How Students Think When Doing Algebra
How Students Think When Doing Algebra
Harrington
Algebra is the gateway to college and careers, yet it functions as the eye of the Starr
needle because of low pass rates for the middle school/high school course and
students’ struggles to understand.We have forty years of research that discuss-
es the ways students think and their cognitive challenges as they engage with
algebra. This book is a response to the National Council of Teachers of Mathe-
matics’ (NCTM) call to better link research and practice by capturing what we
have learned about students’ algebraic thinking in a way that is usable by teach-
ers as they prepare lessons or reflect on their experiences in the classroom.
How
lems) are closely tied to CCSS topics.
Students
Over time, veteran math teachers develop extensive knowledge of how
students engage with algebraic concepts—their misconceptions, ways of think-
ing, and when and how they are challenged to understand—and use that
knowledge to anticipate students’ struggles with particular lessons and plan
THINK
accordingly. Veteran teachers learn to evaluate whether an incorrect response
is a simple error or the symptom of a faulty or naïve understanding of a
concept. Novice teachers, on the other hand, lack the experience to anticipate
important moments in the learning of their students. They often struggle to
make sense of what students say in the classroom and determine whether the
response is useful or can further discussion. The purpose of this book is to
Algebra
accelerate early career teachers’ “experience” with how students think when
doing algebra in middle or high school as well as to supplement veteran teach-
ers’ knowledge of content and students. The research that this book is based
upon can provide teachers with insight into the nature of a student’s struggles
When
with particular algebraic ideas—to help teachers identify patterns that imply
underlying thinking. Doing
Our book, How Students Think When Doing Algebra, is not intended to be a “how
to” book for teachers. Instead, it is intended to orient new teachers to the
ways students think and be a book that teachers at all points in their career Steve Rhine
continually pull of the shelf when they wonder: “How might my students strug-
gle with this algebraic concept I am about to teach?” The primary audience for
Rachel Harrington
this book is early career mathematics teachers who don’t have extensive expe- Colin Starr
rience working with students engaged in mathematics. However, the book can
also be useful to veteran teachers to supplement their knowledge and is an
ideal resource for mathematics educators who are preparing preservice teach-
ers.
By
Steve Rhine
Rachel Harrington
Colin Starr
The CIP data for this book can be found on the LIbrary of Congress website (loc.gov).
Paperback: 978-1-64113-411-8
Hardcover: 978-1-64113-412-5
eBook: 978-1-64113-413-2
1. Introduction.................................................................................... 1
3. Algebraic Relations........................................................................ 99
v
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
INTRODUCTION
An error is ...
a symptom of the nature of the conceptions
which underlie (a student’s) mathematical activity.
— Balachef, 1984
The Cognitively Guided Instruction project (CGI) found that in-service teachers
who learned about common conceptions and sources of those conceptions could
predict students’ struggles. The teachers made fundamental changes in their
4 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
ALGEBRA
going to see an increase in [failure] rates [in algebra] if you raise the instructional
requirements and you don’t raise supports.”25
Many school districts are responding to the algebra crisis by creating two- or
three-year algebra programs or having students take double periods of algebra,
but the student success rate is not changing significantly.26 It is becoming apparent
that the amount of time spent studying algebra is not the issue. There is some evi-
dence that new curricula from projects like the Connected Mathematics Program,
College Preparatory Mathematics Program, and West Ed. are improving students’
success with algebra,27 but many students still struggle to understand and there is
some controversy over reform math’s effects.28
Research has long documented students’ mental hurdles in making the transi-
tion from arithmetic to algebra.29 Over 900 articles spanning the last three decades
examine why students struggle in algebra. Teachers need a resource that will eas-
ily allow them to understand those common errors and conceptual misunderstand-
ings in algebra, and the paths that students take in coming to understand algebra.
Publishing companies are beginning to incorporate some research on students’
thinking into teacher editions of textbooks. These often take the form of one or
two sentence sidebars that point out a typical struggle that students might have.
Although these are helpful, they are not comprehensive.
This book synthesizes those 900 articles into what is most valuable for teach-
ers to understand about how students think when doing algebra. While we break
down students’ algebraic thinking into the individual elements explored by re-
searchers, it is important to see each exploration as a window into a larger system.
The creation of categories and chapters in this book is a matter of convenience in
the two-dimensional realm of print that does not do justice to the three- or four-
dimensional reality of a student’s brain. Ideas about variables are woven through-
out equations, functions throughout graphing. They are not distinct. However, by
looking at the vast complexities of the development of algebraic thinking through
a particular lens, teachers may be able to develop greater insight into students’
mental systems overall.
be made that the study of algebra not only ‘trains the mind’ but is necessary for
everyone desirous of participating in our democracy … we can and should make
algebra part of the curriculum for all students.”34
Perhaps the most plausible perspective on many students’ failure in algebra
is one that does not blame teachers’ skills or students’ intellectual potential. In-
stead, Herscovics suggests we consider the notion of cognitive obstacles in which
students encounter ideas in algebra that require them to restructure the way that
they think about a concept in order to move forward in their thinking. Cognitive
obstacles are best understood in terms of Piagetian theory.
Piaget believed that people develop mental structures—or ways of thinking
about something—called schema. There are two ways that people acquire knowl-
edge: assimilation and accommodation. First, a child may have an idea about
something based on experience, such as “I have a dog” (Figure 1.1). When she is
faced with new information that is similar to what she knows, she acquires new
knowledge by assimilation: fitting the new information into a schema she has
already developed. “That Great Dane is very different from my dog, but I can put
it into my thinking about dogs. There are different kinds of dogs” (Figure 1.2).
When she encounters information that does not fit into her existing schema she ex-
periences disequilibrium and her brain strives to make sense and come back into
balance. She must accommodate her brain structure by creating a new schema.
She restructures old ways of thinking in order to integrate the new information.
“That pony is not much bigger than the Great Dane, but it is very different, so I
need a new category called horses and a bigger category to hold them all called
animals” (Figure 1.3).
Teachers need to decide whether their instructional goal for students is as-
similation (“This is just like…”) or accommodation (“I need to think about this
in a new way”). Accommodation is the restructuring that is often necessary when
students experience cognitive obstacles in algebra. When they face ideas that, in
order to be fully understood, must be thought about in a radically different way
than they have thought about them before, they cannot simply add them to their
memories. Learning, therefore, “sometimes requires the significant reorganiza-
tion of existing knowledge structures and not just their enrichment.”35 As students
move from the concrete world of arithmetic to the abstract world of algebra they
often encounter cognitive obstacles that compel them not to simply assimilate but
to accommodate the new ideas by restructuring how they think about mathemat-
ics. It is the teacher’s challenge to figure out ways to facilitate that process.
Another reason algebra may be difficult to understand is based on our struggle
to define algebra. Some argue that the traditional emphasis on understanding the
structure and manipulation of expressions, and then applying those skills to word
problems, is the most useful. From this perspective, algebraic thinking is the abil-
ity to transform and represent or recognize forms.36 This equation-based approach
emphasizes the use of symbols to model and explore relationships. Others con-
tend that algebra classes should emphasize the concepts of functions and families
of functions. A function-based approach examines relationships between vary-
ing quantities, representing those relationships, and using those representations
to analyze, generalize, and make predictions. Students should engage with real-
world situations and understand relationships that can be described with sym-
bols and models.37 This latter perspective significantly influenced the Principles
and Standards for School Mathematics38 and is somewhat driven by advances in
technology, such as graphing calculators and computers that can do algebraic ma-
nipulation. There are also those who try to find a middle ground between the two
perspectives—that techniques and conceptual understanding in algebra should be
complementary components.39 There is evidence that students’ conceptions relate
to the type of instruction and curriculum chosen.40
We can certainly view algebra from all of these perspectives, but math educa-
tors have “found it difficult to connect function- and equation-based views in
our instruction … when both views exist simultaneously in an algebra course,
they are not well-integrated.”41 Middle school curricula often approach algebra
through a function-based perspective while high schools typically focus on the
equation-based approach. Accordingly, students struggle to reconcile these two
perspectives themselves. Teachers can help students make sense of algebra by
keeping both of these perspectives in mind and integrating multiple representa-
tions and approaches where possible.
Regardless of approach, as students move from the concrete world of arithme-
tic to the abstract world of algebra they must use prior knowledge as a foundation
for new ways of thinking. Many arithmetical rules are extended into algebra; for
instance, commutativity with numbers is the same with variables. However, stu-
dents sometimes experience mathematics in elementary school that conflicts with
ways of thinking in algebra, such as the functioning of the equal sign as a left to
right process rather than a balance of equal sides (see Chapter 3: Algebraic Rela-
tions for a more detailed discussion). Educators are currently identifying ways to
transform elementary-school mathematics teaching to better establish the founda-
tions of algebraic thinking for study in later grades.42
Introduction • 9
FIGURE 1.4 Math Grades and Mindsets. Adapted from Blackwell, Trzesniewski, &
Dweck, 2007, p. 51.
Piaget found that children often had logical reasons for their wrong answers: they
based their reasons on some truth they extrapolated incorrectly. They also had
tendencies, or similar ways of thinking about a concept. Math education research-
ers have since found that these tendencies are often influenced by the structure of
problems; more specifically, as students try to understand algebra, the nature of
the mathematics may lead students to particular incorrect—as well as correct—
ways of thinking. For example, students in arithmetic notice that, for the numbers
they are working with, multiplication makes quantities bigger faster than addition.
While we know that this is not true for all numbers, students can think multiplica-
tion makes numbers bigger faster always. This can impact their understanding of
an algebraic problem such as “Which is bigger, 2n or n+2?”
Balachef’s quote at the beginning sums up the essence of this book: errors
made by a student are often the tip of the iceberg of his or her conceptions of the
mathematics beneath. Errors can result from simple carelessness, such as 5 + 3 =
9, or from forgotten rules. The student can quickly identify the mistake in such
cases. Researchers have also found that “some kinds of errors are widespread
among students of different ages, independent of the course of their previous
learning of algebra” and that “a significant number of students did not use the for-
mal methods taught in schools, preferring some intuitive strategies.”52 Similar to
Piaget’s findings, some students make the same type of errors regardless of their
experience with algebra, indicating common struggles that have more complex
sources than simple calculation or memory errors.
Introduction • 11
Incorrect answers are rarely due to guessing, low intelligence, or low mathematical
aptitude. They result from systematic strategies or rules which usually have sensible
origins and are based on beliefs or misconceptions—they are usually distortions or
misinterpretations of sound procedures. (Perso, 1992)
to right. Using these rules may help students be successful on one type of prob-
lem, which is typically the more simple form. However, when the problem has a
variation or becomes more complex, their method often no longer works because
they lack the underlying understanding necessary for success. Quasi-rules, on the
other hand, are not consistently applied, are often formulated in the moment, and
can change haphazardly. For instance, a student may believe that the statement
“one less than y ” can be interpreted as x rather than “y - 1” because x is one let-
ter before y in the alphabet.72 It is an arbitrary rule applied in a specific context.
Misconceptions are generally faulty sets of rules or inappropriate use of rules
that guide students’ choices. Research indicates that students who are following
rules can learn more accurate rules and be successful. Students who are following
quasi-rules often have a much more difficult time understanding the mathematics
behind the procedures and need more significant intervention by teachers.
Misconceptions that exist in students’ minds prior to instruction can be differ-
ent from students’ misinterpretations during the teaching and learning of algebra.
For instance, as students enter algebra classrooms, researchers have shown that
students bring with them a “natural” tendency to believe that a letter has only one
value.73 The contexts students encounter in algebra require them to restructure and
accommodate their conceptions of variables to include the possibility of letters
representing multiple quantities or ranges of numbers.74 Based on their earlier ex-
periences, students may interpret letters in a variety of unexpected ways that can
negatively influence their ability to successfully work with problems in algebra.
As in Calvin’s case at the beginning of this chapter, what may be emphasized in
arithmetic is the final numerical answer and not the process, so errors in thinking
can go unnoticed. Manipulation of algebraic symbols, however, may require stu-
dents to make use of processes that they have avoided when dealing with simple
arithmetic problems. Students’ encounters with algebra, therefore, can bring to
light the misconceptions and confusions that they may already have experienced
in arithmetic, but have gone unnoticed.75
Similar misconceptions appear in spite of differences in age and experience
with algebra and can persist in spite of direct instruction. For instance, miscon-
ceptions about the nature of variables have been shown to continue into college,
in spite of students taking higher-level algebra courses.76 Themes to these mis-
conceptions can be traced to students’ ideas about the focus of algebraic activity
and the nature of “answers,” the use of notation and convention in algebra, the
meaning of letters and variables, the kinds of relationships and methods used in
arithmetic, and other notions.77
Misconceptions that students have in algebra often relate to its abstract na-
ture. Students have a certain degree of reliance on “reality.” Their experience in
mathematics prior to algebra often consists of considering what is empirically
verifiable, what they can manipulate through objects, or what they can easily con-
nect to experience. As students move from thinking about small numbers to large
numbers outside their ability to verify, and then to generalizations, their challenge
14 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
is to develop a grasp of the abstract structure of things. In dealing with this new,
abstract world, when understanding is difficult for students to come by, miscon-
ceptions often fill the gaps.
Of perhaps even more interest than the kind of question that children solve correctly
is the nature of the errors that they make, especially when the same error is made by
large numbers of children. A study of such errors is important because of the infor-
mation it provides concerning the ways in which the child views the problem and
the procedures that are used in attempting to solve the problem. This information
is of interest not only because it might suggest ways of helping children to avoid
these errors, but also because it might explain children’s apparent lack of progress
in attaining higher levels of understanding in Algebra.78
ALGEBRAIC THINKING
Algebraic reasoning in its many forms, and the use of algebraic representations such
as graphs, tables, spreadsheets and traditional formulas, are among the most power-
ful intellectual tools that our civilization has developed. (Yet,) the traditional image
of algebra, based in more than a century of school algebra, is one of simplifying
algebraic expressions, solving equations, learning the rules for manipulating sym-
bols—the algebra that almost everyone, it seems, loves to hate...
...School algebra has traditionally been taught and learned as a set of procedures dis-
connected both from other mathematical knowledge and from students’ real worlds.
(Students) memorize procedures that they know only as operations on strings of
symbols, solve artificial problems that bear no meaning to their lives, and are graded
not on understanding of the mathematical concepts and reasoning involved, but on
Introduction • 15
their ability to produce the right symbol string. Without some form of symbolic
algebra, there could be no higher mathematics and no quantitative science; hence
no technology and modern life as we know them. Our challenge then is to find ways
to make the power of algebra (indeed, all mathematics) available to all students…79
the idea of symbols representing numbers, and the relationship between arithme-
tic and algebra.85
While algebraic thinking develops quite differently—depending on the ap-
proach you use and at what ages you introduce algebraic concepts—there are
some commonalities of purpose. Mathematical power and flexibility in algebra
lie in four themes or activities:
• Generalizing,
• Solving equations and making sense of their solutions,
• Exercising algebraic rules, and
• Building models and learning from them.86
The first source is the meaning students find from using letters to represent un-
knowns, seeing the ideas that are embedded within the symbols,88 and knowing
how the manipulation of algebraic variables and expressions help the student
solve a problem. A key to finding this meaning is comprehending the connec-
tion between the letters or symbols representing an idea and the numbers that
are the foundation of that relationship. Difficulties emerge because of the chal-
lenge that arises in relating algebraic symbols to natural language. A student
may be able to explain relationships between elements of a situation accurately
through language, but be unable to express those same relationships in alge-
braic symbols.89
Introduction • 17
struggle to see the connections and relationships between the symbols and the
situation. Developing an understanding of how symbols can represent real-world
relationships—even when the symbols are not closely aligned with the situation
or with language—is having symbol sense. Students’ mathematical power in al-
gebra depends on how well they ultimately develop that sense.
Structure Sense is an extension of Symbol Sense.97 Structure Sense includes
students’ ability to:
Recognize a familiar structure in its simplest form (as in seeing 81 – x2 as the differ-
ence of squares and being able to factor it accordingly).
Deal with a compound term as a single entity and, through an appropriate sub-
stitution, recognize a familiar structure in a more complex form (as in addressing
(x – 3)4 – (x + 3)4 by seeing (x – 3)2 and (x + 3)2 as single entities and factoring as
the difference of squares).
Choose appropriate manipulations to make best use of a structure (as in seeing the
possibility of the difference of squares in 24x6y4 – 150z8, so choosing to extract the
common factor of 6 in order to get 4x6y4 – 25z8 and deal with 2x3y2 and 5z4 as single
entities, factoring as the difference of two squares).98
and conventions used in algebra are a significant part of what students must learn
to engage in algebraic thinking. Second, algebra is also about a habit of mind: a
way of mathematically thinking when you encounter a problem situation.102 This
might include “recognizing and analyzing patterns, investigating and represent-
ing relationships, generalizing beyond specifics of an example, analyzing how
processes or relationships change, or seeking arguments for how and why rules
and procedures work.”103 More specifically, these habits of mind could be de-
scribed as interactions with functions in three ways:104
Persistent, well retained bodies of knowledge and skill are those which are richly
inter-connected and that fresh ideas are often rejected until they become so strong
that they force a reorganization of the existing material into a new system, holding
together the new idea and the transformed old ones.106
20 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
can help teachers make appropriate and effective instructional decisions to facili-
tate conceptual change.
It is not the purpose of this book to advocate for a particular method of address-
ing misconceptions. Teaching requires “an adequate theory of knowledge, reason-
ing, and learning [that] must include a range of cognitive and affective structures
and processes.”120 However, there are some general principles for achieving cog-
nitive change:
their students’ algebraic thinking. Yet the insights into how students think when
engaged with algebra has tremendous potential value to novice and veteran math-
ematics teachers alike. This book is intended to be a bridge between this research
and teachers’ practice in the classroom. The goal for the book is to “broaden
and support teachers’ awareness, judgment, and inquiry” regarding their students’
algebraic thinking.126 Findings from research have been selected and organized
into a form that is readily usable as a resource for teachers of students preparing
for—and in—classes with algebraic ideas. Based on research into students’ think-
ing over the past three decades, this book provides an easy-to-use library of the
development of algebraic ideas, misconceptions, and ways of thinking that are
challenging for students to understand.
Four questions form the foundation of the core sections of the book, and guide
organization of each section:
Answers for each question are organized under five algebraic categories: Vari-
ables & Expressions, Algebraic Relations (e.g., equations), Analysis of Change
(e.g., graphing), Patterns & Functions, and Modeling & Word Problems. Each cat-
egory has multiple sections that summarize research on students’ algebraic think-
ing with particular types of algebraic problems. The range of problems presented
is dictated by what has been researched. Each section focuses on a particular
theme about students’ algebraic thinking that has been identified by researchers.
Sections include problems that are typical of the algebra curriculum. Each prob-
lem raises various mathematical issues based on the unique ways students think
and have misconceptions about that type of problem—thereby addressing some
core issues in the development of algebraic thinking. Not all research includes
student interviews to identify thinking or suggestions of how to address a stu-
dent’s thinking; therefore, when possible, a reasonable possible conversation is
given. Interviews directly from research are footnoted.
Each of the categories begins with an introductory section in which some over-
all ideas are laid out to help you organize the different ways of thinking about
each of the algebraic concepts with which students struggle. Words in bold italics
indicate a section. The structure of the book unintentionally implies the idea that
these categories are distinct and that each of the problems examined can be iso-
lated from the greater realm of algebraic thinking; however, a student’s thinking
Introduction • 23
is a complex system of ideas that is continuous and not discrete. In fact, the ideas
of variables, equations, functions, graphing, and word problems are thoroughly
woven together; the book’s organization, therefore, revolves around what struck
the authors as the most central theme of a particular idea, with the understand-
ing that other themes are certain to be involved as well. A teacher who honors a
student’s process of developing thinking and retains that sense of an interrelated
web of ideas will help students to feel a sense of growing success, rather than an
eternal battle against errors and flawed thinking.
REFERENCES
Ainley, J. (1999). Doing algebra type stuff: Emergent algebra in the primary school. Paper
presented at the 23rd Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of
Mathematics Education, Haifa, Israel.
ENDNOTES
1 Wallach & Even, 2005
2 Sherin, Russ, Sherin, & Colestock, 2008, and Ho & Tan, 2013, among many others
3 Sherin, Russ, Sherin, & Colestock, 2008, p. 28
4 Stockero & Van Zoest, 2012
5 Davies & Walker, 2005, p. 275
6 Schoenfeld, 2008, p. 57
7 Leatham, Peterson, Stockero, & Van Zoest, 2015
8 Shulman, 1986
9 Leatham, Stockero, Peterson, Van Zoest, 2011, p. 839
10 Peterson & Leatham, 2009; Stockero & Van Zoest, 2012
11 Mason, 1998, p. 247
12 Leatham, Stockero, Peterson, Van Zoest, 2011, pg. 840, 842
13 Smith & Stein, 2011
14 Leatham, Stockero, Peterson, & Van Zoest 2011; Peterson & Leatham, 2009
15 Tirosh, Even, & Robinson, 1998, p. 51, based on Shulman, 1986
16 Carpenter, Fennema, Loef Franke, Levi, & Empson, 2000
17 Ball, Bass, Sleep, & Thames, 2005, p. 3
18 Fennema, et al., 1996
19 NCTM, 2014
20 Liston & Donoghue, 2010
21 Katz, 2006, p. 6
22 Ladson-Billings, 1998; Moses & Cobb, 2001
23 Helfand, 2006, p. 1
24 Murray, 2010
25 Viadero, 2009, ¶8
26 Nomi & Allensworth, 2009
27 e.g. Moseley & Brenner, 2009; Riordan & Noyce, 2001
28 e.g. Milgram, 1999
29 e.g. Booth, 1984; MacGregor & Stacey, 1997; and Moseley & Brenner, 2009
30 Herscovics, 1989
31 Herscovics, 1989, p. 60
32 ibid
24 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
33 NCTM, 2000, p. 5
34 Katz, 2006, p. 6
35 Christou, Vosniadou, & Vamvakoussi, 2007, p. 283 based on Vosniadou, 1999
36 Pimm, 1995, and Saul, 1998
37 Fey & Good, 1985, and Heid, 1996
38 NCTM, 2000
39 Kieran, 2004, and Lester & Ferrini-Mundy, 2004
40 Payne & Squibb, 1990, Sutherland, 1991, and Tall & Thomas 1991
41 Star, 2009
42 See, for instance, Carpenter, Levi, & Loef Franke, 2003
43 Dougherty & Zilliox, 2003
44 Carraher, Schliemann, & Brizuela, 2001
45 Radford, 2010
46 Hewitt, 2012
47 Hewitt, 2012, p. 158
48 Dweck, 2006
49 Mueller & Dweck, 1998
50 Moser, Schoder, Heeter, Moran, & Lee, 2011
51 Blackwell, Trzesniewski, & Dweck, 2007
52 Demby, 1997, p. 48, discussing Hart, 1981, Booth, 1984, and Kuchemann, 1981
53 Anderson & Smith, 1987
54 Matz, 1980
55 Based on Capraro, Capraro, Parker, Kulm, & Raulerson, 2005
56 Brown and Burton, 1978, pp. 155–156
57 See Smith, diSessa, & Roschelle, 1993
58 e.g., Leinhardt et al., 1990; Nesher, 1987
59 e.g. Brown, J. S. & VanLehn, K. (1980
60 e.g. Novak, 1985
61 e.g., Confrey, 1990, based on Mevarech & Kramarsky, 1997
62 Lannin, Arbaugh, Barker, & Townsend, 2006
63 For a discussion of ‘phenomenological primitives’ such as “multiplication makes numbers
bigger” and a counter-argument to the misconceptions perspective, see Hammer, 1996, and
diSessa, 1993
64 Matz, 1982, pp. 25–6
65 Kajander & Lovric, 2009
66 MacGregor & Stacey, 1997, p. 3 based on Cohors-Fresenborg, 1993, Sutherland, 1991, and
Tall & Thomas, 1991
67 Ainley, 1999
68 Proulx, 2007
69 Kieran, 1992
70 Brown et al., 1988
71 Demby, 1997
72 Macgregor & Stacey, 1997
73 Kuchemann, 1981
74 Kuchemann, 1978, 1981 and Ursini & Trigueros’ work on their 3UV model (2001, 2004,
2008).
75 Bills, Wilson, & Ainley, 2006; Carraher & Schliemann, 2000; Cerulli & Mariotti, 2001;
Cooper, Boulton-Lewis, Atweh, Pillay, Wilss, & Mutch, 1997; Filloy & Rojano, 1984; Filloy
& Rojano, 1989; Gonzalez, Ambrose, & Castro Martinez, 2004; Herscovics & Linchevski,
1994; Linchevski & Herscovics, 1996; Nathan & Koellner, 2007; Russell, Schifter, & Bastable,
2011; Warren, 2003
76 Trigueros & Ursini, 1999
Introduction • 25
Carraher, D., & Schliemann, A. (2000). Bringing out the algebraic character of arithmetic:
Instantiating variables in addition and subtraction. Paper presented at the 24th an-
nual Psychology of Mathematics Education (PME) Conference, Hiroshima, Japan.
Carraher, D., Schliemann, A., & Brizuela, B. (2001). Can young students operate on un-
knowns? Paper presented at the 25th Conference of the International Group for the
Psychology of Mathematics Education., Utrecht, Netherlands.
Cerulli, M., & Mariotti, M. A. (2001). Arithmetic and algebra, continuity or cognitive
break? The case of Francesca. Paper presented at the 25th Conference of the Inter-
national Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Utrecht, Netherlands.
Chalouh, L., & Herscovics, N. (1988). Teaching algebraic expressions in a meaningful way.
In A. F. Coxford (Ed.), The ideas of algebra, K–12 (1988 Yearbook) (pp. 33–42).
Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Christou, K. P. & Vosniadou, S. (2005). How students interpret literal symbols in algebra:
A conceptual change approach. In B. G. Bara, L. Barsalou, & M. Bucciarelli (Eds.),
Proceedings of the XXVII Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp.
453–458). Italy.
Christou, K., Vosniadou, S., & Vamvakoussi, X. (2007). Students’ interpretations of literal
symbols in algebra. In S. Vaosniadou, Baltas, A., & Vamvakoussi, X. (Ed.), Refram-
ing the conceptual change approach in learning and instruction. New York, NY:
Elsevier.
Chun-Yi, L., & Ming-Puu, C. (2008). Bridging the gap between mathematical conjecture
and proof through computer-supported cognitive conflicts. Teaching Mathematics
& its Applications, 27(1), 1–1.
Cohors-Fresenborg, E. (1993). Integrating algorithmic and axiomatic ways of thinking in
mathematics lessons in secondary schools. In the Proceedings of Southeast Asia
Conference on Mathematics Education (SEACME-6) and the Seventh National Con-
ference on Mathematics (pp. 74–81). Kampus Sukolilo, Surabaya.
Confrey, J. 1990. A review of the research on student conceptions in mathematics, science,
and programming, In C. E. Cazden (Ed.), Review of Research in Education, 16,
3–56.
Cooper, T. J., Boulton-Lewis, G., Atweh, B., Pillay, H., Wilss, L., & Mutch, S. (1997). The
transition from arithmetic to algebra: Initial understanding of equals, operations,
and variables. Paper presented at the 21st Conference of the International Group for
the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Lahti, Finland.
Cuoco, A., P. Goldenberg, E., & Mark, J. (1996). Habits of mind: An organizing principle
for mathematics curricula. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 15(4), 375–402.
Davies, N., & Walker, K. (2005, July). Learning to notice: One aspect of teachers’ content
knowledge in the numeracy classroom. In Building connections: Theory, research
and practice (Proceedings of the 28th annual conference of the Mathematics Educa-
tion Research Group of Australasia)(pp. 273–280). [AU: City, state?]
Davis, R. B. (1989). Three ways of improving cognitive studies in algebra. In S. Wagner
& C. Kieran (Eds.), Research in the learning and teaching of algebra (pp. 115–119).
Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Demby, A. (1997). Algebraic procedures used by 13-to-15-year-olds. Educational Studies
in Mathematics, 33(1), 45–70.
diSessa, A. A. (1993). Toward an epistemology of physics. Cognition & Instruction,
10(2/3), 105.
28 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Dougherty, B. & Zilliox, J. (2003) Voyaging from theory and practice in teaching and
learning: A view from Hawai’i. In N. Pateman, B. Dougherty, & J. Zilliox (Eds.),
Proceedings of the 27th conference of the international group for the psychology of
mathematics education (vol. 1, pp. 31–46). College of Education, Honolulu: Uni-
versity of Hawaii.
Driscoll, M. (1999). Fostering algebraic thinking: A guide for teachers grades 6–10.
Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Durkin, K., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2012). The effectiveness of using incorrect examples to
support learning about decimal magnitude. Learning and Instruction, 22(3), 206–
214.
Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset. New York, NY: Random House.
Fennema, E., Carpenter, T., Franke, M. L., Levi, L., Jacobs, V., & Empson, S. (1996). A
longitudinal study of learning to use children’s thinking in mathematics instruction.
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 27(4), 403–434.
Fey, J. T., & Good, R. A. (1985). Rethinking the sequence and priorities of high school
mathematics curricula. In C. R. H. M. J. Zweng (Ed.), The secondary school math-
ematics curriculum (Yearbook of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics).
(pp. 43–52). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Filloy, B., & Rojano, T. (1989). Solving equations: The Transition from arithmetic to al-
gebra. For the Learning of Mathematics: An International Journal of Mathematics
Education, 9(2), 19–25.
Filloy, E., & Rojano, T. (1984). From an arithmetical to an algebraic thought: A clinical
study with 12–13 years old. Paper presented at the Sixth Annual Meeting of the
North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Math-
ematics Education, Madison: University of Wisconsin.
Gonzalez, M., Ambrose, R., & Castro Martinez, E. (2004). In the transition from arithme-
tic to Algebra: Misconceptions of the equal sign. Paper presented at the Psychology
of Mathematics Education 28.
Greer, B. (2006). Designing for conceptual change. Paper presented at the the 30th Con-
ference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education,
Prague, Czech Republic.
Hammer, D. (1996). Misconceptions or P-Prims: How may alternative perspectives of cog-
nitive structure influence instructional perceptions and intentions. Journal of the
Learning Sciences, 5(2), 97.
Hart, K. (1981). Children’s understanding of mathematics: 11–16. London, UK: John Mur-
ray.
Haspekian, M. (2003). Between arithmetic and algebra: A space for the spreadsheet? Con-
tribution to an instrumental approach. Paper presented at the Third Conference of
the European Society for Research in Mathematics Education, Pisa: University of
Pisa.
Heemsoth, T. (2014). Learning fractions from reflecting the rationale behind one’s own
errors. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association, Phila-
delphia, PA.
Heid, M. K. (1996). A technology-intensive functional approach to the emergence of
algebraic thinking. In C. K. N. Bednarz, & L. Lee (Ed.), Approaches to algebra:
Perspectives for research and teaching (pp. 239–255). Dordrecht, The Netherlands:
Kluwer.
Introduction • 29
Helfand, D. (2006, January 30). A formula for failure in L.A. schools. Los Angeles
Times. Retrieved from http://www.latimes.com/news/education/la-me-dropout-
30jan30,1,2605555.story.
Herscovics, N. (1989). Cognitive obstacles encountered in the learning of algebra. In S. W.
C. Kieran (Ed.), Research issues in the learning and teaching of algebra (Vol. 4, pp.
60–86). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Herscovics, N., & Linchevski, L. (1994). A cognitive gap between arithmetic and algebra.
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 27(1), 59–78.
Hewitt, D. (2012). Young students learning formal algebraic notation and solving linear
equations: Are commonly experienced difficulties avoidable? Educational Studies
in Mathematics, 81(2), 139–159.
Ho, K. F., & Tan, P. (2013). Developing a professional vision of classroom practices of a
mathematics teacher: Views from a researcher and a teacher. Teaching Education,
24(4), 415–426.
Hoch, M. (2003). Structure sense. In M. A. Mariotti (Ed.), Proceedings of the 3rd Confer-
ence for European Research in Mathematics Education. Bellaria, Italy: CERME.
Hoch, M. (2007). Structure sense in high school algebra. Unpublished doctoral disserta-
tion, Tel Aviv University, Israel.
Hoch, M., & Dreyfus, T. (2004). Structure sense in high school algebra: The effect of
brackets. Paper presented at the 28th Annual Meeting of the International Group for
the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Bergen, Norway.
Huff, D. (1993). How to lie with statistics. New York, NY: W.W. Norton & Company.
Kajander, A., & Lovric, M. (2009). Mathematics textbooks and their potential role in sup-
porting misconceptions. International Journal of Mathematical Education in Sci-
ence and Technology, 40(2), 173–181.
Kaput, J. (1989). Linking representations in the symbol systems of algebra. In S. W. C.
Kieran (Ed.), Research issues in the learning and teaching of algebra (Vol. 4 of
Research agenda for mathematics education, pp. 167–194). Reston, VA: National
Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Kaput, J. (1995). A research base supporting long-term algebra reform? Paper presented
at the Annual Meeting of the North American Chapter of the International Group for
the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Columbus, OH.
Kaput, J. (1999). Teaching and learning a new algebra with understanding. In E. Fennema
& T. A. Romberg (Eds.), Mathematics classrooms that promote understanding (pp.
133–155). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Katz, V. (2006). Algebra: Gateway to a technological future. Mathematical Association of
America. Retrieved from: http://www.maa.org/news/new-report-algebra-gateway-
technological-future.
Kieran, C. (1990). Cognitive processes involved in learning school algebra. In P. N. J. Kil-
patrick (Ed.), Mathematics and cognition: A research synthesis by the International
Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (pp. 96–112). Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Kieran, C. (1992). The learning and teaching of school algebra. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.),
Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 390–419). New
York, NY: Macmillan.
Kieran, C. (2004). Algebraic thinking in the early grades: What is it? The Mathematics
Educator, 8(1), 139–151.
30 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Kieran, C. (2007). Learning and teaching algebra at the middle school through college
levels. In F. Lester (Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching
and learning (pp. 707–762): Information Age Publishing.
Kuchemann, D. (1978). Children’s understanding of numerical variables. Mathematics in
School, 7(4), 23–25.
Kuchemann, D. (1981). Algebra. In K. M. Hart (Ed.), Children’s understanding of math-
ematics (pp. 102–119). London: John Murray.
Ladson-Billings, G. (1998). Teaching in dangerous times: Culturally relevant approaches
to teacher assessment. Journal of Negro Education, 67(3), 255–267.
Lannin, J., Arbaugh, F., Barker, D., & Townsend, B. (2006). Making the most of student
errors. Teaching Children Mathematics, 13(3), 182–186.
Leatham, K. R., Peterson, B. E., Stockero, S. L., & Van Zoest, L. R. (2015). Conceptualiz-
ing mathematically significant pedagogical opportunities to build on student think-
ing. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 46(1), 88–124.
Leatham, K., Stockero, S., Peterson, B., & Van Zoest, L. (2011). Mathematically important
pedagogical opportunities. Paper presented at the 33rd Annual Meeting of the North
American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics
Education, Reno, NV.
Lee, L., & Wheeler, D. (1989). The arithmetic connection. Educational Studies in Math-
ematics, 20(1), 41–54.
Leinhardt, G., Zaslavsky, O., & Stein, M. K. (1990). Functions, graphs, and graphing:
Tasks, learning, and teaching. Review of Educational Research, 60(1), 1–64.
Lester, F. K., & Ferrini-Mundy, J. (2004). Proceedings of the NCTM Research Catalyst
Conference. Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Linchevski, L., & Herscovics, N. (1996). Crossing the cognitive gap between arithmetic
and algebra: Operating on the unknown in the context of equations. Educational
Studies in Mathematics, 30(1), 39–65.
Liston, M., & O’Donoghue, J. (2010). Factors influencing the transition to university ser-
vice mathematics: Part 2. A qualitative study. Teaching Mathematics and Its Ap-
plications, 29, 53–68.
MacGregor, M., & Stacey, K. (1993). Cognitive models underlying students’ formulation
of simple linear equations. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 24(3),
217–232.
MacGregor, M., & Stacey, K. (1997). Students’ understanding of algebraic notation: 11–15.
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 33(1), 1–19.
Magiera, M., Van Den Kieboom, L., & Moyer, J. (2010). An extensive analysis of preser-
vice middle school teachers’ knowledge of algebraic thinking. Paper presented at the
American Educational Research Association, Denver, CO.
Mason, J. (1998). Enabling teachers to be real teachers: Necessary levels of awareness and
structure of attention. Journal of Mathematics Teacher Education, 1(3), 243–267.
Matz, M. (1980). Toward a computational theory of algebraic competence. Journal of
Mathematical Behavior, 3(1), 93–166.
Matz, M. (1982). Towards a process model for high school algebra errors. In D. S. J. S.
Brown (Ed.), Intelligent tutoring systems. (pp. 25–50). New York, NY: Academic
Press.
Introduction • 31
McCann, N. (2014). What could go wrong? Error anticipation relates to conceptual and
procedural knowledge in algebra students. Paper presented at the American Educa-
tional Research Association, Philadelphia, PA.
Mevarech, Z. R., & Kramarski, B. (1997). From verbal descriptions to graphic representa-
tions: Stability and change in students’ alternative conceptions. Educational Studies
in Mathematics, 32(3), 229–263.
Milgram, R. J. (1999). An evaluation of CMP. Retrieved from ftp://math.stanford.edu/pub/
papers/milgram/report-on-cmp.html.
Monk, S. (1992). Students’ understanding of a function given by a physical model. In G. H.
E. Dubinsky (Ed.), The concept of function: Aspects of epistemology and pedagogy.
USA: Mathematical Association of America.
Moseley, B., & Brenner, M. E. (2009). A comparison of curricular effects on the integration
of arithmetic and algebraic schemata in pre-algebra students. Instructional Science:
An International Journal of the Learning Sciences, 37(1), 1–20.
Moser, J., Schoder, H. S., Heeter, C., Moran, T. P., & Lee, Y. H. (2011). Mind your errors:
Evidence for a neural mechanism linking growth mindset to adaptive post error
adjustments. Psychological Science, 22, 1484–1489.
Moses, R. P., & Cobb Jr, C. (2001). Organizing algebra: The need to voice a demand. So-
cial Policy, 31(4), 4–12.
Movshovitz-Hadar, N., & Hadass, R. (1990). Preservice education of math teachers using
paradoxes. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 21(3), 265–287.
Muldoon, K. P., Lewis, C., & Francis, B. (2007). Using cardinality to compare quanti-
ties: The role of social-cognitive conflict in early numeracy. Developmental Science,
10(5), 694–711.
Murray, D. (December 20, 2010). Algebra in elementary school? As demands change, edu-
cators look for new ways to teach math. The Grand Rapids Press. Retrieved from:
http://www.mlive.com/news/grand-rapids/index.ssf/2010/12/algebra_in_elemen-
tary_school_a.html
Nathan, M. J., & Koellner, K. (2007). A framework for understanding and cultivating
the transition from arithmetic to algebraic reasoning. Mathematical Thinking and
Learning: An International Journal, 9(3), 179–192.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2000). Principals and standards for school
mathematics. Reston, VA: NCTM.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2014). Algebra as a strand of school math-
ematics for all students: A position of the National Council of Teachers of Math-
ematics. Retrieved from: http://www.nctm.org/Standards-and-Positions/Position-
Statements/Algebra-as-a-Strand-of-School-Mathematics-for-All-Students/.
Nesher, P. (1987). Towards an instructional theory: The role of student’s misconceptions.
For the Learning of Mathematics, 7(3), 33–40.
Nomi, T., & Allensworth, E. (2009). “Double-dose” algebra as an alternative strategy to
remediation: Effects on students’ academic outcomes. Journal of Research on Edu-
cational Effectiveness, 2(2), 111–148.
Novak, J. D. (1985). Metalearning and metaknowledge strategies to help students learn
how to learn. In L. West & A. Pines (Eds.), Cognitive structure and conceptual
change,(pp. 189–207). New York, NY: Academic Press.
32 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Novotna, J., & Hoch, M. (2008). How structure sense for algebraic expressions or equa-
tions is related to structure sense for abstract algebra. Mathematics Education Re-
search Journal, 20(2), 93–104.
Payne, S. J., & Squibb, H. R. (1990). Algebra mal-rules and cognitive accounts of error.
Cognitive Science, 14(3), 445–481.
Peterson, B., & Leatham, K. (2009). Learning to use students’ mathematical thinking to
orchestrate a class discussion. In L. Knott (Ed.), The role of mathematics discourse
in producing leaders of discourse (pp. 99–128). Charlotte, NC: Information Age
Publishing.
Piaget, J. (1975). The origin of the idea of chance in children. London: Routledge and
Kegan Paul Ltd.
Picciotto, H., & Wah., A. (1993). A new algebra: Tools, themes, concepts. Journal of Math-
ematical Behavior, 12, 19–42.
Pimm, D. (1995). Symbols and meanings in school mathematics. London, UK: Routledge.
Proulx, J. (2007). Addressing the issue of the mathematical knowledge of secondary math-
ematics teachers. Paper presented at the 31st Conference of the International Group
for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Seoul, Korea.
Radford, L. (2010). Layers of generality and types of generalization in pattern activities.
PNA, 4(2), 37–62.
Rea-Ramirez, M. A., & Clement, J. (1998). In search of dissonance: The evolution of dis-
sonance in conceptual change theory. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the
National Association for Research in Science Teaching, San Diego, CA.
Resnick, L. B., Cauzinille-Marmeche, E., & Mathieu, J. (1987). Understanding algebra.
In J. A. Sloboda & D. Rogers (Eds.), Cognitive processes in mathematics (pp. 169–
203). Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.
Riordan, J. E., & Noyce, P. E. (2001). The impact of two standards-based mathematics
curricula on student achievement in Massachusetts. Journal for Research in Math-
ematics Education, 32(4), 368–398.
Russell, S. J., Schifter, D., & Bastable, V. (2011). Developing algebraic thinking in the
context of arithmetic. In J. Cai & E. Knuth (Eds.), Early algebraization (pp. 43–69).
Heidelberg: Springer Berlin.
Saul, M. (1998). Algebra, technology, and a remark of I. M. Gelfand. Paper presented at
The nature and role of algebra in the K–14 curriculum: Proceedings of a National
Symposium organized by the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, the
Mathematical Sciences Education Board, and the National Research Council, Wash-
ington, D.C.
Saul, M. (2008). Algebra: The mathematics and the pedagogy. In C. Greenes & R. Ru-
benstein (Eds.), Algebra and algebraic thinking in school mathematics (pp. 63–79).
Reston, VA: The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Schoenfeld, A. H. (2008). Chapter 2: On modeling teachers’ in-the-moment decision mak-
ing. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education. Monograph, 14, 45–96.
Sfard, A. (1991). On the dual nature of mathematical conceptions: Reflections on pro-
cesses and objects as different sides of the same coin. Educational Studies in Math-
ematics, 22(1), 1–36.
Sfard, A., & Linchevski, L. (1994). The gains and the pitfalls of reification—The case of
algebra. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 26, 191–228.
Introduction • 33
Sherin, M. G., Russ, R. S., Sherin, B. L., & Colestock, A. (2008). Professional vision in
action: An exploratory study. Issues in Teacher Education, 17(2), 27–46.
Shulman, L. S. (1986). Those who understand: A conception of teacher knowledge. Ameri-
can Educator, 10(1), 9–15,43–44.
Sierpinska, A. (1995). Understanding mathematics. London: Falmer Press.
Smith, J., diSessa, A., & Roschelle, J. (1993). Misconceptions reconceived: A constructiv-
ist analysis of knowledge in transition. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 3(2), 115.
Smith, M., & Stein, M. K. (2011). 5 practices for orchestrating productive mathematics
discussions. Reston, VA: NCTM.
Sowder, L. (1988). Children’s solution of story problems. Journal of Mathematical Behav-
ior, 7, 227–238.
Stockero, S., & Van Zoest, L. (2012). Characterizing pivotal teaching moments in be-
ginning mathematics teachers practice. Journal of Mathematics Teacher Education,
16(2), 125–147.
Star, J. R. (Producer). (2009, February 10, 2009). Why students struggle with algebra and
how schools are helping. Making algebra easier. Retrieved from http://www.ed-
week.org/ew/marketplace/webinars/webinars.html.
Sutherland, R. (1991). Some unanswered research questions on the teaching and learning
of algebra. For the Learning of Mathematics, 11(3), 40–46.
Tall, D., & Thomas, M. (1991). Encouraging versatile thinking in algebra using the com-
puter. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 22(2), 125–147.
Tirosh, D., Even, R., & Robinson, N. (1998). Simplifying algebraic expressions: teacher
awareness and teaching approaches. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 35(1),
51–64.
Trigueros, M., & Ursini, S. (1999). Does the understanding of variable evolve through
schooling? Paper presented at the Conference of the International Group for the
Psychology of Mathematics Education., Haifa, Israel.
Trigueros, M., & Ursini, S. (2008). Structure sense and the use of variable. Paper presented
at the Proceedings of the 32rd Conference of The International Group for the Psy-
chology of Mathematics Education.
Ursini, S., & Trigueros, M. (2001). A model for the uses of variable in elementary algebra.
Paper presented at the 25th Conference of The International Group for the Psychol-
ogy of Mathematics Education., Utrecht, Netherlands.
