Cognitive Learning Style A Review of The Field Dependent Field Independent Approach
Cognitive Learning Style A Review of The Field Dependent Field Independent Approach
Pithers R. T.
To cite this article: Pithers R. T. (2002) Cognitive learning style: a review of the field dependent-
field independent approach, Journal of Vocational Education and Training, 54:1, 117-132, DOI:
10.1080/13636820200200191
R.T. PITHERS
University of Technology, Sydney, Australia
Background
One issue that has continued to be explored in the educational,
psychological and organisational research literature for almost 30 years
is the concept of cognitive learning style. Cognitive style is said to be the
relatively stable strategies, preferences and attitudes that determine an
individual’s ‘… typical modes of perceiving, remembering and problem
solving’ (Messick, 1976, p. 5). They are thought of as the modes by which
learners approach, acquire and process information, as well as including
the consistent ways in which an individual memorises and retrieves
information (Witkin & Goodenough, 1981). Messick (1996) has gone
further to show how cognitive learning style goes beyond learning to
involve an individual’s psychological and social functioning.
The concept is different from cognitive ‘abilities’, which are usually
considered to be more domain specific. Cognitive learning styles,
117
R.T. Pithers
118
COGNITIVE LEARNING STYLE
119
R.T. Pithers
120
COGNITIVE LEARNING STYLE
121
R.T. Pithers
possibility that cognitive style may be malleable. This view was based on
their research in organisations into the ability of successful managers to
take on or select or adapt to styles (presumably more field independent
and analytic) needed in these positions.
Another enlightening study in this area, was carried out by Rush &
Moore (1991). They discovered that it was possible to train people to
change the strength of their cognitive style. For example, FDs were taught
the restructuring or analytical skills that FIDs tended to use more
regularly and then to apply these in new problem solving. They improved
their performance on two subsequent dependent measures. Thus, there
appears to be an emerging implication from this research, that cognitive
style in fact may after all be a reasonably flexible construct. Nevertheless,
there is not much supporting research published yet in this area. It
should be noted that even if FD-FID styles remain relatively stable,
behavioural strategies in teaching or at work may be seen to be quite
varied, reflect conscious decisions and be context specific (Steufert &
Nogami, 1989).
122
COGNITIVE LEARNING STYLE
123
R.T. Pithers
matter content and its coverage and were less inclined, than FD teachers,
to worry about creating positive student classroom attitudes and
relationships.
Witkin & Goodenough (1981) had little doubt, based on available
research evidence at that time, that the FD-FID cognitive style construct
was associated with learning outcomes. They claimed that FID
individuals, including teachers, relied on a more internal frame of
reference when they were faced with ambiguous material. Subsequently,
they were more likely to impose or provide their own structure; be
analytical with material and be more likely to provide a different
orientation to the visual field or subject matter content than the one
suggested by its elements. These authors claimed that ‘stronger’ FID
individuals were more capable in all of these areas. These observations
made about teachers whose predominant style and behaviour were either
more FD or FID, tended to give more substance and detail to Witkin &
Goodenough’s earlier reports.
Self-awareness of cognitive or information-processing style is not
only an important issue in developing more successful managers of
organisations (Murphy & Doucette, 1997), it is important for developing
successful teachers as educational leaders. For instance, if FD learners
tend to favour more structure and feedback in learning, and FID learners
prefer more autonomy and less interpersonal interaction, then perhaps,
vocational teachers should provide a variety of teaching methodologies
to accommodate the range of FD-FID styles likely to be present in their
classes.
A simpler approach would be to find out the predominant cognitive
style in the student group (i.e. FD or FID) and then match that style with
the attitudes and behaviour of an ‘appropriate’ teaching/training style
(i.e. more FD or FID), examples of which have already been outlined. This
approach, however, is problematic. One major problem is that FD-FID
scores from the GEFT are conceived of as following a normal distribution.
This, in practice, means that in any group there will usually be individual
learners who exhibit FD and FID strength in various degrees. This means
that if a teacher or trainer adopts one predominant cognitive teaching
style to ‘match’ with a similar cognitive learning style (i.e. FD or FID), they
are bound not to ‘connect’ with some of the learners’ cognitive style
preference.
It may be argued that learners whose predominant style is FD, who
are exposed to teachers whose predominant style is FID (in particular, in
areas such as management or marketing), become more FID over the time
of their course. The question, however, of whether learners can modify or
change their predominant cognitive style over a relatively short time
period or whether individuals of different predominant styles (i.e. FD or
FID) are more likely to be drawn to a different occupational area, is still a
point of issue in the sparse research literature on this subject.
124
COGNITIVE LEARNING STYLE
125
R.T. Pithers
126
COGNITIVE LEARNING STYLE
127
R.T. Pithers
Concluding Comments
Hayes & Allison (1998) concluded their paper by stating that cognitive
learning style is a potentially potent education and training variable
capable of influencing the quality of learning at the individual and at the
organisational level. There is certainly support for this notion in the
research literature reviewed here, using one of the major constructs in
the area of cognitive style: namely, field dependence-field independence.
