Ross and Strack e 2016
Ross and Strack e 2016
net/publication/310607730
CITATIONS READS
8 341
1 author:
Andrew S. Ross
The University of Sydney
21 PUBLICATIONS 80 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Andrew S. Ross on 14 October 2017.
1. Introduction
Until recently, emotions had received scant attention in applied linguistics re-
search. Emotions lie at the very heart of the human experience and are present
in all aspects of life. Generally, we feel happy if we succeed at something, embar-
rassed if we make a mistake in class, or angry when we feel as though we have been
wronged. Learning a language is a challenging task and it is likely any language
learner would testify that the experience of learning a language and using it both
inside and outside the classroom has triggered a range of positive and negative feel-
ings and experiences. One particular emotion is pride, generally seen as a positive
emotion as “our most meaningful achievements, both everyday and life changing,
are accompanied by feelings of pride” (Tracy & Robins, 2007, p. 147). Pride is an
especially important and powerful emotion within educational contexts, emerg-
ing in different students in different manifestations and situations, thus providing
valuable insight into language learner psychology. Nonetheless, the discrete emo-
tion of pride is not necessarily always ‘positive’. It is possible to see that within indi-
vidual emotions – pride included – there exists a degree of dimensionality that can
manifest in different forms ranging from an absence of the feeling of pride (where
jbid177713 IP: 101.184.148.117 On: Thu, 06 Apr 2017 10:23:46
2. Literature review
There are differing opinions on how emotions can be defined. Keltner and Ekman
(2000) posit emotions are individual responses to events or experiences involv-
ing experiential, psychological, and behavioural activity. Kalat and Shiota (2007)
put forward four useful criteria for understanding exactly what basic emotions
such as happiness are. The first criterion states that a basic emotion is necessarily
universal. Universal does not mean that a basic emotion should be described the
same way linguistically in different cultures and countries, but just that it exists
in all cultures. Of course, some emotions will have an exact translation in mean-
ing, while others might not have an equivalent word in other cultures, a situation
Pavlenko (2008) refers to as conceptual non-equivalence. The second criterion is
that basic emotions are caused by a specific life event. For example, most would
find it hard to disagree that the death of a loved one would evoke sadness. The
third criterion is that basic emotions should be evident early in life, which helps
to distinguish complex and basic emotions. Basic emotions can be expressed in
the facial expressions of infants (happiness/sadness) whereas complex ones can-
not until later in life (such as guilt). A final criterion is that there must be a way to
express the emotion that is built into human physiology and neurobiology such as
274 Andrew S. Ross and Elke Stracke
facial expression and voice. Izard (2011) provides a more general and simplistic
definition of basic emotions in stating that they are those emotions critical to hu-
man nature such as fear and happiness.
Another more complex category of emotions is that of ‘self-conscious’ emo-
tions. Self-conscious emotions require an evaluation of the self in relation to a spe-
cific event to be successfully evoked (Lewis, 2008; Tangney, 2002). Pride belongs
in this category, and in particular the category of the positive self-conscious emo-
tions due to its association with feelings of achievement and satisfaction (Stoeber,
Kobori, & Tanno, 2013).
Emotions are also generally divided into the categories of positive and nega-
tive emotions. These categories need to be briefly explored, particularly in light
jbid177713 IP: 101.184.148.117 On: Thu, 06 Apr 2017 10:23:46
of the focus of the current study. Fredrickson (1998, 2001, 2003) defines positive
emotions as carrying a pleasant subjective feeling such as joy or happiness. In con-
trast, negative emotions can carry an unpleasant subjective feeling such as sadness
or fear. Positive emotions have tended to be particularly underrepresented in emo-
tion research in psychology, and this remains true within the field of applied lin-
guistics (Dewaele & MacIntyre, 2014) in comparison to negative emotions such as
language learner anxiety. The distinction between positive and negative emotions
can be problematic considering that emotions are grouped as such according to
how they are commonly perceived, without giving weight to the different manner
in which they might or might not be experienced.
