2020 Book TheModelsOfEngagedLearningAndT PDF
2020 Book TheModelsOfEngagedLearningAndT PDF
John Willison
The Models
of Engaged Learning
and Teaching
Connecting
Sophisticated Thinking
from Early Childhood
to PhD
SpringerBriefs in Education
We are delighted to announce SpringerBriefs in Education, an innovative product
type that combines elements of both journals and books. Briefs present concise
summaries of cutting-edge research and practical applications in education.
Featuring compact volumes of 50 to 125 pages, the SpringerBriefs in Education
allow authors to present their ideas and readers to absorb them with a minimal time
investment. Briefs are published as part of Springer’s eBook Collection. In
addition, Briefs are available for individual print and electronic purchase.
SpringerBriefs in Education cover a broad range of educational fields such as:
Science Education, Higher Education, Educational Psychology, Assessment &
Evaluation, Language Education, Mathematics Education, Educational Technology,
Medical Education and Educational Policy.
SpringerBriefs typically offer an outlet for:
• An introduction to a (sub)field in education summarizing and giving an over-
view of theories, issues, core concepts and/or key literature in a particular field
• A timely report of state-of-the art analytical techniques and instruments in the
field of educational research
• A presentation of core educational concepts
• An overview of a testing and evaluation method
• A snapshot of a hot or emerging topic or policy change
• An in-depth case study
• A literature review
• A report/review study of a survey
• An elaborated thesis
Both solicited and unsolicited manuscripts are considered for publication in the
SpringerBriefs in Education series. Potential authors are warmly invited to complete
and submit the Briefs Author Proposal form. All projects will be submitted to
editorial review by editorial advisors.
SpringerBriefs are characterized by expedited production schedules with the aim
for publication 8 to 12 weeks after acceptance and fast, global electronic
dissemination through our online platform SpringerLink. The standard concise
author contracts guarantee that:
• an individual ISBN is assigned to each manuscript
• each manuscript is copyrighted in the name of the author
• the author retains the right to post the pre-publication version on his/her website
or that of his/her institution
This Springer imprint is published by the registered company Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.
The registered company address is: 152 Beach Road, #21-01/04 Gateway East, Singapore 189721,
Singapore
MELT
This book is about the Models of Engaged Learning and Teaching (or MELT for
short), which fluidly connect to 100 years of educational research and 100,000
years of human learning and sophisticated thinking. The MELT are a family of
models with an ancestry that shares common features and that necessarily evolve to
fit new and emerging contexts. Because of their adaptability and shared features, the
MELT have provided a way for teachers, academics, sessional and professional
staff to bring together the years of education, from primary/elementary school
education through to the middle years, high school, technical education, under-
graduate, master’s and doctoral study. This connection was evident, for example,
when teachers, academics and professional staff from these contexts shared their
adaptations and uses of the MELT at the International MELT conference in 2017
and especially through informal conversations about learning. Context-specific
examples of MELT use are found throughout the book and associated website.
The MELT may provide you with a way to deepen your understanding of the
crucial role that you play in students’ learning and where your work fits in the span
of their educational experiences. Along with this insight, the MELT may also
enable to you enhance the learning of students under your care.
These models, when used in concert, have helped teachers, academics, profes-
sional staff and especially students to make connections between different subjects
and disciplines, from accounting to zoology, as well as transdisciplinary studies.
Sophisticated thinking is not context-bound, but rather learned best in a variety of
contexts, where connections between learning tasks may not be readily apparent but
need to be made explicit not only to students but also to teachers in different
contexts. The MELT also enable conversations between people with differing,
sometimes polarised, educational philosophies—for example, between behaviour-
ists employing Direct Instruction and social constructivists who employ discovery
learning.
vii
viii Preface
Place Value
While I swam in the ocean at the end of the tropical day, the children caught my
attention as they played unsupervised under the spreading tree down by the
shoreline. Their ages ranged from three to six years old. One child played with
shells, one laid out a mat as if proffering items to sell, two were in different
branches of the tree, one shook the branches, one just hung on, and two more were
hidden from my view but darted into sight occasionally. Each seemed to occupy a
separate world of play and exploration, of choosing what to do and where to do it.
But despite these separate activities, all the children were within a radius of
6 metres, whether horizontally or vertically, and chattered in their mother tongue.
