Theoretical Models and Processes of Reading
Theoretical Models and Processes of Reading
mem e
ection of
U.S:DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH::
EDUCATION A WELFARE-
= - OFFICE OF EDUCATION7
THISsDOCUMENT,=HAS=HEEN _REPRO._
DUCED,EXACTLY=AS RECEIVED FROM=
THE-PERSON OR ORGANIZATION_ORIG =
== INATING ITzPOINTS OF VIEW OR OPIN
-=- =IONS -STATED=DO,NOT-sNECESSARILYn: ==
REPRESENPOFFICIAGOFFICE_IOPEDU-
CATION POSITION OR POLICY,==--,
W/7/11..
47G 7:7 V, n
--\,w
mversit
attors4n
eactio eoreuca a
a esear srz
u strata-
____- ______ _-
an Um,- ro jret_
iforn a-
F
I
F
F
F
F
IF
I IF
h
F
1I
F
F}I F F
F
F 1
L
Fik r
L, F' F F
hF
FF I
F
F F
F FF
F
F
F
F
FF1
F F
F
F
hi
,==='-'a_3
c z__---- i --_____- -___ --_ --:_T__ =
_
-=-_=if_:_--_i_r=i---_---- ____
___
_
rrerace
--=--
=
i=----rAtid-14&=tocomplete what we do when we read
ol 'st's achievements, for it
would be most-intricate workings
of the human iind,= as we o-uthel the tangled -story
ei,thost-irernarkablerispeci
=
ormance that civiliation
ame-
_
-===-
_
= s
:_=--
_ _ = _ _ =__ _=___
T
=-=
-TT:
RETICAL MODELS 1::AND-= PROCESSES;
- __ = =_ s= -
_ _ _ _
__
r-E
-
-
trictec
e=weou
=early istu
-_-__ =-
_
_
-- ----------_:= - __,,_
_
_
ereri
vague-
, _-_-
-
individu s
formed come
-reinitiative=4
ro em solvin
leving= readers
su
rsiiteiY
-- -----
THEORETICALi'MODELS-_AND PRoCESSES-Dr-_READING
tructure twationa
------f-c---7--, ---
- -_. _- --- =
-,_--__,_,__,______
=
z
---- ,------
habits, of course, has partly been a consequence of the view that
linguistic mastery has been regarded as functionally complete by
school entrance age. His summary of language development stud_ ies
during the elementary school years provides a useful overview of the
kinds of investigations which contemporary language theory= has
=
35
done using college subjects: With= one exception, the study using
college subjects was identical to the one using kindergarten children.
Some =of the college subjects getting look-say training were able to
read words on the transfer list on first presentation whereas virtually
none of the look-say kindergarten subjects could do this An-analysis
of how the college subjects did this revealed that those subjects
who had used their knowledge of reading and who had learned
letter sound correspondencei =on their own, were able to transfer
this knowledge to reading the= transfer list. Taken together, the two
studiei give strong support to the= notion that knowledge of letter
sound correspondence is an important basis for transfer to reading
new words.
In the kindergarten study of the effect of phonic versus look-say
training, a task analysis of the skills necessary for transfer to reading
new words indicated that knowledge of letter-sounds as well as the
ability to blend these sounds was required. As the data from the
experiment revealed, the group getting botli kinds of training was
superior to the groups which did not get the necessary training. The
implications of this experiment for the teaching of reading are that
task analyses can be most helpful in planning instructional sequences.
In doing_task_analyses,_specific objectives are_identified.Then,-pre-
on the other hand, may pose another kind of possible mismatch,
that which results from differences between the formal features of
the language of instruction and the formal features of language
which the child has learned in his particular linguistic community.
The instructional resolution of such a mismatch is currently a topic
of Considerable* debate- specifically regarding whether or not focus
should be placed =on altering the language of instruction to match
the language of the child or vice versa. Pathological variation, in
turn, poses the possibility that, in addition to whatever develop-
mental and dialectal differences might exist, special instructional
techniques may be needed that-are not ordinarily encompassed in
a reading program; as, for example, the techniques that may be used
in working with the visually or auditorily= handicapped child.
These three factorsdevelopmental, dialectal, and pathological
are not intended-as inclusive fcirmk of linguistic variation. None-
theless, they do call attention to the need_for a model of reading
instruction to outline instructional-interVentions which are cued by
the kinds of linguistic =variations which can and do occur in the
language of the elementary school child.
A concluding remark: despite the growing data about language
acquisition, Ruddell's admonition that subitantial work in other
dimensions of language in relation to reading is needed is war-
ranted. His presentation 'clearly indicates that, despite some signif-
icant theoretical forinulations and equally significant research, there
is still "a good distance to go in = developing =a theory of reading and,
from such a theory, a model of reading instruction. Ruddell's own
contributions, and hie' comprehensive summary of the contributions
of other researchers, are welcome additions to the accumulating data
about the reading process and its language base.
-to learn to-read. One group learned-a list-where the words were easy
to -_visually-discriminate -from each, other. This group ___was called- -the
high diScriminab-, - -group- and theit-Avords were-spelled BE,-
MI, so.- Another groty -was- called-- the low-discriMinability group and
their words-were difficult to Visually_-_discriminate from each other.
The words were spelled_ SE, -SA,- -ME, .MA. coMparison- bet*een -the
two groups on-speed:of-learning :to read :the -words indicated that the
-group gettingthe-list with-highlydiScritninablei-vordS'excelled: Then
a-,test--wasiivento-deterininetthetrettersluiedifas_tties-,forrecOgnition:
The test_reyealed_=_t which had-
learned =more-quickly,- =hatF,Ustd1--single tertues-- as tbt-basis-- for
retognitiOn._Tht ictAkf:distriminabiligEgrou ;--although= earning-less
-rapidlyhad=use&bothzletteislfss-Ahef -_-xteognitis*=14Then
children -own-a!wordpelled,__mo,--a_mord=they
-in
_
Classical Research -Which Has Influenced
Current Thinking
The research since the -1960's indicates that children tend to
select a detail such as a letter as_ a cue for word recognition. This
finding: is at-variance with the more commonly= held= belief that
children-_userhe whole word or word the cue. How-did this
latter view ongrnate?
-Prior to: 1900;- Cattell,- And -1
e-zettrrenCere_
whole words livor --=_Aticressitri*Or -reco
*art, partly_:_tonsU o
ooks on reading=have continued to=refer tti; these studies Because of
=
that shape was the_ cue least_ used -by- children. Furtherindre,- it is
apparent- -that- a strategy-of learninglo -read using word shape and
length provides a poor-basis for transfer to-reading new words.
Cattell (6), in 1885,=pUblished a Study-which led to the present
belief that beginning readers use the whole word- in word recogni._
don. The-major finding in this study was that pnderatould recognize
a short common word in-slightly less time than it-takes to recognize
a single letter. There are several flaws- in this study which should
make the reader cautious about concluding that the results of this
study apply to children learning how to read. In this study Cattell
used a small number of highly educaied adults. He had them read
aloud as quickly as possible a= passage from Oulliver's Travels, spell
the letters contained in the words, and then read a passage = consisting
of 100 common nouns. The most serious error was that the time
to pronounce the words is confused with the time it takes to recognize
the words. Secondly, he used only skilled readers, and consequently,
the findings =-are- not valid for children.
In ttel lis
thus eliminatin lem in _the - other study where
the time to ronoMice e =word con un e wi e tfine to
recognize e =word: oun ex ure thne, two
unconnected letters or two unconnected wor could _recognized.
in he used adults but he nientiOn one- nine - year -old -boy in
the study Who was descr as being su "or in reading ability to
some of the adults.
The Cattell studies demonstrate= that = skilled teaders do not
engage in letter-by-letter processin en the time for
recognizing a-word would be the - sum =of=the time= necessary_ for
recognizing each of the letters. Manypeopie have interpreted Cattell's
results to mean that a skill er uses the entire word as the unit
for recognition. = =Tfi s interpretation =is not valid because Cattell's
. -
experiments were not designed to answer the= question= of what cues
are actually used by skilled readers in word recognition. It is possible
that Cattell's readers recognized_ just some of the letters in the word
and were able to correctly identify the word from a partial percept.
To infer = from these studies that naive readers use the entire word as
a cue in learning to read is an error, partly because naive readers were
not used in these studies.
Second, it is now known that naive readers tend to select a detail
rather than the entire word. Third, while-it is known that the adult
can perceive several letters together as a unit in word recognition, no
one knows at the present time when beginning readers perceive these
higher order units.
A higher order unit is a spelling pattern having invariant spell-
ing-to-sound correspondence. For example, a higher order unit might
be gh in words like rough or tough. Adults can recognize higher
34 THEORETICAL- MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
order units which conform to English spelling rules even when they
are presented in nonsense words (9). The critical question is How
do beginning_ readers learn the higher order units?
In =order to study how beginning readers learn higher order
units, Gibson et al (10) gave kindergarteners and first graders a task
in which it was possible for them to learn patterns =of spelling. The
child was given -a set of eight cards.-=Four= of the cards =had words with
a higher order unit such as LACK, MUCE, DECK, an SOCK. e other
four car a wor such as = LA ttrcFr EEZ and so x -will no
gher order unit. The attls were- esen
r rzE), and the child= simply -point o one of the =cards -If he
pointed _to e card _wi
cx,he=was-told drat he e =to consisten
point to -flie correctca e
critninatin er uni was i
Gibson n at for some to nce-im ove
indicating a were learning nate an a tract
_incriminate
the common s llin
The- final topic w ich iscussed _relates to the = findings of
a= laboratorystudy testing the a ect o onic versus-look-say reading
training on transfer to tea in -newAvor n is study (11) kinder-
garten children were given = phonic - blend- training and= then were
randomly assigned to a = look- say, phonic, or control group k-say
training consisted of learning to-rea a list of words: e = letters
of these words= were = used in_ new combinations to rm-the words
used in =the transfer list. Phonic training consisted of learning letter
sounds. These letters were= used in the transfer list of words. The
control group got an irrelevant task to perforin. Following training, -
all the subjects were given the same list of-transfer words. Pint the
subjects were shown the words and were asked to read them-without
any help. Then they were given instruction and the number of trials
required for learning the entire = list was computed. The results in-
dicated that the phonic trained group was significantly better than
the other two groups -in number of words read without any help
and speed of learning the entire list. There was no significant differ-
ence between the look-say and control groups on either o_f these
measures, indicating that= look-say training did not provide a basis
for positive transfer to reading new words. A similar study (2) was
Cues_ Used in Learning_ to Read," Journal of Edutational Psychology, 57
(1966), 337-340.
Samuels, S. J., -and _M.-_C. Wittrock. "Word Association Strength and Read-
ing-Acquisition." JournOl of Psychology, in -press. ._
26:-iaticrC,=-Tinker, -Teacking-Elementary=Rea4ing.--llew-York:
_ =
on the basis of overall shape. This had been the least salient cue
for the children.
These results remind us once more that ;t should not be
assumed thai adults and thiiciren behave in the same manner on this
type of visual matching task. Indeed, it -is most interesting to note
the fact that the mast widely used reading method over the past
thirty years (the look-say or whole word) has stressed identification
of words on the basis of overall shape and configuration. But it is
rchildren-wIro-sonretimes-show-this-strategrin-word----
recognition.
'The second general basis -for word recognition lies in the re-
lationship between the graphic characteristics and the nature of the
spoken -language. Early work focused on the correspondences be-
tween individual letters and the sounds they represented. However,
English spelling is quite irregular wh-en individual letters and sounds
are considered "Auk; fact suggested-that thinking of correspondences
in term_ s of single letters was not useful, which led in turn- to- a
search for a more appropriate unit. It appears_that clusters of letters
do have more stable relationships with sound patterns, and it has
been suggested that these "spelling patterns" are critical units for
perception (5). That is, while the letter c alone is not sufficient to
signal a consistent pronunciation, initial ca or ce does correlate well
with specific phonemic patterns.
Eleanor Gibson's hypothesis (5) that spelling-to-sound invari-
ance accounts for the fact that "pronounceable" pseudowords such
as glurch are more easily recognized in a tachiStoscopic exposure
than are unpronounceable pseudowords such as crurgl has not held
up, largely oti the buds of her own research showing that deaf chil-
dren behaVe in the same way as normals (6). This leaves us with
the question of what it actually is that makes certain "spelling pat-
tern?' or letter clusters more easily recognized than others. Perhaps
it will turn out after all not to depend heavily on the spoken lan-
guage.
Here may be an appropriate point -to bring up another experi-
ment that dealt with methods of training. Sometimes assumptions
have been made and accepted wholeheartedly with little or no ob-
jective data to support them. Such is the character of the recom-
mendation, made by Bloomfield (2) and echoed by almost all other
proponents of linguistics reading methods, that only simple, single
WILLIAMS 43
such findings as these to actual instruction. For one thing, few pro-
grams teach letter-sound correspondences in isolation. Further work
is in progress, focusing on spelling patterns presented in the con-
text of words. At present, however, in the absence of sufficient data
on which to base a final decision, it would seem reasonable to provide
at least some variationsome kind of concurrent trainingwhen
presenting multiple grapheme-phoneme correspondences. Certainly
the data indicate that critical questioning of some of the time-
honored assumptions and recommendations is desirable.