2.2 Pride
Pride can be triggered both by an individual’s perception that they have achieved
some kind of outcome of personal value or that they are valued as a person by oth-
ers. Such perceptions establish an important self/other distinction in terms of the
evocation of pride; that is, the individual has achieved something of value to the
self, or something that is appraised as valuable and good by others thus allowing
and fostering an improvement in self-esteem or self-worth, as well as improving
others’ perceptions (Muris & Meesters, 2014). In relation to the self, Tracy and
Robins (2004) differentiate between authentic pride based on specific individual
achievements (as in “I’m proud of what I just did”) and hubristic pride, which
refers to a pride in the global self (“I’m proud of who I am”). Similarities with self-
esteem might be seen in both of these forms of pride, but there is a distinction that
needs to be made on the basis that pride is considered to be a feeling or a state, and
self-esteem a trait. The difference between a state and a trait rests in the temporary
nature of a state, where an emotion is evoked in response to a particular event or
situation, whereas a trait is a more stable personality characteristic. Brown and
Marshall (2001, p. 581) have expanded on this connection between pride and
Learner perceptions and experiences of pride in second language education 275
self-esteem and explain that because pride involves “an appraisal of one’s worth as
a person” it is a significant predictor of self-esteem. Pride can therefore contribute
to the trait-like nature of self-esteem, but should not be seen as synonymous.
Discussion of authentic and hubristic pride helps to reveal some of the dis-
tinct positive and negative connotations of the emotion. Essentially, authentic
pride carries positive connotations and provides a connection with self-esteem.
For example, after achieving success, people experience feelings of pride. These
may eventually promote self-esteem (Tracy & Robins, 2007), making authentic
pride the more socially desirable experience of pride. On the other hand, the nega-
tive connotations of pride cannot be ignored. In fact, pride as a negative emotion
can be traced back to the Bible and its presence among the ‘seven deadly sins’.
jbid177713 IP: 101.184.148.117 On: Thu, 06 Apr 2017 10:23:46
Emotion research within applied linguistics was significantly lacking for a signifi-
cant period of time as recognized by Scovel (2001) who stated, “affective variables
are the area that SLA researchers understand the least” (p. 140). More recently,
Swain (2013, p. 205) has contended that there is still some truth to this situation
and has described emotions in SLA research as “the elephants in the room – poor-
ly studied, poorly understood, seen as inferior to rational thought”. Her statement
highlights the focus on cognition, rather than affect and emotions, in SLA research
in earlier decades.
Nonetheless, to date there has certainly been valuable research, and the vol-
ume of research continues to grow. Firstly, language learner anxiety has received
extensive coverage. Horwitz, Horwitz, and Cope (1986) produced the most no-
table work in this context when they developed the concept of Foreign Language
Classroom Anxiety, the most researched emotion to date in SLA. Further research
included that of Schumann (1997), who used neurological and psychological ex-
posure to stimulus material to explore the role of emotion in the SLA process.
276 Andrew S. Ross and Elke Stracke
that this shifting research paradigm is the result of a move away from considering
emotions only as part of a group of ‘affective factors’ including those of anxiety,
motivation and willingness to communicate, and towards a focus on emotions
in their own right.
Although this move away from merely focusing on affective factors is un-
derway, Dörnyei’s (2005, 2009) L2 motivation self system still holds relevance in
the current study due to the close link between emotions and motivation. In fact,
MacIntyre, Mackinnon, and Clément (2009, p. 47) described emotions as “funda-
mentally important motivators”. This link should not be ignored. The key compo-
nents of the L2 Motivational Self System of significance in the current study are
the Ideal L2 self and the Ought-to self. The Ideal L2 self refers to the learner’s goals
and aspirations as a language learner, whereas the Ought-to self is concerned with
attributes the learners believe they ought to possess in order to meet external ex-
pectations. Emotions in general, and pride specifically, can be seen to play a role in
the Ideal and Ought-to L2 selves.
