Cinema Trance Cibernetics
Cinema Trance Cibernetics
RECURSIONS
Ute Holl’s Cinema, Trance and Cybernetics discusses the cinematic apparatus
as an interface between mind and machine. Rather than simply a medium
for representing altered states, cinema is considered as a cultural technique of
trance. Close investigations of the Soviet avant-garde connect Dziga Vertov
to Russian psycho-reflexology and V.M. Bechterev’s theory of trance.
The anthropological tradition of cine-trance is viewed in the context of
feed-back, as conceived of by Jean Rouch, as well as of the New American
Cinema, following Maya Deren and Gregory Bateson, in conceptualizing the
reiteration of time, space, and movement, to prove that feedback is the basic
UTE HOLL
strategy of cinematic transformations. Holl’s influential stance suggests that
cybernetics is not only an instrument of control, but that the homeostatic
forces of film are steps to an ecology of the cinematic mind that finds it
origins in the nineteenth-century laboratory techniques of measuring the
senses, movement and behaviour.
C I N E MA T R A N C E & C YB E R N E T I C S
“Cinema, Trance and Cybernetics will change the way you see the cinema’s past. Through an
impressive synthesis of psychology, cybernetics, anthropology and the cinematic arts, it
reveals how cinema was born in the scientific laboratory and grew into a machine for
controlling, but also emancipating, mental life. Providing a powerful historical account that
brings Maya Deren in contact with Vladimir Bekhterev, amongst others, the book shows
how cinema ultimately came to shape us into its own image.”
‒ Film and screen scholar Pasi Väliaho, who has written a new Preface to the English edition
of Cinema, Trance and Cybernetics.
Amsterdam
AUP.nl University
9 789089 646682 Press RECURSIONS
Cinema, Trance and Cybernetics
The book series Recursions: Theories of Media, Materiality, and Cultural
Techniques provides a platform for cuttingedge research in the field of media
culture studies with a particular focus on the cultural impact of media technology
and the materialities of communication. The series aims to be an internationally
significant and exciting opening into emerging ideas in media theory ranging
from media materialism and hardware-oriented studies to ecology, the post-
human, the study of cultural techniques, and recent contributions to media
archaeology. The series revolves around key themes:
– The material underpinning of media theory
– New advances in media archaeology and media philosophy
– Studies in cultural techniques
These themes resonate with some of the most interesting debates in international
media studies, where non-representational thought, the technicity of knowledge
formations and new materialities expressed through biological and technological
developments are changing the vocabularies of cultural theory. The series is also
interested in the mediatic conditions of such theoretical ideas and developing
them as media theory.
Editorial Board
– Jussi Parikka (University of Southampton)
– Anna Tuschling (Ruhr-Universitat Bochum)
– Geoffrey Winthrop-Young (University of British Columbia)
Cinema, Trance and Cybernetics
Ute Holl
Amsterdam University Press English-language titles are distributed in the US and Canada by
the University of Chicago Press.
Some rights reserved. Without limiting the rights under copyright reserved above, any part of
this book may be reproduced, stored in or introduced into a retrieval system, or transmitted,
in any form or by any means (electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise).
Table of Contents
In the Beginning 19
Part I
1. Cinema 23
2. Cybernetics 33
3. Knots 39
4. To Whom it May Concern 47
Part II
1. Discretions 57
2. Depersonalizations 77
3. Deviations 117
4. Compressions 137
Part III
1. Mental Apparatuses 161
2. Psycho-Motor Activity 187
3. Psycho-Drama 197
4. Psycho-Technology 205
5. Psycho-Reflexology 219
Part IV
1. The Truth Won by Means of Film 249
Part V
1. After All: Return to Receiver 281
Notes 285
Bibliography 309
Index 321
Foreword
Fade into Black
Pasi Valiaho
Goldsmiths, University of London
Cinema, Trance and Cybernetics is driven by a refrain. The paths and forks
of the book’s reflections always return to a primal scene, a “ritual”, as Ute
Holl calls it, which keeps appearing in different guises: the “passage into the
darkness of the cinema” that begins as soon as the lights of the movie theatre
are turned off. This primal scene retains its familiarity to us. Despite the fact
that televisual screens have rendered their occurrence as virtually superflu-
ous, we can still remember and experience those anticipatory moments of
becoming enveloped into the movie theatre’s artificial blackness before the
film projector begins its duty of casting movements of light and shadow
on the screen’s blank surface. Cinema, Trance and Cybernetics reminds
us how this “passage” bears particular anthropological significance. The
movie theatre’s darkness is one in which the faces of our loved ones become
covered with the strangest masks, and we grow either closer or more alien
to ourselves.
Cinema, Trance and Cybernetics can be read as a detective story: Who
turned off the lights? What happened when the darkness fell for the first
time? The book engages a range of actors from the later 19th century and
the first part of the 20th to divulge the necessary clues. Readers should be
notified, however, that its protagonists are not the usual (male) figures of
early cinema and the Hollywood movie industry but instead scientists as
well as scientific and experimental filmmakers. Rather than the Lumière
brothers, Alfred Hitchcock, Steven Spielberg, the Wachowskis, etc., the
dramatis personae that for the most part populate the following pages
include Maya Deren, Jean Painlevé, Jean Rouch and Dziga Vertov, in addition
to numerous (more or less well-known) figures from the history of the life
sciences as well as anthropology, including Margaret Mead and Gregory
Bateson, Etienne-Jules Marey, Gustav Fechner, Hermann von Helmholtz,
Vladimir Bekhterev, and the Harvard psychologist of German origin, Hugo
Münsterberg, who published one of the first theoretical studies on the
“photoplay” in 1917. Cinema, Trance and Cybernetics takes the reader to
a journey that spans a range of different sets (seen as if from a bird’s eye
view, through a virtual camera flying over different locations): Bali (where
8 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
Mead and Bateson did their fieldwork with film cameras and typewriters),
Albert Londe’s photographic studio at the Salpêtrière in Paris, Marey’s
physiological station in Bois de Boulogne, the international symposium
of neuroscientists and psychologists at Wittenberg College, Ohio, in 1927,
Deren’s house in Los Angeles where Meshes of the Afternoon (1943) was
filmed…
Cinema, Trance and Cybernetics in a sense rewrites film history. Its stakes
are nothing less. But instead of debating the historical record (who did what
when), the book’s implicit concern is on how these questions should be
posed. Rather than merely telling stories about how cinema was conceived
by various inventors and has been used by artists and entrepreneurs, Cin-
ema, Trance and Cybernetics is preoccupied by what the medium of film
has done to us, how the cinema has changed us. That is what the mystery of
darkness is about. The book thus refocuses the lens through which we look
at the past; it adjusts our conceptual understandings and approaches. Under
Holl’s scrutiny, cinema doesn’t appear as a medium of mass entertainment,
not even as a particular aesthetic form per se, but above all as an “anthropo-
logical machine” the stakes of which involve articulations between human
and animal, conscious and unconscious, or speaking being and living being.1
Above all, Cinema, Trance and Cybernetics explores the film medium’s place
and meaning in the scientific and technological upheavals of the twentieth
century that came to radically reshape our composition.
A key thread of the book’s narrative fabric is to show how cinema was
born out of the spirit of the experimental scientific laboratories developed in
the nineteenth century. “The unknown avant-gardists of film history”, Holl’s
cogent observation goes, were in fact “natural scientists”. It was the forerun-
ners of modern neurology, physiology and psychology that first came up with
techniques of probing perception and consciousness, and above all the “li-
men” that separates conscious mental states from non-conscious ones. After
initial experimentations in the lab, commercial and artistic applications,
which capitalized on cinema’s liminal psychology, followed. Jean-Martin
Charcot’s and Albert Londe’s visualizations of the movements of so-called
hysterical female bodies at the Salpêtrière in Paris, for instance, provided
the breeding ground for Lillian Gish’s performances in David Griffith’s films.
Gustav Fechner’s three-year state of trance, which the founder of psychophy
sics induced himself into with excessive experiments on afterimage effects,
amongst other things, might just as well describe the somnambulists flocking
to watch Griffith’s dramas, or alternatively today’s video game players.
Throughout Cinema, Trance and Cybernetics one can hear the echo
of Friedrich Kittler writing in Gramophone, Film, Typewriter (originally
Foreword 9
published in 1986 in Germany): “Since its inception, cinema has been the
manipulation of optic nerves and their time”.2 Kittler’s insight was to show
how technological recordings and reproductions of acoustic and optical
“data” have critically changed the “state of reality.”3 For Holl, likewise,
cinema performs an experiment on our being; on what can be sensed, im-
agined and dreamt. Conceptually, Cinema, Trance and Cybernetics is firmly
grounded in the German-speaking tradition of media and film theory,
and proves what this tradition of scholarship is capable of doing with its
vigorous mixing of philosophical thought with a sustained inquiry into and
critique of cinematic (and other medial) modes of being and thinking. Film
comes here across as a cultural technique the research of which, to borrow
Bernhard Siegert’s words, “amounts to an epistemological engagement with
the medial conditions of whatever lays claim to reality.”4
Cinema had its throne as the medium of the 20th century capable of
dictating how we are able to perceive and conceptualize the world. Cinema
had the power, to paraphrase Kittler’s words, to “define what really is.”5 Holl
investigates this power by deftly drawing conceptual parallels between
cinema and cybernetics, parallels that have largely remained to be explored,
one might surmise, simply because they perhaps aren’t the most obvious.
Whilst cinema, at first sight at least, comes across as a technology of the
industrial era, belonging to the family of mechanical apparatuses that
includes the steam engine, the bicycle, the train, and the clock, cybernet-
ics is an invention of the Second World War, of (electronic) signals and
computation. Whilst cinema was originally conceived by the Scientist and
then quickly appropriated by the Capitalist, cybernetics belonged in its very
beginnings to the General, that is to say, to the military-industrial complex.
Norbert Wiener’s “anti-aircraft predictor” (built in 1948) was one of the first
incorporations of the cybernetic idea of modeling and controlling a system’s
behavior and particularly its future states. How does this compare with film,
a technology we normally associate with storage rather than anticipation,
pure recording rather than feedback? The common denominator can be
found in the etymology of “cybernetics”, that is to say, governance and
control. Both cinematic and cybernetic systems link living beings with
machines and in so doing administer and regulate human cognition and
behavior, or in other words, the movements of bodies and souls. Both
“feedback” into our nervous systems and brains. “The cinema is thus a good
place to examine cybernetic processes, since the links between nervous
systems and apparatuses have constantly been synchronized, aligned, and
optimized in its history”, Holl writes. Synchronization and optimization of
the senses and psychic life coupled with the mechanics of the film apparatus
10 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
– this is the operating function of the feedback loop between the spectator
and the screen that is established in the dark of the movie theatre.
Cinema’s power, then, is not merely located in the contents of its imagery,
however moving, luring or explicit, but in the exchanges, repetitions and
relays that take place between the movie machine and our nervous sys-
tems and brains. Cinema is a machine for controlling and regulating our
impulses, sensations and emotions. But above all – and this is the point
that recurs in Cinema, Trance and Cybernetics like Karl Marx’s ghost in our
neoliberal nightmares – cinema is a machine for shifting and displacing
the consciousness, and for “inducing trance”. Combining film experience
with the trance state is the second key innovation of the book, in addition
to refocusing on cinema through the lens of cybernetics.
“Trance”, Holl writes, “as a dissolution or diversion of the conscious-
ness under the impact of certain technologies, is the gap in film theory.”
Much ink has indeed been spilled on the unconscious mechanisms of film
spectatorship; on the dream state induced by film viewing, on the mirror-
ings, misrecognitions and identifications that arguably take place in the
movie theatre’s darkness. In the film theory of the 1970s, the moviegoer was
even reduced to an infantile of sorts. Cinema, Trance and Cybernetics does
point towards a similar kind of loss of control in the spectator’s position
as soon as the lights in the movie theatre are turned off. But rather than
mere regression, the idea of trance designates here how the spectator’s
self-regulatory system yields control to external forces – the sheer force
of movement, light and shadow on the silver screen taking charge of inner
physiological and mental events. This means the emergence of alternative
bodily and psychic states, affective and emotional conditions, within the
individual. In this respect, Holl’s analysis can be seen to converge with
recent attempts at re-conceptualizing cinematic subjectivity, which seek
analogies between the filmic apparatus and hypnosis in particular.6 Here,
the power of cinema is considered in terms of suggestion, even possession,
which opens our minds, not simply to external control and machination,
but also to new dispositions and capacities.
In this respect, trance indeed appears in what follows as an ambiguous
concept, a concept with at least two faces. On one side, it is a question of
“command and control” – the movie machine’s steering of our nerves and
psyches like the God sending orders by means of rays of light to Daniel Paul
Schreber (the [in]famous case of paranoia “treated” by the psychiatrist Paul
Flechsig and theorized by Sigmund Freud, and later Gilles Deleuze and Félix
Guattari). But on the other side, trance gestures towards ruptures, erratic
tics and fits within the dominant order of things.
Foreword 11
We might get a better grasp of this ambiguity by taking a brief look at the
Hauka movement, which began among the Songhay people in Niger in 1925
and was popularized for the Western audience in Jean Rouch’s ethnographic
film The Mad Masters (Les Maîtres fous, 1955). Rouch’s cinematic works play
a key role in Cinema, Trance and Cybernetics, as they, according to Holl,
tinker “with the technology until new imaginary spaces start to emerge,
in which mental states are realized that are non-integrated.” The Hauka is
a famous example of such “non-integrated” as well as ambivalent states.
The participants would dance and become possessed by the spirits of their
European colonizers – military officers, administrators, governors, and so
on. The proceedings would be physically intense, as Paul Stoller describes:
Notes
of cinematic experience which, as I see it, turns out to explain even more
of the ambivalences of communication and control in social and personal
media today than it had done for the critique of the old mass medium
which was still the dominant type of viewing film’s when the book was
written. Rereading my study I was astonished to find that in the light of
the latest developments of technical devices its assumption that technical
media carry the threat of social trance and oblivion, while also providing
perspectives of cultural transgression and the transformation of alterities
proved sustainable. Or rather, it seems much clearer today than when
Hollywood’s model of the cinema was considered classical, its apparatus
homogeneous and its impact compulsory. In today’s cinematic devices
from personal pads to gigantic screens on urban architecture, the issue
of feed-back, of senso-motoric integration or disintegration of bodies and
screens – or cameras for that matter – is obvious to every user. The history
of the cinema as an interface is a central thread in the book.
Venturing back into the psycho-physiological experiments of the 19th
century, I set out to discover a common history of technique, laboratory
studies of behaviour and desire. In this sense, the book seemed risky in a
methodological perspective. In a fortuitous encounter of luck and serendip-
ity I found these fields connected in the work of experimental filmmaker
Maya Deren. Following her biographical cues, I traced the genealogy of
cinematic experiments into the psycho-physiological laboratories of the
19th century and discovered close bonds between scientific experiments
of psychologists and scientists on the one hand and the studies of trance,
possession and altered states on the other. At their interface, major and
resilient filmic topoi emerged, the mad scientist as well as the hysteric
clairvoyant. Cinema’s history in the field between avant-garde film, 19th
century experimental psychology and anthropology had been discussed
before. But a study on trance and feed-back in cinematic perception also
shows that not only plots and content derive from cinema’s prehistory, but
also their technical adaptations to the minds of the 20th and now, differently,
to the minds of the 21st century.
Maya Deren’s work does not only link the fields of psychology, anthropol-
ogy and experimental cultures, she was also acquainted with a concealed
network of counter-psychological thought. Through her personal relations
to Soviet Psychology I discovered the strange networks of knowledge
established by Vladimir Bekhterev, who turned out to link experimental
practices connected to names like Paul Flechsig, Jean-Martin Charcot,
Dziga Vertov, Walter Cannon and finally Jean Rouch and Gregory Bateson.
Cybernetics as historically established feed-back relations or interfaces
Preface to the English Tr ansl ation of Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics 15
between the nerves, the senses and the minds on the one hand and technical
devices on the other – a context concisely called cinema-eye, kino glaz by
Vertov –, relations that at the same time escape the conscious perception
of users seems to be a very old project, which is taking possession of newer
cultures of seeing and listening only now, in the presence of personal and
personalized screens.
Maya Deren’s invitation to follow the Voudoun divinity Legba into the
darkness and towards the cross-roads of cultures and ecstatic practices also
led my studies. The path of research has thus lead from cinema’s darkness
into the flicker-lit set-ups of laboratories and to utopian and dystopian
models of technical devices connecting instruments and physical bodies. To
day, the darkness of the movie houses is only a memory of a trance technique
which is about to disappear. When writing the book, gramophone, film
and typewriter had just been replaced by Walkman, digital video formats
and personal computers. The culture of I-pods, smart phones and pads as
well as of streamed data which form into personal audio-visual viewing
dispositives was just on the verge of breakthrough. Once they reached their
subjects, they increasingly reduced them to consumers. Deren’s call for ap-
propriating technical skills and devices in the service of revolutionary and
ritual cultural forms seems to be right on time now. Gregory Bateson had
hoped that the notion of gods meant nothing far out but the implementation
of ecological thought against the competition of capitalist society. I kept
returning to this hope in the presence of ever more drastic exploitation of
people and continents. What sort of hybrids we have become under the rule
of the new and colder race of gods – as William James had predicted – will
have to be inferred as extension of the book at hand.
As opposed to the studies of the Canadian School of media theory,
contemporaries of Maya Deren she had not really taken notice of, Deren’s
thinking was concerned with syncretistic and unknown cultures rather
than with the decline of a Western world of literacy. And compared to
the Canadian School her work was inspired by feminist attitudes. Film
studies, and specifically in Germany, insisted that cinema is a form of
thinking liberated from the obligation of assuming philosophy’s single and
disembodied spirit – Geist – in favour of the multiplicity of heterogeneous
forms combining different views, gazes, bodies and minds. In this feminist
culture of film studies to which my research owes its foundations, Deren’s
work provided crucial forms of knowledge. This farewell to the formation
of a singular form of mind will also have to be reconsidered in the presence
of cinemas on multiple screens.
16 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
La Cybernétique n’est pas une Super-Science, le cybernéticien n’est pas un Super-Savant: ce sera
bientôt un spécialiste comme les autres, installé en un carrefour, mais non pas pour faire la loi. –
G.Th. Guilbaud 1957. In: What is Cybernetics?, London 1959. p. 28.
In the Beginning
If film were less of a plutocratic production form, nerves and film tech-
nology could enter into reciprocal communication, and what Vladimir
Mikhailovich Bekhterev maintained for nervous activity in general could
also be applied to the cinema: that not only does the human being have
to adapt psycho-physically, but that there is “also a modification of the
external conditions, that is, an ancillary adaptation of external conditions
to internal conditions.”1
With the technological unconscious, the condition of which is record-
ing and projecting discrete single images on film, the gaze unconsciously
moves in the pictures and the shots of the photographic surface. This is
the beginning of film criticism. Siegfried Kracauer saw this photographic
quality of images, which depict the external world in its unposed, random,
fragmentary reality, as the very essence of cinema.2 Hugo Münsterberg
pointed out that this surface is not only exhibited in its melancholy, absti-
nent visual clarity, as Kracauer had envisioned for ideal photography, but
that the photographic production of the film image also indicates a material,
meaningless, but functional side, which in turn spurs on a dramaturgy and
a way to draw the attention beneath the threshold of conscious perception:
“The shading of the lights, the patches of dark shadows, the vagueness of
some parts, the sharp outlines of others, the quietness of some parts of
the picture as against the vehement movement of others all play on the
keyboard of our mind… ”3
All three basic functions of film technology, camerawork, editing, and pro-
jection, can thus also be seen as psycho-physical technologies, as consciously
treating perception and reality at the same time, but which entirely evade
conscious perception, “a conscious manipulation designed to create effect”,
as Maya Deren put it.4 The basis of all film technology is cutting up, recording,
and projecting single images. And this is the technological consequence from
the old chronometry in physiology and psychology. After a unified time-frame
was incorporated as the basis of all experiments in the laboratories of the nine-
teenth century, this led in the twentieth century to an interest in expanding,
compressing, or accelerating this timeframe, or even in letting it run backwards
and in loops, leading the spectator astray. But all the illusionary techniques
of the cinema, which is also “the truth 24 times per second”, have to relate to
the spatio-temporal parameters of basic neurophysiological research. The
exceptions, construing reality in a different way and recording movement in
time differently, may not be called cinema and cannot technically be screened
in cinemas: such as focal-plane shutters which do not cut up the flow of time,
but recording flowing colors and forms on a running track and representing
the intensities of the world as rubber mat distortion.
26 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
analysis. The means to get there, however, are set up beneath the threshold
of consciousness and have no actual intention to rise above it.
The avant-garde films from the Soviet Union, called “Russian films” in the
twenties, dialectically produced a conscious clash of different images, from
which some third thing was meant to emerge and which was conceived as
a substitute for thinking. Of course Eisenstein, Dovzhenko, and Pudovkin
realized that film had to avoid conscious perception in order to be film,
but they nonetheless claimed that their montage was a method by which
relations could become deliberate in their dialectic, since they provided their
own expression. The cinema was meant to be a mirror in which ideological,
false, bourgeois thinking encountered and corrected itself as other. In this
respect, even the optical metaphysics of Tarkovsky or Kieślowski belong to
this tradition. In the term dialectic images, as Walter Benjamin coined it, this
concept even applies beyond any film theories as a historico-philosophical
attempt to prop up the hegemonic intellectual movement on the foundation
of its technological pre-conditions. Thinking could thus not only be moved
from an initially external principle, but also sedated or exploded.7 The actual
optical unconscious in cinema, however, the deception and intoxication of
perception as principle, which Vertov had his sights on, was also suspect
in Soviet cinema, for most directors as well as for the state agencies that
scrutinized and censored every film project in writing.
The difference between the Russian and American techniques, between
montage and editing, are not entirely subsumed in the opposition between
the conscious and unconscious manipulation of perception. In both East
and West, rules were provided that were meant to teach correct montage,
and all these rules served the goal of not tearing the spectator out of his
or her film trance. Whether it was supposed to be teaching an “I SEE”, the
“US”, or dialectical thinking in this trance was the subordinate problem for
those working at the editing table on both sides of the Curtain. It is certainly
true that over the course of time those techniques that are conspicuous as
changes in the field of vision – and that interrupt the trance – disappeared
from the repertoires of the editors: multi-screen, split-screen and different
dimensions for the screen, such as Eisenstein had wanted,8 iris wipes, but
also time lapse, black frames, and all the operations that the spectator
is aware of as intrusions into perception. By contrast, technologies that
simulate and stimulate involuntary activity in the brain, as Münsterberg
described, for instance light slow motion, lighting effects, certain estrange-
ment effects through graininess or layering, split focus shots, the use of
different focal lengths, etc., belong to the standard repertoire of tricks for
all camera operators and editors.
Cinema 29
The only thing which decided the order of shots was the desire to achieve
the most satisfactory results. […] The medium was extremely flexible in
that there was no physical reason why one should not cut from practically
anything to anything else.9
Continuity and connection, heavily loaded terms anyway, to this day have
to serve in editing technique for something that is constantly changing. If
the first spectators were shocked by close-ups being edited in, because they
thought they were looking at limbs that had been cut off, by the end of the
twenties discontinuous scenes, edited like jazz music, in which musicians
and instruments elided into one another – like in Murnau’s Sunrise – were
no longer disturbing, but were the necessary variety that promised to be
entertaining. At the beginning of the thirties people were astounded by
sync sound, and the graphic dance montages of Berkeley, in which bodies
were arranged serially according to their limbs, were already a part of mass
entertainment. Changing montage rhythms made Jules Dassin’s Naked City
in 1948 the forerunner of films that relied less on story than on the rhythmic
montage of urban landscapes and movements. In 1960 Hitchcock acceler-
ated the performance of reception with the 70 cuts of the shower curtain in
30 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
Psycho, setting new standards for the stimulation that was expected in mov-
ies. The West was discovering the montage techniques from the twenties in
the East, material time-space montage, which had disappeared there due to
the demands of Stalinism. The “visible cut”, montage that was introduced
against the cultural imperialism of Hollywood, could only elucidate until it
itself became invisible from habit. Who was still disturbed by the “wrong”
cuts in Breathless, who even noticed them anymore? Even Lars von Trier’s
cubist montage in The Kingdom, of shots that are incompatible according to
traditional spaces in time, and which showed that even at the beginning of
the nineties, in the age of MTV, there were still conventions that could still
be shockingly transgressed, has already become standard in commercial
film production. And also a standard of what we have been trained to
expect – and demand – in terms of speed in the cinematic experience. The
only thing that is still disturbing is anything that does not make use of the
acceleration of stimulation: Straub/Huillet and all those whose films we no
longer see in the cinemas for just that reason.
Even montage that tried to be conscious as an operation remains unno-
ticed as a technique. Often the effect on the spectator from the screen is no
longer a feeling of surprise, without her immediately becoming aware of the
breach in the conventions of perception as a breach against technological
conventions. This montage shows that genres are combinations of techni-
cal rules, which produce certain combinations of feelings. If these genres
are mixed, they therefore also produce new, literally artificial mixtures
of feelings in the cinema, which have caused people to be surprised at
themselves, but also disturbed, and which have not always immediately
been so well received.
Speaking about Une Femme est une Femme, Godard says:
Les comédies sont jamais filmées en gros plan, elles sont toujours filmées
en plan général. Et alors là, quand elles sont filmées en gros plan, elles
deviennent pathéthiques. Alors, il exprime des sentiments dans une
situation comique, c’est le beau dans le film. Mais, pour ça, le film n’a
pas marché.11
Genre means that space and time in film unconsciously guide the “mood”
in the sense of the old psychology of Wilhelm Wundt.
The attempt to describe the cinema as the extension of a dispositif that
usurped human minds and psyches as movement-chronograph and at the
same time as a rhythm machine is supposed to replace the dichotomy
between the conscious and the unconscious manipulation of perception.
Cinema 31
At the beginning was the question of the particular reality of the cinema and
the question of the subject and its transformation through the links between
perception and technology in the cinema. At the end a variety of networks
have been extended in which historical cinematic perception can be seen
as psycho-physical training and the as implementing social technologies
by using devices. The history of cybernetics as the science of multifaceted,
regulating, balancing, and communicative processes can be used – and not
only because it historically crisscrosses the history of cinema – to theorize
communication as automatic and automating, a communication that, in
the cinema, cannot be understood as the experience of the senses alone.
This may come back to haunt us.
The cybernetic process in the cinema would then be a matter of changing
perception in the cinema and regulating this perception through the effects
of film, even before the meanings of these effects are even formed. The
cinema is thus a good place to examine cybernetic processes, since the links
between nervous systems and apparatuses have constantly been synchro-
nized, aligned, and optimized in its history. This means that a feedback
process had already emerged in the research, before any film screening, as
a gradual refinement of the trance in the act of seeing movement.
The cinema is also a good object for examining cybernetic processes
since the work of every filmmaker consists in using time manipulation
and other cinematic techniques to re-apply a recorded series of events
back to that series of events itself, and through such operations to bring the
future of the messages into the imagination of the dreaming spectator. The
spectator’s trance attests to loops of self-adaptation where the perception
in the cinema is located.
On the other hand, the cinema is a highly inappropriate object for a
cybernetic hypothesis because it neither has any clear signal, nor is it one.
In order for cinema as cinema to become a signal in statistical mechanics,
and thus for it to be predictable, it must also be reduced to the flickering
and fluttering of light and darkness, such as occurred in laboratories and
34 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
was researched in the history of science and its artificial regulation was
improved upon – one highpoint of this development was the symposium
“Feelings and Emotions” in Ohio, at which this research, mediated by
Bekhterev and Cannon, who could not be mediated there, reached back
into the early history of cybernetics.
On the other hand, the movement of the body was itself examined in just
the same tradition. Using instantaneous photography and cinematography,
human movements and expressions were depicted so that they could be
further processed, treated, and most likely would have been endlessly
projected on one another in the primal scene of Salpêtrière if the female
and male hysterics had not finally refused to play along. Medical diagnoses
as well as artistic depictions of certain states were the result of the recording
technologies that transform time and that could thus dissect the individual
in order to subject this depiction, and thus the subject itself, to new and
immemorial causal relations.
On the one hand perception of the human body was made alien to the
human mind with cinematography, on the other hand human vision was
itself transformed by cinematography. Two sides of a process that made it
impossible to distinguish any more between, for instance, the representa-
tion of cinematically depicted bodies on the screen on the one hand and
the cinematic perception of these bodies in the space of the move theater
on the other. Even without producing clear emotional attributions, the
representation of a body in slow motion shows both certain unforeseen
qualities of the body depicted and certain unforeseen possibilities for the
spectator’s perception. The bon mot that a cinematic image is created by
fusing retina and screen – in the cinema, in the head, or in the body – now
had to be extended.
Bodily movements and emotions fuse like dancers and the dance in
or through the image. At any rate, this takes place somewhere that is not
supposed to exist according to the instructions given to the projection.
They fuse with technical effects, with other, now divided individuals, or
with themselves as others.
It is astonishing that, in a system that complies to a certain degree out of
paranoia, differences can still be shown at the structural level. In neurology,
in cybernetics, and in the cinema there are quite different basic models
of the mind and the bio-socius, or rather, of how cinematic technology is
applied: a hierarchical model of self-control on the one hand, and operative,
reversible, feedbacking connections on the other, in which an exchange
of experiences could be organized and at the same time technologically
put together and newly put to use. A vision that constructs a genealogy of
38 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
In the beginning is the darkness of the projection. Early film theorists, for
instance Münsterberg, Mauerhofer, Kracauer, or Arnheim,1 analyzed the
strange state that spectators indulge in as part of film perception. Since
the shutter strobes the projected beam in an established rhythm while the
Geneva drive intermittently transmits individual frames, moviegoers are
sitting in darkness for nearly half of the projection time, while their optical
nerves are stimulated to the beat of these mechanics. Hugo Münsterberg
was the first to draw the parallels between film perception and experiments
of isolated acts of perception in psychological laboratories. Hugo Mauer-
hofer, a psychologist, biographer of Hesse, and emigré in exile in Britain,
analyzed the transformation in psychic reception behavior of moviegoers
in four phases, diagnosing a state similar to that of daydreaming. Accord-
ing to Mauerhofer, the only proper object of scientific film theory is the
psyche itself, since every film critique, due to unconscious perception in the
cinema, is nothing more than a more or less inept report about individual
fantasies.2
Films are not simply seen. They transform the subject in the cinema. The
consciousness that, according to Kracauer, withdraws from the scene in the
cinema3 itself appears to the film critic as one that is under the influence of
a technologically evoked lull. Under the spell of early German experimental
psychology, represented in the Major Film Theories by the persons of Hugo
Münsterberg and Rudolf Arnheim, students of Wundt and Wertheimer
respectively, 4 examining the technologies that manipulate perception
made up a large part of American film theory. References to historical
trance techniques came from French film theory. Raymond Bellour was
the first to systematically equate film perception with hypnosis.5 But as a
relation of domination, that is, as gaze, seeing can only be classified and
criticized once the physiological conditions of its movement are discovered
as technologies, technologies that establish social orders while themselves
remaining invisible. Films are not simply seen, they allow for seeing.
Seeing or being seen: blind spots and blackouts from the very begin-
ning. Joseph Plateau, who carried out the first experiments on strobo-
scopic seeing, long before there even was film or cinema, went blind after
experimenting on himself to study retinal afterimages. The experiment
was not differentiated enough: his phenakistiscope, literally eye-deceiver,
had actually already showed him that it was not positive afterimages, but
successive, albeit discrete single images that were the necessary condition
40 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
The historical answer to Arnheim’s question first comes out of the labora-
tories: since cinematography was invented to analyze movement by taking
apart a temporal continuum by means of a variety of procedures, every
new development of the apparatuses was initially done in this tradition:
practical human decisions. It first became uncanny in the history of science
when the models of human perceptual psychology evidenced similarities
42 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
This made the artificiality of viewing cinema even more complex. The
functions of the apparatuses assume an alliance with the functions of the
nervous system. But the spectator needn’t know anything about this to see
motion in the cinema.
Cinema from the viewpoints of psycho-technology, as Münsterberg had
named his applied psychology, had to be examined precisely as a social
technology when its tricks were unconscious, such as in the transformation
of single images into a flow of moving images. Or the perception of motion
independent from the perception of a form: Wertheimer’s phi phenomenon.
Already in 1913 experiments in which white stripes were edited in between
film images showed that, despite the frequency of 24 images per second, as
is common in film projection, no flow of motion emerged as a cinematic
illusion, since the light impulse of the light fields suppressed the perception
of the previous images.15 The darkness in the cinema is initially neither the
metaphorical re-staging of Plato’s Cave,16 nor a mere refuge for lovers without
a room. The darkness in the cinema is above all a perception-physiological
necessity for viewing films. And for just that reason, as Benn describes it, it is
intoxicating. Just as Kracauer had suspected, darkness dismisses conscious-
ness from film perception.17 A phase of nerve stimuli slips in between film
projection and reception that only a visitor from the Gutenberg Galaxy like
Walter Benjamin could affably call it “distraction.”18 Since control by the
apperceiving consciousness is systematically undermined in cinema by the
technical equipment, it would be more precise to call this distraction trance.
Trance, as a dissolution or diversion of the consciousness under the
impact of certain technologies, is the gap in film theory. Here physiology
enters the humanities, challenging the idea of the subject to its very
Knots 43
of reality. Cultural technologies are mediations of the law, but they only
work when they can promise the subject – fragile, divided, powerless – a
new integrity for its submission. When it experiences itself in the joy of the
dance and the trance, it doesn’t notice anything of cultural technologies;
when it notices the cultural technologies, it experiences itself as other and
falls apart. In between the two is where female scholars and machinists
experiment.
The fact that the machinists’ effect is at the same time affect, the conse-
quence and the pursuit of their artistic methods, is one of the oldest insights
of experimental culture. In his text on the Marionette Theater, Heinrich
von Kleist described the relationship between cultural technologies and
souls as being dance-like, at any rate not as directly mechanical, but as a
relationship between various transformations: “Somewhat artificial” is
the relationship between puppeteers and the dancing puppets, says the
leading dancer of the opera, explaining this as an engineer: like that of
numbers to their logarithms. The path taken by emphasizing the puppet
in the intermedial relationship between machine, puppeteer and puppet is
not only the effect of the technical construction, but of the whole spiritual
complication, of psycho-physical dispositivs avant la letter, in which the
functions of perception, of consciousness, and of the soul appear as a
parable, the ends of which – author, narrator, subject – disappear in the
endlessness of the function “making dance.” A secretive line is the trace
of this interference:
It is nothing other than the path to the soul of the dancer, and Herr C.
doubted that it could be proven otherwise that through this line the
puppeteer placed himself in the center of gravity of the marionette; that
is to say, in other words, that the puppeteer danced.22
Ego and consciousness pursue one another hyperbolically in the tracks and
loops, the meshes and circles of medial constructions, and so the trance
can merge with the knowledge of how it came to be.
The film avant-gardists of the twentieth century let themselves become
fascinated by archaic trance techniques and transgressions time and
time again. African, Pacific, Caribbean cults became visible for the first
time outside their ritual spaces on 16mm film. The information that this
“visibilité” could deliver into the heart of the colonial powers thus suddenly
appeared as the dark collaboration of filming ethnologists. In the trance
films of Jean Rouch and Maya Deren techniques become visible that seek
to surrender knowledge not to power, but to powerlessness. Such trance
Knots 45
films experiment with feedback that no longer allows for any distinction
between puppeteer and puppet, between dance and dancer: cinematically
initiated entropy. “Going native” as fading out the messages of indigenous
informants – through feedback, through joining the ritual and ruthless
integrity of the technical medium. But the subversive usage of technology
does not automatically guarantee this entropy. The relationship between
the decomposing strategies of the avant-gardes and the mapping adminis-
trations is shockingly parasitical.
The American experimental films of the 1940s examine the emotional
effects of cinematic processes. But precisely the 16mm technology to
which they owe their art had only been developed and ref ined because
of the Second World War. Not only were 16mm film cameras and material
easier to get after being discarded by the Off ice of War Information,
new f ilm forms were being invented in the f ield at lightening speed.
Almost all of the big Hollywood directors had worked making newsreels
in the army. John Ford, for instance, was shooting The Battle of Midway
when an explosion ripped the f ilmstrip from the sprockets, producing
a skewed exposure of the material. This new form of newsreel realism
was legitimated in that it made the technical device itself visible. What
experimental f ilm theorists called for years later as a strategy against
Hollywood fiction had already been realized by chance in the field under
f ire. 23 The intoxication is the effect, was the message of the medium.
Effects of apparatuses and not of meaning stood at the beginning of
all experiments in cinematic space. In 1964 Marshall McLuhan would
explain this as a characteristic of the electronic age: “Concern with ef-
fect rather than with meaning is a basic change of our electric time, for
effect involved the total situation and not a single level of information
movement.”24
So if the effects of the electronic age place us in the synthetic totality of
a dance, we must first stumble into the process in order to be able to get
any insight into the foundations and chasms of subjectivity. A disturbing
experience. Subject and apparatus are reciprocal perturbations in the
sense of neurobiology. Changes in the structure of one’s own system,
which is not caused, but is provoked by another system or the surround-
ings – cinema is not always and everywhere, but wherever the work of
filmmakers encounter the functions of the apparatus and the perception
of the spectator, unsettling one another in the process. According to the
findings of neurobiology, the human being should be seen as a being that
not only operates a complicated communication system directed outward,
but also directed inward, proprioception. 25 The body moves, and from
46 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
If it goes well, what gets developed and enhanced in the cinema is our
own thinking in the rhythm of images and films. But it is also clear that
this kind of message transmission does not stop at the limits of the body
or the sense organs, and that the quality of transmission can be improved
or inhibited by various external circumstances. Designating it as psychic
only means reducing long processes and complex relationships to a single
switchpoint. The machinists in art have a more wide-ranging approach.
They also attempt to switch themselves in to the self-guidance of inner
messaging systems in order to mobilize proprioceptors and effectors,
curves and arabesques, emotions and feelings, thus disturbing an inner
system so that its vibrations react to this in their own way. Examining the
cinema cybernetically therefore does not simply mean writing the history
of controlling and steering sense perception and unconscious structures of
watching under the conditions of cinematography. Rather, cinema should
be pursued in the sense of the perturbation of Walter Benjamin’s wish:
“The most important social function of film is to establish equilibrium
between human beings and the apparatus.”28 There are describable and
historical relationships and interactions between the technology of cinema
and human perceptual functions. But this is not simply as rhizomatic as
Deleuze and Guattari wanted to claim when they wrote that the mari-
onettes strings are attached “to a multiplicity of nerve fibers.”29 Strings
and knots do not simply grow like mushrooms. A proper knot is a science
of its own.
4. To Whom it May Concern
The sciences are novels about heroes such as Hegel, Freud, Lacan.
The authors are the titles.
– Hubert Fichte, 1980
“We have decided to call the entire field of control and communication
theory, whether in the machine or in the animal, by the name Cybernetics,
which we form from the Greek κυβερνήτης or steersman”, wrote Norbert
Wiener when he was reporting on the creation of this epistemological
program in 1947.1 At the time it was assumed in the military that form-
ing a theory of communication would need precise neurological and
mathematical research, which was supposed to be able to prognosticate
reactions and future developments in a system. During the Second World
War Wiener had not only worked on the project of an electric calculator
as a “form of communication apparatus concerned more with messages
than with power.” Kept awake with massive doses of Benzedrine, which,
as he reported it, caused him to tremble, since he was afraid of blabbering
about war secrets, he had calculated the predictability of the trajectories
of fighter planes. While at first the human element, the seemingly incal-
culable reactions and emotions of pilots and shooters, was supposed to
be excluded from technological warfare, later the human being and the
machine were merged into a joint venture of medicine and electronics.
Wiener, along with Arturo Rosenblueth, had shown that nerves and
electronic machines were compatible, and if they were wired together
they could cause messages to be transmitted.2 The goal of navigation,
however, was not sure. Guilbaud, a thoughtful historian of cybernetics,
added:
…les machines supérieures, les plus evoluées, les plus récentes, celles
qui jouissent de la remarquable propriété d’adapter leur fonctionne-
ment aux variations du monde extérieur, les machines ‘réflexes’ ont leurs
constructeurs mais non leurs architectes.3
Society has a memory of its own, far more durable and far more varied
than the memory of any individual belonging to it. In those societies
which are fortunate enough to possess a good script, a large part of this
communal tradition is in the writing, but there are societies which, with-
out writing, have preserved a whole tradition in the form of a technique
of ritual memorization of tribal chants and histories. 4
original, and authentic in the rituals of the colonies and the bodies of
the colonized, the involuntary intoxication, was precisely the object of
the avant-garde research into their own cultures. Western science and
medicine mapped the body anew according to a logic of contact noise and
of intoxication, and bodies were wired up to new apparatuses, creating
new entities.
In the fall of 1947 Artaud declared war on the organs. Deleuze and Guat-
tari, who continued working on this reorganization, invoke Gregory Bateson
when they use the term “plateau for continuous regions of intensity.”7 For
Artaud, organs create the connection between divine will and the capitalist
abuse of bodies.8 At the end of 1947 many young French people also flee
from this abuse. Alfred Métraux and Michel Leiris also travel to Haiti, with
the desire “to take one’s revenge on a life with which one was not satisfied.”9
(Traveling the other way around, André Breton had been stationed in Haiti
on his way back to Paris in 1945, had been feted by the revolutionary youth,
and was expelled after the fall of the country’s government.) The young
engineer Jean Rouch goes back to Africa, where he had already studied and
filmed rituals of possession during the war. The experimental filmmaker
Maya Deren, when she travels to Haiti in 1947, senses the flip side her own
culture on her own body. Her film study of minoritarian cultures ends for
her part in becoming-minoritarian, albeit minoritarian like a goddess.
