Open navigation menu
Close suggestions
Search
Search
en
Change Language
Upload
Sign in
Sign in
Download free for days
100%
(2)
100% found this document useful (2 votes)
2K views
Conversational Routines in English - Convention and Creativity PDF
Uploaded by
Choi Philip
AI-enhanced title
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content,
claim it here
.
Available Formats
Download as PDF or read online on Scribd
Download now
Download
Save Conversational routines in English- Convention and... For Later
Download
Save
Save Conversational routines in English- Convention and... For Later
100%
100% found this document useful, undefined
0%
, undefined
Embed
Share
Print
Report
100%
(2)
100% found this document useful (2 votes)
2K views
Conversational Routines in English - Convention and Creativity PDF
Uploaded by
Choi Philip
AI-enhanced title
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content,
claim it here
.
Available Formats
Download as PDF or read online on Scribd
Download now
Download
Save Conversational routines in English- Convention and... For Later
Carousel Previous
Carousel Next
Save
Save Conversational routines in English- Convention and... For Later
100%
100% found this document useful, undefined
0%
, undefined
Embed
Share
Print
Report
Download now
Download
You are on page 1
/ 134
Search
Fullscreen
Conversational Routines in English vitorStudies in language and linguistics General editors: GEOFFREY LEECH & MICK SHORT Lancaster University Already published: A Dictionary of Stylistics ‘The Communicative Competence of Young Children. SUSAN H. FOSTER Linguistic Parisi GEORGE THOMAS ‘Women, Men and Language Second edition JENNIFER COATES Lexical Ambiguity in Poetry SOON PENG SU Understanding Metaphor in Literature English Spelling and the Computer ROGER MEFTON Conversational Routines in English: Convention and Creativity, KARIN AMER CONVERSATIONAL ROUTINES IN ENGLISH: Convention and Creativity KARIN AIJMER Longman London & New York‘Addisom Wesley Longman Limited Edinburgh Gate Harlow, Essex CM20 2] and Associated Comproiesthroughout the world Published inthe Ute Stats of America ‘by Addison Wesley Longman, New York (© Addison Wesley Longman Limited 1996 ‘The right of Karin Aijmer tobe identified as the author of this Work has been asserted by her in accordance with the ‘Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988. Allright reserved na part of his publication maybe fad, sored in aretieval syst, oF wansraed inany form or by any means electronic, mechanical Photicopying recording or ctherwise without either the Prior wen permission the Publishers or licence rmiing restated copying in the United Kingdom issued bythe Copyright Licnaing Agency Li 90 Tatenkarn Court Read London WIP HE First published 1996, ISBN 0582 08212-9 Cs ISBN 0582 08211-0 Ppr British Library Cataloguing-in-Publieation Data ‘Accatalogue record for this book is available from the British Library jng-n-Pablcation Data ‘Ames Karin ‘Conversations routines in English convention and creativity / avin Aer {em — (Gries in anguage ad linguistic) Indies biblographia reference and nde ISBN 0-582-O861120 (paper) — BBN 0-582-08212-9 cas) {-Englsh language-Spoken Engl. 2. English anguast— Discoue analy 3 English anguoge—Prenk aalis 4.Computationat linguists. 3.Comveration 6 Semitic {hte Series Stales in language std lngustcs Landon, England PEIOI SAT? 1956 ‘a4 s6-17540 cP Set #n9/1 RPO gu ad Produced by Longman Sng hers Pe Paedngoppre fete 25% To Celia, Kristofer, Paula and LovisaContents Preface Acknowledgements CHAPTER ONE: Introduction Wl 12 13 4 15 16 17 18 19 110 un 12 1B ‘Aim and scope of the present study Material and method Frequency of conversational routines in spoken language Psychological aspects of conversational routines ‘Conversational routines and ritualization Lexicalization, grammaticalization and idiomatization Conversational routines and meaning Criteria of fixedness 1.8.1 Repetitive phrases and pragmatic idioms 1.8.2 Prosodic fixedness ‘The processing of conversational routines Routines and discourse Conversational routines and grammatical analysis 1111 Grammatical deficiency 1.11.2 Syntactic integration and position ‘A model for describing the structural flexibility of conversational routines ‘The pragmatic function of conversational routines 1.13.1 Conversational routines and illocutionary force 1.132 Indirect speech acts, 10 n 2 13 “4 15 7 18 18 9 a m4 4 4 viiCONTENTS Charrer Two: 1.133. Conventionalization of inditect speech acts 1.14 The pragmatics of conversational routines 1.14.1 Conversational routines and frames 1.14.2 Factors of speech-act frames 1.15 Conversational routines and language teaching Thanking 21 Introduction 22. Thank youlthanks as anillocutionary force indicating device 23. Thanking and politeness 24 Strategies of thanking, 25. Gratitude expressions 2.6 Continuation patterns 27 The grammatical analysis of gratitude expressions: 28 Prosody and fixedness 29 Distribution of thanking over different texts 2.10. Thank you/ thanks as stems 2.10.1 Expanded forms of thanking. 2102 Thanking and intensification 2.10.3. Prosody and intensification 2.104. Patterns of compound thanks 2.11 The functions of gratitude expressions 2.111 Thanking and ritualization 212. Thanking asa discourse marker 2.12.1 Thanking as a closing signal in adjacency triplets 2.122 Thanking in proposal-acceptance sequences 2.123. Thanking in telephone closings 2.124 Thanking in different turn positions 2.13. The pragmatics of thanking. 2.13.1. The constraints caused by the object of gratitude 2.14 Frames for thanking 2.141 Variation in standard situations 2:18 Conclusion (CHarren Taree: Apologies 3.1 Introduction 32 Defining apologies 33 Apologizing strategies 25 26 26 7 28 33 35 35 39 39 41 41 2 45 v 8 SI 51 52 56 58 6 2 78 BL (Carrer Four 34 35 36 37 38 39 310 an 32 33 314 35 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 CONTENTS The form of apologizing a Continuation patterns 87 ‘The grammatical analysis of apology expressions 88 Apologies and prosody 88 Distribution of apologies over different texts 89 Collocational fixedness and flexibility 31 39.1 Fully expanded apology expressions 91 39.2 Apologizing and intensification 93 3.93 Prosodic devices emphasizing the politeness expressed by the apology 94 394 Compound apologies 94 Apologies and function 7 Retrospective and anticipatory apologies 98. 