When I left the water at twilight, the children of the village who were over seven
years old played volleyball on the sand and shell court, with an eight-year old
officiating and making sure they adhered strictly to international rules.
The next day, I attended the Year 1/2 class in the primary school of the village
and saw three of the children who had been playing in and under the tree the day
before.
Preface ix
The thirty-one children sat closely packed together on a Pacific Island i-TaliTali
mat. The teacher began with some maths terminology and showed flash cards to the
children.
Reading the card being held up by the teacher, Ms. Kristi, one child called out,
‘Plus value’.
All the children chanted, ‘Plus value, plus value’.
Ms. Kristi corrected ‘Place value’, and the children chanted, ‘place value, place
value’.
Referring to the concept of numeral position in a two-digit number, Ms. Kristi
asked the students, ‘Is place value on your right or on your left?’
‘Left’, called some children.
Then eight children were selected to go to the front and each took a number from
a box and held it to their chest for all to see. Numbers chosen were 9, 41, 11, 57, 5,
17, 20 and 29.
Ms. Kristi asked these eight children to arrange themselves in increasing
numerical order. They began to shuffle, reorder, pause, look at each other’s num-
bers and shuffle some more until they stood in a straight line in the sequence ‘5, 9,
11, 41, 17, 20, 29, 57’. Guided by their teacher, who pointed to each number in
turn, the children on the mat read the numbers in order from their right to left: ‘five,
nine, eleven, fourteen…’.
Ms. Kristi paused at this number and began, ‘For…’ and one student completed
‘Forty-one’.
Ms. Kristi paused thoughtfully. Then she said, ‘Let’s move on’ and resumed
pointing.
The children called out ‘Forty-one… seventeen… twenty… twenty-nine…
fifty-seven’.
‘Is it correct or not?’ asked Ms. Kristi.
The children called in unison, ‘Not’, so Ms. Kristi asked, ‘which number is
incorrect? Not in the proper place?’
One girl standing in the sequence and holding ‘5’ pointed to the fourth student
and said, ‘forty-one’.
‘How do you know that forty-one should not stand here?’
The student did not answer. No student answered.
‘So this forty-one should go where?’
Many children pointed to the next position up, so the teacher prompted forty-one
and seventeen to shuffle and change places.
The sequence became: 5, 9, 11, 17, 41, 20, 29, 57.
‘There. How do you know this forty-one should stand there?’
No answer.
Ms. Kristi continued, ‘Any other number is in the wrong place?’
‘Twenty-nine’, a student called out.
‘Twenty-nine should go where?’
‘Besides seventeen’, one student called out, and the student with twenty-nine
shuffled there, passing forty-one.
5, 9, 11, 17, 29, 41, 20, 57
Preface xi
understanding of place value, this would have negative implications for their
enduring engagement with maths. From a constructivist perspective, if students
were corrected on an answer that they volunteered in good faith, this could be
counterproductive to their ongoing confidence and willingness to engage in
learning. But from the pragmatic perspective of the MELT, a ‘bricolage’, a blending
of what works, is not only possible, but desirable, providing that teachers under-
stand what they are trying to achieve, how they will achieve it and especially know
why this is their goal.
Learning that is structured in order to help students acquire mandated knowledge
is not, by default, powerful, but neither is learning through play. One could argue
that without guided teaching, students involved in play (such as the children in the
tree) may merely be reproducing their cultural setting and knowledge, not learning
to be critical thinkers. This would suggest that play can be more reproductive and
less conceptually free than it appears on the surface. But Direct Instruction and
learning through play can be powerful, and in the context of the MELT, the
combination of these and other modes of learning provides a powerful incentive for
students to learn to think in sophisticated ways.
Learning and teaching are complex, and individuals, groups and systems will see
the same things differently through the MELT, as each will inhabit different social
and cultural worlds. I offer the MELT as a way in which our educational worlds can
become a little less polarised, and a little more connected and fluid across the years
and contexts in order to improve student learning.
xiii
Contents
xv
xvi Contents
xix
Chapter 1
What Is Our Purpose?
