able to transmit part of the frame to the operational module (characteris-
tics of Z 1, current position, expected final position and the conditions required
to attain this goal, e.g. the light remains green). Then, the operational module
determines the elementary actions to be taken (e.g. on the peddles and steering
wheel) to change from 40 to 25 km/h along the ZI straight length. This procedure
only partially describes COSMODRIVE reasoning for decision making. In fact it
is also equipped with anticipatory capacities: TRG usually does not need to take
decision itself, but only validates the decisions previously taken by anticipation
and decision agents.
(risk judged too high, for example), decision will choose a 'wide first' strategy in
order to examine other ARs liable to offer a more appropriate solution.
these two sources of variation (driving experience, on one side, and secondary
task impact, on the other side). This research aims also to compare the accuracy
of the CTR, according to a combined effect of these two dimensions.
18.3.2 Methodology
A specific methodology has been developed in order to study these questions
(Bailly et al., 2003; Bailly, 2004). The experimental plan comprises 40 video
sequences of driving situations. Participants are invited to look at these video films
on a TV screen. At the end of each sequence, the video suddenly stops. Then, the
last picture of the video film appears on the TV and subjects have (a) to determine
if something has changed or not and (b) to indicate the nature and the location
of the detected modification(s). Modifications can occur on events (e.g. add or
suppress car) or on road infrastructure components (e.g. add or suppress road
signs or change traffic lights colours). Forty participants (20 novices and 20 EDs)
took part in this study. During the first phase of the experience, the subjects' task is
limited to looking at 20 video sequences (simple task condition). During the second
phase, video observation is done with an additional task (mental arithmetic; dual
task condition). At this level, the aim is to analyse the impact of a secondary task
on the tactical representation content and the attention sharing strategies required
through lack of cognitive resources. Note that the 40 video sequences have been
divided into two series. According to a turning experimental plan, each of the two
series has been equally used in the simple task (ST) and in the dual task (DT)
conditions.
100 , -- - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - ---,
90 + - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - l
~ 80 -t-- - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - ---\
.2 70 -t--== - - - - - - --j
ti
~ 60
c 50
1:
40
a:'0
Cl
30
<ft 20
10
o
global simpl e task dual task
FIGURE 18.9. Percentage of right detection according to the experimental conditions (ST
versus DT).
100
90
til
80
c:
.2 70
ti
Ql 60
Qi
c 50
1:
a:
Cl
40
'0 30
-:!!.
0
20
10
0
Infrastructure Events
FIG URE 18.11. Percentage of right detection accordingto the distance of modifications.
the drivers (respectively, 79.1 % versus 57.3 % for ED and 60.9 % versus 36.4 for
novice drivers ). On the other hand , no significant effect was found concerning
event modifications according to the ST versus DT condition (even if the perfor-
mance is globally better for ST condition). The result is the same for EDs (70%
in ST versus 66.3% in DT, but khi , = 0.594, p = 0.441 ) and for novice drivers
(55.8% in ST versus 50 % in DT, but khi- = 1.278, p = 0.258). In other words ,
event modification detection is clearl y more ' robust' (i.e. lesser effect of the DT
condition) than infrastructure components modification detection.
The last comparison presented herein concerns the distance of the modified
component. Four distances have been defined: 0 to 15 m (nearby zone), 15 to
25, 25 to 50, and beyond 50 m (jar zone). Performances for each distance are
compared according to the driver s' experience and to the secondary task effect (Fig.
18.11). In ST conditions, EDs obtain better results than novice s for modifications
concerning the nearby zone (81.3 % versus 60%) and also for modifications in the
15 to 25 m zone (87 .1% versu s 67.1%). Beyond 25 m, differences between ED
and novices are not significant. For the DT conditions, differences between experts
and novices are significant for three of the four distances con sidered. Performances
are, respectively, 67 .5% (expert) versus 51 .3% (novices) for the nearby zone (0
to 15 m), 67.1 % versus 42 .9% for the 15 to 25 m zone , and 70% versus 41.4%
for the 25 to 50 m zone. No significant effect is found for the far zone. If we now
con sider, for each population of drivers, the secondary task impa ct according to the
distance of modifications, the result s reveal some relevant differen ces. For EDs, a
DT significant effect occurs in the nearby zone (8 1.3% versus 67.5 %) and for the
15 to 25 m zone (87.1% versus 67. 1%). However, for novices drivers, DT impact
336 Bellet et al.
concerns the 15 to 25 m zone (67.1 % versus 42.9%) and the 25 to 50 m zone (64.3%
versus 41.4%). For all the drivers, no significant difference is found beyond 50 m,
probably because the performances were already weak in ST conditions. The other
main relevant 'non-significant' effects of the DT conditions concern specifically
(a) only novice drivers for the 0 to 15 m zone and (b) only EDs for the 25 to 50 m
zone.
and foremo st focused on the nearby environment of the vehicle. For EDs, the focal
point is between 25 and 50 m ahead the car (between 2 and 3.5 s).
FIGURE 18.12. Novice (view) versus experienced drivers (right) mental model in a ST
condition.
338 Bellet et al.
FIGURE 18.13. Novice (left) versus experienced (right) drivers' mental model in dual-task
condition .
is observe and known concerning the traffic flow in the intersection itself. On the
contrary, EDs focused as a priority the residual attention on the 25 to 50 m zone. If
some secondary events are potentially missing, experienced drivers have generally
integrated at this time the most significant events (i.e. car coming from the opposite
direction) in relation with their own "left-turn" goal.
Lastly, Fig. 18.14 illustrates the COSMODRIVE simulation interest in order
to dynamically visualise mental models evolution, during the whole approaching
phase of the crossroad. If we consider, for example, the previous novices' mental
model in DT condition, drivers' attention will gradually be focused on the area
ahead as they progress on the road and the traffic flow will be finally considered
(and partly integrated) a few seconds latter.
FIGURE 18.14. Example of simulation of a novices' mental model evolution during the
whole crossroad approaching phase (zone Z I of the driving frame) .
18. Cognitive Modelling and Computational Simulation 339
1 The MUSETTE theory provides a method for encoding traces of human behaviours.
The first level of trace, called rough trace, is made up of a succession of observables,
called objects of interest (01). The second level of trace, called primitive trace, is a suc-
cession of states and transitions embedding the Ols. Ols are of three kinds: 'entities'
(static facts) allowing description of states, 'events' allowing description of transitions
and 'relations' from entities or events to each other. In Fig. 18.15, states are represented
by circles, transitions by rectangles, entities are labelled En, events are labelled Ev and
340 Bellet et al.
(a) Normal
Zl Z2 Z3
how it is possible to connect a low-level trace of the driving activity (i.e. opera-
tional sub-schemas) to the upper descriptive level embodied by the tactical driving
frames. This work falls under the LESCOT's effort to better understand the car
driving activity. Within this new framework, the possibility of automatically as-
sess the driver's situation awareness from driving behaviours could have multiple
uses such as to get a better understanding of the accidents or to identify needs or
possibilities of assistance. In a long-term perspective, it could also be a first step
towards adaptive technologies based on a mental model sharing between, on one
side, the human driver and, on the other, driving aids (Bellet et aI., 2003).
Providing assistance that is adapted to the current driver's needs and to the spe-
cific characteristics of the driving situation of the moment is the aim of adaptive
technologies. The objective is to head towards the contextualisation of driving aids.
It is easy to imagine that driver's needs may be different depending on the type
relations are labelled R. The cutting into states and transitions is useful to identify episodes,
which consist of passing from one state to another through a succession of intermediate
states.
2 The creation of the primitive traces led us to widen a tactical driving frame to a finer
granularity level. This level is called operational sub-schema. This representation allows us
to describe tiny differences between situations. Figure 18.16shows how we can represent a
'surprised driver' sub-schema that is characterised by an initial 'Cruise' state that encroaches
on the approach zone (Zl ) of the infrastructure, followed by a 'strongly decelerate' state
characterised by a value of deceleration higher than in the normal sub-schema. From future
developments we want to implement search functionalities, which enable us to find patterns
in the primitive trace likely to match with a tactical driving frame, according to specified
criteria (Georgeon et aI., 2005) .
18. Cognitive Modelling and Computational Simulation 341
of the driving task (e.g. monotonous driving on motorways, going through roads
junctions in urban areas, overtaking), depending on the driving conditions (e.g.
traffic density, weather conditions), or depending on the driver's permanent (e.g.
age) or momentary capacities (e.g. cognitive resources available, state of stress,
fatigue). Contextualising aid involves developing technologies that are 'open to
the world', that is to say capable to assess - in real time - the driver's activities and
all the requirements dictated by driving conditions. This must all be done to (a)
determine if it is necessary or not to assist the driver in the present situation and (b)
identify the kind of aid required in this specific context and the form it should take
(giving information to the driver, warning him of a danger, or taking control of the
vehicle). We can also imagine using this kind of analysis capacity for diagnosing
human errors: Is the driver's behaviour appropriate according to the characteristics
of the driving situation? Alternatively, does the driver's behaviour present certain
risks not taken into account by human awareness? Several researches have been
done at LESCOT concerning adaptive technologies (see Bellet et al., 2003), with
the aim to manage on-board information or to design cooperative collision avoid-
ance system. Nevertheless the previous works were based on behavioural analysis
methods, but not on a cognitive simulation model. This new research will explore
this possibility by using some part of COSMODRIVE model. It could be a first step
to bring man-machine cooperation closer to man-man cooperation situations in
which two human agents (the driver and the co-driver) co-ordinate their efforts to
carry out the driving task together. In this context, each partner mentally imagines
the activities of the other partner (i.e. activities he does and those of which he is in
charge) and uses this internal model for interacting with him in an appropriate way.
In terms of man-machine cooperation, this means 'symetrising' the man-machine
relationship (Amalberti and Deblon, 1992) and developing technology capable to
represent itself the human partner activity (by using a more or less complex internal
model) like the human partner imagines (through a more or less true mental model)
the technological system in charge to assist him or her (i.e. what it is supposed to
do, what it really does and, if needs be, the way it goes about doing it).
References
Allen, T.M., Lunenfield, H. and Alexander, G.J. (1971). Driver information needs. Highway
Research Record, 366, 102-115.
Amalberti, R. and Deblon, F. (1992). Cognitive modelling of fighter aircraft's control pro-
cess: A step towards intelligent onboard assistance system. International Journal of
Man-Machine Studies, 36, 639-671.
Amidi, O. (1990). Integrated mobile robot control. Technical Report CMU-RI-TR-90-17.
Carnegie Mellon University Robotics Institute.
Bailly, B. (2004). Conscience de la situation des conducteurs: Aspects fondamentaux,
methodes, et applications pour la formation des conducteurs. PhD Thesis, Universite
Lyon 2.
Bailly, B., Bellet, T. and Goupil, C. (2003). Driver's mental representations: Experimental
study and training perspectives. In Proceedings of the 1st International Conference on
Driver Behaviour and Training, Stratford-upon-Avon, England. CD-ROM, 8 pp.
342 Bellet et al.
Mayenobe, P., Blanc, C., Trassoudaine, L., Bellet, T. and TattegrainVeste, H. (2003). Active
perception tasks driven by a cognitive simulation model. In Proceedings of the IEEE
Intelligent Vehicles Symposium: IV-2003, Columbus, OH (pp. 378-382).
Mayenobe, P., Trassoudaine, L., Bellet, T. and TattegrainVeste, H. (2002). Cognitive sim-
ulation of the driver and cooperative driving assistances. In Proceedings of the IEEE
Intelligent Vehicles Symposium: IV-2002, Versailles, June 17-21,2002 (pp. 265-271).
Mc Knight, A.J. and Adams, B.B. (1970). Driver Education Task Analysis. Vol. I: Task
Descriptions. Human Resources Research Organisation HumRRO, Alexandria.
Mourant, R.R. and Rockwell, T.H. (1972). Strategies of visual search by novices and expe-
riences drivers. Human Factors, 14(4),325-335.
Michon, 1.A. (1985). A critical view of driver behavior model: What do we know, what
should we do? In Evans and R.C. Schwing (Eds.). Human Behavior and Traffic Safety
(pp. 485-520). Plenum Press, New York.
Minsky, M. (1975). A framework for representing knowledge. In P. Winston (Ed.). The
Psychology of computer vision (pp. 211-281). McGraw-Hill, New York.
Naatanen, R. and Summala, H. (1976). A model for the role of motivational factors in
driver's decision-making. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 6, 243-261.
Ochanine, V.A. (1977). Concept of operative image in engineering and general psychology.
In B.F. Lomov, V.F. Rubakhin and V.F. Venda (Eds.). Engineering Psychology. Science
Publisher, Moscow.
Otha, H. (1993). Individual differences in driving distance headway. In A.G. Gale (Ed.).
Vision in Vehicles IV. Elsevier, Netherlands.
Rasmussen, 1. (1983). Skills, rules and knowledge; Signals, signs and symbols and other
distinctions in human performance models. IEEE Transaction on Systems, Man and
Cybernetics, 13(3), 257-266.
Recarte, M.A. and Nunes, L.M. (2000). Effect of verbal and spatial-imagery task on eye
fixations while driving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6, 31-43.
Rumbaugh, 1., Blaha, 1., Eddy, F., Lorensen, W. and Premerlani, W. (1991). Object Oriented
Modeling and Design. Prentice Hall, Londres.
Salvucci, D.D., Boer, E.R. and Liu, A. (2001). Toward an integrated model of driver behavior
in a cognitive architecture. Transportation Research Record, 1779, 9-16.
Schneider, W. and Schiffrin, R.M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information
processing I: Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84(1), 1-66.
Schneider, W. and Chein 1.M. (2003). Controlled and automatic processing: Behavior,
theory, and biological mechanisms. Cognitive Science, 27, 525-559.
Sukthankar, R. (1997). Situation Awareness for Tactical Driving. PhD Thesis, Carnegie
Mellon University.
Thorn, R. (1991). Predire n'est pas expliquer. Eshel, Paris.
Van der Molen, H.H. and Botticher, M.T. (1988). A hierarchical risk model for traffic
participants. Ergonomics, 31(4), 537-555.
