Escapism in Relation To Leisure Activity - Priyambada
Escapism in Relation To Leisure Activity - Priyambada
Priyambada Bhagawati
Department of Psychology
Delhi University
CERTIFICATE
This is to certify that the project entitled “Escapism in relation to Leisure Activity
Engagement” submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirement of the degree of
Bachelor of Arts in Psychology (honors) from Daulat Ram College, University of
Delhi, Delhi, is a faithful record of bonafide research work carried out by
Priyambada Bhagawati under my guidance and supervision. No part of this project
has been submitted for any other degree. All the assistance received during the
course of investigation has been acknowledged by her.
Department of Psychology,
University of Delhi,
Delhi
Acknowledgement
Every work, big or small, is invariably a product of collective effort. This project, in this regard,
is no different. First, I would like to acknowledge my research partners, Neti Ranjan and Nishtha
Jain without whom it would have been impossible to carry out this research. This research is an
output of collaboration of their great minds and collective hardwork.
I would like to acknowledge my project guide Dr. Preeti Kapur. She guided me at every stage
very patiently and actually turned out to be the wind beneath my wings by her constant
encouragement. At times when I was myself not very sure about the direction the project was
taking, she egged me on and also saw to it that my faith in myself returned. I am deeply indebted
to her for my growth as a researcher as well as a person during the last two months of working
on this project. She is my inspiration.
In their own way, my parents and siblings also have had much to contribute. My heart thanks to
all of these special people in my life, for just being there.
I wish to express special thanks to the Department of Psychology (Daulat Ram College) for
providing its best facilities and resources for the purpose of carrying out this project.
Priyambada Bhagawati
TABLE OF CONTENTS
List of Tables
List of Figures
Abstract
1. Introduction 8-25
2. Methodology 26-28
3. Analysis 29-36
Conclusion 40
References
Appendices
List of figures
A common understanding of escapism might be that it represents different types of reliefs from
the routines and obligations of everyday life (Cohen & Taylor, 1992). Escapism has even been
proposed to be a survival mechanism typical of humans (Tuan, 1998). However, the term raises
several questions: From what do we escape? To where do we escape? How do we escape? In the
psychological literature, the escapism term has been used to describe coping with emotional
distress by repressing acknowledgement of the stressors causing the stressful situation (Folkman
& Lazarus, 1980, 1985; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). In a similar manner, Baumeister (1990,
1991) describes suicide attempts, eating disorders, and drug abuse as escapes from the self.
Individuals may find relief in these harmful activities because they withdraw focus from the self
and direct it toward the execution of concrete actions. Baumeister suggests that performance of
these actions induces cognitive narrowing, a state characterized by lowered self-awareness and
decreased self-evaluation. At the same time, research conducted in sports and leisure contexts
indicates that individuals engage in health-promoting activities in order to momentarily reduce
self-awareness and forget about their daily hassles (e.g., Hutchinson, Baldwin, &Oh, 2006;
Iwasaki, 2003). For example, the phenomenon of flow (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990), described as a
highly satisfying psychological state obtained through activity engagement, is characterized by
an enhanced focus on one’s actions and reduced self-evaluation. In other words,
Csikszentmihalyi’s flow state, regarded as nourishment for one’s well-being, resembles
Baumeister’s description of cognitive narrowing in behaviors associated with psychological
malfunctioning. This illustrates a paradox in the sense that an escape from the self is found in
both self-destructive acts and activities regarded as conducive to optimal psychological
functioning (Ryan & Deci, 2000).
Although the characteristics, the number, and the importance of domains must be regarded as a
personal issue, most people have one thing in common: these domains are constantly tapping
from the same sources of energy, time, and money available in our lives. We only possess a
limited number of hours pr. day, and we have to attend to our daily and weekly obligations to
function in a modern society. This matter of limited resources entails that we constantly have to
regulate the resources invested into the different life domains. In this context, the leisure domain
constitutes a potential source of conflict and distress when having a craving to unconditionally
allocate large portions of resources into a leisure activity. On the other hand, a life domain
perspective on leisure may also highlight the uniqueness of the leisure domain in opposition to
other life domains. Humans are determined to dedicate their resources to activities that have
some sort of contingencies attached to them (Deci & Ryan, 2000). People’s appreciation of
leisure activities and their strong motivation to engage in them indicate that these activities offer
inputs that are less available in other life domains.
Self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 2000) suggests that optimal functioning depends on
the satisfaction of basic psychological needs, namely autonomy, competence, and relatedness.
Applied to a leisure activity context, the satisfaction of individuals’ need for autonomy
(deCharms, 1968) involves the experience of engaging in an activity for intrinsic reasons, and
that engagement in the activity is in accordance with one’s personal goals and values. The
satisfaction of the need for competence (White, 1969) involves a feeling of being capable of
mastering the challenges the activity brings, and about perceiving some sort of progression
through one’s engagement in the activity. The need for relatedness (Baumeister & Leary, 1995)
pertains to the experience of being included in a social context, and applied to people’s favorite
activities this implies that one feels close to individuals that one cares for and is appreciated by
when engaged in the activity. Several studies have shown that autonomy, competence, and
relatedness satisfaction are closely attached to well-being (see Deci & Ryan, 2000) In accordance
with the leisure-domain approach, one of the reasons that leisure activities are so highly
appreciated may be that they contribute uniquely to the satisfaction of these three basic
psychological needs.
