Emotional Intelligence PDF
Emotional Intelligence PDF
INTELLIGENCE
WHY IT CAN MATTER MORE THAN IQ
DANIEL GOLEMAN
From the
Library of Unviolent
Revolution
UnviolentPeacemaker
at ThePirateBay
3/661
BLOOMSBURY
For Tara, wellspring of emotional wisdom
Contents
Aristotle's Challenge
PART ONE
THE EMOTIONAL BRAIN
PART TWO
THE NATURE OF EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE
PART THREE
EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE APPLIED
9. Intimate Enemies
10. Managing with Heart
11. Mind and Medicine
PART FOUR
6/661
WINDOWS OF OPPORTUNITY
12. The Family Crucible
13. Trauma and Emotional Relearning
14. Temperament Is Not Destiny
PART FIVE
EMOTIONAL LITERACY
Notes
Acknowledgments
Aristotle's Challenge
Anyone can become angry —that is easy. But to
be angry with the right person, to the right de-
gree, at the right time, for the right purpose, and
in the right way —this is not easy.
ARISTOTLE, The Nicomachean Ethics
OUR JOURNEY
In this book I serve as a guide in a journey through these
scientific insights into the emotions, a voyage aimed at
bringing greater understanding to some of the most per-
plexing moments in our own lives and in the world
around us. The journey's end is to understand what it
means—and how—to bring intelligence to emotion. This
understanding itself can help to some degree; bringing
cognizance to the realm of feeling has an effect
something like the impact of an observer at the
14/661
Impulses to Action
One early spring day I was driving along a highway over
a mountain pass in Colorado, when a snow flurry sud-
denly blotted out the car a few lengths ahead of me. As I
peered ahead I couldn't make out anything; the swirling
snow was now a blinding whiteness. Pressing my foot on
the brake, I could feel anxiety flood my body and hear
the thumping of my heart.
The anxiety built to full fear: I pulled over to the side
of the road, waiting for the flurry to pass. A half hour
later the snow stopped, visibility returned, and I contin-
ued on my way—only to be stopped a few hundred yards
down the road, where an ambulance crew was helping a
passenger in a car that had rear-ended a slower car in
front; the collision blocked the highway. If I had contin-
ued driving in the blinding snow, I probably would have
hit them.
The caution fear forced on me that day may have
saved my life. Like a rabbit frozen in terror at the hint of
a passing fox—or a protomammal hiding from a ma-
rauding dinosaur—I was overtaken by an internal state
25/661
memories date from the first few years of life, in the re-
lationship between an infant and its caretakers. This is
especially true for traumatic events, like beatings or out-
right neglect. During this early period of life other brain
structures, particularly the hippocampus, which is cru-
cial for narrative memories, and the neocortex, seat of
rational thought, have yet to become fully developed. In
memory, the amygdala and hippocampus work hand-in-
hand; each stores and retrieves its special information
independently. While the hippocampus retrieves in-
formation, the amygdala determines if that information
has any emotional valence. But the amygdala, which
matures very quickly in the infant's brain, is much closer
to fully formed at birth.
LeDoux turns to the role of the amygdala in childhood
to support what has long been a basic tenet of psycho-
analytic thought: that the interactions of life's earliest
years lay down a set of emotional lessons based on the
attunement and upsets in the contacts between infant
and caretakers.9 These emotional lessons are so potent
and yet so difficult to understand from the vantage point
of adult life because, believes LeDoux, they are stored in
the amygdala as rough, wordless blueprints for emotion-
al life. Since these earliest emotional memories are es-
tablished at a time before infants have words for their
experience, when these emotional memories are
55/661
field. Nor did they have the greatest life satisfaction, nor
the most happiness with friendships, family, and ro-
mantic relationships.4
A similar follow-up in middle age was done with 450
boys, most sons of immigrants, two thirds from families
on welfare, who grew up in Somerville, Massachusetts,
at the time a "blighted slum" a few blocks from Harvard.