3.11.1. Disarming apologies 100 3.1.1.1 Disarmers and corrections 102 311.2 Disarmers as requests for repetition 102 3.11.13 Disarmers in dispreferred responses 103 ‘Thestructural function of apologies 106 3.121 Apologies in telephone openings 106 3.12.2 Apologies intelephoneclasings 106 ‘The type of offence 108 3.13.1 Talk offences 109 3.13.2 Time offences ua 3.133. Space offences m5 3.13.4. Offences involving social behaviour 115 3.135 Offences involving inconvenience 116 Apologies and pragmatic frames m8 3.14.1 Frames for standard situations 9 Conclusion 121 Requests and offers 124 Introduction 14 ‘The speech act assignment mechanism and indirect speech acts 124 Indirect specch acts and pragmatic principles 126 Indirect speech acts and implicature 127 Pragmatic ambiguity 128 Defining requests 129 Requestive strategies 130 ‘A taxonomy of requests 131 48‘CONTENTS (CHAPTER PVE: 48.1 Requestives, advisories and offers 482 Explicit and implicit indirect requests 49° Requests and politeness 49.1 Assertive and tentative indirect requests 492 Requests and style 4.10 Continuation patterns 411 The grammatical analysis of requestive routines 4.12. Describing request expressions 4.12.1 Prosodic modification 4122. Requests and discourse type 4.13. Indirect requests and speech act stems 4.14 Types of stem 4.14.1, Mitigated indirect requests in the form of declarative sentences 4.142 Want and need statements 4.143. Mitigated indirect requests in the form of interrogative sentences 4143.1 Canfeould you 41432. Willfoould you 41433 Requests in the form of permission questions 4.15. Lexical mitigating devices 4151 Please 4152 Just 4.16. Internal and extemal modifiers 4.161 Requests and extemal modifiers 4162. Combinations of modifiers 4.17 Referential strategies 4.18 Requests and pragmatic conventions 4.18.1 Requests and the situation 4.18.2 Frames fr requests 4.19 Imperatives 4.19.1, Imperatives and politeness 4191.1 Do-+ imperative 419.12 You + imperative 4.20 Patterns expressing offers 421 Conclusion Discourse markers as conversational routines 51 Introduction 52 Coherence and discourse markers 53 Discourse markers characterized 54 The metalinguistic function 14 136 139 40 141 ww M4 45 M5 146 Ww 148 49 154 156 157 159 161 163 166 169 170 170 178 175 7 178 180 182 183 187 189 195 200 200 201 203 References Index 58 59 5.10 Sn 512 CONTENTS Relevance theory and communication 55.1 The interpretation of discourse markers in relevance theary ‘The approach to discourse markers in this work “The linguistic properties of discourse markers 57.1 ‘The discourse marker slot’ 57.2 Prosodic fixednoss 5.7.3 Positional fixedness 574 The grammatical analysis of discourse markers as stems Contextual properties of discourse markers 5.8.1 Discourse markers.as deictic ‘pointers’ referring backwards and forwards in the discourse 582 Discourse markers and person deixis, Functional properties of discourse markers 59.1 Global and local discourse markers 59.11 The pragmatic functions expressed by local discourse markers 59.12 Functions of global discourse markers ‘Combinations in the discourse marker slot Discourse markers and cognitive frames Condlusion 208 209 21 a 212 216 216 a7 218 218 20 221 21 226 231 232 235 246Preface The aim of he prevent work is to sty the range ad frequency of tional routines in the London-Lund Corpus of Spoken English ‘The numberof spoken texts which have been colleced and transcribed make up almost haifa million words. The original recordings on which the texts are based were made mainly during the 1960s and 1970s at University College, London, as a part of the project Survey of English Usage (SEU). ‘The samples represent various text types such as face-to-face conversa- tions, radio discussions and telephone conversations, described in more
howdy Finally, the phrase is fixed in the sense that ¢he idiomatic function (= a greeting) is associated directly and unambiguously with the linguistic form. A high degree of fixedness is also indicated by the fact that, in a 2 INTRODUCTION discourse perspective, how do you do must be followed by another greet- ing, usually a copy of the same phrase (A: How do you do? ~B: Hew do you o?). Gee section 1.10.) ‘According tothe same tests, the greeting how are you isles fixed. It can either be interpreted literally or have an idiomatized pragmatic function The idiomatic meaning is illustrated by the following exchange: How are you? Fine how are you? However, the phrase can be understood literally asa question fits asked by one’s doctor and answered by a specification of one’s health (Makai 1972:175). Grammatically its also les fied. The form of the verb can be changed (How haze you been?) the subject can be the third person (ow is he?) The phrase can be embedded in another structure (He wondered how 1 yas) and it can be expanded (How are you today?).2 ‘The majority of routines are like how are you, which is fixed according to some criteria but not others. Thank yu is another example of how each phrase needs to be described on the bass of several differen¢ criteria. The phrase cannot be passivized (“you are thanked), and the verb cannot be replaced by a (near-synonym (‘appreciate you). However, thank you can be modified (thant you very much), embedded (I wish fo thank you), etc in ‘ways which will be further discussed in Chapter2. Haggo and Kuiper (1983: 541) account for the grammatical and seman- tic restrictions on formulas and their potential variability ina finite state grammar without loops (see section 1.12). Another model is provided by lexicalized sentence stems, an alternative I explore in section 1.12. 