Madonna-minority.
All of them described their travels as crises provoked in their own identity,
as desired transformation. Heiner Müller, who designated people like John
Cage and himself as the “revenge of dead Indians”, much later pointed out
the difficulties of producing art looming in the power imbalance between
cultures and subcultures. The technologies of production have already
attached themselves to the body before the artist even chooses his weapon:
“I didn’t know then, but already foresaw that one cannot remain an Indian
if one wants to do something with art. We all shoot from the hip, and in
art doing something means doing away with something, beginning with
oneself.”10 This goes for everyone, researchers or artists, who work with
technological images.
In a sciences’ history of the cinema, which examines the preliminary
neurological works on cinematic perception, parallels to pre-cybernetic
research and researchers quickly appears of its own accord. The artistic
induction of feelings and emotions that takes place in the cinema and the
technical motion of gazes are cybernetic regulations of an apparatus that
links living beings with machines. The name of this apparatus is cinema
in the broadest sense, from the moment in which it is technologically
50 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
proposed a joint film project in Haiti, but also Norbert Wiener himself, who
asked the research couple Mead-Bateson to comment on the sociological
uses of cybernetics models during the Macy conferences, drew connections
between the question of social technologies, which was so virulent in the
1940s, and a theory of communication.
Against this historical backdrop, Maya Deren proposes the practice of
a technology of depiction that is simultaneously an artistic and a social
process. Film can become ritual technology if the rules of transformation
are set.
In the second chapter, “Depersonalizations”, possession is placed in
a diachronic context. Deren’s comparison of hysteria and possession in
Haitian voodoo is placed into relation with her film theory and her practi-
cal film transformations of the coordinates of space, time, and perception.
The precondition of all of her art is that Deren is familiar with the essence
of technical procedures. For all her discretion, what she nonetheless forgot
is: Knowledge about the rules of transformation do not necessarily protect
the subject from being infected itself, in other words, from becoming
possessed.
One of Deren’s more fortunate doppelgängers is Jean Rouch, an engineer
in street and bridge building, who developed the method of ciné-trance in
Africa, the technological correspondence between film and possession.
Using the example of his film Les maîtres fous, “the mad masters”, about
the cult of the Haouka in Accra, I will present Rouch’s reflections of “film
feedback as anthropological return gift”, and his practice of using film
footage as an art of transformation: “Deviations.”
It is not by chance that the threads of research about possession and
suggestions coincide at a place that also forms one of the primal scenes
of f ilm: Salpêtrière, where Albert Londe developed new cameras with
which doctors like Charcot could bring some order into the confusion of
hysterical gestures. With these cameras, they could then present themselves
as the masters of madness. Not only Sigmund Freud, but also Vladimir
Mikhailovich Bekhterev was a spectator at these stagings, the mastery of
which was based on a technology: “Compressions.”
The second part of the book looks into the prerequisites for all these
cultural technologies of trance in the history of sciences. The cinema
appears there as part of the history of psychology and its experiments,
localizing the soul in the nervous system and establishing the connec-
tion between human beings and machines, as it also defined art for the
electronic age: the birth of cinema from the laboratories of the neuro-
physiologists. The apparatus of the cinema appears in the line of medical
52 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
Maya Deren, Glas. Photo: Alexander Hammid. Mugar Library, Boston, NY.
Part II
1. Discretions
dancers and young women in trance were expanded into long undula-
tions on the screen. At the same time Margaret Mead’s commenting voice
dropped an octave, as if she were tying to prove the thesis that all cultural
coding is also always a transformation, carried out on her own body and
using the example of the most significant ethnological binary. The “grande
dame” of American anthropology, who had written her most important
books about the cultural differentiation of gender, sounded like a man.
What was thus shown in the “acoustic mirror”3 was a distorted acoustic
image of Margaret Mead, unclear in the cultural context but nonetheless
identifiable. A real fake.
Karl Heider justified his harsh manipulation of the playback speed in
the name of texts that Mead and Bateson themselves had written about
the conspicuous calm and even, slow rhythm of Balinese culture. Rituals,
ceremonies, and educational methods on Bali, as the two anthropologists
had shown, were all aimed at interrupting emotionally cumulative, that
is, increasingly sensational processes in social relations, and at regulat-
ing emotional states in terms of uniform stability and non-competitive
behavior.
What Heider provided for his students to see, manipulated in this way,
in fact matched the normal speed of Balinese dancers and rituals, since
Bateson, in order to economize on the expensive film material, had shot
certain parts of his footage at 16 frames per second. Margaret Mead had
simply edited these in between the remaining material later. In a regular
projection, the presumably calm dancers hysterically floundered around
on the screen during these sequences. The regularity of the film projector
could only present either the cultural integrity of the anthropologist or that
of the Balinese, and when Heider pulled back the tempo of the mechanism,
thus violating the correct cultural identity of the anthropologist, he put
the film apparatus at the service of Balinese culture. Its transformation in
the cinema occurred consciously and for pedagogical reasons. As a good
reader of Mead, the speed was significant for him, and not just the schema
of the course of movement: “Mead’s voice drops an octave or so but it is still
understandable, and the Balinese pace can be appreciated.”4 Now it was
exactly the goal of Mead and Bateson’s research trip to use new inscription
methods to transform themselves in the field of Balinese culture. Their goal
caught up with them in the end, but only belatedly, involuntarily, and with
Heider’s technological help.
Ethnographic films for the purposes of research had already existed in
the prehistory of cinema. In 1895 the doctor and anthropologist Félix-Louis
Regnault, together with the cameraman Charles Comte, prepared chrono
Discre tions 59
people, beyond all linguistic valuation, before the second step of analyz-
ing certain patterns and then discovering anthropological and cultural
typologies in the images.
The film material that Gregory Bateson and Margaret Mead brought
back from Indonesia at the beginning of the forties does in fact represent
a milestone in the history of anthropology. Since the two anthropologists
claimed to have discovered a model of non-aggressive social dynamics on
Bali and – anachronistic to all geopolitical escalation – a model of non-
competitive social behavior, the trip could very much have been understood
as a peace mission in dark times. The Balinese footage, however, caused
such a stir above all because it represented the basis for a future dynamic
anthropology. Already in the thirties, as part of their research in New
Guinea, Bateson and Mead had observed behavioral patterns among young
villagers on the Sepik river that they described as “schismogenetic”, meaning
a process of progressive cultural differentiation that was increased and
extended through symmetrical or complementary intersubjective feedback.
Examples of this are male and female behavior in everyday life in the play
between voyeurism and exhibitionism, in the back and forth of relationships
and gazes. Cultural identity, the two anthropologists could demonstrate,
could be understood as inter-relation, as interplay.
Since that time, anthropology no longer simply focuses on exploring an
unfamiliar society, but on discovering what recursive processes are used to
differentiate human behavior in a society into particular cultural or even
moral types. The goal of anthropological examinations was not simply
customs, rites, rituals, and relational forms, but the model that is visible
behind all variable behavior: the rules of transformation. The charm in the
heart of darkness.8
Due to his research in New Guinea, Bateson received a Guggenheim
grant to explore a theory of social transformation. The wording is as fol-
lows: “A formulation of a nucleus of theory relating to concepts of culture,
personality and character formation and the extension of this nucleus to
cover the phenomenon of cultural change.”9 Due to their research, Mead and
Bateson were invited, anthropologists among physicists, mathematicians,
and neurologists, to the exclusive Macy conferences to investigate “feedback
mechanisms and circular causal systems in biological and social systems”,
which launched both the term and the epistemology of cybernetics. Ques-
tions of how social relationships and social transformation are modeled and
regulated were at the core of this important American research program
for a (post-)war world that had got out of hand, the predictability of which
was supposed to be restored with the aid of electronic machines.
Discre tions 61
There had been too much emphasis that there were temperamental differ-
ences among children, so that you responded differently to a hyperactive
baby than you did to a quiet baby. But the extent to which there was a
system in which the mother was dependent on what the child had learned
as the stimulus for the next position wasn’t well articulated until we got
the cybernetics-conferences going.10
Cybernetics was therefore – much like navigating the sea, this master
pattern of cyberneticists and helmsmen – to be understood as a conse-
quence of situational assessments and the corresponding corrections for
readjustment. Observing this behavior would then also have to be kept to
a consecutiveness of actions in hierarchies of logical and temporal arrange-
ments that were only realized over the course of time.
Cinematography, with its temporal organization of events, thus initially
promised to provide the ideal recording instrument for ethnologists. Never-
theless, Bateson and Mead’s efforts in Bali to use film to raise the scientific
methods of anthropology to new heights of data processing11 initially proved
to be only an unexpected “quantum leap.”12 From their stay between 1936
and 1939, Mead and Bateson brought back 25,000 photographs and roughly
22,000 feet of 16mm film material, that is, more than 12 hours worth, which
they wanted to evaluate as the basis for their studies of trances and dances in
Balinese rituals. What was missing was the corresponding leap in methodol-
ogy. Only when viewing the material after their return to New York did it
become clear to the two researchers that the footage was only the necessary
preliminary work for a medial revolution in anthropological paradigms.
Editing the films, which required a structural organizational principle,
emerged as an unresolved problem. What was lacking in the editing room
was any convention that would have scanned the significant moments and
events in the uniform calm of Balinese images, given visibility to relations
62 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
and behavior, and thus suggested some sort of editing principle. In the
images of scenes from everyday life, rituals, and trance dances, which were
recorded in all kinds of lighting, from various perspectives, and with dif-
ferent speeds, one cannot initially spot any coherent units or elements that
might have organized the material. But this is precisely what would have
been necessary for any scientific systematization in structural anthropol-
ogy. At the beginning of his universal ethnological examination apparatus,
Claude Lévi-Strauss made the following remark: “In any field a system of
significances can be constructed only on the basis of discrete quantities.”13
Bateson and Mead had selected an analog recording medium right at
the point in time when the avant-garde of anthropologists were working on
formalizing reality in the direction of a strict binary. Claude Lévi-Strauss,
who was teaching at the New School for Social Research in Manhattan at
the beginning of the forties, where Bateson was also lecturing, developed
his methods of structural anthropology in analogy with linguistics, which,
as Lévi-Strauss observed with “some, let us say, melancholy, and a great
deal of envy”,14 precisely involved a technological collaboration with the
engineers of that new science called cybernetics. Just as linguists differenti-
ated phenomena in language, Lévi-Straus discovered “distinct entities”
and “pairs of oppositions” in ethnological analysis, for example of kinship
relations, which could be ascribed to certain ways of behaving. So, just as
Freud had called for deciphering the contents of dreams not according
to their value as image, but according to relations of signification, Lévi-
Strauss noted that “the error of traditional sociology, like that of traditional
linguistics, was to consider the terms, and not the relations between the
terms.”15 Lévi-Strauss formalized these relations to make them available in
an almost exemplary way for further extrapolation by the “great modern
electronic machines.”16 The relations that Lévi-Strauss had discovered as
cultural forms in societies could all be notated and systematized as “+” or
“-” so that “each culture is a unique situation”,17 while remaining part of a
general law. Following this system, one could write an algorithm for each
culture, which would illustrate its patterns, processes, communications,
and transformations – the dream of every computer administration.
Binary coding for Lévi-Strauss, as an anthropologically constant
fundamental law – and this is the surprising turn in his argument – is a
formalization due to the unconscious activity of the mind.
The activities of the mind that humans beings are unaware of is not only ap-
propriate for Turing’s universal machine, it can also realize and integrate all
cultures in world history with their institutions and customs as a Hegelian
universal machine.
Unlike Lévi-Strauss, Gregory Bateson, who never tired of emphasizing
the value of “loose thinking”19 in scientific processes, deliberately kept the
relationship between linguistic and ethnological order casual and porous
from the very beginning in order to avoid some as yet undiscovered uni-
versality of human forms of relationship turning out to be a self-fulfilling
prophecy of a European, Cartesian mind. He systematically jeopardized his
own intentionality when speaking and naming. In contrast to the elegant
and elementary systematics of structuralism, he deployed Anglo-Saxon
“trial and error”:
When I am faced with a vague concept and feel that the time is not
yet ripe to bring that concept into strict expression, I coin some loose
expression for referring to this concept and do not want to prejudge the
issue by giving the concept too meaningful a term… I can go on using the
vague concept in the valuable process of loose thinking – still continually
reminded that my thoughts are loose.20
Bateson thus casts out his ever-refining net of signifiers, which is meant
to catch unfamiliar wild thinking and acting, while he remains categori-
cally camouflaged like a hunter. Like the entrapped Narcissus, however, he
himself becomes the first victim of this strategy.
Initially, however, Bateson wanted to transfer this method to film in
order to counter the French universalism of the mind with a psychosomatic
ecology, which was to be viewed in Norbert Wiener’s sense as the circulation
of the whole ecosystem, of the “organism-plus-environment.”21 Correspond-
ingly, the researcher couple got down to the task in a holistic way, albeit
always maintaining a division of labor. Margaret Mead recorded the course
of events in written form, while Gregory Bateson photographed and filmed
at the same time – sometimes assisted by anthropologist Jane Belo. In
one photo book, which the New York Academy for Science published in a
special edition, almost 800 photos are combined into thematic tableaus
64 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
irrevocably brought symbolic conventions into the field of the Balinese and
even Balinese culture. What Bateson had ignored was that photography
and film, with their mechanical equipment, optical devices, and chemical
processes, were just as historically determined, and were just as foreign to
the Balinese as the typewriter had been. Bateson had ignored that he, with
Heiner Müller, was always already shooting from the hip.
The film material initially remained unedited. Instead, Bateson began
to organize all the open questions of social and cultural patterns in terms
of system theory at the New School for Social Research. In this context he
developed a graphic solution – halfway between writing and image – to the
problem of anthropological illustration. Typical Balinese behavior, which
consisted in systematically interrupting the “perhaps basically human
tendency towards cumulative personal interaction”29 and in it as duration
without escalation, becomes manifest, in its ordinate pattern of “cumula-
tive action” to the abscissa of time, in the form of a plateau. It is just this
plateau that would later come to prominence in the history of philosophy
with Deleuze and Guattari. Bateson was thus proposing one of the most
important lines of flight out of the limitations of western thought:
The trouble is that these paired adjectives are very inadequate descrip-
tions of human relationships, that in fact, to make a pair, the dominance
and the submission have to be of such special sorts that the submission
is the sort of submission which is appropriate to that particular sort
of dominance… Dominance may be linked with spectatorship, and
submission may be linked with exhibitionism, or the pair of polarities
may be reversed – dominance being linked with exhibitionism etc. The
important thing is that there shall be an ethological system of some
sort underlying the relationship between the two contrasting elements.
Beyond this is it probably necessary for the two elements to be talking
about the same thing – e.g. sex, or drawing a hopscotch line, or whatever.32
Last night the Bateson theory lecture and the mix up about Balinese
‘startle.’33 I suspect it doesn’t sit in there right, because it is a ‘symptom’ of
something which is an order, and it is not itself an order in the sense that
the ‘other feedbacks’ are orders. Anyway, that dominance-submission
business feels very wrong somehow but I don’t dare speak as strongly as
I should like to because I’d have not the right thing to offer instead. At
least if he would use arrows of dynamic movement (what the hell is the
name of them?) rather than make those directional signposts! Time Time
Time – not Space. Energy – not matter.34
68 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
set up and left in the same place, large batches of material can be collected
without the intervention of the film-maker or ethnographer and without
the continuous self-consciousness of those who are being observed.”38 In
misrecognizing the degree to which the usage of cultural recording tech-
nologies is conditioned and marked by tradition, she summarily threatens
those who put up resistance to their filming of her behavior with definitively
losing their history:
[…] the isolated group or emerging new nation that forbids filmmaking
for fear of disapproved emphases will lose far more than it gains.[…] they
will rob of their rightful heritage their descendants, who […] may wish
to claim once more the rhythms and handicrafts of their own people.39
Film archives or institutions in which the young and by now urbanized and
electrified indigenous population might once again take possession of its
legacy of ritual and handwork with the aid of anthropology have not pre-
vailed. In contrast, the subcultural use of electronic instruments in various
musical styles like the blues of the Delta, certain jazz music, or Hendrix’s pop
music might suggest an immediate and physical link to ritual techniques.40
Margaret Mead’s well-meaning colonialism, which also ignored the role of
technology, was avenged not by the descendants of the dead Indians, but,
as her Brown students heard, by the projecting institution itself.
Parallel to the ongoing debate with Gregory Bateson, Maya Deren wrote
an article about montage, Creative Cutting, in which she sees through the
anthropologists’ game with f ilm technology: “It is the phenomenon of
duration as tension which explains why slow motion – which may have
in it very little activity – often makes for greater tension than normal or
rapid motion for the tension consists in our desire to have our anticipation
satisfied.”41 It is this transformation of “duration” into “tension” that creates
emotionality in film. The intensity that appears in film as certain editing
sequences or in anthropological depictions of interactions that do not
provoke a differentiation between groups as a continual, sustained curve,
is created from elements that had previously been made discrete. If editing
technique remains in common conventions, it remains unnoticed as a
function of illustrative technology and thus unaware of the transformation.
If it appears, as it did at the beginning of film art, when there were barely
any film conventions, or later in the montage of cinéma verité, in Jean
Rouch, or in Jean-Luc Godard’s “wrong”, visible cuts, it troubles the field of
vision, thus instigating a disturbance to culture, its technologies of storing
and recording, and thus the vey conditions of subjectivity.
Discre tions 71
The immediate physical contact with the film, the nearness of the image,
the automatic muscular control of its speed – the fact that as I wound –
my impulses and reactions toward the film translated themselves into
muscular impulses and so to the film directly with no machine – buttons,
switches, etc. – between me and the film[…]. Later of course, I shall use
the projector to get proper speed, etc. But first this intimate copulation
between me and the film must take place… 43
While Bateson had just given up hope that film might be a medium to
depict circular-causal and feedback mechanisms in social systems, he had
created all the necessary conditions to put such mechanisms in gear. The
cinematic feedback that the anthropologists had presumably sought in
the field occurred in the cinema. Only during projection could and must
Margaret Mead and Gregory Bateson be confronted with their cinematically
72 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
construed Balinese selves, and only then could they see the transformation
of the stranger as their own. Heider’s students were plied and flattened by
the same transformation, by an endless, and in McLuhan’s sense, medial
extension, which is echoed by itself over and over again, and which can
be described as a baker’s transformation, to use Deleuze’s terminology. At
the same time this medial extension is further processed further into a
multifarious “mille feuille”. It is the processing of one’s own sensibility, of
one’s own trance, which would not be induced by Indonesian drums and
dancers, but by the small Trojan horse of a 16mm projector in the lecture
hall. Heider’s dauntless speed switching constantly kept this processing
from becoming the usual procession of institutional anthropology.
In their model of the thousand-fold plateau, Deleuze and Guattari merged
the double sense of the anthropology-order into a direction for philosophy
to move. With regard to the subject, they turned out to be film philosophers:
Starting from the forms one has, the subject one is, the organs one has,
or the functions one fulfills, becoming is to extract particles between
which one establishes the relations of movement and rest, speed and
slowness that are closest to what one is becoming, and through which one
becomes. This is the sense in which becoming is the process of desire. 44
Mead: […] he’s a good filmmaker, and Balinese can pose nicely, but his
effort was to hold the camera steady enough long enough to get a sequence
of behavior.
Bateson: To find out what’s happening.
Mead: When you’re jumping around taking pictures …
Bateson: Nobody’s talking about that, Margaret, for God’s sake.
Mead: Well.
Bateson: I’m talking about having control of a camera. You’re talking
about putting a dead camera on top of a bloody tripod. It sees nothing.50
Whether cameras can see, whether the control or steering of the gaze can
be placed or even concealed in the detail of the apparatus, whether human
being and machine develop symbiotic or parasitical relationships in the
matrix of cybernetic artificiality – in the 1940s these were not just questions
for anthropologists with movie cameras. In the Second World War, camera
and radar eyes on airplanes and machine guns could not only record and
transmit visual material, they could also calculate trajectories and guide
projectiles. But while the devices learned to behave more precisely, human
behavior under the conditions of war had turned out to be highly unreliable
and uncontrollable. People who were just supposed to shoot could suddenly
Discre tions 75
no longer trust their own senses and nerves, their muscles, and their self-
assurance, entrapped in irresolvable sensory-motor convolutions. They
started to tremble, to stutter, to stagger, and, at best, simply to collapse.
The mathematician Norbert Wiener, who was familiar with all these states,
tried to find a remedy to these break-downs while working for the US Air
Force. He discovered a new principle of integrated control: “This method
of control appeared to us not unlike a method already known in electric
circuits and now being applied in servomechanisms, or systems by which
we switch in an outside source of power for control purposes.[…] We call
this negative feedback.”51
It was this method of negative feedback that Bateson was looking for
when he sought to achieve control over the camera, which would see in
his place so that he could finally get the goal of his gaze in the viewer
– a constant feedback between technology and the gaze, which rather
precisely describes the difficult to achieve balance between self-control
and absent-mindedness that is necessary for all artistic production. Bate-
son became increasingly interested in his own cultural transformation,
which obviously included the world around him as an ecosystem. By this
time, ethnologists had been able to further deconstruct the image of the
stranger – if necessary, in a brutal and liberating way, such as Karl Heider
had done when he showed his students that sometimes all that is needed
to get the genie in the anthropological bottle to appear is a valiant grip
into the apparatus.
In a letter from December 20, 1967 to the neurophysiologist Warren
McCulloch, one of the pioneers of the mathematical calculation of neuronal
network processes and one of the founders of the Macy conferences, Bates
writes about the knowledge of the gods, which appears at the end of this
lifelong research report that meanders through the sciences:
I suggest that one of the things that man has done through the ages to
correct for his short-sighted purposiveness is to imagine personified enti-
ties with various sorts of supernatural powers, i.e., gods. These entities,
being fictitious persons, are more or less endowed with cybernetic and
circuit characteristics.52
she noted on March 16, 1947. Shortly thereafter, however, she encountered
the gods in Haiti, who appeared on the scene in such transformations, who
seized bodies and intervened in human relationships. This case makes
clear how such good gods form an alliance with the media of storage and
transmission.
2. Depersonalizations
Body Balance
While Gregory Bateson’s oblivious use of film technology was making him
the object of cinema-trance unwillingly, Maya Deren, a pioneer of American
experimental film, is going the opposite way: as a levelheaded machinist,
she first appropriates the technological functions of film, only to take
her leave as often as possible in a kind of cinema-sleep. “[…]you may find
me many evenings in the motion-picture theater, sharing with the other
sleepers […] the selected dream without responsibilities.”1 While the British
Bateson dragged visions of analytical symmetry to the USA modeled after
his great role model William Blake, Deren smuggled an unusual legacy of
Russian knowledge in among the American filmmakers. Her father had
studied reflexology in St. Petersburg with Bekhterev, and had worked at
his Psychoneurological Research Institute in the 1910s, a time when experi-
ments were being done on methods of group therapy, methods of collective
suggestions and mutual psycho-physical equilibration, methods that were
unknown in the USA. Only in the 1960s were similar forms of systematic
therapy developed there for schizophrenia patients: by a research group
surrounding Gregory Bateson.
Maya Deren will come close to circular-causal thinking, as she newly
discovered it in Bateson’s lectures at the New School for Social Research
in New York, when she begins systematically reflecting on the cinema.
Her most important text about cinema, Cinema as an Artform, in which
she produces the relativity of spatio-temporal perception and historical
technology, begins with the dedication: “To my father, who, when I was a
child, once spoke to me of life as an unstable equilibrium.”2 Her theory of
cinema examines just such an unstable equilibrium as a microstructure
of a web of relations, not only between subjects, but especially between
people and apparatuses.
Cinema, like dance, is about movements that produce an equilibrium of
moments which are unstable in themselves. This is why Deren constantly
78 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
Today the airplane and the radio have created, in fact, a relativistic
reality of time and space. They have introduced to our immediate real-
ity a dimension which functions not as an added spacial location but
which, being both temporal and spacial, relates to all other dimensions
with which we are familiar. There is not an object that does not require
relocation in terms of this new frame of reference, and not least among
these is the individual.3
For Deren, “relativistic” reality not only refers to the theory of a once
new scientif ic way of thinking, but also to complexity and ambiguity,
asynchronicity, blurriness, and imbalances in human perception, which
have become scientifically competent. With analogue media it was not
reality, but movements and relationship between realities that became the
object of research. For Deren, parameters like time and space are subject
to historical and technological developments – as is shown by the fact that
she insistently and repeatedly pointed out the simultaneous development
of cinema, radio, and rocket technologies since 1945. But cinema itself has
a part in this transformation of times and spaces. In the comprehensive
sense that Deren gives to it, cinema, by drawing technology and perception,
machines and human bodies, images and gazes together in its rhythm,
can itself create reality: “Cinema – and by this is understood the entire
body of techniques, including camera, lighting, acting, editing, etc. – is a
time-space art with a unique capacity for creating new temporal-spatial
relationships and projecting them with an incontrovertible impact of
reality[…].”4
Against the magic of film projection, the human head is powerless. Deren
examines the two operations of film production, technological storage and
the technological assembling of stored data into a new reality, in its effect
on subjectivity on both sides of the screen. “…filmmaking consists of two
distinct but interrelated processes: photography – by which actuality is
recorded and revealed […] in its own terms; and editing, by which those
elements of actuality proper may be re-related on an imaginative level to
create a new reality.”5 In photography every object portrays its own illustra-
tion onto light-sensitive surfaces or filmstrips, at any rate, as Deren repeats,
Depersonalizations 79
not before it has passed the analysis of recording technology: “the refined
optics of the lens, the slow-motion analysis of the movement, etc.”6
Only after being treated technologically does reality become visible, open
to experience and communicable, and thus reality is always a historical one.
From the very beginning Deren examines the quality of an image not for its
possible similarities with what is depicted, but for the effect of this image
on thinking and the imagination, for the medial effect as a mental effect:
“…the term ‘image’[…] presumes a mental activity.”7 The form of reality
that is photography marks its place value in a series of transmissions that
encounter and alter perception. This is why the photomechanical means of
illustrating, which can be relayed as the impression of light itself, constitutes
the form of reality that is every photograph:
The photograph not only testifies to the existence of that reality […] but
is, to all intents and purposes, its equivalent. This equivalence is not at
all a matter of fidelity but is of a different order altogether. If realism is
the term for a graphic image which precisely simulates some real object,
then a photograph must be differentiated from it as a form of reality itself.8
While Rudolf Arnheim thought that photography had “raised our demands:
we like reproductions not only to be faithful to the object but also to
guarantee their faithfulness by being mechanical manifestations of the
reproduced object itself”,9 for Deren photography is the tool that provides
art with reality.
After 1945, however, it was precisely technological devices that threat-
ened to replace human senses and possibly also human thinking. Every con-
nection to machines played with the inevitable self-dissolution of human
sensibility, and not only in art. The early researchers in cybernetics feared
that the individual as an active agent in the world was being reduced to the
minimum in the world of digital machines. In the opening speech of the
Macy conferences it is stated: “Wiener in his introduction in ‘Cybernetics’
points out […] that the complexity of the computing machine type of mecha-
nism is so great and can be pushed so far now that it potentially threatens
individual decision.”10 This is the disappointing way that the integration of
the human into the digital human-machine was being presented.
At the same time, Maya Deren was experimenting on analogue film with
the effects of technological images on human seeing, seeking to establish a
new reality assembled out of horizontal, reticulate, multiple functionalities
and relationships for the purpose of rescuing human sensibility.11 She was
simultaneously attempting to replace the idea of the individual with a
80 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
how one’s thinking it directed in the cinema, or the shock of a cut can be
experienced as mental falling and tumbling. Incursions and consterna-
tion. In Deren’s films, however, it is above all bodies themselves that are
freed from the preconfigured gaze by which they are shackled and held
fast in symbolic poses and gestures. She takes up the unpredictable and
uncontrollable in movements, but not simply as letting-oneself-go or as
relaxation, not as coincidence nor as an accidental shot, neither random
nor chance. In order to create sequences of falling, spinning, jumping, or
tumbling, Deren demands enormous discipline from the dancers that she
works with, for what is supposed to emerge as feeling in the cinema must
be assembled technologically from precisely calculated fragments into a
new film reality. “‘Film-Spontaneity’ is impossible”,16 she writes succinctly
and in a snipe to the surrealists with the pig’s eye under the razor blade.
In order to have an experience in art or in the cinema, one has to grasp
the essence of the production-device, not the essence of an object depicted
by them: “…that experience would be created out of the nature of the art
instrument by which it was, in fact, realized.”17 Deren’s note is much more
significant for film theory than for the visual arts. From the beginning,
examinations of film perception such as Arnheim’s or Panofsky’s have
insisted that we can no longer speak of representation in the cinema, since
in particular the perception of movement in cinema is a completely distinct
and singular form of perception, “not the re-experience, but the experience
of motion.”18 In her films, Maya Deren will show that the same is true for
perception of film spaces and times.
In all of Deren’s films, from Meshes of the Afternoon to the raw mate-
rial that she shot about voodoo rituals in the Caribbean, the shifting and
interlocking of relations of time and space is not only a film process, but
also determines what could be designated as action or rather “incident.”
The protagonists, dancers, lose themselves in spatio-temporal labyrinths
and encounter themselves again, this time as others on their way through
the foreign spaces. These odysseys are not to be seen as the progressive
formational journeys of heroes, but as circular movements in spaces that
displace any unambiguous gaze. Spaces reflect and refract gazes and raise
questions about how the ego is optically involved in the relation to others.
The film Meshes of the Afternoon, which was shot, according to Hammid,
as a “home movie” at 1466 Kings Road in Los Angeles, a few blocks north of
82 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
Sunset Boulevard, combined everything that the Bolex – this Swiss preci-
sion clockwork among 16mm cameras – allowed for, including changing
film speeds, fades, and focal lengths, and equipped with its frame counter
to precisely calculate multiple exposures. The film became a labyrinth
of perception, which in film history has turned out to be a labyrinth of
self-perception. The gaze in this film is led into all the paradoxes provided
by the functions of seeing. Meshes of the Afternoon realizes what Lacan
designated as the prison-house of desire in an image: “In this matter of the
visible, everything is a trap, and in a strange way […] entrelacs (interlacing,
intertwining). There is not a single one of the divisions, a single one of the
double sides that the function of vision presents that is not manifested to us
as a labyrinth.”19 The interlacing of Meshes of the Afternoon is also about the
seeing of seeing itself, and how the subject is displaced and shifted in the
process. What does not stop shifting the subjects into meshworks of space-
time would then be the uncanniness of the home movie. The means are
banal and yet fundamental, as Panofsky succinctly pointed out for cinema:
“These unique and specific possibilities can be defined as dynamization of
space and, accordingly, spatialization of time. This statement is self-evident
to the point of triviality but it belongs to that kind of truth which, just
because of its triviality, is easily forgotten or neglected.”20
Using simple tricks, the little house in Kings Road becomes a villa with
endless staircases and interlocking rooms. While for example in one wide-
angle shot (Nr. 6) the protagonist climbs a couple of steps to the front door
of the house, these very same steps will seem to have been transformed
right afterwards into an infinitely long staircase by editing together several
close-ups of the key falling down the many steps in slow motion (Nr. 9-12).
The literary “…it would seem that…” becomes being in the film by manipu-
lating time. Right at the beginning of the film then, after the protagonist
has approached the front door of her lover’s house with decisive steps,
she loses control over herself along with the key – as is conveyed by the
slow motion – and her perception of her surroundings. The slowed-down
movements of the key appear to the identified and infected perception
of the spectators as a paralysis of their own deciding power, their own
ability to intervene. The suddenly transformed steps defy any perceptual
intentions, or to put it the other way around, they no longer have control
over the temporal spaces of the world. The problem of any interpretation of
this film remains that the intensity of a feeling produced by compressing
time – of paralysis, of resistance, or of heaviness – always leaves open vari-
ous other justifications at the same time, or, to use the terms of objective
psychology, it links together different relations. With this first use of slow
Depersonalizations 83
Film reality is not created from existing relations, but from optically
constructed ones. Exertion, fear, and the feeling of futility that befall the
protagonist are not mimicked, but simulated in film perception.
A further trick to manipulate spatial feeling is to link spaces by linking
two flash pans. A long and constantly accelerating pan (Nr. 16) through a
room is edited during the blurriness of motion together with a pan that
constantly decelerates and ends in a completely different room (Nr. 17). Later
in the film (Nr. 42) there is a pan away from these same rooms without a cut,
and a staircase that was not there before becomes visible in the center. This
gives rise to a new topological context of a space that seems to be familiar.
An interlacing space that conceals its knots in the editing. The discomfort
in this constellation appears not only as a serious doubt in the protagonist’s
perceptive capacities, but also as a slight doubt in one’s own ability to re-
member. Such paradoxical spaces appear complex, but they are the result
of simple editing tricks, which, due to their speed, fall below the threshold
of conscious perception and intertwine feelings of time and space. Meshes,
used as a technical term, not only means a network, but also feedback loops,
that is, a term that meant more than handwork and handcraft in 1942. These
feedbacks of perception, which are due to non-perceptible manipulations,
the cinematic tricks of single frames, are what induces one into a cinematic
trance. The discreet charm of the Maltese cross. Since he overlooked this
systematic dismantling of duration in time done by recording technology,
Gregory Bateson could not analyze trance – at least not in film.
84 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
Hammid and Deren also create illusionary continuity by using the flash
pan to compress time. In a chase sequence between the protagonist and
a figure dressed in black, flash pans link up not only different spaces, but
also different recording speeds. The figure in black walks up the street in
slow motion, the camera follows her, then pans very quickly back. In the
blurriness of the motion the next shot is edited in, which begins with a
flash pan that slows down, and then ends on the young woman who, in
normal speed, takes up the chase (Nr. 32-38). Thus arises the paradoxical
time structure that the person walking away moves extremely slowly while
the chaser is walking very quickly and yet cannot catch up with the other.
To conscious perception, however, they are in the same space and thus
in the same time continuum. The paradox that the quick walker cannot
catch up with the slower woman is resolved by the visual perceptual logic
that knows nothing of technological tricks in that it draws compensatory
“conclusions” that here could be called “unconscious conclusions”, to bor-
row a term from Helmholtz. Because the spaces cannot be differentiated
optically, the different movement qualities of the two figures get attributed
to these figures themselves as emotional qualities. The woman in black
appears “uncanny” and “threatening” in her movements, the woman being
chased “desperate” or “frustrated.”
The exact reversal of this construction, in which the change in time takes
place over the course of the pan or as the scene goes on, is seen at the end
of the film Ritual in Transfigured Time, in which a woman, who is fleeing
in normal time, cannot escape her pursuer, who chases her in slow motion.
Here as well, the uncanniness of the spatio-temporal situation created by
film gets attributed to the man as an omnipotent fiend.
This transposition of the quality of a movement, which gets recorded
technologically and thus produced and defined cinematically, to the charac-
ter of a person who moves is fundamental to watching cinema. It is a cultural
means of perception, and it repeats the tactical history of the dispositif: the
way from physiologically measuring a person to psychologically assessing
him, which masks the operation of measuring. According to the cinema-
convention we spectators attribute cinematically produced variations of
speed and movement to the actors’ bodies, not to the film technologies
that actually create them. But even if Deren initially only sees her tricks
as the poetry of the medium, the paradoxical structures simultaneously
expose the functions of the medium. The simplest manipulations of the time
structure can make two different characters out of the same actress – in
Meshes of the Afternoon it is always Maya Deren herself. Rudolf Arnheim
described this logic in the sense of the experiments from Gestalt theory:
Depersonalizations 85
“The change of speed not only served to adapt visual movement to the
range of human perception, but also changed the expressive qualities of
an action.”22
Deren and Hammid have strategically reversed here what turns up
again in the discussion with Bateson about recording and assessment in
the documentary material from Bali. The parameters of illustration allow
what is filmed to appear in a particular and preconfigured system that has
already structured and shaped the innocent observation of the senses, be it
those of the ethnologist of the spectator in the cinema. While it is also not
possible to perceive at all except through such a matrix, the gaze should
nonetheless – and this is the point of Meshes – be drawn to the meshes of
the film.
Another trick for interweaving chronology is the repetition of certain
shots that are edited with different connections over the course of the film,
that is, into a different temporal context. Shot Nr. 21 for example shows a
close-up of a pillow, then follows with a pan right to a record player where
a record is spinning; the woman’s hand picks up the needle and pauses the
record player. The shot fades out into blurriness after another pan to the
left. Much later, shot Nr. 61 begins with a medium shot from below of the
woman, looking down and holding out her hand. Shot Nr. 62 is a medium
close-up of the record player, the woman’s hand comes into the picture
as the camera zooms in on the record player. When the framing is almost
identical with that of Nr. 21, she lifts the needle up again, once again paus-
ing the recording player… This repetition of the action appears in a new
spatio-temporal context, thus disturbing the logic of the story. Once again
the paradoxical structure actuates the circuits of logical attribution. Our
perception in the cinema tends to adjust what it sees to the usual experi-
ences of time and space, thus falling into paradoxical loops of causality. If
the same action is shown twice, then it will be a memory of the protagonist.
If, however, as its introduction suggests, it is a second, identical action,
then there must be something from outside that deceives the protagonist.
What is specific to film in this structure is that spaces and chronologies,
but also identities and movements are first identified and organized in an
automatism of reception, and are always only perceived as contradictory
after the fact. Optical illusions in painting can be distinguished from those
in film in that the latter occur in linear time and cannot be verified over the
course of projection. The constant belatedness with which consciousness
recognizes the steps of perception as missteps thus becomes a disturbance
of perception and of consciousness itself. All these dissociations, which
are called “dreamlike” in interpretations, arise from simple technological
86 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
trance. The time of perception and the perceived time periods displace one
another without there being any absolute time of consciousness as a holding
point. When a close-up of the woman’s eye movements follows the scene of
the sea in the film, which ultimately allows for no conclusions whatsoever
to be drawn as to whether the material is running backwards or once again
forwards, seeing has to be let go. Perception is squarely placed under the
regime of film movement. The following movements by the protagonist are
then set apart from all reality of the beach with a hyperreality. Her move-
ments make her appear as a stranger in her surroundings. At the same time
her sensual presence challenges the photographic realism of the sea waves.
Deren not only employs inversions of chronology, but also slow motion
and time lapse in a way that disturbs the perception of speed and reflects
on their determination from outside. When, for example, some of an actor’s
movements are executed very quickly, but recorded in slow motion, the
movement is seen in projection at a “normal” speed, albeit with qualities
that only slow motion produces. The impression of emotional closeness that
slow motion can produce is therefore also seen in normal time. In At Land
there is an example of this in a scene in which the actor’s head movements
seem to be a continuation of the previous scene in normal speed, but the
delayed speed of her hair in the wind show this movement to be accelerated
movement shot in slow motion. So this illusion makes it clear that any
so-called normality of movement is no longer a matter of normal speed or of
the normal course of movement, but a matter of a movement-combination
achieved through a camera technique.
For Deren, as for the directors of the French Nouvelle Vague twenty years
later, it is about mixing emotional affects against conventions, genres, and
topoi. In this sequence as well, two different feelings of time are aroused
in parallel, and a feedback loop of the senses produces a kind of visual
echo in the brain. The different movements on screen place the spectator
in particular but contradictory positions in relation to the image. We are
temporarily displaced. By cutting things up into single frames, the physiol-
ogy of the actor’s movements can be reassembled in any number of ways.
“(Slow motion) can create movements which are deceptively normal”, wrote
Deren.26 In the natural and idyllic surroundings of the beach everything
seems calm and unthreatening when suddenly there is the movement of
the girl out of the water itself. But the sensual quality of her movements,
the innocence of her appearance, and the fact that we are not immediately
aware of the technical manipulation shift the uncanny quality back – in
the spectator’s perception – to the carefree paradisiacal beach. Emotions
in the cinema are thus constantly compressed and shifted further.
88 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
When the woman from the sea finally goes onto shore, there comes
another spatial trick, which is an extension of the scene with the steps
from Meshes of the Afternoon. The protagonist draws up from the beach
and onto a tree stump. This is recorded from three different camera angles
– from above against the backdrop of the sand, horizontally, and from
below against the backdrop of the sky – so that the way to get there seems
insurmountably long. The impression is made even stronger by the use of
barely perceptible slow motion. Furthermore, her climbing is interwoven
with another scene edited in parallel, in which the protagonist scales the
table at a dinner party (Nr. 20-29). Maya Deren had hoped and claimed
that the identification effect of photography – “the compulsive reality of
photography” – that is, the identification of the woman on the beach with
the woman at the dinner table, would link the scenes into a continuity.
“There is a central personage who is identifiable in At Land: and whose
identity serves as a continuity method.”27 In fact, however, the perceived
continuity, which connects the obvious difference between the locations,
is made even stronger as the protagonist’s movements are slowed down
over the course of the edit and are executed more intensely as she feels her
way. Physical movements, time, and space are connected in the film in
contrast to the various conventions that the protagonist is working against.
What is fascinating in Deren’s spectacle is how easily she adapts, bodily
and sensually, to the artificial times and spaces. Like a cat she feels her way
through the film spaces, in springy, dancing movements, which in front
of the camera must in part have been completely artificial, slowed down,
sped up, or fragmented. Complementary to this, the woman at the camera
is also meant to adapt herself to the apparatus in order to make moving
images: “[…] the body with its complex combinations of joints, swivels,
etc., mounted on very adequate legs, can put, at the service of the camera,
a variety and combination of movements which even the most elaborate
tripod could not begin to offer.”28 The film At Land has been described as a
process of a woman growing up and becoming an artist,29 but this transition
can be seen as more than becoming a camerawoman in a doubled sense. A
connection emerges between body and apparatus, which appears as a new
being in the movements on screen.