In the study by Vallerand et al. (2003), where people were asked to name their favorite activity,
the average time spent pr. week in the activity was 8.4 hours. Later studies on people’s favorite
activities have reported mean values of around ten hours (Vallerand, Rousseau, Grouzet,
Dumais, Grenier, & Blanchard, 2006; Vallerand, et al., 2007). That such an amount of time is
devoted to people’s favorite activity indicates that the activity has a special position in their
everyday lives, and that the activity is not one of several activities of a similar character.
The Dualistic Model of Passion (Vallerand et al., 2003; 2006; 2007) has recently gained
additional power through a number of studies investigating its applicability in different
populations such as professional dancers (Rip, Fortin, & Vallerand, 2006), dramatic art students
(Vallerand et al., 2007), and different categories of athletes including basketball players, water
polo players, synchronized swimmers (Vallerand et al., 2006) and hockey players (Amiot et al.,
2006). Most of the findings are compatible with the model’s theoretical assumptions in terms of
the proposed dualism of passion towards activities. The theoretical background of the model
derives from the principles regarding internalization of behavior as outlined by self-
determination theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 2000). In SDT, internalization refers to the integration
of values, beliefs, and attitudes from the environment into the self, and thereby constitutes an
important part of the process of socialization. Thus, when certain activities are internalized into
the self, they may become central parts of one’s identity. When the activity has been transformed
from being an action to being stated as a personal feature, then the internalization process has run
its course. As shown in previous studies (Vallerand, 1997; see also Deci & Ryan, 2000), values
and regulations concerning activities can be internalized into one’s identity in either a controlled
or an autonomous fashion.
The Dualistic Model of Passion proposes that obsessive passion stems from a controlled
internalization of the passion activity, while harmonious passion stems from an autonomous
internalization of the activity into one’s identity. A controlled internalization is posited to derive
from intrapersonal or social pressure, like performing to be rewarded, to enhance self esteem, or
because the excitement experienced through the activity takes control over the individual and
leads to a more excessive involvement in the activity. Vallerand and his coworkers (2006) found
support for the existence of these two distinctive ways of internalization in relation to passion
activities. In a study on recreational athletes they found that harmonious passion was related to
an autonomous personality orientation, whereas obsessive passion was related to a controlled
personality orientation. Since personality orientations are likely to be stable over time, these
findings support the idea that the way the activity has been internalized is a highly relevant
predictor for the development of harmonious or obsessive passion. Furthermore, individual
differences regarding internalization may explain several of the consequences that are observed
in relation to the two types of passion. A controlled internalization helps explain a compulsive
pattern of activity engagement and its negative consequences, including lack of positive affect
(Vallerand et al., 2003) and addiction-like psychological effects when prevented from
engagement in the activity (Mageau & Vallerand, 2007; Vallerand et al., 2003, study 1). When
internalizing an activity for causes beyond one’s own, one seems to loose the intrinsically driven
initiative that supports healthy engagement in the activity. In contrast, when internalizing the
activity freely without any contingencies attached to it, this seems to assure the positive spin-offs
observed in relation to harmonious passion, including positive affect (Vallerand et al. 2003) and
subjective well-being (Vallerand et al., 2007).
In other words, if one belongs to an environment that fully accepts an obsessive approach to
activity engagement, then no detrimental effects on well-being might be present. Likewise,
being a harmonious individual in an obsessive environment, this conflict might attenuate or
evaporate the positive effects on well-being expected from this approach to the activity.
The Two-Dimensional Model of Escapism makes use of Regulatory Focus Theory (RFT;
Higgins, 1998) to explain why action attention can be related to both ill-being and well-being.
Based on the hedonic principle that people approach pleasure and avoid pain, RFT states that
behaviors performed to obtain well-being origins from two very different motivational
directions, namely promotion focus or prevention focus. A promotion focus directs individuals
towards the obtainment of their superior standards and ideals. Promotion focus motivates the
person towards nurturance of the self and to allocate attention towards the positive outcomes of a
desired end-state. In contrast, motivation incited from a prevention focus leads the individual
towards obtainment of concordance between one’s actions and perceived duties and obligations.
Engagement based on prevention focus draws one’s attention towards the inhibition of negative
outcomes related to a potential failure of reaching a desired end-state. A prevention focus
complies with motivation to protect the self from threats towards its unity and stability. Inspired
by the hedonic principle incorporated in Regulatory Focus Theory, we suggest that individuals
differ in their self-regulatory viewpoint with regards to intentions to engage in escape-facilitating
activities. Specifically, while one person can approach action attention to promote the
achievement of prospective positive experiences in activity engagement, another person might
engage in order to prevent thinking on precedent negative experiences or prospective challenges.
The goal of obtaining the state of action attention through activity engagement will be similar for
both persons and both will be concerned with the temporary enhancement of well being.
