Cramer2010 3
Cramer2010 3
The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 198, Number 9, September 2010 www.jonmd.com | 619
Cramer and Kelly The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 198, Number 9, September 2010
suppression of the attachment system occurs via deactivating strat- regarding the relation between ego defense mechanisms and attach-
egies, which are characteristic of the Avoidant (Dismissive and ment style. The present study will address this question.
Fearful) attachment styles. These strategies involve the defense There are several methods available to assess the use of ego
mechanism of denial— denial of attachment needs, denial or over- defense mechanisms (Cramer, 2006). One approach that has been
looking one’s own weaknesses or faults, failure to attend to attach- used successfully in both experimental and observational research is
ment related cues, and avoidance of emotional involvement (Shaver based on the coding of narrative material. Using a detailed Defense
and Mikulincer, 2008). In the case of the Anxious (Preoccupied) Mechanism coding Manual (DMM; Cramer, 1991a), the use of 3
style, hyperactivation occurs, including rumination, increased efforts defenses— denial, projection, and identification— can be ascertained
to gain the significant other’s attention, and loyalty to the other. For from a series of Thematic Apperception Test stories (Murray, 1943).
Preoccupieds, the desire for closeness with the relationship partner Defense scores based on this coding have been shown to increase
can lead to the use of the defense of identification, seen in an illusory when the story-teller is under stress (e.g., Cramer, 1991b, 1998), to
sense of similarity and blurring of boundaries between self and be related to self-reported violence (Porcerelli et al., 2004), and to be
other. The use of both the deactivating and hyperactivating defenses related to psychopathology (Cramer, 1999).
may contribute to adjustment, but they are likely to break down
under severe stress. EGO DEFENSES AND ABUSE: THEORY
Research has determined that deactivating defensive opera- Theoretically, the 3 defenses of denial, projection, and iden-
tions are preemptive—i.e., they occur at the time of encoding tification have been implicated in the occurrence of child abuse.
stimulus events, rather than being postemptive, or occurring at the Abusive parents are described as having distorted perceptions of
time of memory retrieval (Fraley et al., 2000; Fraley and Shaver, their children, because of their reliance on these defenses. There are
1997; Fraley and Brumbaugh, 2007). Fraley and Brumbaugh (2007) several reasons why the use of denial might be characteristic of
also noted that preemptive strategies result in the individual being abusive parents, especially whether they themselves were abused as
less likely to recognize the emotional implications of interpersonal a child. That is, because the abuse of these individuals occurred at an
events. This suggests the possibility that the person who uses a early stage in which they were unable to physically defend them-
preemptive defensive strategy would be less likely to recognize the selves against the abuser (Arcaya and Gerber, 1990; Briere and
negative implications of abusive treatment of a child. Runtz, 1993; Prodgers, 1984), and because denial was the defense
that was available to the young child to defend against both the
physical pain and the anger over the emotional betrayal of the
ATTACHMENT AND EGO MECHANISMS OF caretaker, it is likely that the use of the defense of denial was
DEFENSE intensified at that time (Lewis, 1992; Spaccarelli, 1994) and became
The idea that there are nonconscious mechanisms of defense a characteristic style of reacting to stress as the child advanced in
was early proposed by S. Freud (1894) and developed by A. Freud age. In fact, research has shown that children with stressful early
(1936). These ego mechanisms of defense include denial, projection, lives make excessive use of the immature defense of denial when
and identification, among others. In the attachment literature, use of they become adults (Cramer and Block, 1998). In adulthood, this use
these defense mechanisms contributes to deactivating and hyperac- of denial could prevent the individual from recognizing his/her
tivating defense strategies. As described above, the deactivating abusive behavior.
strategy depends on various manifestations of the ego defense of Projection may also play a role in the parent’s abusive
denial. The deactivating strategy may also include the defense of behavior. Parents may project their own negative qualities onto the
projection, which has been described as being especially character- child, who is then perceived as having the disowned negative
istic of the Avoidant (Dismissive and Fearful) attachment styles. In characteristics. In turn, this provides the parent with justification to
defensive projection, unwanted self-traits are projected onto others. use harsh or abusive disciplinary measures (e.g., Bradley and Peters,
The defense of projective identification (Mikulincer and Horesh, 1991; Bugental and Johnston, 2000; McGuigan et al., 2000).