There is a preliminary form in Meshes of the Afternoon to the editing
through continuous motion, which Deren herself would describe in retro-
spect as the prototype for all her successive works. At the end of this film (Nr.
131-136) one of the many Deren doppelgängers strides through the world with
a knife in her hand in order to cut her own throat when she’s arrived at the
other end. We see her feet in close-up as she walks in a continual path over
Depersonalizations 89
the beach, over grass, over a street, and finally stepping up onto a staircase.
“What I meant when I planned that four stride sequence was that you have
to come a long way to kill yourself, like the first life emerging from primeval
waters”, she wrote to James Card. “Those four strides, in my intention, span
all time.[…] As I used to sit there and watch the film when it was projected
for friends in those early days, that one short sequence always rang a bell
or buzzed a buzzer in my head. It was like a crack letting light of another
world gleam through.”30 Indeed, the film does have a different, earlier ending
in which the protagonist escapes the trap of self-perception, but the quasi
last ending, which is a murder of the self among doppelgängers, contains
yet another peculiar twist. Among the doubled women, the one who dies
is the one who was always alone in the picture, who never let herself be
copied or simulated, who could not move through the artificial cinematic
spaces and therefore could not merge with them. Those doppelgängers that
gather around the table in their multiplicity, chat, and finally pass a death
sentence on the one – who is presented as the original – thus turn out to be
resistant to romance love, to the deceptions of technical apparatuses and
to the threats raised by the transforming mirror images. They stay cool,
they are twentieth-century-minded or even cinematomorphic. Meshes of
the Afternoon marks the beginning of the evolution of a cinema-being in
Deren’s films, a being for whom going through technical processes and
images possibly means intensifying relations to the world. An intensifying
that becomes perceptible to sleepers in the cinema as an emotionalization.
At Land does not yet signify the arrival into the new world that Deren had
referred to in the letter to James Card, but is a search for forms of new, tech-
nologically manipulated relations to time and space. Stop trick sequences
and illusionary connections between movements are deployed in ever new
variations and combinations in order to push perception to the limits of its
capacity to integrate. But wherever clear-cut self-consciousness, which is
supposed to go along with clear-cut perception, has to be abandoned, the
emotional side effects continue on unbroken. For Deren this test was a way
of trying out the technical effectivity of film methods: “This concern with
time and space is not purely technical and one is not aware of the devices
of the cinema because of the emotional ramifications of this concern.”31
The odyssey through time frames in film tests the relativizing of one’s
own body in space and time through sensual impulses – and not only on the
protagonist, but also on the spectator in the cinema. The difficulty of build-
ing up a stable relationship to the fragmented and contradictory elements
of new time-spaces is not only the topic of the film, it is at the same time
adapted and altered for viewing cinema. The adjustment of the individual
90 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
The first camera construction in the film is a very long pan in which
the dancer Talley Beatty shows up at four spots between the trees of a
wooded area. This pan is assembled from four shorter, very even pans,
shot from a tripod. The second construction is a variation on movement
continuity across spatial distances. The dancer stretches out in long circular
movements, the first part of which is still in the wooded area, while the
following parts take place in a variety of interior spaces, and the extension
of the time of certain phases of movement is repeated and overlap. Over the
course of this dance sequence the dancer – this would be the third purely
optical choreography – ends up in front of a mirror, and the moving figure is
doubled in the image: a pas de deux through the mirror. The fourth and fifth
tricks are accelerations of the movement by means of spatial or temporal
contraction. In the shot in the Egyptian Court at the Metropolitan Museum
a sequence of jumps on the diagonals toward the rear corner of the room,
and then back to the camera, seems uncannily accelerated because the
use of a wide angle lens compresses the spatial relations. The reduced focal
length functions as a mental time lapse. The space gets a dynamism which
then creates artificial emotions. The same can be said of the treatment
of time. When the dancer spins in front of a four-headed Buddha, Deren
accelerates the pirouette by turning the film speed down in the Bolex from
its maximum 64 frames per second to 8 frames per second, thus achieving
an extreme fast motion – while an assistant closes the aperture so that the
lighting conditions remain stable. The manipulation of the camera appears
as art and skill, as a quality of the dancer.
When Beatty eventually advances to one last great cinematically con-
structed leap, so that his head, torso, legs, and finally even his heels fly up
and out of the image on the screen, the spectators are already deceived
by this first phase of movement, since his jump is a landing cut in reverse
into the film, a fall edited in reverse. But since the jump continues, all that
remains from the beginning is the impression of a particularly buoyant
take off. In the end the various phases of movement of a jump that is shot
in silhouette against the sky are edited together into almost 30 seconds of
floating in space before the dancer finally lands. The camera constructs
the reality of an artificially cinematic dance.
Beatty’s jump, which, as one critic noted, was actually Deren’s jump,36
displays the marionette figures whose movements are the movements that
we see on screen. Three of the dance tricks described here are not even due
to the editing, but merely to the recording technique. This makes it clear
that every movement on screen is the event of a technical construction done
by those holding the camera. By selecting the lens, the framing, the speed,
92 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
and the spatial perspective, they create movements and times that only exist
through the film. “Certainly the camera is unique, among art instruments,
in its ability to itself create time – not merely a sense of it – abut an actual
period. It can create time which is accelerated, attenuated or arrested.”37
It is such camera and cinematic constructions that Deren wanted to draw
attention to, and that exceed ordinary entanglements of time in the cinema:
In her last film The Very Eye of Night the dancers’ movements finally be-
come composed of camera movements in such a way that the dancers are
floatingly danced through the cinematic space in a quiet ballet. Body and
movement meet each other in ever more abstract relations.
In her experiments with time and space, Deren was initially working with
dance movements because they can be more easily edited, since they have
their own rhythm. In the film Ritual in Transfigured Time, however, she
transforms ordinary movements into dance movements. Once again, this is
due to the choreography of the camera: “…first I should state, for the benefit
and ease of anyone overawed by the film’s title, that the transfigurations
of time are, on one level, just technical devices…” wrote the critic Ken
Kelman.39 In the film’s most famous choreographic moment, gestures of
greeting were assembled from numerous encounters into a social ritual,
although the course of movement during the repeated shootings was already
so heavily ritualized that the movements recorded cannot be called either
voluntary or involuntary. In a variety of sequences in which there are vari-
ous degrees of slow motion, Deren organized the rhythm of the movements
with a metronome, the basic instrument of all psycho-laboratories.
In a further development of the examples from Meshes of the Afternoon,
in the film Ritual in Transfigured Time various recording speeds are also
assembled into a space that had been established in the film as continuous.
The experiment is carried out in mathematical regularity. In one sequence
at the beginning of the film (Nr. 11-23) the two protagonists meet, played
by Maya Deren and the dancer Rita Christiani. Maya Deren allows woolen
yarn to unwind from her outstretched arms with great, even rocking move-
ments, and Rita Christiani then winds it into a ball either in countershot
Depersonalizations 93
But apart from such scientific uses, slow-motion can be brought to the
most casual activities to reveal in them a texture of emotional and
psychological complexes. For example, the course of a conversation is
normally characterized by indecisions, defiances, hesitation, distractions,
Depersonalizations 95
shape. All attributions are dissolved over time in the multiple reflections
of the gazes. In the cinema, as Deren’s films were able to show despite all
apparatus theory, it is not the confirmation of an original ego function
that is repeated, such as takes place in the mirror stage. Rather, the whole
allure of secondary narcissistic identifications is placed into the work, in
which everyone is constantly changing, or desperately wishes to change,
into a different futurist ego.
To this end, Deren uses the photographic image on screen, edited into
the film, which is capable of deceiving the others, all the more so when it
lets down its own self-perception, time and time again. As a good player
Deren puts herself completely into the task, allowing herself to be optically
fragmented, putting her own involuntary, unconscious movements of the
camera’s illustration on display and merging them in such a way that the
spectators see the Other of the others, the depersonalization in every film
shoot, causing them to be deceived about their own conscious self in the
cinema as well. The doublings, depersonalizations, and extensions on screen
are experiments with the self, in which perception and self-perception are
constantly and mutually modified: proprioception in the technologies of
perception. This auspicious fusion with the camera, however, only works if
no identities, truths, or diagnoses are attached to it. Seeing in the cinema
is the deceptive illusion under which a subject is constituted, and possible
meshes and impossible rituals are transformed in these deceptions.
In the film At Land there is an episode that at first seems to belong in
the repertoire of slapstick. The protagonist, who had already gone through
various stations of going on land as an experience of civilization, encounters
a cinematically multiplied personality in the man. While walking along
a country lane she is shown to be in dialogue with a man who is always
a different one (Nr. 56-64). The counterpart to this changing identity is
her own constitution, but not as identity, but as continuity, that is, as an
artificial duration of a sequence of time:
This trick, that a new man jumps into the image behind the back of the
camera as it pans back and forth with the dialogue, is so unspectacular
Depersonalizations 99
that the small adjustments in perception provoked by the scene are barely
noticeable. It is not the similarity of the men that poses the question of iden-
tity. Rather, it is the establishment of a continual timeframe in the film, in
which the four men appear in the same spot, walking in the same direction
and taking part in the same conversation, which seeks to force cinematic
understanding to identify in the first place.54 By means of this trick, identity
is presented without mediation as intersubjective, as interrelational and
techo-social determination, in which the subjects, the man and woman in
the film, exactly like actors in the studio or on screen and the spectators in
the cinema, are subjected to one another in their temporal-spatial relations.
All desire that arises between them is shown to be the waste product of an
optical construction. This waste product, however, is the actual pleasure
in films, as it is in every relationship.
Pasolini staged quite similar sequences in Mamma Roma. While Anna
Magnani walks in long takes along the bleak whoring streets on the outskirts
of the city, she is accompanied time and again by single men or groups of
boys who engage in her monologue, answer her, calling in commentary
and jokes. For all their differences, these men also possess a single timeless
identity. Mamma Roma’s monologue could in turn be spoken in first person
by two and a half thousand years of Roman history. What she has to say
is defined by her relation as a whore to the men and to friends, and by the
dilapidated roads of the outskirts. The men, however, also appear on the
world’s stage only because they are allowed to be in relation to Roma – and
in the mirror reverse at the intersection, to amoR.
The sequence in Deren’s At Land unfolds its meanings only in the context
of the entire film, which is crisscrossed by a symbolic play by and with chess
pieces. At the beginning, animated chess pieces move before a single chess
player at the end of a dinner party. Over the course of this party the black
queen captures a white pawn which is dragged from the field, falls over,
tumbles off the board and – in a movement continued past the cut – tumbles
outside through meadows and into the sea, where it begins an odyssey
through natural landscapes. The chess game relativizes all other actions in
the film, albeit not through the strategic moves on the board, but through
the relation of the characters to the rules of the game. Animated by the
film’s tricks, the chess pieces leave the space and the patterns of movement
through which they are defined in their possible opposing power relations.
Away from the board they can no longer meet according to the old rules of
art, but are exposed to new laws of motion from bubbling brooks, breaking
waves, or blowing winds. In the space of the chess board or of geometral
optics, as Lacan calls it in a language game, we are “in space partes extra
100 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
at any rate, if only to be able not only to double the single figures, but to
multiply them even more. In Meshes of the Afternoon three doppelgängers
come together for a round table discussion about the fate of the first, three
Maya Derens sit in judgment over themselves at the same time (Nr. 115),
much like how a photo from 1917 shows five Marcel Duchamps pondering
themselves.
Ever since Otto Rank declared the doppelgänger as an anthropological
mental constant in 1914, the prototype for creating doppelgängers in film
has been The Student of Prague, as it was brought to screen by Stellan Rye’s
cameraman Guido Seeber. Seeber, who was responsible for all the technical
tricks at German Bioscop from 1908 to 1914 (and who later shot The Joyless
Street and Secrets of a Soul for Pabst), used this trick of masking in his film
from 1913 in order to transfer the old fable of the selling one’s mirror image
to film. In his images, Rank first observed that representation in film “in
numerous ways reminds us of dream-work.”56 This was, as Friedrich Kittler
showed,57 already a misrepresentation and supersession of the history of
psychoanalytic knowledge, since it was not film that imitated the dream. On
the contrary, film, as Freud had previously seen it, as a chronophotographic
dissection of bodies and a way to store symptoms at Salpêtrière, was the
technical prerequisite for psychological and dream analysis. Before one
could find symptoms in the body, before the ego could be confronted with
a symptomatic Other, the body as body had to be able to be recorded and
stored with all its marks and defects.
The doppelgänger in Deren’s films are reworkings of human subjectivity
in the age of the time machines, among which the camera, alongside the
radio, the airplane, and concepts of relativity, is one of the most impor-
tant.58 Doppelgängers do not have a psychic or psychological origin, but
come from the history of cinema itself, and take possession of realistic
bodies. Since it became possible to indicate identity as the information of
technological media by bertillonage, doppelgängers have been a problem of
lighting. The relevant originary dispositif in film history of all later double
and triple exposures, long before the cameraman Guido Seeber’s tricks
were constructed, was indeed Étienne-Jules Marey’s multiple exposures
of a single photographic plate on which he recorded the course of motion.
One and the same soldier jumped, fought, and ran in the depiction, vainly
competing with himself, but since he was a being synthesized out of his
own jumps in the stop-motion images, he could also float above all reality
like the dancers in Deren’s film.
Time and again, Deren’s delight in mobilizing doppelgängers generates the
constellations in which the characters “see themselves seeing themselves”
102 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
possession of the flower, which has turned into a knife at just this moment.
The protagonists’ gazes – and with them the spectators’ gazes – shift in
both directions between love and aggression and are also crisscrossed
by side views of the reflective knife. Once again, spectators presume the
mutual assumptions running between the protagonists’ gazes. Although
the woman, quite in keeping with classical dramaturgy, lies submissively on
the bed, and in one shot we see only her mouth, which opens slightly, while
the man sits upright, coolly and anxiously looking down, this does not add
up to any clear order of emotions. The spectators will attempt to recognize
clear reactions in the protagonists’ facial expressions, but devotion, concern,
lust, fear, betrayal, and revenge are interpretations that can be seen one after
the other in the ever constant faces, or better, can be presumed. In the duel
of gazes the woman suddenly grabs the knife and throws it in the direction
of the man. This signifies a change in the optical conditions as a whole. The
sharp cut to the next shot shows that the image of the man must have been
a mirror image, which has now been shattered. A large hole is now gaping
where his eyes once were, providing a view to the sea.
Throwing the knife opens the gaze up to that optical space that surrounds
the space of the duel and had obviously surrounded it the whole time. What
is uncanny about this scene is that underneath, in the mirror’s blurriness,
which supplants the space “opposite”, despite its shattered surface, the
remaining partial portrait of the man, his face now without eyes and gaze,
continues, speaking, to move. What is uncanny is that he is not simply
dead, but has been displaced into a different, unknown, optically real
but not logical space, in which he can no longer give any response to the
woman’s inquisitive gaze, although he continues to have an influence on
her reality. This image is once again reminiscent of the other images in
the film in which the medial voices from other spaces are silenced. The
telephone receiver that is put back on the cradle over and over again, the
record player that is stopped time and again. What is uncanny in Meshes of
the Afternoon is that the connections to the absent and to the apparatuses,
the talking and the reflection of others, which structures the ego’s desire,
cannot be turned off. “It” keeps going. The second, less uncanny turn taken
in Meshes of the Afternoon with this break in the optics is that the man’s gaze
is cleared away as the sole answer to the woman’s desire, and not as killing
the consciousness of the other, but as an optical switch. In the following
shot we see the seashore with its beach and waves, blinking shards of mirror
floating in the water reflecting patches of the sky. A re-relativized space
has emerged by optically frustrating the structure of the gaze. Hard linear
gazes turn into waves that play with and wash around the mirror images.
104 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
This ending of Meshes of the Afternoon, in which the mirror flies to the
beach as shards, is the first ending, the “happy ending” of the film. It replaces
imaginary and eerie identification in the couple’s relationship with a mul-
tiplication of spaces. It is likely that this is the ending that Deren wanted,
since is a female gaze that no longer wishes to be required to look someone
else in the eyes in order to be recognized. And yet it does not heroically look
away, but invents new optical illusions that serve as a strategic eye-catcher:
becoming subject with one’s own devices. It is at any rate the film ending
that Deren chose as her own, for the film At Land will start a couple of
years later with almost the very same shot at the beach that Meshes of the
Afternoon ends with, but instead of shards of mirror there are eyes, bodies,
and the crests of waves in the image.
The mirrors that have played a central role in Meshes of the Afternoon as
a cinematic function of optical reflection and as a functional metaphor of
the gaze will no longer turn up in Deren’s later films – except for the one
mirror in Choreography for the Camera, in front of which the dancer briefly
dances a pas de deux with his doppelgänger. The function of the gaze will be
absorbed completely in the optical operations of the camera and the film.
Reflection becomes refraction. The tricks and techniques of the camera
itself provoke the feedback of perception about the technical conditions
that effect the reactions and emotions through projection.
The question of identity in the film will increasingly get shifted from
imagination to production. In At Land it was experiments with the gaze,
as experiments with editing together times and spaces, through which the
protagonist was meant to be put into a relation to a universe relative to
herself: “The problem of that individual, as the sole continuous element, is to
relate herself to a fluid, apparently incoherent, universe.”62 In Choreography
for the Camera the film replaces the logic of a person with the logic of a move-
ment: “Here the identity which unites space and time is not the personalized
identity of a given, individual character, for there is no effort to treat the
dancer in these terms; rather, it is the identifiable nature of his movement
which constitutes the compulsive continuity.”63 In Ritual in Transfigured
Time these two forms of identification, which after all are necessary for
“continuity” in the montage, are, as perception itself infers from a pattern
of movement or a photographic shape, supplemented by a third mode:
Associations, or, since it is the same thing for Bleuler, the thinking that
follows these traces according to fixed rules, “can best be understood by
comparing it with the switches in an electrical plant. These switches may
connect different machines with one another or let them run independently
of each other; they can switch them on or off.”73 Bleuler’s theory of switching
on and off in the nervous system is not only one of the clearest example of
what, as Kittler noted, “every psychology or anthropology only subsequently
spells out which functions of the general data processing are controlled
by machines, that is, implemented in the real.”74 It justifies a functional
theory of the unconscious, which Bleuler only distinguished from the then
competing system of reflexes because he was examining reactions that did
not run by subcortical means, but by nerve tracts and cerebral cortices.
Such unconscious processes can also help explain perceptions that do not
arise from stimuli or sensations.
Learning by Feeling
It is not fixed designs made up of optical machines and human bodies, but
vectors of permanently shifting force fields that would be the right model for
Deren’s film theory. Her films present feedback mechanisms and circular-
causal processes as they were being researched in the US during the forties
by anthropologists, psychiatrists, computer specialists, and engineers. The
cinema was to be conceived as an apparatus that linked individual percep-
tion and sensibility with times and spaces formed by technology. Techno-
logical images would form emotions that were nonetheless due to an exact
experience of reality. What is always “recorded subjectively as emotion”, as
Norbert Wiener conjectured, “may not merely be a useless epiphenomenon
of nervous action, but may control some essential stage in learning, and in
other similar processes.”80 Like affectivity for Bleuler. The nervous system
stores our experiences and models the perception accordingly.
Gregory Bateson had underestimated the intrusions that filming released
in the plateaus of Balinese culture, and its transformation by its connection
with the apparatus was surprising to him. Deren as well will be surprised by
an odd connection between human bodies and apparatuses in Haiti. But not
like Bateson. In Haiti, Deren encountered the archaic techniques of voodoo
rituals that she had previously studied and from which she had developed
a radical critique of her own culture and its techniques. But while the term
ritual had previously granted her a way to see the transcendences of western
cultures as equally producing and projecting in its cultural technologies, a
few years later she uses the eye of the cinema to look at the techniques with
which the Haitian gods can artificially be called into action. Like in the
cinema, manipulations of time are introduced into the rituals as gateways
to different perceptual levels, as “doors of perception”. “There are even ritual
details in which inversion and reversal suggest a mirror held to to time.[…] It
is like a motion-picture projected in reverse, a diver shooting back up out of
112 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
the water on to the springboard.”81 Times, spaces, and movements are linked
in certain cultures according to particular laws initiated, altered, and formed
by the perception of those living in this culture. The integration of these forms
can be understood in the service of gods or of media, the decisive factor is that
this cultural aspect does not represent any law or fixed schema, but a rule of
transformation that has to be realized, embodied in constant performance.
When Maya Deren lands in Port-au-Prince in the autumn of 1947, she
brings along two cameras, tripods, and material for 16mm film footage,
a 6 x 6 still camera and equipment to record sound on magnetic wire. In
Haiti she records optical and acoustic signals separately, not at random,
as Bateson had attempted on Bali, but following an integrity that she had
recognized as the rule of transformation both of ritual and of the cinema
as well. In rhythms and light effects, in temporal and spatial compressions,
in fragmentations and compositions of collective body movements she
assembles a reality of gods and devices. Afterwards she thinks she was able
to differentiate manipulating and recording in ritual procedures:
In ritual, the rhythm of the drumming reassembles the ego, which had fallen
apart. The dance and the dance are indistinguishable, linked through the
acoustic “order”, arrangement and command, that structures time. The next
division takes places in the visual aspects of the mirror, which simultane-
ously constructs and divides the ego, demonstrating its interconnectedness
with the doppelgänger. At this moment the gaze of the dance becomes the
eye of cinema, which sees stroboscopically, shifting into slow motion, as if
an imaginary projection speed were being reduced. A flicker starts, black
holes emerge between the single frames, and the cinematographer loses
herself not between the ego and the mirror image, but in the nothingness
between image and image, which is never perceived in the cinema but
nonetheless governs cinematic perception:
The film breaks in the projection room. What lies between image and image
as trance remains unconscious. It nonetheless turns up in the protocol
precisely as the technology of cinema. The fact that Deren no longer wants to
114 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
edit her own material after this coinciding of film image and identification
is everything but the rejection of the medium of film. It is much more about
promoting it into the pantheon of recording technologies that have formed
souls, spirits, and memories. The great alliance of cinema and anthropology
has brought the gods down from heaven in order once again to connect
them to the bodies of human beings. They are meant to change the world.
But the stories that they tell the anthropologists are stories of media that
have been changing people, their societies, and their histories from time
immemorial. Sometimes we forget that, sometimes we are reminded, for
instance by Godard when in Le Mépris he shows that Fritz Lang knew it.
Bateson had observed that people in occidental cultures still speak and act
as if dissecting and codifying reality on the one hand and subjectively evalu-
ating this reality on the other were processes that were mutually separable,
while in fact every visual perception of another person and every physical
reaction can only register the combined process of codifying and evaluating,
that is, behavior.87 This indicates the emergence of a new concept of the
human being, which has its preconditions in the cinematic recording of
human characteristics. But only in the fifties of the twentieth century did
“l’homme imaginaire”, whose behavior always had to be accompanied by a
double, get established in everyday life as well. Edgar Morin examined the
imaginary man in an ethnological study of his own society in its dependence
on the technology of cinema: “J’ai fait en même temps de l’anthropologie du
cinema et de la cinématographologie de l’anthropos, selon le movement en
boucle: l’esprit humain – éclaire le cinema – qui éclaire – l’esprit humain
etc.”88 Working with Jean Rouch in the summer of 1960, Morin also turned
his diagram of the flow between the functions of anthropology and the
function of the cinema, which projects all behavior onto the surfaces of the
body, into movements and voices beyond the symbolic, into a film experi-
ment. The reactions by various inhabitants of Paris to the questions “Are you
happy?” was combined into a kaleidoscope of French society: Chronique
d’un Été, a film commissioned by the Musée de l’Homme. A film about
film and anthropology, in which Batesonian binaries such as man/woman,
dominance/submission, strange/familiar are relativized into a historical
image of post-war society, where taboos suddenly push their way back into
the light and onto the big screen.
Bateson himself abandons cinematography and takes up circular-causal
processes in neurology and psychiatry. This is exactly the reason he remains
an anthropologist. His studies on homeostatic processes in schizophrenia
research examine group processes in which they attempt to understand
“the use of relations”, as Lacan says, the usage of relationships between all
Depersonalizations 115
the members of a group, from outside, from the behavior and strategies
of talking. Bateson’s research has affiliations with Vladimir Bekhterev’s
studies, which he had carried out in St. Petersburg a half century earlier.
These two strands of the history of psychiatry, which are separated from
one another by all the systematic, ideological, and institutional borders in
the world, can – and not by chance – be linked by the name Maya Deren.89
Par hasard the history of sciences as novel.
3. Deviations
Following the end of the war, poets, painters, and ethnologists were traveling
out of the ruins of Europe and into Africa, the South Seas, or the Caribbean
to research occultism, mysticism, ritual practices and possession, and, as
Michel Leiris wrote, “to forget their mediocre little ‘white man’s manias’ (as
certain blacks say), and also to lose what they conceived of as their identity
as intellectuals.”1 They dragged along their own cultural technologies with
them, subjecting the objects of their research to the very same literary,
iconological, or medial orders that they were hoping to escape, a fact that
they remained oblivious to, and at any rate did not make their readers
aware of – with very few exceptions, who tended to be engineers rather
than humanities scholars. Jean Rouch, for example, “Ingénieur des Ponts
et Chausées”, had already signed up to go to Africa at the beginning of the
Second World War, since the only thing left for him to do in France was to
blow up bridges before the enemy could get to them. He wanted to use his
knowledge constructively in Africa and what interested him as an engineer
were how things worked, or how they had to be built so that they worked.
Coming from a family of Parisian artists and adventurers, having observed
the surrealist scene in Paris in his youth, and having been a regular visitor
at Café Flore, he was also interested in how things become dysfunctional,
how they produce erratic effects, and how they took place on the bridges
of unforeseen encounters between cultures. Rouch was a cartographer of
dépaysement.
In 1941 Rouch was deployed to the area around Niamey in Niger. His
predecessor gave him some advice: “Just don’t go native! Keep away from
those niggers! After all, you’re representing French culture!” Many years
later, in a conversation with Hanns Zischler, he would add: “I did not take
his advice.”2 Rouch represents his life as a clear case of “going native”, which,
118 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
the while. When he films them all traveling to the beach together, Rouch
comments on his view, the images, and glamorous Abidajni stars at the
same time: “Voici enfin l’Afrique en vacances.” Moi, un Noir examines what
images an “I” would produce if it had the cinema.
“For Rouch, it is a matter of getting out of his dominant civilization and
reaching the premises of another identity.”8 Rouch’s strategy was every bit
as careful as Deleuze presents it to be. Because he was aware that he was
not only the symbolic representative of French culture, as it was called
during the war, but that he was also the engineer that had seen to creating
the conditions for traffic and mobility in Niger, he also wanted only to
use film technology to set up constructions on which identities could be
positioned, according to their usage, with or against cultural conventions.
Rouch examined how people learned to move, identify, and differentiate
on the bridges and streets and on other symbolic orders that the colonial
masters had saddled the country with. This is why he does not merely film
indigenous dances and music, but wild speech, the art of fabulating, which
explodes the masters’ discourse through speech acts that have gone mad.
“‘I is another’ is the formation of a story which simulates, of a simulation of
a story or a story of simulation which deposes the form of truthful story.”9
Cinéma vérité is a truth of the cinema that differs from other truths due to
its technological processes of production, but that no longer needs to claim
to be absolute. Films are a productive force that builds new bridges and con-
nections, on which syncretized forms of transitioning beyond established
forms of consciousness can be cultivated:
research and starting point for his films. His data and depictions, all of
which – except for those that stem from chronicles in the name of and in
the archives of the Muslim rulers – were the result of empirical investiga-
tions in the field, could be supplemented by the African spectators in the
projections on the village square. His justification for him viewing writing
as inadequate for his purposes of transitory and reciprocal anthropology
was that the Songhay had torn only the photos out of the dissertation that
he had written about them and hung them up in their huts because they
could not read. Because the interventions of the African spectators in the
film work had proven to be a helpful corrective, however, Rouch began to
propose ethnology methodologically as a self-correcting scientific system.
Here as well, he made a detour around the institutional ways. Only after
the fact does pragmatism turn out to have been an epistemological leap:
I was convinced that using the means of f ilm one could contribute
substantially more to the research of foreign cultures than a scientific
discourse can, however thorough it might be. When, for example, one
witnesses and records an initiation, one is struck with how the individuals
being initiated react (this is most obvious with circumcision): classical
anthropology doesn’t even take note of this at first, it reduces the suf-
fering or jovial person to a fresco, an overall tableau. It takes itself out of
the reality that it itself has seen like a mask.13
like in the film Bataille sur le grand fleuve, a film about the hippopotamus
hunt, which spectators in Niamey criticized after it was completed because
Rouch put hunting melodies – played on authentic string instruments! – on
the soundtrack. Moi, Rouch, later admits what he was trying to do: “I wanted
to ‘make movies.’”14 Moi, un cinéaste! The Africans, however, let him know
that hippopotamus hunting required silence, a different nervous tension
than that provoked by the music in the prepared dance ceremonies. They
made their arguments ritually, cinematographically, and as hunters all at
once. Voici l’Afrique.
Damouré Zika, who had seen himself on screen for the first time in
Bataille sur le grand fleuve, then wanted to appear as an actor in future
films: “on va jouer!” And so it was in many other films, and the so-called
ethno-fiction film was invented. In turn, other spectators of Bataille sur le
grand fleuve suggested that Rouch make a film about their lion hunt. This
led to the film La Chasse au lion à l’arc. The new cinema that Rouch initiated
in Africa developed at the level of these expressed wishes.
Rouch’s specific methods also consisted in using cinematography not
only as a recording process, but as a potential and transcultural feedback
procedure.
but in order to make it loud and clearly audible. In the porcelain shop of the
fine information society you have to shatter the old connections, “casser
les vitres” in a double sense:
“Coutand was prepared to develop the super light Éclair, which he had
built for satellites, for civilian use as well.”18 He tested the new technology
on colleagues in Paris:
An Éclair-centaur, a camera-man.
The improved optical network system shifts and displaces the iden-
tity positions of its participants to the point that they do not recognize
themselves.
Just like the first people I attempted to interview, I was highly surprised
by the provocative strength caused by this contact. It was a conversa-
tion with Paul Lévi, a student of Marcel Mauss, with whom I carried
out a highly intense conversation, the camera always at my eye. I posed
questions and got responses that I (and as he himself told me afterwards,
also he) had never experected.[…] I call this state “ciné-trance.” This
expression plays with the Vertov’s coining of the term “kino pravda”
(“cinéma vérité”).20
Dancer, the Magician, the Sorcerer, the Filmmaker, and the Ethnographer.”21
Spectators in the cinema can add themselves to this list. From the begin-
ning, possession has been a state that occurs not only in the ritual space of
the ceremony, but in the cinematic space that reaches far beyond the optical
field of the camera (or the projection space of the cinema).
In his description of possession, Rouch relativizes the distinction be-
tween dance acts and speech acts emphasized by Deleuze. In the time travel
that is one of the elements of dances of possession, language and movement
both become messages sent from the beyond. In possession (as in all ritual
processes) both the dance act and the speech act get their rhythm from the
outside. This is familiar to Europeans in dancing, but uncanny in speaking.
“The gods speak through the dancer, his body, and his mouth. When people
dance, they dance their history; history appears as they know it from the
continual narrative.”22
The messages are brought from the beyond with the aid of the camera,
which at the time was not even a satellite camera. Just as Deren surmised in
her examination of phenomena of possession, the power of the imaginary
established in rituals is dependent on the use of the symbolic and is thus
provoked and intensified by recording technology. The truth that emerges
is due on the hand quite simply to the cinematic restructuring of a space.
Because behavior itself, that is, the transforming interaction between
subjects, is made visible, cinéma vérité can register the precise truth of
lies. Rouch explains this with an example of a particular encounter, over the
course of which notions of center and periphery, of power and submission,
got inverted and twisted in a dual back-and-forth of presumptions. In the
Comédie Française Rouch saw De Gaulle talking with the Moroccan ambas-
sador during the intermission of a performance of Racine’s Andromaque.
Rouch recounts:
I got within a few centimeters of de Gaulle and he, when he realized the
camera was rolling, turned to to the ambassador and asked: ‘Excellence,
who do you prefer, Andromaque or Hermione?’ ‘Hermione, mon Général!’
‘How right you are!’ And I am quite certain that this dialogue would never
have existed in this form without the camera.23
takes sides with the one who renounces her own origins. Of course, since
the camera’s rolling, this could all be a matter of displacements, deferrals,
false statements, which is what produces cinéma vérité in this feedback.
Trojan horses for General de Gaulle. The camera instigates a show in which
everything is off-center – even if the center and the power located there
have not yet been abolished. Rouch studied this in turn with his footage
of possession rituals: “It’s a short step to fiction, for what occurs in these
dances is in principle nothing other than the birth of tragedy. Dionysus is
always passing by. The heart of the Corybantes is transformed into the hears
of a filmmaker with a camera and a Nagra.”24 It is not a matter of authentic
culture, but rather of people and gods in cultural transit.
Before the classics can appear in film with symbolic shifts, however,
and even before the imaginary can extend its wings in the cinema, Rouch
describes the simplest alliance of film with possession, one located at the
most elementary level of the camera’s technology. The process of rhythmi-
cally chopping up, with which the camera dissects the imaginary identity of
bodies into individual frames, can be connected in its rhythmic regularity
to the technique of inducing trance. Like drum rhythms, the intermit-
tent scansions of the camera throw consciousness out of sync with the
self-referential consciousness. The noise of the camera indicates that it
allows an Other from outside to take the place of the ego. Maya Deren
explained this process in terms of the physical properties of the nervous
system: “Sustained rhythmic regularity and the fact that the source of it
is outside the individual rather than within, means that consciousness is
unnecessary, as it were, in the maintenance of this concentration.”25 Rouch
provides a technical description based on footage shot for a film from 1971
about two particular drummers, Tourou et Bitti, and about the music and
the states of possession that they create. When he comes to the village with
the soundman Moussa Hamidou, the musicians are already playing, but
nothing has yet happened among the dancers:
I wanted to come into the village with the camera rolling. The problem
for Moussa, who was coming with me, was to choose the best of the
various sounds, the best of many things said, which of course did not
always happen identically with the image. We reached the dance square,
where an old dancer, old Albeydou, Sambou Albeydou, was just then
approaching the orchestra. I also went to the orchestra and then – sud-
denly – the orchestra paused. Normally I would have had to stop, but I
left the camera running, I squinted over at Moussa, he nodded to me, we
kept shooting, and the people around us sensed or knew that the camera
128 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
was still running and at that moment possession took over.[…] The priests
and the dancers went along, they knew that their dance and their music
were now being recorded, which stimulated them greatly.26
Here the camera replaces the function that has to precede all trance and
suggestion, as Deren had described for the leveling of the stimulus frequency
in acoustically inducing trance. An expectant attitude has to be introduced,
in which sense data are processed differently:
Only when the confusing, noisy, and cacophonous flow of data has been
artificially chopped apart and systematized and a certain artistic space has
been established – in the acoustics of the archaic drum rhythms or in the
visual aspects of the cinematically rhythmatized space – can the imaginary
and symbolic relations be connected to it. This is the physiological pre
requisite of possession. Rouch, however, does not so much stress the process
of rhythmically dissecting the movement, but the reverse of the process, the
continuity of the uniformly formalized elements, such as the 24 frames per
second in the projector, the cinema-trance, which continues even when a
black frame appears on the screen. Both sides of this process, the dissection
and the imaginary synthesis, form the social technique that combines the
peculiarities of the gods into the body images of the dancers. In order to
preserve the integrity of the ritual, intervening subjective decisions have
to be reduced. This is why Rouch insists on long takes, on shooting with the
camera without interruption as long as there is material.
To emphasize it once again, what was significant was the long take, the
continuity of filming during the ritual. My efforts went to shooting even
longer, substantially longer takes – an exclusively technical problem,
which, as I hoped, could be solved by Jean-Pierre Beauviala, the camera
engineer and inventor from Grenoble.28
realness of the cinematic space that unlock the dark continent of the psyche
in possession, creating new insights and new overviews and allowing for
journeys into the heart of darkness. Psychic space is differentiated by the
technology of the camera. A new topology emerges, which stands completely
within the logic of European, western technology, but which also provides
an image of how a Deux Chevaux makes its way down the river along the
bridge African-style. As long as the engineers manipulate the technology,
they definitely maintain an awareness of the fact that the paths that they
are plotting are also those that they have have to “get over.” They can and
must also know that they themselves are transformed by this technology:
Vertov said that the eye of the camera is a mechanical eye, the micro-
phone is a mechanical ear or sense of hearing (so-called radio hearing),
this mechanical anatomy is characterized by a certain variability in the
optical (lenses, apertures, focusses) and in the acoustic (axial registration
with directional microphones). If I, equipped with these instruments,
make a film, I myself am a ciné-observateur and find myself in a state
of ciné-trance. In other words: I, Rouch, get up, move, and do something
that I otherwise would never do.29
to their cult, and precisely not as persons of authority from the colonial
powers, but as servants: “à la bonne.” In addition, a camera, which is called
Bell & Howell, must have appeared to these Haouka priests as a convenient
omen for the change of “his master’s voice.”
The title already announces yet another application of the film. On the
one hand the “maîtres fous” are traditionally “the masters of the wind that
brings madness in its wake”, that is, the priests who have overcome mad-
ness. At the same time, “maîtres fous” is the name of the mad masters, the
colonialists with their cultural and symbolic orders, their science, their
traffic systems, and their military parades. The syncretic sect of the Haouka,
beginning with the order that they gave to start shooting, provided the
occasion for a film of reciprocal anthropology. Les Maîtres fous is, according
to Rouch, “in the first place a film about us, the whites, about the image
that Africans make of us.”31
Rouch’s camera had to be rewound after 45 seconds. The “mise-en-scène”,
the dramaturgy, arose during shooting, in the seconds necessary to rewind
the spring mechanism again. Since Rouch was shooting in long takes, as
always – that is, making no interruptions and no cuts as long as the cam-
era ran in the space of the ritual activities, but only movements – he had
placed himself under the periodic standards of the camera and thus in a
ritual space. What caught his attention as he was rewinding the camera
determined the next take. This was, according to Rouch, the thinking in
the film, that had got lost with the incorporation of motors into cameras.
The gods of technology rule the parameters of cinema-thinking.
The sound, on the other hand, could be recorded continuously for 30
minutes. In order to apply it later, the editor listened to the whirring of
the camera, which was heard scene for scene in the original sound of the
ritual in the Ghanaian bush. Editing the film followed the chronology of
its recording.32 The shots, their beginnings and ends, are so haphazard and
noticeable due to the camera’s limitations that there’s no way to claim any
overview, any overall perspective. In the dramaturgy on the ritual stage,
which is extremely difficult to comprehend, the cameraman is optically
and firmly integrated in the events, in one role among many, subject to the
direction of the gods.
The techniques used to induce trance in the ritual are not separately
described in the film. What can be recognized is that rhythms are created by
pounding wooden guns and that large amounts of gin have been poured out
on the central sites of the scene. The first signs of possession are shot from
various camera angles, almost textbook cases from a catalog of convulsions:
first in the left foot, then in the right, in the leg, through the back, to the
Deviations 131
shoulders, to the head. Rouch captures the eyes askew, the contorted joints.
The degree to which the camera, the appointed cameraman, and the shoot
are stimulating the Haouka cannot be determined. The first possessed
person to get up, “Kaporal Gardi”, the corporal of the guard, does so in fact
directly in front of and looking at the camera. Over the course of the ritual
various other gods appear without being filmed from the beginning. The
trance as ciné-trance is evidently only one among many in this ritual. At
any rate, everything, let’s not forget, that we see of possession is cinema.
Among the gods that appear after the corporal of the guard, there are
the locomotive driver, Captain Malta of the Red Sea, the General Secretary
Gomno, the Major Commandant Mugu, Samkaki the truck driver, and
Chemoko, the son of the corporal. They are recognizable through their
movements, but additionally for the spectator by the fact that a running
commentary paraphrases what is said by the other participants, sometimes
also already gods, sometimes still human beings, as they greet the new
gods, test them, and adorn them with the appropriate accessories. The
perversions of colonial history are presented in many small details. Even
more than the ridiculed medals, helmets, suits, and flowery phrases, small
shifts in the relations show that the gods know what is driving them on. For
instance, in Les Maîtres fous it is the wife of the doctor, “Madame Lokotoro”,
who appears, not the doctor himself, whose figure also appears in Foucault
as the “master of madness”, the one “who makes it appear in its truth and
[…] dominates it, pacifies it, and gradually makes it disappear after having
artfully unleashed it.”33 In the ritual, the doctor’s Other appears, what moves
and controls him: Madame.
Much more than any symbolic staging, however, it is in the embodiment
of movements that the Haouka gods make themselves known. The corporal,
the captain, the locomotive driver are distinguishable through their gaits,
in which man and corresponding machine are partially fused, whistling
and fizzling, into a single way of moving. The gods therefore also appear
as a farce in the official history of anthropological film, which constantly
disavows its ergonomic beginnings and its initial regulation of bodies.
Cinematography is supposed to differentiate racially. Here, however, all
the movements of the maîtres fous appear as awkward, synchronized, stiff
convulsions set to a militarily strict pre-established rhythm: “In all the other
films that I shot on dances of possession the gestures of the dancers are
rounded out, soft, themselves dissolved in the greatest turmoil, while in Les
Maîtres fous they are angular, coarse, and ludicrous.”34 But they are precise
in a double sense. In the middle of the footage of the possessed, Rouch
inserts an old film document of a British military parade in order to be able
132 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
to show how the cult dancers simulate or distort these odd movements and
accessories. Through this material, however, it becomes clear that our gaze
among les maîtres fous has already begun to differentiate, and we become
attentive to differences between the gestures and movements of the British
and French occupiers. Under the gaze of the Africans, the Blacks, as they
could proudly be called in the négritude of the fifties, it becomes visible how
colonization had also subjected European bodies to a manifest violence.
