Predicting Trajectories
Predicting Trajectories
net/publication/42583194
CITATIONS READS
39 98
3 authors:
John H. Laub
University of Maryland, College Park
93 PUBLICATIONS 14,765 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Bianca E. Bersani on 23 May 2014.
Abstract
Distinguishing trajectories of criminal offending over the life course,
especially the prediction of high-rate offenders, has received considerable
attention over the past two decades. Motivated by a recent study by
Sampson and Laub (2003), this study uses longitudinal data on conviction
histories from the Dutch Criminal Career and Life-Course Study (CCLS)
to examine whether adolescent risk factors predict offending trajectories
across the life span. The CCLS is particularly well suited to study
developmental offending trajectories as it contains detailed information
on individual criminal offending careers for a representative sample of all
individuals convicted in the Netherlands in 1977 (n = 4,615) beginning at
12 years of age and continuing into late adulthood. To assess predictive
1
University of Maryland, College Park, MD, USA
2
Leiden University, The Netherlands; Utrecht University, The Netherlands
Corresponding Author:
Bianca E. Bersani, Department of Criminology and Criminal Justice, University of Maryland,
2220 LeFrak Hall, College Park, MD, 20742, USA
Email: [email protected]
Bersani et al. 469
ability, the authors employ two different analytical approaches. First, the
authors examine whether offending trajectories can be prospectively
differentiated by risk factors identified in adolescence. Second, the
authors use group-based trajectory analysis to retrospectively identify
distinct developmental offending trajectories and employ a cross-
validation technique to examine the ability to predict the probability of
an individual’s membership in a particular trajectory group. Overall, the
results support the notion that it is difficult to predict long-term patterns
of criminal offending using risk factors identified early in the life course.
Keywords
age and crime, trajectories, prediction, desistance
Over the past two decades, the developmental course of criminal offending
has received considerable theoretical and research attention. On one hand,
Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990) argued that the age-crime relationship is
invariant and that all offenders commit fewer offenses as they age. In con-
trast, Blumstein and colleagues (Blumstein, Cohen, and Farrington 1988;
Blumstein et al. 1986) maintained that the age-crime relationship varies
and that a relatively small group of chronic offenders do not desist from
crime with age (see also Piquero, Farrington, and Blumstein 2007). Along
similar lines, Moffitt (1993, 1994) contended that there is a dual taxonomy
of offenders: a life course–persistent group wherein offending does not
decline with age and an adolescence-limited group wherein offending is
concentrated during the teenage years (for similar typological approaches,
see Loeber and LeBlanc 1990; Patterson and Yoerger 1993).
To date, research using a variety of data collected from different
sociohistorical times and geographic locales has found evidence of con-
siderable heterogeneity in offending patterns over the life course (Ezell
and Cohen 2005; Laub and Sampson 2003; Piquero et al. 2007). One
question that has emerged from these studies is whether it is possible to
distinguish or predict these varying criminal offending trajectories,
especially the chronic high-rate offender group, using risk factors iden-
tified early in the life course. Using a particularly rich data set, Sampson
and Laub (2003) tackled this question of prediction by examining long-
term criminal trajectories for a sample of high-risk boys using a variety
of factors occurring in childhood and early adolescence. Contrary to the
work of Moffitt, among others, Sampson and Laub found that prediction
was at best problematic as there was no evidence that individual, child-
hood, and/or family risk factors could predict long-term trajectories of
470 Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 46(4)
criminal offending (see also Laub and Sampson 2003; Sampson and
Laub 2005).
Although Sampson and Laub’s (2003) findings are compelling, the
generalizability of their results is questionable. Specifically, we do not
know the extent to which their findings are specific to the sociohistori-
cal context of their study or the individuals in their analysis. Moreover,
the fact that these findings have significant implications for extant crim-
inological theory as well as policies that seek early intervention in
childhood to ward off serious adolescent and adult offending, a replica-
tion of their analysis is essential. The aim of the current research is to
assess the robustness of these important findings utilizing data gathered
in a more contemporary, cross-national context with a sample contain-
ing nearly 5,000 convicted individuals.