1999), which increases the similarity between the self and other, Further, in carrying out the current abuse, the abusive parent
thus avoiding a sense of separateness, has been considered as part of may have adopted the punitive critical attitude of their own parent
the Anxious (Preoccupied) style, contributing to the hyperactivating (Prodgers, 1984; Finzi et al., 2003) – i.e., they may identify with
strategy. Mikulincer and Horesh define projective identification as a their own abusive parent. “By impersonating the aggressor, assum-
mechanism in which the individual projects actual-self-traits onto ing his attributes or imitating his aggression, the child transforms
others, thus increasing self-other similarity and a sense of closeness. himself from the person threatened into the person who makes the
Although projective identification is a developmentally immature threat” (Freud, 1936, p. 121)—i.e., the child transforms the self
defense, when defined in this way it fits under the larger category of from being powerless to being powerful. Identification also
the defense of identification, as assessed with the Defense Mecha- preserves the attachment with the abusive parent, which is
nism Manual (Cramer, 1991a). Research on the immature ego adaptive in childhood. In adulthood, it is maladaptive because it
defense of splitting—the failure to integrate good and bad images of prevents the individual from accurately perceiving abusive be-
self or of other— has found this defense to be related to both havior (Blizard and Bluhm, 1994).
attachment Anxiety and Avoidance (Lopez, 2001). Thus, theoretically, denial, projection and identification may
Understanding the use of ego defense mechanisms makes it all play an important role in the personality and behavior of the
clear that the insecure individual’s negative perception of others, abusive parent. Further, since early abuse is believed to interfere
which is characteristic of the Dismissive and Fearful attachment with the maturation of ego functions, defense mechanisms, and
style, is derived not only from their own negative attachment impulse control (Blizard and Bluhm, 1994; Arcaya and Gerber,
experience—i.e., their personal history of abuse— but also from the 1990) greater use of the immature defense of denial by these adults
use of the ego defenses of denial, projection, and splitting. Similarly, might be expected.
the idealization and wish for enmeshment that is characteristic of the
Preoccupied attachment style (Main et al., 1985) is linked with the EGO DEFENSES AND ABUSE: RESEARCH
use of (projective) identification and splitting. However, apart from Only a small amount of research has studied the relation
the studies cited above, there is little research evidence available between defense use and abuse. We look first at defense use by
the total score. For example, a Relative Denial score was com-
TABLE 2. Defense Mechanism Manual Scoring Categories: puted according to the formula: Raw Denial score/Total raw
Denial, Projection, Identificationa defense score (Raw Denial ⫹ Raw Projection ⫹ Raw Identifica-
Denial tion). These Relative Defense scores were used throughout in the
Omission of major characters or objects analyses that follow.
Misperception The DMM measure of defense mechanisms has been demon-
Reversal strated in previous studies to have adequate inter-rater reliability,
with children, adolescents and adults (e.g., Cramer, 1991a, 2006;
Statements of negation
Cramer and Kelly, 2004; Hibbard et al., 1994; Porcerelli et al., 1998;
Denial of reality
Sandstrom and Cramer, 2003). The construct validity of the DMM
Overly maximizing the positive or minimizing the negative has been demonstrated with experimental studies designed to test
Unexpected goodness, optimism, positiveness, or gentleness specific hypotheses derived from defense mechanism theory. For
Projection example, based on the theoretical tenet that the use of defense
Attribution of hostile feelings or intentions, or other normatively unusual mechanisms should increase under conditions of stress, both chil-
feelings or intentions, to a character dren and college students have been found to show an increase in
Additions of ominous people, animals, objects, or qualities age-appropriate defense use following stress-inducing experimental
Magical or autistic thinking interventions, such as presumed failure (Cramer and Gaul, 1988),
Concern for protection from external threat rejection by a playmate (Sandstrom and Cramer, 2003), criticism of
Apprehensiveness of death, injury or assault creative ability (Cramer, 1991b) and challenge to sex-role orienta-
Themes of pursuit, entrapment and escape tion (Cramer, 1998).
