Principles of Behavior Modification
Principles of Behavior Modification
~ ~ - .
* • • - - . ^ _ -~t - -r— . _. _
J
:' '.'!.';
!*! ,*' '.
Digitized by the Internet Archive
in 2012
http://archive.org/details/principlesofbehaOOband
~^&r
A7
Principles of Behavior Modification
Albert Bandura STANFORD UNIVERSITY
Principles
of
Behavior
Modification
Dallas •
Montreal Toronto London Sydney
• • •
to Ginny, Mary, and Carol
While this book was being written the author contributed chapters on
modeling processes to Volume II of Advances in Experimental Social
Psychology (Bandura, 1965) and to the Ciba Foundation Symposium:
The Role of Learning and Psychotherapy (Bandura, 1968). Chapter 3
contains a revised and updated version of some of the material that
originally appeared in the latter publications.
Many people contributed in one way or another to this venture. To
Ted Rosenthal and Rogers Elliott, who read preliminary versions of the
manuscript and made many valuable suggestions, I offer my sincere
thanks. I am also indebted to countless students and colleagues who have
helped through collaborative research and sharing of ideas to enhance the
value of what I have written. I owe a special personal debt to my former
student and colleague, Richard Walters, who died tragically at the height
of his productive career. Although he never read what I have written
here, our lively discussions during collaborative projects did much to
clarify some of the theoretical issues discussed in this book.
The preparation of this volume involved considerable work, and I
PREFACE V
1 Causal Processes 1
Theoretical Interpretations of
Reinforcement Processes 217
Essential Components of Reinforcement Practices 225
Ethical Implications of Reinforcement Practices 234
Applications of Contingency Systems 242
Social Organizational Application of
Reinforcement Contingencies 261
Summary 282
REFERENCES 284
6 Extinction 355
The discussion thus far has been concerned with the deviant behavior
of members of groups, who mutually support and reinforce each other's
ideologies and actions. Some individuals display gross behavioral eccen-
tricities that appear totally inexplicable; persons from different sub-
groups who do not share the same normative systems are apt to view
these eccentricities as pathological manifestations. Even in these in-
stances, when the idiosyncratic social-learning history for the behavior is
Cornelison, Terry, & Fleck (1958) report a case, for example, in which
sibling schizophrenics believed, among other strange things, that "dis-
agreement" meant constipation. This clearly inappropriate conceptual be-
Causal Processes 5
gotta go with you/ Myself, I don't want to go, but when they start talkin'
about what they gonna do, I say, 'So, he isn't gonna take over my rep.
I ain't gonna let him be known more than me.' And I go ahead [p. vii]."
I would have stabbed him. That would have gave me more of a build-up.
People would have respected me for what I've done and things like
that. They would say, There goes a cold killer' [p. 8]." Similar reinforce-
Causal Processes 9
but they are then invoked as the causes of the same behavioral referents.
For these reasons, so-called symptomatic behavior can be more ade-
quately explained in terms of social learning and value theory than
through inappropriate medical analogizing. An extended account of a so-
cial-learning taxonomy of behavioral phenomena generally subsumed un-
der the term "psychopathology" is presented elsewhere ( Bandura, 1968 )
The preceding discussion reviewed some of the principal factors deter-
mining the attribution of sickness to deviant behavior. Similar social
judgment processes are, of course, involved in the attachment of de-
scriptive labels such as aggression, altruism, dependency, or achievement
to particular response patterns.
—
bizarre pattern of behavior which was developed, maintained, and sub-
sequently eliminated in a schizophrenic woman simply by altering its re-
—
inforcing consequences was interpreted erroneously as a symptomatic
manifestation of complex psychodynamic events by diagnosticians who
were unaware of the specific conditions of reinforcement regulating the
patient's behavior.
Unfortunately, the exact antecedents of deviant behavior are rarely
known, and in the absence of powerful techniques that permit adequate
control over behavioral phenomena, clinical endeavors have until re-
cently lacked the self-corrective features necessary for eliminating weak
or invalid theories of psychopathology. As a consequence, rival inter-
pretations of social behavior have 4
decades retained a secure status
for
with little risk that any one type of theory might prove more cogent
than another.
In recent years there has been a fundamental departure from con-
ventional views regarding the nature, causes, and treatment of behavioral
dysfunctions. According to this orientation, behavior that is harmful to
the individual or departs widely from accepted social and ethical norms
is viewed not as symptomatic of some kind of disease but as a way
that the individual has learned to cope with environmental and self-
He was not only afraid of horses biting him . . . but also of carts,
must have been his father; and it was this that had enabled him to
regard Fritzl as a substitute for his father when the accident happened at
Gmunden. ... In the end his father went into the lumf symbolism,
and recognized was an analogy between a heavily loaded cart
that there
and a body loaded with faeces, between the way in which a cart drives
out through a gateway and the way in which faeces leave the body, and
so on . .\ [p. 126-127].
We can now recognize that all furniture- vans and drays and buses
were only stork-box carts, and were only of interest to Hans as being
symbolic representations of pregnancy; and that when a heavy or heavily
loaded horse fell down he can have seen in it only one thing — a child-
birth, a delivery. Thus the falling horse was not only his dying father
but also his mother in childbirth [p. 128].
these characteristics were derived from the circumstance that the anxiety
originally had no reference at all to horses but was transposed on to them
secondarily [italics added] and had now become fixed upon those ele-
ments of the horse complex which showed themselves well adapted for
This exposition fails to account for the variation in both the pattern
and the intensity of Hans's anxiety reactions under different circum-
stances. In fact, the case data provide considerable evidence that ex-
ternal cues served as the primary eliciting and controlling stimuli for
Hans's phobic responses rather than simply as incidental targets for pro-
jected feelings.
Let us consider the major traumatic episode which was related to the
onset of Hans's phobia. While out walking with his mother Hans saw a
large bus-horse fall and kick with its feet. He was terrified and thought
the horse was killed in the accident. There were three important ele-
ments in this stimulus —
complex large horse, heavily loaded transport
vehicle, and horse and vehicle traveling at high speed. The occurrence
and intensity of Hans's subsequent phobic reactions varied predictably
as a function of the specific patterning of these three critical stimulus
elements. Hans was more frightened of large dray horses than of small
horses, more frightened of a rapidly moving vehicle than of a slowly
moving one, more frightened of heavily loaded vehicles than of empty
ones, and frightened when a horse-drawn cart made a turn:
fall down.
father: So you're not afraid with a small cart?
hans: No. I'm not afraid with a small cart or with a post-office van. I'm
most afraid too when a bus comes along.
Causal Processes 13
father: What did you think when the horse fell down?
hans: Now it'll always be like this. All horses in buses'll fall down . . .
[p. 49].
father: When the horse fell down, did you think of your daddy?
hans: Perhaps. Yes. It's possible . . .
[p. 51].
loaded full up, then I'm afraid; but when there are two horses and
it's loaded full up, then I'm not afraid.
father: Are you afraid of buses because there are so many people
inside?
hans: Because there's so much luggage on the top.
father: When Mummy was having Hanna, was she loaded full up too?
[pp. 90-91].
The Oedipal interpretation fails not only to account for the discrimi-
native pattern of Hans's phobic behavior but also to explain satisfac-
torilywhy he was afraid of railways and locomotives as well, a phobia
which probably generalized from the transport vehicle stimulus complex.
The psychoanalytic interpretation would demand that the locomotive and
the railway tracks were likewise symbolic representations of the castrating
father and the impregnated mother.
The conceptual structure of causal sequences in psychodynamic the-
ories of behavior is beleaguered by serious problems. An amorphous
internal determinant cannot possibly account for the remarkable variety
of heterogeneous behaviors as well as changes both in their incidence
and magnitude under different stimulus conditions, toward different
persons, and at different times. How can a horse phobia be attributed
to an underlying Oedipus complex and projected castration fears if a
person responds phobically to one horse pulling a heavy loaded vehicle,
but is relatively unafraid of two horses drawing a loaded vehicle? When
diverse stimulus inputs produce correspondingly diverse behavioral ex-
pressions then any internal mediators implicated in the causal sequence
must be at least equally specific and their activation must be closely
regulated by discriminative environmental stimuli.
The conceptual difficulties associated with psychodynamic formula-
14 CAUSAL PROCESSES
leaves one ill-prepared for devising and implementing methods that are
successful in promoting favorable social change. Had educational proc-
esses, which also depend upon neurophysiological functioning, been his-
torically misconstrued as principally medical phenomena, our society-
would undoubtedly be faced with the same critical shortage of educa-
tional facilities and well-trained instructional personnel that characterizes
our current "mental health" enterprises.
Although the designation of behavioral eccentricities as manifesta-
Causal Processes 17
( 1961 ) cogently points out, continued adherence to this analogy has be-
come a serious hindrance. Many people who would benefit greatly from
psychological treatment avoid seeking help because they fear being stig-
matized as mentally deranged, which often carries deleterious social con-
sequences. Those who are compelled by chronic distress to seek a solu-
tion to their interpersonal problems are typically ascribed a sick role
and are regarded as relatively helpless, dependent, and incompetent in
managing their daily lives. By having their behavioral deviations treated
as expressions of internal psychic pathologies they are thereby relieved
of the natural consequences of their actions. In this connection, it is
derived from social-learning principles are now available that can effec-
tively eliminate such phobias in any person in a few sessions (Bandura,
Blanchard, & 1968). Psychological centers that offer brief and
Ritter,
highly efficacious treatments for specific behavioral dysfunctions would
provide valuable therapeutic services to many persons who would other-
wise endure unnecessary restrictions in certain areas of their psychologi-
cal functioning.
18 CAUSAL PROCESSES
signals and other guiding cues, for example, and who remained indif-
ferent to important social and symbolic stimuli, would suffer a painfully
rapid extinction.
sponses, but in cases where they are under stimulus control, social-learn-
ing procedures that are capable of neutralizing the emotion-arousing
properties of stimulus events offer the most direct and effectiye treatment.
Autonomic responses can be most readily brought under the control of
environmental stimuli through classical conditioning operations. If a
formerly ineffective or conditioned stimulus is closely associated with an
unconditioned stimulus capable of eliciting a given physiological re-
sponse, the former stimulus alone gradually acquires the power to evoke
the physiological response or Although some types of
its equivalent.
autonomic responses are more difficult to condition than others, almost
every form of somatic reaction that an organism is capable of making.
including respiratory and heart-rate changes, increases in muscular ten-
sion, gastrointestinal secretions, vasomotor reactions, and other indices
of emotional responsiveness (Bykov, 1957; Kimble. 1961), has been classi-
cally conditioned to innocuous stimuli. Environmental eyents can like-
100 r
90
80
?0
Io
60
I
_c
"5 50
o
u>
(0
£ 40
o
I 30
20
10
r
J I I L
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24
Baseline Interaction Solitary I Interaction reinforced
reinforced play
reinforced I
Days
are rewarded each time they exhibit the desired behavior (continuous
schedule), and later the reinforcement is completely withdrawn, they
are likely to increase responsiveness for a brief period of time and then
to display a rapid decrease in performance, often accompanied by emo-
tional reactions.
Sometimes behavior is reinforced only after a specified period of time
has elapsed schedule). Pay periods, eating schedules,
(fixed-interval
recreational times, and other regularly scheduled rewarding activities
illustrate the temporal cycles of reinforcement regulating some aspects
of human behavior. When rewards are dispensed on a fixed temporal
basis the payoff is the same regardless of the amount of behavior pro-
28 CAUSAL PROCESSES
& Liebert, 1966; Rosenhan, Frederick, & Burrowes, 1968) show that self-
rewards are most sparingly administered when stringent performance
standards have been consistently modeled and imposed, whereas social-
learning conditions in which persons both model and reinforce lenient
behavioral demands produce generous self-reward patterns of behavior.
Interpretation of Causal Processes 35
3000
Boys
2500
fe 2000
| 1500
o
E
i 1000
500
that they do not exist, but that they are not relevant in a functional anal-
ysis.We cannot account for the behavior of any system while staving
wholly inside it; eventually we must turn to forces operating upon the
organism from without [p. 35]."
The common practice of invoking spurious inner states or agents as
determiners of behavior has also produced justifiable wariness of inferen-
tial variables. After a given response pattern has been attributed to the
action of a psychic homunculus, the search for controlling conditions
promptly ceases. Although the use of the more colorful animistic entities
in explanatory schemes is declining, the tendency to offer new descriptive
labels for behavioral phenomena in the guise of explanations remains a
flourishing practice.
The phenomena results primarily from
relative neglect of experiential
their limited accessibility.Thought processes are directly accessible only
to the person within whom thev occur and therefore their presence, ab-
sence, and exact nature cannot be independently verified. As a conse-
quence, one is forced to rely upon verbal self-reports and other indirect
indices of events occurring at a private level. In discussions of the me-
thodological problems and theoretical issues regarding symbolic processes
it is customarv to belabor the limitations and inaccuracies of self -reports.
40 CAUSAL PROCESSES
the concluding chapter, persons can not only reliably discriminate inter-
nal events, but they can manipulate them by making self-reinforcement
contingent upon their occurrence. Furthermore, thought-induced affective
reactions may be successfully employed for purposes of controlling one's
own overt behavior. In the above instances implicit activities constitute
either important phenomena in their own right or causal antecedents
rather than mere internal accompaniments of behavioral and environ-
mental events.
There are innumerable psychological processes in which internal me-
diating events must occur before external stimuli will exercise control
over overt performances. Verbal mediators, in the form of self -instruc-
tions, implicit categorizing responses, or linkages through common word
associates, are perhaps the most prevalent symbolic regulators of behav-
ior. Persons must often relv on verbal self-control when external stimuli
for correct responses are absent (Bern, 1967; Luria, 1961). Also, in many
forms of conceptual behavior or in semantic generalization persons dis-
awake in hell before the morning. After some time I had a little rest,
and then, actuated by the same spirit, I took a like position on the floor,
where I remained, until I understood that the work of the Lord was
perfected, and that now my salvation was secured; at the same time the
guidance of the spirit left me, and I became in doubt what next I was to
do. I understood that this provoked the Lord, as if I was affecting igno-
rance when
knew what I was to do, and, after some hesitation, I heard
I
the command, to "Take your position on the floor again then," but I had
no guidance or no perfect guidance do so, and could not resume it. I
to
was told, however, that my
depended upon my maintaining that
salvation
position as well as I could until the morning; and oh! great was my joy
when I perceived the first brightness of the dawn, which I could scarcely
believe had arrived so early [pp. 28-29].
would befall him, for that I was a prophet of the Lord. He was not a
whit shaken by my address, so, after again and again adjuring him, by
the desire of the Spirit whose word I heard, I seized one of his arms,
desiring to wither it; my words were idle, no effect followed, and I was
ashamed and astonished.
Then, thought I, I have been made a
fool of! But I did not on that
account mistrust the doctrines by which had been exposed to this error. I
there were not wanting voices to suggest to me, that the reason why
the miracle had I had not waited
failed, was, that for the Spirit to
guide my action when
word was spoken and that
the I had seized the
man's arm with the wrong hand ... [p. 33].
the power of the Holy Spirit, redeem myself, and rid myself of the spirits
of blasphemy and mockery that had taken possession of me.
The way in which I was tempted to do this was by throwing myself
on the top of my head backwards, and so resting on the top of my head
and on my feet alone, to turn from one side to the other until I had
broken my neck. I suppose by this time I was already in a state of fever-
ish delirium, but my good sense and prudence still refused to undertake
again attempted what I was enjoined. The man seized me, I tore myself
from him, telling him it was necessary for my salvation; he left me
and went down stairs. I then tried to perform what had begun; but
I
now I found, either that I could not so jerk myself round on my head,
or that my fear of breaking my neck was really too strong for my faith.
In that case I then certainly mocked, for my efforts were not sincere.
order to get rid of my two formidable enemies; and then again I was
told to drink water, and the Almighty was satisfied; but that I was not
satisfied (neither could I be sincerely, for I knew that I had not fulfilled
ant came up with an assistant and they forced me into a straight waist-
coat. Even then I again tried to resume the position to which I was again
Social Learning as a Reciprocal Influence Process 45
instead, one analyzed the data for the amount of aversive stimulation cre-
ated by each subject, then the environment becomes the changeworthy
event that may vary considerably for different subjects and at different
times for the same subject. Within the framework of environmental anal-
ysis, one might, for instance, administer alcohol to one group of subjects
in the Sidman paradigm and water to another, and then compare the
ceding stimulus act on the part of one person was the major determinant
of the other person's response. In approximately 75 percent of the in-
stances, hostile behavior elicited unfriendly responses, whereas cordial
antecedent acts seldom did. Aggressive children thus created through
their actions a environment, whereas children who displayed
hostile
friendly interpersonal of response generated an amicable social
modes
milieu. These findings demonstrate that persons, far from being ruled by
an imposing environment, play an active role in constructing their own
reinforcement contingencies through their characteristic modes of re-
sponse. The theon' of social interaction advanced by Thibaut & Kelley
(1959) relies heavily upon mutual reinforcement contingencies. Research
Social Learning as a Reciprocal Influence Process 47
dox overlooks the fact that reciprocity is rarely perfect, since one's be-
havior is not the sole determinant of subsequent events. Furthermore,
fused to go to school. He lay in bed, ordered his sister to get his break-
fast, bring his clothes, and struck her when she disobeyed [p. 163]. . . .
in turn creates the very conditions likely to perpetuate it. Thus while
Symptom Substitution
Her constant and compulsive pacing, holding a broom in the manner she
does, could be seen as a ritualistic procedure, a magical action. . . .
Her broom would be then: (1) a child that gives her love and she gives
50 CAUSAL PROCESSES
him in return her devotion, (2) a phallic symbol, (3) the sceptre of an
omnipotent queen . . . this is a magical procedure in which the patient
carries out her wishes, expressed in a way that is far beyond our solid,
mutually extinguish one another's limited social behaviors. Nor have con-
ventional methods of behavioral change had much beneficial impact upon
the widespread problems of alcoholism, drug addiction, and a host of other
major social problems which, in some instances, require modification of
social systems rather than the behavior of isolated individuals.
Even in the restricted sample of persons who consult psychotherapists
and are accepted for treatment, the dropout rates and the estimates of
behavioral change for those who remain in treatment give little cause
.
CRITERIA OF CHANGE
The two-thirds improved figure, which has been widely and uncriti-
cally accepted as the typical base rate of change accompanying interview
therapies, overestimates the amount of benefit that people actually derive
from such treatment. The criteria upon which judgments of therapeutic
efficacy are usually based leave much to be desired. In many instances
psychotherapists' global impressions of their results serve as the major
indicants of outcome. Considering that such ratings reflect upon thera-
pists' professional competence, it is reasonable to assume that therapists
ioral changes as a measure of success had not only been seriously neg-
lected, but often derogated as superficial. Indeed, there exists no other
avowedly humanitarian enterprise in which clients' major concerns are
so cavalierly disregarded. Whatever personality changes a psychothera-
pist may choose to promote, they should be considered of dubious value
if thev arc not reflected in To take an analo-
the client's social behavior.
gous example, medical treatments on the basis of physicians' impres-
that,
sions and other ambiguous indicants, supposedly effected profound phys-
iological change's but, in actuality, produced no evident changes in clients'
suffering and physical dysfunctions, would be summarily dismissed as
both ineffectual and misleading. Clearly, objective measures of changes
in behavior constitute the most stringent and the most important criteria
of the power of a given treatment method. Since the areas of functioning
that require modification may differ extensively from person to person,
global, all-purpose measures of change must be replaced by behavioral
criteria that are specific and individually tailored to the treatment objec-
tives selected by the client (Pascal & Zax, 1956). Findings of comparative
studies utilizing indices of improvement based on behavioral change
(Fairweather, 1964; Lazarus, 1961; Paul, 1966) yield success rates that
are substantially below the legendary two-thirds improved figure cus-
tomarily quoted for interview therapies.
Moreover, improvement figures usually present a misleading picture
of the effectiveness of interviewmethods because dropouts have been in-
variably excluded from statistical analyses. When a particular procedure
yields a relatively high attrition rate, discarding terminators in assessing
psychotherapy becomes especially critical. Let us assume, for instance,
that of 100 persons who
entered treatment, 80 withdrew after several
initial interviews, while all of the remaining 20 cases exhibited significant
percent effective when, in fact, only 20 percent of the cases have been
benefited. It will be recalled that a sizable percentage of clients who enter
into interview treatments terminate after a few visits.
sults that a hospitalized patient has little to gain from undergoing client-
centered treatment and may, in fact, suffer some slight losses if his thera-
pist happens to be lacking in amiability.
Faced with growing evidence that interview therapies have limited
efficacy, some researchers concluded that outcome studies should be held
in abeyance while intensified efforts are made to elucidate the process
underlying these procedures. Outcome studies were therefore promptly
downgraded, investigators became absorbed in minute analyses of verbal
interchanges between therapists and their clients and, in the absence of
any promising alternatives, the traditional practice's not only survived es-
sentiallv unaltered but were professionally sanctified. The possibility that
a conversational approach to the modification of deviant behavior is in-
herently too weak to justify exhaustive process studies was rarely enter-
tained. Under conditions where a given treatment procedure exercises
weak behavioral control many other extraneous variables (e.g., personality
characteristics of therapists, social attributes of clients, minor technical
variations in procedures) singly or in combination will emerge as deter-
and others do not, whether or not thev are involved in formal therapy.
Comparative investigations of the attributes of clients who terminate
treatment prematurely with those of clients who remain and improve are
particularly relevant in this respect. Relative to persons who continue in
treatment, terminators typically come from lower socioeconomic levels,
are nonconforming toward authority figures, are impulsive, relatively non-
anxious, report a history of antisocial behavior, present deficits in verbal
and emotional responsiveness, exhibit a relative inability to establish and
to maintain soeial relationships, and acknowledge little contingency be-
tween their own behavior and the actions of others toward them. By con-
trast, remainers generally come from higher socioeconomic levels, are bet-
ter educated, are willing to explore their personal problems, are responsive
to soeial reinforcement, are suggestible, introspective, relatively anxious,
and self-condemning (Auld & Myers. 1954; Frank et al.
self-dissatisfied,
1957; Hiler, 1954;Imber et al.. 1955; Katz, Lorr. & Rubinstein. 1958; Lorr,
Katz. & Rubinstein, 1958; McNair, Lorr, & Callahan, 1963; Rubinstein &
Lorr, 1956). Except for socioeconomic and educational indices which—
generally correlate significantly with continuation in treatment but tend
to be unrelated to outcome —most of the latter personality variables are
likewise predictive of rated subsequent improvement in psychotherapy.
Thus the type of people who continue to participate and improve in psy-
chotherapy have attributes similar to those of persons who, in laboratory
studies of conformity, attitude change, and conditionability, show more
responsiveness to almost any form of social influence procedure (Berg &
Bass, 1961; Biderman & Zimmer, 1961; Janis & Hovland, 1959).
The above findines indicate that the social characteristics of clients,
Efficacy of Conventional Methods of Behavioral Change 59
rather than the chosen psychotherapeutic method, are the main determi-
nants of the successes of traditional psychotherapy. This may explain why,
in spite of wide conceptual divergences, all "schools" of psychotherapy
achieve very similar rates of improvement (Appel et al., 1951; Miles,
Barrabee, & Finesinger, 1951 ) and, although differences may
occasionally
slightly favor the treated groups ( Frank, Gliedman, Imber, Stone, & Nash,
1959; Learv & Harvey, 1956), the magnitude of behavioral change exhib-
ited by nontreated cases is not substantially less than change in clients
who have undergone some traditional form of psychotherapy (Bergin,
1966 ) The types of clients who derive some benefit from participation in
.
Summary
This chapter has presented a social-learning interpretation of the
mechanisms regulating behavior and contrasts this approach with the-
ories that tend to assign causal properties to hypothetical internal forces.
The differences in conceptual models are especially striking in explana-
tions of deviant behavior that have traditionally been depicted as symp-
tomatic by-products of a quasi-mental disease. From a social-learning
may be detrimental to the individual or that
perspective, behaviors that
depart widely from accepted social and ethical norms are considered not
as manifestations of an underlying pathology but as ways, which the per-
son has learned, of coping with environmental and self-imposed demands.
Psychopathology is not solely a property inherent in behavior but re-
flects the evaluative responses of societal agents to actions that violate
prescribed codes of conduct. The social labeling of a given response pat-
tern as a pathological expression is. in fact, influenced by numerous sub-
jective criteria including the aversiveness of the behavior, the social at-
tributes of the deviator, the normative standards of persons making the
judgments, the social context which the behavior is performed, and a
in
host of other factors. Consequently, the same response pattern may be
diagnosed as "sick" or may be normatively sanctioned and considered
emulative by different groups, at different times, or in different environ-
mental settings. Considering the arbitrary and relativistic nature of the
social judgment and definition of deviance, the main value of the normal
versus abnormal dichotomy lies in guiding the social and legal actions of
societal agents concerned with the maintenance of an efficiently function-
ing society. This dichotomy, however, has little theoretical significance,
because no evidence exists that the behaviors so dichotomized are either
qualitatively different or are under the control of fundamentally different
variables.
Personality theories generally assume that energized traits and con-
cealed motivational states impel behavior in a variety of directions. These
hypothetical internal conditions tend to be regarded as relatively autono-
mous of external stimulation and their relationship to behavior remains
somewhat loose. In social-learning theory both deviant and prosocial be-
haviors are acquired and maintained on the basis of three distinct regula-
tory systems.
Some response patterns are primarily under external stimulus control.
Autonomic responsiveness, such as changes in cardiovascular and gastro-
intestinal reactions, and emotional behavior, can be effectively brought
under the control of environmental events through their contiguous asso-
ciation with either direct or vicarious affective experiences. Instrumental
References 63
REFERENCES
Appel, K. E., Lhamon, T. W., Myers, J. M., & Harvey, W. A. Long term
psychotherapy. In Psychiatric treatment. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins,
1951. Pp. 21-34.
Auld, F., Jr., & Myers, ]. K. Contributions to a theory for selecting psycho-
therapy patients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1954, 10, 56-60.
Ay lion, T., & Azrin, N. H. The measurement and reinforcement of behavior
64 CAUSAL PROCESSES
Neither traits nor types, as concepts, have any real existence. They are
merely words, and words do not exist in the eye of the observer nor in
the people observed. A man can not be said to have either a type or a
trait, but he can be said to fit either a type or a trait. At present the fit
will be inexact, for dimensions of personality have not yet been quantified
well enough to permit of accurate measurement. In the case of height,
the measurement can be precise, and little confusion results from saying
that a man has a certain height. Observation and concept are so closely
related that the phrase is not ordinarily understood to mean more than
72 VALUE ISSUES AND OBJECTIVES
urable terms, becomes readily apparent when the methods have suc-
it
ceeded, when they have failed, and when they need further development
to increase their potency. This self-corrective feature is a safeguard
against perpetuation of ineffective approaches, which are difficult to re-
tire if the changes they are supposed to produce remain ambiguous.
thal, 1955; Truax, 1966; Winder et al., 1962), it is surprising that many
therapists continue to view the psychotherapeutic process as one that does
not involve behavioral influence and control.
In later writings, Rogers 1956), a leading proponent of the anticontrol
(
& YVestlev. 1955 . The members of his reference group, in turn, serve to
reinforce and to uphold Ins self-prescribed standards of conduct. A person
who chooses a small select reference group that does not share the values
of the general public may appear highly individualistic and "inner-
directed." whereas in fact he is very much dependent on the actual and
—
Ross. & Ross. 1963 and they respond to new situations in a manner con-
(,
sistent with the model's dispositions even though they have never ob-
served the model's behavior in response to the same stimuli Bandura &
Harris. 1966; Bandura & McDonald. 1963; Bandura & Mischel, 1965'.
These findings indicate that after the model's attitudes and behavioral
attributes have been adopted, he continues to influence and indirectly to
control the subject's actions, though he is no longer physically present.
In fact, in Rogers' 1951 I
conceptualization of maladjustment, introjected
parental values are construed as continuing pathological influences that
maintain disturbing incongruities in the clients' self-structure. However,
after internalized parental values are supplanted by adoption of the
therapist's attitudes and standards, the client is flatteringly portrayed
—
by the psychotherapist as self-actualized, flexibly creative, and self-
directed!
Much of the controversy between Rogers and Skinner centers around
theirown value preferences for others. Skinner advocates that people be
made "truly happv. secure, productive, creative, and forward-looking";
Rogers argues in favor of self-direction and self-actualization of potential-
ities as the prescribed objective of social influence. It might be noted
within this highly structured interaction, the therapist must exercise re-
and the types of options that they are likely to consider for themselves.
In many instances, for example, persons are unable to participate freely
in potentially rewarding social interactions because of severe phobias;
they are unable to engage in achievement, aggressive and heterosexual
activities; and they deny themselves socially permissible gratification be-
cause of austere, self-imposed standards of conduct. Treatment programs
designed to reduce rigid self-restraints are rarely viewed as ethically
objectionable, since they tend to restore spontaneity and freedom of
choice among various options of action. Ethical issues arise only if a
change agent uses his influence selfishly or to make his clients socially
irresponsible.
Behavioral deficits also greatly restrict freedom of choice and other-
wise curtail opportunities for self-direction. Persons' positions in various
status and power hierarchies are to a large extent determined by their
social, educational, and vocational competencies. The degree of control
that one can exercise over one's own activities, the power to form and
to modify one's environment, and the accessibility to, and control over,
desired resources increase with higher status positions. Persons who have
developed superior intellectual and vocational capabilities enjoy a wide
latitude of occupational choices; they are granted considerable freedom
to regulate both their own activities and the behavior of others; and they
have the financial means of obtaining additional privileges that further
increase their autonomy. By contrast, high school dropouts who lack
sociovocational proficiencies are relegated to a subordinate status, in
which not only is their welfare subject to arbitrary external controls, but
they are irreversibly channeled into an economic and social life that
further restricts their opportunities to use their potentialities and to affect
their own life circumstances. Eliminating such behavioral deficits can
substantially increase the level of self-determination in diverse areas of
social functioning.
Societally imposed restrictions on freedom of self-expression occur as
responses to deviant behavior that violates legal codes. Chronic alcoholics,
drug addicts, sexual deviates, delinquents, psychotics, and social noncon-
formists and activists may have their liberties revoked for fixed or indefi-
nite periods when their public actions are judged to be socially detri-
mental and therefore to be subject to social control. Special ethical prob-
lems are most likely to arise wherever restoration of his freedom is made
contingent upon the individual's relinquishing socially prohibited pat-
change acts in opposition to the society
terns of behavior. If an agent of
which supports him institutionally, then he evades his broader social
responsibilities with which he has been entrusted. If, on the other hand,
he imposes conditions upon his captive clients designed to force con-
formity to social norms, he is subverting the client's right to choose how
Factors Impeding Specification of Objectives 87
he shall live hislife. These moral dilemmas are less difficult to resolve in
commend them for their achievements or to penalize them for their trans-
gressions. It would be more from this
sensible, point of view, to praise and
to chastise the external determinants. But since these events are also un-
avoidably determined by prior conditions, the analysis results in an infinite
regression of causes. Some degree of freedom is possible within a deter-
ministic view if it is recognized that a person's behavior is a contributing
factor to subsequent causal events. It will be recalled from the previous
discussion of reciprocal influence processes that individuals play an active
role 1
in creating their own controlling environment.
From a social-learning point of view freedom is not incompatible with
determinism. Rather a person is considered free insofar as he can partly
influence future events bymanaging his own behavior. One could readily
demonstrate that a person can, within the limits of his behavioral capabil-
ities and environmental options, exercise substantial control over his
social lifeby having him plan and systematically carry out radically dif-
ferent courses of action on alternate daws. Granted that the selection of
a particular course of behavior from available alternatives is itself the
result of determining factors, a person can nevertheless exert some con-
trol over the variables that govern his own choices. Indeed, increasing use
is being made of self-control systems (Ferster, Nurnberger, & Levitt, 1962;
Harris, 1969; Stuart, 1967) in which individuals regulate their activities
own wishes by deliberate self-management of reinforce-
to fulfill their
ment contingencies. The self-control process begins by informing indi-
viduals of the types of behaviors they will have to practice to produce
desired outcomes, of ways in which they can institute stimuli to increase
the occurrence of requisite performances, and of how they can arrange
self-reinforcing consequences to sustain them. Behavioral change pro-
cedures that involve role enactment also depend upon the self-determina-
tion of outcomes through clients' regulation of their own behavior and
the environmental contingencies that reciprocally influence it. Contrary
to common belief, behavioral approaches not only can support a human-
istic morality, but because of their relative effectiveness in establishing
self-determination these methods hold much greater promise than tradi-
tional procedures for enhancement of behavioral freedom and fulfillment
of human capabilities.
Factors Impeding Specification of Objectives
another. Each school gives its own particular brand of insight. Whose
are the correct insights? The fact is that patients treated by analysts of
all these schools may not only respond favorably, but also believe
strongly in the insights which they have been given. Even admittedly
'inexact' interpretations have been noted to be of therapeutic value!
Moreover, the problem is even more complicated than this; for, depend-
ing upon the point of view of the analyst, the patients of each school
seem to bring up precisely the kind of phenomenological data which
confirm the theories and interpretations of their analysts! Thus each
theory tends to be self-validating. Freudians elicit material about the
Oedipus Complex and castration anxiety, Adlerians about masculine
strivings and feelings of inferiority, Horneyites about idealized images,
Sullivanians about disturbed interpersonal relationships, etc. The fact
is that in so complex a transaction as the psychoanalytic therapeutic
process, the impact of the patient and the therapist upon each other,
and particularly that of the latter upon the former, is an unusually pro-
found one. What the analyst shows interest in, the kinds of questions he
asks, the kind of data he chooses to react to or to ignore, and the inter-
pretations he makes, all exert a subtle but significant suggestive impact
upon the patient to bring forth certain kinds of data in preference to
others [Marmor, 1962, p. 289].
94 VALUE ISSUES AND OBJECTIVES
2.2
2.0
1.8
1.6
1.4
1.2
1.0
0.8
0.6 h
0.4
0.2
0.0
Moderate High Extreme
Discrepancy
(masculine for girls) than they judged themselves to be. Later the
students rated themselves again so that changes in their self-evaluations
could be assessed. Students in the low credibility condition likewise
completed the initial self-ratings, received one of the three levels of
discrepant interpretations concerning their masculine status, and then
repeated the self -evaluation. In these cases, however, the judgments were
made in a decrepit basement office by a scrawny youngster on the basis
of casual observation.
The results, presented graphically in Figure 2-1, show that under
high credibility conditions the more divergent the interpretation the
greater the change in self-attitudes; on the other hand, when inter-
amount of attitude
pretations issued from a source of low credibility, the
change decreased with increasing discrepancy between the judgments
of the participants.
Although the generality of the self-evaluative conforming behavior
cannot be determined from the findings of the foregoing study, it never-
theless suggests strongly that people are willing to adopt erroneous
underlying attributes suggested to them by prestigious specialists. It
might be supposed that the persuasive efforts of psychotherapists would
be especially effective because the same interpretations are made re-
peatedly during prolonged treatment and are directed not only toward
assumed unconscious determinants but also toward clients' resistances
against the prompted insights.
Suggestive communications offered by prestigious agents under con-
ditions of ambiguity and high personal distress may be well suited for
98 VALUE ISSUES AND OBJECTIVES
imparting insights to clients, but after the self-beliefs have been socially
induced their maintenance is strongly governed by existing conditions
of reinforcement. Results of innumerable verbal conditioning experiments
and analyses of client-therapist interactions, which have been cited
earlier, furnishample evidence that psychotherapists selectively reinforce
conformity to their own opinions about the causes of behavior, and that
clients can readily secure their therapists' appreciation and approval by
reiterating the appropriate insights.
It would seem from the findings presented above that interpretive
psychotherapies may primarily represent a conversion of the client to the
therapist's point ofview rather than a process of self-discovery. It is not
surprising, therefore, that insight can be achieved without helping the
client with the difficulties for which the client originally sought help.
There is no reason to expect, for example, that a stutterer converted to
Freudianism, Jungianism, Existentialism, Behaviorism or to any other —
theoretical system —
will begin to speak fluently. His stuttering is more
likely to be eliminated bv necessary relearning experiences than by the
gradual discovery of predetermined insights. To account for the lack of
relationship between insight and social behavior, different varieties of
insights have been distinguished. On the one hand, there is "intellectual
insight," which is believed to exist when cognitive responses are present
but the accompanying social or emotional behavior is absent. Then there
is "emotional insight" which is typically defined in terms of the effects
cies can markedly influence overt performances. Unlike the arbitrary and
enigmatic nature of psychodynamic events, the controlling function of
environmental contingencies is readily demonstrable and amenable to
testing and verification.
plied until an adequate conception of mental health and the nature of the
"good life" is developed. The fact that a universal conception of mental
health would require value standardization is usually obscured by the
abstract nature of the discourse. On the other hand, when the issues are
cast in a more specific form, it becomes apparent that the search for
uniform criteria of "good" functioning is not only a fruitless pursuit; it is
also one that would raise serious ethical concerns if standards were ever
officially adopted and imposed on the populace. Who is to prescribe what
is the "healthiest" occupational activity, the "healthiest" political or reli-
gious belief, the "healthiest" style of living, the "healthiest" form of marital
or social relationships, or the "healthiest" artistic preferences?
People differ widely across social groups and over time in their views
of the ideal pattern of life. Indeed, as noted in the introductory chapter,
modes judged abnormal and a source of distress in
of behavior that are
one social group may be regarded
as commendable and emulative in
another subculture. In a society that values individualism the "good life"
may assume a wide variety of acceptable patterns. Although some com-
mon elements might be abstracted from the heterogeneity, the distillation
would most likely yield a set of general bland attributes. Social scientists
can make their greatest contributions in the ethical domain by assessing
the consequences of different styles of life. Such information would pro-
vide others with useful bases for making value choices.
persuasion), they are less appropriate for situations in which the learner
selects his own objectives. In fact, awareness of and commitment to speci-
fied outcomes that are shared by agents of change tend to enhance posi-
Decision Processes in the Selection of Objectives 101
where.
Special problems in goal selection also arise when persons are confused
over their own values and purposes, or when they exhibit severe deficits
in reality-oriented behaviorand low capacity for communication. It might
be questioned whether such persons are capable of selecting meaningful
objectives for themselves. Fairweather, Sanders, Maynard, and Cressler
( 1969 ) have shown in their work with chronic schizophrenics that such
individuals can successfully participate in the selection of personal goals
provided the alternatives are defined in comprehensible terms of per-
formance and the clients are given responsibility for decision-making that
affects their daily lives. Some grossly deviant persons, of course, may
refuse to seek modifications of any sort. Often they constitute threats to
Decision Processes in the Selection of Objectives 103
SEQUENTIAL DECISION-MAKING
Decisions about objectives are not irrevocable. Consequences resulting
from behavioral changes representing the initially selected outcomes may
lead to revision of subsequent aims. The initial objectives should be
assigned a provisional status in order to provide the client opportunities
104 VALUE ISSUES AND OBJECTIVES
After the goals and requisite learning experiences have been estab-
lished, another set of decisions arises in the selection of change agents
who, by virtue of their specialized training or close relationship with the
client, are best suited to implement treatment procedures. In traditional
clinical practice, changes in behavior are characteristically effected by
professional psvehotherapists in office settings, mainly through the modifi-
cation of verbal-symbolic contents. Although the decided preference for
artificial environments and symbolic substitutes for naturally occurring
grams conducted in natural settings are far superior to similar ones ad-
ministered in psychiatric institutions ( Fairweather, et al., 1969).
It follows from principles of generalization that the optimal conditions
for effecting behavioral changes,from the standpoint of maximizing trans-
would require people to perform the desired patterns of be-
fer effects,
havior successfully in the diverse social situations in which the behavior
is most appropriate. On the other hand, when treatment is primarily cen-
tered around verbal responses expressed in an invariant, atypical context
one cannot assume that induced changes will necessarily generalize to
real-life performances to any great extent.
Issues regarding the locus and content of treatment are closely linked
with the choice of change agents. From a social-learning perspective those
who have the most intensive contact with the client, if given appropriate
training, can serve as the most powerful agents of change. Their potential
efficacy derives from the fact that in such positions they exercise consider-
able control over the very conditions that regulate the behavior. Success-
ful applications of this general principle are provided in new approaches
to child therapy in which parents are utilized in the treatment of their
own (Hawkins, Peterson, Schweid, & Bijou, 1966;
children's behavior
O'Leary, O'Leary, 1967; Patterson, Ray, & Shaw, 1968; Risley &
& Becker,
Wolf, i966; Russo, 1964; Wahler, Winkel, Peterson, & Morrison, 1965;
Williams, 1959).