181 Repetitive phrases and pragmatic idioms Conversational routines can also be fixed because they always appear a ceriain form. Manes and Wolfson’s work on compliments (1981) is of special interest here. To begin with, Manes and Wolfson noticed that there isan almost infinite number of (indirect) realizations of a compliment. However, some variants are never chosen although they are grammatical ‘and would be functionally appropriate. Manes and Wolfson found, for ‘example, that as many as 85 per cen (out of 686 compliments) fell into one of only three syntactic patterns such as I (really) like NP (Manes and Wolfson 1981: 123) Furthermore, just two verbs (like and love) occurred in 8% per cent ofall the compliments containing verbs expressing a positive ‘evaluation (Manes and Wolfson 1961: 1279, Of the compliments with an adjective, 229 per cent contained nice (Manes and Wolfson 1981: 117) (See Figure 12) Manes and Wolfson concluded that compliments were formulas (routines), just as much as greetings or thanks, because speakers used only a restricted set of expressions, As a result, compliments are a(CONVERSATIONAL. ROUTINES IN ENGLISH ie fm [B] on r) 2 ‘ta ie] ne PRO is realy (a) ADV NP You Via! vealy ADJ NP NOUV (NPY ealiy) ADV Yu have (al eally) ADU NP (Wit) AD NPP ADIN fant ABU NP Other patorns| ee zemnpo Figure 12 _The distribution of syntactic pattern in compliments (after Manes and Wolfson 1981: 121) constrained as o patter productivity just. as much as thanks or apologies. Itshould be remembered, however, that even if the patterns which can be established by quantitative methods are of interest, they have different properties from other routines At this point we need to make a distinction between different types of routine, According to Stenstrém, structures can be analysed both on the ‘grammatical and pragmatic level (ee Stenstroms 1990: 172F multi-level analysis of so-called discourse markers). Coulmas (1979: 240), on the ‘other hand, discusses the possibility of classifying routines into repetitive (recurrent) phrases and pragmatic idioms. “Repetitive phrases’ (such as really like NP) do not have special syntactic chatacteristics, ie. they are not restricted as to pattern productivity and they are not transforma- tionally defective. Pragmatic idioms’ are characterized by typical idiom features, such as a special meaning, fixed form and grammatical irregularities. Finally it should be noted that there is a fuzzy boundary between dif- ferent types of phrase. As Lamb points out (1992, unpublished), there are a great many phrases which are only weakly or slightly idiomatized, Le they keep ther literal meaning. Such transparent phrases are, for example, the probiem is .., what Fmean i .., let me tell you something. 18.2 Prosodic fixedness Prosodically, conversational routines correspond to tone units. Tone unit boundaries and hesitation pauses within the tone unit therefore provide additional clues about the degree of fixedness of a conversational routine (cee Altenberg and Beg-Olofsson 1990: 18; Peters 1983: 10). There is a fixed set of tones to choose from. As Kuiper and Haggo point out, a fixed pro- sody makes ‘storage and retrieval easier, as. fling system using cards of “ INTRODUCTION Afferent colour in each section is easier to use than a single colour system’ (0984: 228), Keller (1981: 1061) regarded “a drop in the intonational contour" as a sore reliable criterion for ‘gambits' (conversational routines with a dis course-organizing function) than syntactic independence or postion in the uterance. Moreover, a characteristic intonation pattern can be chosen in stereotypic or standard situations illustrated by the tone on sorry when cone bumps into someone (see Ladd 1978 ~ "stylized intonation’). Finally, it should be stressed that although intonation is always to some extent fixed, a conversational routine may have several prosodic patterns each ‘with complicated conditions of use. Sory i, for example, used with arise (fall-rise) tone ifthe speaker asks the hearer to repeat Something, but with a fall tone to signal sincere regret. A fixed prosody also means that the stress cannot be moved from its normal position to any other element of the phrase (see section 1.1). There are, however, degrees of prosodic fixedness, and changes in the prosodic patter can be made for certain purposes (thank you ery moch-> Hak you ery much) 1.9. The processing of conversational routines On a general level, the analysis of routines and idioms depends on whether language performance is regarded as governed by grammatical rules or as the ‘putting-together’ of ready-made or partly ready-made cognitive units stored in the long-term memory. Inhis connection itis fruitful to think of the ‘language text’ as being produced and interpreted according to two different principles. ‘According to the so-called ‘open choice principle’ discussed by Sinclair (1987:379), the language text isthe result of a number of ‘slot-by-slot’ choices made by the language user on different levels. A language text organized according to the ‘idiom principle’ imposes heavier constraints on what is said” since the principle presupposes that language users have at their disposal a number of lexicalized or semi-lexicalized phrases which are stored in the long-term memory (see Sinclair 1987: 320). Such, phrases can be interpreted directly by the hearer without the need of semantic rules operating on the syntactic structure in a compositional fashion (see Morgan and Horn’s notion ‘short-circuited implicature’; see section 4.4). The idiom principle is compatible with a tendency to eco~ rnomy in Janguage (see Leech 1983: 67) since the speaker can easily and rapiclly get access to the lexicalized or idiomatized chunks which are stored in the long-term memory. Looking things up in the long-term memory is quick and provides the speaker with extra time to plan ahead. Sinclair stresses the fact that the two models of language are incompat- Ible with each other: There is no shading of one into another; the switching, 1sCONVERSATIONAL ROUTINES IN ENGLISH from one model to the other will be sharp’ (1987: 324). On the other hand, the principles or processes for interpreting sentences do not represent an either/or choice in discourse interpretation, but operate in a parallel fashion: ‘We should also recognize thatthe choice between understanding a combination om the bass of is components and