1.1 Purpose
There are a lot of hidden similarities in education. Differences are often emphasised,
and similarities hidden, by the articulation of context-specific terminology and tech-
niques. These include differences associated with ways of learning and teaching, such
as pedagogy versus andragogy; epistemological perspectives including objectivism
versus constructivism; pedagogical approaches, for example, Direct Instruction ver-
sus discovery learning; disciplinary ways of speaking and doing such as counting
and accounting versus zoo visits and zoology; and ways of researching, as evident
in qualitative versus quantitative methods.
Between all of these perspectives and approaches are genuine differences that are
good and helpful, for they mirror ways of learning, teaching and researching in a
variety of contexts. Such differences in perspective and approach mirror the differ-
ences between the communities to which they belong. However, these perspectives
and approaches also share quite a few similarities.
This book is about the similarities. The similarities are important because they
can be the connective elements across formal education, for teachers, principals,
academics, communities, education systems, parents and especially for learners.
Frequently, education comes across to students one learning activity, one assignment,
one subject at a time. But all those integrally involved aim for education to become a
forest of learning for students, rather than a sequence of individual trees. That forest
is a complex ecosystem of interactions, involving all year levels, all subjects and all
educational concepts growing in health and harmony. Where all the individual parts
join together into a complex whole is the location that students develop 21st century
skills, becoming critical thinkers and problem solvers who are research-minded and
information-savvy.
In order to connect the similarities so that students learn more effectively to solve
complex problems, think critically, research and make evidence-based decisions,
this book introduces and explains the Models of Engaged Learning and Teaching
(MELT). The MELT provide an understanding of the connections between diverse
animals provided a breeding ground for many killer diseases that resulted in large-
scale human and animal suffering. Wide adoption of crops led to the extreme use of
herbicides and insecticides, and the clearing of natural habitats. And technologies
that closed the distance between people through online social engagement across
the globe enabled cyber-abuse to enter people’s bedrooms and addictive behaviours
to control the tempo of modern relationships. Solutions produced fresh problems,
each of which required increasingly sophisticated solutions of their own. Thus, we
became the problem-solving ape [11], in part because we had to learn to solve the
problems we made for ourselves and for the whole planet. As a species, we have a
knack of solving problems with solutions that create new, more complex problems.
The MELT provide a way to gather together and connect educational ideas and
energies in a way that may help us to break out of the vicious circle of our solutions
that cause more problems. This is possible because, as noted, the MELT connect to
100,000 years of human learning and 100 years of educational research; with this
diverse set of otherwise conflicting set of understanding, the MELT can enable an
education that is broadly savvy of the influences on education. The MELT intention-
ally draw together and represent disparate views of education theory and practice, so
as to capture the broad sweep of learning and teaching, including Direct Instruction
to discovery learning; objectivist perspectives on learning through to social con-
structivist thought; primary/elementary school to Ph.D. studies; and accounting to
zoology and interdisciplinary notions; The MELT then can be used as a conceptual
set to connect those with different roles such as caregivers, lab managers, learn-
ing advisors, learning designers, lecturers, librarians, principals, professors, parents,
programme coordinators, sessional staff, supervisors of higher degree by research,
teachers, vice chancellors and, crucially, students from primary/elementary school
to Ph.D.
MELT is frequently put into action to help students understand their own sophis-
ticated thinking and see more clearly the purpose for their own education that nec-
essarily revolves around the further development of that thinking. It is not easy for
teachers to have or develop a sense of purpose for students that goes beyond the
immediacy of daily lessons to the big picture. For both students and teachers, MELT
may be used as a thinking routine [12] that becomes habitual (but not mundane)
and which, through repeated exposure, prompts growth in sophisticated thinking,
not only about what to do and how to do it, but also metacognitive awareness.
When considering the last 100 years of educational research, our understandings
of learning and teaching can seem more disparate than ever, and theory has not
connected well with practice. For example, it is common for teachers to omit the
explicit use of theoretical frameworks for their lesson planning and when leading
other teachers [13]. However, we are also at a point of knowing an amazing amount
about the complexities of teaching and learning. The MELT were formulated not to
be theoretically pure, but through a consideration of major aspects of educational
research and simultaneous reflection on classroom practice [14], as discussed in
Chaps. 2 and 4. The models provide a conceptual framework for action, not a theory
or set of theories. A ‘conceptual framework’ pertains here to a structure that guides
thinking, that sets the parameters for considering learning and teaching.