In a well-designed program a thorough behavioral analysis is first con-
ducted to identify the social conditions that maintain the various behavior
disorders. Next the deviant response patterns to be eliminated and the
desirable behaviors to be strengthened are clearly specified. The parents
are then given a detailed description of how they must alter their charac-
teristic ways of reacting to their child's behavior to achieve therapeutic
changes. This typically involves a reversal of parents' differential rein-
forcement practices. Whereas the child's deviant behavior previously
commanded attention and his desirable behavior received little special
notice, the parents are advised now to ignore or to reinforce negatively his
aberrant behavior and to respond positively to the forms of behavior they
wish to promote. In the case of deficit problems (Lovaas, 1966), a pro-
gram of graduated modeling is also devised, while in fear-motivated dis-
orders (Bentler, 1962) a graduated reexposure to threatening situations
isimplemented by the parents.
It should be noted in this context that attempts to modify behavior
through giving advice, have an extended history, mainly negative. Its pal-
try outcomes probably result from the nature of the advice given and from
the fact that instructions alone are of limited effectiveness unless they are
combined with other procedures that help to alter and to support parental
.
100
First Second
1 experimental experimental
period period
m 80 -
1 1
a
tt 60
E 40 -
:
/J
Figure 2-2. Number of 10-second intervals in which the boy displayed objec-
tionable behavior during each one-hour session. Hawkins et al., 1966.
Value conflicts arise not only in formulating common goals, but also
in selecting methods for inducing preferred changes. In one way or an-
other decisions aremade about how much social objectives are advanced
through coercive methods, through positive reinforcement of appropriate
behaviors, or through provision of models for emulation who exemplify
the desired behavioral patterns.
The notion of planned social change is likely to arouse in people's
minds negative associations of regimentation, invasion of privacy, and
curtailment of self-determination. In fact, as Benne (1949) and Mann-
heim (1941) have cogently argued, collectively planned social change,
rather than being anti-individualistic, generally safeguards and extends
human freedom. The need for social planning stems from the fact that, in
many areas of behavioral functioning, people's outcome experiences are
jointly determined by each other's actions. Thus if motorists did not have
the benefit of traffic codes they would repeatedly obstruct and injure one
another, whereas agreeing to a few sensible regulations greatly enhances
their personal welfare and freedom of movement. Without some social
controls over human behavior, personal freedoms would be continuously
in jeopardy. Paradoxically, zealous individualists often attack the very
social institutions that are established to protect freedom of self-expres-
sion.
Problems of dysfunctional restraints often occur when social control is
Summary
One of the major obstacles to the development of effective change
programs arises from the failure to specify precisely what is to be accom-
plished, or the more common practice of defining the intended goals in
terms of hypothetical internal states. When the aims remain ambiguous,
learning experiences are haphazard, and whatever procedures are con-
sistently appliedtend to be determined more by personal preferences of
change agents than by clients' needs.
The appropriate methods and learning conditions for any given pro-
gram of behavioral change cannot be meaningfully selected until the
desired goals have been clearly defined in terms of observable behavior.
Rapid progress is further assured by designating intermediate objectives,
which delineate optimal learning sequences for establishing the compo-
112 VALUE ISSUES AND OBJECTIVES
REFERENCES
Alexander, F. Psychoanalysis and psychotherapy; developments in theory,
techniques, and training. New York: Norton, 1956.
Alexander, F. The dynamics of psychotherapy in the light of learning theory.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 1963, 120, 440-448.
Asch, S. E. Social psychology. New York: Prentice-Hall, 1952.
Ayllon, T., & Azrin, N. H. Reinforcement and instructions with mental patients.
Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1964, 7, 327-331.
Bandura, A. Social learning through imitation. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska
symposium on motivation: 1962. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press,
1962. Pp. 211-269.
Bandura, A. Vicarious processes: A case of no-trial learning. In L. Berkowitz
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology. Vol. II. New York:
Academic Press, 1965. Pp. 1-55.
Bandura, A. Social-learning theory of identificatory processes. In D. A. Goslin
(Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research. Chicago: Rand
McNally, 1968.
Bandura, A., Blanchard, E. B., & Ritter, B. The relative efficacy of desensiti-
zation and modeling approaches for inducing behavioral, affective, and
attitudinal changes. Unpublished manuscript, Stanford University, 1968.
Bandura, A., Grusec, J. E., & Menlove, F. L. Vicarious extinction of avoidance
behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1967, 5, 16-23.
Bandura, A., & Harris, M. B. Modification of syntactic style. Journal of Experi-
mental Child Psychology, 1966, 4, 341-352.
Bandura, A., & Huston, A. C. Identification as a process of incidental learning.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1961, 63, 311—318.
Bandura, A., & Kupers, C. J. Transmission of patterns of self-reinforcement
through modeling. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1964,
69, 1-9.
Bandura, A., Lipsher, D. H., & Miller, P. E. Psychotherapists' approach-
avoidance reactions to patients' expressions of hostility. Journal of Con-
sulting Psychology, 1960, 24, 1-8.
Bandura, A., & McDonald, F. J. The influence of social reinforcement and
the behavior of models in shaping children's moral judgments. Journal
of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 67, 274-281.
Bandura, A., & Mischel, W. The influence of models in modifying delay of
gratification patterns. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1965,
2,698-705.
Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. Imitation of film-mediated aggressive
models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 66, 3-11.
Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. Adolescent aggression. New York: Ronald, 1959.
Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. Social learning and personality development.
New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1963.
Benne, K. D. Democratic ethics in social engineering. Progressive Education,
1949, 26, 201-207.
Bentler, P. M. An infant's phobia treated with reciprocal inhibition therapy.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1962, 3, 185-189.
114 VALUE ISSUES AND OBJECTIVES
of Matheties. 1962.87-109. 1.
Hart, B. M.. Reynolds. N. }.. Baer. D. M., Brawlev, E. R., & Harris. F. R. Effect
References 115
McConnell, J.
V., Cutler, R. L., & McNeil, E. B. Subliminal stimulation: An
overview. American Psychologist, 1958, 13, 229-242.
Mager, R. F. Preparing objectives for programmed instruction. San Francisco:
Fearon, 1961.
Mahrer, A. R. The goals of psychotherapy. New York: Appleton-Century-
Crofts, 1967.
Mannheim, K. Man and society in an age of reconstruction. New York: Har-
court, Brace & World, 1941.
Marmor, J. Psychoanalytic therapy as an educational process: Common de-
nominators in the therapeutic approaches of different psychoanalytic
schools. In J. H. Masserman (Ed.), Science and psychoanalysis. Vol. 5.
Psychoanalytic education. New York: Grune & Stratton, 1962. Pp. 286-
299.
Murray, E. J. A content-analysis method for studving psychotherapy. Psycho-
logical Monographs, 1956, 70 (.13, Whole No. 420).
Mussen, P. H., & Parker, A. L. Mother-nurturance and girls' incidental imita-
tive learning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1965, 2, 94-97.
O'Leary, K. D., O'Leary, S., & Becker, W. C. Modification of a deviant sibling
interaction pattern in the home. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1967, 5,
113-120.
Page, M. L. The modification of ascendant behavior in preschool children.
University of Iowa Studies in Child Welfare, 1936, 12, 7-69.
Patterson, C. H. Control, conditioning, and counseling. Personnel and Guid-
ance, 1963, 41, 680-686.
Patterson, G. R., & Brodsky, G. A behavior modification programme for a child
with multiple problem behaviors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
chiatry, 1967, 7, 277-295.
Patterson, G. R., Ray, R., & Shaw, D. Direct intervention in families of deviant
children. Unpublished manuscript, University of Oregon, 1968.
Pentony, P. Value change in psychotherapy. Human Relations, 1966, 19, 39-46.
Pratt, C. C. The logic of modern psychology. New York: Macmillan, 1939.
Risley, T., & Wolf, M. M. Experimental manipulation of autistic behavior and
generalization into the home. In R. Ulrich, R. Stachnik, & J. Mabry (Eds.),
Control of human behavior. Glenview, 111.: Scott, Foresman, 1966. Pp.
193-198.
Rogers, C. R. Client-centered therapy. Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1951.
Rogers, C. R., & Skinner, B. F. Some issues concerning the control of human
behavior: A symposium. Science, 1956, 124, 1057-1066.
Rosen, J. N. Direct analysis: Selected papers. New York: Grune and Stratton,
1953.
Rosenthal, D. Changes in some moral values following psychotherapy. Journal
of Consulting Psychology, 1955, 19, 431-436.
Rosenthal, T. L. Anxiety-proneness and susceptibility to social influence. Un-
published doctoral dissertation, Stanford University, 1963.
Russo, Adaptations in behavioural therapy with children. Behaviour Research
S.
and Therapy, 1964, 2, 43-47.
Shoben, E. J., Jr. The therapeutic object: Men or machines? Journal of Coun-
seling Psychology, 1963, 10, 264-268.
Speisman, J. C. Depth of interpretation and verbal resistance in psychotherapy.
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1959, 23, 93-99.
References 117
Kohlberg ( 1963 ) on the other hand, reserves the term "identification" for
,
as imitation.
It is possible, of course, to draw distinctions among numerous descrip-
tive terms based on antecedent, mediating, or behavioral variables. One
might question, however, whether it is advantageous to do so, since there
is every indication that essentially the same learning process is involved
120 MODELING AND VICARIOUS PROCESSES
ditions under which modeling occurs; and because of the vehement reac-
tions against the instinct doctrine, until recently even the phenomena
subsumed under the concept tended to be either repudiated or widely
ignored in theoretical explanations of learning processes.
REINFORCEMENT THEORIES
With the advent of reinforcement principles, theoretical explanations
of learning shifted the emphasis from classical conditioning to instrumen-
tal response acquisition based on reinforcing outcomes. Theories of mod-
point of view was most clearly expounded by Miller & Dollard (1941) in
the classic publication, Social Learning and Imitation. According to this
122 MODELING AND VICARIOUS PROCESSES
50
•4 15 25 35
Sessions
since one would not expect similarity cues to lose their rewarding value
that suddenly. Rather, the intrinsic rewards arising from precise response
duplication should sustain imitative behavior forsome time even in the
absence of externally administered reinforcers. Studies including more
extensive variations in incentive conditions, indeed, show that generalized
imitation is under incentive control rather than its inherently re-
largely
warding value. Berkowitz (1968) found that retarded children who were
rewarded for imitative responses only at the end of the experimental ses-
sion displayed a high rate of matching behavior as long as the food re-
wards were present in the room. During sessions when food was not
displayed, imitation dropped significantly; it was promptly re-established
by introducing the sight of food.
It should be noted that the laboratory phenomenon that has been la-
beled "generalized imitation" involves only imitation across responses
under conditions where subjects are instructed to repeat the experiment-
er's behavior. A more stringent test of generalized imitation would in-
126 MODELING AND VICARIOUS PROCESSES
ing tendencies that will not be restricted to the treatment setting but will
generalize to other, more natural settings. On the basis of a secondary
reinforcement hypothesis, the treatment program should include consid-
erable imitation training under a generous schedule of reinforcement.
The assumption made is that the more reinforcement a person experiences
for behavioral matching, the more reinforcing it will become for him to
imitate in any situation. On the basis of a discrimination hypothesis, on
the other hand, theprogram would involve only as much reinforcement
as necessary to establish matching behavior, which would then be re-
is
program.
The Skinnerian analysis of modeling phenomena relies entirely upon
the standard three-component paradigm S d -» R -» S r where S d denotes
,
havior in either the model ( Bandura, 1965a; Kanfer, 1965; Parke & Wal-
ters, 1967 ) or the subjects ( Kanareff & Lanzetta, 1960; Lanzetta & Kana-
reff, 1959; Mctz, 1965; Schein, 1954; Wilson & Walters, 1966); whereas
following the display of aggressive behavior; in the second, the model was
generously rewarded with delectable treats and lavish praise; the third
condition presented no response consequences to the model. A post-expo-
sure performance test of imitation revealed that the reinforcement con-
tingencies applied to the model's responses resulted in differential degrees
of matching behavior. Compared to subjects in the model-punished con-
dition, children in themodel-rewarded and the no-consequence groups
spontaneously performed a significantly greater variety of imitative re-
sponses. Moreover, bovs reproduced substantially more of the model's
behavioral repertoire than girls, the differences being particularly marked
in the model-punished treatment (Figure 3-2).
Following the performance test, children in all three groups were of-
fered highly attractive incentives contingent upon their reproducing the
model's responses in order to promote performance of what they had ac-
quired through observation. As shown in Figure 3-2, the introduction of
Positive incentive
No incentive
o 4
ai 3
DC
I 2
Q
with regard to the affective feedback theory. Even though a model's re-
warding qualities are equally associated with the different types of be-
haviors he performs, modeling effects nevertheless tend to be specific
rather than general. That is, model nurturance enhances the reproduction
of some responses, has no effect upon others, and may actually diminish
the adoption of still others (Bandura, Grusec & Menlove, 1967a). A lim-
ited study by Foss (1964), in which mynahs were taught unusual whis-
tles played on a tape recorder, also failed to confirm the proposition that
ferentation studies (Taub, Bacon, & Berman, 1965; Taub, Teodoru, Ell-
man, Bloom, & Berman, 1966) also show that responses can be acquired,
performed discriminatively, and extinguished even though sensory soma-
tic feedback is surgically abolished by limb deafferentation. It would seem
from these findings that the acquisition, integration, facilitation and in-
hibition of responses can be achieved through central mechanisms inde-
pendently of peripheral sensory feedback.
132 MODELING AND VICARIOUS PROCESSES
problem, Mowrer (1960) has conjectured that the initial scanning and
selection of responses may occur primarily at the symbolic rather than at
the action level.
Human functioning would be inflexible and unadaptive if responsive-
ness were controlled by affectivity inherent in the behavior itself. Because
social responsiveness is highly discriminative, it is extremely doubtful that
behavioral patterns are regulated by affective qualities implanted in be-
havior.To take aggression as an example, hitting responses directed to-
ward parents, peers, and inanimate objects differ little, if at all; neverthe-
less, physically aggressive responses toward parents are generally strongly
whereas physical aggression toward peers is freely expressed
inhibited,
(Bandura, 1960; Bandura & Walters, 1959). Moreover, in certain well-
defined contexts, particularly in competitive, physical contact sports such
as boxing, people will easily and maintain unattenuated, physi-
initiate
cally aggressive behavior. One would,
therefore, predict more accurately
the expression or inhibition of identical aggressive responses from knowl-
edge of the stimulus context (e.g., church, athletic gymnasium), the ob-
ject (e.g., parent, priest, policeman, or peer), and other cues that signify
predictable consequences, than from assessment of the affective value of
aggressive behavior. It has been amply demonstrated (Bandura, 1968)
that the selection and performance of matching responses is mainly gov-
erned by anticipated outcomes based on previous consequences that were
directly encountered, vicariously experienced, or self-administered.
Although feedback conceptions of modeling do not require a response
to be performed before it can be learned, they nevertheless fail to explain
the acquisition of matching behavior when reinforcers are not dispensed
either to the model or to observers. Moreover, a vast majority of the re-
sponses that are acquired observationally are not affectively valenced.
This is exemplified by studies of observational learning of perceptual-
motor tasks from filmed demonstrations ( Sheffield & Maccoby, 1961 ) that
do not contain positive or aversive stimuli essential for the classical con-
ditioning of emotional responses. Mowrer has, of course, pointed out that
sensory experiences not only classically condition positive or negative
emotions, but also produce conditioned sensations or images. In most
cases of observational learning images or other forms of symbolic repre-
sentations of modeling stimuli may be the only important mediators. Sen-
sory-feedback theories of imitation may therefore be primarily relevant to
instances in which the modeled responses incur relatively potent reinforc-
Theoretical Conceptions of Observational Learning 133
CONTIGUITY-MEDIATIONAL THEOBIES
automobile the bell alone tends to elicit imagery of the car. Under condi-
tions where stimulus events are highly correlated, as when a name is con-
sistently associated with a given person, it is virtually impossible to hear
the name without experiencing imagery of the person's physical char-
acteristics. The findings of studies cited above indicate that, in the course
of observation, transitory perceptual phenomena produce relatively en-
during, retrievable images of modeled sequences of behavior. Later re-
instatement of imaginal mediators serves as a guide for reproduction of
matching responses.
The second representational system, which probably accounts for the
notable speed of observational learning and long-term retention of
modeled contents by humans, involves verbal coding of observed events.
134 MODELING AND VICARIOUS PROCESSES
80 Labeling •-
Imagery •«
Verbalization •-
Control •
70
§ 60
50
40
30
Immediate Delayed
Reproduction Reproduction
ATTENTIONAL PROCESSES
Since repeated contiguous stimulation alone does not always result in
response acquisition, it is evident that additional conditions are required
being repeatedly rewarded for imitating certain types of models and not
rewarded for matching the behavior of models possessing different char-
acteristics, persons eventually learn to discriminate between modeling
cues that signify differential probabilities of reinforcement. Thus, models
who have demonstrated high competence (Gelfand, 1962; Mausner,
1954a, b; Mausner & Bloch, 1957; Rosenbaum & Tucker, 1962), who are
purported experts (Mausner, 1953) or celebrities (Hovland, Janis, &
Kelley, 1953), and who possess status-conferring symbols (Lefkowitz,
Blake, & Mouton, 1955) are likely to command more attention and to
serve as more influential sources of social behavior than models who lack
these qualities. Other distinctive characteristics, such as age (Bandura
& Kupers, 1964; Hicks, 1965; Jakubczak & Walters, 1959), sex (Bandura,
Ross, & Maccoby & Wilson, 1957; Ofstad, 1967; Rosenblith,
Ross, 1963a;
1959, 1961), socialpower (Bandura, Ross, & Ross, 1963b; Mischel &
Grusec, 1966), and ethnic status (Epstein, 1966), which are correlated
with differential probabilities of reinforcement, likewise influence the
degree to which models who possess these attributes will be selected for
emulation.
The affective valence of models, as mediated through their attractive-
ness and other rewarding qualities (Bandura & Huston, 1961; Grusec &
Mischel, 1966), may augment observational learning by eliciting and
Theoretical Conceptions of Observational Learning 137
socioeconomic and racial status (Beyer & May, 1968); and countless
studies have shown that the effects of modeling stimuli are partly de-
termined by the sex of observers. Persons who have been frequently
rewarded for displaying matching behavior (Miller & Dollard, 1941;
Schein, 1954) are also apt to be most attentive to modeling cues. Finally,
motivational variables and transitory emotional arousal significantly alter
perceptual thresholds and in other ways facilitate, impede, and channel
observing responses (Bandura & Rosenthal, 1966; Easterbrook, 1959;
Kausler & Trapp, 1960).
It is difficult to evaluate from performance measures alone whether
the effects of observer characteristics reflect differences in degree of
observational learning or in willingness to perform what has been learned.
Results of several studies employing a learning analysis of modeling
(Bandura, Grusec, & Menlove, 1966; Grusec & Brinker, 1969; Maccoby
& Wilson, 1957) disclose that observer characteristics can serve as deter-
minants of observational learning.
Viewers' observing behavior can be effectively enhanced and focused
through arrangement of appropriate incentive conditions. Persons who
are informed in advance that they will later be asked to reproduce a
given model's responses and rewarded in terms of the number of elements
performed correctly would be expected to pay much closer attention to
relevant modeling stimuli than persons who are exposed to the same
modeled events without any predisposition to observe and to learn them.
The facilitative influence of incentive set on observational learning will
be most operative under exposure to multiple models requiring selective
attentiveness to conflicting cues. Indeed, incentive control of observing
behavior can, in most instances, override the effects of variations in ob-
server characteristics and model attributes. It should be noted, however,
138 MODELING AND VICARIOUS PROCESSES
that in the present theory reinforcement variables, to the extent that they
influence the acquisition process, do so principally by augmenting and
sustaining attentiveness to modeling cues.
In addition to attention-directing variables, stimulus input conditions
( i e., rate, number, distribution, and complexity of modeling stimuli pre-
sented to observers) will regulate the acquisition of modeled responses
to some extent. The observer's capacity to process information sets
definite limitson the number of modeling cues that can be acquired
during a single exposure. Therefore, if modeling stimuli are presented
at a rate or level of complexity that exceeds the observer's receptive
capabilities, observational learning will necessarily be limited and frag-
mentary. Under such conditions repeated presentations of the modeling
stimuli would be required in order to produce complete and precise
response matching.
Finally, the rate and be affected
level of observational learning will
by the discriminabilitv modeling stimuli. Modeled characteristics that
of
are highly discernible can be more readily acquired than subtle attributes
which must be abstracted from heterogeneous responses differing on
numerous stimulus dimensions. In highly intricate response systems, such
as language 1
behavior, for example, children typically encounter consider-
able difficulty in acquiring linguistic structures because the identifying
characteristics of different grammatical constructions cannot be readily
distinguished within extremely diverse and complex utterances. However,
when verbal modeling cues are combined with procedures designed to
increase syntactic discriminabilitv (Bandura & Harris, 1966; Lovaas,
1966a; Odom, Liebert, & Hill, 1968) relatively complicated linguistic
patterns of behavior can be acquired and modified observationally.
In therapeutic applications of modeling procedures observational
learning is often retarded by discrimination failures arising from defi-
ciencies in cognitiveskills, sensory-motor handicaps, or faulty prior learn-
RETENTION PROCESSES
The discussion thus far has been concerned with sensory registration
and symbolic coding of modeling stimuli. Another basic component func-
tion involved in observational learning, but one that has been virtually
ignored in theories of imitation, concerns the retention of modeled events.
In order to reproduce social behavior without the continued presence
Theoretical Conceptions of Observational Learning 139
On the other hand, observers who lack some of the necessary components
will, in all probability, display only partial reproduction of a model's
behavior. In such cases, the constituent elements first must be established
through modeling and then, in a stepwise fashion, increasingly complex
compounds can be acquired imitatively. Thus, for example, when a mute
autistic child failed to imitate the word baby,
the therapist modeled the
component sounds, and were established through
after these elements
imitation, the child readilv reproduced the word baby (Lovaas, 1966b).
As will be illustrated later, graduated modeling procedures have proved
highly effective for modifying gross behavioral deficits.
In many instances modeled response patterns have been acquired
and retained in representational forms but they cannot be reproduced
behaviorally because of physical limitations. Few basketball enthusiasts
could ever successfully match the remarkable performances of a towering
professional player regardless of their vigilance and dutiful rehearsal.
Accurate behavioral enactment of modeling cues is also difficult to
achieve under conditions where the model's performance governed by is
A person may acquire and retain modeled events and possess the
capabilities for skillful execution of modeled behavior, but the learning
may rarely be activated into overt performance if negative sanctions or
unfavorable incentive conditions obtain. Under such circumstances, when
positive incentives are introduced observational learning is promptly
translated into action ( Bandura, 1965b ) Incentive variables not only reg-
.
ulate the overt expression of matching behavior, but they also affect ob-
servational learning by exerting selective control over the modeling cues
to which a person is most likely to be attentive. Further, they facilitate
selective bv activating
retention coding and rehearsal of
deliberate
modeled responses that have high utilitarian value.
It is evident from the foregoing discussion that observers do not func-
Establishment of New Response Patterns through Modeling 143
70 Reinforced model •-
Reinforcement alone
60
C
<1>
E
#50
—)
"to
| 40
Z-30
O
£20
10
1 2 3
Treatment phase
Blocks of 4 Scores
& Branch, 1969), both types of influences produce their effects through
verbal modeling and they differ only in the explicitness with which the
required responses are defined. As might be expected, greater perform-
ance gains are achieved when the desired behavior is clearly specified
than when it must be inferred from a few examples.
The basic components in the development of complex integrated units
of behavior are usually present in subjects' behavioral repertoires as prod-
ucts either of maturation or of prior observational learning and in-
strumental conditioning. For example, persons can produce a variety of
elementary sounds as part of their natural endowment. By combining
existing sounds one can create a novel and exceedingly complex verbal
response such as supercalifragilistieexpialidocious. Similarly, people are
endowed with the capacity to move their fingers, but intricate sequen-
tial arrangements of movements are required to perforin a piano concerto.
produce new responses which the\ have never formed or seen before,
that learning principles cannot account for innovative behavior. Theories
employing modeling principles have often been similarly questioned on
the mistaken assumption that exposure to the behavior of others can
produce at most mimicry of specific modeled responses.
In most experimental investigations of modeling processes a single
model exhibits a limited set of responses, and observers are subsequently
tested for precise response duplication under similar stimulus conditions.
These restricted experimental paradigms cannot yield outcomes that ex-
tend bevond the particular responses demonstrated. On the other hand,
studies employing more complex procedures indicate that innovative be-
havior, generalized behavioral orientations, and principles for generating
novel combinations of responses can be transmitted to observers through
exposure to modeling cues. Under conditions in which opportunities are
provided to observe the behavior of heterogeneous models (Bandura,
Ross, & Ross, 1963b), observers typically display novel patterns of be-
Establishment of New Response Patterns through Modeling 149
objects on tables, on
on boxes and on other objects, and simultane-
chairs,
ously verbalized the common prepositional relationship between these
objects, a child would eventually discern the grammatical principle. He
could then easily generate a novel grammatical sentence if a toy hip-
popotamus were placed on a xylophone and the child were asked to
describe the stimulus event enacted.
Unlike social responses which are often readily acquired, language
learning is considerably more difficult, because sentences represent com-
plex stimulus patterns in which the identifying features of syntactic
structures cannot be easily discriminated. The influential role of both
modeling and discrimination processes in language development is
shown by findings of an experiment (Bandura & Harris, 1966) designed
to alter th^ syntactic style of young children who had no formal gram-
matical knowledge of the linguistic features that were manipulated. The
grammatical constructions chosen to be modified were the prepositional
phrase, which has a high base rate of occurrence, and the passive voice,
which is grammatically more complex and rarely displayed by young
children.
As might be expected, social reinforcement, even when combined
with a strong attentional set to identify the characteristics of "correct"
sentences, was ineffective in increasing the use of passives in sentences
generated by the children in response to a set of simple nouns. The
majority of subjects did not produce a single passive sentence, and con-
sequently, no responses occurred that could be reinforced. Nor were the
children able, within the relatively brief exposure period, to discern the
critical syntactic category simply from observing a model construct a
series of passive sentences. In contrast, children generated significantly
more passives when verbal modeling cues were combined with procedures
designed to increase syntactic discriminability. The most powerful treat-
ment condition was one in which the attentional set was induced,
modeled passive constructions were interspersed with some sentences
in the active voice so as to enhance differentiation of relevant grammati-
cal properties, and both the model and the children were rewarded for
passive constructions. In the case of a syntactic category as common as
prepositional phrases, reinforcement together with an active attentional
set were effective in altering children's usage of prepositions, but model-
ing cues were not a significant contributory factor.
Further evidence for the influential role of modeling processes in lan-
guage acquisition is provided by naturalistic studies involving sequential
Establishment of New Response Patterns through Modeling 151
program is based on the view that the total rehabilitation of autistic and
schizophrenic children can be best achieved through the establishment of
stimulus functions which make one amenable to social influence. This
process primarily involves developing children's responsiveness to model-
ing cues, increasing the discriminative value of stimulus events so that
children attend and respond appropriately to aspects of their environ-
ment that they have previously ignored, and endowing social approval
and other symbolic stimuli with reinforcing properties. After a strong
modeling set has been created, and children have become adequately
responsive to environmental influences, the major task of broadening
children's social and intellectual competencies can be effectively carried
out by parents, teachers, and other agents. Since interpersonal communi-
cation and social learning are extensively mediated through language, the
development of linguistic skills is also selected as a central objective of
treatment.
As noted previously, modeling outcomes depend upon accurate per-
ceptual input. Autistic children generally show defective reception of
external stimuli, a deficitwhich has been attributed by some researchers
to neurophysiological impairment (Hutt, Hutt, & Ounsted, 1965; Rim-
land, 1962 ) It cannot be determined from the available data whether the
.
the child so he cannot easily ignore the responses that are being modeled.
Second, during the session the child is not permitted to avoid the thera-
peutic task by withdrawal or by resorting to bizarre activities. If neces-
sary, the therapist physically restrains the childfrom turning away, he
establishes eye contact by asking the and he may
child to look at him,
withhold positive attention, address the child sharply, or even slap him
on the thigh to terminate stereotyped bizarre behavior. Firm intervention
of this type, if thoughtfully employed, may serve a therapeutic function
when failure to respond appropriately to situational demands reflects un-
willingness rather than inability. This is dramatically illustrated in a tell-
BYE-BYE
ARM
BUBBLE
NiGHT
CORN
HAT
RUN
GO
COOKIE
HAIR
HAND
MAMA
MOMMY
MY
MILK
ME
MEAT
MORE
NO
WHY
BREAD
BOTTLE
TA
BED
BILLY
DOLL
DA
DADDY
BOY
BALL
(BLOW)
we we we WE
E_
oo oo oo oo oo OO
baby baby baby baby baby BABY
10 11 12 13 14 15 16
Figure 3-5. Rate of verbal imitation by a previously mute autistic child during
the first 26 days of training. The words and sounds are printed in lower case
letters on the days they were introduced and trained, and in capital letters on
the days they were mastered. Lovaas, Berberich, Perloff, & Schaeffer, 1966.
100
Prepositional training will illustrate the basic discriminations that are de-
veloped. Behavioral matching of a verbal stimulus can be more easily
achieved by autistic children than verbally labeling nonverbal events.
Therefore, initially the adult gives a verbal instruction involving a prepo-
sition (e.g., "Put the ball inside the box")" and the child is rewarded
but it may result in speech that is lacking in spontaneity and overly de-
Establishment of New Response Patterns through Modeling 157
pendent upon specific external cuing. To remove this problem, after the
requisite skills for generative grammatical speech have been established,
children are taught to use their language to initiate and maintain social
interactions, to express their feelings and desires, and to seek and ex-
change information about their environment. Self-generated spontaneous
speech is initially fostered in several ways. First, by withholding desired
objects and activities until children verbalize their wants, they are taught
to influence and control their environment verbally; second, they are
encouraged to develop comments and stories about activities depicted
pictoriallv in magazines and books and are rewarded for increasingly
elaborate and novel verbalizations; third, they are asked to recount, in
detail, past experiences; and finally, the concepts that they have learned in
the formal tasks are extended into informal daily interactions. Indeed, as
treatment progresses the formal training procedures are incorporated into
more natural interpersonal interactions, where verbal approval, affectional
expressions, plav activities and a sense of accomplishment replace primary
rewards as major reinforcing events.
Self-care skills, plav patterns, appropriate sex-role behaviors, intellec-
tual and interpersonal modes of behavior can be established in
skills,
the treatment of adult psychotics. This is all the more surprising con-
sidering that a majority of the chronic cases suffer from debilitating be-
havioral deficits which must be overcome if they are to function effec-
tively in community life. The relative neglect of this powerful approach
probably results in large part from therapists' strong allegiances solely to
operant conditioning methods or to interview procedures in which a great
deal of time is devoted to analyzing patients' ineffectual behaviors.
Establishment of New Response Patterns through Modeling 159
18
16
14
v>
| ,2
o
Q.
c/J
Q)
DC 10
O
a>
i 8
2 h
12 Symbolic modeling
Control condition
Nonisolate baseline
10
he were, in fact, the person portrayed in the sketch. For example, a pas-
sive nonassertive person may be assigned an active assertive role. The new
behavioral patterns, which are usually in marked contrast to the client's
customary modes of responses, are consistently enacted for several weeks
or some other preselected period. This phase of the program is structured
to the client as representing brief experimentation with, rather than per-
manent adoption of, new characteristics. Moreover, the client is never
told that he should be the new character, only that he should act like
him on a trial basis. The emphasis on brief experimentation and simula-
tion is considered essential for minimizing the initial threat of making
sweeping changes in one's mode of life.
Prescribing a role by itself will be of limited value unless a person
knows how to translate it into concrete actions under a variety of circum-
stances. In Kelly's approach the treatment sessions, usually scheduled on
alternate days, are mainly devoted to rehearsing the prescribed role as
it might apply to everyday events involving vocational and social relation-
Therapist and client usually alternate in the role enactment. Through such
role-reversal the client not only benefits from the therapist's demonstra-
tion of skillful ways of relating to others, but he also experiences how
people are likely to be affected by the behaviors being modeled.
After new forms of responsiveness to different types of interpersonal
164 MODELING AND VICARIOUS PROCESSES
situations have been adequately rehearsed, and the client's actual experi-
ences in implementing the role have been thoroughly discussed, the
client decides whether or not he wishes to adopt the new role behaviors
on a more lasting basis. If he has found the new role effective and wishes
to go on with the program the behavioral rehearsals are continued as
long as necessary. With further experience the client becomes increasingly
skillful and comfortable in the new role behaviors until eventually they
dered by their inadequacies, then these methods are unlikely to fare any
better than interpretive interview approaches that similarly accentuate
the negatives. On the other hand, treatment approaches that employ
modeling procedures to establish effective modes of behavior often lack
an adequate transfer training program in which clients are provided
with opportunities to test their newly acquired skills under conditions
likely to produce rewarding consequences. If change agents themselves
portray requisite interpersonal competencies, and arrange optimal con-
ditions for their clients to learn and to practice more effective means of
coping with potential problems, then this type of approach is almost
certain to prove successful.
Before turning to other issues we should like to comment briefly on
the nature of the effects produced through modeling processes. When
people are deliberately instructed to observe and to reproduce either the
behavior exemplified by others or an imaginatively reconstructed role,
there may be a tendency to view the resultant changes as feigned and
superficial. In fact, as will be shown in the concluding chapter, role
enactment techniques have proved to be one of the most effective means
of inducing stable affective and attitudinal changes. These findings pro-
vide support for the view that self-evaluative and cognitive events may be
partly epiphenomena arising from one's competencies and the conse-
quences of one's behavior. Modeling, even under simulated conditions,
can have far-reaching effects.
are partly accountable for their life situations because of their objection-
able and duplicitous behavior, the therapist himself consistently models
self-disclosure and personal accountability.
During the course of conversational treatment some of the therapists'
attitudes and personal preferences are inevitably revealed through their
selective responsiveness and interpretive comments (ParlofI, Iflund, &
Goldstein, 1960). These inferred attitudes are likely to be emulated by
clients even though therapists -may strive to maintain neutrality in the
value domain. Some suggestive evidence of this effect is reported by
Rosenthal (1955) who found that clients who were judged as showing
greatest clinical improvement changed their values in the areas of sex,
aggression and authority in the direction of their therapists' values
whereas clients who were rated unimproved became less like their
therapists. The occurrence of value congruences during the course of
therapy is shown by Pentony (1966). It cannot be determined from
also
these data, however, whether the value similarities are attributable to
modeling or to differential reinforcement of clients' verbalizations; un-
doubtedly both kinds of influence processes are operative.
There have been several recent demonstrations that the classes of
responses that traditional psychotherapists are interested in modifying
can be significantly influenced by modeling procedures. Schwartz &
Hawkins (1965) found that adult schizophrenics whose emotional state-
ments were positively reinforced in group therapy increased affective
expressions when their group was provided with two patient models who
frequently verbalized their feelings; under the same reinforcement con-
ditions affective responsiveness was decreased when the added models
displayed predominantly nonaflective verbalizations. Marlatt, Jacobsen,
Johnson, & Morrice (1966) found that interviewees were more inclined
to reveal personal problems after witnessing a brief waiting-room conver-
sation in which a model's self-disclosure was either accepted or socially
rewarded by the interviewer than if the model's behavior was discouraged
or subjects had no exposure to a problem-admitting model.
One of the obstacles to efficient conduct of interview therapy arises
from the fact that clients are usually confused about what they are sup-
posed to do in order to achieve beneficial effects, and verbal explanations
inadequatelv convey the requisite role behaviors. This ambiguity can be
easily overcome by providing clients with concrete examples of appro-
priate therapeutic responsiveness (Marlatt, 1968a, 1968b). In several
studies Truax and his colleagues (Truax & Carkhuff, 1967) demonstrated
that clients who listened to tape-recorded excerpts exemplifying self-
variety of personality tests than did clients who received the same type
of treatment without the initial modeling experience.
The foregoing studies indicate that modeling procedures can be suc-
cessfully employed to induce changes in verbal behavior. However, con-
sidering the weak relationships that exist between alterations at the verbal
—
level whether in the form of value preferences, verbal statements, or
—
endorsements of personality test items and nonverbal modes of response,
it would seem that models could be used far more advantageously to
studies have attempted to identify the social cues that are most influ-
ential in producing vicarious arousal, while still others have been designed
to elucidate the social-learning conditions whereby social cues become
endowed with emotion-eliciting potency.
One of the earliest studies of vicarious affective arousal was reported
by Dysinger & Ruckmick (1933), who measured the autonomic responses
of children and adults to movie scenes depicting dangerous situations and
romantic-erotic displays. The findings showed that scenes of danger, con-
flict, or tragedy elicited the greatest emotional reactions among young
children, but responsiveness decreased progressively with increasing age.
The inverse relationship obtained was attributed to the greater ability of
older persons both to discriminate between fantasied and realistic situa-
tions and to attenuate the aversiveness of danger cues by forecasting
eventual favorable outcomes. As would be expected, emotional reactions
to erotic scenes were stronger among subjects in older age groups.
More recent demonstrations of vicarious emotional instigation through
filmed stimulation is provided in a series of experiments by Lazarus and
his associates (Lazarus, Speisman, Mordkoff, & Davison, 1962). Con-
tinuous recordings of subjects' autonomic responses were obtained during
presentation of a film portraying a primitive puberty ritual of an Aus-
tralian tribe in which a native boy underwent a crude 4
genital operation.
College students displayed heightened autonomic responsiveness while
viewing the genital subincision scenes, the reactions being particularly
marked when the operation was accompanied by sobs and other pain
cues on the part of the young initiate*. Both the deletion of the vocal pain
cues and the provision of sound-tract commentaries that minimized the
aversiveness of the depicted operation significantly reduced the subjects'
level of emotional arousal; conversely, commentaries highlighting the
suffering and hazards of such operations enhanced observers' physiological
arousal (Speisman, Lazarus, Mordkofl, & Davison, 1964).
In an erudite analysis of vicarious processes, Berger (1962) restricts
the phenomenon of vicarious instigation to situations in which an observer
responds emotionally to a performer's presumed affective experiences.
Since the emotional state of another person is not directly observable, its
Miller and his colleagues ( Miller, Banks, & Ogawa, 1962, 1963; Miller,
Murphy, & Mirsky, 1959) have identified, through the use of an ingenious
cooperative avoidance-conditioning procedure, some of the social cues
that serve as conditioned stimuli for affective arousal in observers. Rhesus
monkeys were first trained to avoid an electric shock by pressing a bar
whenever a stimulus light appeared. After the avoidance training, the
animals were seated in different rooms, and the bar was removed from
the chair of one monkey and the stimulus light from the other. Thus, the
animal having access to the light stimulus had to communicate by means
of affective cues to his partner, equipped with the response bar, who could
then perform the appropriate instrumental response that would enable
both animals to avoid painful stimulation. Distress cues exhibited by the
stimulus monkeys in anticipation of shock were highly effective in eliciting
fear in their observing companions as reflected in increased heart rate and
rapid performance of discriminated avoidance responses (Miller, 1967).
The finding that color slides showing the stimulus animal in fear or pain
170 MODELING AND VICARIOUS PROCESSES
affected by the pain responses of another rat; the control group showed
little empathetic responsiveness; and animals whose past distressing
experiences were unassociated with the pain responses of another mem-
ber of their species showed an effect intermediate between the two
groups.
Conditioning in humans is frequently mediated through self-generated
symbolic stimulation, which also plays an influential role in vicarious
responding (Bandura & Rosenthal, 1966; Stotland, Shaver, & Crawford,
1966). In personality theory vicarious emotional arousal is typically dis-
cussed under the concept of empathy. Within the personality framework it
of the models.
Laboratory investigations of vicarious classical conditioning in humans
(Barnett & Benedetti, I960; Berger, 1962) typically involve the condi-
tioning of autonomic responses to neutral environmental stimuli through
observational experiences. In Berger's (1962) studies, for example, one
group of observers was informed that the performing model would receive
a shock whenever a light dimmed, the dimming of the light being in
each trial preceded by a buzzer. A second group of observers was in-
structed that the performer would make a voluntary arm movement
whenever the light dimmed but that he was receiving no aversive stimu-
lation. In two other conditions the model was supposedly shocked but
refrained from making arm movements, or the model was neither shocked
nor withdrew his arm. The measure of vicarious conditioning was the
frequency of observers' galvanic skin responses to the buzzer, which
served as the conditioned stimulus. Observers who were informed that
the model was receiving aversive stimulation and who witnessed the
model simulate pain responses by jerking his arm displayed a greater
degree of vicarious conditioning than observers in the other three groups.
In a further extension of socially mediated conditioning, Craig & Wein-
stein ( 1965 ) found that observation of a performer experiencing repeated
failure produces vicarious emotional arousal that becomes conditioned
to previously neutral environmental cues.