Van Elslande, P. (1992). Les erreurs d'interpretation en conduite automobile: mauvaise
categorisation ou activation erronee de schernas? Intellectica, 15(3), 125-149.
Wierda, M. and Aasman, J. (1992). Seeing and driving: Computation, algorithms and im-
plementation. Traffic Research Centre, University of Groningen, Netherlands, Haren:
Verkeerskundig Studiecentrum, VK91-06.
Wilde, G.J.S. (1982). Critical issues in risk homeostasis theory. Risk Analysis, 2(4), 249-
258.
19
Simple Simulation of Driver
Performance for Prediction and Design
Analysis
P. C. CACCIABUE, C. RE, AND L. MACCHI
19.1 Introduction
19.1.1 Modelling Human Behaviour in Modern Technology
The ability to model and predict Human-Machine Interaction (HMI) with consis-
tent approaches is a crucial aspect of modern technological systems, where the con-
sideration for the presence of humans in control of systems is necessary at design
level as well as during production and implementation processes. A reliable and
realistic approach that enables to anticipate what will be "done" at different levels
of cognition and behaviour by a human being, enables the prevention of erroneous
or risky behaviour, as well the implementation of means of intervention exploiting
the power of modem technologies and the decision making skill of humans.
The literature of the last decades is rich with methods that focus on this subject.
A comprehensive and short review of methodological approaches and theoretical
constructs has been given by Moray (1997), covering the last 30 years of research
and development in the field of human factors applied to different domains and
industrial settings.
In modem systems, modelling human behaviour entails considering primarily
cognitive processes and system dynamic evolution, i.e., mental activities, resulting
from the time-dependent interaction of humans and machines. In this sense, a model
of human behaviour cannot be developed in total isolation and abstraction, but it
must be coupled and linked to a model of plant performance in order to develop
the simulation of a 'joint' human-machine system, also called 'Joint Cognitive
System' (Hollnagel and Woods, 1983; Hollnagel, 1993). Therefore any model of
human behaviour comes necessarily framed in a clear interaction context with the
associated technological system.
The formulation of models ofHMI is an evolutionary process whose origin lies in
the cybernetic paradigm of Wiener (1948), who developed the analogy between the
human operator and the servomechanism, and in the similarities between comput-
ers and brains pointed out by von Neumann (1958). These analogies have increased
the awareness of the closed-loop nature ofHMI, with the specific goal-oriented be-
haviour of the human controller. However, the most substantial theories on human
344
19. Simulation of Driver Performance 345
behaviour derived from these first paradigms did not consider the predictive nature
of human behaviour, as all relevant human activities were practically associated
with manual control and direct interaction with physical phenomena. The inclusion
of the human element in the control loop of a process was thus a simple exercise
of mathematical consideration of action delays and parameters estimation.
The progress of technology towards supervisory control and automation re-
quired the formulation of much more complex models of human reasoning and
decision making processes, able to account primarily for cognitive rather than
manual activities. The need of simulating the man-plus-the-controlled-element was
then enunciated in the 1960s by McRuer and colleagues (1965). Other pioneering
research in this area, combined with the development of computer technologies,
inspired the first formulations of theoretical models of cognition (Neisser, 1967)
and, in the early 1970s, the metaphor of the operator as Information Processing
System (Newell and Simon, 1972).
Since then, a variety of paradigms of human behaviour have been developed
(Rouse, 1980; Rasmussen, 1983; Stassen et aI., 1990; Sheridan, 1992; Wickens,
1984, 2002), which aim to cover different levels of complexity and depth in repre-
senting mental processes, cognitive functions as well as behavioural performances.
These models vary quite substantially for many reasons. Certain models focus on
a specific domain of application. Other models pay attention primarily to cognitive
functions, such as decision making or diagnostic processes. Certain models con-
centrate on the role of operators and their interactions, such as supervisory control
process and teamwork.
This variety of models offers the analysts and designers of joint cognitive sys-
tems the possibility to choose the most suitable approach and paradigm to apply
for specific applications. However, in order to make the best selection in relation
to the problem at hand, the scope of application of the available methods and
models must be recognised. The existence of conceptual frameworks that enable
to identify boundaries and areas of consideration for modelling human behaviour
is discussed in detail in Chapter 2 of this book.
Users
A large variety of people can obtain a driver license. These include teenagers,
elderly and disabled persons. In other domains, the types of users are much less
346 Cacciabue, Re, and Macchi
1. The model must enable rapid assessments of DVE interactions with different
configurations so as to evaluate prototypes of different conceptual nature.
2. The dynamic interactions between driver, vehicle and environment need to be
included in the simulation. The vehicle and environment should offer dynamic
changing situations and interactivity with driver actions.
3. Cognitive aspects of behaviour and 'joint modelling' between humans and
vehicle-environment must be considered.
4. The model needs to account for behavioural adaptation to different types of
ADASs and possible emotional/attitudinal aspects.
5. The model should enable to consider possible driver errors, in addition to nor-
mative behaviour, in different traffic conditions. Error-generation mechanism
needs to be included in the model.
6. The overall model and simulation should be developed in such a configuration
to facilitate the evolution towards a 'real-time' simulation approach of the DVE
system.
Elementary Functions, which represent the basic activity that cannot be further
subdivided into simpler components.
Elementary Task, which is a task made of elementary functions only.
Complex Task, which is a task made of a combination of elementary tasks and
elementary functions.
The first step to perform is to define the correlations between what has been
called 'task' and individual 'Elementary Functions'. Table 19.1 contains a set of
Elementary Functions that can be accounted for in order to describe the basic
actions of a driver. As an example, the Elementary Function 'Receive Perceptual
Input' is associated with visual, aural or haptic perception.
Table 19.2 shows a sample of correlations that are defined between tasks and
elementary functions. Several tasks have been developed for implementing the
simulation, such as 'Attain higher speed', 'Attain lower speed', 'Stop vehicle',
'Reverse vehicle', 'Tum left', 'Tum right', 'Change lane', 'Pass Vehicle', 'Over-
take', 'Keep lateral safety margins', 'Keep longitudinal safety margins', 'Maintain
speed', 'Give way at intersection', 'Emergency manoeuvre', etc.
The tasks, orframes, are simulated implementing the associated procedure and
sequence of elementary tasks/functions by means of sequences of rules and actions
described through object oriented programming languages. Attributes (pre- and
post-conditions) are correlated according to rules and entity-attribute matrices.
The logical dynamic sequence of tasks is defined according to a hierarchy derived
from the satisfaction of the pre- and post-conditions, coupled with a model based
on decision making and definition of intentions. This model is crucial for the
entire dynamic evolution of the DVE interaction. A simple model of this nature is
described in next section and may be associated with the maximum allowed speed
and on specific road and traffic conditions.
Another fundamental characteristic of the SSDRIVE simulation is the consid-
eration for the permanent or automatic tasks. These tasks are identified by the
fact that they are permanently carried out during a DVE interaction and do not
require specific pre-conditions to be launched. These are stereotypes of what may
be called 'skill-based behaviour' in a very 'classical' modelling architecture based
on a skill-rule-knowledge type behaviour that has been the most known imple-
mentation of the information processing paradigm proposed in the early 1980s for
describing human behaviour (Rasmussen, 1983). Two permanent tasks are con-
sidered in the present simulation approach, namely 'keep lateral safety margins'
and 'keep longitudinal safety margins' .
The concept of permanent tasks is quite straightforward, as it is associated with
the fact that drivers 'normally' keep their vehicle within lane margins and at a
reasonable safety distance with preceding vehicles or obstacles, without specific
352 Cacciabue, Re, and Macchi
effort and cognitive demand. Consequently, these two tasks are permanently active
in the DVE loop and are always performed every time the driver simulation is
activated. This simulation requirement, associated with the fact that there is no
cognitive load in performing these' simple vehicle control' tasks, in normal condi-
tions' has generated the choice of a classical (optimal) control modelling approach
(Weir and Chao, 2007, this volume) for their simulation in the DVE loop.
In practice, every time the driver simulation is activated in the DVE loop, the
permanent tasks are performed first, unless a human error is underway. They
aim at keeping the vehicle under control with respect to longitudinal and lateral
coordinated of the driving environment (road and traffic) (Fig. 19.1). The other
tasks are then performed according to the model of decision making and intentions
described above.
Step 2 Step 3
I
I
I
I I
I
DVE DVE DVE I
I
I
Interaction Interaction Interaction
I
I
I
I
I I
I I
I i I I I
o 2 4 ....a time
controls that are known and, in many cases, familiar, according to experience. As
the driving process is very dynamic, these actions are continuously selected or
developed from the knowledge base of the driver. However, prior to this activ-
ity a process of information management and formulation of goals and tasks is
necessary.
Consequently, independently of the specific model selected for describing the
driver behaviour, the following four steps of cognitive and behavioural interactions
must be considered:
1. Perception of signals from vehicle and environment.;
2. Interpretation of information.
3. Formulation of goals and intentions and selection of tasks to be carried out.
4. Performance of actions on control panel and on vehicle commands.
This is a typical formulation of a model based on the paradigm called 'Information
Processing System' (IPS), which has been applied in almost all fields to account for
human interaction with technology at different levels of automation (Cacciabue,
2004). The important characteristic of this approach is that it allows considering the
behavioural as well as the cognitive aspects of human performance. This paradigm
is the reference model for the SSDRIVE simulation.
The way in which the various cognitive functions of the model are implemented
in the simulation is quite simple and straightforward and depends on two possible
types of modelling architecture: the simple and linear normative driver behaviour
or the complex and more realistic descriptive driver behaviour. These two types
of simulation are now described in some detail.
354 Cacciabue, Re, and Macchi
y
N
1. All signals and signs that are produced inside and outside the vehicle are per-
ceived.
2. Interpretation is conforming to the meaning associated with signs and
signals.
19. Simulation of Driver Performance 355
3. Intentions are formulated in relation to the ongoing task and information per-
ceived and interpreted. A task is then either continued or newly started. This
task is called active task.
4. Actions are carried out according to the active task.
The discussion on the reasons for selecting these parameters accounting for de-
scriptive driver behaviour goes beyond the scope of this paper and can be found
in Carsten (2007, this volume). The definitions that are associated with each pa-
rameter are shown in Table 19.3. However, it is primarily noticeable that the two
static parameters of Attitude/personality and Experience/competence enable the
definition of some initial characteristics of behaviour that affect all decision mak-
ing processes and performances throughout the simulation. On the other side, the
dynamic parameters enable to account for the variability of conditions and envi-
ronmental changes that are encountered during a typical journey. In this sense,
both types of parameters are very important and may playa crucial role in the sim-
ulation of DVE interactions. Naturally, this implies that the model and associated
simulation are sufficiently powerful to incorporate all these effects.
It is important to note here that the parameter Task Demand is in most cases
equivalent to Driving Demand. However, as the objective of the correlations be-
tween parameters and behaviour covers all potential aspects affecting driver per-
formance, it is preferable to retain in this parameter also the factors associated
19. Simulation of Driver Performance 357
with secondary task performance, including social and personal demands derived
from the overall vehicle context.
The set of variables that govern the dynamic driver model is directly correlated
with the ability of the model to account for the above functions at different levels
of depth. The crucial open issues that remain to be resolved with respect to the
DVE parameters are of three types:
The forms that these functions may take are numerical or logical expressions.
In the SSDRIVE approach, the choice of utilising fuzzy descriptions has been
made. Consequently, during the simulation, each (independent) variable of inter-
est is evaluated by a numerical correlation and then associated to a fuzzy value.
Subsequently, the estimation of the (fuzzy) value of each parameter requires the
application of typical fuzzy rules that combine the (fuzzy) values of the associated
independent variables.
In practice, the five parameters that govern the SSDRIVE are fixed. A set of
default correlations between parameters and measurable independent variables is
available. These require in input only the limits for each independent measurable
variable to structure the fuzzy correlations. The fuzzy rules that define the values
of the parameters are fixed, while screening on the fuzzy correlations associated
to each independent variable is possible via input data to the simulation tool.
On the other side, if the user of the simulations intends to study and apply
new functions and rules, then the input requires the definition of new independent
variables, to be selected amongst those calculated by the three modules of the DVE
simulation, and the formulation and programming of new correlations that replace
the default ones. This is a very flexible aspect of the simulation tool that is based
on a 'modular' structure. However, this requires programming ability and detailed
knowledge of the simulation software.
The correlations adopted as default for each dynamic parameter will now be
briefly discussed.
1. Task Demand (TD) is assumed to be a fuzzy function with values High, Accept-
able, and Low, and is correlated only to visible level of traffic and environment
(Visibility), and to the number of vehicles present on the road (Complexity of
traffic, CoT):
TD = f (Visibility CoT)
2. Visibility is measured in terms of distance and is considered
Good, when the driver can see at a distance of more than 100 metres;
Acceptable, for distances of clear vision between 70 and 100 metres;
Bad, for less than 70 metres of clear vision.
3. Complexity of traffic (CoT) is measured in terms of vehicles per kilometre on
the road:
Good, when the density of vehicles is less than 100 per kilometre;
19. Simulation of Driver Performance 359
Vis CoT
G G
A A
B B
70 100 m 100 250 No. of
vehic1es/Km
FIGURE 19.3. Example of fuzzy correlations and fuzzy rule between Task Demand versus
Complexity of traffic and Weather Conditions (Visibility).
Acceptable, when the density of vehicles is between 100 and 250 per kilome-
tre; and
Bad, when the density of vehicles is greater than 250 per kilometre.
4. The fuzzy rule applied to calculate the fuzzy value of task demand is
TD == High if min (Visibility, CoT) == Bad;
TD == Medium if min (Visibility, CoT) == Acceptable;
TD == Low if min (Visibility, CoT) == Good.
This means that the less favourable value is associated with TD between the
two fuzzy values of Visibility and Complexityof traffic.