4 1 What Is Our Purpose?
The MELT provide a practical philosophy, then, connecting theories with theories
and practices with practices, and especially connecting theory with practice. The
MELT makes the skills associated with sophisticated thinking explicit, with the
intention of encouraging coherent, explicit and cyclic development of such thinking
across students’ education.
This book spans early childhood education (ECE) to postgraduate study and con-
tains examples across those contexts. But why would an early childhood teacher care
about Master’s level study, or undergraduate or high school? Why would a Ph.D.
supervisor care about primary school learning? I suggest a reason is that the MELT
can help with the connections across education, ultimately improving learning and
teaching in ECE, primary and secondary school, technical education, undergraduate,
master’s, Ph.D. and employment contexts. Another reason is that all students use and
teachers value the skills and attitudes associated with the MELT, because the models
encapsulate what we do when we engage in sophisticated thinking [14].
A brief history of MELT
Beginning in 2004, my colleague Kerry O’Regan and I synthesised disparate liter-
ature and reflections on classroom practice, culminating in the first fully-developed
version of the MELT, called the Research Skill Development (RSD) framework [15].
The RSD was employed in two national studies [15–17], which were designed to
determine its efficacy in higher education contexts. However, the word ‘research’ did
not always connect to people’s practice. For example, Sue Bandaranaike coordinated
student placements in industry called Work Integrated Learning or Cooperative Edu-
cation and knew that ‘research’ did not fit her context. In 2009, Sue re-articulated
the sophisticated thinking expressed in the RSD in terminology that was true to
employment contexts, producing the Work Skill Development (WSD) framework
[18]. Then, in quick succession, colleagues in Oral Health developed the Clinical
Reflection Framework in 2012 [19], student-tutors developed a pentagon-shaped ver-
sion for engineering, called the Optimising Problem-Solving pentagon [20] and an
early childhood music teacher developed a song version called ‘Research Mountain’
[21], both in 2014. Colleagues from the University of the South Pacific developed a
process-based version [22], using the metaphor of weaving a Pacific Island mat, the
italitali mat that students were sitting on in Place Value. In 2018 Monash University
developed the Digital Skills Development (DSD) framework [23] and in 2019 the
Blended and Engaged Learning Zones (BELZ) [24] was devised for the design and
evaluation of modes that are explicitly blended with e-learning. (Note that ‘learning’
is used preferentially to ‘e-learning’ in this book because modern learning is so often
bound up with, or mediated fully by, the electronic that it is often not helpful to
differentiate [25]).
By 2016, the number of RSD-based models had grown to such an extent that one
name was chosen to be emblematic of the characteristics and purpose of all of them:
the Models of Engaged Learning and Teaching [15]. It took from 2004 to 2016 to
determine the core characteristics of these models, and to find a name that could
connect them conceptually. The MELT evolved over time to become a set of related,
but context-specific, representations of how sophisticated thinking could be taught
1.1 Purpose 5
and learned, in keeping with Homo sapiens’ history [9] and contemporary learning
environments [14, 15].
Contemporary issues
By unpacking the MELT, this book will address four perennial, yet contemporary,
issues:
• How educators may effectively help students think in sophisticated ways, includ-
ing understanding their own thinking processes. Sophisticated thinking takes many
forms, and includes researching, problem-solving, evidence-based practice, clin-
ical reasoning, ethical reasoning, critical thinking, discovering, inquiring and
understanding concepts, as well as metacognition.
• How to connect different aspects of education so that they mutually reinforce and
complement each other:
– Across students’ education, from early childhood through to school completion,
technical and further education, undergraduate, master’s and Ph.D. level, onto
employment and continuing professional development.
– Across subjects and disciplines from counting and accounting to zoo visits and
zoology, multidisciplinary, interdisciplinary and transdisciplinary learning.
– Between sometimes competing paradigms, theories and teaching practices.
• How to deepen educators’ understanding of the dimensions and practicalities of
student autonomy in learning. This understanding will illuminate connections
between disparate discourses around student-centred learning, Direct Instruction,
Cognitive Load Theory, Threshold Concepts, discovery learning and networked
learning, as well as student cultural, language and learning diversity.