The ritual in the film becomes a lesson in reciprocal anthropology. In this
regard the possession dances of the maîtres fous are unequivocal. They
show how domination does not remain on an administrative level, but
how it, unconscious and supra-personal, takes power through bodies. This
magic was first applied in Europe during the nineteenth century under
no less brutal rituals and then, through physical drill and psycho-physical
identification, transferred to the bodies of the colonized. The divine appear-
ances of the Haouka reproduce and disfigure the technologies precisely to
the degree that power has made itself subjects through drill and stereotypes
in Africa. The transfer in the film works so well because chronophotography
had been the most important tool in establishing new bodily regimes in the
nineteenth century. This is repeated here in reverse. In the commentary
we hear a comparison between ritual and real colonial administration: “Ici,
l’ordre est different, ici et là, le protocole est bien le même.” The order that
the ritual dictates is the order (and the command) of the Haouka gods. The
protocol that governs how subjects (like the British subjects in the film)
should behave has the same prehistory. It is due to the dispositif by which
movements can be stored, controlled, and reproduced in the first place: the
protocol of cinematography.
Because Rouch subjected himself to the recording rhythm of the camera
– the micro-rhythm of 24 frames per second (here, unlike Bateson, he was
accurate) and the macro-rhythm of the 45-second takes – and because he
allowed the persons to depersonalize strictly according to the rules of the
Haouka in his montage, which maintained the continuity of the movements,
of the torches, of the colors, and of the relationships, he accomplished his
task. The terror that seized the audience at the screening of Les Maîtres fous
at the Musée de l’homme and that made the film unforgettable repeated and
distorted one of the strategies of possession itself: that of tying memory un-
consciously to the body by means of affects. “For it is the defeated who know
best which of the opposing tactics were irresistible”, wrote Maya Deren. In
Rouch’s film as well, the subordinated can indicate the tactics with which
they were codified to become part of a powerful empire, in order then, as
dispossessed, to flail on the fishhooks of power. The possessed persons first
Deviations 133
of all incorporate the “corporal”, the one who holds the corporeal body, then
the officers, the administration, the traffic system, and the “forbidden” other
sex, namely the wives of two dignitaries. Freedom, equality, civic fraternity,
identity, and happiness are all promised by power, capturing their subjects
with tricks that, if all goes well, leave behind weapons, quick jobs, old cars,
and sex. The Haouka ritual and the film give it a name: violence.
What shocked spectators when the film was shown for the first time
in 1955 at the Musée de l’homme was the violence with which possession
usurped the participants’ bodies: the screams, the cramps, the shaking, the
wide-open, rolling eyes, the foaming mouths, and the staggering, buckling
extremities. In the film, as Rouch shot it, the real of the bodies is made
painfully present in the imaginary of the appearance of the Haouka gods.
The gods “de la ville, de la technique, de la force” demonstrate how they
physically displace the whole person, leaving nothing left of the old remains
of the soul, which might still indicate a consciousness that accompanies
these transformations. No “je-sais-mais-quand-même”, as Leiris has seen
or only hoped for in Haiti, suggests that the possession might be simulation
or theater. The camera shows an absolute power takeover of civilization in
the interior of the nervous control function, and shows itself to be part of
the technology of civilization. The spectators were appalled, the majority
left the room in protest, but obviously not many of them were ready to
realize what was being presented: the mercilessness and horror with which
cultural technologies penetrate the body. Perhaps this is also exactly why
they fled. To a much smaller degree, but perhaps even more uncannily, this
process is repeated in the cinema. Rouch was reviled by Europeans and by
Africans, by whites and blacks, accused of having gone too far. Obviously,
however, none of the critics heard any message in this presentation of the
ritual that might have conveyed the takeover by power as a takeover of
bodies. Not consciously at least, for the message is crystal clear: “to whom
it may concern.”
What had been particularly shocking to the spectators was the depic-
tion of a dog sacrifice. In order to show that they are Haouka and thus not
subject to the order of human beings, the possessed persons eat of the dog,
which had previously been killed by the priest. Rouch did not shy away
from showing how the animal was torn apart and dog’s blood dripped
out of foaming mouths. The gods made fun of any attempt to rationalize
the horror. In one scene of anticipatory anthropology “the captain” has a
conference convened in order to pose the decisive question of whether
the dog should be eaten “raw or cooked”. In the meantime the dog is lacer-
ated further by the gods and swallowed up in large pieces. Rouch makes
134 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
no attempt to rationalize. The little dog in the film cannot even stand in
metaphorically for the allegiance with which it obeys the master’s voice,
archaically and technically. Bell and Howell. The master eats it up. When
the technology provides the rhythm, there is no more faithful allegiance,
but only involuntary loyalty. The violence is the intrusion of the real, not as
the making visible of the apparatus on the screen, as Baudry had imagined,
but as the twitching of the bodies, the real of which sweeps up once again
against colonial codification in the technically controlled space. Rouch
cinematically doubles the space of symbolic violence, thus bringing it into
the screening room. That was unbearable. It is said that Lacan left the room
without a word along with all the others.
The demented paths of the gods provided the film’s appeal to psycholo-
gists, anthropologists, and cinéastes. It was well received in these circles,
where the last trance left to central Europeans was being organized in
the fifties: the mass mobilization on the streets and the air with the goal
of forgetting oneself for a couple of days. “Ici, l’ordre est different, ici et
là, le protocole est bien le même.” The film, which the gods of the cities,
of technology, and of violence had commissioned and in which the gods
bloodthirstily show that they recognize no taboos, received the first prize in
Venice in 1957 in the popular category of ethnography, geography, tourism,
and folklore. “Voici enfin l’Afrique en vacances.”
An almost uninterrupted commentary runs on the soundtrack through the
entire film, which is normally an effective method of establishing distance
between the images and a secure order outside the film. For instance, we
have confidently watched lambs die in dozens of films about Jesus. Rouch’s
commentary, however, is unsettling even at the level of language. It emerges,
like the images, in the mutual displacement of the participants and repeats
the African fabulation. Rouch reports that the speech of the possessed was
a speaking in tongues: “Une langue, qu’on peut interpréter mais qu’on ne
peut pas traduire mot à mot.”35 At first he has the speeches translated into
into an African language and then conveyed in French afterwards. The
commentary that is added in the final version was spoken by Rouch in a
crazed state that he himself calls glossolalia, and that ordered him in the
darkness of the projection to the microphone in the recording studio. In the
studio Rouch repeats what befalls the good workers at Accra when they are
seized in the ritual of the gods. In the space of the recording studio, in which
the projected images set the rhythm of the speech, in which the foreign
speech of the gods is taped as the movements of one’s own tongue, in which
one’s own voice returns distorted and alienated as the carrier of technically
processed, foreign speech, consciousness takes leave, a consciousness that
Deviations 135
might have been able to imagine where the ego might be. The physical is
left to the laws of technology. It carries the mental functions in place of a
feedbacking consciousness. The glossolalia that Rouch reports seems only
to have an anecdotal character. Technically it corresponds exactly to the
experiences of our present culture: Gods only come into the studio.36
The god that we are presumably waiting for in the film is the incarnation
of the camera-man: the figure that inevitably belongs to the personnel of a
colonial troupe. This even seems to be signalized when above the “general
secretary, at the termite hill-royal palace” an old cinema program is filmed,
Le Signe du Zorro, the film about the revenge of the disinherited. Only when
the locomotive driver blows through the image, and the governor berates
those present in French, when Captain Malia from the Red Sea stumbles
through the scene like Groucho Marx in the British “slow march”, Madame
Lokotoro inaugurates and desecrates statues while wearing an elegant sum-
mer dress, and the priest Mountyeba chats at the alter like a radio reporter,
do we notice that the camera-man is also on the scene and is shooting as
the kino-eye. We as spectators suddenly notice that we do not comprehend
the gaze that rips us out of our anchoring by seeing; only afterwards can
be reconstruct it. The camera is our consciousness on the scene. By techni-
cally implementing the gaze on oneself, Rouch repairs to the level and the
program of the gods. He transforms himself by filming. What our gaze
identifies on the scene is a technically equipped white man, and thus, like
all the other gods present, a Black, a Negro, a Noir, who understands his
ego as other through the many interconnections and relations. What we
ourselves see are the pictures of a cameraman who has linked himself and
the camera to a cinematic occupation of the world. A maître fou. In Vertov’s
tradition Rouch has linked himself to the maîtres fous. In Vertov’s tradition
he has become a black: “In many of my films I freed the camera from its
prescribed usage, “converted” it. In today’s capitalist-industrial-socialist
world there is no other way to make films – you have to use the camera in
ways that were not intended or prescribed.”37
At the end of the film Rouch’s commentary speaks of the fact that pos-
session might be a cure for some people, with which the Africans might be
able to integrate into the system of normality. At the end, this sentence is
introduced somewhat rhetorically: Has the camera not already shown how
its remedy works? Isn’t the film about how to use media in a way that they
become remedies? The gods of the city, of technology, and of violence have
shown us this unambiguously.
Much like Deren in her comparison with hysteria, Rouch also designates
possession as a way of dealing with states that are merely considered
136 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
Diagnosis had slid into functionality; whoever could set it in motion had
explained it.
Raising reproduction to the level of explanation initially meant that
the goal of treatment was no longer therapy, but controlling the illness.
Freud will not follow his teacher in this. Charcot’s oracle, that no theory
would prohibit what happened in the clinic from existing – “ça n’empêche
pas d’exister” – is something that Freud only pointed out in response to
the search for an “it” in the hysterics’ spells, an untouched “ça” that had a
mysterious, disturbing, and modifying influence on consciousness, which
Freud wished to examine. Freud praise of his teacher, however, was a
paraphrase of Charcot’s sentence:
For Charcot, theory was first and foremost the “known like” of clinical
practice, which made hysterics publicly mad time and again, and especially
Tuesday for Tuesday. The transfer of the symptomatic order to the bodies
of the patients was the ideal of the diagnostics, although what was missing
in Charcot’s methodology was exactly any description of the “means of
reproduction” that he claimed to have at hand.
How the transfer of signs to the bodies takes place, how the oddly
reciprocal interferences of desire were performed as regular mechanics
or dramaturgies between body and soul, was something that Charcot did
not want to know anything more about – in contrast to Freud, who will
later get his first indications toward a theory of the unconscious from this
relationship. Charcot only submitted evidence that “it” did not let itself be
disturbed.
According to Foucault, the technological and discursive apparatus that the
various scientific and related services at Salpêtrière made available belongs
to a third epoch of truth production, which became established at the end
of the eighteenth century. Truth is generated in the experiment, and thus
must be repeatable. In the laboratory that was Salpêtrière, the techniques
of hypnosis and suggestion were refined to the point that the ill persons
obviously did what they assumed to be the demands of the doctors without
resistance. The hysteric, according to Foucault, “herself retranscribed the
effects of medical power in forms that the doctor could describe in terms
140 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
Any one who has studied hysteria as an epistemological effect since then has
had to be prepared for feedback mechanisms with one’s own scientific work.
During the 1980s, when the first effects of new electronic developments
were breaking into ordinary life, coinciding with a crisis in theoretical
feminism and instigating a discussion about femininity and representation,
a renaissance in the historiography of hysteria began in which the visual
presentation of hysterics, as Brouillet’s picture shows, was examined as a
staging of gender difference. Hysteria was declared the hospitalization of
a desire that, following the dictates of the individual hysteria researcher’s
discipline, could be analyzed as relations of media, power, and gender.
Once Charcot’s reorganization of the body had been established, it was
seen that his experiments had not only been affairs between doctors and
patients, but manifestations of a new science of the body. This presumed
the development of new optical devices that got their finishing touches in
the laboratories of physiologists and replaced a geometric optics with a psy-
chological one. The technical developments appeared to the tableau painter
André Brouillet as a crisis of his own profession at the sector of imaging, and
he had treated them in his picture of Charcot’s lesson. Alongside the first
staging to shows the construction of typical femininity in the male space
of medicine, a second staging lies hidden in the image, one about seeing
and being seen, about becoming-visible and remaining-hidden: about the
chemical-physical machinery behind the stage that allows for the staging
and makes the truth from Salpêtrière available to science.
Charcot, the director on the small stage of hysteria, is himself only a bit
player in a much more broadly encompassing reform of the medicinal gaze.
Brouillet’s picture shows that Charcot is also only another person to look at
in this play, exactly like the helpless patient and the heroically composed
assistant. Behind the doctors’ heads, that is, at a site that the three women
in the picture could see if they had not lowered their heads or had not shut
their eyes in helplessness, hangs a picture, painted after a photo from the
photography archive at Salpêtrière: it is the picture of a woman whose body
forms an arc stretched out backwards from her head to the tips of her toes.
In the synopsis of the hysterical poses that Richer had prepared as drawings
based on the photographic models of the doctor Paul Regnard, this is the
“arc de cercle”, the “great hysterical arc” or “das Gewölbe” [“the arch”], as
Sigmund Freud translated more beautifully because more Kleistian. This
arc represents the highpoint in the course of the hysterical attack in the
order of Salpêtrière. Better yet, it presents it. In the hall of the clinical
presentations the “great hysterical arc” counters that all the patients are only
doppelgängers of the recorded originals, in which poses and bodies coincide,
142 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
full, in a white apron tied at the waist and a black silk cap: the traditional
garb of an assistant doctor. This was simply presumptuous, since Londe
was not a physician, he was a trained chemist and amateur photographer
and earned, at the time the picture was painted, 1200 francs a year, roughly
the equivalent of an office boy’s salary.13 Thus while the facial expression
of most of the audience and even of Charcot were due to an instantaneous
photograph taken with a quick camera, which Londe had developed, Londe’s
portrait is the result of hours of sitting in the studio: a character study in
the manner of the old painters. This difference is as difficult to make out
in Brouillet’s illustration as is the reproduction of the “arc de cercle” on the
wall as being the result of photographic recording technology. Nonetheless,
Brouillet placed the destitute Londe in the position of the benefactor, for
the painter knew better than anyone that without Londe’s photographic
constructions, not only would this picture not have been possible, but
there also could not have been any “Leçon clinique à la Salpêtrière” in the
1880s. Londe’s techniques had completely restructured the medicinal gaze.
Brouillet’s picture is secretly an homage to the new era of recording and
depiction, which had become established at Salpêtrière since 1884.
Brouillet and Londe knew from their own experience that the disparate
eras of recording that were hidden in the picture were the key to mastering
madness. The mastery of the neurologists was due to the technologies and
procedures of depicting an illness, which showed no visible physical lesions,
it was “sine material.” Charcot had prided himself in being able to direct a
gaze that visually defined the forms of nervousness. His methodological
problem consisted in being able to document this gaze. Charcot’s clinical
practice can be distinguished from the experimental medicine of Claude
Bernard’s school, the famous vivisector, in that the patients were monitored
and observed, their symptoms catalogued and systematized in order for
the data recorded to be compared posthumously in the autopsy with the
nervous-physiological findings in vivo. According to Charcot, the clinical
rooms at Salpêtrière were not laboratories, but a
women”15 for poor people were treated free of charge at Salpêtrière provided
they committed every part of themselves, including their nervous systems,
to the institution – functioned as a hysteria exhibition only so long as
the exhibited objects were still alive. In the pathology in which Freud
had worked, neither lesions nor forms of trembling could be discerned.
Although Charcot had stylized himself as a seeing, as “visuel”, he could not
provide any proof for his unerring diagnostic gaze to the patients. This is
why the throne of science remains empty in Brouillet’s painting. In place of
a scientist there is the technology that makes the new knowledge possible
in the first place.
In 1882, when the parliament approved the budget for a new professorship
in neurology under Gambetta, the chemist Albert Londe was originally
hired as préparateur for the museum. When he started he found a photo
lab there, which he called “prehistoric”,16 and promptly set about renovating
it and equipping it with new machinery. Stages and mounting devices for
lighting patients were brought to his light-workshop, and the darkroom was
brought up to the latest standards. Londe developed a half dozen camera
prototypes, and in 1893, when Charcot was already dead, the (in)famous
“Piste de la Salpêtrière” was installed outside, on which the patients had to
complete long courses of movement in front of a serial photographer and
even later in front of a film camera.
With this “photographic service”, as his division was officially called,
Londe had greater visions than simply the task of depicting patients. Ulti-
mately it was not Londe that had introduced photography to Salpêtrière.
Long before he started his photographic service the famous volumes of
photographs made by his predecessors, the Iconographie photographique
de la Salpêtrière, were already in the laboratory. The three volumes had
been brought out between 1876-1879 by the neurologists Bourneville and
Régnard. But these photographs were, as Bourneville wrote in the foreword
to the first volume, merely illustrations of the medical gaze, and had only
been printed because “our excellent master Monsieur Charcot” encouraged
doctors “to publish the observations gathered by us in his halls, illustrating
them with photographs taken by Monsieur Régnard.”17
Londe, on the other hand, did not wish to illustrate the medical gaze, but
to thoroughly modify it. In “prehistoric” research, photography itself played
no scientific role. Londe, however, envisioned an epistemological function
for the new technology. The photograph itself was supposed to be the test
case, the proof, in the sense of evidence and of the artistic copy, “épreuve”,
of the illness hysteria, because it discovered things that the naked eye could
not discern. Just as the photographer Londe is asserted to be a physician in
Compressions 145
Brouillet’s painting, the value of the photo lab is also meant to be raised to
being a scientific laboratory.
The moment of Londe’s attempt at a power grab was opportune. In the
1870s Jean-Martin Charcot had worked on localizing various nervous ill-
nesses in the brain. Since 1878 he had examined and systematized hypnotic
states, eventually presenting his research at the Académie des sciences in
1882, thus achieving scientific recognition for hypnosis. In his opening
lecture of a series in the winter of 1885, at which Sigmund Freud most likely
also participated, Charcot confronted his listeners with a new, unresolved
medical problem:
But you are aware, gentlemen, that there still exists at the present time
a great number of morbid states, evidently having their seat in the nerv-
ous system, which leave in the dead body no material trace that can be
discovered. Epilepsy, hysteria, even the most inveterate cases, chorea,
and many other morbid states which it would take us too long to enumer-
ate, come to us like so many sphinx, which deny the most penetrating
anatomical investigations. These symptomatic combinations deprived
of anatomical substratum, do not present themselves to the mind of the
physician with that appearance of solidity, of objectivity, which belong
to affections connected with an appreciable organic lesion.18
Only Londe was able to provide the “material trace that can be discovered” of
hysterical illnesses, as the material trace on a photographic plate, thus advanc-
ing both hysteria to an illness with verifiable and reproducible symptoms,
and photography to a scientific method at the same time. Londe’s technical
invention made the epistemological revolution possible. The material trace
that he sought was not to be found under the surface of the body, but in the
folds of the temporal extension of its process. Time had to be manipulated,
enlarged, taken under the magnifying glass, in order to get to the bottom of
hysteria. The decisive shift in Londe’s methodology, as opposed to the old shots
by Régnard, was the drastic shortening of exposure times. In the 1870s, that
is, at the time when the first Iconographie photographique de la Salpêtrière
had been photographed, the usual exposure time in interior spaces lasted
between 15 and 30 minutes. In 1880 Londe’s exposure of an external shot with
45 seconds got a great deal of recognition, around 1884 “temps de pose”, as
they were called in French from the perspective of those photographed, under
one second were still quite rare.19 Only in 1886 did Londe define the snapshot
as a photograph that was exposed for less than ¼ of a second. Most of his
spectacular inventions and improvements in photographic technology – a
146 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
new coating for the plates, new circular shutter techniques, the chronophoto
apparatus, and ever the improvement of his lenses – served to reduce the
exposure time for the purpose of instantaneous photography.
Londe’s technical inventions were the requirement to open up a new field
of research at Salpêtrière with the objectivity of scientific methods: the logic
of the involuntary and the unconscious in human movements. The short
exposure times and intervals with which Londe’s camera depicted moments of
movement could no longer be consciously perceived. This altered the diagnos-
tics of hysteria absolutely. The “system of relations” [“réseau”]20 between the
various discursive, institutional, or iconographic elements, which could only
be created thanks to instantaneous photography, captures the unconscious
as a “mental trace that can be discovered” by scientists for the first time.
This was something that Régnard’s “prehistoric” photographs with their long
exposure times could neither hope for, nor could they technically achieve it.
In a certain respect, what happened in Londe’s studio was nothing more
than in the “composite portraitures” of Francis Galton or of bertillonage, the
facial photography developed by the director of the photographic service of
the prefecture of Paris, Alphonse Bertillon, which quickly became mandatory
for European police departments.21 A person was photographically reduced
to a portrait view and a brief moment of posing in the simplest time-space
coordinates. Such shots were made in series with multiple exposures of
individuals, and then assembled by the eugenicist Galton as familial groups
or by Bertillon as criminal groups, so that types began to emerge with char-
acteristic deviances, visible at first glance. These could then be determined,
numbered, and transmitted by telegraph to aid in manhunts.22 Londe showed
how something similar could also be made visible in neurology, something
that only existed in the series of photographs: the facies, typical expressions
of an illness that could not otherwise be materially detected.
The trick images are engraved into the physician’s memory, the picture of
the illness emerges in the photo lab: “C’est ainsi, en particulier, que dans
les maladies du système nerveux, on a pu établir des types rigoureusement
Compressions 147
The only way out of this practice based on devices, which Londe called
“mauvais à priori”, 27 was to develop mechanisms and equipment that
148 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
from the object being observed, and to value depiction over observation. To
photograph meant to store and process an event for science.
Hysteria is therefore rightly called a sickness of representation.31 Physi-
cians at Salpêtrière had turned the iconography of hysteria into an object
without reflecting on the separation of their object of examination from
the patients. As long as they might stare at them, they could only learn as
much about them as an astronomer who stares into the sun and only sees
the flickering of his own eyes. The work of the photographer consisted in
inventing techniques that necessarily resolved the transferal of the moving
body to the image so true to nature and so scientifically that, on the one
hand, clear information could be gained from raving and trembling bodies,
and on the other hand, nothing of the bodies was missing in the images. The
technical service is the condition for the discursive order that Charcot was
able to set up in “all the wilderness of paralyses, spasms and convulsions.”32
Even in Salpêtrière, the Masters of Madness could only appear and keep the
protocol because the Master of Technology gave the “ordre” of the course.
By means of the instantaneous photograph, the meshes of the networks,
the physical and the sensual can be captured as signs and organized, decid-
edly finer. No longer just poses and gestures, but also involuntary move-
ments and also a frothing and vibrating can thus be fixed and systematized
in order to render a diagnostic judgment about the unconscious. A realm
of raving and mad movements beyond scientific ascertainability, in which
both male and female hysterics could send rebellious messages and cries for
help to all concerned through their attacks and behavior. While exposure
times of ½ hour or even ½ minute can still be described as an authentic
personal statement, self-showing, self-expressing, quick photography in
milliseconds seizes evidence from movements in moments that are so brief
that the patients do not notice, much less have the chance to react. Since
Londe the drama of hysteria is that utterly everything has been requisitioned
for the processing of signs with the new medical camera gaze. Even the
mad productivity of the hysterics’ parodies, acting, and performances, like
Charcot’s stage work, has to let itself be synchronized and processed by the
new cameras. What we see, read, and know about hysteria emerges in photo-
graphic networks. What can be captured as the message of male and female
hysterics had previously passed through the processes of technical storage,
as they can graph and corresponding dramatize physical expressions in the
realm of the millisecond in the case of instantaneous photography.
In his 1976 study, psychoanalyst Lucien Israel examined hysterical
symptoms “dans la ‘pathologie’ relationnelle de l’hystérie”,33 stressing the
difficulty of separating message and medium in hysteria:
150 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
the right to the left arm by using a magnet: “Mlle Wittman, transfer d’une
attitude au moyen de l’aimant.”
What was already presented as a chronophotographic series, as a con-
tinual depiction of a course of movement, however, in reality still belongs
to the era of the pose. Londe later admitted that the individual exposure
time amounted to a second in each case, that the suspension of movements
therefore lay with Mlle Wittman, and was not any chopping up of fluid
motion by the camera. But on whichever side of the lend the intermittent
twitches were made: the photographic preparation of reality by means of
photography is entropic, no event can be reconstructed from the photo-
graphic manipulations. This is something that photographers know as well
as painters do, when they offer themselves as chroniclers.
It was only the next camera that Londe built that finally allowed for
technical chronophotography strictly speaking, that is, serial shots in which
the individual photo is taken independently of particular and regular time
intervals. Instead of two lenses, nine lenses were arranged in a circle on a
plate, “qui nous permet de faire neuf épreuves successives à des intervalles
de temps rapprochés.”38 Nine times, one after the other, the photographic
plate captured a snapshot of the patient as she turned in new steps behind
the lens: the real Revolution Nr. 9 of the history of the sciences. These new
recordings of hysterical attacks took place in fractions of seconds, in periods
of time in which patients could no longer simulate because the short inter-
vals allowed for no conscious reactions, in the realm of the technological
unconscious. That means that male and female hysterics, even if they were
familiar with photographic services, could not turn the attack of illustrative
technology to their self-image. The random shots caught something real,
which could not merge itself with any imaginary image. The doctors of the
Salpêtrière could organize what became visible on the photos as they saw fit.
In the nine-lens camera a battery provided an electrical impulse to the
mechanism that controlled the shutters, while the impulse in turn was kept
in rhythm by a metronome. Thus, the moments of movement that were
photographed were initially determined by a unified division of time. This
meant that a technology was implemented in the “appareil photo-électrique”
that had otherwise been the domain of the doctor during inducing hypnotic
or hysterical attacks: the induction of trance by a rhythmic pattern. In this
case, it belonged to the mechanism of the apparatus itself. The tick-tock of
the metronome set the rhythm that set the pace for the patients’ nervous
crises while at the same time setting the rhythm by which the crisis would
be recorded. Subjective and objective in the recording shifted positions
with every stroke.
152 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
The time in which Londe was taking his first series with this “appareil
photo-électrique à neuf objectifs” corresponded exactly with the shift in
Charcot’s research from localizing nervous source of the disease in the brain
to the study of illness as “sine materia.” In the foreword to his translation of
Charcot’s lectures, Freud, observing that Charcot had left the research into
the organic studies of neural diseases, wrote: “At about the same time at
which Breuer was carrying on the ‘talking cure’ with his patient, the great
Charcot in Paris had begun the researches into hysterical patients at the
Salpêtrière which were to lead to a new understanding of the disease.”39 It
was this camera that made is possible to store the neuroses – at any rate
it also made it irrelevant to have the cooperation of the male and female
hysterics, and had made an allegory of the rapport between doctor and
patient. As such it was examined further in the iconographic research into
hysteria. The engineer in doctor’s clothing, who refined the power over
madness, remained unobserved as an assistant.
Because the single frames of the camera with nine lenses on the 13 x 18 cm
photographic plate were tiny, Londe was developing a prototype for another
camera at the beginning of the 1880s. Its twelve lenses would be overlapped
so that the square images would almost entirely fill up the wide format of
the photographic plate. With this prototype Londe delivered his medical
masterpiece in 1884: He photographed the patient Rosa Guillot, whose
unusually fast attacks, lasting for only seconds, could not be identified by
any of the resident doctors at Salpêtrière. With his chronophotographic
series Londe showed that the crises could indeed be classified as epileptoid
hysterical in the synoptic pattern of Salpêtrière. When, a few weeks later,
the crises slowed down and Rosa Guillot’s neurosis could be identified
by the naked eyes of the doctors, Londe was shown to be correct with his
photographic slow-motion diagnosis, and he was credited with the first
application of photography that was not merely for depiction, but could aid
in diagnosis. 40 Did he feel vindicated after this triumph for appearing in
the garb of the assistant doctor when he sat for the painting in the artist’s
studio?
The serial camera with twelve lenses also chopped up the image of the
body, which might still have been a draft of the hysterics’ “ego”, into a clinical
picture that consists of sections of twitching that cannot be consciously
controlled, and that had appeared indistinguishable to the naked eye of
the doctors and students. In the 1870s, when “walking through our halls”
with his students and assistants, Charcot, the physician, had incited this
in order to sharpen the medical gaze in the muddle of the body:
Compressions 153
At first glance one might well think that a monotonous spectacle of its
gazes was present. In fact, if one is satisfied with a superficial look, the
symptom of trembling seems to be identical or nearly the same in all the
women. Only one factor is remarkable, namely the changing intensity and
distribution that the rhythmic vibrations presenting by their members. 41
After processing with the photographs, after 1885, Charcot was able, ac-
cording to Freud’s own memories of the “rounds with his senior in one
of the departments of the Salpêtrière”, to bring some order to “all the
wilderness of paralyses, spasms and convulsions for which forty years
ago there was neither name nor understanding”, 42 or simply no recording
technology.
A short time later, Londe’s works was also publicly recognized: At the
beginning of 1885 Charcot illustrated his depiction of the case of Guinin, a
male hysteric, with a 12-part chronophotogrpahic series of Londe’s. Charcot
presents himself here as quite as aesthete of hysteria and he underscores its
beauty precisely through the illustrated example of a male body:
This elevation of photographic methods over clinical ones must have been
irksome to Charcot, and he took his revenge by relegating photography to
the second tier of aesthetics in relation to clinical work. In a counter move,
Londe had representatively enhanced his own value through painting and
might have been relieved that his methods did not have to withstand any
serious scientific controls, for he had readily delivered illustrations that did
not serve the cause of science, but the power plays of the physicians. The
photo series of Guinin’s attack in twelve images, arranged as if they were
taken from a single plate, was just as manipulated in time as the series of
Blanche Wittman had been. Londe had compiled it in the photo lab from
two different series of twelve in order better to simulate the aesthetics of
the attack. Charcot’s verdict, that hysterics “simulate without any particular
intention.[…] and cultivate the art for their own sake”, 48 goes for both the
medial staff as well as the photographic personnel. Simulation is the method
per se at Salpêtrière. And Londe’s time compression, achieved through the
technology of photography, successfully replaced all the physiotherapeutic
compressions, braces, and brackets with which the hysterics’ bodies had
previously been fixed in the clinic.
Unperturbed by all of this, Londe pursued his wish to perfect the photog-
raphy of the involuntary and the unconscious as a scientific method vis-à-vis
medical practice. For example, he exposed some patients for so long that
Compressions 155
The technical instructions regulate the classical course of the attacks, which
can be seen in the photo series; they are the result of a particular dramaturgy
of time. The seemingly regular and continuous course of movement that
can be seen in the pictures is already carried over into compressed and
expanded time periods. The schematic attacks of the series, which mark the
medical gaze, are compiled from chronophotographic slow-motion and time
lapse. Unlike the series by Marey and Muybridge, Londe’s series allow for no
inferences about the temporal course of the patients’ movements, because
each shutter can be set differently. On the other hand, the attack of Guinin,
simulated in the photographic sense, is the best example of this. Of the twelve
photos on the print, those with the quickest course of movement (pictures
3, 5, 12) are shot with the shortest exposure and the shorted intervals, while
the pictures of the “arc de cercle” (pictures 6, 7) are taken in extremely large
intervals, for the poses lasted from five to ten minutes. These temporal
distinctions can no longer be extracted from the sequences of images.
Compressions 157
Jules Etienne Marey, Movements of a tightrope walker taken by Londe with his twelve lense camera,
1893 Société de Photograhie. In: Marta Braun, p. 88.
Part III
1. Mental Apparatuses
Least interesting of these shivers are the ones with a perfectly steady frequency,
no variation to them at all. The next-to-least interesting are the frequency-
modulated kind, now faster now slower depending on information put in at the
other end, wherever that might be. Then you have the irregular waveforms that
change both in frequency and in amplitude. They have to be Fourier-analyzed
into their harmonics, which is a little tougher. There is often coding involved,
certain subfrequencies, certain power-levels-you have to be pretty good to get
the hang of these.
– Thomas Pynchon, 1973
Experimenting
analyze this shivering with no problem, belong to the 1940s. This is when
reflexology, chemistry, and electronics formed the unholy alliance to which
Pynchon dedicates his novel.
During the forties experimental artists in the USA – including Sydney
Peterson and James Broughton, Marie Menken and Willard Maas, Kenneth
Anger and Norman MacLaren – began making films about the sensations
and feelings that occur when human bodies are confronted with non-human
technologies. In the forties Maya Deren wrote articles about film art for a
living, working day and night with the help of Benzedrine proscribe by Max
“Dr. Feelgood” Jacobson, the side effects of which would eventually kill her.
Norbert Wiener, also on Benzedrine, calculated how airplane trajectories
could be predictable for human brains, commissioned by the Army.
Technologically recording and systematizing “feelings and emotions”
and then inducing them again in the nervous system in order to control
and manipulate them: this was a focal point of the research on human
beings as it was being carried out by the military-medical complex of the
nineteenth century in preparation for the twentieth. The idea of a freely
mobile spirit on the one hand and the simple reach of the technically or
chemically manipulated nervous system on the other are ultimately only
still linked by historical-critical paranoia, by a thought: pensiero. Eddie
Pensiero, whose Benzedrine perception is therefore brought up to speed by
all available means, reports the history of his own nervous condition with
the novel’s catalog of shivers, shortly before his visual center becomes so
quick that it can decode the stroboscopic messages of Byron the Bulb: … you
pretty much have to be on speed to get the hand of these. The bulb in turn
can only express itself because it is has entered into a nervous-electrical-
circuit feedback with a human being who engages its electrical generator. Its
flickering therefore not only simulates the heart of all cinema projections,
in which devices and nerves also have to be interconnected in intermittent
movements so that film stories can be told. Its flickering also tells the newer
story of the human body, which has become wired with medical test devices
and is in a feedback loop to the prurient bundle of nerves when it is either
analyzed or – in the experiment – depersonalized.
At the end of the nineteenth century the cinema as an entertainment
apparatus declined in physiological research. At the beginning of the history
of cinema, not only are there the optical devices that had been improved in
the nineteenth century, which could be manufactured in mass with more
precise lenses.2 At the beginning of the history of cinema there are also
devices that had been developed in the fields of physiology and psychology
in order to examine perception itself as a way of treating impulses in the
164 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
nerves. The goal of all the inventions of the technicians in the laboratory
was to record bodily functions through observation as much as possible
without blurriness and to store the data for later objective analysis. Over
the course of the medical attempts to investigate the functions of the mind
and perception, bodies were newly organized according to the findings of
nerve physiology, charted, interpreted, and in the process they changed
perception itself. Recording devices were at one and the same time also
machines for staging. This side of experimental physiology and psychology
is also part of the history of cinema.
The technical prehistory of cinema, the history of cinematic perception,
and finally the change to the body itself through the recording systems
are three different aspects of the same process: apparatuses, nerves, and
their physiological interfaces are linked into media. The individual has
to enter into relations of perception that can be no longer be consciously
controlled. In the 1940s experimental films from the USA are putting just
these three components of media communication – technology, perception,
and corporality – up for negotiation. They experiment with the feedback
between bodies and devices, with the limits of the body that are meant
to separate internal from external and that had become permeable in the
impulse-reaction circulation of the nerves. Experimental films thus always
introduce fragments of a psycho-physiological or psycho-technological his-
tory of cinematography. This is why the simple history of cinema by military
devices, as Virilio wrote, should be compared with, or better yet, included
in a medical one.3 It will be able to show that the term experimental in the
genre definition of many films can be correctly related to the methods of
physiology and psychology in the nineteenth century.
Shocking
In his essay about Baudelaire, Walter Benjamin describes the “shock” of the
photographic process as a technical realization of the power of destruction,
which it would be remembering under the conditions of modernity, because
there would no longer be any experience to be had in it. The human being
is aligned to the machine and, as Benjamin writes with horrible precision
in 1939, it is “part of society’s preparation for total war that training is shift-
ing from techniques of production to techniques of destruction.”4 These
preparations for war had already been in progress for a long time. In the
nineteenth century “technology.[…] subjected the human sensorium to a
complex kind of training. There came a day when a new and urgent need
Mental Appar atuses 165
Over the course of the century the methods of getting experience went
under the skin and perforated the body. The first film experiments that
physiologists created at the end of the nineteenth century are simple con-
tinuations of those experiments with cut-up body parts in the laboratories,
and they served the same purpose: to reanimate what had previously been
destroyed for the sake of the experiment. This includes 35mm films that
Ludwig Braun made in Vienna, first on artificially animated dog’s hearts
and then, in 1898, on “live, exposed human hearts”, 14 just like Charles
François-Franck’s films about artificially induced reflexes on the leg of an
anaesthetized dog.15
In the clinical practice of the eighteenth century, as Michel Foucault
has demonstrated, the “first scientific discourse concerning the individual
had to pass through the stage of death.”16 The triumph of physiology in the
nineteenth century can thus be described as a renewed interconnection
of dead or dissected body parts and as the successful reanimation of a
harmonic and balanced body. But the bodies, according to the experimental
experience in the laboratory, are not the same ones as before. The experi-
mental set-ups left behind traces, which became part of the body that they
were examining. Without the media that extended these apparatuses into
our bodies and – as Marshall McLuhan more positively observed – that
extended our bodies into the apparatuses, no experience would have been
possible in the twentieth century. Pynchon lets Pensiero think that only a
brain on speed can create the connections between laboratory experience
and shivering. There is, however, a bit of data on this paranoia in the history
of science that might also illuminate brains that run somewhat slower.
Registering
Wilhelm Wundt in 1879 in Leipzig. The question of the mind was no longer
a big issue, but it also no longer had much to do with what physiologists
had previously picked apart in the experiment. In 1874 Wundt explained
that, under the term mind, psychology had made “the whole realm of inner
experience” their object. This inner experience, however, as Claude Bernard
had claimed, had been “provoked” by “external” experiments. Wundt had
to and could demonstrate his elaboration of the life of the mind with 400
pages of fundamental physiological research.18 Henceforth the mind was
seen as a complex of individual psychic functions, which were disclosed to
each apparatus used to test them in the laboratory.
The analysis of the mind continued as nerve studies. Even philosophical
institutes used technical devices to speculate. Johann Friedrich Herbart,
Kant’s successor in Königsberg in the professorship that Hugo Münsterberg
is supposed to have turned down later in favor of Harvard, proposed an order
of concepts and connections between concepts already in 1824 according
to the laws of statics and mechanics. Around 1830 microscope technologies
were significantly improved and found a process of cutting nerve tissues
from the brain in thin slices and chemically hardening them. Augustus
Volney Waller, who in 1851 had demonstrated that every nerve fiber is linked
to a nerve cell, invented a method of damaging nerves in such a way that
their channels could be traced through the brain and spinal cord due to
secondary degeneration. The functions of the mind and its paths were
suddenly revealed to be nerves.19 When Fritsch and Hitzig announced the
localization of motor functions in the cerebrums of dogs, they also noticed
that the methods produced the following result: The smaller the damage
to the brain, the more differentiated the mental functions appeared. The
thought that the “mind is a kind of overall function”, they scoffed, can only
still be accepted due to all too “colossal mutilations of the brain” in the
experiments.20 Total damage equals total mind.
Technologies and devices decide the question of the mind, and so it is
no wonder that the expert that was called from Zürich to Göttingen for
arbitration was precisely an inventor of physiological devices for research-
ing mental activity: Carl Ludwig, founder of “quantifying experimental
physiology.”21 The list of students that studied with him in Zürich, Vienna,
and Leipzig shows what influence his work had on the development of
experimental psychology in the second half of the nineteenth century:
Granville Stanley Hall, who later founded the first laboratory for psychol-
ogy at Johns-Hopkins University in the US, Ivan Mikhaylovich Sechenov,
founder of Russian reflexology, Ilya Fadeyevich Tsion, or Élie de Dyon, as he
was called in Paris, and Ivan Petrovich Pavlov as well as Ernst Mach and Paul
Mental Appar atuses 169
Emil Flechsig, who dedicated his famous inaugural speech as rector to Carl
Ludwig.22 In psychological laboratories the world over experiments were be-
ing carried out with devices built by Ludwig. American psycho-technology
and Russian psychoreflexology are indebted to Ludwig for their methods of
interconnecting nerves and apparatuses to apply isolated stimuli in order
to read and systematize result by means of the apparatuses.
Ludwig’s most important invention was the kymograph in 1846, a reg-
istration device that “set the course of all further physiological research
more than any previous observations of physiological findings.”23 As was
common at the time for physiologists, Ludwig assembled the device, which
recorded the pressure fluctuation in the central arteries and simultaneously
in the rib cage and thoracic cavity himself according to the needs of the
experiment. In the place of an observing physician, the body attached to the
apparatus could record its functions and its circumstances itself. The body
no longer had to be destroyed and killed in order to get the measurements,
but it still was injured, drilled into for the interconnection. Only then did
the immediate representation of the body appear as a graphic curve, as
can be seen beating even faster today on electrocardiographs: the rationale
for a method that “means something similar for biological research as the
alphabet for human culture.”24 If history “runs parallel with the development
of its methods of registration”25 is as true for physiology and it is for modern
biology, this is the founding act.
Defining, separating, and recording the functions were considered the
basic operations of the physiological method. With scrawlings similar to
the attempts of a first-grader to write, the bodily functions reported with
black “rubber-based ink” on a white piece of vellum paper. Samples of this
are still stored today at the Physiological Institute at the University of Turin.
Ludwig would later inscribe the back of this to his student and biographer
Angelo Mosso, who noted that the paper “designated the origins of the
graphic method in the history of science”, writing: “These first stammerings
of the heart and breast are donated to the collection of my friend Mosso. C.