Although the phenomenon of vicarious conditioning has been clearly
~
demonstrated, people differ widely in the rate with which they develop
conditioned emotional responses observationally and in the stability of
the acquired responses. Since this process requires the observer to ex-
perience painful consequences vicariously, thereby producing affective
arousal, variables that influence an observer's general level of emotionality
are likely to enhance or retard vicarious learning. There is some evidence
(Bandura & Rosenthal, 1966) that emotional arousal is, indeed, a significant
determinant of vicarious conditioning, but the latter variables are not
related in a simple linear fashion. In this experiment groups of adults
observed another person undergoing aversive conditioning experiences
in which a buzzer sounded at periodic intervals and shortly thereafter
the model feigned pain, supposedly in response to having received pain-
ful electric shocks. Prior to the vicarious conditioning phase of the study,
the groups of observers were subjected to differential degrees of emotional
arousal manipulated both psychologically and physiologically through the
administration of varying doses of epinephrine, a sympathetic stimulant.
The frequency with which observers manifested conditioned galvanic
skin responses to the buzzer alone was found to be a positive function of
the degree of psychological stress (Figure 3-9). However, a monotonic
decreasing function is obtained when, in addition to situational stress,
80 Nonthreat ^—
Placebo injection •—
Placebo + shock threat #•—
70
Epinephrine, 0.2 cc •—
Epinephrine, 0.5 cc •»—
I 60
Q)
C
o
S 50
o 40 -
20
10 -
Figure 3-9. Mean percentage of GSRs exhibited by subjects during the ac-
quisition phase, in which the tone and model's pain cues occurred in close tem-
poral association, and during tests in which the formerly neutral tone was
presented alone to assess its conditioned aversive properties. The five treatment
conditions represent increasing degrees of affective arousal. Bandura & Rosen-
thal, 1966.
174 MODELING AND VICARIOUS PROCESSES
freely with the control items, but actively avoided objects that accom-
panied supposedly painful experiences for another animal.
Although emotional behavior is probably often developed in everyday
situations through vicarious means, there are few occasions when aversive
forms of classical conditioning might be intentionally employed for thera-
peutic purposes. There are clinical reports (Miller, Dvorak, & Turner,
1960), however, in which aversive counterconditioning has been applied
in a group setting for creating aversion to alcohol in chronic alcoholics.
Aversion reactions are rapidly established under such conditions, and
most of the clients display strong vicarious conditioning effects. Positive
vicarious conditioning, on the other hand, has rarely been employed
systematically to develop empathy, pleasurable reactions, and favorable
social attitudes.
Vicarious Extinction
suffer apprehensions and force themselves into tense contact with feared
objects.
As part of a program of research designed to elucidate the phenome-
non of vicarious extinction, several efficacious modeling procedures have
been developed for modifying anxiety disorders. The first study in the
series (Bandura, Grusec, & Menlove, 1967b) involved a stringent test of
the degree to which strong avoidance behavior of long standing can be
extinguished vicariously. It also explored the possibility that induction of
positive affective responses in observers during exposure to potentially
threatening modeling cues may expedite the vicarious extinction process.
Young children, who exhibited fear of dogs as revealed by parental
ratings and an actual test of dog avoidance behavior, were assigned to
one of four treatment conditions. One group participated in eight brief
sessions during which they observed a fearless peer model exhibit pro-
gressively more fear-provoking interactions with a dog. For these chil-
dren, the modeled approach behavior was presented within a highly
positive party context designed to counteract anxiety reactions. The fear-
arousing properties of the modeled performances were gradually increased
from session to session by varying simultaneously the physical restraints
on the dog, the directness and intimacy of the modeled approach re-
sponses, and the duration of interaction between the model and his canine
companion. A second group of children observed the same graduated
modeled performances, but in a neutral context. In the two treatment
conditions described the stimulus complex contained both modeling cues
and repeated observation of the feared animal. Therefore, in order to
measure the effects of exposure to the threatening object itself, a third
group of children observed the dog in the positive context but with the
model absent. A fourth group participated in the positive activities but
was never exposed to either the dog or the modeled displays.
Following completion of the treatment series, children were readmin-
istered the avoidance test consisting of the graded sequence of dog inter-
action tasks. They were asked, for example, to approach and to pet the
dog, to release her from a playpen, to remove her leash, to feed her dog
biscuits, and to spend a fixed period of time alone in the room with the
animal. The final and most difficult set of tasks required the children to
climb into the playpen with the dog and, after having locked the gate, to
pet her and to remain alone with the animal under the confining, fear-
arousing conditions.
Evidence that deviant behavior can be modified by a particular
method is of limited therapeutic significance unless it can be demon-
24
22
20
a> 18
16
< 14
12
positive context •—
Model h neutral context •—
10
Dog + positive context •—
Positive context *—
Multiple model ^—
Single model •—
12
Control group •-•
10
5 6
4 -
ing treatment. The increased boldness of one of the control children who
had been subsequently treated is portrayed in Figure 3-12. The top frames
show the model's dauntless behavior; the lower frames depict the child's
fearless interaction with the animals, both of which she boldlv corralled
in the playpen, after the formal test.
Comparison of results of the two experiments suggests that symbolic
modeling is less powerful than live demonstrations of essentially the same
behavior. Although the single-model treatment effected significant reduc-
tions in children's avoidance responses, it did not sufficiently weaken
their fears to enable them to carry out the threatening terminal approach
behavior. However, the diminished efficacy of symbolic modeling can be
offset by a broader sampling of models and aversive stimulus objects.
Children who received the diverse modeling treatment not only showed
continuous improvement in approach behavior between the post-test and
follow-up periods, but also achieved terminal performances at rates com-
parable to equally avoidant children who, in the previous experiment,
observed fearless behavior performed by a single real-life model. Hill,
Vicarious Extinction 181
Liebert, & Mott (1968) and Spiegler, Liebert, McMains, & Fernandez
(1968) have also successfully eliminated persistent avoidance behavior
in children and adults through brief symbolic modeling, but in the latter
studies the modeled performances are accompanied by a persuasive nar-
rative and other fear-mitigating variables.
The potency of modeling influences in the transmission of anxiety is
widely acknowledged, but their therapeutic value has sometimes been
questioned (Jersild & Holmes, 1935) on the grounds that fears persist
even though modeling frequently occurs under ordinary conditions of
life. The effectiveness of any principle of learning depends not only on its
tioning in various ways. Some of the people were unable to perform their
jobs in situations in which there was any remote possibility that they
might come into contact with snakes; others could not take part in recrea-
tional activities such as hunting, gardening,camping, or hiking, because
of their dread of snakes; and still others avoided purchasing homes in
rural areas, or experienced marked distress whenever they would be un-
expectedly confronted with pet snakes in the course of their social or oc-
cupational activities.
In the initial phase of the experiment the participants were adminis-
tered a behavioral test that measured the strength of their avoidance of
snakes. In addition, they completed a comprehensive fear inventory to
determine whether elimination of fear of snakes is associated with con-
comitant changes in other areas of anxiety. Attitudinal ratings on several
scales describing various encounters with snakes and on the evaluative
dimensions of the semantic differential technique were also obtained. The
latter measures were included to furnish data regarding the interesting
but inadequately explored attitudinal effects of behavioral changes in-
duced through social-learning methods.
The cases were individually matched on the basis of their avoidance
behavior and assigned to one of four conditions. One group participated
in a self-administered symbolic modeling treatment in which they ob-
served a film depicting young children, adolescents, and adults engaging
in progressively threatening interactions with a large king snake ( Figure
3-13). To increase even further the power of this method two other fea-
tures were added: subjects were taught to induce and to maintain anxiety-
inhibiting relaxation throughout the period of exposure, and they were
permitted to regulate the rate of presentation of stimuli by means of re-
mote control starting and reversing devices. The rationale for the second
feature was that a self-regulated modeling treatment should permit
greater control over extinction than one in which persons are exposed to
a sequence of aversive cues without regard to their anxiety reactions.
Subjects were instructed to stop the film whenever a particular modeled
performance provoked anxiety, to reverse the film to the beginning of the
aversive sequence, and to reinduce deep relaxation. They then reviewed
the threatening scene repeatedly in this manner until it was completely
neutralized before proceeding to the next item in the graduated sequence.
After subjects became skillful in handling the projector controls and the
self-induction of relaxation, the experimenter absented himself from the
situation, and the subjects conducted their own treatment until their anx-
ieties to the depicted scenes were thoroughly extinguished.
were tested initially with the familiar brown-striped king snake and then
with an unfamiliar crimson-splotched corn snake that was strikingly dif-
ferent in coloration; the remaining subjects were tested with the two
snakes in the reverse order. The behavioral test consisted of a series of
tasks requiring the subjects to approach, look at, touch, and hold a snake
with bare and gloved hands; to remove the snake from its cage, let it
loose in the room, and then replace it in the cage; to hold it within five
inches of their faces; and finally to tolerate the snake in their laps while
they held their hands passively at their sides. Immediately before and
during the performance of each task subjects rated the intensity of their
fear arousal on a 10-interval scale to measure extinction of affective
arousal accompanying specific approach responses.
As shown in Figure 3-14, control subjects remained unchanged in
avoidance behavior, symbolic modeling and desensitization produced sub-
stantial reductions, and live modeling combined with guided participa-
tion eliminated snake phobias in virtually all subjects (92 percent). The
modeling procedures not only extinguished avoidance responses of long
standing, but they also neutralized the anxiety-arousing properties of the
Desensitization •-
26
Symbolic modeling *_
Control »-
24
22
3> 20
16
14
12
10
^L
Pre -Test Post -Test
fected the greatest attitudinal changes. These findings will be given more
detailed consideration in a later chapter specifically concerned with proc-
esses governing the modification of attitudes.
Analysis of the fear inventory scores disclosed some degree of fear
reductions beyond the specifically treated phobia, the decrements being
roughly proportional to the potency of the treatments employed. Non-
treated controls showed no changes in either number or intensity of fears.
Desensitization produced a decrease only in severity of fears toward other
animals, whereas symbolic modeling was accompanied by a reduction in
the number of animal fearsand a general diminution in the intensity of
anxiety in several other areas of functioning. Participant modeling, on the
other hand, effected widespread fear reductions in relation to a variety
of threats involving both interpersonal and nonsocial events. The transfer
obtained reflects the operation of at least two somewhat different proc-
esses. The first involves generalization of extinction effects from treated
stimuli to related anxiety sources. The second entails positive reinforce-
ment which mitigates emotional
of a sense of capability through success,
responses to potentially threatening situations. Having successfully over-
come a phobia that had plagued them for most of their lives, subjects re-
ported increased confidence that they could cope effectively with other
fear-provoking events
After the posttreatment assessment, subjects in the control group re-
ceived the symbolic modeling treatment without the relaxation com-
ponent. Symbolic modeling alone achieved substantial decrements in
fear arousal and avoidance behavior: 45 percent of the subjects ex-
hibited terminal performances toward both snakes. No significant dif-
ferences were found in approach behavior between subjects who were
administered symbolic modeling alone and those who received symbolic
modeling with relaxation. However, subjects who paired modeling with
relaxation required fewer reexposures to neutralize the aversive scenes,
188 MODELING AND VICARIOUS PROCESSES
28
26 -
24 -
22
c/>
to
o 20
a
^
Symbolic modeling •-
Desensitization •-
-tl J L
Pre -Test Post -Test Post -live Follow up
again in a follow-up study conducted one month later. Bandura, Blanchard, &
Ritter, 1968.
Vicarious Extinction 189
100
Modeling + information + participation I
Modeling I
80
Control
70
60
50
40
30
20 -
10 -
3.0
2.5
2 2.0
<
1.5 -
1.0
-
0.5 -
12 Number
3
of
4
Exposures
5 6
Figure 3-17. Mean level of fear arousal evoked by the modeling stimuli
initiallyand by each subsequent exposure to the same filmed scenes in subjects
receiving symbolic modeling with relaxation and symbolic modeling alone.
The data are averaged across scenes at each exposure and plotted for the first
six exposures only since subjects rarely required more than six presentations to
neutralize any given scene. Bandura, Blanchard, & Ritter, 1968.
192 MODELING AND VICARIOUS PROCESSES
ganization of attitudes.
The findings of studies reviewed above indicate that a powerful form
of treatment is which therapeutic agents themselves model the de-
one in
1
ances of subjects who have observed the same modeled behavior without
any consequences. In the experiment cited earlier (Bandura, 1965b), for
example, children who had observed a model's aggressive behavior result
in severe punishment performed significantly fewer matching responses
than subjects who observed the same actions result either in reward or in
no evident consequences. Indeed, the vicarious punishment produced
virtually complete suppression of imitative aggression in girls, whose
inhibitions regarding physical forms of aggression are initially relatively
strong. Further evidence for the suppressive effects of vicarious punish-
ment is furnished by studies comparing consistent vicarious reward with
successive reward and punishment of the model's behavior (Rosekrans &
Hartup, 1967). Subsequent punishment tends to cancel the behavioral en-
hancement effects of rewarding consequences to the model.
The above studies demonstrate the inhibitory influence of observed
negative outcomes to a model on the aggressive behavior of viewers. Wal-
ters and his associates (Parke & Walters, 1967; Walters, Leat, & Mezei,
1963; Walters, Parke, & Cane, 1965) have likewise shown that witnessing
peer models punished for engaging in forbidden play activities increased
observers' resistance to deviation when they were similarly tempted with
the prohibited objects. In a comparative study, Benton (1967) found
that observers who witnessed others reprimanded for handling prohibited
toys latershowed the same amount of response inhibition as did the pun-
ished performers. The possible mechanisms through which vicarious pun-
ishment produces inhibitory effects are discussed in some detail in the
introductory chapter of this book.
In many instances persons respond with self -punitive and self-deval-
uative reactions to behavior of their own that may be considered permis-
sible, or even rewardable, by others. Results of investigations concerned
through vicarious means. The main reason for this difference is that
behavior which is customarily subject to negative sanctions is often
positively reinforcing for the user, but
it is socially suppressed for the
processes (Miller & Dollard, 1941; Skinner, 1953) have, in fact, been
almost exclusively concerned with the discriminative function of social
Response Facilitating Effects of Modeling Influence 197
Hence, persons are most frequently rewarded for matching the behavior
of models who are intelligent, who possess certain social and technical
competencies, who command social power, and who, by virtue of their
adroitness, occupy high positions in various status hierarchies. On the
other hand, the behaviors of models who are ineffectual, uninformed,
and who have attained low vocational, intellectual, and social status, are
apt to have considerably less utilitarian value. As a result of differential
reinforcement for matching models who possess diverse attributes, the
identifying characteristics gradually come to serve as discriminative stim-
uli probable consequences associated with behavior
that signify the
modeled by different social agents. Moreover, through the process of
Utilization of Modeling Principles in Planned Sociocultural Change 199
to alter sociocultural patterns are to meet with success, they must employ
powerful change procedures to overcome the unfavorable conditions of
reinforcementinitially associated with unaccustomed practices. Attitude-
cepted when they produce immediate inspectional benefits and the causal
relationship between new behavior and advantageous outcomes can be
Utilization of Modeling Principles in Planned Sociocultural Change 201
they advocate yield highly favorable outcomes. After they have thus
enhanced their credibility and modeling potency they are in a more
favorable position to attempt modifications that conflict with existing
traditions and vested interests.
Summary
This chapter is principally .concerned with modeling processes where-
by new modes of behavior are acquired and existing response patterns
are extensively modified through observation of other people's behavior
and its consequences for them.
A
multiprocess theory of observational learning was advanced, accord-
ing to which modeled stimulus events are transformed and retained in
imaginal and verbal memory codes. Later, reinstatement of these repre-
sentational mediators, in conjunction with appropriate environmental
cues, guide behavioral reproduction of matching responses. Performance
of observationally learned responses is largely regulated bv reinforcing
outcomes that may be externally applied, self-administered, or vicariously
experienced. Since modeling phenomena are controlled by several interre-
lated subprocesses, the absence of modeling effects in any given case
may result from either failures in sensory registration due to inadequate
attention to relevant social cues, deficient symbolic coding of modeled
events into functional mediators of overt behavior, retention decrements,
motor deficiencies, or unfavorable conditions of reinforcement.
Modeling procedures have been extensively employed, with consider-
able success, for many purposes, especially for developing conceptual and
interpersonal modes of behavior. In this approach agents of change model
requisite behaviors and arrange optimal conditions for recipients to
learn and to practice the activities until they are performed skillfully
and spontaneously. In addition to the utilization of modeling principles
for establishing social and cognitive competencies, emotional responsive-
ness can be conditioned and extinguished on a vicarious basis. In the
case of vicarious affective conditioning, exposure to a model's emotional
reactions arouses in observers emotional responses which become condi-
tioned, through contiguous association, to distinctive cues present in the
situation. However, the degree of vicarious responsiveness is partly de-
pendent upon an intermediary self-stimulation process involving symbolic
representation of similar consequences occurring to oneself in the same
situation. Affective expressions of a model are most likelv to elicit high
self-arousal in observers under conditions where the participants have
experienced similar pleasurable or painful experiences.
Vicarious extinction of emotional behavior is achieved by exposing
REFERENCES
Adler, L. L., & Adler, H. E. Age as a factor in observational learning in puppies.
American Dachshund, 1968, March, 13-14.
Allport, F. H. Social psychology. Cambridge, Mass.: Riverside Press, 1924.
Alyokrinskii, V. V. Imitatsiya det'mi (4-7 let) russkikh nekotoroykh anglui-
i
Corsini, R. J.,
& Putzey, L. J. Bibliography of group psychotherapy. Beacon,
N.Y.: Beacon House, 1957.
Corsini, R. J., Shaw, M. E., & Blake, R. R. Roleplaying in business and industry.
New York: Free Press, 1961.
Corson, J. A. Observational learning of a lever pressing response. Psychonomic
Science, 1967, 7, 197-198.
Cowan, P. A., Hoddinoth, B. A., & Wright, B. A. Compliance and resistance
in the conditioning of autistic children: An exploratory studv. Child De-
velopment, 1965, 36, 913-923.
Craig, K. D., & Weinstein, M. S. Conditioning vicarious affective arousal. Psy-
chological Reports, 1965, 17, 955-963.
Crawford, M. P., & Spence, K. W. Observational learning of discrimination
problems by chimpanzees. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1939, 27,
133-147.
Crooks, J. L. Observational learning of fear in monkeys. Unpublished manu-
script,University of Pennsylvania, 1967.
Darby, C. & Riopelle, A. J. Observational learning in the Rhesus monkey.
L.,
Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1959, 52, 94-98.
deCharms, R., & Rosenbaum, M. E. Status variables and matching behavior.
Journal of Personality, 1960, 28, 492-502.
Drakeford, J. W. Integrity therapy. Nashville, Tenn.: Broadman, 1967.
Dubanoski, R. A. Imitation as a function of role appropriateness of behavior
and response consequences to the model. Unpublished manuscript, Uni-
versity of Iowa, 1967.
Duncker, K. Experimental modification of children's food preferences through
social suggestion. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1938, 33,
489-507.
Dysinger, W. S., & Ruckmick, C. A. The emotional responses of children to
the motion-picture situation. New York: Macmillan, 1933.
Easterbrook, J. A. The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organiza-
tion of behavior. Psychological Review, 1959, 66, 183-201.
Ellson, D. G. Hallucinations produced by sensory conditioning. Journal of Ex-
perimental Psychology, 1941, 28, 1-20.
Emmerich, \V. Parental identification in young children. Genetic Psychology
Monographs, 1959, 60, 257-308.
Epstein, R. Aggression toward outgroups as a function of authoritarianism and
imitation of aggressive models. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 1966, 3, 574-579.
Erasmus, C. J. Man takes control. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press,
1961.
Feshbach, N. D. Variations in teachers' reinforcement style and imitative be-
havior of children differing in personality characteristics and social back-
ground. Unpublished manuscript, University of California, Los Angeles,
1967.
Flanders, J. P. A review of research on imitative behavior. Psychological Bul-
letin, 1968,69,316-337.
Flanders, J.
P., & Thistlethwaite, D. L. Effects of informative and justificatory
variables upon imitation. Unpublished manuscript, Vanderbilt University,
1969.
Foss, B. M. Mimicry in mynas (Gracula religiosa) : A test of Mowrer's theory.
British Journal of Psychology, 1964, 55, 85-88.
208 MODELING AND VICARIOUS PROCESSES
Metz, J.
R. Conditioning generalized imitation in autistic children. Journal
of Experimental Child Psychology, 1965, 2, 389-399.
Michael, D. N., & Maccoby, N. Factors influencing the effects of student par-
ticipation on verbal learning from films: Motivating versus practice effects,
"feedback," and overt versus covert responding. In A. A. Lumsdaine (Ed.),
Student response in programmed instruction. Washington, D. C: National
—
Academy of Sciences National Research Council, 1961. Pp. 271-293.
Miller, E. C, Dvorak, A., & Turner, D. W. A method of creating aversion to
alcohol by reflex conditioning in a group setting. Quarterly Journal of
Studies on Alcohol, 1960, 21, 424-431.
Miller, N. E. Some implications of modern behavior theory for personality
change and psychotherapy. In P. Worchel & D. Bvrne (Eds.), Personality
change. New York: Wiley, 1964. Pp. 149-175.
Miller, N. E., & Dollard,
J.
Social learning and imitation. New Haven: Yale
University Press, 1941.
Miller, R. E. Experimental approaches to the physiological and behavioral
concomitants of affective communication in rhesus monkeys. In S. A.
Altmann (Ed.), Social communication among primates. Chicago: Uni-
versity of Chicago Press, 1967. Pp. 125-134.
Miller, R. E., Banks, J. H., & Ogawa, N. Communication of affect in "cooper-
ative conditioning" of rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, L962, 64, 343-348.
Miller, R. E., Banks, }. II., Jr., & Ogawa, N. Role of facial expression in
"cooperative avoidance conditioning" in monkeys. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 1963. 67, 24-30.
Miller, R. E., Caul, W. & Mirsky, I. A. Communication of affect between
I'..
5,424-431.
Rosenthal, D. Changes in some moral values following psychotherapy. Journal
of Consulting Psychology, 1955, 19, 431-436.
Ross, D. Relationship between dependency, intentional learning, and incidental
learning in preschool children. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 1966, 4, 374-381.
Ross, S. A. The effect of deviant and nondeviant models on the behavior of
preschool children in a temptation situation. Unpublished doctoral dis-
sertation, Stanford University, 1962.
Schachter, S., & Hall, R. Group-derived restraints and audience persuasion.
Human Relations, 1952, 5, 397-406.
Schein, E. H. The effect of reward on adult imitative behavior. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1954, 49, 389-395.
Schwartz, A. N., & Hawkins. II. L. Patient models and affect statements
in group therapy. Proceedings of the 73rd annual convention of the
A.P.A. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association, 1965.
Pp. 265-266.
Sheffield, F. D. Theoretical considerations in the learning of complex sequential
tasks from demonstration and practice. In A. A. Lumsdaine (Ed.),
Student response in programmed instruction. Washington, D. C.: National
—
Academy of Sciences National Research Council, 1961. Pp. 13-32.
Sheffield, F. D., & Maccoby, N. Summary and interpretation of research on
organizational principles in constructing filmed demonstrations. In A. A.
Lumsdaine (Ed.), Student response in programmed instruction. Wash-
ington, D. C.: National Academy of Sciences — National Research Council,
1961. Pp. 117-131.
Sherman, J.
A. Use of reinforcement and imitation to reinstate verbal behavior
in mute psychotics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1965, 70, 155-164.
Skinner, B. F. Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan, 1953.
Sloane, H. N., Jr., Johnston, M. K., & Harris, F. R. Remedial procedures for
teaching verbal behavior to speech deficient or defective young children.
In H. N. Sloane, Jr. & B. A. MacAulay (Eds.), Operant procedures in
remedial speech and language training. Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1968.
Pp. 77-101.
Slobin, D. I. Imitation and grammatical development in children. In N. S.
References 215
DRIVE-REDUCTION HYPOTHESIS
for this is that strong stimuli can lose their activating function if pre-
sented in gradually increasing values, if they have been associated with
rewarding experiences, or they become discriminative for less active
if
SENSORY-STIMULATION HYPOTHESIS
Although some reinforcement effects may be governed by visceral
drive states, there are many reinforcing conditions that do not appear to
involve reduction of physiological needs or removal of aversive stimuli,
unless one to invoke a host of sensory and activity drives. Animals
were
will learn to perform responses that produce visual and auditory stimula-
tion or opportunities to engage in manipulative and exploratory activities
(Barnes & Baron, 1961; Butler, 1958a; Kish, 1966; Miles, 1958). A num-
ber of studies, conducted principally with infants ( Rheingold, Stanley, &
Doyle, 1964) and children (Odom, 1964; Stevenson & Odom, 1964), have
likewise shown that visual and auditory feedback can be effective in
modifying and sustaining behavior over time. These findings would seem
to indicate that much human behavior —particularly approach, attending,
and manipulative responses — is reinforced feedback that
by the sensory
is automatically produced.
Investigations of factors that might contribute to the reinforcing
properties of auditory and visual events have revealed that novel and
complex stimuli function as more effective reinforcers than simple and
familiar stimulus events. The data furthermore indicate that, as is the
PREPOTENT-RESPONSE HYPOTHESIS
In describing the essential properties of reinforcing events, emphasis
has usually been placed on the nature of the reinforcing stimuli (e.g.,
and approval, intracranial
food, money, novel sensations, social attention
stimulation, etc. ) , and their efficacy under varying conditions of depriva-
tion. On the basis of results from an ingenious series of experiments,
Premack ( 1965 ) has presented an explanation of reinforcement that em-
phasizes the reinforcing response rather than the reinforcing stimulus. In
these investigations the reinforcement values of different activities are
estimated from the duration for which subjects spontaneously engage in
particular behaviors when no time or response restrictions exist. If the
opportunity to engage in the more rewarding activity is made conditional
upon the prior performance of low probability behaviors, then the latter
responses increase in frequency. Based on these findings, Premack has
222 POSITIVE CONTROL
The discussion thus far has been primarily concerned with the per-
formance-enhancing effects of various contingent events, whether they be
drive-reducing, sensory, or in the form of prepotent activities. Two differ-
ent explanations have been proposed as to how reinforcing consequences
affect behavior. Some reinforcement theories assume that positive response
outcomes have a direct strengthening effect on stimulus-response associa-
224 POSITIVE CONTROL
INCENTIVE SYSTEM
stimulus with any reward value. The contrasting results were attributed
to differential attentivenesson the part of the children. They were highly
attentive to external cues during negative stimulation, whereas in sessions
employing rewards they engaged in considerable self -stimulatory behavior
and appeared oblivious to the relevant social stimuli. It was therefore
decided to employ an instrumental conditioning paradigm in which the
children received food rewards only if they approached the therapist
whenever he said the word "good." The children were thus required to
attend closely to the appropriate verbal cue and to discriminate it from
other stimuli occurring at the same time. After the social stimulus had
been established as discriminative for primary reinforcement, the chil-
dren's approach responses were intermittently rewarded on a gradually
increasing ratio in order to further enhance the rewarding capacity of the
verbal cue. This procedure proved highly effective. In later phases of the
experiment, new responses could be established and maintained in autistic
children through contingent presentation of verbal approval alone. More-
over, the social stimulus retained its reinforcing potency over an extended
period on the basis of periodic association with food rewards.
With less severely autistic children social reinforcers were established
more readily. In these eases verbal approval and affection in the form of
demonstrative pats and hugs sustained the children's positive responsive-
ness during numerous sessions devoted to language learning and the
acquisition of social skills. Occasionally, however, food rewards accom-
panied the social reinforcers as a means of preserving their efficacy. Many
of the change programs discussed later rely heavily upon interpersonal
reinforcers in which desired behavior is responded to with attention,
interest, and approval while undesired activities are either consistently
ignored or socially disapproved.
A stimulus that has been associated on numerous occasions with many
types of primary as well as secondary reinforcements acquires the capacity
to function as a generalized reinforcer. In the treatment of young chil-
dren or adults for whom positive social and verbal have weak
stimuli
incentive value, tangible generalized reinforcers are frequently employed.
Appropriate performances are rewarded with monetary credits, tokens, or
points that can later be used to obtain a variety of rewarding objects and
special privileges. A token incentive system has several advantages over
other forms of material rewards: The reinforcing value of tokens is rela-
ARRANGEMENT OF CONTINGENCIES
After appropriate reinforcers that have sufficient incentive value to
maintain stable responsiveness have been chosen, the contingencies be-
tween specific performances and reinforcing stimuli must be arranged.
Parents, teachers, and psychotherapists intuitively employ rewards in
their attempts to influence and modify behavior, but their efforts often
produce limited because the methods are used improperly, incon-
results
sistently, or inefficiently. In many instances considerable rewards are
bestowed, but they are not made conditional upon the behavior that
change agents wish to promote; long delays often intervene between the
occurrence of the desired behavior and its intended consequences; spe-
cial privileges, activities, and rewards are generally furnished according
to fixed time schedules rather than performance requirements; and, in
230 POSITIVE CONTROL
ing privileges for deviant behavior and infractions of house rules. Within
an institutional setting in which noncontingent rewards are provided at
a high level, the staff members are cast in the unenviable role of punitive
agents, and the boys can move onlv in a downward direction. Thus, the
threat of punishment is ever present, but the positive incentives for be-
havioral change, though abundantly available, are poorly managed. Under
these circumstances, the majority of the participants comply halfheartedly
with the minimum demands of the institution in order to avoid penalties
for any breach of the rules. Similarly, in most psychiatric facilities, pa-
tients can best maximize their rewards by merely adopting a passive
patient role.
The necessity for arranging appropriate reinforcement contingencies
is dramatically illustrated by studies in which rewards are shifted from
a response-contingent to a time-contingent basis ( Lovaas, Berberich, Per-
loff, & Schaeffer, 1966; Baer, Peterson, & Sherman,
1967). During sessions
in which rewards are made conditional upon occurrence of the desired
behavior, the appropriate response patterns are exhibited at a consist-
ently high level; by contrast, under conditions where the same rewards
are given but after a certain time has elapsed, independent of the client's
behavior, there is a marked drop in the desired behavior. Reinstatement
of response-contingent reinforcement promptly restores the high level of
responsiveness. These behavioral changes are particularly striking con-
sidering that interpersonal relationship factors and the amount of reward
remain the same in phases of treatment except for the arrangement
all
1967).
In an effective program of change reinforcement contingencies should
be arranged to provide positive guidance and support for new modes of
behavior, rather than to extract minimal compliance with situational de-
Essential Components of Reinforcement Practices 231
formance level, although he receives his total payment at the end of the
month rather than in small amounts immediately after each unit of work
has been completed.
With young children, grossly deviant adults whose behavior is under
weak stimulus control, and individuals whose efforts extinguish rapidly
under delayed reinforcement contingencies, it may be necessary initially
to employ immediate concrete rewards; otherwise, such persons are likely
to display rapid decrements in responsiveness if reinforcing consequences
are postponed. On the other hand, persons who are responsive to instruc-
tional control are usually able to function adequately under delayed
reinforcement provided the contingencies arc explicitly defined and the
incentives are sufficiently attractive. Moreover, immediate satisfactions
derived from the activity itself and signs of progress often supplement,
and may eventually replace, ultimate extrinsic reinforcements in main-
taining behavior.
initially set too high, most, if not all, of the person's responses go unre-
warded, so that his efforts are gradually extinguished and his motivation
diminished. Consequently, in the beginning stages a low criterion for
reinforcement is generally adopted so that responses that are within the
individual's capabilities, but may have only slight resemblance to the
desired behavior, are reinforced. After gross approximations to the com-
plex pattern of behavior become more frequent, reinforcement is made
contingent upon a closer response variant. The criterion for reinforcement
is thus raised in small successive steps in the direction of more compli-
cated forms of behavior until eventually only the desired behavior is
reinforced.
The effective utilization of successive approximation procedures is
so, and when the customary rewards are discontinued they will cease
responding altogether. It is further assumed that rewarding practices not
only establish weak and unenduring behavior, but that contingent rein-
forcement is likely to interfere with the development of spontaneity,
creativity, intrinsic motivational systems, and other highly valued self-
determining personality characteristics. Some of the more intemperate
criticism considers the deliberate use of reinforcement to be deceptive,
manipulative, and an insult to the personal integrity of human beings.
For reasons presented above most persons whose own behavior is
change agents work with mature and intrinsically motivated persons, but,
rather than appealing to higher symbolic motivations, insist on imposing
crass materialistic incentives upon them. There are undoubtedly some
practitioners who apply incentive procedures thoughtlessly and ineffec-
tively. Ordinarily, however, primary rewards are employed in initial
stages with persons who are not reinforceable with other types of events
and who would otherwise remain inaccessible to treatment. In the latter
cases it would be no more appropriate to rely upon developmentally
advanced incentives than to teach young children how to count by
commencing with the principles of advanced mathematics. After rein-
forcing functions have been imparted to social and symbolic stimulus
events, then more subtle and naturally occurring reinforcers are increas-
ingly employed. Without the initial concrete training, psychologically in-
capacitated persons arc relegated to a subhuman existence in custodial
institutions.
REINFORCEMENT SYSTEMS
AND DURARILITY OF BEHAVIORAL CHANGES
Demonstrations that behavior can be maintained at a satisfactory level
through reinforcements mediated by change agents are of limited signifi-
cance unless the response patterns endure long after the specially created
contingencies have been discontinued. There arc several ways in which
reinforcement systems can be devised and altered during the course of
treatment to ensure* that existing behavior docs not readily extinguish.
ment from play activities with peers, adult rewards for interaction with
children were progressively diminished to a normal amount of attention,
and the schedule for nonreinforcement of adult contacts gradually
relaxed. Eventually the treatment program was discontinued altogether
and no special contingencies were arranged thereafter. The increase in
social interaction with other children nevertheless endured, as revealed
in observations of behavior conducted at various times following the
termination of the program. Other case studies, specifically designed to
investigate the durability of behavioral change (Baer & Wolf, 1967), have
shown that if adults maintain their reinforcement support of social be-
havior in children until they achieve reciprocally rewarding interactions
with peers, the children's behavior comes increasingly under peer control
and is little affected by withdrawal of adult social reinforcement.
Results from the above studies, and others conducted in the same
manner, show that established patterns of behavior maintain their
strength after specially arranged consequences are discontinued provided
the behavior is brought under the influence of favorable contingencies
within the individual's social milieu. In cases, however, where the rein-
forcement practices in naturalistic situations are either deficient or grossly
deviant it is doubtful that lasting behavioral changes can be achieved,
unless the program is extended to encompass significant members of the
individual's social environment.
100
Baseline Operant consequences Instructions
plus operant
consequences • • •
50 -
(
' *
v- J
t t t • I t I
20 40
Meals
Figure 4-1. Percentage of patients who picked up cutlery during the baseline
period, during the reinforcement phase in which appropriate responses were
promptly rewarded, and during a period when instructions were combined
with reinforcement. Ayllon & Azrin, 1964.
100
Baseline Instructions
r^- Instructions plus
operant consequences
50
^>— ••<
,4-
10 20 Mil 120 130 v
221 230
Meals
cial behavior (Colman & Baker, 1968), and innumerable other types of
procedure in which both the child and his peers initially earn desired
rewards for his good behavior. The material reinforcers are then gradually
withdrawn is entirely maintained by
until eventually the child's behavior
social reinforcement from teachers and peers. Following termination of
the formal treatment program, telephone contact is maintained on a
diminishing schedule, and home observations are conducted periodically
over a six-month follow-up period.
Results based on six families that have participated in the above pro-
gram show that parents reduced the frequency with which they positively
reinforced deviant behavior from an average rate of 35 percent during
the baseline period to 10 percent at the end of the intervention program.
Modification of familial contingencies not only decreased the family's
output of deviant behavior, but it increased the amount of positive social
reinforcement in the entire social system, and it produced a more recipro-
cal quality to the interactions between the various family members.
248 POSITIVE CONTROL
SYMBOLIC LEARNING
In recent years, reinforcement procedures have been used effectively
in conjunction with programmed instructional materials to establish com-
plex symbolic forms of behavior. Staats' (1965) program of research on
the acquisition of reading behavior furnishes one such example.
Reading involves complicated processes which children must learn
in
both to discriminate among symbols and to associate
intricate verbal
appropriate verbal responses to them. Complexity arises primarily be-
cause the same elements in a compound word stimulus must elicit differ-
ent responses depending upon the context in which they occur. Since
words contain many common stimulus properties (e.g., counsel, council)
and in most cases, word differentiation relies upon subtle cues, the devel-
opment of reading responses constitutes a demanding associative form-
discrimination task. In addition to difficulties created by high stimulus
similarity, the instructional material itself typically serves as a weak
source of positive reinforcement, particularly for young children. An
Applications of Contingency Systems 249
1964; Staats, Minke, Finley, Wolf, & Brooks, 1964) regarding the potential
value of this approach for establishing reading behavior in preschool
children. Further, the influence of schedules of reinforcement on rate of
reading acquisition has been studied systematically in several cases with
250 POSITIVE CONTROL
6 i- Experimental training
Baseline period
[regular school training)
E
5 2
<
? 1
3 4 5 6
Number of School Years
Figure 4-3. Reading test scores achieved after 8/2 years of regular classroom
instructions and after 4J2 months during which reading responses were positively
reinforced. Staats & Butterfield, 1965.
subjects for the first time in his turbulent academic career, and he mark-
edly decreased and eventually ceased his aggressively defiant behavior.
The entire program, which was administered by a probation officer, in-
volved a total expenditure of $20.31 for token exchange items.
Essentially the same procedures were applied with some degree of
success by adult volunteers and high school seniors in teaching reading
skills to retarded, emotionally disturbed, and culturally deprived children
should be emphasized that the critical issue is not reliance upon mechan-
ical versus social presentation of stimulus material, but rather which
tutorial systems, applied either singly or in combination, best approximate
optimal conditions for learning. Although social commentators often
attribute legions of virtues to conventional modes of instruction and hosts
of pernicious effects to programmed methods, many instructors do not,
in fact, provide the type of content organization that would ensure rapid
learning and effective retention; many present material in ways that
extinguish students' intellectual interests; and often many inadvertently
establish strong avoidance tendencies toward the subject matter being
taught. As a consequence, many students, particularly those who are
weakly motivated or less well endowed intellectually, display marked
intellectual deficits despite numerous years expended fruitlessly in school
attendance.
Provided that they are skillfully designed and adaptive to individual
requirements, self-instructional systems possess several features that can
facilitate the learning process. First, they present material to the student
in a well-organizedgraduated order. The utilization of logically ordered
sequences prevents students from becoming contused or lost through
omission of essential intermediate steps in exposition; this removes one
major aversive aspect of conventional instruction. Second, they provide
the student with immediate feedback about the accuracy of his responses,
helping him to continuously monitor his comprehension of the subject
matter. Third, since a student can proceed to new information only by
making correct responses to preceding items, the required active partici-
pation of the student forces careful observation of stimulus material. Thus,
if a student should lapse into classroom reverie, the instructional content,
like Old Man River, keeps rolling along, whereas in programmed instruc-
tion the patient tutor remains idle as long as the student is disengaged.
Fourth, the self -pacing feature of programmed teaching methods makes
individualized instruction possible for persons who differ in ability and
mastery of the material. In computerized systems, in which new instruc-
tional content is selected at each step on the basis of the learner's past
performances, students can generate their own optimal learning se-
quences. Finally, because errors are drastically reduced by gradual pro-
gression in content difficulty, learning from self-instructional programs is
gram of change. The goals that individuals choose for themselves must be
specified in sufficiently detailed behavioral terms to provide adequate
guidance for the actions that must be taken daily to attain desired out-
comes.
To further increase goal commitment participants are asked to make
contractual agreements to practice self-controlling behaviors in their daily
activities. Thus, for example, in modifying smoking behavior (Tooley &
Pratt, 1967) and obesity (Ferster, Nurnberger, & Levitt, 1962), clients
agree to restrict increasingly, in graduated steps, the times and places in
which they will engage in the undesired behavior. Under conditions
where individuals voluntarily commit themselves to given courses of
action, subsequent tendencies to deviate are likely to be counteracted by
negative self -evaluations. Through this mechanism, and anticipated social
reactions of others, contractual commitments reinforce adherence to
corrective practices.
Satisfactions derived from evident changes help to sustain successful
endeavors. Individuals can, therefore, utilize objective records of behav-
ioral changes as an additional source of reinforcement for their self-
controlling behavior. In studies of self-directive processes by Kolb, Winter,
& Berlew (1968) students used miniature counters to keep an accurate
record of the frequency with which they displayed desired and undesired
behavior throughout each day. These data were plotted graphically to
provide a clear picture of the behavioral improvements students were
accomplishing by their own efforts. Daily feedback of this type not only
serves a reinforcing function but it also safeguards against irregular and
halfhearted implementation of self-prescribed procedures. In a study
designed to improve self-instruction behavior, Fox ( 1966 ) found that
students who recorded their daily productivity continued to work on
assignments until they exceeded their preceding performances, thus
ensuring continued improvement.
