Document Rime: 11101 Plus Postage. PC Not Available From ERRS
Document Rime: 11101 Plus Postage. PC Not Available From ERRS
ABSTRACT
Since E.L. Thorndike's landmark 1917 study of the
complexity of reading comprehension, inferential research has
generally focused oa either inference as a developmental process or
the nature of inferences made during reading. In his 1930 study, R.
M. Tyler established that inferior* could be objectively measured. S.
G. Paris conducted several studies in the 1970s showing that readers'
age and ability level influenced their inferenciag skills, and
indicating that young childres'are capable of producing inferences
but usually do not do so spontaneously. Examinations of the nature of
inferences have not been as conclusive as developmental studies. S.
T. Goets's 1977 study of high school students found no main effects
for the importance or salience of the reading material. A 1979 study
by P. D. Pearson, J. Hansen, and C. Gordon revealed that prior
knowledge has a simple effect for Inferable questions but no effect
for explicitly stated questions. In a major contribution on the
nature of inferencing, R. Tierney, C. A. Bridge, and M. Coma
discovered that good readers made more causal and conditional
connections between propositions while poor readers more often
overgeseralised. One point of unanimity in reading research is that
inferential ability is vital to mature readers. (MM)
***********************************************************************
* Reproductions supplied by EMS are the best that can be made *
* from the original document. *
***********************************************************************
TAIL 011PAI1ERINT Of SONCA116111
NATIONAL INSTEIVIE Of ESUCATION
EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATION
CENTER iEluCI
)dhs dOMMOI hog limn ispeamod
amtoml Item Ow porton WOOWSION
044111640 4
4001 AO** basil 0i00, a) 4m
MMAKUMOWft
Poems of vow at opinions stood in *adieu
wont do not notwtototty musuom Wheel NO
moon Of pokey
INFERENCE: A RESEARCH REVIEW
JoBeth Allen
Assistant Professor
"PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE MS
MATERIAL IN MICROFICHE ONLY
HAS SEEN GRANTED BY
JoBeth Anal
have emerged. The present paper will examine one of these factors,
inference, which was an almost unheard of topic before 1970. Calfee and
While many authors agree that there is such a skill as drawing in-
defined two ways in which inferences are made. The first involves text
2
even definitions, inference has been the topic of much research in the
plexity of comprehension for the first time; up to that point reading had
factual recall and inference. Thorndike was topressou with the extensive
variety of answers to his questions, and found that the responses did
(Thorndike, 1917):
closely tied to the reasoning process; thus, his title in reporting the
4
3
Reading."
each word must produce a correct meaning (text factor), which the reader
must weigh correctly ih relation to other words (reader factor), and then
conducted in 1941 with college freshmen, concluded that the two most
important factors in reading were memory for word meanings and reasoning
The second study was a much more carefully controlled and planned in-
maerices.
4
Table 1
3. factual recall 10
Davis again interpreted his data to support word meanings and drawing
inferences as the two factors accounting for over half of the unique
valiance.
Spearritt (1972) used Davis' 1968 study and applied the statistical
factor, but felt that three other factors, drawing inferences from context,
reasoning.
adopter! the goal that each student should have "An ability to draw inferences
6
from facts, that is, to propose hypotheses." To measure students' ability
The students took two tests in the same day, one requiring production of
interpret the facts" (Tyler, 1930, p. 476). A sample paragraph and logical
inference were provided. Papers were collected, and the students were
given the same items in multiple choice form. One might expect a rather
of this data was that "the ability to select the most reasonable inference
from a given list is not the same as the ability to propose an original
inference " (p. 477-8), Tyler did not consider the possibility that
inferences drawn in the first measure might have interferred with the ability
member of the education faculty use for sorting samples of student inferences.
student" (p. 479). He further validated 'he procedure with new faculty
testing inference had, very low correlation (r..29) with a test of factual
7
6
knowledge over the same material. He concluded that inference was not only
a separate skill, but that it could be measured and "the merit of an in-
ference is not the purely subjective quality often claimed" (p. 479).
cern until the seventies. The only mention of inferential ability was in
studies such as Davis' (1944), where it was considered as part of the hier-
archy of reading skills. Even these studies dealt with .nature readers,
rather than with children learning the basics of the reading process. As
words, syllables and even letters. Rate was easily measured, as were eye
coaprehension were examined. Reading research was given added impetus from
seventies produced intense discussion and theory about the nature of the
reading process; the theories are presently being tested through research.
The studies evolving from this atmosphere pertinent to inference are re-
ported here.
ducted by Paris (Paris & Upton, 1974; Paris, 1975; Paris & Lindauer, 1976).
