Martin, A. J., Marsh, H. W., Williamson, A., y Debus, R. L. (2003) - Self-Handicapping, Defensive Pessimism, and Goal Orientation. A Qualitative Study of University Students.
Martin, A. J., Marsh, H. W., Williamson, A., y Debus, R. L. (2003) - Self-Handicapping, Defensive Pessimism, and Goal Orientation. A Qualitative Study of University Students.
discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/228611433
CITATIONS READS
107 62
4 authors, including:
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Andrew J Martin on 12 January 2017.
Martin, A.J., Marsh, H.W., Williamson, A., & Debus, R.L. (2003). Self-handicapping, defensive
pessimism, and goal orientation: A qualitative study of university students. Journal of Educational
This article may not exactly replicate the authoritative document published in the journal. It is not the
copy of record. The exact copy of record can be accessed via the DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.3.617.
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 1
Self-Handicapping, Defensive Pessimism, and Goal Orientation: A Qualitative Study of University Students
Raymond L. Debus
Faculty of Education
University of Sydney
Author Note
Requests for further information about this investigation should be sent to Dr Andrew Martin, SELF Research
Centre, University of Western Sydney, Bankstown, Locked Bag 1797, Penrith South DC, New South Wales,
Self-Handicapping, Defensive Pessimism, and Goal Orientation: A Qualitative Study of University Students
Abstract
Interviews with first year university students selected as high or low in either self-handicapping or defensive
pessimism identified (a) personal perspectives on the nature of self-handicapping and defensive pessimism, (b) the
perceived reasons why they engage these strategies and the perceived advantages that follow from them, and (c)
the extent to which ego goals and task goals mark their approach to their studies. The qualitative data not only
confirm previous quantitative research but also extend and illuminate what is currently known about these
strategies. The data also provide important contextual information on phenomena that have typically been studied
from quantitative perspectives. An important yield of approaching these phenomena from a qualitative perspective
is that new critical issues can emerge through giving expression to students voices. Such issues in the present
study included congruencies and differences in goal orientation for self-handicappers and defensive pessimists, the
social and academic costs of self-protective behavior, the control students feel they have over their self-protective
behavior, and the roles of the family and students culture in their tendency to self-protect.
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 3
Self-Handicapping, Defensive Pessimism, and Goal Orientation: A Qualitative Study of University Students
For some students, protecting self-worth is of paramount importance. In the academic context, students
self-worth is most threatened when they fail to perform successfully at a given task and there is the risk that they
may be seen to have low ability. According to the self-worth theory of motivation, ability is closely tied to self-
worth and so when there is doubt as to individuals ability, there is doubt as to their self-worth (Covington, 1984,
1992). A priority of some students, therefore, is to protect their sense of ability and to try to influence others
evaluations of their ability. The present study examines self-handicapping and defensive pessimism as two ways
students are able to do this. Given that the present analysis of self-handicapping and defensive pessimism is
located in an educational context, we consider it important to also investigate motivational constructs that may be
closely related to them and that are relevant to students academic experiences. One such construct, goal
orientation, is central to much educational research and is also explored in this study.
We hope to gain a number of insights through our analysis. First, we seek to explore more fully the
specific ways students engage in self-handicapping and defensive pessimism. Secondly, we aim to uncover
students views on the advantages that they see as emanating from their self-handicapping and defensive
pessimism. Thirdly, we examine what goal orientations underpin self-handicappers and defensive pessimists
educational approaches. Much research is now positing goal orientation as central to students approaches to their
studies and locating self-handicapping and defensive pessimism in this context brings these strategies into line
with central educational theory. Finally, quantitative perspectives on psychological phenomena can limit (through
pre-designed surveys or experimental manipulation) the extent to which new insights and information can emerge
from students and so this study is an opportunity to transport quantitative constructs into a qualitative
There has, in recent years, been growing recognition that quantitative and qualitative research methods
can complement each other in a way that both consolidates findings and sheds light on issues that could only be
researched by one and not the other. Numerous commentators over the past three decades have noted the benefits
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 4
to be derived from both approaches to research (Brewer & Hunter, 1989; Campbell, 1975; Cook & Campbell,
1979; Cronbach, 1975; Firestone, 1987; Gage, 1989; Glesne & Peshkin, 1992; Patton, 1990). In terms of self-
handicapping and defensive pessimism, the research to date has adopted a quantitative perspective. It is considered
that a qualitative analysis of these issues is not only timely but can add important personal insights into the lived
scales would have difficulty accessing. Moreover, self-handicapping and defensive pessimism are strategies that
may be manifested in a variety of ways for a variety of subtle or not so subtle reasons. It is not until individual
respondents are interviewed that the richness of this information can be used to better understand the constructs
For the most part, self-handicapping and defensive pessimism have been assessed amongst university
students using experimental manipulation or through self-report questionnaires. Data derived from studies using
either of these methodologies provide insights into causal relationships between constructs or provide
correlational information about associated constructs. As a result of this research, we know much about their
correlates or the experimental conditions under which they can be evoked. What we do not know much about are
the finer-grained features of self-protection embedded within them. Our qualitative study provides an opportunity
to find such information. In relation to goal orientation, the quantitative data are not able to provide the personal
detail about the ways self-handicappers and defensive pessimists differ in their orientation to their studies, and the
enables individuals to deflect the cause of poor performance away from their competence and on to the acquired
impediment. In doing so, self-handicappers avoid disconfirmation of a desired self-conception (Rhodewalt &
Davison, 1986). Examples of self-handicapping include the strategic reduction of effort, procrastination, ingestion
of drugs or alcohol, or the choice of performance debilitating circumstances (see Berglas & Jones, 1978; Higgins
& Harris, 1988; Martin, Marsh, & Debus, 2001a, 2001b, in press; Rhodewalt & Davison, 1986; Shepperd &
Arkin, 1989; Tice & Baumeister, 1990). In the event of failure, individuals have a ready excuse for it: For
example, the lack of effort is seen as the cause and not their lack of ability. Self-handicapping is to be
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 5
distinguished from attributions in that it occurs prior to rather than after an event. It should also be noted that not
all procrastination (and the like) is self-handicapping and that an anticipatory self-protective (or enhancing)
Defensive pessimists are students who set unrealistically low expectations prior to tasks that undergo
some form of assessment. There are a number of advantages associated with the defensive pessimistic strategy.