DoD- IL1Speed I
L i
H I
H
I
,
1
A A I A
I i
I
H ----~-----
I
L
I
I
1---------1----------- L I
---------1-----_----
i
60 180 min 10 30 Km/h deg
Bad } High
DS Acceptable if Min [ Max( I L1Speed I , I L1q> I), DoD} Acceptable
{ Good Low
FIGURE 19.4. Example of fuzzy correlations and fuzzy rule between Driver State versus
Duration ofDrive, Speed change and Steering.
3. Variation of steering (I ~cp I) is associated to the road geometry and not to the
yaw disturbance. Variation of speed (I~Speedl) is associated to the traffic con-
ditions and road type/geometry. Therefore, it is assumed that in the case of
roads with constant geometry (straight roads or small bends and curves, e.g.,
highways) very little change of steering and speed occurs. These are causes of
possible drowsiness in drivers. Changing either of them, over a period of 10
min, is considered sufficient to retain an acceptable level of vigilance. They
are therefore combined in defining a fuzzy measure of Driver State. The fuzzy
correlations for the variation of speed and steering (for a period of driving ~t =
10 minutes) are as follows:
Low, when I~Speedl < 10 km/h or I~cpl < 5.
Medium/Acceptable, when 10:s I~Speed I :s 30 km/h or 5 :s I~cpl :s 30.
High, when I~Speedl > 30 kmlh or I~cpl > 30.
4. The fuzzy rule applied to calculate the fuzzy value of Driver State is as follows:
DS = Bad if = Low
DS = Acceptable if min [max(I~Speedl, I~cpl), DoD] = Acceptable
DS = Good if = High
This means that the less favourable value is associated with DS between the
fuzzy values of Duration ofDriving and the combination of Speed change and
Steering.
19. Simulation of Driver Performance 361
Dis (Vj)
L L
50 100 200 250 Vj 3 5 v2
(no. of veh./km) no. of active
indicators & IVIS
Dis = f[no. ofveh./km (vI)' no. of active indicators & IVIS (v2)]
SA -j.at SA = h(DS)
Bad if TD=Low Bad if DS=Bad
SA Good if TD = Medium SA Acceptable if DS = Acceptable
{ {
Bad if TD=High Good if DS = Good
SA = h(Dis)
Bad if Dis = High
SA Acceptable if Dis = Medium
{
Good if Dis = Low
FIGURE 19.5. Example of fuzzy correlations and fuzzy rules between Situation Awareness
versus Driver State, Task Demand and Distraction.
Finally, default fuzzy correlations and rules for the evaluation of Situation Aware-
ness are shown in Fig. 19.5.
The complete set of fuzzy correlations and fuzzy rules that govern the SS-
DRIVE simulation is quite complex and requires a considerable effort of data
input and data definition. This is not surprising as the actual combinations of vari-
ables, parameters and effects are distributed and cannot be oversimplified with
19. Simulation of Driver Performance 363
the risk of undermining the overall effort of modelling carried out at theoretical
level.
The actual formulation of the parameters that govern the model of the driver is
obviously critical for the overall DVE modelling. For this reason, the implementa-
tion of the correlations linking all static and dynamic parameters, i.e., EXp, AIT,
DS, TD, and SA, and measurable variables is kept wide open in the simulation
approach of SSDRIVE. This means that the users have two main options:
1. To utilise the set of default correlations, i.e., those that are described in this
paper, and adapt simply their boundary conditions and limits by input data.
This alternative is obviously easy and simple.
2. To apply a set of specific correlations, by developing appropriate programming
routines that will then be interfaced with the main SSDRIVE simulation. This
makes the input process more complicated and time consuming. However, in
this way, the flexibility of the overall simulation is widely improved and makes
possible to test different formulations and combinations of (fuzzy) functions
and rules that combine parameters and variables.
1. the default settings are kept as simple as possible in order to make the develop-
ment of standard cases relatively easy and fast and
2. the development and implementation in the simulation of new algorithms will
require a more complex definition of the input and deeper knowledge of the
simulation architecture and programming languages.
The set of defaults conditions existing in the simulation are now briefly dis-
cussed. These are simple correlations that govern primarily development of inten-
tions and decision making, and error generation.
364 Cacciabue, Re, and Macchi
and
ifRoadcond == Bad --+ 1/J(Roadcond.) == - 1
if Roadcond == Acceptable --+ 1/J(Roadcond.) == 0
if Roadcond == Good --+ 1/J(Roadcond.J == 1.
In this way, the intended speed may vary between the two following maximum
and minimum values:
Max Speedintended == 2 * Speedmax-allowed
and
..- ..- ..
... - '"
I I I I
o 2 4 6 8
I
CD"~~:~_ " T"
A 8
DS D" "
river Pa rameters at time t4
.. TO
~
C
DS
vehicle and environment conditions, i.e., the dynamic variables calculated at pre-
vious time steps, and evaluates the new actions that are carried out on the vehicle
controls (Fig. 19.7, upper part). These are essentially the settings of indicators,
ADAS, IVIS etc., and the basic vehicle control action s of steering and acceler-
atinglbraking in order to obtain to the desired position and speed. The cognitive
processes and behavioural adaptation that lead to decisions have been discussed
in the previou s sections. In the following, the implementation of control actions is
discussed in some detail s.
Ifeath er conditions
Road conditions
jDR IVE R Made/l
Traffic
.....
t f eedback
I
----------------,
r- ! I
I
F....., IJriw"V.Iud- Modd
I
U/. tJ ' +~ l4(I)
,
j FrotIf
l Driwr
i EIW;n;HtIrW,1I
70 fi: i .
i .IIod. t
~ 60 ------t-------t------ :------i--------- ----------
50 --.- ---- , -- --- - --- ,-- --- -----. - -- -------
40 _ . ~ . __ A ~ _ _- : :
10 20 40 50
FIGURE 19.7. Control theory model of the SSDRIVE simulation and desired speed profile
during transition from 50 to 80 km/h with a Tstyle of 20 s.
Environment
r.
~
Env ironmental
Parameters
Obstacles I
Vehicle speed Weather condrtions
Vehicle position Traffic and road conditions
...... ~Ir
......
Simulation
Dr iver
I ~
Manager
!Dr ive r' s Parameters Task analysis &
,,
Driver's Actions
Next LH OIL
I 11\
Lane position (steering angIe)
Vehicl~ speed
Speed profile (brake/accelsrator)
Vehicle posrtion
Control setlings (ADAS, IVIS,
ADAS interface
indicators, ......)
IVIS interface ,Ir
...... ....
Vehicle Dynamic
The detailed description of the control theory model applied within the SS-
DRIVE simulation goes beyond the scope of the present chapter and can be found
elsewhere (Re and Carusi, 2005). In brief, the simplified driver behaviour rules
applied in the simulation are associated to the desired lane position (lpd) and de-
sired speed profile (Ud) (Fig. 19.7). These quantities are evaluated according to
the following rules:
Desired Lane Position (lpd): the car maintains the centre of the lane.
Desired Speed Profile (U d ) : while the rule for desired lane position is regulated
also in terms of driving good practice, to define the ideal speed profile is not
equally simple. Taken into account the rule of minimising travel time according
to driving at the maximum allowed speed (which means that after the transition
Ud is almost constant in a highway), the desired driver speed profile is obtained
by means of a smooth profile. This allows to increase or decrease speed after the
decision of attaining higher or lower speeds taken by the more cognitive part of
the simulation.
During the transition from a speed limit to the successive speed limit, Ud is
modelled as UTransition:
o-:: (t)
_= ~ (1 +
V
sin(wl - n
2
/2)) '
where w = n /Tstyle and 1 = 0 -:- Tstyl e. The outcome of the optimal control process
is then transformed into steering angle and actions on accelerator/brakes. The
optimal control model does not affect the positions of controls and indicators of
ADAS/IVIS and other settings, as these are evaluated by the cognitive part of the
simulation.
The overall dynamic interaction within the DVE simulation of the SSDRIVE is
finally summarised in Fig. 19.8. In particular, at each time step of the simulation,
the driver model calculates the implementation of tasks and actions and generates
profiles of steering angle and actions on the break or the accelerator, in order to
accommodate for the desired lane position and speed profile.
The Simulation Manager is the governing module of the simulation, which
receives input from all three main DVE components and evaluates the overall
error-generation mechanism by means of the DIL and keeps account of the syn-
chronisation of the simulation. The effect of ADAS and IVIS, if contained by the
vehicle simulation, is also fed to the simulation manager.
Ud(S) + U(s)
----...-j.. . ( X ) - - - - + 1
Ipd(S) +
--+0
lp(s! I.. _
FIGURE 19.9. Control model in terms of the equivalent inner open loop Laplace transfer
function of heading angle control.
Discrete values of these quantities must be assigned in input in order to allow the
simulation of limited sets of sequences of events.
An example of improper steering, due to driver distraction or other causes, is
shown in Fig. 19.10, where a driver is simulated and will not correct the heading
angle by means of the steer controller and will not decrease the speed for a certain
time (Tdistraction). This type of approach has also been attempted by Salvucci (2001).
However, the SSDRIVE approach offers a wider variety of inadequate actions and
causes as well as permitting the application of novel and diversified correlations
of human error generation.
19. Simulation of Driver Performance 371
~.+ iuo
~
U(s)
~ lp.(s)
7S
O
Constant control
= value applied by
the Driver
x = Ignoredinput
The two sample cases discussed hereafter have the only objective to demonstrate
the functionality of the simulation, rather than discussing detailed applications of
predictions of DVE interactions according to well-defined scenarios and dynamic
situations. The reason for such type of approach is dual: firstly, it would be too long
to discuss in detail an application that attempts to predict DVE interactions, as the
overall scenarios setting and DVE correlations would require an extended discus-
sion and a accurate scientific justification. Secondly, the results of the simulation
runs should also be analysed for their implications on the design and associated
safety issues.
However, these issues go beyond the objectives of this paper, which is focused
at demonstrating the availability of a DVE simulation tool, centred on the driver
behaviour modelling, and at showing its potentialities in terms of diversity of
behavioural adaptation, error generation and parametrical correlations. For these
reasons, the following two sample cases discuss rather simple situations and highly
theoretical situations that stress the computational power of the simulation rather
than representing realistic behaviours of drivers in similar situations.
19.3.1 Case 1
The first sample case discussed here is based on a simple path considering a series
of round turns and multiple lanes roundabouts trajectories. The lateral and angular
deviations have been evaluated. The path is composed of two turns, one anticlock-
wise and one clockwise, and several times lane change on a three-lanes roundabout.
No human error or inadequate behaviour is considered. The driver simulation fol-
lows the normative behaviour. The desired and actual paths calculated by the driver
lane control model are illustrated in Fig. 19.11. It is clear that the desired path and
the actual position of the vehicle are almost completely overlapping, even in the
presence of a rather complex trajectory.
...,
-.J
N
n
~
(")
S'
. ': ~
25 - - Desired Path
- - Actual Path
~
20 ~
::I
15% 0..
15 "
z~
10 (")
10%
e:
5
g 0 5%
-5
0%
-10
-15
O.606Oll
-20
Steeri>J Angle
-25 Adjustment I'ad]
L
0 10 20 :J] 40 50 60 70
[m) O.om
FIGURE 19.11. Study Case I : Vehicle path analysis and centre lane deviation against steering angle adjustment.
19. Simulation of Driver Performance 373
As the speed profile was chosen constant, the effect of the adjustments of steering
angle versus lateral deviation from the desired centre of lane could be evaluated.
Negative values of steering angle correspond to steers in the right direction. As
shown in Fig. 19.11, there are no particular trends in terms of steering angle adjust-
ment and centre of lane lateral deviation. The histogram contains the distribution
of the samples. The peak (20%) is around position (0.0; 0.0) and represents the
samples coming from straight sections of the path. Most of the samples (>90%)
are concentrated in the angular adjustment interval -0.01 to +0.01 radiant and
more than 95% of samples have a centre of lane lateral deviation less than 0.29 m.
19.3.2 Case 2
The second sample case discussed here is the performance of the same paths
simulated in the first sample case, where the difference is introduced in terms
of a loss of control. We will not discuss here the reasons for such human error
since the goal of the case study is to demonstrate the power of the simulation
to represent the consequences of erroneous behaviour. The simulation of loss of
control is performed by modelling the error as a freezing of steering control. As a
consequence, no further adjustment of the steering angle occurs in order to follow
the road centre lane.
As in the previous case, the desired and actual paths calculated by the driver
lane control model are reported in Fig. 19.12. It is clear that at a certain instant,
the desired path and the actual position of the vehicle separate completely and,
unless a recovery action takes place, the vehicle is actually driven off the road. In
-2 r---,---_r--_~___,--::--r ____,_-____,_-____r-_r__------___,
- - Desired Path
-4 - - Actual Path
-+- Distraction Path
- - Lane Boundaries
-6
-8
I
-10
-12
-14
-16
FIGURE 19.12. Study Case 2: Vehicle path analysis in the case of erroneous steering.
374 Cacciabue, Re, and Macchi
this case, no recovery action was simulated and the overall DVE interaction was
stopped at the time of expected vehicle loss of control.
19.4 Conclusions
In this paper, we have described the SSDRIVE. While the governing model and the
basic theoretical considerations are derived from a specific paradigm described in
referto thepaper (Carsten, 2007), in this paper we have discussed the set of default
correlations and algorithms that are implemented in the simulation. The overall
architecture of the SSDRIVE simulation has been conceived with the objective
of maximum flexibly from the user point of view. In other words, the simulation
is based on a modular structure and the user is able to include in the simulation
different models and modules describing specific driver support and information
systems, in accordance to specific needs or design options.
Following a set of preliminary tests, the model can be considered stable and
reliable. The development of the model and simulation will continue in terms of
deviation analysis during control delay and control freezing conditions. In partic-
ular, the analysis of a variety of IVIS and ADAS interventions will be gradually
included in the simulation, following the same principle of maximum flexibly for
the user. In this way, the core features of the SSDRIVE simulation will remain
unchanged, in terms of basic modelling architecture and fundamental principles,
such as the consideration for task analysis process, normative versus descriptive
driver behaviour, number of parameters that govern driver behaviour and basic
DVE interactions. At the same time, thanks to the modularity of the simulation ap-
proach adopted, the users of SSDRIVE will be able to test specific configurations,
typical proprietary models of ADAS and IVIS, as well as particular algorithms for
human errors and correlations between parameters and environmental variables.
