Differences in Executive Functioning Betwen Current and Former Gang Members
Differences in Executive Functioning Betwen Current and Former Gang Members
Title Page
Dissertation
Presented to the
In Partial Fulfillment
DOCTOR OF PSYCHOLOGY
By
Heather Farris
January 2013
UMI Number: 3557654
In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript
and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed,
a note will indicate the deletion.
UMI 3557654
Published by ProQuest LLC (2013). Copyright in the Dissertation held by the Author.
Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC.
All rights reserved. This work is protected against
unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code
ProQuest LLC.
789 East Eisenhower Parkway
P.O. Box 1346
Ann Arbor, MI 48106 - 1346
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING ii
Copyright page
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING iii
Signature page
________________________
DATE
Dissertation Committee:
________________________
Noor Damavandi, Ph.D., Chairperson
Alliant International University
__________________
Daniel Levinson, Ph.D., Committee Member
Alliant International University
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING iv
Acknowledgements
Industries for their participation in this research. You are all an inspiration. Without
your contribution, my research efforts would not have been possible. I would like to
expressly thank James Horton at Homeboy Industries for his diligence and hard work.
Without your assistance, this most certainly would not have been possible. Homeboy
Industries does amazing work, and I hope my journey brings me back there one day.
To Jami, few words can ever express what your friendship means to me. I am so
grateful for your honesty and willingness to tell me what I needed to hear no matter what.
I wish more people were able to experience what it is like to have a truly unconditional
friendship. Your words stuck with me even when our schedules and distance kept us
from catching up for periods of time. Your wisdom and strength carried me through so
much of this process. For that, and so much more, I am forever grateful. You are my
To Rose, Robyn, and Alana, I could not have gotten through this process without
your friendship. I will always hold close our conversations, whether laughing or crying.
I would like to thank my brother, Jason, and sister-in-law, Denise, for your love
and friendship. Life keeps us busy, but I am grateful to you for always keeping me close
in your hearts and extending invites for get-togethers and getaways. They are just what I
needed.
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING v
To Mom and Dad, thank you for being a part of my dream. I attribute my
successes to your unwavering support. You have always been there for me, guiding me,
but letting me find my own way. You are the epitome of devotion. I hope to one day
Finally, to my new niece Kamila, the sky is the limit, sweet girl. Anything is
Table of Contents
Table of Contents
Results ............................................................................................................................... 93
CHAPTER V .................................................................................................................. 101
Discussion ....................................................................................................................... 101
Limitations .................................................................................................................. 105
Internal Validity....................................................................................................... 105
External Validity...................................................................................................... 107
References ....................................................................................................................... 108
Appendix A ..................................................................................................................... 112
Table 1 ......................................................................................................................... 112
Table 2 ......................................................................................................................... 112
Table 3 ......................................................................................................................... 112
Appendix B ..................................................................................................................... 113
Table 4 ......................................................................................................................... 113
Table 5 ......................................................................................................................... 113
Appendix C ..................................................................................................................... 114
Demographic Questionnaire ........................................................................................ 114
Appendix D ..................................................................................................................... 115
Demographic Table ..................................................................................................... 115
Appendix E ..................................................................................................................... 117
Informed Consent ........................................................................................................ 117
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING ix
Abstract
aggression (Blair, 2007; Giancola, Roth, and Parrott, 2006; Dolan & Anderson, 2002). It
perspective of like-minded teens (Moffitt, 1993). Within this theory, it is believed that
Most delinquency remits after adolescence; however, a small percentage persists into
adulthood, and some continues for a lifetime. The antisocial behavior of gang members
can be thought of as stable and persistent, and the gang culture breeds an enmeshment
that is difficult to break from; hence, the cycle of antisocial behavior persists. However,
some gang members make a choice to break from the gang, and live a more prosocial life.
This decision and planning can be considered an executive function. The outcomes of
antisocial behavior in midlife have seldom been examined. This research will examine
executive functioning differences between current and former gang members, and what
role it might play in a gang member’s ability to leave the gang. This researcher proposes
there will be significant differences in executive functioning between current and former
gang members, with former gang members exhibiting greater executive functioning
abilities.
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING x
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 1
CHAPTER I
Overview
Executive function (EF), housed in the frontal lobes of the brain, includes the
capacity for the most complex brain functions. In a broad sense, EF is linked to
allows a person to process and respond to social cues via self-regulation. Subtle damage
to the frontal lobe can cause apathy and indifference (Goldberg, 2009). More important
to society, EF damage has been linked to aggressive behavior, difficult temperament, and
impulsivity (Blair, 2007; Dolan & Anderson, 2002; Giancola, Roth, and Parrott, 2006).
Gangs pose a different construct from mainstream society for the individuals
discrimination, and community violence are normal daily concerns for individuals,
prior to an individual joining a gang, which enables him to transition into antisocial
behavior, or whether these changes occur after joining the gang, as a result of antisocial
activity. Additionally, what bearing does the timing of these cognitive deficits have on
As a young adult gang member begins to question his or her place in the world,
the jump from contemplating criminal acts versus prosocial activities (e.g., job,
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 2
persists throughout the life course (Moffit, 1993; Piquero, 2001; Raskin, White, Bates, &
Buyske, 2001). Specific to gangs, this study will examine differences in executive
functioning between former and current gang members, and determine whether they can
Problem Statement
continuity. Delinquency trajectory theory posits that the majority of adolescents who
engage in delinquent behavior will outgrow this behavior by late adolescence or early
adulthood (Moffitt, 1993); however, it is still a decision, which falls under the umbrella
of executive function. Factors that lead to a gang member’s decision to leave should be
further researched so that executive function can be better understood in its role in
Purpose Statement
In addition, studies on antisocial behavior have not been specific to gangs or specific
factors that contribute to a young male exiting this lifestyle. The purpose of this study is
to examine executive functioning differences between current and former gang members
in the crucial developmental stage of late adolescence/early adulthood, when the frontal
lobe reaches full development. This researcher hopes this study will contribute additional
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 3
data to the understanding of executive functioning and its role in antisocial behavior in
late adolescence and early adulthood, as well as the causes that may lead this behavior to
may have a more critical role than supporting the delinquency trajectory theory of falling
that there are significant differences in executive functioning, it may support the idea that
behavior (Giancola et. al., 2006). This may lead to a paradigm shift in the treatment of
antisocial behavior throughout the life span in gang members and other delinquents.
executive function measures among current gang members and former gang members.
Research Questions
aggressiveness, and delinquency (Blair, 2007; Dolan & Anderson, 2002; Giancola et. al.,
2006). Once engaged in this line of behavior, what factors may exist that allow a person
to make a change in their life course trajectory? The research question is as follows:
Conceptual/Theoretical Framework
antisocial behavior, specifically aggression and impulsivity (Blair, 2007; Dolan &
Anderson, 2002; Giancola et. al., 2006). This researcher theorizes there will be
significant differences in executive functioning, with the former gang members having
greater executive functioning abilities than the current gang members. This will confirm
prior studies that adolescence-limited delinquency has less executive functioning deficits
(Donnellan, Ge, & Wenk, 2000; Huesmann, Dubow, & Boxer, 2009; Raine, Moffit,
Assumptions
1. The BRIEF-A and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) are valid
The scope of this study encompasses executive functioning levels of current gang
members compared with former gang members. Delimitations which will narrow the
scope of the study include the self-report measures used to capture gang membership as
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 5
neurocognitive measures being used. In addition, the participants recruited will be from
one non-profit organization that assists with gang intervention and prevention; in essence,
the participants will be in a frame of mind that is open to assistance. Although some may
still be in the gang, they are seeking support, therefore, the scope of current gang
affiliation will be narrower than someone enmeshed in the gang and not seeking help. In
addition, many life-course persistent offenders may not be available because they are
incarcerated or deceased.
Limitations
3. Substance and alcohol abuse will be limitations in that the use of these
members may invite increased risk for altercations and possible head
Chapter Summary
Executive functions include the capacity to plan, reason, organize thoughts, and
aggression (Blair, 2007; Dolan & Anderson, 2002; Giancola et. al., 2006). These deficits,
as well as psychosocial factors, heritability factors, and delinquency trajectories, all play
an intricate role in the antisocial behavior associated with gangs as suggested in the
literature to follow. This study will examine whether differences in executive function
CHAPTER II
Literature Review
The normative view of gangs tends to follow the idea that they are outlaw
having conduct disorder, and having antisocial personality disorder as their behavior
extends into adulthood. Conduct disorder is defined as a repetitive and persistent pattern
of behavior in which the basic rights of others or societal norms or rules are violated
(APA, 2000). These behaviors include aggression towards people or animals, destruction
of property, deceitfulness and theft, and a serious violation of rules that may include
addition to risk factors identified by Maxmen, Ward, and Kilgus (2009), as always being
offers more temptations. As a gang member grows into adulthood and continues to be
irresponsibility (APA, 2000). According to Maxmen et al. (2009), estimates place the
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 8
likelihood that those with conduct disorder will develop Antisocial Personality Disorder
Some gang members enter the gangs with these characteristics; others develop
lying, truancy, participating in graffiti, selling drugs, robbery, auto theft, and assault. In
their annual gang threat assessment, the National Gang Intelligence Center (NGIC, 2009)
identified drug trafficking as the most frequent gang-related activity, but noted human
2008, approximately one million gang members belonging to more than 20,000 gangs
Some of the current literature (e.g., Craig, Vitaro, Gagnon, & Tremblay, 2002;
Hill, Howell, Hawkins, & Battin-Pearson, 1999; Lahey, Gordon, Loeber, Stouthamer-
Loeber, & Farrington, 1999) measures factors that predict gang involvement as well as
differences between gang members and non-gang members; however, it is possible that
some of these differences may arise after a youth has entered gangs and may not be a
contributing factor in their decision to join (Lahey et al., 1999). Gangs and gang
the criminal justice system and society. There are other factors that must be looked at
that lead youth to joining gangs and participating in antisocial gang behavior.
involvement in street life and delinquent behavior (Valdez, Kaplan, & Codina, 2000).
Adolescent gang identities are complex and range from normal to extreme antisocial
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 9
behavior. There are many reasons non-antisocial youths may join gangs, which may
include self-esteem, power, and protection, but then are encouraged to participate in
antisocial behavior by the group after joining (Lahey et al., 1999). There are some gang
means of maintaining their gang status (Valdez et al., 2000). Children growing up in
these communities come to know the world as dangerous and view aggression and deceit
as the safest and most effective response to situations. There are youths on the other end
of the spectrum who are predisposed to violence, and whose behavior is rewarded within
The causal mechanisms of gang membership are multiple and complex. Some
theories are more developed than others, and each have a psychosocial or biological
gangs. The question remains why some youth continue with the gang life and why some
leave. More importantly is how these individuals come to the decision to leave the gang
life, and what cognitive processes take place that might differentiate them from those that
examines the microsystems a child experiences in everyday life and how they might
postulated that delinquency, violence, and substance abuse are not often found to be
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 10
Using multiple self-report measures completed by the youth, their parents, peers,
and teachers, Craig, Vitaro, Gagnon, and Tremblay (2002) addressed multiple objectives
in their study. In particular, they examined the stability of gang membership between the
ages of 10 to 14; compared the family, behavioral, peer and school profiles among stable,
unstable, and non-gang members; and identified grade achievement among the
participants.
The subjects of the Craig et al. (2002) study were originally part of a longitudinal
study in a low socioeconomic area in Quebec, Canada that included 1,034 boys that were
in kindergarten in 1984. To control for culture, both parents of the boys had to be born in
Canada and their first language be French. A subsample of 142 boys was selected for
this study. The boys were divided into three groups: stable gang members, identified as
children who belonged to a gang at ages 13 and 14; unstable gang members, identified as
children who belonged to a gang at either age 13 or 14; and non-gang members. There
for this comparison group, to provide a total of 66 non-gang members, 25 stable gang
Measuring the stability of gang membership year over year, Craig et al. (2002)
employed four loglinear models using the self-report data. Each of the models between
each age was found to be non-significant, with the exception of ages 13 to 14, which
were found to have a significant association. They determined the probability for gang
members at age 13 to belong to a gang at age 14 was .49, and were able to define stable
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 11
gang members as belonging to a gang at ages 13 and 14. These results were the rationale
for the identification of each participant group. This prompted the researchers to question
what factors may be at play at 13 and 14 that cause this shift from inconsistent
With their self-report measures, Craig et al. (2002) found that at ages 10 and 11
there were no significant differences between the behavior of the unstable gang members
and non-gang members in their sample. Their research suggested that as adolescents
engage themselves in gang activity at the ages of 10 and 11, they are not exhibiting
significant antisocial behavior different from those of non-gang members; however, their
levels of deviant behavior increase as membership in the gang becomes more stable.
They found that gang members are not uniformly deviant, but their delinquent behavior
escalates as they engage further with the gang. This is important for future research to
determine what factors at this age lead a person who does not exhibit more antisocial
In their research, Craig et al. (2002) also examined the difference between stable
gang members and non-stable gang members, and differences in their behavior profiles.
They hypothesized that stable gang members have more behavioral problems than the
non-stable gang member. At the ages of 12, 13, and 14, the stable gang members scored
higher than unstable and non-gang members, on fighting, stealing, and vandalism. From
ages 11 to 14, the stable gang members reported more school deviance than the unstable
gang members, and stable gang members reported more friend delinquency than unstable
gang members and non-gang members. This suggests the more behavioral problems an
individual has going into a gang, the more likely they are to become a stable gang
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 12
member. Craig et al. (2002) determined that as stable and unstable gang members age,
they engage in wider range delinquent behavior than non-gang members, and the
later dropping out completely. Craig et al. (2002) performed a chi-square analysis to
examine grade attainment among distributions of the three groups. They found a
significant relationship between gang membership and grade achievement at age 14; p =
.03. 59% of stable gang members were behind in school, versus 47% of unstable gang
members and 41% of non-gang members. Craig et al. (2002) suggested that high dropout
rates among gang members is due to the fact that school is an arena in which individual
deficits may become apparent, and dropping out to hang with a peer group where these
Craig et al. (2002) suggested that there is both continuation and accumulation of
deviant behavior problems with development. The pathway to stable gang membership
may include antecedent behavior problems, as well as low levels of anxiety and failure to
learn prosocial alternatives to antisocial behaviors. Craig et al. (2002) found that both
stable and unstable gang members had antecedent behavior problems in a variety of
contexts as rated by self, parents, and teachers; however, unstable gang members
exhibited fewer problematic behaviors than stable gang members, which suggested that
these two groups may have different developmental trajectories. These differing
trajectories may be due to unstable gang members being on the outside of the antisocial
peer group of gang members. They are not “street” enough, but have to portray this
image to survive in their neighborhood (Anderson, 1999). They may have a tendency
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 13
towards more prosocial behaviors and peer groups, but view the gang as a way to
maintain friends and status in their neighborhoods. Their activities are for appearances,
While Craig et al. (2002) examined factors that might lead to stability of gang
membership for younger youth, Hill, Howell, Hawkins, and Battin-Pearson (1999) took
self-reports of 10 to 12 year olds and measured the risk factors of them joining a gang
between 13 and 18 years old. Data was collected annually from ages 13 to 18. Some of
the demographic information to note is that 77% of the 10 year olds were in schools
located in high crime areas; 46% of parents reported an annual income of less than
$20,000; and more than half (52%) of the student sample participated in the school lunch
program at some point during the study, indicating they were from families living in
poverty. The sample was multiethnic with 46% European American, 24% African
American, 21% Asian American, 2% Native American, and the remaining 7% of other
ethnic backgrounds, primarily Hispanic. A self-report measure was used and data
obtained from multiple sources, including the youth, their parents or caretakers, teachers,
Hill et al. (1999) found that cumulatively, 15.3% of the sample reported ever
belonging to a gang between age 13 and 18, and membership peaked at age 15. The
specific risk factors examined, neighborhood, family, school, peer, and individual, all
youth with neighborhood risk factors demonstrated the highest odds ratio (OR) for
neighborhood had more than three times greater odds of joining a gang than youth that
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 14
did not. Youth with neighborhood peers that were in trouble had three times greater odds
of joining a gang than those youth who were not exposed to other troublesome youth.