However, their initiative for entering the pleasant state of escape is instigated from very different
standpoints. The promotion-guided person engages in order to attain positive affect, whereas the
prevention-guided person will engage in order to experience absence of negative affect. A
promotion focus in escapist engagement will be accompanied by a favourable anticipation of the
positive experiences attainable through getting immersed in the activity. The activity is viewed
upon as a self-developing opportunity which offers complementary experiences compared to
other important activities in life. Individuals with a promotion focus are inclined to evaluate their
engagement in their favorite activity in a positive manner, both regarding the situational
outcomes in terms of i.e. affect and need satisfaction, but also in a more holistic context by
means of a positive apprehension of it’s impact on life satisfaction. Escapist engagement out of
promotion intentions will correspond to a volitional organismic integration process known to be
facilitative for optimal functioning (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Accordingly, this type of activity
engagement is defined as self-expansion. In contrast, approaching action attention with a
prevention focus will be accompanied by a deconstructive apprehension of one’s attributes,
which in turns restricts the person’s possibilities with regards to obtaining self-developing
experiences through activity engagement. The activity is viewed upon as an instrument to avoid
the apprehension of negative aspects of self and to temporary direct one’s attention away from
disturbing memories or future obstacles. In line with principles of emotion dynamics (Gross &
Levenson, 1993), an allocation of the cognitive resources in direction of a moderation of
negative affect will also reduce the likelihood of enhancement of positive affect. Engaging out of
prevention intentions will temporary block self-threatening objects from consciousness, but it
will not facilitate the self-enhancing and satisfying experiences in activity engagement known to
be conducive to well-being. Therefore, escapist engagement out of prevention motives is defined
as self-suppression.
In sum, the Passion model and the Escapism may complement each other towards a better
understanding of peoples’ engagement in leisure activities.
Self-esteem is a disposition that a person has which represents their judgments of their own
worthiness. In the mid-1960s, Morris Rosenberg and social-learning theorists defined self-
esteem as a personal worth or worthiness. Nathaniel Branden in 1969 defined self-esteem as "the
experience of being competent to cope with the basic challenges of life and being worthy of
happiness." According to Branden, self-esteem is the sum of self-confidence (a feeling of
personal capacity) and self-respect (a feeling of personal worth). It exists as a consequence of the
implicit judgment that every person has of their ability to face life's challenges, to understand
and solve problems, and their right to achieve happiness, and be given respect
REVIEW OF LITERATURE
Psychological escapism: predicting the amount of television viewing by need for cognition
By Henning, Vorderer (2006)
Abstract:
Germans spend an average of more than 3 hours watching television each day. Among them,
there are many who turn on their TV sets for less than 1.5 hours a day, and others who do this for
more than 8 hours a day. What accounts for these differences? The central thesis in this paper is
that individuals may be distinguished by their attitude toward thinking and that differences in
their need for cognition explain the differences in their time spent with TV. The lower viewers'
need for cognition is, the less pleasant they feel when they have nothing to do because there is
nothing left to do but think. The easiest way for individuals to escape this pressure to think is by
watching TV. Thus, individuals will watch more TV when they have a lower need for cognition.
Results of a survey study show that the concept of escapism proves to be useful in explaining TV
use when it takes over a psychological perspective as well as a sociological one.
Abstract:
In three papers, the present thesis examined the applicability of psychological theories on
motivation in a leisure activity engagement context. It is argued that research on leisure
motivation could benefit from a less contextual and a more idiosyncratic perspective on leisure
activities. The concept of activity affordances is presented in order to highlight the person-
activity fit that takes place when a certain activity is approached. This concept suggests that
activities with different attributes may offer the same psychological experiences as well as
proposing that individuals may approach identical activities with different mindsets depending
on their well-being, need satisfaction motives, personality characteristics, and so on. These
motivational dynamics that evolve between the person and the activity is the primary objective of
the present thesis. In these three papers, the Dualistic Model of Passion is investigated in a
leisure context. This model is dualistic in the sense that it proposes two distinctively different
motivational constructs in relation to passionate engagement in activities. Specifically, the
Passion model includes harmonious passion, a motivation which origins from an autonomous
interest in an activity that leads to positive emotions and general well-being, and obsessive
passion, which stems from a controlled internalization of the activity that undermines flexible
engagement in the activity and constitutes a threat to well-being. In addition, the present thesis
includes the development of another dualistic model in relation to leisure activity engagement
that pertains to motives to escape from the self through investing interest in an activity. Two
dimensions of escapism were proposed: self-suppression and self-expansion. Self-suppression
escapism derives from motivation to avoid negative evaluation of self by getting focused on an
activity, whereas self-expansion is motivated from facilitations of positive experiences by getting
immersed in an activity. The results supported the theoretical proposals, showing that these two
dimensions have different determinants and outcomes. Self-suppression was related to ill-being
and poor psychological adjustment, whereas self-expansion was related to flexible activity
engagement and positive affective outcomes. The findings from the studies in the present thesis
give conditional support for a motivational dualism in leisure activity engagement. Harmonious
passion and self-expansion were related to positive affective outcomes and subjective well-being,
whereas obsessive passion and self-suppression were related to general and activity-related
negative effect, poor psychological adjustment, and intrapersonal conflicts regarding the
resources invested in the activity. The Passion model and the Escapism model complemented
each other in terms of affective outcomes from activity engagement. In sum, these findings show
that leisure activity engagement can be conducive to well-being, but also, and in contrast to the
traditional view upon leisure activity engagement, that leisure activities also may pose a threat to
life satisfaction.