In the present configuration, the simulation tool is mainly applicable for pre-
dictive analyses of DVE interactions and for studying hypothetical scenarios for
design and safety assessment purposes. However, once the simulation is validated
and streamlined by means of fast running parametrical expressions and specific al-
gorithms, it could be implemented in vehicle technology for real-time assessment
of DVE interaction. It could therefore be applied for detecting and anticipating po-
tential risky behaviours and dangerous conditions, and preventing them by either
suggesting possible driver intervention and/or by taking over control of the vehicle,
recovering the normal situation or containing the consequences of a incident.
References
Cacciabue, P.C. (2004). Guide to Applying Human Factors Methods. Springer-Verlag,
London.
Hollnagel, E. (1993). Human Reliability Analysis: Context and Control. Academic Press,
London.
Hollnagel, E. and Woods, D.D. (1983). Cognitive Systems Engineering: New wine in new
bottles. International Journal ofMan-Machine Studies, 18, 583-606.
19. Simulation of Driver Performance 375
Kirwan, B. and Ainsworth, L.K. (1992). Guide to TaskAnalysis. Taylor and Francis, London.
Kondo, M. (1953). Directional stability (when steering is added). Journal of the Society of
Automotive Engineers ofJapan, 7(5/6),104-106, 109, 123, 136-140. (In Japanese.)
McRuer, D.T., Allen, R.W., Weir, D.H. and Klein, R.H. (1977). New results in driver steering
control. Human Factors, 19,381-397.
McRuer, D.T., Graham, D., Kredel, E. and Reisener, W. (1965). Human pilot dynamics in
compensatory systems - theory, models and experiments with controlled elements and
forcing function variations. Report AFFDL-TR65-15. Wright Patterson AFB, OH.
McRuer, D.T. and Krendel, E.S. (1974). Mathematical models of human pilot behavior.
AGARDograph No. 188. NATO Advisory Group for Aerospace Research and Develop-
ment.
Michon, J.A. (1985). A critical review of driver behaviour models: What do we know? What
should we do? In L.A. Evans and R.C. Schwing (Eds.). Human Behaviour and Traffic
Safety (pp. 487-525). Plenum Press, New York.
Minsky, M. (1975). A framework for representing knowledge. In P. Winston (Ed.). The
Psychology of Computer Vision. McGraw-Hill, New York.
Moray, N. (1997). Human factors in process control. In G. Salvendi (Ed.). Handbook of
Human Factors and Ergonomics (pp. 1945-1971). Wiley, New York.
Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive Psychology. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York.
Newell, A. and Simon, H.A. (1972). Human Problem Solving. Prentice-Hall, Englewood
Cliffs, NY.
Rasmussen, J. (1983). Skills, rules and knowledge: Signals, signs and symbols; and other
distinctions in human performance model. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and
Cybernetics, 13(3), 257-267.
Re, C. and Carusi, E. (2005). Driving stability correlated with automated in-vehicle systems.
In 24th Annual European Conference on Human Decision Making, Athens.
Rouse, W.B. (1980). Systems Engineering Models of Human-Machine Interaction. North
Holland, Oxford.
Salvucci, D.D. (2001) Predicting the effects of in-car interface use on driver performance:
An integrated model approach. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 55,
85-107.
Stassen, H.G., Johannsen, G. and Moray, N. (1990). Internal representation, internal model,
human performance model and mental workload. Automatica, 26(4), 811-820.
Sheridan, T.B. (1992). Telerobotics, Automation and Human Supervisory Control. The MIT
Press, Cambridge, MA.
von Neumann, 1. (1958). The Computer and the Brain. Yale University Press, New Haven,
CT.
Wiener, N. (1948). Cybernetics. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Wickens, C.D. (1984). Engineering Psychology and Human Performance. Merrill,
Columbus, OH.
Wickens, C.D. (2002). Multiple resources and performance prediction. Theoretical Issues
in Ergonomics Science, 3,159-177.
Weir, D.H. and Chao, K.C. (2007). Review of control theory models for directional and
speed control. In P.C. Cacciabue (Ed.). Modelling Driver Behaviour in Automotive En-
vironments. Springer, London.
VIII
Simulation of Traffic
and Real Situations
20
Real-Time Traffic and Environment
Risk Estimation for the Optimisation of
Human-Machine Interaction
ANGELOS AMDITIS, ARIS POLYCHRONOPOULOS
AND E. BEKIARIS
20.1 Introduction
The development of next generation driver-vehicle interaction systems should be
focused towards obtaining a safe and sustainable mobility, with the aim to half
the number of road accidents as proposed by the European Commission (2001).
Mobility should be promoted in the future towards 'intermodality' in order to
reduce traffic congestion and to optimise travel planning; however, towards this
aim there is an increasing demand for on-board information systems. These needs
together with the demand for new on-vehicle support and services and the need of
the users to be connected to their own information cell (mobile phone, PDA, etc.)
will unavoidably increase the number of interaction of the driver with the vehicle
thus raising the potential risk of driver's distraction and fatigue, which are among
the main causes of road accidents.
While conventional vehicle safety measures (seatbelts and airbags) have con-
tributed significantly to the reduction of fatalities in the last decades, their safety
contribution is reaching its limits and currently further improvement is difficult to
be achieved at a reasonable cost. Today, the development of new advanced driver
assistance systems (ADAS, e.g. collision avoidance, lane-keeping aid and vision
enhancement systems) offers great potential for further improving road safety, in
particular by means of mitigating driver errors. In addition, the number of in-
vehicle information systems (IVIS) increases rapidly in today's vehicles. These
systems have the potential to greatly enhance mobility and comfort (e.g. navigation
aids and traffic information systems, media players, web browsers, etc.), but at the
same time increase the risk for excessive and dangerous levels of inattention to
the vehicle control task. Furthermore, many IVIS functions are today featured on
portable computing systems, such as PDAs or advanced mobile phones, which are
generally not designed for use while driving. In the near future, many of these func-
tions could be expected to be easily downloadable from a remote service centre,
directly to the vehicle or the nomadic device.
This variety of systems and functions that interact with the driver in one way or
another leads to a number of challenges, both technical and human factors related,
for the designer of the future automotive HMIs. These challenges include the HMI
379
380 Amditis, Polychronopoulos and Bekiaris
design for the individual systems as well as the question as to how to integrate a
range of different systems into a functioning whole with respect to their interaction
with the driver. Another challenge concerns how to best exploit the technological
possibilities of adaptive interfaces in the automotive domain.
The latter is the main proposition that the paper deals with. It primarily addresses
the adaptivity features of human-machine interaction related to the external traffic
and environmental conditions. In particular, the environment is modelled and risks
are calculated based on the current macroscopic driver's behaviour. A real-time
supervision system is being developed and presented, which detects, analyses and
assesses the environment. The outcome is a calculated level of risk on several
driving scenarios or states of the environment. According to the risk level, a mes-
sage can be presented to the driver or not, enabling the aforementioned adaptivity
features of integrated HMI systems.
In the sequel, two different use cases are presented that represent two different
systems and concepts: the AWAKE system, in which the traffic and environment
supervision is used to adapt a safety application to the environment - an adaptive
hypovigilance driver warning system (DWS) - and the AIDE system that offers
an adaptive integrated HMI solution for conflicts between on-board messages that
compete in order to be presented to the driver. AWAKE was the first attempt
to calculate an overall level of traffic risk, whereas AIDE proposes a complete
algorithm that covers most of the critical traffic scenarios.
Hypovigilance diagnosis module (HDM) that detects and diagnoses driver hy-
povigilance in real time. Based on an artificial intelligence algorithm this module
fuses data from on-board driver monitoring sensors (eyelid and steering grip data)
and data regarding the driver's behaviour (lane tracking, gas/brake and steering
position data).
A traffic risk estimation module (TRE) that assesses the traffic situation and
the involved risks. It matches, following a deterministic approach, data from an
enhanced digital navigational map, a positioning system, anti-collision radar, the
odometer and a driver's gaze direction sensor. This module is not designed as a
complete new system to estimate traffic risk, but rather as an expert combination
of existing ADAS technology. The output of this module was used by the HDM
to re-assess the state of the driver and by the DWS to determine the adequate
level of warning.
A DWS that uses acoustic, visual and/or haptic means. The module uses inputs
from the HDM and the TRE to determine the adequate warning level for a certain
situation.
specific situation. In the AWAKE project, the evaluation of a general risk index is
used as input to the warning system. In summary, risk is a function of
the state of the vehicle (its dynamic condition described by speed, acceleration,
etc.);
road scenario (type of road: motorways, urban roads, etc.);
traffic scenario (complexity, speed, density);
environment (weather conditions, visibility);
driver conditions (perception of the surrounding environment, distraction).
A complete and integrated TRE system would be very complex and was far
from an industrial exploitation phase in AWAKE. It was not an objective of the
AWAKE project to develop new sensors to create such advanced system. However,
the concept of the TRE system is modular and can therefore be easily adapted to
the sensor system of the vehicle in which it is installed. TRE module consists
of four main blocks (Damiani et al., 2003). The first unit, sensors, includes both
the sensors that are standard available in the car, and which are shared with other
on-board functions (ABS, etc.), and specific sensors added to the vehicle for the
TRE module. The second unit, scenario assessment (SA), integrates all information
coming from the sensors. The third unit, warning strategies, identifies discrete risks
and their risk level, while the fourth unit, risk-level assessment (RLA), integrates
the discrete risks into a unique overall RLI.
that represent the risk level for each single event. The warning strategies unit
employs several algorithms that implement the specific warning criteria. As output,
a warning level index is generated as described in Table 20.1.
1 Normal situation (no risk estimated, scenario identified, factor detected) Green
2 Slight cautionary case (single scenario or factor) Yellow
3 Severe cautionary case (combination of scenarios and factors) Orange
4 Imminent case (highest risk) Red
384 Amditis, Polychronopoulos and Bekiaris
traffic risks to the same denominator. For this purpose, a method was proposed by
Damiani et al. (2003) for the classification of critical traffic scenarios according
to the associated level of risk. The more promising approach seems to be a mix
of a simple rule-based system with a weighted sum approach, which assigns a
pre-defined value to each single warning originating from a specific function. In
particular, the proposed rules are as follows:
Normal situation - If the levels of warning coming out from the different functions
are all at their own minimum value, then the output will be always 1.
Intermediate cases - In order to distinguish between levels 2 and 3, the criteria are
based on a sum of weighed values (a particular pre-defined value is assigned to
a warning level originating from a specific function).
Imminent case - As soon as a maximum level of warning is provided by one of
the functions, the output will be always 4.
(a) Traffic and environment risk estimation (vehicle, traffic, environment and their
relationships) - TERA
(b) Cockpit activity assessment (driver's activities not related to the driving task) -
CAA
(c) Driver availability estimation (driver's activities related to the driving task) -
DAE
(d) Driver state degradation (driver's physical ability) - DSD
(e) Driver characteristics (driver's individual behaviour and preferences) - DC
The (a) component is a successor of TRE module, namely the TERA module and
it is described in details in this paper.
20.3.1 Overview
Traffic and environment risk assessment (TERA) in AIDE monitors and measures
activities outside the vehicle in order to assess the external contributors to the en-
vironmental and traffic context and also to predict the driver's intention for lateral
manoeuvre. Existing sensors used by collision-warning (long-range radars), lane
departure warning (cameras), blind spot warning (cameras), maps and position-
ing systems combined with a table of corresponding roadway characteristics and
vehicle inertial sensors could be used to help understand the environment outside
the vehicle and adapt the HMI accordingly. The supervision algorithms collect all
available raw (e.g. radar signals) and processed data (e.g. tracked object list). The
role of TERA is threefold:
to calculate in real time a total level of risk related to traffic and environmental
parameters;
to calculate environmental and traffic parameters according to the requirements
of the other DVE modules and/or warnings if a function is absent in the vehicle
(e.g. the collision warning function could be produced from TERA if a radar is
available );
to estimate the drivers' intention (e.g. for manoeuvre of a possible lane change).
Radar
object hSl, wammg
Flag, rrc, TH TEM algorhh rns ,
,~--------.,I
,
..
: TE RA Output :
I
Blind $pO'
carner. ~
I "_. _. _. _. _... !
I--
Cartographic da la ban and
positioning system: Speed
hmlt , road type .
--
Curvalure. inlerse ction
l.t:!P Lann . Pnonty, Pol, Sl~' .......
20.3.2 Architecture
The role of TERA is represented by the two physical blocks, namely the risk
assessment and the intention detection. In Fig. 20.1, the connections with the
other components are indicated in the vehicle environment. The physical inter-
faces are out of the scope of this document (CAN , TCP, etc.). An adequate soft-
ware module is under development, to combine all different pieces of informa-
tion from the various subsys tems in a coherent whole, running on an on-board
computer.
Input sensor array includes input control sensors (e.g. steering wheel angle sen-
sor, pedal position sensor), enviro nment sensors (radar, laser, IR, etc., but also GPS
and digital maps) and vehicle dynamic state sensors (speed sensors, accelerome-
ters, yaw rate sensors).
High traffic risk situations - High traffic risk value should signify a quite dan-
gerous situation where a warning might have already been generated or the
possibility to be generated is high. In those cases , adaptation can only have the
20. Real-Time Traffic and Environment Risk Estimation 387
(a) the road infrastructure consisting of the lanes, the road borders and the infras-
tructure elements (e.g. speed limit, traffic signs);
(b) the subject vehicle and its dynamics;
(c) the moving and stationary obstacles;
(d) the traffic flow representing the number of obstacles ahead or/and their dis-
tances from the ego-vehicle;
(e) environmental parameters.
Using the vehicle data (e.g. yaw-rate sensor) and the data coming from 'exter-
nal sensors' (radar and possibly camera), it is possible to compute and assess the
subject vehicle's trajectory. The method that is used within AIDE TERA module
is the scenario representation by analysing the following state elements (Poly-
chronopoulos et al., 2005):
388 Amditis, Polychronopoulos and Bekiaris
(a) Object State = the dynamics of the objects in the longitudinal field.