Tierney and Cunningham (1980) summarize the first study by noting that "Paris
(1975) found young children are less able to relate their own background ex-
with the opinion of those who feel that young children do not have sufficient
7
factor in his next study (Paris & Upton, 1974). 'Paragraphs were read to
grade. Pupils were asked to respond to four literal and four inferential
affects for grade and question type (p. < .01), was that younger children
ducted an experiment using subjects from first, third and fifth grades.
either explicit (The man dug a hole with a shovel) or implicit (The man
dug a hole.) They were then tested on the four implicit and four explicit
sentences by being given the cue (which had been stated in only half of
the sentences) and asked to recall the sentence. Analysis showed a signi-
ficant difference between grades one and three, and one and five, but not
between three and five, indicating that the development of the skill
second and fourth graders. In this design, all sentences implied an in-
strument, but did not state it. Questions were asked using as recall cues
9
8
ten first graders act out the implicit sentences. The children were in-
structed to perform the actions for "The man dug a hole." The results
the difference between explicitly cued and implicitly cued sentences was
70% of the implicit-cue sentences. Paris and Lindauer concluded that young
children were capable of drawing inferences, but that they had no spon-
unfortunate that Paris and Lindauer did not use a large enough sample to
report their results in terms of significance; and that no one has sought
process identified by Paris. Iiildyard (1976) had sixteen first, third and
fifth graders listen to passages, end sixteen third and fifth graders read
knowledge.
10
9
and its effect on recall, examined the types of inferences children made in
terms of their appropriateness. Their subjects were first and fourth grade
.36; 4th - .31; adult - .35). There were developmental trends in two other
with the original text increased with age (1st - .15; 4th - .41; adult - .49).
age (1st - .40; 4th - .29; adult -.16). Wandler and Johnson interpreted these.
fourth grades.
information (e.g., a mean, selfish fox helps a weak bear catch and cook a
fish). When asked for exact recall, first graders recalled only the story
line, not the discrepant description of the fox. Fifth graders recalled
both the story and the description; some even made some inferences about
the fox's motives. When asked to "fill in anything that is missing," first
Fifth graders made inferences as to the reasons for the incongruous action,
10
and sought to provide resolution to the story. While her report contained
of 144 children. Their material was written in two forms; the control
Table 2
K 3 K 3
difficulty in der mg with ambiguity for first and third graders. However,
the older students were better able to make use of added information in
resolving ambiguity.
Poulsen, Kintsch, Kintsch,and Premack (1979). They found that four and
r
six year old children can both make up stories from pictures presented in
logical order. However, when the pictures were presented in random order,
the younger children reverted to simply labeling each picture. The older
children tried to make sense of the pictures as presented, and made in-
ferences about thoughts and emotions in order to tie a story line together.
in the Poulsen, et al. study were six; the younger children in Stein's
study were first grade, probably six or seven years old. The age that
One possible explanation is that in the Stein study, the story was read to
subjects; therefore, they did not have the text for reference. Subjects
congruity, and could ponder and process with more ease. Also, the required
(which both four and six year old children in the study did),- requires
the logical connection of ideas and events. In the case of the incon-
create a cohesive story. In contrast, the Stein task was only loosely
13
12
mental nature, has to do with the nature of inferences made. Much of the
reading theorists (Anderson, 1977; Rumelhart & Ortony, 1977; Spiro, 1977).
Various systems for analyzing inferences have been devised, such as Collins,
Based Inferences has been used in several research designs as a system for
inference.
further separated into high and low verbal ability sections. The material
One story, for example, presented the reader with the information that a
woman was late to the airport because she stopped to talk to her son. In
the talk causes her to miss her flight, which crashes; thus the time with
her son saved her life. Other text variables included salience and
13
There were six sets of eight passages with various combinations of im-
Subjects were asked five questions about each passage. The first was
spersed were basic premise questions and questions used to control for
test after reading the passage and before answering the questions; this
Main effects were reported for grade level and verbal ability. There
was no main effect for importance; however, there was an interaction be-
tween importance and explicitly stated items. Goetz reports this inter-
action:
15
14
avert that they were making inferences, since they rated significantly
A study designed to examine when and how inferences are made was
passages with C coming after the A-B relationship; the other group read
3)-6) other questions about the passage, designed to mask the intent
7) C information.
scale.
There were significantly (p < .015) more errors when C was read after
A-B than when C occurred before. In the C-after condition, subjects often
reported that the inference was not in the text (B-mention errors) or that
the opposite was true (B- incorrect errors). Spiro and Esposito concluded
readers are more likely to make errors after the inference has been made,
findings. The authors report sample sizes of only four students in each
of the experimental gra,'Ps, and eight in the control croup. While they
report that group differences accounted for 23.7% of the reported variance,
they did not specify whether this analysis was of the experimental versus
control, between the two experimentals, or among all three. Four remains
problem surfaces in examining the instructions given the students and the
time allowed for reading. Subjects were instructed to read and understand
each passage, and asked not to look back. However, they were given nine
minutes for each 250 word passage. Sonic must have filled the eight minutes
with rereading, if only out of boredom. Others might have processed the
Implied Propositions by Good and Poor Readers Using Three Types of Assess
Achievement Test; poor readers were from the same grade reading at a
having readabilities of 3.0, 4.0 and 5.0 on the Fry graph (although the
17
16
Hain effects were found for ability, explicitness and question type.