Taking into account family structure, Hill et al. (1999) found that youth living
with one parent, with one parent and other adults, and youth with no parents in the home,
when compared to youth living with two parents (biological or adoptive), had more than
two times greater odds of joining a gang. In addition, poverty and parental pro-violent
attitudes contributed to having more than two times greater odds for joining a gang.
School level predictors of joining gangs were also found to be significant with
more than three times greater odds of joining a gang than those with higher academic
achievement and no learning disabilities. Hill et al. (1999) stressed the importance of
elementary school experiences as predictors in later gang membership, and an area that
indicators of gang membership were marijuana experimentation and violence, which was
reported as fighting, throwing objects, and hitting a teacher, at more than three times
greater odds than those who do not engage in this behavior. Other individual factors
included antisocial beliefs (OR = 2.0), externalizing behavior (OR = 2.6), hyperactivity
Overall, Hill et al. (1999) demonstrated that neighborhood, family, and school
influences all have an independent impact on the likelihood a youth will join a gang.
Taking into consideration the impact of multiple risk factors, and creating an index of
four quartiles to group risk factors from no risk to high risk, Hill et al. (1999) determined
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 15
that with each increase of risk factors, the odds of joining a gang approximately doubled,
and youth with exposure to seven or more risk factors at ages 10 to 12 had more than 13
times greater odds of joining a gang than if exposed to 0 to 1 risk. Collectively, the more
risk factors a youth is exposed to, the more likely they are to join a gang.
that the factors that predict gang entry would include both antisocial behaviors prior to
joining the gang as well as family and neighborhood characteristics. They controlled for
antisocial behavior prior to gang entry to determine whether any of the contextual factors
Lahey et al. (1999) used data from the second cohort of the Pittsburgh Youth
Study with a sample of 347 boys being in the seventh grade in 1987-1988 to measure
serious antisocial behavior. Data was collected in waves over six and a half years,
varying between six and 12 month intervals; however, two of the waves were not used in
order to analyze the data on an annual basis. They identified two definitions of gang
and serious gang membership, whose members engaged in at least one antisocial
behavior (fighting, selling drugs, robbery, stealing cars, or homicide). Participants were
classified as either high risk or low risk, with high risk scoring above the 67th percentile.
Of the 347 boys, 181 were classified as high risk and 166 were placed in the low risk
group. The boys’ family median income was $16,579 (in 1996), and 49% lived with a
single parent.
Of the 347 participants, 95 (27%) boys reported ever entering a gang. When
looking specifically at serious gang membership, 62 boys (18%) reported entering a gang.
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 16
Using only African American participants in a multivariate analysis, Lahey et al. (1999)
found that both higher antisocial behavior at the beginning of the study and increases in
this conduct disorder over time, independently predicted future gang involvement. In an
against property, and drug sales), Lahey et al. (1999) suggested that only crimes against
persons independently predicted adolescents joining gangs after other types were
controlled.
With regards to peer delinquency, Lahey et al. (1999) identified four types that
were reported: school related delinquent acts; covert delinquent acts; aggressive
delinquent acts; and drug use, and found that only the school-related delinquent acts were
significantly associated with gang involvement when the others were controlled. The
of gang entry were household structure, family income, parental supervision, and
However, upon combining prior antisocial behavior and the contextual factors,
using entry into any gang as the dependent variable, Lahey et al. (1999) did experience
some significant findings. They found that boys who committed crimes against persons
Although Lahey et al. (1999) did find conduct disorder behaviors in their first
wave to predict serious gang entry, they also found a high degree of overlap in the
conduct disorder behaviors of future gang members and boys who did not join a serious
gang. Some boys with low baseline conduct disorder behavior entered gangs, and others
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 17
with high conduct disorder behavior had not entered a serious gang by the end of their
study. This suggests the complexities that are involved in gang membership; it is not
solely due to socio-cultural reasons, nor solely due to high conduct disorder delinquent
behavior.
When examining various interactions with age, comparing early adolescence with
late adolescence, Lahey et al. (1999) also discovered significant findings. Peers that
engaged in aggressive delinquent acts significantly influenced gang entry only in early
adolescence. In addition, the higher the family income and the less parental supervision
available, the higher the risk for serious gang entry, but only in early adolescence. The
association between an adolescent’s initial conduct disorder behavior and the likelihood
of entering a serious gang weakens as age increases. The study suggested that boys who
exhibited antisocial behavior were already on this path of gang entry. As Lahey et al.
(1999) posited, “…gang entry might be thought of as the next developmental step in the
Peer Influences
Kupersmidt, Coie, and Howell (2004) postulated that peer groups have the most
the norm. A question was posed whether problems among aggressive youths are
associated with friendships, and Kupersmidt et al. (2004) answered with a definitive, yes.
They reported that antisocial youths confirm that their antisocial friends negatively
influence them and even teach them how to do things that get them into trouble. They
behavior. Kupersmidt et al. (2004) suggested the proliferation of gang activity in the
United States reflects the tendency of antisocial youths to reinforce other’s antisocial
behavior.
In their study, Craig et al. (2002) examined peer relations as an antecedent, and
compared the friendships of boys who belong to gangs to those who do not belong to a
gang. They allude to the idea that unstable gang members may spend less time with their
delinquent peers and have less knowledge about their friends’ delinquent activities than
do stable gang members. This raises a question of whether it is pressure from the gang
that contributes to these youth participating in the deviant behaviors associated with
gangs or some other influence, external or internal, that allows them to participate in this
behavior.
Hill et al. (1999) found that associating with delinquent friends was a potent
factor for predicting gang membership, and having a strong attachment to conventional
peers decreased the probability of joining gangs. Having peers that engaged in problem
behaviors, assessed by the number of friends that got them into trouble with the teacher
or tried alcohol without parents’ knowledge, accounted for a twofold increase in the
These studies reveal the complexities of gang involvement. While some found
that neighborhood, family, school, and peer influences affect gang entry; others found
that these factors only had influence when there was early antisocial behavior. Craig et
al. (2002) pointed out that understanding the mechanism of the catalyst for the initiation,
ambivalent in the disadvantaged communities gangs tend to form in; however, not all
youth respond in the same manner to these opportunities. As found in prior studies,
neighborhood context has a fairly significant influence on the youth. Beyers, Bates,
Pettit, and Dodge (2003) suggested that neighborhoods are characterized by their social
structure, that is, the aspects of the individuals who compromise them, as well as by the
nature of the way individuals relate to one another. Neighborhoods where parents talk to
each other about their children’s activities or about parenting strategies have more social
neighborhoods where adults intervene if they see a group of teenagers acting out, they
have more collective efficacy and improve children’s opportunities than neighborhoods
Dupere, Lacourse, Willms, Vitaro, and Tremblay (2007) examined the interaction
gangs. They expected that children with psychopathic tendencies, that is, high
hyperactivity, low anxiety, and low prosociality, would be more at risk to affiliate with
The sample used was from the National Longitudinal Survey of Children and
children and adolescents throughout the ten provinces. For this study, the sample was
family SES, residential instability, and childhood affiliation to deviant peer groups. Non-
intact family variables were also considered, and neighborhood characteristics were
measured using the 2001 Census of Canada. Three parent-reported scales were used to
assess late childhood psychopathic tendencies, and approximately 10.7% of the sample
presented the psychopathic tendency profile. Six percent of the participants reported on a
single self-report item that within the last 12 months they had been “part of a gang that
broke the law by stealing, hurting people, damaging property, etc.” (Dupere et al., 2007).
childhood were approximately 1.6 times more likely to have a gang affiliation in mid-
adolescence than their peers without these tendencies. They found neighborhood
economic disadvantage had 1.6 times greater odds of youth joining gangs than those who
was found to have 2.7 higher odds of youth gang affiliation than adolescents living in
average neighborhoods.
once family background contextual factors were controlled for, psychopathic tendencies
and residential instability were found to be independently associated with youth gang
independent factor. They also found independent significant associations with gang
membership and the various family background variables such as non-intact families,
family SES, family residential instability, and previous deviant peer group affiliation.
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 21
Upon adding interaction terms to the model, Dupere et al. (2007) found that youth
membership if the youth also had the psychopathic tendencies (higher hyperactivity, low
anxiety, and low prosociality). Youth living in residential instability communities have a
five times higher probability (5.2% vs. 26.5%) of joining a gang if they also demonstrate
the psychopathic profile. There was no significant interaction found between the
residential instability were associated with less parental monitoring, and less parental
likely to drastically reduce collective capacity to supervise youth groups within the
neighborhood. They suggested that single parent households, which are high in unstable
neighborhoods, reduce parent-to-child ratio, and fewer guardians are available to oversee
youth activities. Beyers et al. (2003) concluded that it is important to address family
functioning within the neighborhood context when considering the development of youth
conduct problems.
hypothesized that the more exposure one has to community violence would lead to
increased beliefs that support retaliatory aggression, which in turn would lead to lower
aggressive behavior.
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 22
elementary schools of four classes of sixth through eighth graders, and a longitudinal
model of a third elementary school with five classes of sixth through eighth graders,
where Time 1 (T1), exposure to community violence and retaliatory beliefs about
aggression lead to Time 2 (T2), self-efficacy and aggressive behavior. There were a few
The cross-sectional sample included 126 youth (43 boys, 83 girls), ages 10 to 15,
and was conducted in the fall of the academic year. The longitudinal sample included 81
students (35 boys, 46 girls). There were 129 students at T1 and 106 students at T2. The
longitudinal study was conducted during the fall and spring of the following academic
year.
All three schools served urban African American youth who lived in a Chicago
public housing development community, of which 96-100% were low income, which was
defined as families that receive public aid, are supported in foster homes with public
funds, or are eligible to receive free or reduced priced lunches. The schools in this study
also had chronic truancy rates (8.1 – 14.2%) that differed dramatically from those of the
state of Illinois (2%), and were of particular interest to this study (McMahon et al., 2009).
aggression, self-efficacy, and aggressive behavior for each sample of students. There
was a slight difference in the assessment of exposure to violence between the two groups.
The cross-sectional sample was administered the 51-item Screening Survey of Exposure
physical threats, use of weapons, burglary, and gang and drug activity. The longitudinal
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 23
sample was administered the 12-item Children’s Exposure to Violence Scale due to a
need for a shorter assessment tool. McMahon et al. (2009) thought the self-report
Other measures included the Normative Beliefs about Aggression Scale, which
measures retaliatory beliefs through two subscales: one that presents situations, and one
that poses more general questions; the Self-Efficacy to Control Aggression, a 5-item
scale from the Teen Conflict Survey and assesses a person’s confidence in controlling
their anger; and the Aggressive Behavior Scale, which measures the frequency of self-
McMahon et al. (2009) hypothesized that the more exposure one has to
community violence would lead to increased beliefs that support retaliatory aggression,
which in turn would lead to lower self-efficacy to control aggressive behavior, and
eventually lead to actual increased aggressive behavior. This model was tested using
structural equation modeling. In addition to this model, McMahon et al. (2009) proposed
there would be a direct link between violence exposure and aggressive behavior.
The cross-sectional data from the first sample was tested first. It was found that
the effect sizes of each of the model pathways were significant and their hypotheses were
confirmed. Exposure to violence was significantly related to retaliatory beliefs (p < .05),
retaliatory beliefs was significantly related to self-efficacy (p < .01), and self-efficacy
was significantly related to aggressive behavior (p < .01). McMahon et al. (2009) also
found a significant relation between exposure to violence and aggressive behavior (p <
.01). In essence, a youth living in a community where they are exposed to violence learn
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 24
to develop beliefs that retaliatory behavior is the norm, and this in turn leads to beliefs of
inadequacy in the ability to control their aggression, and these negative views on
McMahon et al. (2009) then tested the model with the longitudinal sample where
exposure to community violence and retaliatory beliefs about aggression were introduced
to the model at T1, and self-efficacy and aggressive behaviors were included in the model
at T2. Results from this longitudinal sample also confirmed the model. Exposure to
violence was significantly related to retaliatory beliefs (p < .05), retaliatory beliefs was
significantly related to self-efficacy (p < .05), and self-efficacy was significantly related
to aggressive behavior (p < .01). Exposure to violence was also found to be significantly
McMahon et al. (2009) also examined the correlations of each variable across
time from T1 to T2, and found a substantial amount of continuity across time: violence
aggressive behavior, r=.54. This stability suggested that this process that leads to
There were limitations to the study that included small sample size with limited
power, assessments were limited to self-reports, and the longitudinal study was only over
two time points in the course of one year. Despite these limitations, within this specific
violence mediates a number of cognitions with potential long term negative effects,
“The colossal individual and social costs of youth gang membership calls for a
better understanding of its antecedents and the process leading up to it” (Dupere et al.,
2007). Additional research on diverse populations, where the majority of gangs exist, can
Socio-Cultural Perspective
between the ages of 15 to 30 years. She gathered self-reported information with semi-
influenced by gangs. The data reported is from a larger study, conducted from 1992-
1994, of the socioeconomic status of Black youth in Canada, Great Britain, and the
United States.
schools, and lack of jobs, were among the five major issues identified as impacting these
survey participants. Drugs and gang/violence were the top two concerns. Eighty percent
of the participants in Los Angeles and 55% of the participants in London either
personally knew a gang member, lived in communities claimed by specific gangs, or had
Gibbs (2000) was able to identify multiple functions and reasons these
individuals joined gangs, including safety, friendship, surrogate family, social status,
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 26
respect, excitement, income, power, and to alleviate boredom. The individuals viewed
any engagement in violence as instrumental in achieving these listed goals, rather than an
end in itself. Their sole intent was not to inflict harm on people; it was a means to an
end. The gang members in the study held the perspective that the gang has its own norms
and internal structures, and serves functions other than perpetuating deviant behaviors.
Valdez et al. (2000) postulated that adolescents that grow up in these communities
may not internalize so called mainstream orientations to norms, values, expectations and
behavior. In essence, they adapt to the social structures and conditions they are
surrounded by. According to Anderson (1999), families from the “street” are more
invested in the unwritten “code of the street” and may aggressively socialize their
children into the “code” in a normative way. A community can become so alienated that
an oppositional culture, like gangs, can develop and flourish. A number of community
residents learn not to conform to the dominant society in order to survive the street
(Anderson, 1999). The DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000) notes that concerns have been raised
living in a threatening or high crime area. The issue of gangs is more complex than a
Many gang members agreed that the gang provided a more stable social structure
with clear rules, discipline, and norms, in contrast to their home environments (Gibbs,
2000). Some participants stated they derived greater social status from the gang members
than from their family, particularly when they were successful in generating income.
Some rationalized that the income gained from illegal activities is a form of reparations
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 27
structure through which the members can meet a number of psychological, social and
economic needs in a society in which they feel excluded Gibbs (2000). Many find
themselves adrift between the street and more conventional society (Anderson, 1999).