• How educators can effectively engage with educational theory in ways that offer
practical value for teaching and learning environments.
The book introduces the MELT as a way to conceptualise how such enabling,
connecting, deepening and engaging may take place.
This chapter details the purpose and features of the book and of its namesake sub-
ject, the Models of Engaged Learning and Teaching. Section 1.2 outlines the MELT’s
six facets of sophisticated thinking, elaborated along a continuum of learning auton-
omy. Section 1.3, called Parachute, comprises a story about two students, Shelly and
Katie, each engaged in an individual short project in the first year of high school.
As well as covering some of the types of learning that are common across formal
education and in many different disciplinary and interdisciplinary contexts, this story
is true to the sophisticated learning that humans have engaged in for 100,000 years.
In Sect. 1.4, 100 billion brains, humans are contrasted with beavers, who were the
premier engineers of the past 20 million years, but who were stuck in their mode
of learning. Unlike beavers, humans have developed multiple ways of learning, sug-
gesting that a multiplicity of teaching strategies are not just possible but desirable.
Section 1.5, One billion brains more, outlines the absolute need to develop sophisti-
cated thinking in order to address educational and planet-wide problems, requiring
a conceptualisation like MELT to connect different teaching approaches and ideas.
6 1 What Is Our Purpose?
Section 1.6 provides the structure of this book, where each chapter’s title is one
of the seven questions that are central to each MELT facet and to learning auton-
omy. The chapter concludes with Sect. 1.7, Student learning that resonates. For the
MELT, recognising and fostering a diverse range of teaching and learning strategies
is absolutely central to effective education. Therefore, this book explicitly articulates
the connections between disparate educational ideas, placing them all on the same
learning autonomy continuum of the MELT, in the hope that these ideas will be taken
together as a set and become more mutually supportive.
The MELT comprise the six facets of sophisticated thinking elaborated along a
continuum of learning autonomy.
The facets of the MELT concern the ‘what’ of learning and teaching. Content varies
subject-by-subject, lesson-by-lesson and in the MELT focus, the ‘what’ concerns
the skills and attitudes of sophisticated thinking as applied to, and mediated by, the
content.
Figure 1.1 shows a version of the MELT that was inspired by engineering students
who tutored in a large first year course. The student/tutors adapted MELT and devised
a version that they called the Optimising Problem-Solving (OPS) pentagon. That ver-
sion stripped out a lot of detail, resulting in a representation that is student friendly
and focuses on the facets, rather than explicating learning autonomy directly to the
students. Context-specific adaptations of MELT, portrayed in the pentagon configu-
ration, are used, with students from Year 4 of primary school to master’s level, and
in introductions of MELT to educators in schools and universities.
Complex learning from ECE to Ph.D. always requires something akin to the six
MELT facets. In many ways, these facets are clear and uncontroversial in nature if
not in name, and commonly made explicit in education. Each MELT facet comprises
a name made of verb couplets, an associated affective adjective, and a corresponding
question, as shown in Table 1.1 (as well as a description, provided in Chap. 2).
The facets of MELT are quintessential processes whose descriptions act as triggers
and connectors. As quintessential processes, the MELT facets can’t independently
capture the meaning for every context. They are fully dependent on the educators
who adopt them, each of whom knows or is coming to know, what needs to happen
in any learning situation that they are facilitating, and how to articulate the processes
they are facilitating.
Each facet of the MELT is designed to ‘trigger’ words and phrases that better
describe the facet’s concepts in a particular context. These words and phrases then
1.2 MELT Components 7
Fig. 1.1 The MELT Pentagon’s six facets, each with a pair of verbs, a key question and an adjective
in blue which represents the affective domain
may conceptually connect to other contexts which use different words and phrases for
the same concepts being triggered by the facet. Without this conceptual connection,
the processes associated with the same facet may otherwise seem unrelated to students
and educators. For example, the facet embark and clarify may trigger terms that
suit the start of a process such as ‘pose research question’, ‘define problem’ or
‘determine need’, depending on the terminology of the context and the purpose at
hand. These terms do have useful differences, but they also have conceptual overlap
that is frequently overlooked. As triggers, the facets are not generic skills, because
‘generic’ implies skills that students maybe able to generalise from one context
to another. The facets may be better thought of as ‘connectable skills’ rather than
transferable skills.