There is a third general category of, cues that is used in word
-recognition: the context in which the word appears. That is, a reader
makes use of the information contained in the rest of the sentence
in his efforts to recognize the word. For example, in the sentence "He
. walked toward the . . . ," there are certain limitations,. of both a
grammatical and a semantic nature, on the words that can complete
the sentence in an acceptable fashion. Investigators-have =found that
it is indeed easier to recognize a word when it is presented in context
than when it is .presented alone. For example, Samuels has demon-
strated that recognition speed for the response item in a word pair
can be facilitated or interfered with by an appropriate selection of-
stimulus items.
More emphasis is being placed on context, or, to state it another
way, the search for cues when units larger than a- single word are
considered. Only a few years ago, psychologists were comfortable
with a very simple and narrow definition of reading, one that con-
centrated on what was distinctive about reading. Such definitions put
major emphasis on the decoding process, of course.
Today the focus has shifted. Decoding is necessary but not
sufficient, and other aspects of "reading"notably, of course, com-
prehensionhave been attracting attention (11). The emergence of
such interest undoubtedly reflects the very strong influence of
cognitive psychology. Reading now tends to be tied to information-
processing and other related concepts. Definitions also seem to be
growing more general and less focused on what is unique to reading.
One can reasonably describe skilled reading, I believe, as a process
in which the reader samples the cues on the printed page. Using
these partial cues together with previous knowledge both about
printed pages and about the world, the reader forms hypotheses (or
WILLIAMS 45
to the five year old. More work of the type that is so well represented
by Samuels' experiments is sorely needed. .1
REFERENCES
1. Ackerman, M. D., and J. P. Williams. "Simultaneous vs. Successive Discrim-
ination Trainin& as a Function of Stimulus Similarity," paper presented at
meetings of the American Educational Research Association, February 1969.
2. Bloomfield, L., and Clarence L. Barnhart. Let's Read: A Linguistic Ap-
proach. Detroit: Wayne State University Press, 1961.
3. Blumberg, E L., D. V. Williams, and J. P. Milliams. "What Cues Are Used
in Visual Word Recognition?" paper presented at meetings of the Eastern
Psychological Association, April 1969.
4. Francis, N. "Commentary on Chapters One Through Three," in H. Levin
and J. P. Williams (Eds.), Basic Studies on Reading. Harper and Row,
forthcoming.
5. Gibson, E. J., et al. "A De_ velopmental Study of the Discrimination of
Letter-Like Forms," Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology,
55 (1962), 897-906.
6. Gibson, E. J., A. Shurcliff, and-A. Yonas. "Utilization of Spelling Patterns
by Deaf and Hearing Subjects," in H. Levin and J. P. Williams (Edi.),
Basic Studies on Reading. Harper and Row, forthcoming.
7. Levin, H., and J. Watson. "The Learning of Variable Grapheme-to-Phoneme
Correspondences: Variations in the Initial Consonant PoSition," A Basic
Research Program on Reading, U. S. Office of Education Cooperative
Research Project No. 639. Ithaca, New York: Cornell University, 1963,
8. Marchbanks, G., and H. Levin. ''Cues by which Children Recognize Words,"
Journal of Educational Psychology, 56 (1965), 57-61.
9. Taber, J. I., and R. Glaser. "An Exploratory Evaluation of a Discriminative
Transfer Learning Program Using Literal Prompts," Journal of Educational
Research, 55 (1962), 508.512.
10. Williams, J. P. "Successive vs. Concurrent Presentation of Multiple Gra-
pheme-Phoneme Correspondences," Journal of Educational-Psychology, 59
-(1968), 309-314. 1
11. Williams, J. P. "From Basic Research on Reading to Educational Practice,"
in H. Levin and J. P. Williams (Eds.), Basic Studies on Reading. New York:
Harper and Row, forthcoming.
12. Williams, J. P., and H. Levin. "Word Perception: Psychological Bases,"
Education, 87 (1967), 515518.
Models of Perceptual Processes in Reading
JOHN J. GEYER
Rutgers University
Classical Theory
Historical background
The span of attention. Central to- the classical theory of per-
ception in reading is the hypothetical construct of the span of
perception, a concept which stems directly-from the span of attention
experiments- of early psychology. The second oldest- experiment in
experimental psychology, the span of attention was a lecture topic
in- the classes of Sir William Hamilton-(73:176-177) prior to 1859:
If you throw a handful of marbles on the floor, you will find
it difficult to view at once more than six, seven at most, without
confusion; but if you group them in twos, or threes, or fives,
you can comprehend ai many groups as you can units, because
the mind considers these groups only as units, it views them as
wholes, and throws their parts out of consideration.
Sir William's marble-throwing demonstration was developed by
Wundt, Cattell, and others into precise experimental piocedures
which became central to the theoretical disputes of the turn-of-the-
century. In Germany, Wuait and his pupils Dietze, Cattell, Hill,
and Jastrow worked to establish the limits of- the Umfanea problem
which the latter three were to bring across -the Atlantic with them.
Exner experimented on the "primary memory" and Richet on the
"elemental memory?" In England such giants as -Galton, Sully, and
Bain were concerned with the problem. Their techniques and
theories were reflected in France by the work of Billet, Henri, and
Simon who were the most successful of all at applying- the new
techniques to the new -field of- mental testing. To this latter group
of scientists, the measurement, of the span of attention was a method
by which they- could investigate and demonstrate individual differ-
ences.
In the United- States, the leader of the Wundtian school of
Structuralists was Titchener, assisted by, a host of students which
included Erdniann, Dodge, Huey, Hylan, and others. Their ap-
proach to psychology.was to determine the elemental units of the
mind, chiefly through introspection, and to build these units into
larger and larger blocks in a direct analogy to chemistry. Since the
span of attention appeared to remain constant under a variety of
GEYER 49
GOldscheider and Muller (68) had likewise held that the letters
were important and that the "deterniining letters" were the initial
letter, "ascenders," "descenders," and vowels. While certain words
might be read from word shape, they concluded from their-experi-
ments that such reading was not the rule. Crosland and Johnson (42)
attempted to rank the letters in terms of relative legibility and
found their results in general agreement with Goldscheider and
Muller. The determining= letters were thought to arouse an = auditory
image of the word, and-the word was then completed by means of
associations. =Huey (90), in the first comprehensive treatment of the
psychology of reading,- presenteit essentially the same- View and
further suggested that the speed of association was determined by
"expectancy."
Pillsbury (121:349-350) projected words= with omitted letters,
incorrect letters, or an x overprinted on one or more letters. His
five subjects were all mature readers and trained introspectionists
as well; in fact,- one was Titchener. His results indicated that
. . . there is a striking decrease in the percentage of recog-
nition [of disfigurements] as we proceed from the first letter to
the last throUghout the word . . . . This seems to indicate a
general tendency of the subject to read through the word- -from-
left to right (emphasis in- original), and thus to give the -first
letters in the word a more prominent_ part in the recognition of
the word as a whole.
Further evidence.of the same tendency-is offered by the fact
that in the words with more than one letter changed, whenever
52 THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
more letters were identifiable, and these, together with some general
characteristics such as length, provided better and better guesses.
The early experimental findings tending to contradict the
"whole word" view could be extended considerably. That the con-
clusions arrived at during the last century by Cattell and by Erd-
mann and Dodge should have survived so many years in the face
of a large amount of contradictory evidence seems due to two pri-
mary factors. In the first place, these experiments went to the heart
of the extremely bitter theoretical warfare being waged between
the Structuralists and the Functionalists. When the introspective
base of Structuralist methodology was discredited by the rising tide
of Behaviorism, the Functionalists were left with the field. Secondly,
at the very time that the Functionalists needed explanations to dis-
-
that they saw all the elements but forgot some before they could
be reported. This report is so universal and so tenaciously believed
by the subjects, that even its introspective origin was not sufficient to
discard it altogether.1 That =such reports contradicted the assump-
tions underlying the span of perception and "total word picture"
views of reading was recognized, but the implications were - largely
ignored. Thus while Woodworth and Schlosberg (149) tell us on
page 104 that "as children advance in reading ability they become
familiar -with many word pictures, and their span for words- in-
creases much faster than their span for unconnected letters," they
also tell us on page 102 chat
. . . the visual impression received during a= brief= exposure
must be much more complete and detailed than is implied by
the phrase 'general = word shape' . . . . Even when a subject can
report only a few of the letters shown, he firmly believes he has
seen all of them during the actual exposure. He forgets some
before he can report them all because mere memory cannot re-
tain so many disconnected items.
Woodworth and Schlosberg's more detailed view of perception
in reading seems to imply immediate and simultaneous sensing of
all the parts of the visual field, but successive identification of its
component parts, limited in a brief exposure by immediate memory.
On page 507 they describe the reading act as a ". . . continuous
process in that the perceptual development of meaning goes on
steadily. Perhaps one can think of it as a continuous production
process, a machine into which the raw material- is tossed by the
shovelful."
One of the experiments which forced this more detailed view
was that done 13) Glanville and Dallenbach (66). In a definitive study
of the span of attention, these investigators exposed two rows of
letters across the visual field for an exposure length of 78 msec. Light
pre- and post-stimulus fields were used, a factor the importance of
which will be discussed below. Their results showed that the letters
to the left of center in the upper row were most correctly reported.
Introspective reports given by their highly trained observers indi-
cated that most of the letters were reported from a "memory after-
image." Glanville and Dallenbach (66:225) concluded that:
. . the usual Jrder of report followed by the O's was to name
the letters from left to right, and those in the top row before
those in the bottom. This plan places those in the upper
row at an advantage over those in the lower. This theoretical
advantage is borne out by the objective results.
They also reported that the subjects could change the order of
_report at will.
Very similar results were found by Crosland (40) when he ex-
posed random- letters in series ranging from I to 9 for exposures of
100 msec. A falling shutter tachistoscope =of a type hich gave ap-
proximately equal lighting of the_fields was used His results showed
-that most letters reported =were from the = left field, and that the
curve of correct reports by letter position declined rapidly= from
left to right. The sharpest decline came between the fourth and
fifth positions. Since it was clear that these findings did not agree
with the doctrine of the day which held that perception of all ele-
ments was simultaneous and that vision was clearer in the center
and less clear on the periphery, Crosland explained these results as
indicating a "left-to-right mindedness."
When Crosland (41) extended his experiment to groups of good
and poor readers of elementary school age, he found the left-to-right
effect more pronounced in the good- readers than in the poor but
apparent in both groups when the letters were flashed to the center
of fixation. When the L ord was presented to the left of fixation, how-
ever, the poor readers reported more of the letters at the' ninth posi-
tion. Anderson and Dearborn (5 :281) find this to be evidence that
"good readers are attracted by the beginning of words. Poor readers
not so much so."
The experimental findings were further extended in an unpub-
lished experiment by Crosland and, presumably, Anderson (re-
ported in Anderson and Dearborn, 5:227-228), by repeating the
procedure with bilingual Jewish children. In reading English, these
children showed the same results as had been obtained before, but
GEYER
57
f2 SYSTEM PROGRAM
GEYER 67
0
interpretation. Mackworth (108,109), for example, equates Sperling's
"visual image" with her own use of the term and with Glanville and
Dallenbach's "memory after-image." But there are difficulties with
this interpretation when applied to normal reading conditions.
In the normal reading situation, all "fields" are equally lighted
at the level to which the ,eye is adapted. Tachistoscor ::! variations
from this luminance procedure create' difficulties in applying the
results to normal reading phenomenon. Figure 2 plots the data of
Sperling with data reported by _Mackworth (110), showing the effects
on number of elements reported by an interaction of exposure dura-
tion and various luminance schedules. In the area below 50 nisec.,
the luminance schedule is critical. 'The use of dark pre- and post-
stimulus fields at short exposure duration provides the subject with
a visual input equal to that provided by much longer durations.
Cattell's results (31), therefore, reflect, response variables such as
familiarity and contextual 'constraint, - and provide no data for
inferences regarding the "primary cue" in reading.
The effects on form recognition caused by varying the luminance
of tachistoscopic fields has been extensively investigated by Eriksen
and coworkers (53, 54, 55), who ascribe the effects to a combination
1
I
6
0......... P. cod Postfields dark, stioulue fad& 3 It. (Mesckwettlt, 1963)
1J
//
cs........ Pre. and Post 0.14. dims stimulus 0.14, 31 ft.d. (Spoiling, 1960)
410. Pre. and Pee4elds32 ftel: stimu!te Aeld 3 9 fa (Morkwonit, 1963)
2
i Hypothstaind scanning rote
./
O
1 I
20
i L i-i
40 60
I 1
60
I 1
100
I I
120
l'il 140
I
160
I I
110
I
200
I
14-
.
68
THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
e_= at:-_prOtessingdoes_liot-iiecess4-rilyittopWithetheetisation- _
of -1C-al_f_stintuhiv;;additional==controlprOCedittes- :_=-Ivere
.