We wish to note the growing recognition of two salient factors within
criminology that are of particular relevance to the current study. First,
there is an increasing appreciation that research should not be restricted
to data from the United States. Although recognition of the need for
more cross-national comparative studies1 is not new (see Glueck 1964),
this concern has grown in recent years (see Adler 1996; Farrington and
Wikström 1994; LaFree 2007). For instance, in his presidential address
to the American Society of Criminology, Gary LaFree (2007:14) noted:
“Stating that you are in favor of more comparative cross-national
research in criminology is a bit like saying that you are opposed to pre-
meditated murder—hardly anyone will disagree with you.” Our study
adds to the small but growing collection of comparative cross-national
criminological research. To the extent that our findings are similar or
different to those of Sampson and Laub (2003), we garner an increased
understanding of the generalizability of the findings regarding the abil-
ity to predict long-term trajectories of criminal offending.
Second, whether it is through the replication of key findings or the
systematic statistical summary of information via meta-analysis, there is
a growing recognition of the importance of doing a better job of cumu-
lating knowledge (Gendreau 2001; Lowenkamp, Cullen, and Pratt
2003). Our article contributes to the need of replication studies in crimi-
nology and expands upon previous studies of prediction of high-rate
offenders. The research presented here is especially important given that
we are just beginning to understand the development of offending over
the life course and because the implications of predicting distinct offend-
ing trajectories hold promise for public policy initiatives. We begin with
a brief review of the relevant literature before moving on to a detailed
discussion of the research presented here.
Bersani et al. 471
Current Study
The conclusions proffered by Sampson and Laub (2003) appear rather
dismal for criminological theory and public policy interventions rooted
in an early risk factor approach, but it would be imprudent to make rec-
ommendations based on these findings alone. Nevertheless, the
availability of data for conducting an investigation similar to the one
undertaken by Sampson and Laub are rare, as such an analysis requires
Bersani et al. 473
Subsample
In addition to analyzing the full CCLS sample, we also carry out sepa-
rate analyses on a random subset of male offenders (n = 689) from the
original CCLS data. Research supports that factors such as low intelli-
gence and psychological instability are important predictors of chronic
offending trajectories (DeLisi 2005; Fergusson and Horwood 2002;
Moffitt, Lynam, and Silva 1994). To measure the predictive utility of
these factors, we utilize data collected from the Ministry of Defense,
which contain information on individual traits compiled from psycho-
logical and physical assessments conducted when the men were 18 years
of age and drafted for military service.3
a
1.0
0.9
Mean Rate of Total Criminal
0.8
0.7
Convictions
0.6
0.5
0.4
0.3
0.2
0.1
0.0
12 14 16 18 20 22 24 26 28 30 32 34 36 38 40 42 44 46 48 50 52 54
Age
b
0.5
Mean Rate of Violent Criminal
0.4
Convictions
0.3
0.2
0.1
0.0
12 14 16 18 20 22 24 26 28 30 32 34 36 38 40 42 44 46 48 50 52 54
Age
Risk Factors
To examine whether trajectories of offending can be differentiated by
risk factors identified in adolescence, we employ four risk factors: two
measures of criminal history (i.e., early onset, chronic offending) and
two measures of individual traits believed to be related to crime (i.e.,
low intelligence, psychological instability).
Early onset of criminal offending has been identified as one of the
strongest predictors of long-term offending (Blumstein et al., 1986;
DeLisi 2005; Farrington et al. 1990; Loeber and LeBlanc 1990; Piquero
et al. 2007). Research suggests that individuals who begin offending at
early ages are likely to have longer, more varied, and more serious crim-
inal careers than those who initiate their criminal behavior at a later age
(for thorough reviews, see LeBlanc and Loeber 1998; Thornberry and
Krohn 2003). In the present study, we capture early onset with a mea-
sure of age at first conviction obtained from official reports. Those
respondents who had a criminal conviction history at 15 years of age or
younger are categorized as having an early onset of offending (early
onset = 1, comprising 17 percent of the sample).