Ludwig, Leipzig, August 15, 1874.” In turn, it was Mosso who designed the
ergograph in Ludwig’s tradition, with which work and exhaustion could be
measured.26 Ludwig’s change in methodology was in fact revolutionary. If
the mind could no longer speak, the body could now write. Initially it was
not possible to get anything from its scribblings without having to drill
into it. The segmentation of the functions and the organs is suspended in
the fluid script of the heart.
Étienne-Jules Marey, who had introduced graphic methods into physi-
ology in France, would dissect his studies of movement into individual
170 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
images with a photographic gun, which he also built himself. Just as script
can be cut out of discrete heartbeats, continuous movement can also be
fragmented, and with other apparatuses the horses and soldiers concerned
can learn to run again as images. Indeed, Marey is considered one of the
inventors of cinematic projection, but for him it was much more a matter of
measuring bodily movements, and he did not take the illusion of movement
on screen for the reality of the gait. Marey paid his respects to Ludwig’s
inventions in 1895, the year of the so-called birth of the cinema, when
he had to announce his death as president of the Parisian Académie des
sciences: “On lui doit l’introduction en physiologie de méthodes précises et
fécondes en progrès. Ludwig créa le premier des instruments enregistrateurs
aujourd’hui si nombreux dans les laboratoires de physiologie.”27 Marey’s own
invention from 1860, the spygmograph, with which he had the rhythm of
the human pulse recorded on a sooty cylinder, was a further development
of the kymograph, this simple device that nonetheless set up a prototypical
register for mental functions and thus established the parameters for all
future programs of measurement and training.
Going Wild
While mental activities were being tested for their chemical and physi-
cal foundations in the laboratories, states of exception of the sense and
perception organs cropped as night views of this research at the same
time: trances and raptures, erratic attacks, fits and ticks, which should
precisely have been excluded by research. It seemed as if the concentrated
investigation of a certain mental accomplishment set other nerve connec-
tions in motion as well, letting the body become confusingly muddled.
New apparatuses were constructed to determine the laws of these states
of confusion, in turn calling up other odd effects. So, right around 1848, in
the years of the great expulsion of spirits, ever new unknown phenomena
were turning up.
The appearance of feedback effects in the laboratories, when observing
nerves and observing perception were interconnected, was first seen in
the middle of the century in a famous case. In 1840, after experimenting on
himself with disturbances in seeing color and afterimage effects, Gustav
Theodor Fechner lapsed into a three-year-long trance, from he he only
awoke through a kind of self-healing that he described as rebirth, such as
it known in shamanism. A metamorphosis took place that marks every
shamanistic journey. During his illness, the physician transformed into the
Mental Appar atuses 171
As the starting-point for our enquiry, I should like to pick out one from
among many remarks made upon the theory of dreaming by those who
have written on the subject. In the course of a short discussion on the
topic of dreams, the great Fechner puts forward the idea that the scene
of action of dreams is different from that of waking ideational life. This is
the only hypothesis that makes the special peculiarities of dream-life
intelligible. What is presented to us in these words is the idea of psychical
locality. I shall entirely disregard the fact that the mental apparatus
with which we are here concerned is also known to us in the form of an
anatomical preparation, and I shall carefully avoid the temptation to
determine psychical locality in any anatomical fashion. I shall remain
upon psychological ground, and I propose simply to follow the suggestion
that we should picture the instrument which carries out our mental func-
tions as resembling a compound microscope or a photographic apparatus,
or something of the kind. On that basis, psychical locality will correspond
to a point inside the apparatus at which one of the preliminary stages
of an image comes into being. In the microscope and telescope, as we
know, these occur in part at ideal points, regions in which no tangible
component of the apparatus is situated.32
Freud, unlike the biologist Cannon, had not checked Fechner’s calculations
and detected mistakes his formulas, but, after previously having thought up
Mental Appar atuses 173
Resonating
But even for an ordinary professor, it can become bad, as Fechner noted,
when the connections in the brain are so multifaceted that perception
impulses are impeded, displaced, duplicated, and a series of asynchronous
thoughts ends up in a mad dash. Helmholtz was able to show how self-
awareness functions under normal circumstances, and he also provided
possible physiological explanations for confusions and trances. “My most
significant result was that the sensations of the senses are only signs to
configure the outside world, and the interpretations of them can only be
learned through experience.”40 His explanations of the physiology of percep-
tion and the achievements of the brain come ever closer, thanks to Werner
von Siemens, to communications technology. In the physiology of nerves
it had been assumed that a specific sense energy and a correspondingly
specific nerve energy determined the various perceptions of the senses.
In his On the Sensations of Tone Helmholtz showed that there were indeed
various sense organs and nerve conducts, for example, for optical or acoustic
signals, but that each individual nerve fiber can only establish different
impulse strengths. He illustrated his model with the comparison, which
176 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
the theory of the arts, I hope that I have kept the regions of physiology
and esthetics sufficiently distinct. But I can scarcely disguise from myself,
that although my researches are confined to the lowest grade of musical
grammar, they may probably appear too mechanical and unworthy of the
dignity of art, to those theoreticians who are accustomed to summon the
enthusiastic feelings called forth by the highest works of art to the scientific
investigation of its basis.”46
Helmholtz lets the art theorists know that enthusiastic feelings are
only effects and are not themselves open to scientific inquiry, and that the
physical “and that this analysis of the sensations would suffice to furnish
all the results required for musical theory.”47 No special mental activity is
needed in order to appreciate art. Helmholtz, who was passionate about all
the arts all his life had no need for any extra illusions: all that resonates in
perception are the oscillations of the world, which is something that any
dancer can verify.
Rhythmizing
Now it can be seen that even Wundt, over the course of his publications,
constructed the model of a psychic apparatus according to the prototype
of his experimental set-up. The devices from the laboratory were tacitly
projected into the interior of people. In the fifth edition of his Physiological
Psychology from 1902, in which Wundt summarized his research in Leipzig,
the technical requirements of psychological measuring, the chronoscope
and the complication clock, were relegated to the third volume. The experi-
ences were supposed to no longer know anything about their prerequisites
in the set-up of the experiments. The body, moved in regular rhythms, as it
had been synchronized by the clockwork over the course of research, seems
to Wundt to represent in part the nature of the body and of consciousness.
In one of his final works, the Introduction to Psychology, which was consist-
ently reprinted up to 1922, rhythm seems to be a quality of the organism,
and thus a quality of perception itself: “Our consciousness is rhythmically
disposed. The reason of this scarcely lies in a specific quality, peculiar to
consciousness along, but it clearly stands in the closest relationship to
our whole psycho-physical organization. Consciousness is rhythmically
disposed, because the whole organism is rhythmically disposed.”51
This idea would be nice and easy if it weren’t for the fact that the rhythm
that is meant to dominate here is always based on the regular ticking of
a clock as the norm. When Wundt wrote the Introduction to Psychology
as a book of popular science, he replaced the complication clock with a
simple device that was available in every modest home with a piano: the
metronome. Using this device, anyone could conduct psychological tests in
self-experiments. Experiments with one’s own perception as “the entrance
of some content into consciousness – an entrance that can be in fact proved”
and with one’s own apperception as “the grasping of this by the attention”52
thus became a kind of parlor game. When the self-experimenter gets going,
that is, comes into “attitudes of expectation”, he or she has the experience
of a psychic (personal) achievement, which appears involuntarily. Simple,
uniform ticks become a series of strokes in which various emphases can
be perceived. From the pure ticking, a “content” suddenly enters “into
consciousness” through perception. The perception itself undergoes a
marvelous metamorphosis and transforms the world according to the
rhythm of the organs.
in the order of language even faster, that is, it can read. Within a familiar
word in a familiar language, consciousness can grasp as many as 20 elements
at a time. Wundt’s example of this is “Wahlverwandtschaften” [“elective
affinities”], which can keep together even if it is not spelled correctly.
For acoustic stimuli as well, the maximum number of events that can
be grasped is six. But integrated into a regular rhythm with three different
beats, six units of combinations can also be noted. Classical spelling literacy
and two-beat variations of a metronome ticker are appropriate to the nature
of the psyche, polyrhythms such as those in jazz or in the complicated
rhythms of voodoo rituals55 cannot be grasped by it. Central Europeans
no longer notice anything when Africans or Haitians know that the gods
are among them.
If, according to Wundt, “our means of locomotion are in a certain sense
natural pendulums”, as regular as those of the metronome, all that is lacking
is the methodology of the Kleistian puppeteers to make the connection
and provide the beat to appear as the inspiration for the gracefulness of
dance. “The limbs, which were no more than pendula, following along
mechanically, without any extra help, of their own accord”, as is described
in the marionette theater. Because the puppeteer and his mechanics cannot
be seen, what appears is the artistic and mysterious “path of the dancer’s
soul.” Wundt also staged the grace of rhythmic perception as path of the
soul simply by forgetting that the art of the movement of the soul is a trick
that allows the machinery to disappear into the wings. This as well is an
important preliminary step to staging perception in the cinema.
Wundt is also responsible for the insight that in cinema all meaning is
preceded by a physiological rhythmic structure that regulates what provides
that meaning:
It is at once obvious that these states, which we shall call for shortness
the contrasts of strain and relaxation, have the very same right to be
called feelings. For feelings, wherever they arise, accompany, as subjective
reactions of consciousness, sensations and ideas, but are never identical
with them.57
While the Germans came to the cinema attempting to optimize the mind’s
functions, the French invented it to take care of their bodies, and it is
believed that one can still recognize this difference in national film produc-
tions to this day. In 1872, after France had lost the war against Prussia and
the good citizens were shocked by the vitality, energy, and mobility of the
communards on the barricades in Paris, a patriotic movement was formed in
France that traced the catastrophic outcome of the war back to “decadence”,
to the soldiers’ instability and their weak wills. A gymnastics movement was
instigated to restore elegance and more stamina to the French. One of its
leaders was Georges Demeny, who had founded a “cercle de gymanstique” in
Paris to develop and train new bodies, which were not meant to be drilled in
any authoritarian Prussian sense, but trained in an economic French style
to optimize energy use, avoid fatigue, and learn sequences of movement in
which the organism functioned without resistance, in short, to introduce
grace in the Kleistian sense as an artificial paradise. Demeny made great
efforts to ground the new forms of movement in science and registered as
an assistant to the physiologist Étienne-Jules Marey.
After his medical practice had failed, Marey studied the physiology of
movement as an independent researcher in his own laboratory and de-
veloped his own methods to read these movements from the living body.
He wanted to liberate physiology from the anatomy that it had always
been subordinated to. While still as assistant doctor in 1859, Marey had
developed the sphygmograph, a machine to read the pulse, which became
so popular in medical practices that Marey could live off the royalties
from the patent.1 The sphygmogrpah differed from Ludwig’s Kymograph
in that it was applied on the outside, on the skin, and did not have to be
inserted under the skin. It was made of aluminum and wood, which made
it very light, and instead of working with weights, it worked with a spring
that pressed the apparatus onto the artery. Instead of using a steel pin,
Marey’s sphygmograph used a spring to write down what the change in
the receptacle dictated. But although Marey’s technologies sought to record
inconspicuous movements like the beat of a butterfly’s wings, his most
spectacular and bloodiest experiment in these years was a cardiograph
188 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
of the heart of a living horse, which he carried out together with Auguste
Chauveau, the laboratory director of the veterinary clinic in Lyon. A rubber
plunger was inserted into the cardiac chamber of a horse and transmitted its
contractions as neat three-track notations. Blood pressure in both cardiac
chambers and the beats of the heart were recorded in an “indirect” method.
The graphic notations only translated: “Nos courbes n’exprimaient donc
pas les mouvements du cœur à proprement parler; mais elles permettaient
cependant de déduire des changements de pression qu’elles traduisaient…”2
If Carl Ludwig is considered the inventor of the recording system where
bodily states leave their traces on soot or papered cylinders without the aid
of a physician, Marey was the theorist of the graphic methods of notating
body movement. Perceiving and representing motor phenomena coincided
in the body’s self-transmission mounted in the inscription machines. Marey
proposed devices and built models that allowed all possible movements
to be brought into the one-dimensionality of a single line. In the 1860s he
examined human and animal motor activity and made notations according
to intensity, duration, and form. In addition he constructed the thermograph,
the cardiograph, the pneumograph, and the myograph, which he also built in
a lighter version than Helmholtz’s prototype so as to be able to record the fine
movements of muscular tremors. Marey’s apparatuses, however, did have the
disadvantage that they limited freedom of movement when they were used,
that is, they altered the movements that they were meant to measure. Only in
1878, when he saw Muybridge’s photo series in the journal La Nature, did Marey
invent mechanisms to record living creatures in presumably free movement.
Following Jules Janssen’s model he constructed a photographic revolver,
which he tested in the summer of 1882 with his assistant Otto Lund in the
radiant light of Naples. 12 pictures could be taken on a rotating photographic
plate at 1/720 of a second, and Marey brought another 25 photographic plates
in a dark bag so he could reload his photographic gun at the beach.3 The
images made on the plates, however, were as small as postage stamps, and a
series of 12 pictures was too few to be able to determine the exact trajectory
of the bird they had photographed in its speed and direction. So Marey
altered the parameters. He built a camera with a fixed photographic plate.
Several phases of a movement were exposed over one another on the same
plate by rotating slots at 1/1000 of a second in intervals of 1/10 of a second.
In contrast to Galton, who standardized the traces of many bodies in the
contracted space of double exposure, he envisioned analyzing a body’s
course of motion in its graphic-spatial expansion. 4
Marey used these multiple exposures to create his pictures in phases,
which went against all western conventions of representation and which
Psycho -Motor Activit y 189
Later a pole was installed from which views could be recorded from a bird’s
eye perspective. In comparison to the equipment that two German physiolo-
gists put on a “test individual” in order to measure the “gait of the human
being”, Marey’s installation was very free. Wilhelm Braune and Otto Fischer
attached small discharges – although not until 1891 – and later Geissler
tubes to the test subjects, which were charged by a Rühmkorff inductor
coil. With this equipment the measurements documented nothing but the
gait that the subject could take under such cumbersome circumstances and
the difficulties of the experimental conditions. In contrast the conditions
of the light French runners could be seen as natural, although they also
restricted the illustrative apparatus of body movements from a distance.
In contrast to the moving camera work, which Londe, for instance,
was developing at the same time, every stretch in the Parc de Princes was
supplied with scales, measuring rods, and chronometric gauges so that
temporal and spatial parameters could be meticulously documented on
the photographs, that is, in fraction of seconds. The objects on the runway
were no longer bird and insects, as had been the case with Marey, but hu-
man beings, who followed a course once it was set up and could carry out
particular movements. Together with Demeny, Marey tested gymnastics
students and soldiers from the military school at Joinville. Walking, run-
ning, jumping, fencing, riding, waving flags, carrying backpacks.[…] The
military disciplines were examined, with the support of the ministry of war,
for their trajectories, their energy consumption, and training programs.
It immediately became clear in the Parc des Princes that mechanical
discipline was required for all the measurement. In cinematic tests, for
instance, a runner on the level circuit of exactly 500 meters had to interrupt
the contacts of a telegraph circuit, which were installed along the track
every 50 meters, as he passed them. Inside the laboratory an odograph
wrote down these interruptions, and thus the speed of the runner, in
corresponding zigzagging lines. The speed could only be related to the
steps of the runner because he was synchronized by a bell in the middle of
the race track. Rhythmatized from a distance, the runner ran his rounds.
“Cette sonnerie peut prendre toutes les fréquences possibles depuis 40
jusqu’à 120 coups à la minute.”6 Truffaut’s first film, about the dark side of
post-war education, was called Les Quatre Cent Coups. In the film, rhythms
and strokes characterize the authorities that structure the individual’s life
from outside. The history of discipline and the history of cinema and the
history of French society. In France, the idea that the cinema realizes its
culturalization beneath conscious perception and catches up with it again
at the level of the narrative, is always present.
Psycho -Motor Activit y 191
In dynamic tests the soldiers in the Parc des Princes were laden down
with different weights in order to calculate the relations of weight and speed,
pressure and mobility, exertion and achievement in phases of movement.
What had previously been treated as a question of character – stamina,
resolution, deportment – was, in the new French body politics, divided “dans
la marche ou dans la course” into individual physical sizes, transferred into
graphic grids, and made calculable. This in fact allowed for the creation of
guidelines for gymnastics lessons, which would be whipping people into
shape throughout the twentieth century all over Europe: “Il y a donc, pour
chaque allure, une cadence optimum: celle où la vitesse croît plus vite que
la dépense du travail.”7 There is an optimal rhythm at which achievement
increases faster than effort is made. What immediately appeared to the
Germans, following Wundt, as a natural component of their organism,
was first worked out as a scientific special form by the French, who also
let themselves go, but then was trained into the body with tambourines
and bells. A certain rhythm that makes patriots mobile: “Les premiers
résultats que nous avons obtenus one fait l’objet d’un rapport au Ministre
de la Guerre.”8
On October 15, 1888 Marey presented the first long filmstrips that he had
shot with Demeny to the Académie des sciences. They were approximately
50 centimeters long and – in order to comply with copyright – were made
of paper. What could be seen was the flight of a pigeon: Land in sight! And
a hand opening and closing. The films had to be run through the camera
between two sockets on the shutter, and held still in each case in order to ex-
pose 20 images per second with 1/500 of a second. In 1889 in Paris one could
get films coated with a gelatin-collodion layer from the Lumière company,
while the photographer Nadar bought nitrogen films from the American
competitor Eastman. But neither the filmstrips nor the machinery worked
quite right. The strips became charged with static, were too short and the
coatings were too irregular, the strips did not run evenly. The pioneers of
cinematography, Marey, Reynaud, and Edison did indeed exchange notes
at the World’s Fair in 1889, but Marey had little interest in perfecting the
illusion of motion, for the apparatuses that he manufactured were meant
to serve the purpose of analysis. Nonetheless, although he was merely a
technical agent in the history of cinema, he had made the decisive step
toward filming human beings: namely, qualities that had previously been
considered mental were physiologically analyzed and then recombined.
Inner values were measured as external. The movement of the soul from the
outside, as is common in archaic and shamanistic trance techniques, could
now also be realized in the heart of Europe through cinematography. While
192 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
neurophysiologists had established that the nervous system could use body
movement to transform data from the surroundings into information, while
physiologists measured movement in anatomy in order to gain information
from it about the body and character, while soldiers ran their exhausting
laps for the text on the drum of the sphygmographs, what appears at the
interface of these undirected, but every finer data nets is the cinema, which
can turn movements and the transmission of stimuli into psychological
stories. What becomes clear on Marey’s testing grounds is that, since 1871,
the “armament of the souls” is a complex that combines apparatuses, bodies,
nature, and technology into a new aggregate. This is why Marey is the model
for all media theory that sees running such machines as the process that
makes it impossible to distinguish our physical and psychic breathlessness.
Even Max Ernst, who assembled the Parc des Princes into a landscape of
souls in his cycle “La Femme 100 têtes”, discovered the way that the psyche
could go crazy around 1900 in the apparatuses with which it was measured.
He showed that lost souls could be found again outside in nature, which,
itself integrated into the culture machine as a technical auxiliary construc-
tion, regulated movements and forms of human bodies. He gave the subtitle
“die landschaft wird im höchsten grade unbewußt” [“the unconsciousness
of the landscape becomes complete”] to one of his pictures from 1929, in
which Marey’s track is supplemented by trap-like devices and by the verti-
cally split and displaced body of a woman. Female bodies, the woman, as
Bresson then finds, were completely missing in the measuring being done
at the historical Bois de Boulogne. Ernst imported them from other research
stations and among other parameters and, foreign and anachronistic, placed
them in the paths of the male soldiers in order to transform desire into pure
escalation of energy… while the gait at 40 and 120 beats per minute remained
constant. “Cette sonnerie…” Heralded gender difference. Hammered gender
difference. The secrets of the new souls lie outside the body, where the
naked eye sees nothing, just as on the highway to Beaune, on the way into
the Musée Marey, there is suddenly a sign with three waves signaling that
in the invisible landscape beyond the street there should be a river, called
“La femme sans tête.” A hundred years later, what Marey had recorded
conforms to every serial human being as an image of the soul, even more, it
is enough for an optical bundling of desire, as Max Ernst prophesied in his
pictures. Documents of biometry become sentimental views. The curators
of the Musée Marey in Beaune ensure that the soul has changed under the
measurement into beautiful, cryptic affectivity, unconsciously of course:
“L’homme de Marey est repéré, mésuré, millimétré, cadré […] au risque
de perdre son âme: Il préfigure les images du corps numérisées, obtenues
Psycho -Motor Activit y 193
the entry of the bullet before exploding completely as the bullet exits
the other side. Another well-known filmmaker was the medical student
Jean Comandon, whose f ilms were purchased as educational f ilms by
the UFA. In 1909 he shot a film about microbes in mouse intestines with
dark f ield illumination and in the same year a f ilm about the syphilis
virus, films show cells dancing with great tenderness. The most famous
of Marey’s successors, however, must be Charles Emile François-Franck,
Marey’s assistant at the pathological physiology department at the Collège
de France. Under Charcot at Salpêtrière in the 1880s François-Franck had
investigated circulation and localization in the brain, and had published
a book about the motor brain functions, for which Charcot wrote the
foreword. This work included film footage that, unlike with Londe, was no
longer meant to show the external movements or symptomatic expressions
of emotional stimulations, but the inner paths of transmitting stimuli.
François-Franck’s experiments are complementary to Londe’s illusionary
and imaginary images, which abstracted fragments from the living ap-
pearance of the body. François-Franck electrically anesthetized, dissected,
and reanimated animal bodies for cinematic recordings. His films are the
missing link between the experiments of Duchenne de Boulogne and the
first close-ups by D.W. Griffith and the sine qua non of all Frankenstein
movies.
Although Meliès and the Lumière Brothers are considered the actual
pioneers of film, the natural scientists, the unknown avant-gardists of film
history, developed a wealth of styles and techniques that slowly paved the
way for film into art. These include slow motion and time lapse, which make
it possible to see the movements of plants, animals, and cells, underwater
photography and the microscope, the x-ray and radio cinematography. It is
in biology films that the first artificial worlds emerge that only exist through
the fusion of camera and human perception. Among natural scientists film
was used in a filmic sense from the very beginning, and not as a replacement
for vaudeville, theater, or literature. Feelings that arise while watching the
staging of microbes in mouse intestines are undoubtedly artificial and
undoubtedly genuine.
The first artists’ films in came into being among the cinéastes in the
laboratories in France. Jean Rouch recalls how his uncle, a professor
at the medical university, regretted that in 1920 he had to fail his most
talented student in physics, chemistry, and biology. This was Jean Pain-
levé, the son of the mathematician, two-time Minister of Defense, and
Minister President Paul Painlevé. Since 1924 Jean had been publishing
Neo-Zoologisms in the journal “Surréalism”, which he had founded as a
Psycho -Motor Activit y 195
tradition, but also the structure of shooting and the gaze, which turns
floating seahorses, disoriented dogs, and sick girls into stars.
In his brilliant outline of film history, Dalí wrote:
Following the dull years during which the technique is improved the
cinema […] suddenly reaches its true golden age in the achievement of
the first materialist films of the Italian school (prewar and the beginning
of the war). I am referring here to the grandiose period of hysterical
cinema…14
3. Psycho-Drama
The films of Painlevé, Buñuel, or Dalí make it easy to forget the murderous
legacy of Marey’s inventions. Following the model of Londe, who had provided
an iconography for unconscious poses and developed dispositifs of the female
or male unconscious, film became the model and then also the substitute
for conventional medial diagnoses. “Film reproduces” was plainly stated in
the protocols, and one enthusiastic researcher among the clinicians, Emil
Kraepelin, who had completed his education with Wundt, would turn to the
cinema in relief in his Munich psychiatric practice. The cinematogram archive
would spare him the confusion and madness of the patients at his station:1
camera and away from it, clothed at first, then with bare legs, presumably
better to see the play of the muscles, and then finally nude. After a group
of adults, children are presented as well. They hobble, buckle, have trouble
moving forwards and getting through the area captured by the frame. Their
movements look even more awkward due to the mise-en-scène of their
skinny little bodies in the completely empty space of the picture.3
Marinescu’s camera stands fixed to one spot. Only the reactions of a few
patients who laugh or wave when they go to the camera draws attention to
the fact that there is someone behind the controlling gaze that the spectator
is positioned in, someone who is directing this. With the children, who
obviously attempt to evade the direction not to make eye contact with
the camera-eye, the sequence in which they walk towards the camera is
edited down to a few seconds. By using looks and gestures to point out what
surrounds the area where they are being judged, the children break through
the formal objectivity of the visual structure, and exactly this moment is
edited out in the name of science. Shortly before the cut, however, we see
them laugh briefly, presumably because they recognized someone behind
the camera. Only the cuts point to a different space, to the off-screen space
of the shot, and thus to the fragile masks of scientificity. If we watch the
images without the commentary that was added later on, the cinematic
strategy of eradicating those recorded becomes even clearer. We see name-
less, stumbling bodies shot from the side in wide-shots, largely without
faces. The young men have shaven heads. All personal characteristics that
could be manifest in clothing have been taken away from the children, with
the exception of a couple or social, or rather asocial, markings: the girls
have disheveled braids, are unkempt, their stocking hanging down. Nudity,
which is meant to reinforce the impression of physical health in Marey’s
hopping soldiers, and which by now looks a little ridiculous, reinforces the
impression of helplessness in the small patients in Bucharest, robbing them
of any human characteristic. Around 1900 medical exams were already
being used to slate children for treatment like the one that would lead
millions of so-called abnormal or pathological persons to their deaths over
the course of the century by more or less totalitarian regimes.
The iconography of annihilation in these first films by Marinescu can
be seen in all further films about ataxia or catatonia. It is correct that the
titles of films were only the names of the illness involved, for what is being
portrayed is the image of the illness with all its specific symptoms and
particularities, an image that was meant to train less experienced neurolo-
gists in diagnosis. In line with to the fact that it was not the person in need
of help, who was suffering under seizures or manifestations of paralysis,
Psycho -Dr ama 199
and whose appearance might cause fear or pity, that was represented, but
only a body befallen with symptoms, the extermination of just this body
appears as a coup de libération, which would no longer take any account
of anything human behind it or in it – a logic that is repeated in horror
films as valiant impalement and, incidentally, often as burning by body
snatchers and the living dead. Unlike in German fairy tales, it seems that
no good and enchanted soul will be able to work its way out of these film
bodies possessed by evil.
In the same year that Marinescu was shooting his f irst psychiatry
films, Albert Londe was carrying out a series of experiments with chrono-
apparatuses. Some of the ones that survived show male and female patients
on the “Piste de la Salpêtrière”, series recorded with the camera with twelve
lenses. At the end of the twentieth century, film scholars edited these im-
ages into short 35mm films to show early examples of movement analysis. To
give their film students the most authentic illusion possible of the medical
position on the “Piste de la Salpêtrière”, they even compensated for the
parallel shifts that had come about through the distances between the
twelves lenses. Didactically and artificially they attempted to created a
central perspective of unified space after the fact, which would allow for
only a single and continuous ideal gaze, whereas previously a series of
views and vantage points had only allowed for interpolating the patients’
movements. But despite the best efforts of the film scholars to create an
absolute and – as it was implemented in the history of perspective from
the beginning – self-denying gaze in a homogenous optical space, the clips
jerk and jump a little, just enough to make every student aware that these
images and these historical medical views are a carefully arranged optical
construction, exactly like every cinematic space. But it is precisely the dif-
ference between these awkwardly reconstructed not-yet-films by Londe on
the one hand and the films by Marinescu, staged in the coherent space of the
film camera, on the other, that makes it possible to trace the new sovereign
power of the camera’s gaze as visual diagnostics in medicine. The dysfunc-
tional aspects of watching movies also provide some elucidation here. If the
film historians had wanted to analyze the power, the source of which had
been concealed in the construction of the apparatuses, they would have
been better served if they had exposed the technical parameters of working
on film rather than continuing to conceal it with further manipulations.
When Londe himself later shot some footage with a film camera, he seems
to have implicitly taken the role of cinematography into account. In 1999
three of Londe’s films were restored by the Archives des films of the CNC
for an exhibition in Paris. The first showed a Cheval entraînant un moulin,
200 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
the second the Abbatage d’un bœuf, and finally the third was a cinematic
Démarche pathologique de la Salpêtrière. Training, fragmenting, staging:
there could be no more precise summary of the cinematic body politics
that reworked Marey’s measurements into dramas.
One example that shows that cinematic recordings around 1900 was
already no longer serving to analyze movement, but had become quixotic
evidence for medical diagnoses, is the film Neuropatologia by Camillo
Negri from 1908, and it can be taken as representative for the cinema that
Salvador Dalí had in mind when he rhapsodized about the “grandiose period
of hysterical cinema.”
In his clinic in Turin, Negri had himself and his assistant filmed as they
treated a woman having a nervous attack. The woman was wearing a mask,
presumably to protect her identity. Already because of this, the sequence has
an undertone of a Sadistic boudoir play, and is reminiscent of the stagings
done at Salpêtrière in which hysterics had to incarnate classic theatrical
roles or zoological forms. After the woman has been brought into the picture
she gives a brief explanation, which is of course kept from the spectator, and
its wording clearly belongs to the medical protocol. Immediately following
this she falls into a state of twitching. Negri, who is bent over her busily and
business-like, communicates during the whole shoot with the cameraman
or the technical personnel behind the camera, obviously to check whether
everything happening can be seen in the image, and what instructions to
give to get the spectacle to run its course. Toward the end of the film, when
the patient is already giving the impression of being completely exhausted,
Negri once again gives a sign to shoot and then begins to press the woman’s
abdomen against the mattress. A new attack begins. From the point of view
of the film, there is no way to tell if the woman moved on her own accord
or whether the professor’s strong push and the feathers of the bed started
up the whole movement. One might say that hysteria itself thus appears in
front of the running camera. The subject and object of the hysterical move-
ment are indistinguishable. Just as the title says, neuropathology appears
in the image as a malaise of representation, as a trick of the neurologists,
as a mise-en-scène of the cameraman, as a reaction of a female body to
diverse psychic and physical factors. Because the identity of the woman
on the screen behind the masks of the “great universal matrix”, the “Caput
mortuum” of the female, as Michel Leiris called the practice in the case of
his research in Africa, has disappeared, it is no longer her twitching and
trembling that appears on the screen, but an attack, under the guidance
of the professor. What the film shows cannot give any information about
the physiological course of the attack. Recording it on film is no longer
Psycho -Dr ama 201
measuring it for the camera, but staging it for the camera. The complicitous
looks that Negri gives over and over again to the camera confirm how sure he
can be of getting the applause of his colleagues. The scientific demonstration
merges with its interpretation by the operating specialist. At the end of the
film the two men in the laboratory are presented like actors at the final
curtain, holding up a woman between them: the weak sex, patient, diva,
object of scientific ambition, whether she shares this now or not. 4 Staging
and diagnosis run in parallel and are demonstrated with shots that are not
relativized by any accountability beyond the technical aspects of filming the
experiment. Hysteria once again appears as the overwhelming corporality
of a symbolic order. Structures that supply meaning get into the image by
means of technical workmanship. Only when the attention is shifted to
cuts, dissolves, zooms, and pans does it become clear how film and medical
orders are dramatized into social contents. L’âge d’or du cinéma hystérique.
How to distract attention away from technical workmanship was
taught by a German assistant doctor, Hans Hennes. Like Charcot, what
he cherished about cinematography was that it could reliably store and
differentiate where human objects of view behaved erratically. “Once the
footage is shot, the picture is available for reproduction at any time, the film
is always ‘in the mood,’ there are no misfires.”5 Only with time will it turn
out that “reproduction” is a euphemism for dressing. The degree to which
cinematography in Germany found its way into medical diagnostics is
documented in the thorough overview published by the medical officer and
radiologist Dr. Martin Weiser, a student of Ewald Hering’s, just after the war
in 1919. His book Medizinische Kinematographie is both a film handbook and
a scientific report, revealing a number of inventions, techniques, and tricks
that were being used by physicians with the camera. Already in 1911 one Dr.
Summer had emphasized “the value of cinematography for the differential
diagnosis of epileptic seizures against hysteria and organic diseases of the
brain.”6 The great model of all neurologist-filmmakers, however, was the
Italian brain physiologist and cinema pioneer Osvaldo Polimanti, who
had taken it on to supplement his written protocols about neurological
deficits after extirpations of certain brain areas with “successful cinematic
records.”7 Weiser emphasized – and almost in the same words that Londe
had used to formulate his visions – that doctors should make use of the
cinematic impression of the medical gaze as a “natural” perceptual method,
in distinction from writing:
to the camera. The staging reduces the madness of the patient to his illusion
in thinking that he is a human being just like the doctor and he could enter
into eye-contact with him at will. But the naked man does not have his
gaze in the space of the camera and is thus no longer the subject. Beyond
the implied diagnostic level – the analysis of nervous twitching – there is
a second level of the feud of gazes, which only one of them, technologically
equipped, knows about and survives: the medical gaze that is reinforced
by the camera. If we think that a certain horror has got a name and a face
in Dr. Kirchof’s appearance, it soon becomes clear that the technology of
filming also seeks to subordinate the gaze of the spectator to this logic
of annihilation. The staging consequently ends in a sequence that shows
the patient writing his name. We see the trembling hand, then the patient
holds up the paper and his name, now visible in full on the piece of paper,
is illegible. Even on the symbolic level, the identity of the patient is thus
definitively negated. Even more: the film suggests that the patient has
forfeited his humanity himself through his uncontrollable movements. The
film always only seems to show what is, but shots, gaze manipulation, and
montage constantly generate meaning. Given the euthanasia campaigns
during the Third Reich, the sequence, in which the man’s trembling results
in his failure to write his name, means that there is no identity here anyway.
In this discrete film sense, annihilation would only be the completion of a
process that, as the film seeks to establish in all objectivity, has long been
the patient’s destiny.9
In view of such obvious efforts at staging in medical films, it is astounding
that the scientific claim of “medical cinematography” (Weiser), that is, the
legitimacy of recording and registering bodily functions by simply storing
them on film, could hold for so long. Certainly the fact that cinematography
lies in the direct tradition of inventing methods to record physiological
movement meant that systematizing movement, and thus also formalizing
and pathologizing it, seemed to be guaranteed by film. But even if cinematic
methods of measuring by analyzing single images and sequences can be-
come an artificial synthesis of movements, various levels of staging come
forth in projection, on the screen, and the findings turn out to be imaginary
anyway. Interventions by a cinematically examining doctor can only be
reconstructed in the case of the basic stroboscopic illusion of movement if
slow motion and time lapse can be sensed or even noticed as “significant.”
All variations beneath this perceptible threshold, be they due to conscious
manipulation or to the contingent dysfunction of the apparatuses, remain
unnoticed. In contrast to the failed comparison of parallel axes mentioned
above, which made the reformation of chronophotographic analysis visible
204 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
It was not fables, forms, or colors that were raised to the quality of artworks
at the end of the last century, but excitation frequencies. Inasmuch as per-
ception had been dissected in the laboratory and examined for its individual
functions, art was no longer examined as an aesthetic phenomenon, but
also for the way it affected the nerves. Physiologists had previously analyzed
body movements and then, from outside, neurologically identified the
qualities of character. Psychologists had established the effect of images
and rhythms on the psyche. In psycho-physiological analysis art once again
became physical: a mental reaction to nerve stimuli.
One of Wundt’s students, Paul Linke, continued his experiments as
studies in cinema perception. In March 1916 he reported in Dresden on
his attempts to induce seeing motion artificially. “Dr. Paul Linke/Jena […]
breaks with the old, purely physiological views, which sought to explain the
problem of movement merely through the phenomenon of the afterimage
and amalgamation. According to Paul Linke, seeing motion in cinematog-
raphy is an illusion of identification”, was written in 1917 in Photographische
Korrespondenz.1 Linke provided the technological basis for a psychological
explanation of Wertheimer and Köhler’s experiments. Working in Wundts’s
laboratory, he had tested how many images in a series were necessary to
create the illusion of a course of movement. “What emerges from his work
is that our psyche possesses a quite astounding and previously unknown
capacity to reproduce a course of movement out of two different single
images.”2
As proof Linke had built “the tautoscope for the psychological cin-
ematographs” which put two different images from a series on screen,
“two perceptions of a face […] that have so little spatial difference from
each other that they can be identified, that is, can be referred to one and
the same object.” The two elements had to be perceived separately, but “be
simultaneously present in the consciousness, indeed, simultaneous, for
206 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
instance, like the sound of a spoken word or the rhythmic beat of the notes
of a melody.”3 Linke shows his audience that two visual impression can be
separated from one another by dark phases for so long that an afterimage
effect could no longer be considered a connector between the two images.
Stroboscopy, he demonstrated, calls on a capacity of the brain itself, so that,
as one spectator reported, “a clear impression of motion (emerges) in that
the psyche automatically supplements the phases in between.”4
Martin Weiser, who had written the first handbook for filmmaking physi-
cians, initially considered Linke’s studies to be psychological suprematism:
it only worked for points, lines, circles in black-and-white. But Linke was also
able to delude him with transformation effects: apples into pears. Weiser
remarked that Linke’s trick of affecting the brain was a typical trick form
Wundt’s laboratory: “In Linke’s experiments we must still bear in mind
that a quite specific rhythm is maintained, to which the psyche adapts.”5
Rhythm is the missing link between film and medical diagnosis, between
Ewald Hering’s panmnemism as a “surrogate science of the soul”,6 Bleuler’s
mental apparatuses, which could replace broken nerve functions, and the
stroboscopic affection of the psyche. Rhythm creates the tension in which
we are prepared to let signals in the nerves become data: expectation in
Wundt’s complex sense. Or in the sense of a certain drum rhythm, which
calls to action a certain dance in the limbs and a certain god.
Also in 1916, when the German Friends of Photography and Film were
rubbing their eyes after Linke’s screenings, in the USA the first psycho-
technical film theory was being written, The Photoplay: A Psychological
Study, and by one of Wundt’s students, Hugo Münsterberg. Historical and
practically, Münsterberg described the compatibility between psyche and
cinematic apparatus. In his film theory, which was one of the first film
theories at all, he reversed the relationship between subject and object,
familiar from art reception, when it came to watching movies. “In the
photoplay our imagination is projected on the screen”,7 wrote Münsterberg,
and by “imagination” he did not mean fantasy, but the imaginative power of
the mind. “The mental function involved is that of expectation or, when the
expectation is controlled by our feelings, we may class it under the mental
function of imagination.”8 Results and terminology from years of research
in the laboratory in Leipzig, starting with the first complicated expecta-
tions of astronomers up to systematic differentiations and interferences of
feelings and sensations, turn out to be perfectly suited to the experience
of the cinema. Perception studied by nerve physiologists as transferring
and processing impulses, can easily be extended into the apparatus of the
cinema. Münsterberg could easily demonstrate that, “for the first time in the
Psycho -Technology 207
We say our will is fulfilled when the idea we try to maintain becomes
realized. What does realization mean? […] It means the identity of content
between the preceding and the resulting experience.12
relations that form human beings: “The world of values is the only true world,
and for every one who wants to have a world at all, all the relations which
result from the self-assertion of the experiences must be acknowledged as
absolutely valid for the true world.”13
Since they psycho-physically dissolve into single mental functions, values
are the contemporary replacement for old categories. They are the tools with
which the internal, external, and social worlds are linked by neurological
interconnections and flows of data. With these “eternal values” Münsterberg
can remain both moral philosopher and technician of brain functions at the
same time. In 1914 he published the first version of his popular scientific
work Die Grundzüge der Psychotechnik, a discipline that he had founded,
and which was meant “to be an application of psychology in the service of
cultural tasks”,14 a science that relates to psychology like “the engineering
sciences do to physics or the agricultural sciences do to botany.”15 In a more
efficient American sense, culture simply meant ergonomics, optimizing
performance, social hygiene. Here Münsterberg once again goes after
psychological methods and an overly hasty invocation of consciousness:
I did not think that we either of us had been doing automatic writing, we
always knew what we were doing how could we not when every minute
in the laboratory we were doing what we were watching ourselves do-
ing, that was our training, but as he wrote the article after all I was an
undergraduate and not a professional and I am always very docile, and
all the ideas had been his all that had been mine were the definitions
210 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
of the characters of the men and women I had seen naturally it was as
if I had written that I did that automatic writing. I did not think it was
automatic I do not think so now, I do not think any university student is
likely certainly not under observation is likely to be able to do genuinely
automatic writing.21
states that even Solomons and Stein rested on the relay that gave the states
a convention. At the end of their research they stated that hysteria is “a
disease of the attention”,23 although they might have understood attention
to mean the neurological, sensory-motor interconnection and feedback
of intellectual subroutines, which could be experimentally altered. It is
notable throughout that over the course of training, the subroutines of
“minds” could be more reliably switched on and off with books, images,
or apparatuses than with “one’s own” central consciousness. Münsterberg
had arrived at Harvard.
Just as he had had the connections between dissociated mental functions
and apparatuses tested in the Psychological Laboratories, Hugo Münster-
berg examined the psycho-physiology of cinematic perception in 1916. His
theory of cinema is, like the Eternal Values, divided into “outer” and “inner”
“Development of the Moving Picture”, that is, into routines of technological
devices and those of the mind. As such, the Cinematic Apparatus is being
designated, already in 1916, as something that goes straight through all
the old corporal boundaries, part apparatus and part perception. “Our
imagination is projected on the screen!”
