Psychologists: Behavioral
Psychologists: Behavioral
Psychologists
Behavioral
Ivan Pavlov
John Watson
B.F. Skinner
Edward Thorndike
Albert Bandura
Humanist
Abraham Maslow
Carl Rogers
Cognitive
Jean Piaget
Jerome Bruner
Edward Tolman
Anne Treisman
Donald Broadbent
Albert Ellis
Aaron Beck
David Kolb
Wilhelm Wundt
Leon Festinger
Psychodynamic
Sigmund Freud
Erik Erikson
Carl Jung
Social
Solomon Asch
Stanley Milgram
Hofling Nurse Study
Philip Zimbardo
Serge Moscovici
Henri Tajfel
Memory
Richard Atkinson
Alan Baddeley
Fergus Craik
Elizabeth Loftus
Developmental
Lev Vygostksy
Jerome Bruner
Mary Ainsworth
John Bowlby
Lawrence Kohlberg
Theories
Behavioral
Behaviorist Approach
Classical Conditioning
Operant Conditioning
Schedules of Reinforcement
Law of Effect
Social Learning Theory
Humanist
Humanistic Psychology
Hierarchy of Needs
Carl Rogers
Personality
Personality Theories
Type A
Cognitive
Cognitive Psychology
Information Processing
Attention
Perception
Cognitive Dissonance
Memory
Memory Intro
Short Term
Long Term
Multi Store Model
Working Memory
Levels of Processing
Forgetting
Eyewitness Testimony
Sigmund Freud
Psychodynamic Approach
Psychoanalysis
Id, Ego, Super-Ego
Unconscious Mind
Psychosexual Stages
Defense Mechanisms
Social
Social Psychology
Conformity
Obedience
Attitudes
Self Concept
Prejudice
Developmental
Jean Piaget
Sensorimotor Stage
Preoperational Stage
Concrete Stage
Formal Operational Stage
Lev Vygostksy
ZPD
Jerome Bruner
Attachment
John Bowlby
Psychosocial Stages
Studies
Conformity
Obedience
Attachment
44 Thieves
Strange Situation
Schaffer & Emerson
Van Ijzendoorn & Kroonenberg
Harlow's Monkeys
Hodges & Tizard
Genie
Behavioral
Pavlov's Dogs
Little Albert
Bobo Doll
Memory
Stress
Kiecolt-Glaser
SRRS - Rahe
Developmental
Scaffolding (ZPD)
Heinz Dilemma
Three Mountains Task
Object Permanence
Research Methods
Experimental
Lab Experiment
Field Experiment
Natural Experiment
Experimental Designs
Variables
Non-Experimental
Case Study
Interviews
Questionnaire
Likert Scales
Observations
Conducting Research
Hypotheses
Sampling
Scienific Approach
Lab Report
Ethical Guidelines
Data
Qualitative Quantitative
Reliability
Validity
Correlation
Statistics
Home › Cognitive Psychology › Perception
Summary
A lot of information reaches the eye, but much is lost by the time it
reaches the brain (Gregory estimates about 90% is lost).
Therefore, the brain has to guess what a person sees based on past
experiences. We actively construct our perception of reality.
Gregory has demonstrated this with a hollow mask of a face (see video
below). Such a mask is generally seen as normal, even when one knows
and feels the real mask.
The Necker cube is a good example of this. When you stare at the
crosses on the cube the orientation can suddenly change, or 'flip'.
Once the face is discovered, very rapid perceptual learning takes place
and the ambiguous picture now obviously contains a face each time we
look at it. We have learned to perceive the stimulus in a different way.
2. Perceptual Development
A perplexing question for the constructivists who propose perception is
essentially top-down in nature is 'how can the neonate ever perceive?' If
we all have to construct our own worlds based on past experiences why
are our perceptions so similar, even across cultures? Relying on
individual constructs for making sense of the world makes perception a
very individual and chancy process.
3. Sensory Evidence
Perhaps the major criticism of the constructivists is that they have
underestimated the richness of sensory evidence available to perceivers
in the real world (as opposed to the laboratory where much of the
constructivists' evidence has come from).
However, in the real world, retinal images are rarely seen in isolation (as
is possible in the laboratory). There is a rich array of sensory
information including other objects, background, the distant horizon and
movement. This rich source of sensory information is important to the
second approach to explaining perception that we will examine, namely
the direct approach to perception as proposed by Gibson.
Gibson argued strongly against the idea that perception involves top-
down processing and criticizes Gregory’s discussion of visual illusions
on the grounds that they are artificial examples and not images found in
our normal visual environments. This is crucial because Gregory
accepts that misperceptions are the exception rather than the norm.
Illusions may be interesting phenomena, but they might not be that
informative about the debate.
James Gibson (1966) argues that perception is direct, and not subject to
hypotheses testing as Gregory proposed. There is enough information in
our environment to make sense of the world in a direct way. His theory
is sometimes known as the ‘Ecological Theory’ because of the claim
that perception can be explained solely in terms of the environment.
For Gibson: sensation is perception: what you see if what you get.
There is no need for processing (interpretation) as the information we
receive about size, shape and distance etc. is sufficiently detailed for us
to interact directly with the environment.
The starting point for Gibson’s Theory was that the pattern of light
reaching the eye, known as the optic array, containing all the visual
information necessary for perception
If the flow appears to be coming from the point, it means you are
moving towards it. If the optic array is moving towards the point you are
moving away from it.
Invariant Features
Affordances
OPTICAL ARRAY: The patterns of light that reach the eye from
the environment.
Visual Illusions
Gibson's emphasis on DIRECT perception provides an explanation for
the (generally) fast and accurate perception of the environment.
However, his theory cannot explain why perceptions are sometimes
inaccurate, e.g. in illusions. He claimed the illusions used in
experimental work constituted extremely artificial perceptual situations
unlikely to be encountered in the real world, however this dismissal
cannot realistically be applied to all illusions.
For example, Gibson's theory cannot account for perceptual errors like
the general tendency for people to overestimate vertical extents relative
to horizontal ones.
References
DeCasper, A. J., & Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns
prefer their mothers' voices. Science, 208(4448), 1174-1176.
Slater, A., Morison, V., Somers, M., Mattock, A., Brown, E., & Taylor,
D. (1990). Newborn and older infants' perception of partly occluded
objects. Infant Behavior and Development, 13(1), 33-49.
Comments (18)
Sort by: Date Rating Last Activity
Reply Report
Reply Report
Reply Report
Reply Report
Physics helps me investigate accidents and engineer controls when possible. Engineered controls in my industry are not often possible in the
short term. Psychology helps me understand the mental processes of my drivers that often lead to unsafe behaviors so I can properly train
operators of potentially very dangerous machinery today and in the very present tense. Understanding visual perception is, obviously, a large
part of that. Thank you for your article and I would encourage those studying psychology to keep in mind: What you learn and publish today
may save the loss of life and limb when those of us in the field of Industrial Safety can use it to prevent an unsafe behavior that statistically
leads to an untoward occurence.
Thank you for your altruistic contributions to a humanitarian endeavor. Great work here folks. Keep it up.
Reply Report
Reply Report
Reply Report
1 2 Next »
Name Email
Further Information
Perceptual Set
Visual Perception
Follow us
Back to top