Ursini, S., & Trigueros, M. (2004). How do high school students interpret parameters in
algebra? Paper presented at the 28th Conference of the International Group for the
Psychology of Mathematics Education, Bergen, Norway. http://search.ebscohost.
com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=eric&AN=ED489663&site=ehost-live.
Viadero, D. (2009, March 11). Algebra-for-all policy found to raise rates of fail-
ure in Chicago. Education Week. Retrieved from http://www.edweek.org/ew/
articles/2009/03/11/24algebra.h28.html?tmp=1587018900.
Vosniadou, S. (1999). Conceptual change research: State of the art and future directions. In
W. Schnotz, S. Vosniadou, & M. Carretero (Eds.), New perspectives on conceptual
change (pp. 3–13). Bingley, United Kingdom: Emerald Group Publishing.
Wallach, T., & Even, R. (2006). Hearing students: The complexity of understanding what
they are saying, showing, and doing. Journal of Mathematics Teacher Education,
8(5), 393–417.
34 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Warren, E. (2003). The role of arithmetic structure in the transition from arithmetic to alge-
bra. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 15(2), 122–137.
Watson, J. M. (2002). Inferential reasoning and the influence of cognitive conflict. Educa-
tional Studies in Mathematics, 51(3), 225.
Watson, J. M. (2007). The role of cognitive conflict in developing students’ understanding
of average. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 65(1), 21–47.
CHAPTER 2
INTRODUCTION
We can often trace students’ struggles in algebra back to their understanding of
variables.1 There are many uses of the term variable, which is often the basis for
students’ difficulties in understanding.2 Research has shown that some students
struggle with the differences between the name of an object (e.g., the person Mi-
chael), the name of an attribute (e.g., Michael’s height), and the name of a mea-
surement or quantity (h units).3 They can often capably manipulate variables with-
out really understanding the power and flexibility of the symbols.4 Students may
interpret letters or algebraic expressions based on intuition, simple guessing, or
comparison with other symbol systems they know.5 Moreover, there is evidence
to show that students’ understanding of variables can remain largely unchanged
through many years of schooling, all the way to the college level.6 Yet variables
are the core of algebra, and understanding of their nature and behavior is critical
to students’ success.
Some of the confusion over the nature of variables reflects how our views of
the concept of variables have changed over time. When researchers asked math-
ematicians, educators, linguists, computer scientists, and logicians for a one-word
definition of variable, they heard “symbol, placeholder, pronoun, parameter, argu-
ment, pointer, name, identifier, empty space, void, reference, and instance.”7 Each
person uniquely defined the word based on his or her experience, prior knowl-
edge, and context. A variable has historically been understood as “a changing
number,” “representing numbers,” able to have “two or more values,” “a symbol
for which one substitutes names for some objects, usually a number,” and more
recently, “a symbol for which things . . . can be substituted.”8 A letter can repre-
sent a constant, a specific unknown, and a variable.
Moreover, how we use variables in algebra can take the same form, but have
a different sense.9
1. A = LW
2. 40 = 5x
3. sin x = cos x tan x
4. 1 = n (1/n)
5. y = kx
Although each of the above equations has a term equal to a product of two other
terms, the way the variables are used in each case communicates a different idea
about the mathematics involved. In the first case—a formula—a relationship is a
mathematical tool. Each letter has a distinct connection to a physical reality, the
dimension of a rectangle. In the second, x is an unknown quantity that students
can solve. The third use of the variable x is more abstract, in that students can
algebraically manipulate this trigonometric identity without finding a specific val-
ue; students can manipulate the identity to learn about mathematical systems and
structure. Fourth, n is in a property that defines mathematical operations, a value
based on a generalized pattern. Finally, in the fifth use, y and x are truly variable in
the full sense of the word, communicating an independent/dependent relationship
that takes its shape with k representing some constant value.
In the above discussion on algebraic thinking, it was proposed that there are
four purposes or themes to algebra: generalizing, solving equations and making
sense of their solutions, exercising algebraic rules, and building models and learn-
ing from them. Each of these four approaches to learning algebra leads to a dif-
ferent conception of the idea of variable.10 When you plug 2 into a calculator and
repeatedly press the + sign, you get the pattern expressed by 2x. The use of x helps
students translate the mathematical ideas into a generalized form. There are no un-
knowns to seek; the variable simply helps in representation and students are “fin-
ished” when they find an appropriate algebraic form. In contrast, working with an
equation such as 40 = 5x leads students to simplify and solve for the unknown,
Variables and Expressions • 37
represented by the variable. Solving problems can also involve variables treated
as constants, such as k in the equation y = kx. Algebra can work on a more abstract
level, as well: one in which students manipulate letters based on algebraic rules.
In this use, there is no connection between the variable and a specific value or
values. Letters can be “arbitrary marks on paper” as students study structures and
systems of variables and operations.11 Finally, students use variables to represent
and study situations in the real world with mathematical models. Within this use,
variables relate to each other in important ways. In the area formula A = LW, each
variable refers not only to an aspect of a rectangle, but also to the relationships
among the aspects. Students do not need to put specific values into the model to
consider the relationships of the variables. Variables stand for numbers on which
other numbers depend, thereby acting as a parameter of the real-world situation,
explaining dependent and independent relations. Students need to understand the
many meanings within the name of variable in order to access the powerful math-
ematics behind the word.12
The way we conceive of variables is also parallel to the way we use language.
For instance, consider how we use the word hat. When we refer to “Lincoln’s
hat,” it has a specific value—a black top hat—as might the equation 40 = 5x in
which x has the specific value of 8. We also might say “Michael always wears a
hat.” Knowing that Michael wears a range of hats, it could be a baseball cap, a be-
ret, or a fedora. Our understanding of hat, given the constraint that it is Michael’s,
lets us know that it could be a few specific hats. Given the constraints that x is a
positive integer and x < 4, we know x could be 1, 2, or 3. We could also refer to “a
lady’s hat,” which is constrained to the type of hats worn by ladies, but an unlim-
ited range of possibilities, in a similar way that |x| < 5. The variable x in this case
has an infinite number of possibilities, but is also constrained so that it cannot be
just any number. We might even say, “a woman’s choice of hat depends on what
she wears.” There is a relationship between the hat and clothing, similar to the
relationship between the two variables in y = kx. The parameter k quantifies the
relationship between x and y. If she chooses a particular dress, that corresponds
to a particular hat. There is some kind of relationship between the two, such as
color coordination or style. The variable y similarly depends on what you choose
for x, but k defines how the two are connected. It is the words that surround hat
that help define what we are thinking when we say “hat,” just as the numbers,
variables, functions, etc. surrounding a letter define how a variable is understood.
We have interesting research that explores What Can Variables Stand for?1 and
Can Variables Change? that is explored in more detail later.
Research on students’ understanding of variables has also evolved over the
past thirty years. There are many ways that researchers have categorized students’
developing understanding of variables, but the following categories can be help-
ful to teachers. Based on tests and interviews of hundreds of students, researchers
1
Bold and italicized print indicates a reference to a section in the book
38 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
have identified different ways that students think about letters representing un-
knowns.13 The first three categories describe students’ struggles to develop under-
standing of what a letter can represent. Each of the next three categories indicates
different uses of variable in algebra, as well as increasing levels of sophistication
of thought about the use of letters.
• Letter evaluated
Students assign the letter a numerical value from the outset.
• Letter not used
Students ignore the letter, or at best acknowledge its existence, but without
giving it a meaning.
• Letter used as an object
Students regard the letter as shorthand for an object or as an object in its
own right.
• Letter as an unknown number
Students regard the letter as a specific but unknown number, and can oper-
ate upon it directly.
• Letter used as a general number
Students see the letter as representing, or at least as being able to take,
several values rather than just one.
• Letter used as a functional relationship
Students see the letter as representing a range of unspecified values, and
a systematic relationship is seen to exist between two such sets of values.
These categories help us consider how students’ thinking about letters evolves into
thinking about variables. Initially, these categories were considered to be a Piag-
etian hierarchy of levels of increasingly sophisticated thinking about variables.14
However, research has demonstrated that students’ understanding of variables is
deeply impacted by their prior knowledge and experience and does not necessar-
ily have a linear developmental path.15 Each of these categories is described in
more detail later in this chapter.
The last three categories of specific unknown, general number, and functional
relationship are broken down in Table 2.1 as a decomposition of the concept of
variable. The authors of this research suggest that difficulties with understanding
the multiple perspectives of variables are reinforced when each of the different
aspects are taught separately, particularly when teachers stress manipulation and
transformation rules.16 Research indicates that Algebra teachers typically focus
on the use of letters as specific unknowns rather than variables.17 Instead, teach-
ers should emphasize how the same symbolism, syntax, or rules for manipula-
tion apply to the different uses of a variable, while having different purposes and
meaning. Understanding how context can influence the use and meaning of letters
is an important aspect of algebraic thinking. Again, helping students understand
how letters mean different things in different situations might involve examining
Variables and Expressions • 39
language. Avid crossword puzzle doers, for example, realize that the art of a good
puzzle is in the ability of words to have multiple meanings. The word train, for
instance, could mean a locomotive, preparation for a baseball game, or the end of
a bridal dress. These are very different uses of one word, depending on how and
when it is used. Similarly, symbols take on different purposes in different math-
ematical contexts.
Students of arithmetic typically believe mathematics flows from left to right,
to a numeric conclusion. For instance, in one study, 145 sixth-grade students were
given the problem 8 + 4 = ? + 5. All 145 students answered either 12 or 17.18 Chil-
dren as young as kindergarten come up with the same conclusion. This tendency
leads to problems in algebra when students encounter expressions, such as x + 5.
They often struggle to understand that an “answer” can be an expression rather
than a number. They want to “complete” the problem by continuing the flow from
left to right. Researchers describe the ability to see an expression as an answer
(and not need a single number) as an Acceptance of Lack of Closure, or the abil-
ity to overcome the Expected answer obstacle (expecting an answer to be a single
entity without operations).19 Arithmetic leads many students to believe that math
requires specific numerical answers, so they may be reluctant to give an algebraic
answer, assuming that something else must have been intended. Students struggle
40 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
to find meaning when given expressions because they lack an equal sign and
something on the other side.20
Algebra requires a shift in thinking so that an expression such as x + 5 can be
seen as an instruction for a procedure to add 5 to the variable x; or as an answer,
the number that is 5 more than x; or as a function, mapping the values achieved for
values of x onto another. This notion that an expression can represent a procedure
and an answer simultaneously is called the Process-Product Dilemma21 and is
challenging for students in early algebra to accept.22 Students must be able to see
that 13 can be written as 5 + 8, which can represent the number of items in two
sets, 5 and 8, as a + b can represent the number of items in two sets containing
a and b items.23 The ability to see a + b as an object as well as a process can be
initiated as students learn arithmetic. Some of the difficulty for students can be
traced back to their understanding of Algebraic Notation, as learning notation in
mathematics can be as challenging as learning a new language. The rules of nota-
tion can be based on a particular rationale or simply mathematical convention,
and it is difficult for students to differentiate.
Finally, variables are used as a tool to help solve problems. While the roles
of variables in modeling and word problems are discussed in a later chapter, it is
useful to explore some research in regard to using variables in context. We have
organized the research on variables in these contexts into the final two topics
in this chapter: Area, Perimeter, and Variables and Translation Difficulties in
Changing Words to Algebraic Sentences.
In the following quote, researchers describe our primary goal for this chapter:
As soon as children are unable to give meaning to concepts, they hide their difficul-
ties by resorting to routine activities to obtain correct answers and gain approval.
Once committed to such a course, it easily degenerates into a never ending down-
ward spiral of instrumental activity: learning the “trick of the week” to survive,
soon leading to a collection of disconnected activities that become more and more
difficult to coordinate, even at a purely mechanistic level. Therefore, the beginning
phase of the subject—giving meaning to the variable concept and devising ways
of overcoming the cognitive obstacles—is fundamental to laying a foundation for
meaningful algebraic thinking.24
This chapter on Variables and Expressions can be used as a basis to help explain
some of the mathematical issues that may underlie possible answers that students
give when encountering algebraic problems. A clear and comprehensive under-
standing of variables is at the heart of algebraic thinking.
Variables and Expressions • 41
Can ‘n’ stand for 37? Yes (30%, 67%, 81%).
As students had more experience with variables, their success on these questions
increased. However, at only 40%, the percentage of eighth-grade students who
thought n could be 3r + 2 was still quite low after instruction about variables. A
student gave explanations for some of his thinking in a follow-up interview: in
the case of n being 4, the student explained, “letters are also used as variables and
so n could be any number.” However, when asked, “Could n stand for 15 + 27?”
the student responded:
I think so. Well, actually, I do not think so really because variables just stand for
one number. You could have n plus another letter or variable and n could be 15 and
the other variable could be 27. n = 15 and p can equal 27 so then if you did n + p it
would equal 42.27
42 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
The student could not accept that the addition sign could be part of what n repre-
sented—that it could represent an expression.
He had developed the rule in his mind, likely from his experience, that variables
can only represent single numbers. This is an example of the Process-Product
Dilemma described in detail later in this chapter.
Upon further questioning, the student also had developed some erroneous rules
about the impact of parentheses on the potential for what n could represent (see
Table 2.2). In the interviews, when parentheses isolated expressions, students re-
sponded differently. For instance, writing the expression as (3r + 2) instead of (3r)
+ 2 or simply putting r into parentheses altered students’ perspectives so that they
were more comfortable thinking about what that expression might represent as an
object, as opposed to thinking of it as a process. However, the student in this case
struggled with the idea that an expression (with or without parentheses) could be
one thing. He seems to have understood that you typically evaluate what is inside
parentheses, which influenced his thinking about what n could represent: if it was
evaluated first, then it would work. Researchers found that using parentheses in
different ways might be an effective approach that could help bridge the Process-
Product Dilemma for students.
Students have quite a bit of difficulty thinking about a variable being a negative
number as well as thinking about the negative or opposite of the value of a vari-
able. Research indicates that students’ experience with natural numbers strongly
influences their interpretation of symbols in algebra. Specifically, when thinking
about the values of variables in algebraic expressions, students tend to believe that
the letters can only represent natural numbers. As students move from working
with natural numbers to real numbers, their understanding of what variables can
represent must change similarly.
As the -b part of this problem implies, some students interpret the sign in front
of the variable as the sign of the numbers that they represent. When engaged with
expressions such as k + 3 or d + d + d, some students believed that the variables
stood for positive numbers and a negative number could not be substituted. Fifty
percent of students believed that a positive number could not be put in for b in
a term such as -b. See Algebraic Relations: Negatives for further discussion on
this topic.
In one study, the interviewer asked four different students to explain their un-
derstanding of the variable x when given the problem (see Table 2.3): What is the
value of 10 – x, when x = 6?
The mathematical convention of considering 1x to be the same as x may be
confusing to students. For some, their prior experience with numbers next to each
other may negatively influence their interpretation of this convention of leaving
out the multiplication symbol. For instance, students’ experience with compound
fractions (e.g., 2 ½ = 2 + ½) and place value (43 = 4 tens + 3 ones) is contrary
to the convention of 1x meaning multiplication.30 Others may get confused as
they try to make sense of what they are learning about mathematical practice in
algebra, with the use of letters in places such as textbook exercises labeled 1a,
1b, 1c, etc.31 Asked, “When I show you something like 3a, what does it mean
to you?” five of six students in one study interpreted 3a as a subdivision label,
saying: “third problem, first part” or “like 1, a, b, c.”32 In addition, students may
misunderstand
what teachers mean when they say “x” without a coefficient means 1“x” . . . the
power of x is 1 if no index is written and that x with zero as the (power) equals 1
(e.g., x = x1 and x0 = 1). . . . The student gets a vague message that the letter x by itself
is something to do with 1.33
As a result, researchers have noted that some students assume that x = 1, as the
students explain above.
Another common misconception regarding what letters represent is thinking
there is some relationship between the choice of letter used and its value. In par-
ticular, many students believe that there is a correspondence between the letter’s
position in the alphabet and its value. Problems such as “Find three consecutive
integers that add to 63” may be set up initially as x + y + z = 63, in which x is the
first integer, y is the second, and z is the third. This strategy makes organizational
sense but can have the unintended effect of teaching students that there is some
connection between a letter’s position in the alphabet and the answer. Students
may bring this type of understanding to the learning of algebra, so it is difficult for
them to believe that the use of letters is arbitrary and not related to other letters.
This interpretation can also be influenced by decoding puzzles or other contexts
that connect numbers to particular letters, such as in Table 2.4. This misconcep-
tion might be reinforced by letters used in close association with specific num-
bers, such as α = 1, β = 2, etc.34
In another research problem, students were asked to “Simplify e + 2 + 6.”
Some students responded “13” and explained, “I added 2 and 6 to get 8, but I
didn’t really know what to do next so I guessed that because e is the fifth letter of
14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26
N O P Q R S T U V W X Y Z
Variables and Expressions • 45
the alphabet, the answer must be 13.”35 Students answering in this manner did not
have Acceptance of a Lack of Closure. They may have wanted to “complete” the
problem by combining the numbers and letter. Not having enough information
about the letter e to “finish” the problem, the student assumed that e represented 5
because it is the fifth letter of the alphabet. In another problem, when asked if they
could simplify 10 + h, some students answered “10 + h = r” because ten letters
after h is r.36 When researchers posed the question, “if e = 3 and g = 5, what can
you say about the value of f?” a large proportion of students answered “4” because
f is between e and g, so the answer must be between 3 and 5. Students may not
understand that what a letter represents is independent of the letter used. This is an
important point that teachers will want to make in the classroom.
Some students may confuse what the variable can represent with place value.37
For instance, one interview asked: “If x = 6, what is the value of 2x?” The student
responded: “26. You put the 6 in for the x because it is supposed to be something
more than 20.” In this case, the 6 is a digit in the number 26 representing the ones
column. Students may assume that the x in 2x is the second digit in a number of
twenty something. There is implicit addition in arithmetic that may lead to this
confusion.38 For instance, 26 represents 20 + 6 and 5 ½ represents one half more
than five. This type of thinking, called concatenation in the research, may also be
encouraged with digit problems, in which x might stand for the tens digit and y
might stand for the ones digit.
A significant part of the movement from concrete to abstract thinking is under-
standing what a variable can represent. There are significant differences between
how numbers work and how letters work in mathematics. In Table 2.5, researchers
have summarized the important differences between numbers and letters that stu-
TABLE 2.6.
4
Problem 2: What numbers might work for d in the problem 5d > ?
d
Interview
Interviewer: Could we try with a negative number; let’s say -2?
Student: Well, yes, of course. Mmm! That is -10 > -2, which is not valid. Well, that changes
everything.
Interviewer: So, now do you think we could find a number which wouldn’t hold for the
inequality?
Student: Certainly, the negative numbers.
Interviewer: Any other kind of number?
Student: We have tried everything, positives and negatives.
Interviewer: Could we try with a fraction; let’s say 1/2?
Student: Yes, we could [he tries it]. This also wouldn’t hold.
Interviewer: Why didn’t you try with such kind of numbers before?
Student: I don’t know, I should have thought both of negatives or fractions.
Source: Christou & Vosniadou (2012)
Teaching Strategy
It often helps to tap into students’ prior knowledge and build on it toward an
algebraic idea. In regard to the idea of representation, consider the approach in
Table 2.7 that builds an understanding of variable on students’ understanding of
Variables and Expressions • 47
number. Similar strategies are found in math curriculum such as Connected Math
with “coins and bags.” Coins represent numbers and bags represent variables.
Teachers
• Help students understand how letters mean different things in different
contexts. How does a particular context lead to a particular interpretation
of the use of a letter?
• Help students understand the role of parentheses and how they effect what
a variable can represent.
• Help students understand how negatives affect the value of variables.
• Consider how arithmetic rules may influence students’ thinking about the
use of variables.
• Help students understand that a letter’s place in the alphabet or the choice
of a particular letter has no impact on its value.
• Sometimes use random letters for variables, rather than always using con-
secutive letters in the alphabet.
48 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Students need to understand when letters can vary and when they can’t. Letters
can vary across problems but not within a problem. For example, in the problem
x + y = 2x – 3, x represents the same number whenever it is used on both sides of
the equation. This is confusing in that x is actually a different number depending
on what y is, so it does change based on y. Possibly adding more confusion for
students, in a system of equations, x could be the same across equations (at the
point of intersection) but could also be different. However, in two different prob-
lems, x could be a different number or range of numbers. It could also be the same
number, but for different reasons. Conservation of relations is just one common
misconception students might develop when working with equations, functions,
and variables. Teachers can address this possibility by purposefully designing ex-
periences focused on changing the alphabetic variable and nothing else about a
relation.
An understanding of when variables can change and when they stay the same
is critical when students get to the complex situation of systems of equations.
When solving systems of equations, students might have the following two equa-
tions: x + y = 3x - 5 and 2x + 3y = x + 2. Within those two equations, the xs have
52 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
the same value. Across the two equations, the xs are different values except at
their point of intersection (3, 1), where they are the same values. This can be very
complicated for students to understand. Success with systems of equations begins
with the foundation of understanding the stability of variables in single equations.
Teachers
• Help students understand that variables within an equation have the same
value (e.g., x + 9 = 2x - 8).
• Help students understand that letters chosen to represent values do not have
any influence on their value. For instance, when m is chosen to represent
a value and n is chosen to represent another value, the value of m is not
necessarily less than n. Also, m and n could represent the same value.
Variables and Expressions • 53
Sometimes, students have more difficulty with the representation than with the
concepts, such as Student 3 (see Table 2.14), who can describe his thinking but
struggles with what to write.
Other researchers describe this struggle with closure as the Process-Product
Dilemma.55 Algebra requires a shift in thinking so that an expression such as “x +
5” can be seen as an instruction for a procedure to add 5 to the variable “x,” or as
an answer, the number that is 5 more than “x,” or as a function giving the output
values resulting from the possible values of x. Whether “x + 5” is to be understood
as a process or as an object/product depends on the situation. This notion is chal-
lenging for students in early algebra to accept. The ability to see a + b as an object
as well as a process can be initiated as students learn arithmetic. Students must
be able to see that 13 can be written as 5 + 8, which can represent the number of
items in two sets, 5 and 8, as a + b can represent the number of items in two sets
containing a and b items.
“Many features of an item determine students’ responses, one being the size
of the number and the ease of automatic responses.”56 For instance, students cor-
rectly answered Problem 13 (add 12 to x) at a high rate but erred more often
with Problem 8 (add 4 onto 3n), perhaps because of the coefficient in front of the
variable. For some students, mathematical convention is confusing, particularly
the elimination of the multiplication operation using symbolism such as 10 x h =
10h.57 Students working on Problem 12 may generalize this procedure by writing
10h and intending 10 + h. Students may struggle with the representation of the
mathematical idea rather than the concept. Some suggest that students’ struggles
are due to conventions in natural language. For instance, the similarity between
the words and and plus may lead students to consider ab to mean the same as a +
b.58 It is important for teachers to explore students’ rationale for why they conjoin
numbers and variables in problems such as these, in order to determine if students
accept lack of closure.
Researchers find that exploration of examples and non-examples by plugging
in values for variables helps students overcome their inability to accept lack of
Variables and Expressions • 55
Teachers
• Help students understand how an expression can be an answer.
• Help students understand when they can evaluate a variable and when they
cannot.
• Have students test numbers in the original expression and conjoined ex-
pression to see if they are equivalent.
56 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
LETTER EVALUATED
Common Core Standards: 6.EE.2, 7.EE.2, A-SSE.1, A-SSE.2, A-SSE.3
When students have very little understanding of variables, they may assign the
letter a numerical value from the outset. The problems in Table 2.15 make this
way of thinking evident. In Problem 14, the student may struggle to understand
that an “answer” can be an expression. The interview (see Table 2.16) indicates
that the students may not be comfortable with leaving an expression such as “8
+ g” and feel compelled to come up with a numerical answer (see Acceptance of
Lack of Closure). When students have the Letter Evaluated way of thinking, they
figure out numbers for the letters using any means possible. This numerical think-
ing indicates the student’s belief that the variable is a single number.61 The first
student interviewed assumed that the value of letters is related to their position
in the alphabet, and evaluated the letter accordingly. The student in the second
interview for Problem 14, as well as the student in Problem 15 (see Table 2.17),
assumed that there are three letters, so three equal numbers would be the answer.
This again may be an example of the Letter Evaluated way of thinking, in which
the student does not consider the letters as representing unknowns—or that they
have any relationship to each other—but simply puts numbers in their places.
In Problems 16 and 17 (see Tables 2.18 and 2.19), the student believes x can be
evaluated as the number 1 (see Representation: What Can Variables Stand for?),
so evaluates the expressions accordingly. Sometimes students are confused when
teachers say, “1x is just like x,” and they interpret this as x = 1. Students solving
Problem 18 for a may be a bit more advanced in algebra, but still are challenged
in thinking that a must be evaluated, so they figure out a way to find that value.
In the interview for Problem 18, the student appears to understand the problem
and can describe how he or she might get the answer, but struggles to understand
that the answer can be something with a letter—challenged by the symbolism in
the answer, rather than the thinking behind it. The student wants to evaluate the
problem into a specific number, and is therefore confused as to how to represent
the answer. The tendency to evaluate the letter in the expression is based on many
faulty assumptions such as these and can persist into college.
In order to help students not feel the need to evaluate letters in expressions
into numbers, research suggests that teachers might consider having students use
more language in algebra, such as having students write out words to describe
their thinking, and then help students understand how to symbolize that thinking
in algebraic terms.
Teachers
• Help students understand that the letters chosen to represent a value(s) do
not have any influence on the value(s) that letter represents.
• Help students become comfortable with answers that are expressions, such
as h + 10.
• Help students understand when variables can be evaluated and when they
cannot.
Variables and Expressions • 59
Teachers
• Help students understand that variables represent values.
• Help students understand how variables influence expressions and equa-
tions.
• Help students understand the impact of eliminating variables from expres-
sions and equations when it is mathematically incorrect, versus when it is
mathematically correct. For instance, the difference between subtracting an
x from both sides of the equation 2x + 3 = x - 7 versus ignoring the variable
in the expression 4n + 7 to get 11.
• Help students understand mathematical convention with variables. For in-
stance, 12a meaning 12 multiplied times a—rather than added—and why
1x is the same as x.
Variables and Expressions • 61
sometimes instruct students to treat the a as apples and b as bananas so they don’t
add 3a and 5b to get 8ab because “You can’t combine apples and bananas.” This
type of thinking is described by researchers as fruit salad algebra,72 which encour-
ages students to think of letters as names or labels of things. While this approach
might help students in the moment in learning to combine like terms, this can lead
to long-term confusion and “the belief that one cannot multiply expressions such
as 2a and 5b because one cannot multiply apples and bananas.”73 Instead, teach-
ers should clearly distinguish between the names and numbers of objects, as in
“a represents the unknown number of apples.” Teachers understand the shortcut
language “apples” as implying “unknown number of apples” but students do not
necessarily understand it: in early algebra, they are struggling to comprehend how
letters are used in different ways, and in this context, letters do not signify objects,
but unknown quantities of those objects—an important distinction.
In Problem 26 (Table 2.24), many students understood the letters to represent
the objects “cakes and buns” rather than the cost of cakes and the cost of buns.
Those students who answered incorrectly were found to make poor progress in
their understanding of literal symbols as variables. Research indicates that teach-
ers’ choice of letter used to represent the quantity may influence students’ ten-
dency to think about the letters as objects instead of variables (see Figure 2.174).
In one study, only 37% of Grades 6–8 students answered Problem 26 correctly
when c and b were used as the variables while 57% answered correctly when x
and y or Ψ and ɸ were used to represent the quantities instead.75 While students’
performance in this study changed depending on the letters used to represent the
quantities, other researchers caution against the assumption students treat the let-
ters as objects and encourage teachers to have students explain their rationale.76
In order to have a comprehensive understanding of the interaction between
numbers and variables and operations in a term such as “4c” students need to
understand the value of the number, the potential value of the letter, and that the
representation of 4 next to c implies multiplication. Students may rely on their
previous experiences and interpret 4c as “four of some object that is represented
by c.” On the other hand, if the student can understand 4c as that combination of
number, variable, and operation, in Problem 26 they must understand 3b in the
same way. Finally, the student must understand 4c + 3b as a similar encapsulated
quantity that represents the total cost of cakes and buns, with each variable signi-
fying potential values.77 What might seem simple to those of us who teach math—
and have been successful in many math classes beyond algebra—is actually quite
involved, with multiple pieces of understanding coming together to represent the
concept of the cost of cakes and buns.
Researchers have shown that when students were asked to use their intuition
and self-generate ways of representing a problem instead of relying on the teacher
or instructional materials, it led to better understanding of what the variable rep-
resented (see Algebraic Relations: Student Intuition and Informal Procedures).
Although, in the short term, the use of mnemonic devices such as “think of a as
apple(s) and b as banana(s)” may be beneficial, it may reinforce students’ naïve
conception that letters stand for labels instead of variables. We can’t teach that
variables don’t ever stand for objects. In day-to-day life, we use letters to stand
for objects all the time. Recipes call for 4c, or 4 cups, of flour. A grocery list might
have shorthand for items. To counteract students’ experiences, teaching variables
should include when letters stand for labels and when they stand for quantities;
this will help students understand the difference. It is important to help students
understand the range of uses of letters and when each use is appropriate, in what
context, and how the meaning of letters changes in each context.
Teachers
• Help students understand the many different uses of letters in algebra.
• Help students differentiate between letters as signifying objects and letters
as variables.
• Be aware of how your use of language in the classroom may encourage or
discourage students’ thinking of letters as objects.
• In initial work with story problems, not always using the first letter of the
item indicated when naming variables might help students understand what
variables represent.
Variables and Expressions • 65
Teachers
• Create opportunities for students to understand how letters can represent
more than one value, such as x + y = 10.
• Help students understand that testing one number in a situation is not suf-
ficient justification for an argument.
68 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
influenced students’ thinking about the possibilities for x and y. Students were
reluctant to pick values for either x or y that were greater than c.85 As in this prob-
lem, students typically did not pick numbers greater than 10 for either x or y. Few
students considered the possibility that c could be a negative number, constrained
by their thinking about the composition of the number 10. While many students
in the research had an understanding of the letter possibly representing more than
one number, few students in the research understood the infinite possibilities of
that relationship. A student with a sophisticated understanding of Letter Used as
a Functional Relationship is able to comprehend the full range of possibilities
for the letter in a mathematical context. A student with the letter used as a general
number level of thinking, on the other hand, may have some self-imposed con-
straints on what those numbers might be.
A comprehensive understanding of a letter used as a general number requires
that students:86
Teachers
• Help students understand a multiple-values interpretation of letters.
• Probe students’ answers to determine the extent of their understanding of
the range of possibilities. Would 3.3 work for c? Would -11? Would num-
bers between 4 and 11?
70 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
the two expressions. A student at the level of letter used as a functional relation-
ship recognizes how one set of values (2n and n + 2) changes as another set (n)
varies in a dependent relationship. A sophisticated understanding of this problem
requires students to view the variable as “something that can not only take on
multiple values, but must have those static values compared to each other.”89
This level of understanding is perhaps best seen in the problem noted in the
previous section on Letters Used as General Number: “If c + d = 10, and c is
always less than d, what values may c have?” It is not that c can be anything, but
that there is a dependent relationship. If c is 8.7, then d must be 1.3. The choice of
the independent variable dictates what the dependent variable will be. Students at
this level are able to take into account the “if . . . then” relationship and consider
constraints, understanding how the answer might change, depending on the value
of c. While Students #3 and #4 may be at that level of thinking with Problem 32,
it is not clear from their answer the extent of their reasoning, which would require
further probing.
“An understanding of variable implies the comprehension of all these aspects and
the possibility to shift between them depending on the problem to be solved.”91
Given that there are so many ways to interpret variables, it is important that stu-
dents develop the flexibility to adapt, depending on the specific situation. A rich
understanding of variable requires students to have facility in manipulating the
symbols, while maintaining a sense of what the variable represents in the problem.
Teachers
• Help students understand how changes in one variable affect the other
variable(s).
Variables and Expressions • 73
Problem 34 Problem 35
In the right-angled triangle 1. What happens to the triangle
the height is twice its base. as x changes?
What is the value of x? 2. For what value of x is the
height of the triangle twice its
base?
ALGEBRAIC NOTATION
Common Core Standards: 6.EE.1, 6.EE.2, 6.EE.3, 6.EE.4, 7.EE.4,
A-SSE.1, A-SSE.2, A-SSE.3, F-IF.2
The ability to think algebraically comes before the understanding of nota-
tion.94 Sometimes students’ struggles with algebraic notation are not based on the
mathematical concepts, but the mathematical conventions. “For students learning
formal notation, all they are given is a collection of someone else’s symbols.”95
For instance, it can be quite confusing for students that 4½ means 4 + ½ and 89
means 8 tens + 9 ones, but 4x means 4 multiplied times x.96 The symbol “3” can
have multiple meanings depending on its location, such as 34, 43, 43 , √3, and 4/3.
“Words gather meanings from the company they keep,”97 and so it is with the
symbols of mathematics.
There are specific ways of symbolizing in algebra that students need to learn.
Algebraic notation exists because we needed a way to be consistent, and to be
able to communicate mathematical ideas clearly. While many uses of notation
can seem second nature to teachers, they can be considered a similar challenge as
learning a new language for many students.98
Yet, as mentioned earlier, students as young as eight years of age can learn to
express algebraic relationships100 and operate on unknown values with letters.101
Consider the problems in Table 2.33.
Basic Operations
All students understand the concept of one person being taller than another
by a certain amount. The issue for students is how to express that algebraically.
In Problem 36, students who write h10, for instance, can intend the notation to
mean h + 10. This type of thinking is apparent in the interview for Problem 39.
The interviewer asks: “Are t + t and t + 4 ever equal? If so, when?” The student
responds: “t + t is 2t and t + 4 is 4t so t + 4 would be bigger.” In the next section on
Area, Perimeter, and Variables there are multiple examples of this type of think-
ing—writing a collection of variables and numbers with the operation missing,
but intended. As students begin formal algebra, they may just put terms together
because they don’t know what else to do, or they may generalize the mathematical
convention that 3y means 3 multiplied times y to 3y also being able to symbolize
3 + y. They may collapse h + 10 into h10 because they don’t have Acceptance
of Lack of Closure and need to come up with a single-term answer as described
previously with Problem 39.
Lastly, the sequence of natural English language is reading from left to right.
This may lead students to
read the symbol ab as a and b, and interpret it as a + b. Or the student may read the
expression 2 + 3a from left to right as 2 + 3 giving 5, and consider the full expression
to be the same as 5a.102
While this type of difficulty appears to diminish over time, as the data from Prob-
lem 37 suggests, with 93% of students answering correctly by 15 years of age,
students still had significant difficulty with Problem 38 in the same study. Stu-
dents in early algebra also have considerable difficulty interpreting the equal sign
as a balance of equal quantities, rather than a “get the answer” sign (see Algebraic
Relations: Variables on Both Sides). Ultimately, how problems are structured
may lead to the types of confusion with notation described here.
Students learning algebra may not understand when there is mathematical ra-
tionale for notation being the way it is, and when mathematicians just agreed on
a particular practice. Much of notation in algebra is arbitrary, and simply agreed
upon by mathematicians for the sake of convenience. This can lead students to
the perception that math is just a bunch of things to memorize without mean-
ing. Math teachers may contribute to this perception by using tricks or mnemon-
ics to help students learn mathematics, such as FOIL (first, outer, inner, last for
multiplying two binomials) or PEMDAS (parentheses, exponents, multiplication,
division, addition, subtraction for order of operations). These also can include
sayings for students to memorize, such as “collect together like terms,” “of means
76 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
multiply,” “add the same thing to both sides,” “change sides, change sign,” and
“to divide, turn upside down and multiply.”103 While helpful in that moment, it
may contribute to the feeling some students have: that the nature of mathemat-
ics is procedures you do by rote, rather than understanding. Furthermore, those
strategies may be limiting (e.g., what do you do when you have to multiply a
binomial with a trinomial?) or misleading (e.g., multiplication does not always
precede division in the order of operations).104 It is important for teachers to make
distinctions between what is arbitrary—agreed-upon notational conventions that
must be memorized—and what is necessary—procedures that have mathematical
rationale.105
Generalization
Perhaps the primary reason for algebraic notation is the need to generalize. In
arithmetic, mathematical symbols indicate concrete ideas with numbers. In al-
gebra, variables are introduced in order to represent mathematical ideas such as
unknown solutions (x + 9 = 13) and the character of possibilities or the relation-
ship in the pattern (5m = n). In the first case, a single answer is represented by the
letter. In the second, a general relationship is described that for every case of m,
n is five times as large.
In arithmetic as well as algebra, symbols typically refer to some reality. The
number 5 can refer to five apples in a basket, and a letter can refer to the solution
of the equation (x + 9 = 13) or the dependent part of the relationship (n depends
upon what m is). Understanding the reference106 and representing generalization107
are concepts that are typically very difficult for novice algebra students to under-
stand. One of the mental shifts in algebra is for students to be able to manipulate
symbols as a goal in itself—without any connection to a reference reality—but
based on rules regarding relationships between symbols and the notation. In other
sections of the book we discuss generalization in further detail, such as Letter
Used as General Number and Patterns and Functions: Promoting Generaliza-
tion. The main issue addressed here is not the act of generalization, but rather
developing students’ understanding of how generalization is represented in the
form of letters.
Student: I’d do two fives, two u’s, and one six (writes 25 + 2u + 16).
Interviewer: You’ve written two fives there. Now what do you mean by that?
Student: Oh! It should be smaller! Because otherwise you’ll think . . . (writes 2 with the five
5
as a subscript).
Interviewer: What do you mean by that?
Student: Two fives, multiply it. Two multiplied by five, two multiplied by u . . .
Interviewer: You do not mean twenty-five?
Student: No! Two lots of five. Oh . . . (writes 16 instead of 16).
Interviewer: Why did you write 162u?
Student: 5 plus 5 plus 6 is 16 and two “u’s”
fives, it is evident that, in the absence of clarity, students can be creative in using
such mathematical representations as subscripts. This confusion may come from
other experiences with symbols, such as chemical notation, in which CO2 means
a carbon atom plus two oxygen atoms. Students often struggle to understand the
meaning and purpose of subscripts such as y1 or Fn-1.109 For instance, in one study,
students would write Fn-1 + 2 = Fn as Fn - 1 + 2 = Fn or Fn - 1 + 2 = Fn, indicat-
ing that they didn’t really understand the difference between the notation and the
terms in the equation. In particular, using this type of notation for recursive equa-
tions is challenging, as the following interview suggests:110
The researchers found that only 12% of 34 seventh-grade students were able
to use the notation correctly as they engaged in recursive problems.111 Students
viewed Fn as a single symbol for the answer and interpreted Fn-1 as “back up one
from the answer,” that is, subtract one from the answer, rather than move to the
previous case before the nth case.
Another common wrong answer is when students express powers instead of
products (e.g., n2 instead of 2n). In one study, the misconception of using pow-
78 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
ers instead of products increased from 5% at Year 7 (11–12 years old) to 18% at
Year 10 (14–15 years old), indicating that new learning often pushes students to
reinterpret past learning and contributes to misconceptions.
Finally, there are many factors that may influence students’ interpretation of
algebraic notation:112
The bottom line is that students may have understanding of the algebraic con-
cepts, but struggle to determine how to represent that understanding in a way that
is deemed appropriate by the mathematical community. Rather than immediately
considering students’ incorrect answers as representative of incorrect thinking,
teachers may want to question students about the rationale behind their use of
algebraic notation to differentiate between understanding of concepts versus rep-
resentation of those concepts.
Teachers
• Help students understand the difference between mathematical convention
and mathematical necessity.
• Help students convey the meaning behind their use of notation, in order to
assess whether they struggle with representation of the answer/idea or with
mathematical concepts.
• Help students understand the utility of letters to represent generalization
not simply as a placeholder for a solution.
• Help students understand the difference between uses of notation in mathe-
matics that are different than uses of similar notation in other subject areas,
like science (e.g., CO2 versus x2 - x1).
Variables and Expressions • 79
42. Part of this figure is not drawn. There are n sides altogether, all of length 2.
• A number between 32 and 42 (25%)a
–– correctly with 2n (9%)
––33% gave a correct verbal description of how to get an
answer (such as “Two times however many sides there
are”) but were unable to give a symbolic representation
––the rest provided no answer
• A number between 32 and 42 (18%)b
––correctly with 2n (38%)
––the rest provided no answer
a
Percent of 13-year-old students in Booth (1984).
b
Percent of 14-year-old students in Hart and Kuchemann (2005).
80 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
a
Percent of 13-year-old students in Booth’s (1984) study.
Problems 43 and 44 in Table 2.37 focus on finding the area. Research, includ-
ing the interviews in Tables 2.38 and 2.39, suggests that there are four possible
reasons for students’ answers. The student:
Errors in thinking may occur at one of several points in the process of solving the
problem. Students may have misread the question or part of the problem, or, in
spite of a correct interpretation of the problem, students may have used an incor-
rect method in solving the problem. They also may have understood the problem,
but had difficulty putting the answer into a format that is understood by math-
ematicians. Considering an input-process-output model of students’ engagement
with problems may help gain a clearer picture of the points at which students’
understanding of the problem breaks down.
Problem 42 in Table 2.35 is a different style of perimeter problem that de-
mands an understanding of variables used for the purpose of generalization (see
Letter Used as a General Number). In order to deal with the unknown situation,
students may simply substitute a number or complete the figure in their minds.
The first student interviewed for Problem 42 (Table 2.40) simply ignored the let-
ters and incompleteness of the problem. The student simply counted the number
of sides, rather than consider the figure as going on “n” times, getting an answer
of “32.” Other students physically or mentally continued the shape, closed the
figure, and then counted, getting an answer such as “42.” These students may
have lacked the ability to generalize, or simply did not consider it as an option.
Students may have difficulty visualizing a figure in which there are many more
sides than what they can see.
The interview with the second student for Problem 42 demonstrates that stu-
dents are learning not only the mathematical procedures, but also the mathemati-
cal expectations. Similar to our earlier discussion of algebraic representation with
perimeter problems, the student here may simply need to learn what is “okay” in
mathematics and mathematical conventions. A comprehensive understanding of
Variables and Expressions • 83
Teachers
• Have students explain their thinking so you can determine their under-
standing of variable as collecting things or their understanding that vari-
ables represent values.
• Differentiate between students who are struggling with the concept of pe-
rimeter or area versus those who have conceptual understanding, but lack
the understanding of mathematical notation or conventions, and those who
struggle with what variables represent.
• If students have difficulty with letters in perimeter or area problems, con-
sider walking through a problem with just numbers to help them get the
overall understanding.
84 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
rather than equality”128 and functions as a separator symbol, rather than indicat-
ing an equivalent relationship: 8 Chinese for every 1 English.129 Reviewing the
meaning of the equal sign may be helpful here. The Interviewer suggests using the
letters C and E, but neither designates the meaning of those variables.
The student’s struggles from that point forward imply that he is using static
comparison and considers the letters to represent labels for Chinese (C) and Eng-
lish (E), rather than the number of Chinese or English people. Being careful to de-
fine the variables and continually refer to the variables as “the number of” rather
than simply “Chinese” or “English” may help students understand the role of the
variable in the problem. This mistake may have its roots in the English language,
in which adjectives precede nouns. Students who are used to writing “red car” and
“six students” in English may use the same logic in mathematical representation.
Teachers might unintentionally reinforce this tendency by switching between la-
bels, units, and variables readily when working on word problems. While these
are perfectly reasonable uses of letters in mathematics, these problems indicate
that students are not as adept at differentiating between the different uses. Re-
search also suggests that this same type of error (following a familiar pattern for
direct translation) may be due to students’ familiarity with the standard form of
an equation as ax = y.130
Some research suggests that using different letters than those that represent the
initial letters of words such as x and y or NC and NE may encourage a more accu-
rate use of variable, but the results are inconclusive.131 One strategy that appears
to help students not make the reversal error appears to be placing a multiplication
sign between the number and variable.132 Writing the problem out as 8 x E = 1 x
C may help students not think of the letters as labels.
Other research suggests that the reversal error is not solely evident with multi-
plication problems, but also with problems involving addition, and that syntactic
translation only describes a small subset of the reasons for students’ difficulty.
More broadly,
reversed equations and expressions are produced when students attempt to represent
on paper cognitive models of compared unequal quantities. . . . Students need expe-
rience in recognizing when a problem sentence needs to be reorganized before it can
be translated syntactically and instruction in how the reorganizing can be done.133
One study found that requiring students to alter their equation into a division
format (y/a = x) significantly increased their success with the Student/Professor
problem.134
In contrast, the interview in Table 2.43 demonstrates a student’s semantic
understanding of the Students/Professors problem. Researchers have found that
there is a significant connection between language and mathematics; in particular,
high language scores tend to be associated with high algebra scores.135 In order
for students to understand language, they need to understand symbols, syntax, and
Variables and Expressions • 87
ambiguity in the words used that need context for understanding. Similar skills
are needed to understand algebra.