Our second risk factor captures the frequency of official convictions
during adolescence (i.e., prior to age 18). Beginning in the early 1970s,
criminal career researchers identified a small subgroup of individuals,
“chronic offenders,” who account for a disproportionately large number
of crimes (Wolfgang et al. 1972). This research suggests that individuals
identified as chronic offenders have an increased likelihood of maintain-
ing their criminal behavior into adulthood (Farrington 2003; Piquero
et al. 2007). Therefore, by placing an additional restriction on our desig-
nation of at risk, including only those individuals with a history of five
Bersani et al. 477
a c
1.0 1.0
Chronic = 1
0.3 0.3
0.2 0.2
0.1 0.1
0.0 0.0
18 20 22 24 26 28 30 32 34 36 38 40 42 44 46 48 50 52 54 18 20 22 24 26 28 30 32 34 36 38 40 42 44 46 48 50 52 54
Age Age
Figure 2. Mean predicted probability of conviction for risk factors: early onset,
chronic offending (ages 18–55; n = 4,615)
2a. Any criminal conviction by early onset
2b. Violent criminal conviction by early onset
2c. Any criminal conviction by chronic offending
2d. Violent criminal conviction by chronic offending
a
1.0
0.9 Early and Chronic = 1
Mean Predicted Probability of
0.8
0.7
0.6
0.5
0.4
0.3
0.2
0.1
0.0
18 20 22 24 26 28 30 32 34 36 38 40 42 44 46 48 50 52 54
Age
b
0.5
Early and Chronic = 1
Mean Predicted Probability of a
Violent Criminal Conviction
0.3
0.2
0.1
0.0
18 20 22 24 26 28 30 32 34 36 38 40 42 44 46 48 50 52 54
Age
Figure 3. Mean predicted probability of conviction for risk factor: early onset and
chronic offending (ages 18–55; n = 4,615)
3a. Any criminal conviction by early onset and chronic offending
3b. Violent criminal conviction by early onset and chronic offending
a c
1.0 1.0
b d
0.5 0.5
0.3 0.3
0.2 0.2
0.1 0.1
0.0 0.0
18 20 22 24 26 28 30 32 34 36 38 40 42 44 46 48 50 52 54 18 20 22 24 26 28 30 32 34 36 38 40 42 44 46 48 50 52 54
Age Age
Figure 4. Mean predicted probability of conviction for risk factors: low IQ and
psychological instability (ages 18–55; n = 689)
4a. Any criminal conviction by low IQ
4b. Violent criminal conviction by low IQ
4c. Any criminal conviction by psychological instability
4d. Violent criminal conviction by psychological instability
there are magnitude differences during early adulthood, we find yet again
that all groups evidence a sharp declining pattern with age.
Subsample Analysis
Using a random subsample of cases, we investigate the predictive abil-
ity of two additional individual difference measures (low intelligence
and psychological instability) understood to be important predictors for
future criminal behavior. The analytical strategy mirrors that of the full
sample described previously. We begin with the measure of intellectual
ability and examine both any criminal conviction and violent criminal
conviction (see Figures 4a and 4b, respectively). Unlike the magnitude
differences displayed previously using official statistics to capture early
onset and chronic offending, the differences depicted in these graphs
appear negligible. Next, we examine differences in long-term offending
trajectories taking into account psychological instability. We present the
findings for any criminal conviction and violent criminal conviction in
Figures 4c and 4d, respectively. Similar to the previous analyses, the
general pattern remains substantively the same.9
482 Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 46(4)
In sum, the findings from our prospective analyses are consistent and
suggest that the ability to prospectively predict a group of high-rate, chronic
offenders defined by a flat rate of offending over the life course based on
key adolescent risk factors is problematic. Regardless of crime type or risk
predictor employed, the substantive results presented here do not change
demonstrating the robustness of the findings. The pattern among all groups
of high-risk offenders reveals a high conviction rate in young adulthood
followed by a significant declining pattern with age. Overall then, although
magnitude differences are apparent, the criminal offending trajectories
derived from the CCLS follow a general path that declines over time.