Gibbs (2000) also found that many of the gang members age out and tire of the risk
taking behavior, usually around age 30. But as Vigil (2003) points out, many gang
members, who usually have been raised in marginalized, highly stressful families, have
their social development arrested and remain peer-dependent into their thirties and
forties.
Gibbs (2000) identified some of the socio-cultural factors that may influence a
young personal to join a gang. However, gang membership is not as simple as labeling it
Self-Control
In light of McMahon et al.’s (2009) study on the role that exposure to violent
briefly review literature on the role family and community factors play in self-control.
neighborhood and community. In their study, Pratt et al. (2004) examined the
conditions. They also examined race as a variable. Pratt et al. (2004) used data from the
National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY). For purposes of their study, they
limited their sample to participants who were age 10 in 1992 (n = 463), and examined
measures cross-sectional as well as longitudinally when the participants were 12. From a
parental socialization perspective, Pratt et al. (2004) measured parental supervision and
their measures as well as self-control. All four of these factors were measured using self-
report questionnaires.
Using ordinary least squares regression equations, Pratt et al. (2004) first
examined predictors of the two measures of parental socialization. They found parental
suggesting that parents, who were male, non-White, and exposed to adverse
neighborhood conditions, were less likely to supervise their children. With regards to
parental monitoring/discipline, Pratt et al. (2004) found being non-White and exposed to
Pratt et al. (2004) then examined predictors of self-control. Again using ordinary
least squares regression equations, they found poor parental supervision and higher levels
that being non-White is significantly associated with better self-control. In measuring the
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 29
relationship between adverse neighborhood conditions and self-control, it was found that
indicates that in the neighborhoods that were reported to exhibit poorer conditions, the
children are more likely to exhibit lower self-control. In the longitudinal analysis of the
variables at age 10 measured against self-control at age 12, the findings were replicated.
Pratt et al. (2004) then looked at predictors of self-control by race. At age 10,
Whites and non-Whites exhibited similar results: both were more likely to experience
levels of discipline, and living in neighborhoods with adverse conditions. The only
difference between these two groups was the effect of race, and it was only found to be
marginally significant for Whites (p<.10). Looking at the longitudinal analysis, among
Whites, poor parental supervision and high parental discipline are significantly associated
with lower self-control; whereas with non-Whites, poor parental supervision, high
Pratt et al. (2004) also examined the total effect and fit of the models with
structural equation modeling. Overall, the model provided a good fit to the data and the
results from the previous analyses were replicated. Self-control was found to be
In examining the same structural equation model, this time split by race, Pratt et
al. (2004) found that parental supervision, parental discipline, and adverse neighborhood
conditions, all predict self-control for both Whites and non-Whites. However, an
was found. Adverse neighborhood conditions was found to have a significant effect on
parental supervision for non-Whites but not for Whites, indicating that in neighborhoods
with adverse conditions, non-White children are less likely to be supervised by their
parents. Beaver, Wright, and Delisi (2007) examined the possibility of self-control as an
executive function. Both of these concepts incorporate the ability to regulate emotions
and sustain attention. They argue that variability in self-control is likely due to
variability in the function of the prefrontal cortex, or executive function area of the brain.
control.
The data for their study came from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study,
representative sample of American children and thus far has had six waves of data
collected. For purposes of this study, Beaver et al. (2007) limited their data to the fall
and spring of kindergarten (Waves 1 and 2) and the spring of first grade (Wave 4). Due
to the large sample size, 15% of the sample was randomly selected, and a final sample of
almost 3,000 male and female children was obtained. There were three measures Beaver
based on parent and teacher responses from the Social Skills Rating Scale (SSRS) at each
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 31
wave. Neuropsychological deficits were measured using the Early Screening Inventory-
Revised (ESI-R) administered at Wave 1. A Fine Motor Skills scale and Gross Motor
Skills scale were used from the ESI-R. Six socialization measures were developed which
physical punishment, and neighborhood disadvantage. They used gender and race as
controls.
al. (2007) found that both fine and gross motor skills are predictive of low self-control for
males, suggesting that higher motor skills are associated with lower self-control.
However, when prior self-control was introduced, gross motor skills were no longer
physical punishment. When the measure of prior self–control was introduced, only
parental withdrawal and physical punishment had a significant effect on low self-control.
Beaver et al. (2007) next tested whether the neuropsychological measures have
the ability to predict low self-control later in childhood from the scale measured in Wave
4. For the male sample, both neuropsychological measures were significantly related to
low self-control in both models, with and without prior self-control. Three of the
parental involvement, parental withdrawal, and physical punishment. When prior self–
control was incorporated, only parental withdrawal had a significant effect on low self-
control.
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 32
Parenting and other social measures had some effect on self-control, but results were
small and inconsistent. One of the limitations is the sample age, and Beaver et al. (2007)
neuropsychological tests that measure motor skills were included. Other measures might
be used that specifically test for executive functioning. In addition, the motor skills
scales may have identified dysfunctions that are not related to self-control.
Ratchford and Beaver (2009) examined the relationship between social and
biological risk factors, self-control, and delinquency in a three step study, using ordinary
least squares regression equations, which first looked at whether social factors (family
and community) and neuropsychological deficits, among other biological risk factors, are
dependent variable to determine predictor variables that might be associated with these
variables in delinquency.
Ratchford and Beaver’s (2009) data was drawn from the National Survey of
Children (NSC), a longitudinal survey that includes three waves of data collected over 10
years. Ratchford and Beaver (2009) only used data from Waves 1 and 2 for purposes of
their study. Wave 1 was collected in 1976 when the children were between the ages of
six and 12, for a total participant pool of 2,301. Wave 2 data was gathered five years
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 33
later in 1981, when the participants were between 12 and 17 years with a total of 1,423
respondents.
For the self-control measure, Ratchford and Beaver (2009) developed their own
composite self-control scales based on information gathered from parents and teachers
from the NSC. Biological risk was assessed by measuring neuropsychological deficits as
well as birth complications and low birth weight as reported by the mothers in Wave 1.
The neuropsychological deficits were measured with the Peabody Picture Vocabulary
In the first step, measuring self-control with any social or biological predictor
variables, Ratchford and Beaver (2009) found that neuropsychological deficits are a
punishment and neighborhood disadvantage had significant effects on the parental low
self-control scale as well as the composite scale. The teacher low self-control scale was
predictive of only parental punishment. In other words, the more parental punishment and
neighborhood disadvantage was indicated, the lower the self-control. Gender and race
were also found to have significant associations with low self-control, indicating males
Ratchford and Beaver (2009) found that self-control is influenced by both social
and biological factors; however, the neuropsychological deficits had the most consistent
neuropsychological deficits, only considering social risk factors and the other two
biological risk factors, as well as gender and race. They found that birth complications,
low birth weight, gender, and race all had significant effects on neurological deficits. In
other words, male, non-White children who had suffered birth complications and were
born with a low birth weight, had more neurological deficits than children who did not
Finally, Ratchford and Beaver (2009) considered delinquency and its predictors,
including the parent and teacher self-control variables as well as the socialization and
biological variables. Parental punishment was the most consistently significant of the
social predictors, indicating that the more parental punishment, the more likely the child
will be delinquent. None of the biological measures had an effect on delinquency. Each
of the low self-control scales (parent, teacher, composite) in both waves had significant
effects on delinquency, suggesting that the less self-control a child has; the more likely
Although biological factors were not predictive of delinquency in this study, they
the prior studies, also found collective socialization to be predictive of self-control and
delinquency. These suggest that a biosocial approach, incorporating both social and
Heritability Factors
Gene-Environment Interactions
behavior, and how it differs among varied socioeconomic status, or what they referred to
Their sample included 2,133 16 to17 year old twins and 1,067 parents from the
third wave of the Twin study of CHild and Adolescent Development (TCHAD)
Stockholm University. The items were found to have high validity (97.5%) and
reliability (90%), as well as high internal consistency with a Cronbach’s alpha of .92.
Family socioeconomic status was measured with self-reported data from the parents of
conditions were measured using five variables: ethnic diversity, basic education level,
phenotypic variance factors are also portioned into interaction effects with socioeconomic
status, as well as a main effect of socioeconomic status on antisocial behavior. They also
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 36
behavior and genetic, shared environment, and non-shared environment factors. Testing
on the interaction between antisocial behavior and socioeconomic status indicated that
surrounding environment. They found that genetic factors are more expressed in a
adolescent lacks the environmental risk factors that push or predispose him/her to behave
antisocially. Likewise, they found genetic factors to be weaker and the shared
backgrounds because the environment in these areas will conceal the genetic contribution
be associated with low self-control, associating with delinquent peers, as well as the
Beaver, Shutt, Boutwell, Ratchford, Roberts, and Barnes’ (2009) study was to expand on
many criminological studies and their ideas that low self-control among delinquents is
due to social learning theory. They posited that biology and genetics should also be
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 37
associations. Beaver, Shutt et al. (2009) specifically wanted to determine any effects of
genetic and environmental factors, both shared and non-shared, in the development of
Data was from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add
twelfth grade among 80 high schools and 52 middle schools, who were administered a
self-report survey in 1994. There were three waves of data collected over seven years,
with the final wave occurring with the participants ranging between 18 and 26 years.
Within the sample of participants was a subsample of siblings, who were asked to
indicate whether they were part of a twin pair. Beaver, Shutt et al. (2009) only included
monozygotic (MZ) twin pairs and same-sex dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs. For purposes of
their study, they only used data from the first two waves; their final analytical sample
Beaver, Shutt et al. (2009) used low self-control and drug-using peers as the
drug-using peers, they used the DeFries-Fulker (DF) augmented analysis, a regression-
based approach that allows for analysis of data with sibling pairs. Beaver, Shutt et al.
(2009) used double entry of the twins to allow for each twin to be used as the dependent
Their results were broken into four models with Wave 1 represented by Models 1
and 2, and Wave 2 represented by Models 3 and 4. Models 1 and 3 accounted for
genetic, shared, and non-shared environmental influences, and the four specific non-
self-control, Beaver, Shutt et al. (2009) found that shared environment did not account for
any of the variance in low self-control. In Wave 1, 56% of the variance was accounted
for by genetic influences and the non-shared environment accounted for the remaining
44% of variance. When the four parental non-shared environmental factors were
introduced, genetic influences still accounted for more than half (52%) of the variance;
however, of the four measures, only maternal disengagement was statistically significant
Among Wave 2 participants, genetics accounted for 40% of the low self-control
variance, and the non-shared environment accounted for 60% of the remaining variance.
When the four non-shared environmental variables were introduced, genetic variance
increased to 44% and non-shared variance reduced to 56%. Two of the parental variables
parental permissiveness.
Using the same four model structure, when looking at drug-using peers, Beaver,
Shutt et al. (2009) found some slightly different results. Shared environment was found
to have an effect on Wave 1, but not on Wave 2. In Wave 1, Beaver, Shutt et al. (2009)
found that genetics accounted for 37% of variance, the shared environment for 27% of
variance, and the non-shared environment and measurement error accounted for 36% of
variance. When the four non-shared variables were added, the variance of genetic effects
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 39
increased to 40% and the shared environment variance decreased to 22%. There was one
maternal attachment.
In Wave 2 of assessing for drug-using peers, 53% of the variance was found to be
accounted for by genetics, a large jump from Wave 1. There was no shared environment;
hence, the remaining 47% of variance was accounted for by the non-shared environment
and measurement error. Lastly, when the four non-shared environment variables were
added, the genetic influence increased to 62% variance, and the non-shared environment
accounted for 38% of the variance. None of the four variables were found to be
In short, Beaver, Shutt et al. (2009) found that both genetic and non-shared
environmental factors play a role in low self-control, as well as engaging with drug-using
this study, the specific four measures were limited and had small, inconsistent effects.
Another item to note is that this analysis was based on a sample of MZ and DZ twins, and
may not generalize to the larger population. Future research might concentrate on
specific genetic causes to low self-control as well as the complex ways the non-shared
Beaver, DeLisi, Vaughn, and Wright (2010) examined the existence of overlap
Second, MAOA will not have any effects on low self-control or delinquency; however,
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 40
Beaver, DeLisi et al. (2010) used data from the National Longitudinal Study of
Adolescent Health (Add Health), which included in Wave 1 more than 90,000 students in
seventh through twelfth grade in 1994-1995 from 132 middle schools and high schools.
Three waves of in-home interviews occurred over six years. In Wave 3, a subsample of
participants who had a sibling or co-twin who was also participating in the study was
asked to submit buccal cells for genotyping. This resulted in 2,574 respondents being
genotyped. Beaver, DeLisi et al. (2010) limited their study to white males as they did not
have sufficient statistical power to examine other groups. After removing one
monozygotic twin from each monozygotic twin pair, they had a final sample of 767 white
males.
The MAOA was measured by dividing the gene into two groups: low MAOA
activity and high MAOA activity. The low MAOA activity group comprised the 2- and
3- repeat alleles, and the high MAOA activity group included the 3.5-, 4-, and 5-repeat
alleles (Beaver, DeLisi et al., 2010). The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT)
score of five questions that were confirmed via factor analysis to be significant indicators
of a latent construct. Delinquency was measured via summed responses that created a 15-
Beaver, DeLisi et al. (2010) focused their analyses on Waves 1 and 2 to measure
the delinquency scale as the dependent variable. Using a negative binomial regression,
they ran three models. First MAOA, neuropsychological deficits, age, and age squared
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 41
were examined as predictor variables of delinquency; only age and age squared were
between MAOA x neuropsychological deficits was added. This interaction was found to
themselves. In the third model, the Wave 1 low self-control scale was introduced and
found to be significant and have the strongest predictor effect on delinquency. The
addition of low self-control also weakened the effect of the MAOA x neuropsychological
deficits interaction.
Wave 1. Only age and age squared had a significant effect on delinquency. The
interaction between MAOA x neuropsychological deficits when added was again found
significance level after adding the low self-control variable. Neuropsychological deficits
were no longer significant in this wave after adding the MAOA x neuropsychological
interaction. Low self-control was also found to have a significant positive effect on
delinquency scales in Waves 1 and 2. In the first model, only neuropsychological deficits
were found to have a significant positive effect on violent delinquency in Waves 1 and 2.
MAOA was not found to have a significant main effect. When the MAOA x
neuropsychological deficits interaction was added, it also had a significant positive effect
on violent delinquency in both Waves 1 and 2; however, when low self-control was
introduced in the third equation, this interaction no longer had a significant main effect
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 42
delinquency. As in the prior analysis, low self-control was the strongest predictor of
violent delinquency.
Lastly, Beaver, DeLisi et al. (2010) used the low self-control scale as the
dependent variable to examine any effects MAOA and neuropsychological deficits may
have on low self-control. They found that neither MAOA or neuropsychological deficits
on their own have a significant main effect on low self-control; however, when the
findings were similar; however, the addition of the MAOA x neuropsychological deficits
Beaver, DeLisi et al. (2010) confirmed most of their hypotheses, and found that
have any effects on low self-control or delinquency independently, but did interact with
Some limitations include the small sample of respondents that were genotyped
and the affect this may have on generalizability, as well as the use of a delinquency scale
Add Health sample was a community sample and more than likely did not include an
adequate number of LCP offenders. Lastly, the sample only included white males. This
affects generalizability by only including one ethnic group and not a diverse group of
participants.