One strategy suggested by researchers to address students’ difficulty with this
type of problem is to teach the concept of variables using visual representations
of data, such as tables or images, rather than reliance on problem-solving pat-
terns. “Generalizing from tables is significantly correlated with understanding the
variable concept.”136 For instance, assist students in drawing pictures or building
models for the problem. A chart that is developed prior to writing an equation may
be useful. For example:
Students should then translate the problem statement into an algebraic sentence.
Following this, they may substitute their numbers from the chart into the equa-
tion. Another study found that visual cues (i.e., boxes representing containers for
variables) as well as feedback based on test cases helped some students overcome
reversal errors.137
A strategy to address students’ tendency to simply use the same order as the
words for the equation —word order matching—is to give them a more familiar
context. For example, have students consider how to write the expression 5 less
than 11 as 11 - 5. In this case, as in the problems above, the representation of the
words does not follow the order of the words.
Finally, there should be explicit discussion to help students discover the short-
comings of the incorrect equation(s) that were generated by students. Creating
a hypothetical situation and plugging in numbers for S and P makes it clear that
88 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
the representation 6S = P does not accurately convey the sentiment that there are
more students than teachers.138 Teachers should also consider emphasizing the
meaning of a variable (as opposed to a label) and of the equal sign during this
activity.
Teachers
• Have students test their constructed equations from word problems with
different values to ensure the equations accurately reflect the situation.
• Help students identify key words and phrases that help them write equa-
tions while simultaneously having them consider broader ideas in the prob-
lem, and whether their translation makes sense with the broader problem.
• Encourage students to use multiple representations to help them accurately
construct equations from word problems, such as drawings, tables, and
graphs.
ENDNOTES
1 Kieran, 1992, and Stacey & MacGregor, 1997.
2 Schoenfeld & Arcavi, 1988.
3 MacGregor & Stacey, 1997, p. 16.
4 Wagner & Parker, 1993, p. 330.
5 MacGregor & Stacey, 1997, p. 15.
6 Trigueros & Ursini, 1999, and Trigueros, Ursini, & Reyes, 1996.
7 Schoenfeld & Arcavi, 1988, p. 421.
8 Based on Hart & Kuchemann, 2005, and May & Van Engen, 1959, from Usiskin, 1988, p. 9.
9 Usiskin, 1988, p. 9.
10 Usiskin, 1988
11 Usiskin, 1988, p. 17.
12 Bazzini, 1999, p. 113.
13 These six categories integrate the work of Küchemann, 1981, and Ursini & Trigueros’ work on
their 3UV model (2001, 2004, 2008).
14 Hart & Kuchemann, 2005 and Trigueros & Ursini, 2003
15 Stacey & MacGregor, 1997, and Weinberg, Stephens, McNeil, Krill, Knuth & Alibali, 2004.
16 Trigueros, Ursini, & Reyes, 1996, pp. 4–321.
17 Küchemann, Hodgen, & Brown, 2011
18 Falkner, Levi, & Carpenter, 1999.
19 Collis, 1974, 1978; Tall & Thomas, 1991, p. 126.
20 Chalouh & Herscovics, 1988, and Kieran, 1981.
21 Davis, 1975. Sfard, 1991, and Tall, 1994, frame this similarly, explaining that abstract math
can be seen as structural (objects) or operational (processes).
22 Booth, 1988.
23 Kieran, 1992.
24 Tall & Thomas, 1991, p. 127
25 Küchemann, 1981
26 Weinberg, et al., 2004
27 Weinberg, et al., 2004, p. 7
28 Weinberg, et al., 2004, p. 8
Variables and Expressions • 89
REFERENCES
Arzarello, F. (1991). Pre-algebraic problem solving. In J. P. Ponte, J. F. Matos, J. M. Matos,
& D. Fernandes (Eds.), Mathematical problem solving and new information tech-
nologies (pp. 155–166). NATO ASI Series F, 89. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Arzarello, F., Bazzini, L. & Chiappini, C. (1993). Cognitive processes in algebraic think-
ing: Towards a theoretical framework. In I. Hirabayashi, N. Nohda, K.Sheigematsu,
& F-L. Lin (Eds.), Proceedings of the 17th International Conference for the Psy-
chology of Mathematics Education, (Vol. 1, pp. 138–145). Ibaraki, Japan: Univer-
sity of Tsukuba, Tsukuba.
Arzarello, F., Bazzini, L., & Chiappini, G. (1994). The process of naming in algebraic
problem solving. Paper presented at the 18th Annual Conference of the International
Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (PME), Lisbon, Portugal.
Barrett, J. E., Clements, D. H., Klanderman, D., Pennisi, S.-J., & Polaki, M. V. (2006).
Students’ coordination of geometric reasoning and measuring strategies on a fixed
perimeter task: Developing mathematical understanding of linear measurement.
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 37(3), 187–221.
Bazzini, L. (1999). On the construction and interpretation of symbolic expressions. Paper
presented at the Proceedings of the First Conference of the European Society for
Research in Mathematics Education.
Booth, L. R. (1984). Algebra: Children’s strategies and errors. Windsor, Berkshire: NFER-
Nelson.
Booth, L. R. (1988). Children’s difficulties in beginning algebra. In The ideas of algebra,
K–I2, 1988 NCTM Yearbook (pp. 20–32). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers
of Mathematics.
Brown, M., Hodgen, J., & Küchemann, D. (2014). Learning experiences designed to de-
velop multiplicative reasoning: Using models to foster learners’ understanding. In P.
C. Toh, T. L. Toh, & B. Kaur (Eds.), Learning experiences that promote mathemat-
ics learning (pp. 187–208).Singapore: World Scientific.
92 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Carraher, D., Schliemann, A. D., & Brizuela, B. M. (2001). Can young students operate on
unknowns? In M. van den Heuvel-Panhuizen (Ed.), Proceedings of the 25th confer-
ence of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, (Vol.
1, pp. 130–140). Utrecht, The Netherlands: PME.
Chalouh, L., & Herscovics, N. (1983). The problem of concatenation. Paper presented at
the Fifth Annual Meeting of the North American Chapter of the International Group
for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Montreal.
Chalouh, L., & Herscovics, N. (1988). Teaching algebraic expressions in a meaningful
way. In A. F. Coxford (Ed.), The ideas of algebra, K–12 (1988 Yearbook, pp. 33–
42). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Christou, K. P., & Vosniadou, S. (2012). What kinds of numbers do students assign to
literal symbols? Aspects of the transition from arithmetic to algebra. Mathematical
Thinking & Learning, 14(1), 1–27.
Christou, K., Vosniadou, S., & Vamvakoussi, X. (2007). Students’ interpretations of lit-
eral symbols in algebra. In S. Vaosniadou, Baltas, A., & Vamvakoussi, X. (Eds.),
Reframing the conceptual change approach in learning and instruction. New York,
NY: Elsevier.
Clement, J. (1982). Algebra word problem solutions: Thought processes underlying a com-
mon misconception. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 13(1), 16–30.
Clement, J., Lochhead, J., & Monk, G. (1981). Translation difficulties in learning math-
ematics. American Mathematical Monthly, 88, 286–290.
Coady, C., & Pegg, J. (1993). An exploration of student responses to the more demanding
Küchemann test items. Paper presented at the 16th Annual Mathematics Education
Research Group of Australasia Conference.
Cohen, E., & Kanim, S. E. (2005). Factors influencing the algebra “reversal error.” Ameri-
can Journal of Physics, 73(11), 1072–1078.
Collis, K. (1974). What do we know abut K–14 students’ learning of algebra? Paper pre-
sented at the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics national symposium, The
Nature and Role of Algebra in the K - 14 Curriculum, Washington, DC.
Collis, K. F. (1978). Operational thinking in elementary mathematics. In J. A. Keats, K. F.
Collis & G. S. Halford (Eds.), Cognitive development: Research based on a Neo-
Piagetian approach (pp. 221–248). Bath: The Pitman Press.
Cooper, M. (1986). The dependence of multiplicative reversal on equation format. Journal
of Mathematical Behavior, 5(2), 115–120.
Cooper, T. J., Boulton-Lewis, G., Atweh, B., Pillay, H., Wilss, L., & Mutch, S. (1997). The
transition from arithmetic to algebra: Initial understanding of equals, operations,
and variables. Paper presented at the 21st Conference of the International Group for
the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Lahti, Finland.
Davis, R. B. (1975). Cognitive processes involved in solving simple algebraic equations.
Journal of Children’s Mathematical Behavior, 1(3), 7–35.
English, L., & Warren, E. (1994). The interaction between general reasoning processes
and achievement in algebra and novel problem solving. Paper presented at the 17th
Annual Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia Conference.
Falkner, K. P., Levi, L., & Carpenter, T. P. (1999). Children’s understanding of equality: A
foundation for algebra. Teaching Children Mathematics, 6(4), 232–236.
Filloy, E., & Rojano, T. (1984). From an arithmetical to an algebraic thought: A clinical
study with 12–13 years old. Paper presented at the Sixth Annual Meeting of the
Variables and Expressions • 93
North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Math-
ematics Education, Madison: University of Wisconsin.
Filloy, B., & Rojano, T. (1989). Solving equations: The transition from arithmetic to al-
gebra. for the learning of mathematics. An International Journal of Mathematics
Education, 9(2), 19–25.
Fisher, K. M. (1988). The students-and-professors problem revisited. Journal for Research
in Mathematics Education, 19, 260–262.
Fisher, K., Borchert, K., & Bassok, M. (2011). Following the standard form: Effects of
equation format on algebraic modeling. Memory & Cognition, 39(3), 502–515.
Franzblau, D. S., & Warner, L. B. (2001). From Fibonacci numbers to fractals: Recursive
patterns and subscript notation. Yearbook (National Council of Teachers of Math-
ematics), 2001, 186–200.
Fujii, T. (1993). A clinical interview on children’s understanding and misconceptions in
school mathematics. Paper presented at the 17th PME International Conference.
Fujii, T. (2003). Probing students’ understanding of variables through cognitive conflict
problems: Is the concept of variable so difficult for students to understand? Paper
presented at the 27th International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Educa-
tion Conference, Honolulu, HI.
Galvin, W., & Bell, A.W. (1977). Aspects of difficulties in the solution of problems involv-
ing the formulation of equations. Nottingham, UK: Shell Centre for Mathematical
Education, University of Nottingham.
Graf, E. A., Bassok, M., Hunt, E., & Minstrell, J. (2004). A computer-based tutorial for al-
gebraic representation: The effects of scaffolding on performance during the tutorial
and on a transfer task. Technology Instruction Cognition and Learning, 2, 135–170.
Greer, B. (1994). Extending the meaning of multiplication and division. In G. Harel &
J. Confrey (Eds.), The development of multiplicative reasoning in the learning of
mathematics (pp. 61–85). Albany, NY: SUNY Press.
Halford, G. S. (1978), An approach to the definition of cognitive developmental stages in
school mathematics. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 48, 298–314.
Hart, K., & Kuchemann, D. (2005). Children’s understanding of mathematics: 11–16. East-
bourne, UK: Anthony Rowe, Ltd.
Herscovics, N. (1989). Cognitive obstacles encountered in the learning of algebra. In S. W.
C. Kieran (Ed.), Research issues in the learning and teaching of algebra (Vol. 4, pp.
60–86). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Herscovics, N., & Linchevski, L. (1994). A cognitive gap between arithmetic and algebra.
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 27(1), 59–78.
Hewitt, D. (2012). Young students learning formal algebraic notation and solving linear
equations: Are commonly experienced difficulties avoidable? Educational Studies
in Mathematics, 81(2), 139–159.
Hodgen, J., Brown, M., Coe, R., & Küchemann, D. (2012). Surveying lower secondary
students’ understandings of algebra and multiplicative reasoning: to what extent do
particular errors and incorrect strategies indicate more sophisticated understand-
ings? In J. C. Sung (Ed.), Proceedings of the 12th International Congress on Math-
ematical Education (ICME-12) (pp. 6572–6580). Seoul, Korea: International Math-
ematics Union.
94 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Hodgen, J., Küchemann, D., Brown, M., & Coe, R. (2008). Children’s understandings of
algebra 30 years on. Paper presented at the British Society for Research into Learn-
ing Mathematics.
Kaput, J. (1992). Technology and mathematics education. In D. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook
of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 515–556). New York, NY:
MacMillan Publishing.
Kaput, J. J., & Sims-Knight, J. (1983). Errors in translations to algebraic equations: Roots
and implications. Focus of Learning Problems in Mathematics, 5(3), 63–78.
Kieran, C. (1981). Concepts associated with the equality symbol. Educational Studies in
Mathematics, 12, 317–326
Kieran, C. (1990). Cognitive processes involved in learning school algebra. In P. N. J. Kil-
patrick (Ed.), Mathematics and cognition: A research synthesis by the international
group for the psychology of mathematics education (pp. 96–112). Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Kieran, C. (1992). The learning and teaching of school algebra. In D. Grouws (Ed.), Hand-
book of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 390–419). New York,
NY: Free Press.
Kieran, C. (2007). Learning and teaching algebra at the middle school through college lev-
els. In F. Lester (Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching and
learning (pp. 707–762). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.
Kim, S.-H., Phang, D., An, T., Yi, J. S., Kenney, R., & Uhan, N. A. (2014). POETIC: In-
teractive solutions to alleviate the reversal error in student–professor type problems.
International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 72(1), 12–22.
Kinzel, M. T. (1999). Understanding algebraic notation from the students’ perspective.
Mathematics Teacher, 92(5), 436–441.
Kirshner, D., Awtry, Y., McDonald, J., & Gray, E. (1991). The cognitivist caricature of
mathematical thinking. Paper presented at the Thirteenth Annual Conference of the
North American Chapter of the International Group of the Psychology of Mathemat-
ics Education, Blacksburg, VA.
Knuth, E., Alibali, M., McNeil, N., Weinberg, A., & Stephens, A. (2005). Middle school
students’ understanding of core algebraic concepts: Equivalence & variable. ZDM,
37(1), 68–76.
Küchemann, D. (1978). Children’s understanding of numerical variables. Mathematics in
school, 7(4), 23–25.
Küchemann, D. (1981). Algebra. In K. M. Hart (Ed.), Children’s understanding of math-
ematics (pp. 102–119): London, UK: John Murray.
Kuchemann, D. (2005). Algebra. In K. M. Hart (Ed.), Children’s understanding of math-
ematics: 11-16 (11th revised ed.). United Kingdom: Antony Rowe Publishing Ser-
vices.
Küchemann, D., Hodgen, J., & Brown, M. (2011). English school students’ understanding
of algebra, in the 1970s and now. Der Mathematikunterricht, 57(2), 41–54.
Laborde, C. (1990). Language and Mathematics. In P. N. J. Kilpatrick (Ed.), Mathematics
and cognition: a research synthesis by the international group for the psychology of
mathematics education (pp. 53–69). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Lannin, J. K. (2005). Generalization and justification: The challenge of introducing alge-
braic reasoning through patterning activities. Mathematical Thinking & Learning,
7(3), 231–258.
Variables and Expressions • 95
Trigueros, M., & Ursini, S. (2008). Structure sense and the use of variable. Paper presented
at the 32nd Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathemat-
ics Education.
Trigueros, M., Ursini, S., & Reyes, A. (1996). College students’ conceptions of variable.
Proceedings of the XX PME International Conference (pp. 315–22.). Valencia,
Spain.
Ursini, S., & Trigueros, M. (2001). A model for the uses of variable in elementary algebra.
Paper presented at the 25th Conference of The International Group for the Psychol-
ogy of Mathematics Education., Utrecht, Netherlands.
Ursini, S., & Trigueros, M. (2004). How do high school students interpret parameters in
algebra? Paper presented at the 28th Conference of the International Group for the
Psychology of Mathematics Education, Bergen, Norway.
Usiskin, Z. (1988). Conceptions of school algebra and uses of variables. In A. Coxford
& A. Schulte (Eds.), The ideas of algebra, K–12 (pp. 8–19). Reston, VA: National
Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Wagner, S. (1981). Conservation of equation and function of variable. Journal for Re-
search in Mathematics Education, 12, 107–118.
Wagner, S., & Parker, S. (1993). Advancing algebra. In P. S. Wilson (Ed.), Research ideas
for the classroom: High school mathematics (pp. 120–139). New York, NY: Mac-
millan.
Wagner S., Rachlin S. L., & Jensen R. J. (1984). Algebra learning project—Final report.
Department of Mathematics Education, University of Georgia.
Warren, E. (1999). The concept of variable: Gauging students’ understanding. Paper pre-
sented at the 23rd Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of
Mathematics Education, Haifa, Israel.
Weinberg, A. D., Stephens, A. C., McNeil, N. M., Krill, D. E., Knuth, E. J., & Alibli, M. W.
(2004). Students’ initial and developing conceptions of variable. Paper presented at
the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Diego,
CA.
Wilson, K., Ainley, J., & Bills, L. (2005). Naming a column on a spreadsheet: Is it more
algebraic? Paper presented at the Sixth British Congress on Mathematics Educa-
tion, University of Warwick.
CHAPTER 3
ALGEBRAIC RELATIONS
INTRODUCTION
One of the fundamental components of an Algebra course is learning to solve
equations and inequalities. “Students’ understanding of core algebraic concepts of
variable and equivalence influences their success in solving problems, the strate-
gies they use, and the justification they give for their solutions.”1 They acquire a
new level of mathematical power when they comprehend the relationships and
processes of equation solving. Solving for x, finding x, and solving for the un-
known are all ways that we characterize the use of algebraic relations for a pur-
pose. However, 60% of 16-to-18-year-old students in one study believed that k =
5 was not an equation because there was no operation, 65% thought that a = a
was not an equation, and others thought that 3w = 7w – 4w was not an equation
because it was in the wrong order.2 A solid understanding of algebraic relations
begins with a strong understanding of what an equation is and the role of the
equal sign. In addition to these characterizations, the literature on the teaching
and learning of algebra tells us we must also consider the fundamental arithmetic
skills that students use when manipulating the terms in an expression and the
structure of the task itself. In the end, students need to become fluent enough
with these skills and concepts that they can apply solution strategies successfully
across a variety of tasks.
One- and Two-Step Equations1 represent a class of algebraic relations that
are often the easiest for students to solve. These equations often take the form of
ax + b = c where a, b, and c are constants. Students may visualize what has been
“done to x” and “undoing what has been done” as an informal way of approaching
the task that is often successful. For example, in the equation x – 6 = 7, students
can verbalize the task and say, “x is a number such that when I take away 6, I get
7.” From there, the transition to knowing that I can find x if I have 6 and 7 is a
small cognitive leap. The literature demonstrates that because of this, One- and
Two-Step Equations are a good entry point for learning to work with algebraic
relations.3
In this task, the student must operate on rational numbers with unlike denomina-
tors. Even if the student can accurately translate the task into an equation (see
Modeling: Translating Word Problems into Algebraic Sentences), those unable
to perform the necessary addition or subtraction will not be able to find the an-
swer. The literature indicates that students need to review fundamental operations
on rational numbers as they move into algebra. This review should begin with
whole numbers and continue through fractions, as students who have difficulties
with operations on fractions will struggle even more with operations on rational
expressions.
1
Bold and italicized print indicates a reference to a section in the book
Algebraic Relations • 101
In the same way that Rational Numbers are a challenge, students will often
struggle with Equations Involving Negative Numbers. These challenges take two
forms. First, students might not recognize that a negative number is in the expres-
sion as they focus their attention on other features like the presence of variables.
Second, once the negative expression is recognized, the student might not have
the necessary skills to operate on it. In the following series of tasks, we can see
these challenges illustrated:6
1. 12 – x = 7
2. 4–x=5
3. -4 – x = 10
4. -x = 7
The first task seems to be the easiest for students as it can be solved through rea-
soning (12 minus what is seven?). The remaining tasks are more complex as their
solutions are not as transparent. For example, what can I subtract from four to get
five? It is now in the student’s best interest to perform operations on this equation
to find the unknown value. But how do you “undo” subtraction of x? What do I
get when I take five from four? In these situations, the teacher might try to come
up with a representation of the equation that is fixed in the real world. However,
many of the models for looking at Equations Involving Negative Numbers are
problematic as well. Some of the issues involved with equations have their roots
in students’ conceptions of what variables can represent, particularly when nega-
tives are involved (see Variables & Expressions: Representation).
In some situations, students can perform operations accurately, but then fail
to understand the meaning of the simplified relation. Some algebraic relations
have elements that lead to solutions like 0x = 7, 0 = 7, or x = 0 once simplified.
Students will see these algebraic relations with Solutions Involving Zero and
misunderstand what can be done with the zero. Teachers will need to generate ad-
equate representations, either graphically or in words, that can demonstrate how
the students should interpret the resulting answer.
As students transition to more complex tasks, error patterns appear. When stu-
dents begin Solving Equations with Variables on Both Sides, they are prone to
arithmetic errors. Because these tasks are more complex, arithmetic errors can go
unnoticed by students: is difficult to reason the solution in words as they did in
One- and Two-Step Equations. Furthermore, when given an equation like 6x – 2
= 3x + 4, we often use informal language to characterize the procedures necessary
to simplify the equation. Language like “move the 3x to the other side” can lead
students to ignore the sign of the coefficient and make the following error:
6x – 2 = 3x + 4 6x + 3x – 2 = 4
The 3x term has “moved,” but the resulting equation is inconsistent with the
original. Similar difficulties can occur when students misunderstand the meaning
of the equal sign.7 Given an equation like 3x – 2x + 7 = 4x + 4, students might re-
spond by subtracting 4x from every x-term in the equation. The result is (3x – 4x)
– (2x – 4x) + 7 = (4x – 4x) + 4. The teacher’s challenge is to help students under-
stand the equal sign as the balance point rather than as an operator that implies a
flow from left to right to complete an answer.
Once students have progressed through One- and Two-Step Equations and
Solving Equations with Variables on Both Sides, it is common for algebra cur-
ricula to move to solving Inequalities. The procedures used for operating on in-
equalities are very similar on the surface to what students have experienced with
operating on equations. If the students focus too much on solving inequalities
algebraically, and neglect numeric and graphical solutions, the distinction of this
class of algebraic relations can be unclear. How is it different when the equal
sign is replaced by a “greater than” symbol? The procedural knowledge of how
to handle division by negative numbers is insufficient to prevent errors; students
need additional strategies to effectively operate on order relations. Once again, the
root of students’ struggles with inequalities may lie in their ability to understand
what a variable can represent. It is a big leap to comprehend how a variable can be
a range of possible solutions rather than a single answer (see Variables & Expres-
sions: Letter Used as a Functional Relationship).
We then discuss Factoring as a means of solving quadratic equations. Back
in 1989, NCTM advocated for a “decreased emphasis on factoring and an in-
creased emphasis on using computer utilities and graphing calculators to solve
problems that involve factoring.”8 Some researchers argue that factoring should
not be taught at all; it has very little applicability because it requires integer coeffi-
cients between 10 and -10. “Rote mechanical methods like FOIL (First Outer, In-
ner, Last for factoring trinomials) . . . contribute little to student understanding.”9
Instead, students should rely on completing the square and the quadratic formula
because those can be applied more universally.10 Yet most algebra classes still
spend quite a bit of time teaching students to factor. With the advent of phone apps
like Wolfram Alpha that will factor anything and show you the steps, solutions,
Algebraic Relations • 103
and representations, it is inevitable that factoring will lose its importance. That
said, we focus here on students’ struggles with understanding the mechanics of
factoring, but even more so on how they understand the meaning of solutions and
the structures involved in the process.
SOLUTION STRATEGIES
By understanding the different elements of algebraic relations and recognizing the
nuances of different types of algebraic relations, the student can develop a toolkit
of successful solution strategies without just seeing the toolkit as an unrelated list
of procedures to memorize. It should be our goal for students to have a variety
of strategies that they can use as needed. The strategies should be both formal
(graphing, algebraic algorithms, successive approximation) and informal (guess-
and-check, reasoning, intuition).
Formal procedures are a reliable way to solve algebraic relations. When em-
ployed properly, these procedures work every time, regardless of the complex-
ity of the task. However, students will never truly understand a procedure until
they have internalized it, realized the impact of operations on the structure and
not the solution (i.e., the answer doesn’t change when you add 3 to each side),
and made it their own. Watching repeated demonstrations of formal procedures
is insufficient. This does not mean that the teacher has no role in the develop-
ment of student understanding of procedures; rather, students need guidance when
developing new ideas. Such guidance can come in the form of a rich variety of
examples that offer the opportunity for students to see patterns and common char-
acteristics across a variety of scenarios. The development of student understand-
ing means that we nurture Student Intuition and Informal Procedures as well,
while exploring the role of formal procedures in their work. Informal procedures
that are mathematically correct but less efficient might make more intuitive sense
to students. Students can be confused if teachers imply the methods are incorrect,
rather than less efficient. Both formal and informal procedures enhance different
aspects of student understanding, and one should not be sacrificed for the other.
As students develop their toolkits for solving equations, it is critical that they
understand how these different strategies are related. They must also know how
and when to select the proper strategy, given a particular context. This is a dem-
onstration of Flexible Use of Solution Strategies. Knowing how to solve an equa-
tion by graphing it is an element of procedural knowledge. Knowing when it is
best to solve equations graphically is conceptual knowledge.11 For example, when
given an equation like 3x + 4 = 2 + 3x + 2, will the student employ algebraic
manipulation to solve it? Should we take the time to enter the equations into a
calculator? While both strategies can generate a correct answer, it would be most
efficient to examine the task and see that the two sides of the equation are the
same. We can immediately jump to the identity as the solution, and no further ac-
tion is needed. A quick check of a variety of values of x will confirm our idea. In
contrast, if asked to solve 3(x + 5) = 20, a graphical or algebraic solution might be
104 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
more efficient. Thinking through this task, or trying to plug in values for x, might
eventually give us a correct solution, but it would be laborious. Having a variety
of strategies, and knowing when to use each, is the hallmark of true conceptual
understanding.
•••••
-5x = 10
If students do not understand the symbols, they become confused about whether
to add 5 or divide by –5. In this case, teachers can ask students to compare and
contrast 5x, 5+x and 5*x. In each case, what has been “done to x”?
Four metaphors are often used to help students understand the ideas of an
equation:12
In regard to a story about a number, consider the problem [4(x – 1)– 2]÷ 2 =
9. This can be interpreted as a story: “There was a number x. You take away 1,
then multiply by 4, then subtract 2, then divide by 2. The answer is 9. The recipe
metaphor considers that problem similarly as the process of getting 9. Instead of
seeing the equation as a sequence of procedures, the balance model conveys the
sense of equilibrium between the sides (see Figure 3.1). The function machine
metaphor is discussed in detail in Patterns & Functions: Function Machines.
Research indicates that teachers may assume students automatically transfer the
metaphors to the new situation of an equation. However, students may remember
the procedures for solving, while not understanding equivalence between the left
and right sides of an equation. Each metaphor may help with certain aspects of the
concept of equation, but teachers should be aware of each metaphor’s limitations.
Linear equations in which the variable only appears on one side of the equal
sign are a good starting point for students learning to solve equations because
manipulation of variables is minimal. In this class of equations, the manipula-
tions involve explicit numbers. Previous misconceptions about number properties
can confound the solving of these types of equations, though. For example, in
the problem in Table 3.1, we see that the student does not understand when the
Commutative Property of Addition can be applied. Or, when given an equation
106 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
3
such as 5 x - = 1 , misconceptions about rational numbers can lead to incorrect
solutions. 2
To develop complete understanding of linear equations, students need to see
equations as a whole, recognizing them as an entire structure rather than seeing
them as an accumulation of numbers, unknowns, and operations. The main idea is
to transform the original equation into a simpler one with the solution x = a, based
on relationships between operations. Even when students understand the purpose
and procedures for solving, they do not necessarily understand the concepts.
In general, students who rely on computational thinking have more difficulty
solving equations than students who use relational thinking. Relational thinking
means that the students are able to take advantage of relationships between num-
bers to rewrite expressions in ways that make the computation easier. For ex-
ample, a student thinking relationally might rewrite 47 + 38 as 50 + 35, which is
somewhat simpler to compute. Knowing the properties of numbers and operations
is an advantage when moving from concrete numbers to abstract manipulation of
variables. Students struggle with the relations when they only spend time with
the procedures. Consider Problem 2 in Table 3.2, in which students are asked to
think conceptually about the impact of procedures on equations.13 The problem is
a bit more abstract and demonstrates the challenges that students face when they
don’t understand the relationships in equations. See Variables & Expressions:
Acceptance of Lack of Closure and Letter Evaluated for more insight regarding
this problem.
When introducing solution strategies for equations, teachers often begin with
simple equations where the answer is apparent and the algebraic procedures are
not necessary. However, giving students easier equations and insisting they use al-
gebraic methods to solve them does not encourage algebraic skill development or
conceptual comprehension of equivalence (see Student Intuition and Informal
Procedures). Instead, we should challenge students right away with equations
whose procedures require the use of algebraic methods, such as when the solu-
tions are not whole numbers.
Some teachers depend on models for use in introductions as well. It is impor-
tant to remember that what may seem like “real-life” situations may not be real for
all students. For example, the balance model (see Figure 3.1) may be confusing
for students who have never seen a balance. One must know the students and their
experiences when selecting a model that will make sense to them. Students should
be encouraged to verify that their solutions solve the original equation: checking
the validity of their solutions leads to improved confidence. However, care should
be taken when asking the students to justify their steps. If forced to explain their
steps, students may only do the steps that they can explain. It is critical that teach-
ers probe thinking to uncover misconceptions, rather than just looking at student
work.
One of the areas that has been well researched is students’ difficulties in under-
standing the equal sign.14 Researchers found that few preservice teachers under-
stand that many students “hold misconceptions about the equal sign.”15 Because
elementary students typically handle only problems that flow from left to right,
they often develop an “operational conception” of the equal sign. They see it as an
indication to “do something,” or as the end of the problem (“and the answer is...”),
instead of a symbol that signifies the equivalence of two expressions.16 Research-
ers have found that math textbooks encourage these misconceptions by “rarely
present(ing) equal signs in contexts most likely to elicit a relational interpreta-
tion,” such as having operations on both sides.17 As a result, students may believe
equations should be read left to right and therefore perceive an equation such as 5
= x + 2 as backward. Students with these conceptions of the equal sign ultimately
108 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
x+5 = 9
5 Æ -5
x = 9-5
FIGURE 3.2. “Change Sides, Change Signs.”
Algebraic Relations • 109
Teachers
• Equations with variables on one side are a good introduction to solving
equations.
• Encourage students to imagine the entire equation as a unit in balance.
Models can be helpful, but informal reasoning for simpler equations should
also be encouraged.
• Forcing students to apply procedures to simple tasks can frustrate them
and hinder their mathematical reasoning. Challenge students with difficult
tasks where procedures are tools that help.
• “Generalization can only develop from a broad range of experiences. So
students need to encounter, early on, equations which have other than small
positive integer coefficients, or solutions.”24
• Analyzing the nature of students’ errors when solving equations may be
insightful for making instructional decisions.25
• The Center for Algebraic Thinking’s Algebra Equation Builder app26 (see
Figure 3.3) gives students practice in maneuvering numbers, letters, and
operations in order to create a true algebraic equation with a variable on
one side of the equation.
RATIONAL NUMBERS
Common Core Standards: 6.NS.6, 6.EE.7, 7.NS.1, 2, 7.EE.1,3
Researchers find that students have many misconceptions when it comes to
fractions.27 One of the reasons for difficulties with fractions is that students are
often encouraged to memorize algorithms without developing an understanding
of them. Understanding rational numbers and being fluent with operations on ra-
tional numbers are important building materials for algebra. In order to be fluent
with rational numbers, students need to understand five concepts:
1. Part–whole relationships,
2. Ratios,
3. Quotients,
4. Measures, and
5. Operators.28
TABLE 3.3.
Problem 3: Rational Numbers as n Gets Very Large, What Happens to 1/n?
a) It gets close to 1
b) It gets close to 0
c) It gets very large
Source: Brown & Quinn, 2006.
12 3
- is about how much?
13 7
a) 1 15.4% chose a) or c)
32.2% said they didn’t know
1
b)
2
c) 0
d) I don’t know.
Percent of 143 students aged 13–15 years in Brown & Quinn (2006)
a
112 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
x + y 1+ y2 x 1
= or =
x2 x x + y2 1+ y2
Problem 7
x + 3 x − 10 ( x + 5)( x − 2) ( x + 5) 5
2
= = =
x 2 + 2 x − 8 ( x + 4)( x − 2) ( x + 4) 4
Source: Hall (2002); Rossi (2008)
x2 term and stopped, and 11.1% factored the numerator and denominator correctly
but did not cancel the (x – 2) term.31 Generally, students get the idea that they can
cancel terms from the top and bottom but misunderstand when that is appropriate.
Teachers
• Researchers suggest the following strategies for helping students make the
transition to algebra with a solid understanding of rational numbers:32
• Fraction operations should be developed as a generalization of whole num-
ber operations, and students should be given the chance to construct their
own algorithms.
• The development of the formal definitions of fraction operations should
progress through students’ experiences and algorithms, and also prepare
students for the abstraction necessary in algebra.
• When teaching about fractions, start with manipulation of concrete objects
and the use of pictorial representations such as unit rectangles and number
lines.
• Develop fraction notation, but avoid introducing formal procedures. Stu-
dents need tasks that require reasoning above formal steps.
• Focus on building a broad base of experience that will be the foundation
for a progressively more formal approach to learning fractions.
Some of the manipulatives that could be used to develop the meaning of frac-
tion and operations with fractions are Cuisenaire Rods, pattern blocks, Geoboards,
number lines, and fraction bars. These manipulatives are commonly available
tools found in many schools and are also freely available as online tools through
Shodor, NCTM, and the National Library of Virtual Manipulatives. Another tool
Algebraic Relations • 113
is the Center for Algebraic Thinking app, Algebra Card Clutter (see Figure 3.4),
which can help students develop understanding of the relative sizes of fractions
by having students organize cards from lowest to highest.
114 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Students begin learning about integers with the assumption from early arithmetic
that addition increases quantities and subtraction decreases.37 This can be a diffi-
cult, naive conception for students to let go of and may impact their understanding
of negatives when variables are involved.
Researchers have found that students’ difficulties with negative numbers fall
into three categories of understanding:38
At the heart of this issue is students’ typical difficulty with basic arithmetic
with negative numbers. Teachers will see consistent errors when students lack a
strong knowledge base for operations on negatives, reflecting these categories of
errors. Operationally, these errors can be classified into five types:42
1. Adding a number to its additive inverse (the student has ignored the
negative sign):
-3 + 3 = 6
-8 + 3 = 5 or 11 or -11
-3 – 8 = -5, 5, or 11
116 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
3–8=5
5. Subtracting a positive number from 0 (the student has ignored the zero,
assuming it does nothing):
0 – 8 = 8 or 0
There are other difficulties that arise with negatives in equations, separate from
those arithmetic difficulties. Equations are full of other symbols. For instance, it
is easy for a student’s eye to skip over minus signs while trying to focus on “more
important” things like variables. It is not always clear to a student what way is
appropriate to eliminate an unwanted term or factor from an equation when that
term or factor includes a minus sign.
Arithmetic issues are compounded when considering negative variables, as
many students have significant difficulty interpreting the value of a variable when
it is negative (see Variables & Expressions: Representation). When presented
with a task like 13 – x = 5, students can often reason through the task without
formal operations in the same way elementary school students might deduce what
goes in the box for a task like 13 – □ = 5. However, researchers find that students
can lose sight of this intuitive approach to problems and instead struggle to get
x by itself procedurally (see Students’ Intuition and Informal Procedures). In
13 – x = 5, the problem is simple intuitively but complex procedurally (having
to deal with the negative of a variable). The negative in this problem does not
pose as much of a cognitive challenge for students, perhaps because it is simple
subtraction of a smaller positive number from a larger; so, in a sense, there are no
“negative” numbers involved. This changes, though, as the tasks become more
grounded in integer computations. Examples like 5 – x = 7 or -5 – x = 11 pose
more of a cognitive challenge.
As students begin solving multi-step algebraic relations (see Solving Equa-
tions with Variables on Both Sides), the negative terms continue to cause prob-
lems. In the following task, a student has detached the negative preceding the 3x
and this produces the error.
2 – 3x + 6 = 2x + 18
-2x -2x
2 – 1x + 6 = 18
thinking. Incorrect answers can result from either misconceptions or simple mis-
takes, and it is important to determine the difference. In the conversation in Table
3.6, an interviewer poses a task and the student presents and defends an incorrect
response.
Initially, this error appears to be an error of the third type described above. If
we only look at the answer (-5), then we cannot know if the student thinks that
the smaller number should be subtracted from the larger number or if the student
has ignored the sign. Based on the verbal response, it is still unclear which of the
errors has occurred (or if this is a new type of error). In this situation, the teacher
needs to consider how to frame questions to access the student’s way of thinking
in order to have a clear understanding of the mathematical issues at play. Perhaps
the teacher could ask, “How is this different from 8 – 3 or -8 + 3?”
We know that it is easier for students to learn to work with positive numbers
because they can map their understanding of operations and properties to physical
materials. Developing a similar way of understanding negative numbers is harder
because negative amounts do not appear physically in their world. A comprehen-
sive understanding of “negative” includes:43
• An understanding of subtraction/operations,
• An understanding of direction,
• An understanding of value or magnitude, and
• A symmetrical understanding of opposites.
-2–3, which is arithmetically correct but creates confusion between the negative
sign and the sign for the subtraction operation. This shortcut could confuse stu-
dents when they try to develop the meaning of the negative sign as distinct from
the subtraction sign. The money/debt metaphor can confuse students in problems
like (-8) – (-2). Some students tend to translate this expression incorrectly as “You
owe $8 and then you owe another $2.” This ignores the fact that the subtraction
sign in the middle indicates that you are taking away from your debt, which does
not necessarily make sense in the students’ world, although it may in the real
world.
Having a mental number line is also often considered a component of number
sense that helps to build an understanding of negative numbers.46 Internalization
of negative numbers is the stage when a person becomes skillful in performing
subtractions. Understanding the numerical system and relative size of the num-
bers, including the number zero, is an important part of having a mental number
line. Absolute value is also an important and very powerful concept to compre-
hend in this aspect.
The problem -x = 7 is one of the most difficult equations for students to under-
stand. They consider -x statically just like -4 and treat them as the same. Students
can struggle with the idea that -x = 7 could give x = -7 because to them, the right-
hand side of the original equation would have had to be -7. A problem such as 6x =
7 is easier for students to work than -5x = 10. They may not recognize that -5 and x
are being multiplied. The negative sign persuades students that there is a subtrac-
tion of 5 that needs to be reversed with addition rather than multiplication by -5.
It is essential that students are grounded in variables and notation before solv-
ing equations. Students need to distinguish between the x and the minus sign as
being two distinct pieces of information. Being precise with language is important
when working with negative numbers. For example, in a problem such as “6 – -3,”
saying “6 minus minus 3” rather than “6 subtract negative 3” can hinder students’
understanding.47 Thinking of the negative sign as signaling the opposite of x could
help with relational reasoning (see Variables & Expressions: Representation for
further discussion on students’ thinking with negative variables).
Teaching Strategies
Once teachers can recognize potential student errors and have an understand-
ing of the mathematical issues involved, the focus moves to teaching strategies
that are effective for addressing these misconceptions. Using different models for
negative numbers and knowing the pros and cons of each model will help students
to understand negative numbers and their operations.48 Teachers can then move
to a parallel discussion using negative coefficients of variables. Researchers have
found that reasoning about negative numbers using metaphors can be helpful to
some for understanding their use in that narrow context, but confusing to others
when they don’t already understand the concept.49 In fact, “integers do not appear
as tools that are necessary for solving problems in the world, but as an artificial,
Algebraic Relations • 119
purely mathematical lens that we can apply to real-world contexts when that is
the game to be played.”50 The “real-world” problems we typically use in school
do not need negative numbers to be solved. In any case, teachers should alternate
among models to compensate for the imperfection of each model; however, such
alternations should be done in concert with pointing out the issues in the model so
students can appreciate how negative numbers can be used.
Some commonly used models are:
• Debt or owing: We need to be careful as we use this model because not all
operations can be modeled naturally in this way. For example, subtracting
a negative number from a negative number can become problematic for
some students. Instead of the debt or owing model for negative numbers
and operations, we can use the story of a letter carrier delivering checks
or bills to an address and the person at the address keeps track of the bills
and checks as they are delivered. In this story line, subtracting a negative
number from a negative number can be conveyed by delivering a bill to the
wrong address and the postman comes back to take away the wrong bill.
• Two-color integer chips (sometimes called Equilibrium): One color repre-
sents positive numbers, and the other color represents the negative num-
bers. With this model, most of the operations are modeled naturally. How-
ever, for subtracting negatives, pairs of negative and positive chips need to
be generated, and this is somewhat artificial. For example, for 4 – (-3) four
black chips are displayed then 3 pairs of black and white chips are formed
to display zeros, then three white chips are taken away to display the value
of the operation, 7, with seven black chips (see Figure 3.5).
• Elevators: Numbers are used to represent both position (the third floor)
and the action (going up three floors). It might be hard for students to grasp
these two meanings of the number; however, this is a model that aligns well
with the number line.
• Time: A scale from B.C. to A.D. could be used to model numbers. How-
ever, time is already a hard concept for students to grasp, and adding and
subtracting dates does not make much sense.51
• Temperature: This model may be beneficial to use if students already have
a conceptual understanding of temperature, particularly what it means to be
“below zero.” However, if they don’t grasp the temperature idea it might be
hard for them to understand what it means to add or subtract temperatures.
This is another model that aligns well with the number line, though, for
those students who do understand temperature.
• Number line model for operating with negative numbers: In this model,
addition and subtraction operations indicate which direction you face: for
addition, you face the positive numbers and for subtraction, you face the
negative numbers. The sign of the number indicates how you move on
the number line: if you have a positive number, then you move forward
120 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
in the direction you are facing; if you have a negative number, you move
backward in the direction you are facing. For example, for (-7) + (-2) you
start at -7 on the number line and since you are adding, you face positive
numbers. Since you have -2, you move backward 2 units facing positive
numbers and land on -9. This model helps students to operate with negative
numbers systematically. However, it is possible for students to memorize
the steps to produce the answers without really understanding the meaning
behind the operation. Thus, providing an alternative model to develop the
meaning of negative numbers is highly suggested. The Hop the Number
Line app (see Figure 3.6) can be a useful tool for helping students practice
their thinking with addition and subtraction of integers on the number line.
• Computer animations: Animations, such as a seagull flying above an ocean
and then diving into the sea after fish, have been found helpful for students
trying to understand the dynamic between negative numbers.52
Understanding the benefits and drawbacks of these models is helpful when de-
signing lessons or addressing individual student needs. As students move beyond
arithmetic operations, researchers find that there are additional teaching strategies
when working with equations that contain negative terms:
• Relational thinking (“the same as”) can help students solve questions like
5 – x = 7 or -5 – x = 11. Teachers should emphasize that 5 – x is the same
as 7. Simpler tasks could be solved using reasoning instead of procedural
operations that introduce the potential for errors. This is also an effective
strategy for Solving Equations with Variables on Both Sides.
Algebraic Relations • 121
• Asking students to justify (not just describe) their steps in writing forces
them to do only steps that they can explain. Ignoring signs becomes dif-
ficult under such types of questioning, and defending the validity of their
solutions may also improve students’ confidence.
• Common errors should be discussed at the appropriate point, both in les-
sons and in textbooks (especially at the introductory level), because it pre-
vents development of bad habits and misconceptions on the part of the
student.
• Interviewing students rather than just looking at written work will help
uncover misconceptions. This is especially true for students who are Eng-
lish Language Learners, as they may have differing conceptions of various
terms.
• Algebra tiles extend the strategy of two-color integer chips to variables.
Positive and negative x are represented by different colors and are different
sizes than positive and negative numbers (see Figure 3.7).
Teachers
• Students need to have strong arithmetic skills in order to solve equations
in algebra. Review of operations on negative numbers is critical even in
algebra.
• Take the opportunity to interview students as well as analyze their indi-
vidual work. This can reveal the actual mathematical issues underlying
misconceptions and faulty logical reasoning.
• Use models for reviewing operations on negative numbers. Researchers
found metaphors as generally useful depictions of positive and negative
numbers.
• The Center for Algebraic Thinking offers two apps to give students practice
with Integers. The first, Hop the Number Line (see previous Figure 3.6),
asks students to place the rabbit on the correct spot on the number line in
order to combine the integers accurately. The second, Algebra Card Clut-
ter (see previous Figure 3.4), has students organize cards from lowest to
highest. It has levels for students to organize cards that include fractions,
decimals, exponents, absolute value, and square roots.
Algebraic Relations • 123
INEQUALITIES
Common Core Standards: 6.NS.7a, 6.EE.5, 8, 7.EE.4b
For many students, inequalities represent a difficult next step after equations.
The Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS) asked stu-
dents to solve the inequality: 9x – 6 < 4x + 4. Only 17% of eighth-grade students
internationally could solve that problem accurately.53 In the United States, 21%
correctly solved the inequality. The procedures for solving inequalities are de-
ceptively similar to those for solving equations, but there are significant differ-
ences that students often overlook. For example, division by a negative number
requires a change in the sense of the inequality. Why does it change? Sometimes
we ask students to “break apart” the inequality and develop two simultaneous
solutions. Sometimes it is best to simply reason about the inequality as a com-
parison of two expressions. Sometimes students need to simplify the inequality
down to a single appearance of the variable. When should that happen? Why?
