0% found this document useful (0 votes)
171 views19 pages

Lives Without Imagery - Congenital Aphantasia

Cortex Volume Issue 2015 [Doi 10.1016%2Fj.cortex.2015.05.019] Zeman, Adam; Dewar, Michaela; Della Sala, Sergio --

Uploaded by

John
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
171 views19 pages

Lives Without Imagery - Congenital Aphantasia

Cortex Volume Issue 2015 [Doi 10.1016%2Fj.cortex.2015.05.019] Zeman, Adam; Dewar, Michaela; Della Sala, Sergio --

Uploaded by

John
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 19

Accepted Manuscript

Lives without imagery – congenital aphantasia

Adam Zeman, Michaela Dewar, Sergio Della Sala

PII: S0010-9452(15)00178-1
DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.05.019
Reference: CORTEX 1484

To appear in: Cortex

Received Date: 3 March 2015


Revised Date: 18 May 2015
Accepted Date: 20 May 2015

Please cite this article as: Zeman A, Dewar M, Della Sala S, Lives without imagery – congenital
aphantasia, CORTEX (2015), doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.05.019.

This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to
our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo
copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please
note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all
legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

PT
Lives without imagery – congenital aphantasia

RI
SC
Adam Zemana, Michaela Dewarb, Sergio Della Salac

U
AN
a
Professor of Cognitive and Behavioural Neurology, University of Exeter Medical
School, College House, St Luke’s Campus, Exeter EX1 2LU (corresponding author)
M

[email protected]
D

b
Assistant Professor and Research Leader, Psychology, School of Life Sciences, Heriot-
Watt University, Edinburgh EH14 4AS
TE

[email protected]
EP

c
Professor of Human Cognitive Neuroscience, Human Cognitive Neuroscience,
University of Edinburgh, 7 George Square, Edinburgh EH8 9JZ
C

[email protected]
AC
2
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

1. Introduction

Visual imagery is, for most of us, a conspicuous ingredient of everyday experience, playing a

prominent role in memory, daydreaming and creativity. Galton, who pioneered the

PT
quantitative study of visual imagery with his famous ‘breakfast-table survey’, reported a wide

variation in its subjective vividness (Galton, 1880). Indeed, some participants described ‘no

RI
power of visualising’. This phenomenon has received little attention since, though Faw

reported that 2.1-2.7% of 2,500 participants ‘claim no visual imagination’ (Faw, 2009).

SC
The experience of voluntary imagery is associated with activity in fronto-parietal ‘executive’

U
systems and in posterior brain regions which together enable us to generate images on the
AN
basis of our stored knowledge of appearances (Bartolomeo, 2008). The relative contributions

of lower and higher order visual regions to the experience of visual imagery are debated
M

(Bartolomeo, 2008). Clinical reports suggest the existence of two major types of neurogenic
D

visual imagery impairment: i) visual memory disorders, causing both visual agnosia and
TE

imagery loss, and ii) ‘imagery generation’ deficits selectively disabling imagery (Farah,

1984).
EP

In 2010 we reported a particularly ‘pure’ case of imagery generation disorder, in a 65 year


C

old man who became unable to summon images to the mind’s eye after coronary angioplasty
AC

(Zeman et al., 2010). Following a popular description of our paper (Zimmer, 2010), we were

contacted by over twenty individuals who recognised themselves in the article’s account of

‘blind imagination’, with the important difference that their imagery impairment had been

lifelong. Here we describe the features of their condition, elicited by a questionnaire, and

suggest a name – aphantasia - for this poorly recognised phenomenon.


3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
2. Results

21 individuals contacted us because of their lifelong reduction of visual imagery. We

explored the features of their condition with a questionnaire devised for the purpose and the

Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire (VVIQ) (Marks, 1973) (see supplementary

PT
material for further details). Participants typically became aware of their condition in their

teens or twenties when, through conversation or reading, they realised that most people who

RI
‘saw things in the mind’s eye’, unlike our participants, enjoyed a quasi-visual experience.