A third had IQs below 90. But again IQ had little rela-
tionship to how well they had done at work or in the rest
of their lives; for instance, 7 percent of men with IQs un-
der 80 were unemployed for ten or more years, but so
were 7 percent of men with IQs over 100. To be sure,
there was a general link (as there always is) between IQ
and socioeconomic level at age forty-seven. But child-
hood abilities such as being able to handle frustrations,
control emotions, and get on with other people made
the greater difference.5
Consider also data from an ongoing study of eighty-
one valedictorians and salutatorians from the 1981 class
in Illinois high schools. All, of course, had the highest
grade-point averages in their schools. But while they
continued to achieve well in college, getting excellent
grades, by their late twenties they had climbed to only
average levels of success. Ten years after graduating
from high school, only one in four were at the highest
74/661
A DIFFERENT KIND OF
INTELLIGENCE
To the casual observer, four-year-old Judy might seem a
wallflower among her more gregarious playmates. She
hangs back from the action at playtime, staying on the
76/661
silent. When her therapist then asked her how she felt at
that moment, she said she felt "awful," but couldn't cla-
rify her feelings beyond that. And, she added, from time
to time she found herself crying, but never knew exactly
what she was crying about.12
And that is the nub of the problem. It is not that alexi-
thymics never feel, but that they are unable to
know—and especially unable to put into words—pre-
cisely what their feelings are. They are utterly lacking in
the fundamental skill of emotional intelligence, self-
awareness—knowing what we are feeling as emotions
roil within us. Alexithymics belie the common-sense no-
tion that it is perfectly self-evident what we are feeling:
they haven't a clue. When something—or more likely,
someone—does move them to feeling, they find the ex-
perience baffling and overwhelming, something to avoid
at all costs. Feelings come to them, when they come at
all, as a befuddling bundle of distress; as the patient
who cried at the movie put it, they feel "awful," but can't
say exactly which kind of awful it is they feel.
This basic confusion about feelings often seems to
lead them to complain of vague medical problems when
they are actually experiencing emotional distress—a
phenomenon known in psychiatry as somaticizing, mis-
taking an emotional ache for a physical one (and differ-
ent from a psychosomatic disease, in which emotional
105/661
Cooling Down
Once when I was about 13, in an angry fit, I walked
out of the house vowing I would never return. It was
a beautiful summer day, and I walked far along lovely
lanes, till gradually the stillness and beauty calmed
and soothed me, and after some hours I returned re-
pentant and almost melted. Since then when I am
angry, I do this if I can, and find it the best cure.
the real thing, I won't get well. And if I don't get well
I'll never be happy. 12
MANAGING MELANCHOLY
The single mood people generally put most effort into
shaking is sadness; Diane Tice found that people are
most inventive when it comes to trying to escape the
blues. Of course, not all sadness should be escaped; mel-
ancholy, like every other mood, has its benefits. The
sadness that a loss brings has certain invariable effects:
it closes down our interest in diversions and pleasures,
fixes attention on what has been lost, and saps our en-
ergy for starting new endeavors—at least for the time
being. In short, it enforces a kind of reflective retreat
from life's busy pursuits, and leaves us in a suspended
state to mourn the loss, mull over its meaning, and,
140/661
Mood-lifters
145/661
them are a bit greater than usual, and they are able to
give more than usual. If there is too little demand on
them, people are bored. If there is too much for them to
handle, they get anxious. Flow occurs in that delicate
zone between boredom and anxiety."28
The spontaneous pleasure, grace, and effectiveness
that characterize flow are incompatible with emotional
hijackings, in which limbic surges capture the rest of the
brain. The quality of attention in flow is relaxed yet
highly focused. It is a concentration very different from
straining to pay attention when we are tired or bored, or
when our focus is under siege from intrusive feelings
such as anxiety or anger.
Flow is a state devoid of emotional static, save for a
compelling, highly motivating feeling of mild ecstasy.
That ecstasy seems to be a by-product of the attentional
focus that is a prerequisite of flow. Indeed, the classic
literature of contemplative traditions describes states of
absorption that are experienced as pure bliss: flow in-
duced by nothing more than intense concentration.
Watching someone in flow gives the impression that
the difficult is easy; peak performance appears natural
and ordinary. This impression parallels what is going on
within the brain, where a similar paradox is repeated:
the most challenging tasks are done with a minimum ex-
penditure of mental energy. In flow the brain is in a
184/661
mother cry, one baby wiped his own eyes, though they
had no tears.