First, individuals are able to work through potential failure and steel themselves for this outcome. According to
Norem and Illingworth (1993; see also Norem & Cantor, 1986, 1990; Martin et al., 2001a, 2001b, in press),
defensive pessimists acknowledge apprehensions and work through them cognitively. A defensive pessimistic
strategy can also cushion the individual against debilitating anxiety prior to stress-provoking tasks and
motivate continued persistence in the face of that stress (Cantor & Norem, 1989, p. 93). In fact, projecting
lowered expectations can serve to set performance standards that are less difficult to achieve (Martin et al., 2001a,
2001b; Showers & Ruben, 1990), and may even lower the threshold for satisfactory performance (Baumgardner &
As noted earlier, much quantitative research exists in relation to self-handicapping and defensive
pessimism derived from correlational studies utilizing psychometric scales or from experimental manipulations. A
need exists for more focused in-depth perspectives on self-handicapping and defensive pessimism, particularly
from individuals high and low on these traits. This is considered important not only because such data can shed
further light on the quantitative research findings, but also because they can provide new perspectives on the
concepts and how they function in relation to educationally-relevant constructs such as goal orientation. Indeed if,
as may be the case, self-handicapping and defensive pessimism are ultimately not adaptive in the educational
context, students own views of these strategies and reasons for their occurrence must be taken into account if
A good deal of motivational and educational research has identified goal orientation as a pivotal construct
influencing the way students go about their studies. Goal orientation is comprised, inter alia, of ego-orientation
and task-orientation (Duda & Nicholls, 1992; Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Martin & Debus, 1998; Nicholls, 1989).
Consistent with previous work, we propose that individuals goal orientation plays a role in the degree to which
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 6
they are motivated to protect their self-worth. Ego-oriented individuals tend to be competitive, feel most
successful outperforming others (Duda & Nicholls, 1992; Thorkildsen & Nicholls, 1998), and see outcomes as due
primarily to ability rather than effort (Martin et al., 2001a, 2001b, in press; Middleton & Midgley, 1997; Nicholls,
Patashnick, & Nolen, 1985). From a self-protection perspective, ego-oriented students are particularly vulnerable
Task-oriented individuals, on the other hand, are more concerned with the task itself than with
outperforming others and see success as due primarily to effort rather than ability. From a self-protection
perspective, failure seen to be due to insufficient effort is not so threatening because the individual usually
believes he or she has control to avoid it next time by simply investing greater effort (Covington, 1992).
Hence, the quantitative data suggest that self-handicappers and defensive pessimists are more inclined to
have a history of concern about their ability relative to others and feel most successful when they outperform
others. The data also suggest that individuals low in the tendency to self-handicap or be defensively pessimistic
are more likely to have a history of mastery and task orientation. We seek to explore the extent to which this
emerges through students stories and then we explore the specific ways students goal orientations play out in
Method
Sample
A questionnaire assessing self-handicapping and defensive pessimism (see Martin, 1998) was
administered to 584 first year Education students enrolled in three universities in metropolitan Sydney (Australia).
Included in the questionnaire was an invitation for students to record their names and phone numbers if they were
willing to be interviewed at a later time. In total, 134 students volunteered this information. There was no
significant difference in self-handicapping between these students and the larger sample, t(575)=1.26, p=ns, nor
were there any differences in defensive pessimism, t(581)=.30, p=ns. A list of these students was generated on
which they were sorted in ascending order on the basis of their responses to the self-handicapping scale (e.g. I
often fool around the night before a test or exam so I have an excuse if I dont do as well as I hoped) and
defensive pessimism scale (e.g. No matter how well I have done in the past, I go into academic situations
expecting to do worse) in the questionnaire (adapted from Midgley, Arunkumar, & Urdan, 1996; Norem &
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 7
High self-handicappers in the eligibility list, for example, were the students who volunteered their contact
name and phone number and who were the highest ranked of these volunteers on the self-handicapping score.
Further sorting was done by sex, age, and institution. Respondents were selected using purposive sampling such
that the sample was intended to match approximately age, sex, and institutional composition of the larger sample
as well as ensuring that those sampled represented the four target academic profiles (low and high self-
handicapping and low and high defensive pessimism). The self-handicapping subscale used to select low and high
self-handicappers was reliable for students in the qualitative study (Cronbachs = .97) and for the larger sample
from which the qualitative sample was drawn (Cronbachs = .92). This was also the case for the defensive
pessimism subscale in terms of the subsample examined in this study (Cronbachs = .98) and the larger sample
Twenty-four students were selected and agreed to a follow-up interview. The mean age of respondents
was 21 years 10 months. Six respondents were male and 18 were female. Eight respondents were drawn from
each of the three institutions in such a way that two were high self-handicappers, two were low self-handicappers,
two were high defensive pessimists, and two were low defensive pessimists. Sample composition appears in Table
1. In aggregate, there was no significant difference between those interviewed and the larger sample in terms of
self-handicapping, t(575)=.47, p=ns and defensive pessimism, t(581)=.95, p=ns. In terms of self-handicapping and
defensive pessimism, then, the interview sample was not markedly different from the larger sample of 584
students.
Procedure
Data were collected using a semi-structured interview schedule (see Appendix A). Students were informed
that their honest and frank opinions were what the researchers were interested in and at no stage were any of their
responses reacted to with anything other than acceptance. At no stage were the terms self-handicapping or
defensive pessimism used by the interviewer in the interviews. Descriptions of the behavior rather than their
labels were used. The interview began with some questions about what subjects respondents were studying, their
thoughts about the course and university life in general. When some rapport had been established through the
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 8
introductory questions, the interviewer then began exploring some of the issues relevant to the study. The average
duration of each interview was approximately 45 minutes. The interview was audio taped and transcribed
Data Analysis
Formation of Categories
The manner in which the data are coded and categorized directly influences the way in which they are
analyzed and interpreted and so data categorization is one of the most important aspects of qualitative research.
Categorization can dictate the way themes are generated, the way these themes are integrated in subsequent
analyses, and ultimately the conclusions drawn from the study. One criticism of qualitative research is that
researchers often do not document how their categories are formed and so do not leave the important audit trail
for the reader (Lincoln & Guba, 1985). In suggesting ways by which the categorization process can be more
transparent, Constas (1992) has proposed that researchers document their category development by outlining the
components of categorization and the temporal aspects of category formation. He divides the components of
categorization into various elements, three of which - origination, verification, and temporal designation - are
relevant here.
Origination refers to where the responsibility or authority of categorization resides. In this study, authority
of categorization lay in (a) the researchers interests and theoretical viewpoints for example, the protective
rather than enhancing dimensions of self-handicapping were explored as the researchers were pursuing a research
program with self-protection as a central component, (b) the literature for example, conceptualizing in the
research literature was the basis upon which students goal orientations were differentiated, and (c) participants
responses for example, students identified issues not anticipated by the researchers or central in the literature
(issues such as family and cultural contexts relevant to self-handicapping). Also, subtleties in how questions were
positioned were influenced by participants reactions in the interview for example, questions about self-
handicapping were often couched in the third person so that students did not feel threatened or uncomfortable
when interviewed. Verification refers to the strategies used to justify the creation of categories. In this study,
categorization was guided by (a) rational considerations in which categories have face validity and the appearance
of logical connectedness (for example, categorization directly reflected the core research questions revolving
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 9
around advantages and disadvantages of self-handicapping and defensive pessimism and the precise ways in
which these are played out in students lives); and (b) referential considerations in which established research
findings are used to justify the category generation (for example, goal orientation research addresses the issue of
multiple goals and so categorization was developed to account for this). Temporal designation refers to the timing
of category development. In this study, many categories were determined a priori because they are largely based
on the extant quantitative literature which is quite prescriptive in terms of findings and relevant arguments and
issues. Notwithstanding this, there was a good degree of iterative processing in that additional categories were
formed in response to participants reports which, on the basis of the researchers interpretation, went beyond the
predetermined category structure (for example, the Thats interesting category was an outlet for such responses).