(b) Subject State = the dynamics of the subject-vehicle.
(c) Road State = the geometry of the lanes and road borders.
(a) The road borders and lanes are described by a clothoid model (Kirchner and
%
Heinrich, 1998): Y (x) = co~ + Cl + Yo, where Yo is the offset from the
ego-vehicle's position, Co is the road curvature and YOl the rate of the curvature.
Different offsets YOl and YOr represent the left and the right border locations.
Thus, the following state vector can describe the road (or the lane): XRB =
(co Cl YOl YOr)T. The measurement space includes higher level parameters
from a camera (and its image processing unit for lane detection), map and
positioning data and inertial sensors (odometer, yaw rate sensor).
(b) The state of the subject vehicle (SV) contains kinematics, attributes and prop-
x
erties. A typical state vector of the former case is sv = ( V awe)T, where
e
V is the velocity, aas the tangential acceleration, is the heading and to the
yaw rate.
(c) The state of the obstacles contains also kinematics, attributes and properties
such as: Xo = ( x Y Vx Vy ax ay W h )T. Here, (x, y) are the Cartesian co-
ordinates in a local coordinate system (i.e. the subject's vehicle coordinate
system) and V, and a is the relative velocity and acceleration respectively in
the two axis. Wand h refer to the properties of the tracked obstacles.
(a) Road data - Type of road context (country, urban, peri-urban, highway, urban
highway), priority level of the road, number of lane, speed limit, presence of
school, etc.
(a) Vehicle data - Blinkers status, wipers' position and light position.
On the basis of these three basic conditions, TERA risk assessment produces a
set of traffic risk functions that take into consideration not only this detection of
discrete dangerous situations around the subject vehicle but also the traffic and the
environment context where these situations occur. However, in order to ensure the
correct system's output in case of a dangerous situation, this conditions' correlation
with the traffic and environment context takes place only when the basic conditions
are not classified as imminent.
In the following, the rule-based approach adopted for TERA algorithms is illus-
trated explaining the functions' derivation. In each of the TERA functions, the first
coefficient reflects the main condition to be detected, where the second coefficient
reflects the risk contributor derived from specific characteristics (parameters) of
the current driving scenario. Each coefficient inside the second coefficient repre-
sents a sub-condition (subsidiary condition) and is multiplied with a number W
between 0 and 1 (confidence weight) where Lm Wi == 1 and m is the amount of
selected sub-conditions (if main condition NOT imminent):
TERA F unct ion == (main-condition) x {[(sub_condition 1) x
wl]+[(sub_condition2) x W2]+ ... [(sub-condition) x wm ] }
where (float) TERAFunction E [0,1] and each of the main conditions and their cor-
responding sub-conditions can either have deterministic (wipers status) or fuzzy
(TTC) values. In the first case, variables that are involved in the conditions or
sub-conditions' derivation are modelled by membership functions (trapezoidal
or Gaussian) and thus, the operations among variables are replaced from op-
erations among fuzzy sets. In the second case, relevant variables have scalar
or Boolean values. In addition, a sub-condition can also be the logical output
of two or more conditions following the binary aggregation according to the
Boole algebra. Weights give the possibility to externally control system's out-
puts. Moreover each condition can influence in a different way each function
which gives the system a more realistic-intelligent overview of the current see-
nano,
Thus, the enhanced rule-based approach adopted for each of the TERA traffic
risk functions is based on estimating one main condition (high level situation
description), which is then modulated by a factor representing the confidence
assigned to the main condition detection. This factor consists of a weighted sum
of the sub-conditions considered to influence the specific main condition. Such
390 Amditis, Polychronopoulos and Bekiaris
sub-conditions may involve consideration of the data coming from the vehicle
inertial sensors (including environmental information) and the data coming from
the map and positioning system combined with a table of corresponding roadway
characteristics in order to create a more global picture of the environment outside
the vehicle within the current driving scenario. However, it should be noted once
again that this approach is valid only if the main condition has not been classified
as imminent.
The main conditions to be detected lead to the three following traffic risk
functions:
First weights will be defined from experts' opinions and previous experience
in the field (Bekiaris and Portouli, 1999; Damiani et al., 2003). TERA function
concerning environment risk assessment will be similar to the way that traffic risk
is calculated.
In the second step, an overall risk assessment function associates these higher
level functions with each other and eventually produces the two risk functions
required by the ICA in order to adapt the system's HMI with respect to traffic and
environment risk estimation. This association of the individual higher level risk
functions adopts a weighted-aggregation approach based on experts' opinions and
TERA team's previous experience in the field.
The final overall traffic risk function (TrRisk) can be thus derived using a
weighted sum of the three internal risk functions:
TrRisk = (DangCurveAppr > thres1) x WI + (RiskLFColl > thres2) x W2 +
(RiskRLExit> thres3) x w3
First weights will be defined from experts' opinions and previous experience in
the field and modulated in accordance with real data sets tests.
The frontal/lateral collision risk function has the aim to evaluate the risk due to
an obstacle present on the host-vehicle trajectory. Such a function uses a long-
range radar to assess the distance, the velocity and the angular position of the
objects ahead or aside. The fundamental point is to understand if one (or more)
object(s) detected by the radar in front of the vehicle is dangerous, and in case
it becomes an obstacle, or not. In order to assess this event, the object has to be
inside the driving path (a path wide 1.5 to 2 m, more or less the same width of
an ordinary sedan): If this is the case, it is considered as a potentially dangerous
obstacle. In the method used by conventional ACC systems, one main assumption
is made about the motion of the objects (deterministic method which ignores the
time parameter): The host-vehicle obstacle is regarded as moving with constant
speed (straight uniform motion).
TTC is the time that results from the distance ~x (between leading and rear
bumper) from a leading car (carleading) to the own car (carsystem), divided by the
difference of velocity (vsystem - Vleading) between both of them
tTTC == ~x I.
I vsystem - Vleading
Headway is the time that results from the above-named distance ~x and the
velocity vsystem of the system car
~x
tHeadway == - - .
vsystem
The corresponding warning distances are calculated with a predetermined time
(tTTC and tHeadway) for several dangerous situations (cautionary and imminent). The
larger calculated warning distance is defined as criterion of the system activation.
The determination of the specific thresholds of TTC and headway for both danger-
ous situations is accomplished deriving from NHTSA (see below). The imminent
case applies to
TTC is larger than in the imminent case but not larger than 10 to 14 s (5 s ~
TTC ~ 10 to 14 s);
headway is larger than in the imminent case but not larger than 2 to 3 s (1.5 s ~
headway ~ 2 to 3 s).
392 Amditis, Polychronopoulos and Bekiaris
A
2
r
tY\
v..
h Ay\ ~ iJAlI
o
-1
\A f ~ ~ ( .....
'y~
-2 ~ I
~
-3
~
-4
-5
o 200 400 600 800 1000 1200 1400 1600 1800 2000
I
1
1
I
1
1
I- manouver type I
I
I
1
1 1 I 1 1
0.8 - 1 I
r----------r---------~----------l--
1 1
I
1
1
1
--- --- ---- -----+----------
I
1 I 1 1 I
1 I 1 I 1
-----+----------
I 1 1 I
--- --- ----
1
0.6 - ~----------~---------~----------~--
1 1 1 1 I I
I 1 I 1 I I
I 1 I I I 1
L
I __________ 1
L _________ J I __________ 1 __ 1 _ ____ L1 __________
0.4 - --- --- I ----
~
1 I 1 I 1
1 1 1 I I I
1 1 1 1 I I
1 1 1 I I I
0.2 -
1 1 1 1
r----------~---------4----------~--
I 1 I 1
--- --- 1 ----
I
-----T----------
1
1
I 1 1 I 1 I
I 1 1 I 1 I
o
I I 1 I 1 1
1 1 1
- 1
--- "--~---- 1
----,-
1 1 1 1 I 1
1 1 1 1 I 1
1 1 I I I 1
-0.2 ---r-------
I
I
1
--~---------4---
I
I
1
-
I 1
----~----------~----------T-
I 1
1
I
------ -
1 1 1 1 I 1
1 I 1 1 I 1
___ 1 _______
L 1 _________ J 1___
__ L _ ___ 1__________ I __________ L1 _
- ------ -
~ ~
-0.4 1 1 1 1 I 1
I 1 I I I 1
I 1 I I 1 I
I I 1 1 1 1
-0.6 ---r-------
I I 1 1 1 I
- -r - - - - - - - - - -1- - - - ----~----------~----------T- ------ -
1 1 1 1 1 1
1 1 I 1 I 1
1 1 I I 1 I
___ 1 _______
L __ 1L _________ J I ___ _ ___ 1 __________ I __________ L_
1
-0.8 - ------ -
~ ~
I I 1 I 1 I
I I 1 1 1 1
1 I 1 1 1 1
1 1 1 1 1 1
1 1 1
I-- ~I ~ ,.. - ~ - - - - - - - -1- - '" - ..,...._ _.__ - - _ _...+-.._._ -.............-+
I 1 1
I I 1
I 1 1
0.9 _______ 1 L
1 _ I
--------------- -------------------------
~
1 1 I
1 1 1 1 1
1 1 I I 1
I 1 1 I I 1
1 1 I 1 1 I
0.8 _ _ _ _ _ _ _ L
1
1
1
_ ______ L_____
1
J
I
L
I
L
I
_
1 I 1 1 1 1
,
I 1 1 1 I 1
I , 1 1 I 1
0.7 -- - - - - - - - - - - - - -,-- - - - - - - - -
I , 1
-----.-- 1
--1--------,--------.-------
I I 1
'E
Q) 1 I I 1 I 1
I 1 I 1 1 I
E I 1 1 1 1 1
c 0.6 1
- - - - - - - r - - - - - - - -,- - - - - - - - , - -
1 I
-r
1
----,--------r--------r-------
I I 1
0> 1 I I 1 I I 1
'00 1 I I 1 1 , 1
en 1 I I I , 1
Cl3
0.5 -------r------- ------,--------r--------r-------
1 I I I I 1
-j- - - - - - - - '1 - - - - -
~
1 , , I 1 I
1 , 1 I 1 1
:.0 1
1
1
1
1
,
I
I
,
I
I
1
Cl3
..c 0.4 1 1
a.
0 I 1
1 1
_ _ _ _ _ _ _ L1 1
, I
1 _ _____ L___
1
__J
1
_ LC
0.3 I 1 I 1 1
1
I
1
I
,
,
1
1
1
I
no LC
1 I I 1 1
union
-------:-----
I I I I
0.2 - - - - - - - ,
I
- - - - - - - -1- -
1
- - - - - -
:-----~---------~
1 1 I I
I I I
0.1 1 1 I I '
I I 1 I I
1 I 1 1 I
1 I 1 1 I
0
-0.6 -0.4 -0.2 o 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8
m/s
The basic probability assignment, for each evidence, is calculated through proper
trapezoidal fuzzy membership functions. An example is given in the following
figure for the derivative of the lateral offset. The figure shows that if the value of
the derivative is 0.2 rn/s then 0.9 is the BPA assigned for the lane change and 0.1
for its negation (Fig. 20.4).
Assume that belief function Bel(i) are assigned to independent sources of ev-
idence in same frame of discernment and the relevant basic probability assign-
ment is m i, ma. etc. Then according to Dempster's rule of combination, the new
belief function Bel(A) and basic probability assignment meA) may be yielded
via
LateraIOffeset
velocity ObjectNumber
LaneWindth
yaw rate filtered radial velocity
Lanecurvature
steering angle ObjDistance
LaneCurvatureRate
wiper position ObjAngle
Type of Lane marking
(headlamps) (ObjClass,ObjID)
(neighbourlanes)
JJ
Figuring out the
traffic algorithm which will environment
provide the envisioned
traffic and environment
risk assessment Environmental Risk:
OveraliTrafficRisk: f(weather conditions, type
f(DangCurveAppr, of road), to be reflected in two
RiskFLColI, separate conditions:
RiskRoLExit) - LowVisibility
- Low Audiblityin the Cockpit
(dueto differentHMI adaptation
requested)
FIGURE 20.6. Available signals mapped onto TERA traffic risk functions.
20. Real-Time Traffic and Environment Risk Estimation 397
, e-d _ _
...- _... ., .,
r
C.aIiIDl :
~o.,..,:
055
005 ...
FIGURE 20.7. A screenshot from 'Fuzzy TERA development tool 1.0 ' laboratory tool.
The algorithms both for risk assessment and driver intention have been im-
plemented in MATLAB and C++. A tool (fuzzy TERA development tool 1.0)
has been developed that loads data sequences and video files in a playback
mode, synchronises data , runs off-line the TERA algorithm and displa ys re-
sults in a graphical user interface. The tool work s both with real (recorded from
test cars and truck s) and simulated data and it is developed for evaluation pur-
poses. The tool (Fig. 20.7) shows internal traffic risk conditions' values, video
and mark s whether the driver is considered to be in high traffic risk situation
(three detection levels). The TERA traffic risk conditions' threshold s are man-
ually tuned by varying several thresholds and weights and comparing the algo-
rithm with recorded video s of real driving. The algorithm for the detection of
possible manoeuvres is integrated in the same GUI. The C++ version is in-
tegrated in an automotive proce ssing unit and will run in AIDE demonstrator
vehicles.
20.4 Conclusion s
The paper presented a novel approach on the real-time assess ment of the traffic
and environmental situation with respect to the risk that the driver is exposed
to. The work focuses on the descripti on of the relevant risk in a higher level of
398 Amditis, Polychronopoulos and Bekiaris
References
Amditis, A., Polychronopoulos, A., Belotti, F. and Montanari R. (2002a). Strategy plan
definition for the management of the information flow through an HMI unit inside a car.
In e-Safety Conference Proceedings, Lyon.
Amditis, A., Polychronopoulos, A., Bekiaris, E. and Antonello, C. (2002b). System archi-
tecture of a driver's monitoring and hypovigilance warning system. In IEEE Intelligent
Vehicle Symposium Versailles, France.