Since behavior is extensively under external stimulus control, persons
can regulate the frequency with which they engage in certain activities
by altering stimulus conditions under which the behavior customarily
occurs. Overeating, for example, will arise more often when appetizing
256 POSITIVE CONTROL
whereas their aversive consequences are not experienced for some time.
Conversely, self-control measures usually produce immediate unpleasant
effects while the personal benefits are considerably delayed. Self-reinforc-
ing operations are, therefore, employed to provide immediate support for
self-controlling behavior until the benefits that eventually accrue take
over the reinforcing function.
The contingencies that individuals arrange for themselves may involve
Applications of Contingency Systems 257
() 3 6 9 12 () 3 6 9 12 3 6 9 12 3 6 9 12
Months Months Months Months
145 175 \i i
J 165
i i i
Vj
150
() 3 6 9 12 ( D 3 6 9 12 3 6 9 12 3 6 9 12
Months Months Months Months
Figure 4-4. Weight losses achieved by eight women using self-control proce-
dures. Stuart, 1957.
VERBAL CONDITIONING
The contradictory and weak generalization effects noted above are not
at all surprising when one considers that experimental manipulations in
most conditioning studies barely suffice to produce a conditioning effect,
ables to the role of awareness in the learning process. Results of this line
of research and their implications for theories of behavioral change will
be reviewed later.
Social Organizational Applications of Reinforcement Contingencies 261
paid." . ."You mean if I work at the lab I won't get paid? I need tokens
.
to buy cigarettes for my boy friend and to buy new clothes so I'll look nice
like the other girls [pp. 363-365]."
In a subsequent experiment, when patients were paid the tokens on a
264 POSITIVE CONTROL
Preferred job
N =1
9 t » t » t;
20 30
Days
Figure 4-5. Mean number of hours patients worked per day when positive
reinforcement was varied between preferred and nonpreferred jobs. Ayllon &
Azrin, 1965.
Reinforcement
not
50 contingent
upon
•
v.-'-.. performance
•• • •• •©••
••••• • ••
• • • •• ••
•
40
1
|
Reinforcement Reinforcement
contingent contingent
upon upon
performance 1 performance
30 •
1
•
20 -
1
N = 44
10
\
I
1
20 40 60
Days
triously; when they were simply given the tokens noncontingently they
gradually stopped working; and when contingent reinforcement proce-
dures were reinstated their participation was restored immediately and
maintained at the previously high level.
It is when the incentives were completely
of particular interest that
withdrawn and the institutional rewards and privileges we™ ^ade freely
available in a manner similar to usual hospital practices, a marked loss in
behavior resulted ( Figure 4-7 )
The rapid behavioral changes produced by alternating incentive con-
ditions does not necessarily demonstrate that reinforcement is the sole
controlling factor. In social situations behavior always remains partly
under modeling stimulus control. Persons who occupy a prestigeful posi-
tion in a social group usually serve as major sources of social behavior
for other members. Consequently, to the extent that the incentive condi-
tions modified the behavior of prestigeous models, other patients may
266 POSITIVE CONTROL
No
reinforcement
50
• • • •• • »
• • •• • • •
* •• • •
•
•
I
>40
CD
|
T5 Reinforcement Reinforcement
contingent contingent
upon upon
performance performance
30
I
20 -
N = 44
10
V •• •
•
••
•
•
15 30 45
Days
Figure 4-7. Total number of hours spent each day by the group of 44 patients
performing "on-ward" activities during periods when rewards were given upon
completion of work assignments, when positive incentives were not used and
the various activities and privileges were freely available, and when the
reinforcement contingencies were reintroduced into the social system. Ayllon &
Azrin, 1965.
had become a permanent abode, ventured out for the first time in 43
years!
Discharge rates also verified the beneficial effects of the new rein-
forcement practices. Twenty-one patients left the hospital via the privi-
leged group, almost double the discharge rate for the same ward the
previous year. The overall findings of this project indicate that alteration
of contingency structures in a hospital social system can not only coun-
teract the stupefying effects of prolonged institutionalization, but also
produce generalized increases in self-directive and interpersonal modes of
behavior.
A comprehensive treatment program does not simply aim to produce
effective functioning in circumscribed areas within the institution, but
Social Organizational Applications of Reinforcement Contingencies 269
lacking in personal and social resources, present the most challenging re-
habilitation problem. Results based on follow-up studies disclose that ap-
proximately 70 percent of chronic patients who are discharged from men-
tal hospitals return within 18 months regardless of the type of treatment
received during the period of hospitalization ( Fairweather, Simon,
Gebhard, Weingarten, Holland, Sanders, Stone, & Reahl, 1960; Fair-
weather & Simon, 1963 ) As a consequence of this high readmission rate,
.
modifying the behavior of each member, and for implementing the in-
centive system, was delegated to the group. It met daily to discuss the
progress of individual members, their problems, and constructive ways in
which these might be managed or modified. Any staff member could be
invited to furnish factual information needed by the group to reach a
reasonable decision, but the staff refrained from recommending what
courses of action should be taken.
Each week the group also met with the staff to present their recom-
mendations concerning each individual member's step-level and con-
comitant money and passes for the following week, the actions taken
with respect to problem behavior, and their evaluations of the progress,
morale, and functioning of their group. The staff then either approved all
recommendations made bv the group, approved some and disapproved
others, or rejected their recommendations. If warranted, the entire group
could be rewarded or penalized by being raised or lowered one step-
level, depending upon the appropriateness of the group's decision-making
behavior.
Thus, the social-role behaviors required of both the patients and the
staff differed markedly two programs. In the conventional treat-
in the
ment the was primarily responsible for regulating the
hospital staff
patients' daily activities, for making decisions concerning money allot-
ments, passes and other types of privileges, and for implementing dis-
ciplinary and remedial courses of action. The patients, in turn, occupied
the usual subordinate, patient role. Bv contrast, although the staff in the
group decision-making program could overrule or amend action-oriented
recommendations proposed by patients, staff members functioned pri-
marily as consultants and resource persons. This social structure provided
patients considerable freedom and responsibility to manage their own
activities and to make decisions that significantly affected each other's
behavior.
In an effort to control for possible effects of different staff characteris-
ticson patients' social behavior, the two sets of staff members changed
wards halfway through the experiment. The relative efficacy of the
treatment approaches was objectively assessed in terms of diverse criteria
including a multitude of behavior ratings, sociometricallv derived pref-
erences, self-evaluations, administrative indices, and attitude question-
naires.Most of the behavioral assessments were conducted throughout
the 27 weeks that the experiment was in progress, while others were
obtained at the completion of the study, and six months later.
The voluminous data from this ambitious, well-executed field study
demonstrate that the program specifically designed to reinstate inter-
personal responsiveness and self-directive behavior in patients yielded
Social Organizational Applications of Reinforcement Contingencies 271
7 9 11 13 15 17 19 21 23 25 27 11 13 15 17 19 21 23 25 27
Week Week
(c) (d)
40 [45 50| 55
Baseline Manager Fines Manager
fortuitous factors.
In view of the generalized changes in interpersonal responsiveness
achieved by Fairweather, it would be of considerable interest to compare
systematically the relative efficacy of staff-administered reinforcement
systems involving an elaborate set of precise contingencies of the type
employed by Ayllon, and Atthowe & Krasner, with one in which some in-
centives are used to foster strong group cohesiveness but controlling
functions are in large part delegated to group members. The merits of
these two approaches, which involve many common principles, could
be easily combined to form a program in which a specified set of rein-
forcement contingencies is developed and implemented by the patients
themselves under staff guidance.
Another important contingency variable that requires systematic
investigation is concerned with whether rewards are tied to individual
performances or to entire sets of behavior. In the latter system, successive
phases are devised which require increasingly higher levels of function-
ing in several different areas. As individuals progress through these
sequential steps by adopting the requisite patterns of behavior they
receive increased rewards and privileges. In treating a group of delin-
quent adolescents, Martin, Burkholder, Rosenthal, Tharp, & Thorne
(1968) found that a phase-contingent system of reinforcement produced
more rapid and uniformly positive changes in behavior than a previous
system in which specific responses were individually reinforced. Indeed,
the latter contingency structure produced much wrangling and accusa-
tions of unfairness because, in an effort to ensure adequate reinforcement
of progress made by youngsters functioning at different levels, they were
required to meet different behavioral standards and achievements for
similar rewards. The authors attribute the greater efficacy of the rein-
forcement system linked to role behaviors to the fact that clear specifica-
tion of sequential goals and the behaviors required for promotion from
one phase to the next serve as prompts and positive guides for changes
in desirable directions.
In most applications of reinforcement principles to severely inca-
pacitated persons, behavioral improvements are initially achieved by
immediate reinforcement of specific performances. However, as their
competencies are increased, individuals are promoted to a phase system
analogous to hierarchical reinforcement structures existing in community
life.
Transition Lodge
begins closes
100 i-
Lodge group
Control group
80
28 33
mos mos
60
« 40
20
12 18
JiilkL
24
Months
30 34 40
Figure 4-10. Percentage of time that patients in the lodge and hospital pro-
grams spent in the community for 40 months of follow-up. The lodge program
was discontinued after 33 months. Fairweather et al., 1969.
are even more striking in their vocational functioning: the lodge system
enabled patients to maintain gainful employment, while none of the
patients who received treatment within the hospital setting were employed
full time (Figure 4-11). These beneficial results were obtained at an
individual cost of $6 per day, as compared to $14 at the hospital from
100
Lodge group
Control group
80
40
't
20
Figure 4-11. Percentage of time that patients in the lodge and hospital pro-
grams were employed full time for 40 months of follow-up. Fairweather et al.,
1969.
278 POSITIVE CONTROL
which the participants were drawn, $12 at a local state hospital, $45 at a
local private hospital, and $56 at a local county hospital.
The main purpose of the type of residential program discussed above
is to create a semi-autonomous subcommunity in which marginal indi-
rewards.
Some suggestive evidence concerning the relative efficacy of individ-
ual and group contingency systems is provided by Wolf & Risley ( 1967 )
They studied the amount of disruptive classroom behavior displayed by
a child in the absence of any special reinforcement and during subse-
quent periods when either she alone earned five points, or she and her
immediate peers each earned one point for her commendable behavior.
It is interesting to note that the child's activities were more effectively
controlled under the peer contingency even though it produced only one-
fifth of the amount of reinforcement provided on the individual basis.
Apparently, through the group reward, change agents were able to enlist
the peers' aid in modifying the behavior of their companion. The findings
of the present case study, and those cited earlier, are sufficiently interest-
ing to warrant further systematic exploration of the effects of different
types of group contingency structures on social performance.
Group-oriented reinforcement practices have been adopted on a
society-wide basis in the Soviet Union for the explicit purpose of develop-
ing strong collectivistic morality in its citizenry ( Bronfenbrenner, 1962).
This aim isimplemented by use of school collectives where children's
behavior is regulated by rewards and punishments administered on a
group basis, so that all members of a given social unit are affected by
the actions of each individual. Socialization at the school is commenced
in the primary grades by assigning children to row units. Daily records
are kept of each group's performance on a variety of social and academic
activities. The grades that a person receives are based on the overall
Summary
The present chapter reviewed theories of reinforcement in terms of
their relative emphasis upon associative-strengthening or incentive func-
tions of reinforcers. Evidence bearing on alternative explanations of re-
REFERENCES
Allen, K. E., & Harris, F. R. Elimination of a child's excessive scratching by
training the mother reinforcement procedures. Behaviour Research
in
and Therapy, 1966, 4, 79-84.
Allen, K. E., Hart, B., Buell, J. S., Harris, F. R., & Wolf, M. M. Effects of
social reinforcement on isolate behavior of a nursery school child. Child
Development, 1964, 35, 511-518.
Allen, K. E., Henke, L. B., Harris, F. R., Baer, D. M., & Reynolds, N. J.
Control
of hyperactivity by social reinforcement of attending behavior. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 1967, 58, 231-237.
Atkinson, R. C. Computerized instruction and the learning process. American
Psychologist, 1968, 23, 225-239.
Atthowe, J. M., Jr., & Krasner, L. Preliminary report on the application of
contingent reinforcement procedures (token economy) on a "chronic"
psychiatric ward. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1968, 73, 37-43.
Ayllon, T. Intensive treatment of psychotic behavior by stimulus satiation and
food reinforcement. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1963, 1, 53-62.
Ayllon, T., & Azrin, N. H. Reinforcement and instructions with mental patients.
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1964, 7, 327-331.
Ayllon, T., & Azrin, N. H. The measurement and reinforcement of behavior
of psychotics. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1965,
8, 357-383.
References 285
References 287
J.
M. Shlien (Ed.), Research in psychotherapy. Washington, D. C:
American Psychological Association, 1968. Pp. 103-129.
Lovaas, O. I., Berberich, J.
P., Perloff, B. F., & Schaeffer, B. Acquisition of
imitative speech by schizophrenic children. Science, 1966, 151, 705-707.
Lovaas, O. I., Freitag, G., Kinder, M. Rubenstein, B. D., Schaeffer, B., &
I.,
Martin, M., Burkholder, R., Rosenthal, T. L., Tharp, R. G., & Thorne, G. L.
Programming behavior change and reintegration into school milieux of
extreme adolescent deviates. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1968, 6,
371-383.
Meehl, P. E. On the circularity of the law of effect. Psychological Bulletin,
1950, 47, 52-75.
Miles, R. C. Learning in kittens with manipulatory, exploratory, and food in-
centives. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1958, 51,
39-42.
Miller, L. B., & Estes, B. W. Monetary reward and motivation in discrimination
learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1961, 61, 501-504.
Miller, N. E. Some reflections on the law of effect produce a new alternative
to drive reduction. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motiva-
tion: 1963. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1963. Pp. 65-112.
Miller, N. E., & Dollard, ]. Social learning and imitation. New Haven: Yale
University Press, 1941.
Miller, N. E., & Kessen, M. L. Reward effects of food via stomach fistula com-
pared with those of food via mouth. Journal of Comparative and Physio-
logical Psychology, 1952, 45, 555-564.
Minge, M. R., & Ball, T. S. Teaching of self-help skills to profoundly retarded
patients. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1967, 71, 864-868.
Mowrer, O. H. Learning theory and behavior. New York: Wiley, 1960.
Murray, E. J. A content-analysis method for studying psychotherapy. Psycho-
logical Monographs, 1956, 70 (13, Whole No. 420).
Myers, A. K., & Miller, N. E. Failure to find a learned drive based on hunger:
Evidence for learning motivated by exploration. Journal of Comparative
and Physiological Psychology, 1954, 47, 428-436.
Narrol, H. G. Experimental application of reinforcement principles to the
analysis and treatment of hospitalized alcoholics. Quarterly Journal of
Studies on Alcohol, 1967, 28, 105-115.
Odom, R. D. Effects of auditory and visual stimulus deprivation and satiation
on children's performance in an operant task. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 1964, 1, 16-25.
290 POSITIVE CONTROL
the ongoing behavior. These studies reveal that both types of aversive
contingencies decrease responding, but there is disagreement as to which
method produces greater behavioral suppression. In the original experi-
ment, Estes (1944) found that animals which had been shocked only at
times when they were not engaging in lever-pressing behavior displayed
essentially the same degree of suppression and subsequent recovery of
lever-pressing responses as subjects whose punishment was strictly con-
tingent upon the occurrence of the response. In a further test of whether
suppressive effects are governed bv environmental stimuli or response-
produced cues, animals whose lever-pressing responses had been pun-
ished were left in the situation for an adaptation period with the lever
removed. This arrangement prevented elicitation of the punished re-
sponse but permitted the neutralization of threatening situational cues.
A subsequent test for extinction revealed that mere re-exposure to the
fear-provoking situation with no further unpleasant experiences resulted
in almost complete extinction of conditioned suppression. The findings of
this study thus suggested that internal cues accompanying the punished
response exercised relatively little influence upon the inhibitory process.
Hunt & Bradv (1955) extended the above research in a comparative
study of the influence of response-contingent and stimulus-correlated
punishment upon the acquisition, generalization, and extinction of condi-
tioned suppression of responses that were intermittently rewarded. For
subjects in the stimulus group, shocks were contiguously associated with
a tone, but lever-pressing responses were never punished; on the other
hand, in the response condition, the tone was presented and animals
were shocked only when they pressed the lever in the presence of the
auditory stimulus. Both procedures resulted in almost complete response
suppression whenever the tone was presented. The method designed
specifically to endow the environmental cue with aversive properties,
however, produced greater emotional disturbance, and greater general-
ized inhibition thatwas more resistant to extinction. Essentially identical
results were obtained in an earlier study (Hunt & Bradv, 1951) even
though subjects in the response-contingent treatment received more
shocks. In a well-designed experiment that equated for the number and
temporal distribution of shocks, Hoffman & Fleshier (1965) found that
animals that were punished only if they responded in the presence of
certain cues displayed less behavioral suppression and extinguished more
rapidlv than their counterparts that were punished during presentations
of the same cues without regard to their behavior. The foregoing results
thus provide evidence that, under certain conditions, inhibitions are
primarily situation- rather than response-bound.
At variance with the above conclusion, Azrin (1956) found that
response-produced punishment was considerably more effective than
300 AVERSIVE CONTROL
attendant inhibitory responses are not evoked until after the disapproved
act has been completed.
Before evaluating the main findings of these studies it should be
noted that tests of internalized behavioral control typically involve so
many external stimulus supports that response suppression cannot be
attributed solelv to intrinsically mediated consequences. Post-training
measures of self-control are characteristically obtained by the same ex-
perimenter, during the same experimental session, in the same experi-
mental room, in which children are presented either identical or similar
302 AVERSIVE CONTROL
fects; while intense punishment typically results in large and stable reduc-
tions in behavior.
The conflicting evidence may be interpreted in several ways. In the
above experiments, punishing consequences were actually administered
contingent upon occurrence of transgressive behavior, whereas the dis-
sonance studies involved a single verbal threat of punishment. A second
and more critical difference concerns the type of behavior that is being
controlled. In the dissonance paradigm approach responses are inhibited
toward one of several positive alternatives. Under these advantageous
conditions the instigation to transgression is apparently so weak that a
mild verbal threat is sufficient to produce compliance in all subjects re-
gardless of whether the prohibiting agent is present or absent. Given a
response tendency of any strength, transgressive behavior is ordinarily
performed more frequently in situations free of social surveillance than
when the disapproving agent is physically present (Hicks, 1968). By
contrast, in studies of aversive control investigators either select responses
that are highly resistant to change, or the behavior to be eliminated is
1.0
0.9
0.8
1 ° 5
CD
5.0.4
Q.
w 0.3
. /^— Pre -stress
0.2 gradient
Y
-
0.1
0.0 I l l I I l
verbal aids. By clearly labeling the modes of behavior that are permissible
and those that are punishable, and by specifying the times and places at
which certain courses of action are appropriate or unsuitable, greater
specificity of punishment effects can be ensured.
about sex and remain sexually inhibited in later life when such behavior
is socially approved and expected of him. When marked temporal or sit-
siderably easier, though less effective, for example, to punish the antiso-
cial behavior of delinquents than to remove the subcultural contingencies
that mold and control their actions.
In most of the research reviewed earlier, punishment was applied to
responses after the rewards maintaining them were removed, in order to
determine whether the addition of aversive consequences accelerated the
Of considerably greater significance are investigations
extinction process.
of the effects of punishment on behavior that is concurrently maintained
by positive reinforcement, since the response patterns that people fre-
quently attempt to modify result in some rewarding outcomes for the
performer. The available evidence generally indicates that punishment
does not have enduring reductive effects on behavior that is simultane-
ously being maintained by a favorable schedule of positive reinforcement.
Mild and moderately punishing stimuli typically reduce the occurrence
of intermittently reinforced behavior, but as punishment is continued
subjects adapt to the aversive consequences and exhibit some recovery
of responsiveness even while the punishment contingency is still in effect
(Azrin, 1959, 1960; Holz, Azrin, & Ulrich, 1963). Parents who make fre-
subjects could turn the shock off completely by striking a pedal. Since
Presentation of Negative Reinforcers 315
lay on that board with no occupation but thinking. And plan future jobs.
to reward competing tendencies. It has also been shown that even mild
punishment, which has more informative than inhibitory value, may facil-
itate behavioral change to the same extent as severe levels of punishment,
provided that alternative responses are concomitantly rewarded (Boe,
1964).
In a comprehensive analysis of punishment effects Solomon ( 1964
has termed the widespread belief that punishment is only a temporarily
SPEECH DISORDERS
Aversive contingencies have been extensively employed by Goldia-
mond (1965a) in both experimental production of stuttering behavior
and its elimination. Before discussing the details of this approach and its
28 Control
Experimental
24
20
g> 16
4 -
12 3 4
Readings
5 6 7
200
160
120
100
80
60
40
20 -
w 10
CJ
I 8
~ 4
0.6
0.4
0.2
0.1
gradually removed. By the eighth and final day the client was reading 256
words per minute without manifesting a single disfluency.
Quantitative data are presented for eight chronic stutterers. In each
case fluent speech was achieved and maintained in the laboratory situa-
tioneven when rate of verbalization was increased and negative conse-
quences withdrawn. Goldiamond also reports concomitant improvements
in clients' speech in naturalistic settings, but apparently no objective as-
326 AVERSIVE CONTROL
26
256 WPM
25
24
23
22
21
20
o 19
x
-Q 18
•E 17
"g 16
S/DF
| 15
5
£ 14
-. 13
£l2
a)
I 11
Reading rate
| 10
| 9
Io 8
5 7
Stuttering rate
2
J L J L J
12
I
3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Oct. 29 30 N1 4 h5H 6 h-7H
Sessions
straints.
The maintaining conditions of self-injurious behavior are not fully
understood, but several experiments have shown that it is amenable to
control through variation of reinforcing consequences. Lovaas, Freitag,
Gold, & Kassorla (1965) found that the self-injurious responses are
readily cued off by stimuli signifying withdrawal of social reinforcement
for other behaviors and that they tend to increase in frequency and
intensity when social reactions are made contingent upon their occur-
rence. Demonstrations that self-injurious behavior can be reduced through
reinforcement of physically incompatible responses and increased by
extinction of competing activities cast little light on the variables that
control this behavior. Of much greater interest are the changes pro-
duced by variation of the contingencies applied directly to self-injurious
behavior.
Applications of Aversive Contingency Systems 329
lit T~T«
14 16 18 20 22 24 26 28
|
2 2 1 121131214455
pnJJ f_t
J
-!
N
to the boy it was employed as the consequent event in the first phase of
the treatment program. A series of daily walks was arranged in each of
which the therapist removed his hand from the child's grasp and ceased
talking whenever a self-injurious response occurred; physical contact
was reinstated if the boy did not hit himself for a period of three seconds.
As can be seen in Figure 5-7, the time-out contingency produced a
dramatic reduction in self-injurious behavior.
In the second phase of the program, response-contingent shock was
used to eliminate the remaining head-banging that threatened further
damage to his eyes. It was explained to him that if he continued to hit
himself he would receive painful shocks. The aversive contingency, com-
bined with verbal praise and affectionate reactions for desirable be-
havior, permanently eliminated the self-injurious responding. Whereas
previously the boy had been physically restrained in bed, after the
treatment program was completed he participated freely in daily activities
with increased enjoyment and spontaneity.
Risley (1968) provides a detailed report of a case in which social
consequences were totally ineffective in decreasing self -injurious behavior.
Applications of Aversive Contingency Systems 331
10.0
Control Experimental
D Control Experimental
Days
MOTOR DYSFUNCTIONS
In one of the early applications of aversive contingencies, Liversedge
& Sylvester (1955; Sylvester & Liversedge, 1960) treated 39 cases of
writer's cramp with a procedure employing response-contingent shock.
In the majority of cases, tremors and spasms of hand muscles were
elicited only by highly specific writing stimuli, but the same muscle groups
were unaffected when involved in nonwriting situations. As has been
found in other types of deviant behavior, the individuals exhibiting this
occupational impairment shared no common psychological characteristics,
suggesting that specific reinforcement contingencies rather than psychody-
namic factors were the critical determinants. It is therefore not surprising
that a number of these clients who had undergone various conventional
forms of psychotherapy experienced little or no amelioration of their
"craft neurosis." Consequently, Liversedge & Sylvester explored the
efficacy of aversive consequences for altering each component of the
physical disorder.
In order to remove tremors, one element of the motor disability, the
clients were required to insert a stylus into a series of progressively smaller
holes; each time the stylus made contact with the side of the hole it
factory quality was restored in 24 of the cases; the clients were able to
resume work, which often involved writing for extended periods, and
follow-up studies conducted up to four and a half years later disclosed
that the improvement was being maintained. Five clients responded
favorably to the treatment but subsequently experienced a recurrence of
muscular dvsfunction. while 10 cases showed no improvement. The
had exhibited the motor disability over a longer period (6 to 21
failures
Applications of Aversive Contingency Systems 333
years), which may partly explain why they were less responsive to the
treatment.
The procedures devised by Liversedge & Sylvester are sufficiently
effective to merit controlled studies to isolate factors responsible for the
success of this treatment approach. Since the technique involves both
response guidance and punishment of spasmodic and tremorous responses,
it is conceivable that guided retraining with nonaversive feedback may in
itself effect needed regarding the
changes. In addition, information is
logical treatments. According to the client's report, the tics developed after
a frightening experience in the armv when he awoke one night with a
choking sensation accompanied bv a momentarv inabilitv to breathe or
swallow. At the time of the study, his motor pattern included contractions
of neck, shoulder, chest, and abdominal muscles, head-nodding, eye-
blinking, mouth-opening, other facial movements, and swallowing diffi-
culties.
SEXUAL DEVIATIONS
Chapter 8 describes classical conditioning procedures designed to
eliminate sexual aberrations by endowing stimuli that elicit the behavior
with aversive properties. Some attempts have been made to bring sex-
ually deviant behavior under control through response-contingent aver-
sive stimulation. Feldman & MacCulloch (1964, 1965) provide a detailed
account of a treatment method, primarily based on an avoidance con-
ditioning paradigm, that they have developed for the modification of
homosexuality.
Clients are asked initially to rate the attractiveness of an extensive se-
depicting both clothed and completely nude males. A simi-
ries of slides
duced when the client consistently requests the return of the preceding
feminine item. This same process isrepeated with succeeding pairs of
stimuli in the pictorial hierarchy. A typical session involves completion of
about 30 trials and requires some 20 minutes to conduct. The treatment
series, which averages about 15 sessions, is continued until a client ex-
Applications of Aversive Contingency Systems 337
that after several sessions the boys lost their interest in female under-
wear and permanently discontinued their clothesline forays. The methods
employed in the above studies appear to hold some promise for modify-
ing deviant sexual behavior, but full evaluation must await controlled
studies.
was not equated, and possible order effects were not controlled. How-
ever, the findings of Tolman & Mueller are essentially corroborated by
McMillan (1967), who assessed the relative efficacy of contingent shock
and temporary withdrawal of rewards in eliminating a concurrently re-
inforced response. Both types of punishers reduced responding to about
the same degree, but the time-out procedure was associated with less
behavioral recovery.
The adjunctive use punishment bv reinforcement withdrawal has
of
certain advantages over physically aversive procedures. As previously
shown, aversive interventions may arouse fear and avoidance of punish-
ing agents, and thus weaken their potential influence. In contrast, meth-
ods that chiefly involve the removal of positive reinforcers not only
generate much weaker emotional effects, but they tend to foster and
maintain orientation toward the agents who control the desired positive
resources. If restoration of the positive reinforcers is made contingent
Removal of Positive Reinforcers 341
tween the child's disruptive conduct and its social consequences. At this
meeting, each participant's role is specifically outlined. It is explained
that the school cannot permit a child continually to disrupt the educa-
tional activities ofan entire class. The child's help is enlisted to control,
as best he can, behavior that has disturbing effects on all concerned.
Whenever he displays behavior that exceeds certain clearly defined
limits, the teacher must ask him to leave school for the remainder of the
which the child is removed, and the attractiveness of the setting from
which he is withdrawn. Findings of studies employing time-out contin-
gencies to control grossly deviant behavior indicate that much briefer
periods of exclusion might work as well, or even better, than full-day
suspensions from classroom activities.
Summary
The present chapter has discussed the processes whereby response
patterns are eliminated through the use of punishing stimuli. Punishing
consequences may involve either removal of positive reinforcers or
presentation of aversive events. Punishment is believed to achieve its
REFERENCES
Appel, j. B. Punishment in the squirrel monkev Saimiri sciurea. Science, 1961,
133,36.
Aronfreed, }. Conduct and conscience. New York: Academic Press, 1968.
Aronfreed, J., 6c Reber. A. Internalized behavioral suppression and the timing
of social punishment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1965,
1,3-16.
Aromon. E.. & Carlsmith, [. M. Effect of the severity of threat on the devalua-
tion of forbidden behavior. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,
1963,66,584-588.
Ayllon, T.. 6v Azrin, N. II. Punishment as a discriminative stimulus and con-
Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. Adolescent aggression. New York: Ronald, 1959.
Barrett, B. Reduction in rate of multiple ties by free operant conditioning
methods. Journal of Xervous and Mental Diseases, 1962, 135, 187-195.
Beach, F. A., Conovitz, M. W., Steinberg, F., & Goldstein, A. C. Experimental
inhibition and restoration of mating behavior in male rats. Journal of
Genetic Psychology, 1956, 89, 165-181.
Beech, H. R. The symptomatic treatment of writer's cramp. In H. J. Eysenck
(Ed.), Behavior therapy and the neuroses. New York: Pergamon, 1960.
Pp. 349-372.
Benton, A. A. Effects of the timing of negative response consequences on the
observational learning of resistance to temptation in children. Disserta-
tion Abstracts, 1967, 27, 2153-2154.
Black, A. H. The extinction of avoidance responses under curare. Journal of
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1958, 51, 519-524.
Black, A. H., Carlson, X. J., & Solomon, R. L. Exploratory studies of the con-
ditioning of autonomic responses in curarized dogs. Psychological Mono-
graphs, 1962, 76 (29, Whole No. 548).
Bloodstein. O. Conditions under which stuttering is reduced or absent: a re-
view of literature. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1949, 14,
295-302.
Boe, E. E. Extinction as a function of intensity of punishment, amount of train-
ing, and reinforcement of a competing response. Canadian Journal of
Psychology, 1964, 18, 328-342.
Boe, E. E., & Church, R. M. Permanent effects of punishment during extinc-
tion. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1967, 63,
486-192.
Bolles, R. C. Theory of motivation. New
York: Harper & Row, 1967.
Bond, I. K., & Evans, D. R. Avoidance therapy: Its use in two cases of under-
wear fetishism. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 1967, 96, 1160-
1162.
Brookshire, R. H., & Martin, R. R. The differential effects of three verbal
punishers on the disfluencies of normal speakers. Journal of Speech and
Hearing Research, 1967, 10, 496-505.
Brutten, E. J., & Shoemaker, D. J. The modification of stuttering. Englewood
Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall, 1967.
Bucher, B., & Lovaas, O. I. Use of aversive stimulation in behavior modifica-
tion. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Miami symposium on the prediction of be-
havior, 1967: Aversive stimulation. Coral Gables, Fla.: University of
Miami Press, 1968. Pp. 77-145.
Buehler, R. E.. Patterson, G. R., & Furniss, J.
M. The reinforcement of behavior
in institutional settings. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1966, 4, 157-
167.
Burchard, J., & Tvler, V., Jr. The modification of delinquent behaviour through
operant conditioning. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1965, 2, 245-
250.
Camp, D. S. Response suppression as a function of punishment intensity7 and
of response-punishment contiguity. Dissertation Abstracts, 1965, 26, 3494.
Chapman, R. W. School suspension as therapy. Personnel and Guidance Jour-
1962, 40, 731-732.
nal,
Church, R. M. The varied effects of punishment on behavior. Psychological
Review, 1963, 70, 369-402.
Cohen, H. L. Educational therapy: The design of learning environments. In
350 AVERSIVE CONTROL
J.
M. Shlien (Ed.), Research in psychotherapy. Washington, D. C: Ameri-
can Psychological Association, 1968. Pp. 21-53.
Colman, A. D., & Baker, S. L., Jr. Utilization of an operant conditioning model
for the treatment of character and behavior disorders in a military setting.
Unpublished manuscript, Walter Reed Army Institute of Research, Wash-
ington, D. C, 1968.
Daly, D. A., & Cooper, E. B. Rate of stuttering adaptation under two electro-
shock conditions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1967, 5, 49-54.
Dardano, J. F., & Sauerbrunn, D. Selective reinforcement of concurrent pro-
gressive ratio behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,
1964, 7, 51-65.
Davis, D. M. The relation of repetitions in the speech of young children to
certain measures of language maturity and situational factors: Part I.
Journal of Speech Disorders, 1939, 4, 303-318.
Davis, D. M. The relation of repetitions in the speech of young children to
certain measures of language maturity and situational factors: Part II.
Journal of Speech Disorders, 1940, 5, 235-246.
Desiderato, O. Generalization of conditioned suppression. Journal of Compara-
tive and Physiological Psychology, 1964, 59, 434-437.
Dinsmoor, J. A. Punishment: I. The avoidance hypothesis. Psychological Re-
view, 1954, 61, 34-46.
Elliott, R., & Tighe, T.
J.
Breaking the cigarette habit: A technique involving
threatened loss of money. Psychological Record, 1968, 18, 503-513.
Estes, W. K. An experimental study of punishment. Psychological Monographs,
1944,57 (3, Whole No. 263).
Feldman, M. P., & MacCulloch, M. J. A systematic approach to the treatment
of homosexuality by conditioned aversion: Preliminarv report. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 1964, 121, 167-171.
Feldman, M. P., & MacCulloch, M. J. The application of anticipatory avoid-
ance learning to the treatment of homosexuality. 1. Theory, technique
and preliminary results. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1965, 2, 165-
183.
Ferster, C. B. Control of behavior in chimpanzees and pigeons by time out
from positive reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 1958, 72 (8,
Whole No. 461).
Ferster, C. B. Suppression of a performance under differential reinforcement
of low rates by a pre-time-out stimulus. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 1960, 3, 143-153.
Ferster, C. B., Appel, J. B., & Hiss, R. A. The effect of drugs on a fixed-ratio
performance suppressed by a pre-time-out stimulus. Journal of the Ex-
perimental Analysis of Behavior, 1962, 5, 73-88.
Flanagan, B., Goldiamond, I., & Azrin, N. H. Instatement of stuttering in
normally fluent individuals through operant procedures. Science, 1959,
130, 979-981.
Fowler, H., & Miller, N. E. Facilitation and inhibition of runway performance
by hind- and forepaw shock of various intensities. Journal of Comparative
and Physiological Psychology, 1963, 56, 801-805.
Freedman, J.
L. Long-term behavioral effects of cognitive dissonance. Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology, 1965, 1, 145-155.
Goldiamond, I. Fluent and nonfluent speech (stuttering) Analysis and operant
:
Shames, G. H., & Sherrick, C. G., Jr. A discussion of nonfluency and stuttering
as operant behavior. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1963,
28, 3-18.
Sheehan, J. G. The modification of stuttering through non-reinforcement.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1951, 46, 51-63.
Sheehan, J. G. An integration of psychotherapy and speech therapy through
a conflict theory of stuttering. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders,
1954, 19, 474-482.
Sheehan, J. G. Conflict theory of stuttering. In J. Eisenson (Ed.), Stuttering:
A symposium. New York: Harper, 1958. Pp. 121-166.
Sheehan, J. G., & Martyn, M. M. Spontaneous recovery from stuttering.
Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1966, 9, 121-135.
Siegel, G. M., & Martin, R. R. Punishment of disfluencies in normal speakers.
Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1966, 9, 208-218.
Sloane, H. N., Jr., Johnston, M. K., & Bijou, S. W. Successive modification of
aggressive behavior and aggressive fantasy play by management of con-
tingencies. Unpublished manuscript, University of Illinois, 1968.
Solomon, R. L. Punishment. American Psychologist, 1964, 19, 239-253.
Solomon, R. L., & Turner, L. H. Discriminative classical conditioning in dogs
paralyzed by curare can later control discriminative avoidance responses
in the normal state. Psychological Review, 1962, 69, 202-219.
Solomon, R. L., Turner, L. H., & Lessac, M. S. Some effects of delay of pun-
ishment on resistance to temptation in dogs. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 1968, 8, 233-238.
a
Stassi, E. J.
Disfluency of normal speakers and reinforcement. Journal of Speech
and Hearing Research, 1961, 4, 358-361.
Storms, L. M., Boroczi, C, & Broen, W. E., Jr. Effects of punishment as a
function of strain of rat and duration of shock. Journal of Comparative
and Physiological Psychology, 1963, 56, 1022-1026.
Sylvester, J. D., & Liversedge, L. A. Conditioning and the occupational cramps.
In H. J. Evsenck (Ed.), Behaviour therapy and the neuroses. New York:
Pergamon,'l960. Pp. 334-348.
Tate, B. G., & Baroff, G. S. Aversive control of self-injurious behavior in a
psychotic bow Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1966, 4, 281-287.
Timmons, E. O. Weakening verbal behavior: A comparison of four methods.
Journal of General Psychology, 1962, 67, 155-158.
Tolman, C. W., & Mueller. M. R. Laboratory control of toe-sucking in a young
rhesus monkey by two kinds of punishment. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 1964. 7, 323-325.
Tyler, V. O., Jr., & Brown, G. D. The use of swift, brief isolation as a group
control device for institutionalized delinquents. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 1967, 5, 1-9.
Van Riper, C. The influence of empathic response on frequencv of stuttering.
'
COMPETING-RESPONSE THEORY
In the interference interpretation of extinction (Guthrie, 1935; Estes,
1959), the decrement of a response during nonreinforced evocation re-
sults from the appearance of incompatible responses strong enough to
supersede the ongoing behavior. These competing responses may be
linked either to the same stimuli or to different stimulus events. In the
latter case, response diminution primarily reflects external inhibition
brought about by simultaneous occurrence of new prepotent stimuli that
evoke antagonistic tendencies, or by attentional shifts to other distinctive
features of the environment. In the former instance, which essentially in-
volves a counterconditioning process, extinction results from the develop-
ment of new incompatible responses to the same stimuli, or the reappear-
ance of interfering responses that have been previously learned.
Any conditions, apart from the omission of reinforcement, that reduce
the probability of occurrence of the original behavior will facilitate the
appearance of competing response patterns. Some of these conditions,
358 EXTINCTION
In the initial study (Page & Hall, 1953), animals learned to avoid shocks
administered in one compartment of a shuttle box by escaping into a neu-
tral chamber. The avoidance responses were then extinguished in two
different ways: Control animals were given regular extinction trials in
which they performed avoidance responses until they stopped running
from the threatening compartment; the experimental subjects were de-
tained in the fear-provoking box for the first five trials and then given
traditional extinction trials. The barrier group extinguished approximately
three times as fast as the controls.
In order to determine if elimination of avoidance responses in the first
study was due to the acquisition of competing protective responses or to
neutralization of the fear-evoking stimuli, Page ( 1955 ) conducted a sec-
ond experiment that proceeded in the following manner: The initial phase
of the study, which duplicated the procedure of the earlier experiment,
similarly demonstrated that animals first detained in the threatening com-
partment subsequently extinguished much more rapidly than controls
given regular extinction trials. In the second phase of the study, designed
to measure the aversive properties of the conditioned stimuli, the animals
Interpretations of the Extinction Process 359
were placed in the neutral chamber after being deprived of food and the
speed with which they entered the shock compartment for food was meas-
ured. In addition, a control group of animals never exposed to shock stim-
ulation was tested. The approach response latencies averaged approxi-
mately 25, 60, and 110 seconds for the control, regular extinction, and
response-prevention groups, respectively. It seems clear from the two sets
of data that under forced exposure to fear-arousing stimuli a dominant
mode of avoidance behavior was eliminated, but the animals nevertheless
retained some fear of the negative compartment. These findings indicate
that the threatening stimuli continued to generate aversive stimulation
and whatever protective responses the animals adopted in the situation
were reinforced by the omission of painful shocks.
In the extinction of avoidance behavior, absence of expected adverse
consequences provides a powerful source of reinforcement for competing
responses. In the elimination of behavior previously maintained by posi-
tive reinforcement, reduction of aversive emotional arousal produced by
omission of anticipated rewards may likewise constitute the main rein-
forcement for antagonistic responses. According to the frustration inter-
pretation of extinction (Amsel, 1962; Wagner, 1966), nonrewarded repe-
tition of responses generates aversive arousal capable of evoking conflict-
ing response tendencies that interfere with the ongoing behavior. By sup-
planting the nonrewarded behavior the competing responses reduce dis-
turbing emotional arousal and are thereby negatively reinforced. Consist-
ent with these theoretical speculations, it has been shown that nonreward
produces aversive effects analogous to punishment operations. Stimuli
previously associated with nonreward acquire arousal properties (Wag-
ner, 1963), their presence attenuates responding (Amsel & Surridge,
1964), and escape from cues signifying nonreward can reinforce new per-
formances (Wagner, 1963).