Teachers can help students recognize these differences between solving
equations and solving inequalities. When teachers introduce inequalities, stu-
dents will try to connect what they already know about how to solve equations.
In word problems, the context of a problem can help students recognize the
differences between situations of inequality and equality.54 Students must real-
ize that with an inequality, you are not seeking a precise answer: the inequality
has many potential solutions—an interval of values. Students often depend on
algebraic methods for solving equations and inequalities, and use strategies in-
terchangeably (and sometimes incorrectly). This misconception is understand-
able because equations and inequalities “look” similar. Students struggle with
solving linear and quadratic inequalities, especially if they are provided with
only one method of solving them. Typical student difficulties when solving in-
equalities are:55
1. Is there a value for x that will make (2x – 6)(x – 3) < 0 true?
2. Let p and q be odd integers between 20 and 50. For these values, is
5p – q > 2p + 15 always true, sometimes true, or never true?
3. Is there a value for x that will make the following statement true?
Each of these problems does not require students to solve for x or simplify. Re-
searchers suggest that this approach may be effective at “eliciting a greater vari-
ety of anticipatory behaviors”57 because it can encourage students to predict the
answer, rather than perform any actions. In Question 1, the factored form of the
inequality is 2(x – 3)2. Students need to recognize that no values can make that
inequality true because you will only get positives with the squared term. In Ques-
tion 2, the task is not looking for a single solution, but rather whether it can be
true and when. In Question 3, students should be able to recognize the structure
of the problem—both sides contain 6x – 8 – 15x and 12 > 6—and deduce that any
value would work. It might be helpful for students to begin with guess-and-check
to get a sense of the question, and then see where graphical and algebraic solu-
tions take them.
Question 3 taps into the similar ideas that are mentioned in Algebraic Rela-
tions: Student Intuition and Informal Procedures. That is, instead of automati-
cally beginning to use a set of procedures, it is good for students to look at the
structure of the inequality and recognize the commonalities that lead to a conclu-
sion about the relationship. Consider two students’ responses to Question 3 in
Table 3.7. Both students combined like terms to simplify the equation. However,
Student 2 began by noticing that the expressions in the parentheses were the same.
Teachers should encourage students to develop and work with their intuition to
seek understanding first, and then what procedures might be appropriate.
Second, when introducing inequalities, it can be helpful to explicitly distinguish
between different classes of equations and expressions. In each of the following,
students can have difficulties understanding them as equations or inequalities:58
In particular, researchers have found that students struggle with logical con-
nectors and knowing whether a response requires “or” or “and.”61 For instance,
x −5
with the inequality < 0 , students wrote the answer x – 5 > 0, x + 2 < 0 with-
x+2
out any logical connection. This is also true of inequalities with absolute value
signs, such as |x| < 3 and |x – 2| < 1, which 65% and 77.1% of high school students,
respectively, were unable to solve with the correct logical connection.62
Research indicates that students have more success with graphical approaches
than algebraic approaches because the “visual characteristics can help students to
understand the differences between equalities and inequalities and the meanings
of the different signs used to express these relationships.”63 Graphing each side of
an inequality as a separate graph and doing a logical comparison can help promote
an understanding of the result. As noted above, students generally have quite a bit
of difficulty with logical connectives. They have a difficult time understanding
when “or” or “and” is appropriate for a solution, as is evident for the problem in
Table 3.9 and the interview in Table 3.10.64
If students are already familiar with graphing functions, then solving inequali-
ties with graphic methods can help them to understand the solution better. The
x−5
<0 x+2<0x–5>0
x+2
x < -2 x>5
Source: Tsamir, Almog, & Tirosh, 1998; Tsamir & Almog, 2001
Algebraic Relations • 127
graphical method for solving inequalities also helps these students to answer ques-
tions related to the functions (solutions, etc.). Furthermore, by focusing on graphi-
cal methods of solving equations and inequalities, students see representations of
equations and inequalities that look different, they are less likely to incorrectly
apply strategies from one type to the other, and the visuals help them interpret
the results65 (also see Algebraic Relations: Flexible Use of Solution Strategies).
Teachers
• Choose tasks that demand more than just a procedure to identify an answer.
Tasks should highlight the ways that inequality solutions differ from solu-
tions to equations.
• Take time to analyze the features of different tasks in algebra (inequalities,
different classes of equations, expressions, etc.). Students need to know the
meaning behind the form of each task.
• Provide more than one method to solve inequalities and help students to
choose a method they understand well.
• Help students develop the habit of thinking about what the inequality ques-
tion is asking first, before starting to manipulate the variables and numbers.
• Research suggests that having students compare inequalities with the equal
sign “facilitated a relational understanding of the equal sign more than a
lesson in which students learned about the equal sign alone in the same
amount of time.”66
• The Center for Algebraic Thinking’s Inequality Kickoff app (see Figure
3.8) gives students practice in thinking about inequalities with variables.
128 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
In step 1, some students may ignore the sign of the variable and just “move the
x to one side.” In this case, we would see 8x + 3 = -5. In step 3, it is common for
students to make one of two errors. Some students divide a negative by a nega-
tive and get a negative answer. Others “divide backward” and incorrectly get the
error of x =. These errors can be extended when there are multiple like terms on
one side. When working on tasks like 5x – 3x + 1 = 4x – 3, it can be common for
students to add 3x to every x term regardless of the equal sign. Simply teaching
students to combine like terms prior to adding or subtracting terms from both
sides of the equation may miss addressing this apparent lack of understanding of
the equal sign’s role. Another common error for students is the “deletion error,” in
which they eliminate numbers and variables incorrectly.67 For instance, 2yz – 2y
may be equated to 2z (because students may misapply the fact that 2y – 2y = 0),
or 3x + 5 = y + 3 is simplified to x + 5 = y (because students believe 3x – 3 = x).
Growing research indicates the power of worked examples for students learn-
ing to solve equations.68 In particular, comparison of correct and incorrect solu-
tions can help students focus on certain features of a problem or typical mis-
takes.69 For instance, in Table 3.11 Alex and Morgan each simplified the given
expression differently and solved an equation differently. Researchers found that
students who compared and contrasted alternative solution methods increased
their procedural proficiency more than students who simply reflected on the same
solution methods one at a time.70 An important component in the success of these
comparison tasks is teachers’ ability to bring out the important mathematics in the
discussions of the problems.
130 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
During discussions, students should be required to justify (not just report) their
steps. Asking the students to justify their steps forces them to only do steps that
they can explain. This is difficult when solving equations is taught as a system
of procedures, rather than using algebraic properties. Students should explicitly
use the reflexive, symmetric and transitive properties of equality to improve their
understanding of the use of equal sign and teachers should reinforce the idea of
inverses.71 For example, instead of going from x + 6 = 3x + 1 directly to 2x + 1
= 6, teachers could show the in-between steps by indicating the properties being
used: “x + 6 = 3x + 1 subtracting x from both sides, we get 6 = 2x + 1 and by the
symmetric property of equality, we arrive at 2x + 1 = 6.”
During instruction, solving single unknown equations by introducing a second
variable can translate an algebraic problem to a graphical one. For example, 2x
+ 3 = 4x + 1 can be rewritten as y = 2x + 3 and y = 4x + 1. These two functions
can then be compared graphically to find the common solution. Common errors
should be discussed and analyzed at the appropriate point, especially at the intro-
ductory level, taking care to not create confusion. Such discussion helps students
recognize faulty reasoning, prevent formation of students’ bad habits and devel-
opment of inaccurate constructions, and see errors as opportunities for learning.72
As students can have selective attention, there is the danger that they will simply
remember “the teacher said something about…” and store the error in memory.
It is important that when teachers are proactive in discussing common errors that
they formatively assess the impact of that instruction to ensure that the message
intended was the message received.
There are different challenges when there are multiple appearances of a vari-
able in an equation than with a single appearance of a variable. The complexity in-
creases when a second variable is introduced, such as in the following problem:73
The perimeter of a rectangle is five times its width. Its length is twelve meters.
What is its width?
The problem results in the equations P = 5a, P = 24 + 2a. Researchers find that
“algebraic competences that deal with handling a single unknown are not sponta-
neously extended to two-unknown cases.”74 Just because students understand how
Algebraic Relations • 131
to solve equations such as 2x + 5 = 9 does not mean they will understand the dy-
namics when there are two different variables involved. One of the abilities need-
ed to solve the perimeter problem above is based on a more complex understand-
ing of the equal sign. Besides the understanding of the equals sign as signaling
the same value on both sides of the equation, “the notion of substitution is also an
important part of a sophisticated understanding of mathematical equivalence.”75
The idea of substitution, in this case, is based on the transitive property: if a = b,
and b = c, then a = b; c can be substituted in for b in the first equation. For the
above problem, P can be replaced in the second equation with 5w from the first
equation to get 5a = 24 + 2a. The sense of “sameness” for equivalence must ex-
tend not only within an equation, but across equations.76 This structural sense of
equality is the next step in advancing students’ mathematical power. Consider the
construct map of the development of the idea of equivalence in Table 3.12.
Teachers
• Require student to justify solutions using algebraic properties, rather than
just reporting steps.
• Introduce graphical solutions as a way to understand the meaning of a lin-
ear equation with variables on both sides. Consider graphing each side of
the equation separately prior to transformation.
132 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
FACTORING (FACTORIZATION)
Common Core Standards: A-REI.4.b, F-IF.8.a
Textbooks describe factoring as the reversal of multiplying polynomials or
changing an expression from a sum of terms to a product.77 Students have varying
beliefs about what factoring is:78
Reversal: The process of undoing or reversing a multiplication by apply-
ing the distributive property or multiplication of binomials or
both.
Deconstructive: The process of breaking down or simplifying an expression.
Evaluative: A process that you can check by multiplying out your answer.
Formal: A factored expression that is in the form of a product.
Numeric: Factoring has to do with decomposing numbers into products of
primes.
Each of these beliefs includes some truth about factoring. Students may hold more
than one of these beliefs simultaneously, but if they only hold one in isolation,
they are likely to struggle. For instance, a student that only sees factoring as rever-
sal, or ‘unFOILing’ as some students put it, might factor (3ax2 – 4ax) as (3x – 4)
(ax + 0) and be unable to see how to factor (3x + 6 – ax – 2a). A student who has
the deconstructive belief might factor (x2 + 5x – 24) as x(x + 5 – 3*8), changing
each term into its factors. A student with the numeric belief may focus on the
numbers, such as factoring (x2 + 5x – 24) as (x2 + 5x + 3*2*2*2) or (3ax2 – 4ax)
as (3axx – 2*2ax). It is important for teachers to understand what students believe
factoring is, as that will guide how students approach problems. Knowing their
belief systems, teachers can work to expand any limited definitions.
When working with quadratic equations, students are often confused by the
various forms that quadratics take:79
Students need to recognize when a particular format is the most appropriate for a
context and how the structure of each form lends itself to different kinds of infor-
mation about the function. They need the procedural skills to be able to transform
an equation into one of the forms or from one form into another easily.
There are the basic methods for solving quadratic equations:
• Factoring
• Factoring by grouping
• Applying the quadratic formula
• Completing the square
134 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
1. The inspection method. 3x2 can be factored with 3x and x. 2 can be fac-
tored as 2 and 1 or -1 and -2. Based on the middle term of 7x the factored
form is (3x + 1)(x + 2).
2. The decomposition method. Multiply 3x2 and 2 to get 6x2. Decompose
7x into the sum of two terms whose product is 6x2 or 7x = 6x + x. Factor
3x2 + (6x + x) + 2 by grouping: 3x2 + 6x + x + 2 = (3x2 + 6x) + (x + 2)
= 3x(x + 2) + 1(x + 2) = (3x + 1)(x + 2).
Strategies such as the diamond method may help students organize their thinking
and figure out the factors for trinomials (See Figure 3.12 for factoring the equation
x2 – 8x + 15 = 0. See the Center for Algebraic Thinking’s Diamond Factor app
(Figure 3.9) for practice on that skill).
Understanding the dynamic and patterns between numbers in multiplication
and division is at the heart of factoring equations. Many secondary mathematics
students struggle with basic multiplication table facts, negative factors, and mul-
tiple factors. This may “make factoring simple quadratics (ax2 + bx + c, a = 1) a
FIGURE 3.9. Two Numbers That Multiply to the Top Number and Add to the Bot-
tom Number from the Diamond Factor App.
Algebraic Relations • 135
Researchers found that students without structure sense and with only procedural
understanding tend to remove any parentheses or brackets first.88 In interviews
about strategies for solving quadratic equations, 11th-grade students said: “I get
rid of the brackets. The fewer brackets the better” and “First I always open the
brackets.”89 In one study, over 80% of errors solving the problem (2x – 3)(x + 2)
= 0 involved expansion errors.90 Table 3.13 shows some of the common errors
found. As mentioned earlier, an important part of learning about factoring is de-
veloping structure sense—not just focusing on getting solutions, but knowing the
role of parentheses and having a sense of purpose for manipulations.
Students also tend to make the following error:
x2 – 10x + 21 = 12
⇒ (x – 7)(x – 3) = 12
so, either x − 7 = 12 or x – 3 = 12.
They do not understand the null-factor law or remember why or when you want
to set each binomial equal to a number (zero). Students may follow procedures
blindly, leading to generalization of those procedures inappropriately, as in this
case. Sometimes students may also choose the constants in the binomial expres-
sions as the solutions rather than setting each factor equal to zero to solve. For
example, students might say that the solutions of (x + 3)(x + 5) = 0 are 3 and 5.
It is important to reinforce students’ understanding that they are looking for what
value of x would make each binomial factor equal zero (x + 3 = 0 x = -3 or x + 5 =
0 x = -5).91 In Table 3.14 the interview demonstrates that some students might see
the two solutions of a quadratic equation as the two numbers to input simultane-
ously, rather than each being a single potential solution. That is, if x = -3, ((-3) +
3)((-3) + 5) = 0 because (0)(2) = 0.
Since factoring of quadratics is the writing of polynomials as a product of
polynomials, students need to have both a strong conceptual understanding of
multiplication of polynomials and the procedural knowledge to retrieve ba-
sic multiplication facts effectively. “It is useful for students to have conceptual
knowledge of how products of terms relate to one another (i.e., exponent laws,
addition of like terms, etc.). With this understanding, students can do the neces-
sary procedural steps in factorization but also step back and ask themselves if the
results make sense.”92
Manipulatives such as algebra tiles have been shown to be effective for some
students in learning the structural relationships of polynomials and conducting
factoring.93 Concrete models can help students visualize abstract ideas. Algebra
Tiles and Algebra Discs can be used to represent variables and constants, as in
Figure 3.10; they are used to represent polynomials for factoring or expansion.
The ability to physically represent each of the terms and manipulate the tiles or
discs into polynomials can support students’ efforts to develop understanding
of factoring. Algebra Tiles appear to be a stronger representation because they
FIGURE 3.10. Algebra Tiles for factoring x2 + 5x + 3 (left). Algebra Discs for fac-
toring x2 – 2x – 3 (right). Based on Hoong, et al. (2010)
5 y + 6 x + 10 y 2 + 3
10 y 2 + 5 y + 6 x + 3
5 y (2 y + 1) + 3(2 y + 1)
(5 y + 3)(2 y + 1)
In this problem, students must understand that they need to group the first line
into terms that have something in common, such as 5y, and then use the distribu-
tive property twice to come up with the final factored form. Researchers find that
common errors students make with the distributive property are based on what the
factors are. For instance, for the expression mv–mu students may factor it as m2
(vu) or m2 (v – u).98 One possible source of this type of error is students’ difficulties
with the idea of exponents. Some believe that t2 is equal to 2t because it is t * t,
which is the same as 2t, so t2 – 2t = 0.
Again, visual representations might help students comprehend the structural
dynamics when grouping (see Figure 3.11). Consider the expression: xy + 2y +
138 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
3x + 6 and the use of algebra tiles to demonstrate the organization, structure, and
dynamic between terms and expressions when grouping.
When working on real-world problems, it is rare that an equation will be fac-
torable, yet most exercises in school textbooks are factorable, except when work-
ing with the quadratic formula.99 Accordingly, some mathematicians argue that
completing the square and the quadratic formula are the most usable strategies.
Research on how students think about and understand the quadratic formula is un-
fortunately limited. Generally, however, students struggle with understanding the
derivation of the formula, leading to mindlessly plugging in values for variables
to acquire solutions.100 Students often make procedural errors, particularly with
the negative signs (i.e., finding -b when b is negative).101 They also tend to have
conceptual difficulty with the two different roots, due to the ± sign. They may
have difficulty in determining the values for a, b, and c, sometimes incorrectly
identifying those values before writing the equation in the form ax2 + bx + c =
0.102 Students may believe that an equation must have three terms to be a quadratic
equation. They may need to be reminded that b and c can equal 0, but a cannot be
0, as that would make the equation linear and the fraction undefined.
It is generally accepted that visual, geometric approaches are most effective in
helping students learn how to complete the square.103 For instance, for the equa-
tion x(x + 10) = 39, a student might divide the shaded rectangle in Figure 3.12 into
two parts (with size 5x) and rearrange them into the second figure. A new square
is created in the lower right corner with area 5 x 5 or 25. Adding that area “com-
pletes the square.” Procedurally, x2 + 10x + 25 = 39 + 25 or (x + 5)2 = 64. Taking
the square root of both sides leads to a solution of x = 3. The solution of -13 does
not apply in this case because a negative solution cannot be a measurement of a
side of the rectangle. The inability to get a negative solution can therefore be a
limitation of the geometric model.
Algebraic Relations • 139
Finally, researchers have found that students can often factor and solve qua-
dratic equations, but are confused about the concept of a variable (see Variables
& Expressions) and of a “solution” or “root” to a quadratic equation.104 In the
section Variables & Expressions: Conservation of Variables, we noted that some
students responded to a problem such as x + x + x = 12 by stating that (2, 5, 5)
or (10, 1, 1) would be acceptable solutions, not understanding the consistency of
a variable within an equation. Many students do not realize that if a variable ap-
pears twice in an equation—for example, with x2 – 8x + 15 = 0, or (x – 3)(x – 5)
= 0—then it has the same value in the different places in which it appears.105 For
example, in the equation (x – 3)(x – 5) = 0, even if students obtained the correct
solutions, x = 3 and x = 5, some students thought that the two xs in the equation
stood for different variables, indicating they lacked relational understanding and
relied on rote procedures.106 Some students do not understand why there would
be two solutions. “Part of the problem is that students who generate solutions to
an equation often do not understand that the only number(s) which ‘make the
equation true’ are the solutions.”107 Research indicates that many students do not
understand what quadratic equations are, from a mathematical point of view, so
they struggle with the meaning of the solutions.108 Just because students can fac-
tor, complete the square, or use the quadratic formula does not mean that they un-
derstand quadratic equations. Teachers should work with students to understand
the structures and conceptual meaning of manipulations (relational understand-
ing) of equations and expressions, along with developing their procedural skills
(instrumental understanding).109 “We do not want meaningless symbol manipula-
tion; if students use symbolic expressions, we want them to use the symbols with
understanding.”110
Teachers
• Boosting students’ confidence in their multiplication facts may be a first
step in preparing them for success with factoring.
140 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
• Discuss the meaning of solutions with students often, rather than empha-
sizing the procedural skill alone.
• In one study, Algebra I students who were learning to factor used graphing
calculators to investigate graphs of families of quadratics, to explore real-
world applications of factoring, and to apply the data collected to concepts
of factoring. As a result, their basic skills in factoring were the same as a
control group while being able to engage in higher-order thinking.111
• When plugging in values for the quadratic formula, encourage students to
use parentheses to help them manage negative values. For example, b = -4,
-b = - (-4).
• Use parentheses/brackets as an intermediate tool to help students see and
understand structure. For instance, xy + 2y + 3x + 6 = (xy + 2y) + (3x + 6)
= y (x + 2) + 3(x + 2).
Algebraic Relations • 141
When comparing methods, it is critical for the teacher to draw students’ atten-
tion to the important features—this makes them actively compare the methods.
“Show-and-tell” is not enough. Developing flexibility in solving equations re-
quires students to know which strategies are more efficient than others under par-
ticular conditions. Instead of focusing on the difficulty of a question that students
can solve, teachers can assess the sophistication level of the solution strategy. One
potential guide for how to assess sophistication of these strategies is in Table 3.16.
In one study of an algebra classroom in China, researchers identified the strate-
gy of teaching with variation as a key reason for the success of East Asian students
in comparison with their Western counterparts on international tests.121 Teaching
with variation involves “illustrating the essential features by using different forms
Algebraic Relations • 143
1 1 1 1 1
( x + 14) = ( x + 20) ( x + 15) = - ( x - 7)
7 4 5 2 3
144 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Teachers
• Provide tasks that can be solved using a variety of methods (guess-and-
check, table, algebraic, graphical). Students need to be comfortable with
all methods.
• When comparing methods, it is critical for the teacher to draw students’
attention to the important features of the task that make different methods
more efficient.
• De-emphasize the difficulty of the task and instead focus on the sophistica-
tion of the solution strategy. Introduce concepts of elegance and efficiency
in regard to solution strategies.
Algebraic Relations • 145
• Nil zero: Zero has no value and students act as if it wasn’t there.
• Place value zero: Zero is used as a placeholder in a large number when
there is none of that place value (over half of students up to the eighth grade
could not write a number such as “two hundred thousand forty three”).125
• Implicit zero: The zero does not appear in writing, but is used in solving a
task. A student might solve a problem by thinking about obtaining a zero in
the process. For example, 5 – 17 = 5 – 5 – 12 = 0 – 12 = -12 might be the
thought process while 5 – 17 = -12 is the only thing written.
• Total zero: The combination of number opposites. For example, 34 + (-34) =
0.
• Arithmetic zero: The result of an arithmetic operation.
• Algebraic zero: The result of an algebraic operation or the solution of an
equation.
1 – 2x < 2(6 – x)
1 – 2x < 12 – 2x
-2x + 2x < 12 – 1
0 < 11
146 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
This problem was written in order to provoke cognitive conflict with students with
the following key features: the disappearance of x, the expression “Here Mr. A got
into difficulty,” and the answer 0 < 11. Fourteen percent of students responded
with a correct answer. Of those who answered incorrectly, 34% responded with
18
an inequality including x (e.g., x > ) and 18% did not have an x (e.g., 0 > 11).127
11
Many students struggled with their perception that they must get an answer in a
particular form, such as x > a, claiming that a final answer without x was not pos-
sible. Other students followed strict procedures without significant understanding
of the meaning of the solutions, treating x as merely an object in transforming an
expression. In a study of high school students, most did not know how to interpret
the results of their manipulation of symbols when the result was an identity or no
solution.128
To overcome these challenges, teachers can apply a number of strategies that
develop students’ understanding. Connecting the solutions to real-life situations
such as sharing cookies can help with understanding. If students get 0x = 7 (for
7
example) and conclude that x = = 0 , teachers could ask them whether they can
0
they take 7 cookies and share them among 0 people as a way of explaining why
division by zero is undefined. In general, asking the students to justify their steps
forces them to do only steps that they can explain. Furthermore, students should
be expected to check the validity of their solutions for confidence improvement.
The checking process becomes important in contextual problems because the
meaning that is associated with the check will confirm the correct interpretation of
the solution. One final strategy is to prevent students from aligning work horizon-
tally (e.g., 8x + 4 = 28, 8x = 24, x = 3). To show the progression of ideas leading to
a solution more clearly, they should align their work vertically (see Equal Sign).
Teachers
• Give students an opportunity to work with equations involving zero, but
have explicit discussions about the solutions and what they mean.
• Provide examples that allow for contrasting special case solutions like x = 0,
x = x, and x = no solution.
• Require students to check validity of solutions and to justify this validity.
Algebraic Relations • 147
• Do the computation without recording any steps and indicate that x=4;
• Graph both sides of the equation as functions and indicate that the x value
of the intersection is 4;
• Bring all the terms to the left, factor, and then write down the value that
will produce an output of 0; or
• Divide all the coefficients by 5 and then solve.
• Automatization: Students do it, but cannot explain how or why they did
what they did (“I don’t know why, it’s just what my teacher said to do”).
• Formulas: Students apply a memorized formula (like difference of 2
squares).
148 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Student: Yes, I can multiply both sides by 3 over 1, then subtract 4 from both sides and get x = 2.
Interviewer: Can you solve for x without writing anything on your paper? Can you do this one in
your head?
Student: That is a lot to keep track of in my head. I would probably make a mistake because
I’m not good at mental math. I could guess and check but that always takes forever. I
thin k it would be hard but I could probably do it.
In Table 3.18, the student solved in a procedural way. Instead, the student
could have applied an informal procedure and solved the equation by thinking
it through. “One third of something (x + 4) is 2, so that ‘something’ must be 6.
Something (x) plus 4 is 6, so that ‘something’ is 2. Then, x = 2.” This method is
often described as the cover up method. Students cover up part of the equation (x
+ 4 in this example) in order to intuitively think about the problem. Researchers
find that students can solve complex problems intuitively that they could not solve
procedurally, such as the one in Figure 3.13. The app Cover Up by the Center for
Algebraic Thinking provides practice in using this skill.
Presenting mathematical concepts through standalone examples and repeti-
tious practice does not foster understanding. We appear to teach algorithms too
soon and assume that once taught, they are remembered. This approach does not
help students overcome misconceptions. Instead, we should provide examples of
problems that help students see the benefit of using informal procedures, as in the
example above. Other examples help them to see the necessity for formal proce-
dures, as in a task like: 6 = 15 . When students encounter tasks like this, they can
a
Algebraic Relations • 149
Is there a value for x that will make the following statement true?
A student who had impulsive anticipation, for instance, might immediately begin
to combine like terms and isolate a variable. A student who thought with analytic
anticipation might notice that there were xs on each side of the inequality and that
there was an opportunity to simplify what was inside the parentheses. The student
might ultimately combine like terms and isolate the variable, as the student with
impulsive anticipation might, but he or she would not act “without thinking.” The
student who had coordination-based prediction might recognize that there were
two quantities in parentheses that had identical structures. Then he or she might
reason that the effect on the inequality would be equivalent, leaving the 12 > 6.
Using these descriptions as a guide, teachers might identify students’ ways of
thinking and look for opportunities to develop students’ sophistication in anticipa-
tion, reasoning, and analysis of problems prior to and during action.
152 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Teachers
• Avoid introducing procedures with linear equations. This appears to elimi-
nate student intuition regarding the meaning of equations. Encourage stu-
dents to develop hypotheses of how to approach problems and discuss the
merits and disadvantages of different approaches.
• Consider working toward strategies based on patterns. For instance, show-
ing students 5*x = 10, students should be able to intuitively come up with
an answer, rather than perform procedures. Extend those examples and help
students articulate a strategy for handling problems with that structure.
• Provide examples of problems that help students see the benefit of using
informal and non-standard procedures.
• Contrast intuition-invoking examples with problems that help students see
the benefits of procedures.
• Use metaphors (stories, recipes, function machine, balance model, etc.)
when describing equations.
Algebraic Relations • 153
ENDNOTES
1 Tahir & Cavanagh, 2010.
2 Godfrey & Thomas, 2008.
3 Linsell, 2008.
4 Ding & Li, 2010.
5 Brown & Quinn, 2006, 2007a, 2007b.
6 Vlassis, 2002.
7 Knuth, Stephens, McNeil, & Alibali, 2006.
8 Kennedy, 1991, p. 118.
9 Kennedy, 1991, p. 118 based on Usiskin, 1988.
10 Bossé & Nandakumar, 2005.
11 Attorps, 2003.
12 MacGregor, 1999.
13 Kuchemann, 1981; Hodgen, Kuchemann, Brown, & Coe, 2008.
14 de Lima & Tall, 2006; Kieran, 1981; Knuth, Stephens, McNeil, & Alibali, 2006.
15 Stephens, 2006, p. 249.
16 McNeil & Alibali, 2005; Molina & Ambrose, 2008.
17 McNeil, Grandau, Knuth, Alibali, Stephens, Hattikudur, & Krill, 2006, p. 367.
18 Carpenter et al., 2003; Koehler, 2004; Saenz-Ludlow & Walgamuth, 1998 .
19 Bodin & Capponi, 1996; de Lima & Tall, 2006; Freitas, 2002; Vaiyavutjamai, 2004a, 2004b.
20 O’Rode, 2011, p. 25.
21 Jones, Inglis, Gilmore, & Dowens, 2012, p. 166.
22 Barcellos, 2005; Sakpakornkan & Harries, 2003.
23 Barcellos, 2005.
24 Falle, 2005.
25 Hall, 2002.
26 All Center for Algebraic Thinking apps are available at no cost for iOS devices, except Math
Flyer and Help Me Choose! which are $0.99.
27 Behr, Harel, Post, & Lesh, 1992; Brown & Quinn, 2007a; Brown & Quinn, 2007b; Lamon,
2011.
28 Kieran, 1980, p. 134.
29 Brown & Quinn, 2006.
30 Rossi, 2008.
31 Based on 180 9th- to 11th-grade students in Hall, 2002.
32 Brown & Quinn, 2006.
33 Peled & Carraher, 2008.
34 Peled & Carraher, 2008, p. 325.
35 Gates, 1995, p. 283.
36 Based on Gallardo, 2002; Gallardo, 2003; Peled, 1991.
37 Hefendahl-Hebeker, 1991; Kilhamn, 2011.
38 Adapted from Atiparmak, 2010 and Kilhamn, 2009.
39 Ball, 1993; Schwarz, Kohn, & Resnick, 1993.
40 Schwarz, Kohn, & Resnick, 1993; Vlassis, 2004.
41 Gallardo, 1995.
42 Based on Hayes, 1994; Hall, 2002; Hayes, 1996; and Seng, 2010.
43 Vlassis, 2004.
44 Hayes, 1994, 1996.
45 Kilhamn, 2008, 2009; Schwarz, Kohn, & Resnick, 1993.
46 Fischer & Rottman, 2005.
47 Hayes, 1994.
48 Peled, Mukhopadhyay, & Resnick, 1989 and Schwarz, Kohn, & Resnick, 1993.
154 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
.
112 Skemp, 1976.
113 From Attorps, 2003 based on Hiebert & Lefevre, 1986
114 Hiebert & Lefevre, 1986, as quoted in Attorps, 2003, p. 2.
115 Star, 2005; Baroody, Feil and Johnson, 2007; Star, 2007; Kieran, 2013; Star & Stylianides,
2013; Foster, 2014.
116 Foster, 2014.
117 Sfard & Linchevski, 1994.
118 Steinberg, Sleeman, & Ktorza, 1990.
119 Huntley, Marcus, Kahan, & Miller, 2007.
120 Sfard & Linchevski, 1994, p. 204–5.
121 Gu, 1994; Gu, Huang, & Marton, 2004.
122 Li, Peng, & Song, 2011.
123 Anthony & Walshaw, 2004, based on Sheffield and Cruikshank, 2001.
124 Anthony & Walshaw, 2004; Gallardo, 2005; Gallardo, 2006.
125 From Anthony & Walshaw, 2004, p. 38, citing Crooks and Flockton, 2002.
126 Fujii, 2003.
127 Based on 123 lower secondary students in Fujii, 2003.
128 Huntley, Marcus, Kahan, & Miller, 2007.
129 Demby, 1997, p. 68.
130 Lim, 2006.
131 Lim, 2006, p. 2–106.
REFERENCES
Allaire, P. R., & Bradley, R. E. (2001). Geometric approaches to quadratic equations from
other times and places. The Mathematics Teacher, 94(4), 308–313.
Almog, N., & Ilany, B. (2012). Absolute value inequalities: High school students’ solutions
and misconceptions. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 81(3), 347–365.
Altiparmak, K., & Ozdogan, E. (2010). A study on the teaching of the concept of negative
numbers. International Journal of Mathematical Education in Science and Technol-
ogy, 41(1), 31–47.
156 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Anthony, G., & Walshaw, A. (2004). Zero: a “none” number? Teaching Children Math-
ematics, 11(1), 38.
Asghari, A. (2009). Experiencing equivalence but organizing order. Educational Studies in
Mathematics, 71(3), 219–234.
Attorps, I. (2003). Teachers’ images of the ‘equation’ concept. Paper presented at the Third
Conference of the European society for Research in Mathematics Education, Bel-
laria, Italy.
Aufman, R. N., & Barker, V. C. (1991). Intermediate algebra. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Ayres, P. L. (2000). An analysis of bracket expansion errors. Paper presented at the 24th
Annual Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics
Education Hiroshima, Japan.
Ball, D. (1992). Magical hopes: Manipulatives and the reform of math education. Ameri-
can Educator: The Professional Journal of the American Federation of Teachers,
16(2), 14–18, 46–47.
Ball, D. (1993). With an eye on the mathematical horizon: Dilemmas of teaching elemen-
tary school mathematics, Elementary School Journal, 93, 379–397.
Barcellos, A. (2005). Mathematical misconceptions of college-age algebra students. Un-
published dissertation. University of California, Davis.
Baroody, A. J., Feil, Y., & Johnson, A. R. (2007). An alternative reconceptualization of
procedural and conceptual knowledge. Journal for Research in Mathematics Educa-
tion, 38, 115–131.
Behr, M. J., Harel, G., Post, T. R., & Lesh, R. (1992). Rational number, ratio, and propor-
tion. In D. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learn-
ing. New York, NY: Macmillan Publishing.
Bodin, A., & Capponi, B. (1996). Junior secondary school practices. In A. J. Bishop, M.
A. Clements, C. Keitel, J. Kilpatrick, & C. Laborde (Eds.), International handbook
of mathematics education (pp. 565–614). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Aca-
demic Publishers.
Bossé, M. J., & Nandakumar, N. R. (2005). The factorability of quadratics: Motivation
for more techniques. Teaching Mathematics and its Applications: An International
Journal of the IMA, 24(4), 143–153.
Brown, G., & Quinn, R. J. (2006). Algebra students’ difficulty with fractions: An error
analysis. Australian Mathematics Teacher, 62(4), 28–40.
Brown, G., & Quinn, R. J. (2007a). Fraction proficiency and success in Algebra: What does
research say? Austrailian Mathematics Teacher, 63(3), 23–30.
Brown, G., & Quinn, R. J. (2007b). Investigating the relationship between fraction profi-
ciency and success in Algebra. Australian Mathematics Teacher, 63(4), 8–15.
Burger, E., Chard, D., Hall, E., Kennedy, P., Leinwand, S., Renfro, F., Roby, T., Seymour,
D., & Waits, B. (2008). Holt California Algebra 1, Student Edition. New York, NY:
Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
Carpenter, T. P., Franke, M. L., & Levi, L. (2003). Thinking mathematically: Integrating
arithmetic and algebra in elementary school. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Carry, L. R., Lewis, C., & Bernard, J. (1980) Psychology of equation solving; An informa-
tion processing study, Austin, TX: University of Texas at Austin, Department of
Curriculum and Instruction.
Crooks, T., & Flockton, L. (2002). Mathematics assessment results 2001. National educa-
tion monitoring. Dunedin, New Zealand: Educational Assessment Research Unit.
Algebraic Relations • 157
de Lima, R., & Tall, D. (2006). The concept of equations: What have students met before?
Paper presented at the 30th Conference of the International Group for the Psychol-
ogy of Mathematics Education, Prague, Czech Republic.
De Souza, V., Nogueira de Lima, R., & Campos, T. (2015). A functional graphic approach
to inequations. Revista Lanioamericana de Investigacion en Matematica Educativa,
18(1), 109–125.
Demby, A. (1997). Algebraic procedures used by 13-to-15-year-olds. Educational Studies
in Mathematics, 33(1), 45–70.
Didiş, M. G., Baş, S., & Erbaş, A. (2011). Students’ reasoning in quadratic equations with
one unknown. Paper presented at the 7th Congress of the European Society for Re-
search in Mathematics Education, Rzeszów, Poland.
Ding, M., & Li, X. (2010). An analysis of the distributive property in U.S. and Chinese el-
ementary mathematics texts. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American
Educational Research Association, Denver, CO.
Dobbs, D., & Peterson, J. (1991). The sign-chart method for solving inequalities. Math-
ematics Teacher, 84, 657–664.
Dreyfus, T., & Eisenberg, T. (1982). Intuitive functional concepts: A baseline study on
intuitions Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 13, 360–380.
Falle, J. (2005). From arithmetic to Algebra: Novice students’ strategies for solving equa-
tions. Paper presented at the 28th annual conference of the Mathematics Education
Research Group of Australasia.
Filloy, E., Rojano, T., & Solares, A. (2010). Problems dealing with unknown quantities and
two different levels of representing unknowns. Journal for Research in Mathematics
Education, 41(1), 52–80.
Fischer, M. H., & Rottman, J. (2005). Do negative numbers have a place on the mental
number line? Psychology Science, 47(1), 22–32.
Foster, C. (2014). ‘Can’t you just tell us the rule?’ Teaching procedures relationally. Paper
presented at the 8th British Congress of Mathematics Education, University of Not-
tingham.
Freitas, M. A. de. (2002). Equação do primeiro grau: métodos de resolução e análise de
erros no ensino médio. [Equations in the first degree: Methods of resolution and
analysis of errors in high school]. Master’s Dissertation. São Paulo: PUC-SP.
Fujii, T. (2003). Probing students’ understanding of variables through cognitive conflict
problems: Is the concept of variable so difficult for students to understand? Paper
presented at the 27th International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Educa-
tion Conference, Honolulu, HI.
Gallardo, A. (1995). Negative numbers in the teaching of arithmetic. In D. Owens, M.
Reeds, & G. Millsaps, (Eds.), Proceedings of the 17th Annual Meeting for the Psy-
chology of Mathematics Education, (North America Chapter, vol. 1, pp. 158–163.)
ERIC Clearinghouse for Science, Mathematics and Environmental Education, Ohio,
USA.
Gallardo, A. (2002). The extension of the natural-number domain to the integers in the tran-
sition from arithmetic to algebra. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 49,171–192.
Gallardo, A. (2003). It is possible to die before being born? Negative integers subtraction:
A case study. Paper presented at the 27th International Group for the Psychology of
Mathematics Education Conference, Honolulu, HI.
158 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Gallardo, A. (2005). The duality of zero in the transition from arithmetic to algebra. Paper
presented at the 29th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of
Mathematics Education, Melbourne.
Gallardo, A., & Hernandez, A. (2006). The zero and negativity among secondary school
students. Paper presented at the 30th Conference of the International Group for the
Psychology of Mathematics Education (3: 153–160). Prague, Czech Republic.
Gates, L. (1995). Product of two negative numbers: An example of how rote learning a
strategy is synonymous with learning the concept. Paper presented at the 18th An-
nual Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia Conference. Salma Ta-
hir Macquarie University.
Godfrey, D., & Thomas, M. O. J. (2008). Student perspectives on equation: The transition
from school to university. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 20(2), 71–92.
Gu, L. (1994). Theory of teaching experiment: The methodology and teaching principle of
Qinpu. Beijing: Educational Science Press.
Gu, L., Huang, R., & Marton, F. (2004). Teaching with variation: A Chinese way of pro-
moting effective mathematics learning. In L. Fan, N. Wong, J. Cai, & S. Li (Eds.),
How Chinese learn mathematics: Perspectives from insiders. Singapore: World Sci-
entific.
Hall, R. (2002). An analysis of thought processes during simplification of an algebraic
expression. Philosophy of Mathematics Education, 15.
Hattikudur, S., & Alibali, M. W. (2010). Learning about the equal sign: Does compar-
ing with inequality symbols help? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107,
15–30.
Hayes, B. (1994). Becoming more positive with negatives. Paper presented at the 17th An-
nual Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia Conference.
Hayes, B. (1996). Investigating the teaching and learning of negative number concepts and
operations. Paper presented at the 19th Annual Mathematics Education Research
Group of Australasia Conference: Melbourne, Australia.
Hefendehl-Hebeker, L. (1991). Negative numbers: Obstacles in their evolution from intui-
tive to intellectual constructs. For the Learning of Mathematics, 11(1), 26–32.
Hiebert, J., & Lefevre, P. (1986). Conceptual and procedural knowledge in mathematics:
An introductory analysis. In J. Hiebert (Ed.), Conceptual and procedural knowl-
edge: The case of mathematics (pp. 1–28). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Hoch, M., & Dreyfus, T. (2004). Structure sense in high school Algebra: The effect of
brackets. Paper presented at the 28th Annual Meeting of the International Group for
the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Bergen, Norway.
Hodgen, J., Kuchemann, D., Brown, M., & Coe, R. (2008). Children’s understandings of
algebra 30 years on. Proceedings of the British Society for Research into Learning
Mathematics, 28(3), 36–41.
Hoffman, N. (1976). Factorisation of quadratics. Mathematics teaching, 76, 54–55.
Hoong, L. Y., Fwe, Y. S., Yvonne, T. M. L., Subramaniam, T., Zaini, I. K. B. M., Chiew,
Q. E., & Karen, T. K. L. (2010). Concretising factorisation of quadratic expressions.
Australian Mathematics Teacher, 66(3), 19–24.
Hu, F., Ginns, P., & Bobis, J. (2015). Getting the point: Tracing worked examples enhances
learning. Learning and Instruction, 35, 85–93.
Algebraic Relations • 159
Huntley, M. A., Marcus, R., Kahan, J., & Miller, J. L. (2007). Investigating high-school
students’ reasoning strategies when they solve linear equations. The Journal of
Mathematical Behavior, 26(2), 115–139.
Jones, I., Inglis, M., Gilmore, C., & Dowens, M. (2012). Substitution and sameness: Two
components of a relational conception of the equals sign. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 113(1), 166–176.
Jones, I., & Pratt, D. (2012). A substituting meaning for the equals sign in arithmetic notat-
ing tasks. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 43, 2–33.
Kaput, J. (1989). Linking representations in the symbol systems of algebra. In S. W. C.
Kieran (Ed.), Research issues in the learning and teaching of algebra (Vol. 4 of
Research agenda for mathematics education) (pp. 167–194). Reston, VA: National
Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Kennedy, P. A. (1991). Factoring by grouping: Making the connection. Mathematics and
Computer Education, 25(2), 118–123.
Kieran, C. (1980). The interpretation of the equal sign: Symbol for an equivalence relation
vs. an operator symbol. Paper presented at the Psychology of Mathematics Educa-
tion Conference, Berkeley, CA.
Kieran, C. (1981). Concepts associated with the equality symbol. Educational Studies in
Mathematics, 12(3), 317–326.
Kieran, C. (2004). The equation/inequality connection in constructing meaning for in-
equality situations. In M. J. Hoines & A. B. Fuglestad (Eds.), Proceedings of the 28th
Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Educa-
tion, 1, 143–147.
Kieran, C. (2013). The false dichotomy in mathematics education between conceptual
understanding and procedural skills: An example from algebra. In R. K. Leatham
(Ed.), Vital directions for mathematics education research (pp. 153–171). New
York, NY: Springer New York.
Kieren, T. E. (1980). The rational number construct: Its elements and mechanisms. In T. E.
Kieren (Ed.), Recent research on number learning (pp. 125–149). Columbus, OH:
ERIC Clearinghouse for Science, Math, and Environmental Education.
Kilhamn, C. (2008). Making sense of negative numbers through metaphorical reason-
ing. Paper presented at the 6th Swedish Mathematics Education Research Seminar,
Stockholm, Sweden.
Kilhamn, C. (2009). The notion of number sense in relation to negative numbers. Paper
presented at the 33rd conference of the International Group for the Psychology of
Mathematics Education, Tessaloniki, Greece.
Kilhamn, C. (2011). Making sense of negative numbers. (Doctoral Thesis), University of
Gothenburg.
Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruc-
tion does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-
based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41(2),
75–86.
Knuth, E. J., Stephens, A. C., McNeil, N. M., & Alibali, M. W. (2006). Does understanding
the equal sign matter? Evidence from solving equations. Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education, 36(4), 297–312.
Koehler, J. L. (2004). Learning to think relationally: Thinking relationally to learn. Un-
published dissertation research proposal, University of Wisconsin-Madison.
160 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
McNeil, N. M., Grandau, L., Knuth, E. J., Alibali, M. W., Stephens, A. C., Hattikudur, S.,
& Krill, D. E. (2006). Middle-school students’ understanding of the equal sign: The
books they read can’t help. Cognition and Instruction, 24(3), 367–385.
Molina, M., & Ambrose, R. (2008). From an operational to a relational conception of the
equal sign: Third graders’ developing algebraic thinking. Focus on Learning Prob-
lems in Mathematics, 30(1), 61–80.
Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Gonzalez, E. J., Gregory, K. D., Garden, R. A., O’Connor,
K. M., Christowski, S. J., & Smith, T. A. (2000). TIMSS 1999 International Math-
ematics Report: Findings from IES’s Repeat of the Third International Mathematics
and Science Study at the Eighth Grade. Chestnut Hill, MA: Boston College.
Nanney, J. L., & Cable, J. L. (1991). Intermediate algebra. Dubuque, IA: Wm. C. Brown.
Novotna, J., & Hoch, M. (2008). How structure sense for algebraic expressions or equa-
tions is related to structure sense for abstract algebra. Mathematics Education Re-
search Journal, 20(2), 93–104.
Nustad, H. L. & Wesner, T. H. (1991). Principals of intermediate algebra with applica-
tions. Dubuque: Wm. C. Brown.