understanding a lexcalized formation as a Unit isnot anelier-or choice at al since these two proceses may both operate, in parallel, Its very likely tht such parallel operation is usual fF com: binations that areas yet only slightly leaned, (Cam 1982, unpublished, p. 1) Parallel processing implies thatthe hearer has @ choice between recog rizing a combination of words asa lexicalized unit or analysing it on the basis of the grammar and the lexicon, and that switches from one prin- ciple to another can take place on-line. In Lamb's model there would be two possible processing routes (see Figure 1.3). Routines Recognition of Processing by means texcaizes unit, tthe grammar and the lesicon Figure 1.3 Parallel processing of conversational routines (strongly and weakly ‘idiomatic phrases) Even when a lexeme has been ‘strongly’ idiomatized, its components can still operate as such fo provide connotations based on the meanings they have when occurring in isolation (cf. frankly speaking, to tell yor the ‘ruth. If we take it that there are parallel ways of processing idioms, we «an also explain that the semantic information associated with the com- ponent parts of the idiom can be recovered by means of ‘recycling’ (cf Haggo and Kuiper 1983: 549 ~ ‘semantic reanalysis’; Kjellmer 1991: 123 — ‘manipulation’. An example is when How do you do? is responded to by How do Ido what? for husmorous effect (Makkai 1972: 173). This view of language processing and interpretation means that many phenomena, which traditionally belong to grammar, may also be analysed as routines and patterns. Makkai advocates the view that much of gram- ‘mar ough to be studied in an idiomatic perspective (1992, unpublished). Here are some examples: + uses of words which are collocationally restricted (now you come to mention it) 6 INTRODUCTION + uses of words cooccurring with certain complements (hark you for coming, sorry to trouble you) + words which are ordered in a special way (perhaps you would like to come) ‘+ combinations of words which repeatedly occur together (indirect requests ike cm you, would you, etc) There are many problems with studying utterarces as routines or larger routinized patterns.” Even if one believes thatthe bulk of language is to some extent routinized, its clear that speakers may differ with regard 0 what patterns they have stored and use. Moreover, itis difficult to for- rmulate pragmatic rules or frames for indirect requests which are suffi ently constrained to explain how the extralinguistic situation is matched with lexical forms, 1.10 Routines and discourse Minimally, a routine is a single word (eg, thanks, sorry), but most routines consist of several words. Is clear that speakers can store quite long phrases in their long-term memories (eg. the routine can ! keep you for a ‘moment and tell you something which can preface a message). A rostine need not even be a syntactic constituent as is shown by routine phrases, such as the fact is. cam you ... . Some routines are discontinuous with a slot for lexical insertion: as far as / .../is concerned. Routines do not only occur alone, but they can be repeated and juxta- ‘posed. The structures which emerge are composed of single building- blocks, stored! as units in the speaker's long-term memory, which appear in the order that they are generated. The combination of routines can be ‘more ot lese fixed: ) B Aldvelye thank* you !very ‘much in-ded# (9.2.568-9) In general, the length and complexity ofthe gratitude phrase depend on the social situation and the amount of politeness it requires (see ‘Chapter 2). Besides, we find sequences of routines. The smallest ofthe for- ‘mulaic discourse structures can be described as aft adjacency pair ‘Richards and Schmidt 1983a: 128; Schegloff and Sacks 1973). Adjacency pairs are sequences of utterances produced by two successive speakers such that the ‘first pair part’ constrains the other speaker to provide a response in the ‘second pair part. An exact copy of the frst part is char- acteristic of pairs which are completely formulaic such as greetings (Hello = Hello). In discourse structures which are less tightly constrained, there isa choice between different strategies in the second part ot the second part may be optional. Compare, for example, the following adjacency vCONVERSATIONAL ROUTINES IN ENGLISH pairs consisting of a request and different second pair parts (from Richards and Schmidt 1983a: 129) Request Grant A: Can you mail these for me please? B. Sure ~ Put off B: Sure, but won't have time today. =Challenge _B:_ Why do you always ask me to rasil them for you? ~ Refusal Bi: Sorry, but I won't be near the Post Office, Discourse patterns (which can be regarded as a special kind of adjacency pairs) may occasionally consist of three pars, al of which are routines, ‘A: ‘Would you pas the milk” % ‘Were’ ‘Ac “Thanks” (atter Gottman 1975 65) “The first two moves of the conversational exchange are a request by A and the granting of the request by B. The third move (which is optional) acknowledges the response by B and closes the conversational exchange. Routines are frequent in certain discourse types which impose constraints on the speaker’s short-term memory stich as auctions (see Kuiper and Haggo 1984), sports commentaries (ee Kuiper and Austin 1990), elgious ceremonies (see Paul 1989). Another example is telephone calls where routines are found in the opening and the closing ofthe call (see Hopper 1989; Schegloff and Sacks 1973). This will be illustrated in the following chapters. 1.11 Conversational routines and grammatical analysis ‘To what extent can conversational routines be described grammatically? In the following sections \sball discuss if and what syntactic information must be specified for conversational routines. 