As an educational trigger, each facet has four vital inter-related components. Three
are introduced above (verb couplets, affective adjective, key question), and the fourth
component, introduced in Chap. 2, is a sentence description of each facet. Some
educators may focus on the cognitive aspect (e.g., embark), some on the affective
(e.g., curious), some on the question (what is our purpose?), some on all three.
But together, these aspects provide the sense of what we are after across education,
and that sense can be explicitly connected from one context to another. Subject and
discipline-mediated ways of understanding and representing the facets vary widely
[16–19], as demonstrated in Chap. 3’s look at MELT use in a variety of contexts.
The continuum of learning autonomy in MELT concerns the ‘how’ of learning and
teaching, that is, the ways that the facets maybe developed, making the continuum an
explicit articulation of the teaching process for scaffolding the development of sophis-
ticated thinking. It is also possible to enable students to understand their engagement
in the learning process, and representation of the continuum of learning autonomy
expressly for students is shown in Fig. 1.2. In the figure, the red pentagon represents
lower levels of learning autonomy, where students emulate; a yellow pentagon at
mid-levels of learning autonomy, where students improvise, and a blue pentagon
representing high levels of learning autonomy, where students initiate. Students may
emulate, then improvise, then initiate sophisticated learning and then proceed to
emulate once more, for example, if the learning context shifts so that students are
unfamiliar with new content, if conceptual demand goes up or as the expected rigour
increases. In other words, learning autonomy in MELT is not unidirectional towards
high autonomy, but rather shuttles back and forth, according to the young child’s or
the Ph.D. student’s learning needs [27].
Learning autonomy in MELT maybe engineered by teachers, and matrix ver-
sions of MELT often articulate a five-level, teaching-oriented continuum of learning
autonomy, shown in Fig. 1.3. Five levels of differentiation are sometimes helpful for
teachers, whereas three levels are typically enough for students.
1.2 MELT Components 9
Fig. 1.2 A student-oriented continuum of learning autonomy, here represented by three verbs and
corresponding colours: emulate, red; improvise, yellow; and initiate, blue (see www.rsd.edu.au/
framework)
Fig. 1.3 A teaching-oriented continuum of learning autonomy, here represented by five verbs and
corresponding colours: prescribed, red; bounded, orange; scaffolded, yellow; open-ended, green;
and unbounded, blue (details removed: see www.rsd.edu.au/framework)
realities. It is ultimately the most contentious question and because of this has the
potential to connect disparate ideas in education:
How much guidance?
The six facets of MELT elaborated along the continuum of learning autonomy frame,
but cannot answer, the above question. The question can only be answered by indi-
vidual teachers and their students, by school communities, by systems and, maybe
soon, by Teaching Machines (see Chap. 5) who understand the context of learning.
The MELT explication of the continuum of learning autonomy provokes answers,
however, around a healthy shuttling where students emulate, improvise and initiate
and then proceed once more to emulate. The amount of guidance depends on the rela-
tionship of the student to what is learned, to the teacher and to the broader context,
and the complexity of such relationship is best discussed and debated.
Sharing the inter-relationships of facets and learning autonomy, the MELT takes
on many forms and may be revisited in different guises along a student’s learning
journey. Some forms include tables with text, pentagon or jig-saw shapes, songs with
actions, and a weaving metaphor; diverse MELT models are presented in Chap. 3.
Formats and phrasings depend on the purpose chosen for each of the MELT, the
intended audience, and educators’ professional judgement, and so the MELT are
necessarily fluid. With a growing number of emerging MELT, the models show the
potential of working together as a set that conceptually connects the disparate ideas
and energies of education. Multiple manifestations and uses of the same overarching
framework by many educators, researchers and parents may, over time, richly develop
the sophisticated thinking that enables students to create solutions to problems—
solutions that do not become the cause of further problems.