2.5
Chronic Offender
Mean Rate of Total Criminal Convictions
Classic Desister
2.0 Low-rate Offender
Sporadic Offender
1.5
1.0
0.5
0.0
18 20 22 24 26 28 30 32 34 36 38 40 42 44 46 48 50 52 54
Age
Figure 5. Latent offending trajectories for total criminal convictions (ages 18–55)
484 Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 46(4)
15 percent of the sample) follows a path that rises steadily through early
adulthood and begins declining in the mid- to late 30s. The third group
(classic desisters; 11 percent of the sample) follows the classic age-crime
curve with conviction rates peaking in early adulthood and declining
steadily thereafter. The final group, chronic offenders (4 percent of the
sample), demonstrates a high rate of convictions throughout the 20s and
30s, followed by a declining pattern beginning in the late 30s.
With the trajectory groups identified, we examine whether these groups
can be prospectively differentiated by adolescent risk factors. We display
the means for our risk factors and individual characteristics (i.e., gender,
ethnicity, marital status at age 18, and parental status at age 18) by group
membership for total criminal convictions in Table 1. Significant mean
differences are estimated using an ANOVA test with a post hoc comparison
to determine which groups significantly differed from one another. We
report the significant differences using chronic offenders as our compari-
son group. The results illustrate some significant differences in the means
across the four groups of offenders. For example, classic desisters and
chronic offenders are more often characterized by early onset. In addition,
as defined in the data, chronic offenders are characterized by chronic
offending in adolescence. In contrast, we find few significant differences
across the four groups when looking at individual characteristics. Overall,
although there are some mean differences across the four trajectory groups,
individuals identified as being at risk for high-rate, chronic offending in
adulthood (i.e., those defined by early onset, chronic offending in adoles-
cence, low intelligence, and/or psychological instability) are dispersed
across all four trajectory groups. Moreover, there is no consistent pattern-
ing of risk factors distinguishing the chronic offender group.
In the final step, we address our main research question and test the
extent to which group membership in a trajectory group can be ade-
quately predicted based on risk factors identified in adolescence and
individual characteristics. That is, we examine whether a person’s pre-
dicted group membership corresponds with or differs from a person’s
observed trajectory group. We present these results in Table 2.
We discuss only the results for the full sample (upper table) because
the results for the subsample (bottom table) are very similar. Overall, the
findings reveal that we accurately predict group membership for 71 per-
cent of the cases (in bold in table). Although the general predictive
accuracy of our model is informative, a more meaningful analysis for
criminologists interested in criminal careers involves an assessment of
the model’s predictive accuracy within groups.
Due to their continued involvement in crime into later adulthood, we
believe the two groups of greatest substantive interest are the low-rate
and chronic offender groups. When we inspect the findings from our
estimation analysis, we find that of the 328 cases observed to be mem-
bers of the low-rate offender group, only 5 are predicted to be a member
Concluding Remarks
This article contributes to the empirical literature on prediction by ana-
lyzing long-term patterns of offending (ages 18 to 55) for a large sample
of convicted men and women in the Netherlands. Data collected by the
Netherlands Institute for the Study of Crime and Law Enforcement offer
a unique opportunity to conduct a partial replication and extension of
the Sampson and Laub (2003) study. Two important findings stand out
in our analysis. First, regardless of the type of crime or the specific risk
predictor assessed, our findings indicate that although magnitude differ-
ences are readily apparent, criminal offending trajectories derived from
the CCLS follow a general path that declines over time for all convicted
offenders. These results lend support to Sampson and Laub’s argument
for a general process of desistance from crime. Second, despite the use
of data covering a large portion of the life course, containing a large
sample of serious offenders, and the application of an advanced statisti-
cal technique, our results are consistent with previous findings regarding
the difficulty of predicting high-rate offenders prospectively (see e.g.,
Auerhahn 1999; Gottfredson and Gottfredson 1994; Laub and Sampson
2003; Sampson and Laub 2003).