Bizarre story or theme Personal History of Abuse
Identification Information regarding the participant’s own experience of
Emulation of skills abuse was obtained from clinical interviews with the participants,
Emulation of characteristics, qualities, or attitudes carried out by a licensed psychologist. This self-reported Personal
Regulation of motives or behavior History of Abuse was coded as No Abuse, Domestic Violence,
Self-esteem through affiliation Neglect, and Physical or Sexual Abuse. Where more than one type
Work; delay of gratification of abuse was reported, the more serious form was coded.
Role differentiation
Moralism RESULTS
a
A more complete version of the coding system appears in Cramer (1991a); and Attachment Styles
available at: williams.edu/Psychology/Faculty/Cramer/cramer.html.
Of the 86 participants, 41% were rated as Secure, 14% as
Preoccupied, 21% as Dismissive, and 24% as Fearful. As compared
with other, nonabusive samples of college students (Bartholomew
not matter much which quantitative measure is used” (Brennan and and Horowitz, 1991; Brennan and Shaver, 1995, 1998; Table 3), the
Shaver, 1998, p. 839). present sample has more Dismissive and Fearful individuals, and
fewer rated as Secure.
Defense Mechanisms
All participants were seen individually. Each was given the Defense Mechanisms
standard instructions for telling stories to 6 TAT cards (Murray, 1943). The Defense scores are given in Table 4. A Defense (3) ⫻
These included Cards 1 (boy with a violin); 2 (country scene with Sex (2) repeated measures ANOVA indicated a significant effect
young woman in foreground, man and pregnant woman in back); 3 GF for Defense, F (2,168) ⫽ 32.88, p ⬍ 0.001, eta2 ⫽ 0.28. The
(boy huddled on floor, gun alongside); 4 (woman clutching shoulders of interaction with Sex was not significant. F (2,168) ⫽ 0.66. The
man, who is turned away); 12 M (man lying on couch, older man significant Defense effect was due to the high use of Denial and
leaning over him); and 13 MF (woman lying on bed, man standing with Projection, as compared with the low use of Identification, ts (85) ⫽
arm across face). Stories were recorded and subsequently coded, using 8.10 and 7.38, ps ⬍ 0.001, ds ⫽ 1.49 and 1.64. The high use of Denial
the Defense Mechanism Manual (DMM: Cramer, 1991a). (42%) is especially striking for a sample of adults as compared with
Each of the 516 stories told was scored by a highly experi- other adult samples (e.g., Cramer and Block, 1998, age 23, 27%;
enced coder who has demonstrated high inter-rater reliability for 15 Cramer and Tracy, 2005, age 30, 26%; Cramer, 2008, age 38, 21%)
different previous samples. The rater had no knowledge of the (Table 3).
story-teller’s Attachment category, personal history of abuse, or sex.
The stories were scored for the presence of 3 defense mechanisms— Personal History of Abuse
denial, projection, and identification—according to the DMM. For Thirty-six persons (42%) reported no Personal History of abuse.
each defense, there are 7 categories representing different aspects of Fifty persons (58%) reported that they had experienced some maltreat-
the defense; each category is scored as many times as it occurs in ment as children. This included 9 cases (10%) of Domestic violence, 5
each story. (Table 2). The scores for each category are then summed, cases (6%) of Neglect, and 36 cases (42%) of Physical or Sexual abuse
yielding a total score for denial, for projection, and for identification. (Table 5). Because of the small number of cases in some subgroups, this
In turn, these 3 defense scores may be added together to provide a factor is coded Some/None in subsequent analyses.
Total Defense score. A recent large scale study of adults in the general population
To control for differences in story length, which might influ- found an incidence of 4.9% of documented abuse (including neglect,
ence the possibility of receiving a defense score, raw defense scores physical and sexual maltreatment as a child), and of 9.1% when
were transformed into proportional, or relative defense scores. For relying on self-reported abuse (Johnson et al., 1999). As compared
each person, the sum of all 3 defenses was computed to obtain a with the general population, the incidence of personal abuse in the
total defense score. Then, each individual defense was divided by present sample (58%) is noticeably higher.