The classification and measurement of FD-FID over almost 30 odd years,
predominantly using the GEFT, has generated much useful data, and a
host of theoretical conceptions relevant to understanding the wider area
of an individual’s more global or more analytic type of thinking. Cognitive
style research and especially work on the field dependence-field
independence dimension, certainly showed much early promise. This was
especially so in terms of the practical idea of the vocational teacher
matching his or her cognitive style to a that of their students’, but now
this simple approach has been shown to have serious limitations.
Overall, the weight of research evidence in the area of teacher-
learner style matching, has not led to many clear, long-term suggestions
or solutions for improving learning and learned performance, that is as
distinct from the possibility of reported improvement in classroom
attitudes and short-term increases in student satisfaction. The approach,
nevertheless, has certainly led to many suggestions for improving variety
in teaching methodology and to acknowledgement of the importance of
the teacher demonstrating and engaging in critical thinking and problem-
solving behaviour of different forms.
Furthermore, it has led to a notion of the importance of the learner
attempting to learn and apply style flexibility, based on the type of
problem to be solved. Fortunately, the simple solution of matching the
teacher’s and the learner’s cognitive style (i.e. a teacher adopting a field
dependent style and relevant set of behaviour with a group of students
128
COGNITIVE LEARNING STYLE
Correspondence
R. T. Pithers, Faculty of Education, University of Technology, Sydney,
PO Box 123, Broadway, New South Wales 2007, Australia
([email protected]).
References
Allison, C.W. & Hayes, J. (1996) The Cognitive Style Index: a measure of intuition
analysis for organisational research, Journal of Management Studies, 33,
pp. 119-135.
Brodzinski, D. (1982) Relationship Between Cognitive Style and Cognitive
Development, Developmental Psychology, 18, pp. 617-626.
129
R.T. Pithers
Derussey, E. & Futch, E. (1971) Field Dependence-independence as Related to
College Curricula, Perceptual and Motor Skills, 33, pp. 1235-1237.
DeSanctis, G. & Dunikoski, R. (1983) Group Embedded Figures Test: psychometric
data for a sample of business students, Perceptual and Motor Skills, 56,
pp. 707-710.
Dyer, J.E. & Osborne, E.W. (1996) Effects of Teaching Approach on Problem
Solving Ability of Agricultural Students with Varying Learning Styles, Journal
of Agricultural Education, 37, pp. 36-43.
Fergusson, L.C. (1993) Field Independence and Achievement in College Art: a
reexamination, Perceptual and Motor Skills, 77, pp. 1104-1106.
Griffin, R. & Franklin, G. (1996) Can College Academic Performance Be Predicted
Using a Measure of Cognitive Style? Journal of Educational Technology
Systems, 24, pp. 375-379.
Hayes, J. & Allison, C.W. (1998) Cognitive Style and the Theory and Practice of
Individual and Collective Learning in Organisations, Human Relations, 51,
pp. 847-871.
Kahtz, A.W. & Kling, G. (1999) Field Dependent and Field Independent
Conceptualisations of Various Instructional Methods: a qualitative analysis,
Educational Psychology, 19, pp. 413-429.
Kelleher, W. E. (1997) The Group Embedded Figures Test: field
dependence/independence and undergraduate commerce major, College
Student Journal, 31, pp. 115-121.
Leonard, N.H. Scholl, R. & Kowalski, K. (1999) Information Processing Style and
Decision Making, Journal of Organisational Behavior, 20, pp. 407-420.
Lusk, S.C. (1998) The Relationship Between Cognitive Styles and Academic
Achievement, British Journal of Educational Technology, 29, pp. 137-147.
Lusk, E. & Wright, H. (1981) Differences in Sex and Curricula on Learning and
Group Embedded Figures Test, Perceptual and Motor Skills, 53, pp. 8-10.
March, J. (1991) Exploration and Exploitation in Organizational Learning,
Organizational Science, 2, pp. 71-87.
McKenna, F.P. (1990) Learning Implications of Field-dependence, Independence:
cognitive style vs cognitive ability, Applied Cognitive Psychology, 4,
pp. 425-437.
McRae, L. & Young, J. (1990) Group Embedded Figures Test: psychometric data
for a sample of Canadian undergraduate business students, Perceptual and
Motor Skills, 67, pp. 195-198.
Melancon, J.G. & Thompson, B. (1989) Measurement Characteristics of the
Embedded Figures Test, Psychology in Schools, 26, pp. 69-78.
Messick, S. (1976) Personality Consistencies in Cognition and Creativity, in
S. Messick & Associates (Eds) Individuality and Learning. San Francisco:
Jossey-Bass.
Messick, S. (1996) Bridging Cognition and Personality in Education: the role of
style in performance and development, European Journal of Personality, 10,
pp. 353-376.
130
COGNITIVE LEARNING STYLE
131
R.T. Pithers
Witkin, H.A., Moore, C., Goodenough, D. & Cox, P. (1977a) Field Dependent and
Field Independent Cognitive Styles and their Educational Implications, Review
of Educational Research, 47, pp. 1-64.
Witkin, H.A., Oltman, P., Goodenough, D., Friedman, F., Owen, D. & Raskin, E.
(1977b) Role of Field Dependent and Field Independent Cognitive Styles in
Academic Evolution: a longitudinal study, Journal of Educational Psychology,
69, pp. 197-211.
Young, J., Keller, W. & McRae, L. (1989) Field Independence and Business
Students’ Achievement, Perceptual and Motor Skills, 69, pp. 997-998.
132