As a f irst indication of the fact that watching movies is a special
internal labor of the brain, Münsterberg also presents seeing motion as
a phi phenomenon, using examples from the museum of stroboscopic
apparatuses, and in doing so offers a list of all Wundt’s fellow students and
colleagues, who had discovered the cinema through their psychological
experiments: Stricker, Exner, James, Fischer, Marbe, Wertheimer, Korte,
and Linke. They had all done away with the illusion of the afterimage
effect as the basis for seeing motion: “The apparent movement is in no way
the mere result of an afterimage […]. The movement is in these cases not
really seen from without, but is superadded, by the action of the mind, to
motionless pictures.”24
Just as seeing motion is a mental act that is initiated by the apparatuses,
seeing depth in the cinema is a function of the brain induced from outside.
The experienced psyche combines perspectival relations, shadowing, and
various level of movement into plasticity where there is only a flat screen. In
the studios of the “Vitagraph Company”, which were first set up on Nassau
Street in New York, high above on the roof of the Morse Building, and later
were moved to Brooklyn, Münsterberg adopted, alongside directors and
actors, directorial tricks as one of the physiological justifications: “For in-
stance, by a well-known optical illusion the feeling of depth is strengthened
if the foreground is at rest and the background is moving.”25 This is the
neuronal depth and melancholy of road movies.
212 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
The impression of depth and seeing motion are only two of the per-
ception forms that can automatically be instigated in the cinema by
technical tricks as an artif icial analysis and synthesis of elementary
brain functions. All cinema dramaturgy is fundamentally based on this
mixture and consolidation of such routines, and Rudolf Arnheim, in his
f ilm theory grounded in Gestalt psychology 16 years later, at any rate
without previously having taken note of Münsterberg, will then extend
his fundamentals of perception elements in the cinema. Münsterberg,
however, emphasized what Arnheim would notice only much later, in
relation to the films of Maya Deren, namely the psychological trance in the
cinema, which emerges from the complexity and complication of neuronal
entanglements: “It brings our minds into a peculiar complex state; and we
shall see that this plays a not unimportant part in the mental make-up of
the whole photoplay.”26
Attention, and its possible dissociation from the familiar conventional-
ized sensory-motor context that Solomons and Stein had trained, represents
the pivot point in Münsterberg’s theory of the cinema. With his psycho-
technical analyses of both theater and cinema reception as a dynamic of
attention, Münsterberg disappointed all the ideals of the educated class.
It was not only true for the cinema, ostracized by the pedagogues anyway,
but also for the stage, that the attention is involuntarily drawn toward what
is loud, shiny, and unusual, or what blinks and flickers. While the staging
in the theatre has to make efforts to direct the attention consciously, the
cinema is nothing other than the guiding and misguiding of involuntary
attention itself, by means of the various techniques of the camera and of
montage: “…there is surely no lack of means by which our mind can be
influenced and directed in the rapid play of the pictures.”27
Münsterberg enumerates these means better than any handbook of
directing film, indeed, as a cineaste-purist, not even dealing with text or
music as aids to distracting attention. Camera angles and focal lengths, sets
and framing enhance the movements and relations that distract attention.
If it wants to be more than filmed theater, cinema must assert itself as direct
and unconscious distraction and affection of the nerves.
And Münsterberg was American enough not to view the reality of the
cinema, which corresponds to real mental experience, as secondary, but as
reality with its own rules, which can also stand up to the scientific scrutiny.
The fact that in 1916 there were still numerous technical shortcomings
that distorted these mental mechanisms does not alter the fundamental
autonomy of experience: “It is a unique inner experience, which is charac-
teristic of the perception of the photoplays.”28
Psycho -Technology 213
The horror which we see makes us really shrink, the happiness which we
witness makes us relax, the pain which we observe brings contractions
in our muscles; and all the resulting sensations from muscles, joints and
tendons, from skin and viscera, from blood circulation and breathing, give
the color of living experience to the emotional reflection in our mind.35
Psycho -Technology 215
Even at this early stage, Münsterberg was coming up with recipes for
experimental films. At any rate his suggestions were not appreciated. On
the contrary, film criticism, including Kracauer’s “psychological history
of film”, represented such tricks and uses of the material as superficial.
Kracauer, for instance, considered “one-turn-one-pictures”, with which
Fritz Arno Wagner created the jerky movements of the vampire’s coach
in Murnau’s Nosferatu, a frivolous effect: “It is noteworthy that such an
amount of picture sense and technical ingenuity served the sole purpose
of rendering horrors.”38 For Münsterberg, in the specific case of film it was
not about mirroring emotions, but of transmitting them.
In experimental films, manipulating material is the prerequisite for
experimenting with transmitting states or feelings. The question of the
conditions of human seeing always arises, but so does the question of the
possibilities of extending or destroying human emotionality through new
experiences of seeing. In the fiction film these technological tricks are indeed
more or less subsumed under the course of a narratable story and a surface
216 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
iconography, but they nonetheless always have a decisive effect on the style
and effect of a film in the cinema. It would seem that the most reliable people
to be able to analyze films as films would be trance specialists and test
subjects from the psycho-technological laboratories. Like Münsterberg, Maya
Deren, in her examinations of suggestibility and possession, also studied
the basics of film technology, which lie outside sociology, in the textbooks
of psychiatrists, formulating her film theses as a psycho-physicist: “Ideas
without an accompanying affect do not act suggestively; or, one might put
it this way – the greater the emotional value of an idea, the more contagious
or suggestive it is…”39 …and the better the transmission.
Deren’s most important reference, Eugen Bleuler, also described, in
his natural history of the soul, how important affect is as a medium for
transmitting an idea.
The adult cannot remain cheerful among mourners, not because of the
ideas underlying mourning, but because of the perceived expressions of
affect. The fact that related ideas might also be suggested along with the
affect is self-evident in the close connection between the two and in the
influence on logic by affect, quite unrelated to the fact that it may well
be the goal of the mechanism to transmit ideas as well. 40
While Münsterberg found himself under political fire in real life, because he
opposed the American entry into war and intervened for a “Nordic alliance”,
Psycho -Technology 217
The fact that millions are daily under the spell of the performances on
the screen is established. The high degree of their suggestibility during
those hours in the dark may be taken for granted. Hence any wholesome
influence emanating from the photoplay must have an incomparable
power for the remolding and upbuilding of the national soul. […] The
people still has to learn the great difference between true enjoyment
and fleeting pleasure, between real beauty and the mere tickling of the
senses. 42
We sometimes cry when we catch sight of an object, not because this object
generates a sad feeling in and of itself, but because it reminds us of a dead friend
through an association.
– Vladimir Bekhterev, 1913
of being politically sidelined at the time, and in 1927 in the Soviet Union
this meant much more than a travel ban.
What is certain is that the Wittenberg Symposium in 1927 was one of the
last great international psychological congresses before the Second World
War. It is also clear that the topic “Feelings and Emotions” was so explosive
that the National Research Council in Washington supported the sympo-
sium. The invitations and the interest of the speakers show that the problem
of feelings and sensations was the focus of neurological and psychological
research all over the world. What we can see from the contributions is that
the theories of how to explain, measure, control, and regulate emotions
were still quite diffuse. It is clear that there were certain convergences in
the discussions in Ohio. It is also clear that one person was definitively not
invited: the inconvenient Sigmund Freud. “Feelings and emotions” were no
longer available to simple therapeutic or cultural critical work. At the time,
Freud was writing Civilization and its Discontents, expressing doubt that
a strictly physiological description could do justice to the historical and
cultural networks in which emotions are differentiated. For this reason he
advises starting from associations, images, and representations in order to
infer from this the hidden, underground world of feelings.
In Ohio it was exactly the ideational content that was suppressed. Shortly
before the lights had gone out in Europe – to borrow a documentary film
title from Alexander Hammid – before the various fascist systems could
practically demonstrate that they could stabilize their power in a targeted
manner through mass mobilization of certain feelings, the scientists were
attempting to find their way in the darkness of “feelings and emotions.”
They wanted to get from describing and systematizing feelings and sen-
sations in the laboratory to inducing and regulating these feelings. The
symposium at Wittenberg College represented an international brainstorm,
in which psychology gathered together its practical clout in order to be
able to promise that they could direct emotionalized bodies in the future,
physiologically, pedagogically, and even aesthetically. The alliance with
chemistry expressed in the new architecture of Wittenberg College would
in fact determine the immediate future of psychology and psychiatry.
Psycho -Reflexology 221
In 1927, however, there were still quite different concepts of artif icial
emotionalization.
It is possible that Bekhterev was not in Ohio in October, 1927, but his
lecture was definitely given. In it he presented a proposal for a linked system
in which nerves and apparatuses, metabolism and chemistry would be
amalgamated beyond the individual body. Feelings are the measurable signs
of this interconnection. Bekhterev’s proposal, which he had worked out in
parallel to the first effective mass use of the press in the second half of the
nineteenth century and the significant use of the press and film in the First
World War, marked the aesthetic concepts of many avant-garde artists in
Russia, then in the Soviet Union, during the tens and twenties. Meyerhold’s
theater of biomechanics, in which bodily expression simulated and induced
mental activities as the primacy of the “external”, Eisenstein’s montage of
attractions, Kuleshov’s coordinate system of feelings for film, or Vertov’s
kinoki concept are directly or indirectly based on Bekhterev’s research and
are inconceivable without his medial turn in reflexology. With his lecture
in Ohio Bekhterev was attempting to establish transatlantic contact. He
directed his message at those to whom it would concern, due to their own
research: the physicians surrounding Walter B. Cannon from Harvard, this
“shadow community” who had also begun to examine not only individual
organs, but connections and relations between physiological circumstances
and homeostasis within complex physiological systems.5 Emotions, which
had always been considered an infraction into the controlled experiment in
laboratory medicine, were now to be seen as signs and signals of an affective
interaction, as information about the states of the body! Bekhterev wanted
to place his biomedial vision in the context of this research. It came, like
Münsterberg’s proposal ten years earlier, too early for science and too late
for the author.
Whoever experiments must also take into consideration that his hypothesis
might get confirmed. Vladimir Mikhailovich’s death becomes a legend,
like in a Kleistian biography, at the moment that his life comes to its heroic
highpoint, and at the same time it falls into the grotesque. In December
1927, when the great old man of Russian hypnosis and reflexology, who was
now over seventy, was being celebrated in Moscow at the First All-Union
Congress of Neuropathologists and Psychiatrists, he unexpectedly got a
summons from Stalin to pay him a medical visit. Bekhterev diagnosed him:
222 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
paranoia. Following this Stalin had him poisoned on the very same night
with two specially prepared ice cream desserts.6 Daniil Kharms made the
story famous in Soviet literature:
So the professor’s wife was sitting drinking coffee. Suddenly a ring. What’s
that? – A parcel for you.
The professor’s wife was really pleased; smiling all over her face, she
thrust a tip into the postman’s hand and was soon unwrapping the parcel.
She looked in the parcel and saw an urn of ashes, with a message: ‘Here-
with all that remains of you spouse.’7
Bekhterev, who added a “von” to his last name in German, was a celebrity
in international psychiatry8 until he was murdered, and the memory of
his name for the western world became reduced to a disease of the bone
marrow, which results in a stiffening of the spine. In the east his name
remained current because one of the most experimental clinics for nerv-
ous diseases is named after him, which is familiarly referred to as “the
Bekhterev” to this day.9 Alongside Ivan Mikhailovich Sechenov, his teacher,
and Ivan Petrovich Pavlov, his lifelong rival, Bekhterev is considered the
founder of Russian reflexology. The comparison with Pavlov’s working
method, strictly concentrated on the physiology of the nerves, does not quite
do justice to the grandiose vision of a unity of all sciences of the human
being and its multiple relationships that Bekhterev wanted to realize in
his Psychoneurological Institute. Because Bekhterev’s experiments and
proposals are almost completely unknown, at least in West Germany, but
nonetheless represent the missing link in a neurological media history,
because they so decidedly influenced the arts and artists in the Soviet Union
and then also in the western world, and because they unexpectedly link the
experiments of the tens and twenties to the later cybernetic experiments of
the sixties, I will present them here, at least to the degree that they affect
an archaeology of the cinema.
Bekhterev had studied neurophysiology in Paris with Charcot, experi-
mental psychology with Wundt in Leipzig, brain anatomy with Paul Emil
Flechsig, and, like Sechenov and Pavlov, had worked in the psychological
laboratory of Carl Ludwig before becoming professor of psychiatry in
Kazan in 1885.10 The condition that he took on for his appointment at Kazan
was to set up a psycho-physiological laboratory in the classical Leipzig
style: with a pneumograph, a reflexograph, a reflexometer, an apparatus
for measuring brain volume, and a Hipp’s chronograph for measuring
psychic processes. This equipment was standard in the laboratories that
Psycho -Reflexology 223
were founded all over the world by Wundt students toward the end of the
nineteenth century.11
In 1884, when Bekhterev was Flechsig’s associate at the University Nerve
Clinic, the lawyer Dr. Daniel Paul Schreber had just been admitted, whose
Memoirs of My Nervous Illness appeared in German in 1906, which formed
the basis for Freud’s Psychoanalytic Comments on an Autobiographical Ac-
count of a Case of Paranoia (Dementia paranoids).12 Schreber’s memoirs as
“Iatrogenic (i.e., professional) psychosis”13 made it possible to see the mirror
inversion, the feedback between the systematic delusions of psychiatrists
and patients, and Bekhterev’s later misjudgment of this is thought to have
laid the groundwork for his abrupt end. Schreber’s memoirs, as a view to
the dark side, or better yet, the inner side of brain anatomy, would serve to
set the protocol for what was researched in Leipzig under Flechsig.
God to start with is only nerve, not body, and akin therefore to the hu-
man soul. But unlike the human body, where nerves are present only in
limited numbers, the nerves of God […] have in particular the faculty
of transforming themselves into all things of the created world; in this
capacity they are called rays.14
data. The transformation of the body in the signal system was in full gear
everywhere, which would lend itself well to broadcast news technology.
Ever since there have been methods of brain anatomy “to take apart the
organ with its billions of ganglion cells and nerve conductors, mechanical
observations of mental appearances have been recognized by science.”16
They are supposed to serve to direct the tributaries in their ever more
mobile existence and also to guide them morally. Brain specialists hoped “to
provide a physiological foundation for ethics […] in order to be able to base
law on this whenever possible”, as Flechsig suggested in his rector’s speech
in 1894.17 At the end of the nineteenth century neurology was asserting itself
directly as a science of power.
Bekhterev, who had studied at all of the centers of this European
neuropower, began to link hysteria, hypnosis, and suggestibility research
on the one hand with nerve-physiology and brain-anatomy on the other.
He examined unconscious mental activity with laboratory apparatuses,
with which he undertook to check and regulate crazy thoughts. And like
Bleuler, Bateson, Deren, or Rouch, he examined hypnosis and suggestibility
as historical and social phenomena.
One of the Bekhterev’s first great cases was that of the sect founder
Kondrat Malyovany, whom he had described in the study Suggestion and
its Role in Social Life. In this encyclopedic examination of historical mass
illusions, mass hallucinations, and mass possessions, in which Bekhterev
investigated the psychopathological background of religious and political
group dynamics, Malyovanism stands in a long series of psychopathologies
between medieval demon possession and modern mass panic. Bekhterev
did not settle for any psychiatric judgment “of primary madness or of para-
noia”,18 but reconstructions, in “numerous examinations” with Malyovany,
the system of mad perception, although he was less interested in a clear
diagnosis than in the way to stimulate and convey Malyovany’s thoughts,
with which he wanted to account for the relations between consciousness,
nerves, and their collective interconnection.
Not only in his perception, but also in Malyovany’s movements, in his “de-
cisive gestures”, his “restless speech”, the “trembling” of his hands, Bekhterev
discovered affective feedback that extended beyond the person of the sect
leader. His attacks “of trembling or shaking” while preaching led to “some
of those present, namely the women, also coming down with convulsions
or cramps.”19 Bekhterev judged the different expressions of Malyovanian
possession according to Charcot’s system of hysteria.20 Unlike at Salpêtrière,
however, and similar to how Deren will describe Haitian possession in
contrast to clinical hysteria, Bekhterev diagnoses the states not as a sign
Psycho -Reflexology 225
The error of all […] teachers consists in the attempt to fathom the es-
sence of things speculatively, while the task of true science does not lie in
investigating the essence of things itself, but is geared toward pursuing
the mutual relationships between appearances.23
Making relations, rather than essence, the object of science was the episte-
mological turn from which Bekhterev formed the basis of an avant-garde
science of the human being.
226 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
Here, however, it must be emphasized that in our use of the term ‘energy’
we are in no way associating this with the common usage of ‘physical
energy,’ as is currently assumed. In our view, the energy or force for the be-
ing is nothing other than an active principle disseminated in the nature of
the universe. We know nothing more of the essence of this active principle,
which appears as the milieu of the global aether, but we see the expressions
themselves in the constant conversion of substance all around us.28
Personality Changes
By the turn of the century, in order to create a secure basis for his research
and his researchers, Bekhterev had already planned for a private institute
Psycho -Reflexology 229
Why do the masses move inexorably at a mere wave of the leader […],
why does everything strive toward the same goal as if on command?
[…] However one might think about this, and whether or not one wishes
to accept special ‘psychic waves’ that simultaneously extend over great
masses of people and are even supposed to be capable of a moving
backwards under certain circumstances, what cannot be doubted as
the bases of the whole appearance are the powerful effects of reciprocal
suggestion in masses of people. […] Such mutual suggestion leads to a
kind of ‘electrical charge’ of singular individuals, whose sensations now
increase into extraordinary tension. 42
In the “psychic waves” and “electrical charges” are the electrified media nets
without which the leaders of the uprisings of 1905 and 1917 could not have
brought their commands to the masses, presented as an ideal revolutionary
model from the control room of brain physiology. Bekhterev’s scientifically
defined agitation lends the artistic and dramaturgical metaphors of the
revolution a neurological foundation. The “shock theater”43 of Meyerhold
and Eisenstein invoked this research and and the concept of a personality
that enters into contact and exchange with its social surroundings when
something is communicated. This concept could only be conveyed with
great effort or incompletely without the corresponding reflex “social theory.”
Some German revolutionary theater artists were indignant. Piscator wrote:
electrifying, jolting the spectator. If that really were the task of revolution-
ary drama, the effect could be just as easily achieved by putting on a
boxing match. 44
Anyone who spends a lot of time with the people and has his own experi-
ences knows what the value of logical persuasion is. In the best case it
only has a very slow effect, while suggestion through encouragement or
command almost always leads quickly and surely to the goal. 45
The tsar did not wish to hear anything about experimentally tested propa-
ganda. In 1917 the Psychoneurological Institute was once again the center
of the uprisings. First the new rulers appreciated the research: under the
Bolsheviks the institute was indeed dependent on state subsidies, but was
developed further. On 32½ dessiatines of land within the city of Leningrad,
ten more buildings were constructed. Bekhterev published journals and
organized congresses on special psycho-neurological topics such as develop-
ing personality, criminology, the psycho-physiology and reflexology of
work, which are very similar to Münsterberg’s psycho-technology. For the
third edition of his General Principles of Human Reflexology in 1925, the
“second after the Revolution”, Bekhterev’s work is officially recognized in
the foreword as the consummation of the history of psychology. Bekhterev’s
teaching on associative reflexes “paves the way to constructing an objective
teaching of the human personality.”46 The Bolsheviks, inspired by thoughts
of a mass empire that could be uniformly governed by general electrifica-
tion, welcomed the attempt to research the nervous system as the state basis
232 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
Ways of Behaving
Nerve Priming
“associative areas” in the nerves, which grow up in the end to be the organs
of mental activity.60 To this day, the representation of corporal sensation is
localized in the areas designated by Flechsig.61 Flechsig had outlined certain
areas in the brain as “spheres of bodily feeling”, in which the shape of the
body was illustrated. As an inner mirror relationship, this constituted the
“consciousness” in physiological reflexivity, in which the system of inner
perception is the first area that develops and with the the self-feeling of
every small child is more developed than motor self-control.62 Bekhterev
adopts this concept and its topology. From the correspondences he draws
the conclusion that all behavior is not simply the expression of mental
activity, but much more the mirror of all neurophysiological processes of
stimulus and reaction. Nonetheless, the idea that he has of the structure of
the conductors in the brain and marrow can be distinguished significantly
from that of Flechsig. Flechsig’s topography was passionately against the
democratization of the model of nerves in early research, which sought to
replace the idea of a hegemonic soul. “It is not the republic, but the monarchy
that is realized in the organization of the mental organs”, thought Flechsig.63
He had presented the coordination through associative centers as “com-
mand hierarchies of the fabric of the nerves.”64
Bekhterev’s studies, on the other hand, especially concentrate on those
tracts that run between the various nerve centers, convolutions, cores,
functional joints, as if the hierarchical, monarchic structure from Flech-
sig’s model of the brain wanted to be rhizomatically undermined by new
conductors. At any rate, determining some order in the nervous system,
in which “the individual fiber tracts in the marrow and the brain lie in
close contiguity without perceptible boundaries, often even commingling
[…] or form meshworks, even sometimes intersections”,65 is not an easy
task. Bekhterev viewed the “associative tracts” as “intrinsic fibers” that
internally interconnected the cells of the cerebellum or the cerebral cortex
like a “functional collectivization of its various fiber systems.”66 Bekhterev
structures the topology and functions of the brain as anarchy, against the
position of his teacher, until he, much later, can describe the “function of
the brain” as “the most important apparatus of relating in communication
with the outside world.”67
As a last relay in the communication between inner and outer, nerves and
surroundings, the question of transmission or contact between the nervous
tracts had to be explained. Around the turn of the century there were two
competing hypotheses: one that assumed the continuity of the nervous
networks as a fibrous mesh and the other being neuron theory, which,
following the research of Forel, His, and Ramón y Cajal, assumed discrete
236 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
Reflex Arcs
Here at the very latest Bekhterev goes beyond what can be considered a
reflex arc in science today, which only inherited Pavlov’s coup from reflexol-
ogy. In nerve interconnections communication with the surroundings is
opened up as complex behavior, extended, and – this links Bekhterev with
the biologists and neurologists at Harvard – fed back: “the cortex of the
cerebrum, in association with lower centres, establishing a correlation of
the organism with the environment on the basis of individual experience.”73
Reflex tracts function through a physiological constraint as a storage
space. Thinking and remembering fall under these processes of correlation
that are interpreted and postponed with time. “The associative processes
influenced by operations of constraint often demand a significant time
interval until the reaction is triggered, so that in certain cases the reaction
can appear to be an independent appearance.”74 So what can be considered a
motor reflex is not only the act of speaking, but also complex operations like
giving speeches, reading letters, writing books, or earning money. This ex-
pansion of the understanding of reflex was of course contested.75 Bekhterev’s
hypothesis that “consciousness”, “ego”, and all personal activity were made
up of reflex combinations, which could be completely explained through
biochemical processes, was not entirely original. Bekhterev would have
been able to draw on, for example, the teachers of Freud as predecessors
of reflexology: even Brücke, Meynert, and Exner had seen the foundations
of the ego as a thinking subject in the reflexes and their connections in a
system of cortical tracts.
238 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
What was new in Bekhterev’s proposal was that he followed the path of
stimuli and reactions beyond any anthropomorphic framework. Thanks to
objective examination aided by apparatuses, a complex view of reflex inter-
actions in living creatures was opened up. In this case the cinematograph
is the aid to the objective psychologist. By enlarging motion, the camera
can make visible that the operations at the basis of nerve transmission are
the same in all living creatures:
Quite in the sense of Londe, the camera shows what the naked eye can-
not recognize. In the series of these tropisms, reflexology f inally also
places the behavior of human beings in those “complicated forms that
are conventionally described as acts.” These series of reflexes, which are
guided by a goal, “that is, by a stimulus”, can be traced back to “offense,
defense, concentration, and in certain cases to imitation and symbol-
ism.” Neurological modal and cinematic illustration prove in turn to be
complementary. They provide, if they transcend the unity of the single
individual, the same discrete basic signals, from which the movement of
life itself is composed.
“Feelings and emotions” are good examples of such reflex complexes.
Movements of an “emotional character” are called mimic reflexes in
Bekhterev’s systematics. They are only partly observable: for instance as
facial gesture, breaking out in a sweat, blushing, or speeding up the rate of
breath. Mimicry is “obviously a very complicated and peculiar association
of the motor, secretory, and blood vessel reactions of the organism, which
can be distinguished in their complexity from simple reflex movements.
Nonetheless, these appearances run exactly as mechanically as do ordinary
reflexes”,77 wrote Bekhterev in 1913, when he formulated psycho-reflexology
for the first time as a “teaching.” Psycho-reflexological methods show ex-
actly which physiological processes belong to an emotional complex. The
analysis of mimic movements is more complicated if the organic-reflex
movements appear as components of associated movements. The various
organic reactions were measured in Bekhterev’s laboratory according to
time, intensity, and in their phylogenetic and individual emergence, and
reflex mimicry was physiologically classified according to the perceiving
Psycho -Reflexology 239
After Bekhterev had disassembled the personality into the discrete single
parts of its basic neuronal functions, he finally attempted to construct a
theory of relativity of the psychic process.
But if matter is a fiction, and only energy is real, there is no ground for
the contraposition of the psychic to the material, and vice versa, and we
have ask ourselves: Is it not possible to reduce psychic activity, too, to
physical energy?79
Only after he had taken the analysis of nerve connectors as far as the
methods of natural science allowed, and after he had reconstructed the
transmission qualities of the nerve tracts in the laboratory and clini-
cal practice and newly integrated them into his therapeutic forms did
Bekhterev publish his global theory of relations of a worldwide network
of nervous current, which might appear a metaphysical paranoia, but
he could actually prove the functions of all the individual connectors in
experiments. Bekhterev’s anarchism included the idea that vision was not
conceived as the world on a wire, but as a multiplicity of relations, which
animate reality as a flow of energy between parts, which are smaller than
240 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
electrons, and between complexes that are larger than the planetary
system.
In this way, also, the external world – of course, not that which we
perceive and imagine, but that which exists in reality –is subject to
the law of causality or, more accurately, to the laws of relations. And
when we prosecute our analysis to the end, we must acknowledge one
fundamental and first principle of all being, and this we call energy.
In the concept of energy we have the idea of various manifestations
of movement under the form of great masses […]. To the basis of this
movement, a basis which must be common to all phenomena of nature,
including ourselves as a part of the universe, we give the name universal
energy.80
The glands can, and undoubtedly do, react to the chemical composi-
tion of the blood (chemical reflexes). Thus there is established a sort of
equilibrium between the various glands, due to the direct effect of the
chemical composition of the blood upon the chemical elements of the
glands themselves.84
242 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
of fits of rage, he had only cited Bekhterev from very old experiments from
his time with Flechsig. He seemed not to be familiar with the new develop-
ments from Leningrad or, worse, they did not interest him. The historical
encounter between the two proto-cyberneticists ends in the darkness of
the protocol: “Dr. Walter B. Cannon requested that the following question
may be submitted to Dr. Bekhterev”, we read, then the daily minutes end.
And while in Ohio chemistry and psychology were being fused, in the name
of hormones and vitamins, into a single science that was meant to control
feelings in a more contemporary way than opium and hashish, in Moscow
power was being seized in the old style.
Bekhterev’s last work, Collective Psychology, is an elegant synthesis of
all his research, at once mass psychology, war psychology, psycho-history,
political theory, and – social critique from the perspective of objective
psychology. What Bekhterev opposes to Flechsig’s “ethics grounded in physi-
ology”90 as the coming project of a biosocial society are accumulator-bodies,
linked to one another but at the same time freely developing, which learn
and learn to learn in exchange with their surroundings, much like anti-
authoritarian cyberneticists like Bateson and von Foerster will imagine and
try out much later. If we take Cannon’s and Bekhterev’s research seriously,
feelings would be chemo-neurological complexes that guide social life, an
“autopoietic” variant of reflexology according to Maturana and Varela. In
1913 Bekhterev had imagined that
‘How to arouse emotions of the apathetic, and direct the energies of the
frustrated into the war effort, is the informational Challenge.’ That was
domestic information from the OWI, with an emphasis on emotion and
not intelligence or education which its Soviet or Nazi counterparts could
have easily understood.93
Not much was left of all the research, all the high-ranking symposia, all
the publications. All that “feelings and emotions” attested to now were
the remaining traces of the human in the function of the medial. And yet,
cinema people turn up over and over again that bring the dream of the
unknown Bekhterev to the screen.
Dziga Vertov, Jumping Top. Private Collection.
Eisenstein, Still from the Film The General Line (UdSSR 1920).
Part IV
1. The Truth Won by Means of Film
Everyone who cares for his art seeks the essence of his own technique.
– Dziga Vertov, 1922
Signals
Many of Bekhterev’s students were artists, including the writer Isaac Babel,
the directors Grigori Boltanski and Abram Room and, one of the most
famous, Dziga Vertov.1 This has been pointed out by Herbert Marshall,
who had worked as Joris Ivens’s assistant in the Soviet Union and who
had been personally acquainted with Vertov since his visit to Germany in
1929. Marshall was in fact surprised that many artists had studied at the
Psychoneurological Institute with no intention of becoming physicians,
but then maintains Viktor Shklovsky’s claim that the institute was the only
place that had not restricted access to Jews through quotas, thus providing
an opportunity to assimilate into Russian society by means of an academic
degree.2 This explanation established the connection for film history be-
tween Vertov’s theory of the kino-eye and the experiments that Bekhterev
was conducting in his laboratories. In Vertov’s journals and workbooks,
however, there are a handful of revealing comments about how much his
films and his theories are indebted to the methods of objective psychology.
When he began working at the Moscow Film Ministry immediately after the
October Revolution, Vertov noted in the third person – observing himself:
had experimented with using letters to write down the sound of a sawmill
and sounds of nature. The outcome of these attempts to depict reality in
a symbolic order had above all clarified for him the limits of writing as a
recording technology, which could only manage more or less successful
reductions in the realm of acoustic events with its possible combinations
of 32 Cyrillic, preordained individual elements. Vertov then bought an
old phonograph, with which he could record, store, and manipulate the
vibrations of the noises of the world on cylinders, still without a microphone,
according to frequency and amplitude.5 In this continuous analogue record-
ing Vertov made his own cuts and edited the noises and voices into audio
plays. Initially he recorded human language in traditional literary form
and then as free speaking. Liberated by phonographic recording from the
compulsion of rhyme or rhythm, the old mnemomics, language could then
become voices, breathing, faltering, or hoarsely croaking like a sawmill in
the distance.
The experiments at the Psychoneurological Institute ultimately led
to Vertov’s theory of documentary film. Just as the reflexologists turned
thoughts – which, according to Sechenov and Bekhterev, are inhibited
reflexes – reactions, and behaviors into objects of objective psychological
examination as processes that can be observed, measured and stored by
means of apparatuses, Vertov replaced the old recording technology of
writing, which had implemented its transformative rule as a spirit in things
and people, with technical media that could work without introspective
additions, without the poetry of self-perception, because they stored
acoustic and optical reality in their physical qualities. Using experimental
and applied psychology, the human being – just like things and animals
– was transformed into the events and dimensions of data, which were
transformed according to the corresponding new rules. A new image of
people could only be realized through this transformation, and Vertov was
one of the most careful of its engineers.
Vertov’s study of psychic reactions and reflexes in his self-experiments,
that is, his attempts at self-portrayal with the camera, make it possible to
calculate the “ego” on film and to “relate it to the machine.” Thus arose
an ego-kino-eye or a kino-eye-ego, which could move through the world
beyond the limits of the old consciousness. A kino-eye that speaks from the
manifestos in the first person, like in the text entitled “Kinoks-A Revolution”:
sixteen-seventeen frames per second, free of the limits of time and space,
I put together any given points in the universe, no matter where I’ve
recorded them. My path leads to the creation of a fresh perception of the
world. I decipher in a new way a world unknown to you.6
Human being and machine form a new optics, which dismantles the staged
presentation of the world into the individual parts of its movement, thus dis-
covering and displaying how the historical treatment of reality-data at the
basis of human perception works. Vertov transfers the signal character from
his experiments with acoustic recordings to film. The “zhizn’ vrasplokh”,
the life caught unawares, or, to go back to Walter Benjamin’s anti-bourgeois
methodology of observing history, life viewed as “non-intentional”, shows
the historical reality by technical means. Sophie Küppers, the wife of El Lis-
sitzky, translated the texts for Vertov’s trip to Europe in 1929 and introduced
once and for all the term “überrumpeltes Leben” [“life caught unawares”]
into German literature for Vertov’s technique of documentary film.7
From the beginning Vertov’s technique was not only seen as an attack
on bourgeois conventions and tragedies [Trauerspiele], but on the bourgeois
subject of seeing. The truth exposed by the “life caught unawares” are the
multiple, universal reactions and relations that remain unconscious – al-
though still visible and measureable – when staging the everyday. The truth
of this “zhizn’ vrasplokh”, of the life unconsciously caught unawares, means
that all seeing and measuring of social relationships is based on historical
technologies. The truth of depiction cannot be other than an analysis of the
character of the technology through which it is produced. That this is no
tautology in the practical work with apparatuses, but becomes epistemol-
ogy, was repeated by Godard when he calls film truth 24 times per second
in Le Petit Soldat. Vertov experienced this with own body.
Vertov described his first foray into film history as a self-experiment in
the terms of objective psychology. In 1918 he jumped from a height of one
and a half stories behind the film committee’s palace, while a cameraman
filmed the jump in slow motion. Vertov’s different emotional states on the
way down could then be analyzed using this film. While in real time all that
could be seen was him bowing, smiling, and jumping, the slow motion of the
film established the physical sequence of fear and indecisiveness, embar-
rassment, growing conviction, and so on. These were basic organic-reflex
types that the associative-reflex physicality of the heroic could be traced
back to, according to the experiments at Bekhterev’s institute. Manipulat-
ing the time axis allowed for about 18 truths per second in 1918. The time
became relative, so that even the terms “time lapse” and “slow motion” were
252 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
From the viewpoint of the ordinary eye you see untruth. From the
viewpoint of the cinematic eye (aided by special cinematic means, in
this case, accelerated shooting) you see the truth. If it’s a questions of
reading someone’s thoughts at a distance (and often what matters to us
is not to hear a person’s words but to read his thoughts), then you have
that opportunity right here. It has been revealed by the kino-eye. It is
possible, by means of the kino-eye to remove a man’s mask…8
When Vertov jumped, the truth of observation was still invoked against
the culture of writing from the old bureaucracy, which sought to grasp
and censor the spirit of artworks and artists. Storing human data on film
was established precisely in opposition to the bureaucratic version. In 1935,
when Vertov wrote down his memories of the jump, and when his film Three
Songs about Lenin was not explicitly censored, but was put aside by the
distribution offices, it had thus already become a bureaucratic technique
to censor without words and writing. There was plenty of opportunity “not
to hear a person’s words but to read his thoughts.”9 What is essential about
Vertov’s discovery, however, is that the truth of film does not lie on its visual
surface, but in the processing of discrete individual images, through which
new qualities of reality appear that are invisible without film. Vertov’s
experiments situate film exactly between its signal character, which allows
for manipulating the time axis, and the visual character, which causes all
kinds of shapes to appear on the surface.
In the tradition of the experiments at the Psychoneurological Institute, in
which Vertov placed his film experiments, the self-experiment corresponds
to an experiment from the series of “physical reflexes”, with which move-
ments and reactions could be tested “that characterize the inner state under
certain conditions.”10 It strictly adheres to the psycho-reflex method of
rejecting introspection, but giving free reign to subjective processes in order
to compare them with the results of simultaneous objective observation.
Bekhterev had written:
Agitations
animation filming one of the “chief positions”13 in the Kinoki’s work, he views
his method as a way to explain reality. He harshly criticizes practices that
obfuscate the consciousness of the population. He thus vilifies, moralizingly
and unexpectedly misogynistically, the hypnosis so valued by Bekhterev
in therapy by calling it an infamous trick from the arsenal of the feature
film and all other art forms that constrict consciousness. In the manifesto
Kinoglaz he writes of the film drama:
Just as Bekhterev had drawn the public’s attention to the infirmity of their
consciousness under neuro-physiological points of view in his Petersburg
lectures, Vertov also showed the audience how consciousness can be de-
ceived in the cinema, and self-awareness can also be disappointed. Vertov’s
films, above all Man with a Movie Camera, present cinematography as a
technology in all its manipulative possibilities in order then to raise aware-
ness of the “magic” of everyday life.
Only consciousness can fight the sway of magic in all its forms.
Only consciousness can form a man of firm opinion, firm conviction.
We need conscious men, not an unconscious mass submissive to any
passing suggestion.
Long live the class consciousness of the healthy with eyes and ears to
see and hear with!
Away with the fragrant veil of kisses, murders, doves, and slight-of-hand!
Long live the class vision!
Long live kino-eye!15
Both figures, Bekhterev and Vertov, battle with the paradox of recognizing
media technologies as not enabling conscious choices, but as having to
use it nonetheless to fulfill their mission to enlighten. What is common to
both is the attempt to make consciousness itself to appear as foreign and
different by means of the outside of the apparatus, and thus to be able to
judge oneself as different. This is also why the title Celovek s kinoapparatom
should be translated quite literally not as Cameraman nor as Man with a
Movie Camera, but more exactly as Person through the Cinematic Apparatus,
The Truth Won by Means of Film 255
who then turns up as a kind of causa formalis. What is shown is the person
produced according to objective psychology through the cinematic ap-
paratus in his or her objective truth.
The paradox of wanting to produce a self-awareness from unconscious, that
is, by means of film tricks or hypnosis not accessible due to the technical bases
of conscious perception, from which a “man of firm opinion, firm conviction”
can be formed, persists at any rate as an ambiguity of technical suggestion and
enchantment. Over and over again, the montage sequences of the Kinoki films
intoxicatingly circle around the topic of ecstasy, of shamanistic practices, of
stirring rhythm, of madness, and of the dance, like around the blind spot
of their own theory. An example is the beginning of the first Kinoglaz film:
One sequence is called “Kino-Eye, At the Church Holiday, “or”, The Effect of
Homemade Vodka on the Village Women”, and all the opium of the masses
is vilified in images of women dancing madly. Until it becomes clear that,
for the spectator, the village women’s frenzy is solely due to the frenzy of the
editing and thus to the cinema as a Platonic opium den. The succession of the
first 58 short shots shows that they are enhanced by a pattern of constantly
accelerating repetitions into a rhythmic crescendo of their own.16
What the art drama is meant to display as religious madness, “stupefac-
tion and suggestion”, is here conducted by the anti-drug agitation itself
by means of the technology of cinema. The women’s stupefaction, which
is announced in the caption as an example of misdirected ecstasy, has
too much momentum for an anti-alcohol campaign – or, as it is called in
more technical terms in the Kinoki manifestos, “rhythm of movement,
slowed and accelerated” – indeed, precisely that momentum that, from
so-called gypsy music to the music of Kusturica’s mad Balkanites, has time
and again been considered corrupting by orderly states and has thus been
banned. Just as the limbs of marionettes sway logorhythmically to the
mental lines of dancers, bottles sway in the hands of intoxicated female
dancers, and their bodies in turn sway logarhythmically to the gazes that
go into the camera-eye, constant and unflinchingly direct, like a steadfast
axis. The vilification of the dancing women turns into a sermon straight
from the mouth of a preacher that is itself intoxicated and intoxicating: the
cinematic apparatus. The question is whether, with so much movement and
excitement, for agitation is nothing other than that, it is ultimately only
rhythmic interference patterns that arise in the spectator’s perception in
place of a healthy class consciousness, and whether it is even possible to
speak of objectively analyzing reality anymore. In fact, the goal of analyzing
reality seems to have yielded once and for all to synthesis. In the Provisional
Instructions to Kino-Eye Groups from 1926 we read:
256 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
I can point to the dancing of the drunken peasant women in the first
section of Kinoglaz as an example of a montage moment not limited by
time and space. They were filmed at different times, in different villages,
and edited together into a single whole. The beer house and the market,
actually all the rest[…] were also done through montage.17
This montage study is not so much for editors as it is for spectator brains,
which themselves are brought to dance. “For the first time, we feel that the
obvious drawback of such a film screening can be found not on-screen but in
the theater”, was Rudolf Arnheim’s attempt in 1929 to explain this odd state
in Vertov’s cinema.20 The regularity of this exercise is one in which percep-
tion is imposed as something from outside, as a rule of movement-intervals,
a synthetic manipulation of the brain. The kinoks manifesto continues:
“The result of this concerted action of the liberated and perfected camera
and the strategic brain of man directing, observing, and gauging – the
presentation of even the most ordinary things will take on an exceptionally
fresh and interesting aspect.”21 The attempt to wire up apparatus and brain
and therefore to use film tricks to get around consciousness, since it is
moving on the familiar pathways of experience, was still an experiment
for the kinoks themselves in 1923, and its outcome could only vaguely be
prognosticated as “fresh and interesting.”
Many years later Rudolf Arnheim, who himself had done his doctorate
in Gestalt psychology on physiognomy and the problem of expression
and its interpretation, could not make rhyme or reason out of the lack
of any meaning on screen.22 Vertov’s first sound film, which bears the
258 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
title Enthusiasm, which is significant for the ecstasy complex, was heav-
ily criticized by Arnheim in the “Weltbühne” on September 29, 1931. It is
not by chance that Kleist’s formulation of marionette movements, “fairly
artificial”, is brought into the mix, and he is annoyed by the “flickering”,
which Münsterberg would have been particularly pleased about: “Montage
alone is supposed to provide the film’s structure, and this quite artificial,
theoretically thought-out restriction results in an editing overload – a
flickering of images, held only loosely together, which strongly taxes the
viewer’s nerves.”23 Using the puzzling formulation that the film Enthusiasm
remains an “unmodified structure”, perhaps Arnheim wished to aid Vertov’s
intention to assert its rights of not making reality available randomly, but
to construct an optics in which reality becomes viewable in its various
patterns. For Arnheim, however, all this was glowing splinters that showed
the nerves all that remained as movement: an attitude toward life, the
good “vibrations” of the Soviets, which Arnheim was justifiably calling
into question in 1931. But in doing so the documentary quality of the kinoks
operation was obscured. Vectors and power relations, movements and the
method of results seemed only to be empty claims before a party hierarchy
that had solidified into a block of matter. This caused what was new in
Vertov’s discovery, that the nerves could be moved without implying any
statement or contents, to disappear behind the critical mistrust that he was
trying to arouse enthusiasm for a corrupt system. In 1931 Soviet art was no
longer the great hope it had once been.