The dark side of leisure: obsessive passion and its covariates and outcomes
Stenseng, Rise & Kraft (2009)
Abstract:
The present paper challenges the notion that engagement in leisure activities is exclusively
conducive to well‐being. By applying the Dualistic Model of Passion in a leisure context on 467
participants, the measurement of obsessive passion for a leisure activity was related to indicators
of ill‐being and unrelated to general well‐being. In Study 1, obsessive passion for a leisure
activity was associated with activity addiction, escapism engagement, and intrapersonal
conflicts. In Study 2, obsessive passion was negatively related to basic need satisfaction in
activity engagement as well as positively related to negative affective outcomes from activity
engagement. We argue that the present findings may lead towards a broader understanding of
motivation and outcomes in leisure activities
PRESENT STUDY
Escapism has been proposed to be a survival mechanism typical of humans (Tuan, 1998). To
cope with the complexities of life and break the shackles of responsibilities and duties we often
indulge in so called ‘healthy leisure activities’. Leisure time is for most people more or less filled
with different types of structured activities. These activities can range from cultural activities,
such as going to the movies, reading books, and painting, to sports activities like jogging,
playing football, and parachuting. But are these leisure activities really healthy? Or does it have
an impact over our lives? A leisure activity becomes passion for people if they like it, it is
important to them, and in which they spend a significant amount of time on a regular basis.
Individuals approach their favorite activities with different escapism motives according to their
regulatory focuses and their life circumstances. According to the dualistic model of escapism,
escapism is of two types: self expansion and self suppression. Even though two individuals may
engage in the same activity for the same amount of time, they might not get equal amount of
satisfaction from the activity. It is because the motive behind the indulgence can be quite
different depending on various factors. Thus it becomes important to know not only what activity
we indulge in but also why we indulge in it. Also, our indulgence in various leisure activities
seems to have an impact on other life domains.
Two of the most common leisure activities that the youth indulges in are television viewing and
reading books. For some, TV viewing and reading are not just leisure activities, but it has come
to play a critical and vital part of their life. With time such activities have become very important
to them and they often spend a significant amount of time for it. Thus, this research has been
conceptualized to study the type of escapism that results in the indulgence of either of the two
activities i.e. TV viewing and reading books among youth and it’s relationship with depression,
interpersonal problems and self esteem.
OBJECTIVES
The objective of the present research is to study escapism in relation to leisure activity
engagement namely tv viewing and book reading in relationship with depression, interpersonal
problems and self esteem.
HYPOTHESES
On the basis of the concepts and review of literature discussed above, the following hypotheses
were arrived at:
1. There will be a significant relationship between depression levels and type of escapism
a. The individuals with self suppression type of escapism will have higher depression
levels.
b. The individuals with self expansion type of escapism will have lower depression
levels.
2. There will be a significant relationship between interpersonal problems and type of
escapism.
a. The individuals with self suppression type of escapism will have higher interpersonal
problems.
b. The individuals with self expansion type of escapism will have lower interpersonal
problems.
3. There will be a significant relationship between self esteem levels and type of escapism.
a. The individuals with self suppression type of escapism will have higher self esteem
levels.
b. The individuals with self expansion type of escapism will have lower self esteem
levels.
METHODOLOGY
The present study focuses on the dualistic concept of escapism in relation to leisure activities on
the dynamics of depression, interpersonal conflicts and self esteem.
There is a healthy and an unhealthy approach to engagement in favorite activities, and that they
evolve dependent on conjunctive effects of personality characteristics and external factors. To
explore this journey that is undertaken, it was decided to study university students who engage in
either tv viewing or reading books or both.
Mixed method approach was used to collect data and towards this end, both qualitative and
quantitative methods were employed because of the resultant methodological pluralism which
results in richer data. The quantitative measure used was College Adjustment Scale (Anton and
Reed, 1991). The qualitative measure used was structured interview schedule. It was employed
to gain an insight as to whether the indulgence in the leisure activities i.e television viewing and
reading novels is driven by self expansion or self suppression type of escapism.
Delhi university students were decided upon as the sample. The age of the participants ranged
from 18 to 23 years. College students were taken as sample as with the sudden increase in
responsibilities and with the increasing competition and expectations, students indulge in various
activities as a way of coping mechanism. The two of the most common leisure activities that
today’s youth indulge in are tv viewing and reading novels. Thus, only those students who
indulge in either of these two activities were selected in the sample.
For the purpose of this study, 40 Delhi University students were taken as a representative
sample. Of the 40 students, 20 were male students and 20 were female students. The subjects
were selected following the purposive sampling procedure. Kerlinger and Lee (2000) explained
purposive sampling as another type of non-probability sampling, which is characterized by the
use of judgment and a deliberate effort to obtain representative samples by including typical
areas or groups in the sample.