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 43
Delinquency Trajectory
hypothesizing that temporary and persistent antisocial individuals are comprised of two
distinct types of people. She posited that timing and duration of antisocial behavior are
the defining features of her two proposed types of offenders. A larger group of
individuals are thought to engage in delinquent behavior for a short period of time in
that engages in some kind of antisocial behavior at every stage in life, she identified as
course-persistent.
have no notable antisocial behavior in childhood and little chance of such behavior in
adulthood. Their delinquency is often abrupt, particularly during the onset and
desistance, and their behavior is inconsistent. They may ditch school but abstain from
in that participation in antisocial behavior that reaps rewards is maintained and antisocial
behavior that has severe consequences is extinguished. Moffit (1993) suggested two
between childhood and adulthood, and influence of life-course persistent antisocial role
antisocial activity is social mimicry, which leads one to believe that delinquency leads to
an end with a desirable resource. This desired resource is postulated to be a mature status
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 44
which attempts to close the gap between biological age and social age. Once the
developmental process takes hold and a teen gets closer to adulthood with adult roles,
their interest in antisocial behavior diminishes as this maturity gap closes; they no longer
have anything to prove. Moffit (1993) suggested that the antisocial behavior that was
once thought of as rewarding is now punitive, and remits based on them now having
something to lose.
continuity of antisocial behavior over their life. They bite and hit at age four, shoplift and
are truant at age 10, sell drugs and steal cars at age 16, rob and rape at age 22, and
commit fraud and child abuse at age 30. She suggested that there is no reason to assume
that life-course persistent delinquents assume prosocial tendencies after several decades
of antisocial behavior.
In those whose behavior is stable and persistent, Moffitt (1993) suggested you
have to look for roots in early development. There could be disruption in the
development of the fetal brain by maternal drug abuse, poor prenatal nutrition, and
deficits empirically associated with antisocial behavior are verbal and executive
functions. Verbal deficits may include difficulties with receptive listening and reading,
problem solving, expressive speech, writing, and memory. Executive functioning deficits
well as maintain it over a life course. One is evocative interaction, which occurs when a
child’s behavior evokes particular responses from others (Caspi, 1987). The other two
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 45
interactions are reactive and proactive. Reactive aggression occurs when a person reacts
to an environment based on their style. A person may read into cues as harmful intent
even if there is none, and respond aggressively. Proactive interaction occurs when people
create an environment that supports their style, such as being selective with others they
Moffitt (1993) posited two sources of continuity that she suggested are reasons
why life-course persistent delinquents are impervious to change. One is failing to learn
prosocial alternatives to behavior. She suggested that once the deviant behavior begins, a
lack of recourse to more prosocial alternatives keeps them on the course toward life
persistency. They miss opportunities at every stage of development due to being rejected
by peers and adults. Second is becoming entangled in a deviant life-style due to crime’s
consequences. Poor self-control and impulsivity increase the risk that antisocial youth
Moffit (1993) suggested that the stability of antisocial behavior is linked to its
extremity; however, those that engage in extreme antisocial behavior will not necessarily
have stability over time. Gangs are generally involved in extreme antisocial behavior,
ranging from assault, selling drugs, car theft, and murder. They are a stable phenomenon
in the sense of their extreme crimes, but their stability is not implied and change is
possible depending on their own capacities for change. There have been multiple studies
Raskin White, Bates, and Buyske (2001), examined delinquency that persists into
adulthood and hypothesized three trajectories would identify themselves in their study:
their behavior from adolescence into adulthood. They also thought these three groups
environmental risk.
Raskin White et al. (2001) used data collected as part of a larger longitudinal
study on adolescent development from the Rutgers Health and Human Development
Project (HHDP). Participants of the HHDP study included 698 males and 682 females
tested between 1979 and 1981 (Time 1, T1). Raskin White et al. (2001) only used male
participants for their study due to life-course persistent delinquency usually being
inclusive of men. Three cohorts were tested at T1, with the youngest cohort being age
12, the middle cohort being age 15, and the third cohort being age 18. Participants of
each cohort were tested three more times over fifteen years, with the first retest in 1982-
1984 (T2), the second in 1985-1987 (T3), and the last in 1992-1994 (T4). Of the original
1979-1981 sample, 1,257 returned for the T4 testing, providing a 91% response rate. The
original sample was 90% Caucasian with a median family income between $20,000 and
$29,000 in 1981.
Delinquency was measured by the number of times a respondent engaged in petty theft,
grand theft, motor vehicle theft, breaking and entering, avoiding payment, vandalism,
assault, and armed robbery over the prior three year period. An aggressive scale was also
assessed by frequency of armed robbery, assault, and fight with a weapon or in a gang.
birth risk factors: low birth weight and premature birth. At T3, additional
functioning. Verbal ability was measured by the Shipley Institute of Living Scale (SILS)
Vocabulary Test. Executive functioning was assessed by the Trail Making Test - Part B
Raskin White et al. (2001) also assessed sensation seeking with the Disinhibition
the youngest cohort due to lack of reliability prior to age 14. Family adversity was
assessed with three measures: family SES, family structure, and family hostility.
technique to identify any delinquency trajectories. Seven age scores for delinquency
were used to model the trajectories: 12, 15, 18, 21, 24.5, 28, and 31. Each age group was
added and the analyses repeated to determine the optimal number of groups that best
describes the data. An EM algorithm was used to determine the trajectories and the
membership probabilities. Raskin White et al. (2001) then measured the association
between delinquency trajectory and the risk factors and their interactions with
Raskin White et al. (2001) identified a four group trajectory model as best fitting:
delinquency scale than adolescence-limited at all ages except 12; however, the aggressive
scale only had statistically significant differences at ages 18 and 24.5. The escalating
group did not show any statistically significant differences until age 21, compared to the
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 48
adolescence-limited group, and became significantly more delinquent than the persistent
personality, and environmental risk factors, and whether their influences could
distinguish the delinquency trajectories. They found that the main effects of these
predictors and age cohort were significant, as were the addition of the neuropsychological
-environmental interaction, which improved the model. This indicated that individually,
environment interactions did not significantly improve the model fit, indicating these two
In examining odds ratios (OR), Raskin White et al. (2001) found that delinquents
had significantly greater odds of having higher impulsivity (OR = 1.15), lower harm
avoidance (OR = 1.11), higher disinhibition (OR = 1.41), higher parental hostility (OR =
1.03), and one parent families (OR = 2.16), than non-delinquents. Disinhibition was the
persistent delinquency, with an OR of 1.19. They also found that persistent delinquents
have significantly greater odds of higher disinhibition (OR = 1.20), lower verbal ability
(OR = 1.06), lower harm avoidance (OR = 1.15), higher parental hostility (OR = 1.03),
and coming from a one-parent family (OR = 3.49), than escalating delinquents.
Raskin White et al. (2001) not only identified the three trajectory models, but a
fourth model was noted, which was unexpected. They were able to identify some
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 49
differences in the escalating delinquents from the persistent delinquents; however, more
follow up is needed on these two trajectories. Some of the predictor variables did not
produce significant effects, and more research into this area is needed. This may be a
matter of the risk factors being unable to distinguish adolescence-limited from persistent,
or from the escalating group identified, and because other risk factors need to be
researched to better gauge this older population and their trajectories for delinquent
behavior.
and four manifestations of life-course persistent offending: early onset, violent and
hypothesized that poor performance on the neuropsychological tests would predict the
Piquero (2001) gathered data from three sources: the National Collaborative
The NCPP followed over 56,000 pregnancies from pre-term to age seven. For purposes
of his study, Piquero (2001) identified 207 African–American youth (144 males, 63
The four dependent variables were coded for ease of analysis. Onset of offending
was identified using age 14 as a cut-off; life-course persistent offenders were coded has
having an onset prior to age 14, and adolescent-limited offenders were coded as having
an onset at or after age 14. Offenders were also classified as either being nonviolent or
having offenses that were both nonviolent and violent. Seriousness of criminal offending
was identified via a score from the Sellin and Wolfgang seriousness scale. This scale
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 50
takes into account the type of criminal acts and the aggravating factors associated with
the acts (Piquero, 2001). Finally, chronic offending was differentiated on the basis of
number of offenses incurred by age 18: four or less offenses were less chronic, and five
Independent variables examined were low birth weight, weak family structure,
biosocial interaction, and neuropsychological tests. Low birth weight was differentiated
by being either above or below six pounds. Weak family structure was measured by
summing the scores of three indicators when the child was age seven: number of changes
in the mother’s marital status, who the child lives with, and whether the father of the
child was present in the household. Biosocial interaction was assessed by examining the
relationship between low birth weight and weak family structure. The
neuropsychological test administered was the short form of the Wechsler Intelligence
Piquero (2001) also included two control variables: gender and socioeconomic
status (SES), which was a composite measure of three indicators collected when the boys
were age seven: education, income, and occupation of the head of household.
In looking at the relation between the WISC and the four manifestations of life-
course persistent offending, Piquero (2001) found that verbal and performance subscales
of the WISC were significantly positively correlated with one another. However, in
looking specifically at each of the subscales, the four manifestation variables were all
found to be negatively correlated with both the verbal and performance subscale of the
WISC; only some were significant. The verbal subscale was found to have the most
-.201, p<.05), and chronic offending (r = -.193, p<.05). The performance subscale was
Using hierarchical logistic regressions, Piquero (2001) first examined how each of
the five independent variables and two control variables might predict the four
he added the two WISC subscales to measure any effect they may have on the
In the first regression, Piquero (2001) examined early onset of offending and how
birth weight, family structure, gender, SES, and biosocial interaction might predict its
occurrence. He found that low birth weight, SES, and the biosocial interaction of low
birth weight and family structure, significantly predicted early onset of offending. When
adding the WISC subscales, Piquero (2001) did not find any significant effect on early
onset.
In the results for involvement in violent and nonviolent offending, Piquero (2001)
found only gender to have a significant effect on violent and nonviolent offending in
model 1. In model 2, when the WISC subscales were added, gender remained a
significant effect, but the verbal subscale was also determined to have a significant effect
on involvement in violent and nonviolent offending. Essentially, those who score lower
on the verbal subscales are more likely to engage in both violent and nonviolent offenses.
score in seriousness of offending, Piquero (2001) found that in model 1, only gender was
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 52
subscales in model 2, gender remained a significant effect, but the verbal subscale was
who score lower on the verbal subscales are more likely to be involved in serious
offending.
the variables in the first model are significantly indicative of this behavior. When the two
WISC subscales are added in model 2, gender and the verbal subscale of the WISC are
found to have a significant effect on predicting chronic offending. As found in the prior
two predictor regressions, males are significantly more likely than females to be involved
in chronic offending, as well as those who score lower on the verbal subscales.
Piquero (2001) determined that poor verbal ability has a significant relation to
being more likely to engage in both violent and nonviolent offenses, serious offending,
occurred simultaneously: the emergence of language and the explosive rise of executive
functions, or more specifically, the frontal lobes. Although any relationship of these two
developments is not often discussed, Goldberg (2009) posited their emergence at the
same time in evolution is not a coincidence. Language has a generative power to build
new constructs; however, this ability may depend on the frontal lobes to make use of and
manipulate the language constructs into more complex operations. The question remains
whether the poor verbal ability demonstrated in this study is the result of poor cognitive
Although Piquero (2001) found that poor neuropsychological test scores can
to his study. First, the generalizability of the study is limited by the class and race of the
sample. Second, the WISC was not able to directly measure neuropsychological risk.
The use of more direct tests may provide stronger results. Lastly, criminal history data
was only available through age 17. The neuropsychological test scores obtained as an
adolescent may affect the ability to predict criminal activity into adulthood, and affect the
Also, the criminal history of the sample is from the 1980s, and criminal offenses
committed then may have limited generalizability to the offenses committed by offenders
today.
analyzed the data from the Columbia County Longitudinal Study, initiated in 1960 with
856 children in third grade, and followed their aggressive and antisocial behavior from
age eight to 30. Huesmann et al. (2009) extended this study to predict criminality
This study was initially broken into three waves; Huesmann et al. (2009) added a
fourth wave. The first wave included 856 children at eight years old. Over 90% were
Caucasian, and 51% were male. The number of ethnic minorities was too small to allow
separate analyses, so they were excluded from this study. Eighty-five percent of the
participants’ mothers and 71% of the fathers were interviewed in this wave.
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 54
Wave 2 was conducted in 1970 when the participants had a modal age of 19.
Four hundred and twenty-seven of the original 856 children participated, with 211 boys
and 216 girls. They had completed an average of 12.6 years of education. In 1981,
Wave 3 was conducted with 409 of the original participants. The modal age was 30, and
consisted of 198 males and 211 females. The average education level was some college
Wave 4 was completed between 1999 and 2002. Five hundred and twenty-three
of the original 856 sample completed this wave (61%). This sample consisted of 268
males and 255 females, with an average age of 48.46 years. The average education level
was between some college and a college degree. The sample sizes vary considerably
over all four waves depending on the variables that are studied and what information is
available. For this study and aggression, complete data is available for 285 participants
for the first three waves and 230 participants for all four waves (Huesmann et al., 2009).
Huesmann et al. (2009) incorporated the three specific aggression measures used
in the original study into their fourth wave. Peer-nominated aggression was assessed at
ages eight and 19 using ten peer-nominated aggression items that cover physical, verbal,
acquisitive, and indirect acts of aggression. Severe physical aggression was assessed at
ages 19, 30, and 48, via the participants’ self-reports of how often in the prior year they
had engaged in each of the following four behaviors: choked someone, slapped or kicked
Aggressive personality was assessed at ages 19, 30, and 48, with the sum of scales 4, 9,
Adult outcomes were identified with 13 variables in the last wave of the original
study, and Huesmann et al. (2009) applied these same items to wave 4. These items,
which assist in measuring the outcome of the participants’ adult life, include self-reports
religious attendance.
(2009) used structural equation modeling first over the first three waves, and then
recomputed the data with their current data over all four waves. The first three waves
produced disattenuated stability coefficients of .50 for males and .35 for females. After
recomputing the model with their data from wave 4, the disattenuated continuity
coefficients were very similar, .50 for males and .42 for females.
of initially high and low participants that stay high or low was compared on all four
waves. Two criteria were used to identify those that scored high on aggression on all
four time points, and who scored low on all four time points. The first criterion was
those that fell above or below the overall median in any wave, and the second was those
that fell above or below the 33rd and 67th percentiles in any wave.
participants stay high (35%) when using the median. This suggests the continuity of
aggressiveness is not solely due to the high-aggressive staying high. When the results are
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 56
broken down by gender, males tend to hold their position in their classification (40%)
more than females (33%). Males stay in the high category (38%) more so than females
(18%); however, on the other end, males and females tend to consistently stay in the low
Huesmann et al. (2009) used logistic regression to examine what child and family
characteristics can predict life-course persistent aggression. They compared the life-
course persistent aggressive group with the life-course persistent nonaggressive group
and found that being male (OR = 4.2), having parents with lower education (OR = 0.25),
having parents who hit you (OR = 3.5), and having parents who reject you (OR = 1.3), all
have significant effects in increasing the odds a child will have life-course persistent
aggression.
composite aggression scores. The subsets are life-course persistent low aggressive, life-
aggressive, and late-onset aggressive. In looking at the scores of each of these trajectory
groups on the adult outcome measures, the life-course persistent high aggressive group
had consistently poorer outcomes at age 48 than any other group. For example, 7% of the
life-course persistent aggressive group were arrested between ages 30 and 48, compared
to none of the other groups being arrested during that time. This group scored
were significantly more likely to report problems with alcohol and scored significantly
more depressed.
