Virtual Reality and Education: By: Giti Javidi Submitted To: Dr. James White EME7938
Virtual Reality and Education: By: Giti Javidi Submitted To: Dr. James White EME7938
EME7938
Introduction
Background
One of the challenges in working with instructional media is that developers and
technology’s capabilities. The business and entertainment sectors are driving a fast-
paced evolution of the devices people have in their workplaces and homes. Researchers
and educators are scrambling to assess the potential, develop pedagogical strategies,
create instructional materials, and implement a school- based infrastructure for today’s
technologies— only to find that computers and communications are “morphing” into new
media of even greater power. Students of today experience a society and a workplace
that is entirely different from those that their parents faced. To allow educational tools to
fall behind the pace of technological advance is to sell out a generation of learners.
size and cost of these devices has decreased while their power has grown. First,
computers were seen as number- crunching machines, then came data processing, now
we live in the age of tools that manipulate symbols and information. Virtual Reality (VR)
research is based on the growing certainty that the next evolutionary stage is computers
channel within which content flows, but a virtual place to live that competes directly with
reality for the attention of many, especially new generation of students. For this reason,
charting the strengths and limits of virtual reality is vital for educational technology.
1
Educating children now and in the future to live in an information society is
critical. There is also a need to provide life-long education for all citizens and to support
atomic or planetary scales, and to visit environments and interact with events that
distance, time, or safety factor make unavailable. The types of activities supported by
this technology promote current educational thinking that students are better able to
master, retain, and generalize new knowledge when they are actively involved in
medium for enhancing certain skills. For example, effectively coordinating sensory-
motor skills; gaining situation awareness through use of simulations; and training in
design skills. The commercial success of virtual environments in pilot training has led to
speculations about the application (Krueger, 1991) of virtual environments to other areas
of education, such as in virtual science laboratories. This kind of approach could give
focused on skill acquisition, i. e. the development of coordinated sensory motor skills and
2
Designers and evaluators of immersive VR systems have many ideas concerning
how VR can facilitate learning (Dede, 1996b), but there is little information concerning
which of the VR's features best enhance understanding or how to customize those
affordances for different learning environments. Other factors such as the concepts or
skills to be learned, individual characteristics, the learning experience, and the interaction
experience all play a role in shaping the learning process and learning outcome.
construction are described and then the nature of the confluence of VR and constructivist
learning theory or the "goodness of fit" between the two is examined. A discussion on
whether constructivism is the best basis for building a theory of learning in virtual
environments is included. Finally, the effect of learner characteristics (e.g. learner age,
Approach
The literature search is designed to answer some of the most obvious but critical
traditional methodologies?
instructional practices?
3
• What is the impact of learner characteristics (e.g. age, gender, race, or
VR?
The term "Virtual Reality" has been applied more widely to include graphics
applications that allow users to walk through a simulated environment and, possibly, to
interact with objects in it. Therefore, this paper is concerned only with immersive VR.
In addition, this paper will include both educational uses of VR technology and
training applications, but not with those instances where VR technology itself is being
taught, but rather where VR technology is being used as the learning medium.
4
Literature Review
The early stages of development of any new technology or theory are often
confounded by controversy over what that technology actually involves. Any definition
VR capabilities. The technology's undeveloped state seems to be the only barrier to the
achievement of the science fiction fantasies that are provided by the current press. A
environmental simulation controlled only in part by the user (Spring, 1991). VR requires
hardware and software that furnish a sense of (1) immersion, (2) navigation, and (3)
manipulation (Helsel, 1992). VR falls into three major categories: text-based, desktop and
textually on the Internet where people interact by typing commands and "speak" by
typing messages on their computer keyboards. This has been valuable in distance
without being considered immersive. Immersive VR, the focus of this paper, involves a
5
mixture of hardware, software and concepts that allow the user to interact with a three
(2) Datagloves which allow the user to interact with the environment by
tracking the users motion and giving tactile reinforcement to the visual
(3) Wands or other devices which allow the user to manipulate objects in the
virtual world.
The major software required for VR includes high resolution image generators
which allow real time rendering so the virtual world is updated as the user acts upon it;
and software which allows localized stereo sound and in some cases smell and voice
immersion and active learner participation as defining educational factors of VR. Winn
(1995) describes the result of VR's mixture of hardware, software and concepts as a
world is a valid, though different, form of reality". This phenomenon has been compared
appears to involve less effort on the part of the audience or user, with far more
convincing effects.
6
Virtual Reality and Simulation-Based Training
that requires the user to be fully immersed into the computer-generated, real-time, and 3D
technology.
synonymous as well as remain distinct, depending on their design and most importantly
how they are used in a learning interaction. Simulations-based programs can become
According to Thurman & Mattoon (1994), the concept of VR and the wave of
research and development accompanying it are creating new form of simulation that may
lead to fundamental improvements in simulation-based training. They call this new form
computer-based simulation that is controlled, in part, by the user. In a VR, users perceive
a synthetic environment instead of their immediate, physical surroundings, and they are
included as part of the simulation (Thurman et. al., 1994). Chiou (1995) supports this
technology in which a learner could behave like an active participant and an active
The main points of VR are full immersion and inside participation. A simulation
technology. There is much simulation software today that can simulate concrete things
7
and abstract concepts without requiring the user to be inside the simulated world.
He clarifies his point by arguing that today's multimedia software on personal computers
can simulate and present much information using a variety of forms, but a multimedia
Mattoon (1994) summarize these as integration, interface and verity. They claim that VR
is a type of interactive simulation that includes the human user as a necessary component.
It is, however, fundamentally different from other interactive simulations, because the
user's sense are simultaneously partitioned from the real world and integrated with the
synthetic world of VR. Integration of the user in the virtual environment is claimed to be
The concept of interface for VR is such that the interface disappears, allowing the
user to interact directly with the virtual world. Verity is defined as whether or not the
virtual world is "true to life", representing either real world or abstract concepts and
relationships. This concept is entirely variable and is one of the most important aspects
of VR in education.
immersive VR-based technology, immersive input and output devices are necessary.