The appearance of new behavior that is antagonistic to nonreinforced
responses will undoubtedly accelerate the extinction process. In many
instances, however, rapid elimination of nonrewarded behavior results
from the development of expectations about the future probability of re-
inforcement rather than from the gradual conditioning of incompatible
responses to the same controlling stimuli. The discrimination theory of
extinction, which reviewed next, treats extinction
is as a centrally, rather
than a peripherally, mediated phenomenon.
DISCRIMINATION THEORY
about, the original one. There no reason to suppose that under such
is
larly associated with primary reinforcement (Moltz & Maddi, 1956). The
latter outcome would likewise serve to hasten the extinction process.
The research cited above has been primarily confined to infrahuman
subjects; perhaps for this reason fractional anticipatory response mecha-
nisms and associated proprioceptive cues have frequently been invoked
as explanatory factors. In the case of humans, who possess superior dis-
criminative and symbolic capacities, the informative value of observa-
tional experiences regarding reinforcement contingencies would assume
considerably greater importance in eliminating nonreinforced behavior.
According to this more cognitive view, extinction primarily reflects the
operation of inhibitory sets rather than the loss of behavior or its discon-
nection from previous controlling stimuli. For this reason behavior can be
discarded even without having been performed on the basis of observa-
tion that such behavior is no longer reinforced, it can be promptlv re-
placed by more utilitarian modes of response, and readily reinstated
whenever the original reinforcement contingencies are restored.
The theory of cognitive control of extinction is supported by several
lines of evidence, some of which are discussed fully in the concluding
chapter. Extinction is greatly facilitated by awareness that the usual con-
sequences have been discontinued; and, conversely, it is retarded under
diverting instructions that reduce discriminability of the change in rein-
forcement (Spence, 1966). In fact, when presentation of reinforcement is
Continuous-noninformed »
Partial -informed «»
4 5 6
Extinction Trial
tablished under these less advantageous conditions are indeed more per-
sistent than those that are rewarded continuously, immediately, and at
little expenditure of effort.
Other investigators have, of course, attributed the influence on extinc-
tion of these reinforcement variables to the operation of other mechanisms
involving discrimination processes, frustration effects, and countercondi-
tioning of competing responses. These alternative theories, therefore, need
to be tested under conditions where they make opposing predictions. For
example, resistance to extinction following both a highly variable and an
entirely regular schedule of the same total partial reinforcement has been
studied. The absolute number of unrewarded trials is identical in both
conditions; consequently, subjects are provided with the same number of
occasions on which dissonance could be aroused and presumably reduced.
Dissonance theorv would predict the same rate of extinction under both
conditions, whereas discrimination theorv would lead one to expect the
unpredictable schedule to produce the more durable behavior. Bitterman
and his associates have conducted several such experiments in which
subjects are reinforced on 50 percent of the training trials; for one group
the rewards are administered haphazardly, while subjects in the other
group are regularly reinforced on odd-numbered trials. Behavior is much
more resistant to extinction (Tyler, Wortz, & Bitterman, 1953) after ran-
dom 50 percent reinforcement than after regularly alternated 50 percent
reinforcement. Analogous results are obtained in extinction of autonomic
reactions which do not involve performance of any effortful responses
( Longenecker, Krauskopf, & Bitterman, 1952). Moreover, increased train-
ing, which provides more dissonance-reducing opportunities, facilities ex-
tinction following alternating reinforcement, but it has no effect on be-
havior rewarded according to an unpredictable pattern (Capaldi, 1958).
Whether or not findings of the type reported above contradict dis-
sonance theory cannot be resolved as long as there exists some ambiguity
as to the conditions most likely to produce high dissonance. Do subjects
who repeatedly perform effortful behavior knowing that it is unlikely to
be rewarded experience more or less dissonance than if they had expected
rewarding outcomes? The authors assume that the latter condition is more
dissonance producing. It would seem, however, that subjects in the former
condition are exhibiting the more irrational behavior and would, there-
fore, experience greater pressure to justify their actions by persuading
themselves that they really enjoy the activity. On the other hand, subjects
who performed because they expected to be rewarded have adequate
justificationwithout needing to endow the activity with additional attrac-
tions. If reluctantperformance of an action is accepted as behavioral evi-
dence for the existence of dissonance ( Lawrence & Festinger, 1962 ) then
subjects trained under alternating reinforcement in the above experiments
366 EXTINCTION
6.0
5.0
4.0
3.0
2.0
1.0
1 3 10 12 14 15 17 19 21 23 25 27
Baseline "Sit-down" Baseline "Sit-down" Praise given for sitting
commands commands
Consecutive Morning Observations
Figure 6-2. Number of children standing in class during baseline periods and
when such behavior produced verbal admonishments or incompatible responses
were positively reinforced. Madsen et al., 1968.
55 First extinction
50 Second extinction
45
</>
(D
| 40
^ 35
1
g 30
° 25
o
| 15
Q
10
3 4 5 6 7 8 10 11
Figure 6-3. Duration of crying in two extinction series in which tantrum be-
havior was no longer socially reinforced. Williams, 1959.
chotic talk occurred during the ninth week of treatment, when, unknown
to the ward personnel, a social worker had been conducting interviews
with the patient and inadvertently reinforcing her psychotic verbaliza-
tions; the effects of these interviews generalized to the patient's interac-
tions with the nursesand other patients as well. Reinforcements provided
by hospital employees and other visitors to the ward produced other tem-
porary increases. However, the psychotic talk still remained less frequent
than it had been at the commencement of the extinction program and
370 EXTINCTION
behavior were measured. When extinction was first instituted, the num-
ber of bizarre letters increased from a baseline rate of approximately 13
letters a week to 43 letters in two days, after which it declined and stabi-
lized at about 5 letters a week. The relative frequency of disturbed be-
havior also decreased from 71 percent during the baseline period to only
16 percent when the extinction program was in effect. Both disturbed
behaviors and the number of psychotic letters increased after the staff
reverted to their own preferred practices, whereas deviant performances
diminished when the nurses were again persuaded to withhold attention
from psychotic verbalizations.
As part of a program of research in the development of procedures
for the modification of psychotic behavior, Ayllon and his associates
(Ayllon & Haughton, 1962; Ayllon & Michael, 1959) provide numerous
examples in which deviant behavior of hospitalized psychotics is extin-
guished by withdrawal of its positively reinforcing consequences. In one
study (Ayllon & Haughton, 1962), a group of schizophrenics, who exhib-
ited severe eating problems of long standing, remained totally unrespon-
sive to announcements that meals were being served and to other persua-
sive appeals. Consequently, the patients had been individually escorted
to the dining room by ward personnel, spoon-fed, tube-fed, and subjected
to electroshock "therapy" and other forms of infantilizing and punitive
treatments.
It was assumed by the research staff that the nurses' inadvertent social
5 -Day Blocks
Figure 6-4. Reversals in the incidence of psychotic and neutral verbal behavior
as a result of variations in social reinforcement of these two classes of verbali-
zations. Ayllon & Haughton, 1964.
372 EXTINCTION
Baseline
180 fly
Extinction
I 0)
.2 £
E o>
o <i>
90
en CC
. y
°l
20 40 60 80 100
5 -Day Blocks
Figure 6-5. Frequency of somatic complaints during the baseline period and
while somatic verbalizations were successively rewarded with attention and
ignored. The temporary increase in somatic complaints shown by the arrow in
the fourth phase of the treatment coincides with a visit by a relative. Ayllon
& Haughton, 1964.
Extinction of Positively Reinforced Behavior 373
Baseline
180
ri Extinction Reinforcement Extinction
•
-<
90 -
—^S
•/
/••
•
la )
•\
•1
\ • •
• • S^
•• • •
• • \ • • •
• • •^
1
1
o—^f*
10 20 30 40 50
10- Day Blocks
Figure 6-6. Frequency of somatic complaints during the baseline period and
while somatic verbalizations were successively rewarded with attention and
ignored. Ayllon & Haughton, 1964.
376 EXTINCTION
100
With adults
80
60
40
20
Z 100
With children
80
60
40
20
12 3 4 7 8 9 10 11
w\
J I I I L
12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 2122232425
J—
31384051
Baseline Reinforced Reversal Reinforced interaction Post checks
interaction with children
with children
Days
formance of aggressive responses, but for the most part they overlook
skill component.
the essential
The process of acquisition and subsequent utilization of aggressive
behavior is best exemplified in laboratory studies employing markedly
different training and test situations. Walters & Brown (1963) showed
that boys who had been intermittently reinforced with marbles for punch-
ing an automated Bobo doll later exhibited more physically aggressive
behavior toward other children in a competitive situation than did boys
who had received no prior training in punching responses. Conversely,
in the previously reviewed study by Chittenden (1942), children whose
aggressive responses were reduced in a doll-play situation through differ-
entialmodeling and vicarious reinforcement were much less prone to re-
spond aggressively to interpersonal thwarting, both in situational tests
and in their every da v interactions.
There is a substantial bodv of evidence (Bandura & Walters, 1963)
that novel modes of aggressive behavior are readily acquired through
observation of aggressive models. Findings of these controlled investiga-
tions lend support to field studies demonstrating the crucial role of mod-
eling in the genesis of antisocial aggressive behavior (McCord & McCord,
1958), and in the cultural transmission of aggressive response patterns
( Bateson, 1936; Whiting, 1941 ).Modeling influences continue to regulate
aggressive responsiveness to some extent even after the behavior has been
acquired. The behavior of models continually exerts selective control over
the types of responses exhibited by others in any given situation. More-
over, seeing individuals behaving aggressively without adverse conse-
quences reduces restraints in observers, thereby increasing both the fre-
quency with which they engage in aggressive activities (Wheeler, 1966)
and the harshness with which they treat others ( Epstein, 1966; Hartmann,
1969).
After aggressive patterns of behavior have been learned, they can be
maintained by a variety of reinforcing events. Theories that invoke ag-
gressive drives (Dollard et al., 1939; Feshbach, 1964) assume that pain
cues and other injurious consequences experienced by the victim consti-
tute the major reinforcers of aggressive behavior. The process whereby
signs of injury and distress acquire positively reinforcing properties has
never been established. Sears, Maccoby, & Levin (1957) suggest that ex-
pressions of pain and discomfort produced in others by aggressive behav-
ior are frequently followed by removal of frustrations or rewarding out-
comes for the aggressor. Through such paired association pain cues
acquire conditioned reward value. One might also expect expressions of
suffering to gain satisfying properties under conditions of interdependent
competing consequences in which success for one member produces pun-
380 EXTINCTION
ishing outcomes for the other. Feshbach (1964 ) offers a somewhat differ-
ent interpretation of the phenomenon. Through example and precept
children learn a retaliation norm: infliction of injury requires that the
initial aggressor must be hurt. It is further assumed that perception of
382 EXTINCTION
384 EXTINCTION
modeling cues. The fact that the negative findings occurred in studies in
which other variables were highly influential lends support to the view
that frustration is only one, but not necessarily the most important, vari-
able determining aggressive behavior. Indeed, according to social-learn-
ing theory, one could readily produce highly aggressive individuals by
providing them with successful aggressive models and intermittently re-
warding aggressive behavior, while keeping frustration at a low level. It
would follow from the findings reviewed in the preceding sections that
lasting changes in aggressive behavior can be most successfully achieved
by reducing the utilitarian value of aggression through the development
of more effective alternative modes of response.
A variety of social-learning procedures has been employed with suc-
cess in modifying extreme aggressive behavior. Chittenden ( 1942
al., 1966; Sloane, Johnston, & Bijou, 1968; Zeilberger, Sampen, & Sloane,
1968) have eliminated violent temper tantrums and physically assaultive
behavior by reducing the amount of social reinforcement that parents and
teachers provide for such behavior. In the foregoing programs aversive
Extinction of Negatively Reinforced Behavior 385
386 EXTINCTION
-1
-2
B-1 2-4 5-7 8-10 11-13 14-16 17-E 2-4 5-7 8-11
Acquisition Extinction
Trials
eliminated. Using a similar method, Page & Hall (1953) likewise demon-
strated that the response-prevention technique can accelerate extinction,
provided it is employed on every trial in a lengthy series during the initial
phase of extinction. Weinberger (1965) has further shown that the rate
of extinction of avoidance behavior is increased with longer durations
of forced exposure to fear-provoking events.
Response blocking in the presence of aversive stimuli can produce
behavioral changes through several different means. It may extinguish
the aversive properties of threatening stimuli so that they lose their
capacity to evoke fear and avoidance. Alternatively, it may eliminate the
obstructed avoidance responses without altering the arousal potential of
feared stimuli by producing new forms of defensive behavior that are
inevitably successful in forestalling nonexistent threats. This process of
response substitution is best exemplified by Miller's (1948) experiment,
in which animals confined in a threatening situation acquired a long suc-
cession of avoidance responses as each preceding one was obstructed.
The importance of distinguishing between changes reflecting stimulus
neutralization and response substitution is further illustrated by evidence
that subjects that have extinguished avoidance responding to a given CS
may nevertheless be somewhat fearful of that stimulus, as measured by
suppression of rewarded behavior whenever the stimulus is presented
(Kamin, Brimer, & Black, 1963).
Assessments of the varied effects accompanying response prevention
suggest that this method may produce rapid behavioral changes without
achieving fear extinction. This is shown by evidence that, compared to
.
havior.
The clients were requested to perforin threatening activities (e.g.,
touching door knobs, handling dust bins, imagining sexual relations with
the Holy Spirit, eating sausages, etc.) and the nursing staff prevented
them from engaging in the ritualistic behavior designed to forestall fore-
boding consequences. The women displayed intense distress when per-
formance of the ritualistic behavior was first blocked. However, their
emotional reactions gradually diminished, and both avoidance behavior
and the compulsive rituals were substantially reduced after the restric-
tions had been removed. According to follow-up studies, the first client
continued her washing routines, but she was much less disturbed by dirt,
her family relationships improved greatly, she resumed sexual relations,
and she was able to participate in a number of social activities which she
previously had avoided for fear of contamination. The second client de-
creased the ritualistic behaviors from approximately 80 to 4 per day and
the occurrence of intrusive thoughts was similarly reduced. These en-
390 EXTINCTION
Graduated counterconditioning *
Graduated extinction 9
Flooding °
Counterconditioning •—
Regular extinction e—
6 7 10 12
Sessions
The person with a severe neurosis who does reach the psychotherapist
is a specially selected case with extremely strong avoidance tendencies.
Therefore, trying to increase his motivation to approach goals will only
increase his fear and conflict. This increase in misery will tend to drive
him out of therapy. This is indeed what seems to happen. Therapists
Extinction of Negatively Reinforced Behavior 393
tion effects that will generalize to the more strongly inhibited forms of
behavior, thus reducing the entire avoidance gradient. In this manner
anxiety associated with successively closer variants of the desired be-
havior can be progressively extinguished until clients are able to execute
the goal responses without experiencing undue emotional arousal. This
strategy has, in fact, been successfully applied to the modification of
agoraphobias (Jones, 1956; Meyer, 1957; White, 1962), claustrophobias
(Meyer, 1957; Walton & Mather, 1963a), compulsive response patterns
394 EXTINCTION
She could pass men in the street, sit next to them on public vehicles,
wait in shop or bus queues with them and speak to them. She related
396 EXTINCTION
two such incidents with satisfaction. She had waited with a young man,
a stranger, for half an hour at a bus stop and had become engaged in a
lengthy conversation. This almost resulted in a date. On a second occa-
sion she renewed a childhood acquaintanceship with a young man of
her age [p. 167].
provided his own volume control with which he could regulate the
amount of aversive stimulation.
An average of 12 showings of approximately 15 minutes each effec-
tively extinguished the soldiers' intense emotional responses, as shown
by reactions of calm, and even boredom, to scenes that had previously
terrifiedthem. Additional evidence that the soldiers had been successfully
desensitized is provided by their relatively undisturbed responses to a
test film of a Marine invasion depicting intense combat and severe casu-
Moreover, they were able to attend commercial movies, which most
alties.
of them had previously avoided because of the newsreels, and they dis-
played a generalized diminution of emotional responsiveness to a variety
of sounds, noises, and even music to which they had been formerly hy-
persensitive.
Results of the above study cannot be fully evaluated in the absence
of an untreated control group and more systematic assessment of changes
in emotional responsiveness. However, the favorable outcomes yielded
by modeling studies utilizing films graduated in aversiveness (Bandura
& Menlove, 1968) suggest that group extinction procedures involving
pictorially presented threats could be employed effectively to extinguish
common fears that are no longer appropriate.
presented at low intensities that are easily tolerable, and more stressful
situations are gradually introduced as emotional responses to weaker
threats are progressively eliminated. Considering that in laboratory in-
vestigations extinction is typically carried out in relation to aversive
stimuli at training intensity, it is evident that fear extinction can be
achieved without stimulus graduation. Indeed, even prolonged or massive
exposure to aversive stimuli at high intensities may produce rapid and
stable extinction of avoidance responses.
Polin ( 1959) trained animals to jump a hurdle at the sound of a buzzer
in order to avoid electric shock. The animals were then given four days of
differential extinction training: One group received 20 trials daily of
five-second exposures to the buzzer with a physical barrier erected to
prevent the avoidance response; the "flooding" group each day received
4 6 8 10 12
from the case material cited that the heterogeneous cues selected for
extinction could have occurred sequentially in traumatic conditioning.
There is also some ambiguity in the implementation of implosive proce-
dures because no explicit criteria are presented for determining when
treatment should be confined to the evident determinants of avoidance
behavior or extended to hypothetical sources of anxiety. Clients may
therefore be needlessly subjected to aversive stimulation while therapists
are neutralizing hypothesized determinants of questionable relevance.
The loose relationship between conceptual rationale and practice is
further shown in experimental evaluations of implosive therapy where
supposedly dynamically significant contents are never pursued.
Results of animal experimentation (Polin, 1959; Poppen, 1968) and
a few clinical applications (Malleson, 1959) indicate that avoidance
behavior can be extinguished by prolonged or massive exposure to sub-
jectively threatening stimuli. Preliminary studies (Hogan, 1966; Levis
& Carrera, 1967) demonstrating that implosive therapy produced greater
reduction in deviant responses on the MMPI test than conventional
treatment were somewhat unconvincing because of the weak criterion
measure employed. Subsequent laboratory investigations present evi-
dence, based on objective measures of behavioral change, that this
method can achieve extinction of avoidance behavior. In one experiment
(Kirchner & Hogan, 1966) coeds who feared rats were either assigned to
a control condition in which they were instructed to imagine pleasant
scenes while listening to music, or they received group implosive therapy.
To minimize possible social influences, subjects in the latter condition
listened through earphones in a language laboratory to a one-hour tape-
recording that described, among other frightening scenes, rats biting,
ripping flesh and attacking a person en masse. A test for avoidance be-
havior disclosed that 62 percent of the subjects in the implosive condi-
tionwere able to pick up a white rat, while 26 percent of the controls
performed the same behavior. Essentially similar outcomes were obtained
in a second experiment (Hogan & Kirchner, 1967) on the basis of a
single session of individual implosive treatment. Sixty-seven percent of
the treated subjects, and 9 percent of the controls, could pick up a rat in
a subsequent behavioral test.
Desensitization
Implosive treatment
Modified desensitization
16 Pseudo- desensitization
Control
15
14
13
12
10
the exercises at home (Jones, 1960) and recorded the number of tics
that extinction effects achieved in the drug condition often fail to trans-
fer to the nondrug state.
the back with a concealed weapon while being taken captive near enemy
lines. After disarming two other soldiers, he burst through a doorway in
a small farmhouse to find 12 enemy troops in the process of wakening. He
stood guard over them for 10 strained hours, finally shot their sergeant,
who kept urging the soldiers to rush their captor, and brought in the
prisoners when fell. The next day he developed a temporary para-
night
plegia when a grenade exploded nearby. Following his army dis-
rifle
duce emotional responding to a stable zero level. Had Freud extended the
extinction series it is probable that his original "cathartic" procedure
might have proved more efficacious than the protracted interpretive form
of treatment that he subsequently adopted.
An interesting laboratory demonstration of the progressive decline of
emotional behavior with repeated hypnotic abreactions of a traumatic
episode is furnished by Lifshitz & Blair (1960). The subject, a 23-year-
old female, revivified under hypnotic age regression a near drowning that
she had experienced at 10 years of age. "She was at the beach with her
father and waded too far out into the water, was knocked down by a
succession of waves, inhaled and swallowed water, and was in fear of
drowning when rescued by her father [p. 248]."
Under hypnosis the subject spontaneously recalled this specific
episode seven times, during each of which the following autonomic reac-
tions were continuously recorded duration of abreaction as revealed prin-
:
+J -G *J
JS o -C
.be
.SP o
ta £ GO
if -a
7 £ >
° £
.£P
>
a. f.
—o a w a
a; ^o Cv
CO c
eo
> O be
Eh
5, CO CO
—CO CO
-
+2 H- ^3 e
CO
no
be - £ 0)
-
CO CJ
CO S 5
9J > --
G tf -5
CO
CO 45 — E^3 o
= CO 5
CD
-
- Z CO 6
S3
DC
a be
Xa;
go
3 be
/.
be
E a;
S lo ;> g
X
—- o 3 "Eh
© p
'*
•E
z O
ed s-
_c
00
6 T5
"cc
<M
CO
— -3
CO
22
Summary 413
Summary
In the process of extinction, when the reinforcing consequences for a
particular response pattern are consistently discontinued, the recurrence
of the behavior is diminished and eventually ceases. Since the decre-
mental effects of nonreinforcement are controlled by many variables,
several different theoretical interpretations of extinction have been pro-
posed.
Contrary to the connotation of the term, extinguished behavior is dis-
placed rather than permanently lost. In fact, nonreinforced behavior is
abandoned.
Under conditions where no reinforcement is externally administered
during the extinction phase, assumed that continued performance
it is
REFERENCES
Abi Rafi,A. Learning theory and the treatment of tics. Journal of Psycho-
somatic Research, 1962, 6, 71-76.
Agras, S., Leitenberg, H., & Barlow, D. H. Social reinforcement in the modi-
fication of agoraphobia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1968, 19, 423-
427.
Alexander, F. Psychoanalysis and psychotherapy. New York: Norton, 1956.
Allen, K. E., Hart, B., Buell, J. S., Harris, F. R., & Wolf, M. M. Effects of
social reinforcement on isolate behavior of a nursery school child. Child
Development, 1964, 35, 511-518.
References 415
Dittes, J.
E. Galvanic skin response as a measure of patient's reaction to
therapist's permissiveness. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,
1957,55,295-303. (b)
Dollard, J.,
& Miller, X. E. Personality and psychotherapy. New York: McGraw-
Hill, 1950.
Dollard, Miller, N. E., Doob, L. YV., Mowrer, O. H., & Sears, R. R. Frus-
J.,
tration and aggression. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1939.
Dyal, J. A. Latent extinction as a function of number of training trials. Psy-
chological Reports, 1963, 13, 407-414.
Edmonson, B. W., & Amsel, A. The effects of massing and distribution of
extinction trials on the persistence of a fear-motivated instrumental re-
sponse. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1954,
47, 117-123.
Elam, C. B., Tyler, D. YV., & Bitterman, M. E. A further study of secondary
reinforcement and the discrimination hypothesis. Journal of Comparative
and Physiological Psychology, 1954, 47, 381-384.
Epstein, R. Aggression toward outgroups as a function of authoritarianism and
imitation of aggressive models. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 1966, 3, 574-579.
Estes, W. K. The statistical approach to learning theory. In S. Koch (Ed.),
Psychology: A study of a science. Vol. 2. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1959.
Pp. 380-491.
Etzel, B. C., & Gewirtz, J. L. Experimental modification of caretaker-maintained
high-rate operant crving in a 6- and a 20-week-old infant (infans
tyrannotearus ) Extinction of crying with reinforcement of eye contact
:
420 EXTINCTION
/
White, J.
G. Neurotic habit formations and the experimental analysis of learn-
ing (conditioning). Paper read to the Ulster Neuropsychiatric Society,
Belfast, April, 1962.
Whiting, J.
W. M. Becoming a Kivoma. New Haven: Yale University Press,
1941.'
Wickens, D. D., Allen, C. K., & Hill, F. A. Effects of instruction on extinction
of the conditioned GSR. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1963,
66, 235-240.
Williams, C. The elimination of tantrum behavior by extinction procedures.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1959, 59, 269.
Williams, C. D. Extinction and other principles of learning in the treatment
and prevention of children's disorders. Paper read at the meeting of the
American Psychological Association, St. Louis, September, 1962.
Wilson, W. J., & Dyal, J. A. Effects of nonresponse acquisition on latent
extinction in a differential reinforcement setting. Psychological Reports,
1963, 13, 879-884.
Wolfgang, M. E., & Ferracuti, F. The subculture of violence. London: Tavistock
Publications, 1967.
Yablonsky, L. The violent gang. New York: Macmillan, 1962.
Yates, A.J.
The application of learning theory to the treatment of tics. Journal
of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1958, 56, 175-182.
Young, A. G. Resistance to extinction as a function of number of nonreinforced
trialsand effortfulness of response. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
1966,72,610-613.
Zeilberger, J., Sampen, S. E., & Sloane, H. N., Jr. Modification of a child's
problem behaviors in the home with the mother as therapist. Journal of
Applied Behaviour Analysis, 1968, 1, 47-53.
7 Desensitization
through
Counterconditioning
fects even when animals were classically conditioned under curare, which
prevents skeletal responding. However, curare procedures do not conclu-
sively rule outmotor mediators because, under lower levels of curariza-
tion,electromyographic responses can be increased through contingent
reinforcement and later can facilitate the occurrence of avoidance re-
sponses in the normal state (Black, 1967). In addition, as Rescorla &
426 DESENSITIZATION THROUGH COUNTERCONDITIONING
dence thus indicates that mechanisms other than autonomic arousal gov-
ern avoidance responding. Indeed, the latencies of autonomic reactions
and their associated feedback are much longer than those of skeletal re-
sponses; consequently, avoidance behavior is typically executed before
autonomic reactions could possibly be elicited. This factor alone pre-
cludes autonomic control of avoidance behavior.
In a comprehensive review of the pertinent literature, Rescorla & Solo-
mon (1967) propose the tenable view, principally on the basis of exclusion
rather than direct corroborative evidence, that instrumental responsive-
ness is mainly regulated by central mediators which can be established
and eliminated through classical conditioning operations. Since central
processes exert control over both autonomic and instrumental responding,
these two response systems are, in general, partially correlated. Major
obstacles to clarification of the role of central mediators in avoidance be-
havior are created by the failure to specify the locus and nature of the
mediating systems and the most valid indices of their activities. The
problem is further complicated by suggestive evidence ( Lacey, 1967 ) that
—
the different arousal systems electroencephalographic, autonomic, and
—
behavioral are functionally separable. Although they generally appear
concomitantly, physiological and behavioral arousal can be markedly dis-
sociated pharmacologically. Thus organisms may be centrally aroused but
behaviorally unresponsive, or conversely, they may be behaviorally ex-
.
30 Graduated extinction •
Graduated counterconditioning ».
25
20
10
unguided counterconditioning. A
major advantage of methods that include
counter-response elicitation is and strength of compet-
that the occurrence
ing activities are managed rather than left to fortuitous factors; this per-
mits greater control over desired outcomes.
sponses are coeffects rather than causally linked events. To the extent that
extinction is governed by mutually inhibitory mechanisms, they are more
likely to operate subcortically rather than in the autonomic system. It is
interesting to note in this connection that some evidence exists (John,
1961) for two reciprocally inhibitory arousal systems in the reticular for-
mation which mediate defensive and approach behavior.
432 DESENSITIZATION THROUGH COUNTERCONDITIONING
100 250
A. Relaxation alone
95 |
4 -
Q.
O
90 o3 210
£ 3
- 85 - 190
- 80 - 170 <
75 -> 150
4 5 6
Weeks
100 -i 250
B. Relaxation with suggestion
- 95 £ - 230 i
£ 3 90 £ 210 =
UL
o3
a. CL
id
g2(- 85 190 2
(0 Ol
D)
< <5
1
80 170 <
75 150
100 250
c 230
4 - 95
|
Q,
O
_c/>
5>
3 90 210 -
¥
o3
Q.
UL
I/}
Q.
85 190
< It
Q-
80 170 <
^
^
75 150
Figure 7-2. Changes in reported asthmatic attacks, and two physiological meas-
ures of respiratory function associated with each of three treatment conditions.
Moore, 1965.
Controlling Variables in Desensitization 437
trol group, but it appears from the summary data that the treatments do
not differ significantly among themselves.With regard to the procedures,
since the "cognitive rehearsal" involved both stimulus exposure and posi-
tive imagery that has counterconditioning potential, this method actually
represents a variant form of desensitization. It might also be noted in
passing that if visualization of aversive stimuli is conceptualized as an
insight operation, then the term has little meaning.
The various findings, taken as a whole, indicate that relaxation is a
than a necessary condition for elimination of avoidance
facilitative rather
behavior. Evidence that relaxation often hastens the extinction process
does not verify that the benefits derive from the explicit manipulation of
muscular activities. Indeed, Rachman (1968) has argued that feelings
of calmness induced by the procedure rather than muscular relaxation
per se is the decisive factor at work. In this alternative explanation, re-
laxation instructions and presentation of pleasant scenes to the imagina-
tion reduce affective arousal which attenuates responsiveness to aversive
stimuli. This interpretation is consistent with the view advanced in this
book that behavioral changes are largely governed by central mechanisms
rather than by peripheral processes.
If the competing activities that function to reduce the arousal capacity
of threatening stimuli are, in fact, symbolically mediated, then certain
changes in the standard desensitization practice may be advantageous.
As Rachman suggests, greater emphasis would be placed on the develop-
ment of tranquil and pleasant imagery than on motor relaxation exercises.
This issue can be best resolved by laboratory studies of the anxiety-
mitigating effects of positive imagery and muscular relaxation when used
alone and in combination with each other.
provoking events. On the other hand, when aversive stimuli are intro-
duced in attenuated form and gradually increased to their full threat
value, extinction effects can be attained with minimal anxiety arousal.
Indeed, by initially presenting an aversive stimulus in a weak form so
that it will not elicit any anxiety, and by increasing its duration and
intensity in small, progressive steps, it should be possible to extin-
guish emotional responsiveness without the occurrence of emotional re-
sponses.
Anxiety-free extinction in aversive situations has received little study.
Experiments with infrahuman subjects (Kimble & Kendall, 1953; Poppen,
1968) have shown that exposure to stimuli graduated in aversiveness
produces more rapid extinction of emotional behavior than when they
are repeatedly presented at their full value. Terrace (1966) has provided
considerable evidence that discriminative behavior can be established
1
16
15
14
13
10
Social desensitization • •
Taped desensitization • •
ior produced toward the attenuated threats, though in the predicted di-
rection, were essentially comparable regardless of whether subjects ex-
pected the treatment to be effective or ineffective. It should also be noted
that "principles of conditioning" would not lead one to predict that sev-
eral random pairings of shock with a blank card interspersed in a hun-
dred trials should necessarily increase avoidance of snakes or spiders.
Indeed, the opposite outcome is entirely possible considering that sub-
ing capacity for matched subjects after they have undergone desensitiza-
tion treatment. To the extent that emotional arousal is reduced below the
threshold which would activate avoidance responses, people will be able
to engage in approach behavior, although with some residual anxiety.
In clinical practice symbolic desensitization is typically supplemented,
either deliberately or unwittingly, with other procedures that tend to fa-
cilitate transfer effects. Symbolic desensitization is most often combined
with performance extinction in which clients are urged to perform for-
merly inhibited behavior in carefully selected naturalistic situations as
their fears extinguish to equivalent imagined threats. Even though change
agents may not prescribe appropriate performance tasks, most people
nevertheless eventually engage in approach behavior as their avoidant
tendencies gradually weaken through treatment.
The desire to please the change agent and others may induce individ-
uals to venture fear-provoking behavior. Positive social reinforcement and
other rewarding outcomes accruing from successful performance of pre-
viously inhibited activities may further extinguish any residual anxieties.
In some cases desensitization is also supplemented by modeling proce-
dures which, in themselves, can produce substantial vicarious extinction
of emotional arousal. Thus, for example, in the frequently quoted case by
Jones (1924), extinction of the boy's animal phobia was achieved not
only by feeding him his favorite food in the presence of gradually in-
creasing anxiety-arousing stimuli, but also by having him observe the
positive response of other children as they played with the feared animal.
Abrupt increases in approach behavior were associated with each of sev-
eral modeling experiences.
In laboratory investigations, of course, these various "extraneous" in-
fluences are intentionally excluded. Because clinical outcomes are usually
obtained by diverse combinations of methods, results are difficult to eval-
100
Counter-conditioning
Insight r
Attention -placebo
|
Control group I
in to
SI
= 1
o c
Figure 7-5. Percent of su bjects in each of the four conditions who displayed
Measures
tion treatment. Two years after the project was completed, 90 percent
of the students who received group desensitization had either graduated
or were completing their studies in good standing, whereas 60 percent
of the nontreated controls had dropped out of school. The grade point
averages for students in the group desensitization and control conditions
in the follow-up semester were 3.5 and 2.4, respectively.
The beneficial effects of desensitization on academic performance
are further corroborated by Mann & Rosenthal (1969) with elementary
school children. Compared to nontreated controls, children who suf-
fered from examination anxiety showed significant changes in test anxiety
and reading achievement scores after receiving either individual or group
desensitization. Interestingly, participant observers benefited to the same
degree as did direct recipients of the treatment procedures.
Another comparative test of the efficacy of counterconditioning meth-
ods for modifying diverse forms of phobic disorders is provided by
Lazarus ( 1961 ) , who employed an experimental design in which the
behavioral outcomes of group desensitization were compared with those
of conventional group psychotherapy. The experiment included acropho-
bics, claustrophobics, cases ofimpotence, and clients who each exhibited
a different type of phobic reaction. The participants were matched in
pairs on the basis of age, sex, and severity of the phobic behavior, and
were randomly assigned to desensitization and interpretive treatment
conditions. In addition, a third group of phobic clients, who received
interpretive treatment plus relaxation at the conclusion of each session,
was subsequently added in order to assess the possible effects of relaxa-
tion per se on avoidance behavior. The same person served as the thera-
pist for all three treatment conditions.
Only clients who exhibited severe phobic behavior, as measured by
actual behavioral tests, were selected for the experiment. In order to
minimize the possible influence of preliminary relationship experiences,
the relevant anxiety hierarchies were constructed from clients' written
responses to questionnaire items, rather than from personal interviews.
The clients who participated in the group desensitization were treated
in small, separate, homogeneous groups. For the acrophobic clients, a
common stimulus hierarchy was constructed utilizing primarily a physi-
cal proximity dimension beginning with a scene in which a subject is
looking down from a height of about 10 feet, and terminating with highly
anxiety-provoking items. The claustrophobic anxiety hierarchy repre-
sented a stimulus continuum in which the degree of spatial constriction
and ventilation were varied simultaneously from scenes depicting the
client "sitting in a large and airy room with all the windows open" to
"sitting in front of an open fire in a small room with the doors and win-
dows shut." Finally, the stimulus hierarchy items constructed for the
456 DESENSITIZATION THROUGH COUNTERCONDITIONING
functions that are not readily evident. The operation of these more intri-
cate stimulus determinants is best illustratedby applications of desensiti-
zation procedures to the modification of diverse sexual disorders.
According to Bond & Hutchison (1960) the most frequent classes of
exposure-eliciting stimuli for sexual exhibitionists are stress experiences
provoking inadequacy and females who possess certain physical char-
acteristics that have been endowed by the exhibitionist with unusually
high sexual valence. The authors therefore employed both sexual and
devaluation hierarchies in treating a 25-year-old male who presented a
long history of persistent genital exposure leading to 24 charges of
indecent exposure including eleven prison convictions. The client had
undergone a variety of treatments without benefit, including individual
and group therapy, carbon dioxide abreaction therapy, moralistic exhor-
tations under hypnosis; finallv, in desperation, he resorted to a specially
designed chastity belt that his wife locked in the morning and unlocked
at night. Even these physical restraints failed to control the client's
behavior as he was once again arrested for indecent assault as he at-
tempted to grasp the legs and breasts of a young woman while wearing
his chastity belt.
Three hierarchies of exposure-provoking stimuli were constructed
for the desensitization treatment. One set of stimuli was graded on the
basis of the age and physical appearance of the females, ranging from
older women who minimally provoked exposure to young attractive fe-
males. These stimuli were presented in each of four settings in which
genital exposure frequently occurred (i.e., in department stores, on
beaches, on sidewalks, and in automobiles). In addition, a separate
hierarchy was constructed on the basis of washroom situations, since
they served as the most potent contextual stimuli for exhibitionism. The
third stimulus dimension contained social situations giving rise to feel-
ings of inadequacy. These sets of eliciting stimuli were then progressively
paired with hypnotically induced relaxation over a period of 30 sessions.
The client was also instructed to practice relaxation and to initiate this
chain of responses by the word "relax."
As treatment progressed the client became less emotionally aroused
by provocative females, his exhibitionistic urges and sexual fantasies
diminished in frequency and intensity, and he displayed increasing volun-
tary control over his exposure behavior on occasions when he experienced
some degree of emotional arousal. As the client made continued improve-
ment, he was able to participate in group activities involving close
heterosexual contacts without experiencing any tension or urges to expose
himself. He showed no exhibitionistic behavior for a period of 13 months
following the termination of therapy (Bond & Hutchison, 1964). Sub-
sequently, the client exposed himself on a few occasions to women in
Identification of the Stimulus Determinants of Emotional Behavior 467
arousing properties, but that the greatest benefits will be derived from
focusing on the particular events that the change agent wishes to neutral-
ize, regardless of whether they constitute the original or the generalized
In the treatment strategies discussed thus far the change agent man-
ages the presentation of both the emotion-provoking and the anxiety-
competing stimuli so that responses to the latter cues prevail over the
former. To the extent that a person can be trained to manage skillfully
instructed to lie in bed with his partner in a relaxed way, but to confine
the sexual activity initially to caresses and preliminary love play. In
order to avoid any possible reinforcement of anxiety, no attempt at inter-
course is made until sexual inhibitions have been sufficiently reduced. As
deconditioning proceeds, the client is likely to exhibit a gradual increase
in sexual responsiveness, and eventually coitus can be attempted after
adequate erections have been achieved. Additional examples of the use
of relaxation by individuals in the self-management of chronic anxiety
reactions are provided by -Jacobson (1964), and by Haugen, Dixon, &
Dickel (1958).
The extent to which a stable change in behavior can be produced
by deliberate utilization of self-induced responses that compete with
anxiety and supplant it depends upon whether they serve primarily to
are absent during this process he will remain vulnerable to the dis-
still
time, usually at the end of the day, the eliciting and neutralizing events
are essentially uncorrelated.
Neutralization of Threats in Symbolic or Realistic Forms 479
rabbit placed on the feeding table or even in his lap, but spontaneously
verbalized a fondness for the animal which previously had terrified him.
Further objective tests revealed that the anxiety extinction effects had
generalized to all the other furlike objects that he had previously feared.
Some therapists (Bettelheim, 1950), working within a psychodynamic
framework, have made extensive use of appetizing foods in counteract-
ing anxiety responses of emotionally prone children whenever they are
about to be exposed to potentially fear-arousing situations.
That food might serve as an effective anxiety neutralizer gains sup-
port from the suggestive evidence cited earlier (John, 1961) that the
reticular formation possesses reciprocally inhibitory arousal systems that
mediate conditioned defensive and alimentary activities. During ali-
mentary activation, response to aversive stimuli is essentially eliminated.
It is generally assumed that counterconditioning procedures employing
his eyes and told to imagine a sequence of events which is close enough
to his eveiyday life to be credible, but within which is woven a story
concerning his favorite hero [p. 192]." After a sufficient degree of posi-
tive affect has been created, the therapist introduces into the narrative
the lowest item in the hierarchy and the child is instructed to signal if
he feels afraid, unhappy or uncomfortable. When the child registers
disturbance, the threatening element is immediately withdrawn, and the
positive imagery is further enhanced. This procedure is continued until
the most phobic item has been neutralized. In most cases arousal-reduc-
ing imagery can be presented and controlled more effectively in dis-
crete conditioning trials rather than in the form of a continuing narrative.
Pleasant imagery and mollifying thoughts are often used in this manner
with adults to enhance the tranquilizing effects of relaxation procedures.
No attempts have been made, however, to assess the physiological effects
of positive imagery, or to determine whether it accelerates extinction of
avoidance behavior.