The broad categories into which the data were sorted are presented in Appendix B. Major divisions within
this categorization are illustrated with extracts from the interviews. The structure of the categories very much
reflects the structure of the interview. Where respondents provided information that went beyond the pre-
determined categories, their responses were assigned to a category labeled Thats interesting. Examples of such
data included details about respondents family or cultural background as deemed relevant to the central issues,
respondents perceived control over their defensive maneuvering, aspects of their behavior that went beyond
previous accounts in the literature, and unforeseen consequences of respondents defensive maneuvering. Hence,
in addition to the a priori determination of categories, there was ample scope provided for more inductive data
Analytic Methods
Goetz and LeCompte (1981; see also LeCompte & Schensul, 1999) identify a number of qualitative
analytic strategies including analytic induction, pure analytic induction (or constant comparison see Glaser &
Strauss, 1967), typological analyses, enumerative systems, and standardized observational protocols. The former
strategies are aligned to more grounded and naturalistic theoretical approaches while the latter are more typical of
conventional analytic approaches (see Lincoln & Guba, 1985). Because this study explores the ground lying
between traditionally quantitative approaches and qualitative perspectives, an intermediate approach was selected
as the analytic method of choice. On the Goetz-LeCompte continuum this method is the typological analytic
approach (see also Lincoln & Guba, 1985). Here the researcher draws on theory to develop questions, categories,
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 10
and assignment of data. Following this approach, we drew on theory seminal to self-worth motivation (Covington,
1992), self-handicapping (Berglas & Jones, 1978), defensive pessimism (Norem & Cantor, 1986), and goal
orientation (Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Nicholls, 1989). The typological approach is primarily based on what Goetz
and LeCompte refer to as a deductive-verificatory-objective process of data analysis typically drawing on previous
research and theorizing but which also enables inductive-generative-subjective processing so that additional or
qualifying themes can emerge (consistent with the categorization methods described above).
In terms of interpretation and conclusions drawn from the data, it is important to recognize that because
this is not a quantitative study some level of subjectivity was required. In the context of qualitative research,
summary statements such as more likely, less likely, most, least, and less are not quantitatively derived
nor statistically testable with confidence. Rather, they tend to reflect trends, nuances, and profiles. For example,
the fact that all self-handicappers do not share ego goals in a similar way or to a similar extent does not
necessarily mean that ego goals are not relevant to this type of student. Indeed, this is consistent with the fact that
self-handicapping and performance goals do not share 100 percent variance (see Martin et al., 2001b) and that
students holding ego goals can respond to these goals in different ways (see Dweck & Leggett, 1988). It should
also be recognized that task and ego goals are typically not mutually exclusive (Harackiewicz, Barron, Pintrich,
Elliot & Thrash, 2002; Martin & Debus, 1998) and a growing body of research shows that these goal orientations
are sometimes positively correlated (e.g., Marsh, Craven, Hinkley, & Debus, in press). Hence, some students can
endorse both task and ego goals. Indeed, related to the research issues under focus here, it is of interest whether
defensive pessimists are more likely than self-handicappers to reflect this complementary mix.
Data were analyzed using the NUDIST (Non-numerical Unstructured Data Indexing, Searching, and
Theorising) software program (Richards, Richards, McGalliard, & Sharrock, 1993). NUDIST is designed to
integrate a number of documents (in this case interview transcripts) for analysis; index components of text (in this
case interview responses); search for text; search indexing; and reorganize indexing systematically in a manner
that can both harness and generate theory. NUDIST assists in organizing coded data into nodes and node patterns
that essentially constitute the fabric of data analysis and theorizing findings.
Results
Self-Handicapping
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 11
the extremity and diversity of ways in which it occurs in their day-to-day lives. When asked how he went about
assignments and study for exams, Peter (HISH see Table 1 for acronyms) indicated that his self-handicapping
It's just like if I have an assignment due, say it's due on Monday and it's the weekend, I seem to just want
to watch TV or go out. It's just something that happens. You know you've got to do something but you
Sophie (HISH) actually had a special term for her self-handicapping. When asked whether she engaged in
such behavior she replied: Pointless time wasting? Yeah. I'll say, I've got study to do, well I really need to clean
my wardrobe. Yeah, I relate to that. I'm the queen of pointless time wasting. Sophie (HISH) reported that when
exams or assignments were approaching she would engage in such diverse activities as cleaning her wardrobe,
tidying under her bed, cleaning the fish tank, and cleaning the garage. Sophie (HISH) would even visit her out-of-
town grandmother who, curiously, only received visits when Sophies (HISH) exams were looming.
Carol (HISH) would be inclined to watch television and leave her study until very late in the night.
Christine (HISH) was much the same, reporting that she had been having difficulty adjusting to academic demands
and finds herself becoming occupied on other things, leaving study to the very last minute. Moreover, despite the
fact that she would promise herself not to do this in the future, each time an exam or assignment approached, she
seemed to do the same, no matter how hard I try. Rachel (HISH) said that when an exam approached she would
When it was put to low self-handicappers that some students seem to place obstacles in the way of
success, they, for the most part, did not identify with such behavior. Tania (LOSH) was well aware of the potential
distractions surrounding her while studying at home (she mentioned distractions as wide ranging as the television
and food in the refrigerator) and so that these would not interfere with her study, she went to the library. Reg
(LOSH) said that he could not go out to nightclubs and parties and expect to stay focused on his studies, so he
stayed at home. Gina (LOSH) reported that she was so inclined to get on top of her assignments and study that she
had no social life. She said that she declined invitations to go out in case it interfered with her study.
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 12
A range of reasons was given by the high self-handicappers as to why they engaged in such behavior. For
example, Rachel (HISH) reported that if she sat down and spent too much time studying she would get stressed
out and lose the plot. She said that procrastination was not so much used as an excuse as it was to avoid
becoming stressed. Interestingly, however, she later reported that if she did fail, she usually identified something
that interfered with her success such as going and getting drunk every night. It seems, then, that while she
explicitly denies using her behavior as an excuse, when failure does occur, she invokes her self-handicapping
behavior for this purpose. Carol (HISH) also felt somewhat uncomfortable with the notion that her behavior could
be used as an excuse. Instead, she suggested that her behavior was used as an explanation for poor performance.