Virtual worlds, like simulations, are programmed environments with which participants
can interact in real time. VR goes further than simulations, however, in that virtual
8
worlds can embody arbitrary objects, abstract or concrete, and can be programmed to
behave in ways that have no equivalence in the real world (Winn & Bricken, 1992).
there have been four generations of development. The first generation was shaped by
behaviorist theory. This theory developed traditional drill and practice tutorial
learner. The second and third generations have been informed by cognitive theory's
focus on the processes involved in assimilating and encoding information. The second
stage of instructional design focuses on the designer and strategies he or she may use to
reduce the cognitive load on students thereby facilitating instruction. The third
generation focuses on the relationship between the user and the information presented.
This stage would include intelligent tutors that attempt to adapt to individual learning
styles by responding to the user's interaction with the program. The fourth generation
focuses on the constructivist assumption that the learner constructs the knowledge and is
9
constructionism learning. Papert (1993) uses the term "constructionism" to label his
will do best by finding for themselves the specific knowledge they need. The goal is to
teach in such a way as to produce the most learning for the least teaching.
"Constructionism" differs from "constructivism" in that it looks more closely than other
the role of constructions in the world as a support for those in the head, thereby becoming
Machine" which would allow children a rich exploration of the world. Primitive
books" and "virtual reality". It seems that immersive VR is very much close to what
Papert has had in mind when discussing the concept of the "Knowledge Machine".
While discussing Microworlds and VR, one may wonder about the differences
and similarities between these two technologies. A Microworld has two essential
characteristics that may help in such analysis. First, a Microworld embodies the simplest
offers an initial point of entry that matches the user's cognitive state to allow interactions
to take place.
experiences in a natural way where the interaction disappears due to the immersive nature
of VR. In LOGO, for instance, the keyboard, mouse, or screen comes between the
10
student and the program. In VR there is no interface (Bricken, 1991). Winn & Bricken
(1992) argue that interaction with the virtual world is intuitive because students interact
Microworld which involves the estimation of distances by using the LOGO command
FORWARD to move the turtle from one point to another on the screen with as few
program where the learner can grab the object and move it from one location to another.
actively construct knowledge from their experience in the world. It also adds the idea
that people construct new knowledge with particular effectiveness when they are
commonality between LOGO Microworld and VR is the fact that both technologies
some extent subjective (we all experience the same world but interpret it on the basis
of our own knowledge and beliefs) (Winn, 1993). Simply put, constructivists argue
11
collaboration, discussions with their teacher, self-assessment, and reflection.
that the traditional instruction focuses on designing instruction that has predictable
onto the student's knowledge. Constructivism, on the other hand, focuses on instruction
that fosters the learning process instead of controlling it and it focuses on learning
The constructivist paradigm is relatively new, and many issues must be resolved
before it can become a sound educational framework. In particular, there are many
questions about how educational experiences can be designed that fit with the
principles to help with this process. He proposes six principles of constructivist learning
12
• Support collaborative constructions of knowledge, rather than encouragement
refine their mental models, computational tools that complement human memory and
intelligence are made available. In parallel, transitional objects (such as Logo's "turtle")
are used to facilitate translating personal experience into abstract symbols (Papert, 1993).
representations and applications can mediate interactions among learners and natural or
social phenomena.
Dede (1995) argues that, constructivism theory fits very well in a virtual
environment. He claims that like Alice walking through the looking glass, learners can
themselves and reality itself shifts the focus of constructivism from peripherally
enhancing how a student interprets a typical interaction with the external world to
"magically" shaping the fundamental nature of how learners experience their physical and
social context.".
13
VR Applications in Education
strong benefits that can support education. For some, VR's ability to facilitate
constructivist learning activities is the key issue (Rose, 1995). Others focus on the
potential to provide alternative forms of learning that can support different types of
learners, such as visually oriented learners. Some of the research seeks to locate learning
within a very general educational setting. Some studies have investigated the impact of
studies and other research is based on longitudinal case studies, while researchers are
uses follows.
Visiting Virtual environments (VE) help students learn content under some
circumstances (Byrne, 1996; Dede, 1992, 1995; Rose, 1995). There are three contributing
what they are learning about in an entirely new way. VEs can simulate objects and
actions that occur in the real world. But in particular, VEs can represent in directly visible
forms, concepts and procedures that are intangible and invisible in the real world (Winn,
1997).
14
According to Bricken (1991), for immersive VR, the interface has ceased to exist
interface between computer and user is a necessary condition for immersion in VR. The
participant, in a sense, "wears the computer" and is "inside" the data. As a result, the
participants can interact with the virtual world, which might be the simulation of some
aspect of the real world, as naturally as they do with the real world
Bricken (1991) identified two other changes that take place as a result of
immersion that are very important for education. First, the subject-object distinction
between students and what they learn disappears. Immersion in a virtual world removes
the interface allowing us to cross the subject-object boundary that exists between the
machine and us. Second, immersion allows non-symbolic interaction with the world.
Immersive VR allows students to interact with the world using what Bricken (1991) calls
the "natural semantics" of the world. What Bricken meant by that is that the students can
interact with the objects and can actively experience phenomena in the virtual world in
ways that are more natural than those normally employed when interacting with
computers. For example, in Dede’s "Science Space", a student may experience what it is
like to be a ball that reacts to forces acting on, and to collisions with another ball, (Dede
at al., 1996). The student can learn Newtonian mechanics by becoming and by observing
interaction that VEs foster. A study by Byrne (1996) suggested that interaction is a more
important facilitator of learning than immersion for some kinds of tasks. Educational
technologists have always understood that a student must interact with an environment
15
for learning to occur (Psotka, 1995). Psotka points out that naturalness of interactions
with objects in a VE makes interaction much easier and therefore more useful than in
Engaging: The third factor that contributes to VR is that students find VEs
entirely engaging (Bricken & Byrne, 1993; Winn, 1997). Winn (1997) explained that part
of the reason for this is doubtless the novelty of VR and its association in children's
minds with computer and video games. Another reason is the uniqueness of the
experience and the empowerment it brings to young students who can control the
computer to do their bidding in complex and sophisticated ways. Winn (1997) also
believes that it also enables some students to understand concepts and principles that
have till now been opaque and baffling which is intrinsically motivating.
It has been argued that immersive VR allow students to learn concepts and to
students to master abstract and esoteric symbol systems before they can understand the
content (Winn, et. al., 1997). Indeed, the symbol system can be learned subsequently
Winn (1993) argued that traditional education requires learners to learn complex
symbols before concentrating on concepts that are imparted indirectly via educators.