We acknowledge two important limitations of the current study.
First, due to data limitations we are only able to examine four adoles-
cent risk factors. Moreover, the indicators of low intelligence and
psychological instability are not optimal as they were gathered from
judgments made by a military examination committee, which may be
influenced by bias. It is important to note though that the patterns for
low intelligence and psychological instability match those found for
the early onset and chronic offending analyses. Second, our analysis
488 Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 46(4)
Funding
The author(s) received no financial support for the research and/or authorship of this
article.
Notes
1. We note that our study is not a conventional illustration of a comparative cross-
national study (see Farrington and Wikström 1994). However, we undertake a
replication of a prediction study by Sampson and Laub (2003) using data from
the Netherlands and compare the findings within an international framework.
Although somewhat limited, this form of a quasi–cross-national comparative
work is not new in criminology (see Farrington 1999).
2. For more information on the full Criminal Career and Life-Course Study
(CCLS) sample, we refer readers to the CCLS codebook (Nieuwbeerta and
Blokland 2003) and earlier publications based on this data set (Blokland,
Nagin, and Nieuwbeerta 2005; Blokland and Nieuwbeerta 2005).
3. The selection of the 689 men was carried out as follows. First, because the
archive of the Ministry of Defense does not hold detailed records for per-
sons born prior to 1940, the sample was restricted to men born after 1940
(N = 3,515). Second, due to resource limitations, data collection efforts were
restricted to gathering information for one-fifth of the cases in the total sample
(N = 736). These men were randomly selected from the full sample. Finally,
information for 47 of the men in the randomly selected subset could not be
traced. In sum, files containing full medical and military records are available
for the remaining group of 689 men. The final group is representative of men
convicted in 1977 between the ages of 12 and 37.
4. While the General Documentation Files contain information on all offenses
that lead to any type of judicial interference, here we use only information
on those offenses that were either followed by a conviction or a prosecutorial
disposition due to policy reasons, thereby excluding cases that resulted in an
acquittal or a prosecutorial disposition due to insufficient evidence. The results
of analyses conducted with these cases in the data are substantively similar.
5. Given its prevalence in 1977, the sample for driving under the influence was
confined to 2 percent. Less common, serious offenses were oversampled, including:
490 Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 46(4)
25 percent of all robbery, public violence, and battery cases; 100 percent of all
cases involving murder (including attempts), offenses against decency, rape,
child molesting, and other sexual assaults; and 17 percent of all drug offenses.
In addition, because the sample was one of cases not people, offenders who had
two or more adjudications in 1977 were more likely to be included in the study.
To deal with this sampling strategy we include a weight in all analyses so that
the weighted sample is representative of the distribution of offense types and
individuals tried in 1977.
6. The CCLS data is a conviction cohort consisting of persons who were tried
in 1977. Therefore, unlike birth cohort studies, the age range in the sample
is broad and skewed, ranging from 12 to 79 with a peak at age 18. This has
two implications. First, the convictions recorded for the sample cover a long
period—from 1924 to 2002 (when data collection concluded). Second, indi-
viduals were not randomly sampled from the entire population; they were all
criminally active in 1977. To examine the extent to which historical changes
and sampling properties affected our results, we divided the sample into three
cohorts based on the individuals’ age in 1977. The first cohort was comprised
of offenders aged between 12 and 21 in 1977, the second of those aged 22 to
31, and the third of those aged 32 and up. Because the substantive findings
reported in the following remain the same, we do not report the results by
cohort here.
7. Negative binomial regression models using a measure of the frequency of con-
victions were also analyzed. The results mirror those obtained using the logistic
regression models. We also graphed the raw data trajectories and found that the
substantive findings did not change. These results are available upon request.
Due to space considerations, we present only the smoothed trajectories based
on the logistic regression models.
8. The slight up-tick in the mean predicted probability of conviction at older ages
does not represent a trend in the observed data but is an artifact of the model.