Such motor mimicry, as it is called, is the original
technical sense of the word empathy as it was first used
in the 1920s by E. B. Titchener, an American psycholo-
gist. This sense is slightly different from its original in-
troduction into English from the Greek empatheia,
"feeling into," a term used initially by theoreticians of
aesthetics for the ability to perceive the subjective ex-
perience of another person. Titchener's theory was that
empathy stemmed from a sort of physical imitation of
the distress of another, which then evokes the same feel-
ings in oneself. He sought a word that would be distinct
from sympathy, which can be felt for the general plight
of another with no sharing whatever of what that other
person is feeling.
Motor mimicry fades from toddlers' repertoire at
around two and a half years, at which point they realize
that someone else's pain is different from their own, and
are better able to comfort them. A typical incident, from
a mother's diary:
she treated each boy. When the boys were just three
months old, Sarah would often try to catch Fred's gaze,
and when he would avert his face, she would try to catch
his eye again; Fred would respond by turning away more
emphatically. Once she would look away, Fred would
look back at her, and the cycle of pursuit and aversion
would begin again—often leaving Fred in tears. But with
Mark, Sarah virtually never tried to impose eye contact
as she did with Fred. Instead Mark could break off eye
contact whenever he wanted, and she would not pursue.
A small act, but telling. A year later, Fred was notice-
ably more fearful and dependent than Mark; one way he
showed his fearfulness was by breaking off eye contact
with other people, as he had done with his mother at
three months, turning his face down and away. Mark, on
the other hand, looked people straight in the eye; when
he wanted to break off contact, he'd turn his head
slightly upward and to the side, with a winning smile.
The twins and their mother were observed so
minutely when they took part in research by Daniel
Stern, a psychiatrist then at Cornell University School of
Medicine.7 Stern is fascinated by the small, repeated ex-
changes that take place between parent and child; he be-
lieves that the most basic lessons of emotional life are
laid down in these intimate moments. Of all such mo-
ments, the most critical are those that let the child know
199/661
they are having the desired effect. In that sense, they are
like skilled actors.
However, if these interpersonal abilities are not bal-
anced by an astute sense of one's own needs and feelings
and how to fulfill them, they can lead to a hollow social
success—a popularity won at the cost of one's true satis-
faction. Such is the argument of Mark Snyder, a
University of Minnesota psychologist who has studied
people whose social skills make them first-rate social
chameleons, champions at making a good impression.8
Their psychological credo might well be a remark by W.
H. Auden, who said that his private image of himself "is
very different from the image which I try to create in the
minds of others in order that they may love me." That
trade-off can be made if social skills outstrip the ability
to know and honor one's own feelings: in order to be
loved—or at least liked—the social chameleon will seem
to be whatever those he is with seem to want. The sign
that someone falls into this pattern, Snyder finds, is that
they make an excellent impression, yet have few stable
or satisfying intimate relationships. A more healthy pat-
tern, of course, is to balance being true to oneself with
social skills, using them with integrity.
Social chameleons, though, don't mind in the least
saying one thing and doing another, if that will win
them social approval. They simply live with the
237/661
sons they go into more detail about the causes and con-
sequences of emotions like anger (probably as a caution-
ary tale).
Leslie Brody and Judith Hall, who have summarized
the research on differences in emotions between the
sexes, propose that because girls develop facility with
language more quickly than do boys, this leads them to
be more experienced at articulating their feelings and
more skilled than boys at using words to explore and
substitute for emotional reactions such as physical
fights; in contrast, they note, "boys, for whom the verb-
alization of affects is de-emphasized, may become
largely unconscious of their emotional states, both in
themselves and in others."5
At age ten, roughly the same percent of girls as boys
are overtly aggressive, given to open confrontation when
angered. But by age thirteen, a telling difference
between the sexes emerges: Girls become more adept
than boys at artful aggressive tactics like ostracism, vi-
cious gossip, and indirect vendettas. Boys, by and large,
simply continue being confrontational when angered,
oblivious to these more covert strategies.6 This is just
one of many ways that boys—and later, men—are less
sophisticated than the opposite sex in the byways of
emotional life.