Thus many learners may fail because they have problems with the symbolic nature of
education, rather than the concepts being communicated. Winn (1993) suggested that
reflection and directly, is a possible powerful tool in VR for education, where learners
16
These claims are also supported by a study conducted by Winn and Bricken
(1992). The authors describe an interesting virtual world in which the relationships
between objects follow algebraic and arithmetic set of rules. In their proposal, the user
can directly manipulate the algebraic elements, and by physical placement the
environment can give them feedback on the correctness of the mathematical operations
which the user has available to solve the puzzle. Winn & Bricken (1992) argue that it is
perfectly possible for students to learn the conceptual basis of Algebra without learning
its conventional symbols provided that the learning experience is direct, personal and
requirement may get in the way of the natural course of learning. The authors believe
that the proposed Algebra virtual world removes the impediments imposed by the
Having students construct their own VEs enables them to learn content (Osberg,
models, into an entire world of their own creation (Winn, et, al., 1997). Arriving at the
construct knowledge for themselves, under guidance, rather than have it fed to them
(Dede, 1995; Winn, 1993). Constructing a VE engages those cognitive and perceptual
skills that are brought to bear when a student makes any physical construction (Harel &
Papert, 1991).
17
The University of Washington's Human Interface Technology Lab (HITL) is one
of the research labs most involved in experimenting with students in real educational
settings using current VR technology. The HITL lab makes the following technology
available to it researchers. Therefore, the HITL studies reviewed in this paper use the
is useful in helping students improve their knowledge of chemistry and if so, whether
VR's interactivity and immersion were the reasons for this improvement and whether the
The main difference among the treatments was the varying degrees of
immersion. The Mac Interactive treatment also consisted of high interactivity, but no
immersion (desktop version of VR treatment). The Video treatment and the Mac Run
18
For the VR treatment, 38 high school students, 25 females and 13 males, who
were in their second semester of junior level high school chemistry class participated.
They had been taught atomic and molecular structure in their first semester of class. They
were all novice users of VR. The Video treatment consisted of twenty high school
students, 10 females and 10 males, who were in their second semester of junior level high
school chemistry. The students for the Mac Interactive treatment were drawn from a
similar, but different population than for the VR and video treatments and consisted of 14
students, 7 females and 7 males. The Mac Run students were drawn from the same
population as the Mac Interactive group. There were 14 students, 5 females and 9 males.
The control group had 7 students, 5 females and 2 males drawn from the same population
as the VR and video treatments. The student population for the long-term retention study
was a subset of the students who participated in the VR, video, or control treatments. 18
students from the VR treatment, 17 students from the video treatment and 5 students from
conceptual understanding of how atoms are built; though not their recall of facts, relative
to other instructional strategies. However, she also found that the key to the success of
her VE was the interaction it permitted not the fact that students were immersed in the
VE. Students learning from Atom World performed no better than students learning in a
non-immersive interactive desktop version of the same program. The interactive desktop
program was more effective than a non-interactive, but still immersive version of the VE.
This conclusion was due to the fact that students in the VR and Mac Interactive
19
treatments scored well on both of the tests. In most comparisons, their scores were
significantly better than the Mac-Run, Video, and Control groups. However, the VR and
Mac-Interactive students were not significantly different from each other, which leads to
the conclusion that interactivity is the important feature not the immersion. Issues of
training, world design, assessment, hardware resolution, and student population were
Cell biology serves as another example of a test case to determine the impact of
study was conducted at the Computer Museum in Boston and hence the target audiences
for this project were the visitors to the museum. Participants' ages ranged 5 to 50, while
their education ranged from high school through graduate school. There were more
males than females in this study (percentage not reported). Only 26% had computer
related jobs, but 84% used computers in some way at work or at home. None of the
participants had much biology experience. The platform used for this study was 486/50
PC based system. The software used in this study was Sense8’s WorldToolKit (WTK).
The function of the VE was to teach how the different kinds of cells require different
organelles in order to perform their different functions. Gay (1994) wanted to test for
two characteristics: immersion and interaction. The same information was presented as
videotape and as a virtual world and the only difference in information was whether the
user was actively involved. This study presented the world in both immersive and the
The initial application was designed much like a textbook in that the users first
learned about cell requirements and cell function before building a cell and testing its
20
structure. In the first part of the evaluation, the researchers realized that it took users a
while to get used to VR interface which distracted them from concentrating on the
content. Based on few findings, the cell biology was redesigned. The new design
allowed the users an opportunity to get familiar with the interface before any learning
began. In addition, it was improved to provide instant feedback their correct usage of the
system when building cells. The researcher compared the impact of immersive, desktop,
and videotape viewing of the refined application. Generally, the interactive (immersive
and desktop) users scored higher on post-testing of symbolic and graphic retention.
Desktop users performed slightly better than the immersive users, although researchers
suggest that the differing resolution between the HMD used and monitor might account
for this difference. However, immersive user did report more enjoyment, and these users
stated that they would be more likely to take a free biology class, as compared to users in
the other two groups. The users of the fully immersive system showed much less
"simulator sickness" than expected. Overall, this study concluded that people learn more
from an interactive learning experience than from a passive one. In addition, the results
show that immersion did not have significant impact on student' learning.
being immersed in VR. The author sought to assess how learner’s ratings of presence in
the VE were related to their enjoyment of, navigation in and ability to perform tasks in
VEs. Subjects were 1001 elementary, 922 middle school and 949 high school students
from a range of social, economic, ethnic backgrounds and geographic areas in the states
21
brainstorming session, and they visited an immersive VE. In the presentation, the
students learned how 3-D computing was similar to conventional computing with the
tracking system, the substitution of the HMD for a flat screen and the use of a wand
instead of a mouse. Students used a pointing device called a wand to “fly” from place to
place in the VE and could easily move large objects by inserting one’s virtual “hand” in
the object while depressing a button on the wand. After the visit they completed a
questionnaire featuring a five points rating scale. The questionnaire asked students to
rate their enjoyment of the experience, their sense of “presence”, whether they were
disoriented, and whether it was easy to move around and interact with objects.