Tool Used
The following tools were used for the purpose of data collection on 120 Delhi University
students:
Procedure
After the sample and the measures had been selected, a pilot study was carried out on a sample
of 4 Delhi University students. On the basis of their responses on interview, certain changes were
made before the final data collection.
After a brief introduction of self, a casual conversation was made with the subject in order to
make him/her feel relaxed and a brief explanation of the purpose of the study was given. Then,
two interviewers took the interview of the participant. One interviewer was involved in the
interaction process while the other was silently recording the responses. Probe questions were
asked during the interview. After taking the interview, the participant was asked to fill the CAS
answer sheet. Instructions were given as per the manual. The doubts or queries put forth by the
participants were resolved. The participants were requested to give free and frank responses,
preferably the responses that come first to mind. It was made clear to the participants that there
were no right or wrong answers. They were also assured that their responses would be kept
confidential and used strictly for the purpose of the research. To maintain the confidentiality of
the participants, unique codes were assigned to them. The 4 letter code was formed by taking the
first two alphabets of the participant’s name followed by the short form of his/her college.
It took approximately 10-15 minutes for each participant to complete the CAS answer sheet and
7-10 approximately to take the interview of one participant. The participants were thanked
thereafter for their time and cooperation.
After the data was collected, the responses were analyzed and the results obtained have been
reported in the next section.
DATA ANALYSIS
Quantitative Analysis
The CAS answer sheets of 40 participants were scored as per the instructions given in manual.
The raw scores thus obtained were converted into percentiles using norms tables ( Table1, Table
2, Table 3, Table 4;Appendix C). Out of the 9 scales assessed, only 3 scales namely depression,
interpersonal problems and self esteem were taken into consideration. The percentiles for each of
the scales were divided into low (<P25), moderately low (P25 – P50), moderately high (P50 – P75)
and high (>P75). The mean of the raw scores for each of the scale was also calculated.
Qualitative Analysis
For analyzing the responses obtained on the interview schedules, the categories of behavior
consistent with the type of escapism were organized in a tabular format (Table 1, Appendix B).
Based on the responses obtained on the interview schedules of 40 participants an analysis table
was constructed. The dualistic concept of escapism is better viewed on a scale ranging from
high to low within the two dimensions. While analyzing the responses it was seen that some of
the participants had extremely high levels of self expansion or self suppression type of escapism
while some had only borderline levels of self expansion or self suppression type of escapism.
Thus, each of the participants was categorized into: high self expansion, low self expansion, high
self suppression or low self suppression.
The results showed that out of 40 participants, only 10 participants had a self expansion type of
engagement in the leisure activities under study (tv viewing and reading books). Out of theses 10
participants, 4 participants had high self expansion type of engagement in the leisure activities
while 6 had low self expansion type of engagement. The remaining 30 participants had a self
suppression type of engagement in the above mentioned leisure activities. Out of these 30
participants, 10 participants had low self suppression while 20 participants had high self
suppression type of engagement in the leisure activities.
Type of Escapism
Fig. 3.1 Pie Diagram indicating the type of escapism of the sample.
An interesting fact to be noted here is, out of the 20 females participants, only 3 participants had
low self expansion type of escapism while 11 participants had high self suppression and the
remaining 6 participants had low self suppression type of escapism. Of the 20 male participants,
only 4 participants had high self expansion type of escapism, 3 had low self expansion type of
escapism while 9 participants had high self suppression and 4 participants had low self
suppression type of escapism.
Females
Fig.3.3 Pie Diagram indicating the type of escapism of the male participants
After the quantitative and qualitative analysis, the levels of depression, interpersonal problems
and self esteem were compared with the type of escapism of the participant. As we can see from
Table 6 (Appendix D), out of the 40 participants,10 participants have high depression levels
(>P75) of which 6 participants fall in the category of high self suppression type of escapism, 3
participants in low self suppression and only 1 participant falls in low self expansion escapism.
11 participants have moderately high depression levels of which 8 participants fall in the
category of high self suppression type of escapism, 2 participants in low self suppression
escapism while only 1 participant falls in the category of high self expansion type of escapism.
10 participants have moderately low depression levels of which 5 participants fall in the category
of high self suppression type of escapism, 2 participants in low self suppression, 2 participants in
low self expansion and 1 participant in high self expansion. 9 participants have low depression
levels of which 1 participants fall in the category of high suppression escapism, 3 participants in
low self suppression escapism, 2 participants in high self expansion escapism while 3 participant
in low self expansion escapism.