19/21 were male. 5/21 reported affected relatives. 10/21 told us that all modalities of imagery

SC
were affected. Our participants rating of imagery vividness was significantly lower than that

U
of 121 controls (p<.001, Mann Whitney U test – see Figure 1). Despite their substantial
AN
(9/21) or complete (12/21) deficit in voluntary visual imagery, as judged by the VVIQ, the

majority of participants described involuntary imagery. This could occur during wakefulness,
M

usually in the form of ‘flashes’ (10/21) and/or during dreams (17/21). Within a group of

participants who reported no imagery while completing the VVIQ, 10/11 reported
D

involuntary imagery during wakefulness and/or dreams, confirming a significant dissociation


TE

between voluntary and involuntary imagery (p<.01, McNemar Test). Participants described a
EP

varied but modest effect on mood and relationships. 14/21 participants reported difficulties

with autobiographical memory. The same number identified compensatory strengths in


C

verbal, mathematical and logical domains. Their successful performance in a task that would
AC

normally elicit imagery – ‘count how many windows there are in your house or apartment’ -

was achieved by drawing on what participants described as ‘knowledge’, ‘memory’ and

‘subvisual’ models.
4
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
3. Discussion

φαντασία, phantasia, is the classical Greek term for imagination, defined by Aristotle as the

‘faculty/power by which a phantasma [image or mental representation] is presented to us’

(Aristotle, 1968). We propose the use of the term ‘aphantasia’ to refer to a condition of

PT
reduced or absent voluntary imagery. Terms used previously in related contexts include

‘defective revisualisation’ (Botez, Olivier, Vezina, Botez, & Kaufman, 1985) and ‘visual

RI
irreminiscence’ (Nielsen, 1946).

SC
Sceptics could claim that aphantasia is itself a mere fantasy: describing our inner lives is

difficult and undoubtedly liable to error (Hurlburt & Schwitzgebel, 2007). We suspect,

U
however, that aphantasia will prove to be a variant of neuropsychological functioning akin to
AN
synaesthesia (Barnett & Newell, 2008) and to congenital prosopagnosia (Gruter, Gruter, Bell,

& Carbon, 2009). Indeed, aphantasia may have some specific relationship to these disorders,
M

as congenital prosopagnosia is associated with unusually low (Gruter et al., 2009), and
D

synaesthesia with unusually high (Barnett & Newell, 2008), VVIQ scores.
TE

The participants described here were self-selected and some of our findings, such as the male

predominance, may reflect the readership of a science magazine like Discover. There is a
EP

need, therefore, for further study in a more representative sample. The existence of lifelong
C

‘aphantasia’ raises numerous additional questions. How commonly does congenital


AC

aphantasia occur? Existing data suggest a frequency of around 2% but there is no fully

reported large scale study. The evidence of familial occurrence should be investigated further.

Does congenital aphantasia have objective neuropsychological associations? Correlations

between imagery vividness and cognitive functioning have been elusive in the past, but

recently developed measures of autobiographical memory (Levine, Svoboda, Hay, Winocur,

& Moscovitch, 2002), imaginative thinking and ‘visual-object intelligence’ (Blazhenkova &
5
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Kozhevnikov, 2010) open up new avenues for exploration. Personality and mood may also be

relevant variables. Are there subtypes of congenital aphantasia? The descriptions given by

our participants suggest that in some cases visual memory is preserved even if visual imagery

is absent, while others may rely entirely on non-visual representations in memory tasks; the

relationship between aphantasia and congenital prosopagnosia also deserves further study. If,

PT
as we hypothesise, the absence or reduction of visual imagery has neural correlates, can we

RI
discover these? We are optimistic that modern structural and functional brain imaging may

help to answer questions about the nature of visual imagery that were first posed in ancient

SC
Greece and first quantified at Sir Francis Galton’s breakfast table over a hundred years ago.

U
AN
M
D
TE
C EP
AC
6
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Figure legend

Figure 1: Distribution of Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire (VVIQ) scores in


participants with aphantasia and control participants (VVIQ range extends from 16, lowest
imagery score to 80, highest imagery score).