The findings indicate that all students enjoyed their experience of VR. When
enjoyment were compared across type of school, the study revealed significant declines
from elementary to middle and from middle to high school students (all p’s < .05). Also,
data collected from Washington public school students indicated that boys enjoyed
The findings also indicate that students experienced a high degree of presence in
VR. However, these convictions were significantly more marked in younger than older
students did. Students found it easy to identify objects in the VE and did not report any
disorientation. Based on the results of the study, Wayne (1997) suggested that VR is a
feasible tool to be used in classrooms. He also argued that any potentials for helping
student learn the content is likely to arise from the attribute of presence.
22
In an evaluative study, Bricken and Byrne (1992)(HITL scientists) evaluated the
potential of VR as a learning environment. The context of the study was the Technology
Academy, a technology-oriented summer camp for students ages 5-18. The academy
offered seven camp sessions. Student activities center on hands-on exploration of new
technology. A total of 59 students participated in the study. The average age of the
students was 13 years (72% male, homogeneous ethnic origin). None of the students had
any experience with or had even heard of VR. The camp allowed seven groups of
students, seven weeks to create their own virtual worlds. HITL provided students with
Macintosh modeling software package Swivel 3D and several Mac II computers for
students to construct their virtual world. Students were provided with a Swivel file
containing a “protoworld”, which consisted of two basic elements of a virtual world. The
first element was the participant’s virtual body, represented by a graphic head and hand.
The virtual head is the position responsive point-of-view, and the virtual hand is the
digital analogue of the participants physical hand, used for gesture commands such as
“fly” and “grab”. The second element was a ground play extended to the maximum size
that the rendering software could handle. On the last day of each session, the students
were taken on the 15 minutes ride to HITL at the Washington Technology Center, located
at the campus of University of Washington. At HITL, students were able to get inside
their worlds using VR interface technology. Bricken and Byrne (1992) used RB2
software on a Macintosh FX rendered by one Iris 320 VGX with a video splitter. First-
generation Eyephones were used for viewing and a right-handed datagloves was used for
gesture-command activity.
23
In collecting information on both student response and system usability, the
activities, elicited student opinions with surveys and collected informal observations from
teachers and researchers. The students' worlds were dynamic and interesting and the
The themes of the worlds ranged from Cloudlands to a Moon Colony. For
instance, the students who created Cloudlands agreed to disagree on topics so each
elaborate house to a sea world. The moon colony project was a more collaborative
rendition of the moon in the future, complete with monorail, mountains, futuristic
buildings and spacecraft. The students experienced each other's worlds at the end of the
seven weeks. In using the worlds the students on the whole had no problems navigating,
individual reactions to the virtual environment. Interacting with objects was considerably
more difficult, but Bricken and Byrne note that this may be due to the creator's lack of
experience. Behaviors such as bending down and reaching out were common, with
Bricken and Byrne noting an avenue for research into the implications of VR for
kinesthetic learners.
The findings of this study indicated that the students were fascinated by their
experience and entering virtual world. The students were motivated to achieve functional
competence in the skills to design and model objects, demonstrated a willingness to focus
significant effort toward finished product, and expressed strong satisfaction with their
24
comprehension of complex concepts and skills. They learned computer graphic concepts,
3-D modeling techniques, and world design approaches. Overall, Bricken and Byrne
The most relevant work on the issue of how learning is mediated through the use
Mason University. Since February 1994, the project team has worked collaboratively to
Electrostatics, leading up to the concept of Gauss' Law. PaulingWorld, the most recent
All three worlds have been built using a polygonal geometry. Colored, shaded
polygons and textures are used to produce detailed objects. These objects are linked
together and given behaviors through the use of NASA-developed software that defines
the virtual worlds and connects them to underlying physical simulations. Interactivity is
achieved through the linkage of external devices (e.g., a head-mounted display) using this
same software. Finally, graphics rendering, collision detection, and lighting models are
provided by other NASA-developed software. The key hardware items used are a high-
performance graphics workstation with two video output channels; a color, stereoscopic
for the head and both hands; and, in some cases, a haptic display. Interaction in these
25
worlds is principally carried out with a three-dimensional mouse. The software is called
Custom (Dede, et. al., 1996b). Haptic feedback has been employed in NewtonWorld and
effectiveness. The haptic feedback was provided by two different haptic vests, both of
Dede, et. al. (1996b) evaluated MaxwellWorld as a tool for (1) remediating
misconceptions about electric fields and (2) teaching abstract concepts with which
students are unfamiliar. During the sessions, they administered one to three lessons
superposition, test charges, and field lines), learning experiences about electric potential
(potential and kinetic energy, potential difference, work, and potential vs. force), and the
The authors claim that although these evaluations are still underway, they report
preliminary findings based on 14 high school students (and four college students) who
have participated in the evaluations thus far. Each session lasted for approximately two
hours. Students were scheduled on consecutive days for the first two sessions, while the
third session was conducted approximately two weeks later; thus providing a measure of
The results of the study indicate that all of the students who were post-tested
enjoyed learning about electric fields in MaxwellWorld. When asked about their general
reactions to MaxwellWorld, a majority of the students commented that they felt it was a
more effective way to learn about electric fields than either textbooks or lectures.