256/661
TOXIC THOUGHTS
266/661
other, and feel alone within the marriage. All too often,
Gottman finds, the next step is divorce.
In this trajectory toward divorce the tragic con-
sequences of deficits in emotional competences are self-
evident. As a couple gets caught in the reverberating
cycle of criticism and contempt, defensiveness and
stonewalling, distressing thoughts and emotional flood-
ing, the cycle itself reflects a disintegration of emotional
self-awareness and self-control, of empathy and the
abilities to soothe each other and oneself.
specific advice for men and for women, and some gener-
al words for both.
Men and women, in general, need different emotional
fine-tuning. For men, the advice is not to sidestep con-
flict, but to realize that when their wife brings up some
grievance or disagreement, she may be doing it as an act
of love, trying to keep the relationship healthy and on
course (although there may well be other motives for a
wife's hostility). When grievances simmer, they build
and build in intensity until there's an explosion; when
they are aired and worked out, it takes the pressure off.
But husbands need to realize that anger or discontent is
not synonymous with personal attack—their wives' emo-
tions are often simply underliners, emphasizing the
strength of her feelings about the matter.
Men also need to be on guard against short-circuiting
the discussion by offering a practical solution too early
on—it's typically more important to a wife that she feel
her husband hears her complaint and empathizes with
her feelings about the matter (though he need not agree
with her). She may hear his offering advice as a way of
dismissing her feelings as inconsequential. Husbands
who are able to stay with their wives through the heat of
anger, rather than dismissing their complaints as petty,
help their wives feel heard and respected. Most espe-
cially, wives want to have their feelings acknowledged
277/661
Calming Down
Every strong emotion has at its root an impulse to ac-
tion; managing those impulses is basic to emotional in-
telligence. This can be particularly difficult, though, in
love relationships, where we have so much at stake. The
reactions triggered here touch on some of our deepest
needs—to be loved and feel respected, fears of abandon-
ment or of being emotionally deprived. Small wonder
we can act in a marital fight as though our very survival
were at stake.
Even so, nothing gets resolved positively when hus-
band or wife is in the midst of an emotional hijacking.
One key marital competence is for partners to learn to
soothe their own distressed feelings. Essentially, this
means mastering the ability to recover quickly from the
flooding caused by an emotional hijacking. Because the
ability to hear, think, and speak with clarity dissolves
during such an emotional peak, calming down is an im-
mensely constructive step, without which there can be
no further progress in settling what's at issue.
Ambitious couples can learn to monitor their pulse
rates every five minutes or so during a troubling en-
counter, feeling the pulse at the carotid artery a few
281/661
Detoxifying Self-talk
282/661
Practicing
Because these maneuvers are to be called upon during
the heat of confrontation, when emotional arousal is
sure to be high, they have to be overlearned if they are to
be accessible when needed most. This is because the
emotional brain engages those response routines that
were learned earliest in life during repeated moments of
anger and hurt, and so become dominant. Memory and
response being emotion-specific, in such moments reac-
tions associated with calmer times are less easy to re-
member and act on. If a more productive emotional re-
sponse is unfamiliar or not well practiced, it is extremely
difficult to try it while upset. But if a response is prac-
ticed so that it has become automatic, it has a better
287/661
BRINGING EMOTIONAL
INTELLIGENCE TO MEDICAL CARE
The day a routine checkup spotted some blood in my ur-
ine, my doctor sent me for a diagnostic test in which I
was injected with a radioactive dye. I lay on a table while
an overhead X-ray machine took successive images of
the dye's progression through my kidneys and bladder. I
had company for the test: a close friend, a physician
himself, happened to be visiting for a few days and
offered to come to the hospital with me. He sat in the
room while the X-ray machine, on an automated track,
rotated for new camera angles, whirred and clicked; ro-
tated, whirred, clicked.
The test took an hour and a half. At the very end a kid-
ney specialist hurried into the room, quickly introduced
himself, and disappeared to scan the X-rays. He didn't
return to tell me what they showed.