PT
RI
U SC
AN
M
D
TE
C EP
AC
7
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Reference List

Aristotle, t. b. D. W. Hamlyn. (1968). De Anima. Books II and III (with certain

passages from Book I). Oxford: Clarendon Press.

PT
Barnett, K. J. & Newell, F. N. (2008). Synaesthesia is associated with enhanced, self-

RI
rated visual imagery. Conscious.Cogn, 17, 1032-1039.

SC
Bartolomeo, P. (2008). The neural correlates of visual mental imagery: an ongoing

debate. Cortex, 44, 107-108.

U
AN
Blazhenkova, O. & Kozhevnikov, M. (2010). Visual-object ability: a new dimension

of non-verbal intelligence. Cognition, 117, 276-301.


M

Botez, M. I., Olivier, M., Vezina, J. L., Botez, T., & Kaufman, B. (1985). Defective
D

revisualization: dissociation between cognitive and imagistic thought case report and short
TE

review of the literature. Cortex, 21, 375-389.


EP

Farah, M.J. (1984). The neurological basis of mental imagery: a componential

analysis. Cognition, 18, 245-272.


C

Faw, B. (2009). Conflicting intuitions may be based on differing abilities - evidence


AC

from mental imaging research. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 16, 45-68.

Galton, F. (1880). Statistics of mental imagery. Mind, 5, 301-318.

Gruter, T., Gruter, M., Bell, V., & Carbon, C. C. (2009). Visual mental imagery in

congenital prosopagnosia. Neurosci.Lett., 453, 135-140.


8
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Hurlburt, R. T. & Schwitzgebel, E. (2007). Describing Inner Experience: proponent

meets sceptic. Cambridge, Massachusets: MIT Press.

Levine, B., Svoboda, E., Hay, J. F., Winocur, G., & Moscovitch, M. (2002). Aging

and autobiographical memory: dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychol.Aging,

PT
17, 677-689.

Marks, D.F. (1973). Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures. British

RI
Journal of Psychology, 64, 17-24.

SC
Nielsen, J. (1946). Agnosia, apraxia, aphasia: their value in cerebral localisation.

U
(2nd ed.) New York: Hoeber. AN
Zeman, A. Z., Della Sala, S., Torrens, L. A., Gountouna, V. E., McGonigle, D. J., &

Logie, R. H. (2010). Loss of imagery phenomenology with intact visuo-spatial task


M

performance: a case of 'blind imagination'. Neuropsychologia, 48, 145-155.


D

Zimmer, C. (2010). The Brain: Look deep into the mind’s eye. Discover, March Issue,
TE

28-29.
C EP
AC
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

PT
RI
U SC
AN
M
D
TE
EP
C
AC
1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Lives without imagery – congenital aphantasia

Supplementary data

1. Material sent to participants

1.1 Email sent to people who contacted us to say that they lacked visual imagination

PT
I am writing in response to your message about Carl Zimmer’s article in Discover on visual
imagination. I am the first author of the paper Carl Zimmer’s article referred to.

RI
You were one of around 15 people who made contact following this article to report that – in most
instances - you had never experienced visual imagery (while, of course, the patient we describe, MX,
had experienced vivid visual imagery until he lost it abruptly in mid-life).

SC
Although it has been recognised since the 19th century that some people get along very well without
having the experience of visual imagery, surprisingly little scientific attention has been paid to this
fascinating fact.

U
If you are willing to help us, my co-authors and I felt that we should take this unexpected
AN
opportunity to learn a little more about visual imagery – and its absence.