Students cited the 3-D representations, the interactivity, the ability to navigate to multiple
26
perspectives, and the use of color as characteristics of MaxwellWorld that were important
to their learning experience. Pre- and post-lesson evaluations showed that students
The authors also report significant individual differences in the students' abilities
to work in the 3-D environment and with 3-D controls, as well as their susceptibility to
some students learned to use the menus, manipulate objects, and navigate very rapidly,
others required guidance throughout the sessions. Most students experienced nothing
more than slight eyestrain; however, two students experienced moderate dizziness and
slight nausea during the first session, and, consequently, did not return for the second
session. No student complained of any symptoms during the first 30-45 minutes of the
reference frame usage in learning. Thirty high school students with at least one year of
high school physics participated in this study. Each trial required 2 1/2 to 3 hours;
learning tasks in the HMD required 1 to 1-1/4 hours. During the sessions, students
thought aloud as they performed learning tasks that focused on relationships among
force, mass, velocity, momentum, acceleration, and energy during and between
collisions. For each task, students began by predicting what the relationships or behaviors
would be, then experienced them, and finally assessed their predictions based on what
they observed. To assess the utility of the multisensory experience, the authors formed
27
three groups of subjects differentiated by controlling the visual, tactile, and auditory cues
that students received while performing learning tasks: (1) visual cues only; (2) visual
comments, usability questionnaires, interview feedback, and pre- and post-test knowledge
understanding relationships among force, motion, velocity, and energy. Most students
found the activities interesting and enjoyed their learning experience. Additionally, many
users stated that they felt NewtonWorld provided a good way to explore physics
concepts. When asked to list the features they liked most, almost all students cited the
cues were provided. In fact, students receiving sound or sound plus haptic cues rated
NewtonWorld as easier to use and the egocentric reference frame as more meaningful
than those receiving visual cues only. For example, students who received haptic cues in
addition to sound and visual cues performed slightly better than students in other groups
observed that students receiving haptic and sound cues were more attentive to these
Overall, the students found the environment easy to use. The authors argue that
the analysis of the learnability data suggests that younger users might gain more from
students. The findings MaxwellWorld and NewtonWorld studies reported here (Dede, et.
28
al. 1996b) demonstrate that students find virtual worlds attractive learning environments.
In some topical areas, especially abstract concepts in electrostatics, students did learn
In another study, Salzman, et. al. (1999) designed an evaluation to compare the
those of a highly regarded and widely used 2D learning environment, EM Field (EMF),
while tightly controlling the learning activities and instructional content of the lessons.
Lessons were designed to provide the same content and learning activities using each of
the application, focusing on concepts pertaining to the distribution of force and energy in
electric fields.
Stage one of the study compared MW and EMF on the extent to which
and interaction outcomes. In the stage two of the study, MW’s full range of capabilities
(such as multisensory input) were utilized to ascertain the value these affordances added
to the learning experience. Fourteen high school students completed the lessons in both
MW and EMF. Seven of students came back after 5 months for completing the stage two
of the study. The authors examined pre- and post lesson understanding of each of the
groups. They also assessed retention for those students that returned to stage two. They
and gaming experience, and motion sickness history. The also examined whether
interaction experience (sickness and usability) differed between the groups and whether
they predicted learning outcomes. At the end of the first stage, both groups demonstrated
29
significantly improved conceptual understanding, with MaxwellWorld students better
able to define concepts than students who used EMF. Also, MaxwellWorld students
performed better in demonstrating concepts in 3D, were able to predict how change to a
source would affect the electric field, and could recognize symmetries in the field.
Student ratings indicated that they felt significantly more motivated by MW than EMF (F
(1,12) = 7.66). On the scale from –3 to +3, mean ratings (and standard deviation) were
2.03 (.29) and 1.11 (.82) respectfully. Neither motivation nor meaningfulness of the
representation significantly predicted learning outcomes. The findings also report that the
more time the participants spent using computers, the higher usability was rated.
lesson in MaxwellWorld, this time supported by auditory and haptic cues. The results
F-tests were significant at p < .05). Students learned more from the visual and
multisensory representations used in the lesson. Mean motivation was reported the same
as stage one. Overall, these results suggest that an immersive 3D multisensory world can
stage two, the enhancement of visual representations with multisensory cues appeared to
facilitate learning, especially for students who had trouble grasping the concepts.
Salzman, et. al.(1999) report the following findings based on all their studies
discussed above:
• Evaluation outcomes indicate that VR’s features affect not only learning, but
the quality of the interaction and learning experiences
30
• Outcomes show that 3-D immersive representations can be motivating and can
support learning
• VR’s features sometimes support the learning of one concept, but hinder the
learning of another
• Learning experience is affected by VR’s features
• The success or failure of VR learning environments in practice depends on the
web of relations among VR’s features, the concepts to be learned, learner
characteristics, the learning experience, the interaction experience and more
informed design.
glove, 3-D earphones and tracking equipment, Chiou (1995) felt there is potential for VR
as a learning medium. The author believes that constructivism rationale can be applied to
VR and that computer-based tools need to be under the learner's control. He argues that
children between the age of 8 and 11 who were enrolled in an elementary summer school
program in Navota, California. Two different computer apparatuses were used: computer
workstation and VR system developed by Autodesk Inc. The abilities under investigation
were mental imagery, spatial relation, displacement and transformation, creativity and
spatially problem solving. The hypothesis was that children’s cognitive abilities could be
enhances by having them develop, displace, transform, and interact with 2D and 3D
computer generated models. Although children had some difficulties in using the
peripheral devices, they had become quite proficient in the system by the end of the
study. The results of the project showed that displacement, transformation, visualizing,
31
and mentally manipulating 2D objects were significantly related to spatially related
problem solving abilities of children (R=68, F= 8.05, ndf = 1.20, p = .00). Although
abilities, the relationship between the two is uncertain. The author concluded that VR is
Andolsek (1995) suggests that since virtual worlds are totally engaging, they
immerse the student entirely, both cognitively and affectively. Passivity becomes
impossible. Virtual environments may have great potential in terms of increasing the
learner motivation. If learning is made more interesting and fun, students may remain
engaged in an activity for longer period of time. Bricken & Byrne (1992) claim that since
VR places learners in a 3D visual and auditory environment, the sensation the learner
receives are pervasive and convincing and indeed engaging and motivating. However,
the challenge would be, how to increase the learner motivation to stay engaged in a
virtual environment. This is where the design and usability issues become very important
students in VE and motivate the learner to stay engaged, then it would provide solutions
learning.
Due to the fact that VR is a new technology, its use as an educational tool is quite
new with very little empirical research related to learner characteristics. Various studies
have addressed the fact that it is important to investigate whether factors of gender, race,
or student experience impact their interaction with and enjoyment of VR. Dede (1996a)
32
identified several learner characteristics as important factors: gender, domain experience,
spatial ability, computer experience, motion sickness history, and immersive tendencies.