354/661
HEART START
The impact of parenting on emotional competence starts
in the cradle. Dr. T. Berry Brazelton, the eminent Har-
vard pediatrician, has a simple diagnostic test of a
baby's basic outlook toward life. He offers two blocks to
an eight-month-old, and then shows the baby how he
369/661
teacher told me. "The kids all listen to see if it will stop
here or go on." For several weeks many children were
terrified of the mirrors in the restrooms; a rumor swept
the school that "Bloody Virgin Mary," some kind of
fantasied monster, lurked there. Weeks after the shoot-
ing a frantic girl came running up to the school's prin-
cipal, Pat Busher, yelling, "I hear shots! I hear shots!"
The sound was from the swinging chain on a tetherball
pole.
Many children became hypervigilant, as though con-
tinually on guard against a repetition of the terror; some
boys and girls would hover at recess next to the
classroom doors, not daring to venture out to the play-
ground where the killings had occurred. Others would
only play in small groups, posting a designated child as
lookout. Many continued for months to avoid the "evil"
areas, where children had died.
The memories lived on, too, as disturbing dreams, in-
truding into the children's unguarded minds as they
slept. Apart from nightmares repeating the shooting it-
self in some way, children were flooded with anxiety
dreams that left them apprehensive that they too would
die soon. Some children tried to sleep with their eyes
open so they wouldn't dream.
All of these reactions are well known to psychiatrists
as among the key symptoms of post-traumatic stress
386/661
EMOTIONAL RELEARNING
Such traumatic memories seem to remain as fixtures in
brain function because they interfere with subsequent
learning—specifically, with relearning a more normal re-
sponse to those traumatizing events. In acquired fear
such as PTSD, the mechanisms of learning and memory
have gone awry; again, it is the amygdala that is key
among the brain regions involved. But in overcoming
the learned fear, the neocortex is critical.
397/661
PSYCHOTHERAPY AS AN EMOTIONAL
TUTORIAL
Fortunately, the catastrophic moments in which trau-
matic memories are emblazoned are rare during the
course of life for most of us. But the same circuitry that
can be seen so boldly imprinting traumatic memories is
presumably at work in life's quieter moments, too. The
more ordinary travails of childhood, such as being
chronically ignored and deprived of attention or tender-
ness by one's parents, abandonment or loss, or social
409/661
CHILDHOOD: A WINDOW OF
OPPORTUNITY
The human brain is by no means fully formed at birth. It
continues to shape itself through life, with the most in-
tense growth occurring during childhood. Children are
born with many more neurons than their mature brain
will retain; through a process known as "pruning" the
brain actually loses the neuronal connections that are
less used, and forms strong connections in those syn-
aptic circuits that have been utilized the most. Pruning,
by doing away with extraneous synapses, improves the
signal-to-noise ratio in the brain by removing the cause
of the "noise." This process is constant and quick; syn-
aptic connections can form in a matter of hours or days.
Experience, particularly in childhood, sculpts the brain.
The classic demonstration of the impact of experience
on brain growth was by Nobel Prize-winners Thorsten
Wiesel and David Hubel, both neuroscientists.10 They
showed that in cats and monkeys, there was a critical
period during the first few months of life for the devel-
opment of the synapses that carry signals from the eye
to the visual cortex, where those signals are interpreted.
If one eye was kept closed during that period, the num-
ber of synapses from that eye to the visual cortex
dwindled away, while those from the open eye multi-
plied. If after the critical period ended the closed eye
432/661
AN EMOTIONAL MALAISE
These alarming statistics are like the canary in the coal
miner's tunnel whose death warns of too little oxygen.
Beyond such sobering numbers, the plight of today's
children can be seen at more subtle levels, in day-to-day
problems that have not yet blossomed into outright
crises. Perhaps the most telling data of all—a direct
443/661
TAMING AGGRESSION
In my elementary school the tough kid was Jimmy, a
fourth grader when I was in first grade. He was the kid
who would steal your lunch money, take your bike, slug
you as soon as talk to you. Jimmy was the classic bully,
starting fights with the least provocation, or none at all.