There is clear indication that emotion research is continuing to gain momen-
tum as part of an ‘affective turn’. Further evidence can be seen in the influence
of the positive psychology movement, which has become visible within applied
linguistics. The category of positive emotions represents one of the core elements
of positive psychology (MacIntyre & Mercer, 2014) defined as development of
the ability to “understand and foster the factors that allow individuals, communi-
ties, and societies to flourish” (Fredrickson, 2001, p. 218). Dewaele and MacIntyre
(2014) highlight the growing attention to positive emotions in a study that contin-
ued the interest in the negative emotion of anxiety, but also focused on the positive
emotion of enjoyment. Another study by Gregersen, MacIntyre, Finegan, Talbot,
and Claman (2014) explores how the emotional intelligence of language learn-
ers can be used to harness positive emotions and aid in the language acquisition
process. Within SLA the interest in positive psychology continues to grow, and a
Learner perceptions and experiences of pride in second language education 277
recent volume on the topic edited by MacIntyre, Gregersen, and Mercer (2016)
can be taken as further evidence.
Playing our own part in Pavlenko’s (2013) ‘affective turn’, we present our find-
ings regarding the self-conscious, positive emotion of pride and explore percep-
tions and experiences of pride of tertiary international ESL students in Australia.
In the following section we offer some background information for the study be-
fore providing relevant information about the participants, the interviews, and the
subsequent data analysis.
3. The study
jbid177713 IP: 101.184.148.117 On: Thu, 06 Apr 2017 10:23:46
The focus of the current paper is drawn from a large-scale mixed methods re-
search project investigating the emotional experiences and motivation of tertiary
English language learners in Australia (Ross, 2015). During the project, the emo-
tion of pride evoked a multitude of salient responses from the participants. This
resulted in the current project that focuses solely on pride from a qualitative per-
spective. We explore how pride is, and is not, experienced by these ESL learners
both in the context of learning, that is the classroom; and in the context of social
interaction, that is, the communicative context (Batstone, 2002). We also consider
these experiences in relation to the implications they present for language teach-
ing and learning.
The participants were 12 English language students at university level and under-
taking academic English courses (English for Academic Purposes – EAP) on a
degree pathway program at an Australian university.
In qualitative research, generally the overarching aim is to “describe the as-
pects of an idiosyncratic experience rather than determining the most likely, or
mean experience within a group” (Dörnyei, 2007, p. 126). It was therefore vital
that the research participants were able to deliver relevant, rich and varied insights
into the phenomena in question. Thus, a purposive sampling approach was ad-
opted wherein participants were selected in accordance with certain criteria. The
primary criterion was related to language proficiency. The participants needed to
be learners of academic English in order for us to have a degree of confidence that
they could actually engage with the interviewer and the questions and topics of the
interviews effectively enough to produce comprehensive data. Proficiency is also
an important factor in relation to the target emotion of pride as learners accom-
plish goals in their studies, as they progress and develop as language users. The
278 Andrew S. Ross and Elke Stracke
participants were drawn from three different university language centres in major
Australian cities and were unknown to the researchers.
Other criteria concerned a desire for a gender and nationality mix. As can
be seen in Table 1, however, half of the participants were Chinese. Although not
our initial desire, the large proportion of Chinese participants eventuated, re-
flecting student numbers at each participating institution where students from
China make up the greatest part of student populations. The gender mix is as
good as we were able to achieve, with the proficiency and nationality criteria taken
into account first.
The details of the participants can be seen in Table 1.
ter a greater understanding of the manner in which people experience second lan-
guage learning and subsequently make sense of that experience.
The first author conducted the interviews at a pre-arranged time on the re-
spective participant’s campus. All interviews were audio-recorded in addition to
notes being taken by the interviewer. Each interview started out with general con-
versation before focusing on the topic of the research. During the actual interview
process, the interviews attempted to:
1. Gain a general overview of the participants’ experience of learning English
(where/how long/teachers and teaching style/learning style/beliefs etc.)