Those who often mediate pleasant experiences or the reduction of
discomfort in others are likely to acquire positive properties; conse-
quently, the mere presence of such an individual will elicit positive affec-
Antagonistic Activities in Counter conditioning 483
tive responses that can serve as anxiety neutralizes. Frequent social con-
tact, even though unaccompanied by nurturant functions, may also
endow others with positive valence (Cairns, 1966; Homans, 1961). That
familiar social stimuli can function as anxiety reducers has been clearly
shown with both humans and infrahuman subjects. Mason (1960) found
that responses indicative of emotional disturbance were exhibited less
frequently to stressful situations by monkeys in the presence of peers
than in the company of adult monkeys ( whom they had rarely seen since
birth ) , other animals, or when they were left alone in the situation. The
influence of familiarity on social stress reduction receives further sup-
port from a study by Kissel (1965) conducted with college students. A
friend's company was found to be more effective in diminishing auto-
nomic arousal to induced failure than that of a stranger, whose presence
had no distress-reducing value.
There is reason to expect from the above laboratory findings that
relationship-induced responses can serve to mitigate emotional arousal
to some extent. Wolpe ( 1958 ) in fact, contends that favorable outcomes
,
ministered epinephrine but was uninformed about its side effects, while
other students received a placebo injection of saline solution. Immedi-
ately after the experimental manipulation of physiological arousal, all
subjects were sent to a room where they were exposed to the experi-
menter's confederate, supposedly another subject, who displayed con-
siderable anger and verbal aggression toward the experimental pro-
cedure. Subjects in the epinephrine-uninformed group displayed more
anger than students in either the epinephrine-informed or placebo con-
dition, which did not differ from each other. In another phase of this
experiment, four treatments were employed, the three described above
and one in which subjects were injected with epinephrine and deliber-
ately misinformed concerning its side effects so they had no adequate
Physiological Accompaniments of Emotional Behavior 489
Summary
In this chapter the principle of counterconditioning has been dis-
cussed in relation to the modification of emotional behavior by neutral-
izing the arousal potential of threatening stimuli. The reconditioning
process is achieved by inducing activities incompatible with emotional
responses in the presence of anxietv-arousing stimuli. This mode of be-
havior change based on the fact that classically conditioned effects can
is
REFERENCES
Agras, W. S. Transfer during systematic desensitization therapy. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 1967, 5, 193-199.
References 493
Lang, P. J.,
Lazovik, A. D., & Reynolds, D. J.
Desensitization, suggestibility,
and pseudo-therapy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1965, 70, 395-402.
Lazarus, A. A. The elimination of children's phobias by deconditioning. In
H. Eysenck (Ed.), Behavior therapy and the neuroses. New York:
J.
Pergamon, 1960. Pp. 114-122.
Lazarus, A. A. Group therapy of phobic disorders by systematic desensiti-
zation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1961, 63, 504-510.
Lazarus, A. A. The results of behaviour therapy in 126 cases of severe neurosis.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1963, 1, 69-79. (a)
Lazarus, A. A. The treatment of chronic frigidity by systematic desensiti-
zation. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1963, 136, 272-278. (b)
Lazarus, A. A. Crucial procedural factors in desensitization therapy. Be-
haviour Research and Therapy, 1964, 2, 65-70.
Lazarus, A. A., & Abramovitz, A. The use of "emotive imagery" in the treat-
ment of children's phobias. Journal of Mental Science, 1962, 108, 191-195.
Lazarus, A. A., & Rachman, S. The use of systematic desensitization in psycho-
therapy. South African Medical Journal, 1957, 31, 934-937.
Lazovik, A. D., & Lang, P. J. A laboratory demonstration of systematic
desensitization psychotherapy. Journal of Psychological Studies, 1960,
11,238-247.
Leitenberg, H., Agras, W. S., Barlow, D. H., & Oliveau, D. C. Contribution
of selective positive reinforcement and therapeutic instructions to system-
atic desensitization therapy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1969, 74,
113-118.
Leon, H. V. Reciprocal inhibition: An evaluation of group procedures with
"normal" snake phobic subjects. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Uni-
versity of Tennessee, 1967.
Liddell, H. Some specific factors that modify tolerance for environmental stress.
Research Publication of the Association for Nervous and Mental Disease,
1950, 29, 155-171.
Madsen, C. H., Jr., & Ullmann, L. P. Innovations in the desensitization of
frigidity. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1967, 5, 67-68.
Mann, J.,
& Rosenthal, T. L. Vicarious and direct counterconditioning of test
anxiety through individual and group desensitization. Unpublished manu-
script, University of Arizona, 1969.
Marder, M. The effects of experimenter attractiveness and negative reinforce-
ment on verbal conditioning. Dissertation Abstracts, 1961, 22, 1243.
Marks, I. M., & Gelder, M. G. A controlled retrospective study of behaviour
therapy in phobia patients. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1965, 111,
561-573.
Mason, W. A. Socially mediated reduction in emotional responses of young
rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1960, 60,
100-104.
Melamed, B., & Lang, P. ]. Study of the automated desensitization of fear.
Paper read at the meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association,
Chicago, May, 1967.
Melvin, K. B., & Brown, J. S. Neutralization of an aversive light stimulus as
a function of number of paired presentations with food. Journal of Com-
parative and Physiological Psychology, 1964, 58, 350-353.
Meyer, V., & Crisp, A. H. Some problems in behaviour therapy. British Journal
of Psychiatry, 1966, 112, 367-381.
498 DESENSITIZATION THROUGH COUNTERCONDITIONING
likely to retain their attractiveness. Although the classical and the in-
strumental approaches can be easily differentiated operationally, the lat-
somewhat arbitrary.
with the use of biochemical agents because of the gradual, and often
unpredictable, onset of physiological reactions. As a consequence, the
presentation of conditioned stimuli is typically delayed until some time
(Fromer & Berkowitz, 1964) that aversive stimuli with a gradual onset
produce significantly weaker aversion responses than those with a sud-
den onset. Since there is no effective way of terminating abruptly drug-
induced aversive states, the nauseous reactions are needlessly prolonged.
Moreover, because they also tend to subside gradually, the therapist
lacks reliable criteria for timing the withdrawal of conditioned stimuli
to prevent their association -with the reduction of discomfort.
Apart from the problems created by inadequate control over the rate
of onset, duration, intensity, and recovery time of drug activity, unde-
sirable physiological side-effects are sometimes produced, requiring the
administration of additional medicinals. Some of the actions of drugs
that accompany, but are unrelated to, the effects for which they are
being administered may, of course, impair the conditioning process
itself. Central depressants have been shown to decrease conditioning,
1
more, Thorpe, Barker, Conway, & Lavin, 1963) of the client in con-
junction with the elicitors of the undesired behavior. A number of shock
Development of Conditioned Aversion and Avoidance 505
trials are presented during each of the sessions, which are scheduled
over a period of a week or two.
It is sometimes difficult to introduce into treatment the stimulus
events for deviant behavior in the forms and intensities in which they are
ordinarily encountered in everydav situations. Consequently, aversion
reactions may be conditioned to verbal, pictorial, or imaginal representa-
tions of the actual stimulus objects, with the hope that sufficient trans-
fer will occur to inhibit approach tendencies to the real-life counterparts.
McGuire & Vallance (1964) have devised a portable electric stimulation
apparatus that makes it possible for the client, after some preliminary
training, to carry out his own counterconditioning in naturalistic settings
whenever he feels impelled to perform the deviant behavior.
The utilization of the self-conditioning procedure is illustrated in
the treatment of a 25-year-old graduate student who had been mastur-
bating to fetishistic fantasies with considerable guilt about three times
a day for 10 years. The began aversion therapy after having
client
participated in conventional treatment, which had reduced neither his
fetishisticmasturbatory behavior nor the attendant guilt feelings. In the
initial phase of treatment, the client was asked to produce the usual
fantasies and to signal bv raising his hand when the fetish objects were
clearly visualized, at which time a shock was administered. At later
sessions, when the client was unable to conjure up the sexually provoca-
tive fantasies, photographs of persons attired in the fetishistic clothing
were employed as the conditioned stimuli. In addition, he was en-
couraged to use the conditioning apparatus at home whenever he felt
instigated to masturbate. The fetishistic fantasies were completely elim-
inated within a short period, the incidence of masturbatory behavior
was substantially reduced, and on the occasions when the client did
masturbate, for the first time in his life this behavior was accompanied
by heterosexual fantasies. The authors report favorable outcomes in the
use of the self-conditioning procedure for reducing obsessional rumina-
tions, obesity, smoking, and alcoholism. Similarly, Wolpe (1965) was
able to achieve temporary control over drug-addictive behavior in a
physician by having him administer to himself a shock from a portable
apparatus whenever craving for the drug appeared.
In the methods discussed thus far, aversive counterconditioning is
The negative verbal contents are usually drawn from disagreeable, pain-
ful, or revolting experiences that have previously arisen either in con-
nection with the pleasurable objects and activities or in other contexts.
As in the other paradigms, the conditioning trials are continued until
the formerly positive stimuli alone elicit feelings of revulsion.
Miller (1959, 1963) provides several illustrations of the successful
use of symbolic aversion methods in modifying homosexuality and al-
coholism. Most homosexuals have experienced specific disgust reactions,
at one time or another, in intimate relationships with certain male part-
ners. During treatment hypnotically revivified nauseous reactions, which
the client has experienced in previous homosexual contacts, are repeat-
edly associated with visualized homosexual practices involving current
male companions. One client, for example, had felt strong revulsion to
the smell and taste of urine and stale perspiration while performing
fellatio with an uncircumcised male. These past experiences were, there-
fore, employed as aversive verbal stimuli in the treatment. The author
reports that after several sessions the client became nauseated by his
male lovers and eventually broke off all homosexual contacts.
Avoidance responses established in this manner are typically re-
inforced by supplementary conditioning trials at monthly intervals for
the first year. In addition to attaching negative valence to homoerotic
responses and love objects, symbolic counterconditioning procedures are
also employed to enhance the positive reinforcing value of heterosexual
stimuli. In the preceding illustrative case the client participated con-
currently in a number which certain feminine attributes
of sessions in
that he found desirable were hypnotically augmented and associated
with women. Although on the few occasions when the client had for-
merly dated girls he had selected masculine types, following the counter-
conditioning treatment he was attracted to, and dated, women possess-
ing notable feminine attributes.
In aversion treatment of alcoholism, which
is reviewed later, verbally
of this technique are that it has no adverse side effects, it is highly adapt-
able, and people can be taught to administer the treatment to themselves
in the naturalistic situations in which their problem behavior is apt to
arise.
Development of Conditioned Aversion and Avoidance 507
The manner which aversive procedures are employed and the dura-
in
bility of resultant aversions are likely tobe influenced to a considerable
extent by one's conceptualization of the mechanism through which aver-
sive stimulation produces its effects. Most traditional accounts of counter-
conditioning convey the impression that, as a result of paired association
with negative experiences, formerly positive stimuli directly and auto-
matically evoke aversive reactions. The temporal relationship between
stimulus events is therefore considered to be a major determinant of the
strength and durability of conditioned aversions. As will be shown in the
following chapter, this nonmediational view is at variance with certain
experimental findings. It has been demonstrated, for example, that both
conditioned autonomic and avoidance responses promptly disappear when
shock electrodes are removed or subjects are merely informed that a
given stimulus will no longer be accompanied by painful stimulation.
Moreover, conditioned emotional responses can be established cognitively
without the immediate support of external aversive stimulation. Since in
aversion treatments the negative experiences associated with pleasurable
activities are arbitrarily rather than intrinsically related to the behavior,
individuals can readilv discriminate that in everyday life the same activi-
ties be unaccompanied by unpleasant consequences but
will not only
may, in fact, prove highly rewarding. Given cognitive control over con-
ditioning effects and markedly different situational contingencies, one
might expect conditioned aversions to be readily extinguishable and to
show little transfer from treatment to real-life situations. On the other
hand, there is considerable evidence that established revulsions usually
generalize across situations and that they can be relatively long lasting.
An alternative view of counterconditioning effects is that external
stimuli acquire the capacity to activate a self-stimulation mechanism
which, in turn, produces the aversive reactions. Thus, for example, after
a person has repeatedly experienced strong nausea in conjunction with
alcoholic beverages the mere sight or smell of alcohol leads him to re-
vivify his past nauseous experiences. In this conceptualization aversive
reactions are, in large part, self-induced rather than automatically evoked.
If the aversive self-stimulation established through counterconditioning
is potent enough, a personmay be able to counteract the disposition to
engage in deviant behavior by symbolically reinstating nauseous reac-
tions whenever the need arises.
The manner in which counterconditioning is applied will differ in
several important respects depending upon whether it is viewed as a form
of automatic immunization or as a technique of self-control. In the former
case, methods are favored that permit precise management and split-sec-
508 AVERSIVE COUNTERCONDITIONING
ond timing of stimulus events. In the latter approach, on the other hand,
the procedures are designed to develop strong and readily recreatable
aversions to certain objects or activities. For this purpose, verbal and
pharmacological agents may regenerate more natural and symbolically
reproducible aversions than electric shock, the physiological manifesta-
tions of which are relatively subtle. An optimal procedure might initially
involve the combined use of verbal induction and either emetic drugs or
shocks to create vivid aversive reactions. In subsequent sessions, how-
ever, verbal stimuli alone would be used as the conditioning agent al-
though they might be paired occasionally with emetic drugs or shock to
preserve their potency. After an aversive self-stimulation system had been
established in sufficient strength, individuals would be instructed to avoid
engaging in the deviant behavior by deliberately inducing nauseous re-
actions. When conditioned aversions are regarded as self-induced reac-
tions rather than as automatic products of stimulus pairings, the change
agent assumes greater responsibility for arranging positive incentives to
ensure that individuals utilize this potentially effective means of self-
control. In a comprehensive treatment program this practice would, of
course, be used in conjunction with other methods of self-control, as well
as procedures designed to reduce the instigation to engage in the deviant
behavior.
When some persons who have undergone aversive treatment later re-
outcomes are often attributed to defi-
vert to their deviant activities these
ciencies inherent in the conditioning procedure itself. Faulty timing and
sequencing of stimulus events, selection of inadequate aversive agents,
and conditionability deficits are typically invoked as explanatory factors.
Similarly, recommendations for enhancing the efficacy of this method
single out conditioning variables. These include the use of intermittent
schedules of reinforcement, continuation of the trials for a sufficient pe-
riod to ensure overlearning, and inclusion of booster treatments at peri-
odic intervals after the formal program has been discontinued ( Eysenck,
1963). There exists suggestive evidence (Voegtlin, Lemere, Broz, &
O'Hollaren, 1942) that conditioned aversion and avoidance can be suc-
cessfully maintained through periodic reconditioning trials. Portable de-
vices that permit self-administration of aversive stimuli and the judicious
use of symbolically generated consequences would undoubtedly also re-
duce the disposition to engage However, there is lit-
in deviant behavior.
tle reason to expect that intermittent reinforcement would increase the
durability of aversive reactions. Partial reinforcement retards the rate of
extinctionby reducing discriminability of the occasions when customary
consequences will or will not occur (Spence, 1966). Administration of re-
inforcements in an unpredictable manner may produce stable conditioned
responses during treatment, but the conditions of reinforcement prevail-
Development of Conditioned Aversion and Avoidance 509
ing in treatment situations and in everyday life differ markedly and are
easily distinguishable. The situational change would ordinarily result in
a rapid decrement in responsiveness unless cognitive functions were uti-
designed to eliminate.
The problem of negative transfer can be even more complicated in
cases where the existing behavior is entirely satisfactory, but the evoking
stimuli are so bizarre and disturbing to others that attempts are made to
nullify their arousal capabilities through aversive procedures. This situa-
tion arises most frequently in the treatment of heterosexual fetishists and
transvestites who, in order to obtain an erection and to engage in sexual
intercourse, must wear their wives' clothing (Blakemore et al., 1963),
rubberized mackintoshes (Oswald, 1962), be kicked with rubber boots
or high-heeled shoes (Marks, Rachman, & Gelder, 1965). Such unique
modes of erotic arousal create serious marital conflicts, even conditioning
revulsions in sexual partners. A similar problem exists when efforts are
made to negate the sex-arousing value of self-flagellating, fetishistic, and
sadistic fantasies that arouse masturbatory or heterosexual behavior with-
out suppressing the behavior itself. It is of considerable import that in
many of the published cases more appropriate sexual fantasies emerge as
the arousal potential of bizarre elicitors is eliminated, and desirable sexual
behavior is either maintained at its original level or further enhanced
(Blakemore et al., 1963; Cooper, 1963; Kushner & Sandler, 1966; Marks &
Gelder, 1967;McGuire & Vallance, 1964; Oswald, 1962; Raymond, 1956).
The and extent of generalization of aversion effects can be
direction
regulated through a program of differential reinforcement in which un-
desirable events are repeatedly associated with negative experiences,
while the desired ones are paired with either rewarding or no adverse con-
sequences. Verbal labeling can also be utilized effectively both to delimit
what is being negatively conditioned and to enhance the most relevant
Sexual Deviance 511
Sexual Deviance
nose, or lips, broad pelvis and wide hips or narrow pelvis and slim hips,
light or dark skin color —may be neutral or highly repulsive to members of
another social group. These cross-cultural data showing the range of pre-
ferred sexual reinforcers are striking testimony of the influential role of
social learning in the development of sexual behavior that may be judged
deviant by some social group.
Although our society imposes severe social and legal prohibitions
against some forms of behavior that are believed to have sexual implica-
tions, certain members may nevertheless experience unusual reinforce-
ment and modeling influences serving to promote and to maintain deviant
sexual behavior. A major obstacle to the understanding of human sexual
deviance is that, for ethical reasons, experimentation designed to identify
the conditions governing sexual phenomena cannot be conducted. Conse-
quently, the search for the relevant controlling variables must rely on
naturalistic studies. A number of clinical 'reports have been published
containing data that illustrate the social learning processes whereby cul-
turally inappropriate stimuli and responses acquire unusually strong sex-
ual reinforcing properties.
Litin, Giffin, & Johnson ( 1956 ) describe the development of transvest-
ism in a young boy who continually dressed up in his mother's clothes,
including cosmetics and jewelry, exhibited almost complete feminine-role
behavior, and even adopted a girl's name which his mother suggested.
mother, in response to her son's comment that she looked pretty in her
new shoes, hugged him and offered him her old shoes. He wore these
shoes daily, eliciting considerable maternal approbation. The mother con-
tinued to encourage and reward sex-inappropriate behavior with demon-
strations of affection and approval, while the grandmother and neighbors
supplemented the mother's training by supplying the boy
in transvestism
with generous quantities of old shoes, hats, purses, bridal veils, and other
female apparel. When the boy's inverted behavior met with disapproval
from other persons, the mother attempted some discrimination training
by informing her son, "You must never dress like that in front of company,
only in front of the family." In a study of the mothers and wives of 32
transvestites, Stoller (1967) found that the subjects were initiated into
transvestism by being dressed in girls' clothing or highly rewarded when-
ever they dressed themselves in feminine apparel. The transvestite be-
havior was further elaborated by mothers and wives who devoted many
hours to teaching the subjects how to dress in feminine clothing, how to
apply cosmetics, and how to behave as women.
Litin, Giffin, & Johnson (1956) depict how a mother actively condi-
tioned voyeuristic behavior in her sonby sleeping with him, and by being
physically and verbally seductive while appearing nude before him. When
.
the boy was six years old, the mother had shown him her vagina a num-
ber of times, but she later discontinued her physically seductive behavior
after the son suggested that they engage in sexual intercourse. The boy's
strongly established voyeuristic behavior had generalized to the maid and
other persons; eventually he was apprehended by the police while peep-
ing from a ladder into neighborhood bedrooms.
Generalization of strongly reinforced homoerotic responses is illus-
That erotic experiences can endow formerly neutral stimuli with sexual
arousal properties is supported by results of an interesting study by Rach-
man (1966), designed to create a mild fetish under laboratory conditions.
After a photograph of women's boots was repeatedly associated with
slides of sexually stimulating nude females, subjects exhibited sexual
arousal ( as measured by penile volume changes ) to the boots alone, and
generalized the conditioned sexual responses to other types of black shoes.
In accord with these findings, McGuire, Carlisle,& Young (1965) report
that deviant sexuality often develops through masturbatory conditioning
in which aberrant sexual fantasies are endowed with strong erotic value
through repeated association with pleasurable experiences from mastur-
bation. The details of their interpretive scheme, and its therapeutic impli-
cations, are discussed more fully later.
The foregoing cases represent a small sample of those documented in
the reports to which reference has been made. Three social learning vari-
ables emerge from these naturalistic studies as important determinants of
deviant sexual behavior. The first consideration involves the degree to
514 AVERSIVE COUNTERCONDITIONING
tension-reducing effects.
In applications of aversive counterconditioning to sexual disorders, at-
tempts are made to reverse the sexual arousal value of appropriate and
inappropriate stimuli through differential conditioning procedures.
Sexual Deviance 515
ment in which shocks were administered while they carried out their devi-
ant behavior or imagined themselves performing the same activities. In
each case, the process of treatment was studied by recording changes in
sexual arousal through the use of a penis transducer. The clients' attitudes
180
160
140
120
100
80
60
40
Panties
20 Pyjamas Skirt and blouse
i i '
I I I I I
Sessions
60
40
E
20 CO
hn kQn
Erections after one minute exposure to stimulus
the other items were serially counterconditioned they also lost their sex-
arousing capacity. It is important to note, however, that the client main-
tained high sexual responsiveness to appropriate heterosexual stimuli after
deviant sources of sexual arousal were eliminated. The specific sequence
of behavioral changes obtained by repeated application of aversive stimu-
lation provides convincing demonstrations that the alteration in sexual
arousal was indeed produced by the conditioning procedure. In the cases
studied, sexual reconditioning was followed by corresponding elimination
of deviant sexual desires and activities.
The findings of the above study not only attest to the efficacy of aver-
sive conditioning, but they also help to clarify the stimulus function of
symbolic processes and their modification. Prior to treatment, imagery of
the deviant behavior elicited strong erectile responses. As these fantasies
were repeatedly associated with unpleasant experiences the latency of the
images increased until eventually they could be produced only with con-
siderable difficulty ( Figure 8-1 ) Moreover, the erections which originally
.
Whatever the reasons might be, the differential outcomes associated with
the desire to modify one's sexual orientation reaffirm the view that unless
individuals are committed to the selected objectives, their behavior is
likely to nullify the effects of change programs.
McGuire, Carlisle, & Young (1965) advance the interesting thesis that
in some cases deviant sexual preferences are developed through mastur-
batory conditioning. According to the authors, three factors usually figure
prominently in this form of sexual learning. First, as a result of unpleasant
heterosexual experiences or feelings of physical and social inadequacy, the
person comes to believe that he cannot achieve a normal sex life. Second,
the person usually has a sexual experience that is not sufficient by itself
interest in girls, but rather derived his sexual stimulation almost entirely
from women's underwear which he bought or stole. This helps to explain
how outlandish fantasies can acquire powerful sexual valence through
contiguous association with masturbatory experiences (Marks, Rachman,
& Gelder, 1965; Mees, 1966) and, once established, why they are so re-
fractory to extinction. Other case reports by McGuire and his associates
depict a similar process in which aberrant sexual fantasies are selectively
reinforced; they eventually become able to provoke corresponding homo-
sexual, exhibitionistic,and Voyeuristic behavior. The prevalence of mas-
turbatory conditioning in aberrant sexuality is further shown by Evans
( 1968 ) who found that among a large group of sexual deviates 79 percent
,
In many of the cases cited above, therapists have tried not only to
create aversions to inappropriate objects, but also to develop attraction to
heterosexual stimuli. A variety of differential conditioning techniques
has been employed for this purpose. In one such approach, which is
stimuli with sex arousal value. Nevertheless, Solyom and Miller found,
during treatment of a group of homosexuals, that plethysmograph re-
sponses to female pictures became progressively greater, while responses
to male pictures remained essentially unchanged. This suggests that the
differential conditioning produced a change from predominantly homo-
sexual to bisexual responsiveness. However, these findings are difficult to
interpret because, as the authors themselves acknowledge, their plethys-
mograph measure does not differentiate between sexual and anxiety
arousal. Erotic arousal can be most validly assessed in terms of penile
erectile responses as demonstrated by Bancroft, Jones, & Pullan (1966),
who measured changes in several object preferences in a pedophile
throughout the course of aversion therapy. This measure makes it possi-
ble to conduct systematic investigations of the relative efficacy of different
conditioning procedures for altering erotic preferences. It also provides
an objective criterion for determining the duration of treatment, thus
safeguarding against either premature termination, or needless pro-
longation, of the conditioning sessions.
It should be emphasized here that conditioning of sexual attraction
to appropriate objects constitutes only part of a broader treatment ob-
jective. Persons who have engaged in deviant sexual practices for a
long time must develop not only heterosexual attraction but also intricate
patterns of heterosexual behavior. This may require, among other things,
acquisition of new speech patterns, dress styles, courtship behaviors,
modes of sexual stimulation that are closely associated with heterosexual
coitus, and many other aspects of sex-role behavior. To the extent that
such behavioral changes enable persons to engage in rewarding intimate
interactions, the resultant positive experiences will exert a powerful in-
fluence on the further development of heterosexual feelings and pref-
erences.
He tried out his rubber articles a week after leaving hospital, found they
held no interest for him and discarded them. He went out to dances and
other social events for thefirst time in years. After 6 months he relapsed
and a further 4 months later made known his deviation to the Service
authorities and was invalided. He told me at that time that he intended
to live in London where there were others who shared his interests. He
had been back to the brothel which, he pointed out, advertises in a well-
known week-end publication available at any bookstall, under the guise
of a rubber-clothing store. He had formed a friendship with a male homo-
sexual (but not had sexual relations with him), whom he had first noticed
wearing a black, shiny, rubber mackintosh in Hyde Park one fine summer's
evening [p. 202].
SO
70
\
60
I 50 Insight
Shocked responses
Nonshocked responses
40 No insight
e 30
20 -
10
Trial
Alcoholism
group of animals to approach the lighted end of the alley for food, and
a second group to avoid the lighted end of the alley to escape electric
shocks. Compared to the behavior of sober controls, which had been ad-
Alcoholism 531
had
ministered placebo injections, the avoidance responses of subjects that
received alcohol injections showed a substantial reduction in strength,
but approach responses seemed unaffected.
A few experiments involving human subjects have also demonstrated
the disinhibiting effects of alcohol on verbal expressions of sexual and
aggressive behavior in social drinking situations (Bruun, 1959; Clark &
Sensibar, 1955 ) . Among humans, however, the same dose of alcohol may
have diverse effects because individuals differ in the types of responses
inhibited, the strength of inhibitions, and variations in social conditions
which, in part, serve to define and to control appropriate behavior.
Numerous studies have been concerned with the influence of ethanol
on escape and avoidance responses tested in a variety of aversive condi-
tioning situations involving no rewarded competing responses. In these
experiments animals are initially taught to perform responses which either
avert the onset of aversive stimulation or terminate it after its occurrence.
Changes avoidance and escape responses as a function of
in the rate of
the administration of ethanol are then assessed relative to the perform-
ances of control groups, which receive either water or solutions contain-
ing other types of drugs. Ethanol in moderate doses produces more rapid
extinction of fear-mediated responses (Kaplan, 1956; Pawlowski, Denen-
berg, & Zarrow, 1961), and it reduces the rate of responses designed to
postpone the occurrence of aversive stimuli ( Hogans, Moreno, & Brodie,
1961; Sidman, 1955). Moreover, the capacity of alcohol to reduce emo-
tional behavior is similar to that of other drugs possessing central de-
pressant properties (Korpmann & Hughes, 1959).
The withdrawal of positive reinforcers following a period of reward
generally produces aversive effects that lead to the suppression of associ-
ated responses. Further support for interpreting the behavioral effects of
ethanol in terms of emotion-reducing processes is furnished by experi-
ments concerned with the reinstatement of responses following their
inhibition through frustrative nonreward. Under these conditions, animals
administered ethanol are more persistent than those given a placebo in
performing nonreinforced behavior (Barry, Wagner, & Miller, 1962), and
they increase their rate of response in the presence of stimuli signifying
nonreward (Blough, 1956; Miller, 1961).
The experimental data reviewed so far, based on the forced adminis-
tration ofmoderate doses of ethanol, strongly indicate that alcohol can
produce significant decrements in both autonomic arousal and emotional
behavior generated by aversive environmental conditions. Investigations
concerned with variables governing the voluntary intake of alcohol also
contribute to an understanding of the development and maintenance of
self-intoxication. This research is reviewed next.
532 AVERSIVE COUNTERCONDITIONING
Alcoholism 533
straight whiskey and asked to smell it, to sip it, and to taste it thoroughly.
This same procedure is repeated several times with whiskey either taken
straight or mixed with warm water to afford easy emesis. The rationale
for relying on whiskey exclusively in the initial session and at the
beginning of each subsequent session is that it produces greater gastric
irritation than beer or wine and therefore serves to facilitate the emetic
reaction. Kant ( 1944 ) has seriously questioned the wisdom of using the
conditioned stimulus in order to enhance the unconditioned response,
since this procedure runs a high risk of reinforcing drinking behavior.
If 4 to 6 ounces of alcohol are ingested before emesis occurs, large
quantities of alcohol are likely to be absorbed. Under these conditions,
the immediate reinforcing effects of alcohol may reduce, or even out-
weigh, the effectiveness of subsequent aversive experiences. Considering
the difficulties in precisely timing the onset of emetic responses, this
factor may
partly account for variations in strength of conditioned
aversion developed by different investigators supposedly using the
same method. While Voegtlin has taken necessary precautions to avoid
alcohol absorption by the client during treatment, it is not clear whether
other therapists have paid as close attention to this important point in
the technique.
The procedural changes recommended by Kant (1944, 1945) obviate
the alcohol absorption problem without detracting from the efficacy of
the treatment. During the first two sessions just prior to and during
nausea, the client is asked to look at, smell, and taste the different
alcoholic beverages, but then to spit them out. In subsequent sessions,
the client is requested to drink some alcohol at the height of nausea.
Only during the terminal sessions, when alcohol itself has acquired the
capacity to produce rapid emptying of the stomach, is the client encour-
aged to take several drinks.
It is important to include all varieties and types of alcoholic beverages
beer, wine, and whiskey are used, greatest attention is usually directed
toward the particular type of intoxicant that the client most prefers. The
conditioning trials are continued until the alcoholic stimuli alone elicit
nauseous reactions, and ingestion of the different varieties of liquor
produces prompt emesis. At the conclusion of the treatment the client
is instructed that he must, in the future, abstain totally from all alcoholic
beverages.
There have been some variations in the conditioning procedure
originally developed by Voegtlin. Miller, Dvorak, & Turner (1960) re-
port that excellent aversions to alcohol can be developed by group appli-
cations of this method. The authors report that simultaneous presence of
several persons undergoing treatment frequently produces contagious
emesis, thereby facilitating the negative conditioning process.
Many European therapists have employed a counterconditioning
method devised bv Feldmann ( DeMorsier & Feldmann, 1950 ) in which ,
Number Complete
of Abstinence Duration of
Investigator Cases Avers ive Stimulus (%) Follow-up
tual participation in, periodic reconditioning sessions during the year im-
mediately following treatment (when most alcoholic reversions occur)
Alcoholism 543
Number of Percentage of
Supplementary Sessions Number of Cases Abstinence
None 88 74
One 113 80
Two 57 95
Three 20 90
Four or more 7 100
DISULFIRAM REGIMEN
on the next three days. After the primary intolerance for alcohol has been
established, the client is then given one or more test trials of alcohol in
order to determine the optimum maintenance dosage of Antabuse. The
dosage is adjusted individually to the level where the characteristic, un-
pleasant side effects of the drug are reduced to a minimum, but the dosage
is still adequate to produce sufficiently intense reactions to deter further
drinking. The reactions to the test doses also serve to impress upon the
client the serious physical consequences of ingesting even small amounts
of alcohol while on disulfiram. Following discharge, the client is placed
on a maintenance dosage which usually varies from % to % gram tablet
of disulfiram taken each day either before breakfast or in the evening
(Bowman et al., 1951; Child, Osinski, Bennett, & Davidoff, 1951).
Because of the violent physiological reactions that can be elicited by
alcohol when disulfiram is present in the body, the primary intolerance
to alcohol and the maintenance dose are generally established during a
brief period of hospitalization with the client under careful observation.
However, Martensen-Larsen (1953), who has written authoritatively about
this mode of therapy, describes a therapeutic regimen that may be con-
Alcoholism 545
duced by decreasing the daily maintenance dose (Child et al., 1951; Mar-
tensen-Larsen, 1953). The disulfiram treatment regimen is also generally
contraindicated for clients suffering from cardiovascular disorders, cirrho-
sis of the liver, nephritis, diabetes, epilepsy, advanced arteriosclerosis, and
in cases of pregnancy.
It is possible that eventually an effective anti-alcohol agent will be
discovered that produces few unpleasant side effects. Ferguson (1956),
for instance, reports a drug, citrated calcium carbimide (CCC), whose
action is similar to that of disulfiram in inhibiting acetaldehyde metabo-
lism, but which is free of some of its disagreeable features. A preliminary
experiment in which different groups of alcoholics were treated with CCC
and with disulfiram revealed that fewer subjects in the CCC group dis-
continued medication of their own accord ( Armstrong & Kerr, 1956 )
In view of the possible physical manifestations associated with disul-
firam and the inconvenience of continuous self-medication, the selection
of this mode of therapy over the shorter, safer, and more economical
countcrconditioning methods would be justified only if the pharmacologi-
cal approach were shown to yield significantly higher rates of successful
outcomes. In a study comparing the relative efficacy of aversive counter-
conditioning, Antabuse, group hypnotherapy, and milieu therapy, Waller-
stein (1957) found that Antabuse was most efficacious according to an
aggregate rating based on degree of abstinence, general social adjustment,
"subjective feelings of difference," and changes in "personality structure."
Results for the conditioning group, however, are at such marked variance
with those achieved by other investigators that the findings of this experi-
ment must be accepted with reservation. Yanushevskii (1959) analyzed
the follow-up data on 2000 alcoholics who had received either medication,
psychotherapy, hypnosis, apomorphine-counterconditioning, or disulfiram
in a Moscow Conditioned aversion and disulfiram proved superior
clinic.
val between the ingestion of alcohol on the one hand, and the onset of the
aversive consequences on the other. Consequently, alcohol retains its pos-
itive value and the client is able to drink within several days after termi-
nating medication. Many alcoholics, in fact, will take disulfiram intermit-
tently and go on drinking sprees during periods when their physiological
tolerance for alcohol has been restored. The duration and degree of absti-
nence is, therefore, contingent on the duration and regularity with which
medication is used (Jacobsen, 1950).
The temporal prerequisites for aversive conditioning are also absent
from methods which nauseants are added to alcoholic beverages. Un-
in
der these conditions a person will refrain from drinking emeticized cock-
tails but retain his strong attraction to unmedicated alcoholic drinks. In
must teach new skills; it must furnish exemplary role models; and it
must embody a set of reinforcement contingencies that will counteract
deviant activities and promote more constructive modes of behavior. It
is interesting to note, as dramatically illustrated by the Synanon approach
the use of surgical and dental procedures with patients willing to undergo
a brief painful experience in order to alleviate more deleterious and
long-lasting suffering. The brief discomfort occasioned by a program of
aversion therapy is minor compared to the repeated incarceration, social
ostracism, serious disruption of family life, and self-condemnation re-
sulting from uncontrollable injurious behavior. It is a therapist's responsi-
bility to provide clients with information about the treatment alternatives
available to them and which outcomes are most likely to result from
each choice. Given this knowledge, it should be the client's right to
decide what types of treatment, if any, he wishes to undergo.
As noted earlier, aversion therapy has proved least effective with
sexual deviants who are coerced into treatment in an attempt to change
their behavior in the direction of conformity with more conventional
practices. In cases where conduct threatens the welfare of others,
their
they have the choice of either altering their injurious behavior or having
their freedom revoked. There are other forms of sexual activities, how-
Summary 553
Summary
This chapter is primarily concerned with classical aversion treatment
of exceedingly persistent behavior that is maintained by inappropriate,
potentially harmful, or culturally prohibited positive reinforcers. This
stimulus-oriented approach attempts to establish control over behavior
by endowing formerly attractive stimuli or symbolic representations of
deviant activities with negative properties through contiguous association
with aversive experiences. These negative experiences are typically in-
REFERENCES
Abrams, S. An evaluation of hypnosis in the treatment of alcoholics. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 1964, 120, 1160-1165.
Agras, W. S. Behavior therapy in the management of chronic schizophrenia.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 1967, 124, 240-243.
Alcoholics Anonymous. Twelve steps and twelve traditions. New York: Alco-
holics Anonymous Publishing Co., 1952.
Allchin, W. H. Behaviour therapy. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1964, 110, 108.
References 555
References 557
Hald, J.,
& Jacobsen, E. A drug sensitizing the organism to ethyl alcohol. Lan-
cet, 1948, 2, 1001-1004.
Harris, M. B. A self-directed program for weight control: A pilot study. Journal
Abnormal Psychology, 1969 (in press).
of
Hess, E. H. Pupillometric assessment. In J. M. Shlien (Ed.), Research in psy-
chotherapy. Washington, D. C: American Psychological Association, 1968.
Pp. 573-583.
Hess, E. H., Seltzer, A. L., & Shlien, J. M. Pupil response of hetero- and homo-
sexual males to pictures of men and women: A pilot study. Journal of Ab-
normal Psychology, 1965,70, 165-168.
Hill, M. J., & Blane, H. T. Evaluation of psychotherapy with alcoholics. Quar-
terly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1967, 28, 76-104.
Hoff, E. C, & McKeown, C. E. An evaluation of the use of tetraethylthiuram
disulfide in the treatment of 560 cases of alcohol addiction. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 1953, 109, 670-673.
Hogans, A. F., Moreno, C. M., & Brodie, D. A. Effects of ethyl alcohol on EEG
and avoidance behavior of chronic electrode monkeys. American Journal
of Physiology, 1961, 201,434-436.
Hollingshead, A. B. Views on the etiology of alcoholism. In H. D. Kruse (Ed.),
Alcohol as a medical problem. New York: Harper, 1956. Pp. 56-67.
Holmberg, A. R. The Siriono. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Yale Universitv,
1946.
Hsu, J. J.
Electroconditioning therapy of alcoholics. A preliminary report.
Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1965, 26, 449-459.
Isbell, H., Fraser, H. F., Wikler, A., Belleville, R. E., & Eiseman, A. J. An ex-
perimental study of the etiology of "Rum fits" and delirium tremens.
Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1955, 16, 1-33.
Jackson, J. K., & Connor, R. The skid road alcoholic. Quarterly Journal of Studies
on Alcohol, 1953, 14, 468-486.
Jacobsen, E. Biochemical methods in the treatment of alcoholism, with special
reference to Antabuse. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine, 1950,
43,519-526.
James, B. Case of homosexuality treated by aversion therapy. British Medical
Journal, 1962, 1, 768-770.
Jellinek, E. M. Phases of alcohol addiction. In D. J. Pittman & C. R. Snyder
(Eds.), Society, culture, and drinking patterns. New York: Wiley, 1962.
Pp. 356-368.
Johnson, A. M. Factors in the etiology of fixations and symptom choice. Psy-
choanalytic Quarterly, 1953, 22, 475-496.
Kant, F. Further modifications in the technique of conditioned reflex treatment
of alcohol addiction. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1944, 5,
229-232.
Kant, F. The use of conditioned reflex in the treatment of alcohol addicts.
Wisconsin Medical Journal, 1945, 44, 217-221.
Kantorovich, N. V. An attempt of curing alcoholism by associated reflexes.
Novoye Refleksologii nervnoy i Fiziologii Sistemy, 1928, 3, 436-445. Cited
by Razran, G. H. S., Conditioned withdrawal responses with shock as the
conditioning stimulus in adult human subjects. Psychological Bulletin,
1934,31, 111-143.
Kaplan, H. S. The effects of alcohol on fear extinction. Dissertation Abstracts,
1956, 16, 571-572.
References 559
Mowrer, O. H. Learning theory and the symbolic processes. New York: Wiley,
1960.
Narrol, H. G. Experimental application of reinforcement principles to the
analysis and treatment of hospitalized alcoholics. Quarterly Journal of
Studies on Alcohol, 1967, 28, 105-115.
Oswald, I. Induction of illusory and hallucinatory voices with considerations
of behaviour therapy. Journal of Mental Science, 1962, 108, 196-212.
Pawlowski, A. A., Denenberg, V. H., & Zarrow, M. X. Prolonged alcohol con-
sumption in the rat. 2. Acquisition and extinction of an escape response.
Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1961, 22, 232-240.