We all stood in awe of Jimmy—and we all stood at a dis-
tance. Everyone hated and feared Jimmy; no one would
play with him. It was as though everywhere he went on
447/661
PREVENTING DEPRESSION
456/661
DEPRESSIONOGENIC WAYS OF
THOUGHT
465/661
SHORT-CIRCUITING DEPRESSION
468/661
EATING DISORDERS
During my days as a graduate student in clinical psycho-
logy in the late 1960s, I knew two women who suffered
from eating disorders, though I realized this only after
many years had passed. One was a brilliant graduate
student in mathematics at Harvard, a friend from my
undergraduate days; the other was on the staff at M.I.T.
The mathematician, though skeletally thin, simply could
not bring herself to eat; food, she said, repulsed her. The
librarian had an ample figure and was given to bingeing
471/661
taken firm root in the lives of the young. They are crisis
intervention, the equivalent of solving a problem by
sending an ambulance to the rescue rather than giving
an inoculation that would ward off the disease in the
first place. Instead of more such "wars," what we need is
to follow the logic of prevention, offering our children
the skills for facing life that will increase their chances of
avoiding any and all of these fates.61
My focus on the place of emotional and social deficits
is not to deny the role of other risk factors, such as
growing up in a fragmented, abusive, or chaotic family,
or in an impoverished, crime-and drug-ridden neigh-
borhood. Poverty itself delivers emotional blows to chil-
dren: poorer children at age five are already more fear-
ful, anxious, and sad than their better-off peers, and
have more behavior problems such as frequent tantrums
and destroying things, a trend that continues through
the teen years. The press of poverty corrodes family life
too: there tend to be fewer expressions of parental
warmth, more depression in mothers (who are often
single and jobless), and a greater reliance on harsh pun-
ishments such as yelling, hitting, and physical threats.62
But there is a role that emotional competence plays
over and above family and economic forces—it may be
decisive in determining the extent to which any given
child or teenager is undone by these hardships or finds a
490/661
A LESSON IN COOPERATION
Compare a moment from a class in Self Science with the
classroom experiences you can recall.
A fifth-grade group is about to play the Cooperation
Squares game, in which the students team up to put to-
gether a series of square-shaped jigsaw puzzles. The
catch: their teamwork is all in silence, with no gesturing
allowed.
The teacher, Jo-An Varga, divides the class into three
groups, each assigned to a different table. Three observ-
ers, each familiar with the game, get an evaluation sheet
to assess, for example, who in the group takes the lead
in organizing, who is a clown, who disrupts.
The students dump the pieces of the puzzles on the
table and go to work. Within a minute or so it's clear
that one group is surprisingly efficient as a team; they
finish in just a few minutes. A second group of four is
engaged in solitary, parallel efforts, each working separ-
ately on their own puzzle, but getting nowhere. Then
503/661
A POINT OF CONTENTION
But as the class goes on to mull over the object lessons
in teamwork they've received, there is another, more in-
tense interchange. Rahman, tall and with a shock of
bushy black hair cut into a longish crew cut, and Tucker,
the group's observer, are locked in contentious discus-
sion over the rule that you can't gesture. Tucker, his
blond hair neatly combed except for a cowlick, wears a
baggy blue T-shirt emblazoned with the motto "Be Re-
sponsible," which somehow underscores his official role.
"You can too offer a piece—that's not gesturing,"
Tucker says to Rahman in an emphatic, argumentative
tone.
"But that is gesturing," Rahman insists, vehement.
Varga notices the heightened volume and increasingly
aggressive staccato of the exchange, and gravitates to
their table. This is a critical incident, a spontaneous ex-
change of heated feeling; it is in moments such as this
that the lessons already learned will pay off, and new
ones can be taught most profitably. And, as every good
teacher knows, the lessons applied in such electric mo-
ments will last in students' memories.
"This isn't a criticism—you cooperated very well—but
Tucker, try to say what you mean in a tone of voice that
doesn't sound so critical," Varga coaches.
505/661
TIMING IS ALL
As developmental psychologists and others map the
growth of emotions, they are able to be more specific
about just what lessons children should be learning at
each point in the unfolding of emotional intelligence,
what the lasting deficits are likely to be for those who
fail to master the right competences at the appointed
time, and what remedial experiences might make up for
what was missed.