O’Rode, N. (2011). Latino/a students understanding of equivalence. In K. Téllez, J.
Moschkovich, & M. Civil (Eds.), Latinos/as and mathematics education: Research
on learning and teaching in classrooms and communities (pp. 19–36). Information
Age Publishing.
Parent, J. S. (2015). Students’ understanding of quadratic functions: Learning from stu-
dents’ voices. University of Vermont, Graduate College Dissertations and Theses.
Paper 376.
Parrish, C. (1992). Inequalities, absolute value, and logical connectives. The Mathematics
Teacher, 85(9), 756–757.
Peled, I. (1991). Levels of knowledge about negative numbers: Effects of age and ability.
In F. Furinghetti (Ed.), Proceedings of the 15th international conference for the
Psychology in Mathematics Education (Vol. 3, pp. 145–152). Assisi, Italy: Confer-
ence Committee.
Peled, I., & Carraher, D. (2008). Signed numbers and algebraic thinking. In D. W. Carraher
& M. L. Blanton (Eds.), Algebra in the early grades (pp. 303–328). New York, NY:
NCTM.
Peled, I., Mukhopadhyay, S., & Resnick, L. B. (1989). Formal and informal sources of
mental models for negative numbers. Paper presented at the 13th International Con-
ference for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Paris, France.
Piaget, J. (1960). Introducción a la espistemología genética. I. El pensamiento matemático.
Biblioteca de Psicología Evolutiva [Introduction to genetic epistemology: Math-
ematical thinking. Library of Evolutionary Psychology]. Paidós, Buenos Aires, Ar-
gentina.
Pulsinelli, L., & Hooper, P. (1991). Intermediate algebra: An interactive approach. New
York, NY: Macmillan.
Pungut, M. H. A., & Shahrill, M. (2014). Students’ English language abilities in solving
mathematics word problems. Mathematics Education Trends and Research, 2014,
1–11.
Radford, L. (2004). Syntax and meaning. Paper presented at the 28th International Con-
ference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education,
Bergen, Norway.
162 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Rauff, J. V. (1994). Constructivism, factoring, and beliefs. School Science and Mathemat-
ics, 94(8), 421.
Renkl, A. (2005). The worked-out-example principle in multimedia learning. In R. Mayer
(Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 229–245). Cambridge,
MA: Cambridge University Press.
Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2007). Does comparing solution methods facilitate con-
ceptual and procedural knowledge? An experimental study on learning to solve
equations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(3), 561–574.
Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2009). Compared with what? The effects of different com-
parisons on conceptual knowledge and procedural flexibility for equation solving.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(3), 529–544.
Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2011). The power of comparison in learning and instruc-
tion: Learning outcomes supported by different types of comparisons. In B. Ross
& J. Mestre (Eds.), Psychology of learning and motivation: Cognition in education
(Vol. 55, pp. 199–226). San Diego, CA: Elsevier.
Rossi, P. S. (2008). An uncommon approach to a common algebraic error. PRIMUS ,18(6),
554–558.
Saenz-Ludlow, A., & Walgamuth, C. (1998). Third graders’ interpretations of equality and
the equal symbol. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 35(2), 153–187.
Sakpakornkan, N., & Harries, T. (2003). Pupils’ processes of thinking: Learning to solve
algebraic problems in England and Thailand. Paper presented at the British society
for research into learning mathematics, Oxford, United Kingdom.
Sarwadi, H. R. H., & Shahrill, M. (2014). Understanding students’ mathematical errors
and misconceptions: The case of year 11 repeating students. Mathematics Education
Trends and Research, 1–10.
Schwarz, B. B., Kohn, A. S., & Resnick, L. B. (1993). Positives about negatives: A case
study of an intermediate model for signed numbers. The Journal of the Learning
Sciences, 3(1), 37–92.
Seng, L. K. (2010). An error analysis of form 2 (Grade 7) students in simplifying algebraic
expressions: A descriptive study. Electronic Journal of Research in Educational
Psychology, 8(1), 139–162.
Sfard, A., & Linchevski, L. (1994). The gains and the pitfalls of reification—The case of
algebra. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 26, 191–228.
Sharp, J. (1995). Results of using algebra tiles as meaningful representations of algebra
concepts. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Mid-Western Education
Research Association, Chicago, IL.
Sheffield, L., & Cruikshank, D. (2001). Teaching and learning elementary and middle
school mathematics. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
Sherin, B. L. (2001). How students understand physics equations. Cognition and Instruc-
tion, 19(4), 479–541.
Skemp, R. R. (1976). Relational understanding and instrumental understanding. Math-
ematics Teaching, 77, 20–26.
Sobol, A. J. (1998). A formative and summative evaluation study of classroom interactions
and student/teacher effects when implementing algebra tile manipulatives with ju-
nior high school students (Order No. 9830812). Unpublished Doctoral dissertation,
St. John’s University, New York. Retrieved from http://proxy.lib.pacificu.edu:2048/
login?url=http://search.proquest.com/docview/304449341?accountid=13047.
Algebraic Relations • 163
ANALYSIS OF CHANGE
(GRAPHING)
INTRODUCTION
One of the most interesting graphs ever created is Minard’s image depicting Na-
poleon’s march on Moscow in June of 1812 (see Figure 4.1). The image is a
physical graph imposed on a map of the region. The graph represents the size of
Napoleon’s army (initially 400,000) as he progressed from the Niemen River in
Belarus on the left toward the upper right corner and Moscow. A couple of regi-
ments broke off to scout at the initial top part of the graph. As the French army
chased the Russian army east, the Russians burned everything so the French army
would have no resources. Napoleon reached Moscow in October. The dark graph
then shows his return march in concert with the temperature graph at the bottom,
which is read right to left, starting at 0 degree Celsius. The temperatures steadily
drop down to -30 degrees Celsius by the 6th of December, as Napoleon’s return
was during the bitterly cold Russian winter. Because of the destruction on the
way to Moscow, the French army needed to return by a different route. Due to
the battles and starvation, only 10,000 troops were left when they returned to the
Niemer River. What a powerful visual description of history!
This graph conveys change over time. It is complex and engaging. Can stu-
dents understand the dynamic? Change is one of the fundamental features of our
world that is represented mathematically in algebra. Analyzing change in various
contexts is one of the four fundamental concepts of algebra described in the Prin-
ciples and Standards for School Mathematics1 and is embedded throughout the
Common Core State Standards. One of the generative questions of mathematics
is: How much has a given quantity changed?
Representing and understanding change requires multiple types of actions:
in linear functions and then moves on to non-constant rates of change. While rate
of change can be seen in many forms, a major component of an Algebra course
is Constructing Graphs from Real-World Data, which represents change visu-
ally. Plotting ordered pairs of numbers on a coordinate system leads to students’
observing patterns in the graphical representation of those ordered pairs. An es-
sential aspect of graphing is knowing how choices made for Scaling the axes
influence the nature of the visual representation. Teachers must help students with
Connecting Graphs to Algebraic Relationships in order to understand how the
symbolic representation is reflected in the visual representation and vice versa.
This includes Understanding Slope and how the steepness or direction of the line
communicates information about the change occurring in the problem context.
For example, in the beginning of this book we discussed the discouraging in-
crease in the rates of K–12 students failing algebra in the United States. In writing
this book, we hope to contribute to efforts to change those rates, which is to say to
change the slope of that graph, so that fewer and fewer students fail algebra (or more
and more students pass algebra) as a result of myriad efforts. If, for example, by the
year 2020 we see that students are failing algebra at a lower rate than in 2014, that
might be evidence of a correlation between our work in this text and the learning of
algebra in schools in the United States. Described graphically, the slope of the line
that portrays the failure rate of algebra students in the United States should decrease
or become less steeply slanted upward. We hope it would “flatten out” or even de-
cline, implying an alteration in the rate of change. When teaching about mathemati-
cal descriptions of change (of which graphing is one example) it is more effective to
embed student understanding of graphing in real-world examples.
Recently, National Public Radio reported a rise in the number of stay-at-home
parents in Oregon:
The number of stay-at-home parents in Oregon is on the rise. A new report released
Wednesday finds that one in five mothers is staying home to care for family while
with fathers, it’s one in a hundred. Oregon’s Office of Economic Analysis focused
on the state’s working population, those between the ages of twenty-five and fifty-
four. The report finds that, while mothers account for the vast majority of Oregon’s
stay-at-home parents, the share of fathers staying at home has doubled in the past
decade.4
The story stated that one in five mothers was staying home to care for family. For
fathers, the rate was one in one hundred. What is interesting in this significant
discrepancy between the two genders is that the 1% rate for fathers staying home
represents a doubling of the rate in the past decade. Overall, across both genders
the number of stay-at-home parents is on the rise. Can students understand the
dynamic of change in this article? Figure 4.2 was included in the report. Can
students interpret this graph and the changes over time? What was happening in
2001? How is that different than 2010?
The Common Core State Standards for Mathematics note that “Mathemati-
cally proficient students can explain correspondences between equations, verbal
168 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
descriptions, tables, and graphs or draw diagrams of important features and re-
lationships, graph data, and search for regularity or trends.”5 To make sense of
many policy discussions, purchases (such as homes with mortgages), and ensu-
ing decisions, citizens must understand something about how change is described
mathematically. How might a .25% change in interest rate impact the cost of a
home over time? What if you paid off the home 5 years early? What if you made
extra payments early in the loan versus in the middle of a loan? Understanding the
mathematical nature of change is important for consumers.
There are multiple elements in learning about analysis of change and graphing
in an Algebra course that can be the source of potential student errors. For exam-
ple, to begin to Understand Rate of Change, students must understand that rate
is a numeric relationship between two changing quantities. A weak connection to
that concept is at the heart of many student errors and misconceptions. Research
indicates that students’ experience of the real world has a significant influence on
how they understand graphs. Some misconceptions are based on students seeing
Graphs as Literal Pictures. Generally, one of the main objectives in an Algebra
course is to develop students’ skills in Interpreting Graphs. In this chapter, we
share research on the many common ways that students struggle with making
sense of visual information and the connection to the data and symbolic represen-
tation. Finally, one particular aspect of learning about change is Understanding
Speed as Rate of Change and working with Motion Graphs.
Analysis of Change (Graphing) • 169
6 x
=
14 35
that there is much work to be done to develop students’ ratio and proportional
thinking.
Proportionality is a mathematical relationship between two quantities—a
a c
statement of equality of two ratios, i.e., = .9 Researchers describe propor-
b d
tional reasoning as “reasoning in a system of two variables between which there
is a functional relationship (that) leads to conclusions about a situation or phe-
nomenon that can be characterized by a constant ratio.”10 The key for students is
to learn the nature of that relationship and how it is different than other math rela-
tionships they have learned. Consider how students solve Problem 3 in Table 4.2.
Rather than seek multiplicative relationships, students often find the difference
between 6 and 14 and then subtract it from 35 in order to get an answer of x =
27.11 Prior knowledge can influence students’ thinking about proportions. In this
case, students are used to thinking in terms of additive solution methods, so that
is how they tend to approach the problem. One major study found that 25–40%
of errors on proportion problems were due to students using additive strategies.12
Other incorrect strategies include students simply ignoring some of the data in a
problem, as in the following problem:
In a particular metal alloy, there is 1 part mercury to 5 parts copper and 3 parts tin
to 10 parts copper. How many parts mercury to would you need for how many parts
of tin?
Table 4.3 provides an interview with a student indicating a struggle with taking
into account multiple aspects of the problem simultaneously. Generally, in order
of increasing difficulty, students are challenged by 1:2 ratios, 1:n ratios, m:n ratios
(in which neither term is 1), and non-integer ratios.13
There are four primary difficulty factors for students solving proportion prob-
lems.14
This algorithm does not help students make connections between the relation-
ships of the quantities. One concern is that students only encounter “missing value
problems where three of four values in two rate pairs [are] given and the fourth
is to be found”18 and the cross-multiplication method is the only strategy that stu-
dents learn to solve them.19 Students apply the method to similar problems rather
than being able to compare different problem types and approaches to solving
172 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Susie and her friends were tossing Frisbees. The average distance was 25 feet. Susie’s
throw was 5/4 of the average distance. How many feet did Susie toss her Frisbee?
FIGURE 4.4. Tape Diagram: Throwing Frisbees. Based on Murata (2008, p. 381)
these models can lead to successful thinking about proportions through graphs
and their symbolic representation (see Figure 4.6).32
Proportionality is a simple example of a mathematical function that can be
represented by a linear equation. It is a useful bridge between numerical and ab-
stract relationships such as y = mx. Teachers should work with students to help
them comprehend how algebraic ideas with proportional relationships can be
seen in tables, graphs, equations, and diagrams. The teacher’s goal for students’
proportional reasoning should be more qualitative than quantitative. That is, stu-
dents should be able to compare without specific values. For instance, consider
the problem: “If Nicki ran fewer laps in more time than she did yesterday, would
her running speed be faster, slower, the same, or you can’t tell?”33 When the con-
text is familiar, students should be able to interpret the meaning of two ratios in
this problem without needing numbers to understand the dynamic. Facility with
proportional reasoning in this way will support later thinking with concepts such
as slope in Algebra courses.
Teachers
• Help students understand both the within relationship and between rela-
tionship in a proportion.
• Use visuals to help students understand proportional relationships and con-
nect symbolic representations.
• Don’t rely on rote procedures such as cross multiplication. Help students
see the conceptual ideas and proportional relationships behind symbolic
manipulation.
• Be sure to use non-integers to increase students’ flexibility with propor-
tional reasoning.
Analysis of Change (Graphing) • 175
As is evident in the interviews in Table 4.4, students may have trouble coordi-
nating the two quantities required for a rate. The idea of “rate” typically develops
in stages.36 In the first stage, students do not have a mathematical conception of
rate, and instead describe rate based upon everyday usage. For example, in the
interview, Student 1 refers to movie “ratings.” At a further stage of development,
students see rate as a single numerical quantity. For example, Student 4 says that
the clown is walking at a rate of 8 seconds, and not 8 feet per second. Next,
students may see a relationship between two quantities, but may be unable to
describe that relationship numerically. For example, Student 5 does not use num-
bers, but describes the relationship generically—that as the area of sunlight in-
creases, the height of the window shade also increases. Finally, students might see
that rate is the numerical relationship between two changing quantities. Student 9
identifies a specific relationship by explaining that the rate the man is walking is 4
meters per second, because for every second the man walks four meters. The first
four descriptions indicate gaps in students’ understanding. Comprehension of rate
of change must include some sense of two changing quantities and the relation-
ship between them. A more sophisticated understanding of rate of change would
ultimately involve reasoning that is independent from the context, the concept of
variable rates of change, and developing potential for abstraction into symbols.
Another concern with students’ thinking regarding rate of change is that stu-
dents may appear to understand one representation of rate but not be able to
transfer that understanding to a different representation or context.37 In particular,
students “do not transfer their understanding of rate demonstrated in tables and
graphs to the corresponding symbolic representation.”38 Different representations
provide different rate-related information for different students. Students have
greater success when they are familiar with the setting of the reasoning task.39 For
instance, students often understand the concept of speed, but many are not able to
relate that understanding to a context not involving speed.
Finally, body language can give teachers helpful insights into student think-
ing when talking about rates.40 When a student’s words and gestures match, it is
likely the student has a clear understanding of the concept. Sometimes a student’s
gestures and verbal descriptions do not agree. For example, a student might say
that the shadow is getting bigger while their hand motion indicates that the height
of the shadow is decreasing. The mismatch between words and gestures indicates
that the student does not have a good understanding of the concept. When a stu-
dent cannot verbalize a concept, but correctly demonstrates a concept through
body language, the teacher can help the student focus on vocabulary development
and symbolic representation.
Teachers
• Try to determine the level of a student’s development of the concept of rate
so you know what aspects of the concept they need help with.
• Use multiple representations of rate in different contexts with integers and
non-integers to help students develop a comprehensive understanding.
Analysis of Change (Graphing) • 177
Along that line of reasoning, in order to discuss the way students develop in
their thinking about graphically representing real-world data, we first share the
following problem:
Problem 5: Motorist
A motorist is speeding along a highway and he’s very thirsty. When he sees a store,
he stops to get a drink. Then he gets back in his car and drives slowly away.44
In one study, middle and high school students were asked to create their own
representations to depict the motion described in Problem 5. As a result, students
created a wide variety of representational forms. The work of two students, Jason
and Clare, is shown in Figures 4.7 and 4.8, and the explanations of their responses
to the problem are in Table 4.6.
Students are not likely to choose a coordinate graph to represent a situation if
they have not had experience with that type of model. The first set of interviews
(see Table 4.6) show how students can find other ways to represent a contextual
situation. In each of these students’ representations, you can get a sense of their
perception of rate of change and the base of knowledge they have to work with
as they move toward more sophisticated understanding. They typically use ex-
isting representations from their experience (or “constructive resources” as one
researcher put it) as the fodder for their new representations.45 In Clare’s case, she
“uses arrows of various lengths to show the magnitude of the speed, and dots to
One-to-one correspondence is
Category 3
needs of the situation. One study gave 92 eighth-grade students a problem similar
to the following about the relationship between three students’ time studying and
their scores on tests:
Each student had a different relationship: increasing, constant, curvilinear, and de-
creasing. While 60% of students were able to construct at least one correct graph,
only 28.9% were able to construct all four graphs.49 Students often focus on only
one aspect of the relationship in their visual representation—in this problem, the
amount of time (see Figure 4.9). 50 Thirty-six percent of students created a series
of similar graphs focusing on one dimension. This way of thinking aligns with
Category 2. Thirty-seven percent of students argued that a single point represent-
ed at least one of the relationships (for example, see Student 2, Figure 4.10). In
Figures 4.9 and 4.10, students’ responses reflect similar, narrow focus upon some
aspects of the dynamic and not others.
When students are engaged with data, they often assume there is a linear re-
lationship, in the form of y = mx + b, with m > 0 as in Problems 6b and 6c.51 In
this study, while one problem is constant and the other a parabola, students used
the information to graph a steep line, reflecting the thinking in categories 3–5 (see
Figure 4.11). They use the data the best way that they know how to communicate
the information. An important focus for teachers here is teaching mathematical
conventions of graphing as well as the meaning of each point in the graph.
If the teacher over-emphasizes one type of relationship (e.g., an increasing rela-
tionship) or one type of graph (e.g., line graph), it may lead to a student perception
Student 1
Student 2 Student 3
that all graphs have that form. Teachers should make sure that students are ex-
posed to different types of graphs (e.g., linear, increasing, decreasing, non-linear,
those that go through the origin, and those that don’t). Only using graphs that
are increasing in nature may lead to the common mistake of making all graphs
increasing, even when this does not fit the situation.
Traditional approaches to teaching graphing that begin the process of produc-
ing a graph by first breaking it into step-by-step items in a procedure may, in fact,
perpetuate the perceived problem that graphing is a difficult topic. Instead, present
students with a purposeful task in a familiar context, and students will be able to
act intuitively to use line graphs. Using computers/graphing calculators to help
construct the graphs can help students make the connection between the specific
procedures of creating the graph, parameters of the function, and the meaning of
the graph as it pertains to the data.52 More constructivist approaches toward using
Teachers
• Encourage informal graphing prior to formal graphing as a way for stu-
dents to get on paper their ideas about change.
• Help students see the relationship between the two variables in the dy-
namic of the graph. That is, how are the changes in the graph reflected in
changes in the variables?
• Use students’ attempts at constructing a graph to model a real-world situ-
ation as a basis for conversation about the need to account for multiple
factors in a graph.
186 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
SCALING
Common Core Standards: 8.EE.5 8.F.5
Here we extend the discussion we initiated in the last section (Constructing
Graphs from Real-World Data) on the difficulty students had in understanding
scaling when creating axes for graphs (see also Patterns & Functions: Graph-
ing Functions). “A complete understanding of graphical representation means
realizing what visual features of the graph will not change under the change of
scales (e.g., the x- and y-intercepts) and what features change when the scales are
altered (e.g., the geometrical angles that the line creates with each of the axes).”55
Consider Figure 4.13: If a student constructed one of those graphs, what would
Graph 1 Graph 2
Graph 3 Graph 4
be the difficulty with their understanding? In each case, students struggle with the
conventions of how to label the axes. In graph 2, for instance, students thought
that it was possible to have different scales on the positive and negative parts of
the axes.56
Looking at each of these graphs, it becomes clear that in order to understand
the axes of a graph, students need to comprehend:
• Consistent intervals along an axis (e.g., counting by 2’s, unlike Graph 1),
• Numerical order/continuous (unlike Graph 1 or 4),
• Scaling must be the same along each axis but can be different between axes
(unlike Graph 2), and
• Numbers grow from left to right or from bottom to top (unlike graph 3).
Some students believe that the points on the axes are discrete rather than continu-
ous, thinking that there aren’t any points between the marked points on the axis,
perhaps reflected in an image such as Figure 4.14.57 Students may simply graph
the data available to them. Other students “may place data points on successive,
evenly spaced intervals regardless of their magnitude.”58
When students don’t understand scaling, interpretation of graphs can be heav-
ily influenced by the intervals chosen for the graph.59 Consider the graphs in Fig-
ure 4.15. They each imply a very different story of the state of the stock market.
However, the data is exactly the same. The only thing that has changed is the scale
of the y axis. Similarly, in Table 4.9, students are asked to view three graphs and
determine which two showed the same information. About one-third of students
didn’t realize that the b graph was the same data but differently scaled. In order to
be good consumers and conveyors of information, students must comprehend the
role scaling plays in the construction of graphs.
188 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Graph 1 Graph 2
There are unique challenges with scaling when using graphing calculators—as
opposed to apps or computer software—that can be viewed as limitations of the
technology or great opportunities for conversation. Students often do not pay at-
tention to scale and the format of the window of a graphing calculator. Research-
ers working with students on Problem 8 in Table 4.10 found that less than 16%
recognized that the visual representation of the slope of a linear graph depends on
the scaling of the axes.60 Simply changing the scale on a graph can have a great
impact on the difficulty of the task for students. In many cases, students continue
TABLE 4.9. Problem 7: Scaling Problem--Which Two Show the Same Information?
to read the scale as if it was in single units even when it has been changed to count
by 2s, 5s, or other increments.
Encouraging students to use graphing software to enter data and make several
similar graphs can promote discussion and understanding regarding which type
of graph and scale best portrays the trends and patterns associated with the task
being investigated. Researchers have found some student difficulties when us-
ing graphing calculators, including challenges in interpreting scale changes when
zooming;61 the appearance that perpendicular lines are not perpendicular, depend-
ing on the scale (see Table 4.11 for a student interview);62 and the tendency to ac-
cept whatever picture appears as the initial graph on the screen without zooming
in or out.63 These technology tools can improve student understanding of scale as
well as distort perceptions;64 therefore, care needs to be taken in how teachers ad-
dress student misconceptions based on the technology. Students may benefit from
confronting the limitations of the technology and attempting to explain them. Tab-
let apps such as Math Flyer allow students to pinch zoom in and out to quickly
change the scales and see how those manipulations alter the visual representation
of the graph.
Teachers
• Do not always choose functions that fit neatly into a grid that spans from
-10 to 10 on the x and y-axes. Students need to learn that the shape of the
graph depends on the window you are viewing it through.
• Work with students on graphs in which the axes are not scaled equally.
• Encourage students to manipulate the scales and intervals of graphs and
interpret the visual change in comparison with the data and symbolic rep-
resentation.
• Teachers may want to avoid linear functions when helping students un-
derstand the shape and scale of graphs because linear functions are easy
to graph, but do not contain enough features to develop students’ visual
intuition. It may be more effective to analyze the graphs of linear functions
after more complicated algebraic functions.
Analysis of Change (Graphing) • 191
a
Percent of 18 9th- and 10th-grade students in Moschkovich’s (1999) study.
192 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
either the y-intercept value or the slope. Data from research interviews with stu-
dents indicate that “the use of the x-intercept was more than a superficial mistake
from which students could recuperate.”70 Students had well-thought-out justifica-
tions for their actions, often having a “three-slot-schema” for equations in which
they look for three pieces: the slope, y-intercept, and x-intercept.71 Students may
expect the x-intercept to be just as salient as the y-intercept in the equation as it
is in the graph.
The idea that the value of the x-intercept should appear in the equation “y = mx
+ b” is persistent even with instructional intervention.72 One way to help students
change this misconception is to guide them towards understanding the actual re-
lationship between the x-intercept and the equation y = mx + b. Students should
explore the case where m = 1, and see the connection between how the changing
value of b affects the x-intercept. Next, students should explore cases where the
slope is not 1. As the value of the slope changes, students can see how the shift in
m relates to the steepness of the slope of the graph, and a shift of the x-intercept
toward the origin. This may guide students away from seeing the x-intercept as a
reflection of a change in b and seeing it as also depending on m.73
meaning of the y-intercept in relation to the real-world data. While m will always
represent the rate of change, b may not have a practical interpretation.81 It is im-
portant that students realize the nature of a line as infinitely progressing in two
directions and the y-intercept as the point (0, b) at which the line intersects with
the y-axis. Data can often be interpreted as going in either direction (e.g., the more
tickets you sell, the more money you make or the less tickets you sell, the less
money you make) so the idea of “starting point” may create misconceptions with
students regarding direction of data. The discussion of a linear relationship and
the b value represents exactly the amount that the line has been shifted vertically.
In the form y = mx + b, a change in b shifts the graph horizontally b units, but
−
m
the horizontal shift is impacted by both the b value and the slope of the line.
Technology can play an important role in helping students see the connec-
tion across representations, particularly between graphs and equations. Graphing
calculators have historically been a useful tool for allowing students to problem
solve, develop conceptual understanding, quickly graph multiple functions on the
same set of axes, and consider patterns.86 However, students and teachers often
struggle to understand how to use the devices effectively.87 The new-generation
mathematical apps are much more dynamic and allow for seeing hundreds of
variations in seconds. Apps mentioned earlier, such as Desmos and Math Flyer,
use sliders to allow students to vary the values of m and b, and explore how
the graphical representation of the function is influenced by the coefficients in
the algebraic equation. The Center for Algebraic Thinking’s Linear Model allows
students to manipulate a table, equation, or graph to see changes in the other two
simultaneously. The app Lion Grapher is similar to the former program Green
Globs, in which players must adjust the y = mx + b equation to make a line go into
a lion’s mouth. These apps encourage students to think about how manipulations
of values for equations of a line will influence the path of lines.
Teachers
• Place a greater emphasis on qualitative graphs when helping students make
initial connections with algebraic symbolism, particularly with the mean-
ing of the y-intercept.
• Use a range of discrete and continuous data as a resource for identifying
algebraic relationships and discuss the nature of the data in relation to the
algebraic symbolism.
• Be careful with using informal terminology such as “starting point” when
helping students learn the idea of y-intercept, as misconceptions may de-
velop about the nature of the data and relation to the algebraic symbols.
• Help students understand how the slope of a line is related to the x-inter-
cept.
• It is important to honor how students see and understand changes in graphs,
connect that perspective to the algebraic relationships, and discuss effi-
ciencies of perspective such as seeing line movement as vertical change
because b is easily isolated in y = mx + b.
Analysis of Change (Graphing) • 197
UNDERSTANDING SLOPE
Common Core Standards: 6.EE.9, 8.F.5
“Understanding and graphing slope requires that a student be able to track two
variables and the nature of their co-varying relationship.”88 Slope describes both
the direction (increasing or decreasing) and steepness of a line, and is the visual
representation of rate of change. On one hand, slope may be easy for students
to comprehend because of their experience with the steepness of a hill or ratios
such as two oranges for a dollar. On the other hand, having to consider at least
two different points and the relationship between them can be challenging for
students. Many students think of slope as a geometric ratio—”I go up this much
and I go over this much.” Or they may have a limited algebraic understanding of
slope—simply using the linear equation y = mx + b to identify m as the slope.
Generally, there is quite a bit of research discussing students’ difficulties under-
standing slope.89
Researchers suggest that there are four dimensions of understanding the con-
cept of slope (see Table 4.17). A symbolic understanding simply regards where m
resides in the linear equation, as the coefficient of x. A graphical understanding
is one of visual interpretation: that m communicates the steepness of the line. A
numerical understanding involves the computation of the change in the two y
values of two points, over the change in the two x values. Lastly, understanding of
the slope in context is realizing the meaning of m as the rate of change, which is a
ratio of two values such as 27 miles per one gallon. This framework was extended
by other researchers to include all the concepts in Table 4.18.
Students are generally successful in identifying which term is slope in an
equation, demonstrating symbolic understanding (see Table 4.19, Problem 10),
although they have less success when it is not written in y = mx + b form (see
Table 4.19, Problems 11 & 12) or when m is written as a decimal.90 It is interesting
to note that so many students opted for the ratio answers, likely because they have
extensive experience thinking about slope as rise over run and struggle to think of
a number such as 3 as 3 .
1
Students are less successful in determining whether two lines might be paral-
lel (Determining property). Consider Table 4.20, Problem 13, in which students
are asked to decide whether PQ is parallel to the line y = 3x -5. Students had
considerable difficulty, with less than 10% giving an accurate answer, saying the
lines weren’t parallel because “one is longer than the other” or “because the line
is diagonal” and saying the lines were parallel “because they both go together” or
“because the lines have 3 marks on the y axis all the way along.”91
One common error found in the research is slope-height confusion.92 Students
frequently don’t know whether information from the slope or the height of the
graph is useful for solving a problem. For example, in Problem 14a (see Table
4.21), students were asked to recognize that the slopes of the lines represent the
speed of the objects and Line A rises more steeply than Line B, so Object A is
traveling faster than Object B. In one study, 79.6% of 7th- and 8th-grade students
answered this type of question incorrectly.93 Of those who answered incorrectly,
researchers found that many students chose the higher line “due to failure to re-
alize that information about the velocity cannot be extracted from the height.”94
Students also tended to state that the two objects had the same speed at t = 7, not
realizing that the two lines have different slopes, so they would never have the
same speed.95 Some students simply made graph reading errors or had reasonable
Analysis of Change (Graphing) • 199
3
c. (22%)
2
d. -2 (1%)
11. The slope of the line y= 2 - 3x is: a. 3 (8%)
b. 2 (14%)
3
c. − (16%)
2
d. -3 (56%)
12. The slope of the line 2y = 6x +5 is: a. 6 (14%)
6
b. (11%)
5
c. 2 (4%)
d. 3 (52%)
a
Percent of 252 12th-grade students in Barr (1981).
but faulty logic for their choice, rather than having held any misconceptions about
the “picture,” so it is important for teachers to probe students’ thinking.96 Other
research indicates that this confusion can be effectively addressed by instruction,
particularly with technology.97
Slope can represent real-life situations such as the steepness of a hill, or slope
can measure the rate of change of two varying quantities. When dealing with
physical situations of steepness, students rarely use proportional reasoning (see
Rate and Proportional Reasoning), comparing the height of an object to the base
of the object. Only 22.7% of students in a high school physics class used a ratio
to explain their thinking for Problem 15 (Table 4.22).98 Instead, students focused
on angles (“Measure the angle at the top of the ramp”) or a single attribute, such
as the length of the ramp (“I would find the length of the hypotenuse”) or used
whatever they knew (“One-half base times height”).99
Students are often taught slope as a fraction, with the change in y over the change
in x. With only this instrumental understanding of slope, students may have a dif-
ficult time with the way a line is positioned in a plane or the idea of rate of change.100
They may struggle to understand the idea that the slope is a ratio or rate of change
(functional relationship) and to think of ratio as a measurement of steepness (physi-
cal relationship).101 For instance, when the slope is written as a decimal (y = 3.4x
+ 2) students have difficulty thinking of slope as a rate of change.102 Researchers
find that students tend to have a better understanding of slope as a rate of change
than as a measure of steepness.103 These variations on the idea of slope are based
in students’ understanding of fractions, decimals, and percentages; they are more
likely to use the idea of rate or ratio when dealing with symbolic equations than in
real-world situations. Students are most successful in calculating rate as a function
of time, which in many ways is the most intuitive form of rate, rather than measures
of steepness.104 If a student knows that they have traveled 200 miles in four hours,
they are able to identify the rate as 50 miles in one hour.
An important reason for students to understand slope as rate is to enhance
their understanding of the relationship between the points on a line. Students can
look at the relationship between the points in Figure 4.19 in two ways; first, in an
additive relationship in which each x coordinate is 3 more than the previous one;
and second, that the x coordinate is 3 times the y coordinate. Research indicates
that as students gain understanding of slope, they may have difficulties recogniz-
ing multiplicative relationships.105 This is evident from Problem 16 in Table 4.23,
as students struggle to understand the relationship between the points in order to
determine additional points. There is also quite a bit of research indicating that
students don’t understand that there are infinitely many possible ordered pairs
making up a line.106 In Problem 17, less than 20% of students understand that lines
contain an infinite number of points.107
One way to encourage students to develop an understanding of slope, and par-
ticularly the multiplicative relationship between the infinite number of points on
a line, is the Center for Algebraic Thinking’s app, Point Plotter (see Figure 4.20).
202 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
17. Draw a line through the points Some finite number (e.g., 4, 5, 8) (63.8%,
(2,5), (3,7), and (5,11). 69.2%, 51.6%)
How many points do you think lie on the line “Hundreds,” “lots,” etc. (12.6%, 5.1%, 9.7%)
altogether? “Infinite,” “as many as you like,” etc. (6.7%,
6.2%, 19.6%)
a
Percent of 1789 13-, 14-, and 15-year-old students in Hart, et al. (2004), respectively.
In this game, students must determine as many points as possible between the two
given points in a timed or untimed version. The “easy” points at the grid crossings
are quickly done and leave students to figure out the relationship of the points
between points. In order to be successful in this game, students need to develop an
understanding of the slope and the multiplicative relationships between the points
to determine new points on the line. As students use strategies such as taking half
the distance between two points (developing/using the midpoint formula) or com-
ing up with the slope and equation of a line to calculate new points, they come to
realize the infinite possibilities.
Teachers
• There is often a disconnect between being able to calculate slope and ac-
tually knowing what slope represents, so teachers need to ask students to
explain the meaning of the slope in a given situation. One way to prompt
student thinking is to ask, “What does the slope represent in the context of
this situation?”
• Researchers suggest that, when preparing lessons to teach the concept of
slope to students, a teacher should reflect on the following questions:108
a. What representations of slope are in my concept definitions?
b. Do I have a flexible understanding of representations for slope?
c. What representations of slope do I use?
d. What real-world situations do I use to illustrate the concept of slope?
e. What is my knowledge of student difficulties with slope?
• Both the Submariner and Lion Grapher apps from the Center for Algebraic
Thinking create a context for students to think about how to manipulate
the slope and y-intercept of a linear equation to achieve a goal. As students
play either game, they develop an intuitive understanding of the steepness
and direction of a line by changing the slope and/or y-intercept.
• One activity to promote an understanding of steepness as a ratio is to have
students physically measure real-world handrails, sidewalks, and ramps.
Models of ramps that have slopes that are gentle, moderate, and steep can
also be used. Compare the ratio of the rise of the ramp with the run. Stu-
dents should realize that the ratio with the greatest value relates to the ramp
with the steepest slope.109
204 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Graph 1 Graph 2
due north, then east,” “went along a corridor, then up in a lift, then along another
corridor;” (b) “going along, then turning left,” “going back the way he came;” and
(c) “climbing a mountain,” and “going up, going down, then up again.”119
There are two particular technological tools that may help students make the
connection between reality and the data on a graph. First, computer-based motion
detectors provide an opportunity for students to have a physical, experiential con-
nection to the data regarding position versus time that appears on a graph (see Fig-
ure 4.23). Motion detectors can be used with moving objects as well as with mov-
ing students; activities can include matching a pre-made graph or exploring the
interaction between movement toward and away from the detector and the result-
ing graph. Research suggests that motion detectors encourage students to evaluate
their perspectives regarding the connection between their bodily movement and
what appears on a screen.120 Conversation about their experience and connecting
their bodily motion with mathematical symbolism can enhance learning.121 Also,
due to interaction with a motion detector, students’ abilities were significantly
more developed in interpreting, modeling, and transforming graphical data.122
The second helpful tool is the Center for Algebraic Thinking’s Action Grapher
app (Figure 4.24) that focuses directly on the difficulty students have in under-
Correct Responsesa
a) 9.5%, 8.4%, 15%
b) 11.1%, 9.3%, 15.7%
c) 14.7%, 17.2%, 25.2%
a
Percent of 1798 13-, 14-, and 15-year-old students in Kerslake (2004), respectively.
Source: Kerslake (2004)
206 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
standing the connection between a graph and the motion of a bicyclist on a hill.
The student takes a bike on a journey up and down a hill and sees three different
graphical representations of what is going on, based on elevation, speed, and dis-
tance versus time. First, the student sketches what she thinks each of the graphs
might look like. Next, she plays the animation to see how her hypothesis com-
pares to each of the actual graphs. The student can change the terrain when ready
to test new hypotheses.
Teachers
• Use computers whenever possible to develop students’ understanding of
interpreting graphs. Graphs can be formed instantaneously as students cre-
ate, input, or manipulate data, as graphs are less likely to be seen by stu-
dents as “static pictures and more likely to be seen as involving dynamic
relationships.”123
• Instruction may include tasks in which students collect their own data, cre-
ate a graph, and are then asked questions about the graph they created (e.g.,
Where on the graph does it show...?).
• Consider having students start learning to read graphs within contexts that
are familiar to them, and then move toward traditional curricular topics
once internal graph reading competency has been established. For exam-
ple, students can collect data on the drop height and bounce height of a ball,
or height of a plant over time.
208 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
INTERPRETING GRAPHS
Common Core Standards: 8.EE. 5 & 6, 8.F. 1, 2, 3, 4, & 5
“A graph is useful only to those who know where to focus their attention.”124
Information often comes to us in the form of graphs, whether it is about which
ketchup pours more slowly or how much our mortgage will cost us over time.
“Our society is becoming more reliant on the representation of information in
graphical forms as traditional communication and literacy demands change and
adapt to what could be considered a burgeoning information age.”125 We need
to interpret information in order to be entertained or informed. This ability to
interpret is sometimes termed graph sense or graphicacy (based on “graphical
literacy”), meaning the skill to read graphs and learn from that data.126 The skills
required for graph sense are:127
(a) In which year were television sets first available for purchase in stores?
(b) In which year did 50 percent of the households own at least one television
set? Satisfactory: 10%
(c) Write a sentence or two comparing growth in the percentage of households Incorrect: 21%
with television sets across the three decades of the 50s, 60s, and 70s. Minimal: 35%
(d) The points (1950, 10) and (1970, 90) lie on the graph above. They are also Partial: 29%
solutions of y = 4x – 7790. However, if the graph of y = 4x – 7790 were Omitted: 6%
drawn for 1940 to 1997, it would not look like the graph shown. Explain why
not.
Problem 20
Martine is filling a rectangular fish tank using two hoses that fill the tank at the A) 10%
same flow rate. When the tank is about half full, she turns off one hose but does B) 18%
not change the flow rate of the other hose. Which of the following graphs best C) 52%
represents how the depth of the water in the tank changes over time? D) 15%
E) 4%
a
Percent of students responding on NAEP (2013)
210 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Based on these three levels, teaching graph comprehension might take place at
three successive stages.130 At the initial stage, students should be able to extract
information from the data, or “read the data.” Teacher questions such as “How
many cars were sold in 1980?” is an appropriate cue for this level. To be suc-
cessful at the initial stage, students must understand the conventions of the graph
design, including the scale and labels.
At the intermediate stage, students find relationships within the data, or “read
between the data.” Teacher questions to encourage this type of thinking might
include condensing data categories (such as “How many boxes have more than 30
raisins?”), identification of trends in parts of the data (“What is the relationship
between car sales and engine size between 1970 and 1985?”), and observing rela-
tionships in visual displays without reference to the contextual meaning (“How do
the changes in these two curves compare?”). At the intermediate stage, students
must be able to make comparisons and perform the appropriate computations.
The final stage “reads beyond the data,” and requires interpolation/extrapola-
tion such as prediction or generation. Teacher prompts to develop this type of
thinking might include reduction of the data to a single statement (“From June
15 to June 30, what was the trend in the value of stock X?”), extending the data
to form predictions (“If students opened another box of raisins, how many might
they expect to find?”), and using the data as evidence to support arguments (“If
this graph was offered as a piece of evidence to prove true the statement, ‘The
economy is improving,’ how would you describe the connection between the
graph and the attempt to prove the statement true?”). To be successful in the final
stage, students must also be able to relate the data to the context of the situation.
An important aspect of graph interpretation is the ability to understand the
connection between an equation and a visual representation. Researchers call
this the Cartesian Connection: that each point on a line represents an ordered
212 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
pair that satisfies the equation representing the line.131 If the equation of the
line is y = 3x + 2, for instance, the point on the line (1, 5) satisfies the equation
because the ordered pair makes the equation true: 5 = 3(1) + 2. “Understanding
both the equation-to-graph and graph-to-equation connections is considered fun-
damental in developing the flexibility to move fluently among representations.”132
In a study looking at both directions of process, the majority of students had par-
ticular difficulty moving from the graph to the equation—not understanding that
any point on the graph would be a solution to the equation.133 Students’ difficulties
in this area seem to be caused more by instructional rather than cognitive fac-
tors.134 Teachers can help by moving back and forth between algebraic and graphi-
cal representations so that students can clearly understand the connection.135
Another area in which students struggle when interpreting graphs is under-
standing when a point or an interval is the most useful information.136 For in-
stance, students were asked to determine whether boys or girls are growing faster
in Figure 4.26, and an example of their responses is in Table 4.28. In this conver-
sation, it is clear that the student believes that the highest point (see discussion of
slope-height confusion in Understanding Slope) is what is important in answer-
ing the question. She fails to understand that the rate of change at that point is
what will answer the question, which requires her to look at the interval around
that point. “Instead of identifying the largest increase, students identified the larg-
est value.”137
When studying students’ eye movement as they worked on graph problems,
researchers found that the format of the graph (line, bar, pie, etc.) also had an
influence on students’ understanding and the way they interpreted and inferred
information,138 applying different types of cognition to different kinds of graphs.139
Analysis of Change (Graphing) • 213
TABLE 4.28. Interview for Average Weight of Boys Versus Girls Graph
Student: Em, I wrote Girls.
Interviewer: Why did you write girls?
Student 1: Because they weigh more than boys and this means that they could be taller than ...
they could be eating more.
Interviewer: Ok. So, she is saying that “girls” because they weigh more...
Student 1: Yeah, they weigh more at that point. [Indicating where the boys and girls are 14.]
Interviewer: At that point.
Student 2: But the gradient is steeper.
Student 1: Yeah, I know, but it is not an area, it is just for one point.
Source: Hadjidemetriou & Williams (2001)
The visual characteristics, as well as the context for the information, impacted
students’ ability to make inferences. Students are also often confused about when
data is discrete or continuous.140 They have difficulty in determining the meaning
of a line graph, often only “seeing” the marked points (see Figure 4.27).141 In one
large study, for Problem 21, many students said that there were no points between
two marked points, while others explained that there was just one, probably mean-
ing the midpoint (see Table 4.29).142 In another study, students believed that points
had some mass or size rather than being an abstract entity, which may lead them
to believe they can only “fit” so many in the space.143 They often connect discrete
points when it is not appropriate, indicating that they struggle to understand what
contexts dictate what type of data.144
An integrated approach that emphasizes both the mechanical interpretation of
graphs and the power of graphs as a model for studying relationships between
variables is necessary. Student difficulties appear to arise because variable rela-
tionships presented symbolically are too abstract for most students in the 11–13
age range.145 Students should be involved in the entire process—gathering the
data, creating the graph, and analyzing variables. It is possible to teach graphing
skills in isolation if all we expect is for students to perform algorithmically. How-
ever, if we want them to interpret visual data in a way that indicates they compre-
hend meaning and subtleties, then that requires understanding the whole process.
Teachers
• Graphs presented with only qualitative features (i.e., no numerical values
such as Problem 20 in Table 4.25) might help students draw on their com-
mon sense and reality-checking strategies.149
• Research indicates that spreadsheets have power in helping students under-
stand the connection between data and their graphs.150
• Students should be involved in both graph construction and interpretation
right from the beginning in order for them to make sense of graphs.
Analysis of Change (Graphing) • 215
• The acceleration principle: If the green car is behind the blue car and is
accelerating very rapidly while the blue car is not accelerating at all, then
the green car will get closer to the blue car.
• The speed principle: If the blue car is ahead of the green car at time t0 and
the blue car is going faster than the green car, then the blue car will stay
ahead of the green car and will actually get farther ahead of the green car.
These are logical principles based on his experience. However, they don’t gen-
eralize to more complex situations, such as a car entering a freeway on-ramp.
The red car could be on the freeway traveling at 70 MPH while the blue car is
entering the freeway and accelerating rapidly from 30 MPH to 65 MPH. In that
situation, the blue car would not be getting closer to the red car; in fact, the red car
is moving farther away from the blue car. The speed principle does not generalize
because it considers only the cars’ velocities at a particular point in time and does
not take acceleration into account.159
A complex understanding of speed as a rate of change includes comprehending
the difference between speed and acceleration. Each of the ideas of instantaneous
speed, average speed, and acceleration are integrally tied to the context, so it is
important that students begin to get a sense of the relative comparisons based on
contexts, such as getting on the freeway versus a constant speed on the freeway.