High Depression
high self
suppression
low self
suppression
high self
expansion
low self
expansion
Fig 3.4 Pie diagram: depicting the relation between the type of escapism with high depression
high self
suppression
low self
suppression
high self
expansion
low self
expansion
Fig 3.5. Pie diagram: depicting the relation between type of escapism and moderately high depression
high self
suppression
low self
suppression
high self
expansion
low self
expansion
Fig.3.6. Pie Diagram: depicting the relation between type of escapism and moderately low depression
Low Depression
high self
suppression
low self
suppression
hish self
expansion
low self
expansion
Fig.3.7. Pie Diagram: depicting the relation between type of escapism and low depression
As we can see from Table 7 (Appendix D), out of the 40 participants, 9 participants have high
interpersonal problems of which 5 participants fall in the category of high self suppression
escapism and 4 participants in low self suppression escapism. 10 participants have moderately
high interpersonal problems of which 6 participants fall in the category of high self suppression
type of escapism, 1 participants in low self suppression escapism, only 1 participant in low self
expansion escapism while 2 participant falls in the category of high self expansion type of
escapism. 14 participants have moderately low interpersonal problems of which 8 participants
fall in the category of high self suppression type of escapism, 5 participants in low self
suppression and 1 participant in high self expansion escapism. 7 participants have low
interpersonal problems of which 1 participants fall in the category of high suppression escapism,
1 participants in high self expansion escapism while 5 participant in low self expansion
escapism.
high self
suppression
low self
suppression
high self expansion
Fig 3.8 Pie Diagram: depicting the relation between type of escapism and high interpersonal problems
Moderately High Interpersonal
Problems
high self
suppression
low self
suppression
high self
expansion
Fig 3.9 Pie Diagram: depicting relation between type of escapism and moderately high interpersonal
problems
high self
suppression
low self
suppression
high self
expansion
low self
expansion
Fig. 3.10 Pie Diagram: depicting relation between type of escapism and moderately low interpersonal
problems.
Fig 3.11 Pie Diagram: depicting relation between type of escapism and low interpersonal problems
As we can see from Table 8 (Appendix D), out of the 40 participants,10 participants have low
self esteem of which 6 participants fall in the category of high self suppression type of escapism,
4 participants in low self suppression. 11 participants have moderately low self esteem levels of
which 7 participants fall in the category of high self suppression type of escapism, 2 participants
in low self suppression escapism and 2 participant falls in the category of low self expansion
type of escapism. 11 participants have moderately high self esteem levels of which 4 participants
fall in the category of high self suppression type of escapism, 2 participants in low self
suppression, 4 participants in low self expansion and 1 participant in high self expansion. 8
participants have high self esteem levels of which 2 participants fall in the category of high
suppression escapism, 2 participants in low self suppression escapism, 3 participants in high self
expansion escapism while 1 participant in low self expansion escapism.
Fig. 3.12 Pie Diagram: depicting relation between type of escapism and low self esteem
Fig.3.13 Pie Diagram: depicting relation between type of escapism and moderately low self esteem
Moderately High Self Esteem
high self
suppression
low self
suppression
high self
expansion
low self
expansion
Fig. 3.14 Pie Diagram: depicting relation between type of escapism and moderately high self esteem
Fig.3.15 Pie Diagram: depicting relation between type of escapism and high self esteem
INTERPRETATION AND DISCUSSION
The objective of the present research was to study escapism in relation to leisure activity
engagement namely tv viewing and book reading in relationship with depression, interpersonal
problems and self esteem. In other words, the research was conducted to study the type of
escapism (self expansion or self suppression) of the sample taken. Also, to study the relationship
of self expansion with depression, interpersonal problems and self esteem and the relationship of
self suppression with depression, interpersonal problems and self esteem. For this the sample of
40 students (males=20, females=20) was taken from Delhi university using purposive sampling
technique. The data was collected using self constructed interview schedule and College
Adjustment Scale (Anton &Reed, 1991) was used to measure the three variables.
Traditionally, engagement in leisure activities has been regarded exclusively as favorable for
well-being. In opposition to this view, the present research suggests that leisure also has a dark
side previously unexplored in the leisure literature. By applying the Dualistic Model of Passion
and the Two-Dimensional Model of Escapism on engagement in leisure activities, it becomes
evident that leisure activity engagement can take forms related to maladaptive psychological
functioning. Both models propose that there is a healthy and an unhealthy approach to
engagement in favorite activities, and that they evolve dependent on conjunctive effects of
personality characteristics and external factors. The Dualistic Model of Passion suggests that the
development of either harmonious or obsessive passion is dependent on the onset of the activity.
Harmonious passion evolves from an autonomous internalization of the activity, whereas
obsessive passion evolves from a controlled internalization of the activity. The Escapism model
is built upon the promotion vs. prevention distinction in Regulatory Focus Theory (Higgins,
1998). In relation to task engagement, one of these regulatory focuses will dominate the person’s
motivation. Approaching the task with a promotion focus relates to an emphasis on the positive
outcomes from an accomplishment of the task, whereas a prevention focus relates to an emphasis
on the negative outcomes in case of failing the task.
The Passion model and the Escapism model both illustrate the dualism of leisure activity
engagement. Further, the escapism model is better understood on a scale ranging from high to
low within the two dimensions. In other words, self expansion as well as self suppression has
been further divided as high and low. On the basis of this concept the interview schedules were
analyzed. The results showed that (Table 1; Appendix B) out of 40 participants, only 10
participants had a self expansion type of engagement in the leisure activities under study (tv
viewing and reading books). Out of theses 10 participants, 4 participants had high self expansion
type of engagement in the leisure activities while 6 had low self expansion type of engagement.
The remaining 30 participants had a self suppression type of engagement in the above mentioned
leisure activities. Out of these 30 participants, 10 participants had low self suppression while 20
participants had high self suppression type of engagement in the leisure activities. This stark
difference could be because the sample consisted of only college students of age 18-23 years.