Vit-hPch-=-_,and--Sperling-- used assic-T_pro,
=
ceddre7o jt-t-_=-dernOn-
is--= -tote
stratedihy---Bakt-114;haslongbcen=aniiin _ aant-AC/0 for =the
_
y- isttal!piticessys-and---i-:cer tti.for,th-eoreti
beetiTithOrtitighly-ireview_ccl-,by-Kahneinan- -Canno- t-1)e_ ex-
lensiVeltdisCuised-like.-ImtheifollOwin paradigm t_cance-=to
this--pap0,'-the procedure consist's of= following a stimulus field With
r a field-- corn designed to-- destroy= the inform-a--
-a bright-;flaih,:foundJhat1-01-Msec-.='_fret of-interferentewairnecestary--
for the ,rierteption-:of ,a= single letter and that an-_additional=_ if----insic.
was - required for each -letter:reported:in a-series. Since -his -Masking
stimulus: was brighter -than-his, stimulus fields, _this _
-estimate-
is--probably'slightly-exteisive.---Aveibach and-=Sperling:(8)-;__howeVer,
report estimate -11-e_use-of--"parts of as the-mask.
Sperling_(196S),2goinifOund_thatriq_Insec. of exposure was= re4Oited
THEORETICAL MODELS AND I,AO CESSES =OF READING
-Kinsbotane:and- Warringonninveitigatedr_the
quettion-
quentiaVVerstis:==_
parallef--_-pracetsing_-_=_Isp_resentingT:4-=randll_ltletter
grOups to tviorottheir=subjeCti.
Their-results indicated
. that
the_threshold_ifor-thecOrrectlidentification
-___--- of all the=_
letters lir the_ taclustoscopic:grov _--
never= aceeded-that of- identi=
-ficationiof one -of the -igeoup-:by more titan= -cini6
_ tachistoscopic
step. In _most = cases,- :two -sandsl_threer-Jettersi=were-
simulfaneoitsly
identifiedat the-same-exposure duration as .
It apptaii-that_-andet-ftlie-presenc Conditions=
tandout--pei.;_
ceptioncisEinfluented by the aftercoming randOni pattern much
as- is-single perception:
s,
GEYER 73
Viewing_
The inalysik,presented:2a the :retinal- locus _--eitperi7---_-
mentatiori can -provide-scime7 insight into epossible-__Strategies-
availible.to-Subjectsiirtrachittostopit__experimenW--Reaclingf_can-_,be-
thoughtof a-proceis=brWhichespatiaiiequentei are-zzeiniVerted- to-
temporalrsequekes. -accomplishedlby=processing4r=at least
three levels loosely _ideritifiecf_With=tensbry;tecOgnitional- and motor
processei.-DireCtionalityAs--initiatelk at rhe-:sensory- leirei hut
-parently determinedPby -the-Ilesireds: sequence of _the.;finalE:reporL_
Thus, if a:subject- knOWS=before-the----expOstire the -sequence.-iiilithith
the response' will -be -made, -heseats-che---inaterial- in the- direction-
whiCh produCet- that sequence. Internal =-recognition: takes- _place in
the sequence, and -the externalFresporie is -made-airetogniticin
occurs, -or from a_storage of a covert response-. Ifrhessame-Secfnence
is maintained= from= to output, -the systenisroperattatinakiMuin
efficiency. If a sequence of external- ieiponse---is-required--- which is
counter_ to the-direction, of scanning, however,- the __-most effective
strategy-would-be-to store the totarinternalretponseiand_the report
in the-reverse_ direction. This would limit == the= final report: to back-
ward memory span,_ while -preserving a SeMblance of:the eirectional
effects_ imposed by _the -fading: icon.
Long prattice has apparently= made the English-reading subject
more proficient witl-theleft4o4ightScan-direction. 'Thus, -while he
shows right field= superiority-wheri:reqUited to respond= to _nondirec-
74
THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
When the stimulus field is small= and the presentation very brief,
however, ex_ Perienced subjects might develop the "attending broadly"
technique.
PROGRAM
Storage-but --_
to linninanCe-variablerartifictual-toKthit4nodeFsinterthefda not-
_ --z
occur in normal- reading _ --primarrfunctioir ofithis=system: is -to
-----
act as a -temporalizrbuffer _t_weenT-_-seticory,a-itd__respCitisey'phases-___to
_-
allow integrationzof=thedifferingiprocessingrates.
Internal -Tesponw-syttem.The-pottihle----ketporiset,Matie:=-
- -dnring
_
_
reading are -many= and _-_complex-:and--would-tmvolve-Naried -systems.
-0f-interest to this:paper-arethe--verbalfrespOnses-zrequired in:tachis%
totcopir eicperiments--_aridz-nitistil-not=a11;_--teading.-=:Theinteractions
bettveen the rate_ at which-tresponses:_can'ibe niade,=-__the- elements
making- up response -units,------andilllielength-Utikteticont-temain in
storage are seen as-the-Major fattors-sgoverning speed of reading-and-
eye-MoVement charatteristics; = _ _
Several ingtigatort,Aiting-Auite_-dissimilar_egporimtntalisitua,-
tions, have -pot tiilatedza -5= or evicyde: per second:Central nervous
system response rate (Pladcive11,14).-Lansing;-SchWartz;and Lindsley
(103) have pOstiilated_ a physiolOgical-b-a-sis-= for such a respante-rate,_
centering around the action of the -ascending reticular =activating
system- on cortical resolution and the -transmission of incoming
messages. As- recognized by Davis (1963); these postulated response
rates agree quite well- with the -long known (but little underttood)
fact that the mean -pause length in normal silent readihg-remains
78
THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
memory:(1)-. _Questions
currentlybeingclebatedinclude whether _the
of storage auditory in_ nature-(47, 48,115),
whether coding= is byleininticoricouitieltitaMeters,,(10, 38i- 141i-7
145) =and whether the _
--processes-are decay, interference; or
GEYER 79
msec.,_ four or five digits could be reported- before the icon faded.
Additional increments of -time added to the stimulus presentation
would add additional- digits to the report up to a maximum of
approximately 40 -to 48-_msec. For longer= exposure durations, the
limiting factor would be the interaction, of response -rate and the
_duration of the iconic storage. -Additional increments_ of exposure
time would not add to the report until the duration was long enough
to allow the first digit to be processed before the exposure was
terminated-----a duration_ of -apprOxiinatelys_ 250: msec.. approx-
imately 50-Lk:v=250
_
TABLE
-oLEAN NUMBER-0P WORDS-CORRECTLY REPORTED
- Number of
words in Exposure duration (ms.)
stimulus field 84 126 168 210 252 84e
-_
..aA AXiagilaMMui-ii;
Iteratur
licatidri
tioiL.
___---
_._._
ten tn
taa ner- iieif-
tanite iiilei te
.LATadiiitoscopkStudyof
-dsolotka _
ono a '-= 19 ---;
GSPh:OlogbfthiReUna
ttention a
ustic n usions
(19641:75 83
matnanr7
=7_
ucationa
--_----_- - _=
ntn tatio
______-- ------
-A a %/LIM, _164 a:. _a 110 I" &&&&&& 411Ly7r1V4UVIlLys iV14l11J11511111, _ JUILY7144 111G4pC7_1_._-_
-124,-- Scharf,_ B; and -L _Lefton "Forward and = Backward 11lasking =as a Function=
of Illumina_n_ce,__ p- aper presented EaiteriNy-eh-ologicatAiiititiation0968=
-125, etacontiait,'_ journal o,
z-kriin'efitaliTsyekology--,-,7V(1966);-=_82z
126,- iSchilletc=_P;-
ournal of Ex irinentill;P*holdly_;;66_141963)386=39
127. Schotifierd,: an
_Infraseries Variation;' _Canadian_ Journal of =1201(196W 21-8-227f-_---:
128.- _Schintaiin-,--sF,=mieEkliefinling --lioli=lhichitabeii-Jtii ort-iii==beiMoineti-__
Psychologie,
,-i8thintiann; is serken indigiu-rteiViZeitieh t -ItirsP*4460;08=_0_9217_,
1922). 205,224
130: -_=ItiklarPeteepticirW 0-,'!_iPsychelo
= 6C(1957Nao6=3
1;-s
*Acilogiedl-FM0frogid:hi441(1960) .
132. iSprigie;X.---_1:;:fEffe-ctic11iffetential--I'rainir_ig=_pir
----nititinalteiholds77otirnarbtExperimentat P.i.y_ch6W 1959 427:2,
133; Taylor; j,&=:mfett_i)::fiSe(6UAis6ciatedl;Wof uration Thresho
=Perceptual and otor:=Skilk 956)03
184: Taylor;- J. =- e-ahing-iz-Yrequeney;-an Visual
I n
-19UrnaVO e_iiinent-alrErteliology;:5k(1958); -29=3$=
135. TaIlori
Canadian Journal to -ch79/9-107= (1953),-,42-
cultural deman
wever _inappropriate
seem, em oniy ones
articular circumstances. It must kCe
eenou o accomrnodate-man
it altmative,
cient or more sOCiallyadelita
has had Many experiern
from w CMS
problems, with zest and con ence ct s out his
)robletns, his own world animals who have re.
ceived adequate stimulation and a ection in infancy will ee out
actto
m_ e,Ear ssta
ctors=e4
thitie=sator=
,,---- --_---_-i------_--------r- 1
_
_
RETICAL----MODELS-ANDPROCESSESrm-
-=--===--- --- -
=---_-
= =-=
rocess o cation
w e- =rea m -
-movement= =recor
wit outa eetin e-on r
r_conee
un era le
u
icu
-as a- reader atten er
sense rson
i scanay
.andzinore-:= rune_ o-
-,beenifounctcleficierit-in-ie oltren e eabili to-gauge _-:
-reality-and4 eiize--= -thitritft iti=ipiirci-priatei
_,
_
'reeted-Tacticri-
4-;=ltkiisey: -)liamremarked -=: at rreaders-_-- --
e ess re isticii- leitegiiateso
____
pntelitrat-readers;-vhile-
_
- - .
I
= ,Row--
2 a ___ure:stin=_:ticlus: -in _Ournal-fo
otiOnal= _ncationnUAssociation 19
-___ e:IInderathieiltit Giftedf one,'_'-
a_t-etio_tta - Children ; 1955)247=24 ----:-
._- _rituo_wi-____ "A _ntparativeStutly-o n erachielrewIt_IormalAChievers,- _
and 1 ers-i_it,=; --din--"Aapith itsertatiOn-i-lolva--
State _JJfiiitraity 19
ttitudes = toward Reading An
2C --GrOff,-=_IF.:I_-'_n!Childreiter, Attitudes eir-Criticaljtead,--
,-= _ing:_-:Abilities--:_in'=Fati:_:Ccinterit, T_=ype Materiali,"_ -- JoUrnal_01=-_-Educational
--i_ isearch, 55.196 2), 313-31
=
;
- -
L
76. Witkin, H. A. "Origins of 'dye S e," in Constance Sheerer (Ed.).
Cognition: Theory, Research,, Promise. New York: Harper and Row, 1964.
Witty; Pr= A. "Rea_ding= Success= and Emotional Adjustment," Elementary
English Review;27 (1950),
Zimmerman, Irla L., and . . ebrand. "Personality Characteristics and
Attitudes = toward Achievement of and Poor Readers," Journal of
Educational_ Research, 59 (1965), 28.30.
--
=SKOP"
- V
V
f
V
V
V
for the older children to teach the younger children spontaneously j V
I :
spelling, etc. We have known for years that the best way to learn
V
anything is to have to teach it. 2)4 curriculum built around areas of j V
V
V
interest rather than content subjects. As far as reading is concerned, I
V
throughout the-day; The teacher tries to spend some time each day V V
V
to take about four 'years, from the age of five to nine, to break the
code. Then seems to be no pressure put on him by the teacher-or
in the last few years that trend seems to have reversed. We-hear little
about individualized reading and see kw examples of itin the class- i
room. V
sonal meaning for him. We also maximize the chances that 'reading
V
F
MUSKOPF 123
control over what they read? Free choice responds to personal feelings
and, therefore, would seem to be an important element of any read-
ing program.
Summary
We live in a society in which =the pervasive effects of external
control on humans must be counteracted by encouragement toward
spontaneity, open expression =and= recognition = of basic feelings and
emotions.- The _major eo r -was _to su st several
ibilities or treatin a.nd mod fy ng =a reading program
ools to-enable - children == to = express an recognize tllese feelings
er an repress
REFERENCES
Cognitive -Development
.
Cognitive di op m en 1s t a=c o ntin:uo uv_ con_ truct: o nof m teue.t =-
tual operations at eVolveto als1 ean mobile:systemization
essentially direct 'tot-yard- ibriinn;- tellectual-operations, or
the organization v int _activity, are rooted -in- action- and
are always a part tan of operatiOnsior structured wholes:At all
levels uf:deteelopinent-thereareconstant--- ctions-Cominon-
. . . action presupposes' a_precipita factor: a physiologic:4,
affective, of_ intellectual need; (In, elattet case, the need -ap-
pears in,the-guise of a question or a=probIem). ._. -inin addition
to the -constant functions, there- are the variable structures
(25:4-5).
the problem and to consider ways of dealing with itthat is, to set up
hypotheses" (22:42). Now the learner is becoming more reflective
and less impulsive. He is beginning to want proof, to suspend judg-
ment, to think of information as tentative and relative. Rather
Than seizing on the first idea-that occurs to him, he pauses (suspends
judgment) to note whether or not there- are better ways or Other
alternatives.