9. Similar to our full sample analysis, we repeat the analysis with two alternative
risk factor conceptualizations capturing those with an early onset of offending
and a low intelligence or psychologically unstable designation. Of the sub-
sample, 7 percent meet this criterion. We find that these results remain substan-
tively the same.
10. These estimated parameters of the prediction model are available upon request.
11. The trajectory group and cross-validation analyses were also conducted using
violent criminal convictions as the dependent variable. We found a three-group
model best fits for trajectories of violent convictions. The results of the predic-
tion analysis are substantively similar to those reported using total criminal
convictions as the dependent variable. Specifically, excluding the sporadic
Bersani et al. 491
offender group, our model accurately predicts less than 7 percent of the cases
(results available upon request).
References
Adler, Freda. 1996. “Our American Society of Criminology, the World and the State
of Art.” Criminology 34:1–9.
Auerhahn, Kathleen. 1999. “Selective Incapacitation and the Problem of Prediction.”
Criminology 37:703–34.
Auerhahn, Kathleen. 2006. “Conceptual and Methodological Issues in the Prediction
of Dangerous Behavior.” Criminology & Public Policy 5:771–78.
Bernard, Thomas J. and R. Richard Ritti. 1991. “The Philadelphia Birth Cohort
and Selective Incapacitation.” Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency
28:33–54.
Blokland, Arjan A. J., Daniel Nagin, and Paul Nieuwbeerta. 2005. “Life Span
Offending Trajectories of a Dutch Conviction Cohort.” Criminology 43:919–54.
Blokland, Arjan A. J. and Paul Nieuwbeerta. 2005. “The Effects of Life Circums-
tances on Longitudinal Trajectories of Offending.” Criminology 43:1203–40.
Blumstein, Alfred, Jacqueline Cohen, and David P. Farrington. 1988. “Criminal
Career Research: Its Value for Criminology.” Criminology 26:1–35.
Blumstein, Alfred, Jacqueline Cohen, Jeffrey Roth, and Christy Visher. 1986. Criminal
Careers and Career Criminals. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Blumstein, Alfred, David P. Farrington, and Soumyo Moitra. 1985. “Delinquency
Careers: Innocents, Desisters, and Persisters.” Pp. 187–219 in Crime and Justice: An
Annual Review of Research, Vol. 6, edited by M. Tonry and N. Morris. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Cohen, Jacqueline. 1983. “Incapacitation as a Strategy for Crime Control:
Possibilities and Pitfalls.” Pp. 1–84 in Crime and Justice: An Annual Review
of Research. Vol. 5, edited by M. Tonry and N. Morris. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.
DeLisi, Matt. 2005. Career Criminals in Society. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Ezell, Michael E. and Lawrence E. Cohen. 2005. Desisting from Crime: Continuity
and Change in Long-term Crime Patterns of Serious Chronic Offenders. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Farrington, David P. 1999. “A Criminological Research Agenda for the Next
Millennium.” International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative
Criminology 43:154–67.
Farrington, David P. 2003. “Key Results from the First Forty Years of the Cambridge
Study in Delinquent Development.” Pp. 137–184 in Longitudinal Research in
the Social and Behavioral Sciences: An Interdisciplinary Series, edited by: T. P.
Thornberry and M. D. Krohn. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
492 Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 46(4)
Farrington, David P., Rolf Loeber, Delbert S. Elliott, J. David Hawkins, Denise Kandel,
Malcolm Klein, Joan McCord, David C. Rowe, and Richard E. Tremblay. 1990.
“Advancing Knowledge about the Onset of Delinquency and Crime.” Pp. 283–342
in Advances in Clinical Child Psychology. Vol. 13, edited by B. B. Lahey and A. E.
Kazdin. New York: Plenum.