2. Elicit different times in participants’ language learning that triggered different
emotions
jbid177713 IP: 101.184.148.117 On: Thu, 06 Apr 2017 10:23:46
3. Elicit descriptions of the feelings the participants experienced at that time and
the reaction they had to them and how they processed them.
The initial interview aided in the development of rapport. The majority of the data
for the study came from the second interview. To stimulate the discussion on the
participants’ emotions, they were emailed the first question of the interviews ask-
ing what their best and worst language learning experiences were. This proved to
be an especially useful and beneficial approach to the qualitative researching of
emotions as it allowed the participants time and opportunity to reflect on the in-
formation they wished to divulge and expand on in the interview itself. The third
interview allowed for further clarification of any comments made in the previous
two interviews. More importantly, it provided an avenue for deeper investigation
of anything arising in earlier comments.
The interviews were conceptualised as what Holstein and Gubrium (2003,
p. 68) describe as “interactional procedures of knowledge production” meaning
that the interviews were co-constructed with the researcher, with participants rec-
ollecting subjective emotional experiences (Prior, 2015). The interviews did not
focus solely on the emotion of pride, but it was among those discussed and the
comments of the participants in relation to pride were deemed particularly salient,
and thus it was established as a theme. Sample interview questions can be seen
below, but it should be noted that interview guides are not included as appendices
as the complete guides are not relevant to the current paper due to the focus on
other emotions beyond pride alone.
– Can you describe how you feel when you are able to keep up with the material
in class well? Can you give an example?
– When you can contribute something in class in a discussion or other activity,
what effect does it have on you? How do you feel?
280 Andrew S. Ross and Elke Stracke
– What about outside of class? Have you ever felt proud of yourself about the
way you used English in a social situation?
The data analysis began with the verbatim transcription of the interviews. The in-
terviewer alone transcribed due to the fact that he had been present and was able
to recall other physical aspects of the interview beyond the audio such as facial
expressions and gestures.
The primary analytical approach was Kvale’s (1996) ‘meaning condensation’
approach. While only one researcher carried out the transcription, the coding
jbid177713 IP: 101.184.148.117 On: Thu, 06 Apr 2017 10:23:46
4. Results
the interviews, those presented in the following discussion are the most salient in
relation to these themes.
The most common trigger of feelings of pride in the English language learning
context is generally classroom-based and related to personal achievement. When
speaking in relation to this, Alice stated:
I was so proud of myself the day before yesterday. I got the full mark for reading
and the teacher said who got full marks. Only me!
The participants’ description of their experiences and the effect on them demon-
strated the strong presence of pride in relation to accomplishment in class.
4.2 Communication
Effective use of English in communication outside the classroom was a strong in-
fluence on the participants’ experience of pride. Antonio highlighted this and es-
282 Andrew S. Ross and Elke Stracke
tablished that, along with other emotions, he felt authentic pride when his attempts
to communicate ended in success, and then went on to tell of a specific example:
I feel proud every time I can, you know, when my communication works. I feel
proud about myself but a specific one maybe is when I was trying to, when I
bought a console and it was supposed to have to CDs inside but there was only
one and then I had to come back and argue with the salesperson until they give
me back my CD and then I feel proud like “Oh I got my CD! I could talk with
them in English!”
The situation Antonio described was not an ordinary situation of buying a ticket
or asking for directions, but was in fact one of arguing, or making a case. An en-
counter such as this would have required significant confidence, so the pride he
jbid177713 IP: 101.184.148.117 On: Thu, 06 Apr 2017 10:23:46
Finally, Sepan described the pride he feels in terms of his language use in Australia.
He is aware of how he has grown since his arrival in Australia and in this particular
case he seems to feel pride in his own ability to communicate and achieve what he
set out to do with the language:
One thing I want to tell you is I’m proud every day. When I came here I didn’t
have any information about things. When I asked some people they helped me a
lot. Someday I questioned someone who is Australian about accommodation and
they helped me a lot … I’m very proud about this.
A strong sense of pride in his own communicative ability can be detected in his
description – an ability that he seemingly did not think he possessed.