I am attaching two short questionnaires. I would be very grateful if you could complete and return
these electronically.
M

The first is newly created, and asks a range of questions about matters connected with imagery, for
example whether your dreams contain visual imagery. These questions are often raised when
D

imagery is discussed, and we hope that your answers will help to clarify the experience of people
who lack imagery.
TE

The second is a widely used ‘vividness of visual imagery questionnaire’. We expect you to rate your
imagery at the low end of the spectrum – given what you have written – but it would be helpful to
quantify your experience using this standard measure.
EP

We have been studying imagery in normal folk to follow up our work with MX. We anticipate putting
the data we have gathered from these participants together with what we learn from your answers
C

to write a paper drawing attention to the neglected phenomenon you have described. Looking to
the future, as we now have tools to study the working brain in action, it should soon be possible to
AC

discover why there is such a wide variation in the vividness of imagery. Your responses will highlight
the variation we need to explain.

Any information you provide will be used in an anonymous fashion. Unless you prefer not to be
informed, I will keep you in touch with the results of this small survey, and send you a copy of any
publication that results.

Many thanks for contribution so far, and for helping with this work.

Best wishes

Adam
2
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1.2 Newly devised imagery questionnaire emailed to participants with response letter above

Age

Years of full-time education

PT
Profession

When did you become aware that you were unable to form mental images?

RI
How did you become aware of this?

SC
Did this discovery have an emotional impact on you?

U
Is your lack of visual imagery total, or do you sometimes experience brief flashes of imagery?
AN
Is your ability to imagine affected by whether your eyes are open or closed?
M

Are all types of imagery affected, or can you imagine sounds (including music), textures (by imagined
D

touch), tastes or smells?


TE

Do you dream normally, and in particular do you see visual images in dreams?
EP

Is your ability to recall memorable events from the past, like holidays or celebrations, normal?

Do you think that your lack of a ‘mind’s eye’ has had any other effect on your thought processes?
C
AC

Did it affect your career choice?

Has it had an effect on your relationships (for example by making it more difficult to reminisce, or to
imagine the faces of loved ones)?

Do you think that your lack of a mind’s eye has had any particular advantages?

Do you have any relatives who have similarly noticed that they lack a mind’s eye?
3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Please count the number of windows in your house or apartment. How do you do this? Do you
inspect an image of your house or apartment as you perform this task?

Do you have any other comments?

PT
Thank you very much for your help!

RI
U SC
AN
M
D
TE
C EP
AC
4
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1.3 Vividness of visual imagery questionnaire sent to participants with response letter above

VIVIDNESS OF VISUAL IMAGERY QUESTIONNAIRE (VVIQ)

For each item on this questionnaire, try to form a visual image, and consider your experience
carefully. For any image that you do experience, rate how vivid it is using the five-point scale
described below. If you do not have a visual image, rate vividness as ‘1’. Only use ‘5’ for images that
are truly as lively and vivid as real seeing. Please note that there are no right or wrong answers to

PT
the questions, and that it is not necessarily desirable to experience imagery or, if you do, to have
more vivid imagery.

Perfecting clear and lively as real seeing 5

RI
Clear and lively 4
Moderately clear and lively 3
Dim and vague; flat 2

SC
No image at all, you only “know” that you are
thinking of the object 1

For items 1-4, think of some relative or friend whom you frequently see (but who is not with you at

U
present) and consider carefully the picture that comes before your mind’s eye.
1. The exact contour of face, head, shoulders and body _______________
AN
2. Characteristic poses of head, attitudes of body etc. _______________
3. The precise carriage, length of step etc., in walking _______________
4. The different colours worn in some familiar clothes _______________
M

Visualise a rising sun. Consider carefully the picture that comes before your mind’s eye.
5. The sun rising above the horizon into a hazy sky _______________
D

6. The sky clears and surrounds the sun with blueness _______________
7. Clouds. A storm blows up with flashes of lightning _______________
TE

8. A rainbow appears _______________

Think of the front of a shop which you often go to. Consider the picture that comes before your
mind’s eye.
EP

9. The overall appearance of the shop from the opposite side


of the road _______________
10. A window display including colours, shapes and details
Of individual items for sale _______________
C

11. You are near the entrance. The colour, shape and
details of the door. _______________
AC

12. You enter the shop and go to the counter. The counter
Assistant serves you. Money changes hands _______________

Finally think of a country scene which involves trees, mountains and a lake. Consider the picture
that comes before your mind’s eye. _______________
13. The contours of the landscape _______________
14. The colour and shape of the trees _______________
15. the colour and shape of the lake _______________
16. A strong wind blows on the trees and on the lake causing
reflections in the water. _______________
5
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
2.0 Control participants

Control results for the VVIQ were obtained previously from two groups of healthy participants: 111

healthy Psychology undergraduates and 10 healthy middle-aged professional men (all surveyors or

architects chosen to match the profession of our 2010 patient (Zeman et al., 2010)). We use their

scores for comparison with those in our participants below.