Farrington, David P. and Per-Olof H. Wikström. 1994. “Criminal Careers in London
and Stockholm: A Cross-National Comparative Study.” Pp. 65–89 in Cross-
National Longitudinal Research on Human Development and Criminal Behavior,
edited by E. G. M. Weitekamp and H-J. Kerner. Dordrecht, the Netherlands:
Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Fergusson, David M. and L. John Horwood. 2002. “Male and Female Offending
Trajectories.” Development and Psychopathology 14:159–77.
Ge, Xiaojia, M. Brent Donnellan, and Ernst Wenk. 2001. “The Development of Persistent
Criminal Offending in Males.” Criminal Justice and Behavior 28:731–55.
Gendreau, Paul. 2001. “We Must Do a Better Job of Cumulating Knowledge.”
Canadian Psychology 43:205–10.
Glueck, Sheldon. 1964. “Wanted: A Comparative Criminology.” Pp. 304–22 in
Ventures in Criminology, edited by S. Glueck and E. Glueck. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Glueck, Sheldon and Eleanor Glueck. 1950. Unraveling Juvenile Delinquency. New
York: The Commonwealth Fund.
Gottfredson, Don M. 1987. “Prediction and Classification in Criminal Justice
Decision Making.” Pp. 1–20 in Prediction and Classification: Criminal
Justice Decision Making, edited by D. M. Gottfredson and M. Tonry. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Gottfredson, Michael R. and Travis Hirschi. 1990. A General Theory of Crime.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Gottfredson, Stephen D. and Don M. Gottfredson. 1994. “Behavioral Prediction and
the Problem of Incapacitation.” Criminology 32:441–74.
Gottfredson, Stephen D. and Laura J. Moriarty. 2006. “Statistical Risk Assessment:
Old Problems and New Applications.” Crime & Delinquency 52:178–200.
Gove, Walter R., Michael Hughes, and Michael Geerken. 1985. “Are Uniform
Crime Reports a Valid Indicator of the Index Crimes? An Affirmative Answer
with Minor Qualifications.” Criminology 23:451–501.
Harcourt, Bernard E. 2006. Against Prediction: Profiling, Policing and Punishing in
an Actuarial Age. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Hindelang, Michael J., Travis Hirschi, and Joseph G. Weis. 1979. “Correlates of
Delinquency: The Illusion of Discrepancy between Self-report and Official
Measures.” American Sociological Review 44:995–1014.
Bersani et al. 493
Piquero, Alex R., David P. Farrington, and Alfred Blumstein. 2007. Key Issues in
Criminal Career Research: New Analyses of the Cambridge Study in Delinquent
Development. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Sampson, Robert J. and John H. Laub. 2003. “Life-course Desisters? Trajectories of
Crime among Delinquent Boys Followed to Age 70.” Criminology 41:301–40.
Sampson, Robert J. and John H. Laub. 2005. “A Life-course View of the Development
of Crime.” Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Sciences
602:12–45.
Stattin, Håkan and Ingrid Klackenberg-Larsson. 1993. “Early Language and
Intelligence Development and Their Relationship to Future Criminal Behavior.”
Journal of Abnormal Psychology 102:369–78.
Thornberry, Terence P. and Marvin D. Krohn. 2003. Taking Stock of Delinquency:
An Overview of Findings from Contemporary Longitudinal Studies. Longitudinal
Research in the Social and Behavioral Sciences: An Interdisciplinary Series.
New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
Tonry, Michael. 1987. “Prediction and Classification: Legal and Ethical Issues.” Pp.
367–413 in Prediction and Classification: Criminal Justice Decision Making,
edited by D. M. Gottfredson and M. Tonry. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
Wolfgang, Marvin, Robert Figlio, and Thorsten Sellin. 1972. Delinquency in a Birth
Cohort. Chicago: University of Chicago.
Bios
Bianca E. Bersani is a doctoral candidate in the Department of Criminology and
Criminal Justice at the University of Maryland, College Park.
Paul Nieuwbeerta was a senior researcher at the Netherlands Institute for the Study
of Crime and Law Enforcement (NSCR) when writing this article. Currently he is a
professor in the Department of Criminology at Leiden University and in the
Department of Sociology at Utrecht University in Utrecht.