Learner perceptions and experiences of pride in second language education 283
travelled with my Mum to Hong Kong you know they speak Cantonese and our
language is Mandarin and we didn’t communicate very well so I had to speak to
them in English and then I found out “Oh, English is very useful”, then my Mum
was very proud of me.
Her voice and facial expressions supported this important and rewarding experi-
ence that Cathryn recalls.
Similarly, when asked if he had ever encountered the feeling of pride in his
English language learning experience, Mitchell recounted the following, which
serves to reinforce what Cathryn said about the pride of parents:
Interviewer: Can you think of any examples in your English language learning
past or present when you have felt this?
Mitchell: Well actually, my parents, they didn’t go to university so they just,
after their study of high school they didn’t know English and when I
told them I studied a lot about English they felt proud of me because
they can tell their friends their son can speak English very well and
their son passed the college English test band 6 and their son just
studied abroad in Australia.
We can understand Mitchell’s belief that his parents would be proud of him and
speak of what their son was doing, which involved having to mention his achieve-
ments. Hence, pride also came from external sources and could be just as powerful.
Alice provides a final example in relation to external pride; however, in this
instance the pride was directed from her towards her daughter. She spoke of her
daughter’s rapid English development and, with great excitement and smiling, re-
called her daughter’s use of English at the dinner table one night which surprised
Alice greatly, and evoked a very proud response when she said, “I was very proud
of her and gave her a big hug and kiss”. This reaction on its own might not surprise,
but Alice had trouble reconciling her daughter’s rapid progress in the same skill
she was also trying to master and that she was finding so challenging.
284 Andrew S. Ross and Elke Stracke
4.4 ‘Non-pride’
Generally, the emotion of pride is perceived to be a positive one, but some par-
ticipant responses indicated a different, negative perception. In doing so, the par-
ticipants shed light on a dimensionality that can be said to exist within individual
emotions wherein the experience of the emotion itself does not occur exactly as
one might normally assume. In other words, there is more than one dimension
to the emotion. In the case of pride, the different dimensions can be recognised
when considering instances where pride is felt, and then other instances where it
is absent (but where it would normally be present).
Claudia conveyed this feeling when she compared a ‘good’ feeling to a feel-
ing of ‘pride’ and had trouble reconciling them. Whereas most students would
jbid177713 IP: 101.184.148.117 On: Thu, 06 Apr 2017 10:23:46
Claudia does not feel proud of herself here because she is in a place where she
must use English. Her perception helps to highlight the different circumstances in
which the emotion is felt by different people as a result of a different appraisal of
the situation, as other participants felt it quite strongly for this exact reason – being
able to survive in a foreign environment. In a sense, she felt that she was merely
‘surviving’, but not exhibiting any great proficiency in her use. This perception
contributed to the absence of pride she described.
Other feelings of ‘non-pride’ were related to the situated classroom learning
environment. Interestingly, some participants did not consider the classroom as
being a setting worthy of triggering pride as it is a very safe environment in that
they did not find themselves in encounters with others of L1-standard proficiency.
As a result, students did not feel as challenged as they felt they should in order to
warrant the feeling of pride. Mitchell gave one example when he spoke of not feel-
ing proud when successfully participating in a lesson:
I: What about specifically related to your learning?
M: Well actually, it doesn’t make me proud.
I: Oh ok. Why not?
M: Because everyone knows it very well, not only me, but also the other people
[he refers to students].
Mitchell’s comments here imply that for him to feel pride he needs to be doing
something better than others, or at least doing something that might be a bit more
Learner perceptions and experiences of pride in second language education 285
I feel like in class it’s not really real and then I don’t feel really proud. Even if I am
really good at something I don’t feel really proud because I know that it’s not real
and street people [he refers to ‘ordinary people’] say slangs …
The people he was referring to were just ordinary people that he may see and po-
tentially encounter and interact with in his everyday activities. This is a powerful
perspective to consider as it specifically draws attention to the fact that successful
language experiences in the reality of life outside of the classroom are often what
students want to experience.