When students have a strong understanding of these ideas, they are more likely to
be successful when the context moves to mixture types of problems.
Teachers
• Computer-based activities can be very effective in helping students differ-
entiate their understanding between velocity and acceleration.160 Graphs of
realistic representations of motion can “help students confront paradoxes
that arise when their new view of motion conflicts with their previous un-
differentiated view.”161
• Researchers found that having students interact with motion detectors and
moving objects, then seeing the resulting graphs in real time was helpful in
repairing students’ misconceptions about speed and acceleration.162 From
one student’s perspective: “Doing that one lab where we actually had to
come up with the scenarios and then kind of play them out to see whether
they worked—that helped out the most, I think.”163 When computers are
used to link physical motion to graphical shape, the tools become resources
for understanding. “Graphs created by a motion detector in response to a
student’s own body motion embody a link between a mathematical repre-
sentation and body motion.”164
• Computer simulations like the Center for Algebraic Thinking app Tortoise
and the Hare Algebra (Figure 4.30) can be used to provide examples that
help students explore the effects of speed and different rates of change on
the classic race between the tortoise and the hare. In this app, students ma-
nipulate how many feet (or meters) per second each racer travels—includ-
ing a little nap time for the hare—and then watch the animated race. They
can also change perspectives between seeing the overall map of the race
218 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
and watching individual racers up close to see how the rates of change for
each racer look in each context. This provides additional experience with
the concept of speed as a rate of change as students try to solve challenges
posed in the app, such as how fast would the tortoise need to travel to finish
the 100-yard race in 8 seconds? Or, if rabbit travels at 50 ft/sec and tortoise
travels at 40 ft/sec, how far behind will turtle be when the race ends?
Analysis of Change (Graphing) • 219
MOTION GRAPHS
Common Core Standards: 6.EE.9, 8.F.5
Interpreting a Cartesian graph of objects in motion is a process of understand-
ing the representation of the relationship between selected elements. The research
describes how students attempt to make sense of graphs related to problems of
relative motion. In one study, researchers asked 10th-grade students to interpret a
graph of students in motion (see Table 4.30).165 Many students had difficulty in-
terpreting what was happening at points A, B, C, and D in the graph for Problem
22 because both students were moving. In the first part of the interview (see Table
4.31), Student 1 saw the horizontal segment of the graph as representing that one
of the people stopped, when in fact it meant that the distance between the two
people stayed the same while they were walking.
In the interview, each of the students is at a different place in his or her think-
ing, struggling to make sense of the graph. The diverse perspectives keep them
in conversation trying to resolve their differences. Their primary challenge is to
TABLE 4.32. Interview with Students for Problem 22: Instructor Intervention
I: If Peter is walking faster than Marna, will the distance between them remain the same?
S1: No. What happens between B and C?
S2: Peter stops.
I: Is the CBR still moving?
S3: Yes...No, I don’t know. Did Marna stop? She has the CBR.
The conversation is changing from relative speed to relative distance.
I: What does the point A represent?
S2: 1 meter.
I: Is that a distance or a speed?
S1: A distance.
I: What is the distance measuring?
S2: No, from B to C they are walking at the same speed because the distance between them is
staying the same.
A reasonable explanation for BC has been made by S2.
S3: Then from C to D the distance between Peter and Marna is decreasing so Marna would be
walking faster than Peter. Does that make sense?
S1 and S2 agree. Students have come to a reasonable interpretation of the graph that makes sense.
think about the difference between absolute and relative motion. The dynamic
changes as the instructor enters into the conversation and helps them focus their
attention on particular aspects of the story (see Table 4.32).
Cartesian graphs are used to describe real world phenomena and convey com-
plex mathematical meanings. One of the challenges for students working with
motion graphs is that “space and time have to be understood in relational terms.
Both space and time have to be measured numerically against an arbitrary start-
ing point.”166 Researchers suggest that the logic of interpreting a Cartesian repre-
sentation of relative motion can become apparent through discussion as opposed
to direct instruction. The discussion is a “social process related to the manner
in which students become progressively aware, through personal deeds and in-
terpretations, of the cultural logic of mathematical entities.”167 One researcher
described the process as “progressive disentanglement.”168 The role of the teacher
in helping students move toward particular mathematical ideas was significant.
The interviewer for this problem used a number of questions to probe students’
thinking. Sometimes it is useful for the teacher to help move discussion forward
by breaking down the dynamic of motion in the graph with questions such as the
following:
1. What does point A represent? (The distance between Marna and Peter at
time t=0.)
Analysis of Change (Graphing) • 221
2. What is happening over segment AB? (The distance between Marna and
Peter is increasing.)
3. What is happening over segment BC? (The distance between Marna and
Peter is staying the same.)
4. What is happening over the segment CD? (The distance between Marna
and Peter is decreasing.)
Another important part of the process was students’ gestures to communicate their
thinking. Researchers find that gestures can help students process their ideas as
well as provide teachers with additional formative assessment of students’ think-
ing, particularly with motion graphs.169 “Gestures help construct better mental
representations and mental operations to solve problems.”170 Gestures can be
physical, bodily movements as well as “inked gestures” of drawings on paper.171
Teachers
• Students can bring real-world digital photos and video of motion into
mathematical software such as The Geometer’s Sketchpad and Vernier’s
Video Physics. Using pictures from students’ experience can personalize
mathematics, engage students, and integrate math with other subjects to
help students makes sense of the mathematical dynamics.172
• Research indicates that encouraging students’ use of gestures with hands or
body may help them communicate and think about motion.173
• Another approach to helping students understand motion graphs is the
Center for Algebraic Thinking’s Action Grapher app (see Figure 4.31). It
was built in response to research on students’ thinking with motion graphs.
The biking activity in the app was described earlier. Another activity is fill-
ing flasks with water. The student watches a flask fill with water and must
determine which of three representations accurately describes that motion.
The activity includes a “custom” option in which students can test their
hypotheses by designing their own flasks and filling them with water. The
app automatically constructs three different representations for the student
to choose from as the correct representation.
ENDNOTES
1
NCTM, 2000
2
Demana, Schoen, & Waits, 1993; Leinhardt, Zaslavsky, & Stein, 1990
3
Dugdale, 1993; Leinhardt, Zaslavsky, & Stein, 1990; Mitchelmore & Cavanagh, 2000; Moschk-
ovich, Schoenfeld, & Arcavi, 1993
4
Cisneros, 2014
5
National Governors Association Center for Best Practices, Council of Chief State School Officers
(2010).
6
Jitendra, et al., 2009, p. 250, based on Boyer, Levine, & Huttenlocher, 2008, p. 1478, and Fuson &
Abrahamson, 2005; Lamon, 2007; Post, Behr, & Lesh, 1988
7
Lamon, 1994, p. 90 based on Lesh, Post, & Behr, 1988 and reflected as well in Kilpatrick, Swafford
& Findell, 2001
8
Post, Behr, & Lesh, 1988
9
Tourniaire & Pulos, 1985, p. 181
10
Karplus, Pulos, & Stage, 1983b, p. 219
11
Fuson & Abrahamson, 2005, p. 213; Hart, 1981
12
Hart, 1984, p. 5
13
Tourniaire & Pulos, 1985
14
Tourniare & Pulos, 1985
15
Hart, 1981; Noelting, 1980a, 1980b
16
Tourniare & Pulos, 1985
17
Lesh, Post, & Behr, 1988, p. 93-94
18
Post, Behr, & Lesh, 1988, p. 78
19
Karplus, Pulos, & Stage, 1983; Noelting, 1980a
20
Jitendra, et al., 2009
21
Silvestre & da Ponte, 2012, p. 74, based on Stanley, McGowan, & Hull, 2003
22
Hart, 1981; Tourniaire & Pulos, 1985
23
Hart, 1981
24
Lesh, Post, & Behr, 1988, pp. 104-105
25
Lamon, 1994; Tourniaire & Pulos, 1985
26
Hembree, 1992; Koedinger & Terao, 2002; Mayer, 1989, 2005
27
Rau, 2016, p. 1. Also Booth & Koedinger, 2012
28
Orrill & Brown, 2012; Watanabe, 2015
29
Murata, 2008
30
Middleton & van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, 1995
31
Murata, 2008, p. 379
32
Post, Behr, & Lesh, 1988, p. 86
33
Post, Behr, & Lesh, 1988, p. 79
34
Orton, 1983; Ubuz, 2007
35
Herbert, 2010; Herbert & Pence, 2009
36
Thompson, 1996
37
Karplus, Pulos, & Stage, 1983; Vergnaud, 1980
Analysis of Change (Graphing) • 223
38
Herbert, 2010, p. 246
39
Saunders & Jesunathadas, 1988
40
Herbert & Pierce, 2007
41
Tourniaire, 1984
42
Sherin, 2000, p. 403 quoting Palmer, 1977
43
Sherin, 2000, p. 400
44
Based on Sherin, 2000, p. 408
45
Sherin, 2000
46
Sherin, 2000, p. 415
47
Leinhardt, Zaslavsky, & Stein, 1990, p. 12
48
diSessa, Hammer, Sherin, & Kolpakowski, 1991
49
Mevarech & Kramarsky, 1997
50
Bell, Brekke, & Swann, 1987a, 1987b; Mevarech & Kramarsky, 1997
51
Dreyfus and Eisenberg, 1982; Markovits, Eylon, & Brukheimer, 1983; and Zaslavsky, 1997
52
Barclay, 1987; Burrill, et al., 2002; Ellington, 2003; Hall, & Chamblee, 2013; McClaran, 2013;
Roschelle, Pea, Hoadley, Gordin, & Means, 2000
53
Li & Ma, 2011
54
Delgado & Lucero, 2015, p. 652
55
Leinhardt, Zaslavsky, & Stein, 1990, p. 19
56
Hart, 2004
57
Leinhardt, Zaslavsky, & Stein, 1990
58
Delgado & Lucero, 2015, p. 652 based on Adams & Shrum, 1990; Brasell, 1990; Mevarech & Kra-
marsky, 1997; Vergnaud & Errecalde, 1980; Wavering, 1989
59
American Association for the Advancement of Science, 1993
60
Mitchelmore & Cavanagh, 2000
61
Goldenberg, 1988
62
Vonder Embse & Engebretsen, 1996
63
Steele, 1994
64
Burrill, et al., 2002; Ellington, 2003; Mitchelmore & Cavanagh, 2000
65
Mitchelmore & Cavanagh, 2000, p. 262
66
Goldenberg and KIiman 1988; Mitchelmore & Cavanagh, 2000
67
Delgado & Lucero, 2015; diSessa, Hammer, Sherin, & Kolpakowski, 1991
68
Schoenfeld, Smith, & Arcavi, 1993; Moschkovich, 1989, 1990, 1992
69
Moschkovich, 1999
70
Moschkovich, 1999, p. 170
71
Schoenfeld, Smith, & Arcavi, 1993
72
Moschkovich, 1999
73
Moschkovich, 1999
74
Davis, 2007, p. 388 based on Smith, Arcavi, & Schoenfeld, 1989
75
Hattikudur, Prather, Asquith, Alibali, Knuth, & Nathan, 2012
76
Barr, 1980
77
Bardini & Stacey, 2006, p. 115
78
Davis, 2007
79
Davis, 2007
80
Pierce, Stacey, & Bardini, 2010
81
Pierce, Stacey, & Bardini, 2010
82
Hattikudur, Prather, Asquith, Alibali, Knuth, & Nathan, 2012
83
Hattikudur, Prather, Asquith, Alibali, Knuth, & Nathan, 2012, p. 236
84
Goldenberg, 1988; Moschkovich, 1990, 1999
85
Chiu, Kessel, Moschkovich, & Munoz-Nunez, 2001
86
Boers-van Oosterum, 1990; Demana, Schoen, & Waits, 1993; Dunham & Dick, 1994; Ellington,
2003, 2006; Hennessy, Fung, & Scanlon, 2001; Kenney, 2014; Slavit, 1994,
224 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
87
Dewey, Singletary, & Kinzel, 2009; Kenney, 2014
88
Hattikudur, Prather, Asquith, Alibali, Knuth, & Nathan, 2012
89
Bell & Janvier, 1981; Janvier, 1981a; McDermott, Rosenquist, & van Zee, 1987; Orton, 1984; Si-
mon & Blume, 1994; Stump, 2001
90
Barr, 1980, 1981
91
Hart, et al., 2004
92
Beichner, 1993; Brassel and Rowe, 1993; Clement, Mokros, & Schultz, 1986; Hadjidemetriou &
Williams, 2001; Janvier, 1978; Leinhardt, Zaslavsky, & Stein, 1990; McDermott, Rosenquist, van
Zee, 1983; McDermott, Rosenquist, Popp, & van Zee, 1986
93
Clement, Mokros, & Shultz, 1986
94
Mcdermott, Rosenquist, & van Zee, 1987, p. 504
95
Mcdermott, Rosenquist, & van Zee, 1987
96
Hale, 2000; Monk, 1994
97
Mokros & Tinker, 1987
98
Stump, 2001
99
Stump, 2001,p. 82-83
100
Skemp, 1976; Walter & Gerson, 2007
101
Stump, 2001; Swafford & Langrall, 2000
102
Barr 1980, 1981
103
Simon & Blume, 1994; Stump, 2001
104
Simon & Blume, 1994; Stump, 2001
105
Simon and Blume, 1994
106
Hart, 2004; Mevarech & Kramarsky, 1997
107
Hart, 2004
108
Based on Stump, 1997
109
Based on Stump, 2001
110
Berg & Phillips, 1994; Clement, 1985, 1986; Janvier, 1998; Leinhardt, Zaslavsky, & Stein, 1990;
McDermott, Rosenquist, & van Zee, 1987; Monk, 1992; Nemirovsky & Rubin, 1992; Noble &
Nemirovsky, 1995
111
McDermott, Rosenquist, Popp, & van Zee, 1983
112
Monk, 1992
113
Mokros & Tinker, 1987
114
Kerslake, 2004
115
Clement, 1989
116
Monk, 1992; Schultz, Clement, & Mokros, 1986
117
Barclay & TERC, 1986; Schultz, Clement, & Mokros, 1986
118
Based on Brassel and Rowe, 1993
119
Kerslake, 2004, p. 129
120
Nemirovsky, Tierney, & Wright, 1998
121
Ferrara, 2014; Herman, Laumakis, Dover, & Doyle, 2008; Robutti, 2006
122
Kwon, 2002; Phillips, 1997
123
Glazer, 2011, p. 200
124
Knuth, 2000a, p. 504 based on Larkin & Simon, 1987
125
From Lowrie, Diezmann, & Logan, 2011, p. 1, based on Leu, Kinzer, Coiro, & Cammack, 2004
126
Friel & Bright, 1996; Friel, Curcio, & Bright, 2001; Postigo & Pozo, 2004
127
Friel, Curcio, & Bright, 2001, p. 146
128
Padilla, McKenzie, & Shaw, 1986
129
Postigo & Pozo, 2004, pp. 627-629
130
Based on Glazer, 2011; Friel, Curcio, & Bright, 2001; and Wainer, 1992
131
Knuth, 2000a; Moschkovich, Schoenfeld, & Arcavi, 1993; Schoenfeld et al., 1993; Smith et al.,
1989
132
Knuth, 2000a, p. 501
Analysis of Change (Graphing) • 225
133
Knuth, 2000a
134
Knuth, 2000a
135
Knuth, 2000a
136
Hadjidemetriou & Williams, 2001; Leinhardt, Zaslavsky, & Stein, 1990; Mevarech & Kramarsky,
1997
137
Billings & Klanderman, 2000, p. 441 based on Bell & Janvier, 1981; Janvier, 1981b
138
Carpenter & Shah, 1998
139
Jackson, Edwards, & Berger, 1993
140
Leinhardt, Zaslavsky, & Stein, 1990
141
Janvier, 1983; Kerslake, 2004; Mansfield, 1985; Stein & Leinhardt, 1989
142
Kerslake, 2004
143
Mansfield, 1985
144
Kerslake, 2004; Markovits, Eylon, & Bruckheimer, 1986
145
Knuth, 2000b
146
Cobb & Moore, 1997; DiSessa, Hammer, Sherin, & Kolpakowski, 1991; Roth & McGinn, 1997.
147
Cobb & Moore, 1997, p. 816
148
Glazer, 2011, p. 204
149
From Hattikudur, et al., 2012, p. 232 and based on Goldenberg, 1987 and Krabbendam, 1982
150
Ainley, 2000; Ainley, Nardi, & Pratt, 2000
151
Thompson, 1994, p. 37
152
Lobato & Thanheiser, 1999
153
Hale, 2000; Lobato & Thanheiser, 1999; Monk, 1994
154
Thompson, 1992; Thompson, 1994
155
Billings & Klanderman, 2000
156
Thompson, 1994, p. 28
157
Thompson & Thompson, 1996
158
Billings & Klanderman, 2000
159
Hale, 2000, p. 415
160
Dykstra, Boyle, & Monarch, 1992
161
Hale, 2000, p. 416
162
Arzarello, Pezzi, & Robutti, 2007; Hale, 2000
163
Hale, 2000, p. 416
164
Noble & TERC, 2003, p. 3 based on Meira, 1995; Moschkovitch, 1996; Nemirovsky & Noble,
1997; Nemirovsky et al. 1998; Noble, Nemirovsky, Tierney & Wright, 1999; Noble, Nemirovsky,
Wright, & Tierney, 2001
165
Radford, Miranda, & Guzman, 2008
166
Radford, 2009
167
Radford, Miranda, & Guzman, 2008, p. 166
168
Radford, 2009, p. 12
169
Cook, Yip, & Goldin-Meadow, 2012; Noble, 2004; Radford, Miranda, & Guzman, 2008
170
Segal, 2011
171
Radford, 2009, p. 14
172
Pierce, Stacey, & Ball, 2005
173
Herbert & Pierce, 2007; Noble, Nemirovsky, DiMattia, & Wright, 2004
174
Author created interview to articulate a potential student description.
226 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
REFERENCES
Adams, D. D., & Shrum, J. W. (1990). The effects of microcomputer-based laboratory ex-
ercises on the acquisition of line graph construction and interpretation skills by high
school biology students. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 27(8), 777–787.
Ainley, J. (2000). Exploring the transparency of graphs and graphing. Paper presented at
the Twenty Fourth Annual Conference of the International Group for the Psychol-
ogy of Mathematics Education, Hiroshima, Japan.
Ainley, J., Nardi, E., & Pratt, D. (2000). The construction of meanings for trend in active
graphing. International Journal of Computers for Mathematical Learning, 5, 85–114.
American Association for the Advancement of Science (1993). Benchmarks for science
literacy. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Arzarello, F., Pezzi, G., & Robutti, O. (2007). Modelling body motion: An approach to
functions using measuring instruments. Modeling and Applications in Mathematics
Education, 10, 129–136.
Barclay, T. (1987). A graph is worth how many words? Classroom Computer Learning, 7,
46–50.
Barclay, W. L., & Technical Education Research Center, C. M. A. (1986). Graphing mis-
conceptions and possible remedies using microcomputer-based labs. Retrieved
from http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=eric&AN=ED26412
9&site=ehost-live.
Bardini, C., & Stacey, K. (2006). Students’ conceptions of m and c: How to tune a linear
function. Paper presented at the 30th Conference of the International Group for the
Psychology of Mathematics Education, Prague.
Barr, G. (1980). Graphs, gradients and intercepts. Mathematics in School, 9(1), 5–6.
Barr, G. (1981). Some student ideas on the concept of gradient. Mathematics in School,
10(1), 14,16–17.
Beichner, R. J. (1993). Testing student interpretation of kinematics graphs. Paper presented
at the 5th European Association for Research on Learning and Instruction, France.
Bell, A., Brekke, G., & Swan, M. (1987a). Diagnostic teaching: 4 graphical interpretations.
Mathematics Teaching, 119, 56–60.
Bell, A., Brekke, G., & Swann, M. (1987b). Diagnostic teaching: 5 graphical interpretation
teaching styles and their effects. Mathematics Teaching, 120, 50–57.
Bell, A., & Janvier, C. (1981). The interpretation of graphs representing situations. For the
Learning of Mathematics, 2(1), 34–42.
Berg, C. A., & Phillips, D. G. (1994). An investigation of the relationship between logical
thinking structures and the ability to construct and interpret line graphs. Journal of
Research in Science Teaching, 31(4), 323–344.
Billings, E., & Klanderman, D. (2000). Graphical representations of speed: obstacles pre-
service K–8 teachers experience. School Science and Mathematics, 100(8), 440–450.
Boers-van Oosterum, M. A. M. (1990). Understanding of variables and their uses acquired
by students in traditional and computer-intensive algebra. Unpublished doctoral dis-
sertation, University of Maryland. Dissertation Abstracts International, 51(5), 1538.
Booth, J. L., & Koedinger, K. R. (2012). Are diagrams always helpful tools? Developmen-
tal and individual differences in the effect of presentation format on student problem
solving. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(3), 492–511.
Analysis of Change (Graphing) • 227
Boyer, T. W., Levine, S. C., & Huttenlocher, J. (2008). Development of proportional reason-
ing: Where young children go wrong. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1478–1490.
Brasell, H. M. (1990). Graphs, graphing, and graphers. In M. B. Rowe (Ed.), The process
of knowing. Washington, DC: National Science Teachers Association.
Brassel, H. M., & Rowe, M. B. (1993). Graphing skills among high school physics stu-
dents. School Science and Mathematics, 93, 63–71.
Burrill, G., Allison, J., Breaux, G., Kastberg, S., Leatham, K., & Sanchez, W. (2002).
Handheld graphing technology in secondary mathematics. East Lansing, MI: Mich-
igan State University.
Carpenter, P. A., & Shah, P. (1988). A model of the perceptual and conceptual processes
in graph comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 4, 75–100.
Chiu, M. M., Kessel, C., Moschkovich, J., & Munoz-Nunez, A. (2001). Learning to graph
linear functions: A case study of conceptual change. Cognition and Instruction,
19(2), 215–252.
Cisneros, S. (2014, June 20). New report finds that 1 in 5 mothers stay home to care for
family. Retrieved from http://www.opb.org/news/article/new-report-finds-1-in-
5-mothers-stay-home-to-care-for-family/.
Clement, J. (1985). Misconceptions in graphing. Paper presented at the Ninth International
Conference for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
Clement, J. (1989). The concept of variation and misconceptions in Cartesian graphing.
Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics, 11, 77–87.
Clement, J., Mokros, J. R., & Schultz, K. (1986). Adolescents’ graphing skills: A descrip-
tive analysis. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational
Research Association.
Cobb, G., & Moore, D. (1997). Mathematics, statistics, and teaching. The American Math-
ematical Monthly, 104(9), 801–823,
Cook, S. W., Yip, T. K., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2012). Gestures, but not meaningless
movements, lighten working memory load when explaining math. Language and
Cognitive Processes, 27(4), 594–610.
Coxford, A. F., Fey, J. T., Hirsch, C. R., Schoen, H. L., Burrill, G., Hart, E. W., & Watkins,
A. E. (1998). Contemporary mathematics in context: A unified approach. New York:
Glencoe/McGraw-Hill.
Davis, J. D. (2007). Real-world contexts, multiple representations, student-invented ter-
minology, and y-intercept. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 9(4), 387–418.
Delgado, C., & Lucero, M. M. (2015). Scale construction for graphing: An investigation
of students’ resources. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 52(5), 633–658.
Demana, F., Schoen, H., & Waits, B. (1993). Graphing in the K–12 curriculum: The impact
of the graphing calculator. In T. Romberg, E. Fennema, & T. Carpenter (Eds.), Inte-
grating research on the graphical representation of functions (pp. 11–39). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Dewey, B. L., Singletary, T. J., & Kinzel, M. T. (2009). Graphing calculator use in algebra
teaching. School Science and Mathematics, 109(7), 383–393.
diSessa, A. A., Hammer, D., Sherin, B., & Kolpakowski, T. (1991). Inventing graphing:
Meta-representational expertise in children. Journal of Mathematical Behavior,
10(2), 117–160.
228 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Dreyfus, T., & Eisenberg, T. (1982). The function concept in college students: Linear-
ity, smoothness and periodicity. Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics, 5,
119–132.
Dugdale, S. (1993). Functions and graphs: Perspectives on student thinking. In T. A. Rom-
berg, E. Fennema, & T. P. Carpenter (Eds.), Integrating research on the graphical
representation of functions (pp. 101–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Dunham, P. H., & Dick, T. P. (1994). Research on graphing calculators. The Mathematics
Teacher, 87, 440–445.
Dykstra, D. I., Boyle, F., & Monarch, I.A. (1992). Studying conceptual change in learning
physics. Science Education, 76, 615–52.
Ellington, A. J. (2003). A meta-analysis of the effects of calculators on students’ achieve-
ment and attitude levels in precollege mathematics classes. Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education, 34(5), 433–463.
Ellington, A. J. (2006). The effects of non-CAS graphing calculators on student achieve-
ment and attitude levels in mathematics: A meta-analysis. School Science and Math-
ematics, 106, 16–26.
Ferrara, F. (2014). How multimodality works in mathematical activity: Young children
graphing motion. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education,
12(4), 917–939.
Friel, S. N., & Bright, G. W. (1996). Building a theory of graphicacy: How do students
read graphs? Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational
Research Association., New York, NY.
Friel, S. N., Curcio, F. R., & Bright, G. W. (2001). Making sense of graphs: Critical factors
influencing comprehension and instructional implications. Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education, 32(2), 124–158.
Fuson, K. C., & Abrahamson, D. (2005). Understanding ratio and proportion as an ex-
ample of the apprehending zone and conceptual-phase problem-solving models. In
J. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 213–234). New York,
NY: Psychology Press.
Glazer, N. (2011) Challenges with graph interpretation: a review of the literature. Studies
in Science Education, 47(2), 183–210.
Goldenberg, E. P. (1987). Believing is seeing: How preconceptions influence the percep-
tion of graphs. In J. C. Bergeron, N. Herscovics, and C. Kieran (Eds.), Proceedings
of the 11th International Conference for the Psychology of Mathematics Education
(pp. 197–203), Montreal, Canada.
Gravemeijer, K., van Galen, F., & Keijzer, R. (2005). Designing instruction on propor-
tional reasoning with average speed. Paper presented at the 39th International Con-
ference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education.
Hadjidemetriou, C., & Williams, J. (2001). Children’s graphical conceptions. In M. van
den Heuvel-Panhuizen (ed). Proceedings of the 25th Conference of the International
Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 3, p. 89). Utrecht: Neth-
erlands.
Hale, P. (2000). Kinematics and graphs: Students’ difficulties and CBLs. Mathematics
Teacher, 93(5), 414–417.
Hall, J., & Chamblee, G. (2013). Teaching algebra and geometry with GeoGebra: Pre-
paring pre-service teachers for middle Grades/Secondary mathematics classrooms.
Computers in the Schools, 30(1–2), 12.
Analysis of Change (Graphing) • 229
Karplus, R., Pulos, S., & Stage, E. K. (1983b). Early adolescents’ proportional reasoning
on ‘rate’ problems. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 14, 219–233.
Kenney, R. H. (2014). Investigating a link between pre-calculus students’ uses of graphing
calculators and their understanding of mathematical symbols. International Journal
for Technology in Mathematics Education, 21(4), 157–166.
Kerslake, D. (2004). Graphs. In K. Hart (Ed.), Children’s understanding of mathematics:
11–16 (pp. 120–136). Eastbourne, UK: Antony Rowe Publishing.
Kilpatrick, J., Swafford, J., & Findell, B. (2001). Adding it up: Helping children learn
mathematics. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Knuth, E. J. (2000a). Student understanding of the Cartesian connection: An exploratory
study. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 31, 500–507.
Knuth, E. J. (2000b). Understanding connections between equations and graphs. Mathemat-
ics Teacher, 93(1), 48–53.
Koedinger, K. R., & Terao, A. (2002). A cognitive task analysis of using pictures to sup-
port pre-algebraic reasoning. In C. D. Schunn & W. Gray (Eds.), Proceedings of the
Twenty-Fourth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associated, Inc.
Krabbendam, H. (1982). The non-qualitative way of describing relations and the role of
graphs: Some experiments. In G. Van Barnveld & H. Krabbendam (Eds.), Confer-
ence on functions (pp. 125–146). Enshede, The Netherlands: Foundation for Cur-
riculum Development.
Kwon, O. N. (2002). The effect of calculator-based ranger activities on students’ graphing
ability. School Science and Mathematics, 102(2), 57–67.
Lamon, S. (1994). Ratio and proportion: Cognitive foundations in unitizing and norming. In G.
Harel & J. Confrey (Eds.), The development of multiplicative reasoning in the learning
of mathematics (pp. 89–120). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.
Lamon, S. J. (2007). Rational numbers and proportional reasoning: Toward a theoretical frame-
work for research. In F. K. Lester, Jr. (Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathemat-
ics teaching and learning (pp. 629–668). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.
Larkin, J. H., & Simon, H. A. (1987). Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth ten thousand
words. Cognitive Science, 11, 65–99.
Lehner, J. (2014). Oregon’s stay at home parents. Department of Administrative Services,
Office of Economic Analysis. Retrieved from https://oregoneconomicanalysis.files.
wordpress.com/2014/06/oregons-stay-at-home-parents-062314.pdf.
Leinhardt, G., Zaslavsky, O., & Stein, M. K. (1990). Functions, graphs, and graphing:
Tasks, learning, and teaching. Review of Educational Research, 60(1), 1–64.
Lesh, R., Post, T. R., & Behr, M. J. (1988). Proportional reasoning. In J. Hiebert & M. Behr
(Eds.), Number concepts and operations in the middle grades (pp. 93–118). Reston,
VA: NCTM.
Leu, D. J., Jr., Kinzer, C. K., Coiro, J. L., & Cammack, D. W. (2004). Toward a theory of
new literacies emerging from the Internet and other information and communica-
tion technologies. In R. B. Ruddell & N. J. Unrau (Eds.), Theoretical models and
processes of reading (5th ed., pp. 1570–1613).
Li, Q., & Ma, X. (2011). A meta-analysis of the effects of computer technology on school
students’ mathematics learning. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 215–243.
Analysis of Change (Graphing) • 231
Lobato, J., & Thanheiser, E. (1999). Rethinking slope from quantitative and phenomeno-
logical perspectives. Presented at the North American Chapter of the Psychology of
Mathematics Education Conference. Morelos, Mexico.
Lowrie, T., Diezmann, C., & Logan, T. (2011) Understanding graphicacy: Students’ mak-
ing sense of graphics in mathematics assessment tasks. International Journal for
Mathematics Teaching and Learning, 1–21. Retrieved from http://eprints.qut.edu.
au/48802.
Mansfield, H. (1985). Points, lines, and their representations. For the Learning of Math-
ematics, 5(3), 2–6.
Markovits, Z., Eylon, B., & Bruckheimer, M. (1986). Functions today and yesterday. For
the Learning of Mathematics, 6(2), 18–28.
Markovitz, Z., Eylon, B.,,,,, & Brukheimer, M. (1983). Functions: Linearity unconstrained.
In R. Hershkowitz (Ed.), Proceedings of the Seventh International Conference of the
International Group for the PME (pp. 271–277). Rehovot, Israel.
Mayer, R. (2005). Cognitive theory of multimedia learning. In R. Mayer (Ed.) The Cam-
bridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 31–48). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Mayer, R. E. (1989). Systematic thinking fostered by illustrations in scientific text. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 81, 240–246.
McClaran, R. R. (2013). Investigating the impact of interactive applets on students’ under-
standing of parameter changes to parent functions: An explanatory mixed methods
study. Theses and Dissertations—Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics
(STEM) Education. Paper 2. Retrieved from: http://uknowledge.uky.edu/stem_etds/2.
McDermott, L. C., Rosenquist, M. L., Popp, B., & van Zee, E. H. (1983). Student difficulties
in connecting graphs, concepts, and physical phenomena. Paper presented at the An-
nual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Montreal, Canada.
McDermott, L., Rosenquist, M., & van Zee, E. (1987). Student difficulties in connecting
graphs and physics: Example from kinematics. American Journal of Physics, 55,
503–513.
Meira, L. (1995). The microevolution of mathematical representations in children’s activ-
ity. Cognition and Instruction, 3(2), 269–313.
Mevarech, Z. R., & Kramarsky, B. (1997). From verbal descriptions to graphic representa-
tions: Stability and change in students’ alternative conceptions. Educational Studies
in Mathematics, 32(3), 229–263.
Middleton, J. A., & Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, M. (1995). The ratio table. Mathematics
teaching in the middle school 1(4), 282–288.
Mitchelmore, M., & Cavanagh, M. (2000). Students’ difficulties in operating a graphics
calculator. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 12(3), 254–268.
Mokros, J. & Tinker, R. (1987). The Impact of microcomputer-based labs on children’s
ability to interpret graphs. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 24, 369–383.
Monk, S. (1992). Students’ understanding of a function given by a physical model. In G. H.
E. Dubinsky (Ed.), The concept of function: Aspects of epistemology and pedagogy.
Washington, DC: Mathematical Association of America.
Monk, S. (1994). How students and scientists change their minds. MAA invited address at
the Joint Mathematics Meetings, Cincinnati, Ohio, January.
232 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Moore-Russo, D., Conner, A., & Rugg, K. (2011). Can slope be negative in 3-space?
Studying concept image of slope through collective definition construction. Educa-
tional Studies in Mathematics, 76(1), 3–21.
Moschkovich, J. (1989). Constructing a problem space through appropriation: A case
study of guided computer exploration of linear functions. Paper presented at the
1989 Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Fran-
cisco.
Moschkovich, J. (1990). Students’ interpretations of linear equations and their graphs.
Paper presented at the Fourteenth Annual Meeting of the International Group for the
Psychology of Mathematics Education, Mexico.
Moschkovich, J. (1992). Making sense of linear equations and their graphs. An analysis of
student conceptions and language use. Unpublished Doctoral dissertation.
Moschkovitch, J. (1996). Moving up and getting steeper: Negotiating shared descriptions
of linear graphs. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 5(3), 239–27.
Moschkovich, J. (1999). Students use of the x-intercept as an instance of a transitional
conception. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 37(2), 169.
Moschkovich, J., Schoenfeld, A. H., & Arcavi, A. (1993). Aspects of understanding: On mul-
tiple perspectives and representations of linear relations and connections among them.
In T. A. Romberg, E. Fennema & T. P. Carpenter (Eds.), Integrating research 011 the
graphical representation of functions (pp. 69–100). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Murata, A. (2008). Mathematics teaching and learning as a mediating process: The case
of tape diagrams. Mathematical Thinking and Learning: An International Journal,
10(4), 374–406.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2000). Principles and standards for school
mathematics. Reston, VA: Author.
National Governors Association Center for Best Practices, Council of Chief State School
Officers (2010). Common Core State Standards Mathematics. Washington, DC:
National Governors Association Center for Best Practices, Council of Chief State
School Officers.
Nemirovsky, R., & Noble, T. (1997). Mathematical visualization and the place where we
live. Educational Studies of Mathematics, 3(2), 9–131.
Nemirovsky, R. & Rubin, A. (1992). Students’ tendency to assume resemblances between
a function and its derivative. Cambridge, MA: TERC Communications, January
1992. Working Paper 2–92.
Nemirovsky, R., Tierney, C., & Wright, T. (1998). Body motion and graphing. Cognition
and Instruction, 16(2), 19–172.
Noble, T., & Nemirovsky, R. (1995). Graphs that go backwards. Paper presented at the
XIX Annual Meeting of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics
Education. Recife, Brazil.
Noble, T., Nemirovsky, R., DiMattia, C., & Wright, T. (2002). On learning to see: How do
middle school students learn to make sense of visual representations in mathemat-
ics? Cambridge, MA: TERC.
Noble, T., Nemirovsky, R., DiMattia, C., & Wright, T. (2004). Learning to See. Interna-
tional Journal of Computers for Mathematical Learning, 9(2), 109–167.
Noble, T., Nemirovsky, R.,Wright, T., & Tierney, C. (2001). Experiencing change: The
mathematics of change in multiple environments. Journal for Research in Math-
ematics Education, 32(1), 85–108.
Analysis of Change (Graphing) • 233
Noble, T., & TERC, C. M. A. (2003). Gesture and the mathematics of motion. Retrieved
from http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=eric&AN=ED47819
0&site=ehost-live.
Noelting, G. (1980a). The development of proportional reasoning and the ratio concept Part
I—Differentiation of stages. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 11(2), 217–253.
Noelting, G. (1980b). The development of proportional reasoning and the ratio concept Part
II—problem-structure at successive stages; problem-solving strategies and the mecha-
nism of adaptive restructuring. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 11(3), 331–363.
Orrill, C. H., & Brown, R. E. (2012). Making sense of double number lines in professional
development: Exploring teachers’ understandings of proportional relationships.
Journal of Mathematics Teacher Education, 15(5), 381–403.
Orton, A. (1983). Students’ understanding of differentiation. Educational Studies in Math-
ematics, 14, 235–250.
Orton, A. (1984). Understanding rate of change. Mathematics in School, 13(5), 23–26.
Padilla, M. J., McKenzie, D. L., & Show, E. L. Jr. (1986). An examination of the line
graphing ability of students in grades seven through twelve. School Science and
Mathematics, 86, 20–26.
Palmer, S. (1977). Fundamental aspects of cognitive representation. In E. Rosch, & B. B.
Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization, (pp. 259–303). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Phillips, R. J. (1997). Can juniors read graphs? Journal of Information Technology in
Teacher Education, 6(1), 49–58.
Pierce, R., Stacey, K., & Bardini, C. (2010). Linear functions: Teaching strategies and stu-
dents’ conceptions associated with y = mx + c. Pedagogies, 5(3), 202.
Pierce, R., Stacey, K., & Ball, L. (2005). Mathematics from still and moving images. Aus-
tralian Association of Mathematics Teachers, 61(3), 26–31.
Post, T. R., Behr, M. J., & Lesh, R. (1988). Proportionality and the development of preal-
gebra understandings. In A. Coxford & A. Shulte (Eds.), The ideas of algebra, K–12
(pp. 78–90). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Postigo, Y., & Pozo, J. (2004). On the road to graphicacy: The learning of graphical repre-
sentation systems. Educational Psychology, 24(5), 623–644.
Radford, L. (2009). “No! He starts walking backwards!”: Interpreting motion graphs and
the question of space, place and distance. ZDM, 41(4), 467–480.
Radford, L., Miranda, I., & Guzman, J. (2008). Relative motion, graphs, and the hetero-
glossic transformation of meanings: A semiotic analysis. Paper presented at the
Joint 32nd Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathemat-
ics Education and the 30th North American Chapter. Morelia, Mexico.
Rau, M. A. (2016). Conditions for the effectiveness of multiple visual representations in
enhancing STEM learning. Educational Psychology Review, 29(4), 717–761.
Robutti, O. (2006). Motion, technology, gestures in interpreting graphs. International
Journal for Technology in Mathematics Education, 13(3), 117–125.
Roschelle, J., Pea, R., Hoadley, C., Gordin, D., & Means, B. (2000). Changing how and
what children learn in school with computer-based technologies. The Future of Chil-
dren, 10(2), 76–101.
Roth, W. M., & McGinn, M. (1997). Graphing: Cognitive ability or practice? Science Edu-
cation, 81, 91–106.
Saunders, W. L., & Jesunathadas, J. (1988). The effect of task content upon proportional
reasoning. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 25(1), 59–67.
234 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Schoenfeld, A. H., Smith. J. P., & Arcavi, A. (1993). Learning: The microgenetic analysis
of one student’s evolving understanding of a complex subject matter domain. In R.
Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 55–175). Eillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Schultz, K., Clement, J., & Mokros, J. R. (1986). Adolescents’ graphing skills: A descrip-
tive analysis. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational
Research Association. San Francisco, CA.
Segal, A. (2011). Do gestural interfaces promote thinking? Embodied interaction:
Congruent gestures and direct touch promote performance in math. ProQuest
LLC. Retrieved from http://gateway.proquest.com/openurl?url_ver=Z39.88-
2004&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:dissertation&res_dat=xri:pqdiss&rft_
dat=xri:pqdiss:3453956.
Sherin, B. L. (2000). How students invent representations of motion: A genetic account.
The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 19(4), 399–441.
Silvestre, A., & da Ponte, J. (2012). Missing value and comparison problems: What pupils
know before the teaching of proportion. PNA, 6(3), 73–83.
Simon, M. A., & Blume, G. W. (1994). Mathematical modeling as a component of under-
standing ratio-as-measure: A study of prospective elementary teachers. Journal of
Mathematical Behavior, 13, 183–197.
Skemp, R. R. (1976). Relational understanding and instrumental understanding. Math-
ematics Teaching, 77, 20–26.
Slavit, D. (1994). The effect of graphing calculations on students’ conceptions of function.
Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research As-
sociation, New Orleans, LA.
Smith, J., Arcavi, A., & Schoenfeld, A. H. (1989). Learning y-intercept: Assembling the
pieces of an “atomic” concept. In Vergnaud, G., Rogalski, J., & Artigue, M. (Eds.),
Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the International Group for the Psychol-
ogy of Mathematics Education (pp. 176–183). Paris: International Group for the
Psychology of Mathematics Education.
Stanley, D., McGowan, D., & Hull, S. H. (2003). Pitfalls of over-reliance on cross multipli-
cation as a method to find missing values. Texas Mathematics Teacher, 11(1), 9–11.
Stanton, M., & Moore-Russo, D. (2012). Conceptualizations of slope: A review of state
standards. School Science and Mathematics, 112(5), 270–277.
Steele, D. (1994).The Wesley College Technology Enriched Graphing Project. Unpub-
lished Master’s thesis, University of Melbourne, 1994.
Stein, M. K., & Leinhardt, G. (1989). Interpreting graphs: An analysis of early perfor-
mance and reasoning. Unpublished manuscript, University of Pittsburgh Learning
Research and Development Center, PA.
Stump, S. L. (1997). Secondary mathematics teachers’ knowledge of the concept of slope.
Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research As-
sociation, Chicago, IL.
Stump, S. L. (2001). High school Precalculus students’ understanding of slope as measure.
School Science and Mathematics, 101(2), 81–89.
Swafford, J. O., & Langrall, C. W. (2000). Grade 6 students’ preinstructional use of equa-
tions to describe and represent problem situations. Journal for Research in Math-
ematics Education, 31(1), 89–112.
Analysis of Change (Graphing) • 235
Thompson, A. G., & Thompson, P. (1996). Talking about rates conceptually, Part II. Jour-
nal for Research in Mathematics Education, 27(1), 2–24.
Thompson, P. (1994). The development of the concept of speed and its relationship to
concepts of rate. In G. Harel & J. Confrey (Eds.), The development of multiplicative
reasoning in the learning of mathematics (pp. 181–234). Albany, NY: SUNY Press.
Thompson, P. W., & Thompson, A. G. (1992). Images of rate. Paper presented at the Annu-
al Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco, CA.
Tourniaire, F. (1984). Proportional reasoning in grades three, four, and five. Unpublished
Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.
Tourniaire, F., & Pulos, S. (1985). Proportional reasoning: A review of the literature. Edu-
cational Studies in Mathematics, 16(2), 181–204.
Tufte, E. (2001). The visual display of quantitative information. Cheshire, CT: Graphics
Press
U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for Edu-
cation Statistics. (2013). National Assessment of Educational Progress. Retrieved
from http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/nqt/Home/LegacyGen0Bookmark?subje
ct=mathematics&year=2013&grade=8.
Ubuz, B. (2007). Interpreting a graph and constructing its derivative graph: Stability and
change in students’ conceptions. International Journal of Mathematics Education in
Science and Technology, 38(5), 609–637.
Vergnaud, G. (1980). Didactics and acquisition of multiplicative structures in secondary
schools. In W. F. Archerhold, R. H. Driver, A. Orton, & C. Wood-Robinson (Eds.),
Cognitive development research in science and mathematics. Leeds, England: Uni-
versity of Leeds.
Vergnaud, G., & Errecalde, P. (1980). Some steps in the understanding and the use of
scales and axis by 10–13 year old students. In R. Karglus (Ed.), Proceedings of the
Fourth International Conference for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (pp.
285–291). Berkeley, CA: University of California.
Wainer, H. (1992). Understanding graphs and tables. Educational Researcher, 21(1), 14–
23.
Walter, J., & Gerson, H. (2007). Teachers’ personal agency: Making sense of slope through
additive structures. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 65(2), 203–233.
Watanabe, T. (2015). Visual reasoning tools in action: Double number lines, area models,
and other diagrams power up students’ ability to solve and make sense of various
problems. Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School, 21(3), 152–160.
Wavering, M. (1985). The logical reasoning necessary to make line graphs. Paper pre-
sented at the Annual Meeting of the National Association for Research in Science
Teaching, French Lick Springs, Indiana.
Wavering, M. J. (1989). Logical reasoning necessary to make line graphs. Journal of Re-
search in Science Teaching, 26(5), 373–379.
Zaslavsky, O. (1997). Conceptual obstacles in the learning of quadratic functions. Unpub-
lished Doctoral dissertation, Technion, Haifa, Israel.