In the New Haven program, for example, children in
the youngest grades get basic lessons in self-awareness,
relationships, and decision-making. In first grade stu-
dents sit in a circle and roll the "feelings cube," which
has words such as sad or excited on each side. At their
turn, they describe a time they had that feeling, an exer-
cise that gives them more certainty in tying feelings to
words and helps with empathy as they hear others hav-
ing the same feelings as themselves.
526/661
EMOTIONAL LITERACY AS
PREVENTION
Some of the most effective programs in emotional liter-
acy were developed as a response to a specific problem,
notably violence. One of the fastest-growing of these
prevention-inspired emotional literacy courses is the
Resolving Conflict Creatively Program, in several hun-
dred New York City public schools and schools across
the country. The conflict-resolution course focuses on
how to settle schoolyard arguments that can escalate
528/661
EMOTIONAL SELF-AWARENESS
MANAGING EMOTIONS
• More responsible
• Better able to focus on the task at hand and pay
attention
• Less impulsive; more self-control
• Improved scores on achievement tests
HANDLING RELATIONSHIPS
A LAST WORD
As I complete this book some troubling newspaper
items catch my eye. One announces that guns have be-
come the number-one cause of death in America, edging
out auto accidents. The second says that last year
murder rates rose by 3 percent.19 Particularly disturbing
is the prediction in that second article, by a criminolo-
gist, that we are in a lull before a "crime storm" to come
in the next decade. The reason he gives is that murders
by teenagers as young as fourteen and fifteen are on the
rise, and that age group represents the crest of a mini
baby boom. In the next decade this group will become
eighteen- to twenty-four-year-olds, the age at which vi-
olent crimes peak in the course of a criminal career. The
harbingers are on the horizon: A third article says that
in the four years between 1988 and 1992 Justice Depart-
ment figures show a 68 percent jump in the number of
juveniles charged with murder, aggravated assault, rob-
bery, and forcible rape, with aggravated assault alone up
80 percent.20
547/661
State-specific Reality
The working of the emotional mind is to a large de-
gree state-specific, dictated by the particular feeling as-
cendant at a given moment. How we think and act when
we are feeling romantic is entirely different from how
564/661
EMOTIONAL SKILLS
COGNITIVE SKILLS
• Self-talk—conducting an "inner dialogue" as a way to
cope with a topic or challenge or reinforce one's own
behavior
• Reading and interpreting social cues—for example,
recognizing social influences on behavior and seeing
oneself in the perspective of the larger community
572/661
RESULTS:
• more responsible
• more assertive
• more popular and outgoing
• more pro-social and helpful
• better understanding of others
• more considerate, concerned
• more pro-social strategies for interpersonal problem-
solving
• more harmonious
• more "democratic"
• better conflict-resolution skills
578/661
Paths
Mark Greenberg, Fast Track Project, University of
Washington.
Evaluated in schools in Seattle, grades 1-5; ratings by
teachers, comparing matched control students among 1)
regular students, 2) deaf students, 3) special-education
students.
RESULTS:
• Improvement in social cognitive skills
• Improvement in emotion, recognition, and
understanding
• Better self-control
579/661
• Recognition
• Labeling
• Decreases in self-reports of sadness and depression
• Decrease in anxiety and withdrawal
RESULTS:
• Improved problem-solving skills
• More involvement with peers
• Better impulse control
• Improved behavior
• Improved interpersonal effectiveness and popularity
• Enhanced coping skills
• More skill in handling interpersonal problems
• Better coping with anxiety
• Less delinquent behaviors
• Better conflict-resolution skills
SOURCES: M. J. Elias and R. P. Weissberg, "School-Based So-
cial Competence Promotion as a Primary Prevention Strategy:
A Tale of Two Projects," Prevention in Human Services 7, 1
(1990), pp. 177-200.
M. Caplan, R. P. Weissberg, J. S. Grober, P. J. Sivo, K.
Grady, and C. Jacoby, "Social Competence Promotion
with Inner-City and Suburban Young Adolescents: Ef-
fects of Social Adjustment and Alcohol Use," Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology 60, 1 (1992), pp.
56-63.