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 57
consequences than the life-course persistent low aggressive group. The only differences
reported were more problems with alcohol, and the adolescent-limited group scored
higher on the trait of aggression at age 48 than did the life-course persistent low
aggressive group. The late-onset group showed more negative outcomes than the low-
aggressive group, but not as many as the life-course persistent high aggressive group
that occurs over entire population levels and is not driven by one trajectory group
maintaining their behavior over a life course (Huesmann et al., 2009). Continuity was
found to be the result of low aggressive remaining low as much as it was high aggressive
remaining high.
Donnellan, Ge, and Wenk (2000) tested a hypothesis derived from Moffitt’s
(1993) developmental taxonomy theory that life-course persistent criminals will perform
poorer on cognitive ability tests than offenders who desist from criminal behavior in
adolescence. They used a unique data set of 20 years of arrest records from a large and
The sample was part of a larger study of 4,164 adolescent males committed to the
between January 1964 and December 1965. Using arrest records collected in 1984 and
1985 to classify the participants, 888 in the adolescent-limited category and 774
offenders in the life-course persistent category, Donnellan et al. (2000) identified 1,662
offenders for their final analysis. The ethnic breakdown was 27% African American,
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 58
52% Caucasian, and 21% Hispanic. The ethnic breakdown of the offender classification
system was notably different. The adolescent-limited category was 17% African
American, 68% Caucasian, and 15% Hispanic, while the ethnic breakdown of the life-
course persistent category was 38% African American, 34% Caucasian, and 28%
tests, and official arrest records. The cognitive measures used were the California
Achievement Test (CAT), California Test of Mental Maturity (CTTM), General Aptitude
Test Battery (GATB), and the Raven Test of Progressive Matrices, all administered in
1964 and 1965 upon the youths’ admission to the institution in Tracy.
With such a diverse sample, Donnellan et al. (2000) controlled for any impact of
tests for each ethnic group. Three separate MANOVAs were run for the CAT, CTTM,
and GATB, and an ANOVA was run for the Raven Test.
cognitive measures than did the life-course persistent offenders in nine of 12 tests. The
non-significant results were evidenced in the spatial and perceptual tests of the GATB
In the Hispanic sample, there were significant differences in six of the 12 tests
differences were in the reading and arithmetic tests of the CAT, the language and non-
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 59
language tests of the CTTM, the spatial test of the GATB, and the Raven test. The
observed effect sizes for significant results in both the Caucasian (.17 to .26) and
Hispanic (.22 to .36) samples were small using Cohen’s designations, which was
The African American sample did not provide any significant differences between
expected to be small, in this sample, they were particularly weak with only three greater
than .10. In three of the tests, the effect sizes were negative due to the observed
differences being in the opposite direction from expectations (Donnellan et al., 2000).
offenders (66.3% and 61.7%, respectively) than did Caucasians (30.2%). Future work
would need to address the role of ethnicity in criminal behavior and its influence over
life-course criminality. Second, the study did not control for SES and its role in any
Donnellan et al. (2000) found that the theory of differences in cognitive abilities
One explanation Donnellan et al. (2000) postulates is that cognitive abilities are not as
influential in the contexts in which African Americans live, and we must account for the
These studies that measure delinquency trajectories suggest that life course
persistent offenders begin their trajectory of crime in childhood, much sooner than
adolescent-limited offenders who tend to begin later and age out in late teens or early
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 60
twenties. This may suggest a different cognitive development trajectory, and something
that needs further research. In addition, as found by Donnellan et al. (2000), ethnicity
may play a large role in determining the delinquency trajectory, as does environmental
Executive Functioning
CEO of a company; it coordinates and integrates all the other structures of the brain. He
stated that human cognition is forward looking and we require a strong mechanism that
allows us to plan, dream, aspire, and set goals. The prefrontal cortex is the “seat of
regulate emotion. As a process, the prefrontal cortex is otherwise known to facilitate the
executive functions. Once the abilities of the prefrontal cortex and executive functions
impulsivity and the inability to see another’s point of view. Some research suggests that
behavior.
Since Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990) proposed their general theory of crime,
there have been many studies on the antecedents to antisocial behavior. Some studies
support Gottfredson and Hirschi’s (1990) social theory of crime and others support a
more biogenic cause for antisocial behavior. One neurobiological explanation is by van
Goozen, Fairchild, and Harold (2008), who argue that neurobiological deficits link
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 61
genetic influences and early trauma or adversity to antisocial behavior. They also
From a biological standpoint, van Goozen, Fairchild, and Harold (2008) suggest
that serious stressful events early in life play an important role in “programming” the
stress systems. Those who grow up in adverse conditions and communities, such as gang
members, are commonly in the face of chronic stress in the form of safety concerns,
employment concerns, and where the next meal might come from, among others. This
include avoidance of chronic arousal and energy expenditure that could result in serious
Goozen, Fairchild, and Harold (2008) posited that antisocial individuals are less likely to
avoid stressful situations and are likely more fearless and less concerned about possible
negative consequences.
From the perspective of biology and heritability, there is some research that
becoming deviant has some physiological causation. The literature has demonstrated that
there are psychosocial risk factors for antisocial behavior, as well as biological risk
factors; but how do these two risk factors interact? Raine (2002b) suggested that due to
the human’s ability to live in a close social system, mostly as a result of the prefrontal
there are biological processes that can affect this system regulation and cause reasoning
and decision making to go haywire. Raine (2002b) argued that there is a prefrontal
dysfunction theory based on the delayed growth of the prefrontal cortex, which can
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 62
develop into the 20s and even the 30s. There are enormous demands during late
adolescence where efficient regulation and control of a growing sex drive, challenges to
social status, and planning for the future is necessary. Adolescents in a lower SES
community tend to have more complex challenges and competing day to day struggles.
With this magnified cognitive load, Raine (2002b) hypothesized that an overload on the
prefrontal cortex occurs. This overload may cause prefrontal dysfunction and a lack of
inhibitory control over antisocial, violent behavior that peaks during adolescence (Raine,
2002a). This age is the prime time that youths consider gang activity, and a time when
they are becoming more independent of their parents and learning ways to express
become expressed, and those with prefrontal dysfunction and poor executive functions
those with a less structured, less stable psychosocial environment, which most gang
prefrontal cortex has matured and they have a full capacity to manage and self-regulate
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 63
behavior. This raises the question of how the prefrontal cortex might be altered for life-
instrumental aggression, has been discussed in the literature. Specifically, free will,
learned they occur automatically; there is no thought process that goes into these
responses. Reactive aggression is often the result of a frustrating event and frequently
induces anger. Instrumental aggression incorporates the idea of free will; a person made
one decision when they could have made another. It is goal oriented and involves
executive functioning processes that allow one to come to their desired outcome. Most
antisocial behavior is instrumental, as it usually entails goal oriented acts such as theft,
fraud, or robbery (Blair, 2007). Although some instrumental aggression can become
reactive due to over-learning, particularly due to learned responses to the complex social
People with personality impairments tend to choose an aggressive course of action due to
head injury.
Participants for their study came from the youngest cohort of the Pittsburgh Youth
Study, which measured 21 serious antisocial behaviors. The original sample for the
Pittsburgh Youth Study had 503 participants; however, due to attrition and other reasons,
Raine et al. (2005) had a total of 335 participants for their biosocial study of aggression
and violent behavior. These participants had an average age of 16.5 at the time of the
testing, with an ethnic breakdown of 41.2% Caucasian and 58.8% African American.
the boys themselves, beginning when the participants were age seven, and taken every six
months from ages seven to eleven, then taken annually up to age 17. Participants were
classified into six levels of delinquency seriousness which were determined by the most
Raine et al. (2005) also assessed environmental factors and ADHD at age seven,
and assessed neurocognitive functioning and any history of head injury at age 17. In
order to meet the diagnostic criteria of ADHD, participants had to present with at least six
months duration with eight of the 14 behaviors. The rate of ADHD in this sample at age
seven was 14.1%. Raine et al. (2005) also incorporated child abuse data that was
collected from official court records at the Children and Youth Services offices. In
however, after a factor analysis and Oblimin rotation were performed, a screen test
parental neglect. All 19 of the initial measures loaded on one of the three factors.
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 65
measure verbal and spatial IQ, verbal memory, and visual-spatial memory, using subtests
of the WAIS and Wechsler Memory Scale, as well as tests that can measure for any head
injuries in frontal lobe functioning such as the Continuous Performance Task (CPT), the
scores into four groups. The groups identified were the control group, who remained low
on antisocial behavior throughout the study. The second group was the adolescent
limited (AL) group, who started off near the control group, but progressed to significant
levels of antisocial behavior by late adolescence. The third group is the childhood
limited (CL) group, who has high antisocial behavior up to about age eleven, then it
drops off. Lastly, the life course persistent (LCP) group starts off with high antisocial
significant main group effects on verbal and spatial IQ. The LCP group had lower verbal
scores compared to the ALs, as well as lower verbal and total IQ than the control group.
The CL group scored lower than the control in verbal, spatial, and total IQ measures.
Raine et al. (2005) did not find any significant difference in the three IQ measures
delayed recall on both verbal and visual-spatial memory. The LCP group yielded spatial
but not verbal memory deficits, and the CLs showed deficits in all measures except
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 66
found significant differences on the CPT, with the LCP group and CLs having had
performed worse than the control group. There were no significant effects on the WCST.
right-ear listening, with the CL group performing significantly worse than both the
of head injury, and ADHD, they were all found to have significant effects on antisocial
grouping, with the exception of head injuries. Raine et al. (2005) viewed head injury as a
potential confound, and ran an analysis with uncorrected post hoc comparisons to assess
whether more head injuries would be found in an LCP group. Out of the six types of
head injuries measured, only number or times knocked unconscious provided evidence
that could indicate an LCP group has a greater history of head injuries than the control as
With regards to court-recorded abuse, the LCP group was found to have almost
four times the rate of abuse than the control group, and more than two times the rate of
abuse than the AL group. The CL group had more than two times the rate of abuse than
the control group, but did not differ from the LCP group. ADHD was found to have rates
twice as high in the LCP and CL groups compared to the control and AL groups.
and parental neglect, were found to have a significant influence on antisocial grouping.
The LCP group was found to have higher levels of poverty and parental neglect than the
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 67
control and AL groups, and the CL group had higher levels of all three measures than the
control group.
Raine et al. (2005) needed to determine whether ADHD, particularly with its high
rate of occurrence in this sample, and psychosocial adversity, were confounds that could
significant after these were both added as covariates, and determined that any links in
neurocognitive and antisocial behavior were independent of the influence of both ADHD
behavior with the above results, Raine et al. (2005) determined that offenders
demonstrate spatial and verbal impairments; they are impaired on memory tasks, life
course persistent offenders are more impaired on neurocognitive and psychosocial factors
than the control, childhood-limited offenders do have cognitive impairments, and finally,
cognitive impairments are not attributable to child abuse, psychosocial adversity, head
injuries, or ADHD. Neurocognitive measures were not assessed prior to the onset of any
pertain to those on a path to lifetime offending. Raine et al. (2005) suggested follow up
past age 17 years into adulthood would be needed to truly assess whether these
functioning, Giancola, Roth, and Parrott (2006), proposed to test a hypothesis that
executive function (EF) mediates the relation between difficult temperament (DT) and
included 310 men (n = 152) and women (n = 158) between 21 and 35 years old. There
were 286 Caucasians, 23 African Americans, and one Hispanic. Any reports of drug or
perspective, tests of EF were chosen that encompassed a wide variety of skills such as
attentional control, strategic goal planning, abstract reasoning, cognitive flexibility (set
shifting), hypothesis generation, inhibition, and the ability to organize and utilize
that were selected are generally accepted measures of functions that originate primarily
from the prefrontal cortex. The cognitive tests administered were the Porteus Maze Test,
Go/No-Go Task, Trails B of the Trail Making Test, Stroop Task, Conditional Associative
Learning Test, Tower of Hanoi, and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.
The 10 subscales were summed and a difficult temperament index was created. Physical
aggression was measured using the Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire (BPAQ). This
self-report has four subscales: Physical Aggression, Verbal Aggression, Anger, and
Hostility. For the purposes of this study, aggression was indexed by the scores on the
Giancola et al. (2006) used a linear, 3-step design to measure the mediation of EF
and the relation between DT and aggression. They used regression on: (a) the mediator
(EF) on the independent variable (DT); (b) the dependent variable (aggression) on DT;
and finally (c) aggression on both EF and DT. Mediation is noted as present if: (a) the
relations in equations (a) and (b) are significant; (b) EF is significantly related to
aggression equation (c); and (c) the influence of DT on aggression is reduced following
the inclusion of EF (Giancola, et al., 2006). These tests were performed for men and
women separately.
The design described above was found to be significant for men with: (a) EF onto
DT (B = -.15, p < .05); (b) aggression onto DT (B = -.17, p < .05); and (c) aggression
onto EF and DT (B for DT = -.12, p = ns; B for EF = .31, p < .001). Adding EF to the
equation reduced the relation between DT and aggression by 30%, making it non-
significant, and therefore indicating that EF mediated the relation between DT and
The relation between DT and aggression was not significant for women (B = -.09,
p = ns); therefore, mediation could not be tested. However, Giancola et al. (2006) did
note that the relation between EF and aggression was significant for women (B = .24, p <
.05).
The aggression aspect of this study might have had more valid and reliable
findings if additional measures of aggression had been utilized, like clinical interviews
and collateral reports of the participants’ history of violence. It is also thought that the
generalizability of the results might have been broadened by including individuals with a
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 70
et al., 2006).
can be mitigated, which in turn can decrease the act of aggression (Giancola et al., 2006).
ability can reduce low adaptability to change, distractibility, and impulsivity, and lead to
impulsivity will be highly correlated, and those with high aggression/impulsivity scores
would exhibit greater executive and memory deficits than healthy controls as well as
healthy male staff. All participants were Caucasian, between 18 and 50 years old, had no
current or lifetime Axis I diagnosis, and had been medication-free for at least 12 months.
The average length of hospital stay for the offenders was 5.5 years, and all had a history
The measures administered included two intelligence tests: the National Adult
Reading Test (NART) and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revised (WAIS-R).
Executive function tests included the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), Stroop
Controlled Oral Word Association test of Verbal Fluency (VF), Trail Making Test
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 71
(TMT), and the Cognitive Estimation task. Memory tasks included the logical memory
(LM) and visual reproduction (VR) tests from the Wechsler Memory Scale – Revised
scores of the perseverative error score on the WCST, the B-A trails score, and the
scores. A composite memory (temporal) function score was derived from the summed
scores of the Logical Memory – immediate and delayed, Visual Reproduction Memory –
Withdrawal (Dolan & Anderson, 2002). SHAPS psychopaths were differentiated from
composite impulsivity score was derived by the summed scores of the impulsivity
aggression score was derived from the aggression subscales of the Buss-Durkee Hostility
Inventory (BDHI), the Brown and Goodwin lifetime history of Aggression scale (BGA),
Using an ANOVA, Dolan and Anderson (2002) found that SHAPS psychopaths
SHAPS psychopaths also had significantly higher composite impulsivity scores than both
psychopaths were found to have lower mean WAIS-R full and verbal subscale scores
than both controls and non-psychopaths. The SHAPS psychopaths had lower mean
scores on the WAIS-R performance tests than the non-psychopaths, but did not differ
for faces (RMF), and composite memory score. There were fewer significant group
differences SHAPS psychopaths exhibited poorer performance on, compared with both
Anderson (2002) had all of the executive function and memory tests ran under a principal
components analysis (PCA). They identified four principal components which accounted
for 67.5% of the variance. Factor 1 was identified as frontal/concept formation and
and Recognition Memory for Faces. Factor 2 was labeled semantic/contextual memory
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 73
function and loaded Logical Memory – immediate and delayed recall. Factor 3 was
labeled attention/concentration and loaded Verbal Fluency, Trail Making Test, and
Stroop (SNST) test. Factor 4 was labeled unspecified memory and loaded Visual
performing worse than controls on Factors 1 and 2. Psychopaths also scored significantly
executive function and memory function, Dolan and Anderson (2002) found that SHAPS
psychopaths had significantly lower executive function scores than both the controls and
non-psychopaths, and remained significant after correcting for IQ. There were no
significant differences among the groups on the composite memory scores, even after
function and memory (r = 0.46, p < 0.001) function were also found to be highly
significant.