5. Discussion
One of the most important findings from the study concerns the dual contexts
within which language learners experience the emotion of pride. These contexts
are the situated learning environment of the language classroom, and situations of
authentic social interaction outside the classroom. Batstone (2002) refers to these
settings as the learning and communicative contexts respectively. The learning
context refers to the general classroom setting and places an emphasis on improv-
ing overall linguistic competence, whereas communicative contexts require using
the L2 to participate in social interactions outside the classroom. This distinction
is of vital importance as it is difficult to argue that language learners confront the
affective factors that are inherent in L2 social interaction and communication in
the classroom environment as often as they do outside of the classroom. However,
having made the distinction, it is equally important to acknowledge that these
contexts can, and do, also work together. Communication does occur within the
classroom, and often with a large degree of authenticity when the tasks and mate-
rials create the conditions that allow it to occur (Willis, 1996).
286 Andrew S. Ross and Elke Stracke
The results of the study showed that pride was felt in the learning environment
by the participants, but not always in relation to issues that would normally be
expected. Primarily, the emotion was evoked in response to grades or praise from
others in the class, which establishes a link between the instrumental orientation
dimension of Dörnyei’s (2005; 2009) Ideal L2 Self. Here praise represents a form of
motivation from an external source, which is then internalised in the learners. The
comments and experiences of Alice and Cathryn demonstrated this link the stron-
gest when they spoke of receiving full marks in a reading test or receiving praise
from classmates for a well-done presentation. On the other hand, there was a lack
of pride shown when performing tasks successfully for no tangible reward, which
in turn could be perceived as a lack of intrinsic motivation in the learning context.
jbid177713 IP: 101.184.148.117 On: Thu, 06 Apr 2017 10:23:46
On the other hand, when the comments of the students are considered in rela-
tion to the communicative environment, the outcome was quite different. Firstly,
when the participants used the language successfully outside of the classroom,
the participants spoke of their pride, but of most interest is that for some stu-
dents, the experience of pride in language use is context dependent. Antonio’s
comments best illustrated the influence of context when he spoke of the absence
of pride in the ‘un-real’ classroom environment, but with great excitement about
his experiences of using language and experiencing pride in the ‘real’ communica-
tive context. Such learner comments suggest that the learning and communicative
contexts put forward by Batstone (2002) provide a useful framework when inves-
tigating the emotional experiences of second language learners.
There are similarities to be drawn with the reported experiences of pride of the
participants and the categories that emerged from Dewaele and MacIntyre’s (2014)
study. Comments from Dewaele and MacIntyre’s study about learners’ realisations
of personal progression in their mastery of the L2 can be compared with percep-
tions of pride in the learning environment in this study. Further, learner comments
about the authentic use of the L2 outside of the class in Dewaele and MacIntyre’s
study can be compared with feelings of pride in the communicative environment.
Another point of interest, investigated by Bown and White (2010) in their
social and cognitive approach to affect in language learning, relates to cognitive
appraisals of social environments and situations. Scherer, Schorr, and Johnstone
(2001) also drew attention to cognitive appraisals in positing that the processes in-
volved with cognitive appraisals play a critical role in determining emotions. One
of the main types of cognitive appraisals is situation-appraisal, which refers to the
idea that people will appraise different situations differently, thus producing dif-
ferent emotional reactions. That was certainly the case in the present study where
learners appraise the learning context in a different manner to the communicative
context. The different appraisal led to different reactions, specifically the absence
or presence of the feeling of pride. We posit that the role of cognitive appraisals
Learner perceptions and experiences of pride in second language education 287
deserves greater attention in the future in order for teachers to develop strategies
for mediation of these appraisals in the interest of more frequent encounters with
the feeling of pride and other emotions.