Peacock, L. J., & Watson, J. A. Radiation-induced aversion to alcohol. Science,
1964, 143, 1462-1463.
Perloff, W. H. Hormones and homosexuality. In J. Marmor (Ed.), Sexual in-
version. New York: Basic Books, 1965. Pp. 44-69.
Pittman, D. J., & Gordon, C. W. Revolving door: A study of the chronic police
case inebriate. Glencoe, 111.: Free Press, 1958.
Pittman, D. J., & Snyder, C. R. (Eds.) Society, culture, and drinking patterns.
New York: Wiley, 1962.
Quinn, J. T., & Henbest, R. Partial failure of generalization in alcoholics follow-
ing aversion therapy. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1967, 28,
70-75.
Rachman, S. Aversion therapy: Chemical or electrical? Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 1965, 2, 289-299.
Rachman, S. Sexual fetishism: An experimental analogue. Psychological Record,
1966, 16, 293-296.
Raymond, M. J. Case of fetishism treated by aversion therapy. British Medical
Journal, 1956, 2, 854-857.
Raymond, M. J. The treatment of addiction by aversion conditioning with
apomorphine. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1964, 1, 287-291.
Raymond, ML, & O'Keeffe, K. A case of pin-up fetishism treated by aversion
conditioning. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1965, 111, 579-581.
Richter, C. P. Production and control of alcoholic craving in rats. In Neuro-
pharmacology, Proceedings of the third conference. New York: Josiah
Macy, Jr. Foundation, 1956. Pp. 39-146.
Robinson, L. H., & Podnos, B. Resistance of psychiatrists in treatment of alco-
holism. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1966, 143, 220-225.
Rodgers, D. A., & McCleam, G. E. Mouse strain differences in preference for
various concentrations of alcohol. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol,
1962, 23, 26-33.
Ruck, F. Alkoholentziehungskur mit Hilfe eines bedingten Reflexes (Apomor-
phinentziehungskur). (Conditioned reflex treatment of alcoholism.) Psy-
chiatric, Neurologie, unci mcdizinische Psychologic, 1956, 8, 88—92.
Sargent, W. Battle for the mind: A physiology of conversion and brainwash-
ing. London: Heinemann, 1957.
Shanahan, W. M., & Hornick, E. J. Aversion treatment of alcoholism. Hawaii
Medical Journal, 1946, 6, 19-21.
Shaw, I. A. The treatment of alcoholism with tetraethylthiuram disulfide in
a state mental hospital. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1951,
12, 576-586.
Sidman, M. Technique for assessing the effects of drugs on timing behavior.
Science, 1955,122,925.
562 AVERSIVE COUNTERCONDITIONING
Skolnik, J.
H. The stumbling block. A sociological study of the relationship
between selected religious norms and drinking behavior. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Yale University, 1957.
Smart, R. G. Effects of alcohol on conflict and avoidance behavior. Quarterly
Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1965, 26, 187-205.
Snyder, C. R. Alcohol and the Jeivs: A cultural study of drinking and sobriety.
Glencoe, 111.: Free Press, 1958.
Solyom, L., & Miller, S. A differential conditioning procedure as the initial
phase of the behaviour therapy of homosexuality. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 1965, 3, 147-160.
Spence, K. W. Cognitive and drive factors in the extinction of the conditioned
eye blink in human subjects. Pst/chological Review, 1966, 73, 445-458.
Stevenson, I., & Wolpe, J. Recovery from sexual deviations through overcoming
of non-sexual neurotic responses. American Journal of Psychiatry, 1960,
116,737-742.
Stollak, G. E. Weight loss obtained under various experimental procedures.
Unpublished manuscript, Indiana University, 1968.
Stoller, R. J. Transvestites' women. American Journal of Psychiatry, 1967, 124,
333-339.
Strel'chuk, I. V. C novykh sovremennykh metodakh lecheniya bol'nykh alkogo-
lizmom. [New contemporary methods of treating patients with alco-
holism.] Sovietskaia Meditsina, 1957, 21 (5), 26-33.
Stuart, R. B. Behavioral control of overeating. Behaviour Research and Tlierapy,
1967, 5, 357-365.
Sutherland, E. H., Schroeder, II. G., & Tordella, C. L. Personality traits and
the alcoholic. A critique of existing studies. Quarterly Journal of Studies
on Alcohol 1950, 11, 547-561.
Syme, L. Personality characteristics and the alcoholic. A critique of current
studies. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1957, 18, 288-301.
Thimann, J.
Conditioned-reflex treatment of alcoholism. II. The risks of its ap-
plication, its indications, contraindications, and psychotherapeutic aspects.
New England Journal of Medicine, 1949, 241, 406-410.
Thompson, G. N., & Bielinski, B. Improvement in psychosis following condi-
tioned reflex treatment for alcoholism. Journal of Nervous and Mental
Disease, 1953, 117, 537-543.
Thorpe, J.
G., Schmidt, E., Brown, P. T., & D. Aversion-relief therapy:
Castell,
A new method for general application. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
1964, 2, 71-82.
Thorpe, J.
G., Schmidt, E., & Castell, D. A comparison of positive and nega-
tive (aversive) conditioning in the treatment of homosexuality. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 1963, 1, 357-362.
Voegtlin, W. L. The treatment of alcoholism by establishing a conditioned
reflex. American Journal of Medical Science, 1940, 199, 802-810.
Voegtlin, W. L., & Broz, W. R. The conditioned reflex treatment of chronic
alcoholism. X. An analysis of 3125 admissions over a period of ten and
Annals of Internal Medicine, 1949, 30, 580-597.
a half years.
Voegtlin, W. Lemere, F., Broz, W. R., & O'Hollaren, P. Conditioned reflex
L.,
therapy of chronic alcoholism. IV. A preliminary report on the value of
reinforcement. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1942, 2, 505-511.
Wall, J. H. A studv of alcoholism in men. American Journal of Psychiatry,
1936, 92, 1389-1401.
References 563
Nonmediational Theory
R S
R F^ AWARENESS
Rnnnworhal — —S R
Cognitive Theory
R+
\
\
\
\
\
SR
/
/
/
/
/
AWARENESS R"
Figure 9-1. Schematization of the functional relationship between awareness and
response change. Dashed lines represent temporally contiguous events, arrows
denote causal relationships, and plus signs designate the magnitude of response
change.
4-3-2-1 0+1+2+3+4
Before verbalization After verbalization
Word Blocks
Figure 9-2. (A) Mean percent human noun responses given by aware, unaware,
and control groups in the verbal conditioning task. ( B ) Mean percent of correct
responses given by subjects in the aware group prior to and after verbalization
of the reinforcement contingency. Spielberger & DeNike, 1966.
& Postman (1955), and Sassenrath (1962), who likewise have analyzed
570 SYMBOLIC CONTROL OF BEHAVIORAL CHANGES
-12 -10 -8 -6 -4 + 2 +4
Before Statement of Principle Position of Block After
tile toward the experimenter (Weiss, Krasner, & Ullmann, 1960), and if
Response Rale
No schedule information
Minimal response instruction 17 17
Complete response instruction 161 161
Schedule information
Variable inter\ al 88 43
Fixed interval 5 7
Variable ratio 250 269
tive theories have been unable to find any evidence of verbal condition-
ing in the absence of correct or correlated hypotheses, whereas Postman
and his colleagues report, on the basis of experiments involving more
complex reinforcement contingencies, that a significant amount of learn-
ing can take place prior to verbalization of the basis for reinforcement.
These divergent conclusions do not appear to be attributable to any
major differences in the definition and assessment of awareness. Nor can
they be accounted for in terms of the operation of partially correct
hypotheses, since the use of partially relevant hypotheses in the concept
learning task did not facilitate performance (Hirsch, 1957; Postman &
Sassenrath, 1961), and the phenomenon is evident even when awareness
is defined to include partially correct verbalizations (Sassenrath, 1962).
Some additional suggestive evidence of behavioral change without
awareness is furnished by investigations involving more complex tasks,
such as probability learning, in which persons predict alternative events
or outcomes that vary in their frequency of occurrence. In these situa-
tions, persons' choice behavior gradually adjusts to the event probabilities
even though the vast majority of subjects are not only unable to state the
probability rules, but frequently entertain quite erroneous hypotheses
(Goodnow & Postman, 1955).
Discrepancies in results may, in part, result from the complexity of
the principle governing the administration of reinforcement and the re-
sponse restrictions imposed by the nature of the learning task. Studies
in which verbalization is accompanied by dramatic performance gains
have generally involved relatively explicit response classes or simple dis-
crimination tasks in which subjects are asked to construct sentences by
selecting one of several personal pronouns or verbs printed on cards.
When the critical response class is unambiguous and the response alterna-
tives are severely curtailed, both awareness and "learning" are most
likely to occur as a one-trial event rather than as an incremental process.
Considering the feeble and inconsequential nature of the reinforcers
employed in most verbal conditioning experiments, one might seriously
question whether reinforcement processes, which presumably govern
automatic response-strengthening effects, are even operative in most of
the studies that have been reviewed. This issue is, of course, of little or
no concern to researchers who are quite content with a circular empirical
law of effect. Nevertheless, it is important to distinguish between the
576 SYMBOLIC CONTROL OF BEHAVIORAL CHANGES
naria, goldfish, and other lower organisms, which lack the anatomical
structures for adequate symbolic representation of environmental events,
are totally unaffected by response consequences until they have ac-
curately cognized their experimenter's contingencies. Implicit mediators
would, of course, assume an important role in governing performance
in tasks that require response on the basis of relatively complicated prin-
ciples or rules.
The overall evidence would seem to indicate that learning can take
place without awareness, albeit at a slow rate, but that symbolic repre-
sentation of response-reinforcement contingencies can markedly acceler-
The validity of this view, which assumes
ate appropriate responsiveness.
between awareness and performance gains, seems
a reciprocal interaction
even more probable when one realizes the limitations of paradigms of
verbal conditioning for elucidating the role of symbolic activities in
behavioral change processes.
increases are generally in the order of 20-30 percent, which can hardly
be considered a massive outpouring of correct responses. It is also ex-
tremely likely that if the experiments were extended beyond the usual
single session, symbolic control, in the absence of supporting incentives,
would decrease over time and the desired behavior might eventually
return to its original level. Moreover, even in short-term situations em-
bodying high demand characteristics, a significant number of aware
subjects never do show any change in their behavior (Farber, 1963).
Hence, the findings of verbal conditioning studies, rather than demon-
strating the potency of symbolic control, in fact illustrate the limitations
of approaches that rely primarily upon cognitive variables to effect be-
havioral changes. The experiments do provide considerable evidence,
however, that awareness combined with incentive-related variables can
exert a powerful influence over behavior.
Spielberger, Bernstein, & Ratliff (1966) compared the response rate
of aware and unaware subjects during an initial phase of the experiment
in which "Mm-hmm" served as the reinforcer, and after an effort was
made to bolster the incentive value of the utterance by challenging sub-
produce as many "Mm-hmms" as they possibly could. Students
jects to
who remained unaware throughout both phases of the study showed no
evidence of conditioning; those who discovered the contingency prior
.
uu
95 A
Aware (pre)
90 -
Aware (post)
85 -
Unaware
80 -
75 -
70 -
65 -
60 -
55
50
45
40
35
30
25
20
15
10
5 _ ^^-^
i
21234567
|
Figure 9-4. Mean percent of correct responses given during the baseline (A),
low-incentive (B), and high-incentive (C) phases of the experiment by sub-
jects who either discovered the reinforcement contingency prior to or after the
incentive manipulation or remained unaware throughout the experiment. Spiel-
berger, Bernstein, & Ratliff, 1966.
Levin, & Shepard, 1962) rather than being varied independently like-
wise disclose that, among aware subjects, those who prize the reinforcers
show a high output of criterion behavior. By contrast, aware subjects who
are indifferent to, or annoyed by, experimenters' guttural utterances may
perform some correct responses in order to confirm their speculations, but
otherwise they are about as unproductive as their unaware counterparts.
In addition to the influence of incentives specifically associated with
the desired behavior, more generalized sets may determine the extent to
Symbolic Control of Classical Conditioning Phenomena 579
each of the remaining words would be paired once with aversive stimula-
tion and eventually shocks would cease altogether. A third group was
instructed that a certain number of shocks would be administered during
the experiment without implying a regular contingency. Subjects in the
first two groups, all of whom discerned the correct word-to-shock rela-
tionship, displayed conditioned heart rate responses, whereas subjects
who received minimal information and remained unaware evidenced no
conditioning. The controlling power of symbolic events is further shown
by evidence that aware subjects exhibited strong autonomic responses
to the critical stimulus but they did not generalize these responses
inappropriately along either semantic or physical dimensions. Moreover,
those who were informed when the extinction phase commenced showed
a prompt and virtually complete loss of conditioned responses before
experiencing any nonreinforced presentations of the conditioned stimulus.
In accord with the above finding, the most striking evidence of sym-
bolic control of classically conditioned responses is provided by studies
Grings & Lockhart, 1963; Wickens, Allen, & Hill, 1963). The decrement
ismost sudden and dramatic when subjects who, despite assurances to
the contrary, suspect that they might continue to be shocked are excluded
from the analysis (Bridger & Mandel, 1965). On the other hand, under
circumstances where arousal level is maintained and the operation of
cognitive factors is curtailed by disguising the conditioning procedures
(Spence, 1966), extinction proceeds at a comparatively slow rate after
reinforcement has been discontinued.
Although a strong causal relationship has been established between
cognitive variables and rate of classical conditioning and extinction, it
6.00 r
5.50
5.00
4.50
4.00 h
3.50
3.00
1.50
1.00 -~~.
0.50 v vr.
_l 1 L I I I I L
0.00 I I I I I
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
Acquisition Extinction
Figure 9-5. Mean GSR responses during acquisition and extinction for groups of
subjects for whom the CS was associated with either threat of shock alone or
threat plus shock. The pseudo-conditioning curves show the subjects' GSR re-
sponses to control stimuli that were never paired with either threat or shock. The
latter data provide a control for the effects of general arousal and orienting
mechanisms. Bridger and Mandel, 1964.
were presented during the acquisition period and on the temporal interval
between these stimulus events.
The nonmediational theory of classical conditioning assumes that, in
order for conditioning to occur, the associated stimulus events must at
least be registered in the nervous system of the organism. Therefore, in
studies assessing the role of awareness in conditioning, it would be of
considerable value to obtain evidence that there has, in fact, been input
from the conditioned stimulus. It is not inconceivable that in experiments
employing masking procedures, in which subjects' attention is diverted
to irrelevant features of the task, the conditioned stimuli may not be
registered in a sufficiently consistent manner to produce stable condi-
tioned responses. Hernandez-Peon, Scherrer, & Jouvet (1956) provide
evidence, based upon neurophysiological studies, that attention focused
on a particular stimulus simultaneously reduces afferent signals activated
by other sensory stimuli. The evoked auditory potential in the cochlear
nucleus of cats to a loud auditory stimulus was virtually eliminated when
they gazed at mice, attentively sniffed fish odors, or received electric
shocks that distracted their attention. Horn (1960) has demonstrated a
similar diminution of neural responses to a light flash during active
attention to other visual and auditory cues. Although there is some
584 SYMBOLIC CONTROL OF BEHAVIORAL CHANGES
The material discussed earlier attests to the fact that the most rapid
and enduring changes in instrumental behavior are achieved when
knowledge of contingencies is supplemented with appropriate reinforcing
consequences. In interview approaches interpretations of probable con-
tingencies and suggestions for preferable courses of action are offered
repeatedly, but favorable outcomes are rarely arranged. On the other
hand, practitioners utilizing reinforcement procedures carefully plan
the necessarv behavioral consequences, but often fail to specify the basis
for the reinforcement. It is apparent from the influential role played by
cognitive variables in change processes that in an optimal treatment
program change agents should designate the conditions of reinforcement
in addition to arranging the requisite response consequences.
There is a further potential application of knowledge of symbolic
control that is well worth exploring. It has been amply demonstrated that
behavior is partly regulated by its immediate consequences. Extensions
of this principle to the phenomenon of self-regulation (Bandura &
Perloff, 1967; Ferster, Nurnberger, & Levitt, 1962) provide evidence that
people can exercise a certain degree of control over their own behavior
by arranging favorable contingencies for themselves. Extending this notion
of self-management a step further, it is entirely possible that individuals
may be able to control and alter their behavior by symbolically produced
consequences.
Many forms of behavior that eventually create adverse social or
physiological effects are strongly maintained by their immediately rein-
forcing effects. If the remote consequences could be moved forward, or
if other types of negative outcomes were applied to the incipient pre-
cursors of the behavior, its occurrence might be significantly reduced.
In most cases, this rearrangement of consequences is difficult to achieve
by manipulating actual reinforcing However, there is some sug-
events.
gestive evidence that symbolized outcomes possess reinforcing proper-
ties that are similar to their physical equivalents. Weiner (1965) found
Although the discussion thus far has highlighted the possible tran-
quilizing effectsof attentional changes, they can serve a preventive
function as well. In many cases, a relatively weak external stimulus may
elicit a particular train of thought which, through its associative connec-
tions, activates further ideational contents capable of generating strong
emotional responses. By interrupting this associative sequence in its early
stages, the occurrence of thought-produced arousal may be forestalled
altogether.
Assuming that symbolic activities obey the same psychological laws
as overt behavior, it should be possible to influence significantly the
nature, incidence,and potency of covert events. The difficulties in detect-
ing the presence of implicit responses present a major obstacle to their
control by reinforcement practices if one adheres to the conventional
paradigm in which an external agent monitors the occurrence of the
desired behavior, imposes the contingencies, and administers the rein-
forcers. However, as Homme (1965) points out, the occurrence or ab-
sence of covert events can be easily and reliably detected by the per-
son doing the thinking. Consequently, such responses are most readily
conditioned through self-reinforcement operations. In this type of ap-
proach implicit responses are self-monitored, the contingencies are self-
crimination rarely occurs when the relevant stimuli are below recogni-
tion level.
A second methodological problem arises because verbal reports can
be influenced by nonsensory factors. Subjects are generally reluctant to
admit the presence of a faint stimulus when they are unsure, a negative
response bias that is likely to raise the verbal threshold artifactually. A
cautious person would obtain a verbal threshold much higher than is,
Discrepancy between Response Systems and the Unconscious 589
tively different nature from any response that has been superseded by an
alternative pattern of behavior.
Apart from its more secure empirical status, the concurrent response
system model has many advantages over formulations which assume the
existence of an unconscious mind. The former conceptualization does not
lend itself to pseudo-explanations in which a descriptive label for response
disparities is and assigned causal properties for example, dis-
reified —
crepancy between symbolic and motor or physiological responses to the
same stimulus events is given the descriptive label of "unconscious," which
is then converted into an internal agency that exercises powerful control
manifestations are obtained from subjects who have never been exposed to
the subliminal stimuli Johnson & Eriksen. 1961
|
It would appear from
.
the elusiye and scanty yield of research in this area that subliminal acti-
vation must play a relatiyely inconsequential role in regulating human
behayior. Whereas recognizable stimuli assume a powerful behavior-di-
recting function, nonrecognized stimuli have, at best. weak, inconsistent,
and fragmentary psychological effects.
Attitudinal Consequences of Behavioral and Affective Changes 595
overt actions and consequentlv that any changes brought about in the
attitudinal domain will have widespread effects upon subsequent be-
havior. It is further believed that altered response patterns that are
accompanied by correspondingly altered attitudes will be more stable
over time than behavior that is induced directly without cognitive sup-
ports. For these and other reasons, the development of beneficial attitudes
is often regarded as a major objective of social change endeavors. If it is
revert to their old behavior and the newly established attitudes are
similarly altered to coincide with the actions.
The relative modifiabilitv of attitudes and actions, and the degree of
correspondence obtained between changes in these two sets of events,
may vary with the affective and social consequences accompanying the
behavior. A given social influence might produce analogous changes in
both attitude and action when persons are indifferent to, or favorably
disposed toward, performing the advocated activities. Most attempts to
control consumer behavior through persuasive communications would
fall in this category. Thus, for example, a person who is considering
BELIEF-ORIENTED APPROACH
AFFECT-ORIENTED APPROACH
Live modeling
0.75
with participation
0.50 Symbolic modeling
0.25 Desensitization
Control
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
< - 1.25
1.50
1.75
2.00
2.25
2.50
1 2 1 2
Pre -Test Post -Test
Live modeling
1.75
with participation
1.50 Symbolic modeling
1.25 Desensitization
Control
1.00
0.75
0.50
0.25
0.00
-0.25
-0.50
-0.75
-1.00
-1.25
-1.50
1 2
Pre -Test Post -Test
Figure 9-6. Attitudinal changes for subjects who received either one of the three
treatment procedures or served as untreated controls. The numeral 1 indicates
subjects' attitudes prior to the behavioral test, and the numeral 2 shows their
attitudes immediately after the test of avoidance behavior. Bandura, Blanchard,
and Ritter, 1968.
formation before any treatment procedures were applied. After the test
for snake avoidance behavior, the attitude measures were again adminis-
tered. In the next phase of the experiment subjects received either
systematic desensitization, symbolic modeling, live modeling combined
with guided participation, or no treatment. Following completion of the
606 SYMBOLIC CONTROL OF BEHAVIORAL CHANGES
BEHAVIOR-ORIEXTED APPROACH
minimal threats or coercive inducements; few reasons are given for taking
the discrepant stand; the person receives a high degree of choice in com-
mitting himself to the counterattitudinal performance; there is high ex-
penditure of effort in the attainment of the goal object or in the enact-
ment of the discrepant behavior; the inducing agent is viewed unfavor-
ably; and the person being influenced displays high self-esteem. It should
be noted here that in naturalistic situations it is ordinarily no easy task to
get people to perform personally repugnant actions for any length of time
under such unfavorable incentive conditions.
Evaluation of the major theoretical issues and voluminous empirical
findings bearing on dissonance-arousing variables goes beyond the scope
of this book. For the interested reader, detailed reviews are available
elsewhere (Abelson, Aronson, McGuire, Newcomb, Rosenberg & Tannen-
baum, 1968; Chapanis & Chapanis, 1964; Elms, 1967; Feldman, 1966).
The empirical studies have generally yielded conflicting results; conse-
Strategies of Attitude Change 609
unexpected, and pressing" crisis, and urgent appeals to the subject to help
the experimenter "out of a jam," none of the experimental conditions, re-
gardless of the appended fee, can be considered as providing insufficient
justification for compliance. By contrast, when the inordinate social pres-
sures are absent and the monetary rewards serve as the main justification
MODIFICATION OF SELF-ATTITUDES
activities and more abstract values only indirectly related to the tasks
themselves are measured. The overall results, based upon numerous in-
vestigations of attitudes toward individualism, equalitarianism, theism,
and achievement, show that significant attitudinal changes can be in-
duced by providing individuals with successful task experiences. For
example, college students who worked better in groups than alone be-
came more collectivistic in their attitudes, whereas subjects who experi-
enced greater success when performing tasks independently adopted a
more individualistic orientation. These studies also provide some evi-
dence that attitudes induced by success tend to generalize to related
types of activities and to abstract preferences.
Change agents are often concerned not onlv with altering individual's
evaluations of different forms of behavior but in modifying their self-
may not be too readily achieved on the basis of the types of conditions
prescribed by the client-centered approach.
In many stem from be-
cases, of course, unfavorable self-attitudes
havioral deficits and are repeatedly reinforced through failure experi-
"Internalization" and Persistence of Behavioral Changes 615
and Persistence
"Internalization"
of Behavioral Changes
ganisms and with human subjects that cues regularly correlated with
reinforcement eventually gain control over the associated behavior.
Hence, where reliable discriminative stimuli are present it
in situations
is reasonable to question what has been internalized, and why it is neces-
tered in efforts to gain adequate control over antisocial groups, the com-
mon procedure is to remove a transgressor from his usual environment
and to subject him to some type of social influence.
Severe antisocial behavior can be controlled in residential centers
through differential reinforcement. Moreover, the resultant conforming
behavior is likely to persist as long as the institutional sanctions remain
in effect. The residents may, in fact, come to behave irreproachably
and even to perform obligingly whatever behavior is expected of them
in order to make conditions in the institution as pleasant as possible and
to expedite their release. A beneficent incentive system in a treatment
center may thus extract considerable prosocial behavior from delinquents,
but such persons often revert to their usual antisocial patterns whenever
supervisory staff members are no longer present. The attraction of the
620 SYMBOLIC CONTROL OF BEHAVIORAL CHANGES
Summary
Several theories have been proposed concerning the role of symbolic
processes in the regulation of behavior. These range from nonmediational
views that assume that reinforcing consequences modify behavior directly
and automatically, to cognitive formulations that consider symbolic repre-
sentation of contingencies a prerequisite for learning and performance
change. A reciprocal-interaction theory seems best able to order the
divergent findings bearing on this issue. According to this view, rein-
forcing consequences can alter behavior independently of awareness, but
individuals eventually infer, from observation of their behavior and its
responsiveness.
Symbolic activities not only augment the efficacy of reinforcement
operations, but they are also increasingly employed to generate emo-
tional effects that constitute the major reinforcing consequences in be-
havioral modification programs. In symbolic desensitization both the
aversive stimuli and the emotion-neutralizing responses are in large part
symbolically induced. Similarly, in aversive cognitive countercondition-
ing, avoidance responses toward addictive objects are established by con-
tiguous association of symbolic representations of positively valenced
stimuli with thought-produced nauseous reactions. Imagined conse-
quences may be employed instrumentally as covert reinforcers either
also
to strengthen or to reduce the incidence of overt behavior. Perturbing
trains of thought often disrupt psychological functioning, in which case
the problem becomes one of controlling symbolic events themselves. Self-
control of thought processes can be achieved by redirecting attention to
absorbing activities that elicit competing cognitions and by self-reinforce-
ment of more constructive lines of thought.
The issue of mediational control of behavior is also frequently raised
in the context of attitude theory. Although it is commonly assumed that
attitudinal changes have widespread and stabilizing influences upon
overt actions, induced alterations in attitudes in fact generally have few
enduring effects upon behavior unless they receive sufficient reinforce-
ment support. On the other hand, direct modification of the affective
properties of attitude objects and performance of attitude-discrepant
behavior produce stable corresponding changes in attitudes. This process
of cognitive accommodation to affective and behavioral changes has
been variously attributed to striving for cognitive consistency, to response
generalization of reinforcement effects, to the self-persuasive influence of
and to new experiential consequences re-
counterattitudinal behaviors,
sulting from the induced behavioral changes. It still remains to be
established whether environmental influences have similar but inde-
624 SYMBOLIC CONTROL OF BEHAVIORAL CHANGES
REFERENCES
Abelson, R. P., Aronson, E., McGuire, W. J.,
Newcomb, T. M., Rosenberg,
M. & Tannenbaum, P. H. Theories of
J.,
cognitive consistency: A source-
book. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1968.
Abelson, R. P., & Rosenberg, M. J. Symbolic psycho-logic: A model of attitu-
dinal cognition. Behavioral Science, 1958, 3, 1—13.
Adams, J. K. Laboratory studies of behavior without awareness. Psychological
Bulletin, 1957, 54, 393-405.
Aronson, E. The psychology of insufficient justification: An analysis of some
conflicting data. In S. Feldman (Ed.), Cognitive consistency. New York:
Academic Press, 1966. Pp. 109-133.
Aronson, E., Turner, J.
A., & Carlsmith, J. M. Communicator credibility and
communication discrepancy as determinants of opinion change. Journal
of Social Psychology, 1963, 67, 31-36.
Abnormal and
Ayllon, T., &
Azrin, N. H. Reinforcement and instructions with mental patients.
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1964, 7, 327-331.
Bandura, A. Social-learning theory of identificatory processes. In D. A. Goslin
(Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research. Chicago: Rand
McNally, 1968.
Bandura, A., Blanchard, E. B., & Ritter, B. The relative efficacy of desensiti-
zation and modeling approaches for inducing behavioral, affective, and
attitudinal changes. Unpublished manuscript, Stanford University, 1968.
Bandura, A., & Perloff, B. Relative efficacy of self-monitored and externally
imposed reinforcement systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 1967, 7, 111-116.
Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. Vicarious reinforcement and imitative
learning. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 67, 601-607.
References 625
Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. Adolescent aggression. New York: Ronald Press,
1959.
Bandura, & Walters, R. H. Social learning and personality development.
A.,
New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1963.
Barber, T. X., & Hahn, K. W., Jr. Experimental studies in "hypnotic" behavior:
Physiological and subjective effects of imagined pain. Journal of Nervous
and Mental Disease, 1964, 139,416-425.
Baron, A., & Kaufman, A. Human, free-operant avoidance of time out from
monetary reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,
1966, 9/557-565.
Bateson, G. (Ed.) Perceval's narrative: A patient's account of his psychosis,
1830-1832. Stanford University Press, 1961.
Bern, D. J. Self -perception: An alternative interpretation of cognitive disso-
nance phenomena. Psychological Review, 1967, 74, 183-200.
Bergin, A. E. The effect of dissonant persuasive communications upon changes
in a self-referring attitude. Journal of Personality, 1962, 30, 423-438.
Blanchard, E. B. The relative contributions of modeling, informational influ-
ences, and physical contact in the extinction of phobic behavior. Unpub-
lished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University, 1969.
Bourne, L. E., Jr. Human conceptual behavior. Boston: Allyn & Bacon, 1966.
Breer, P. E., & Locke, E. A. Task experience as a source of attitudes. Home-
wood, 111.: Dorsey, 1965.
Brehm, J. W., & Cohen, A. R. Exploration in cognitive dissonance. New York:
Wiley, 1962.
Bridger, W. H., & Mandel, I. J. A comparison of GSR fear responses produced
bv threat and electric shock. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 1964, 2,
31-40.
Bridger,W. H., & Mandel, I. J. Abolition of the PRE by instructions in GSR
conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1965, 69, 476-482.
Brown, R. W. Social psychology. New York: Free Press, 1965.
Bruning, J.
magnitude of reward and percentage of reinforcement
L. Effects of
on a lever response. Child Development, 1964, 35, 281-285.
movement
Carlin, M. T. The effects
of modeled behavior during imposed delay on the
observer's subsequent willingness to delay rewards. Unpublished doc-
toral dissertation, Stanford University, 1965.
Carlsmith, J.
M., Collins, B. E., & Helmreich, R. L. Studies in forced compli-
ance: I. The effect of pressure for compliance on attitude change pro-
duced by face-to-face role playing and anonymous essay writing. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 1966, 4, 1-13.
Cautela, J. R. Treatment of compulsive behavior by covert sensitization. Psy-
chological Record, 1966, 16, 33-41.
Chapanis, N. P., & Chapanis, A. Cognitive dissonance: Five years later. Psy-
chological Bulletin, 1964, 61, 1-22.
Chatterjee, B. B., & Eriksen, C. W. Cognitive factors in heart rate conditioning.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1962, 64, 272-279.
Clark, D. F. The treatment of monosvmptomatic phobia by systematic desensi-
tization. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1963, 1, 63-68.
Cohen, A. R. Attitude cliange and social influence. New York: Basic Books,
1964.
Collins,B. E. The effect of monetary inducements on amount of attitude
change produced by forced compliance. In A. C. Elms (Ed.), Role play-
ing, reward, and attitude change. Princeton, N. J.:
Van Nostrand, 1969.
626 SYMBOLIC CONTROL OF BEHAVIORAL CHANGES
Cook, W., & Harris, R. E. The verbal conditioning of the galvanic skin
S.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1937, 21, 202-210.
reflex.
Culbertson, F. M. Modification of an emotionally held attitude through role
playing. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1957, 54, 230-233.
Dabbs, J. M., Jr., & Janis, I. L. Why does eating while reading facilitate
opinion change? An experimental inquiry. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 1965, 1, 133-144.
Das, J. P., & Nanda, P. C. Mediated transfer of attitudes. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 1963, 66, 12-16.
Dawson, M. E. Comparison of classical conditioning and relational learning.
Unpublished master's thesis, University of Southern California, 1966.
Dawson, M. E., & Grings, W. W. Comparison of classical conditioning and
relational learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969 (in press).
De Nike, L. D. The temporal relationship between awareness and performance
in verbal conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1964, 68,
521-529.
Dollard, J., & Miller, N. E. Personality and psychotherapy. New York: McGraw-
Hill, 1950.
Doob, L. W. The behavior of attitudes. Psychological Review, 1947, 54, 135-
156.
Dulany, D. E. The place of hypotheses and intentions: An analysis of verbal
control in verbal conditioning. In C. W. Eriksen (Ed.), Behavior and
awareness —a symposium of reasearch and interpretation. Durham, N. C:
Duke University Press, 1962. Pp. 102-129.
Dulany, D. E. Awareness, rules, and propositional control: A confrontation
with S-R behavior theory. In T. R. Dixon & D. L. Horton (Eds.), Verbal
behavior and general behavior theory. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-
Hall, 1968. Pp. 340-387.
Dulany, D. E., Jr., & Eriksen, C. W. Accuracy of brightness discrimination as
measured by concurrent verbal responses and GSRs. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 1959, 59, 418-423.
Dulany, D. E., Jr. & O'Connell, D. C. Does partial reinforcement dissociate
verbal rules and the behavior they might be presumed to control? Journal
of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1963, 2, 361-372.
Duncker, K. Experimental modification of children's food preferences through
social suggestion. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1938, 33,
489-507.
Ekman, P., Krasner, L., & Ullmann, L. P. Interaction of set and awareness as
determinants of response to verbal conditioning. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 1963, 66, 387-389.
Elliott, R. Reaction time and heart rate as functions of magnitude of incentive
and probabilityof success: A replication and extension. Journal of Experi-
mental Research in Personality, 1966, 1, 174-178.
Ellis, R. A., & Lane, W. C. Structural supports for upward mobility. American
Sociological Review, 1963, 28, 743-756.
Elms, A. C. Role playing, incentive, and dissonance. Psychological Bulletin,
1967, 68, 132-148.
Elms, A. C, & Janis, I. L. Counter norm attitudes induced by consonant versus
dissonant conditions of role-playing. Journal of Experimental Research
in Personality, 1965, 1, 50-60.
Eriksen, C. W. Prediction from and interaction among multiple concurrent
References 627
References 631
Rosenthal, R., Persinger, G. W., Vikan-Kline, L. I., & Fode, K. L. The effect
of experimenter outcome-bias and subject set on awareness in verbal con-
ditioning experiments. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,
1963, 2, 275-283.
Sasmor, R. M. Operant conditioning of a small-scale muscle response. Journal
of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1966, 9, 69-85.
Sassenrath, J. M. Transfer of learning without awareness. Psychological Re-
ports, 1962, 10,411-420.
Schanck, R. L. A study of a community and its groups and institutions con-
ceived of as behavior of individuals. Psychological Monographs, 1932,
43 (2, Whole No. 195).
Scobie, S. R., & Kaufman, A. Intermittent punishment of variable interval and
variable ratio responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Be-
havior, 1969, 12, 137-147.
Shepard, R. N., Hovland, C. I., & Jenkins, H. M. Learning and memorization
of classifications. Psychological Monographs, 1961, 75 (13, Whole No. 517).
Sidman, M. Avoidance behavior. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior.
New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1966. Pp. 448-498.
Skinner, B. F. Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan, 1953.
Smith, G. H. The influence of a female model on perceived characteristics of
an automobile. Paper read at the meeting of the American Psychological
Association, San Francisco, August, 1968.
Spence, D. P. Conscious and preconscious influences on recall: Another ex-
ample of the restricting effects of awareness. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 1964, 68, 92-99.
Spence, D. P., & Holland, B. The restricting effects of awareness: A paradox
and an explanation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1962,
64, 163-174.
Spence, K. W. Cognitive and drive factors in the extinction of the conditioned
eye blink in human subjects. Psychological Review, 1966, 73, 445—458.
Spielberger, C. D., Berger, A., & Howard, K. Conditioning of verbal behavior
as a function of awareness, need for social approval, and motivation to
receive reinforcement. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963,
67,241-246.
Spielberger, C. D., Bernstein, I. H., & Ratliff, R. G. Information and incentive
Abelson, R. P., 137, 210, 599, 607, Ashem, B., 459, 493, 541, 555
608, 624, 630 Atkinson, R. C, 253, 284
Abi Rafi, A., 408, 414 Atthowe, J. M., Jr., 262, 267, 284
Abramovitz, A., 482, 497 Auld, F., Jr., 58, 63
Abrams, S., 506, 554 Ax, A. F., 381, 415, 487, 493
Adam, G., 22, 69 Ayllon, T., 10, 17, 27, 49, 63, 64, 108,
Adams, J. K., 568, 570, 624 113, 230, 233, 240, 241, 242,
Adams, J. S., 595, 629 245, 260, 262, 263, 264, 265,
Adler, H. E., 147, 204 266, 268, 284, 285, 297, 315,
Adler, L. L., 147, 204 316, 339, 348, 351, 369, 370,
Agras, W. S., 246, 288, 398, 399, 414, 371, 372, 373, 415, 572, 574,
419, 448, 449, 450, 497, 518, 624
527, 554, 555 Azrin, N. H., 27, 63, 108, 113, 230,
Ahmad, F. Z., 79, 117, 259, 292 233, 240, 241, 242, 245, 262,
Al-Issa, I., 459, 460, 496, 549, 559 263, 264, 265, 266, 268, 284,
Alexander, F., 78, 98, 113, 414 285, 295, 296, 297, 299, 307,
Alford, J.
A., 545, 555 312, 314, 315, 316, 323, 339,
Allchin, W. H., 552, 554 348, 350, 351, 353, 572, 574,
Allen, C. K., 362, 422, 582, 632 604
Allen, D. W., 148, 211
Allen, K. E., 237, 245, 246, 284, 285,
343, 354, 375, 376, 377, 414,
418 Bachrach, A. J., 246, 285
Allen, M. 67
K., 33, Backer, R., 218, 291
Allen, P., 329, 351 Bacon, R. C., 131, 215
Allport, F. H., 121, 204 Baer, D. M., 26, 27, 66, 77, 78, 114,
Alyokrinskii, V. V., 148, 204 122, 123, 124, 125, 204, 230,
Amsel, A., 359, 406, 415, 417 234, 237, 246, 284, 285, 287,
Anant, S. S., 506, 541, 555 339, 348
Anderson, D., 444, 498 Baer, P. E., 581, 627
Angermeier, W. F., 147, 204 Bailey, C. J.,
42, 64
Appel, J. B., 311, 339, 348, 350 Baker, S. L., Jr., 246, 286, 344, 350
Appel, K. E., 53, 59, 63 Bales, R. F., 535, 555
Armitage, S. G., 232, 246, 288 Ball, T., 248, 289
Armstrong, E. A., 197, 204 Bancroft,J.
H. J., 522, 555
Armstrong, J. D., 546, 555 Bandura, A., 3, 9, 14, 17, 23, 30, 32,
Aronfreed, J., 131, 204, 238, 288, 301, 33, 35, 36, 41, 51, 64, 75, 76, 77,
302, 305, 348 79, 81, 82, 91, 99, 104, 113, 117,
Aronson, E., 306, 348, 600, 608, 609, 118, 120, 122, 128, 129, 130,
624 131, 132, 133, 134, 136, 137,
Asch, S. E., 95, 113 138, 141, 144, 145, 146, 148,
634 Author Index
149, 150, 159, 167, 171, 173, Benton, A. A., 193, 205, 302, 305,
177, 178, 179, 180, 181, 182, 349
184, 186, 188, 190, 191, 193, Berberich, J.