TABLE 3. Comparison of Present Sample With Previous Nonclinical Samples: Attachment and Defense Mechanisms
Brennan and Shaver Bartholomew and Horowitz Brennan and Shaver
Cramer/Kelly N ⴝ 86 (1998) N ⴝ 1407 (1991) N ⴝ 77 (College (1995) N ⴝ 242
Attachment (Abusive Parents) (College Undergraduates) Undergraduates) (Standardization Sample)
Secure 41% 48% 47% 53%–58%
Preoccupied 14% 15% 14% 21%–22% (Anxious)
Dismissive 21% 16% 18% 21%–26% (Avoidant)
Fearful 24% 21% 21%
Defense Mechanisms (Relative Cramer/Kelly N ⴝ 86 Cramer and Block (1998) Cramer and Tracy (2005) Cramer (2008) N ⴝ 84
Scores) (DMM) Mean Age ⴝ 31 yr N ⴝ 91 Age ⴝ 23 yr N ⴝ 62 Mean Age ⴝ 30 yr Mean Age ⴝ 38
Denial 0.42 0.27 0.26 0.21
Projection 0.38 0.50 0.49 0.51
Identification 0.20 0.23 0.25 0.28
0.5
0.4
0.35
FIGURE 1. Attachment style and denial. Secure Preoccupied Dismissive Fearful
0.24
Mean Relative Identification Score
0.22
0.2
0.18
0.16
Denial and Projection were used more often than Identification. The experienced Domestic Violence, t (43) ⫽ 1.93, ps ⬍ 0.06, d ⫽ 0.72,
Defense ⫻ Abuse History interaction was not significant (Table 4). both with large effect sizes. In addition, persons who reported Physical/
Sexual Abuse used less Denial than those who reported being Ne-
Further Examination of Personal History of Abuse,
glected, t (39) ⫽ ⫺ 2.60, p ⬍ 0.01, d ⫽ 1.24.
as Related to Attachment Style and Defense Use
Next, a chi square for Personal Abuse History (4) ⫻ Attach-
Although the number of individuals in the Personal Abuse
ment Style (4) was significant, chi square (9) ⫽ 18.53, p ⬍ 0.03. In
History subgroups was small, 2 exploratory analyses were carried
addition to the relation between No Abuse History and Secure
out to examine a possible relation of type of Personal Abuse History
(4) to Defense use (3) and to Attachment Style (4). Because of the attachment mentioned above, a history of physical/sexual abuse was
small N, these results should be considered only suggestive and associated with the Preoccupied attachment style (10/12 ⫽ 83%), as
should be followed up with larger samples. compared with the Dismissive (9/18 ⫽ 50%), the Fearful (9/21 ⫽
First, an ANOVA for Personal Abuse History (4) ⫻ Defense use 43%), or the Secure style (8/35 ⫽ 23%) (Table 5).
(3) was conducted. In addition to the significant effect for Defense, Taken together, these 2 analyses suggest that the Preoccupied
F (2,164) ⫽ 27.86, p ⬍ 0.001, eta2 ⫽ 0.10, there was a significant Attachment Style is associated with the use of Identification as a
Defense x Abuse History linear effect, F (3,82) ⫽ 2.97, p ⬍ 0.04, defense and with a Personal History of Physical/Sexual Abuse. As
eta2 ⫽ 0.10 (Table 4.) Those who reported Physical/Sexual Abuse used will be discussed later, this combination of Identification and a
more Identification than the Neglecteds, t (11.55) (T-test with unequal history of Physical/Sexual Abuse may be seen as contributing to the
variances) ⫽ 3.13, p ⬍ 0.009, d ⫽ 0.85, and more than those who had current abusive behavior of the parent.
0.44
0.42
Mean Relative Projection Score
0.4
0.38
0.36
0.34
0.32
Relation Between Attachment Style and Failure to Dismissive attachment styles both orient away from processing
Encode emotional information—i.e., are both deactivating. The present
Previous research using memory tests has found that deacti- findings suggest that Dismissives may use a somewhat different
vating Dismissing and Fearful (Avoidant) individuals make greater controlling strategy (Cooper et al., 2009; Maier et al., 2005;
use of Preemptive defenses than do hyperactivating Preoccupieds Niedenthal et al., 2002), for Dismissives used denial less often than
(Fraley et al., 2000). Preemptive defenses are characterized by a the Fearfuls, and showed a tendency to use projection more often.