Bekiaris, E. and Portouli, E. (1999). Driver warning strategies Internal Deliverable ID4.1.
IN-ARTE Project TR4014.
Damiani, S., Antonello, C., Tango, F. and Saroldi, A. (2003). Functional specification and
architecture for the traffic risk estimation module. In Proceedings SICICA 2003, Aveiro,
Portugal.
Engstrom, 1., Cacciabue, C., Janssen, W., Amditis, A., Andreone, L., Bengler, K., Eschler,
1. and Nathan, F. (2004). The AIDE integrated project: An overview. In Proceedings of
ITS Congress, Budapest.
European Commission. (2001). White Paper on European Transport Policy for 2010: Time
to decide.
Horne, J. and Reyner, L. (1999). Vehicle accidents related to sleep: A review. Occupational
and Environmental Medicine, 56, 289-294.
Kirchner, A. and Heinrich, T. (1998). Model based detection of road boundaries with a laser
scanner. In Proceedings ofIEEE International Conference on Intelligent Vehicles, Vol. 1,
Stuttgart, Germany, 1998, 93-98.
Montanari, R. et al. (2002). COMUNICAR: Integrated on-vehicle human machine interface
designed to avoid driver information overload. In ITS Conference, Chicago, ILO.
20. Real-Time Traffic and Environment Risk Estimation 399
21.1 Introduction
In transportation research, simulation technologies have acquired a huge relevance
since they permit to reproduce, under controllable conditions, different scenarios
with a growing degree of complexity. In this sense, a general trend can be antici-
pated here since it represents one of the backbone of this paper. The simulators of
traffic scenario are enlarging the numbers of factors to be considered, from the sole
link between vehicles and driving environment to a joint scenario in which drivers'
intentions, autonomous behaviours of different vehicles and adaptive technologies
(providing ad-hoc reactions according to specific driving conditions) are all to-
gether considered and computed. Therefore, the more the complexity grows, the
more the network of factors influencing the reliability of these scenarios becomes
articulated.
Proposing different working cases (some of them still under research), this paper
tries to describe the main technological features of the traffic simulator model, the
corresponding design approaches and the uses that these simulators offer towards
the research community.
According to the results of the surveys done while writing this paper, a key
point in which a reliable simulation is not yet completely achieved (even if many
benchmark initiatives are acquiring promising results) is a study of drivers' be-
haviours and prediction of drivers' status, especially in terms of workload. This
is a crucial challenge since once the level of workload will be properly and in
real time monitored, the researchers will have the chance to actually improve safe
systems, reducing the number (still too high 1) of vehicles' related fatalities. Since
the rationale behind a workload monitoring system seems to be mainly heuristi-
cally detected (Balaban et al., 2004; Dixon et al., 2005; Recarte and Nunes, 2003;
Schvaneveldt et al., 1998; Zhang et al., 2004; Zhang et al., 2004), simulation
plays a decisive role in their assessment and in the evaluation of the most suitable
1 From many accident statistics carried out at European project level (PREVENT, AIDE,
etc.), each year there are around 1,400,000 accidents, out of which 40,000 are fatalities.
400
21. Present and Future of Simulation Traffic Models 401
and qualitative information referring to the evolution of the simulation. With this
knowledge background, a visual representation is created, and it provides an idea
regarding the state of the simulated environment. The main fields of applications
for a road traffic simulator are the following:
Design and improvement of car equipment, where the simulator can be a powerful
tool for the on-board system evaluation. In fact, it allows to save time (and thus
money) among the different phases of a product, from the concept to the series
development, in particular between the validation with the proof of concept
and the sign off with the begin of production. In the evaluation of innovative
steering devices, the driving simulator has been used (e.g. in Toffin et al., 2003) to
investigate different torque feedbacks in the steering wheel. Results indicate that
drivers on the simulator can control their vehicles in curves with quite different
torque feedback strategies, either linear or nonlinear. However, zero torque or
inverted torque feedback makes driving almost impossible. These observations,
which cannot be implemented in real driving conditions, confirm the essential
role of coherent haptic information for driving real cars and simulators and
also suggest the existence of driver adaptation mechanisms in steering control.
Finally, simulators are used in the context of speech-controlled driver information
systems. Manstetten et al. (2001) studied how drivers interact with such a system
if it allows for natural-language communication in simulation environment. The
present contribution to the book on driver's model is more focused on this point.
The other concepts that are also considered as follows:
Training - real-time simulator is increasingly used to educate and train profes-
sional drivers (trucks, buses, etc.) and dedicated personnel (i.e. traffic control
centre) (Dois et aI., 2002).
Testing new structure - to quantify performances according to different design
options before the works start or before the commitment of resource for con-
struction.
Security and environment - with some applications in this area dealing with
studies on intelligent highways and vehicles (Sukthankar et al., 1996; 1998),
impact of road signs and infrastructure towards the driving tasks (Fitzpatrick,
2000; Horberry et al., 2004; Horberry and May, 1994; Horberry et al., 2005),
driver's behaviour analysis in critical scenarios (Pentland et al., 1999; Salvucci
et al., 2001), accident reconstruction, as well as researches on emissions and
pollution.
Studies on drivers' distraction - Karlson (2004) studied driver distraction and its
countermeasures in driving simulator evaluating the gaze direction. Lansdown
et al. (2004) investigated the impact of multiple in-vehicle information systems
on the driver, undertaken using a high fidelity driving simulator. Martens and
Winsum (2000) investigated driver's distraction in simulator using the peripheral
detection task (PDT,2 Olsson and Burns, 2000), that is a very sensitive method of
measuring peaks in workload, induced by either a critical scenario or messages
provided by a driver support system. The more demanding the task, the more
cues will be missed and the longer the response times to the PDT.
Rehabilitation of driving skills for persons with neurological compromise -
Rizzo et al. (2002) performed a study of 54 individuals (21 with traumatic
brain injury, 13 with stroke and 20 healthy controls) who were administered a
driving evaluation using the driving simulator (mainly based on virtual reality
technologies).
Basic research - traffic simulation is used for mathematical and statistical studies,
with the aim to improve traffic flow models, analyse interesting aspects, such as
emergent collective behaviour, swarm intelligence topics and so on (Leonardi
et al., 2004).
opportunity to take into account all the interactions occurring among vehicle, driver
and traffic (that is, the environment). Another practical example is called simulated
highways for intelligent vehicle algorithms (SHIVA): a tool for simulation, design
and development of tactical driving algorithms (for more details, see Sukthankar
et al. (1995)).
Finally, the mesoscopic approach lies somewhat in between. It captures some
microscopic behaviour of traffic such as car-following, but with this approach the
general characteristics of traffic (i.e. speed, flow and density, etc.) are examined.
In other words, mesoscopic model generally represents most entities at a high
level of detail, but describes their activities and interactions at a lower level. For
example, lane change manoeuvres are represented but could be performed as an
instantaneous event (Kemeny, 1993).
Another classification of simulators groups them into two sets: deterministic and
stochastic. Deterministic simulators have no random variables; all entities inter-
actions are defined through exact relationships (mathematical or logical). On the
contrary, stochastic simulators include probability functions. Deterministic mod-
els are well suited to experiments wherein scenarios are intended to be completely
reproducible. On the other hand, the vehicle behaviour may appear too mechanical
and monotonous, thus realism is somehow lessened (Champion et aI., 1999).
A relevant role in the traffic simulator is represented by the programming
methodology that should be used to create the traffic scenario. This can be pro-
grammed in a sequential way3, as for some macroscopic simulators. On the con-
trary, this can be also designed according to object-oriented principles, as for many
microscopic simulators. Starting from this approach, some relevant breakthroughs
have been achieved in the last 10 years, thanks to the development of the so-called
'distributing artificial intelligent' (Brodie and Ceri, 1992). In particular, the most
recent and widely used of these innovations is the agent-oriented method. The
traffic generator mentioned in Champion et al. (1999) has been designed using an
object-oriented method where several software agents are embedded (detailed in
the next section).
Concerning the way to compute and reproduce the driving task, three are the
main relevant levels: strategic, tactical and operational (Michon, 1985). At the
highest (strategic) level, a route is planned and goals are determined; at the inter-
mediate (tactical) level, manoeuvres are selected to achieve short-term objectives
(i.e. deciding whether to pass a blocking vehicle); eventually, at the lowest level,
these manoeuvres are translated into control operations.
Consider the typical scenario depicted in Fig. 21.1: In the figure, the host-
vehicle" (A) is in the right lane of a divided highway, approaching the chosen exit.
Unfortunately, a slow car (B) blocks the lane, preventing (A) from moving at its
preferred velocity. If A desires to overtake (pass) the slower car, a conflict can
arise with the necessity of not missing the exit. The correct decision in this case
3 That is, each program has a beginning, an execution sequence, and an end. At any given
time during the runtime of the program, there is a single point of execution.
4 With host-vehicle, it is meant the vehicle considered, i.e. the one under evaluation.
21. Present and Future of Simulation Traffic Models 405
A c:::J Be=]-
depends not only on the distance to the exit, but also on the traffic configuration
in the area. Even if the distance to the exit is sufficient for a pass, there may be no
suitable gaps in the right lane ahead before the exit. Thu s tactical level rea soning
combines high-level goal s with real-time sensor con straints in an uncertain envi-
ronment. Simulation is essential in developing such systems becau se testing new
algorithms in real traffic is risky and potentially disastrou s. For example, SHIVA
not only model s the elements of the driving domain most useful to designers but
also provides tool s to rapidly prototype and test algorithms in challenging traffic
situations.
Following are some other examples: PHAROS (Reece and Shafer, 1988),
SMARTPATH (Michon, 1985) and SMARTAHS (Gollu, 1995). PHAROS focused
on important perceptu al issues and directed SHIVAs earl y development. SMART-
PATH is well suited to modelling the PATH AHS con cept (even with large numbers
of vehicles) and it uses S0I 5 animatio n package for visualisation too ls (Sukthankar
et aI., 1995). SMARTAHS is an objec t-oriented simulator, which stores its evolu-
tion state in a persistent DBMS 6 .
As discu ssed above, the most recent advances in distributed artificial intelligence
have allowed to study systems charac terised by auton om ous entities, such as a
navigation by autonomous agents for inter section management is investigated in
Dresner and Stone (2004), where multi- agent systems (MA S) is the sub-field of
artificial intelligent (AI) aiming at providing both principles for construction of
complex systems involving multiple agents and mechanism for coordination of
independent behaviour of agents. In Stone and Veloso (2000), a MAS-approach is
used to alleviate traffic con gestion, basically at intersections.
. --- --- -- -- - - - -- . .
,/' Traffic
Interactively
driven vehicle
behind this simulator is that the traffic is the union of the driven vehicle and the
autonomous traffic, which interact mutually. The main design features are:
With reference to the classifications disscused in the previous section , the model
implemented is microscopic with discrete time (possible to set, default value is
20 Hz) and stochastic. The architectural scheme is detailed in Champion et al.
(1999) and illustrated in Fig. 21.2. This architecture allows the parallel execution
of different traffic generation modules within the limits of the network capacity, if
an experiment requires a relatively high-density of traffic (few hundred vehicles) .
Each module is supported by a different machine and manages a subset of vehicles.
Regarding the simulator environment, there are two fundamental elements: the
road network and signs (RNS) and vehicle model properties, used for the de-
scription of the autonomous vehicles in the simulator. We will now discuss RNS,
whereas vehicle model properties is described in the next section.
RNS is used by the autonomous vehicles to drive on the terrain; it is defined by
a set of roads connected to each other through road nodes. Each road can have the
following features: Width, different lanes for one or two directions, road markings,
21. Present and Future of Simulation Traffic Models 407
speed limits, road authorisations (for pedestrians, no trucks, bus only and so on),
road signs (stop, traffic lights, yield, pedestrian ways, etc.), barriers, etc.
RNS has two objects: on the road side, to order list of points, minimum two,
usually placed at the centre of an actual road; on the nodes side, this is a point placed
to connect two or more roads. Each road must be connected with two nodes: one at
the beginning and one at the ending. A road cannot be connected to the same node.
Moreover, each rode can have the following attributes: width, category, length, list
of lanes. Then, each lane can be defined by offset relative to the middle of the road,
initial and final width, direction relative to the road (same or opposite direction),
driving rules like speed limits, left or right markings, vehicle categories enabled
for the lane, etc.
In each point of a road, it is possible to place one or more signs, which refer to
a lane. Road surface database is used to compute tyre contact with the road and
consequent reactions. Usually users can set some parameters such as adherence
factor (to be used by dynamic models), spatial noise factor (how bumpy is the
road), road type (normal road, sidewalk, car park, etc.), and road nature (asphalt,
snow, concrete, etc.). Road surface and user defined information are queried by
the simulation and enter in the vehicle model to compute the behaviour of the
system, the reaction of the vehicle itself and possible feedbacks to the user. Finally,
environment objects are embedded in boxes named collision boxes used in order
to detect collision between the driving vehicle and the environment.
I Cogn itio n I
I Act uation II+- Host-vehicle - --
1-----< Perceptio n Interactively Autonomous
Autonomous I--------e
Vehicle ~
Vehicle
\ ~
Simulated World
I \ I
I Resouces :::> Resouces
I
FIGURE 21 .3. Structure of the vehicles constituting traffic flow.
This model is not sequential; this may result from several iterations (cyclical)
of the other functions. Moreover, in agreement with the initial hypothesis, the
planning function is usually bypassed by the 'automatic' selection of familiar
frames of knowledge that are associated with procedures or sets of several actions
aiming at the fulfilment of the goal of a frame. This function is howeverimportant
as it becomes effective in unknown situations or in the case of novice drivers,
when 'simpler' frames, based on single actions or on a limited sequence of very
simple/familiar actions, are used to deal with the (new) situation.
These four cognitive functions can be associated to either sensorial or cognitive
processes and are activated according to certain rules or conditions (see Table
21.1).
Comparing the PIPE framework with the Fig. 21.3, it is possible to say that:
In the next section, more details on the three aforementioned elements are pointed
out.