Another cognitive advance that occurs at this stage is the ability
to- use numbers; not only-to order things- in uanti 'but
also to see _that relationships can exist =on a nunreri
such= as the = number syStem-has pro ies - properties
that are agreed upon by mathematician& w o _can produ-ce
a sum deals with an abstractiori roperties Of
mathematics. Interestingly u- h; Piaget makes it quite dear that
the understanding of numbers oes hot learning numerals.
The latter part= of=tbe preoperational"stage- fnds_children= Making
judgments largely on asis o rtia* 1 and immediate percePtions
and/or on the basis of o jective s an They judge by the
way
things look and -usually in-terms=of just one of a number of
relevant
dimension& Even so, three= fundamental = operations= can be deter-
mined. They can think- in =terms =of classe& When ptesented with a
group of circles or squares, they can classify the items on the basis
of roundness. They can think in terms of relationships; i.e., Mr.
Jones is the father of Ralph, Mt Jones is = bigger than Ralph, and
Ralph is the oldest of three children. They can think in terms of
quantity or by handling number concept&
In the concrete operations period the thought of the
seven to
eleven age group is more like that of the adult in= that they = think
more in logical terms. Operations is used by Piaget to refer to mental
acts or imminent acts of an internalized nature and taking place in
the mind. These mental acts represent a process of interaction
and
development. whereby new syntheses are formed by discovery. At-
tributes are noted, objects are classified, and categories determined.
These syntheses are real in the sense that they not only have a loca-
tion in time and space but also that they take place in the minds of
human beings. In the process of cognitive growth through
discovery
and synthesis the individual is merely the neural medium 'in the
resynthesis of cultural elements.
STAUFFER 131
Concept = Learning
Contept attainment -is now generally-considered =a =part of the
psychology of- l--earning and the development of cognitive processes
(12:37). Attention is focused on the logical form of concepts with
studies of concept attainment = generally based on inductive methods
and the strategies used (6:37). Strategies differ from person to person,
from discipline to discipline, and from one level of sophistication to
another. While many = concepts are acquired by discovery, learning
through discrimination, abstraction,= differentiation, hypothesis gen-
eration and testing, and - generalization, many more are acquired
through school learning and/or reading. In the latter, the concepts
are learned by means of criterial attributes presented and the relat-
ing of= them to established ideas (2). Thus the acquisition of concepts
can be accomplished inductively by concept formation or deductively
by concept assimilation. Even so, there is a considerable likelihood
that the learner must use much of= the same processes of concept
formation even when appropriate contexts are presentee d as in con-
cept assimilation.
At the Wisconsin Research and Development Center for Cogni-
tive Learning (20), Concept is treated as a superordinate category of
which all concepts are instances and differentiated from other pro-
ducts of learning such as facts, principles, and problem- solving skills.
A concept is referred to as having four characteristicsdefinability,
structure, psychological meaningfulness, and utility. Four bases of
defining concepts are identified in terms of perceptible defining
properties, semantically, operationally, and logical or numerical
relationships or axioms. Structure is determined by the form in
which the concepts are experienced. Psychological meaningfulness
refers to the phenomenological or idiosyncratic nature of concepts
134 THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
Conclusion
Sam A. Fleming (//: "4) said
Dedication and clear thinking are= our need today. It is ironical
that in this period of so much 'learning . . . a- specter of igno-
rance should hang over us . . . that there should be iiresolution
about many of the true values whiCh are fundamental to our
way of life.
Miiih the same might be- said about what is reading and how to
teach reading. Universal agreement can be obtained supporting the
conclusion that comprehension is the invariant condition of read-
ing. Almost universal agreement can be obtained regarding the
conclusion that reading is a process akin .to thinking. Some- few
agree that if the first two premises are true then reading should be
taught as a thinking process (31).
This review of theories and practices concerned with cognitive
functioning and development and its possible relationship with
reading per se and with the teaching of reading may prove helpful if
it will stimulate research that will define ways and means to more
effective reading. Specific thought processes involved in reading for
various purposes and with varied materials should be identified.
There seems little doubt about the nature of the strategies in-
volved in reading-thinking acts. Increase in task complexity most
likely requires cognitive functioning that ranges in complexity from
STAUFFER 139
stage to stage of maturity similar to the stages Piaget and others have
declared. Like related intellectual tasks such as concept attainment,
reading requires of the reader problem solving ability that is logical
and mobile.- Undoubtedly, too, achievement is influenced by a
reader's intellectual potential, his attitudes and values, his intra-
personal and interpersonal dimensions, and the teaching and testing
to which he is exposed. Even though at the college level a renewed
attempt at critical thinking is made by college freshmen, pedagogical
demands do not foster similar changes across the four-years.
Children deal with means-ends relationships as early as the sen-
sorimotor period. They learn to deal with. variables selectively and
to act reflectively in the preoperational period. They make decisions
on logical terms through immanent acts of =an internalized nature.
This they do by discovery and synthesis as they resolve perceptual-
cognitive conflicts of the operational period. Finally at the logical
stage they can construct theories and make sound deductions without
the need for empirical evidenceAnd_in_all this, language (oral or
written), or representational thought plays a= highly significant role.
If reading is akin to thinking and represents a means of generat-
ing predictive systems, and if science is- a search for relationships,
then the science of critical reading 'requires that such actions be
central. We cannot be satisfied with passive reading accomplished
through passive processes and directed by passive teaching and least
of all at the college level. We must require that reading be a think-
ing act and teach it that way. If thought has its roots in action then
reading does too within a developmental interactionist theory of
cognitive activity.
REFERENCES
24. Lehmann, Irvin J., and Paul L. Dressel. "Critical Thinking Attitudes Values
in Higher Education," Office of Education, U. S. Department of Health,
Education and Welfare, Cooperative Research Project No. 590, Michigan
State University, 1962.
25. Piaget, Jean. Six Psychological Studies, David Elkind (Ed.). New York:
Random House, 1967.
26. Russell, David H. Children Learn to Read. Boston: Ginn, 1961.
27. Sigel, Irving E. "The Attainment of Concepts," in Martin L. Hoffman and
Lois Wladis Hoffman (Eds.), Review of Child Development Research. New
York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1964, 209-248.
28. Sinclair-de-Zwart, Herinina. "Developmental Psycholinguistics," In David
Elkind and John H. Flavell (Eds.), Studies in Cognitive Development: Essays
in Honor of Jean Piaget. New York: Oxford University Press, 1969, 315-336.
29. Singer, Harry. "Conceptual Ability in the Substrata Theory of Reading,"
unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California at Berkeley, 1960.
30. Singer, Harry. "Substrata-Factor Theory of Reading: Theoretical Design for
Teaching Reading," in J. A. Figurel (Ed.), Challenge and Experiment in
Reading, Proceedings= of the International Reading Association, 7, 1962.
New York: Scholastic Magazines, 226232.
-31. Stauffer, Russell G. Directing Reading-Maturity-as-a Cognitive Process. New
York: Harper and Row, 1969.
32. Suchman, J. Richard. "Inquiry Training in the Elementary School," Science
Teacher, 27 (1960), 42-47.
33. Suchman, J. Richard. "A Model for the Analysis of Inquiry," in Herbert
J. Kliusmeier and Chester W. Harris (Eds.), Analyses of Concept Learning.
New York: Academic Press, 1966.
34. Taba, Hilda, Samuel Levine, and Freeman F. Elzey. "Thinking in Elemen-
tary School- Children," U. S. Office of Education Cooperative Research
Project No. 1574, San Francisco State College, April 1964:
35. Thorndike, Edward L. "Reading as Reasoning: A Study of Mistakes in
Paragraph Reading," Journal of Educational Psychology, 8 (June 1917),
323-332, .
36. Wolfe, Willavene, Charlotte S. Huck, and Martha L. King. "Critical Read-
ing Ability of Elementary School Children," Office of Education, U. S.
Department of Health, Education and Welfare, Project No. 5-1040, Contract
No. OE-4-10.187, Ohio State University, 1967.
37. Wolfe, Willavene, et al. The Ohio State University Test of Critical Reading
Ability. Columbus, Ohio: Ohio State University Press, forthcoming.
Reactions to Reading as Cognitive Functioning
Roy A. KRESS
Temple University
tfi
144 THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
Comprehension abilities
The development of any one of the commonly listed reading
comprehension abilities must proceed along similar lines. As an ex-
ample, how can a child most effectively learn to respond to and in-
terpret metaphors? The essential process that he must learn is to
recognize when an author is labeling someone or something in a way
that implies a similarity to something else and to figure out what the
intended similarity is To learn this process he.must again think his
-way-by observing aiuMfialrzitiy anguage situa ions w is are-fami-
liar to him_ , which he already understands. In these he can see that
the label is ridiculous if it is interpreted literally. He knows per-
fectly well that there is no -intention = to say his sister is a= rodent
_
with a long, skinny tail when he says,- "She's a fad" Since she is a
human being, albeit a rather distasteful one to him at the moment,
it is patently impossible for her to be some other kind of animal.
The likeness that he is implying may be only the distastefulness to
him of both his sister and a rat, nothing more specific. When he uses
the metaphor, he knows what he means. In the same fashion, he has
learned to know what his mother means when she says, "Don't storm
at me! I didn't break it." He tells someone he got an icy stare or a
cold shoulder with no sign of shivering.
The fact that the child has learned to interpret and to use
certain metaphoric language in- slang, in name calling, and in a
variety of other ways does not, however, guarantee that he is pre-
pared to interpret new instances of metaphor which he meets. Of
course, even recognition of the need for nonliteral interpretation
demands an awareness of the literal meaning. Without this, the
child might not realize that a literal translation was not intended,
could not actually hold up logically. Beyond this, he must have ob-
served many similar experiences with the particular kind of met-
aphor and observed the signals inherent in them. He must have
generalized his observations to the degree that he knows the essential
similarity intended can be a physical characteristic or something
else. It can be based on some reality about the compared object or
on some myth about it. It can- reflect the personal attitudes of the
author of the metaphor towarchhe things being compared. All this
146 THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
he cannot learn at once. He needs to think his way through one kind
of situation at a time.
The effective teacher, therefore, must again help him by struc-
turing the raw materials for his observation and analysis. .One group
of metaphors f;miliar to him may all rest on some physical likeness
in the compared objects (beanpole, giant, midget, squirt). Working
with these, he can be helped to see that this is one avenue of ap-
proach to the meaning of a metaphor how do these things look or
sound or feel alike ? From another group of familiar metaphors he
may abstract another kind of similarity =and generalize some other
possibilities for arriving at .an interpretation. Only after a series of
such experiences in thinking his way through a variety of types of
metaphoric situations can.he generalize at the level which will make
him master of the process of interpreting all metaphors.
Conclusion
It-is not-too surprising-thatin-considerations -of cognitive func--
timing in reading far more attention has been given to dealing
with specific ideas in reading materials than to learning the compo-
nents of reading ability. Early investigations of children's concepts
followed the same patterndealing with the question of what con-
cepts they had and had not developed to the neglect- of the processes
of concept formation and attainment. The day is gone, however,
when attention can legitimately be given to the products without full
study of the processes by which they are produced. The child's
ability to deal cognitively with reading materials will be deter-
mined by the degree to which he has learned reading as a cognitive
process.
Theoretical Models of Reading:
Implications for Teaching and Research
HARRY SINGER
University of California at Riverside
Selected Models
Model of conceptual response to printed words
For one subsystem of reading, instruction could be= organized to
develop= a conceptual response to printed words. A teaching model
for such imtruction is depicted in Figure 1. The model indicates that
the related "materials of thinking," consisting of percepts, images,
memories, information and feeling tone, are organized through =the
process of concept formation and are linked through a linguistic
form to a printed word stimulus that represents a-class of objects?
The rationale for the model has been drawn from several the-
oretical formulations. The definition of a concept and concept
formation is consistent with Russell's formulation (76:117, 248-249):
Concepts develop out of related perceptual experience.
.. ." They are "the means by which a child or adult represents
anything to himself and thereby creates a readiness to respond
2 The original version of this model and its explanation have already been
published (91). In the present version, the concept of lioguistic form (72) has
been- added to the model.
152 THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
Percepts
4
Images
4 Linguistic
Memories Concept Formation Printed Word
Form (Objects 1, 2, 3, ... n)
t
Information
4
Feeling tone
FIGURE 1. A subsystem of related elements mobilized as a conceptualized
response to a printed word stimulus that represents a class of objects. (Modified,
after Russell, 76.) .
Ito. MMee.
Vim.
02 Power
Ind. Ver.
OS Suffixes
06 Prefixes
07 Vocab. 1601.
14 Mental Age
15 Word Recoil.
11 Match Sor-nde
19 Mend Sounds
20 Cons. Phonics
26 Spell. Recog.
27 Spell. Recall
35 40 45 50 55 60
No. Nese
01 Speed
Ind. Var.
03 Aud. We:cab.
04 Aud. Memory
09 Vis. Abstr.
12 Concept Ab.
13 . Cbron. Age
14 Mental Age
26 Spell. Reeog.
* Is 16 Pastest Readers
rs Marion's Scores
35 40 45 50 55 60
which would have even more implications for teaching. The model
also has other research implications.