Christine (HISH), however, was in little doubt that her behavior was used as an excuse if she did not do so
well. She said that it was always important to have an alibi in the form of an excuse. When Trudy (HISH) was
asked why she self-handicapped, she also identified the importance of an excuse:
If I leave it [study] to the last minute then I've got an excuse if I didn't do well. Any excuse is better than,
You're just not smart enough to do it. I know that I should be putting effort in all the time but then I've
Sophie (HISH) said that she engaged in self-handicapping behavior because by not trying then the whole
things not an issue, implying that a lack of effort may rob the event of its importance - with the importance of
the event at a minimum, the threat of failure is not so acute. In fact, when asked at the end of the interview if she
would like to add anything, she returned to her study style, again arguing that her self-handicapping was suited to
her and that she was better off engaging in that kind of behavior because her performance was better served by last
Although several high self-handicappers (Carol, Rachel, and Peter) reported that there were no real
advantages to their self-handicapping, other comments by them suggested otherwise. For example, Peter (HISH)
went on to identify the fact that it was a comfort to him when he did not do so well knowing that there was an
excuse. Christine (HISH) returned to the issue of self-handicapping when asked at the end of the interview if she
had anything to add. In contrast to her earlier thoughts, her comments reflected self-handicapping:
CHRISTINE. Then you've got an excuse . . . It's just easier to cope with if you think you haven't put as
CHRISTINE. From feeling like a failure because you're not good at it. It's easier to say, I failed because I
didn't put enough work into it than, I failed because I'm not good at it.
When the low self-handicappers were asked why some students might self-handicap, they, for the most
part, did not seem aware of the protective possibilities that self-handicapping presented. Angela (LOSH) reported
that now that they were all out of school with no-one to push them, some students were enjoying their new-found
freedom and this is why they behaved in ways that seemed to limit their success. Reg (LOSH) also reported that
he did not really know why but suggested that possibly those students wanted to have a break from study and then
Defensive Pessimism
All the students selected as being high in defensive pessimism reported that no matter how well they had
performed in previous exams and assignments, they expected to perform more poorly in the future. For example,
despite the fact that she had never failed anything, Dianne (HIDP) reported that she always had the feeling she
was going to fail. She reported that although a part of her thinks rationally about her likely performance, she finds
that her academic life is a balancing act between how she feels she will do in upcoming exams and assignments
Robert (HIDP) cited an instance when a lecturer announced that a few students had not performed well in
an assignment and he proceeded to spend the next day worrying that he was one of them (it turned out that he was
not). He said thinking like this was like walking a tightrope between knowing that he has passed everything
before but expecting that he will do more poorly in the future. Cassie (HIDP) also reported that despite having
done well in the past she thought she would do worse in the next assignment or exam. She reported that each time
she begins an assignment or studies for an exam, she starts from scratch in her expectations about how she will
perform. She said that she would slowly build her confidence as she felt she was mastering the subject matter, but
that no matter how confident she eventually feels about it, the next assessment task is met with unrealistic
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 14
pessimism.
All students selected as being low in defensive pessimism reported that they were generally optimistic
about future performance in assignments and exams. In contrast to the students high in defensive pessimism,
when asked how they would perform in the future given previous successful performance, these students replied
that they would be optimistic. As Bernadette (LODP) reported, basically when I'm on a roll, I feel good. I think,
Defensive pessimists were also asked for the reasons why they held unrealistically low expectations.
Gwen (HIDP) cited cultural reasons for why she was defensively pessimistic. According to her, there was much
pressure on her to perform well and that having lower expectations was a way of dealing with the fear of not
meeting these expectations: Coming from an [ethnic background withheld] family, if you get a good mark, they'll
expect you to do the same thing again. Also, she has found in the past that when she has been optimistic, these
positive expectations have not been fulfilled: In the past when I've looked at things optimistically, something bad
Joe (HIDP) reported that by holding unrealistically negative expectations and entertaining the possibility
of not doing so well, he does not feel let down if those expectations are realized. This disappointment seemed to
be what most defensive pessimists were particularly concerned to protect themselves from. As Robert (HIDP)
reported:
I try to be pessimistic 'cause that way I think the fall's less when you do actually come a cropper. . . I
think if I border slightly on the pessimistic then if I do better than I expected then it's a pleasant surprise
and if I do worse than expected then it's less of a fall. You just try to minimize those falls.
When Marie (HIDP), who has been having some difficulty performing in exams, was asked at the end of
the interview whether she had anything else to add, she reported that her negativity was learnt at home: My
parents have always said, Don't set your goals too high because you'll only get disappointed . . . They're always
According to Cassie (HIDP), Robert (HIDP), and Joe (HIDP), thinking more negatively is a good way of
motivating them into action. According to Joe (HIDP), his negative expectations motivated him into action by
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 15
making him feel more stressed. According to him, if youre negative you stress more and a bit of stress is good.
Robert (HIDP) felt that the pessimism was advantageous in that it kept him more emotionally balanced:
It keeps you a bit more even-keeled. You don't have the highs and lows, the peaks and troughs. You're
not walking around one week saying, I'm fantastic, I'm the greatest thing since sliced bread and not
living up to the expectations the next week and feeling like going out to slash your wrists.
Marie (HIDP) saw mixed blessings in her defensive pessimism. She recognized that it probably is not
good for her self-esteem but that in other respects it reduces the potential blow to the self-esteem by not thinking
that you're going to reach the moon when you can only get as far as the clouds. Dianne (HIDP) added that if it
turns out that she does do well, the advantage of having thought negatively is that she is pleasantly surprised.
High self-handicappers were more likely than low self-handicappers to endorse ego-goals and less likely to
subscribe to a mastery orientation. Carol (HISH) and Christine (HISH) reported that outperforming others would
make them feel more successful than having mastered something and that this was primarily because
outperformance was something visible to other people whereas mastery was not. Trudy (HISH) reported that
outperforming others makes her feel as though I'm not as down there as I thought - at least for a little while. She
said that she always likes to know how close to the top of the class she is and when asked whether mastery or
outperformance would make her feel most successful, she replied: Oh, the top of the class. Because that's how
you're measured in society" In fact, Trudy was actually dismissive of a task-orientation in the context of
university: No-one's going to care if you learn something new - maybe you can solve world peace by it or
something. It doesn't matter that you learnt great things getting those marks - that's not what uni's about.
Nonetheless, in some situations low self-handicappers also reported feeling successful when they
outperformed others, but in a slightly different sense than the high self-handicappers. For example, whereas Gina
(LOSH) was more interested in outperforming others, she did not conform to the stereotypical ego-oriented
student in that she held some regard for those she outperformed. When asked how she felt when she outperformed
others she replied, I feel proud, but I also feel sorry for whoever fails. Also, outperforming others was not
enjoyed for the sake of outperforming others, but in confirming her positive self-concept and also providing
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 16
diagnostic feedback: It's sort of the proof of the pudding . . . That I'm right in being confident in my abilities.