The outputs of the sensors can be corrupted by noise if desired. Some sensors are
explored in greater depth, e.g. vehicle detection in particular, since it is of critical
importance at the tactical level.
In SCANeR II, perception is based on the road network since it is used for
trajectory calculations and acquisition of information. The road network designer
should pay attention to the fact that the information has to be available to the vehi-
cles (road sections, signs positioning); moreover, two vehicles have to know each
other's status (position, speed, acceleration and direction). Autonomous vehicles
also use the knowledge of the future route of the other vehicles to foresee their
behaviour when approaching an intersection. Furthermore, designers may select
8 Drivers rely on their experience with the driving task and perceiving the road environment
relies on top-down expectations (Theeuwes, 2002). For example, Theeuwes and Hagen-
zieker (1993) have shown that drivers expect that objects that are likely to appear in a given
scene should occupy specific positions in that scene. Results from their study also showed
that errors in perceiving objects occurred when road users had wrong expectations regard-
ing the location of particular target objects. Therefore, extremely dangerous situations may
occur when the design of the traffic environment induces incorrect expectations regarding
the spatial arrangement of the object in that scene.
410 Tango, Montanari and Marzani
different types of vehicle sensors or a more abstract vehicle sensor that can be
used to prototype reasoning schemes. Once the useful information is obtained
(road curve, signs and marking, position and trajectory relative to the perceived
vehicles, etc.), a vehicle goes into the cognition phase. This phase corresponds to
the reasoning and to the decision-making processes (see also next section).
Text in italics indicate elementary function and text in bold indicate tasks.
then vehicle (A) has to decide whether it can change lane to overtake vehicle (B)
and take exits or simply to follow (B) and wait for the same exit. It is worth to note
that tactical-level is of great importance for the feeling of realism of a simulator
and thus designers should pay particular attention to this issue.
Finally, at the operational level, if (A) decides to overtake (B), then it has to
compute the speed and the relative lateral offset appropriate with the execution of
the lane-change manoeuvre.
Always focusing on task-analysis another example is the so-called 'goals-means
task analysis' (GMTA), (see also Hollnagel (1993), where more details are pro-
vided). In this context, Table 21.3 shows a general instance for its application.
All in all, one of the major difficulties is the hand-tuning of the reason-
ing/deciding object parameters. Thereby, a careful investigation of learning tech-
niques is one of the main activities to carry out in order to make this task automated.
Potential solutions are described in Baluja (1994).
Aerodynamic
Forces
Comfort
(vibration due to road
surface)
Road
DB
interfaces studies, including both innovative devices for primary and secondary
task. The simulator and the corresponding research activities are carried out by the
Human Machine Interaction Group of University of Modena and Reggio Emilia 10
to design, prototype, test and develop innovative solutions for mechatronic systems, such
as control by-wire applications, human factors for mechatronics' systems (e.g. Ergonomic
by-wire steering-wheels), the study of fluid power for mechatronics and advanced materials.
10 www.hmi.unimore.it.
414 Tango, Montanari and Marzani
have their own desired speed, some vehicles move closer and thus begin to interact
with each other. As this type of interactions increases, deceleration and accelera-
tion waves are propagated upstream. On the other hand, if traffic is string-stable,
these waves would be attenuated. Therefore, traffic density is expected to be a
fundamental factor in wave propagation velocity, with average speed that mono-
tonically decreases as density increases. In high-density case, emergency braking
algorithms are used with much greater frequency.
In this section only a general and brief overview of the main results achieved
by a microscopic traffic simulator have been presented, in order to evaluate the
characteristics of the longitudinal driver's model. More details on this research are
available in Lee (2004). All in all, a modified Gipps model has been used to describe
the longitudinal human driving behaviour (and so the' personality' of each vehicle-
entity in the simulator). Traffic flow simulations for several traffic densities were
carried out: the resulting traffic flow and the average velocity results agree with real
traffic data. This means that the modified Gipps model and all the related parameters
can represent in a satisfactory way the macroscopic traffic characteristics, such as
traffic flow rate and average velocity with respect to traffic density. The impact
of intelligent transportation systems in this context is expected to be high and to
influence the traffic rate as well as the speed of shockwave propagation.
This section presents a research carried out by the Human Factors Laboratory of
the MECTRON project (details on MECTRON are reported above). As previously
introduced, the Human Factors Laboratory is led by the HMI Group of University
of Modena and Reggio Emilia.
The main objective of this research is to develop specifications and requirements
of a steer-by-wire device, which has the peculiarity to give primary importance
to the human factors and ergonomics aspects (a so-called ergonomic steer-by-
wire, i.e. ESBW). In particular, this is illustrated and investigated in terms of
driving quality and performances, vehicle controllability 11 and human cognitive
functionalities (such as mental workload and situational awareness). Here, only
the experimental set-up concerning the use of traffic and environment simulation
is pointed out, where the main scenarios are described.
At the moment, the experiments are in course. The basic idea of this work is that
a by-wire device is able to support properly the driving task only if the drivers'
perception in the usage of this system remains the same of a traditional steering
wheel.
11This concept is used as within the ED Integrated Project PREVENT (and particularly it
sub-project RESPONSE 3). For more information, see the web site www.prevent-ip.org/
en/prevent_subprojects/horizontal_activities/response_3 .
21 . Presentand Future of Simulation Traffic Models 415
recon struct the driving scenario (route , weather conditions, traffic density) in a
reliable way and in real-time;
project the scenarios on the dedicated screen ;
perform (and promote) the interaction between user and system, by means of a
reproduction of the automobile cockpit, the use of steering wheel, accelerator
and brake pedals, with force feedbacks and feed forward (to reproduce the same
feeling and dynamics of a car) .
In the aforementioned experiment by this tool, the rules describing the behaviour
of the force-feedback and force-feed forward actuator in the ESBW are under
investigation. To perform such analysis, a previous benchmark on the different
types of steering devices and reactive torque algorithms was evaluated. The analysis
focused on two different kinds of force-feedback; one concerning reactive torque
algorithms based on vehicle behaviour (e.g. lateral acceleration, yaw rate, sideslip
angle) , the other one based only on steering-wheel dynamic behaviour (e.g. steering
angle, steering rate) . In literature, both were investigated from a human factor
point of view, that is the results of tests of driver performances in different driving
scenarios (e.g. lane change, car following, highway, rural road) under primary
and/or secondary tasks . The comparison among these results allowed to define the
best user-centred algorithms, then to design different solutions of an ergonomic
steer-by-wire featuring several force-feedback models . Finally, these prototypes
416 Tango, Montanari and Marzani
are in course of implementation on the steering wheel of the driving simulator; the
aim is to evaluate, during driving tests, the most performing algorithm in terms
of driver response. These assessments are performed comparing the following
dependant variables:
At the moment, experiments are in progress and it is not possible to anticipate any
results. Nevertheless, the simulator will be used not only as an assessment tool but
also to observe how the ESBW can become a supportive system aimed at improving
drivers' situation awareness in specific conditions (e.g. via the implementation of
proactive supportive actions as a major resistance of the steering-wheel in case
the drivers is trying to change the lane, risking a collision with a vehicles which
is already occupying the adjacent lane). These studies will be carried out in the
late phase of MECTRON project, once the ESBW assessment will be properly
completed.
Traffic simulators aim at investigating the impact of these adaptive interfaces 13
with reference to drivers' reactions and expectations; this can represent a valuable
perspective for these tools. Nevertheless, the only simulation, based on traffic,
could not be enough: thus a mutual interaction with environment, drivers' models
and vehicles is needed, as described in details in the next section (introducing
another relevant case study of AIDE project) and regained in the conclusions.
Speed
Acceleration
Yaw-Rate
Deviation
~
FIG URE 21.7. The SHELL configur-
ation.
E
I
L L H
\
l~
S
S Softwa re
H Hardware
E Enviro nme nt
L Liveware
Inside the AIDE project (and particularly in the sub-project which is taking care
of the simulation related topics, i.e. the sub-project 115 ) , the development of the
architecture adopted to represent the interactions between driver, vehicle and en-
vironment is framed in a generic architecture describing the way in which human s
interact with the world and systems around them (Panou et al., 2005). This is
called SHELL (Edwards, 198 8), whose structure describe s the connections exist-
ing between humans , defined Liveware (L), and the other elements of the work-
ing environments (driving, in this case), as described in the following list and in
Fig. 21.7:
The model focu ses on the way in which a process of interaction is influenced
and can be simulated with respect to a single journey. The model intends to deal
Static variables, which account for variables that do not change over the journey
(examples of these are age of driver, gender, personality, procedures, etc.).
Quasi-Static variables, which account for the phenomena and interactions that
may change during ajourney even though these changes are slow and foreseeable
(examples of quasi-static variables are attitudes, behavioural adaptation, etc.).
Dynamic variables, which account for the events and phenomena that occur
during a journey and may be affected by the evolution of the DVE interaction
itself or may not be anticipated (examples of dynamic variables are workload,
stress, traffic conditions, type of roads, weather conditions, traffic situations,
etc.).
~
~lale OfD (IJ SlaleOf~7IJ/
.J 1=1,+1'11
EN VIRON M ENT
dynamic evolution of both vehicle and environment can be recorded and compared
to the simulated data (or derived by them) .
All in all, the activity on DVE model simulator is still in progress and therefore
current formulation may be not cover a number of variables and cases which then
could become very important. This will be clarified by the experiments envisaged
inside the project AIDE, particular in the topic-related subproject.
However, the overview provided in this section is enough to describe the main
idea and objective of the DVE simulator and the rationale of a promising perspec-
tive for the traffic simulator: An integration of the three main actors involved in
the driving scenario that is the driver, the vehicle and the environment. In this case,
the main topic is not the development (at least, not only) of a specific algorithm
for the simulation of traffic/environment or the implementation of a new driver's
model, but to point out the interaction of these three elements, in an integrated
approach.
All these three items can be handled by two different agents. Initially, a user's
profile configuration agent will support different 'types of users' , with some pref-
erences selected by the users themselves. Then, a customisation agent will monitor
the user's driving behaviour and preferences/actions, by keeping and processing
the user's driving record, i.e. average position in the lane, average headway, typical
speeding and braking pattern, preferred seating position, average use of radio and
mobile phone, other services, like navigation, requested often, etc. The self-built
user profile will be always possible to be reviewed and changed by the user.
The Java agent development framework (JADE) is the basis for building inter-
operable agents FIPA 16 compliant. Such a framework ensures that agents' aspects
(message transport, encoding, parsing, agent life cycle, etc.) are dealt. Data will be
stored in a driver smart card (for example), in order to be used in other equipped
cars. This personalisation can help to better estimate the DIL, because it considers
also the effects due to the differences between different driver's typology (recalling
the PIPE framework, at the execution level, as pointed out by the AIDE project).
Furthermore, also taking into account the effects produced by the use of ADAS
and IVIS applications can be included in the human model (i.e. in terms of be-
havioural adaptation), thus enlarging the applicative scenarios of use of the simu-
lation.
All these aspects can contribute to make easier and more realistic the creation
and implementation of driver-vehicle systems as autonomous agents, in order to
deal with a wide variety of traffic conditions and applicative scenarios.
The other item to be considered is the environment, which is closely related to
the local perception in simulated world. We have already described the so-called
RNS framework, where the road network has to be apprehended according to the
local environment of the agent (vehicles ahead, road type, road layout, etc.). In
order to fulfil this perception issue, it is necessary to take into consideration the
interaction between the driven vehicle (that is, the vehicle with more sophisticated
human model and specific system on-board) and the other autonomous agents. The
study and the implementation of the environment model is one of the main tasks
for a traffic simulator, since it could not be too different from real scenarios, but on
the other side a complete imitation of real world is not possible. Therefore several
approaches are used, both considering particular aspects of the traffic management
(as in Dresner et al. (2004) and Qi (1997)), investigating specific types of on-board
systems as collision warning, adaptive cruise control, etc. (Christen and Neunzig,
2002; Miller and Huang, 2002; Ozguner et al., 2004). The ambitious goal of joint-
DVE approach is point towards the merging of these works, including the impact
of ADAS/IVIS applications with a traffic model.
paper has been about the perception issue: even if data are sometimes insufficient
(as quantity) or erroneous (as in real life), a perception based on (simulated)
visual information can alone enable the vehicles to communicate each other their
objectives. For example, if a simulated scenario involves a motorway network,
an insertion has to be simulated (maybe with a congestion caused by an accident
occurred downstream) and vehicles should be able to perceive surrounding entities
intentions and to accord with (for example, entering the motorway, changing lane
manoeuvre, etc.). In other words, it is necessary to implement a structure that
enables an agent to apprehend and answer any request concerning a resource
access: If it is common and shared, agents have to cooperate in order to coordinate
their actions. In case vehicles do not have this capacity and even if they observe the
highway code, a vehicle wishing to access the highway does not have any priority
and thus it risks staying stuck on the access-ramp.
Many architectures nowadays do not allow a cooperative behaviour immedi-
ately. As pointed out, several aspects have to be considered and merged together,
concerning the perception, the cognition and the interaction. They need to be im-
plemented following a DAI/MAS approach. Whilst the agents have only a reactive
behaviour, it is possible to note a multi -agents coordination: in the example pre-
viously shown, flow of vehicle is strongly slowed down, which is normal, but it is
not stopped. In other words, it can be said that an emergent behaviour arises and
different research works can be carried out on this item.
To sum up, considering traffic as a multi-agent system allows to simulate a more
realistic but simulated environment, with which also (more) complex scenarios can
be analysed, by starting a coordination of traffic entities. This environment model
can be coherent with a homogenous integration of the interactively driven vehicles,
using a joint approach of user's behavioural and personalisation model (including
the presence of driving supporting systems on-board). This is the actual area of
research and work of some projects, such as AIDE. In this context, the environment
model is built by considering driving behavior from the point of view of how drivers
perceive, attend and memorize environmental conditions to make choices and take
proper actions to those conditions.