Gender, domain experience, and spatial ability are important factors because they
might influence a person’s aptitude for mastering abstract science concepts (Dillon, 1985;
Dillon & Schmeck, 1983). Spatial ability may be particularly important in influencing
how effectively students use the information provided by VR’s features (Egan & Gomez,
1985; Norman, 1995). Many researchers have implicated that spatial ability as one of the
strongest predictors of performance in math and science (Halpern, 1992). Spatial ability,
along with computer experience and motion sickness history, might also affect the
interaction experience (Dede et. al. 1999). In their work, Dede & colleagues (1999),
found that computer experience can be predictive of how students rate the usability of the
learning environment. Other researchers have found that there is a strong correlation
between spatial ability and computer-based performance (Egan, 1988; Gomez, Egan, &
Bowers, 1986). Immersive tendencies may be useful for explaining how immersed a
student will become in the VR learning environment (Singer & Baily, 1994).
In one exploratory study Winn & few other HITL’s researchers (1997)
investigated to see whether the unique experiences of building and visiting VEs would be
more useful to some students than others. They examined the extent to which students'
general ability, and secondary school students' spatial reasoning ability and spatial
orientation ability predicted performance after learning by building and visiting VEs and
The subjects for this study were 365 students from grades 4 to 12 took part in the
world-building project. Students took post-tests over the content they had been studying.
33
Since each group of students built a different VE, there was a different post-test for each
enjoyment, the sense of` "presence" in the VE (the extent to which students felt they were
really in the VE and not in the classroom), and potential impediments to learning such as
difficulty seeing and moving around in the VE and tendency to nausea. Students who did
not build worlds and who were in a "traditional" class answered an eight-item subset of
these questions that were concerned with the VR experience not with building a world.
groups crossing with high and low ability show no main effect for group. However, the
interaction of group with high ability was significant, F (1,44)=2.91, p<.10. Low-ability
those studying in the traditional way (M=42.55%, SD=26.28), F(1,44)=8.67, p<.01. For
MTraditional=60.89%, SD=19.36).
Students were also blocked on their spatial ability and spatial visualization scores.
No significant main effects or interactions were found for either measure with content
The authors also reported that that high general ability students reported making
paper drawings and 3D models of objects before modeling them on the computer more
than low ability students, t(41) =1.82, p<.10. In addition, high spatial reasoning students
enjoyed visiting their world and experienced higher levels of presence than low spatial
students, t(22)=2.47, p<.05. Results also showed that high spatial reasoning students were
34
also likely to feel less nausea and less dizziness than low spatial students, t(22)=1.74,
p<.10. Furthermore, students with higher spatial visualization scores collaborated more
with other students with low spatial visualization scores, t(22)=1.72, p<.10.
The authors also conducted a two-way ANOVA involving two levels of gender
and two levels of spatial reasoning ability was performed on presence scores. For gender,
they reported F (1,53) = 6.53, p<.05. For spatial ability, they reported F (1,53) = 5.58,
p < .05. For the interaction of gender with spatial ability, they reported F (1,53) = 5.51,
p < .05. Spatial reasoning ability did not affect presence ratings for boys, (MLow Spatial
= 7.47, sd = 1.26, MHigh Spatial = 7.47, sd = 1.93). However, low spatial girls reported
lower presence than high spatial girls, (MLow Spatial = 5.14, sd = 1.96, MHigh Spatial =
Winn, et. al. (1997) report that in general the world-building activity improved the
posttest performance of low ability students who built worlds when compared to those
learning in a traditional manner. This implies that the collection of innovative learning
activities afforded by world building helped students understand the material who do not
have high general ability as measured using a traditional test. The lack of difference
between high ability students in both groups simply reconfirms that brighter students can
learn from a variety of approaches and therefore, for them, the innovative nature of the
Girls with low spatial reasoning ability reported experiencing less presence than
girls with high spatial reasoning ability. This difference was not found for boys, who
reported higher levels of presence than girls. It is possible that boys have different ways
from girls of becoming engaged in a VE. Maybe they have more exposure to computer
35
games and have developed better skills for manipulating the interface than girls. Maybe
they are more easily fooled into believing a VE is real than girls.
The Virtual Reality Roving Vehicles (VRRV) (Rose, 1995) is another project by
HITL scientists which takes VR technology into public elementary, junior high and high
schools and puts it in the hands of students and teachers. The goal of the project is to
evaluate VR as a tool for students to develop broad-based abilities including, but not
limited to: problem solving, building mental models, and developing effective meta-
to instruction that puts each student in charge of his/her own process of learning.
with 120 junior high school students at Kellogg Middle School in Shoreline, Washington.
According to Rose (1996), the purpose of this VRRV project was to look at the issue of
gender. The Kellogg Project integrated the building of virtual worlds into a specially
designed curriculum about Wetlands ecology. Four classes of thirty students participated;
each one was randomly assigned to focus on one of the wetlands life cycles: water,
carbon, energy and nitrogen. Students learned the fundamentals of their respective cycle
then divided into three working groups who each planned and designed a virtual world to
express their understanding of the wetlands cycle they studied (Rose, 1995). The
contributions of the three working groups in each class were brought together and a
single virtual world was constructed for each of the four life cycles. The virtual wetland
worlds were populated with plants, animals, objects and landscapes which students
created on desktop computers using 3D modeling software. As the final step of the
36
learning process, students put on a VR head mounted display and experienced two of the
At the elementary level, self-reports on the amount of learning that took place
indicated that boys benefited more than girls from world building activities. At the
secondary level, boy reported more enjoyment from the use of VR than did girls. With
respect to general abilities, the VRRV data indicated that students performed equally
well. However, students with high spatial ability reported more enjoyment, and feeling
37
Conclusion
This paper has reviewed the uses of VR in education and its implication for
education is still evolving. The studies described in this paper include the results of one-
time uses of virtual world by particular group of students, with only a few results reported
on the long-term use of the VR technology. As a result, the conclusions are not
generalizable to a larger population of students and reflect only the findings of current
studies. Hopefully they will serve to guide further research on the use of VR in
education. The summary of findings are addressed according to the questions initially
raised in this paper. The remainder of this section returns to the questions raised in this
Summary
(1) Do learning in virtual learning environment offer advantages over more traditional
methodologies?
concepts, and visit environments and interact with events that distance, time, or safety
38
technology allow virtual worlds to support a wide range of types of experiential,
The literature review reveals the rudimental state of VR systems and to a certain
existing data support that VR offers significant, positive support for education. They
Studies showed mixed findings for the impact of immersion on student learning.
feature but not immersion. Issues of training, world design, assessment, hardware
resolution, and student population were suggested as possible reasons for immersion's
lack of significance. In the case of both studies, the failure of positive findings in
Other research findings indicate that the value of VR for education lies within its
ability to provide immersion of the user in either realistic or novel and abstract
environments. The immersion allows the user to experience, interact with and
39
discover digital knowledge first hand while the manipulation of the verity of the
virtual world allows the user to make visual or kinesthetic relationships to help
Although, Byrne’s and Gay's findings show uncertain learning benefits for
all of the studies (including Byrne’s) presented in this paper expressed more
conditions.