Low self-handicappers were more inclined than high self-handicappers to endorse mastery goals and to
recognize the contribution of mastery to their academic lives. According to Lucy (LOSH), I love that [mastery] . .
.That's something in yourself that you can feel good about rather than outperforming others and bagging them
out. Reg (LOSH) reported gaining great satisfaction through just doing or knowing something that you've never
done before and getting the hang of it and being able to do something well. Tania (LOSH) saw the long-term
benefits of mastery:
It's something that you're always going to have. As a teacher that's important because it doesn't matter that
you went better than everyone in a first year test. It's best if you can learn something that you use in the
The defensive pessimists also provided reports that were indicative of an ego orientation, but their
endorsement of ego-orientation was more equivocal. For example, Gwen (HIDP) reported that outperformance
would make her feel most successful but made the point that this would mean more to her not so much because
she outperformed others, but because, in true defensively pessimistic fashion, the relief of performing well was
great. Although Joe (HIDP) recognized that mastery was a more laudable goal, outperformance would make him
feel more successful primarily because university is a competitive environment and beating others is consistent
with this climate. He also reported that outperforming others is the best way to gauge how he is doing at
university: Don't get me wrong, I would rather feel better by mastering something, but it's all competition. If
you're doing better than others then you know you're doing well and that's how you gauge yourself
In contrast to self-handicappers who were not mastery oriented, defensive pessimists recognized that there
was value in mastery orientation but that this value was connected to performance issues. Such data are a good
example of the grey terrain to which we alluded earlier. For example, Marie (HIDP) reported that mastery was
more important to her because mastery was required in order to outperform others. When asked what was most
important to her, she replied, probably understanding, 'cause to beat other people, you have to have
understanding first. Cassie (HIDP) and Dianne (HIDP) implied that while success in competition would give
them satisfaction, it was difficult to compare their performance with that of others and so by default, mastery
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 17
became important. For example, Cassie (HIDP) reported that because everyone studied different subjects,
competition with them became difficult. Hence it became important to her to compete with herself and this was
more attuned to a task-orientation than an ego-orientation. Dianne (HIDP) also reported that she did not compete
with others because they may all have done badly and outperforming them was not a testament to much.
As another testament to the grey terrain, some low defensive pessimists were also ego-oriented. Again,
however, this was qualified. For example, Bernadette (LODP) reported that she enjoyed outperforming others but
more to satisfy personal standards: In some ways I like to outperform others but it's more a sense of doing better
for myself like, This is what I got last time and I'd like to do better than last time". Interestingly for Bernadette
(LODP), ego-orientation also held implications for mastery. She reported that she preferred to outperform others
because that's indicative of being able to understand. While it was important to Lynne (LODP) to outperform
others, this was because it made her feel good about herself and not because others had not performed as well as
her. Amanda (LODP), another low defensive pessimist, made the point that outperforming others is in no way a
cause for her to feel successful and in fact reported that she saw competition as competition with herself and not
with others.
Students low in defensive pessimism were more unambiguously concerned with mastery (note however that
for some there was also the presence of performance goals described above). For example, Bernadette (LODP)
reported that she loved challenge and the feeling that she has learnt something new. According to Brendan
(LODP), university is not a place of competition, it's more about learning. Amanda (LODP) agreed: If I can
grasp something by stretching my mind or worked really hard on something then I've made progression in myself
and I'm proud of myself more so than being able to do something better than someone else.
Discussion
A major aim of the present study is to use a qualitative approach to shed further light on self-handicapping
and defensive pessimism as a means to generate new insights, identify key complexities, illuminate the personal
perspectives of students in their first year at university, and give expression to students voices to allow new
perspectives, contexts, and insights relevant to self-handicapping and defensive pessimism emerge.
The qualitative data add to existing knowledge of the idiosyncratic ways in which students self-handicap
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 18
as well as the various lengths to which students will go to protect their self-worth. What these data also underscore
is that there is no shortage of potential handicaps at these students disposal. Motivations and behaviors of the
high self-handicappers contrasted quite markedly with those of the low self-handicappers who were more focused
and conscientious in their studies. The data in relation to low self-handicappers were particularly illuminating in
that these students seemed to be well aware of the distractions that posed a threat to their study. It seems,
therefore, that students are aware of the variety of ways in which they can distract themselves from the central
purpose of study but that high self-handicappers seize opportunities to engage in these distractions whereas the
low self-handicappers actively resist these potential distractions. Importantly, however, although the low self-
handicappers were well aware of potential distractions surrounding them, they were relatively less unaware of the
strategic ways in which these distractions could be used. In this sense the qualitative data lend support to
quantitative research findings which show that individuals who are not inclined to self-handicap are unaware of
the strategic way it can be used and as a consequence are more likely to accept at face value the self-handicapping
Not only do the data contribute to current understanding about the strategic nature of self-handicapping,
they also underscore and confirm quantitative data concerning the strategic nature of defensive pessimism (Cantor
& Norem, 1989; Martin et al., 2001a, 2001b; Norem & Cantor, 1990). For example, consistent with quantitative
work indicating how defensive pessimism can motivate students (Garcia & Pintrich, 1994; Norem & Cantor,
1986), some defensive pessimists used their defensive pessimism partly to motivate them into action. Defensive
pessimism was also strategic for others in that it obviated disappointment in the event of poor performance,
confirming quantitative data that it may cushion the individual in the event of failure (Martin et al., 2001a, 2001b;
Norem & Cantor, 1986) and establish lower and safer standards with which to compete (Baumgardner &
Brownlee, 1987; Martin et al., 2001a, 2001b). Indeed, the fact that some defensive pessimists reported that they
knew they were not going to fail and yet felt they were going to also suggested some cognitive posturing in
The results also indicated that the role of significant others as well as cultural factors can contribute to
defensive pessimism (see for example, Gwen - HIDP). Indeed, not only do results illustrate the family and cultural
factors that potentially underpin defensive pessimism, they also demonstrate how individuals react to others high
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 19
expectations in a defensive way, confirming quantitative findings (Baumgardner & Brownlee, 1987). However,
not only can the family inadvertently influence the tendency to engage in defensive pessimism, they can quite
directly influence students expectations. For example, Maries (HIDP) parents actually taught her that by holding
lower expectations for upcoming events she could avoid the disappointment if she fails. Indeed, early quantitative
work demonstrated that expected negative outcomes are not so hard-hitting as unexpected ones (Feather, 1967).