Resolving Conflict Creatively Program
Linda Lantieri, National Center for Resolving Conflict
Creatively Program (an initiative of Educators for Social
Responsibility), New York City
583/661
10. Such basic responses defined what might pass for the
"emotional life"—more aptly, an "instinct life"—of these
species. More important in evolutionary terms, these are
the decisions crucial to survival; those animals that could
do them well, or well enough, survived to pass on their
genes. In these early times, mental life was brutish: the
senses and a simple repertoire of reactions to the stimuli
they received got a lizard, frog, bird, or fish—and, perhaps,
a brontosaurus—through the day. But this runt brain did
not yet allow for what we think of as an emotion.
11. The limbic system and emotions: R. Joseph, "The Naked
Neuron: Evolution and the Languages of the Brain and
Body," New York: Plenum Publishing, 1993; Paul D.
MacLean, The Triune Brain in Evolution (New York: Plen-
um, 1990).
12. Rhesus infants and adaptability: "Aspects of emotion con-
served across species," Ned Kalin, M.D., Departments of
Psychology and Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin, pre-
pared for the MacArthur Affective Neuroscience Meeting,
Nov., 1992.
temporal lobe below and to the side of the frontal lobe (the
temporal lobe is critical in identifying what an object is).
Both these connections are made in a single projection,
suggesting a rapid and powerful pathway, a virtual neural
highway. The single-neuron projection between the amyg-
dala and prefrontal cortex runs to an area called the orbito-
frontal cortex. This is the area that seems most critical for
assessing emotional responses as we are in the midst of
them and making mid-course corrections.
1. There are many ways to calculate the divorce rate, and the
statistical means used will determine the outcome. Some
methods show the divorce rate peaking at around 50 per-
cent and then dipping a bit. When divorces are calculated
by the total number in a given year, the rate appears to
have peaked in the 1980s. But the statistics I cite here cal-
culate not the number of divorces that occur in a given
year, but rather the odds that a couple marrying in a given
year will eventually have their marriage end in divorce.
That statistic shows a climbing rate of divorce over the last
century. For more detail: John Gottman, What Predicts Di-
vorce: The Relationship Between Marital Processes and
Marital Outcomes (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Asso-
ciates, Inc., 1993).
percent jump over the 1980 rate. Teen arrest rates for for-
cible rape rose from 10.9 per 100,000 in 1965 to 21.9 per
100,000 in 1990. Teen murder rates more than quadrupled
from 1965 to 1990, from 2.8 per 100,000 to 12.1; by 1990
three of four teenage murders were with guns, a 79 percent
increase over the decade. Aggravated assault by teenagers
jumped by 64 percent from 1980 to 1990. See, e.g., Ruby
Takanashi, "The Opportunities of Adolescence," American
Psychologist (Feb. 1993).
65. The figure for boys and girls reporting sexual abuse in the
United States are from Malcolm Brown of the Violence and
Traumatic Stress Branch of the National Institute of Ment-
al Health; the number of substantiated cases is from the
National Committee for the Prevention of Child Abuse and
Neglect. A national survey of children found the rates to be
32 percent for girls and 0.6 percent for boys in a given
year: David Finkelhor and Jennifer Dziuba-Leatherman,
"Children as Victims of Violence: A National Survey," Pedi-
atrics (Oct. 1984).
66. The national survey of children about sexual abuse preven-
tion programs was done by David Finkelhor, a sociologist
at the University of New Hampshire.
67. The figures on how many victims child molesters have are
from an interview with Malcolm Gordon, a psychologist at
the Violence and Traumatic Stress Branch of the National
Institute of Mental Health.
68. W. T. Grant Consortium on the School-Based Promotion of
Social Competence, "Drug and Alcohol Prevention Cur-
ricula," in J. David Hawkins et al., Communities That Care
(San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1992).
69. W. T. Grant Consortium, "Drug and Alcohol Prevention
Curricula," p. 136.
www.bloomsbury.com/danielgoleman
Visit www.bloomsbury.com to find out more about our
authors and their books.
You will find extracts, authors interviews, author events
and you can sign up for newsletters to be the first to
hear about our latest releases and special offers.
@Created by PDF to ePub