CHAPTER 5
INTRODUCTION
Functions are one of the central ideas in algebra and one of the most common
sources of student difficulty. “Teachers’ comprehensive and well-organized con-
ceptions contribute to instruction characterized by emphases on conceptual con-
nections, powerful representations, and meaningful discussions.”2 In contrast,
teachers’ limited views of function have been shown to lead to narrow instruction,
with “missed opportunities to highlight important connections between concepts
and representations.”3 The study of functions begins informally in very early
grades when we ask students to continue or describe a pattern, and continues in
higher grades by Developing the Concept of Function1 with more formal defini-
1
Note: bold and italicized print indicates a reference to an entry in the book
Problems 2 through 6 all describe functions that have some “peculiarity” com-
pared to most of the functions defined by formulas that students encounter. Once
students realize that there are such functions, they should come to a broader un-
Patterns & Functions • 241
We can see that students have very distinct ideas about what functions are. Some
of the ideas show a limited understanding of functions—for example, that a func-
tion is a formula—while others, on the surface, seem to imply a deeper under-
standing but sometimes turn out to be rote definitions that have little or no con-
ceptual understanding beneath them. Asking the student questions is the only way
to figure out the student’s understanding.
A learning trajectory for functions involves multiple aspects of the concept:17
1. Function definition
2. Operations
3. Composition and inverse
4. Linear, polynomial, and rational functions
5. Exponential functions
6. Sequences
7. Rate of change and characteristics of graphs
244 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
8. Quadratic functions
9. Trigonometric functions
10. Transformations
11. Modeling with functions
Notice that only 1, 7, possibly 10, and 11 get away from functions described by
a formula.
Once a teacher has categorized a student’s notion of function, he or she can
begin to probe its depth. For example, for the student who thinks that a function
must be a formula or an operation, a teacher could start with a graph of x2 and ask
if it shows a function, and then modify it just slightly (in such a way that there is
no obvious formula for it) and ask whether it is still a function.
Teachers
Non-standard questions (such as those employing functional equations as in
Problem 2) can test the depth of a student’s understanding of functions. Because
they are non-standard, however, a student is likely to need some assistance on
such a problem. Students may better internalize the notion of a function if they
are exposed to a variety of descriptions of functions, including non-standard prob-
lems. This forces the student to consider what a function is at a more fundamental
level and may dispel misconceptions about functions. While “Skill Drills” are im-
portant for procedural mastery, they often come in large clusters with a single set
of directions (e.g., “Factor each quadratic expression below”). If a similar compu-
tation arises in a different context, students may find themselves with no idea how
to proceed. Plenty of practice with non-standard or differently phrased questions
can help students recognize familiar algebraic steps in unfamiliar contexts.
Consider the following strategies for teaching algebra and functions:18
ing the total cost for some number of pounds of bananas priced at $3.98 per
pound. Such examples bring out the ideas of domain, scale, precision, and rate
of change. To bring in the concept of a y-intercept, teachers can change this to a
commission model (for example) in which a salesperson receives a fixed salary
plus a commission. This revisits all of the ideas listed previously and introduces
y-intercepts. Many cell phone plans charge by the minute, where fractions of a
minute are rounded up; this is an example of a step function. Interest charges
grow exponentially, taxes use a discrete domain and have a piecewise definition,
etc. Building time into the lesson for discussion of the models gives students a
chance to verbalize and solidify their understanding of these ideas. Another es-
sential component is gradually introducing more and more sophisticated ideas
(e.g., linear—quadratic—exponential—trigonometric or smooth—cusped—dis-
continuous—piecewise-defined) over time, which requires an earlier introduction
to the basic concepts.
Teachers can take advantage of everyday function situations to make strong
connections between functions and the ideas students have already internalized.
Rather than starting with the definition of a simple linear function, like f(x) = 3x,
teachers can begin with a concept of function that ties immediately to the stu-
dents’ experiences. Abstraction to function notation and variables should come
easier for students who already understand the underlying principles.
Be aware of three common misconceptions that teachers and students have
about functions with respect to the notion of arbitrariness:19
On the other hand, it is also common to assume a relation that fits these criteria is a
function—a circle being a good example. Teachers can focus on helping students
understand and make proper use of the Vertical Line Test. Too often it is used in
place of a more comprehensive function definition or as a rule that students can
follow to get the right answer, devoid of understanding.
Make sure students understand univalence: “a function is a relation such that
every number in the domain is matched to one and only one number in the range”
(many state it backwards). Many teachers do not know the history that led to func-
tions being defined this way and therefore do not understand the mathematical
need for univalence. Also, students inadvertently impose a constraint of “unique-
ness” (i.e., functions must be one-to-one), leading them to conclude that relations
such as f (x) = 2 are not functions.20
As noted above, students should be exposed to as wide a variety of function
types as possible. “The ability to identify and represent the same concept in differ-
ent representations, and flexibility in moving from one representation to another,
246 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
are crucial in mathematics learning, as they allow students to see rich relation-
ships, and develop deeper understanding of concepts.”21 They should see con-
tinuous and discontinuous functions, functions with algebraic descriptions and
functions with no algebraic description, functions with continuous domains and
functions with discrete domains, etc., and the representations of the functions
should also vary. Furthermore, students should see examples of relations that are
not functions.
Patterns & Functions • 247
This framework provides a structure that will help ensure students see the variety
of functions they need to in order to develop a solid understanding of them. It be-
gins with what a function is, then continues to ways of representing them, funda-
mental operations on functions, a basic library of functions students should know,
and a broader view of functions. For example, consider the characteristic function
of the rational numbers: g(x) = 1 if x is rational and g(x) = 0 if x is irrational. One
student teacher showed a rote understanding of the definition of a function but did
not have a solid grip on the concept: “Yes, there is an assignment of a single value
to each number.” Later in the interview, this gave way to, “I don’t know if it’s a
function. It fits the criteria of mapping, but it does not look pretty. It is not really
graphable. It might just not be.”27
Problems 9 and 10 approach the function concept from different directions and
can be used to assess the strength of a student’s understanding of functions.28 They
include functions that are discontinuous, have discrete domains, are constant, and
that can’t be graphed in any reasonable way. They can be used as a diagnostic tool
to determine where students have gaps in their understanding of functions.
Problem 9
A student marked all of the following (see Figure 5.3) as non-functions (R is
the set of all real numbers. N is the set of all the natural numbers).
a. For each case, decide whether the student was right or wrong. Give rea-
sons for each of your decisions.
b. In cases where you think the student was wrong, try to explain what the
student was thinking that could cause the mistake.
(iv) A correspondence that associates 1 with each positive number, -1
with each negative number, and 3 with zero.
(v) g(x) = x, if x is a rational number, or
0, if x is an irrational number
(vi) {(1, 4), (2, 5), (3, 9)}
Problem 10
When asked to draw a graph of a function that passes through the points A, B,
and C, a student gave the answer in Figure 5.4:
What do you think about this answer? Is it correct? Why?
• If yes--is there any other correct answer? Give me an example. Can you
find another one? Why do you think the student gave this answer?
• If no--What would you like to see as a correct answer? Give me an ex-
ample. Another one. Why do you think the student gave this answer?
Teachers
• Avoid overreliance on the vertical line test.
• Remember that not all functions can be represented by a “formula.”
• Provide examples of functions that are not readily graphed.
• Verify that students understand the connection between the vertical line
test and the definition of a function.
250 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
FUNCTION MACHINES
Common Core Standards: 8.F.1, F.IF.1
Too often, students associate success in algebra with symbol manipulation.
This focus on manipulating symbols leads to students having little flexibility in
their algebraic thinking and often having considerable difficulty with function
notation specifically.
Student self-evaluations from a course on college algebra indicated that many
felt a function machine model helped them make sense of notation, organize their
thinking, and generate equations from given data.29 Students who were the most
successful in the course made references to input and output as well as function
machines in their written work, while the least successful students did not. One
student’s self-evaluation said,
I know that function machines are good models for mathematical relationships when
one wants to clearly identify and separate the input, output, and process. I also can
look at function notation, f(x), and understand what it is stating: that f is a function
of x. I see the input (x), the output f(x), and the process, when in a relationship form
like f(x) = 3x+10 … I know now that “solving” can refer to many different things
including:
This was without being asked specifically to discuss function machines; the text
emphasized a function machine approach for the first few chapters, then did not
mention them. The prompt for the self-evaluation was “What mathematics have
you learned during this time? Write a summary of what you have learned.” Stron-
ger students are able to think flexibly in this way, able to evaluate a function and
to determine which input(s) correspond to a given output.
Another student said,
I had no idea what a function is and what a relation is, but now I do. Relation is
mathematical notation with at least two variables. Independent is input. Dependent
is output. I know that a relation, which has only one output for each given input is a
function. If I see f(8) = x + 7, I know I am given an input. f(x) = 8 gives me output.
I can work with ordered pairs. In case (8, 45), I see 8 as input and 45 as output.”
This student understood function notation, had a grasp of the roles of inputs and
outputs, and understood multiple representations of functions.
Compare the following tasks:
Patterns & Functions • 251
Task 1: Given a table from a TI-82 graphing calculator. What are the
output(s) if the input is -2? What are the input(s) if the output is
-3?
Task 2: Consider the equation y = 3x – 7. What are the output(s) if the
input is 5? What are the input(s) if the output is 0?
Teachers
• A function table from a graphing calculator may help students gain flex-
ibility in thinking about functions.
• The problems above can be used as formative assessments.
• “Function machine” perspective on functions can help students understand
function notation and what a function is from a process perspective.
2
All Center for Algebraic Thinking apps are freely available on iTunes except Math Flyer ($0.99).
252 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
COVARIATION
Common Core Standards: 8.F.4, 5, F.IF.4, 5
The function machine approach is very much focused on functions as a pro-
cess, which, while useful, is a limited perspective. A covariation approach to
functions may help to expand students’ views of functions beyond simple input-
output procedures. In addition, it may be more intuitive for some students than the
usual input-output approach: how do x and y change together? Furthermore, if the
changes in x are unit changes, this ties in very neatly with introducing students to
the concept of rate of change, including non-constant rates of change (see Analy-
sis of Change: Understanding Rate of Change and Ratio and Proportional Rea-
soning as well as other sections in Chapter 4).
In their efforts to chart a visual course to the concept of function as one of
covariation and a rate of change, researchers found that providing images and lan-
guage for a list of function properties (see Table 5.3) helped students use the terms
constant, increasing, and decreasing when describing functions. Students made
the link between a situation and its visual representation but struggled to concep-
tually understand the link between a graph and its symbols. Consider Problem 11:
Increasing
Rate of change
Decreasing
Constant
through them. Take a third point, P, not on the line, and construct H, the foot of the
perpendicular from P to line AB. Students can drag any of the points. The problem
is to determine the relationships among the points (Figure 5.5 shows a possible an-
swer). Ask: “What does it mean to say two functions are equal?”
Problem 11 allows any of the variables (points in the plane) to be the “indepen-
dent” variable, creating a situation in which covariation is the natural way to think
about functions. The interview in Table 5.4 is a conversation regarding two func-
tions being equal, having explored Problem 11 with dynamic geometry software.
Rather than just giving the students a definition of function equality, the teacher
has a conversation with them about what it should mean. During the conversation,
they bring up several misconceptions that are explored and then either built upon
or discarded, as appropriate. In the end, the students have developed a working
definition of function equality that, while it lacks rigor, nevertheless captures the
principle they were working toward.
Teachers
• A covariation approach to functions can help students see beyond the pro-
cess view of functions and gain a larger perspective on functions.
• A covariation perspective can help students see a mutual dependence be-
tween the variables.
The downside of a covariation approach is that, by its nature, there is some im-
plication that the functions are discrete: to see how a function behaves with this
approach, one tries a few values and looks for a pattern. From this perspective, a
function is almost like a sequence. Teachers who adopt this method will need to
be aware of such a pitfall. One way to avoid that trap is to use dynamic geometry
software such as Cabri or Geometer’s Sketchpad. As in Problem 11, teachers can
254 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
provide students with a predetermined construction that is hidden from the stu-
dents. Students then manipulate different components of the diagram (given lines
and points) and observe the resulting changes in the other parts of the diagram.
This is a natural example of a continuously varying function that students can
internalize at an intuitive level; the motion of the points illustrates the covariation
idea since students can move any of the points and see the effect on the others.
Of course, just playing on the computer will not in itself achieve the desired
result. In one study, the following structure helped students understand the con-
cept of a function:32
• Students engage in lab activities, exploring a task with the software. They
choose a point to move and observe how other points are affected; this es-
tablishes a relationship between the “input” (the point they chose) and the
“output” (the point they observe).
• Students write about their experience with the software and any thoughts
or conclusions they have.
• The class discusses the relationship together. The teacher’s role in guiding
this discussion appropriately is essential. She or he must be able to discern
the key concepts when they arise, even if they are not well articulated.
• At some point, students will need to move from this geometric presentation
of a function to numerical or algebraic functions. In order for this approach
to be effective, students need to see the connection with the underlying
principle of functional dependency.
With such an exercise, students experience a function that has no “formula” and
whose inputs and outputs are not just numbers, expanding their concept of what
can be a function. In addition, the functional relationship is visual rather than
algebraic, potentially helping students whose algebra skills are weak to see what
a function is.
256 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
?x + 3y = -6
values for the clinic at Bamako. An error in their subtraction, which was identi-
fied as they checked their results, led to their recognition that they had found the
gradient.
In contrast, for two other students who came together during the final lesson,
this subtask seemed trivial as their notions of gradient were stronger (see Table
5.10). The differences among these pairs of students were not evident in the stu-
dents’ written work, but rather in the conversation.
Teachers
• Students who see functions only as processes or only as objects are limited
by their perspective. For many students, the process perspective is the easi-
est to understand, so they tend to stick to it.
• Problems that require graphical reasoning can lead students to a better un-
derstanding of functions as objects.
• The ability to reason graphically puts another tool in students’ toolboxes.
Sometimes, a graphical argument is more natural than an algebraic one.
• Ask questions for students to explain their thinking in order to reveal the
depth of their understanding of functions.
GRAPHING FUNCTIONS
Common Core Standards: 8.F.2, 3, 4, 5; F.IF.4, 5, 6, 7, 8 9
A graph of a function is perhaps the fastest way to convey large amounts of
information about the function as a whole (or as an object). Our brains become
skilled at extracting that information and interpreting it in context, but it requires
familiarity and practice with graphing and reading graphs.
Problems were posed to students that required them to graph functions and/or
data. The types of errors are summarized in Table 5.11. The first step in helping
a student learn from a mistake is identifying the mistake. Table 5.11 can help a
teacher do so. Some mistakes (such as perceptual illusion) show a fundamental
lack of understanding, while others (such as transfer error) may indicate mere
carelessness. Problem 16 (see Table 5.12) provides some context to examine each
of these errors and the following figures demonstrate examples of each of these
types of errors, as students used graphing calculators.
In Table 5.12 are student responses that illustrate some of the misconception
and error types. There are a few lessons in here for students. First, over-reliance
on a graphing calculator will be a problem: if any of these errors arise, the student
will miss them. Second, it is important for a student to have some sense of how
different standard functions behave so that she or he can decide whether the graph
displayed is reasonable. Finally, a student needs to have some idea of what she or
he is looking for in order to determine how best to display the graph.
πt
S = 74.50 + 43.75sin
6
Graph Illustrating Error Correct Graph
Scaling Issue1
Teachers
• Graphs convey large amounts of information quickly.
• Due in part to confusion about function notation, students can struggle with
graphing correctly.
TRANSFORMATIONS OF FUNCTIONS
Common Core Standards: 8.F.2, 5, F.IF.4, 7, 8, 9
It is not always immediately apparent how altering a parameter of a function
will change the behavior of a function (or how it won’t). It is important for stu-
dents to recognize that the graphs of f(x) = 3x + 2 and g(x) = -2x + 4 share a prop-
erty of “straightness” that is not shared by h(x) = x2, for example, or that k(x) = x2
+ 4 has the same fundamental shape as h. Without such recognition, students may
expect that changing a in ax has the same effect as changing it in x2 + ax.
Using technology, students are able to develop a deeper understanding of the
various components of functions. Function transformations can be approached
by systematically graphing families of functions from equations and changing
the parameters. This can help students see relationships and make generalizations
about them. In the same sort of way, other technologies such as the Function
Probe41 or Linear Model app (see Figure 5.8) allow students to begin by examin-
ing the graph and then focusing on what happens to the tables and equations when
the function is transformed. A critical aspect of understanding functions is con-
necting concepts across representations, but research indicates that students have
significant difficulties moving among the different forms.42 The Linear Model
Teachers
Teachers can help students begin to generalize the properties and behaviors
of individual functions and families of functions and how the various changes
they make influence the representation. For example, if they wanted to create
an “upward” motion, how would they accomplish that on a linear, quadratic, ab-
solute value, etc. function? Similarly, if they wanted to alter the steepness, how
would they do so? Math Flyer (see Figure 5.9) is an app that allows for easy
transformations of functions. It has a built-in library of standard functions with
sliders associated with parameters of the functions. Moving the sliders results in
real-time changes to the graphs of the functions. The app provides instant and au-
tomatic feedback regarding students’ intuitions and predictions about the behavior
of functions and eliminates the tedium of graphing each and every function. How-
ever, it might not be appropriate if the students have not first acquired this skill.
LINEAR FUNCTIONS
Common Core Standards: 8.F.3, 4, 5, F-LE.1, 2, F.BF.1, F.IF.4, 5, 7
Linear functions comprise one of the simplest and yet most important classes
of functions for students to understand, but because the nature of linear func-
tions involves proportional reasoning, they can be somewhat difficult for students.
Their power and utility are apparent throughout mathematics and the sciences. We
frequently make linear approximations to nonlinear functions, for example, and
the savings in computational complexity can be substantial.
In one study, students were asked to explore two dynamic physical models
and explain their thinking. Students were also given pictures, tables, and equa-
tions to represent the situations. A specific protocol for the interview was not
provided since the interviewer followed the lead of the student’s thinking with her
questions. Included are some transcribed interactions between the student and the
interviewer, as well as samples of the student’s written work.
One of the models could be a gear elevating system. As shown in the illustra-
tion in Figure 5.10, there are gears with varying diameters and a weighted object
at the end of the cord. By turning the crank one turn clockwise, the object, if on
the 3-gear with a 3-inch circumference, would rise 3 inches. A counter-clockwise
turn would lower the object 3 inches. The 4-gear has a 4-inch circumference and
would raise or lower the object 4 inches with each complete turn of the crank.
These can be modeled with elementary linear functions, where the height raised is
a function of the number of turns.
The second model was the Etch-a-Sketch™, where one complete clockwise
turn of the left knob creates a 3 cm horizontal segment moving from left to right,
while a counter-clockwise turn moves from right to left. One complete clockwise
turn of the right knob creates a 3 cm vertical segment from a lower position to an
Teachers
• Linear functions are in many ways a natural starting point for an explora-
tion of functions. Students can explore parameter changes that have clear
geometric interpretations, and computations are simple enough that they
do not obscure the idea of function being taught.
• The gear elevating system provides a context for exploring functions and
the associated variables in a non-symbolic way. Exploring with this model
can support understanding regarding how the change in one variable influ-
ences the change in the other variable (see Covariation for more discussion
of this approach).
• Physical models, such as a gear elevating system or Etch-A-Sketch™, can
provide a context for exploring functions to help students understand how
change in one variable influences change in the other variable.
• Students may struggle to understand physical models as generalized pro-
cesses rather than individual actions.
Patterns & Functions • 273
(Continues)
274 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Problem 19
a. Draw the set of points in a plane that satisfies the equation x2 + y2 = 1.
b. Draw in three dimensions the set of points that are the result of assigning a height z = 3 to the
points in a horizontal plane satisfying the equation x2 + y2 = 1.
c. Draw in three dimensions the set of points that may be reached by starting with points in a
horizontal plane that satisfy the equation x2 + y2 = 1 and assigning them an arbitrary height z.
Problem 20
a. Consider the grid for Problem 2 above:
b. Draw a two-dimensional plane the points on the surface that satisfy z=1.
c. Draw a two-dimensional plane the points on the surface that satisfy x=1.
d. Draw a two-dimensional plane the points on the surface that satisfy y=1.
Problem 21
a. A rental car company charges 100 shekels for a day and an additional 5 shekels per kilometer. The
company would like to have a clear description of the price that any client may have to pay when
returning a rented car. Suggest such a description.
b. You have won a 1000 shekels coupon from the rental car company. Provide a detailed description
of all your options to spend the exact amount for renting and driving a car using the maximum of
your winnings.
structions such as the one this problem asks for are needed when analyzing graphs
of two-variable functions. Finally, Problem 21 provides an approach to consider-
ing functions of two variables.51 It may be more suitable for students who are
ready to move beyond graphing points in three dimensions, or teachers may find
it more instructive to allow students to develop those ideas independently through
the problem, depending on the readiness of the students.
Patterns & Functions • 275
Teachers
• Multivariable functions can expand students’ notions of functions and help
them understand the big picture of what a function is.
• Real-world problems are messy, and real phenomena usually depend on
many variables.
• Students will need some preparation to consider multivariable situations
(e.g., an introduction to graphing in three dimensions).
Real-world problems can help motivate many ideas in mathematics, and mul-
tivariable functions are no exception. The rental car example in Problem 5 is easy
for students to grasp and begin thinking about; there is no barrier to immediately
discussing multiple input variables, as there might be with a more abstract ex-
ample.
Interpreting two-dimensional renderings of three-dimensional figures takes
practice. Having manipulatives with which to build graphs such as that of the
function describing the rental car options can help students make the transition
from two-dimensional graphs to three-dimensional graphs. Once students have an
understanding of graphing in three dimensions, visualization software can help
make the graphs more precise and more malleable (i.e., rotatable). When students
can move fluidly between algebraic and graphical representations with multiple
variables, they will have developed a richer understanding of function.
276 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
PROMOTING GENERALIZATION
Common Core Standards: 7.EE.1, 7.EE.2, F.IF.3, A-SSE.1, A-SSE.2,
A-SSE.3
A main goal in studying patterns and functions is to promote generalizations.
The ability to generalize is what separates success from failure in algebra (see also
Variables & Expressions: Letter Used as a General Number). Generalization is
reasoning that leads to a mathematical rule about relationships or properties.52 It is
moving from a focus on individual cases to articulating the “patterns, procedures,
structures, and the relations across and among them.”53 Students can express gen-
eralizations in four ways: (a) arithmetic, (b) algebraic, (c) graphical/pictorial, and
(d) verbal.54 In order to generalize, they need to understand the notion of variable
and the contingency relationship between two variables, and be able to move
between representations.55
In upper elementary grades, students begin to generalize relationships between
two quantities and can express these relationships using words and “non-stan-
dard” equations. As they move into middle and high school, they explore more
complex relationships and can more readily generate equations or inequalities to
correspond with the relationships. An important role for teachers is to ask ques-
tions that help students make conjectures that lead them to develop generaliza-
tions.
One hazard for the new teacher in particular is the fact that “What’s the next
entry in my pattern?” problems are not well defined. There can be multiple rules
or ways of reasoning that justify different answers for the next number in the pat-
tern. Thus, when asking students to extend patterns, it is essential that the student
explain his or her reasoning, and the teacher must be prepared to evaluate the le-
gitimacy of the explanation and equivalent forms of responses (see Patterns and
Generalizations, Justifying Generalizations). For example, a pattern beginning
1, 2, 3 could continue with 4 (counting), 5 (a Fibonacci-like rule), 6 (the sum of all
previous numbers), 7 (the product of all previous numbers plus 1), etc.
Three stages are necessary when generalizing: seeing a pattern, verbally ar-
ticulating what the student sees, and then recording the pattern that the student
recognizes symbolically.56 In interviews with students, researchers found that stu-
dents may be able to present their thinking using words and gestures before they
can accurately utilize algebraic expressions.57 However, students often struggle to
use the signs and symbols of algebra correctly when asked to present generalized
thinking. When looking at patterns, students may arrive at a “rule” through naïve
induction (trial and error) or through generalization. The pattern that they see is
limited. For instance, for the pattern in Figure 5.11 used in one study, students
said “times two plus one” or “times two plus three” and may have only checked
one or two cases to confirm their strategy. One group of students symbolized their
strategy by writing n × 2(+3).
Patterns & Functions • 277
In contrast, one student, Mel, reasoned that “the top line always has one more
circle than the number of the picture and the bottom line always has two circles
more than the number of the picture” and recorded the formula (n + 1) + (n + 2).58
This is an example of a generalization because Mel noticed some common fea-
tures across the three examples that he could project into the future. Mel reasoned
what he needed to add on to the picture to create the next picture in the pattern.
Another study examined the types of rules (i.e., recursive or explicit) students
generate for given problem situations and the connections they make between the
rule and the situation and between the types of rules they generate (see Figure
5.12). “Recursive rules involve recognizing and using the change from term-to-
term in the dependent variable.… Explicit rules use index-to-term reasoning that
relates the independent variable to the dependent variable(s), allowing for the im-
mediate calculation of any output value.”59 Of the four target students in the study,
two rarely understood the connection between the recursive and explicit rules,
while two used the recursive relationship to build the explicit rule. In both cases,
the generalization came from considering a subdivision of the picture. Mel saw
the nth picture as two rows in which were n + 1 and n + 2 circles, which led him
to an explicit rule—using the number of the picture in his thinking about the rule.
Others saw it as a previous picture with two additional circles, giving a recursive
rule. For instance, Mimi saw it as a previous picture with an additional triangle
of circles (see Figure 5.13)—not using the picture number in her thinking to get
a generalized equation of 2n + 3. Similarly, Doug saw two being added to Picture
2 in Figure 5.14 as it grew. He noticed what was in common and how it changed
from one picture to the next. The two of them reasoned recursively.
Students often reason recursively and have a difficult time transitioning to de-
veloping explicit rules and recognizing the generalizability of their solutions. That
is, they focus on specific instances instead of realizing that their rule is generaliz-
able across instances. Whether students see the generalizability can be influenced
by the kind of tasks they are given and by how well they understand the relevant
mathematical operations and how they are related.
Learning to use algebraic symbols knowledgeably requires students to release
their physical and mental attachment to concrete objects, figures, and tables when
generalizing. It also requires students to move away from specific numbers as-
sociated with those physical realities in order to see a general rule.60 Once they
are able to move from those mental limitations to algebraic notation, it is just as
Teachers
• People naturally want to generalize; we seek patterns in everything to help
us understand the world. However, hasty generalizations are often quite
wrong, so students must be taught to articulate the reasoning for their gen-
eralizations so that it can be explored.
• Generalizations can appear in a recursive form or an explicit form. Both
have their uses, and students should try to generalize in both ways.
These are some questions that press learners to clarify what constitutes a pattern:
• Would the following _________ fit into the given sequence? If yes, how
can you tell and where would it appear? If not, why not?
Questions that encourage learners to consider what is staying the same, what is
changing, and how is it changing:
• What are the variables in this situation? What quantities are changing?
• How are the variables related? As one variable increases, what happens to
the other variable?
• How can you represent the relationship using words, concrete objects, pic-
tures, tables, graphs, or symbols?
• What connections do you see across the representations?
• What does the 10th, 25th, or 100th figure of the pattern look like?
• What does the nth figure of the pattern look like?
280 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
• How would you find the total number of tiles (cubes, dots, toothpicks, etc.)
in the pattern?
Teachers can also use tools that support generalization, such as technology.
Appropriate technology allows students to move quickly and easily among rep-
resentations, to see connections among representations, and to transform repre-
sentations in order to more deeply understand how the components of functions
influence how they look (see Transforming Functions).
Patterns & Functions • 281
JUSTIFYING GENERALIZATION
Common Core Standards: 7EE.1, 7.EE.2, A-SSE.1, A-SSE.2, A-SSE.3
“Engaging teachers in discussions focused on the details of students’ compe-
tencies in justifying and proving may provide a basis for enhancing both teach-
ers’ own understandings of proof and their perspectives regarding proof in school
mathematics.”63 Accordingly, consider the two problems in Table 5.16. Both prob-
lems are about proportional relationships, but they are presented in very different
ways. Each has two phases: finding the relationship between the variables and
justifying the generalization. These problems can be useful as formative assess-
ments of the two phases. In the Connected Gears problem, students will need
to understand that the gears mesh; that is, “more teeth” corresponds to a larger
radius. The goal is to work with students to induce “a habit of mind whereby one
naturally questions and conjectures in order to establish a generalization.”64 For
teachers, it is important to go beyond simply having students recognize patterns,
and to help them “attend to those that are algebraically useful or generalizable”
and be able to argue on their behalf.65
One researcher used these two problems to identify five ways to generalize and
justify or develop algebraic proof (see Table 5.17).66 Students practice generaliza-
tion by considering linear relationships and identifying them as such. In the cases
of the Connected Gears and the Frog Walking problems, students examined ratios
and how ratios stayed the same even when the numbers changed. In this process,
students were 1) searching and identifying common features across cases, 2) ex-
tending reasoning beyond the original example, or 3) making statements derived
from broader results from particular cases. Although correct algebraic general-
izations and deductive forms of proof are a critical instructional goal, students’
incorrect, non-deductive generalizations may serve as an important bridge toward
this goal, which has important implications for teaching. These generalizations
give insight into how the students are thinking and provide the teacher a way to
highlight the misconception with a suitable example or other form of instruction.
Generalizing and justifying are often intertwined and mutually influence each
other as students increase their sophistication of explaining.
Researchers hypothesize that there is a learning trajectory for justification (see
Table 5.18) that shows how justifying is “likely to proceed from inductive toward
deductive and toward greater generality.”67 This trajectory can be a useful tool for
teacher and student reflection regarding the sophistication of their justification
approach.
Research examining the implementation of this framework indicated that stu-
dents have an overwhelming reliance (70% of responses from 350 middle school
students) “on the use of examples as a means of demonstrating and/or verifying
the truth of a statement (Level 1/Level 2)” and don’t recognize the limitations of
Teachers
• Students often do not have difficulty with seeing a pattern, but need help
moving from proof by example to proof by relationship.
• Instead of allowing students to “prove” their rule works by showing that
it results in the right answers for a few cases, have students explain each
component in their rule and how it represents a feature of the situation as
it changes across the pattern. Students need to see the limits and inadequa-
cy of a justification that relies on empirical evidence or tests of particular
numbers. A comprehensive understanding of how the unknowns operate in
a generated formula or rule includes not only a sense of generic examples
as well as specific, but also the connection between the two.
• Simply asking students to generalize and justify can go a long way toward
having them succeed at it. In other words, tasks/problems should include
portions that encourage justification and generalization, given that much of
the difficulty students face stems from lack of familiarity and experience
with justifying and generalizing.
• Patterns can be legitimately extended in infinitely many ways. Thus, jus-
tifying the extension the student chooses is the only way he or she can
convincingly argue that the extension is not just a random guess.
Students begin by searching for patterns in the numbers. It is only after they
are asked to explain their thinking that they move from specific patterns to general
principles. Teachers should ask their students to (a) identify an appropriate domain
for their rules and (b) justify their rules.72 This can help students stay connected to
the model and also employ more sophisticated strategies than “guess-and-check.”
While proof, as justification, occurs mostly in geometry classes, it has an
important role to play in other classes, such as algebra. Consider incorporating
justification early into instruction and emphasizing problems that allow for jus-
tification. Teachers should focus on quantities and the language of quantitative
relationships, rather than just number patterns. Students often attend to number
Patterns & Functions • 285
patterns alone, even within the context of a quantitatively rich problem, so teach-
ers might need to intervene to draw students’ attention back toward the quanti-
tative referents. Ask students to shift from pattern descriptions to phenomenon
descriptions. Over time, these become habits of reasoning that students bring to
a problem situation. The Connected Gears and Frog Walking problems are good
examples of these kinds of problems. The problems supported each other in that
students found commonalities between them that helped them to generalize the
idea of a linear relationship.
286 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
lyze relationships among the various representations of the function (e.g., tables,
graphs, and verbal and symbolic rules).
Students engaged with this problem in different ways. Even though the sixth-
grade student in the interview (Table 5.19) could express the generalization about
Tat Ming’s Pool verbally, she could not yet express both generalizations symboli-
cally. She could express the pool’s area as n2 where n is the pool number, but she
could only describe the pool’s border verbally. However, the ability to verbally
describe a relationship appears to be an important first step toward algebraically
describing the relationship.79 The verbal description is, in fact, an algebraic de-
scription without algebraic notation. This would suggest that with the right ques-
tion or with a little more time, this sixth grader could come up with symbolic
expressions for both relationships in this task.
An important role for teachers is to help students recognize patterns in differ-
ent representations (picture [Pool Tile problem], words [Table 5.19], table [Table
5.20], and graph [Figure 5.17]). In addition, students need to understand how the
pattern is connected across the representations. For instance, how do the pictorial
patterns of white and gray tiles in the Pool Tile problem connect to the graph and
table?
It is important that teachers recognize the many different ways students can
perceive a pattern. In a study with 22 ninth-grade students, twenty-three differ-
ent strategies were used to solve Marcia’s Tiling Squares problem (Figure 5.18),
although not all of them were successful. Almost half of the students who worked
on the task used visual strategies such as grouping, proportioning, chunking, or
examining visual finite differences. For instance, a visual grouping strategy for
the black tiles might count each “arm” of the pattern and then multiply to get the
total, as Student 1 did (Table 5.21). A strategy for counting the white tiles could be
identifying four groups of three white tiles forming an L shape around the center
black cross with an additional four center white squares on each side.
The other strategies were primarily numerical, such as systematic trial and
refinement, extending a table, and numerical use of finite differences in a table.
Students who focused primarily on the numbers in a pattern tended to search for
properties by considering procedural rules for combining, obtaining numbers, and
making calculations. However, some had little understanding of what coefficients
in a formula represented in the relationship. Students may have used variables
simply as placeholders with no meaning except to generate a sequence of num-
bers. Students who used more visual methods may have focused on perceptual
strategies, seeing the functional relationship among the numbers and diagrams. In
that case, variables were seen not only as placeholders, but also as representing
the relationship. Students who used both are “representationally fluent” because
they are able to understand how sequences of numbers can consist of both proper-
ties and relationships.81
These types of pictorial patterns can also be very useful in helping students
see the equivalence of different structures of expressions. For instance, in looking
at the white tile pattern in Figure 5.18, students might see the three white tiles at
the end of each arm staying consistent as 4(3). Then add the inside L of the arm
as 4(1), 4(3), and 4(5) tiles across the three different images or 4(1), 4(1 + 2), 4(1
+ 4). This could result in the expression 4(3) + 4[1 + 2(n – 1)]. Another student
might see the four corners of the figure as 4(3), 4(5), and 4(7) or as 4(1 + 2), 4(1
+ 4), and 4(1 + 6), plus the 4 single white tiles in the middle of each arm con-
sistently across the three images. This might result in the expression 4(1 + 2n) +
4(1). Another student might see a U of five tiles around the top black tile and the
same at the bottom or 2(5) and add the three white tiles on either side or 2(3). Fol-
lowing that visual grouping leads to 2(7) + 2(5)—or the three white tiles on each
side plus two additional—for the next image and 2(9) + 2(7) for the third image.
This would lead to the expression 2(3 + 2n) + 2(1 + 2n). Another student might
simply look at the total number of tiles (16, 24, and 32) and recognize that they are
multiples of 8, leading to an expression of 8(n + 1). There are many other potential
ways of chunking these tiles and resulting expressions. These four different ap-
proaches (see Table 5.22) are all equivalent, the first three simplifying to the 8(n +
1) expression. Students often have difficulty understanding how expressions can
be equivalent, but when viewing tile patterns, they can recognize that they are all
simply different ways of counting the tiles.
Researchers found that students typically used one of four methods to solve
ladder types of problem in Table 5.23:82
TABLE 5.22. Equivalent Expressions for Problem 26: Marcia’s Tiling Squares
4(1 + 2n) + 4(1) 4(3) + 4[1 + 2(n - 1)]
2(3 + 2n) + 2(1 + 2n) 8(n + 1)
292 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Problem Response
28. How many matches would you need to make a 20 rungs* (answers typically in 60s)
ladder with 20 rungs? (16%, 32%, 22%, 49%, 50%)
29. How many matches are needed for a ladder 1000 rungs* (answers range from 300–3000)
with 1000 rungs? (8%, 13%, 13%, 24%, 24%)
*500 4th-, 5th-, 6th-, 7th-, and 8th-grade students, respectively, in Stacey (1989) and Bourke & Stacey
(1988).
Teachers
• Mathematics educators agree that pictorial growth patterns provide a con-
text for supporting algebraic thinking—and, in particular, the generaliza-
tion of relationships between two variables.
• Physically building the models prompts observations about what stays the
same and what changes as you move from one figure of the pattern to the
next. Consider having students construct the pattern, and be sure to watch
Patterns & Functions • 293
ENDNOTES
1 p. 251, citing the work of Hershkowitz & Schwarz, 1999 as well as Heid, 1996
2 Lloyd & Wilson, 1998, p. 270
3 Lloyd & Wilson, 1998, p. 270. Based on Stein, Baxter, & Leinhardt, 1990
4 Hatisaru & Erbas, 2010; Hollar & Norwood, 1999
5 Davidenko, 1997
6 Elia, Panaoura, Eracleous, & Gagatsis, 2007, p. 539 based on Vinner, 1992
7 Norman, 1992
8 Dubinsky & Wilson, 2013, p. 85 based on Carlson, 1998; Clement, 2001; Sierpinska, 1992
9 Even, 1990
10 Breidenbach, Dubinsky, Hawks, & Nichols, 1992
11 Even, 1990
12 Markovitz, Eylon, & Bruckheimer, 1986
13 Sajka, 2003
14 Adapted from Vinner & Dreyfus, 1989, problems #3-7.
15 Vinner & Dreyfus, 1989, pp 359–360.
16 Vinner & Dreyfus, 1989,
17 The trajectory for functions is defined at http://turnonccmath.net.
18 Kaput, 1999, p. 134.
19 Even, 1993
20 Markovitz, Eylon, & Bruckheimer, 1986
21 Elia, Panaoura, Eracleous, & Gagatsis, 2007, p. 538 based on Even, 1998
22 Even, 1993
23 Even, 1993, p. 105
24 Jones, 2006
25 Clement, 2001, p. 746
26 Even, 1993, p. 95
27 Even, 1993, p. 106
28 Even, 1993
29 Davis & McGowen, 2002
30 Davis & McGowen, 2002, p. 5
31 Davis & McGowen, 2002, p. 4
32 Falcade, Laborde, & Mariotti, 2007
33 Knuth, 2000
34 Yerushalmy & Shternberg, 2001, p. 259
35 Yerushalmy & Shternberg, 2001, p. 256
36 Yerushalmy & Shternberg, 2001, p. 256
37 Yerushalmy & Shternberg, 2001, p. 260
38 Knuth, 2000, p. 506
39 Yerushalmy & Shternberg, 2001
40 Pilipczuk, 2008.
294 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
41 Confrey, 1991
42 Bloom, Comber, & Cross, 1986; Goldenberg, 1988; Kalchman, 1998; Markovits, Eylon, &
Bruckheimer, 1986; Schoenfeld, Smith, & Arcavi, 1990
43 Kalchman, 1998, p. 11
44 Borba & Confrey, 1996
45 Borba & Confrey, 1996, p.333
46 Gray & Thomas, 2001
47 See Confrey, 1994 for a more detailed description of these approaches. We left one approach
out, as it was complex to explain in this space.
48 Hines, 2002
49 Thompson, 1994
50 Problems #1-5 are from Trigueros & Martínez-Planell, 2010.
51 Yerushalmy & Shternberg, 2001
52 Carpenter & Franke, 2001; Ellis, 2007; English & Warren, 1995; Lee, 1996
53 Kaput, 1999, p. 137
54 Cañadas, Castro, & Castro, 2009
55 Kalchman, 1998
56 Mason, Pimm, Graham, & Gower, 1985
57 Radford, 2010
58 Radford, 2010
59 Lannin, Barker, & Townsend, 2006, p. 300
60 Radford, 2000
61 Radford, 2000
62 Radford, 2000.
63 Knuth, Slaughter, Choppin, & Sutherland, 2002, p. 1699
64 Blanton & Kaput, 2002, p. 25
65 Ellis, 2007, p. 195 based on Blanton & Kaput, 2002; English & Warren, 1995; Lee, 1996; Lee
&Wheeler, 1987; Orton & Orton, 1994; Stacey, 1989
66 Ellis, 2007
67 Simon & Blume, 1996, p. 9
68 Knuth, Slaughter, Choppin, & Sutherland, 2002, p. 1694
69 Zaslavsky & Ron, 1998
70 Lin, Yang, & Chen, 2004, p. 231. Based on Galbraith, 1981 and Harel & Sowder, 1998
71 Lin, Yang, & Chen, 2004, p. 232. Based on Zaslavsky & Ron, 1998
72 Lannin 2005
73 Zazkis, Loljedahl, & Chernoff, 2008, p. 132 based on Stacey, 1989 and Zazkis & Liljedahl,
2002
74 Zazkis, Liljedahl, & Chernoff, 2008, p. 140
75 Dugdale, 1998, p. 203 based on Abramovich, 1995; Cornell & Siegfried, 1991; Dugdale,
1994; Hoeffner, Kendall, Stellenwerf, Thames, & Williams, 1990; Masalski, 1990; Maxim &
Verhey, 1991; Neuwirth, 1995; Sutherland & Rojano (1993)
76 Lannin, 2005, p. 236
77 Zazkis, Liljedahl, & Chernoff, 2008, p. 132 based on Ainley, Bills & Wilson, 2005; Bills,
Ainley & Wilson, 2006; Lannin, 2005
78 Abramovich, 2000; Drier, 2001, Healy & Hoyles, 1999; Lannin, 2005
79 MacGregor and Stacey 1993, Quinlan 2001
80 Becker & Rivera, 2005
81 Becker & Rivera, 2005
82 Stacey, 1989
83 Warren, 1992, p. 253
84 Quinlan, 2001, p. 426
85 MacGregor & Stacey, 1993
Patterns & Functions • 295
86 Quinlan, 2001
87 MacGregor & Stacey, 1993, p. I-186
88 Billings, 2008
89 Billings, 2008
90 Knuth, 2000
91 Pilipczuk, 2008, p. 672
92 Pilipczuk, 2008, p. 673
93 Based on Hines, 2002
94 Based on Lannin (2005), p. 236, adapted from Simon & Blume (1996) and Waring (2000)
REFERENCES
Abramovich, S. (1995). Technology-motivated teaching of advanced topics in discrete
mathematics. Journal of Computers in Mathematics and Science Teaching, 14(3),
391–418.
Abramovich, S. (2000). Mathematical concepts as emerging tools in computing applica-
tions. Journal of Computers in Mathematics and Science Teaching, 19, 21–46.
Ainley, J., Bills, L., & Wilson, K. (2005). Designing spreadsheet-based tasks for purpose-
ful algebra. International Journal of Computers for Mathematical Learning, 10(3),
191–215.
Becker, J. R., & Rivera, F. (2005). Generalization strategies of beginning high school alge-
bra students. Paper presented at the 29th Conference of the International Group for
the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Melbourne, Australia.
Billings, E. M. H. (2008). Exploring generalization through pictorial growth patterns. In
C. Greenes & R. Rubenstein (Eds.), Algebra and algebraic thinking in school math-
ematics (pp. 279–293). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Bills, L., Ainley, J., & Wilson, K. (2006). Modes of algebraic communication—Moving
between natural language, spreadsheet formulae and standard notation. For the
Learning of Mathematics, 26(1), 41–46.
Blanton, M., & Kaput, J. (2002). Developing elementary teachers’ algebra “eyes and
ears”: Understanding characteristics of professional development that promote
generative and self-sustaining change in teacher practice. Paper presented at the an-
nual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA.
Bloom, L., Comber, G., & Cross, J. (1986). A microcomputer approach to exponential
functions and their derivatives. Mathematics in School, 15(5), 30–32.
Borba, M. C., & Confrey, J. (1996). A student’s construction of transformations of func-
tions in a multiple representational environment. Educational Studies in Mathemat-
ics, 31(3), 319–337.
Breidenbach, D., Dubinsky, E., Hawks, J., & Nichols, D. (1992). Development of the pro-
cess conception of function. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 23(3), 247–285.
Brown, J. P. (2007). Early notions of functions in a Technology-Rich Teaching and Learn-
ing Environment (TRTLE). Paper presented at the 30th annual conference of the
Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia.
Cañadas, M., Castro, E., & Castro, E. (2009). Graphical representation and generalization
in sequences problems. Paper presented at the Congress of the European Society for
Research in Mathematics Education.
Carlson, M. P. (1998). A cross-sectional investigation of the development of the function
concept. In A. H. Schoenfeld, J. Kaput, & E. Dubinsky (Eds.), Research in col-
296 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Waring, S. (2000). Can you prove it? Developing concepts of proof in primary and second-
ary schools. Leicester, UK: The Mathematical Association.
Warren, E. (1992). Algebra: Beyond manipulating symbols. In A. Baturo & T. Cooper
(Eds.), New directions in algebra education (pp. 252–258). Brisbane: Queensland
University of Technology, Centre for Mathematics and Science Education.
Yerushalmy, M., & Shternberg, B. (2001). Charting a visual course to the concept of func-
tion. In A. A. Cuoco & F. R. Curcio (Eds.), The roles of representation in school
mathematics: 2001 Yearbook (pp. 251–268). Reston, VA: National Council of
Teachers of Mathematics.