While the data were illustrative of the distinctiveness of self-handicapping and defensive pessimism, they
extend extant research by suggesting a number of ways in which they are similar. Both are strategic in the sense
that students are for the most part aware of their tendency to use the strategies and aware of the benefits that
follow from them. Following from this, another common feature is their self-protective nature: self-handicappers
are provided with an excuse (or explanation) for potential poor performance and defensive pessimists create lower
and safer standards against which to compete, minimizing the chances of failure. Importantly, however, while on
an aggregate basis the two share key features (e.g. self-protection), the diversity of ways in which students engage
these strategies highlights the uniqueness of students experiences and the idiosyncratic ways they negotiate their
Although the broad finding is that ego-orientation is a hallmark of self-handicapping, the data indicated
the unique ways in which ego-orientation occurs as well as the reasons respondents were ego-oriented. Indeed, this
is supported by previous research showing that similar goal orientations can be manifested in different ways
(Dweck & Leggett, 1988). For example, a theme that emerged was self-handicappers concern with outperforming
fellow students because such performance was visible to others (whereas mastery was not). This implies an
extrinsic orientation on the part of self-handicappers and their concern with how others view them. Indeed, this
confirms the relationship between self-handicapping and a public orientation obtained in quantitative research
(Ferrari, 1992; Martin et al., 2001a, 2001b; Shepperd & Arkin, 1989; Strube, 1986). Respondents identified other
reasons for their need to outperform others. For example, Trudy (HISH) reported that society judges individuals
worth on the basis of their academic achievement, and hence outperforming others was more important than
mastery. She also identified her familys emphasis on academic results, again underscoring the extrinsic
dimension of goal orientation. Indeed, the low self-handicappers responses contrasted markedly with those of
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 20
high self-handicappers in the sense that their reports were underpinned by an intrinsic orientation in which they
enjoyed the experience of mastery and challenge. On the other hand, the relationship between goal orientation and
defensive pessimism was not as clear-cut as that with self-handicapping. For example, some defensive pessimists
saw value in mastery and other defensive pessimists endorsement of ego-orientation was qualified. It seems, then,
that ego-orientation does not mark defensive pessimists approach to study to the extent that it does that of self-
handicappers.
Self-handicappers and defensive pessimists responses also indicated that their concern with
outperforming others was partly influenced by the learning climate in which they operated. According to Trudy
(HISH), university is about competition and there is little place for mastery in such an environment while Joe
(HIDP) reported that a concern with outperforming others was consistent with the competitive university climate.
Interestingly, not all students saw university as a place of competition. Brendan, low in defensive pessimism, saw
university as a place in which ones focus was on mastering the material and not a place of competition. Another
difference, then, between low and high self-protective students is not only their individual goal orientation, but
also their perceptions of the learning climate in terms of ego and task goals. This supports quantitative research
that has found a relationship between the (performance/ego-oriented) focus of the learning environment and self-
handicapping (Midgley & Urdan, 1995). More recent research utilizing statistical procedures appropriate for the
analysis of class-level data has confirmed the relationship between students perceptions of and emphasis on
relative ability in the classroom and self-handicapping (Urdan, Midgley, & Anderman, 1998).
The data clarify not only substantive issues relevant to self-protection, but also hold important
implications for educational practice. The data showed that some self-handicappers perceived little or no control
over their self-handicapping. This can be risky from an educational perspective as it can ultimately lead to some
form of learned helplessness (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978). It is important for educators to encourage
students to develop a greater sense of perceived control. For example, encouraging students to attribute outcomes
to effort and strategy promotes perceived control (Perry & Magnusson, 1989), adaptive approaches to learning
(Craven, Marsh, & Debus, 1991) and performance (Perry & Penner, 1990; Martin, 2001). Moreover, in the
educational context, a prime contributor to uncertain control is non-contingent or inconsistent feedback (Perry &
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 21
Dickens, 1988; Thompson, 1994). Administering reinforcement and feedback in a way that is commensurate with
students performance is one means of enhancing their perceived control over educational outcomes (Thompson,
1994).
Ego orientation seemed to mark self-handicappers approach to their studies and to a lesser extent was a
feature of defensive pessimists approach. It is important, then, to identify elements of instruction delivery and the
educational environment that contribute to the pursuit of ego goals. One element seems to be competition
(Covington, 1992; Duda & Nicholls, 1992; Nicholls, 1989). We propose that competitive environments not only
contribute to a tendency to pursue ego goals but also are relevant to self-protection. Competition for those not
guaranteed success could represent a climate in which self-protection may be an appealing option. For example,
Urdan et al. (1998) have identified the effects of the learning environment on self-handicapping. Cooperative
learning environments, on the other hand, are task-oriented and focus more on mastery through collaboration with
ones peers (Qin, Johnson, & Johnson, 1995). Balancing competitive interests with mastery and cooperation may
reduce the extent to which students are motivated to manoeuvre defensively. Importantly, we do not reject the
possible benefits to be gained through an ego or competitive focus (see Elliot & Church, 1997; Harackiewicz,
Barron, & Elliot, 1998; Martin & Debus, 1998). We do, however, suggest that an overly competitive focus at the
expense of mastery and cooperation can incur certain academic costs (Qin et al., 1995). What this implies is that
students may benefit from coordinating a competitive and mastery orientation and this brings into consideration
the issue of multiple goals (Ainley, 1993; Martin & Debus, 1998; Wentzel, 1989).
The data also illuminate the lengths to which students will go to avoid failure and self-protect. Covington
has outlined the variety of ways educational environments promote failure avoidance (Covington, 1992; see also
Martin, 2001) and has also suggested ways that this can be addressed. Essentially, the very nature and bases of
learning must be changed so that motives become goals and draw rather than drive the student (Covington, 1992;
Covington & Roberts, 1994). Such a program would encourage students to gain knowledge for masterys sake
rather than for the sake of performance, encourage students to serve the interests of the group, and to give
expression to their creativity and curiosity. Changing the reward system has also been proposed as a way in which
the purpose of learning can be altered. Covington and Roberts suggest that reward should be based on students
meeting personal standards rather than outperforming others. Hence, the student is encouraged to become success-
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 22
oriented rather than failure avoiding or failure accepting. When students are success-oriented, they are then in a
stronger position to learn and as Covington and Roberts note, have even been shown to perform well in
competitive scenarios (see also Elliot & Harackiewicz, 1994; Harackiewicz & Elliot, 1993).
Limitations
There are a number of limitations that require consideration when interpreting the results. The data in this
study were static - collected at only one point in time. Although qualitative research is not centrally concerned
with establishing causal relationships to the extent that quantitative research often is, our conceptualization of the
proposed model of the self-protection process begs the question of what precedes what. Also, all the data were
derived from self-reports. Although this study is based on students personal perspectives, qualitative research
need not be based on self-report alone. There is scope for observational data and reports by others close to the
student. Also, artefacts such as course results and examples of their work would be useful. It is therefore important
that future research examines the issues using data derived from additional sources.