Good readers performed better than poor readers, with means of 2.85 and
times 2.47; the mean for implicit items was 1.59. Means for multiple
choice, aided recall and free recall were 2.32, 1.40 and .80, respectively.
The authors reported surprise that even the high group recalled only 52%
was not reported whether the scores from the CAT were overall reading
large part of the determinant score, then the poor performance on com-
concern is the readability level of the passages used. All passages were
at an independent level for the good readers. The poor readers, on the
other hand, may have been reading at instructional and even frustration
levels, since their average reading ability was 5.0. This failure to
was the focus of a study conducted by Pearson, Hansen, and Gordon (1979).
tiated by strong and weak schemata for knowledge about spiders. Recall
valid than frame recall. The two groups did not differ significantly on
18
17
answering four questions were dropped from the study; the remaining groups
about spiders, and a narrative line. The story had a readability level of
2.8 on the Spache Readability Formula. Students read the passage once,
silently, and could not refer to it during questioning. Examiners did not
help with unknown words. Explicit and inferential knowledge were measured
during questioning.
prior knowledge and question type, both significant at the .01 level,
Table 3
strong
schema 4.7 2.8 7.5
weak
schema 3.8 1.0 4.8
19
18
findings are not consistent with the prediction that prior knowledge would
affect both inferential and literal recall. However, it seems that the
into the design. The study showed that both groups learned stated facts
equally well. The more difficult process of combining facts with prior
knowledge.
Tierney, Bridge,and Cera (1978-79). The study was concerned with the
groups of good and poor readers on the basis of Stanford Achievement Test
responded to free and probed recall. Probe questions were based on free
recall reg es, and avoided leading readers to insights they had not
answer significantly more questions than poor readers, whether the questions
20
19
Good readers tend to do everything in the reading task better than poor
readers; that is why their SAT scores and teachers' opinions single them
readers.) The third major finding was that during free recall, both
recorded.
the nature of inferences made by good and poor readers. The discussion
section explains:
This fis44ng indicates the need for more specific criteria for
on their basis in text. Warren et al. (1979) point out the necessity
21
20
test vac not reported; groups were assigned according to percentiles (good
these are not equivalent). It was reported that the experimenters dropped
those who scored below 84% word recognition on the experimental material.
many of the poor readers were reading at a frustration level. Did their
with their ability to comprehend? Bid the fact that the passage was read
probing the good readers were willing to extend their recalls beyond the
were tuned into the "right answer syndrome" which emphasizes the recall
posed to elicit inferences, since that was the focus of the study. It
is also logical to assume that the good readers, especially, had already
the responses of nine poor and nine good readers according to five
22
21
connection. Results were significant for the finding that poor readers
deleted more propositions than good readers (p .05), and that good
good and poor readers" (p. 32). While this seems to be an oversimplifi-
but not the central focus of this paper. Nardelli (1957) failed to find
23
22
this was not the result hypothesized. Although their results with college
students may not generalize, Steingart and Clock (1979) found imagery
to have a more facilitating effect than mere repetition for making in-
ferences. Second graders studied by Hansen and Pearson (1980) made more
inferences, and the learning transferred better, when they were psrt of a
group where inferential questions were asked regularly. This group was
This paper has been an attempt to analyze two main branches of inferen-
measured. When several different ages and ability levels of readers were
While almost every study of inferende has shown that older children
most readers give more literal recalls than inferential ones, other
24
23
a task that proved too difficult for four year old subjects (Paulsen, et al.).
the hypotheses that importance, salience and explicitness would affect the
did appear, from reported interactions, that inferences are more likely to
be made when they are important to the 'tory; salience was reported to
79) discovered that good readers made more causal and conditional connections
Bridge (1979) later reported that the key to good reading was the ability
to integrate propositions.
that inferentia' ability is vital in the mature reader. whose who are
devoting their energies to. producing mature readers have accepted the
24
small or ubwortby task to learn 'what the book says" (Thorndikes 1917,
p. 434).
References
Calfee, R. C. & Drum, P. A. How the researcher can help the reading
(Eds.), Theory and practice of early reading (Vol. 1). Hillsdale, N.J.:
Erlbaum, 1979.
(Tech. Rep. No. 40). Urbana, Ill.: University of Illinois, Center for
XXIX, 597-606.
27
Frederiksen, C. H., Frederiksen, J. D., Humphrey, F. M., & Ottesen, J.
Toronto, 1976.
8, 217-227.
Schreiner, R., & Shannon, P. The recall of explicit and implied propo-
May 1980.
150 545).
Steingart, S. K., & Glock, M. D. Imagery and the recall of connected discourse.
323-332.
14, 539-573.
0193-4759)
Trabosso, T. On the making of inferences during reading and their
ED 181 429).
Warren, W., Nicholas, D., & Trabasso, T. Event chains and inferences