At the same time, Vertov’s aesthetic theory and practice, which he had
essentially formulated at the beginning of the twenties, was still, or rather
once again, far ahead of his contemporaries. The camera-eye shows a truth
that also holds outside the cinema, namely that the human eye is disengaged
from the position of power and control that the Renaissance was able to
promise the bourgeois individual with its optical tricks. What is deceived in
this case is a psychology that, despite all experimental research, holds onto
the biographical concept of a coherent individual with a soul, and draws its
conclusions from the introspective method. It is this subjective psychology
from which Vertov also distances himself, as a result of his years of study-
ing and experimenting at Bekhterev’s Psychoneurological Institute. In the
text “We: Variant of a Manifesto” from 1922 he writes: “The ‘psychological’
prevents man from being as precise as a stopwatch; it interferes with his
desire for kinship with the machine.”24 In 1931 Arnheim was not the only one
who refused to dignify the sensory-motor movements that Vertov’s films
spin and cast as collective, intersubjective, and metasubjective as a positive
and productive step. “Vertov does away very radically with everything that
The Truth Won by Means of Film 259
determine the content of the film. The results are utterly remarkable; in
any case, the cameraman presents anything but mere copies of objects.30
experiments in cinema. In the US, Kracauer only mentions Vertov and the
montage of the kinoks at all in a comparison with Ruttmann, as if his initial
enthusiasm now only seemed to him to be an error and an infatuation at
the urging of the communists: “Vertov’s continued survey of everyday life
rests upon his unqualified acceptance of Soviet actuality.”35
In 1947, however, in the middle of the discussion in New York about
surrealism, avant-garde film, and the techniques of political propaganda,
a critical examination by Siegfried Kracauer would surely have resulted in
Vertov, who at the time was once again working under miserable conditions
in Moscow for the weekly newsreels, being discovered as a precursor to a
film practice that critically put the effect of the media itself to the test. Of
course, immediately following the war propaganda that had precisely been
so much about media in its planning, this topic would have been highly risky
and awkward for an asylum seeker. Kracauer, however, never examined
film technologies in relation to – and possibly as in contradistinction to –
political topics, neither then nor later. Ideology critique was not supposed
to be concerned with neuronal manipulations and flicker analysis, but with
the part of the cinematic that was capable of making conscious choices.
Nonetheless, as early as 1948 Kracauer displayed a fascination similar to the
one he had once shown for Vertov when discussing the films of Maya Deren,
who was interested in just those aspects of the cinema that could manipu-
late psychology and perception. Here as well, he qualifies his enthusiasm
with the words “the problem is only what the meaning itself amounts to.”36
Deren’s idea of the unconscious and here critique of psychoanalysis might
also have helped him – or seduced him – to find a new way to approach
the unconscious in and through cinema. But he remained steadfast. As he
had already done with Vertov, he refused to view the media process itself
as social or even political practice. For this reason, Kracauer has always
been considered a steadfast antipode to all media theory, and not only in
American film theory.37
Explosions
Walter Benjamin, who was a Russia traveler for love, tried to make the
phenomenon of the cinema understandable to the bookish set. In the first
version of his work of art essay in 1935, Benjamin attempted, albeit without
mentioning Vertov, the Kinoglaz films, or Chelovek s kinoapparatom, to
negotiate between human and apparatus, between subjective and objective
psychology, and between the the function of media and that of aesthetics.38
262 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
Like Kracauer, he also takes issue with the distinction between conscious
and unconscious, but whereas Kracauer restricts himself to a “more or less
below”, Benjamin let the space of conscious and unconscious structures
get permeated, almost in a kind of cubistic manner. The human being
is – according to Benjamin in his preliminary work on a media theory,
which he would only work out in its various successor texts – not called
into question by the apparatus, but can extend his or her knowledge of the
ego, of the self, by means of the new technological apparatus. The example
that he uses for the optical unconscious is directly comparable to Vertov’s
film jump. (And the reflexive “self” used by the later Frankfurt School social
researchers is seen to be something more than the storing and collecting
rear guard of the ego-function.):
It should be noted that in the second version of the essay, which Benja-
min only finished after the start of the war in December 1939, there is a
harsher syntax used to subject the human being as a subject to film. The
corresponding section XIII about film and apparatus now begins with the
formulation:
The bookish soul cannot get back to the world of its own bookish sensation.
The apparatuses have yanked apperception into depths from which it can
only come back to its senses with the help of science and psychoanalysis. The
following passage from the second version of the Work of Art essay justifies,
quite differently from the first, above all the objectivity of film as a scientific
instrument and, in parallel, the scientific quality of psychoanalysis as an
objective system of storing traces. On the one hand this lends more weight
to technology. “Demonstrating that the artistic uses of photography are
identical to its scientific uses – these two dimensions having usually been
separated until now – will be one of the revolutionary functions of film.”41 On
the other hand, in this second version the possibilities of film is pointedly
placed in light of a disillusionment with the emancipatory power of art.
The enthusiastic experiences that Benjamin had described with the Rus-
sian, Nordic, and American cinema as avant-garde in anti-fascist art must
have softened the insight that fascist artists knew how to use film just as
well. Although he himself never wrote any further film criticism, and in
particular despite never taking account of Vertov’s intentions, Benjamin
had nonetheless laid the foundations of a media critique that had taken up
a different “unconscious” than the individual-psychological one had been.
The “unconsciously affected space” that is screened in the cinema with
slow motion, time lapse, close-ups, fades, and cuts initially transcends the
known and conscious human in this technological sense.
Benjamin’s most famous formulation, however, is found in both the 1935
and the 1939 versions: “Hopeless” is how Benjamin describes the familiar and
common spaces, the bars, railroad stations, streets, offices, and factories
that enclose human beings. “Then came film and exploded this prison-world
with the dynamite of the split second, so that now we can set off calmly
on journeys of adventure among its far-flung debris.”42 In this sentence he
formulates the simultaneity of liberation and horror that affects anyone
who allows the intervention of film into the basic structure of perception
to become a conscious experience. In Benjamin’s formulation of calmly
move through the wreckage of the world we might already see one source
of McLuhan’s thesis that media might have anaesthetizing effects. At any
rate, Benjamin’s analogy is about an exploding out of habit and certainty
by the ego-feeling and its sensations, which had been raised by the new
technology in its origins. This is why cinema is also always a confrontation
with the fragments and the fragmentary in the world, even if involuntary
264 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
brain functions and imaginary energy force the split seconds into artificial
contexts.
Writing about the explosive force of film, Benjamin provides a significant
objection to that theory that sees cinema as a form of the Platonic cave, in
which spectators feel suspended due to the structural “sous-motricité” and
“sur-perception” in the movie seat, because in this mimicry of those bound
in Plato’s cave the reminiscence of the blissful imaginary experience of the
small child is actualized, the experiences of corporal unity are granted
in the optical-visual realm, while every real experience and especially
the violent breakthrough to reality is displaced by imaginary baggage. 43
For Benjamin, on the other hand, precise perception under cinematic
circumstances signifies the expulsion from the familiar as a chance to
discover the relations between consciousness and technology, between
the unconscious and the technical basis for how it works. In contrast to
later film theorists, Benjamin saw this attack on identity and the process
of identification in the cinema spectator not only as disturbance, but also
as a necessary, fragmenting analysis, which releases from the familiar
and common educational processes of the subject. 44 Frighteningly, we
can assert in the cinema that every identification has a technological and
perception-psychological prelude, and that not only works of art, but also
identities fundamentally change with forms of media depiction in the
passage of time.
But Benjamin cannot maintain the confidence of this statement in the
second version of his text. Under the impositions of history he had lost hope
that film might establish “equilibrium between human beings and the
apparatus”, this “equilibrium” that became the shibboleth of the anarchs
among the cyberneticists, who still dreamed of a scientific and artistic
extension and connection of human organisms with the world.
Intervals
In Vertov the interval of movement is perception, the glance, the eye. But
the eye is not the too-immobile human eye; it is the eye of the camera, as
it extends from a point where an action begins to the limit of the reaction,
as it fills the interval between the two, crossing the universe and beating
in time to its intervals. 49
The eye of matter thus functions like human vision in the cinema. It syn-
thesizes impulses and connects action and reaction to a material reflex
arc, albeit one that transcends the boundaries of the individual. If Deleuze
distinguishes between camera-eye and human-eye in his description, then
he only does so to mark the break at which human perception can become
material, just as it had been imagined in reflexology. It is not the human
eye, but the human gaze that can find itself cinematically interrelating to
the world. At any rate, it gets lost, itself tossed about back and forth by the
reciprocally affecting light and reflection punctures of the matter, so that
there is no center any longer in which the old human being could maintain
his position. It is only the multiple ways and movements of actions and
reactions that become a recognizable model of a world in which everything
possible can be conveyed. This would also be a correct description of the
social circumstances in the relational logic of Bekhterevian collective
reflexology.50
Deleuze therefore points precisely to the sticks of Benjaminian dynamite
in cinema, showing which films consciously dismiss an old view of the
human being and which scientists and artists from the beginning of the
twentieth century had already distanced themselves from the anthropology
of the nineteenth century – which was often simply science in the service of
colonial and imperial power. The new structure and communication of mat-
ter, which had made visible a “truth by means of film”, realized movements
and connection beyond the shapes and boundaries of individual bodies:
We have seen that the gap, the interval between two movements sketches
out an empty place which prefigures the human subject in so far as he
appropriates perception to himself. But, for Vertov, the most important
thing was to restore the intervals to matter. This is the meaning of mon-
tage, and of the ‘theory of intervals,’ which is more profound than that
of movement. The interval is no longer that which separates a reaction
The Truth Won by Means of Film 267
from the action experienced, but, on the contrary, that which – an action
being given in a point of the universe – will find the appropriate reaction
in some other point [point quelconque], however distant it is.51
This explains why Vertov, with his concept of universal and reciprocal
matter-connections, of universal reflex-arcs, should be treated as a political
outsider in the new Soviet state, which was then in the process of making
the heterogeneous homogenous and hierarchical. But the interval theory
is even more radical than what the film agencies understood by it. In the
context of interval theory, Vertov’s temporary exclusion of “man as a subject
for film” because of “his inability to control his movements”52 is no mere
anti-humanism.53 It once again refers to an unconscious that has noth-
ing to do with suppressed fantasies, but with the unconscious movement
of perception and of desire – in the technology of the cinema and in the
structure of the gaze, permeated with desire. This is why Vertov embraces
the non-human as liberating from an oppressive, desireless human reality.
This is also a lesson from Bekhterev’s arsenal, which butted up against all
acceptable conventions with its view of being good citizens with Collective
Reflexology.54 Vertov wanted to liberate the cinema from its anthropomor-
phism, constituted from the symbolic remains of the culture of writing, in
order finally to let the dancer merge with the dance. If even he speaks of a
“new man” who “will have the light, precise movements of machines, and …
will be the gratifying subject of our films.”55 Vertovs concept makes a distinc-
tion from the new man demanded by socialism. The model of this new man
is Dziga Vertov, as artificial as the name referring to the man jumping from
the heights. Instead of a hero, cinema shows someone hesitating, balking,
wavering.56 According to this model, the analysis-apparatus ‘cinema’ was an
anti-heroic model, proposed as a universal transformation machine, just as
the personality had also been conceived by Bekhterev as a transformation
machine for global energy. From this perspective Deleuze defended the
Vertovian truth of matter against the reproach of anti-humanism:
…each – even the most charming peasant woman or the most touching
child – was presented as a material system in perpetual interaction. They
were catalysts, converters, transformers, which received and re-emitted
movements, whose speed, direction, order, they changed, making matter
evolve towards less ‘probable’ states, bringing about changes out of all
proportion to their own dimensions. It is not that Vertov considered
beings to be machines, but rather machines which had a ‘heart’ and which
‘revolved, trembled, jolted about and threw out flashes of lightning,’ as
268 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
man could also do, using other movements and other conditions, but
always in interaction with each other.57
What the “life caught unawares” – the declared goal of the cinematic organi-
zation of the world in the Provisional Instructions to Kino-Eye Groups from
1926 – conveys in the facial expression and gestures of the physiological
analysis to the cinema-lab worker corresponds to the truth of objective
psychology. This is why Vertov so disdained the cinema under the direction
of bourgeois psychology: “We consider the psychological Russo-German
film-drama – weighed down with apparitions and childhood memories – an
absurdity.”58 It does not observe with analytical apparatuses, kino-eyes, but
films what had been written, dramatized according to the logic of literary
souls. It remains the cinematic illustration of a Bildungsroman. Vertov’s rage
at fiction films can also be explained by the fact that in 1935 writing and
text were still an unavoidable reference medium for film artists, and served
as the basis for distributing production funds or imposing censorship. Only
previously written-down observations were permitted, after approval, to
be filmed. Any direct recording with the camera, however, was forbidden,
since the bureaucracy of the time had no censorship laws for celluloid. The
journal writer Vertov writes about the director Vertov: “To observe – all of
Pavlov’s students are allowed to do it, even all scholars and writers, but
he is not allowed. To him they say he has to write everything down in the
script, the script is the primary thing.”59
Experiences
After the other film technicians have been introduced, there are in fact
no more intertitles for the next 92 minutes. The reciprocal relationship
between the images and the flickering, which now begin, are less anarchic
than Deleuze’s description would have them be. In one of the first sequences
the projector is introduced in the film, and the sequence is edited with
other shots into a motif of beginning. The film seeks to find the moment
between motionlessness and the introduction of a movement, between
rest and restlessness in the gears and the masses of people. Finally, there is
a shot showing two carbon pencils concurring in the image, and the light
of the arc lamp starts to shine, starting up the projection. This montage
blends into moments of awakening, of rising, of the visual movement of
opening and of the optical opening of perspectives and gazes. The motifs
metaphorically repeat what the technology is realizing.
Just as the principle of the interval is introduced into the montage, one
sequence shows – to put it into words after all – “traffic, movement, and
seeing movement.” The cameraman presented in the film throws himself
into the capital’s traffic, and the “invisible”, filming camera follows him.
Already at the beginning of the sequence unconscious perceptions and
effects of cinema have appeared, effects which work beneath conscious
perception in correspondence to one another, so that human vision itself,
in a parallel to all the images, becomes the object of observation. A relativ-
ity of seeing movement is gradually built up in the cinema. While at first
we only see simple movements in the image, these are then also taken
up by a moving vehicle so that two movements become relativized on
screen. Finally, the whole constellation itself is taken up in traveling, that
is, jolting, so that what is actually invisible, namely the camera that is
filming, becomes visible through its own movements. The brief cutaway
to the locomotive wheels introduces a new kind of movement: the tracks
that, in contrast to the carriage do not jolt, and thus also make it possible
270 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
for the camera to travel without drawing attention to itself. To this day,
tracks allow for cameras to move unnoticed in the cinema as effects of the
spectator’s perception, as Münsterberg had described for artificial atten-
tion. In the last long wide shot the viewing of images gets entangled with
the past impressions of the different movements produced by film. Every
movement that is seen becomes relative to all others and is at the same time
entangled with the new knowledge about how movements are produced. In
part this is a conscious reflection, in part it remains an unremarked shift
in perspective, which allows the spectator barely any conscious inferences
about his or her own seeing. Any fixed spectatorial standpoint is dissolved
in what optically comes from the screen. The spectator’s gaze gets lost in
the chaotic movements of the traffic.
Already the thematizing of traffic feeds back into the cinematic analysis
of movement and the synthetic, illusionary perception of movement. “The
essence of the cinematic movement-image lies in extracting from vehicles
or moving bodies the movement which is their common substance, or
extracting from movements the mobility which is their essence”,61 noted
Deleuze in a different context, but it shows how well armed the gaze is in
the cinema and how it can recognize itself as so armed.
Over the course of the sequence the confusion of moving gazes is pushed
even further. If at first the two perspectives of the cameramen were built up
and differentiated, now they are intertwined, and the spectator’s gaze can
The Truth Won by Means of Film 271
The elements of film technology appear in the image, and we see how neces-
sary technology is to create a moving image in perception. Single images,
strips, the nearly invisible difference between the pictures in succession, the
various speeds on the editing table, the order of the film strips, the editing
and the splicing make clear how much every depiction abstracts and has to
be made open to montage in the individual frames before it can be turned
into a moving form of reality in the cinema. What is demonstrated is that
no image can be seen if the film runs continuously. The invisible intervals
are necessary for an image to emerge. What is also demonstrated is that
quite different images – as in the case of a cut – can be put together by the
The Truth Won by Means of Film 273
brain, like the almost identical images of a still photo: only the movement
pattern and the intervals have to be right.
– Medium shot, from the side the editor, Svilova, at the editing table,
tapes two filmstrips together (3”)
– Still: Portrait of a young girl (3”)
– Medium shot of the editor from below, in the background are the strips
in front of the light box, the editor is looking for a strip (3”)
– Sequence of the young girl, who we have just seen as a still, in full
motion: the girl starts to laugh (3”)
– Medium close-up, the editor at the editing table cranks and looks at
the strips (2”)
– Still: film strip showing two boys in portrait (4”)
– Sequence of the two boys running in full motion (4”)
– Medium close-up, the editor at the editing table cranks and looks at
the strips (2”)
– Still of two other children as filmstrip (4”)
– Sequence of them in full motion (4”)
– Still of the portrait of the old woman who was already seen earlier (1”)
– Still: Wide shot of the large square that was seen earlier (1”)
– The two children just seen as a still, now in motion. The are watching
something, half afraid, half laughing…
– Wide shot of the large square now in motion: Traffic and passersby (6”)
– The old woman in the portrait now moving, arguing with a woman
at the market (4”)
– Medium shot, the editor at the editing table cranks and looks at the
strips (1”)
– Filmstrips run in a grey blur over the light box, stopping at… (2”)
– Still: Medium shot of two women in a carriage (1”)
– The two women in the carriage as a moving image (3”)
– The young woman, seen previously in a still as portrait, now laughs,
speaks to the filming camera, flirts, look into the camera…
In this last part of the sequence the analysis of watching cinema becomes
an analysis of interpersonal perception, a psycho-analysis in the sense of
objective psychology. It goes back to Vertov’s origin, his jump. The still
images, like that of the old woman, the young woman, or the various chil-
dren with their particular facial expressions, with their “somatic-mimic
reflexes”, are shown first so as to communicate these reflexes as emotion
and perception. As they get treated by film technology this impression is
274 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
Centers or centrifuges
That distinctive montage complex within the shot that arises from the
collisions and combinations of the individual stimulants inherent within
it, of stimulants that vary according to the ‘external nature’ but are bound
together in an iron unity through their reflex physiological essence.75
The Truth Won by Means of Film 277
out as “attack into the very heart of objects and phenomena”,79 merely il-
lustrates a literal and highly museum-like arrangement of sacred objects.
The differences between Vertov’s and Eisenstein’s theories of montage
lies above all in differing concepts of how human perception relates to
its technological conditions. The “sorts of vibrations”80 that Eisenstein’s
cameraman Tissé put into the work as the true virtuosity of the medium by
means of camera movements, fluctuations of light, various focal lengths,
gradations, and motivic tensions must, under Eisenstein’s direction,
always remain connected to fundamental dominants in terminology.
Vibrating itself can no longer be a film effect, it must remain hidden, like
the famous dwarf in his chess machine, and the truth of the cinema has
to resign itself to the law of dialectical materialism. Eisenstein counters
the skeptics of his montage theory with a striking argument from Lenin’s
Conspectus of Hegel’s Book “The Science of Logic”.81 The new man need not
be able to recognize the technical conditions of his own existence if only
he understands the dialectic. Eisenstein does not explain film starting
from f ilm technology, but proposes it in networks of signif ication for
persons of the Gutenberg galaxy. His films reenact this position. The well-
known example of the three marble lions that rise up after one another
in Battleship Potemkin is a “movement” that only stands metaphorically
for cinematic perception. Unlike Vertov’s demonstration of watching
movement in f ilm, Eisenstein leaves his spectators with an indication
of what has happened psycho-physically. Psychoanalysis and literature
will lead Eisenstein ever closer to ecstasy than to a “certain flickering” in
the image.82 But they also do not get him any closer to the conditions of
his own perception.
While Eisenstein wanted to reforge the essence of the Soviet man,
Vertov experimented with technology itself to experience the essence of
the new man. But he had to run the risk that he, if he wanted to experi-
ence the essence of technology, would no longer be the one to have this
experience. Anyone who goes so far into relativity can no longer ask what
he has lost:
On the search for cybernetic forms of thinking which might save the
planet, Gregory Bateson, high above the Pacific in Big Sur, believed that
he could identify supraindividual and integrated models of behaving as
functions of the gods: “One of the things that man has done through the
ages to correct for his short-sighted purposiveness is to imagine personified
entities with various sorts of supernatural powers, i.e. gods. These entities,
being fictitious persons, are more or less endowed with cybernetic and circuit
characteristics.” Bateson, we know, learned from cinema. His divine func-
tions can be thought of as cinematic functions from beginning to end.
The fact that it was cinematography, and not the universal Turing
machine that was the prerequisite for all behavioral research is of course
not mentioned by the avant-gardes of cybernetic thinking, but it was cin-
ematography that turned behavior into an object for science in the first
place, placing even its interweaving of past and present into a measurable
order. Cinematography could record, store, and analyze behavior as a
complex interaction between arbitrary points in the universe, open it up
to combination, to repetition, to restaging. In the cinema behavior became
visible as a visible transformation of states, of circumstances, of sensations
and reactions nascent on the axis of time.
Cinema would be the art of combining behavior, which had been turned
into discrete data with the help of chronophotography, into new forms and
formations. This happens all the more precisely and poetically, that is,
counter to the cinema’s history of control and discipline, the more exactly
art moves in the matrices and registers of the medium. This is why in the
end we have dealt with films that were about the historical dispositif cinema
in the image, or at least of artists that went cinematic with the help of
transformation processes of shooting and editing.
For cyberneticists, not supermen of science, as Guilbaud emphasizes,
but simply people at the cross-roads of the disciplines, behavior, if it is
cybernetic, will bring all the points of the universe into equilibrium. “… for
cybernetics is the art of creating equilibrium in a world of possibilities and
constraints. – And I would suggest that this is also a variable definition of the
art of living.” There is nothing homely about this, no self-sufficiency and no
getting comfortable in a safe spaces. Finding equilibrium, in a cybernetic as
well as a cinematic sense, would mean entering into an extreme exchange
with the surroundings or the universe, and one that is decentering for the
individual. It is never a matter of binary relations, much less of exchange
values. Rarely were the travelers to Africa, the explorers of possession
techniques, the disturbers of behavior oriented as to where their journey
was going. It was nonetheless clear that the violence that is still hidden in
284 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
the decolonialized body cannot become visible in any other way than by
the means of those films which also decenter the observer. It was only the
doppelgängers of trance and their blinking paraphernalia, allowing the
light to flash and flicker in disturbing time periods, that were able to bring
photographing strategies of identity formation to light. And only with the
help of the movie camera those mechanical processes become visible in
which men and cameramen turn into organless or multi-organic energetic
complots and complexes, whether consciously or unconsciously.
The equilibrium in question demands of anyone looking to salvage it to
put oneself in danger, to risk jumping into the unknown of a technologically
manipulated time and its dark intervals, to risk crashing or overdosing,
or pirouetting into the spirals of one’s own ego, endlessly running around
the tracks of the measuring stations in the Bois de Boulogne only to learn
that the truth presented in statistical traces and recordings is a truth about
absences. “…in art doing something means doing away with something, begin-
ning with oneself.”
In the decenteredness in which “movements shoot noiselessly through
the room”, the meshes of the disintegrating or networking individual, its
obsessive, neurotic, and nefarious mental statements may therefore be
understood as a gift to the surroundings. “Mental life is not only a sum of
subjective experiences, but always also gives expression to a certain series of
objective appearances. These objective experiences are what the personality
uses to enrich its external social surroundings.” These objective appearances
in the form of sheen, flecks of light and bright spots on the screen, create
new kinds of homeostasis which connect us as spectators medially to a
degenerated, exploited global village gone off balance, and they teach us to
understand cinema as a production of perception, “in matter, as it extends
from the point where an action begins to the limit of the reaction, as it fills
the interval between the two, crossing the universe and beating it time to its
intervals.” This means we are summoned into the cinema in order to realize
crises in our sensory-motor reactions, to incarnate crises and to enter into
circumstances that connect us in ways that the ego had not previously
imagined. The unpredictable and unimaginable states that arise in the
cinema bring to the present exactly what was missing.
Notes
Part I
1. Cinema
3. Knots
10. Wertheimer (1967): “The optical motion resulting from successive stimula-
tion can be seen in reference to the movement as equal to the seeing of
movement in the exposition of a corresponding object that is moving in
reality, and is equally strong, under certain circumstances even more insist-
ent.”, 75 (underscoring in the original).
11. Anderson/Anderson (1980), 87.
12. Arnheim (1957): “Sound-recording devices make the sound vibrations im-
press their own path, either mechanically or upon wax, shellac, or plastic,
or photoelectrically upon film by a beam of light.”, 164.
13. Arnheim (1957), 162.
14. Arnheim (1974), 387.
15. Anderson/Anderson (1980), 88.
16. As Morin (1956/1977), IX and Baudry (1970) would like to see it.
17. Kracauer (1960): “[The consciousness’s] withdrawal from the scene may be
furthered by the darkness in moviehouses.”, 159.
18. Benjamin (1937/2003), “Reception in distraction – the sort of reception
which is increasingly noticeable in all areas of art and is a symptom of pro-
found changes in apperception – finds in film its true training ground.”, 269.
19. Schlüpmann (1995): Heide Schlüpmann, who consistently interwove film
history and philosophy into a critical theory, wrote: “Film realizes the
separation that in philosophy was always at the same time the repression of
the body – and as a consequence of mimetic writing. It thus facilitates the
suspension of repression and the development of the curiosity of looking
in saving the power of intoxication. The cinema begins where philosophy
ended in failure, at the perception of its peculiarity in the separation from
the body but also in distinction to writing.”, 57.
20. Cf. Theweleit (1988).
21. De Lauretis (1987): “Female is what is not susceptible to transformation,
to life or death; she (it) is an element of plot-space, a topos, a resistance,
matrix and matter.”, 43.
22. Kleist (1972), 22-26.
23. Doherty (1993): “…the violation of normative cinematic grammar (the sta-
tionary horizontal plane, sharp focus, unobstructed vision) translated into
heightened impact and added credence.”, 253.
24. McLuhan (1964/1994), 26.
25. Rheingold (1992): “Human proprioception includes a system of internal
sensors at joints and in muscles to detect changes in pressure and position.
A higher-levelprocessing system detects significant patterns among the mes-
sages from the body’s proprioceptors (e.g., this pattern of messages from this
particular set of sensors means that your body is going to topple forward if
you don’t do something about it; that pattern of messages means that you are
pushing something heavy and polished across a low-friction surface). Propri-
oception’s third information system consists of the effectors for transmitting
commands from the sensing and sense-making system to the muscles – the
microadjustements that keep us upright and guide our movements.”, 27-28.
Notes 287
Part II
1. Discretions
jealousy. Cf. Mead (1975), 261. The frightening would in fact not be an order,
but a symptom of upbringing.
34. Deren (1947 Notebook), February 22, 1947.
35. Cf. Kittler (1993), 190.
36. Arnheim (1957), 184.
37. Panofsky (1997), 96.
38. Mead (1974), 9.
39. Mead (1974), 7.
40. Cf. especially Murray (1989) on the lines of tradition in voodoo from Delta
blues to electronic pop and Jimi Hendrix’s sound.
41. Deren (1947 CC), 191.
42. Deren (1947 notebook), February 16, 1947.
43. Deren (1947 notebook), February 16, 1947.
44. Deleuze/Guattari (1987), 300-301.
45. Ruesch/Bateson (1951), Preface to the 1968 edition, VI.
46. Bateson/Bateson (1987), 13.
47. Ruesch/Bateson (1951), 168-227.
48. Wiener (1965), 162.
49. Bateson/Mead/Brand (1976), 37.
50. Bateson/Mead/Brand (1976), 38.
51. Wiener (1956), 252.
52. Bateson (1982), 67.
53. Deren (1947 typescript), March 16, 1947.
2. Depersonalizations
3. Deviations
1. Leiris (1992), 34: “Pour oublier leurs médiocres petites ‘manières de blancs’
(ainsi qui disent certains négres) et ce qu’ils s’imaginent être leur personne
d’intellectuels.”
2. Rouch (1978), 5.
3. Rouch (1978), 6.
4. Deleuze (1989), 158.
5. In the pop culture of the eighties it would be “Phase, lock, loop!” Electronic
Front Populaire.
6. Rouch’s commentary in: Moi, un Noir (France, 1957).
7. Cf. Rouch (1954), 54f.
8. Deleuze (1989), 147.
9. Deleuze (1989), 148.
10. Deleuze (1989), 214.
11. Cf. Stoller (1992), 59.
12. Cf. Stoller (1992), 38.
13. Rouch (1978), 11.
14. Cf. Rouch (1978).
15. Rouch (2003b), 44.
16. Rouch (1978), 24
17. Interview with Rouch from the sixties, in Bazin/Labarthe (1995). Since this
is not a dialogue where Rouch is touting some big program, it has often not
been understood correctly by media studies scholars. “Certainly he wants
to dialogue the research process, a call that he is not alone in making in
Notes 293
4. Compressions
43. Charcot “Apropos de six cas de hystérie chez l’homme,” quoted in: Bernard/
Gunthert (1993), 127f.
44. Charcot, quoted in Didi-Huberman (2003), 29.
45. Cf. Ellenberger (1973/1985), 139ff.
46. Cf. Charcot/Richer (1887).
47. Foreword by Richer, Gilles de la Tourette, and Londe, quoted in: Bernard/
Gunthert (1993), 129.
48. Charcot (1886), 39.
49. Londe (1893), 99
50. Londe (1893), 114.
51. Ellenberger (1981), 99.
52. Cf. Ellenberger (1973/1985), 157.
53. Marey (1885), 2.
Part III
1. Mental Apparatuses
17. In 1854 the debate about materialism in Göttingen was carried out with
the belated verve of the political critique of 1848. Following a formulation
of Carl Vogt’s, according to which the brain did not exude its thoughts any
differently than the liver did bile of the kidneys did urine, the alternative
would be called “soul or secret.” Cf. Vogt (1847) and (1855). Instead of schol-
ars from the humanities, it was now physiologists, chemists, and physicists
who were debating the idea of the soul, and all partisanship had political
and institutional consequences. The physiologist Carl Ludwig, after trave-
ling from Zürich to the meeting of German natural scientists and doctors
in Göttingen, did not receive the appointment to a professorship in Bonn,
which had been previously considered all but certain.
18. Wundt (1874), 10.
19. Cf. Boring (1950), 68ff.
20. Fritsch and Hitzig, quoted in: Rothschuh (1964), 276.
21. Degen (1954), 271.
22. The complete list of Ludwig’s famous students can be found in Schröer
(1967), pp. 287-293. Joravsky (1989) also includes Vladimir M. Bekhterev
among Ludwig’s physiology students in Leipzig.
23. Bauereisen (1956/57), 104.
24. Schröer (1967), 107.
25. Schröer (1967), 107.
26. Cf. Braun (1992), 327.
27. Quoted in Schröer (1967), 110.
28. Quoted in Mattenklott (1984), 173.
29. Hegel (1807/1967), 142.
30. Fechner (1860), vol.2, 13.
31. Cannon (1945), 135.
32. Freud (1953), 969.
33. Freud (1953), 971.
34. Freud (1953), 973.
35. Bernard/Gunthert (1993), 30.
36. Helmholtz (1850), 14ff.
37. Helmholtz (1883), 867.
38. Helmholtz (1883), 870.
39. Helmholtz (1883), 880. Underscoring in the original.
40. Helmholtz (1891/1996), 29.
41. Helmholtz (1895), 149.
42. Helmholtz (1855/1896), 111.
43. Helmholtz (1868/1896), 365.
44. Helmholtz (1895), 43.
45. Helmholtz (1895), 7 and 53.
46. Helmholtz (1895), 6.
47. Helmholtz (1895), 6.
48. Helmholtz first describes it in his lecture “On Human Seeing,” which he
gave on February 27, 1855 in Königsberg; Wundt published it first in his “Lec-
Notes 297
tures on the Theory of Sense Perception” from 1858-1862, but had already
formulated it previously. Cf. Boring (1950), 316ff. and 329ff.
49. Wundt (1902), 27.
50. Cf. Boring (1950), 145ff.
51. Wundt (1912), 5.
52. Wundt (1912), 34.
53. Wundt (1912), 5-6.
54. Thus it was also possible to demonstrate that cinematic projection, in the
speed of the old projectors at 18 frames per second, would just be percepti-
ble as a sequence of single images if the blade shutter did not subdivide it
into two phases.
55. Cf. Deren (1953), 233-239.
56. Wundt (1912), 53.
57. Wundt (1912), 56.
58. Wundt (1902), 344-360.
59. Wundt (1874), 863.
60. Deren (1941), 10.
2. Psycho-Motor Activity
3. Psycho-Drama
4. Psycho-Technology
5. Psycho-Reflexology
Part IV
1. Cf. Marshall (1983), 64 and Tode/Gramatke (2000), 203. Jörg Bochow (1997),
in his study on subject and religiosity in early Soviet film, for which he col-
lected and translated many previously unknown documents, has pointed
to the effects of objective psychology or reflexology, and in particular to
Bekhterev’s concept of mimic reflexes for Kuleshov’s idea of a new actor,
of the ‘naturshik’. The chapter on Vertov and religious ecstasy presents the
connections between Vertov’s musical inspirations and film montage.
2. Marshall (1983), 236, cites Viktor Shklovsky, “Abram M. Room: Life and
Work”, Moscow 1929, 6.
3. Humorous folk songs consisting of four-line couplets.
4. Tode/Gramatke (2000), 162.
5. Cf. Sadoul (1971), 15-46.
6. Vertov, (1984), 17-18.
7. Cf. Tode (1995), 145. Sophie Küppers also linked this expression not to palpa-
ble circumventions, which might be suggested by the somewhat awkward
phrase, but to the precisely planned work of the technician: “Vertov’s tricks
catch us unawares – if he has mystified us, so in the next moment he will
laughingly explain his trick to us. Even as the wild chaos of the street is barely
no longer whirring before us, he already shows us the assistant at her labori-
ous editing work. […] Through his instrument he has rhythmatized seeing;
seeing resounds; the theatre broke into pieces – what we experience through
him is only – REALITY.” Sophie Küppers cited in Tsivian (2005), 359-360.
8. Vertov, (1984), 124.
9. Cf. Annette Michelson’s detailed analysis of the situation in 1935 using
group photos of the All-Union Creative Conference of Workers in Soviet
Cinema, an “icon of dialectics” from which Kuleshov and Vertov are “miss-
ing.” “By 1935 […] Vertov’s situation within the film industry was […] one of
virtual expulsion.” Michelson (1992), 1.
10. Bechterev (1913), 278.
11. Bechterev (1926), 402.
12. Vertov, in Tode/Gramatke (2000), 43.
13. Vertov, (1984), 77.
14. Vertov, (1984), 63.
15. Vertov, (1984), 66.
16. “The agitation against intoxication has itself been raised to the level of
intoxication”, wrote Jörg Bochow concisely (1997). He also added the precise
editing sequence. Bochow (1997), 153f.
17. Vertov, (1984), 76.
18. In this representation and others, Jörg Bochow found the construction of a
transcendental human being, complementary to the “homeless, mutilated,
addicted, and mad” in Vertov’s films, “as he has been thought and imagined
Notes 305
screening took place in Kiev on January 8, 1929 and the Moscow premiere
occurred on April 9, 1929.
31. Kracauer, in Tsivian (2005), 358.
32. Kracauer, (1947/1974), 185.
33. Kracauer, in Tsivian (2005), 358.
34. Kracauer, (1947/1974), 6.
35. Kracauer, (1947/1974), 186.
36. Kracauer, (2012), 58.
37. Cf. Kracauer (1948).
38. Benjamin’s assessment of Vertov’s film A Sixth Part of the World in his 1927
essay “On the Present Situation of Russian Film,” however, was rather criti-
cal. Cf. Benjamin (1999), 13.
39. Benjamin (2003), 117.
40. Benjamin (2003), 265.
41. Benjamin (2003), 265. Italics in original.
42. Benjamin (2002), 117.
43. Cf. Baudry (1970) and Metz (1975) and for a general description and critique
of the apparatus discussion, cf. Winkler (1992).
44. Precisely in Vertov’s films Jean-Louis Baudry designates the appearance of
optical tricks on screen as the uncanny return of the repressed of cinematic
technology, which destabilizes the spectator’s identity: “Thus disturbing cin-
ematic elements – similar, precisely, to those elements indicating the return
of the repressed – signify without fail the arrival of the instrument ‘in flesh
and blood’ as in Vertov’s Man with the Movie Camera. Both specular tranquil-
ity and the reassurance of one’s own identity collapse simultaneously with
the revealing of the mechanisms, that is, of the inscription of the filmwork”
(45). What is correct in this is that the formation of identity is linked to
images and the conscious disillusionment of this identity is linked to the
revelation of the technical conditions of producing images. But knowledge
about the conditions of one’s own identity furthermore presumes a disturb-
ing confrontation with technological, imaginary, and symbolic conditions of
these artistic, or to be more precise, these media worlds. / As to the histori-
cal reception of Vertov’s film, Annette Michelson, from a perspective of
phenomenological film criticism, also emphasizes that simple and reassur-
ing identity would be destroyed in the cinema. While Baudry argues that the
cinema apparatus precisely blocks the path to knowledge, Michelson under-
stands cinematic seeing as an epistemology of the 20th century, and thus she
can appreciate any collapse in identifying reception in the shock as cultural
critical knowledge: “It is the manner in which Vertov questions the most
immediately powerful and sacred aspect of cinematic experience, disrupt-
ing systematically the process of identification and participation, generating
at each moment of the film’s experience, a crisis of belief.” Michelson (1992),
65. / For Benjamin, this disruption is desirable. These techniques can calmly
observed in a cinema like Vertov’s, since they introduce into their analysis of
Notes 307
cinematic conditions film methods by which the conscious and the uncon-
scious become intertwined in their relations and correspondences. The radi-
cal disappointment of the “ego” in the cinema is in fact not a false identifica-
tion, lacking in experience, as it is criticized in the term “imaginary signifier,”
but much more the liberating dis-illusionment of the idea of human identity
beyond all technical or medial ages. Truth becomes results, knowledge of
realities that are won with literal, cinematic, calculating means.
45. Cf. Hein/Herzogenrath (undated) 103f.; Michelson (1992), 67; Farges (1975), 94.
46. Wiener (1956), 23.
47. Vertov, (1984), 8.
48. Deleuze, (1983), 8-9.
49. Deleuze, (1983), 39-40.
50. And for once refers the difference between eye and gaze to a proximity
of the cinematically constituted anti-Oedipal subject to Lacan’s analyti-
cal logic, in which the gaze is also set in motion by an interval, a gap, and
departs from the subject in the endless tours and routines of desire.
51. Deleuze, (1983), 81.
52. Vertov, (1984), 7.
53. Cf. Michelson, (1992): “... the Wings of Hypothesis”: “The resolutely anti-
humanist context in which the interval appears in the very earliest of his
important texts.” 71.
54. Posthumously one can read which illusions about the enlightened self-
consciousness in modern Soviet criticizes, but also in postmodern citi-
zens of the world, Bekhterev would have indulged when he thought that
subjectivism is anachronistic in a world that creates its relations and truths
by means of technological devices. “All this subjectivism borrowed from
psychology is in essence a remnant of the anthropomorphism that is being
radically eliminated from the biological sciences. It should also be expelled
from sociology, history and other humanities.” Bekhterev (2001), 20.
55. Vertov, (1984), 8.
56. Cf. Michelson (1972) about a photograph of Vertov in the air that re-stages
his first jump. “It projects the preoccupation spelled out in the pseudonym,
which replaced, at the very threshold of his working life, the family name of
Denis Kaufman. Dziga Vertov, translated, is ‘Spinning Top.’”, 62. And Georges
Sadoul (1971): “Dziga, déformation de Denis, se réfère au mot ukrainien
qui veut dire toupie, mais aussi Tzigane, peuple eternal errant. Vertov est
dérivé du verbe russe Vertet qui signifie ‘tourner, pivoter, tournoyer.’ […] le
nouveau nom signifia un peu ‘mouvement perpétuel.’” 15.
57. Deleuze, (1983), 39.
58. Vertov, (1984), 5.
59. Vertov, in Tode/Gramatke (2000), 85.
60. Vertov, The Man with a Movie Camera, 1928, opening credits.
61. Deleuze (1983), 27.
62. Bechterev (1913), 21.
308 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
63. Cf. Alf Bold “Experimentalfilm und Politik” in: Petzke (1989), 193.
64. Even the underground encyclopedist Amos Vogel (2005) gets worked up
about Godard’s “Maoist” films, 130.