11L1 Grammatical deficiency According to Bahns etal. (1986: 695), formulas do not show any syntactic peculiarities. Many conversational routines can be analysed as sentences am grateful for fyour help/, '™m sorry, how do you do}, subordinate clauses @s far as Teno, i may sy <0), noun-verb structures (I mean, you know, nnon-finite clauses (to fell you the truth, frankly speaking), prepositional phrases (infact), etc. Tere are also grammatically defective routines such as Sentence fragments, independent subordinate clauses (if you cou ep ‘me), irregular wh-questions (how about coffee), non-sentences (next slide please), and elliptical constructions of different kinds (ant a drink Routinized sentence fragments can be of different kinds. The short phrases sorry, beg your pardon oF thank you are analysed as sentence 8 INTRODUCTION fragments because they lack the subject (or both subject and copula). Since the missing elements are known, the phrases must be analysed as the result of situational ellipsis (Quirk et al. 1985: 11.38). Even if routines have a constituent structure, they are not necessarily {generated by the grammar. To give an example, you know and I men are 8 fixed combination of noun and verb which must be analysed as dis- course markers (see Stenstréim 1990), Outside this group, however, noun- verb structures are an open-ended category: In comment clauses such as he thought, he std, etc, the verb can, for example, appear with an adverbial (he suid immediatly), and itcan be exchanged for other synonymous verbs he reflected, he supposed, he mentioned. ‘As pointed out above, conversational routines are usually multi-word units. One-word routines are difficult to describe in grammatical terms, ‘and many of them would belong to Fillmore et al.'s class of extra-gratn- ‘matical idioms’ (Fillmore et al. 1988: 505). In thanks, for example, itis not clear what elements have been omitted as thanks could be expanded in ‘more than one way. Both I ote you my thanks andl I give yow thanks are possible sources for thanks (Quirk et al. 1985: 12.34). Alternatively, we Could argue that the syntactic properties of the phrase should be deduced from its syntactic subrcategorization. However, this is not very helpful since thanks can be sub-categorized in two different weys. The phrase must be a verb when its followed by an intensifying adverb (eg. nk very much) but it can also be a noun which is premodified (many thanks). We conclude therefore that thanks is grammatically rather like hello which is not amenable to syntactic analysis.” Discourse-organizing routines such aswell, now which function as ‘openers’ are also difficult to analyse grammatically. They can be analysed neither as elliptical struc- tures nor as special parts of speech. Moreover, whole sentences can be syntactically indetertainate, as illus- trated by Haggo and Kuipers (1983: 5434) example there yow are (a formota used when giving somebody something). First, they say this sentence is probably nota transformation of you are there Secondly, we could hay say that you is the subject of arc. Thirdly, te status of there is difficult to detor- rmine.!® Now ifthe phrase isnot simply a transformational variant ofthe ppkrase you ae ther, this could be taken to mean that it should be analysed as a grammatical construction, 2. ‘a symbolic model that pairs syntactic conditions with semantic and pragmatic conditions’ (Lakoff 1987: 471; cf Fillmore et al. 1988). Asa grammatical construction the phrase can base dif ferent syntactic, semantic and pragmatic properties from the corresponding, simple sentences (see also the discussion of stems in section 1.1), LdL2 Syntactic integration and position Conversational routines expressing thanks and apologies constitute inde- pendent conversational moves (see Stenstrim 1990: 147). On the other 8(CONVERSATIONAL ROUTINES IN ENGLISH hand, routines with a discourse-organizing function (called discourse ‘markers in Chapter 5) are partly integrated in another grammatical struc- ture, Following Andersson (1975) they can also be called speech-act adver- bials because they have adverbial function and “because they modify the ~ speech act of saying something rather than what is said’ (Andersson 1975: 27;my translation). The adverbial constitutes a prosodic unit marked by a tone unit boundary. It has initial (or final) position in the wtterance of which itis a part. What is interesting about Andersson's analysis is that speech-act adverbials are characterized not only functionally and prosodically but also grammatically. In Figure 1.4, a distinction is made between speech- act adverbials, which have pragmatic function, and other elements (adverbs, clauses, et), which are integrated in the proposition and havea propositional function. The speech-act adverbial (= a) occupies a separate node in the tree dia- ‘gram, indicating that its pragmatic scope extends over the whole structure although itis not part of the grammatical core. Elements which have a propositional function (= b) must be part of the propositional structure, ‘which is interpreted compositionally by the semantic rules. There are several arguments for distinguishing between two syntactic analyses. First, there is a contrast between speech-act adverbials and adverbs not referring to the speech act which is clear in pairs such as: (a) Frankly (speaking), he behaved well (b) He behaved frankly Also, whole clauses as adverbials can have two interpretations reflected in different structural analyses. To give an example, if] may say o has pragmatic function and is not part of a conditional sentence. The clause contrasts with jFclauses which state the condition under which the main clause is true (fit rans, I carry an umbrella). s s s ADVERBIAL | Ne ve NP ve Aoverstat (@) pragmatic tnetion (©) propesitonaltesetion Figure 14 The constituent structure of (a) speech-actadverbials and (b) of other adverbials i final position (After Andersson 1975: 29) 20 INTRODUCTION Secondly, the analysis proposed in Figure 1.4 can explain the ambi- guity of the following (constructed) example from Sinclair and Renouf (1988: 152) ‘Tobe fair, Tom divided the sweets evenly. In the pragmatic interpretation, the person who is fair is the person speak ing or writing, and the phrase is a judgement on Tom. In this case, the phrase is attached pragmatically as a speech-act adverbial or a disjunct) to the following utterance. Inthe other interpretation, fo could be para~ phrased as in order to’, and the person whois fair i Tom. The infinitive {sa purpose clause and belongs tothe proposition. In the wellknown syntactic analysis of adverbials by Quirk etal (1985 8.121), adverbials which are only loosely integrated inthe syntactic struc- tureare called disjuncts. Their independent status is shown by various grammatical operations. For example, they ae distinc from other syntac- Hc classes of adverbials (adjuncts, subjuncts) because they cannot be rade the focus of aceft sentence (it is rankly speaking that he dd it} or be the basis of alternative interzogation (“wus it frankly speaking or strangely enough that he didi). 