One of the possible future activities is to model how these conditions are related
to risk factors, in order to determine which are the most critical scenarios. There-
fore, the DVE model should include all those parameters from the environment
which drivers indicate as the most attention demanding. This type of environment
model is synthesised into a preliminary joint DVE model, as illustrated before.
Another one, and related to the previous by a certain viewpoint, concerns the
evaluation and assessment of predictive models of driver's workload, in different
contexts and using specific hap-tic devices (such as the steer-by-wire prototype,
as aforementioned).
It is worth to add, however, that a DASIMAS approach cannot be regarded as
a panacea for each problem in simulation, because drawbacks and shortcomings
exist in this case as well; in fact, possible criticism could be related to the use of
neural networks performance as a decision-making framework, since sometimes
revealed limits when applied in real-time systems due to the time spent in learning
phase if any and/or in the evaluation of proper solution among a list of alternatives.
424 Tango, Montanari and Marzani
Therefore, the activities on these topics are continuously running. Some sugges-
tions to overcome possible difficulties of tuning the reasoning object parameters
concern the investigation of learning techniques, such as in Baluja (1994), where
an abstraction of the basic genetic algorithm, the equilibrium genetic algorithm
(EGA), is reconsidered within the framework of competitive learning. This paper
explores population-based incremental learning (PBIL), a method of combining
the mechanisms of a generational genetic algorithm with simple competitive learn-
ing. Moreover, an empirical analysis is presented, with a description of a class of
problems in which a genetic algorithm approach may be able to perform better.
Among the cases discussed in this document, the DVE simulator could be a possi-
ble field of application, especially the part concerning the traffic and environment
agents, based on a training data set constituted by real road data from several
driving session performed by different drivers in different scenarios.
References
Balaban, C.D., Cohn, r.v, Redfern, M.S., Prinkey, r, Stripling, R. and Hoffer, M. (2004).
Postural control as a probe for cognitive state: Exploiting human information processing
to enhance performance. International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, 17,
275-286.
Baluja, S. (1994). Population-based incremental learning: A method for integrating genetic
search based function optimisation and competitive learning. Technical Report CMU-
CS-94-163. Cornegie Mellow University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA.
Bertacchini, A., Minin, L., Pavan, P. and Montanari, R. (2006). Ergonomic steer by wire:
Caratteristiche e modalita funzionale. Deliverable R5.1, MECTRON Laboratory, Reggio
Emilia, Italy.
Brodie, M.L. and Ceri, S. (1992). On intelligent and cooperative information systems: A
workshop summary. International Journal of Intelligent and Cooperative Information
Systems, 1(2),249-289.
Cacciabue, P.C. (2004). Guide to Applying Human Factors Methods-Reference Model of
Cognition Chapter, Springer, Berlin.
Champion, A., Mandiau, R., Kolski, C., Heidet, A. and Kemeny, A. (1999). Traffic genera-
tion with the SCANeR II simulator: Towards a multi-agent architecture. In Proceedings
of DSC'99, Paris, France, pp. 311-324.
Christen, F. and Neunzig, D. (2002). Analysis of ACC and stop and go using the simulation
tool PELOPS. Final Technical Report IKA.
Dixon, K.R., Lippitt, C.E. and Forsythe, lC. (2005). Modelling human recognition of
vehicle-driving situations as a supervised machine learning task. In Proceedings of the
11th Conference on Human-Computer Interaction.
Dols, r, Pardo, r, Breker, S., Arno, P., Bekiaris, E., Ruspa, C. and Francone, N. (2002). The
TRAINER project: Experimental validation of a simulator for driver training. In Driving
Simulation Conference, Paris, France.
Dresner, K. and Stone, P. (2004). Multiagent traffic management: A reservation-based inter-
section control mechanism. In The Third International Joint Conference on Autonomous
Agents and Multiagent Systems, New York, USA.
Drury, C.G., Paramore, B., Van Cott, H.P., Grey, S.M. and Corlett, E.N. (1987). Task
analysis. In G. Salvendy (Ed.). Handbook of Human Factors. J Wiley, New York, pp.
370-401.
21. Present and Future of Simulation Traffic Models 425
Edwards, E. (1988). Introductory overview. In E.L. Wiener, and D.C. Nagel (Eds.). Human
Factors in Aviation. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, pp. 3-25.
European Commission (2002). Final report of the esafety working group on road safety.
Information Society Technologies, Bruxelles.
Ferrarini, C. (2005). Analisi comparata dell'usabilita di un software per la supervisione e
il controllo dei processi industriali: il caso Moviconx. MS Thesis, University of Modena
and Reggio Emilia.
Fitzpatrick, K. (2000). Alternative design consistency rating methods for two-lane rural
highways. FHWA Report RD-99-172.
Fuller, R. (2005). Towards a general theory of driver behavior. Accident Analysis and
Prevention, 37(3),461-472.
Gipps, P.G. (1981). A behavioural car-following model for computer simulation. Trans-
portation Research, 15B, 105-111.
Gollu, A. (1995). Object Management Systems. PhD Thesis.
Helbing, D. and Treiber, M. (1998). Jams, Waves and Clusters. Science, 282, 200-201.
Hoedemaeker, M., De Ridder, S. and Jahnsenn, W. (2002). Review of European human
factors research on adaptive interface technologies for automobiles. TNO Report, TM-
02-C031.
Hollnagel, E. (1993). Human Reliability Analysis: Context and Control. Academic Press,
London.
Horberry, T., Anderson, 1., Regan, M. and Tomasevic, N. (2004). A driving simulator
evaluation of enhanced road markings. In Proceedings of the Road Safety Research,
Policing and Education Conference, Perth, Australia. Peer reviewed paper.
Horberry, T. and May, J. (1994). SpaD Human Factors Study: Directory ofRelevant Liter-
ature. Applied Vision Research Unit. University of Derby. UK
Horberry, T., Regan, M. and Anderson, 1. (2005). The possible safety benefits of
enhanced road markings: A driving simulator evaluation. Transportation Research
Part F, 9, 77-87.
Kangwon, LJ. (2004). Longitudinal Driver Model and Collision Warning and Avoidance
Algorithms Based on Human Driving Database. PhD Thesis. UMI 2004.
Karlson, R. (2004). Evaluating driver distraction countermeasures. Master Thesis in Cog-
nitive Science, Cognitive Science Study Program, Linkopings universitet, Sweden.
Kemeny A (1993) A cooperative driving simulator. In Proceedings of the International
Training Equipment Conference and Exhibition Conference (ITEC), London, pp. 67-71.
Lansdown, T.C., Brook-Carter, N. and Kersloot, T. (2004). Distraction from multiple in-
vehicle secondary tasks: Vehicle performance and mental workload implications. Er-
gonomics, 47, 91-104.
Leonardi, L., Mamei, M. and Zambonelli, F. (2004). Co-fields: Towards a unifying model for
swarm intelligence. In Engineering Societies in the Agents World III: Third International
Workshop (Vol. 2577, pp. 68-81). Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence. Springer-
Verlag, Berlin.
Liang, C.Y. and Peng, H. (2000). String stability analysis of adaptive cruise controlled
vehicles. JSME International Journal Series C. 43(3), 671-677.
Lieberman, E. and Rathi, A.K. (1997). Traffic simulation. InN.H. Gartner, CJ. Messer and
A. Rathi (Eds.). Traffic Flow Theory (chapter 10). Federal Highway Administration and
Oak Ridge National Laboratory.
Manstetten, D., Krautter, W., Grothkopp, B., Steffens, F. and Geutner, P. (2001). Using
a driving simulator to perform a wizard-of-oz experiment on speech-controlled driver
information systems. In Proceedings of the Human Centered Transportation Simulation
Conference (HCTSC 2001), Iowa City.
426 Tango, Montanari and Marzani
Martens, M.H. and van Winsum, W. (2000). Martens, M. H. and van Winsum, W.
(1999). Measuring distraction: The peripheral detection task. Online paper. Available at
www.nrd.nhtsa.dot..gov/departments/nrd-13/driver-distractionlwelcome.htm.
Michon, J .A. (1985). A critical view of driver behaviour models: What do we know, what
should we do? In L. Evans and R. Schwing (Eds.). Human Behaviour and Traffic Safety.
Plenum Press, New York, pp. 485-520.
Miller, R. and Huang, Q. (2002). An adaptive peer-to-peer collision warning systems. IEEE
Vehicular Technology Conference (VTC). Birmingham, AL, USA.
Minin, L. (2005). Prototipazione virtuale nell'interazione uomo-macchina: Benchmark
analysis e testcomparati per supportare la scelta dei software di prototipazione virtuale
piu adeguati a differenti tipologie di interfacce utente. MS Thesis, University of Modena
and Reggio Emilia.
Neunzig, D., Breuer, K. and Ehmanns, D. (2002). Traffic and vehicle technologies. Assess-
ment with simulator PELOPS. ISSE Conference.
Ni, D. and Feng, C. (2002). A two-dimensional traffic simulation model. In Proceedings of
the 7th Driving Simulation Conference.
Nilsson, L. (1995). Safety effects of adaptive cruise controls in critical traffic situations. In
Proceedings of The Second World Congress on Intelligent Transport Systems. pp. 1254-
1259.
Olsson, S. and Bums, P. (2000). Measuring distraction with a peripheral detection
task. The National Highway Traffic Safety Administration Driver Distraction Inter-
net Forum, on-line paper, available at http://www-nrd.nhtsa.dot.gov/departments/nrd-
13/driver-distractionlPapers20006.htm#A6
Ossowski, S., Fernandez, A., Serrano, J.M., Perez-de-la-Cruz, J.L., Belmonte, M.V.,
Hernandez, J.Z., Garcia-Serrano, A. and Maseda, J.M. (2004). Designing multiagent
decision support system-The case of transportation management. Third International
Joint Conference on Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems (AAMAS 2004), New
York,pp.1470-1471.
Ozguner, F., Ozguner, U., Takeshita, 0., Redmill' K., Liu, Y., Korkmaz, G., Dogan, A.,
Tokuda, K., Nakabayashi, S. and Shimizu, T. (2004). A simulation study of an inter-
section collision warning system. In Proceedings of the International workshop on ITS
Telecommunications, Singapore.
Panou, M., Cacciabue, N., Cacciabue, P.C. and Bekiaris, A. (2005). From driver modelling
to human machine interface personalisation. IFAC 16th World Congress, Prague, Czech
Republic.
Pentland, A. and Liu, A. (1999). Modelling and prediction of human behavior. Neural
Computation, 11, 229-242.
Qi, Y. (1997). A Simulation Laboratory for Evaluation of Dynamic Traffic Manage-
ment Systems. PhD Thesis at Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Boston, MA,
USA.
Recarte, M.A. and Nunes, L.M. (2003). Mental workload while driving: Effects on vi-
sual search, discrimination, and decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Applied, 9, 119-137.
Reece, D.A. and Shafer, S.A. (1988). An overview of the PHAROS traffic simulator. In
Rothengater, J .A. and De Bruin, R.A. (Eds.). Road User Behavior: Theory and Practice.
Van Gorcum, Assen.
Regan, M.A. and Horberry, T. (2004). A driving simulator evaluation of enhanced road
markings. In Proceedings ofAustralasian Road Safety Research, Policing and Education
Conference, Australia.
21. Present and Future of Simulation Traffic Models 427
Rizzo, M., Jermeland,1. and Severson, 1. (2002). Instrumented vehiclesand driving simu-
lators. Gerontechnology, 1(4),291-296.
Salvucci,D.D.,Boer,E.R. andLiu,A. (2001).Toward an integratedmodelof driverbehavior
in a cognitivearchitecture. Tranportation Research Record No. 1779,pp. 9-16.
Schvaneveldt, R.W., Reid, G.B., Gomez,R.L. and Rice, S. (1998).Modellingmental work-
load. Cognitive Technology, 3, 19-31.
Spillers, F. (2003). Task analysis through cognitivearchaeology. In D. Diaper, N. Stanton
(Eds.). The Handbook ofTask Analysis for HCI. LaurenceErlbaumAssociates, Mahwah,
N1.
Stone, P. and Veloso, M. (2000). Multiagent systems: A survey from machine learning
perspective. Autonomous Robots, 8(3), 345-383.
Sukthankar, R., Hancock,1.,Pomerleau, D. and Thorpe,C. (1996).A simulationand design
system for tactical drivingalgorithms. In Proceedings ofAI, Simulation and Planning in
High Autonomy Systems.
Sukthankar, R., Hancock,1. and Thorpe,C. (1998).Tactical level simulationfor intelligent
transportation systems.Journal on Mathematical and Computer Modelling, Special Issue
on ITS, 27(9/11), 228-242.
Sukthankar, R., Pomerleau, D. and Thorpe, C. (1995). SHIVA: Simulated highways for
intelligent vehicle algorithms. In Proceedings ofIEEE Intelligent Vehicles.
Theeuwes,1. (2002). Sampling information form the road environment. In R. Fuller, 1.A.
Santos (Eds.). Human Factors for Highway Engineers. Elsevier, Amsterdam.
Theeuwes,1. and Hagenzieker, M.P. (1993).Visualsearchof traffic scenes:On the effectof
location expectations. In A. Gale, LD. Brown, C.M. Haslegrave and S.P. Taylor (Eds.).
Vision in Vehicles (Vol. 4, pp. 149-158). Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
Toftin, D., Reymond, G., Kemeny, A. and Droulez, 1. (2003). Influence of steering wheel
torquefeedbackin a dynamicdrivingsimulator. In Proceedings ofthe Driving Simulation
Conference, North America,Deaborn.
Zhang,Y., Owechko,Y. andZhang,J. (2004).Drivercognitiveworkloadestimation: A data-
drivenperspective. In Proceedings of 77th International IEEE Conference on Intelligent
Transportation Systems, Washington, DC.
Zhang, Y., Owechko, Y. and Zhang, J. (2004). Learning-based driver workloadestimation.
In Proceedings of 7th International Symposium on Advanced Vehicle Control, Amhem,
The Netherlands.
Zijlstra,F.R.H. (1993).Efficiency in Work Behaviour: A Design Approachfor Modern Tools.
Delft University Press, Delft.
Index
429
430 Index