“failure to encode” stimuli, which is conceptually consistent with the The Preoccupied (Anxious) style was differentiated from the
DMM Denial Category 1 (Omission: see Table 2), in that Omission Dismissive and Fearful (Avoidant) groups in 2 important ways.
represents a failure to acknowledge the presence of a stimulus item. First, the Preoccupieds were more likely to report a personal history
To test for Attachment Style differences in “failure to encode,” of having been physically or sexually abused. In addition, in contrast
DMM Omission scores (Denial Category 1) for the 4 Attachment to the deactivating operations of the Fearful group, the Preoccupied
Style groups were compared by a univariate ANOVA. The results individuals were more likely to use the hyperactivating defense
indicated a significant Attachment style effect, F(3,82) ⫽ 3.15, p ⬍ mechanism of Identification.
0.03, eta2 ⫽ 0.10. A comparison of Attachment groups by t test This combination of personal abuse history and identification
indicated that the Fearfuls had higher Omission scores than the other suggests that different factors may contribute to the abusive behavior of
3 groups, ts(54, 31, 37) ⫽ 2.16, 2.54, and 2.23, ps ⫽ 0.03– 0.01, Preoccupieds, as compared with Dismissives and Fearfuls. The ego
ds ⫽ 0.60 – 0.94, which did not differ. defense of Identification contributes to the hyperactivating strategy by
allowing the individual to stay close (i.e., attached), psychologically, to
DISCUSSION the individual’s own parent. At the same time, this identification with an
abusive parent likely contributes, at least in part, to the current abusive
As compared with the general population, the abusive parents
behavior—i.e., identification with the aggressor. If the defense of
in the present sample are more likely to have experienced personal
Identification is combined with Projection, the likelihood of engaging in
abuse as a child, and are more likely to report themselves as having
abusive behavior increases. Just as their own parent treated them as
an insecure attachment style. Further, a most striking finding with
‘bad’ and deserving of punishment, the current Projection of their
these abusive parents is their very high use of denial, as compared
‘badness’ onto their child results in their perception of the child as bad,
with other, nonabusive samples. The use of denial was especially
and because of Identification with their own parent, their child is seen
true of those individuals with a Fearful attachment style. Notably,
as “deserving” of punishment.
Fearful (Avoidant) parents were also found to have higher scores for
Those individuals who reported a Secure attachment style
Omission of (failure to encode) prominent aspects of the stimulus
were less likely to report a history of abuse than the other attachment
picture than any of the other Attachment groups. Projection, previ-
groups. However, like the other attachment groups, they made
ously described as part of the deactivation strategy (Mikulincer and
strong use of Denial. As with the other attachment groups, the use of
Horesh, 1999) was also frequently used by the group of abusive
denial protects the Secure abusive parents from recognizing the
parents. However, it is denial, and its deactivating function, that
harmful effects of their abusive behavior.
most clearly characterizes these parents. Such strong use of denial
likely prevents these parents from recognizing the harm caused by
their abusive behavior. Limitations
These findings are consistent with, and support the position In interpreting these findings, it is important to keep in mind
that the Avoidant attachment style is characterized by preemptive, that both the Attachment measure and the information on Personal
deactivating defensive operations (Fraley and Shaver, 1997; Shaver History of Abuse are self-report measures. As has been noted in a
and Mikulincer, 2008), but they indicate that the Fearful component previous study of abusive parents, the fact that they were seen for
of the Avoidant style use this strategy more than the Dismissives. court assessment likely motivated them to present themselves in the
This latter finding is relevant for the question of whether Fearful and best possible light (Brennan et al., 1990). This likelihood, plus their
strong use of Denial, raises the question of a self-presentation bias, Ammaniti M, Nicolais G, Speranza AM (2004) Trauma and attachment: An
which may have influenced their self-reporting – for example, investigation in abusive parenting. Arch Psychiatry Psychother. 6:9 –22.
reporting a Secure attachment style. However, to the extent that Andrews G, Singh J, Bond M (1993) The defense style questionnaire. J Nerv Ment
Dis. 181:246 –256.
there was a bias in the self-report of attachment style, this would
likely weaken the associations found for the several insecure attach- Arcaya JM, Gerber GL (1990) An object relations approach to the treatment of
child abuse. Psychotherapy. 27:619 – 626.
ment styles with defense use and personal history of abuse.