Zaslavsky, O., & Ron, G. (1998). Students’ understanding of the role of counter-examples.
PME, 22(4), 225–232.
Zazkis, R., & Liljedahl, P. (2002). Generalization of patterns: The tension between alge-
braic thinking and algebraic notation. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 49(3),
379–402.
Zazkis, R., Liljedahl, P., & Chernoff, E. (2008). The role of examples in forming and refut-
ing generalizations. ZDM, 40(1), 131–141.
CHAPTER 6
INTRODUCTION
The phrase “word problems” evokes a strong response from most adults. In a
survey, algebra teachers cited word problems as “the most serious deficiency of
incoming students.”1 Often found at the end of the exercises in a textbook, prob-
lems that involve reading and context have a reputation for being difficult. Most
traditional textbook exercise sets start with concepts devoid of context and then
end with word problems that are perceived to be the challenges. However, in the
same survey, algebra teachers also cited “motivation” as the single biggest chal-
lenge they face as teachers.2 Word problems and modeling have the potential to
make mathematics meaningful and therefore motivational when used effectively.
In spite of the reputation of word problems, there is data to indicate that when
students are presented problems in a context that is understandable to them, they
actually perform quite well.3 Particularly when solving simple algebra word prob-
lems when a variable is mentioned just once, the context often helps students
make sense of the problem. Struggles with story problems are often more about
comprehension of the task and not the generation of a solution.4 The difficulty
students often have with word problems is how to represent the context symboli-
cally.5
Teachers’ beliefs about the potential difficulty of a problem for their students
do not always align with the actual ability of students to solve tasks.6 Teachers can
misread what aspects of a problem are challenging for students and their level of
understanding. Based on their interpretation of students’ development in algebra,
teachers often change how they have students interact with word problems.7 For
instance, teachers’ beliefs about the value of students’ informal methods influ-
ence the shape of their instructional practice. They may believe that only formal
methods are worthwhile. However, students who use informal and formal prob-
lem-solving methods in combination can perform better than those who simply
use formal methods.8 Teachers tend to believe that students engage in cycles of
learning formal, symbolic mathematics and then applying that understanding to
word problems in arithmetic and algebra. Teachers start with the symbolic forms
because they believe they are simplest. However, some research indicates that
starting with words and students’ informal strategies and then moving to symbolic
forms may be more beneficial.9 “Instruction that bridges formal algebra instruc-
tion to previously grounded representations helps students learn processes such as
algebraic modeling of verbally presented relations.”10 Instruction that integrates
students’ informal methods is particularly helpful when students struggle with
manipulation of symbols. Of course, these informal strategies have limitations,
particularly as students engage in word problems that have more complex struc-
tures.
Researchers suggest that the process of engaging with word problems can be
divided into the comprehension phase and the solution phase.11 First, students must
understand the text of the story, as well as any other forms of data such as charts or
diagrams, and “create corresponding internal representations of the quantitative
and situation-based relationships expressed in that text.”12 The comprehension
phase includes the transformation of the quantitative relationships into a symbolic
representation and is usually the source of most problem-solving difficulties and
errors.13 Lastly, students work toward a solution. Solving word problems is not
typically linear, but rather a back-and-forth process of comprehending part of the
task and working to represent that understanding. A first step in the transformation
process is Defining Variables to Represent Generalizations1 related to the prob-
lem context. The research on difficulties with variables is extensive and warrants
1
Note: bold and italicized print indicates a reference to an entry in the book
Modeling and Word Problems • 303
an entire chapter in this book (See Variables and Expressions). In this chapter, we
note that one such misconception stems from generalizing contexts into variables
and confusing letters that represent qualities or quantities.
The next natural step for mathematics students is to Translate Word Prob-
lems into Algebraic Sentences, or change the English context into algebraic
symbols. In elementary school, student intuition about the translation is often ac-
curate. However, as their learning progresses, some students become dependent
on specific examples that they can replicate in similar situations. In this chapter
we discuss specific tasks that can be used to address common student errors with
translation. Another strategy supported by research is paying close attention to
the features of algebraic expressions and how they impact an expression’s value.
A significant aspect of making decisions about the algebraic expression that
best represents a given situation is Choosing Operations that best fit the task.
Many tasks in current algebra curricula follow similar patterns and students have
come to recognize these patterns. Without really understanding their actions, stu-
dents can make decisions that work, even when they do not understand why. For
example, given a context where one number is bigger than another, they might
choose to use division since we rarely have reasons to divide a small number
by one that is large. They might also try taking both numbers and doing all four
operations and then just make a decision about which answer looks best. These
strategies are not mathematically mature, but they often produce correct answers
in the carefully controlled contexts we find in mathematics classes.
After the word problem has been translated into an expression, students now
need to proceed with a solution. They must Choose Between Algebraic and
Arithmetic Solution Strategies, regardless of whether Drawing Models as an In-
termediate Step in Solving a Word Problem would be helpful, or whether Linear
Versus Non-linear Modeling would be most appropriate for the context. These
decisions should be made with mathematical justification. However, the deci-
sions can sometimes stem from something as simple as copying whatever was
demonstrated in class recently. Teachers can challenge this behavior by assigning
specific tasks and by demonstrating problem-solving strategies that work across
contexts. Specific illustrations of this follow in the entries in this chapter.
We would be remiss if we did not acknowledge the Issues of Language in
a Task—the challenges that language presents to students solving word prob-
lems. Mathematics teachers have much to learn from the research done in the
area of English Language Learners. The strategies and techniques supported by
the research can be applied to word problems. We want to be sure that students
who understand the mathematics are not held back by language constraints. The
way we write tasks and the way we assess student learning can address this issue
straight on.
Finally, there is one key choice for algebra teachers to make: should I start with
learning equations and symbols and then move to word problems (the traditional
method of teaching algebra) or should I start with story problems and develop stu-
304 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
1. In a classroom there are 2 chairs beside each table. If there are n chairs,
how many chairs and tables are there altogether?
2. On a math test, the number of students who pass is 3 times the number
of students who fail. If the number of students who pass is p, how many
students are in this class?
3. n coins are to be divided between Jack and Jill. Jill must first get k coins.
The rest of the coins are divided so that for every x coins that Jill gets,
Jack gets z coins. How many coins does Jack get?
In spite of the fact that the questions contained all the variables needed to respond,
42% of students introduced variables that were the first letters of the objects. In
Problem 1, for example, 8.1% of students defined the variables as “t = tables and
c = chairs” and then came up with a formula such as A = (2c + 1t), using the vari-
ables as adjectives rather than quantities. This type of error has been confirmed
in other research.28
306 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
When students attempt to translate words to symbols, they must assign vari-
ables to the quantities of interest. There are some common student errors when
students use variables to model word problems:29
In Table 6.1, researchers asked secondary students to pick variable names that
would help indicate their relationship. Students could discuss the relationship ac-
curately but were unable to use the “algebraic code” to represent the relationship.30
The responses demonstrate that students struggled to understand how to convey
the relationship between the two numbers by defining the appropriate variable. In
another study, when given a word problem, 87% of 245 college students did not
define the variable, or named the variable in an imprecise way, such as “b is for
books.”31
In the interview for Problem 5 (see Table 6.2), a student articulates that she did
not understand what the variable she chose represented.32 The student acknowl-
edges that she didn’t understand what the letter represented and was still impre-
cise in defining letter P, even after prompting by the interviewer. Due to their lack
of understanding of how a letter might be defined for a word problem, students
can arrive at incorrect conclusions or fail to understand what their answer is tell-
ing them about the problem.33 This failure likely stems from students’ attempts to
have the variable represent both quantity and quality. The letter P in Problem 1
cues the student that the letter represents “pounds,” or the weight of nuts (qual-
TABLE 6.2. Problem 5: Pecans and Cashews: Student Interview on Defining Vari-
able
A person went to the store and bought pecans and cashews. He bought a total of 100 nuts. The
number of pounds of pecans he bought was the same as the number of pounds of cashews. Eight
pecans weigh 1 pound and 12 cashews weigh 1 pound. How many pounds of pecans did he buy?
Interviewer: You multiplied [the 5] times 8 and got 40. Then you divided 40 by 8 to get 5 again.
Student: Yeah, so I probably could have stopped but I didn’t recognize that that was the answer.
Interviewer: Why do you think that … you didn’t recognize the answer?
Student: Because probably not until I looked back did I realize that the P I was trying to find
was pounds. If I realized it first, then I would have said P is pounds and P is 5 then 5
pounds of pecans is what he bought. But not until you asked me what was P, what was
P standing for, then I realized that it had to be pounds.
Source: Rosnick (1982, p. 6)
ity), and it stands for the number of pounds (quantity) in the problem equation. To
assist students in overcoming this confusion, teachers may want to provide many
opportunities for students to define variables for simple word problems, translate
the words into multi-variable equations, and then explain how the quantities in
the problem statement connect with the variables in the equation. For an in-depth
review of this topic, see Variables & Expressions: Representation—What Can
Variables Stand For?, Letter as an Object, and Letter Used as an Unknown
Number.
Another common type of problem in algebra is generalizing from a pattern.
Researchers have found three specific strategies that students use when they try to
define a variable for the general term for a pattern, such as in Figure 6.1.34 First, n
is considered as a place holder to be taken by numbers, with the expression for the
pattern determined by trial-and-error. The formula is seen as a procedural mecha-
nism. For example, with the pattern in Figure 6.1, students found the formula n x 2
+ 1 (where “n “ stands for the number of the figure). In Interview 1 (see Table 6.3),
the student knew only that the formula works to get the right number of toothpicks
for each figure. The rest of the interview indicates that students really struggled
with the meaning of the variable and its purpose in the equation. The second strat-
egy recognizes the recursive nature of the problem. Students saw that the number
of toothpicks in one figure was two more than the previous figure (see Table 6.3,
Interview 2). However, students did not equate what they noticed numerically
with a general form. They saw the general pattern, but only through each instance
of recursion. Their definition of the variable was “all that is unknown,” as is ap-
parent with Student 5’s final comment. In the third strategy, students saw the con-
nection between the visual pattern and the general form (see Table 6.3, Interview
3). They understood how the symbolic expression reproduced the shape of the
figures of the pattern.
308 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
a b c
Good problem solvers usually have “a glimpse of a possible path, and put it
implicitly in their first trials of naming; usually they are able to incorporate the
relationships among the elements and prefigure transformations apt to reach the
solution.”35 As students define the variable for the problem, they keep in mind
how they will use the variable to achieve a solution. Novices, on the other hand,
tend to name variables more randomly and superficially, not anticipating their role
in the algebraic sentence.
Teachers
• Students can often accurately discuss what is going on in a word problem,
but may not be able to represent that understanding algebraically. Help
them make the connection between their words and the meaning of the
variable(s) used.
• Help students understand the difference between qualitative information
in a problem and quantitative information that can be represented by a
variable.
• Ask students to justify each variable they choose and how it relates to the
problem. “Students need to make the link between the representation and
the problem context.”36
• Help students be precise in the language they use to define a variable.
• Provide students with opportunities to define variables as representing
an object. Contrast that with tasks where variables represent an unknown
number or a range of numbers.
• In pattern-recognition problems, encourage students to tie aspects of their
formulas to aspects of the visual.
• The use of tables may help students understand the role of variables in
generalizations of patterns.37
310 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Problem 9
Some money is shared between Mark and Jan so that Mark gets $5 more than Jan gets. Jan gets $x.
Use algebra to write Mark’s amount. The money to be shared is $47. Use algebra to work out how
much Jan and Mark would get.
Problem 10
I think of a number, multiply it by 8, subtract 3, and then divide by 3. The result is twice the number
I first thought of. What was the number? Write an equation and solve it.
ing to note that more students had difficulty writing the equation than answering
the first two problems. Only about one third of students were able to create equa-
tions for each problem, while twice that amount were able to get accurate answers
for the first two problems, typically using trial and error. In Problem 10, the most
abstract of the problems, only 17% were able to get an accurate solution.
For many students, the challenge with word problems lies in the translation
between English and mathematical notation and then vice versa, especially when
the task is not in a familiar context.51 “Common solution error patterns are direct-
ly related to the linguistic sophistication possessed by the solver.”52 Researchers
have found that students’ level of literacy is a predictor of their performance on
algebraic word problems.53 The difficulty of the word problem is often correlated
with text comprehension factors such as ambiguous words. For instance, words
such as “some” and “altogether” were often misinterpreted by students. When
researchers used the same problem structure but made simple changes in wording,
such as using the words “more than” instead of “less than,” students’ success sig-
nificantly increased.54 The “errors” students made with word problems were often
correct interpretations or solutions to miscomprehended problems.55 Generally,
word problems can be easier for students to solve when the “problem situation ac-
tivates real-world knowledge that aids students in arriving at correct solutions.”56
Because mathematics is such a dense language, every symbol matters; yet
most students are not accustomed to reading text carefully. For example, the dif-
ference between f ’(x) = 6x and f(x) = 6x is enormous. While students might be
able to solve a problem conceptually, they may still struggle with symbolically
representing the same problem. This leads to difficulties when they are faced with
a problem they cannot solve by simple reasoning and need algebraic symbols to
represent aspects of the problem. “Unsuccessful problem solvers base their solu-
tion plan on numbers and key words that they select from the problem (the direct
translation strategy), whereas successful problem solvers construct a model of
the situation described in the problem and base their solution plan on this model
(the problem-model strategy).”57 Students use the direct translation strategy as a
shortcut that bypasses any need to develop meaning for the problem. They look
for words that suggest mathematical operations (e.g., “more” means addition and
“less” means subtraction)58 so they can “compute first and think later.”59 In con-
trast, students using the problem-model strategy create a mental model of the
relationships in the problem and are more likely to correctly solve the problem
as a result.
The tasks in Table 6.6 were given to ten sixth-grade students.60 In the tasks,
students could typically compute specific cases for each of the problems with
little difficulty. In a number of instances, they were even able to develop reason-
able symbolic representations of each problem. However, very few of the students
were able to use the equations they wrote to explain the general case, or to use
the equation to solve a randomly chosen specific case. This demonstrates that
students come to early algebra instruction with significant prior knowledge, and
underscores the need for teachers to build on this knowledge. These tasks are also
a good resource for teachers to use in their classes. The researchers in this study
found that, as students computed multiple cases over the same context (e.g., The
Border Problem for different dimensions), algebraic generalization became easier.
It is apparent that students become highly dependent on examples and strate-
gies found in their textbooks. One research study compared errors in problem
solving between Japanese and Thai secondary school students and found they
were able to solve problems that were similar to those found in their textbooks,
but unable to create models for problems with features unlike those in their re-
spective texts.61 It appeared that they memorized processes rather than under-
stood the concepts underlying the problems. In contrast, researchers found that
students who learned in a problem-based environment were more flexible and
better able to apply their mathematical understanding to translate word problems
into mathematical notation.62 They had opportunities to explore their thinking first
and then look for mathematical notation to represent that thinking. When teachers
gradually introduced algebraic notation after students informally represented their
Modeling and Word Problems • 313
thinking, the notation helped students record what they understood and helped
them to structure their thinking.63
Students come to algebra with significant prior knowledge that allows them to
solve complex tasks for specific cases. When algebraic notation is introduced in a
way that connects to this knowledge, even young students can be empowered to
solve challenging problems. Students will come to depend on examples presented
in curricula, so it is critical that we select rich, problem-based texts that allow
students to apply their intuition while solving problems.
Teachers
• Don’t take shortcuts to answers by giving students key-word strategies for
solving word problems. Find ways to support students in making meaning
of the problem instead.
Modeling and Word Problems • 315
• Tasks such as those in Table 6.6 can allow students to build on their in-
tuitive knowledge of a problem’s context prior to establishing notation to
represent a problem.
• Students are often able to compute specific examples correctly. Give stu-
dents opportunities to connect their algebraic representation to the specific
examples in order to confirm their thinking.
• Emphasize the important role that each symbol and each letter play in an
expression. Give students experiences that allow them to see the impact of
changing small parts of expressions.
316 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
CHOOSING OPERATIONS
Common Core Standards: 6.EE.6, 7, 7.EE.A.2, 7.EE.B.3, CED.A.1
When translating word problems into operational notation, students often
struggle to identify the correct operation(s) to use (e.g., addition, subtraction, mul-
tiplication, or division). Students often use non-mathematical strategies to select
an operation (or operations) to use when solving a word problem.64 Even their
strategies for choosing correct operations can be limited and superficial. Consider
Problem 16:
A bag of snack food contains 4 vitamins and weighs 228 grams. How many grams
of snack food are in 6 bags altogether?
answer. If the answer should be smaller, the student tries subtraction and
division and then chooses the more reasonable answer.
7. Meaning-based. Students understand what to do based on the meaning
of the problem. Teachers desire this solution, but students need to be
pressed to justify their thinking. Justification is a key component to any
solution, in order to know if it is meaning-based, rather than blind luck.
The first six strategies are mathematically immature, yet students use them
because (1) they work often enough, (2) teachers tend not to push students to use
a more sophisticated approach, (3) teachers may not question students about why
they used a particular strategy, and (4) these are the only strategies students have
at their disposal. However, students who have a well-developed mathematical
understanding of the operations will select operations based on which fit the story
best. Noteworthy in this research was the fact that students who were able to draw
a picture to represent Problem 16 were more likely to use a meaning-based solu-
tion strategy [see Drawing Models as an Intermediate Step]. The goal is for all
students to choose operations based on their understanding of both the problem
situation and the meanings of the operations.
It is important to acknowledge that “correct answers are not a safe indicator
of good thinking” and teachers should require students to justify their choice of
an operation, in addition to requiring that students give an answer to the problem
67
Teachers could have students consider questions like “How did you know to
[divide] here?” or “What words made you think addition was the appropriate op-
eration to use?”—on individual written work, in small-group discussion, and in
whole-class discussion. Teachers can then emphasize the benefits of choosing an
operation based on understanding the meaning of the operation. Shortcomings of
limited or unsophisticated strategies can be highlighted in class discussion after
having students solve problems that involve extraneous information, fractions or
decimals, or multi-step solutions.
Teachers
• Try to identify when students are implementing one of the six “mathemati-
cally immature” strategies.
• Use questioning to press for justification of operation choice. Ask students
to explain more than what they did—ask why they did it.
• Draw explicit connections between what the problem is describing and the
correct operation.
318 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
problems that the equation can become somewhat detached from the context for
students.74
Teachers should recognize that formulating an equation is not an intuitive
way for many students to represent a problem. To help bridge the gap between
arithmetic and algebraic solution strategies, “supports need to be put in place
for students to make connections between formal algebraic representation, in-
formal arithmetic-based reasoning, and situational knowledge.”75 When working
on word problems, “Teachers view themselves as responsible for assuring that
students have learned the canonical method for solving linear equations.”76 They
don’t tend to be open to non-standard approaches that might be more efficient or
easier to comprehend.77 Teachers should not discourage creative uses of arithme-
tic to solve problems, but rather engage students in discussing when algebraic
approaches are helpful and more efficient.
Teachers
• Most students choose arithmetic over algebraic solution strategies, as they
do not see the utility of algebra. Even when the problem is too difficult
to be solved arithmetically, they are more likely to choose some form of
guess-and-check rather than algebraic methods.
• Use simple problems early in instruction to illustrate that arithmetic and
algebraic strategies yield the same results.
• To help students transition to algebraic strategies, teachers must use prob-
lems that cannot be solved with simple arithmetic strategies.
Modeling and Word Problems • 321
One educator suggests that there are five steps that students need to work
through in order to solve a word problem successfully:89
Most errors that students make occur in the first three steps—during the process of
interpreting the words—before they even begin to solve the problem. In order to
help students understand word problems, teachers often focus on key words such
as “more” and “times.” This strategy is useful in the short term, but limited in the
long term because key words don’t help students understand the problem situa-
322 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
tion (i.e., what is happening in the problem). Key words can also be misleading
because the same word may mean different things in different situations. Consider
the following two examples:
1. There are 7 boys and 21 girls in a class. How many more girls than boys
are there?
2. There are 21 girls in a class. There are 3 times as many girls as boys.
How many boys are in the class?
In the first problem, if students focus on the word “more,” they may add when
they actually need to subtract. In the second problem, if students focus on the
word “times,” they may multiply when they actually need to divide.
Students who draw models of word problems may overcome many of the dif-
ficulties associated with translating a word problem directly into algebraic sym-
bols.90 A number of studies have shown that diagrams are helpful, from arithmetic
word problems to algebraic word problems.91 In fact, some research indicates that
students can do better by drawing their own diagrams than students who were
given a diagram.92 For example, significantly more students doing Problem 20
(Table 6.9) recognized that you could not cut 10-cm-tall triangles with 5-cm-wide
metal as they drew their own diagram. “By actively engaging with the diagram
students are, in fact, engaging in the process of meaning construction.”93
There are many different kinds of drawing models students may use. The mod-
el method, used in Singapore, which is one of the top-performing countries on
international tests, involves representing key information of the word problem
using rectangles. Table 6.10 includes examples of a model for solving part-whole
problems, (additive) comparison problems, and multiplication and division prob-
lems.94 After each problem set, there is an example of a rectangle that could be
used to represent the task. In each case, the first rectangle represents the arithmetic
word problem, while the second rectangle represents the algebraic word problem.
The model method is based on a three-phase problem-solving process.95 The
approach has teachers demonstrate all three problem-solving phases for students
before asking them to use the model method on their own.
Consider Problem 21 in Table 6.10. First, the teacher reads the text with students,
confirming that students understand the words and vocabulary. Next, the class
builds a rectangular model. In this case, the problem can be represented by the
part-whole arithmetic model (see Figure 6.2)
Finally, the model is used to determine which equation and procedures should
be used to generate the answer. In this case: 1050 + 1608 = x.96
Models alone do not make a problem directly understandable. Students need
instruction on features of models and how their design represents a context.97
“Representational competency,” or the ability to understand and create repre-
sentations of mathematical ideas, is a critical component to participation in the
community of a classroom. Teachers need to help their students understand the
features of a representation and how it supports the solution of a task.98 Student-
created models can be effective tools to bridge the gap between the task and the
solution, and can aid students in understanding a task and illuminate areas where
they struggle. Research indicates that computer-based, interactive models may
also be effective in helping students reason through algebraic word problems.99
Teachers
• Use the 3-phase Model Method to facilitate the translation of a task from
words to symbols.
• At each phase of the process, students should compare the text, the rect-
angle, and the equation to confirm that each is correct.
• To encourage understanding through models, students could compare cor-
rect models to incorrect (or partially correct) models, discuss multiple cor-
rect models to illuminate different strategies, or compare their symbolic
representations against models.
• When students are struggling with word problems and a particular rep-
resentation, research indicates that a teacher-supported switch to a new
representation may facilitate improved understanding.100
• The Center for Algebraic Thinking app Distance Rate Time Solver can help
students model DRT types of problems (see Figure 6.3).
326 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Students typically respond to the first problem by saying you need to triple the
drying time for the 3 shirts and to the second problem by saying it will take him
10 times as long.103 In each case, they neglect to make sense of the context and
tend to overgeneralize and jump to a proportional relationship. Researchers have
documented this tendency with the problems in Table 6.13. Over 90% of 120
twelve- to thirteen-year old students got the proportional problem correct while
approximately 5% got the non-proportional problem correct.104
A common approach in algebra courses is for the teacher to present a sample
word problem and then offer a solution path for the students to follow. Research-
ers suggest that an alternative approach when trying to help students understand
the dynamics of non-linear relationships is to present a task with no particular
solution path.105 As students have difficulty with the problem, then a series of
interventions are introduced one phase at a time until the student succeeds. Each
phase is explained in Table 6.14, using Problem 28 from Table 6.11 to illustrate it.
Much of a traditional algebra curriculum focuses on linear relationships. When
presented with non-linear tasks, it is not surprising that students rely on previous
linear strategies to solve the problems. Using the phases illustrated above may aid
students in recognizing their errors. Not surprisingly, students better understand
their error in over-generalizing linearity as they are shown more phases. The use-
fulness to teachers is that these phases are cumulative and progress from lesser to
greater levels of cognitive support and direct instruction.
Teachers
• Students tend to believe that all word problems in algebra are linear rela-
tionships. It is important for teachers to provide tasks that dispel this belief.
• Students should be given a chance to solve problems without a provided
solution strategy.
• If errors present, there are ways to challenge student thinking without dic-
tating a strategy. By progressively introducing the challenges in the five
phases listed in Table 6.14, teachers can help students adjust their thinking
while encouraging true understanding.
330 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
The example in Table 6.18 is part of a larger issue around vocabulary use and
development. Even when ELL students display understanding of decontextual-
ized vocabulary and understanding of mathematics, they do not necessarily dis-
play competence in translating narrative problems to algebraic representations.
Studies reveal that syntax variables, such as the problem length (measured by
total number of words), the level of vocabulary, the number of sentences, and the
degree of separation of data within a problem are reasonably good predictors of
performance among age groups.110 Lower levels of language proficiency com-
pound these issues.
One strategy to assist students in solving these tasks is drawing pictures or
building models for the problem (See Drawing Models as an Intermediate Step
in Solving a Word Problem). A chart that is developed prior to writing an equa-
tion may be useful. For Problem 30, we might write:
Students---------Professors
6 ------------------1
12 -----------------2
18 -----------------3
Etc.
Students should then translate the problem statement into an algebraic sentence.
Following this, they may substitute their numbers from the chart into the equation
to confirm the accuracy of the sentence. Discussions about the truth (or not) of the
number sentences will build a sense of usefulness of the algebraic representation.
Another strategy is to reverse the process: given a mathematical sentence (such as
s = 6p), write a sentence in English that describes it.
Rewording problems may also make them easier to solve. Table 6.19 has ways
to reword problems that make it significantly easier for students to solve.111 While
the problems are elementary focused, there are insights for algebra teachers. First,
the more explicit the wording of the problem, the easier it is for students to solve.
Given that we want students to solve complex problems as well, it is good for
teachers to help students deconstruct word problems to clarify the language used
and facilitate students making explicit what is implicit. Consider the dialogue in
Table 6.20. In this conversation, the teacher helps the students deconstruct the
relationships between the dieters’ weights and focus on the changes in quantities
before working toward a solution.
One question for algebra teachers is whether writing story problems based
on individual students’ interests and experiences can have a positive effect (see
Problem 37). The typical intent of personalizing word problems is to increase
motivation and tap into students’ prior knowledge as a means to enhancing their
access to the mathematics. Some studies found that personalized problems did en-
hance learning with students.112 Understanding and seeing relevance in the prob-
lem helped students have access to the mathematics and solutions. In Problem 38
(see Table 6.21), for instance, the student was able to make sense of the solution
because of the context. However, other studies found that personalization is not
always effective and may distract students’ attention, particularly with students
who have lower interest in math.113 In Problem 39 (see Table 6.22), for example,
the student became distracted by her own experience in math classes, in which
about half of students get A’s and B’s and the other half do not.
When translating text-based tasks into algebraic expressions and equations, all
students can struggle to make sense of the language. Students who are English
Language Learners face additional challenges when they have limited vocabulary.
Yet, they also bring rich experiences that influence the way that they interpret
and understand story problems. It is critical for teachers to properly assess each
student’s abilities with regard to language and identify specific supports for areas
that may hinder students’ understanding of word problems.
Teachers
• Misconceptions about the English language may compound difficulties that
students have with translating mathematical tasks into new representations.
• While these language issues are especially pertinent for ELL students, they
are also relevant for all students.
• Rewording problems or discussing what is implicit in the problem may
help students comprehend the text and move toward the mathematical rep-
resentation.
Modeling and Word Problems • 335
ENDNOTES
1 Walkington, Sherman, & Petrosino, 2012, p. 174 based on Loveless, Fennel, Williams, Ball,
Banfield, 2008
2 Loveless, Fennel, Williams, Ball, Banfield, 2008, p. 9–xiii
3 Baranes, Perry, & Stigler, 1989; Carraher, Carraher, & Schliemann, 1987; Koedinger &
Nathan, 2004; Walkington, Sherman, & Petrosino, 2012
4 Koedinger & Nathan, 2004
5 Koedinger, Alibali & Nathan, 2008
6 Nathan & Koedinger, 2000
7 Nathan & Koedinger, 2000
8 Koedinger & Tabachneck, 1994
9 Koedinger & Anderson, 1997; Nathan, Kintsch, & Young, 1992
10 Nathan & Koedinger, 2000, p. 186, based on Koedinger & Anderson, 1997 and Nathan,
Kintsch, & Young, 1992
11 Koedinger & Nathan, 2004 based on Cummins, Kintsch, Reusser, & Weimer, 1988; Hall,
Kibler, Wenger, & Truxaw, 1989; Lewis & Mayer, 1987, Mayer, 1982
12 Koedinger & Nathan, 2004, 131
13 Koedinger & Nathan, 2004 based on Cummins, Kintsch, Reusser, & Weimer, 1988 and Lewis
& Mayer, 1987
14 Koedinger & Nathan, 2004; Nathan & Koedinger, 2000a, 2000b; Nathan & Petrosino, 2003;
Nathan, Long, & Alibali, 2002
15 Walkington, Sherman, & Petrosino, 2012, p. 174 based on Boaler, 1994
16 Baranes, Perry, & Stigler, 1989; Boaler, 1994; Cooper & Harries, 2009; Greer, 1997; Inoue,
2005; Kazemi, 2002; Palm, 2008; Reusser & Stebler, 1997; Roth, 1996
17 Walkington, Sherman, & Petrosino, 2012, based on Blum & Niss, 1991 and Durik &
Harackiewicz, 2007
18 Walkington, Sherman, & Petrosino, 2012, p. 175
19 Also consider Dan Meyer’s website that encourages the use of unstructured problems and
provides numerous examples (http://blog.mrmeyer.com).
20 Kieran, 1992; Usiskin, 1988, 1997
21 Kieran, 2007 based on Radford, 2004
22 Booth, 1989, p. 57
23 Kieran, 2007, p. 711 based on Noss and Hoyles, 1996
24 Radford, 2004
25 Swafford & Langrall, 2000
26 Rosnick, 1982, p. 4
27 Hubbard, 2004
28 Stacey & MacGregor, 1997
29 Stacey & MacGregor, 1993
30 Arzarello, Bazzini, & Chiappini, 1994
31 Rosnick, 1982
32 Rosnick, 1982, p. 6
33 Rosnick, 1982
34 Radford, 2000
35 Arzarello, Bazzini, & Chiappini, 1994, p. 42
36 Swafford & Langrall, 2000, p. 109
37 Swafford & Langrall, 2000
38 Johanning, 2004, p. 372, based on Bednarz & Janvier, 1996; Hall, Kibler, Wenger, & Truxaw,
1989a; Kieran, Boileau, & Garancon, 1996; Nathan & Koedinger, 2000; Rojano, 1996
39 Walkington, Sherman, & Petrosino, 2012, p. 181
336 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
REFERENCES
Anand, P., & Ross, S. (1987). Using computer-assisted instruction to personalize arithme-
tic materials for elementary school children. Journal of Educational Psychology,
79(1), 72–78.
Arzarello, F., Bazzini, L., & Chiappini, G. (1994). The process of naming in algebraic
problem solving. Paper presented at the 18th Annual Conference of the International
Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (PME), Lisbon, Portugal.
Baranes, R., Pery, M., & Stigler, J. W. (1989). Activation of real-world knowledge in the
solutions of word problems. Cognition and Instruction, 6, 287–318.
Bednarz, N., & Janvier, B. (1996). Emergence and development of algebra as a problem-
solving tool: Continuities and discontinuities with arithmetic. In N. Bednarz, C. Ki-
338 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
eran, & L. Lee (Eds.), Approaches to algebra: Perspectives for research and teach-
ing (pp. 115–136). Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Bednarz, N., Radford, L., Janvier, B., & Lepage, A. (1992). Arithmetical and algebraic
thinking in problem solving. Paper presented at the 16th International Conference
for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Durham, NH.
Blum, W., & Niss, M. (1991). Applied mathematical problem solving, modeling, applica-
tions, and links to other subjects: State, trends & issues. Educational Studies in
Mathematics, 22, 37–68.
Boaler, J. (1994). When do girls prefer football to fashion? An analysis of female un-
derachievement in relation to ‘realistic’ mathematics contexts. British Education
Research Journal, 20(5), 551–564.
Booth, J. L., & Koedinger, K. R. (2012). Are diagrams always helpful tools? Developmen-
tal and individual differences in the effect of presentation format on student problem
solving. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(3), 492–511.
Booth, L. R. (1989). A question of structure. In S. Wagner & C. Kieran (Eds.), Research
issues in the learning and teaching of algebra (pp. 57–59). Reston, VA: NCTM.
Briars, D. J., & Larkin, J. H. (1984). An integrated model of skill in solving elementary
word problems. Cognition and Instruction, 1, 245–296.
Brizuela, B. M., & Schliemann, A. D. (2003). Fourth graders solving equations. Medford,
MA: Tufts University.
Brizuela, B., Carraher, D., & Schliemann, A. (2000). Mathematical notation to support and
further reasoning (“to help me think of something”). Chicago, IL: NCTM Research
Pre-session.
Carpenter, T. P., Corbitt, M. K., Kepner, H. S., Lindquist, M. M., & Reys, R. E. (1980).
Solving verbal problems: Results and implications for national assessment. Arith-
metic Teacher, 28, 8–12.
Carpenter, T. P., Fennema, E., Franke, M. L., Levi, L., & Empson, S. B. (1999). Children’s
mathematics: Cognitively guided instruction. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann
Carraher, T. N., Carraher, D. W., & Schliemann, A. D. (1987). Written and oral mathemat-
ics. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 18, 83–97.
Clement, L., & Bernhard, J. (2005). A problem solving alternative to using key words.
Mathematics Teaching in the Middle Grades, 10, 47–75.
Cooper, B., & Harries, T. (2009). Realistic contexts, mathematics assessment, and social
class. In B. Greer, L. Verschaffel, W. Van Dooren, & S. Mukhopadhyay (Eds.), Word
and worlds: Modelling verbal descriptions of situations. Rotterdam, the Nether-
lands: Sense Publishers.
Cordova, D., & Lepper, M. (1996). Intrinsic motivation and the process of learning: Ben-
eficial effects of contextualization, personalization, and choice. Journal of Educa-
tional Psychology, 88(4), 715–730.
Cox, R. (1996). Analytical reasoning with multiple external representations (Unpublished
doctoral dissertation). The University of Edinburgh, Scotland.
Cox, R., & Brna, P. (1995). Supporting the use of external representations in problem solv-
ing: The need for flexible learning environments. Journal of Artificial Intelligence
in Education, 6(2/3), 239–302.
Cummins, D., Kintsch, W., Reusser, K., & Weimer, R. (1988). The role of understanding in
solving word problems. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 439–462.
Modeling and Word Problems • 339
Davis-Dorsey, J., Ross, S., & Morrison, G. (1991). The role of rewording and context per-
sonalization in the solving of mathematical word problems. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 83(1), 61–68.
De Bock, D., Van Dooren, W., Janssens, D., & Verschaffel, L. (2002). Improper use of
linear reasoning: An in-depth study of the nature and the irresistibility of secondary
school students’ errors. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 50(3), 311–334.
De Bock, D., Verschaffel, L., & Janssens, D. (1998). The predominance of the linear model
in secondary school students’ solutions of word problems involving length and area
of similar plane figures. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 35(1), 65–83.
De Corte, E., Verschaffel, L., & De Win, L. (1985). Influence of rewording verbal prob-
lems on children’s problem representations and solutions. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 77, 460–470.
Diezmann, C. M. (1995). Evaluating the effectiveness of the strategy ‘Draw a diagram’ as
a cognitive tool for problem solving. In B. Atweh & S. Flavel (Eds.), Proceedings of
the 18th Annual Conference of Mathematics Education Research Group of Austral-
asia (pp. 223–228). Darwin, Australia: MERGA.
Durik, A., & Harackiewicz, J. (2007). Different strokes for different folks: How individual
interest moderates effects of situational factors on task interest. Journal of Educa-
tional Psychology, 99(3), 597–610.
English, L. D., & Halford, G. S. (1995). Mathematics education: Models and processes.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Filloy, B., & Rojano, T. (1989). Solving equations: The transition from arithmetic to al-
gebra. For the Learning of Mathematics: An International Journal of Mathematics
Education, 9(2), 19–25.
Freudenthal, H. (1983). Didactical phenomenology of mathematical structures. Dordrecht:
D. Reidel.
Gagatsis, A. (1998). Solving methods in problems of proportion by Greek students in sec-
ondary education, ages 13–16. Scientia Paedagogica Experimentalis, 35(1), 241–
262.
Greer, B. (1993). The mathematical modeling perspective on word problems, Journal of
Mathematical Behaviour, 12, 239–250.
Greer, B. (1997). Modelling reality in mathematics classrooms: The case of word prob-
lems. Learning and Instruction, 7(4), 293–307.
Grossen, G., & Carnine, D. (1990). Diagramming a logic strategy: Effects on difficult prob-
lem types and transfer. Learning Disability Quarterly, 13, 168–182.
Hall, R., Kibler, D., Wenger, E., & Truxaw, C. (1989a). Exploring the Episodic Structure of
Algebra Story Problem Solving. Cognition and Instruction, 6(3), 223–283.
Hall, R., Kibler, D., Wenger, E., & Truxaw, C. (1989b). The situativity of knowing, learn-
ing, and research. American Psychologist, 53, 5–26.
Hegarty, M., Mayer, R. E., & Monk, C. A. (1995). Comprehension of arithmetic word
problems: A comparison of successful and unsuccessful problem solvers. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 87(1), 18–32.
Hong, K. T., Mei, Y. S., & Lim, J. (2009). The Singapore model method for learning math-
ematics. Singapore: EPB Pan Pacific.
Hubbard, R. (2004). An investigation into the modelling of word problems. Paper presented
at the 27th annual conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of
Australasia.
340 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Nesher, P. (1996). School stereotype word problems and the open nature of applications
(pp. 335–343). Selected Lectures from the 8th International Congress on Mathemat-
ical Education, Sevilla, Spain.
Newman, M. A. (1977). An analysis of sixth-grade pupils’ errors on written mathematical
tasks. Victorian Institute for Educational Research Bulletin, 39, 31–43.
Noss, R., & Hoyles, C. (1996). Windows on mathematical meaning. Dordrecht, The Neth-
erlands: Kluwer.
Palm, T. (2008). Impact of authenticity on sense making in word problem solving. Educa-
tional Studies in Mathematics, 67, 37–58.
Proudfit, L. A. (1980). The examination of problem-solving processes by fifth-grade chil-
dren and its effect on problem-solving performance (Order No. 8105942). Available
from ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Global. (303024786). Retrieved from http://
proxy.lib.pacificu.edu:2048/login?url=http://search.proquest.com/docview/303024
786?accountid=13047.
Radford, L. (2000). Students’ processes of symbolizing in algebra: A semiotic analysis of
the production of signs in generalizing tasks. Paper presented at the the 24th Con-
ference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education,
Hiroshima, Japan.
Radford, L. (2004). Syntax and meaning. Paper presented at the 28th International Con-
ference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education,
Bergen, Norway.
Rau, M. A. (2017). Conditions for the effectiveness of multiple visual representations in
enhancing STEM learning. Educational Psychology Review, 29(4), 717–761.
Reed, S. K. (1999). Word problems: Research and curriculum reform. Mahwah, NJ: Law-
rence Erlbaum.
Renninger, K., Ewen, L., & Lasher, A. (2002). Individual interest as context in expository
text and mathematical word problems. Learning and Instruction, 12(4), 467–490.
Reusser, K., & Stebler, R. (1997). Every word problem has a solution: The social rational-
ity of mathematical modeling in schools. Learning and Instruction, 7(4), 309–327.
Rojano, T. (1996). Problem solving in a spreadsheet environment. In N. Bednarz, C. Ki-
eran, & L. Lee (Eds.), Approaches to algebra: Perspectives for research and teach-
ing (pp. 137–145). Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Rosnick, P. (1982). Students’ symbolization process in algebra. University of Massachu-
setts, Amherst: Cognitive Development Project, Department of Physics and Astron-
omy.
Roth, W. (1996). Where is the context in contextual word problems: Mathematical prac-
tices and products in grade 8 students’ answers to story problems. Cognition and
Instruction, 14(4), 487–527.
Schoenfeld, A. (1985). Mathematical problem solving. Orlando: Academic Press.
Schwartz, D. L., & Black, J. B. (1996). Shuttling between depictive models and abstract
rules: Induction and fallback. Cognitive Science, 20(4), 457–497.
Shigematsu, K., & Sowder, L. (1994). Drawings for story problems: Practices in Japan and
the United States. Arithmetic Teacher, 41(9), 544–547.
Simon, M. (1986). Components of effective use of diagrams in math problem solving.
Proceedings of the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psy-
chology of Mathematics Education (pp. 1–7). East Lansing, MI: PME.
Singapore Model Method for Learning Mathematics. Singapore: EPB Panpac Education.
Modeling and Word Problems • 343
Singley, M. K., Anderson, J .R., Gevins, J .S., & Hoffman, D. (1989). The algebra word
problem tutor. In D. Bierman, J. Breuker, & J. Sandberg (Eds.), Artificial intelli-
gence and education. Proceedings of the 4th International Conference on AI and
Education (pp. 267–275), Amsterdam, Netherlands.
Sowder, L. (1988). Children’s solutions of story problems. The Journal of Mathematical
Behavior, 7, 227–238.
Stacey, K., & MacGregor , M. E. (1997). Ideas about symbolism that students bring to
algebra. The Mathematics Teacher, 90(2), 110–113.
Stacey, K., & MacGregor, M. (2000). Learning the algebraic method of solving problems.
Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 18(2), 149–167.
Stacey, K. M., & MacGregor, M. (1993). Origins of students’ errors in writing equations.
In A. B. T. Cooper (Ed.), New directions in algebra education, (pp. 205–212). Bris-
bane, Australia: Queensland University of Technology.
Stigler, J. W., Lee, S-Y., & Stevenson, H. W. (1990). Mathematical knowledge of Japanese,
Chinese, and American elementary school children. Reston, VA: National Council
of Teachers of Mathematics.
Suh, J., & Moyer, P. S. (2007). Developing students’ representational fluency using virtual
and physical algebra balances. The Journal of Computers in Mathematics and Sci-
ence Teaching, 26(2), 155–173.
Swafford, J. O., & Langrall, C.W. (2000). Grade 6 students’ preinstructional use of equa-
tions to describe and represent problem situations. Journal for Research in Math-
ematics Education, 31(1), 89–112.
Swee Fong, N., & Lee, K. (2009). The model method: Singapore children’s tool for repre-
senting and solving algebraic word problems. Journal for Research in Mathematics
Education, 40(3), 282–313.
Tabachneck, H. J. M., Koedinger, K. R., & Nathan, M. J. (1995). An analysis of the task de-
mands of algebra and the cognitive processes needed to meet them. Paper presented
at the Annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Pittsburgh, PA.
Usiskin, Z. (1988). Conceptions of school algebra and uses of variables. In A. Coxford &
A. Shulte (Eds.), The ideas of algebra, K-12. 1988 yearbook (pp. 8–19). Reston, VA:
The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Usiskin, Z. (1997). Reforming the third R: Changing the school mathematics curriculum:
An introduction. American Journal of Education, 106(1), 1–4.
Van Dooren, W., De Bock, D., & Depaepe, F. (2003). The illusion of linearity: Expanding
the evidence towards probabilistic reasoning. Educational Studies in Mathematics,
53(2), 113–138.
Van Dooren, W., Verschaffel, L., & Onghena, P. (2003). Pre-service teachers’ preferred
strategies for solving arithmetic and algebra word problems. Journal of Mathemat-
ics Teacher Education, 6, 27–52.
van Essen, G., & Hamaker, C. (1990). Using self-generated drawings to solve arithmetic
word problems. The Journal of Educational Research, 83(6), 301–12.
Verschaffel, L., De Corte, E., & Lasure, S. (1994). Realistic considerations in mathemati-
cal modelling of school arithmetic word problems. Learning and Instruction, 4,
273–294.
Vicente, S., Orrantia, J., & Verschaffel, L. (2007). Influence of situational and conceptual
rewording on word problem solving. British Journal of Educational Psychology,
77, 829–848.
344 • HOW STUDENTS THINK WHEN DOING ALGEBRA
Walkington, C., Sherman, M., & Petrosino, A. (2012). Playing the game of story problems:
Coordinating situation-based reasoning with algebraic representation. Journal of
Mathematical Behavior, 31(2), 174–195.
Wyndhamn, J. & Saljo, R. (1997). Word problems and mathematical reasoning: A study
of children’s mastery of reference and meaning in textual realities. Learning and
Instruction, 7(4), 361–382
Ya-amphan, D., & Bishop, A. (2004). Explaining Thai and Japanese student errors in
solving equation problems: The role of the textbook. Paper presented at the 27th
annual conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia,
Townsville, Australia.
Zbiek, R. M., Heid, M. K., Blume, G., & Dick, T. P. (2007). Research on technology in
mathematics education. In F. Lester (Ed.), Second handbook of research on math-
ematics teaching and learning (pp. 1169–1207). Charlotte, NC: Information Age
Publishing.