College is a high point in a person’s life. A lot of changes occur during the transmission from
school to college. The freedom comes with a burden of responsibilities. Not only academic
commitments but a person also has to face peer pressure. During the interview, a lot of
participants said that scoring low grades, breakup with boyfriend or girlfriend or family issues
caused emotional distress at some point of time in their life. Although at a glance, gaining
knowledge seems to be a major reason for engaging in the leisure activities namely tv viewing
and reading books but at a deeper level the drive behind engaging in the two activities is quite
shocking. Most of the students are indulging in the activities to eliminate the negative threats in
the immediate consciousness and to seek relief. The increasing competition and expectation of
parents and self are the major causes behind the change in the motive for engagement in the
leisure activities by students. They regard tv viewing and/or book reading as their “safe haven”
and approach these more intensely to suppress negative thoughts. However, this might lead to a
downward spiral because this type of activity engagement will not result in elevated energy or
vitality, but only drain resources that should be allocated in direction of sorting out the sources
for the problematic life situation. The activity is viewed upon as an instrument to avoid the
apprehension of negative aspects of self and to temporary direct one’s attention away from
disturbing memories or future obstacles.
An interesting fact to be noted here is, out of the 20 females participants, only 3 participants had
low self expansion type of escapism while 11 participants had high self suppression and the
remaining 6 participants had low self suppression type of escapism. Of the 20 male participants,
only 4 participants had high self expansion type of escapism, 3 had low self expansion type of
escapism while 9 participants had high self suppression and 4 participants had low self
suppression type of escapism. This is a new trend that has been seen as no previous research has
been done regarding the gender differences in type of escapism.
The College Adjustment Scale was used to measure the depression, interpersonal problems and
self esteem of the participants. The raw scores were converted to percentiles using the self
constructed norm tables. The percentiles for each of the scales were divided into low (<P25),
moderately low (P25 – P50), moderately high (P50 – P75) and high (>P75). As we can see from
Table 6 (Appendix D), most of the participants with self suppression type of escapism have high
or moderately high depression while most of the participants with self expansion have low or
moderately low depression. This is in line with the hypotheses, though we cannot say that the
relationship between the two variables is high but nonetheless there seems to be a relation
between the two variables. When the individual fail to live up to own and/or others standards
and expectations feelings of loneliness and helplessness inculcate. These events are attributed
internally, which thereby leads to decreased self-worth. This condition makes self-awareness
painful and therefore intentions to avoid self-evaluation evolve. Then, the person will search for
actions that reduce self evaluation to escape this painful state (Baumeister, 1990). Thus, the
participants with self suppression escapism seek comfort in their favorite leisure activities as
they have a hold on the activity and may feel competent in this domain thereby removing the
negative feelings of helplessness from immediate consciousness. In line with principles of
emotion dynamics (Gross & Levenson, 1993), an allocation of the cognitive resources in
direction of a moderation of negative affect will also reduce the likelihood of enhancement of
positive affect. Engaging out of prevention intentions will temporary block self-threatening
objects from consciousness, but it will not facilitate the self-enhancing and satisfying
experiences in activity engagement known to be conducive to well-being. From Table 7
(Appendix D), it is evident that only participants with self suppression have high interpersonal
problems and most of the participants with self expansion escapism have low interpersonal
problems. The term self-expansion has previously been used in relation to interpersonal
closeness. Aron, Aron, Tudor, & Nelson (1991) described the inclusion of other into one’s self as
self-expansion (see also Blanchard, Perreault, & Vallerand, 1998). The need for relatedness
(Baumeister & Leary,1995) pertains to the experience of being included in a social context, and
applied to people’s favorite activities this implies that one feels close to individuals that one
cares for and is appreciated by when engaged in the activity. Interpersonal problems are
recurrent difficulties in relating to others (Horowitz, Rosenberg, & Bartholomew, 1993). People
with self suppression escapism have low self worth and hence they immerse themselves in their
favorite leisure activities to avoid communicating with people, that is, to reduce self evaluation
by others and also by self. As a result, interpersonal problems develop. To avoid facing these
problems, the person will again seek comfort in their favorite leisure activities, forming a vicious
circle. From Table 8 (Appendix D), participants with self suppression escapism have mostly low
or moderately low self esteem while participants with self expansion escapism have mostly high
or moderately high self esteem. In the event where a person’s self esteem goes down, the favorite
activity will be regarded as a “safe haven” and be approached more intensely to suppress
negative thoughts. However, this might lead to a downward spiral because this type of activity
engagement will not result in elevated energy or vitality, but only drain resources that should be
allocated in direction of sorting out the sources for the problematic life situation.
The affective outcomes from self suppression in activity engagement were not consistent. the
dualism of passion and escapism is not fully “black or white”. Although both obsessive passion
and self-suppression are associated with subjective ill-being, these approaches towards a favorite
activity might also entail positive experiences, but notably, to a significant less degree than for
harmonious passion engagement and self expansion escapism.
CONCLUSION
In conclusion, the results show that self suppression in relation to activity engagement is
positively related to depression and interpersonal problems and negatively related to self esteem.