Azar ST, Robinson DR, Hekimian E, Twentyman CT (1984) Unrealistic expec-
Also, it is conceivable that parents might have biased their tations and problem-solving ability in maltreating and comparison mothers.
self-report of personal history of abuse, although it is not clear J Consult Clin Psychol. 52:687– 691.
whether they would think it advantageous to report no abuse or some Bartholomew K, Horowitz L (1991) Attachment styles among young adults:
abuse. It is noteworthy that the self-reporting in the present sample A test of a four-category model. J Pers Soc Psychol. 61:226 –244.
of having experienced personal abuse (58%) is considerably higher Blizard RA, Bluhm AM (1994) Attachment to the abuser: Integrating object-
than that found in the general population (9%). In any case, if the relations and trauma theories in treatment of abuse survivors. Psychotherapy.
parents misreported no abuse when in fact some abuse had occurred, 31:383–390.
this bias would also likely weaken the present results that demon- Bowlby J (1988). A Secure Base: Clinical Applications of Attachment Theory.
London (United Kingdom): Routledge.
strate the relation of a history of personal abuse with current
Brennan J, Andrews G, Morris-Yates A, Pollock C (1990) An examination of
attachment style and defense use. defense style in parents who abuse children. J Nerv Ment Dis. 178:592–595.
The present study also examines the role of only 3 defense Brennan KA, Shaver PR (1995) Dimensions of adult attachment, affect regulation
mechanisms. It is possible that other defenses are related to attach- and romantic relationship. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 21, 267–283.
ment style, personal history of abuse, and abusive behavior. How- Brennan KA, Shaver PR (1998) Attachment styles and personality disorders:
ever, the present findings present a significant, cohesive pattern Their connections to each other and to parental divorce, parental death, and
showing the interrelation of these factors with the use of the 3 perceptions of parental caregiving. J Pers. 66:835– 878.
defenses. Brennan J, Shaver PR, Tobey AE (1991) Attachment styles, gender and parental
Another consideration is that the sample size of the present problem drinking. J Soc Pers Relat. 8:451– 466.
study may have limited the power to detect some interconnections. Bradley EJ, Peters R (1991) Physically abusive and nonabusive mothers’ percep-
tions of parenting and child behavior. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 61:455– 460.
Because of the relatively small number of individuals in several
subgroups, the results should be considered preliminary and in need Bugental D, Johnston C (2000) Parental and child cognitions in the context of the
family. Ann Rev Psychol. 51:315–344.
of follow up with additional studies. However, this is the only
Briere J, Runtz M (1993) Childhood sexual abuse. Long-term sequelae and
empirical study of abusive parents of which we are aware that implications for psychological assessment. J Interpers Violence. 8:312–330.
examines the inter-relations of personal abuse history, defense use Callahan KL, Hilsenroth MJ (2005) Childhood sexual abuse and adult defensive
and attachment style, and offers a theoretical rationale for these functioning. J Nerv Ment Dis. 193:473– 479.
relations. Cooper RM, Rowe AC, Penton-Voak IS, Ludwig C (2009) No reliable effects of
Finally, in our thinking about the present findings, we are emotional facial expression, adult attachment orientation, or anxiety on the
assuming that many of these abusive adults were insecurely attached allocation visual attention in the spatial cueing paradign. J Res Pers. 43:643–
as children, and that they made strong use of Denial at that time. The 652.
present study does not provide us with information regarding either Cramer P (1991a) The Development of Defense Mechanisms: Theory, Research
and Assessment. New York (NY): Springer-Verlag.
the attachment style or defense use of these individuals when they
Cramer P (1991b) Anger and the use of defense mechanisms in college students.
were children. Indeed, there is very little research information J Pers. 59:39 –55.
available regarding the relation between defense use and attachment Cramer P (1998) Threat to gender representation: Identity and identification.
in children. In the only study of which we are aware, four-year-olds J Pers. 66:335–357.
who were insecurely attached were found to use more denial in Cramer P (1999) Personality, personality disorders and defense mechanisms.
negative situations involving their mothers, as compared with those J Pers. 67:535–554.
who were securely attached (Lay et al., 1995). Nevertheless, lacking Cramer P (2006) Protecting the Self: Defense Mechanisms in Action. New York
this information, the present findings show clearly that abusive (NY): Guilford Press.
adults make unusually strong use of the defense of Denial. Cramer P (2008) Identification and the development of competence: A 44-year
longitudinal study from late adolescence to late middle age. Psychology and
Aging. 23:410 – 421.