0.01), composite executive function score (r = -0.52, p < 0.01), and composite memory
function score (r = -0.34, p < 0.05). The composite aggression score also negatively
correlated with IQ (r = -0.21, p < 0.05), executive function (r = -0.46, p < 0.01), and
memory function (r = -0.29, p < 0.05). Essentially, the higher the impulsivity and
aggressive scores, the lower the IQ score, executive function score, and memory function
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 74
score. When adjusting for IQ, Dolan and Anderson (2002) determined that correlation
between composite impulsivity and aggression scores and memory function lost their
Seguin, Pihl, Harden, Tremblay, and Boulerice (1995) examined what types of
cognitive functions might differentiate boys with a history of physical aggression from
those without such a history. Participants were a community sample of White, French-
speaking boys from 53 schools with the lowest SES index of a Montreal School Board.
The boys were assessed annually from ages six to 12, as were their parents and teachers.
Physically aggressive behavior was assessed at ages six, ten, eleven, and 12, with the
Three items made up this scale: (1) fights with other children, (2) kicks, bites and hits
After removing those boys who either withdrew or had missing values on
aggression at follow up, Seguin et al. (1995) identified 893 boys who had stability and
severity of physical aggression. They defined aggression as stable physical fighting over
a seven-year period from ages six to 12. Nineteen percent of the sample was classified as
stable aggressive boys and fell above the 70th percentile at two or more assessment points
at age six. Nonaggressive boys fell below the 70th percentile at all assessment points, and
comprised 35% of the sample. Participants who did not meet criteria for either of the
prior two classifications were identified as unstable aggressive boys and made up 46% of
the sample.
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 75
criteria. After exclusion criteria were applied, a total of 203 boys agreed to laboratory
testing at age 13. Of those, 177 returned for further testing at age 14. The 177 boys who
completed both years of testing, 63 were classified in the stable aggressive group, 59 in
the nonaggressive group, and 55 in the unstable aggressive group. Seguin et al. (1995)
also compiled a familial adversity composite index from variables collected from the
mother of the boys when they were age six, including parental age at first child’s birth,
paradigm and covered a broad range of abilities, including verbal memory and executive
Ordered Pointing, Spatial Memory, Strategic Problem Solving, Subjective Ordering, and
Verbal Fluency.
Seguin et al. (1995) controlled for age, gender, race, cultural background,
education level, SES, and brain damage. They broke the analysis into two parts: part 1
was the initial study from ages six to 12 where they performed a factor analysis of the test
battery to summarize any pattern of correlations among the large number of variables and
isolate dimensions of cognitive processes; part 2 was the subsample of 13 and 14 year
olds where they used the factor-analyzed dimensions from part 1 in a MANCOVA to
identify which of the three levels of severity and stability of aggression the dimensions of
In the part 1 factor analysis, Seguin et al. (1995) identified twelve variables of
functioning from the nine psychological tests administered that incorporated executive
This resulted in four factors, which accounted for 58% of the variance. Factor 1
contained variables that reflect verbal learning. Factor 2 represented Spatial Incidental
Learning and includes variables that assess spatial memory tasks. Factor 3 represented
cerebral dominance in the form of Tactile Laterality, and Factor 4 included variables that
MANCOVA was performed on each of the four factors in part 2. Seguin et al. (1995)
found the multivariate effect of the family adversity and anxiety covariates to be
significant. In investigating the effect of the covariates on each of the factors with
multiple regressions, they were found to provide significant adjustment only to Factor 1,
verbal learning.
Seguin et al. (1995) entered Spatial Incidental Learning and Tactile Laterality in
the model prior to Verbal Learning in order test the hypothesis that deficits in Verbal
Learning were more associated with physical aggression than deficits in spatial learning
or cerebral dominance. They did the same with executive functioning and entered this
factor into the model last to test the hypothesis that deficits in executive functioning are
associated with physical aggression over and above all other cognitive-
neuropsychological abilities.
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 77
main effects were found on the executive functions factor only (p = .005). The stable
aggressive boys were found to perform more poorly on all factors except cerebral
dominance, or Tactile Laterality. When comparing the classification of boys within the
executive function factor, the comparison between the stable aggressive boys and
nonaggressive boys was significant (p < .0003), as was the unstable aggressive boys
In this multivariate design, executive functioning was the most strongly related to
physically aggressive behavior. When impaired, likely deficits may include an inability
abstractly. One may quickly become overwhelmed with these impairments and unable to
give a more socially appropriate response to a situation, become overwhelmed, and result
personality, and cognitive ability between youth and adult offenders and non-offenders.
They hypothesized that offenders would have lower social competency and ability to
plan, would have elevated scores on personality testing, and that cognition would be an
In this study, Bergeron and Valliant (2001) had 55 male participants, 26 between
the ages of 16 and 18, and 29 between the ages of 20 and 40 years. Thirteen of the youth
offender participants were gathered from a youth incarceration facility and the 13 non-
offenders were gained from a secondary school and had no prior arrests. The adult
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 78
and cognition. The methods employed to asses for executive functioning (EF) were the
planning and foresight, the Paragraph Completion Method (PCM) to obtain abstract
reasoning, thinking style and social maturity/competence, and the Wisconsin Card
(MMPI), Form 168, an abbreviated version of the test, the Coopersmith Self-Esteem
Inventory (CSEI), the Buss Durkee Hostility Inventory (BDHI) to measure aggression,
and the Carlson Psychological Survey (CPS) which measures attitudes towards crime.
Bergeron and Valliant (2001) also administered the Michigan Alcohol Screening Test
(MAST) and Drug Abuse Screening Test (DAST) to measure the degree of alcohol and
To measure cognition (C), Bergeron and Valliant (2001) employed the Test of
Non-verbal Intelligence Form A (TONI) to gauge abstract reasoning, and the Vocabulary
and Block Design subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised (WAIS-R)
which measures general verbal and performance abilities. These cognition measures
were used to gain a baseline of cognitive functioning and then statistically analyzed to
Valliant, 2001).
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 79
Each of the multi-scale tests had separate factor analyses ran in order to avoid
MMPI’s ten scales were reduced to three factors: Psychotic, Neurotic, and Androgyny.
The five scales of the CPS identified two factors as Criminality and Validity. The BDHI
identified two factors as well which were denoted as Ruminative Hostility and Overt
Performance, and Set Failure. Bergeron and Valliant (2001) then ran four stepwise linear
discriminant function analyses to determine the variables that could differentiate the
groups.
indicated statistical significance with a canonical correlation at .88, indicating that this
function explained 78% of the variance between groups. This indicated that the offenders
(Conceptual Level and Block Design), and higher on personality measures (MAST and
Psychotic Factor).
The second discriminant function compared adult offenders and adult non-
offenders, and also indicated statistical significance. The canonical correlation was .97,
indicating this function explained 94% of the variance between these two groups. This
function determined adult offenders would score lower than adult non-offenders on the
Androgyny/Social Introversion factor (P), the Test Performance factor (EF), and
Conceptual Level (EF); however would score higher on the Q-score (EF) and the MAST
(P).
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 80
The third discriminant function compared youth offenders and youth non-
offenders, and again indicated statistical significance with the canonical correlation of
.95, indicating this function explained 90% of the variance between these two groups.
This third function indicated young offenders would typically score higher on the
Psychotic factor (P), the Overt Hostility factor (P), the Q-score (EF), and the DAST (P).
Finally, the fourth discriminant function compared young and adult offenders.
This function indicated statistical significance with the canonical correlation of .74
indicating this function explained 54% of the variance between these two groups. This
function indicated adult offenders would score higher than young offenders on the MAST
(P), Psychotic factor (P), the Vocabulary subtest (C), and the Q-score (EF). Adult
age was masking any of the main effects that resulted and could be explained by a
developmental lag. They determined that the various groups could be differentiated by
variables in personality, executive functioning, and cognition, but could not identify any
Bergeron and Valliant (2001) found that offenders and non-offenders could be
differentiated on the basis of social maturity, measured by the Conceptual Level score of
the PCM (EF), which had the most significant results, accounting for 70% of the variance
between offenders and non-offenders. This score measures foresight, planning, social
interpret social situations and misinterpretation or inability to read these social cues may
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 81
cause frustration (Bergeron & Valliant, 2001). This frustration coupled with reduced
verbal abilities, and when accompanied by inappropriate attitudes toward crime and
behavior, often makes it difficult for these individuals to engage in the normative
behaviors of society.
Summary
in disadvantaged communities where gangs tend to form, is far reaching. The literature
psychosocial and biological variables. Some of the literature confirmed that the
(Beyers et al., 2003; Dupere et al., 2007; McMahon et al., 2009). Yet another study
determined that the delinquency trajectory one follows is ethnicity dependent and must be
If, according to Gibbs (2000), many of the gang members tire of gang life and age
out around 30, can it be hypothesized that most gang members will fall into Moffit’s
structure of the gangs and the needs they fulfill tend to draw those looking for somewhere
to fit in during adolescence, and once adulthood is realized, the gang no longer fills a
need either for fulfillment or for delinquency. There will remain those gang members
who persist into adulthood and maintain their gang status, and there may be even more
adolescence and is not completed until generally the mid-20s, suggesting a biological
(Beaver et al., 2009; Beaver et al., 2010). Additional literature highlighted the notion that
(Bergeron and Valliant, 2001; Dolan & Anderson, 2002; Giancola et al., 2006; Raine et
al., 2005; Seguin et al., 1995). There remains the possibility of aggression and
improving environmental influences (Craig et al., 2002; Hill et al., 1999; Lahey et al.,
1999).
from biological research. Holistic efforts should be made to examine the complex
problem as a whole, including psychosocial and biological factors. “What many often
perceive as merely a simplistic societal deviance from the mainstream on the part
CHAPTER III
Methods
Introduction
current and former gang members. The Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive
and the Wisconsin Card Sort, a test measuring abstract reasoning and ability to set shift
Research Design
examine any significant differences in executive functioning between current and former
gang members as measured by the self-report from The Brief Rating Inventory of
Executive Function (BRIEF-A), and the objective measure of the Wisconsin Card Sorting
Test (WCST).
Appropriateness of Design
Gangs are a complex phenomenon and multivariate in nature, and instances when
a single variable completely explains a phenomenon are rare. This study will examine
determine whether significant differences exist between the two identified samples. In
much of the research cited in the literature review, significant effects of executive
2001; Dolan & Anderson, 2002; Donnellan et al., 2000; Raine et al., 2005; Seguin et al.,
1995). MANOVAs are commonly accepted and utilized by researchers when measuring
This design was chosen in an attempt to follow a procedure most consistent with accepted
The research questions that will be examined in this study are as follows:
Population / Participants
Forty male adults, ranging from 25 to 50 years, are expected to participate in the
American descent, and of lower SES backgrounds. Participants will be recruited from
Los Angeles, California, provides alternatives to current and former gang involved
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 85
individuals. Services include counseling, education, tattoo removal, job training, and
placement.
Making the decision to leave a gang and its lifestyle is not a simple or immediate
decision. In fact, it is a long recovery process. Although individuals may not be active in
their gang, they may live in the same communities and interact daily with former gang
affiliates. For safety measures, it was decided to identify both current and former gang
members as those involved with Homeboy Industries. For purposes of this study, current
gang members will be operationalized as those individuals that have not been active in
their gang for less than one year. Former gang members will be operationalized as those
individuals that have not been active in their gang for more than two years.
Informed Consent
consent prior to participation (See Appendix A). Included in the informed consent will
Sampling Frame
Literature demonstrates that the frontal lobe does not finish developing until the 20s or
even 30s (Raine, 2002b). Most individuals remit antisocial behavior by early adulthood,
and those who persist delinquency into adulthood will perform poorer on cognitive tests
(Donnellan et al., 2000; Huesmann et al., 2009; Moffit, 1993). The population sample
will be limited to this age group in order to gather data within the framework of these two
Confidentiality
including date of birth, ethnic group/race, gender, SES, use of drugs, head injuries, and
as name, address, or social security number, will not be collected. The signature of the
participant will be required on the informed consent. The identifiable information will be
kept separate from the results to ensure confidentiality. The researcher will inform the
participants that the results of their measures will not be able to be linked back to them
informed that the data will be kept locked for three years, and will then be appropriately
destroyed. The researcher and researcher's direct supervisor are the only individuals
Geographic Location
Participants reside in the Southern California region, likely in lower SES areas.
self-report and informant report. The test was designed to measure a wide variety of
empirically derived clinical scales that measure various aspects of executive functioning
clinical scales form two broader indexes: Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) and
Metacognition Index (MI), which form the overall summary score, the Global Executive
Composite (GEC). The BRIEF-A also includes three validity scales (Negativity,
population that mirrored the U.S. population according to key demographics including
age, gender, ethnicity, education, and geographical regions. One thousand and fifty
participants for the self-report and 1,200 participants for the informant report were
recruited through Internet sampling and ranged in age from 18-90 years. The BRIEF-A
has been demonstrated to be reliable and valid. Internal consistency for the self-report
normative sample is strong (alpha range = .73-.90 for clinical scales; .93-.96 for BRI and
MI indexes, and the GEC), and equally strong for the Informant Report normative sample
(alpha range = .80-.93 for clinical scales; .95-.98 for BRI and MI indexes, and the GEC).
Internal consistency was also high for the mixed clinical/healthy sample (alpha range =
.80-.94 for clinical scales; .96-.98 for BRI and MI indexes, and the GEC), as well as the
Informant Report mixed clinical/healthy sample (alpha range = .85-.95 for clinical scales;
.96-.98 for BRI and MI indexes, and the GEC). Test-retest reliability ranged from .82-.93
over an average interval of 4.22 weeks for the Self- Report Form (n = 50) clinical scales,
and.93 for the BRI and MI and .94 for the GEC. Test-retest reliability for the Informant
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 88
Report Form (n = 44) clinical scales ranged from .91-.94 over an average interval of 4.21
weeks, and was .96 for the BRI, MI, and GEC. Correlations between Self-Report ratings
and Informant Report ratings on interrater agreement ranged from .44-.68 for the clinical
scales and from .61-.63 for the BRI and MI indexes and the GEC. With regards to
convergent validity, the Self and Informant Report Form of the BRIEF-A scales, indexes,
and GEC, demonstrated significant correlations with the Frontal Systems Behavior
Questionnaire (CFQ). Although the BRIEF-A was not used in any of the literature cited
for this study, this data concludes that the BRIEF-A is a structurally valid and reliable
instrument.
The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) is an objective measure that assesses
changes. The WCST provides objective measures for both overall ability as well as
maintain set and inefficient learning across several stages of the test). Clinically, the test
The WCST was standardized on individuals aged six and a half to 89 years.