Research implications
The fourth grade model is a byproduct of confirming the
prediction that the major premise of 'the substrata factor theory of
reading could be generalized from the college to the fourth grade
level (79). Further research would determine whether the scope
162 THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
6 Holmes and Singer (52) predicted at the third grade level of Durkin's study
that her early readers would = converge -with the control group when both groups
had reached the eighth grade. The amvergence of these groups may have been a
function of the methods of instruction, which were not controlled in Durkin's
study. However, the *diction applied only to the criterion used for assessing
reading achievement since the two groups could attain the same performance
level through different patterns of= reading abilities. For example, there could
have been persistent differences between the groups in rate or fluency in reading.
Since Durkin di4 not utilize a comkehensive battery of tests and various criteria
of readinTability, her data cannot answer these questions.
7 Although the intent of the First Grade Study was to test similar hypotheses,
admitted weaknesses in the overall design precluded testing them (10). However,
some of the individual studies in this group might lend themselves to substrata
analysis of their data that would provide at least a tentative answer to the above
questions.
THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
Meaning of
Affixes
, Meaning of
Affixes
., Meaning of
`4"-Affix61-7--- 174
Syllabication =
Consistency 7
Variance 66 71% 69% _73%
Explained:
1
I 1 I
1
' I
414''71"A
IIIII,,],
SINGER 167
READING
restrictions
FIGURE 7. A learning structure for the basic skills of reading. (From The
Conditions of Learning by Robert M. Gagne. Copyright @ 1965 by Holt, Rine-
hart and Winston, Inc. Reprinted by permission of Holt, Rinehart and Winston,
Inc.)
170 'THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
I
Mental Processes Unit Class Relations Systems Transformations
Cognition (recogni- Recognition of Recognition of differ- Recognition *word Recognition Of phonic Recognition of word
tion of information) printed word as ence among letters. form and letter de- characteristics, i.e. endings (plurals, or
such words and numbers tails; as little initial and final roots and affixes)
Sounds
Memory (retention Recall of specific Recall of random Recall by word form Recall of complete Recall of word end-
of information) word forms, e.g. words; letters and 'h and details (sight word * combined ings (s, ed, ing)
own first name numbers in isola- vocabulary)' clues: e.g. initial root or affix
house number tion (reading signs) sound, sentence
pattern and form
Divergent Production Neologima,lwerd , Recognition by dia. Recognition, y base Spontaneous phonic Experiments with
(ilogicalmeative form errors tinctive detail as g Word Within larger, and structural gen- derived and base
ideas) in dog; capital compound Words eralizations words
letters
Convergent product, Recognition by pie Recognition by sheer by sen- Recognition by word Recognition of de-
don (conclusions. toiled clues, shape length, as grand- 'Itence pattern; Word families, common rived words by base
inductive thinking) of sign, etc. mother: or.by syllables, roots. etc. plus ending, or root
unusual shape; as plus affixes
elephant
Evaluation (critical Comparisons Of differ- Discrimination of Discrimination by Discrimination by Discrimination of
thinking) ences and Similari- words, numbers. 'form, details and form, details and base and derived
ties in gross form letters pattern phonic elements words by endings,
and sentence letter sounds, MM.
pattern' melee pattern, root
and affixes
I Figures ga, ilk and ec are reprinted from Toward letter Reading by George Spache, Champaign, Ill.: Garrard Publishing Co., 1963. By per-
mission of the author.
1100/11...,11,1.0014,4.4., . r
N.', I 1,14,411,1w, r o,
0
Emu 9. The diagrans indicates. the- interrelationships among the individ-
ual (0), the _classroom- reading situation (S) the individual's -responses-to the
situation_ (R),-__therfresulting_memory--ttatet_gy-amb
th-e_iddiViduars -perception
_(1!) of theneit-sittiation(Reprinted-frcknmThelieadintProCeis
-and Rantifi-
tations."-Iiiinvitational Addfesses, 1965--;Newark,-Delaware: Internationallt.ead-
ing--AsiociatiOn, -I965,-49-74. By permistion-Uf atichor_and--the -publisher.)
. The individual student is represented-by 0 and= consists of a
psychological model of the products, -prerequisites and processes
within the reader. Included in= this model-are physical, physiological,
mental, attitudinal, knowledge;self-concept, desire to read=in short,
all those interacting systems and subsystems within the individual
that function between stimulus input and response -output. Instruc-
tion is next concerned with S or the classroom situation. its decor,
lean.-..ag atmosphere, and types of reading material, which mutually
interact with 0. -The resultant= of this interaction is 1?, the student's-
responses to the situation. This dynamic interaction leaves a trace, T,
which is stored within an individual's nervous system. This trace then
influences the individual's perception, P, of the next situation, which
completes the cycle.
Summary
After a review of the = literature on =deli of reacting develop-
ment,- several were selected for more detailed presentation and for
drawing implications for teaching and researi.h. These models
covered the developmental range from kindergarten to the college
level.
All of the empirical models selected for detailed =presentation
are byproducts of research to test hypotheses of the substrata-factor
theory of reading. They provide some cross-sectional evidence on
psychological factors associated with development of speed and power
of reading. Although this cross-sectional evidence tends to support
SINGER 175
the hypothesis that the structure of skills and abilities associated with
speed and power of reading is hierarchically developed and orga-
nized, a longitudinal study is necessary to fill =in the developmintal
gaps and to reveal individual patterns of reading development.
If a longitudinal investigation is undertaken, important -in
such an investigation would be the formulation and utilization
of a teaching model that is based upon a theory of the hierarchical
structure of learning (29) and a comprehensive = set of objectives,
such as those =constructed by Spache (99) and Barrett (6). =In such
an investigation we would seek to construct a comprehensive pat-
tern of the "common routes =to maturity around which individuals
vary," determine how these common = routes; interact with= the per-
sonality of the individual, his environmental influences, and his
biological support systems. If we can also ascertain whether =or not
variations in educational procedures have durable effects upon the
developmen4 structure, and= functioning of these common routes,
then we would =be in a position to construct-a comprehensive theory
along the lines outlined by Strang (104). This theory would then
serve as a cognitive map for improving development of speed and
power of reading.
REFERENCES
Gilbert, Luther C. "Functional _Motor Effitiency of the Eyes and its Rela-
tion to Reading," University of California Publications in Education, 2
(1953), 159!232. _
-43.-_=1-1614--David0.-Orgnitition-upftBehatiior.=
Holiribila-ck_Ai-="Facrors,UnderlyintsMajcii-=ReadinglDiiabilities at the
ColletelLe-tel,"-Genetic'/34-chology=Monotraphi,;491954)4,95;-_
--- 45. Hanes, Jack A._ "Theikain-sand-,-theReading=Procesi-,"--Clarititontollegi;
---LivinvTwenty4econ-d- Yearliaok_--_of -:the-ClaremOnt_-_
-Reading-Conferenc4-__Claremont,- Cal: -_,-Curri-Culumsl:aboratory__1957=49-67=--
46. - Califor--
PtibliCationt=id Ediscatiou,1241959);-213-29E- ----_-_- =
52. Holints-,=_Jacr A., and --Harry- Singer. "TheoretI-al Models and Trends_
Toward- -Morel-Basic Research id-Reading," Rivieto of Educational
search, 34 (1964), -127-155. -
53. Holmes, Jack A., and Harry Singer. Speed and Po-wer in Read:ng in High
School, U.S. Department of _Health, Education and Welfare, Office of
Education, - Bureau of Research and Development, Catakig
No. F8 5.230: 30016, 1966.
54. Holmes, Jack A., and Harry Singer. The Language Perception Test Series,
Elementary Battery:Pebble Betch;-,Cal.: Psychological-Educational Services,_
1966:
e-and
eretice
_ . _= _ _ -_=
_
=
= _ ===, -
-=- -=
=
--=-- -1
=== ==_
=== _s=
=- ====-
,_
=
=====-==-=
==
=
-=
=
ommuni
_
-i
_
RETICAL -2MODELS OCESSESi= 0
zn-_
, =
MODELS
-_---,----z---
us,--_-_ii4 2=---=--_-----;----_
-;-iHebb,=,TheOiteptizition-af--hehavior.--Ntw-Yotk:=JohttiWiler-_& Sons, -_
peed of Ret
owerof:Rtaditi
:11iMusic-al-Aptittide
.-Pitclkdis-crimin-a 6-
-discrimination
- Toms- intensity= disciiininaton
-
eviations
--c
-
SINGER
in_ the b iv arat t r elati oni h ip:betwe en e ach -predictor itheregrets ion-
equation_withtheicrite ionIneasure;:bothirecti i ear
in-
where
208 THEORETICAL MODELS AND -PROCESSES OF READING--
I
'"11"""111,111,111'1111,1,111,1111:111111,,!:11:11,1,11,11:,:,,:11::111'd1,11,',1',
THEORETICAL -- MODELS AND-PROCESSES OF READING
READING
_
__-_-r_e---,-
_
FIGURE DeveloPmental integration onel an tem for* 3 e 0 rea ing in ea through six The
shows that Recognition of Affixes and accounts n s_ ed of Reading at the,third
grade level, but only indirectly through o at 6 e 'eve
_-_Stitani -_ n-clusions =_ _-
-- -_---
fi the 'statisticall
is atet_ ems= at Auantitative- and_-
izitionachan -et:in--in-strata= -c-tors_-are,,in- ci;;_aiSoCiatediivith
, ___ ----__- -- - _ _e:-
-292-296.
trEi-ENTit; MitinaUforDitrrell,Stillivan reading- capacity
and teadintathievementzteiti.--Ne*Noit:_-_-:-WorldiBook;_193T-7
=and --
GATES, Themannalof direetions_tydritheiGateilReading-=:SurVey-_.: ew-Yorkt
Teachers College, ColtunbiaUnivertityr13ureau-ofzPublications,=1953;--- _:-=_
Thesmanual-o or the_fGatis_ReadingzSuroey-.,New-Nork:_
-Teacliera_College,-"ColumbiacUniVersity"--Bureati-ofiPublicitions, -1958;:
funitional-tribtor effidency_off-theEeyea,anthits_z_relationr_to-reading.
University Of Xduentiorf,-1953,:2,--159-232;
-GILBERT'''. speed --of- protessing:iiinal-stirrinli---and- to reading.
Journairef'Educational-Psychology195%-15-,18.44-_f_
itotAtEs, J. a.- Factors _underlyingfinajor--reading-diiabilitiei,at-- the college level;
Unpublished- doctoral_ -dissertation, -University :Of talifornia,-_Berkeley, 1948.
HOLMES, J. ti.__The-Substrata-faetor_theory of reading. Berkeley:- California Book
1953. (Out of-print) _ _
-
A Theory of Language, Speech, and Writing'
DAVID WREED
University of California at Berkeley
RECENT AWARENESS that the science of linguistics may lie =of consider-
able relevance to research in elementary reading=instruttion could
produce-Very unfortunate== effects=if-lingdittitt :Shotild-"oe :mitunder-
stodd as giving-Wholehearted-support to phonic methods oUteaching=
reading. It is true that during--the4hirties; fortiesanct-fifties of this
century Mostilingdists_ emphasized th-e-spriinarytiattire of speech and
the secondary nature of writing. A feiv were-incautious enough,to
state that writing is-merely_ a,secondary-tepresentation Or identifica=
tion of speech. Many -non- specialists, in their efforts to apply the
Yr' findings-Of-linguistid to the accomplishment -of practical ends -con-
cluded= that speech is-the language. It demonstrated shortly
that these -Snore extreme-positions aboutthoprimacy of:speech over
writing are patently abstird.
Linguistic statements, like other scientific statements, have to be
understood in terms of the:_historical context that produced them.
During most of the nineteenth century and until about 1925, the
primary interest of linguists was in_ historical linguistics -and in the
reconstruction of prehistoric stages of descendant languages. This
interest naturally led investigators to-the written- records of earlier
periods is their primary-Source of information. Around -1925 Several
new interests combined to shift attention for writing to speech.
Ainong these were interest in recording - and analyzing the very large
number of unwritten languages in the world, interest in more ac-
curate descriptions of modern languages that have systems of written
1Tix., paperz,as presented at an International Reading Assodation Con-
ference on Linguistics and Reading held in Detroit, May 1965, and published in
Brother Leonard Courtney (Ed.), Highlights of the Pre-Convention Institutes,
Institute VI, Linguistics and Reading. Newark, Delaware: International Reading
Association, 1966, pp. 4.25. The first part of the paper was also = published in
Elementary English 42, December 1965, 845-851. The second part of the paper
was delivered at the Annual Convention of the National Council of Teachers
of English, Boston, November, 1965.