Vice versa, self expansion in relation to activity engagement is negatively related to depression
and interpersonal problems and positively related to self esteem. Thus the way we indulge in
leisure activities and more importantly why we indulge in leisure activities has an impact over
our other life domains.
LIMITATIONS OF THE STUDY
The sample size for the present study was very small; hence the results of the study cannot be
generalized. To draw more valid and reliable conclusions, the study needs to be replicated with a
large sample size. Also, the College Adjustment Scale (Anton & Reed, 1991) is constructed on
American population. Though, Indian norms were constructed for the purpose of this research,
but due to the small sample size, the norms are not completely reliable.
IMPLICATIONS AND SIGNIFICANCE OF THE STUDY
This research paper represents a promising path for future research on leisure activity
engagement, as well as a contextual approach to activity engagement which complements
laboratory research on human motivation and self-regulation. The present approach also
demonstrates that engagement in leisure activities exists within a broader system of life domains.
In line with a dualistic approach towards leisure motivation, engagement that origin from
adaptive motivational sources, such as harmonious passion and self-expansion, assures that the
engagement is felt as a contribution to other domains and is conducive to general well-being. In
contrast, engagement that origin from maladaptive motivational sources, such as obsessive
passion and self-suppression, inflicts outcomes from leisure activity engagement that are felt as
detrimental to general well-being.
References
The dark side of leisure: obsessive passion and its covariates and outcomes ,Stenseng, Rise &
Kraft (2009)
A dualistic approach to leisure activity engagement : on the dynamics of passion, escapism, and
life satisfaction, Stenseng, Frode(2009)
Psychological escapism: predicting the amount of television viewing by need for cognition By
Henning, Vorderer (2006)
Baron, R. B., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social
psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.
Baumeister, R. F. (1990). Suicide as escape from self. Psychological Review, 97, 90-113.
Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Escaping the self: Alcoholism, spirituality, masochism, and other
flights from the burden of selfhood. New York: Basic Books.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal
attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497- 529.
Cohen, S., Taylor, L. (1991). Escape attempts: The theory and practice of resistance to everyday
life (2nd Ed). New York: Routledge.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO PI-R. Professional Manual. Florida: Psychological
Assessment Resources, Inc.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York:
Harper Collins.
Cury, F., Elliott, A., Sarrazin, P., Fonseca, D. D., & Rufo, M. (2002). The trichotomous
achievement goal model and intrinsic motivation: a sequential mediational model.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and
the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 269-318.
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life
scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 41, 71-75.
Edginton, C. R., DeGraaf, D. G., Dieser, R. B., & Edginton, S. R. (2006). Leisure and life
satisfaction. Foundational perspectives (4th Ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Elliott, A. J., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1994). Goal setting, achievement orientation, and
intrinsic motivation: A mediational analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66,
968-980.
Elliott, A. J., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). Approach and avoidance achievement goals and
intrinsic motivation: A mediational analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70,
461-475.
Fejfar, M. C., & R. H. Hoyle (2000). Effect of private self-awareness on negative affect and
self-referent attribution: A quantitative review. Personality and Social Psychology
Review, 4, 132-142.
Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1985). If it changes it must be a process: A study of emotion
and coping during three stages of a college examination. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 48, 150-170.
Getty, H. A., Watson, J., & Frisch, G. R. (2000). A comparison of depression and styles of
coping in male and female GA members and controls. Journal of Gambling Studies,
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Haughton Mifflin.
Gross, J. J, & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: Physiology, self-report, and
expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 970-986.
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes:
Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 85, 348 – 362.
Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological
Review, 94, 319-340.
Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280-1300.
Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a motivational
principle. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 1 – 46.
Higgins, E. T. (2005). Value from hedonic experience and engagement. Psychological
Review, 113, 439-460.
Hu, L-T, & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indexes in Covariance Structure
Analysis: Conventional Criteria versus New Alternatives. Structural Equation
Modeling, 6, 1-55.
Hull, J. G., Young, R. D., & Jouriles, E. (1986). Applications of the self-awareness model of
alcohol consumption: Predicting patterns of use and abuse. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 51, 790-796.
APPENDIX
Table of contents
S. No. Title
APPENDIX C Raw scores of the participants and norm tables of all the three
variables under study
Table 3: Norm table for the depression scale normalized on a sample of 40 participants
SCORE FREQUENCY PERCENTILE
40 1 >99
39 0 97.5
38 0 97.5
37 0 97.5
36 0 97.5
35 0 97.5
34 0 97.5
33 0 97.5
32 2 97.5
31 0 92.5
30 1 92.5
29 0 90
28 0 90
27 2 90
26 1 85
25 2 82.5
24 0 77.5
23 1 77.5
22 1 75
21 3 72.5
20 1 65
19 4 62.5
18 2 52.5
17 3 47.5
16 3 40
15 4 32.5
14 3 22.5
13 5 15
12 1 <2.5
Table 4: Norm Table for inter-personal scale normalized on a sample of 40
participants
Table 7 (b): Moderately high interpersonal problem (P50 –P75, P50 =23, P75 =28)
Table 8 (b): Moderately low self esteem (P50 –P75, P50=24, P75 =30)