CONCLUSIONS Cramer P, Block J (1998) Preschool antecedents of defense mechanism use in
Parents who are abusive are more likely to report having expe- young adults. J Pers Soc Psychol. 74:159 –169.
rienced abuse when they were a child, are more likely to report having Cramer P, Gaul R (1988) The effects of success and failure on children’s use of
an insecure attachment style, as assessed by the Relationship Question- defense mechanisms. J Pers. 56:729 –742.
naire (Bartholomew and Horowitz, 1991), and are more likely to use the Cramer P, Kelly FD (2004) Defense mechanisms in adolescent conduct disorder
and adjustment reaction. J Nerv Ment Dis. 192:139 –145.
defense of Denial, as assessed with the Defense Mechanism Manual
Cramer P, Tracy A (2005) The pathway from child personality to adult adjust-
(Cramer, 1991a), as compared with the general population. The findings ment: The road is not straight. J Res Pers. 39:369 –394.
suggest that, within this abusive sample, different defenses may be Finzi R, Har-Even D, Shnit D, Weizman A (2002) Psychosocial characterization
associated with different attachment styles. Denial was especially char- of physically abused children from low socioeconomic households in compar-
acteristic of the Fearful style, Projection tended to be associated with the ison to neglected and nonmaltreated children. J Child Fam Stud. 11:441– 453.
Dismissive style, and Identification was more likely to be associated Finzi R, Har-Even D, Weizman A (2003) Comparison of ego defenses among
with the Preoccupied style. physically abused children, neglected and non-maltreated children. Compr
Psychiatry. 44:388 –395.
Finzi-Dottan R, Karu T (2006) From emotional abuse in childhood to psychopa-
REFERENCES thology in adulthood. J Nerv Ment Dis. 194:616 – 621.
American Psychiatric Association (2000) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Fraley RC, Brumbaugh CC (2007) Adult attachment and preemptive defenses:
Mental Disorders (4th ed). Washington (DC): American Psychiatric Associa- Converging evidence on the role of defensive exclusion at the level of
tion. encoding. J Pers. 75:1033–1050.
American Psychological Association (2008). Monitor. Washington (DC): Amer- Fraley RC, Davis KE, Shaver PR (1998) Dismissing-Avoidance and the defensive
ican Psychiatric Association. organization of emotion, cognition and behavior. In JA Simpson, WS Rholes
(Eds), Attachment Theory and Close Relationships (pp 249 –279). New York Main M, Kaplan N, Cassidy J (1985) Security in infancy, childhood and adult-
(NY): Guilford Press. hood: A move to the level of representation. In I Bretherton, E Waters (Eds),
Fraley RC, Garner JP, Shaver PR (2000) Adult attachment and the defensive Growing Points in Attachment Theory and Research. Monographs of the
regulation of attention and memory: Examining the role of preemptive and Society for Research in Child Development 50 (1–2, Serial No. 209, pp
postemptive defensive processes. J Pers Soc Psychol. 79:816 – 826. 66 –106).
Fraley RC, Shaver PR (1997) Adult attachment and the suppression of unwanted Malinowsky-Rummel R, Hansen DJ (1993) Long-term consequences of child-
thoughts. J Pers Soc Psychol. 73:1080 –1091. hood physical abuse. Psychol Bull. 114:68 –79.
Freud A (1936) The Ego and the Mechanisms of Defense. New York (NY): McGuigan WM, Vuchinich S, Pratt C (2000). Domestic violence, parents’ views
International Universities Press. of their infant and risk for child abuse. J Fam Psychol. 20:613– 624.