219
220 THEORETICAL MODELS. AND PROCESSES OF READING
is- totally deaf may be expected to learn to read and write before he
learns to speak. Indeed, he May grow up as a deaf muteone who
never learns to speak and is thus exactly =comparable to the adult
illiterate; who never learns to-read and write. In the-very- interesting
case -of Helen- Keller, who- was rendered deaf and- blind by scarlet
fever before she was-two-_yearsold, the primary system -of representing
the_Englishianguage-maSEa_tattilelystenii jni-which,symbols identify=
ing language are formed -by illefingers_of=orie=persorr_ in =contact with
the hand of another. Braille:becaMeltheEseCondary:systeM,- compar-
able-in functionito_-readingiand-writing-ifor most people,- and speech,
which Mks Keller -never- acquired _perfectly,- was---merely a_ tertiary
system-for_repr sentingErigkish. _
like /red/. One cannot write this correctly unless he knows -it to be
adjective or, alternatively, verb + past.
coin, but each bank may place its own design on the two faces, so long
as _these are always the same for the denomination struck by one
bank and different for different- dmominations. -We actually have
something like this system, since the designs on our coins=are- changed
from time to time. In any -event, --without laboring the analogy
further, the variant-designs on the-obVerseface of a-given -denomina-
tion Would-correspond-;_to the variant meanings-that may be atsociated
with a given linguistic _form, _whereas -== the Variant designs on ---the_=
reverse face would correspond toVarianCr_
variant-representations
hnguistic-fonn in speech, ivnung ta eSt4fCbrieCt=association-
of a written symbol with a spoken symbol is ,accomplished through
identification of the liriguistit-form-rather than,themeining,In the=
saMemay-thatiishOwn4 corn with a picture of Monticello on it one
know that= there are other coini-ofjhesaine-Value,nickel..-
=
tives are formed from such bases, rather than attempting to teach
derivatives as independent, unrelated forms. The correct procedure
A
has always been followed by reasonably intelligent teachers, but it is
important to obseive that it is an outright contradiction of the
method of phoneme-grapheme correspondence.
From what has been noted up to this point, it should be obvious
that the linguistic forms that are represented in = English speech
and writing consist in part of morphophonemes that are different
from the phonemes employed by most, it not- all, speakers in cer-
tain words, and in part of morphographemes that are different from
the graphemes employed =in conventional English spelling in cer-
tain words. It remains to consider how= dialect difference is a factor
in the determination of linguistic form and therefore affects the
relationship between speech and (writing.
Let us consider examples, presented first in their = written
form: 1) <cant>, meaning = "to tilt or slant," or, . alternatively, "a
jargon," 2) <can't>, the contraction of can and not, and 3) <Kant>,
the German philosopher. The first- is pronounced /kmnt/ in most
dialects of English and America; the second- is pronounced /Itx-nt/
in the majority of American dialects, but /Ica:nt/ in Standard South-
ern British and a few American dialects; in the third form, these
vowels are reversed for many speakers, so -that the pronunciation
/ka:nt/ prevails in America, but /kwnt/ is the= only pronunciation
given by Daniel Jones for Standard Southern British. Clearly there
is a linguistic difference among these three forms, and English writ-
ing represents this fact by employing three different spellings for the
forms. In the commonest varieties of British and American speech,
however, only two different pronunciations are employed, Britain
equating forms 1 and 3, America equating forms 1 and 2. English
writing spells all three forms differently, in order to show that they
con_ tain three different diaphonemes. Diaphonemic spelling enables
an American reader to associate the "broad a" pronunciation with.
<Kant>, and the British reader to associate it with <can't>.
The opposite of this situation may be illustrated by the forms
pronounced /wayz/, /sayz/, and /ayz/ (that is, the verb-forming
suffix.) The first and second are spelled <wise> and <size> on
both sides of the Atlantic. The verbal suffix is spelled <ize> . in
America, but <ise> in England. The pronunciation of the suffix in
232 THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
both countries reflects the linguistic identity of the form, and the
different spellings are a matter of variation in written dialects. All
'`It ree forms contain the same diagrapheme, as indicated by their
pronunciations.
We can now say that the linguistic forms that are represented
in English speech and writing consist in part of diaphonemes that are
different from the phonemes employed =by some speakers in some
words and different from the phonemes employed= by= other speakers
in other words, and in part of diagraphemes that are different from
the graphemes employed in conventional English spelling in certain
words. Combining this conclusion with the one previously reached,
we might say that English spelling is "dia-morpho-grapho-phonemic,"
but we= cannot say that the_linvistic =forms represented in speech
and writing are qdiamorphographophonemes," or the like. Actually
the linguistic fornis represented in speech and writing include some
diaphonemes, = some diagraphemes, some morphophonemes, some
morphographemes, and, in all those cases that linguists have thus far
emphasized, where there is a one-for-one correspondence between
phoneme, linguistic form, and grapheme, some mere graph-
ophonemes. We need= a= name for the class of which these various
"emes" are members, and= =we cannot continue to call it merely
"linguistic form," because there are many other linguistic firms, such
as "noun," that are not represented directly by phonemes and graph-
emes. Under these circumstances, I shall venture to coin a new term,
Unguent, to signify the smallest linguistic unit, such as actualized by
phonemes in speech and by graphemes in writing. The term is
formed on the analogy =of proton, electron, neutron, etc. in physical
science, and is similar to such terms as phonon, morphon, lexon, and
semon, that are used in stratificational grammar.
The abstract nature of such a concept as "the English language"
cannot be overemphasized here.- Just as one cannot meaningfully
say, "I drive the American automobile," so he ought to recognize
that when he says, "I speak the English language," he is employing
an oversimplified statement to express some such notion as, "I em-
ploy a system of speech that is derivable by a list of rules from an
abstract structure known as the English language." In terms of the
analogy, a given dialect is like a particular make of automobile, say
Buick, and my idiolect is perhaps analogous to a particular 1949
233
the following rules to convert the linguon into the proper phonemes
and graphemes:
Goth, moth, wroth, toss, boss, (and 3 others with postvocalic s), posh,
bosh, josh, gosh, of, Oz, tor, nor, for, and Thor; watt, squat, watch,
squad, wan, wash, squash, was, and war. The remaining 78 contain
//3://, including taught, caught, naught, aught, Maugham, and
Vaughan; ought, bought (and 4 others with postvocalic t), Sauk, auk,
baud, daub, Maude, Paul, maul, Saul, pause, and gauze; hawk, pawed,
cawed (and 6 others with postvocalic d); pawn, dawn (and 4 others
with postvocalic n), shawl, yawl, awl, paws, taws (and 6 others with
postvocalic z), (Bryn) Mawr, paw, taw (and 12 others with final
//o://); talk, chalk, (and 3 others with postvocalic k), tall, call (and
8 others with postvocalic 0.
It is found that 21 rules are needed to convert the vowel linguons
of these words into their correct English graphemes. Of these 21
rules; 18 relate to the linguon / /3: //, two relate to / /3/ /, and one
relates to / /a: //. The rules follow:
3
(1, 2) //3:// -4 <au> [taught, caught] A
(3) //3:// --) <ou> [X +
(4) //a:// <au>
(5) //3:// -+ <awe> [awe]
(6-10) //3:// -4 <aw> [shawl, yawl, awl, Mawr, hawk]
(11,12) //3:// -4 <aw> [
(13-16) //3:// -+ <au> [Paul, maul, Saul, haul]
(17) / /a: 1 I -) <a> [.._1]
(18) //3:// -> <au>
(19) //3 // -) <a> [w--]
(20) Ho // -* <o>
IL
(21) //a:// --) <a>
These are ordered rules, which is to say that they must be
applied largely in the order in which they have -just been presented.
Specifically, rules (1-4) must be applied in that order with reference
to one another and must precede rule (18): Rule (5) must precede
rules (6-12) and rule (18). Rules (6-10) must precede rules (7-18).
Rules (11-12) must precede rule (18). Rules (18 -16) must precede
rules (17-18). Rule (17) must precede rule (18), and, finally, rule
(19) must precede rule (20).
- s /ow is a linguon represented by <gh> and /0/ (i.e., "zero"). /1+// is
a linguon representing morpheme boundary.
236 THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
Only nine rules are needed to convert the vowel letters of these
words into the correct linguons:
of these two disciplines have caused the reading specialist and read-
ing researcher to ask how the multitude of individual components
are related to one another and in turn to language skills develop-
ment. The purpose of this discussion is to provide an overview of
selected linguistic and psycholinguistic variables related to decoding
and comprehending language, to briefly examine their psycholog-
ical reality, and in summation to incorporate these variables into a
systems of communication model.
Transformational and semantic theories have proposed that lan-
guage may be viewed on several levels. The -first level is considered
to be the surface structure and encompasses morphemic and synactic
structures which are realized in the form of the graphemic. morpho-
graphemic, phonemic, and morphOphonemic systems. It is at this
level that reading- instruction considers the decoding process. The
second level consists of structural and semantic readings which make
provision for processing language for interpretation. The various
transformational and rewrite rules and the structural reading, as
well as an individual's mental dictionary of semantic readings, are
considered to be incorporated into this level.
The third and least-understood level consists of the deep struc-
ture of language where it is hypothesized that the syntactic and
semantic components of the language are integrated for language
interpretation and stored in memory. This article will initially
examine the decoding process, representing- one dimension of the
surface structure level. Next, the comprehension process encompass-
ing the syntactic dimension of surface structure, the structural and
semantic readings, and the deep structure will be considered. A
minor emphasis will be given to the role of affective mobilizers
and cognitive strategies in language processing. And finally, a sys-
tems of communication model will be presented to summarize the
discussion relative to reading and language skills processing.
and Downing (9) have lent support to the value of greater consis-
tency in the introduction of sound-letter correspondences. Addition-
ally, the consistent replication of research findings discussed by
Chall (6) also supports the logical expectation that an approach to
decoding which helps the child grasp the nature of the English
writing code would be of value.
From the standpoint of information transfer the research by
Samuels and Jeffrey (43) emphasizes the value of sound-letter cor-
respondence units. In their research psuedo letters were designed
to- represent English phonemes and kindergarten subjects were
taught to decode on the basis of sound-letter correspondences and
on the basis of "whole words." The findings indicated that subjects
taught by the first- method were more effective in transferring their
skills to "new words" than were those subjects taught by the second
method. The emphasis on individual correspondences appears to
provide a lower error rate and more effective decoding skill than
does attention to word identification based on single featues.
In a later study the same researchers (23) replicated aspects of
the study with similar findings. However, they attributed
their results in part to one 'aspect of the experimental treatment
which taught the subjects to blend phonemes represented by the
psuedo letters into words. These findings are similar- to those of
Silberman (44) in that subjects were unable to transfer correspon-
dence information to new words unless they had received phonic-
blend instruction. The findings may be interpreted to suggest that
sound blending places the phonemes in a natural sound-unit con-
text constituting a =more elaborated decoding unit which is of value
in transferring sound-letter correspondence information to new letter
patterns and words.
Morphographemic-morphophonemic correspondences. If a
decoding program is to account for the nature of the English writ-
ing system, it is necessary to consider spelling units or letter pat-
terns which provide for prediction of sound correspondences beyond
the grapheme-phoneme correspondence level. Venezky (52), Ward-
haugh (53), and Reed (38) have discussed this concern with reference
to the morphophoneme. This unit represents an intermediate unit
between the phoneme and morpheme and may be thought of as a
sound-spelling pattern unit or morphophonemic-morphographemic
RUDDELL 243
ically real units. The experiment of Fodor and Bever (11) also sup-
ports this.contention. In their investigation, a clicking noise of brief
duration was made as a sentence was read. Regardless of the place-
ment of the click (e.g., during T word occurring immediately befc
or after a phrase boundary), the subjects indicated that the c'
occurred at the phrase boundary. Thus their conclusion supports tbc
viewpoint that perceptuai units correspond to sentence constituents
as designated by the linguist.
The .recent work of Ammon (1) has revealed that third-grade
and adult subjects require more time to= process and respond to
phrases. Suci et al (50) reported similar findings, thus providing ad-
ditional support for sentence constituents as- Meaningful processing
units.
Relational meaning-- deep structure. The transformational
theory has proposed that sentences are processed from the surface
structure level to an underlying or deep structure for comprehension
purposes. This deep structure is realized through transformational
2
and rewrite rules and is then integrated with the semantic com-
ponent to convey Meaning.
The work of Miller (32) has demonstrated that when subjects
are asked to transform sentences from one form into another (e.g.,
active affirmative to passive or active affirmative to passive negative),
a positive relationship is present between transformation time and
the comAexity of the transformation. This finding supports the con-
tention that transformations possess psychological reality in thit the
greater the number of transformations the greater is the distance
between the surface and deep structure of a sentence.
Mehler (30) has shown that after subjects have been asked to
memorize a series of complex sentences varying in grammatical type,
they tend to recall the sentence but in a simpler grammatical form.
For example, a sentence in the passive may be r_ ecalled in its active
form. These findings suggest that a recoiling of the sentence has
occurred and that the semantic form is maintained but -the deep
syntactic marker indicating the passive form has been forgotten.
The role of transformations in sentence comprehension has also
been demonstrated in the research of Gough (18) and Slobin (46) .
These researchers have shown that sentence comprehension varies
in increasing difficulty (speed in determining truth value of sentence)-
248 THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
.0
RUDDELL 249
meaning is retained with little regard for the structure. It would also
appear that the deep underlying structure is basic to comprehending
sentences.
Tronsfaimationot
and
Short Rewrite rules
Term
flooSng Memory N t...2_,,,. .