Eight hundred and ninety-nine subjects were aggregated from six distinct samples: 453
normal children and adolescents in public schools in a large urban area of southeastern
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 89
United States; 49 18 year olds and their friends in an urban area of the southwestern
United States; 150 individuals from Texas and Colorado, between 15 and 77 years; 50
subjects from Colorado between 58 and 84 years; 124 commercial airline pilots (119
from Colorado, five from Washington, D.C.), and 73 adults from independent living
years.
The WCST has been demonstrated to be both reliable and valid. In studies with
adults, interscorer reliability had intraclass correlation coefficients (rICC) that ranged from
coefficients (rICC) that ranged from .91 to .96. In studies with children, interscorer
reliability had intraclass correlation coefficients (rICC) that ranged from .895 to 1.000 for
reliability had intraclass correlation coefficients (rICC) that ranged from .828 to 1.000.
The WCST has four stimulus cards and 128 response cards, or 64 response cards
in the computerized version. Participants are asked to match the response card presented
to them to one of the stimulus cards above them. They have the option to match
according to color (red, blue, green, or yellow), form (cross, circle, triangle, or star), or
number of figures (one, two, three, or four). The test will change the matching category
without notice and it is up to the respondent to follow and determine what is being asked
to match.
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 90
The WCST allows the clinician to assess the following frontal lobe executive
shift cognitive sets, goal oriented behavior, and modulating impulsive reactions. The
WCST is one of the leading executive functioning tests to measure abstract ability and
flexibility, and was used in the following studies cited in the literature review: Bergeron
and Valliant, 2001; Dolan and Anderson, 2002; Giancola et al., 2006; Raine et al., 2005.
For purposes of this study, the computerized version of this test will be administered
(Heaton, 1993).
Demographic Questionnaire
characteristics. Information collected will include date of birth, ethnic group/race, gender,
SES, drug use, head injuries, and current prescription of psychotropic medication(s) (see
Appendix C). The questionnaire will be utilized to describe the sample. Furthermore, the
Data Collection
Upon arrival to the designated testing area, which will be in a quiet, climate
controlled setting and conducive to completing the measures, all participants will be
asked to complete an informed consent prior to data collection, which will include their
rights and responsibilities in the study. Participants will be informed that they are being
asked to take part in a study regarding certain planning, organizing, and goal oriented
processes. The researcher will provide the participants with an opportunity to ask
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 91
questions. All questions will be answered to the best of the researcher's knowledge.
Once all questions are adequately addressed, the researcher will comprehensively review
the informed consent form required to enact the study, and place in a manila envelope
once signed. Each participant will then be given the demographic questionnaire to
will be kept in a separate manila envelope and the BRIEF-A will be administered. The
BRIEF-A protocol will be placed in a third manila envelope separate from the informed
consent and demographic questionnaires. The WCST will then be administered via
laptop, furnished by the researcher in an online version of this measure. Once all
participants complete the measures, they will be debriefed on the purpose of the study
and any last questions will be addressed. It is estimated that the administration period
will take approximately five weeks, administering the measures to an average of eight
Data Analysis
All data will be analyzed using PASW statistics software version 18.0. The
This study will examine two dependent variables, the BRIEF-A and WCST, and one
independent variable with two levels, current and former gang members. Dependent
sensitivity to outliers. Wilks’ Lambda will compare the mean of the BRIEF-A and the
WCST, and a resulting F statistic will be examined for main effects. Significance level
will be set at p=.05. If significant main effects are found, post hoc tests will be ran to
Summary
This research study will examine data collected via a self-report of The Brief
the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), by current and former gang members. Using a
MANOVA, the researcher will examine any differences in executive functioning between
the two groups. A MANOVA will enable the researcher to examine multiple dependent
current and former gang members. Following sections will discuss findings and provide
CHAPTER IV
Results
members affiliated with Homeboy Industries in East Los Angeles, California. For
purposes of this study, current gang members were defined as those that have not been
active in a gang for less than one year. Former gang members were defined as those
individuals that have not been active in a gang for more than two years. Ages ranged
from 24 to 53 years old (M=36). Furthermore, 70% of participants were Hispanic, 25%
African American, 2.5% Asian, and 2.5% Other (more detailed demographic information
The present study compared executive functioning between current and former
gang members. The goal was to examine whether any differences in executive
functioning might help explain a person’s decision or ability to exit the gang lifestyle.
functioning between current and former gang members. It was hypothesized former gang
members would exhibit greater executive functioning abilities. The self-reported Global
the number of categories completed on the WCST (M=3.7, SD=2.312) (see Appendix A,
Table 1). This hypothesis was not supported by the data. The former gang members did
not demonstrate greater abilities in executive functioning than current gang members. A
one-way MANOVA revealed an insignificant multivariate main effect for the current
versus former gang members, Wilks’ Lambda = .999, F (2, 37) = .017, p > .05, partial eta
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 94
squared = .001. Power to detect the effect was .052 (see Appendix A, Table 2). Thus,
the hypothesis was not confirmed. This suggests that former gang members do not have
subjects effects was performed to examine the BRIEF-A and WCST independently to
determine if one better measures executive functioning differences among current and
former gang members. The main effect of the between subjects variables of the BRIEF-
A and the WCST was not significant using a critical p of .05, the GEC on the BRIEF-A
with F (1, 1.225) = .011, p > .05, partial eta squared = .000. Power to detect the effect
was .051. The number of categories completed on the WCST had F (1, .100) = .018, p >
.05, partial eta squared = .000. The power to detect the effect was .052 (see Appendix A,
Table 3). This suggests neither the BRIEF-A, or the WCST, demonstrate significant
each clinical scale of the BRIEF-A (Inhibit, Shift, Emotional Control, Self-Monitor,
as well as the Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI), Metacognition Index (MI), and Global
Executive Composite (GEC), to compare current and former gang members with the
population normed on the BRIEF-A. This was done to identify any one area of executive
functioning the gang populations may perform better or worse in than the general
population. There was a significant difference in eight out of the nine factors of the
BRIEF-A, as well as the BRI, MI, and GEC scores, that suggests the two populations
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 95
performed poorer than the general population (see Appendix B, Table 4). There was one
scale, Organization of Materials, in which the two groups performed better than the
between current and former gang members and the normed population t(39) = 3.826, p =
.000. The Inhibit scale measures a respondent’s ability to inhibit or resist an impulse and
to manage behavior at the appropriate time. This difficulty may be seen in adults with
traumatic brain injuries. Individuals with poor impulse control may display inappropriate
physical responses to others and have a general failure to think before acting (Roth,
Isquith, & Gioia, 2005). The results of the exploratory analysis suggest current and
former gang members have more difficulty than the general population in this executive
function.
between current and former gang members and the normed population t(39) = 4.189, p =
.000. The Shift scale measures one’s ability to move from one situation, activity, or
aspect of a problem to another with relative ease. Aspects of this function include
environment. Individuals with difficulty in this domain may respond to situations with
anger or anxiety. Deficits may range from efficiency of problem solving to perseverative
exploratory analysis suggest current and former gang members have more difficulty
SD=9.05) between current and former gang members and the normed population t(39) =
modulate emotional responses. Those with difficulty in this area may demonstrate
with mood lability, or one who goes from zero to 100 in 60 seconds. Results of the
exploratory analysis suggest current and former gang members have more difficulty
SD=10.42) between current and former gang members and the normed population t(39) =
2.746, p = .009. The Self-Monitor scale measures one’s ability to follow their own
behavior and maintain awareness of the effect of his or her behavior on others. In
interactions, this may manifest in one not understanding why another individual may be
upset with them. Results of the exploratory analysis suggest current and former gang
members have more difficulty than the general population in their awareness of the effect
(M=56.025, SD=10.56) between current and former gang members and the normed
appropriately regulate his or her behavior and emotional responses. It is composed of the
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 97
Inhibit, Shift, Emotional Control, and Self-Monitor scales. One’s ability to manage
problem solving (Roth, Isquith, & Gioia, 2005). Results of the exploratory analysis
suggest current and former gang members have more difficulty than the general
population in their overall ability to regulate emotions and transition between changes in
the environment.
between current and former gang members and the normed population t(39) = 2.266, p =
.029. The Initiate scale measures one’s ability to begin activities and generate ideas or
poor motivation; rather they may want to succeed at something but find it difficult to start
and may require prompting. This is often a significant difficulty for those with severe
frontal lobe brain injury (Roth, Isquith, & Gioia, 2005). Difficulty with initiation may
organization. Results of the exploratory analysis suggest current and former gang
members have more difficulty than the general population in initiating tasks.
SD=12.48) between current and former gang members and the normed population t(39) =
2.673, p = .011. The Working Memory scale measures one’s ability to hold information
with impaired working memory have difficulty remembering things even for a short time,
lose track of what they are doing, or forget altogether what they were instructed to do.
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 98
An integral part of working memory is the ability to maintain attention and performance
over time (Roth, Isquith, & Gioia, 2005). Difficulties in this area involve maintaining an
suggest current and former gang members have more difficulty than the general
SD=10.86) between current and former gang members and the normed population t(39) =
component involves the ability to anticipate future events, implement goals, and develop
steps to achieve said goals. The organize component involves the ability to have a level
of order to the actions or information to achieve goals. The way information or tasks are
organized can impact how the information or actions are learned and implemented.
Adults with organizational problems may approach tasks haphazardly or become easily
Difficulty with planning and organizing plays an integral role in many cases of executive
functioning impairment (Roth, Isquith, & Gioia, 2005). Results of the exploratory
analysis suggest current and former gang members have more difficulty than the general
SD=10.747) between current and former gang members and the normed population t(39)
= 3.457, p = .001. The Task Monitor scale measures a person’s awareness of their own
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 99
successes and failures in problem solving. People with difficulty in this area fail to
appreciate their errors when approaching problems or tasks, or may catch errors at the
last minute. Results of the exploratory analysis suggest current and former gang
members have more difficulty than the general population in their awareness of their
(M=45.50, SD=7.296) between current and former gang members and the normed
population t(39) = -3.901, p = .000; however, current and former gang members
performed significantly better than the population norm on this measure of executive
functioning. The Organization of Materials scale measures one’s ability to keep order in
everyday life with respect to orderliness of work and living spaces. This scale relates to
the ways in which a person organizes his/her world. Adults that have difficulty in this
area may demonstrate poor efficiency at home or work because they often misplace
things and do not have them readily available for use. Results of the exploratory analysis
suggest current and former gang members perform better than the general population in
SD=9.608) between current and former gang members and the normed population t(39) =
2.172, p = .036. The MI represents the respondent’s ability to use planning and
organization to solve problems while keeping these efforts in active working memory. It
Organization of Materials scales. The results of the exploratory analysis suggest current
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 100
and former gang members have more difficulty than the general population in cognitively
SD=10.20) between current and former gang members and the normed population t(39) =
2.96, p = .005. The Global Executive Composite (GEC) is a summary score that
incorporates all of the clinical scales of the BRIEF-A. Overall, the results of the
exploratory analysis suggest current and former gang members have more difficulty than
Results of this study concluded that current gang members, identified as those
who have not been active in a gang for less than one year, and former gang members,
identified as individuals who have not been active in a gang for more than two years, do
report measure of the BRIEF-A, it was suggested that the combined current and former
gang population performed worse than the general population on various elements of
CHAPTER V
Discussion
Research has measured and suggested factors that contribute to or predict youth
joining gangs, as well as various differences between gang members and non-gang
members (Craig, Vitaro, Gagnon, & Tremblay, 2002; Hill, Howell, Hawkins, & Battin-
Maxmen, Ward, & Kilgus, 2009). As Lahey et al. (1999) suggested, it is possible that
some of these differences may arise after a youth has entered a gang, and may not be a
remains whether gang members join gangs to attain social status and/or protection in their
Factors that lead to gang involvement are important in being able to take
proactive measures to provide early interventions for youth; however, factors that lead to
gang desistance in adulthood are equally important. This research analyzed whether
there may be scientific factors that contribute to adults leaving a gang, specifically
executive functioning and one’s ability to reason, plan, and make decisions. The decision
to leave a gang, as does any life changing event, requires reasoning, planning, and
execution.
The frontal lobe, or executive functioning area of the brain, controls emotions,
urges, and knowledge of right and wrong; it is the impulse control center, and imposes
appropriate inhibitions. It is also the part of the brain that organizes information, enables
planning, and completion of goals. This research did not provide any significant results
indicating former gang members have better executive functioning skills than current
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 102
gang members. As the data suggested, the two groups performed relatively similar. It is
important to explore other factors that may contribute to adults exiting a gang.
As Gibbs (2000) suggested, many gang members tire of the gang lifestyle, and
age out at approximately 30 years of age. However, the explanation cannot be that
simple. What does “age out” mean? Future research may explore possible scientific
explanations for how individuals involved in gangs ultimately reach the point of desisting
from the lifestyle. Young men who join gangs generally come from a lower
oftentimes, chaos. This has psychosocial implications, but also biological implications.
They become attuned to surrounding stimuli. As van Goozen, Fairchild, and Harold
(2008) suggested, stressful events that occur early in life play an important role in
“programming” the stress systems. This may result in the adapting and down-regulating
of the stress-response system, and may include avoidance of chronic arousal and energy
this lowered stress-response system, van Goozen, Fairchild, and Harold (2008) posited
that antisocial individuals are less likely to avoid stressful situations and are more fearless
and less concerned about possible negative consequences. When few prosocial
alternatives are available to mitigate this change in the response system, immunity to
stressful situations becomes the method of interacting with the world. This ingrained
style of engagement and interaction becomes more difficult to change, and executive
performed more poorly than the general population in eight areas of executive
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 103
control; monitor behavior; regulate behavior; ability to initiate ideas or activities; hold
information in their working memory; planning; and problem solving. This supports
research that suggests aggression and delinquent behavior are affected by impairments in
executive functions (Bergeron & Valliant, 2001; Dolan & Anderson, 2002; Giancola et
al., 2006; Raine et al., 2005; Seguin et al., 1995). Other literature found a connection
between self-control, aggression, and executive functioning (Beaver et al., 2009; Beaver
et al., 2010).
Literature demonstrates that the frontal lobe does not finish developing until the
twenties or even thirties (Raine, 2002b). Most individuals remit antisocial behavior by
early adulthood, and those who persist delinquency into adulthood will perform poorer on
cognitive tests (Donnellan et al., 2000; Huesmann et al., 2009; Moffit, 1993). ). As the
analysis further demonstrated, the current and former gang members continue to perform
more poorly than the general populations into their 30s, 40s, and 50s. The fact that the
two groups are seeking assistance in leaving the gang lifestyle supports the idea that
et al., 2006), or improving environmental influences (Craig et al., 2002; Hill et al., 1999;
Lahey et al., 1999). It further supports possibilities of early interventions from a holistic,
empirically associated with antisocial behavior are verbal and executive functions.
Verbal deficits may include difficulties with receptive listening and reading, problem
solving, expressive speech and writing, and memory. Executive functioning deficits
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 104
include problems with attention and impulsivity. Piquero (2001) also determined that
simultaneously: the emergence of language and the explosive rise of executive functions,
or more specifically, the frontal lobes. Goldberg (2009) posited their simultaneous
emergence serves a purpose. Language has a generative power to build new constructs;
however, this ability may depend upon the frontal lobes to make use of and manipulate
the language constructs into more complex operations. Young men who become
capacities. As was questioned in Goldberg’s (2009) study, the question remains whether
poor verbal ability can be attributed to poor cognitive functioning, or the result of a poor
education that did not allow the subjects the opportunity to build verbal ability that is
afforded others.