1.12 A model for describing the structural flexibility of conversational routines Can conversational routines be accounted for in generative grammar? AS we have seen, conversational routines may be invariable and/or grammatically irregular; they can have idiomatic meanings and be bound to the situation. The majority, however, are partly productive and are not grammatically peculiar. Exactly how one describes con- versational routines and other formulas seems to be a consequence of ‘one's views on lexicalization and the boundary between the lexicon and grammar. Fillmore et al. describe the current grammatical scene as follows: ‘Current formal medels of grammar take a severe view of the distinction between knowing and figuring out: they assign as much work as possible to the ‘computing 6: figuring out part of knowing how to use language, and they attempt to keep a! 2 minimum those aspects of linguistic competence that have tobe represented as stored or known, (illmore etal. 1988: 502) In generative grammar a distinction is made between a grammatical rule component generating sentences and the lexicon representing those aspects of linguistic competence which are stored or known. It is possible, however, that generative grammar is, in a way, ‘too perfect to account for ‘conversational routines. This leaves us with a number of possible models. for describing conversational routines. In so-called Construction a(CONVERSATIONAL ROUTINES IN ENGLISH Grammar (see Fillmore etal 1988), conversational routines would pre- sumably be grammatical constructions specifying syntactic, lexical, semantic and pragmatic information. Recently, one has also seen different proposals for enriching the lexicon in order to account for structures which do not belong clearly either in the lexicon or in the grammar (see Jackendoff's 1975 discussion of full-entry theories using redundancy rules versus impoverished entries ‘and Haggo and Kuiper 1983: 5399) 1m the theoretical framework of the present study, which is based on Frawley and Syder (1988), the form of conversational routines isa fairly abstract sentence stem, consisting ofa ‘core’ with possible extensions. The following lexemes ae linked by family resemblance andl related by a stem containing sorry as its core: )_Y'msorry to keep you waiting, I'm so sorry to have kept you waiting. Mr X is sorry to keep you waiting allthis time. (Pawley and Syder 1983: 210) ‘The stem is a collocational frame” with slots for grammatical categories. INP be-TENSE sorry to keep-TENSE you waiting (Pawley and Syder 1983: 210) Clearly, the stem is different from a productive grammatical rule as it contains (lexical) elements which are not inserted by means of lexical rules. The capitalized elements represent slots into which, for example, a noun phrase or a tense morpheme may be inserted, Lexicalized sentence stems are placed by Pawley in the “ideal lexico- sgraphers’ lexicon’ (1986: 114). On a deeper level they are cognitive units which provide an economical way of storing linguistic variants which show some similarity, and they can be used ready-made in speech pro- duction. The availability and flexibility of the stem may explain why Iexicalized sentence stems are so frequent in spoken language; the number of lexicalized sentence stems in English ‘probably amounts, at Jest, to several hundreds of thousands’ (Pawley and Syder 1983: 213; see also section 1.3) From a formal grammatical point of view, a dictionary entry for a lexicalized sentence stem is a ‘mini-grammar’ (Pawley and Syder 1983: 216). The stem has the capacity to generate a number of ‘possible routine phrases’ and can be exploited by speakers for ‘creative’ departures (Fillmore 1984; 128). Variants are formed from the stem by substituting, elements by ‘inflection’, modifying existing structures, combining ele- ments into sets, embedding structures in larger discourse units and by deleting optional elements. Grammatical constructions or lexicalized sentence stems can be com- pared with a finite state grammar without recursive properties (Kuiper 2 INTRODUCTION and Haggo 1984: 221)" The grammar runs through a number of states ‘with an initial and terminal state as endpoints, producing words at each transition, assigning to them a certain probability. Since the model repre- sents the simplest type of generative grammar, it has little processing ‘capacity (requiring only one slot in the short-term memory). ‘One can notice, finally, that although the lexicalized stems discussed by Pawley and Syder (1983) have the form of sentences, itis obvious that stems can also consist of clauses or phrases with fixed and variable parts and a specification of the elements which can fill an empty slot inthe stern. ADVERB Sorry to very rightfully terribly sxufully truly, ete Discourse-organizing routines (speech-act adverbials) consist of
You might also like
PDF How Languages Are Learned 4th Edition Patsy M. Lightbown download
PDF
100% (7)
PDF How Languages Are Learned 4th Edition Patsy M. Lightbown download
40 pages
Brazil 1997-CVIE-The Communicative Value of Intonation in English
PDF
0% (1)
Brazil 1997-CVIE-The Communicative Value of Intonation in English
101 pages
Carter and Mcarthy - Grammar and The Spoken Language PDF
PDF
50% (4)
Carter and Mcarthy - Grammar and The Spoken Language PDF
18 pages
Carter, R. & McCarthy, M. (1995) - Grammar and Spoken Language. Applied Linguistics, 16 (2), 141-158.
PDF
33% (3)
Carter, R. & McCarthy, M. (1995) - Grammar and Spoken Language. Applied Linguistics, 16 (2), 141-158.