65. “While [Eisenstein] was under the influence of Bekhterev at the beginning
of the twenties, when he was working on the montage of attractions, in the
second half of the decade he familiarized himself with a wide variety of
concepts: Gestalt psychology and psychoanalysis, behaviorism, and ethnic
psychology. Eisenstein carefully studied the writings of Wilhelm Wundt,
Sigmund Freud, Otto Rank, Ernst Kretschmer, James George Frazer, Lucien
Lévy-Bruhl, and met with Wolfgang Köhler and Hanns Sachs.” Bulgakova in:
Eisenstein (1988), 356.
66. Eisenstein (1998), 40.
67. Eisenstein (1998), 43.
68. Eisenstein (1998), 47.
69. Cf. J. Dudley Andrew (1976), 55ff.
70. Eisenstein (1998), 44.
71. Eisenstein (1998), 50.
72. Eisenstein (1998), 111.
73. Eisenstein (1998), 112.
74. This analogy is also not technically precise, for it does not distinguish the
technical distortion in acoustics, which takes place on the recorded sound
material, from optical distortion, which is produced before recording takes
place, through lenses or quick camera movements.
75. Eisenstein (1998), 113.
76. Eisenstein (1998), 112.
77. Eisenstein (1998), 116.
78. All that is does provide is information about Eisenstein’s clearly ethnocen-
tric understanding of religion. While the simplest minds among the specta-
tors can clearly see, through the military meter of the sequence, that they
are attending a deconstruction of the divine image, all differences between
and reflections on what is seen here of the divine are subordinated to a
single idea: that god is nothing more than a block of wood – and overtonal
montage is just as awkward and bulky as its best service.
79. Eisenstein (1998), 123.
80. Eisenstein (1998), 118.
81. Eisenstein (1998), 121-122.
82. Eisenstein (1995): “…Hanns Sachs, a shrewd old salamander with the horn-
rimmed glasses, whose company I came to enjoy much later on my travels
in Berlin. He had a terrifying African mask – ‘a symbol of complexes’ –
which hung above his small, low, patient’s couch. We became great friends.
He gave me a most interesting book about psychoanalysis. Essay in Genital
Theory by Sandor Ferenczi, which explained a great deal of things.” (admit-
tedly post factum!) which I had come across on my obsessive quest to
penetrate the secrets of ecstasy.” 161-162.
83. Vertov (1984), 9.
Bibliography
Clark, Vévé, Millicent Hodson, Catrina Neiman (1988). The Legend of Maya Deren: A Documentary
Biography and Collected Works, vol. I, Parts 1 and 2. New York (cited in the following as
Legend I.1. or I.2.)
Deren, Maya (1941). “Possessed Dancing in Haiti”, typescript from the Maya Deren Collection.
Mugar Library, Boston University.
Deren, Eleanora (1942). “Religious Possession in Dancing” in: Educational Dancing. March,
April, May 1942.
Deren, Maya (1943). “Program Note to >Meshes<”, reprinted in: Film Culture Nr. 39, Winter 1965, 1.
Deren, Maya (1944 FA). “Fellowship Application to the J.S. Guggenheim Jr. Memorial Foundation”
in: Legend I.2., 243-248.
Deren, Maya (1944 P). “Program Note to >At Land<”, New York, reprinted in: Film Culture 39,
Winter 1965, 2.
Deren, Maya (1945 C). “Choreography for the Camera” in: Dance Magazine, October 1945,
reprinted in: Film Culture Nr. 39, Winter 1965.
Deren, Maya (1945 E). “Efficient or Effective” in: Movie Makers, June, 1945, reprinted in: Legend
I.2., 299-304.
Deren, Maya (1946 Letter). “Letter to the Editors”, The Nation March 3, 1946 in: Legend I.2. S. 383-385.
Deren, Maya (1946 M). “Magic is New”, Mademoiselle, January, 1946, reprinted in Legend I.2.,
305-310.
Deren, Maya (1946 A). An Anagram of Ideas on Art, Form and Film. Yonkers, N.Y.
Deren, Maya (1946 CA). “Cinema as an Art Form” in: New Directions Nr. 9, Fall 1946, 110-120.
Reprinted in: Legend I.2., 313-321.
Deren, Maya (1946 RO). “Notes on Ritual and Ordeal” in: Film Culture Nr. 39, Winter 1965, 10.
Deren, Maya (1946 RTT). “Ritual in Transfigured Time” in: Dance Magazine, December 1946.
Reprinted in: Legend I.2., 457-459.
Deren, Maya (1946 CM). “Creating Movies with a New Dimension: Time” in: Popular Photography,
December 1946. Reprinted in: Legend I.2., 612-616.
Deren, Maya (1945-1947). “Film in Progress: Thematic Statement” in: Film Culture Nr. 39, Winter
1965, 11-17.
Deren, Maya (1947 notebook). Handwritten notebook on film about rituals from the Maya Deren
Collection, Mugar Library, Boston University.
Deren, Maya (1947 typescript). Typewritten notes from the Maya Deren Collection, Mugar
Library, Boston University.
Deren, Maya (1947 CC). “Creative Cutting” in: Movie Maker, May 1947, 190-206 and Movie Maker,
June 1947, 242 and 260.
Deren, Maya (1948). “The Artist as God in Haiti” in: The Tiger’s Eye, June 1948, 115-124.
Deren, Maya (1953). Divine Horsemen: The Living Gods of Haiti, McPherson &Company, Kingston, N.Y.
Deren, Maya (1955). “Letter to James Card” in: Film Culture 39, Winter 1965, 28-33.
Deren, Maya (MM). “Film Medium as Muse and Means” in: Film Culture 39, Winter 1965, 38-45.
Deren, Maya (1960). “Cinematography: The Creative Use of Reality” in: Daedalus: Journal of the
American Academy of Arts and Sciences. Winter 1960, Cambridge, Massachusetts.
Deren, Maya and Gregory Bateson (1980). “An Exchange of Letters” in: October Nr. 14, MIT Press,
Cambridge, MA. Fall 1980, 16-19.
310 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
Bechterev, Vladimir and S. Vladiczko (1911). “Beiträge zur Methodik der objektiven Untersuchung
von Geisteskranken” in: Zeitschrift für Psychotherapie und medizinische Psychologie. Albert
Moll (ed.) III. Band. Stuttgart, 87-109.
Bechterev, Vladimir (1912). “Was ist Pychoreflexologie?” in: Deutsche medizinische Wochenschrift,
P. Schwalbe (ed.) Leipzig, 38. Jahrgang, Nr. 32, August 1912, 1481-1487.
Bechterev, Vladimir, (1912a). “Die Anwendung der Methode der motorischen Assoziationsreflexe
zur Aufdeckung der Simulation” in: Zeitschrift für die gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie. A.
Alzheimer et al. (ed.) Berlin. Vol. 13, 183-191.
Bechterev, Vladimir (1913). Objektive Psychologie oder Psychoreflexologie: Die Lehre von den
Assoziationsreflexen. Leipzig and Berlin.
Bechterev, Vladimir (1914). “Über die individuelle Entwicklung der neuropsychischen Sphäre
nach psycho-reflexologischen Befunden” in: Zeitschrift für Psychotherapie und medizinische
Psychologie mit Einschluß des Hypnotismus, der Suggestion und der Psychoanalyse. Albert
Moll (ed.) Vol. V, Stuttgart.
Bechterev, Vladimir (1923). “Die Krankheiten der Persönlichkeit vom Standpunkt der Reflex-
ologie. (Zur Begründung der pathologischen Reflexologie.)” in: Zeitschrift für die gesamte
Neurologie und Psychiatrie. A. Alzheimer et al. (eds.) Berlin. Vol. 80, 265-309.
Bechterev, Vladimir (1926). Allgemeine Grundlagen der Reflexologie des Menschen. Leitfaden für
das objective Studium der Persönlichkeit. Leipzig/Vienna.
Bechterev, Vladimir (1927) in: L.R. Grote (ed.), Medizin der Gegenwart in Selbstdarstellungen.
VI. Leipzig.
Bechterev, Vladimir (1928). Die kollektive Reflexologie. Halle/Saale.
Bechterev, Vladimir (1928 E). “Emotions as Somato-Mimetic Reflexes” in: Feelings and Emotions:
The Wittenberg Symposium. Martin L. Reymert (ed.). Worcester Massachusetts, 70-283.
Bechterev, Vladimir (1973). General Principles of Human Reflexology. North Stratford, New
Hampshire.
Bechterev, Vladimir (2001). Collective Reflexology: The Complete Edition. Herndon, Virginia.
Bellour, Raymond (1990). “Believing in the Cinema” in: Kaplan (1990), 98-109.
Benjamin, Walter (1927/1999). “On the Present Situaion of Russian Film” in: Selected Writings,
vol. 2, Cambridge, 12-15.
Benjamin, Walter (1931/1999). “Little History of Photography” in: Selected Writings, vol. 2,
Cambridge, 507-530.
Benjamin, Walter (1937/2003). “On Some Motifs in Baudelaire” in: Selected Writings, vol. 4,
Cambridge, 313-355.
Benjamin, Walter (1935/2002). “The Work of Art in the Age of its Technological Reproducibility:
Second Version” in: Selected Writings, vol. 3, Cambridge, 101-133.
Benjamin, Walter (1939/1980). “The Work of Art in the Age of its Technological Reproducibility:
Third Version” in: Selected Writings, vol. 4, Cambridge, 251-283.
Berg, Brigitte (1995). “De charmants petits rien” in: Marey, Pionnier de la Synthèse du Mouvement.
Musée Marey, Beaune.
Bernard, Claude (1865). Introduction a l’Étude de la Médecine Expérimentale. Paris.
Bernard, Claude (1961). Einführung in das Studium der experimentellen Medizin, Paris 1865,
German trans. P. Szendroe, biographical introduction by K.E. Rothschuh. Leipzig (Sudhoffs
Klassiker der Medizin, vol. 35).
Bernard, Denis and André Gunthert (1993). L’instant rêvé. Albert Londe. Préface de Louis Marin.
Nîmes.
Bleuler, Eugen (1911). Dementia praecox oder Gruppe der Schizophrenien. Published as: G.
Ascheaffenburg, Handbuch der Psychiatrie. Spezieller Teil, 4. Abt., I.
312 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
Foucault, Michel (1978). The History of Sexuality, Vol. 1: An Introduction. New York.
Foucault, Michel (1980). Power/Knowledge: Selected Interviews and Other Writings, 1972-1977.
New York.
Foucault, Michel (2006). Psychiatric Power: Lectures at the Collège de France, 1973-1974, New York.
Foucault, Michel (1975). The Birth of the Clinic: An Archaeology of Medical Perception. New York.
Freud, Sigmund (1893/1962): “Charcot” in: The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological
Works of Sigmund Freud, Vol. III (1893-1899): Early Psycho-Analytical Publications, (ed.) James
Strachey. London 1953-1974.
Freud, Sigmund (1900/1953): “The Interpretation of Dreams” in: The Standard Edition of the
Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, Vol. V (1900-1901): The Interpretation of Dreams
(Second Part) and On Dreams, (ed.) James Strachey. London 1956-1974.
Freud, Sigmund (1910/1957): “Five Lectures on Psycho-Analysis” in: The Standard Edition of
the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, Vol. XI (1910): Five Lectures on Psycho-
Analysis, Leonardo Da Vinci an Other Works, (ed.) James Strachey London 1953-1974.
Freud, Sigmund (1911/1958): “Psychoanalytical Notes on an Autobiographical Account of a Case
of Paranoia (Dementia Paranoides)” in: The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological
Works of Sigmund Freud, Vol. XII (1911-1913): The Case of Schreber, Papers on Technique and
Other Works, (ed.) James Strachey. London 1953-1974.
Freud, Sigmund (1919/1955): “The Uncanny” in: The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological
Works of Sigmund Freud, Vol. XVII (1917-1919): An Infantile Neurosis and Other Works, (ed.)
James Strachey. London 1956-1974.
Freud, Sigmund (1930/1961): “Civilization and it’s Discontents” in: The Standard Edition of the
Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, Vol. XXI (1927-1931): The Future of an Illusion,
Civilization and it’s Discontents, and Other Works, (ed.) James Strachey. London 1953-1974.
Gardiner, Muriel (ed.) (1972). Der Wolfsmann vom Wolfsmann: Sigmund Freuds berühmtester
Fall: Erinnerungen, Berichte, Diagnosen. Frankfurt/Main.
Gauld, Alan (1995). A History of Hypnotism. Cambridge University Press.
Gölter,Waltraud (1993). “‘Une Illusion qui se sait’ Zur ‘Poesie’ bei Michel Leiris.” Afterword to:
Leiris, Michel, Lebende Asche, Namenlos. Gedichte. Heidelberg, 75-126.
Guilbaud, G.Th. (1957). La Cybernétique. Paris.
Hacking, Ian (1995). Rewriting the Soul: Multiple Personality and the Sciences of Memory. Princeton.
Hegel, Georg Wilhelm Friedrich (1807/1967). The Phenomenology of Mind. New York.
Heider, Karl G. (1976). Ethnographic Film, Austin.
Hein, Birgit and Wulf Herzogenrath (no date). Film als Film 1910 bis heute: Vom Animationsfilm
der zwanziger zum Filmenvironment der siebziger Jahre. Stuttgart.
Helmholtz, Hermann (1850). “Über die Fortpflanzungsgeschwindigkeit in der Nervenreizung”.
Hearing at the Prussian Academy of Sciences, Berlin.
Helmholtz, Hermann (1855/1896). “Über das Sehen des Menschen” in: Helmholtz (1896) Vol. 1,
85-117.
Helmholtz, Hermann (1868). “Die neueren Fortschritte in der Theorie des Sehens” in: Helmholtz
(1896) Vol. 1, 265-365.
Helmholtz, Hermann von (1883). “Über die Methoden, kleinste Zeittheile zu messen und ihre
Anwendung für physiologische Zwecke” in: Wissenschaftliche Abhandlungen. Leipzig.
Helmholtz, Hermann von (1895). On the Sensation of Tone as a Psychological Basis for the Theory
of Music, translated by Alexander J. Ellis. London.
Helmholtz, Hermann von (1896). Vorträge und Reden. Vols. 1 and 2, Braunschweig.
Helmholtz, Hermann von (1891/1966). Hermann von Helmholtz über sich selbst: Rede zu seinem
70. Geburtstag. Leipzig.
Bibliogr aphy 315
Hennes, Hans (1910). “Die Kinematographie im Dienste der Neurologie und Psychiatrie, nebst
Beschreibung einiger seltener Bewegungsstörungen” Medizinische Klinik Nr. 51, 2010-2014
Henke, Silvia, Martin Stingelin and Hubert Thüring (1997). Hysterie – das Theater der Epoche.
Afterword to the German translation of Georges Didi-Huberman (2004).
Herskovitz, Melville Jean (1937). Life in a Haitian Valley. New York.
Herskovitz, Melville Jean (1938). Acculturation. New York.
Hochmeister, Günter von (1991). Handbuch für den Filmvorführer. (ed.) Wirtschaftsverband der
Filmtheater e.V. Bayern. Munich.
Hohenberger, Eva (1988). Die Wirklichkeit des Films: Dokumentarfilm. Ethnographischer Film.
Jean Rouch. Hildesheim, Zurich, New York.
Hörisch, Jochen and Georg Christoph Tholen (eds.) (1985). Eingebildete Texte. Munich.
Israel, Lucien (1976/1993). Die unerhörte Botschaft der Hysterie. München/Basel.
Jacobs, Lewis (1947/48). “Experimental Cinema in America 1921-1947” in: Hollywood Quarterly,
Vol. III, No.2, Winter 1947-48. Reprinted in: Lewis Jacobs (1968) The Rise and Fall of the
American Film, New York, 1968.
Joravsky, David (1989). Russian Psychology: A Critical History. Oxford (UK) and Cambridge (MA).
Kaplan, E. Ann (1990). Psychoanalysis and Cinema. London and New York.
Katz, Ephraim (1994). The Film Encyclopedia. Second Edition. New York.
Kelman, Ken (1962). “Widow into Bride” in: Filmwise 2. Maya Deren. 1962. S. 20-23.
Kersting, Rudolf (1989). Wie die Sinne auf Montage gehen. Zur ästhetischen Theorie des Films/
Kinos. Frankfurt/Main.
Kittler, Friedrich (1984/2013). “Flechsig, Schreber, Freud: An Information Network at the Turn of
the Century” in: The Truth of the Technological World, (ed.) Hans Ulrich Gumbrecht. California.
Kittler, Friedrich (1997/2013). “Romanticism, Psychoanalysis, Film: A Story of Doubles” in: The
Truth of the Technological World, (ed.) Hans Ulrich Gumbrecht. California 2013.
Kittler, Friedrich (1985). Aufschreibesysteme. 1800-1900. Munich.
Kittler, Friedrich (1986). Grammophon, Film, Typewriter. Berlin.
Kittler, Friedrich and Christoph Tholen (1989) (eds.). Arsenale der Seele. Munich.
Kittler, Friedrich (1984/1993). Draculas Vermächtnis: Technische Schriften. Leipzig.
Kittler, Friedrich (1998). “Computergraphik: Eine halbtechnische Einführung”, Lecture given
in Basel, June 1998.
Kleist, Heinrich von (1808/1972). “On the Marionette Theatre” in: The Drama Review: TDR, Vol.
16, No. 3, Cambridge.
Klivington, Kenneth A. (1989). The Science of Mind. Cambridge.
Kracauer, Siegfried (1947/1974). From Caligari to Hitler: A Psychological History of the German
Film. Princeton.
Kracauer, Siegfried (1948). “Filming the Subconscious” in: Theater Arts. February 1948, 37-40.
Kracauer, Siegfried (1960). Theory of Film: The Redemption of Physical Reality. London, Oxford,
New York.
Kracauer, Siegfried, (1960/1964). Theorie des Films: Die Errettung der äußeren Wirklichkeit.
Frankfurt/Main.
Kracauer, Siegfried, (2012). Siegfried Kracauer’s American Writings: Essays on Film and Popular
Culture, Berkeley/Los Angeles.
Kraepelin, Emil (1909). “Demonstration von Kinematogrammen” in Centralblatt für Nerven-
heilkunde und Psychiatrie. 32, 689.
Krumme, Peter (1971). “Zur Konzeption der dialektischen Bilder” in: Text und Kritik 31/32 ( 1979),
Heinz Ludwig Arnold (ed.) Walter Benjamin. München.
316 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
Mead, Margaret (1974). “Visual Anthropology in a Discipline of Words” in: Hockings, Paul (ed.)
(1995) Principles of Visual Anthropology. Berlin, New York.
Mead, Margaret (1975). Blackberry Winter. My Earlier Years. New York.
Mellencamp, Patricia (1990). Indiscretions: Avant-Garde Film, Video & Feminism. Bloomington
and Indianapolis.
Métraux, Alexandre (1986). “Vladimir Bechterevs Psychophysiologisches Laboratorium” in:
Psychologische Rundschau. Göttingen, Toronto, Zurich, 91.
Métraux, Alfred (1958). Le Vaudou haîtien. Paris.
Metz, Christian (1975). “Le signifiant imaginaire” in: Communication 23, 3-55.
Michelson, Annette, (1972). “The Man with the Movie Camera: From Magician to Epistemologist”
in: Artforum Nr. 10, 1. March 1972, 60-72.
Michelson, Annette (1992). “The Wings of Hypothesis. On Montage and the Theory of the Interval”
in: Montage and Modern Life, (ed.) Michael Teitelbaum, Cambridge, 61-81.
Morin, Edgar (1956/1977). Le cinéma ou l’homme imaginaire: Essai d’ Anthropologie Sociologique.
Nouvelle Edition. Paris.
Müller, Heiner (1990). “I would rather be Goliath: Homage to Chaplin”, Germania, New York.
Münsterberg, Hugo (1889). Beiträge zur experimentellen Psychologie. Heft 1-4. Freiburg/Br.
Münsterberg, Hugo (1916/1970). The Film: A Psychological Study. The Silent Photoplay in 1916. With
a new foreword by Richard Griffith. New York.
Münsterberg, Hugo (1909). Eternal Values. Boston.
Münsterberg, Hugo (1914/1920). Die Grundzüge der Psychotechnik. Second, expanded edition.
Leipzig 1920.
Murray, Charles Shaar (1989). Crosstown Traffic. Jimi Hendrix and Post-War Pop. London, Boston.
Neiman, Catrina (1980). “Art and Anthropology: The Crossroads” in October, 14, Fall 1980, 3-15.
Neiman, Catrina (1988). Program Notes to the Alexander Hammid Retrospective, February 12,
1988, Anthology Film Archives, New York.
Nin, Anais (1971). The Diary of Anais Nin, vol. 4. New York.
Nin, Anais (1976). The Diary of Anais Nin, vol. 6. New York.
Noll-Brinckmann, Christine (1984). “Zu Maya Derens AT LAND” in: Frauen und Film Nr. 37.
Avantgarde und Experiment. Frankfurt/Main.
Oppitz, Michael (1981). Schamanen im Blinden Land. Frankfurt/Main.
Panofsky, Erwin (1997). Three Essays on Style. Cambridge.
Painlevé, Jean (1991). Werkkatalog, Documents Cinématographiques. Paris.
Pavlov, Ivan Pedrovic (1957). Fusion of principal, branches of medicine experimentation as
demonstrated by the example of digestion. New York.
Peters, Jan (1996). Von der Eisenbahn zur Datenbahn. Mobilität im Medium Film. Typescript and
photocopy. Diploma thesis at the Hochschule für bildenden Künste Hamburg.
Petzke , Ingo (1989). Das Experimentalfilm-Handbuch: Schriftenreihe des Deutschen Filmmuseums.
Frankfurt am Main.
Phillips-Krug, Jutta and Cecilia Hausheer (eds.). (no date). Frankensteins Kinder: Film und
Medizin. Museum für Gestaltung. Zurich.
Ponomareff, George L. (1986). “Commentary on the Bekhterev Psychoneurological Institute” in:
Current Psychiatric Therapy 23: 281-285.
Pschyrembel Klinisches Wörterbuch (1986). 255th edition Berlin, New York.
Puente, Antonio E. (1995). “Intellectual Perestroika and Russian Neuropsychology” in: Journal
of the International Neuropsychological Society. New York. Sept. 1 (5) 150.
Rabinovitz, Lauren (1991). Points of Resistance: Women, Power& Politics in the New York Avant-
Garde Cinema, 1943-1971. Urbana and Chicago.
318 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
Ragland-Sullivan, Ellie (1989). Jacques Lacan und die Philosophie der Psychoanalyse. Weinheim,
Berlin.
Rank, Otto (1914). “Der Doppelgänger” in: Imago: Zeitschrift für Anwendung der Psychoanalyse
auf die Geisteswissenschaften. Nr. III.2. 97-164.
Reisz, Karel and Gavin Millar (1968). The Technique of Film Editing. Second Edition. London
and Boston.
Renan, Sheldon (1967). An Introduction to the American Underground Film. New York.
Reymert, Martin (ed.) (1928). Feelings and Emotions: The Wittenberg Symposium. Worcester,
Massachusetts.
Rheingold, Howard (1992). Virtual Reality. London.
Rose, Jacqueline (1986). Sexuality in the Field of Vision. London, New York.
Rothschuh, K.E. (ed.) (1964). Von Boerhave bis Berger: Die Entwicklung der kontinentalen Physiolo-
gie im 18. und 19. Jahrhundert mit besonderer Berücksichtigung der Neurophysiologie. Stuttgart.
Rouch, Jean (1954). Les Songhay. Paris.
Rouch, Jean (2003a): “On the Vicissitudes of the Self: The Possessed Dancer, the Magician, the
Sorcerer, the Filmmaker, and the Ethnographer” in: Ciné-Ethnography, Minneapolis, 87-101.
Rouch, Jean (2003b). “The Camera and Man” in: Ciné-Ethnography, Minneapolis, 29-46.
Rouch, Jean (1978). “Jean Rouch erzählt” Transcript and translation of a conversation with Hanns
Zischler, Paris December 1977. in: Filmkritik Nr. 253, January 1978. 5-31.
Rouch, Jean (1996). Television interview. Broadcast on ARTE on June 21, 1997.
Ruesch, Jürgen and Gregory Bateson (1951). Communication: The Social Matrix of Psychiatry.
New York.
Sadoul, Georges (1971). Dziga Vertov, Paris.
Schade, Sigrid (1993). “Charcot und das Schauspiel des hysterischen Körpers” in: Denkräume:
Zwischen Kunst und Wissenschaft. Silvia Baumgart et al. (eds.). Berlin, 461-484.
Schäfer, Andreas (1989). “Drogen und Krieg” in: Arsenale der Seele. Friedrich Kittler and Chris-
toph Tholen (eds.) Munich, 151-169
Schlüpmann, Heide (1984). “Die Emanzipation des Films: Zu Germaine Dulacs und Maya Derens
Theorie der Avantgarde” in: Frauen und Film 37, 38-51.
Schlüpmann, Heide (1995). “Die Wiederkehr des Verdrängten. Überlegungen zu einer Philosophie
der Filmgeschichte aus feministischer Perspektive” in: Frauen und Film. Heft 56/57, 41-58.
Schlüpmann, Heide (1998). Ein Detektiv des Kinos: Studien zu Siegfried Kracauers Filmtheorie.
Basel, Frankfurt/Main.
Schlüpmann, Heide (1998 A). Abendröthe der Subjektphilosophie: Eine Ästhetik des Kinos.
Frankfurt/Main.
Schmid, Hans (1997). “Film und der Frankensteinkomplex” in: Jutta Phillips-Krug, Cecilia Haus
heer (eds.). Frankensteins Kinder: Film und Medizin. Zurich, Museum für Gestaltung, 16-21.
Schmidt, Ulf (2001). Films: Ethics and Euthenasia in Nazi-Germany 1933-1945. Husum.
Schneider, Manfred (1985). “Hysterie als Gesamtkunstwerk”: Aufstieg und Verfall einer Semiotik
der Weiblichkeit in: Merkur 39, 879-895.
Schneider, Manfred (1988). Afterword to J.-M. Charcot and Paul Richer. Die Besessenen in der
Kunst. (ed.) with a new afterword with Wolfgang Tietze. Göttingen. 138-158.
Schneider, Manfred (1994). “Die Allegorie der Hysterie und das Tête-à-tête der Wahrheit” in:
Sigrid Schade, Monika Wagner and Sigrid Weigel, Allegorien der Geschlechterdifferenz,
Cologne, Weimar, Vienna, 137-155.
Schreber, Daniel Paul (2000). Memoirs of My Nervous Illness. New York.
Schröer, Heinz and Carl Ludwig (1967). Begründer der messenden Experimentalphysiologie
1816-1895. Stuttgart.
Bibliogr aphy 319
Schuller, Marianne (1989/1990). “Hysterie als Artefaktum. Zum literarischen und visuellen
Archiv der Hysterie um 1900” in: M.S. Im Unterschied: Lesen, Korrespondieren, Adressieren.
Frankfurt/Main, 81-94.
Sejka, Leonid (1997). Alchemie. (ed.) Peter Urban im material-Verlag. HfbK Hamburg.
Sekula, Allan (1986). “The Body in the Archive” in: October 39, 1-64.
Short, K.R.M. (ed.) (1983). Film and Radio Propaganda in World War II. London and Canberra.
Silverman, Kaja (1988). The Acoustic Mirror: The Female Voice in Psychoanalysis and Cinema.
Bloomington, Indianapolis.
Sitney, P. Adams (ed.) (1970). A Film Culture Reader. New York.
Sitney, P. Adams (1979). Visionary Film: The American Avantgarde 1943-1978. Second Edition.
New York.
Solomons, Leon M. and Gertrude Stein (1896). “Normal Motor Automatism” in: Psychological
Review 3, 492-512.
Staal, Frits (1979). “The Meaninglessness of Ritual” in: Numen: International Review for the History
of Religions, Volume XXVI, 2-22.
Stoller, Paul (1992). The Cinematic Griot: The Ethnography of Jean Rouch. Chicago.
Streck, Bernhard (1987). Wörterbuch der Ethnologie. Cologne.
Stein, Gertrude (1937/1993). Everybody’s Autobiography. Cambridge.
Steinberg, Holger (2001). Kraepelin in Leipzig: Eine Begegnung von Psychiatrie und Psychologie.
Bonn.
Stingelin, Martin (1990). “Die Seele als Funktion des Körpers. Zur Seelenpolitik der Leipziger
Universitätspsychiatrie unter Paul Emil Flechsig” in: Diskursanalysen 2. F.A. Kittler, M.
Schneider, S. Weber (eds.), 101-115.
Stuart, Herbert A. and Gerhard Klages (1997). Kurzes Lehrbuch der Physik. Berlin, Heidelberg,
New York.
Sudre, Alain-Alcide (1996). Dialogues théoriques avec Maya Deren: Du cinéma expérimental au
film ethnographique, Paris.
Theweleit, Klaus (1988). Buch der Könige: Orpheus & Eurydice. Frankfurt/Main.
Tode, Thomas (1995). “Ein Russe projiziert in die Planetariumskuppel. Dsiga Wertows Reise
nach Deutschland 1929” in: Oksana Bulgakowa (ed.). Die ungewöhnlichen Abenteuer des Dr.
Mabuse im Lande der Bolschewiki: Das Buch zur Filmreihe “Moskau-Berlin”.Berlin: Freunde
der Deutschen Kinemathek, 143-158.
Tode, Thomas and Alexandra Granatke (eds.) (2000). Dziga Vertov: Tagebücher/Arbeitshefte.
Konstanz.
Tsivian, Yuri (2005). Early Cinema in Russian and Its Cultural Reception. London.
Turner, Victor W. (1969). The Ritual Process: Structure and Anti-Structure. London.
Tyler, Parker (1969/1995). Underground Film: A Critical History. New York.
Urban, Peter (ed.) (1997). Anton Cechov, Drei kleine Romane. Berlin.
Vertov, Dziga (1984). Kino-Eye: The Writings of Dziga Vertov. Berekeley/Los Angeles.
Virilio, Paul (1986). War and Cinema: The Logistics of Perception, London.
Vogel, Amos (2005). Film as Subversive Art. New York.
Vogt, Carl (1847). Physiologische Briefe für Gebildete aller Stände. Giessen.
Vogt, Carl (1855). Köhlerglaube und Wissenschaft: Eine Streitschrift gegen Rudolph Wagner in
Göttingen. Gießen.
Weiser, Martin (1919). Medizinische Kinematographie. Dresden/Leipzig.
Wertheimer, Max (1967). Drei Abhandlungen zur Gestalttheorie. Darmstadt.
Wiener, Norbert (1954). The Human Use of Human Beings. Boston.
320 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
Wiener, Norbert (1956). I Am a Mathematician: The Later Life of a Prodigy. Cambridge, MA and
London.
Wiener, Norbert (1963). Kybernetik: Regelung und Nachrichtenübertragung in Lebewesen und
Maschine [German translation of 1965 with a new afterword]. Düsseldorf/Vienna.
Wiener, Norbert (1965). Cybernetics, or Control and Communication in the Animal and the
Machine. Cambridge.
Winkler, Hartmut (1992). Der filmische Raum und der Zuschauer: Apparatus – Semantik – Ide-
ologie. Reihe Siegen 110. Heidelberg.
Wolff-Heidegger, G. and A.M. Cetto (1968). Die anatomische Sektion in bildlicher Darstellung.
Basel/New York.
Wundt, Wilhelm. Hypnotismus und Suggestion. Second edition. Leipzig.
Wundt, Wilhelm. Das Institut für experimentelle Psychologie. Sonderabdruck aus der Festschrift
zum 500-jährigen Jubiläum der Universität Leipzig.
Wundt, Wilhelm (1874). Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie. First edition. Leipzig.
Wundt, Wilhelm (1902). Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie. Fifth, completely revised
edition. Leipzig.
Wundt, Wilhelm (1912). An Introduction to Psychology, London.
Zglinkicki, Friedrich von (1979). Der Weg des Films. Textband. Hildesheim, New York.
Ziferstein, Isidore (1966a). “Die therapeutische Gemeinschaft in der Sowjetpsychiatrie” in:
Psyche: Zeitschrift für Psychoanalyse und ihre Anwendungen. Vol XX/1966, 222-230.
Ziferstein, Isidore (1966b). “Die Arzt-Patient-Beziehung in der Sowjetischen Psychotherapie” in:
Psyche: Zeitschrift für Psychoanalyse und ihre Anwendungen. Vol XX/1966, 427-439.
Index
Abidjan 119 Berlin 27, 260
Abramovic, Marina 43 Bernard, Claude 165, 166, 168, 242
Académie des sciences 145, 170, 191, 195 Bernheim, Hippolyte 153
Accra 51, 117, 134 Bertillon, Alphonse / Bertillonage 101, 146
Adler, Alfred 219 Binet, Alfred 142
Adrenaline 162, 241 Blake, William 77
Africa 49, 51, 107, 117, 118, 123-125, 132, 136, 200 Bleuler, Eugen 107-109, 111, 185, 216, 224, 225
283 Benjamin, Walter 23, 28, 42, 164, 165, 261-264
Afterimage 170, 205, 206, 211 Bochow, Jörg 304, 305
Albeydou, Sambou 127 Bolex 82, 86, 91, 93, 100
American film theory 39, 52, 261 Bolsheviks 231
American experimental film 45, 77, 110 Boltanski, Grigori 249
Amphetamine 161, 162 Boulogne, Duchenne de 140, 194
Anger, Kenneth 163 Brain anatomy 222-224
Anthropology / anthropologist 13, 14, 48, 57-65, Brand, Stewart 74
68-70, 72-74, 106, 108, 114, 121-123, 130, 132, Brault, Michel 124
133, 266 Braun, Ludwig 167
Apparatus 23, 24, 26, 34, 35, 38, 41, 45-47, 49, 50, Braune, Wilhelm 190
52, 58, 62, 69, 71, 74, 75, 88, 90, 98, 105, 111, Brecht, Bertolt 231
134, 139, 140, 142, 146, 148, 151, 155, 163, 168, Breton, André 49
169, 172-174, 176, 177, 180, 181, 185, 187, 190, Broughton, James 163
195, 206, 211, 222, 228, 235, 236, 253-257, 261, Brouillet, André 137, 138, 141-145, 150, 157
262, 264, 267, 282 Brown University 57
Apparatus theory 98 Brücke, Ernst Wilhelm von 36, 237
Appolinaire, Guillaume 194 Bucharest 197, 198
Arnheim, Rudolf 39, 41, 42, 52, 68, 79, 84, 212, Bühler, Karl 219
257, 258, 282 Bull, Lucien 193, 195
Astronomy 179, 180 Buñuel, Luis 102, 197
Automatic writing 208-210 Burghölzli 107
Avant-garde 14, 28, 48, 49, 62, 109, 112, 189, 195,
219, 221, 225, 236, 259, 261, 263 Cage, John 49
California 189
Babel, Isaac 249 Cambridge Expedition 59
Babinski, Joseph 142 Canadian School of media theory 15
Balla, Giacomo 189 Cannon, Walter Bradford 14, 37, 38, 52, 53, 157,
Bali 51, 57-61, 64, 66, 71, 73, 74, 85, 112, 281 162, 172, 219, 221, 241-244
Bateson, Gregory 14, 15, 49, 51, 57-75, 77, 83, Card, James 89, 93, 219
85, 111, 112, 114, 121, 122, 132, 224, 225, 243, Cardiograph 187, 188
244, 283 Caribbean 44, 81, 106, 107, 117
Baudelaire, Charles 164 Cattell, James McKeen 219
Bazin, André 40 Charcot, Jean-Martin 14, 51, 107, 137-139,
Beatty, Talley 91 141-145, 149, 152-154, 193, 194, 201, 222
Beauviala, Jean-Pierre 128 Chauveau, Auguste 188
Behavior, behaving 36, 39, 48, 58-62, 64-68, 70, Choreography 90-92, 111
72-74, 94, 108, 114, 122, 126, 149, 162, 232, 233, Christiani, Rita 92, 93
235, 237, 238, 249, 250, 281-283 Chronophotography 94, 132, 151, 155, 156, 193,
Bekhterev, Vladimir Mikhailovich 51-53, 77, 223, 283
219, 221-245, 249, 250, 252-254, 256, 265, 267, Cinéma verité 70, 118, 120, 125-127
274, 275 Ciné-trance 51, 125, 129, 131, 180, 185
Belo, Jane 63 Claparède, Edouard 219
Benn, Gottfried 42 Cold War 27, 228
Benzedrine 47, 161-163 Collective Reflexology 52, 232, 239, 240, 266,
Bellour, Raymond 39 267
Bergson, Henri 265 Collège de France 194
Berkeley, Busby 29 Columbia University 219
322 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics
180-182, 184, 185, 202, 205, 207, 208, 212, 216, Tourou et Bitti, les tambours d’avant
217, 219, 220, 222, 225, 227, 228, 230-233, 236, (1971) 127
238, 240-244, 249, 250, 251, 253, 255, 257, Cocorico M. Poulet (1974) 120
258, 260-262, 268, 273, 275, 292, 301, 302, Royal Observatory Greenwich 179
304-306, 308 Russian Reflexology 168, 222
Psychoneurological Research Institute 77 Russolo, Luigi 189
Psycho-physiology, psycho-physiological 13, 14, Ruttmann, Walter 260, 261
161, 164, 172, 233, 277 Rye, Stellan 101
Psycho-reflexology 52, 169, 219, 227, 232, 238,
253, 274, 302 Salpêtrière 8, 37, 51, 101, 137-149, 151-155, 157, 194,
Psychotechnology, psycho-technical 42, 169, 199, 200, 224, 225, 233, 294, 315, 318
206, 212 Schizophrenia 77, 107, 114, 243, 302
Pudovkin, Wsewolod Illarionowitsch 28 Schlüpmann, Heide 17, 286
Pynchon, Thomas 27, 161, 163, 167, 169 Schomburgk, Hans 59
Schreber, Daniel Paul 182, 223, 240, 301
Racine, Jean 126 Sechenov, Ivan Mikhaylovich 168, 222, 250
Radiology / radiation 53, 157, 308 Second World War 45, 47, 74, 117, 162, 220, 244
Rank, Otto 101, 291 Seeber, Guido 101
Ray, Man 195 Shao Lin 69
Raymond, André 195 Shamans, shamanistic cure 170, 171, 182, 191,
Reflexology, reflex, reflexologist 24, 26, 34, 40, 225, 255
47, 52, 77, 102, 108, 162, 163, 167-169, 173, 208, Shklovsky, Viktor 249, 304
210, 214, 221-223, 227, 228, 230, 231, 233, 234, Shock 81, 164, 165, 187, 230, 236, 307
236-241, 243, 244, 250-253, 262, 266, 267, Siegert, Bernhard 17, 285
273-277, 301, 302, 304 Sierra Maestra 119
reflex arc 40, 210, 236, 237, 240, 266, 267 Snapshot 145, 150, 151, 275
Regnault, Félix-Louis 58, 122 Société française de photographie 150, 155
Reisz, Karel 29, 285 Solomons, Leon M. 208-212, 234, 299
Resonator 162, 177 Songhay 118, 119, 121, 122, 129
Retinal afterimages 39 Soviet Union 28, 220-222, 228, 231, 243, 244,
Reynaud, Emile 191 249, 292, 302
Rheinau 107 Spencer, Baldwin 59
Rheingold, Howard 286, 287 Spencer, Herbert 226, 227
Rhythm 23, 27, 29, 30, 34-36, 39, 46, 50, 52, 58, Sphygmograph 187, 192
70, 78, 83, 92, 106, 109-113, 126-128, 130-132, Stalin, Josef / Stalinism 30, 36, 221, 222, 228
134, 136, 151, 153, 157, 165, 170, 178-185, 190, 191, Stein, Gertrude 208-210, 214, 234
205, 206, 215, 223, 250, 255, 259, 275-278, 281, Stereoscopic camera 150
301, 305 Stern, William 219, 302
Richer, Paul 137, 141, 142, 154, 162, 193, 295 Stop trick 89
Richter, Hans 40 Straub, Jean-Marie 30
Rimbaud, Arthur 118 Stroboscope / strobe / stroboscopic seeing 39,
Rist, Pipilotti 43 40, 42, 113, 163, 179, 180, 203, 206, 211
Rites 57, 60, 110 St. Petersburg / Leningrad 52, 77, 115, 219, 225,
Ritual 15, 19, 34, 38, 44, 45, 48, 49, 51, 57, 58, 61, 228, 229, 231, 243, 244, 249, 253, 281, 302
62, 69, 70, 80, 81, 84, 92-94, 98, 104, 105, 108, Suggestibility 109, 128, 153, 216, 217, 224
110-113, 117-119, 121, 123, 125-132, 134, 136, 137, Surrealist 52, 81, 117, 195
183, 225, 277, 281 Synthetic feelings 184
Robinson, Edward G. 119
Room, Abram 249, 304 Talbot’s Law 40
Rosenblueth, Arturo 47, 242 Talley Beatty 91
Rouch, Jean 36, 38, 44, 49, 51, 70, 114, 117-135, Tarkovsky, Andrei Arsenjewitsch 28
194, 224, 225, 253, 293, 294 Tarzan 119
Bataille sur le grand fleuve (1951) 123 Tautoscope 205
Les Maîtres fous (1955) 51, 129, 130-132 Telegraph 146, 176, 190, 232, 241
Moi, un noir (1958) 119, 120, 293 Teleology 65, 66, 282
Chronique d’un été (1960) 124, 125 Theater of biomechanics 221
La Chasse au lion à l’arc (1965) 123 Thermograph 188
Petit à petit (1969) 123, 124 Third Reich 52, 203, 299
Third World 48
326 Cinema, Tr ance and C yberne tics