Semantic Memory
Grophemie
Contest feedback for verifying
syntactic information Feedback for
Writing Ir 4, O evaluating running
IMorpnographernic w. discourse
Lk..\`.
REFERENCES
hoped,4 a
So education it was t.I opaatits dictionary and picl:ed out a
a PH. He
word that sounded good. "p111/1INZpl_sAlYn/ 4- yelled. Might
what it means 1. Phizo 2. Phiso /soophical
as well study wend mat: -first. "Philesepkiesi: showing calmness
his 1. fort 2. future S. futshion
and courage in the face of ill fortune." I mean I really yelled it. I
guess a fellow has to work off steam once in a while..
4
GOODMAN 261
He has not seen the story before. It- is, by intention, slightly
difficult for him: The insights into his reading process come primiaily
from his errors, which I choose to call miscues in order to avoid value
implications. His expected responses mask the process- of their at-
tainment, but his unexpected responses have been achieved through
the same process, albeit less successfully applied.- The-ways that they
deviate from the expected reveal this proCess.
in the common sense view that I -am rejecting, all deviations
must -be_ treated_aserrois. Furthermore, it _must be assumed= in this=
=
view that an error either indicatet- that:the- reader does not kilo*
something or that he hat-been- "careless" in ihe application of hit
knowledge.
-For_ekample; his substitution Of:the:for your in the first para-
graph of the sample_ must mean =that -he ,_careless, since he has
already read your_anct the-cotrectly-rin---theAiery:same sentence. The
implication_ is=that we must _teach _him= toibemore_ Careful,- that_ is to
be more rreite in idetnifyitig each-word.orietter.
But now feet-take the view that I-haVei-uggested. What sort of
info..matitin :could haVe led to tentatively_ deciding onike in this
situation and not or refiningithis decision?_ There obviously
is no graphic relationship -between_your and= the.lt-may be =of course,
that he picked =up the in the fif-lis visual field; But; there
is ar important non-graphic relationship -between_ the and -your.
They both haVe thesame grammatical fUnctidn..: they are-, in:my term-
inology, noun marker-s._Bither the reader anticipateda noun marker
and supplied one paying no attention to graphic information or he
Used your as a-igrammatical Signal ignoring its graphic shape. Since
tite tentative choice the disturbs neither= the meaning nor the gram-.
mar of the-passage, there_ is-no_reasoni to -reject and-correct it. This
explanation appears to confirmed "by two similar miscues =in the
next paragraph. A and his are-both substituted for the Neither are
corrected. Though the SubstitutiOn-of,:hisichanget the meaning, the
peeuliar idiom used in this dictionary definition; "in the face of ill
fortune apparently has-little meaning to- this-_feader anyway.
The conchisitin this = time is that-he is -using _noun :Markers_ for
grammatical, as well as:graphic, information in reaching- his tenta-
tive conclusions. All together in reading this ten:page story, he made
twenty noun Marker substitution& six omissions and two insertions.
.1p
a,
GOODMAN 263
I've used the example of this youngster's oral reading not be-
cause what he's done is typical of all readers or even of readers his
age, but because his miscues suggest how he carries out the psycho-
linguistic guessing game in reading. The miscues of other readers
G.
GOODMAN 265
fp.
266 THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
'tit in the-44k.:
: too
_ _
occurred preceding train, she could attempt nothing for train. There
appears to be a problem for many first graders when nouns are used
as adjectives.
Common sense says go back and drill her on come, here, can,
stop, rale, and; don'aerher go to the next-book which she is 61) -
viously not ready to read.
But the more advanced story, with its stronger syntax, more
fully formed language and increased load of meaning makes it pos-
sible for the child to use her graphic cues more effectively and supple-
ment them with semantic and syntactic information. Teaching -for
more precise perception with lists and phonics charts may actually
impede this chiles reading development. Please notice, before we
leave the passage, the effect of immediate experience on anticipation.
Every one of the paragraphs in the sample starts with "Jimmy said"
or "Sue said." When the reader comes to a line starting Jimmy, she
assumes that it will be followed by said_ and it is not until her :ex-
pectition is contradicted by subsequent input that she regresses and
corrects her nfiscue.
Since they must learn to play the psycholinguistic guessing game
as they develop reading ability, effective methods and materials used
by teachers who understand the tides of the game, must help them
to select the most productive cues, to use their knowledge of lan-
guage structure, to draw on their experiences and concepts. They
must be helped to discriminate between more and less useful avail-
able information. Fortunately, this- parallels the processes they have
used in developing the ability to comprehend spoken language.
George Miller (7) has suggested ". psycholinguists should try to
formulate performance models that will incorporate -... hypo_ thetical-
information storage and= information processing components that can
simulate the actual behavior of language users."
: like to present now my model of this psycholinguistic guess-
irg game we call reading English. Please- understand that the steps
do not necessarily take place in the sequential or stretched cut-form
they are shown here. [The model appears on page 272.].
1. The reader scans along a line of print from left to right and
down the page, line by line.
2. He fixes at a point to permit eye focus. Some print will be
270 THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
lie, not in its complexity, but in the fact that it is not yet complex
enough to fully account for the complex phenomena in the actual be-
havior of readers. But such is man's destiny in his quest for knowl-
edge. Simplistic folk lore must give way to complexity as we come
to know.
REFERENCES
wax am MINX
acme
rosstsst
wavy ms wutan
TITS Intl: 'Mt
01101CL Ia SteXTIC twins et vsre Met KV CO
AM Sert Ante ACCIASIATTO 05551115 AND
CddLIT MUNK ' STORIO TM Lde
TIM Med
L. 1.0 011;110 MIMS 00.
"iglu
most TTO5 LOTT
MD If PILL
Editin's Note: This chart is an updated version of the model described in Goodman's article. It is taken from a report on "The
Jo II) on ;et.rwaoanto.4 In ?).e I c T
The Reading
Reading Competency Model
RICHARD L. VENEZKY
University of Wisconsin at Madison
AND
ROBERT C. CALFEE*
Stanford University.
r 273
v_
274 THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
r
276 THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
Forward scanning sek out word and phrase breaks, and asks,
along the way, "Is there anythin unfamiliar in the terrain?" If so,
4 tags are assigned to indicate the degree of strangeness. Sarching,
however, ii always oriented towards the biggest digestible gulp j
4
the largest manageable uniC The information stores referenced dur-
-3 For the competent reiIer, fotward scanning always occurs. The degreeof
-,----
S
_
_ -_-_--_-_:=-:=_;- = ----_
_
VENIMKk---AND:CA
to CtyiIi ma
to account
F7_atitittiti
ILETICAL_MODELSI-AND--PROCESSUfbUREAD
estionu_
-_-_,i-:-- --_ --_ -_-- --__--- i__---- _ss;- --- _ :---- -__===_.=-s_ :- -__-_i_
_
---- _-__---- _----f c-_ _- -=-
_ _-_- -_ s=
_
_ = fr- =_--- --=-_ _- -.,_ -_- ___-z_:_ _in -_ --_ -___ - r__-- ---_ --_ --__s_
-___=-_-n,
_
nrmem_
ezau We=
t
_
sr,__-=t,_ ,
a-tarage- and retneva TrotessecareAnvo
don.
,
i
286 111LORETICAL MODELS _AND PROCESSES OF READING
nitrncticiiircat
UcatlOni
esear_
ature"--b
Educationalf-Testink_
erne
. --
THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES_ OF READING
THEORETICAL MODELS AND =PROCESSES OF' READING
_
nnte -w
are $11
prehen
_-=
J.
CARLL 301
4
This latter -view is held by thcse who argue that there should
be an early emphasis on getting the meaning from print, and thit
the child should advance as quickly as possible toward theword-rec-
ognition and meaning-apprehension capacities of the mature reader.
Skills 2, 3, and 5 are intioduced only iftr the child has achieved
considerable progress- towards mastery of skills 4, 6, 7, and 8.
-: These are the two main views about the process of teaching-read-
-
ing. If each one is taken quite strictly. and seriously, there can be
clear differences in the kinds of instructiOnal materials and
procedures that are used. It is beyond our scope to discuss whether
the two methods differ in effectiveness. We would emphasize, rather,
methods may differ ineffectiveness from child to child. Further-
1- more, it is possible to construt othr reasonable orders in which the .
-
3O2
THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
inferenti _= processes?
7.at ire _ e ma or sources of_ individual differences in rate
and accuracy of comprehension in mature readers?
Questions about certain components _of reading shill as they =affect
=
learning
1: In what way= does knowledge of the spoken language interact
_
with learning-to read? What kinds and amounts-of competence are
des irable_before the child undertakes any given task in learning to
read?
2. What is the nature of the ability to discriminate sounds in
the spoken,language and to diisect words-in terms of these sounds?
HoW does it develop and what role does it play in the beginning
reader's learning of letter-sound correspondences? How can this
ability be taught?
3. Hoiv do children learn to recognize and discriminate alpha-
betic letters- in their various forms? When children have difficulty
with letter recognition, how can = these = difficulties = be overcome?
4. = How do children learn the left-to-right principle in orthogra-
phy, both as applied to individual words and to the order of words
in continuous tex e ther children with special difficulties in
learning this component of reading skill? :
5 y w at are e most useful and functional patterns of
ndence in = =what
-Let42sTconsiderbrieflystMe-Cif_the-more.-obvioUs waysin-_which
intention= affectareading:-__ :)--AS,rio-ted,---if=a-:-=persdn_lOoks--idly_=at
page_ of text, with no intentionto read, he may recognize only those
few wordsAanivhicbhis--gaze-:marreit;==2)-:Art :effiCient__ reader may
read_ textlor Meaning-atiUpAo--4,800letteralier- ininUte, _but at -that
rate he is very likelynotitO-notice_minor spellingl_errors_Or omissionsf
therefore,_-this--Agure certainly-di:its-not:titan:that each letter is
actually looked- at, -even =though- coMpreherition scores may-be very
z
iverse
t
Skills 1scai n =an; _be- itrarc t
:--0
.F,cannoti=r
_=res
attended
ef-thatthe !s
_requires_
210CHBERG- AND BROOZS
in-and
thii:hypOthesii=testing
listening-and- speaking 43roce-sses.
_
increadirig,---Which rooted
single fixation -that- is made in-_ the_ -course==of the treading- rprocesS.
Many-years agOi-Cattell- (5,-6)_shoWddrthat- the =exposure Irequirect=to
identify each of-atachistoicopically_-presented sevoft_unrelated letteri;-_
increased -as ==the -number=of-letters--=,intreased. The-=_-exposure:_'thirie
-reqnired--_tkyrecbgriiie-soinefaniihaelirord--0--phrase however, -no-
longer -than that for= a shigle-letterArriother4ords.,-- the- skilled= reader
_Inch ngr_up t =iv hat, e-'-iinskilled:_zeider have- to- _-
REFERENCES
1. -Betti,- A;-_--112.e-ading:-;Pereepttal :Learning," =Edupition.--__ItidianapOlict
=
_ _
ception, Unconscious_ Perception: An= Analysis- in Terms-oflosychophysical
Indicator-Methodology," -P.rychOlogiee&Balletini-=55(1950),
-11. -Graf, R., an d-1.1 "W-_ Torrey. "Perceptioii-of Phrase Structure-in Written
Language," -== Proceedings- of-=the -14th fAnntiaUConvention-Tof the Ameriedn
e_PsychOlogicl- Association,_1966,_83-8t.---
12.-_ Hayes,--W.4._Robinscin;-_and_ I.,-Brown.--''An'Effect-- of -Paitxperience,on
Perception: An_Artifitt,"Anierican-PsycholOgis4-1611961), 420-(Abstract): -_
-zHenle.:M:_ "The F...iperiinental Investigation_ oU_PastEsieriences as-- a-rDeter--
minantiof Visual-TFOrni_Per curnal-,of Ex- --rirnental-Psychology,
1
14.= :MY
Th _ =in Perce lion.
instoir-1969i_309431
nsponents:o lationaianctilEi lort
=sear -acting,-
Eleanor- J. Gibson
TO ERIC AND ORGANIZATIONS
UNDER AGREEMENTS OPERATING
, OF EDUCATION WITH THE US OFFICE
FURTHER REPRODUCTION
OUTSIDE THE ERIC SYSTEM REOUIRES
PER
Learning to Reads MISSION OF THE COPYRIGHT
OWNER"
EDUCATORS and the public have exhibited a keen interest in the teach-
ing of reading ever since tree _public education became a fact '.(1).
Either because of or despite-their interest,- this most important sub-
ject has been remarkably susceptible to the influence of fads and
fashions and curiously unaffected by disciplined experimental and
theoretical psychology. The psychologistt hive traditionally pursued
the study of verbatlearning by means of experiments with nonsense
syllables and the likethat is, materials carefully divested of useful
information. And the educators, who found little in.this work that
seemed relevant to the classroom, hlie stayed with the classroom;
when they perfonned experiments:the method was apt to be a gross
comparison of classes privileged and unprivileged with respect to the
latest fad. The result has been two cultures: the pure scientists in the
laboratory, and the practical teachers ignorant of the progress that
has been made in the theory of human learning and in meth_ ods of
studying it.