35 pages
Discourse On The Move
PDF
No ratings yet
Discourse On The Move
304 pages
More Listening or Better Listeners - John Fields
PDF
100% (2)
More Listening or Better Listeners - John Fields
3 pages
Second Language Learning Theories by Rosamond Mitchell, Florence Myles, Emma Marsden
PDF
100% (1)
Second Language Learning Theories by Rosamond Mitchell, Florence Myles, Emma Marsden
396 pages
Teachers of English To Speakers of Other Languages, Inc. (TESOL) TESOL Quarterly
PDF
100% (1)
Teachers of English To Speakers of Other Languages, Inc. (TESOL) TESOL Quarterly
13 pages
Cook 1999-Going Beyond The Native Speaker in Language Teaching
PDF
No ratings yet
Cook 1999-Going Beyond The Native Speaker in Language Teaching
26 pages
Cook Using The First Language in The Classroom 1 PDF
PDF
100% (1)
Cook Using The First Language in The Classroom 1 PDF
22 pages
Past Tense - Delta
PDF
No ratings yet
Past Tense - Delta
8 pages
Delta Module One Reading List: Language Systems Grammar
PDF
100% (1)
Delta Module One Reading List: Language Systems Grammar
1 page
Grammar and The Language Teacher - 0526143203 - 001
PDF
100% (2)
Grammar and The Language Teacher - 0526143203 - 001
136 pages
Texture and Pedaling in Selected Nocturnes of Frédéric Chopin PDF
PDF
67% (3)
Texture and Pedaling in Selected Nocturnes of Frédéric Chopin PDF
390 pages
Texture and Pedaling in Selected Nocturnes of Frédéric Chopin PDF
PDF
67% (3)
Texture and Pedaling in Selected Nocturnes of Frédéric Chopin PDF
390 pages
Readings in Methodology Articles On The Teaching of English As A Foreign Language-with-cover-page-V2
PDF
No ratings yet
Readings in Methodology Articles On The Teaching of English As A Foreign Language-with-cover-page-V2
262 pages
Talking Back Small Interactional Response Tokens in Everyday Conversation Mccarthy
PDF
No ratings yet
Talking Back Small Interactional Response Tokens in Everyday Conversation Mccarthy
31 pages
Conversation: Frotn Description To Pedagogy: Scott Thornbury and Diana Slade
PDF
0% (3)
Conversation: Frotn Description To Pedagogy: Scott Thornbury and Diana Slade
23 pages
5-9 MCarthy 1991 Discourse Analysis For Language Teachers
PDF
No ratings yet
5-9 MCarthy 1991 Discourse Analysis For Language Teachers
5 pages
Chaudron (1986) - WP5
PDF
100% (1)
Chaudron (1986) - WP5
39 pages
A Cognitive Linguistic Approach To Phrasal Verbs - A Teachers Gui
PDF
100% (1)
A Cognitive Linguistic Approach To Phrasal Verbs - A Teachers Gui
91 pages
Gass, Mackey & Pica (1998) The Role of Input and Interaction in SLA Introduction To The Special Issue PDF
PDF
100% (2)
Gass, Mackey & Pica (1998) The Role of Input and Interaction in SLA Introduction To The Special Issue PDF
9 pages
020 - Doing - Task-Based - Teaching - Dave Willis - Jane - Willis PDF
PDF
No ratings yet
020 - Doing - Task-Based - Teaching - Dave Willis - Jane - Willis PDF
169 pages
Hudson 2007 Teaching Second Language Reading
PDF
No ratings yet
Hudson 2007 Teaching Second Language Reading
15 pages
Delta Module 1 Bibliography
PDF
No ratings yet
Delta Module 1 Bibliography
3 pages
Richards (1990) Design Materials For Listening PDF
PDF
100% (1)
Richards (1990) Design Materials For Listening PDF
18 pages
The Communicative Classroom - Tricia Hedge
PDF
No ratings yet
The Communicative Classroom - Tricia Hedge
20 pages
Discourse and Context in Language Teaching: A Guide For Language Teachers
PDF
No ratings yet
Discourse and Context in Language Teaching: A Guide For Language Teachers
1 page
Bahns Jens Lexical Collocations A Contrastive View
PDF
No ratings yet
Bahns Jens Lexical Collocations A Contrastive View
8 pages
(Ebook) Teaching and Learning Second Language Listening by Christine C. M. Goh, Larry Vandergrift ISBN 9780367254261, 0367254263 instant download
PDF
100% (1)
(Ebook) Teaching and Learning Second Language Listening by Christine C. M. Goh, Larry Vandergrift ISBN 9780367254261, 0367254263 instant download
55 pages
Luiz Otavio Barros: Teaching Spoken English at Advanced Level
PDF
100% (1)
Luiz Otavio Barros: Teaching Spoken English at Advanced Level
22 pages
Phonology For Listening: Trinity Diploma Developmental Record
PDF
0% (1)
Phonology For Listening: Trinity Diploma Developmental Record
12 pages
9781315716893_previewpdf
PDF
No ratings yet
9781315716893_previewpdf
116 pages
AOUNALIWalid 3ASTextbooks
PDF
No ratings yet
AOUNALIWalid 3ASTextbooks
257 pages
The Lexical Syllabus Dave Willis
PDF
No ratings yet
The Lexical Syllabus Dave Willis
17 pages
Lexical Notebooks or Vocabulary Cards
PDF
No ratings yet
Lexical Notebooks or Vocabulary Cards
8 pages
Delta Module Two Lsa 3 - Part One SYSTEMS: Helping Lower Levels To Understand and Use High Frequency Phrasal Verbs Khara Burgess Lord Byron College Centre No: It295
PDF
100% (1)