The setting of the project, which was in the ESL setting of Australia, is likely
to have had an effect on the experiences of the learners, and can produce vastly
different outcomes on the emotional experiences of language learners within lo-
calised EFL contexts. In ESL settings, the learning and communicative contexts
are both very present and real for students, with the learning context representing
the smaller part of their lives and lived experiences. On the other hand, in EFL
settings the communicative context beyond the classroom does not always exist,
so it is mainly within the learning environment that the opportunity to experi-
jbid177713 IP: 101.184.148.117 On: Thu, 06 Apr 2017 10:23:46
The key implications emerging from the study revolve primarily around the learn-
ing context (i.e. in the classroom), as it is in the learning context that pride was
288 Andrew S. Ross and Elke Stracke
to provide more avenues for learners to experience authentic pride due to clearer
benchmarks of achievement.
Conditions for experiencing pride could also be extended to the environment
outside the classroom environment. For example, in EFL settings students could
be encouraged to connect with L2 speakers in the community, such as tourists as
part of a class project or tandem partners in an online project. Following the inter-
actions that take place, the outcomes could be the focus of discussions or reports
in the classroom, providing a link between the in and out-of-class contexts and a
chance for learners to experience pride as a result of how they had communicated
as part of the project or task. These types of tasks would help to stimulate pride in
students who do not see the environment in the classroom as ‘real’.
The suggestions above would be equally applicable to the ESL context, where
learners have plentiful opportunities to engage with L2 speakers in the commu-
nity. Teachers need to be made aware of their students’ feelings of pride and when
and how they experience it with the greatest positive intensity so that those posi-
tive experiences can be built upon in the learning context. Connecting student
experiences of pride in the class environment and the environment outside of it
through well-designed tasks and materials could do this, as in the EFL context.
Regardless of the context, teachers can also encourage greater reflection by
students on their in and out-of-class experiences. The use of tools such as writing
journals could be of help here, and might allow students to see the antecedents
of their emotional experiences which could in turn help them to self-regulate the
emotions they encounter. Within the classroom context as well, teachers could
incorporate a broader range of approaches and strategies that include simulations
and in-class project work, for example. Activities such as these could be organised
to focus on aspects of positive psychology such as savouring (extending the expe-
rience of positive feelings and emotions) and gratitude. With tasks based around
such notions, opportunities could potentially be created for students to praise each
Learner perceptions and experiences of pride in second language education 289
other, and for the teacher to deliver praise, which could in turn act as a trigger for
the emotion of pride and other positive emotions.
To conclude, emotions are critical to the everyday experiences of language
learners – and people in general – and an understanding of student emotions such
as pride as in the present study provides valuable insights not only into the lived
experiences of these students but also clear implications for how we can improve
our practice and thus the learning experiences of our students.
References
Gregersen, T., MacIntyre, P. D., Finegan, K. H., Talbot, K., & Claman, S. (2014). Examining
emotional intelligence within the context of positive psychology interventions. Studies in
Second Language Learning and Teaching, 4(2), 327–353.
Holstein, J. A., & Gubrium, J. F. (2003). Active interviewing. In J. F. Gubrium & J. A. Holstein (Eds.),
Postmodern interviewing (pp. 67–80). Tousand Oaks, CA: Sage. doi: 10.4135/9781412985437
Horwitz, E. K., Horwitz, M. B., & Cope, J. A. (1986). Foreign language classroom anxiety.
Modern Language Journal, 70(2), 125–132. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4781.1986.tb05256.x
Izard, C. E. (2011). Forms and functions of emotions: Matters of emotion-cognition interac-
tions. Emotion Review, 3(4), 371–378. doi: 10.1177/1754073911410737
Jagacinski, C. M., & Nicholls, J. G. (1987). Competence and affect in task involvement and
ego involvement: The impact of social comparison information. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 79(2), 107–114. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.79.2.107
Kalat, J. W., & Shiota, M. N. (2007). Emotion. Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth.
jbid177713 IP: 101.184.148.117 On: Thu, 06 Apr 2017 10:23:46
Keltner, D., & Ekman, P. (2000). Emotion: An overview. In A. E. Kazdin (Ed.), Encyclopedia
of psychology (pp. 162–166). Oxford: American Psychological Association and Oxford
University Press.
Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Kurokawa, M. (2000). Culture, emotion, and well-being:
Good feelings in Japan and the United States. Cognition and Emotion, 14(1), 93–124.
doi: 10.1080/026999300379003
Kvale, S. (1996). InterViews: An introduction to qualitative research interviewing. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Lewis, M. (2008). Self-conscious emotions: Embarrassment, pride, shame and guilt. In M. Lewis
& J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 742–756). New York,
NY: Guilford Press.
MacIntyre, P. D., Mackinnon, S. P., & Clément, R. (2009). The baby, the bathwater, and the future
of language learning motivation research. In Z. Dörnyei & E. Ushioda (Eds.), Motivation,
language identity and the L2 self (pp. 43–65). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
MacIntyre, P. D., & Mercer, S. (2014). Introducing positive psychology to SLA. Studies in Second
Language Learning and Teaching, 4(2), 153–172. doi: 10.14746/ssllt.2014.4.2.2
MacIntyre, P. D., Gregersen, T., & Mercer, S. (2016). Positive psychology in SLA. Bristol:
Multilingual Matters.
Muris, P., & Meesters, C. (2014). Small or big in the eyes of the other: On the developmental
psychopathology of self-conscious emotions as shame, guilt and pride. Clinical Child and
Family Psychology Review, 17, 19–40. doi: 10.1007/s10567-013-0137-z
Pavlenko, A. (2007). Autobiographical narratives as data in applied linguistics. Applied
Linguistics, 28(2), 163–188. doi: 10.1093/applin/amm008
Pavlenko, A. (2008). Structural and conceptual equivalence in acquisition and use of emotion
words in a second language. Mental Lexicon, 3(1), 91–120.
Pavlenko, A. (2013). The affective turn in SLA: From ‘affective factors’ to ‘language desire’ and
‘commodification of affect’. In D. Gabrys-Barker & J. Bielska (Eds.), The affective dimension
in second language acquisition (pp. 3–28). Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters.
Prior, M. (2015). Emotion and discourse in L2 narrative research. NY: Multilingual Matters.
Richards, K. (2009). Interviews. In J. Heigham & R. A. Croker (Eds.), Qualitative research in applied
linguistics (pp. 182–199). Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan. doi: 10.1057/9780230239517_9
Ross, A. S. (2015). An exploration of the emotions and motivation of tertiary English language
learners in Australia. Unpublished doctoral thesis. University of Canberra.
Learner perceptions and experiences of pride in second language education 291
Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (Eds.). (2001). Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory,
methods, research. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Schumann, J. H. (1997). The neurobiology of affect in language. Boston: Blackwell.
Scovel, T. (2001). Learning new languages: A guide to second language acquisition. Boston: Heinle
& Heinle.
Stoeber, J., Kobori, O., & Tanno, Y. (2013). Perfectionism and self-conscious emotions in British
and Japanese students: Predicting pride and embarrassment after success and failure.
European Journal of Personality, 27, 59–70. doi: 10.1002/per.1858
Swain, M. (2013). The inseparability of cognition and emotion. Language Teaching, 46(2), 195–
207. doi: 10.1017/S0261444811000486
Tangney, J. P. (2002). Perfectionism and the self-conscious emotions: Shame, guilt, embar-
rassment and pride. In P. L. Hewitt & G. L. Flett (Eds.), Perfectionism (pp. 199–215).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
jbid177713 IP: 101.184.148.117 On: Thu, 06 Apr 2017 10:23:46
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2004). Putting the self into self-conscious emotions: A theoretical
model. Psychological Inquiry, 15(2), 103–125. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1502_01
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2007). Emerging insights into the nature and function of pride. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 147–150. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00493.x
Willis, J. (1996). A framework for task-based learning. London: Longman.
Authors’ addresses
Andrew S. Ross Elke Stracke
Centre for Teaching and Learning Applied Linguistics and TESOL
Southern Cross University Faculty of Education, Science, Technology
Gold Coast, 4225, Queensland and Mathematics
Australia University of Canberra
ACT 2601
Australia