University of Washington (Bricken (1991); Bricken and Byrne, 1993; Winn (1999);
Winn (1993); Winn (1995); Rose (1995); Byrne (1996) and elsewhere (Dede, C.,
Loftin, R.B., Salzman, M.C., Calhoun, C., Hoblit, J. & Regian, W.(1994); Dede, C.,
Salzman, M.C. & Loftin, R.B., Chen, J.(1999)) gives us an intuitive sense that VR
could be highly useful to promote skills and knowledge which students can apply
across many domains. The interactive and immersive qualities of VR suggest the
In one study, the researchers (Salzman, M.C., Dede, C., & Loftin, R.B., Chen, J.,
- VR’s features affect not only learning, but the quality of the interaction and
learning experiences,
- Three-dimensional immersive representations can be motivating and can support
learning,
- VR’s features sometimes support the learning of one concept, but hinder the
learning of another
- VR's features affect Learning experience
40
Overall, all of the studies demonstrated that the technology known as VR is
Almost all of the studies suggested that constructivism provides the best theory on
way to create learning experience and transference is to allow users to construct and
Studies also suggested that having students construct their own VEs enable them
knowledge for themselves, under guidance, rather than have it fed to them (Dede,
The studies reported here provide initial findings that are suggestive of suitability
whether positive results are due to the use of this learning method, the use of VR, or a
(4) How does student effectiveness of virtual world compares with other instructional
practices?
The studies that have addressed this question were constructed in very different
areas of curricula, with students in different age groups. However, the literature
shows that VR is a powerful tool for education and that it offers tools for increased
41
student participation. Studies indicated that classroom activities may use VR tools
for hands-on learning, group projects and discussions, field trips, and concept
The findings suggest that immersive VR can furnish experiences that are designed
(Winn & Bricken, 1992) in which instead of learning the symbols in Algebra,
students could explore the objects, learn the concepts and construct their own
knowledge. This study suggests that VRs promote the best and probably only
strategy that allows student to learn from non-symbolic experience. Great many
students fail in school because they do not master the symbol systems of the
disciplines they study, although they are perfectly capable of mastering the concepts
that lie at the heart of the discipline. VR provides a route to success for children who
might otherwise fail in our education system as it is currently constructed (Win 1993;
In most of the studies, students reported a positive attitude and enjoyed learning
through immersive virtual environments. Winn et. al., (1997) found that in general
the world-building activity improved the posttest performance of low ability students
who built worlds when compared to those learning in a traditional manner. In the
MaxwellWorld project (Dede, et. al., 1996b), the students felt that it was more
42
under learner's control. Also, students constructed their own knowledge in VR.
Provided in this paper are initial findings that suggest that these capabilities have
value.
(5) What is impact of learner characteristics (e.g. age, gender, race, or previous
According to Salzman, Dede, et. al. (1999) the success or failure of VR learning
environments in practice depends on the web of relations among VR’s features, the
experience and more informed design. However, very few empirical studies look into
the issue of learner characteristics in VR. Only few studies investigated the issue and
gender and reported that boys enjoyed the use of VR more than did girls (Dede, et al.
(1996a,b); Win et al. 1997; Rose (1995), Byrne (1996); Wayne (1997); Bricken &
Byrne (1992)). These studies showed that students performed well in using VR and
enjoyed their experience. Winn (1995) reported few significant differences in terms
of race or gender.
Girls with low spatial reasoning ability reported experiencing less presence than
girls with high spatial reasoning ability. This difference was not found for boys, who
reported higher levels of presence than girls (Winn et. al., 1997).
The Wetland study (Bricken & Byrne, 1992) reported that as students get older,
they enjoy their experience, but slightly less than younger students. Elementary
students enjoyed themselves the most, followed by middle school students. High
school students seemed slightly more jaded about the technology, or at least inclined
43
to be more cool in their response to their experience. In addition, boys reported
elementary level, self-reports on the amount of learning that took place indicated that
boys benefited more than girls from world building activities. At the secondary level,
boy reported more enjoyment from the use of VR than did girls. With respect to
general abilities, students performed equally well. However, students with high
spatial ability reported more enjoyment, and feeling of presence than did those with
It is possible that boys have different ways of becoming engaged in a VE. Maybe
they have more exposure to computer games and have developed better skills for
Concluding Remarks
It is important that researchers begin to identify and solve the issues concerning
VR now so that it can be used to its best advantage by all students in the future. As the
Most of the studies presented in this paper are on going. Therefore, the results
may change as the studies advance. These studies tend to be evaluative and although
have shown very encouraging results, do not provide quantified data to show, for
44
Numerous factors unrelated to the VR technology itself will undoubtedly have a
assortment of social, economic and political variables related to schools, education and
The future research must take closer look at the possible influence of gender and
VR technology. As shown in this paper, there are very few studies that investigate
learner characteristics. This is, however, justifiable due to the fact that the application of
spatial reasoning ability (Wayne, 1997). Gender is a factor here and its precise role must
be determined.
uses of technology change the teacher's role in the classroom. The fact that it is difficult
for teachers to monitor student's moment-by-moment activities can present challenges for
lesson administration. Also, current applications do not provide teachers with assistance
45
for assessing a student's learning or recognizing particular problems a student may have
with the material. The integration of intelligent tutors into educational VR applications
seems a logical next step that should help to resolve some of these problems. Given the
technology. Few of the studies reported student enjoyment and motivation. But does this
enjoyment and increased motivation last? There is no data to answer this question,
although the ScienceSpace researchers felt that learner motivation will remain high, even
long run, research on immersion may produce a deeper understanding of the nature of
human learning. In the same context, an investigation of the interplay among VR and
other factors such as the concepts to be learned, learner characteristics, the learning
experience, and the interaction experience is needed. By understanding how these factors
work together to shape learning, we may be able to target learning and visualization
MaxwellWorld project, if the researchers had not concerned with these factors, they
might have missed several potential findings and would have been less able to identify
As the technology for building and learning in VE advances both the quality of
the VE and the ease of working in it, more carefully controlled studies will be possible.