Freud S (1894) The Neuro-Psychoses of Defence (Std ed, Vol 3, pp 45– 61). Mikulincer M, Shaver PR, Cassidy J, Berant E (2008) Attachment-related defen-
London (United Kingdom): Hogarth Press. sive processes. In JH Obegi, E Berant (Eds), Attachment Theory and Research
Goodman GS, Emery RE, Haugaard JJ (1998) Developmental psychology and the in Clinical Work With Adults (pp 293–327). New York (NY): Guilford Press.
law: Divorce, child maltreatment, foster care and adoption. In IE Sigel, KA Mikulincer M, Horesh N (1999) Adult attachment style and the perception of
Renninger (Eds), Handbook of Child Psychology, Vol 4: Child Psychology in others: The role of projective mechanisms. J Pers Soc Psychol. 76:1022–1034.
Practice (5th ed, pp 775– 876). New York (NY): Wiley.
Murray HA (1943) Thematic Apperception Test manual. Cambridge (MA):
Griffin DW, Bartholomew K (1994) Models of the self and other: Fundamental Harvard University Press.
dimensions underlying measures of adult attachment. J Pers Soc Psychol.
67:430 – 445. Niedenthal PM, Brauer M, Robin L, Innes-Ker AH (2002) Adult attachment and
the perception of facial expression of emotion. J Pers Soc Psychol. 82:419 –
Hibbard S, Farmer L, Wells C, Difillipo E, Barry W, Korman R, Sloan P (1994)
433.
Validation of Cramer’s defense mechanisms manual for the TAT. J Pers
Assess. 63:197–210. Porcerelli JH, Thomas S, Hibbard S, Cogan R (1998) Defense mechanism
Johnson JG, Cohen P, Brown J, Smailes EM, Bernstein DP (1999) Childhood development in children, adolescents and late adolescents. J Pers Assess.
maltreatment increases risk for personality disorders during early adulthood. 71:411– 420.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 56:600 – 606. Porcerelli JH Cogan R, Kamoo R, Leitman S (2004) Defense mechanisms and
Kaufman J, Zigler E (1989) The intergenerational transmission of child abuse. In self-reported violence toward partners and strangers. J Pers Assess. 82:317–
D Cicchetti, V Carlson (Eds), Child Maltreatment: Theory and Research on the 320.
Causes and Consequences of Child Abuse and Neglect (pp 129 –150). New Prodgers A (1984) Psychopathology of the physically abusing parent: A compar-
York (NY): Cambridge University Press. ison with the borderline syndrome. Child Abuse Neglect. 8:411– 424.
Lay KL, Waters E, Posada G, Ridgeway D (1995) Attachment security, affect Romans SE, Martin JL, Morris E, Herbison GP (1999) Psychological defense
regulation and defensive responses to mood induction. In E Waters, B Vaughn, styles in women who report childhood sexual abuse: A controlled community
G Posada, K Kondo-Ikemura (Eds), Culture, Caregiving and Cognition: Per- study. Am J Psychiatry. 156:1080 –1085.
spectives on Secure Base Phenomena and Attachment Working Models. Mono-
graphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60, (Serial No. 244, Sandstrom M, Cramer P (2003) Girls’ use of defense mechanisms following peer
2–3), (pp 179 –198). rejection. J Pers. 71:605– 627.
Lewis DO (1992) From abuse to violence: Psychophysiological consequences of Shaver PR, Mikulincer M (2002) Attachment-related psychodynamics. Attach-
maltreatment. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 31:383–391. Hum Dev. 4:133–161.
Lopez FG (2001) Adult attachment orientations, self-other boundary regulation Shaver PR, Mikulincer M (2008) Adult attachment and cognitive and affective
and splitting tendencies in a college sample. J Couns Psychol. 48:440 – 446. reactions to positive and negative events. Soc Pers Psychol Compass. 2:1844 –
Maier MA, Bernier A, Perkun R, Zimmermann P, Strasser K, Grossmann KE 1865.
(2005) Attachment state of mind and perceptual processing of emotional Spaccarelli S (1994). Stress, appraisal and coping in child sexual abuse:
stimuli. Attach Hum Dev. 7:285–295. A theoretical and empirical review. Psychol Bull. 116:340 –362.