PT
3.0 Questionnaires

RI
We used the two questionnaires provided above. We note that we used a modified version of the

SC
VVIQ in which more vivid imagery is, intuitively, assigned a higher score. In the original version,

higher scores were assigned to less vivid imagery (Marks, 1973). Scores on the 5-point scale can be

U
straightforwardly converted between the two scoring systems by subtraction from 6.
AN
4.0 Aphantasia Subgroups
M

We distinguished two aphantasia subgroups based on participants’ self-rating for vividness of


D

imagery (VVIQ): i) a ‘no imagery’ group (N = 12) containing participants who responded ‘no image at

all’ across all VVIQ tasks, resulting in the lowest possible VVIQ score of 16; ii) a ‘minimal imagery’
TE

group (N = 9) containing participants with a VVIQ score greater than 16 (range = 17 – 30).
EP

5.0 Statistical Methods


C

We used IBM SPSS 21 to analyse our data. As VVIQ scores are ordinal, we applied the non-
AC

parametric Mann-Whitney U test (two-tailed) (i) to compare the sample’s VVIQ scores to those of

the control samples, and (ii) to compare VVIQ scores in the two aphantasia subgroups. We applied

one-way ANOVAs (two-tailed) to compare the two aphantasia subgroups’ age and education, and

the Fisher exact test (two-tailed) to compare their binary (Yes/No) questionnaire data. We used the

McNemar test to compare, within-subjects, voluntary and involuntary imagery in the ‘no imagery’

aphantasia subgroup. Effect sizes were determined using r (.1 = small effect, .3 = medium effect, .5 =
6
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
large effect) for the Mann-Whitney U tests, Cohen’s d (.2 = small effect, .5 = medium effect, .8 =

large effect) for the one-way ANOVAs, and phi (.1 = small effect, .3 = medium effect, .5 = large effect)

for Fisher exact tests (Cohen, 1988).

6.0 Results

PT
The aphantasia groups’ descriptive data are shown in Table 1 (see end of document). The full

aphantasia group’s VVIQ scores (median = 16) differed significantly from those of the 111 psychology

RI
student controls (median = 58) (p < .001, U = 0, r = -.63), from those of the 10 middle-aged male

SC
architects and surveyors (median = 65) (p < .001, U = 0, r = -.82), and from those of the two control

groups combined (median = 59) (p < .001, U = 0 r = -.61, see Figure 1). For comparison, in his meta-

U
analysis McKelvie (1995) calculated a mean VVIQ of 59.2 (SD = 11.07) among 1869 participants. The
AN
equivalent VVIQ means for our controls (students = 58, middle-aged architects and surveyors = 66,

two groups combined = 57.92) are similar to this mean VVIQ score.
M

The two aphantasia subgroups’ data are also shown in Table 1 (at end). The two subgroups did not
D

differ significantly in age (no imagery group: mean = 39.92, SD = 14.03; minimal imagery group:
TE

mean = 44.25, SD = 15.73; F(1, 18) =.416, p = .527, Cohen’s d = -.29) or in years of education (no

imagery group: mean = 16.83, SD = 4.13; minimal imagery group: mean = 15.88, SD = 2.17; F(1, 18)
EP

=.360, p = .556, Cohen’s d = .29). VVIQ scores were significantly higher in the ‘minimal imagery’

group (median = 26) than in the ‘no imagery’ group (median = 16) (p < .001, U = 0, r = .93).
C

Participants in the ‘no imagery’ group were more likely than those in the ‘minimal imagery’ group to
AC

report a total lack of wakeful visual imagery in our questionnaire (6/12 vs 0/9, p < .05, phi = -.55,

Fisher’s exact test). More participants in the ‘minimal imagery’ (4/9) than ‘no imagery’ (0/12) group

reported that their visual imagery was affected by eye closure (p < .05, phi = .56, Fisher’s exact test).