46
Then researchers will be in a position to conduct studies of precisely which features of
VEs facilitate the learning of what kinds of content for which students.
more about the very process of learning. By participating in the development of VR,
educators can guide the growth of this technology, and perhaps influence the course of
educational change.
47
References
Bricken, M., (1991). Virtual Reality Learning Environments: Potentials and Challenges,
Human Interface Technology Laboratory, University of Washington, Seattle, WA.
http://www.hitl.washington.edu/publications/
Bricken, M. & Byrne, C. (1992). Summer students in virtual reality: A pilot study on
educational applications of VR technology, paper presented at the Annual Meeting of
American Educational Research Association, San Fransisco.
ERIC Document, ED358853
Byrne, C. M., (1996). Water on Tap: The Use of Virtual reality as an Educational Tool,
Ph.D. Dissertation. University of Washington, Seattle, WA.
http://www.hitl.washington.edu/publications/dissertations/Byrne/home.html
Chiou, G. F., (1995). Learning Rationales and Virtual Reality Technology in Education,
Journal of educational technology Systems, 23(4), pp.327-336
Cornell, R., Bailey, D., & Bollet, R., (1994). Virtual Reality: Therapeutic Tool or Time
Bomb? Educational Media International, 31(4), pp.247-49
Dietzel, R., Bird, M., & Kohler, A., (1994). Adding Virtual Technology to the
Curriculum - A Fictional Example, Educational Media International, 31(4), pp.238-41
Dede, Christopher J., (1992). The Future of Multimedia: Bridging to Virtual Worlds,
Educational Technology, 32(5), pp.54-60.
Dede, C., Loftin, R.B., Salzman, M.C., Calhoun, C., Hoblit, J. & Regian, W. (1994). The
Design of Artificial Realities to Improve Learning Newtonian Mechanics. In Brusilovsky,
P. (ed.), Proceedings of the East-West International Conference on Multimedia,
Hypermedia, and Virtual Reality, Moscow, Russia, pp.34-41
Dede, C., Salzman, M.C. & Loftin, R.B. (1996a). MaxwellWorld: Learning Complex
Scientific via Immersion in Virtual Reality. In Proc. 2nd International Conference on
learning sciences, Charlottesville, VA, pp. 22-29.
Dede, C., Salzman, M.C. & Loftin, R.B. (1996b). ScienceSpace: Virtual Realities for
Learning Complex and Abstract Scientific Concepts. In Proceedings of IEEE Virtual
Reality Annual International Symposium, pp. 246-253.
http://www.vetl.uh.edu/ScienceSpace/absvir.html
48
Dillon, R. (ED.), (1995). Individual Differences in Cognition. New York: Academic
Press.
Dillon, R., & Schmeck, R. (Eds.), (1983). Individual Differences in Cognition. New
York: Academic Press
Gay, E. (1994). Is virtual Reality a Good Teaching Tool? Virtual Reality Special Report,
Winter, pp. 51-59
http://www.net.org/html/resources/research/vr-research/vrtt.html
Grove, J. (1996). VR and History: Some Findings and Thoughts, VR in the Schools, 2(1),
pp. 3-9.
Gomez, L. M., Egan, D. E. & Bowers, C. (1986). Learning to use a text editor: some
learner characteristics that predict success. Human-Computer Interaction, 2, pp. 1-23
Harel, I., & Papert, S. (1991) (Eds.). Constructionism. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Helsel, S. (1992). Virtual Reality and Education, Educational Technology, 32(5), pp.38-
42.
Hedburg, J., Alexander, S. (1994). Virtual Reality in Education: Defining Researchable
Issues, Educational Media international, 31(4), pp. 214-20.
Krueger, A. (1991). Those Who Can Do Can Teach, Science & Children, 28(5), pp. 32-
33
49
Loeffler, C. E. (1993). Distributed virtual reality: Applications for education,
entertainment and industry.
http://www.nta.no/telektronikk/tema/nr4_93.html
Loeffler, C. E. & Anderson, T. (Eds.) (1994). The Virtual Reality Casebook, New York:
Van Nostrand Reinhold
Osberg, K. M. (1993). Virtual reality and education: A look at both sides of the sword.
http://www.hitl.washington.edu/publications/r-93-7/
Papert, S. (1993). The Children's machine: Rethinking School in the Age of the
Computer, New York: Basic Books.
Psotka, J. (1994). Immersive Tutoring Systems: Virtual Reality and Education and
Training.
http://alex.immersion.army.mil/
Salzman, M. C., Dede, C., Loftin, R. B., Chen, J. (1999). A Model for Understanding
How Virtual Reality Aids Complex Conceptual Learning. Accepted for publication in
Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments
http://www.virtual.gmu.edu/pdf/presence.pdf
50
Traub, D. (1994). The promise of virtual reality for learning. In Loeffler, C. E. &
Anderson, T. (eds), The Virtual Reality Casebook. Van Nostrand Reinhold. New York.
Spring, M. (1991). Informing with Virtual Reality. In Helsel M. & Ruth J. P., Virtual
Reality: The Practice, and Promise, Westport, CT: Meckler
Winn, W. (1993). A conceptual basis for educational applications of virtual reality. HITL
Laboratory.
http://www.hitl.washington.edu/publications/r-93-9/
Winn, W. and Bricken, W. (1992). Designing Virtual Worlds for use in Mathematics
Education: The example of experimental algebra. Educational Technology, 32(12),
pp.12-19.
Winn, W. (1995). The Virtual Reality Roving Vehicle Project. T.H.E Journal, 23(5), pp.
70-75.
Winn, W., Hoffman, H., Hollander, A., Osberg, K., Rose, H., Char, P. (1997). The Effect
of Student Construction of Virtual Environments on the Performance of High- and Low-
Ability Students, Human Interface Technology Laboratory
http://www.hitl.washington.edu/publications/r-97-6/
51