P., 124, 154, 156, 210,
194, 195, 196, 198, 204, 205, 227, 230, 289
230, 235, 256, 259, 285, 292, Berg, C., 92, 113
296, 313, 338, 348, 349, 357, Berg, J. A., 58, 64, 95, 114
364, 379, 380, 382, 384, 397, Berger, A., 578, 631
415, 450, 457, 465, 476, 493, Berger, S. M., 23, 30, 64, 168, 172,
585, 597, 598, 601, 604, 605, 205, 380, 415
617 622, 624, 625 Bei-gin, A. E., 55, 59, 65, 95, 96, 97,
^
Banks, J. H., 169, 212 114, 600, 625
Barber, T. X., 473, 493 Berkowitz, L., 159, 205, 378, 382,
Barends, J., 459, 499 384, 415, 504, 557
Barker, J. C, 502, 504, 510, 511, 518, Berkowitz, S., 125, 205
555, 559 Berlew, D. E., 255, 288
Barlow, D. H., 398, 399, 414, 448, Berlvne, D. E., 220, 285
497, 518, 555 Berman, A. J.,
131, 215
Barnes, G. W., 219, 285 Bernstein, I. H., 577, 578, 631
Barnett, P. E., 172, 205 Bersh, P. J.,
362, 386, 387, 421, 426,
Barnwell, A. K., 196, 205 498
Baroff, G. S., 329, 331, 354 Bettelheim, B., 480, 493
Baron, A., 219, 285, 572, 573, 574, Bever, N. L., 137, 205
625, 628 Beyme, F., 460, 493
Barrabee, E. L., 59, 67 Biderman, A. D., 58, 65
Barrett, B., 334, 349 Bielinski, B., 548, 562
Barrett, B. H., 406, 408, 415 Bijou, S. \\\, 105, 115, 262, 285, 343,
Barry, H., 485, 486, 493, 531, 555 353, 381, 384, 418, 421
Baruch, D. W., 474, 493 Bis, 278, 286
J.,
Bass, B. M, 58, 64, 95, 114, 361 Bisese, V. S., 30, 65
Bass, M. J., 396, 415, 470, 493 Bitter, E., 380, 417,
Bassan, M. E., 53, 68 Bitterman, M. E, 360, 365, 417, 420,
Bateson, G., 42, 64, 379, 415, 617, 422
625 Black, A. H., 42, 65, 131, 175, 205,
Bayroff, A. G., 147, 205 304, 349, 357, 358, 388, 415,
Beach, F. A., 317, 349, 468, 494, 511, 416, 419, 425, 426, 428, 493
557 Blair,J.
H., 411, 412, 420
Becker, W. C., 105, 116, 148, 209, Blake, B. G., 541, 549, 555
262, 290, 384, 419, 420 Blake, R. R., 136, 164, 194, 196, 206,
Beech, H. R. 333, 349 207, 208, 209, 210, 214
Bell, R. W., 31, 68 Blakemore, C. B., 502, 504, 510, 518,
Belleville, R. E., 533, 558 555, 559
Bellugi, U., 151, 206 Blanchard, E. B., 17, 64, 75, 91, 104,
Bern, D. J.,
612, 625 113, 167, 182, 184, 186, 188,
Bern, S. L., 40, 64 190, 191, 204,
206, 364, 415,
Benedetti, D. T\, 172, 205 450, 457, 465, 476, 493, 597,
Benline, T. A., 389, 415 604, 605, 606, 624, 625
Benne, K. D., Ill, 113 Blane, H. T., 540, 558
Bennett, R. E., 544, 545, 546, 556 Bloch, B. L., 136, 211
Bensberg, G. J., 233, 234, 248, 285 Bloodstein, O., 327, 349
Bentler, P. M., 105, 113, 459, 475, Bloom, R. F., 131, 215
484, 493 Blough, D. S., 531, 555
Author Index 635
Boe, E. E., 300, 314, 316, 317, 349 Brush, E. S., 431, 499
Bolden, L., 416 Brutten, E. J., 318, 320, 349
Bolles, R. C, 296, 349 Bruun, K., 531, 555
Bond, I. K., 337, 349, 466, 493, 514, Bryan, J. H., 196, 206
525, 555 Bucher, B., 329, 330, 349
Bookbinder, L. J.,
48, 65 Buchwald, A. M., 195, 206, 244, 285
Bordin, E. S., 92, 115, 483, 493 Buehler, R. E., 6, 65, 344, 349, 381,
Boroczi, C, 317, 354 416
Bourne, L. E., Jr., 40, 65, 567, 625 Buell, J.
S., 237, 245, 284, 288, 375,
Cautela, J.
R., 257, 286, 506, 556, Conway, C. G., 502, 504, 510, 518,
584, 625 555, 559
Chafetz, M. E., 530, 534, 556 Cook, S. W., 362, 416, 580, 581, 626
Chambers, R. M., 218, 286 Cooke, G., 438, 459, 494
Chandler, P. J., 194, 208 Cooper, A. J., 510, 514, 524, 551, 556
Chapanis, A., 608, 625 Cooper, E. B., 322, 350
Chapanis, N. P., 608, 625 Cooper, J.,
609, 629
Chapel, J. L., 459, 493 Cooper, J.
494
E., 458,
Chapman, R. W., 344, 349 Coppock, H. W., 218, 286
Charms, R. de, 137, 207 Cornelison, A. R., 4, 5, 67
Chatterjee, B. B., 581, 625 Corsini, R. J., 164, 207
Child, G. P., 544, 545, 546, 556 Corson, J. A., 147, 207
Chittenden, G. E., 76, 114, 148, 159, Costello, C. G., 407, 416, 459, 494,
160, 161, 206, 379, 384, 416 518, 556
Chu, C. G., 95, 114 Costiloe, J.
P., 485, 499
Church, R. M., 31, 65, 147, 170, 206, Cowan, P. A., 151, 207, 260, 290
295, 300, 307, 317, 349 Cowden, R. C, 460, 461, 494
Clancy, J., 551, 556 Craig, K. D., 172, 207
Clark,' B. S., 195, 206 Crankshaw, E., 18, 65
Clark, D. F., 407, 416, 459, 474, 476, Crawford, M. P., 198, 207
481, 494, 518, 556, 580, 625 Crawford, R., 171, 215
Clark, M., 262, 286 Cressler, D. L., 102, 114, 275, 286
Clark, R., 532, 556 Crisp, A. H., 464, 497
Clark, R. A., 531, 556 Crook, G. H., 544, 545, 555
Clifford, B., 416 Crooks, J. L., 174, 207
Clifford, L. T., 416 Culbertson, F. M, 604, 626
Cloward, R. A., 4, 65 Curran, D., 516, 556
Clugston, H. A., 139, 215 Cutler, R. L., 100, 116
Cohen, A. R., 596, 599, 606, 608, 609,
625
Cohen, D. J., 280, 286 Dabbs, J. M., Jr., 95, 114, 602, 626
Cohen, H. L., 233, 262, 278, 286, Dalv, D. A., 322, 350
344, 349 Darby, C. L., 147, 207
Cohen, J., 278, 286 Dardano, J. F., 312, 350
Cohen, S. I., 460, 494 Darwin, P. L., 476, 495
Colby, K. M., 92, 114, 151, 206 Das, J. P., 603, 626
Cole, J. O., 485, 494 Davenport, J. W., 22, 65
Cole, M., 576, 629 Davidoff, E., 544, 545, 546, 556
Cole, M. W., 488, 495 Davies, D. L., 540, 556
Coles, M. R., 147, 215 Davis, D. M., 318, 323, 350
Collier, R. M., 92, 114 Davis, R. A., 139, 215
Collins, B. E., 609, 610, 611, 625 Davison, G. C., 433, 435, 440, 449,
Collins, B. J., 259, 292, 571, 629 450, 457, 479, 494, 521, 556
Colman, A. D., 246, 286, 344, 350 Davison, L. A., 168, 210, 215
Colwell, C. N., 233, 248, 285 Davitz, J. R., 383, 416
Conant, M. B., 133, 206 Dawson, M. E., 580, 581, 626
Conger, J. J., 530, 556 De Morsier, G., 539, 541, 556
Conn, J. H., 514, 556 De Nike, L. D., 565, 566, 568, 569,
Connor, R., 550, 558 626, 631
Conovitz, M. W., 317, 349 Deese, J., 357, 361, 416
Converse, P. E., 598, 630 Dekker, E., 20, 21, 65
Author Index 637
Filipczak, J.,
278, 286 Geer, ]. H., 460, 495
Finesinger, J.
E., 59,67 Gelder, M. G., 457, 458, 495, 497,
Finley, J.
R., 249, 291 510, 511, 516, 517, 518, 520,
Fjeld, H. A., 147, 216 527, 557, 560, 602, 629
Flanagan, B., 323, 350 Gelfand, D. M., 78, 114, 136, 137,
Flanders, J. P., 148, 198, 207 196, 308
Fleck, S., 4, 5, 67, 513, 557 Gelfand, S., 78, 114
Fleishmann, E., 595, 627 Gerard, R. W., 485, 494
Fleming, R. S., 245, 285 Gerst, M. S., 134, 135, 208
Fleshier, M., 299, 307, 309, 3-10, 351 Gewirtz, 208, 377, 417
L., 123,
J.
Flint, A. A., 58, 68 Giffin, M. E., 512, 513, 557, 559
Fode, K. L., 572, 631 Giles, D. K., 109, 117, 227, 248, 262,
Folkins, C. H., 438, 494 287, 292
Ford, C. S., 468, 494, 511, 521, 557 Gilmore, J. B., 609, 611, 612, 628
Ford, L. I., 460, 461, 494 Girardeau, F. L., 248, 262, 287
Fort, T., 535, 557 Gitelson, M., 18, 66
Foss, B. M., 130, 131, 207 Gittelman, M., 161, 208
Foster, F. M., 394, 417 Glad, D. D., 534, 535, 557
Fowler, H., 296, 350 Glaser, R., 280, 287
Fox, L., 255, 256, 286 Gleitman, H., 356, 418
Fox, S. S., 220, 287 Gliedman, L. H., 53, 58, 59, 65
Frank, G. H., 92, 114 Glynn, J. D., 518, 525, 557
Frank, J. D., 53, 58, 59, 65 Gorfman, E., 261, 287
Franks, C. M, 504, 557 Gold, V. J., 78, 115, 328, 352
Fraser, H. F., 533, 558 Golden, ]. M., 53, 67
Frederick, F., 34, 68 Goldiamond, I., 233, 256, 260, 278,
Freed, A., 194, 208 286, 287, 288, 318, 322, 323,
Freedman, J. L., 206, 350 325, 326, 350, 351, 406, 418,
Freeman, H. L., 394, 417, 475, 494 588, 627
Freitag, G., 78, 115, 227, 289, 328, Goldman, J.
R., 79, 81, 114, 259, 287
352 Goldstein, A. C., 317, 349
Freitag, L., 157, 210 Goldstein, N., 166, 213
Freud, S., 11, 65, 410, 417, 594, 627 Gollub, L. R., 227, 288
Freund, K., 504, 515, 518, 521, 557 Goodnow, J. J., 575, 627
Friedman, D., 459, 485, 494, 495 Goodwin, W., 244, 291
Friedman, I., 262, 287 Goorney, A. B., 511, 557
Fromer, R., 504, 557 Gordon, C. W., 550, 561
Fuhrer, M. J., 581, 627 Gourevitch, S., 593, 629
Furniss, J. M., 6, 65, 344, 349, 381, Gray, B. B., 460, 495
416 Greenberg, L. A., 529, 557
Greenwald, A. G., 595, 596, 627
Grice, R. G., 361, 419
Gale, D. 428, 495
S., Grings, W. W.,362, 418, 426, 481,
Gale, E. N., 428, 495 495, 580, 581, 582, 626, 627
Garcia, J., 502, 557 Groen, J.,
20, 21, 65
Gardner, J. E., 246, 287 Groot, H., 370, 418
Garfield, S. L., 53,66 Grossberg, J. M., 48, 66, 75, 114, 392,
Garfield, Z. H., 476,495 418, 473, 481, 495
Garvey, W. P., 394, 417 Grosser, D., 194, 208
Gebhard, M. E., 269, 286 Grusec, J. E., 33, 64, 75, 113, 130,
Geen, R. G., 384, 417 131, 134, 136, 137, 141, 148,
Author Index 639
177, 178, 193, 195, 204, 208, Hawkins, R. P., 105, 106, 107, 108,
212 115, 381, 384, 418
Grusec, T., 425, 493 Hayes, C, 147, 208
Guild, J., 545, 556 Hayes, K. J., 147, 208
Guthrie, E. R., 296, 351, 357, 358, Headlee, C. P., 218, 286
418, 444, 495 Hearst, E., 312, 351
Hefferline, R. F., 576, 627
Hegrenes, J. R., 394, 417
Heider, F., 607, 628
Hahn, K. W., Jr., 473, 493, 580, 625 Heilbrunn, G., 541, 556
Harm, S. C, 148, 161, 211 Heine, R. W., 9, 66, 94, 115
Hain, J. D., 196, 206, 457, 460, 495 Helmreich, R. L., 609, 610, 611, 625
Hald, J., 544, 558 Helson, H., 196, 208
Hall, J. F., 358, 388, 421 Henbest, R., 511, 538, 561
Hall, K. R., 197, 208 Hendry, D. P., 297, 351
Hall, R., 196, 214 Henke, L. B., 246, 284
Hall, R. V., 109, 117, 227, 246, 262, Henker, B. A., 83, 115, 130, 208
287, 292 Henry, G. W., 469, 495
Halse, D. F., 312, 348 Herbert, J. J., 147, 208
Hamilton, C, 21, 68 Herman, R. L., 315, 351
Hamilton, J., 329, 343, 351 Hernandez-Peon, R., 583, 628
Hanlon, C, 148, 208 Herrnstein, R. J.,
339, 345, 351, 352
Hanson, K., 544, 545, 555 Herzberg, A., 75, 115, 393, 418
Harford, R. A., 576, 627 Hess, E. H., 515, 558
Harlow, H. F., 220, 244, 287, 617, Hicks, D. J., 136, 148, 208, 307, 351
627 Hiler, E. W., 58, 66
Harper, P., 518, 525, 557 Hilgard, E. R., 217, 287
Harris, E., 595, 627 Hill, B., 315, 351
Harris, F. R., 26, 27, 66, 77, 78, 114, Hill, F. A., 362, 423, 582, 632
154, 214, 237, 245, 246, 284, Hill, H. E., 320, 351
285, 287, 288, 343, 354, 375, Hill,
J.
H., 138, 182, 208, 213
377, 414, 418 Hill, M. 540, 558
J.,
Harris, M. B., 83, 88, 113, 114, 138, Hinde, R. A., 197, 208
144, 149, 150, 196, 204, 208, Hine, C. H., 544, 545, 555
257, 287, 528, 558 Hinko, E., 262, 287
Harris, R. E., 362, 416, 580, 581, 626 Hirsch, J., 569, 574, 575, 628
Harsh, C. M., 147, 208 Hislop, M. W., 567, 628
Hart, B. M., 77, 114, 237, 284, 375, Hiss, R. A., 339, 350
377, 414, 418 Hletko, P., 541, 556
Hartmann, D., 379, 384, 418 Hoddinoth, B. A., 151, 207
Hartup, W. W., 193, 213, 444, 495 Hoenig, J., 449, 495
Harvery, J., 416 Hoff, E. C., 545, 558
Harvey, J. S., 59, 67 Hoffeld, D. R., 396, 418, 470, 495
Harvev, W. A., 53, 59, 63 Hoffman, H. S., 299, 307, 309, 310,
Harway, N. I., 92, 115 351
Haslam, M. T., 394, 418, 460, 495 Hoffman, M. L., 313, 351
Hastorf, A. H., 32, 66, 91, 115 Hofstadter, R., 110, 115
Haugen, G. B., 478, 495 Hogan, R. A., 403, 418, 419
Haughton, E., 10, 49, 64, 245, 260, Hogans, A. F., 531, 558
285, 370, 371, 372, 373, 415 Holland, B., 593, 631
Hawkins, H. L., 166, 214, 266, 291 Holland, J. I., 269, 286
640 Author Index
464, 465, 469, 471, 475, 482, Litin, E. M., 512, 513, 557, 559
497, 500 Little, J. C, 410, 420
Lazarus, R. S., 168, 210, 215, 438, Littman, I., 195, 213
494, 588, 629 Littman, R. A., 381, 421
Lazovik, A. D., 432, 433, 449, 457, Liverant, S., 444, 494
465, 471, 474, 496, 497 Liversedge, L. A., 332, 352, 354
Lazowick, L., 119, 210 Llewellyn Thomas, E., 194, 215
Leary, T., 59, 67 Locke, E. A., 613, 625
Leat, M., 193, 215 Lockhart, R. A., 362, 418, 426, 495,
Lee, D., 152, 209 582, 627
Lefkowitz, M. M., 136, 194, 210 London, P., 87, 115
Leitenberg, EL, 246, 288, 339, 340, Longenecker, E. G., 360, 365, 420
352, 398, 399, 414, 419, 448, Longstreth, L. E., 360, 420
497, 518, 555 Lorr, M., 58, 66, 67, 69
Lemere, F., 508, 511, 535, 537, 538, Lovaas, O. I., 75, 78, 90, 105, 115,
541, 543, 559, 562 124, 138, 142, 148, 151, 152,
Leon, H. V., 475, 497 153, 154, 155, 156, 157, 210,
Lessac, M. S., 302, 353 227, 230, 234, 244, 246, 259,
Lesser, G. S., 137, 210 288, 289, 328, 329, 330, 349,
Lester, D., 532, 536, 559 352, 611, 629
Leuba, C, 133, 210, 220, 288 Luchins, A. S., 144, 195, 210
Leuser, E., 396, 421 Luchins, E. H., 144, 195, 210
Leventhal, H., 95, 114 Lumsdaine, A. A., 140, 209, 600, 628
Levin, G. R., 226, 288 Lund, D., 246, 287
Levin, H., 235, 291, 338, 353, 379, Luria, A. R., 40, 67
421
Levin, S. M., 571, 578, 629, 631
Levine, S., 42, 67, 140, 210
Levis, D. J., 401, 403, 419, 420, 422 Maccoby, E. E., 136, 137, 139, 210,
Levitt, E.B., 88, 114, 255, 286, 585 235, 291, 338, 353, 379, 421,
Levitt, E. E., 53, 67 595, 629
Levitt, T., 595, 627, 629 Maccoby, N., 132, 139, 140, 210, 212,
Levy, D. M., 47, 67 214, 595, 629
Levy, L. H., 572, 629 MacCulloch, M. J., 335, 337, 350, 352
Levy, N., 357, 361, 421 Mackay, H. A., 580, 629
Lewis, D. J., 612, 629 Mackintosh, I., 361, 420
Lewis, J., 21, 68 Macklin, E. A., 544, 545, 555
Lewis, M., 238, 288 Maddi, S. R., 362, 421
Lhamon, T. W., 53, 59, 63 Madsen, C. H., Jr., 148, 196, 209,
Lichtenstein, F. E., 311, 317, 352 211, 367, 384, 419, 420, 460, 497
Liddell, H., 484, 497 Mager, R. F., 73, 116, 253, 289
Lidz, T., 4, 5, 67 Mahoney, M. J., 189, 213
Liebert, R. M., 33, 34, 67, 68, 138, Mahrer, A. R., 72, 116
182, 208, 213, 215, 313, 352 Malinowski, B., 511, 560
Lienert, G. A., 529, 559 Malleson, N., 403, 420
Lifshitz, K., 411, 412, 420 Mallick, S. K., 382, 420
Linder, D. E., 609, 629 Mandel, I. J., 362, 363, 364, 416, 580,
Lindley, R. H., 362, 420, 421 582, 583, 625, 629
Lindsley, O. R., 280, 286 Mandler, G., 140, 211
Lippitt, R.,194, 199, 208, 210, 213 Mann, J., 455, 497
Lipsher, D. H., 79, 81, 113, 259, 285 Mann, L. L, 360, 419
Author Index 643
Mischel, W., 14, 15, 34, 53, 68, 83, Nisbett, R. E., 489, 498
113, 130, 136, 146, 148, 149, Noelpp, B., 20, 68
205, 208, 212, 313, 352, 598, 630 Noelpp-Eschenhagen, I., 20, 68
Mitchell, L. E., 485, 498 Notterman, J. M., 362, 386, 387, 421,
Mohonev, J. L., 460, 495 426, 498
Mohr, J.' P., 246, 285 Nurnberger, J.
I., 88, 114, 255, 286,
Moltz, H., 357, 361, 362, 421 585, 627
Moore, N., 21, 68, 435, 437, 498 Nuttin, J. M., Jr., 609, 610, 630
Mordkoff, A., 168, 210, 215
Moreno, C. M., 531, 558
Moreno, J. L., 164, 212
Oakes, W. F., 603, 628
Morgan, C. L., 120, 212
O'Connell, D. C., 567, 630
Morgenstern, F. S., 518, 560
O'Connor, N., 460, 498
Morrice, D. J., 166, 211
O'Connor, R. D., 162, 163, 212
Morrisett, L. N., Jr., 139, 212
Odom, R. D., 138, 213, 219, 289, 291
Morrison, D. C, 105, 117
Ofstad, N. S., 136, 213
Morse, W. H., 339, 352
Ogawa, N., 169, 212
Moser, D., 24, 68
Ohlin, L. E., 4, 65
Mott, D. E. W., 182, 208
O'Hollaren, P., 508, 535, 537, 543,
Mouton, J. S., 136, 194, 196, 206,
559, 562
208, 210
O'Keeffe, K., 518, 561
Mowrer, O H., 7, 68, 119, 130, 132,
O'Leary, K. D., 105, 116, 240, 262,
165, 212, 220, 289, 295, 311,
290
314, 316, 317, 352, 354, 356,
O'Leary, S., 105, 116
385, 417, 421, 431, 498, 503,
Oliveau, D. C, 448, 497
561
Olmstead, J. A., 196, 208
Mowrer, R. A., 248, 287, 378
Opton, E. M., Jr., 438, 494
Moyer, K. E., 362, 420, 421
Osgood, C. E., 607, 630
Mueller, M. R., 340, 354
Osinski, W., 544, 545, 546, 556
Murphy, J. V., 169, 212
Osmond, H. O., 245, 285
Murray, E. 79, 81, 82, 116, 259,
J., Oswald, I., 510, 518, 523, 551, 561
289, 473, 498
Ottenberg, P., 21, 68
Mussen, P. H., 83, 116, 130, 136, 209,
Ounsted, C, 152, 209
212
Myers, A. K., 220, 289
Myers, J. K., 58, 63
Myers, J. M., 53, 59, 63 Page, H. A., 358, 388, 389, 421
Page, M. L., 76, 116
Paige, A. B., 360, 420
Nachmias, J., 356, 418 Paivio, A., 41, 68, 140, 213
Nanda, P. C, 603, 626 Panyan, M., 262, 287
Narrol, H. G., 262, 289, 548, 561 Parke, R. D., 128, 193, 195, 213, 216,
Nash, E. H., Jr., 53, 58, 59, 65, 66 302, 303, 307, 352, 354, 632
Neisser, U., 356, 418 Parker, A. L., 83, 116, 130, 212
Nelson, F., 479, 498 Parloff, M. 213
B., 166,
Nelson, K., 157, 210 Parr, D., 516, 556
Nelson, S. E., 319, 352 Parsons, T., 119, 213
Newcomb, T. M., 598, 608, 624, 630 Pascal, G. R., 54, 68
Newman, R. G., 58, 68 Paschke, R. E., 31, 68
Nigro, M. R., 339, 352 Pastore, N., 305, 353
Nikelly, A. G., 513, 563 Patterson, C. H., 81, 116
Author Index 645
Patterson, G. R., 6, 65, 90, 105, 116, Pullan, B. R., 522, 555
195, 213, 246,
290, 344, 349, Putzey, L. J.,
164, 207
381, 416, 444, 498
421,
Paul, G. L., 54, 68, 452, 453, 454,
465, 471, 481, 498 Quagliano, J., 297, 353
Pawlowski, A. A., 531, 561 Quinn, J. T., 511, 538, 561
Peacock, L. J., 502, 561 Quist, R. W., 319, 353
Pearce, J. F., 518, 560
Pelser, H. E., 20, 65
Pentony, P., 83, 116, 166, 213 Rabon, D., 262, 287
Perkins, R. B., 529, 556 Rachman, S., 11, 69, 435, 439, 449,
Perloff, B., 35, 36, 64, 124, 154, 210, 460, 469, 497, 499, 504, 510,
227, 230, 256, 285, 289, 585, 513, 518, 519, 520, 527, 560,
617, 624 561
Perloff, W. H., 521, 561 Raines, J.,
434, 500
Perry, H. M., 139, 213 Ramsay, R. W., 459, 499
Persinger, G. W., 572, 631 Ratliff, R. G., 577, 578, 631
Peters, A. D., 59, 69 Ratner, S. C., 361, 416
Peters, H. N., 246, 290 Rau, L. C., 79, 117, 259, 292
Peterson, R. F., 105, 115, 117, 124, Rausch, H. L., 46, 68
125, 204, 230, 245, 285, 381, Raush, H. L., 92, 115
418 Rawnsley, K., 460, 498
Philbrick, D. B., 569, 570, 630 Ray, A. A., 447, 500
Phillips, E. L., 240, 273, 290 Ray, R., 105, 116, 246, 289
Piaget, J.,
121, 144, 213 Raymond, M. J.,
510, 518, 528, 551,
Pinshoff, M., 337, 352
J. 561
Pittman, D. J., 534, 550, 561 Razran, G. H. S., 22, 68, 580, 582,
Plager, E., 420 602, 630
Piatt, S. A., 358, 422 Reahl, J. E., 269, 286
Podnos, B., 545, 561 Reber, A., 302, 348
Polansky, N., 194, 199, 208, 210, 213 Redl, F., 199, 213, 226, 290
Polin, A. T., 389, 400, 401, 403, 421 Reed, G. F., 449, 495
Polish, E., 532, 556 Reed, J. L., 148, 213, 460, 494
Pollio, H. R., 91, 108, 117, 246 Rees, W. L., 518, 560
Poppen, R. L., 390, 403, 421, 428, Reis, E. E. S., 488, 496
429, 440, 498 Renner, K. E., 231, 290
Porro, C. R., 193, 213 Rescorla, R. H., 296, 353, 425, 427,
Porter, L. W., 42, 64 499
Porter, R. W., 22, 69 Resnick, L. B., 253, 290
Porterfield, A. L., 535, 557 Reynolds, D. J., 433, 449, 457, 465,
Poser, E. G., 57, 68 497
Postman, L., 566, 568, 569, 570, 575, Reynolds, N. J., 77, 114, 246, 284
627, 630 Rheingold, H. L., 219, 290
Powell, J., 307, 312, 353 Richard, H. C., 260, 290
Powers, R. B., 227, 289 Richter, C. P., 532, 561
Pratt, C. C., 72, 116 Rickles, N. K., 53, 68
Pratt, S., 255, 292 Rifkin, B. G., 460, 499
Preisler, L., 138, 216 Rigler, D., 92, 115
Premack, D., 221, 229, 290, 587, 630 Rimland, B., 152, 213
Prince, A. L, 444, 498 Rimm, D. C., 189, 213
Proctor, S., 438, 479, 498 Rioch, M. J., 58, 68
646 Author Index
Riopelle, A. J.,
147, 207 Sanders, D. H., 102, 114, 275
Risley, T. R., 27, 69, 90, 105, 116, Sanders, R., 269, 286
151, 154, 213, 281, 292, 329, Sanderson, R. E., 551, 555
330, 332, 340, 343, 353, 354 Sandler, J., 297, 353, 504, 510, 518,
Ritter, B. J.,
17, 64, 75, 91, 104, 113, 559
167, 182, 183, 184, 186, 188, Sargant, W., 411, 421
189, 191, 204, 213, 364,
190, Sargent, W., 551, 561
415, 450,
457, 465, 476, 493, Saslow, G., 59, 69
597, 605, 624
604, Sasmor, R. M., 576, 631
Robinson, L. H., 545, 561 Sassenrath, J. M., 40, 69, 566, 568,
Roby, T. B., 218, 291 569, 575, 630, 631
Rocha e Silva, M. L, 249, 290 Sauerbrunn, D., 312, 350
Rodgers, D. A., 532, 561 Saul, L. J.,
396, 421
Rogers, C. R., 55, 68, 81, 82, 83, 92, Savoye, A. L., 320, 353
116, 614, 630 Schachter, J., 381, 421, 487, 499
Rogers, J. M., 235, 259, 290 Schachter, S., 196, 214, 256, 290, 381,
Rokeach, M., 599, 630 421, 488, 489, 498, 499
Rome, H., 396, 421 Schaefer, H. H, 246, 290
Romney, A. K., 595, 629 Schaeffer, B., 124, 154, 210, 227,
Roos, P., 248, 290 230, 289
Rosekrans, M. A., 193, 213 Schanck, R. L., 596, 631
Rosen, ]. N., 92, 96, 116 Schaul, L. T., 147, 204
Rosen, S., 199, 210 Schein, E. H., 128, 137, 194, 214
Rosenbaum, M. E., 30, 68, 136, 137, Scherrer, H., 583, 628
196, 206, 207, 214 Schmidt, E., 518, 521, 527, 562
Rosenberg, M. J.,
598, 599, 606, 607, Schneider, R. A., 485, 499
608, 609, 611, 612, 624, 630 Schoenfeld, W. N., 362, 386, 387,
Rosenblith, J. F., 136, 214 421, 426, 498
Rosenhan, D., 34, 68, 196, 214 Schofield, W., 59, 69
Rosenthal, D., 81, 83, 116, 166, 214 Schroeder, H. G., 529, 562
Rosenthal, R., 446, 499, 572, 631 Schubot, E. D., 435, 437, 440, 457,
Rosenthal, T. L., 23, 64, 95, 116, 137, 499
171, 173, 205, 274, 289, 380, Schutz, R. E., 226, 291
415, 455, 459, 460, 497, 499 Schwartz, A. N., 166, 214, 266, 291
Ross, D., 32, 64, 83, 113, 136, 137, Schwartz, M. S., 261, 291
148, 194, 196, 205, 214, 601, Schweid, E., 105, 115, 381, 418
624 Scobie, S. R., 574, 631
Ross, S. A., 32, 64, 83, 113, 136, 148, Scott, P. M., 385, 421
194, 196, 205, 214, 601, 624 Seager, C. P., 475, 499
Rotenberg, I. C, 307, 353 Sears, R. R., 235, 291, 338, 353, 378,
Rubenstein, B. D., 227, 289 379, 417, 421
Rubinstein, E. A., 58, 66, 67, 69 Sedlacek, F., 515, 557
Ruck, F., 541, 561 Seidel, R. J., 224, 291
Ruckmick, C. A., 168, 207 Seltzer, A. L., 515, 558
Russo, S., 105, 116 Sensibar, M. R., 531, 556
Seward, J. P., 357, 361, 421
Shames, G. H., 318, 353
Shanahan, W. M., 541, 561
Salzinger, K., 258, 260, 290 Shannon, D. T., 454, 498
Salzinger, S., 260, 290 Shaver, K., 171, 215
Sampen, S. E., 244, 292, 384, 423 Shaw, D., 105, 116, 246, 290
Author Index 647
Shaw, I. A., 545, 561 215, 295, 296, 302, 304, 311,
Shaw, M. E., 164, 207 316, 349, 353, 358, 387, 422,
Sheehan, J.
G., 318, 319, 320, 321, 425, 426, 427, 431, 474, 498,
322 353 500
Sheffield' F. D., 132, 133, 139, 140, Solyom, L., 518, 521, 562
209, 211, 214, 218, 291, 600, Speisman, J. C., 92, 116, 168, 210,
628 215
Shepard, M. C, 578, 631 Spence, D. P., 593, 629, 631
Shepard, R. N., 567, 631 Spence, K. W., 198, 207, 362, 422,
Sherman, A. R., 485, 486, 499 460, 500, 508, 562, 582, 631
Sherman, A., 122, 123, 124, 125,
J.
Spiegler, M.
D., 182, 215
158, 204, 206, 214, 230, 246, Spielberger, C. D., 565, 566, 568,
260, 285, 291 569, 577, 578, 631
Sherrick, C. C, Jr., 318, 353 Spohn, H. E., 8, 69
Shlien, J. M., 515, 558 Spradlin, J.
E., 248, 262, 287
Shoben, E. J., Jr., 81, 116, 473, 483, Staats, A.W., 226, 236, 244, 248,
499 249, 250, 251, 291, 603, 631
Shoemaker, D. J., 318, 320, 349 Staats, C. K., 226, 291, 603, 631
Shrovon, H. J., 411, 421 Stampfl, T. G., 401, 422
Sidman, M., 46, 69, 243, 291, 312, Stanley, W. 290
C., 219,
351, 386, 421, 531, 561, 616, Stanton, A. H., 261, 291
631 Stassi, E.
J.,
320, 354
Siegel, G. M., 319, 353 Stein, M., 21, 68
Silber, E., 58, 68 Steinberg, F., 317, 349
Silberman, H. F., 253, 291 Stephens, H. D., 532, 559
Silverman, I., 460, 495 Stephens, L., 329, 343, 351
Simmel, E. C, 389, 415 Stevenson, H. W., 219, 291, 444, 500
Simmons, J. J., 226, 288 Stevenson, I., 457, 460, 467, 495,
Simmons, J. Q., 227, 289 500, 525, 562
Simon, A., 544, 545, 555 Stiles, W. B., 380, 417
Simon, R., 269, 286 Stingle, K. G., 123, 208
Simon, S., 31, 68 Stoke, S. M., 119, 215
Singer, B. A., 476, 495 Stollak, G. E., 528, 562
Singer, J.
E., 381, 421, 488, 499 Stoller, R.
J.,
512, 562
Skinner, B. F., 15, 24, 27, 29, 38, 65, Stolurow, L. M., 253, 291
69, 82, 116, 122, 196, 214, 240, Stone, A. R., 53, 58, 59, 65, 66
291, 360, 406, 417, 421, 565, Stone, G. B., 269, 286
616, 631
627, Storms, L. M., 317, 354
Skolnik, J.
562
H., 535, Stotland, E., 171, 215
Slack, C. W., 227, 291 Strahley, D. F., 474, 500
Slamecka, N. J., 360, 421 Straughan, J.,
246, 291
Slivka, R. M., 308, 352 Strel'chuk, I. V., 539, 562
Sloane, H. N., Jr., 154, 214, 244, 292, Stuart, R. B., 88, 117, 257, 258, 292,
343, 353, 384, 421, 423 528, 562
Slobin, D. I., 151, 214 Stunkard, A. J., 256, 257, 292
Slucki, H., 22, 69 Sturm, I. E., 164, 215
Smart, R. G., 530, 562 Sturmfels, G., 428, 495
Smith, E. W. L., 444, 499 Surridge, C. T., 359, 415
Smith, G. H., 602, 631 Sutherland, E. H., 529, 562
Snyder, C. R., 534, 535, 561, 562 Sweetland, A., 92, 114
Solomon, R. L., 41, 69, 131, 147, 206, Sylvester, J.
D., 332, 352, 354
648 Author Index
Syme, L., 529, 562 Turner, L. H., 41, 69, 131, 215, 302,
Szasz, T. S., 2, 17, 18, 19, 69 304, 353, 425, 500
Turner, R. H., 598, 630
Twining, W. E., 139, 215
Tyler, D. W., 360, 365, 417, 422
Tannenbaum, P. H., 607, 608, 624, Tyler, V. O., Jr., 343, 349, 354
630
Tarde, G., 120, 215
Tate, B. G., 329, 331, 354 Ullmann, L. P., 259, 292, 460, 497,
Taub, E., 131, 215 571, 579, 626, 629, 632
Taylor, D. M., 59, 69 Ulrich, R. E., 314, 351
Taylor, J. A., 460, 500 Uno, T., 481, 495
Teodoru, D., 131, 215 Usdansky, B. C., 58, 68
Terrace, H. S., 25, 69, 440, 500
Terry, D., 4, 67
Terwilliger, J. S., 8, 69 Valins, S., 447, 500
Test, M. 206
A., 196, Vallance, M., 257, 289, 505, 510,
Tharp, R. G., 274, 289 518, 526, 528, 560
Thibaut, J. W., 46, 69 Van Riper, C., 319, 354
Thimann, J., 541, 547, 562 Van Toller, C., 297, 351
207
Thistlethwaite, D. L., 148, Vandell, R. A., 139, 215
Thomas, D. R., 420 Vanderhoof, E., 551, 556
Thomas, J., 233, 260, 288 Varenhorst, B. B., 196, 209
Thompson, G. N., 548, 562 Verplanck, W. S., 565, 631
Thomson, L. E., 246, 288, 419 Vikan-Kline, L. I., 572, 631
Thoresen, C. E., 196, 209 Vinogradov, N. V., 460, 500
Thorndike, E. L., 146, 215, 565, 631 Voegtlin, W. L., 508, 511, 535, 537,
Thome, G. L., 274, 289 538, 541, 542, 543, 547, 559,
Thorpe, J. G., 502, 504, 510, 518, 562
521, 527, 555, 559, 562 Von Mering, O., 262, 287
Thorpe, W. H., 197, 215
Tighe, T. J., 339, 350
Tilton, J.
288
R., 232, 246, Wagner, A. R., 359, 422, 531, 555
Timmons, E. 354
O., 316, Wagner, M. V., 567, 630
Tinbergen, N., 197, 215 Wahler, R. G., 91, 105, 108. 117, 238,
Tobias, S., 259, 292 246, 292
Tolman, C. W., 340, 354 Wall, A. M., 238, 288
Tooley, J. T., 255, 292 Wall, J. H., 535, 562
Tordella, C. L., 529, 562 Wallace, J. A., 541, 563
Tosti, D. T., 85, 115 Wallerstein, R. S., 72, 117, 546, 563
Trapold, M. A., 425, 500 Walters, R. H., 3, 14, 76, 77, 82, 104,
Trapp, E. P., 137, 209 113, 117, 118, 120, 128, 132,
Traxel, W., 529, 559 136, 137, 158, 193, 194, 195,
Truax, C. B., 79, 81, 82, 117, 166, 205, 209, 213, 215, 216, 235,
215, 259, 292 285, 302, 303, 307, 338, 349,
Tucker, I. F., 136, 214 352, 354, 379, 384, 415, 418,
Tulving, E., 140, 215 422, 622, 625, 632
Tupper, W. E., 535, 537, 559 Walton, D., 21, 69, 328, 354, 373,
Turner, D. W., 175, 212, 539, 541, 393, 394, 395, 407, 422, 460,
560 485, 500
Turner, J.
A., 600, 624 Warden, C. J., 147, 216
Author Index 649
Watson, J. A., 502, 561 154, 213, 226, 227, 234, 237,
Watson, J. B., 146, 216 245,246, 248, 249, 262, 281,
Watson, L. S., Jr., 248, 287 284,285, 286, 287, 288, 291,
Weinberg, N. H., 474, 500 292, 329, 340, 343, 353, 354,
Weinberg, S. K., 261, 286 375,377, 414, 418
Weinberger, N. M., 388, 422 Wolfgang, M. E., 381, 423
Weiner, H., 339, 354, 585, 632 Wolpe, J., 11, 69, 75, 76, 117, 164,
Weingarten, E., 269, 286 185, 216, 430, 431, 435, 444,
Weinstein, M. S., 172, 207 457, 458, 459, 460, 464, 466,
Weinstein, W. K., 571, 632 469, 471, 472, 474, 476, 478,
Weisman, R. G., 358, 416, 422 479, 483, 498, 500, 505, 525,
Weiss, R. L., 571, 629, 632 526, 562, 563
Weiss, W. W., 600, 628 Wolpin, M., 434, 500
Werry, J. S., 407, 417 Woodward, M., 516, 563
Wessen, A. F., 261, 292 Worell, J., 594, 632
West, L. J., 617, 627 Worell, L., 594, 632
Westley, W. A., 82, 114 Wortz, E. C, 365, 422
Whalen, C. K., 33, 64, 148, 157, 205, Wright, B. A., 151, 207
210 Wright, D. E., 419
Whalen, R. E., 218, 292 Wulff, J. J., 218, 291
Wheeler, L., 194, 216, 379, 422, 489, Wynne, L. C, 358, 387, 422, 426,
499 474, 499, 500
White, G. M., 196, 214
White, J. G., 393, 422
Whitener, R. W., 513, 563
Yablonsky, L., 6, 69, 381, 423, 551,
Whiting, J.
W. M., 311, 316, 317,
563
354, 379, 423
Yanushevskii, I. K., 546, 563
Whitlock, C, 227, 292
Yarrow, M. R., 385, 421
Wickens, D. D., 362, 423, 582, 632
Yates, A. J., 48, 69, 405, 408, 423
Wike, E. L., 361, 420
Young, A. G., 366, 423
Wikler, A., 533, 558
Young, B. G., 513, 519, 560
Wilde, G. ]. S., 528, 563
Yum, K. S., 530, 532, 560
Williams, C, 368, 422
Williams, C. D., 105, 117, 369, 423
Williams, R. I., 259, 292
Wilson, F. S., 128, 158, 216 Zarrow, M. X., 531, 561
Wilson, H., 473, 495 Zaslove, M., 478, 500
Wilson, W. C, 136, 137, 210 Zax, M., 54, 68, 485, 498
Wilson, W. J., 361, 423 Zeilberger, 244, 292, 384, 423
J.,
Winder, C. L., 79, 81, 117, 259, 292 Zeisset, R. M., 460, 500
Wineman, D., 226, 290 Zerbolio, D. J., Jr., 358, 422
Winitz, H., 138, 216 Zilboorg, G., 93, 117
Winkel, G. H., 105, 117 Zimmer, H., 58, 65
Winokur, S., 425, 500 Zimmerman, D. W., 227, 292
Winter, S. K., 255, 288 Zimmerman, E. H., 246, 292
Wischner, G. J., 318, 319, 354 Zimmerman, J., 246, 292
Wolf, M. M., 26, 27, 66, 69, 78, 90, Zimmerman, J. A., 339, 354
105, 109, 114, 116, 117, 151, Zubin, J., 53, 69
Subject Index