Another concern is the extent to which students are aware of their use of self-protection strategies, or, the
extent to which they are aware of the reasons for their use of these strategies. It may be that individuals cannot
access their self-protective motivations in great detail and to the extent that this is the case, the present data are
limited. Another issue concerns the possibility that some self-protective students are not inclined to acknowledge
their adoption of self-protection strategies - particularly self-handicapping. Although it has been argued that
individuals are prepared to admit to their self-protection (Arkin & Oleson, 1998), the present results must be
interpreted with the possibility of defensive self-reports in mind. Related to this, it must be recognised that
interviews were typically conducted within one hour and in some cases this might mean that more detailed
insights into students academic lives were limited. Future researchers might immerse themselves in students
academic lives on a more extended basis. For example, talking with students just before and after major
assessment periods would provide important data on the strategies under focus. Also related to time constraints,
the time available to interview students required some weighting of the depth to which some constructs were
explored. For example, although students were asked detailed questions about their self-handicapping and
defensive pessimism, questions in relation to self-regulation were broader. Again, immersing in students
academic lives over a longer period of time would provide greater detail about all relevant constructs.
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 23
Future Research
Some self-handicappers and defensive pessimists noted the costs of their defensive manoeuvring, beyond
simply not performing well. These findings suggest that the consequences of self-handicapping and defensive
pessimism are not just restricted to the academic domain but are felt in a variety of areas of students lives. One
direction for future research is to explore more fully the non-academic (e.g., social and affective) consequences of
The data suggested that cultural factors may also underpin some students defensive manoeuvring.
Although some research has examined cultural differences in self-handicapping and negative attitudes (e.g.,
Midgley et al., 1996), this tends to be restricted to ethnic minorities in the North American context. Cross-cultural
comparisons are needed. Related to the issue of culture is the role of the family in influencing students tendency
to engage in self-protective strategies. The present data are suggestive of some family influence in this respect, but
further work is required to determine the precise nature and extent of this influence. Indeed, this need for further
work into cultural and family factors is not only important in relation to self-protection but is also important from
An interesting extension of students goal orientation was their perceptions of the learning climate. The
data indicated that self-handicappers and defensive pessimists tended to see the learning climate as competitive
and this evoked a need to outperform others. This supports previous quantitative findings (Midgley & Urdan,
1995), but some issues remain unclear. For example, the learning climate and its impact on defensive pessimism
have not been addressed to date. Also, the impact of altering the learning climate (e.g., from competitive to
cooperative - see Qin et al., 1995) on students academic strategies has not been studied and future research might
focus on this with a view to identifying interventions that can take place at class and institutional levels. The need
for further research in relation to climate is consistent with recent reviews of achievement goal theory (Urdan,
1997) and data have recently emerged showing a relationship between the nature of the learning environment and
Conclusion
The present study explored students personal insights into self-handicapping and defensive pessimism
Although previous quantitative findings have identified the broad nature of self-handicapping and defensive
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 24
pessimism, our qualitative data have indicated (a) the idiosyncratic ways in which students engage these strategies,
(b) the salience of these strategies in students lives, (c) the strategic nature of these strategies, particularly in the
context of self-worth protection, and (d) the various factors that contribute to students defensive posturing.
Students personal perspectives shed new light on congruencies and differences in goal orientation for self-
handicappers and defensive pessimists, the social and academic costs of self-protective behavior, the control
students feel they have over their self-protective behavior, and the roles of the family and students culture in their
tendency to self-protect. The strength of this qualitative approach to phenomena typically studied from a
quantitative perspective lies in its detailed and contextualized exploration of self-handicappers and defensive
pessimists motivations and behaviors. In listening to students stories about the diverse ways they go about their
studies in a self-protective manner, we can better understand the factors other than ability which lead to success or
failure at university.
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 25
References
Abramson, L.Y., Seligman, M.E.P., & Teasdale, J. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and
Ainley, M.D. (1993). Styles of engagement with learning: Multidimensional assessment of their relationship with
strategy use and school achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 395-405.
Arkin, R.M., & Oleson, K.C. (1998). Self-handicapping. In J.M. Darley & J. Cooper (Eds.). Attribution and social
interaction: The legacy of Edward E. Jones. Washington DC: American Psychological Association.
Baumgardner, A.H., & Brownlee, E.A. (1987). Strategic failure in social interaction: Evidence for expectancy
Berglas, S., & Jones, E.E. (1978). Drug choice as a self-handicapping strategy in response to noncontingent
Brewer, J., & Hunter, A. (1989). Multimethod research: A synthesis of styles. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Campbell, D.T. (1975). Degrees of freedom and the case study. Comparative Political Studies, 8, 178-193.
Cantor, N., & Norem, J.K. (1989). Defensive pessimism and stress and coping. Social Cognition, 7, 92-112.
Constas, M.A. (1992). Qualitative analysis as a public event: The documentation of category development
Cook, T.D., & Campbell, D.T. (1979). Quasi-experimentation: Design and analysis issues for field settings.
Covington, M.V. (1984). The motive for self-worth. In R. Ames & C. Ames (eds). Research on motivation in
Covington, M.V. (1992). Making the grade: A self-worth perspective on motivation and school reform.
Covington, M.V., & Roberts, B.W. (1994). Self-worth and college achievement: Motivational and personality
correlates. In P.R. Pintrich., D.R. Brown., & C.E. Weinstein (Eds.). Student motivation, cognition, and
Craven, R.G. Marsh, H.W., & Debus, R. (1991). Effects of internally focused feedback and attributional feedback
Cronbach, L.J. (1975). Beyond the two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 30, 116-127.
Duda, J.L., & Nicholls, J.G. (1992). Dimensions of achievement motivation in schoolwork and sport. Journal of
Dweck, C.S., & Leggett, E.L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological
Elliot, A.J., & Church, M.A. (1997). A hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation.
Elliot, A.J., & Harackiewicz, J.M. (1994). Goal setting, achievement orientation, and intrinsic motivation: A
Feather, N.T. (1967). Level of aspiration and performance variability. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 6, 37-46.
Ferrari, J.R. (1992). Procrastinators and perfect behavior: An exploratory factor-analysis of self-presentation, self-
Firestone, W. (1987). Meaning in method: The rhetoric of quantitative and qualitative research. Educational
Gage, N. (1989). The paradigm wars and their aftermath. Educational Researcher, 18, 4-10.
Garcia, T., & Pintrich, P.R. (1994). Regulating motivation and cognition in the classroom: The role of self-
schemas and self-regulatory strategies. In D.H. Schunk & B.J. Zimmerman (eds). Self-regulation of learning
Glaser, B.G., & Strauss, A,L. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory. Chicago, IL: Aldine.
Glesne, C., & Peshkin, A. (1992). Becoming qualitative researchers: An introduction. New York: Longman.