Delta Module Two Lsa 3 - Part One SYSTEMS: Helping Lower Levels To Understand and Use High Frequency Phrasal Verbs Khara Burgess Lord Byron College Centre No: It295
14 pages
Computer-Assisted Pronunciation Teaching (CAPT) To Overcome Pronunciation Difficulties Among EFL Beginners
PDF
No ratings yet
Computer-Assisted Pronunciation Teaching (CAPT) To Overcome Pronunciation Difficulties Among EFL Beginners
9 pages
Skehan, P. 2003 Task-Based Instruction LT36
PDF
100% (1)
Skehan, P. 2003 Task-Based Instruction LT36
15 pages
shifts-in-the-electric-vehicle-market-british-english-student-B2-C1
PDF
No ratings yet
shifts-in-the-electric-vehicle-market-british-english-student-B2-C1
8 pages
Beyond_the_Sentence
PDF
No ratings yet
Beyond_the_Sentence
193 pages
Grammar Background
PDF
100% (1)
Grammar Background
10 pages
Summary of Principles in Language Teaching PDF
PDF
No ratings yet
Summary of Principles in Language Teaching PDF
2 pages
COMMUNICATIVE COMPETENCE Presentation
PDF
100% (1)
COMMUNICATIVE COMPETENCE Presentation
19 pages
1997 Dornyei Scott LL
PDF
100% (1)
1997 Dornyei Scott LL
38 pages
CAT: A Framework For Dogme: Ken Lackman & Associates
PDF
No ratings yet
CAT: A Framework For Dogme: Ken Lackman & Associates
10 pages
Native or Non-Native
PDF
No ratings yet
Native or Non-Native
10 pages
Ellis, R. (2005) Instructed Language Learning
PDF
No ratings yet
Ellis, R. (2005) Instructed Language Learning
16 pages
Global English Teaching Pronunciation
PDF
No ratings yet
Global English Teaching Pronunciation
2 pages
Can Pronunciation Be Taught A Review of PDF
PDF
No ratings yet
Can Pronunciation Be Taught A Review of PDF
38 pages
The Differential Effects of Three Types of Task Planning On The Fluency, Complexity, and Accuracy in L2 Oral Production
PDF
No ratings yet
The Differential Effects of Three Types of Task Planning On The Fluency, Complexity, and Accuracy in L2 Oral Production
36 pages
(Cambridge Applied Linguistics) David Singleton-Exploring The Second Language Mental Lexicon-Cambridge University Press (1999)
PDF
100% (1)
(Cambridge Applied Linguistics) David Singleton-Exploring The Second Language Mental Lexicon-Cambridge University Press (1999)
361 pages
Kramsch (1993) Context and Culture in Language Teaching
PDF
No ratings yet
Kramsch (1993) Context and Culture in Language Teaching
149 pages
Learning Teaching Jim Scrivener
PDF
No ratings yet
Learning Teaching Jim Scrivener
435 pages
Author Insights Paul Davis Hania Kryszewska
PDF
No ratings yet
Author Insights Paul Davis Hania Kryszewska
2 pages
LSA3 Pron Final
PDF
100% (1)
LSA3 Pron Final
12 pages
Focused Verus Unfocused Tasks Grammar
PDF
No ratings yet
Focused Verus Unfocused Tasks Grammar
5 pages
English Pronunciation in L2 Instruction - Chapter 1 2 and 3 - History of Pronunciation Teaching Techniques and Tools Attitudes
PDF
100% (1)
English Pronunciation in L2 Instruction - Chapter 1 2 and 3 - History of Pronunciation Teaching Techniques and Tools Attitudes
73 pages
Johns, T From Printout To Handout Grammar and Vocabulary Teaching in The Context of Data Driven Learning
PDF
No ratings yet
Johns, T From Printout To Handout Grammar and Vocabulary Teaching in The Context of Data Driven Learning
21 pages
Lexical Phrases in Language Learning Schmitt and Carter
PDF
100% (2)
Lexical Phrases in Language Learning Schmitt and Carter
5 pages
New Methodology For Listening
PDF
100% (2)
New Methodology For Listening
9 pages
Carter R McCarthy M 1995 Grammar and Spoken Language Applied Linguistics 16 2 141 158 PDF
PDF
No ratings yet
Carter R McCarthy M 1995 Grammar and Spoken Language Applied Linguistics 16 2 141 158 PDF
35 pages
Fragment 19252
PDF
No ratings yet
Fragment 19252
25 pages
Richardson, - 2004a - (Mis) Representing Islam-The Racism and Rhetoric of British Broadsheet Newspapers
PDF
No ratings yet
Richardson, - 2004a - (Mis) Representing Islam-The Racism and Rhetoric of British Broadsheet Newspapers
6 pages
Dunn - 2001 - Representations of Islam in The Politics of Mosque Development in Sydney
PDF
No ratings yet
Dunn - 2001 - Representations of Islam in The Politics of Mosque Development in Sydney
18 pages
A Corpus-Based Analysis of One of The Most Common Lexical Verbs in English
PDF
No ratings yet
A Corpus-Based Analysis of One of The Most Common Lexical Verbs in English
19 pages
Corpus Linguistics at Work - Language Teaching
PDF
100% (1)
Corpus Linguistics at Work - Language Teaching
33 pages
God, The Devil and You: A Systemic Functional Linguistic Analysis of The Language of Hillsong
PDF
No ratings yet
God, The Devil and You: A Systemic Functional Linguistic Analysis of The Language of Hillsong
32 pages
Methodological Problems in The Analysis
PDF
100% (1)
Methodological Problems in The Analysis
233 pages
1992 / 1 / 1a 4.3 Titration Q2
PDF
No ratings yet
1992 / 1 / 1a 4.3 Titration Q2
7 pages