No further subgroup comparisons were significant (all p > .153). In order to explore the contrast

between voluntary imagery and involuntary imagery we compared, within the ‘no imagery’

subgroup, the number of participants reporting voluntary imagery (in VVIQ) and involuntary imagery
7
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
(in questionnaire). One participant did not answer the question regarding visual imagery during

dreams, resulting in a sample n of 11. Whereas none of the ‘no imagery’ participants (0/11) were

able to summon up visual images in the VVIQ test (voluntary imagery), almost all of them (10/11)

reported involuntary imagery during wakefulness and/or dreams, and this dissociation was

significant (p < .01, McNemar test).

PT
7.0 Acknowledgements

RI
Acknowledgements: we are very grateful to the participants in this study for contacting us and

SC
providing the information summarised here; we thank Dr David Mitchell for his linguistic advice; we

thank Professors John Hodges, Claus-Christian Carbon, Christian Bocti and two anonymous reviewers

U
for helpful comments. MD was funded by a Personal Research Fellowship from the Royal Society of
AN
Edinburgh and Lloyds/TSB Foundation for Scotland (grant number 29400 R41225).
M
D
TE
C EP
AC
8
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table 1 Demographic features and questionnaire results in participants with aphantasia

Full sample ‘No imagery’ ‘Minimal imagery’


Measure
(n=21) (n=12) (n=9)
Sample means (SD)

Age 41.65 (14.49) 39.92 (14.03) 44.25 (15.73)

PT
Education 16.45 (3.44) 16.83 (4.13) 15.88 (2.17)

Age aware 29.5 (15.57) 26.91 (11.37) 32.67 (19.84)

RI
Sample medians

SC
VVIQ 16 (mean = 19.9) 16 (mean = 16) 26 (mean = 25.11)

Sample frequencies

U
Sex ratio (M:F) 19:2 11:1 8:1
AN
Trigger event 18 (85.7%) 10 (83%) 8 (88.9%)

Emotional impact 8 (38.1%) 6 (50%) 2 (22.2%)


M

Total lack of imagery 6 (28.6%) 6 (50%) 0

Flashes of imagery 10 (47.6%) 5 (41.7%) 5 (55.6%)


D

Effect of eye closure 4 (19%) 0 4 (44.4%)


TE

All senses affected 10 (47.6%) 7 (58.3%) 3 (33.3%)

Visual images in dreams 17 (81%) 10 (83.3%) 7 (77.8%)


EP

Normal autobiographical memory 7 (33.3%) 4 (33.3%) 3 (33.3%)


C

Effect on cognition 13 (61.9%) 8 (66.7%) 5 (55.6%)

Effect on career choice 5 (23.8%) 2 (16.7%) 3 (33.3%)


AC

Effect on relationships 7 (33.3%) 3 (25%) 4 (44.4%)

Advantages of imagery deficit 14 (66.7%) 9 (75%) 5 (55.6%)

Relatives affected 5 (23.8%) 1 (8.3%) 4 (44.4%)

Window counting - inspect image 2 (9.5%) 0 2 (16.7%)


9
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Reference List

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioural Sciences. Hillsdale, New

Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

PT
Marks, D.F. (1973). Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures. British Journal of

RI
Psychology, 64, 17-24.

SC
Zeman, A. Z., Della Sala, S., Torrens, L. A., Gountouna, V. E., McGonigle, D. J., & Logie, R. H.

(2010). Loss of imagery phenomenology with intact visuo-spatial task performance: a case of 'blind

imagination'. Neuropsychologia, 48, 145-155.


U
AN
M
D
TE
C EP
AC

You might also like