Goetz, J.P., & LeCompte, M.D. (1981). Ethnographic research and the problem of data reduction. Anthropology
Harackiewicz, J.M., Barron, K.E. & Elliot, A.J. (1998). Rethinking achievement goals: When are they adaptive for
Harackiewicz, J.M., Barron, K.E., Pintrich, P., Elliot, A.J., & Thrash, T.M. (2002). Revision of achievement goal
Harackiewicz, J.M., & Elliot, A.J. (1993). Achievement goals and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and
Higgins, R.L., & Harris, R.N. (1988). Strategic 'alcohol' use: Drinking to self-handicap. Journal of Social and
LeCompte, M.D., & Schensul, J.J. (1999). Analyzing and interpreting ethnographic data. Walnut Creek, CA:
AltMira Press.
Lincoln, Y., & Guba, E. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Beverly Hills: Sage.
Marsh, H. W., Craven, R. G., Hinkley, J., D. & Debus, R. L. (in press). Evaluation of the Big-Two-Factor theory
Martin, A.J. (1998). Self-handicapping and defensive pessimism: Predictors and consequences from a self-worth
Martin, A.J. (2001). The Student Motivation Scale: A tool for measuring and enhancing motivation. Australian
Martin, A.J. & Debus, R.L. (1998). Self-reports of mathematics self-concept and educational outcomes: The roles
Martin, A.J. Marsh, H.W., & Debus, R.L. (2001a). A quadripolar need achievement representation of self-
handicapping and defensive pessimism. American Educational Research Journal, 38, 583-610.
Martin, A.J., Marsh, H.W., & Debus, R.L. (2001b). Self-handicapping and defensive pessimism: Exploring a
model of predictors and outcomes from a self-protection perspective. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93,
87-102.
Martin, A.J. Marsh, H.W., & Debus, R.L. (in press). Self-handicapping and defensive pessimism: A model of
Middleton, M.J., & Midgley, C. (1997). Avoiding the demonstration of lack of ability: An unexplored aspect of
Midgley, C., Arunkumar, R., & Urdan, T.C. (1996). If I dont do well tomorrow, theres a reason: Predictors of
adolescents use of academic self-handicapping strategies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 423-434.
Midgley, C., & Urdan, T. (1995). Predictors of middle school students use of self-handicapping strategies.
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 28
Nicholls, J.G. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic education. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Nicholls, J.G., Patashnick, M., & Nolen, S.B. (1985). Adolescents theories of education. Journal of Educational
Norem, J.K., & Cantor, N. (1986). Defensive pessimism: Harnessing anxiety as motivation. Journal of Personality
Norem, J.K., & Cantor, N. (1990). Cognitive strategies, coping and perceptions of competence. In R.J. Sternberg
& J. Kolligian Jr. (eds). Competence considered. New Haven CT: Yale University Press.
Norem, J.K., & Illingworth, K.S.S. (1993). Strategy-dependent effects of reflecting on self and tasks: Some
implications of optimism and defensive pessimism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 822-
835.
Patton, M. (1990). Qualitative evaluation and research methods. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Perry, R.P., & Dickens, W.J. (1988). Perceived control and instruction in the college classroom: Some
Perry, R.P., & Magnusson, J.L. (1989). Causal attributions and perceived performance: Consequences for college
students achievement and perceived control in different instructional conditions. Journal of Educational
Perry, R.P., & Penner, K.S. (1990). Enhancing academic achievement in college students through attributional
Qin, Z., Johnson, D.W., & Johnson, R.T. (1995). Cooperative versus competitive efforts and problem solving.
Rhodewalt, F., & Davison, J. (1986). Self-handicapping and subsequent performance: Role of outcome valance
Richards, T., Richards, L., McGalliard, J., & Sharrock, B. (1993). NUDIST 2.3: Users Manual. Melbourne:
Replee.
Scheier, M.F., Carver, C.S., & Bridges, M.N. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and test anxiety,
self-mastery, and self-esteem): A reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality and Social
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 29
Shepperd, J.A., & Arkin, R.M. (1989). Determinants of self-handicapping: Task importance and the effects of
Showers, C., & Ruben, C. (1990). Distinguishing defensive pessimism from depression: Negative expectations
and positive coping mechanisms. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 385-399.
Smith, D.S., & Strube, M.J. (1991). Self-protective tendencies as moderators of self-handicapping impressions.
Strube, M.J. (1986). An analysis of the Self-Handicapping Scale. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 7, 211-
224.
Thompson, T. (1994). Self-worth protection: Review and implications for the classroom. Educational Review, 46,
259-274.
Thorkildsen, T.A., & Nicholls, J.G. (1998). Fifth graders achievement orientations and beliefs: Individual and
Tice, D.M., & Baumeister, R.F. (1990). Self-esteem, self-handicapping, and self-presentation: The strategy of
Urdan, T.C. (1997). Achievement goal theory: Past results, future directions. In M.L. Maehr & P.R. Pintrich (Eds).
Urdan, T.C., Midgley, C., & Anderman, E.M. (1998). The role of classroom goal structure in students use of self-
Wentzel, K.R. (1989). Adolescent classroom goals, standards for performance, and academic achievement: An
Appendix A
Summary of semi-structured interview schedule
Would you say you feel more successful when you outperform others or when you learn/master something
new?
Why do you say that?
What does outperforming others/mastery mean to you?
If I ask you to look down the track to your next assignment or exam, do you think youll do the same, better, or
worse than youve done before?
Why do you say that?
How does it make you feel when you go into an exam thinking more negatively/positively than previous
experience would predict?
Does thinking this way have any advantages/disadvantages?
Some students do things that seem to get in the way of their success. For example, they might procrastinate
before an upcoming assignment or become occupied on other things before the exams. Other students get onto
the job of doing their assignments and studying for the exam and maximize their chances of success. Would
you relate to either of these two students?
If so, in what ways do you do some of these things?
Why do you think you do these things?
What advantages and disadvantages might follow from doing these things?
Thats all the questions I have to ask you. Is there anything you would like to add? Is there anything you feel I
havent touched or focused on? Is there anything you would like to emphasize? Is there a particular way you go
about your studies that I havent addressed?
Self-Handicapping and Defensive Pessimism 31
Appendix B
Categories into which the data were sorted and extracts from interviews that reflect major categories
1. GOAL ORIENTATION
1.1 Ego-orientation/competitiveness
(e.g. Oh, the top of the class. Because that's how you're measured in society" Trudy, HISH)
1.1.1 Feelings of success through outperforming others
1.1.2 Feelings of success not dependent on outperforming others
1.1.3 Feelings of success only partly determined by outperforming others
1.1.4 Thats interesting
1.2 Task-orientation
(e.g. I love that [mastery]. . .That's something in yourself that you can feel good about rather than outperforming
others Lucy, LOSH)
1.2.1 Feelings of success through mastery and new learning
1.2.2 Feelings of success not dependent on mastery or new learning
1.2.3 Feelings of success only partly dependent on mastery
1.2.4 Both mastery and competitive concerns
1.2.5 Thats interesting
2. SELF-PROTECTION STRATEGIES
2.1 Self-handicapping
Table 1
Participants in study*