There were two sources of continuity Moffitt (1993) suggested are reasons why
prosocial alternatives to behavior. Moffitt suggested that once the deviant behavior
begins, a lack of recourse to more prosocial alternatives keeps them on the course toward
development due to rejection from peers and adults. Second is becoming entangled in a
deviant lifestyle due to crime’s consequences. Poor self-control and impulsivity increase
the risk that antisocial youth will make irreversible decisions that close the doors of
opportunity. Once a male is immersed in the gang lifestyle, little research has examined
what factors may contribute to their decision to leave. Research abounds with
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 105
taxonomies and theories that separate those involved in delinquent behavior, such as
gangs, and desist in their childhood, or continue a path of delinquent behavior for the
duration of their life course (Huesmann, Dubow, & Boxer, 2009; Moffitt, 1993; Raskin,
after several decades of antisocial behavior. The participants in this research have
demonstrated they are making strides in desisting from a gang lifestyle and discovering a
more prosocial way of living. Future research might explore scientific or biogenic factors
that may contribute to the desistance of delinquent behavior, specifically gangs. It might
organize, and execute a desire to desist from gangs, and regulate emotions and impulses
when this life has allowed them, and at times required them, to interact in maladaptive
ways, and how these processes might differentiate them from those unable to or unwilling
to leave.
Limitations
should be examined in light of the following limitations, which may assist in directing
future research.
Internal Validity
Internal validity is the confidence placed on the cause and effect relationship in a
that may have correlated (positively or negatively) with both the independent and
dependent variable. The sample size included two groups at Homeboy Industries: 20
current gang members, defined as those who have not been involved in a gang for less
than one year, and 20 former gang members, identified as those individuals not active in
The limitation lied in being able to safely gauge current gang membership;
therefore, individuals who have had the shortest time seeking assistance to desist from the
gang lifestyle, were identified as the current group. This may have influenced the results
of this study and may not be an accurate reflection of executive functioning in individuals
currently and actively involved in gangs. Their involvement in the organization to seek
help in desisting from gang involvement may put them more closely related to the former
gang member group. It would be advantageous to have a true current group in future
studies.
Another confounding factor is that the level of executive functioning prior to gang
involvement was not able to be obtained. It is not known if there were any impairment in
impairment.
participants. Several participants indicated they have had multiple head injuries resulting
from gunshot wounds, fights, and car accidents. Any effect of these head injuries and
loss of consciousness were not accounted for. In addition, 100% of the participants
indicated they abused substances throughout their life. It may have been helpful to
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 107
inquire about the duration, extent, and pattern of drug use, specifically whether they used
substances as a social function or when they were sad or excited, as well as their method
of ingesting the substances. This may have impacted frontal lobe capacities and should
External Validity
External validity regards the general (causal) inferences in scientific studies. The
relatively small sample size (N = 40) obtained from a single non-profit organization and
geographical location with idiosyncratic features, may have limited the ability to
relationships found in this study, apply to all gang members, or other geographical
References
Anderson, E. (1999). Code of the Street: Decency, violence, and the moral life of the
inner city. New York: Norton.
Craig, W., Vitaro, F., Gagnon, C., & Tremblay, R. (2002). The road to gang membership:
Characteristics of male gang and nongang members from ages 10 to 14. Social
Development, 11(1), 53-68.
Beaver, K., DeLisi, M., Vaughn, M., & Wright, J. (2010). The intersection of genes and
neuropsychological deficits in the prediction of adolescent delinquency and low
self-control. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative
Criminology, 54(1), 22-42.
Beaver, K., Shutt, J., Boutwell, B., Ratchford, M., Roberts, K., & Barnes, J. (2009).
Genetic and environmental influences on levels of self-control and delinquent
peer affiliation: Results from a longitudinal sample of adolescent twins. Criminal
Justice and Behavior, 36(1), 41-60.
Beaver, K., Wright, J., & Delisi, M. (2007). Self-control as an executive function:
Reformulating Gottfredson and Hirschi's parental socialization thesis. Criminal
Justice and Behavior, 34(10), 1345-1361.
Bergeron, T., & Valliant, P. (2001). Executive function and personality in adolescent and
adult offenders vs. non-offenders. Journal of Offender Rehabilitation, 33(3), 27-
45.
Beyers, J., Bates, J., Pettit, G., & Dodge, K. (2003). Neighborhood structure, parenting
processes, and the development of youths' externalizing behaviors: A multilevel
analysis. American Journal of Community Psychology, 31(1-2), 35-53.
Blair, R. (2007). Aggression, psychopathy and free will from a cognitive neuroscience
perspective. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 25(2), 321-331.
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 109
Caspi, A., Elder, G. H., & Bem, D. J. (1987). Moving against the world: Life-course
patterns of explosive children. Developmental Psychology, 23(2), 308-313.
Dolan, M., & Anderson, I. (2002). Executive and memory function and its relationship to
trait impulsivity and aggression in personality disordered offenders. Journal of
Forensic Psychiatry, 13(3), 503-526.
Donnellan, M., Ge, X., & Wenk, E. (2000). Cognitive abilities in adolescent-limited and
life-course-persistent criminal offenders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
109(3), 396-402.
Dupéré, V., Lacourse, É., Willms, J., Vitaro, F., & Tremblay, R. (2007). Affiliation to
youth gangs during adolescence: The interaction between childhood psychopathic
tendencies and neighborhood disadvantage. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology: An official publication of the International Society for Research in
Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 35(6), 1035-1045.
Giancola, P., Roth, R., & Parrott, D. (2006). The mediating role of executive functioning
in the relation between difficult temperament and physical aggression. Journal of
Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 28(4), 211-221.
Goldberg, E. (2009). The new executive brain: Frontal lobes in a complex world. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Heaton, R., Chelune, G., Talley, J., Kay, G., Curtiss, G. (1993). Wisconsin Card Sorting
Test Manual, Revised and Expanded. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment
Resources, Inc.
Hill, K., Howell, J., Hawkins, J., & Battin-Pearson, S. (1999). Childhood risk factors for
adolescent gang membership: Results from the Seattle Social Development
Project. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 36(3), 300-322.
Huesmann, L., Dubow, E., & Boxer, P. (2009). Continuity of aggression from childhood
to early adulthood as a predictor of life outcomes: Implications for the adolescent-
limited and life-course-persistent models. Aggressive Behavior, 35(2), 136-149.
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 110
Kupersmidt, J., Coie, J., & Howell, J. (2004). Resilience in children exposed to negative
peer influences. Investing in children, youth, families, and communities:
Strengths-based research and policy (pp. 251-268). American Psychological
Association.
Lahey, B., Gordon, R., Loeber, R., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., & Farrington, D. (1999).
Boys who join gangs: A prospective study of predictors of first gang entry.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology: An official publication of the
International Society for Research in Child and Adolescent Psychopathology,
27(4), 261-276.
Maxmen, J., Ward, N., Kilgus, M. (2009). Essential Psychopathology and Its Treatment.
W.W. Norton Company & Inc.
McMahon, S., Felix, E., Halpert, J., & Petropoulos, L. (2009). Community violence
exposure and aggression among urban adolescents: Testing a cognitive mediator
model. Journal of Community Psychology, 37(7), 895-910.
National Gang Intelligence Center. (2009). National Gang Threat Assessment 2009
(NGIC Product No. 2009-M0335-001). Retrieved from http://www.fbi.gov/stats-
services/publications/national-gang-threat-assessment-2009-pdf
Pratt, T., Turner, M., & Piquero, A. (2004). Parental socialization and community
context: A longitudinal analysis of the structural sources of low self-control.
Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 41(3), 219-243.
Raine, A. (2002a). Biosocial studies of antisocial and violent behavior in children and
adults: A review. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30(4), 311-326.
Raine, A. (2002b). Annotation: The role of prefrontal deficits, low autonomic arousal and
early health factors in the development of antisocial and aggressive behavior in
children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(4), 417-434.
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 111
Raine, A., Moffitt, T., Caspi, A., Loeber, R., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., & Lynam, D.
(2005). Neurocognitive Impairments in Boys on the Life-Course Persistent
Antisocial Path. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(1), 38-49.
Raskin White, H., Bates, M., Buyske, S. (2001). Adolescence-limited versus persistent
delinquency: Extending Moffitt’s hypothesis into adulthood. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 110(4), 600-609.
Ratchford, M., & Beaver, K. (2009). Neuropsychological deficits, low self-control, and
delinquent involvement: Toward a biosocial explanation of delinquency. Criminal
Justice and Behavior, 36(2), 147-162.
Roth, R., Isquith, P., Gioia, G. (2005). Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function
– Adult Version Professional Manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment
Resources, Inc.
Séguin, J., Pihl, R., Harden, P., Tremblay, R., & Boulerice, B. (1995). Cognitive and
neuropsychological characteristics of physically aggressive boys. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 104(4), 614-624.
Tuvblad, C., Grann, M., & Lichtenstein, P. (2006). Heritability for adolescent antisocial
behavior differs with socioeconomic status: gene–environment interaction.
Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 47(7), 734-743.
Valdez, A., Kaplan C., Codina E. (2000). Psychopathy among Mexican American gang
members: A comparative study. International Journal of Offender Therapy and
Comparative Criminology, 44(1), 46-58. Retrieved from E-Journals database.
van Goozen, S., Fairchild, G., & Harold, G. (2008). The role of neurobiological deficits
in childhood antisocial behavior. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
17(3), 224-228.
Vigil, J. (2003). Urban violence and street gangs. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32,
225-242.
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 112
Appendix A
Table 1
MANOVA Descriptive Statistics
Table 2
Multivariate Tests
Table 3
Tests of Between-Subject Effects
Appendix B
Table 4
One-Sample Statistics
Table 5
One-Sample Test
Test Value = 50
95% Confidence Interval
of the Difference
t df Sig. (2-tailed) Mean Lower Upper
Difference
Inhibit 3.826 39 .000 6.47500 3.0519 9.8981
Shift 4.189 39 .000 6.62500 3.4263 9.8237
Emotional Control 2.290 39 .028 3.27500 .3817 6.1683
Self_Monitor 2.746 39 .009 4.52500 1.1919 7.8581
BRI 3.608 39 .001 6.02500 2.6478 9.4022
Initiate 2.266 39 .029 3.17500 .3413 6.0087
Working Memory 2.673 39 .011 5.27500 1.2838 9.2662
Plan_Organize 3.247 39 .002 5.57500 2.1018 9.0482
Task Monitor 3.457 39 .001 5.87500 2.4380 9.3120
Organization of Mat. -3.901 39 .000 -4.50000 -6.8334 -2.1666
MI 2.172 39 .036 3.30000 .2271 6.3729
GEC 2.960 39 .005 4.77500 1.5123 8.0377
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 114
Appendix C
Demographic Questionnaire
4. Have you ever been knocked unconscious? For example, have you been in a fight
or car accident and you lost consciousness for an extended period of time.
YES ____ NO ____
If yes, how many times? ______
5. Have you ever taken prescribed medication by a doctor for mental health
reasons?
YES ____ NO ____
If yes, what? ___________________________________________________________
When was the last time? ___________________________
6. Have you ever taken the following drugs: Please check all that apply.
___ Methamphetamine (Crank, Beannies, Black Beauty, Chrome, Cristina, Bling Bling)
___ Hallucinogens (LSD or PCP; Acid, Mellow Yellow, Cactus, Mexican Mushrooms)
___ Ecstacy (E, X, Adam, Roll)
___ Marijuana (Weed, Mary Jane)
___ Inhalants (Air blast, Huffing, Moon gas, Air Fresher)
___ Cocaine (Crack, Blow, C, Marching Powder, Nose Candy)
___ Heroin (Big H, China White, Mexican Brown, Smack, Witch Hazel, White Horse)
___ Alcohol
___ Prescription Drugs
___ Other (Please list)_____________________________________________________
When was the last time you used any of these substances?
_______________________________________________________________________
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 115
Appendix D
Demographic Table
Variable N %
Age
24 4 10
25 1 2.5
26 2 5
27 3 7.5
28 1 2.5
29 2 5
30 1 2.5
31 1 2.5
32 0 0
33 0 0
34 1 2.5
35 1 2.5
36 4 10
37 4 10
38 4 10
39 1 2.5
40 0 0
41 0 0
42 1 2.5
43 1 2.5
44 0 0
45 0 0
46 0 0
47 1 2.5
48 1 2.5
49 3 7.5
50 1 2.5
51 0 0
52 0 0
53 1 2.5
54 0 0
55 0 0
56 0 0
57 1 2.5
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 116
Variable N %
Ethnicity
African American 10 25
Hispanic 28 70
Asian 1 2.5
Other 1 2.5
Knocked Unconscious
Yes 22 55
No 18 45
Substance Abuse
Yes 40 100
No 0 0
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 117
Appendix E
Informed Consent
You are being asked to participate in a research study. However, before you give your consent to be a
volunteer, we want you to read the following and ask as many questions as necessary to be sure that you
understand what your participation will involve.
INVESTIGATOR:
Heather Farris
Noor Damavandi, Ph.D., Supervisor
PURPOSE OF THE RESEARCH: You are invited to participate in the research project entitled,
Differences in Executive Functioning between Current and Former Gang Members, which is being
conducted at Alliant International University and the California School of Forensic Studies, under the
direction of Sean Sterling, Ph.D. The purpose of this study is to examine any changes that take place in the
brain, such as the ability to reason or make plans that might offer an explanation for deciding to leave or
remain in a gang.
PROCEDURES TO BE FOLLOWED DURING THE RESEARCH: This research will take place in the
offices of Homeboy Industries. You will be asked to complete a demographic questionnaire and a
questionnaire in which you will evaluate your views on your own reasoning and problem-solving. You will
review a list of statements that describes people’s behavior and indicate whether you have had any
problems with these behaviors over the past month. Examples of statements in the self-report measure are
“I have problems getting started on tasks” and “I have emotional outbursts for little reason”. The last
measure you will be asked to complete is an online test that will evaluate your ability to plan and recognize
changes in the environment. You will be shown decks of cards and asked to match a card with one shown
on the screen. Your participation will take approximately 1 hour.
RISKS: There is minimal risk to participation in this study. Answering the questions in the self-report as
well as the measure you will be tested on might cause you to feel upset or anxious. If so, you may stop at
any time.
BENEFITS OF THE RESEARCH: An incentive of $20 will be provided for your participation. You must
complete this entire study in order to receive the incentive. Others may benefit by learning about the results
of this research.
ALTERNATIVES TO THIS RESEARCH: You have the alternative not to participate in this research.
DIFFERENCES IN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING 118
CONFIDENTIALITY: You have a right to privacy and all information identifying you will remain
confidential (private), unless otherwise required by law. Confidentiality will be maintained by keeping the
consent forms with signatures and demographic questionnaires separate from self-reports and test data,
which will not include names or any other identifiable information and will be presented to others only
when combined with other responses. The researcher will follow the American Psychological Association
Ethical Standards including those for Research with Human Subjects. The results of this study may be
published in scientific journals, or be presented at professional meetings as long as you are not identified
and cannot be identified from it. However, it is possible that under certain circumstances data could be
subpoenaed by court order.
QUESTIONS ABOUT THE RESEARCH: If you have questions regarding this research project or your
participation, you may call Heather Farris at 714-318-2975. You may also contact Dr. Noor Damavandi at
949-812-7478. Should you have any additional concerns, please contact the Institutional Review Board at
Alliant International University at (858) 635-4448 during normal working hours.
We have tried to explain all the important details about the study to you. If you have any questions that are
not answered here, the investigator will be happy to give you more information.