That this split was unfortunate is dear enough. True, most
children do learn to read. But some learn to read badly, so that
school systems must provide remedial clinics; and-a small proportion
(but still a large number of future citizens) remain functional illiter-
ates. The fashions which have led to classroom experiments, such
Reprinted from Science, 148 (1965) 1066.1072 with the permission of the
author and the American Association for the Advancement of Science. The
author is senior research associate in r; Aology at Cornell University. This
article is adapted from a paper read at a c) ,ference on Perceptual and Linguistic
Aspects of Reading, sponsored by- the Committee on Learning and the Educa-
tional Process of the Social Science Research Council and held at the Center for
Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences, Palo Alto, California, 31 October
1963. -PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE THIS COPY.
RIGHTED MATERIAL HAS BEEN GRANTED
BY
315 American Association
Agi.,,as Advancement of
TO ERIC AND ORGANIZATIONS OPERATING
UNDER AGREEMENTS WITH THE US OFFICE
OF EDUCATION FURTHER REPRODUCTION
OUTSIDE THE ERIC SYSTEM REOUIRES PER.
MISSION OF THE COPYRIGHT OWNER'
7
GIBBON
317
forms improved from age 4 to age 8, but the slopes of the error curves
were different, depending on the transformation to be discriminated
(Fig. 2). In other words, some transformations are harder to dis-
criminate than others, and improvement occurs at different rates
for different transformations. Even the youngest subjects made
relatively few errors involving changes of break or- dose, and among
the 8-year-olds these errors dropped to zero.- Errors =for perspective
transformations were very numer- ous among 4-year-olds and still nu-
merous among 8-year-olds. Errors for rotations and reversals started
high fbut dropped to nearly zero by 8 years. Errors for changes from
=
Type of training
Group Standards Transformations Errors
El Same Different 69
E2 Different Same 39
.C Different Different 101
320 THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
.
lists which satisfy these requirements for a specified type c--e were
tried out against the results of a confusion matrix obtained with the
same type (simplified Roman capitals available on a sign-typewriter).
Each of the features in the list in Fig. 3 is or is not a character-
-i GIBSON
321
Features A B C E K I. N.0
Straight segment
Horizontal + + +
Vertical
Oblique / +
+ + ++ +. +
Oblique \ + + +
Curve
Closed +
Open vertically
Open horizontally + +
Intersedion ++ ++ +
Redundancy
+
Cyclic change
Symmetry
Discontinuity
++ + ++ + ++
Vertical
+ + + + +
Horizontal ++
FIGURE 3. Example of a "feature chart." Whether the features chosen are
actually effective for disaiminating ktten must be determined by experiment.
istic of each of the 26 letters. Regarding each letter one asks, for
example, "Is there a curved segment?" and gets a yes or no answer.
A filled-in feature chart gives a unique pattern for each letter. How-
ever, the number of potential features for letter-shapes is very large,
and would vary from one alphabet and type font to another. Whether
or not we have the right set can be tested with a confusion matrix.
Children should confuse with greatest frequency the letters having
the smallest number of feature differences, if the features have been
chosen correctly.
We obtained our confusion matrix from 4-year-old children,
who made matching judgments of letters, programed so that every
letter had an equal opportunity to be mistaken for any other, without
bias from order effects. The "percent feature difference" for any two
letters was determined by dividing the total number of features
possessed by either letter, bat not both, by the total number possessed
by both, whether shared or not. Correlations were then calculated
between percent feature difference and number of confusions, one
for each letter. The feature list of Fig. 3 yielded 12 out of 26 positive
significant correlations. Prediction from this feature list is fairly good,
$22 THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES- OF READING
7
40
6
... 5
o
is: 4
9-
I -11-1-1 1 t t I I -t I I I t- t t -I 11-
0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 22 24
Trio Is
FIGURE 4. Learning curves on transfer task for group tra ned originally
with whole words (W), group- trained with= single letters (L), ant - control group
(q.
the average, a greater number of component letter-sound correspon-
dences, but some subjects in group W hadAearned all 12. Most of
the subjects in group L reported that they had tried to learn by using
knowledge of component correspondences. But so did 12 of the 20
subjects in group W, and the scores of these 12 subjects on the trans-
fer task were similar to those of the letter-trained group. The subjects
who had learned by whole words and had not used individual cor-
respondences performed no better on the task than the control sub-
jects.
It is possible, then, to learn to read words without learning the
component letter-sound correspondences. But transfer to new words
depends on use of them, whatever the method of original training.
Word training was as good as letter training if the subject had ana-
lyzed for himself the component relationships.
unequivocally that this picture is false. For the English language, the
single graphemes map consistently into speech only as morphemes
that is, the names of -the letters of the alphabet. It is possible, of
course, to name letters sequentially across aline of print ("spell out"
1
a word), but that is not the goal of a skilled reader, nor is it what he
does. Dodge (11) showed, nearly 60 years ago, that perception occurs
in reading only during fixations, and not at all during the saccadic
jumps from one fixation to the next. With a fast tachistoscopic ex-
posure, a skilled reader can perceive four unconnected letters, a very
long word, and four or more words if they form a sentence .(1 2).
Even- first graders can read three-letter words exposed for only 40
milliseconds, too short a time for sequential eye movements to occur.
Broadbent (13) has pointed out that speech, although-it consists
of a temporal sequence of stimuli, is responded to at the end of a
sequence. That is, it is normal for a whole sequence to be delivered
before a response is made. For instance, the sentence "Would you
give me your ?" might end with any of a large number of
words, such as "name" or "wailer or "wife." The response depends
on the total message. The fact that the component stimuli for speech
r.c1 reading are spread over time does not mean that the phonemes
or letters or words -are processed one at- a time, with each stimulus
decoded to a separate response. The fact that o is pronounced differ-
ently in BOAT and BOMB is not a hideous peculiarity of English which
must consequently be reformed. The o is read -only in context and is
never responded to in isolation. It is part of a sequence which con-
tains constraints of- two kinds, one morphological and the other the
spelling patterns which are characteristic of English.
If any doubt remains as -to the unlikelihood of sequential pro-
cessing letter by letter, there is recent evidence of Newman (14) and
of Kolers (15) on sequential exposure of letters. When letters forming
a familiar word are exposed sequentially in the same place, it is al-
most impossible to read the word. With an exposure of 100 milli-
seconds per letter, words of six letters are read with only 20 percent
probability of accuracy; and with an exposure of 375 milliseconds
per letter, the probability is still well under 100 percent. But that is
more than 2 seconds to perceive-a short, well-known. word! We can
conclude that, however graphemes are processed perceptually in
reading, it is not a letter-by-letter sequence of acts.
If the single grapheme does'not map consistently to a phoneme,
and furthermore, if perception normally takes in bigger "chunks"
of graphic stimuli in a single fixation, what are the smallest graphic
units consistently coded into phonemic patterns? Must they be whole
GIBSON 329
words Are there different levels of units? Are they achieved at dif-
ferent stages of development?
Spelling Patterns
It is my belief that the smallest component units in written
English are spelling patterns (16). By a spelling pattern, I mean a
cluster of graphemes in a given environment which has an invariant
pronunciation according to the rules of English. These rules are the
regularities which appear when, for instance, any vowel or consonant
or cluster is shown to correspond with-a given prounuciation in an
initial, medial, or final position in the spelling of a word. This kind
of regularity is not merely "frequency" (bigram frequency,- rrigram
frequency, and so on), for it implies that frequency counts are rele-
vant for establishing rules only if the right units and the right re-
lationships are counted. The relevant graphic unit is a functional
unit of one or more_ in a given position .:111in the word,
which is in correspondence with a specified pronunci .tion (17).
If potential regularities exist within wordsthe spelling pat-
terns that occur in regular correspondence with speech patterns
one may hypothesize that these correspondences have been assim-
ilated by the skilledreader of English (whether or not be can
verbalize thezules) aird have the effect of organizing units for percep-
tion. It follows that strings of- letters which are generated by the
rules will be perceived more easily than ones which are not, even
when they are. unfamiliar words or not words at all.
Several experiments testing this prediction were performed by
Gibson, Pick, Osser, and aammond (18). The basic design was to
compare the perceptibility (with a very short tachistoscopic ex-
posure) of two sets of letter-strings, all nonsense or pseudo words,
which differed in their spelling-to-sound correlation. One list, called
the "pronounceable" list, contained words with a high spelling-to-
sound correlation. Each of them had an initial consonant-spelling
with a single, regular pronunciation; a final consonant-spelling hav-
ing a single regular pronunciation; and a vowel-spelling, placed be-
tween them, having a single regular pronunciation when it follows
and is followed by the given initial and final consonant spellings,
repectivelyfor example, cL/tt/c.x. The words in the second list,
330 THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
ample, RAN, NAR, RNA). Some longer pseudo words (four and five
letters) taken from the previous experiments were included as.well.
The words and pseudo words were exposed tachistoscopically to in-
dividual children, who were required to spell them orally. The
-first-graders read (spelled out) most accurately the familiar three-
letter words, but read the pronounceable trigrams significantly better
than the unpronounceable ones. The longer pseudo words were sel-
dom read accurately and were not differentiated by- pronunciability.
The third-grade girls read all three-letter combinations with high
and about equal accuracy, but differentiated the longer pseudo
words; that is, the pronounceable four- and five-letter pseudo
words were more often perceived correctly than their unpronounce-
able counterparts.
These results suggest that a child in the first stages of reading
skill typically reads in short units, but has already generalized cer-
tain- regularities- of- spelling-to-sound correspondence, so -that three-
letter pseudo words which fit the rules are more easily read as units.
As skill develops, span increases, and a similar difference can be
observed for longer items. The longer items involve more complex
conditional rules and longer dusters, so that the generalizations must
increase in complexity. The fact that a child can begin very early
to perceive regularities of correspondence between the printed and
spoken patterns, and transfer them to the reading of unfamiliar
items as units, suggests that the opportunities for discovering the
correspondences between patterns might well be enhanced in pro-
graming reading materials.
I have referred several times to levels of units. The last experi-
ment showed that the size and complexity of the spelling patterns
which can be perceived as units increase with development of read-
ing skill. The other levels of structure, both syntactic and semantic,
contain units as large as and larger than the word, and that percep-
tion of skilled readers will be found, in suitable experiments, to be a
function of these factors is almost axiomatic. As yet we have little
direct evidence better than Cattell's original discovery (12) that
when words are structured into a sentence, more letters can be ac-
curately perceived "at a glance." Developmental studies of percep-
tual "chunking" in relation to structural complexity may be very
instructive.
332 THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING
Summary
Reading begins with the child's acquisition of spoken language.
Later he learns to differentiate the graphic symbols from one an-
other and to decode these to familiar speech sounds. As he learns to
decode, he must progressively utilize the structural constraints which
are built into it in order to attain the skilled performance which
is characterized by processing of higher-order unitsthe spelling and
morphological patterns of the language.
Because of my firm conviction that good pedagogy is based on
a deep understanding of the discipline to be taught and the nature
of the learning process involved, I have tried to show that the psy-
GIBSON 333
REFERENCES
1. See C. C. Fries, Linguistics and Reading (Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, New
York, 1963), for an excellent chapter on past practice and theory in the
teaching of reading.
2. In 1959, Cornell University was awarded a grant for a Basic Research Project
on Reading by the Cooperative Research Program of the Office of Education,
U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare. Most of the work
reported in this article was supported by this grant. The Office of Education
has recently organized "Project Literacy," which will promote research on
reading in a number of laboratories, as -well as encourage mutual under-
standing between experimentalists and teachers of, reading.
3'. E. J. Gibson, J. J. Gibson, A. D. Pick, H. Osser, J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol.
55, 897 (1962).
4. E. J. Gibson, H. Osser, W. Schiff, J. Smith, in A Basic Research Program on
Reading, Final Report on Cooperative Research Project No. 639 to the
Office of Education, Department of Health, Education, and Welfare.
5. The method was greatly influenced by the analysis of distinctive features of
phonems by R. Jacobson and M. Halle, presented in Fundamentals of Lan-
guage (Mouton, The Hague, 1956). A table of 12 features, each in binary
opposition, yields a unique pattern fol. all phonemes, so that any one is
distinguishable from any other by its pattern of attributes. A pair of pho-
nemes may differ by any number of features, the minimal distinction being
one feature opposition. The features must be invariant under certain trans-
formations and essentially relational, so as to remain distinctive over a wide
range of speakers, intonations, and so on.
6. A. D. Pick, J. Exp. Psychol., in press.
7. C. H. Bishop, J. Verbal Learning Verbal Behay. 3, 215 (1964).
8. Current advocates of a revised alphabet who emphasize the low letter-sound
correspondence in English are Sir James Pitman and John A. Downing.
Pitman's revised alphabet, called the Initial Teaching Alphabet, consists of
43 characters: some traditional and some new. It is designed for instruction
of the beginning reader, who later transfers to traditional English spelling.
See I. J. Pitman, J. Roy. Soc. Arts 109, 149 (1961); J. A. Downing, Brit.- J.
Educ. Psychol. 32, 166 (1962); , "Experiments with Pitman's initial
334 THEORETICAL MODELS AND PROCESSES OF READING