Providing students with multiple exposures to MELT in many guises enables the
six facets to become a thinking routine [12], a way of thinking that can conceptually
accompany them throughout their education and remain afterwards. Researchers
found that a hallmark of effective teachers was that they frequently employed explicit
thinking strategies for students to use [11]. Teachers repeatedly introduced, modelled
and used these strategies to facilitate student learning. The researchers called these
thinking routines because they became almost second nature for students, and they
were a vital component of ‘making thinking visible’ to teachers, parents and to the
students themselves. The six facets of the MELT can become for students a thinking
routine if teachers facilitate their use repeatedly. One final-year university student,
looking back over multiple semesters of MELT use said ‘because they have been
consistently applying this structure to all of our assignments, we have come to think
that way for science’ [28]. MELT became a thinking routine for that student.
Education connotes a process of educing, a ‘leading out’ of what is inside stu-
dents, their capacities for sophisticated thinking, whereas the facilitation of learning
1.2 MELT Components 11
1.3 Parachute
This chapter emphasises purpose, and purpose is the underpinning theme in the class-
room story below, called Parachute. A big range of teaching and learning strategies is
evident in Parachute: there is a teacher present, a person who holds a vast amount of
culturally-specific knowledge and who is keen to incorporate that into the students’
own knowledge bases. The teacher has an intention for the learning that may or may
not be realised student-by-student. There is a learner, Shelly, who is willing to take
a risk and innovate, and another learner, Katie, who plays it safe and emulates the
teacher. There are unnamed students who strongly influence the learning dynamic,
even though the focus of the class is nominally an individual project.
This diversity of actors and actions is true to the reality of human learning across
millennia, where the capacity to learn was a huge determinant of the survival and
evolutionary direction of the species. At times in our prehistory, imitation and rote
learning, forms of emulation, were far more effective for survival than discovery
learning requiring innovation. However, there have also been many times where
existing knowledge and practice were insufficient and, like Shelly below, individuals
or groups needed to take risks and innovate. Without such adaptations, human groups
would have perished or been overrun. Or they just may have missed an amazing
opportunity unless they adapted and took risks, as demonstrated by the Polynesians
who sailed into the unknown Pacific about 3,000 years ago [31].
12 1 What Is Our Purpose?
‘Three?’
‘What did you do to get it?’
‘Divide by itself?’ asks Shelly in answer.
‘Sort of,’ I respond, thinking she is getting close.
‘Find what
√ and what… like square roo… like you..?’
I write ‘ 9’.
‘I don’t know how to do that,’ Shelly laments.
I show her the symbol on her calculator. It is term three of Year 8 and Shelly is in
the top mathematics class. She says that does not know how to find the length of the
sides of a square when given an area. She says that she does not know how to find
the square root of a number, even using a calculator.
Much worse to me, though, is that she never thought for a second that there could
be something wrong with the size of the square she had calculated. And she was an
‘A’ grade student in maths.
Shelly finds the square root to be 20.5, runs to her desk and immediately starts to
rule up the plastic. There is now plenty of material to make a square parachute with
sides of 20.5 centimetres.
Mrs. Breen goes to the doorway to speak to a number of students dropping
parachutes outside: ‘Alright everyone, back in.’
As Shelly hears this, she says, ‘Shivers,’ quietly to herself, grabs both parachutes
and heads for the door.
She almost makes it outside, when Mrs. Breen waves everyone back to their seats.
‘I want everyone to listen.’
Back at her seat, Shelly works hastily to make sure her parachutes are ready to
test.
‘I know most of you have done the experiment…’ (Shelly works at a more frenzied
pace) ‘…but I also want you to do a bar graph.’
Mrs. Breen draws an example on the board.
Katie begins to do some test drops with her two parachutes. She is comparing
the drop time of a cloth parachute and a plastic parachute, which was the example
provided by Mrs. Breen. Both parachutes float to earth in a satisfying parachute-like
manner.
Shelly grabs the stop-watch and starts to drop her square parachute. However, the
string pulls off the plastic.
‘Mine doesn’t work. It’s crap,’ she sulks to Katie.
Shelly quickly repairs the square parachute and drops it again (Fig. 1.4).
In the results table, under the column heading ‘round’, Shelly writes ‘0.68’, then
crosses it out. Next to this, she writes ‘1.12’ after another drop. Then, she crosses
this out. She drops again and writes ‘0.93’, yet changes the nine to a six, so her
only number in that column reads ‘0.63’. This dropping, erasing and writing process
continues for several minutes.
Mrs. Breen passes and asks, ‘So what happened? Is it OK?’
‘Yep’, says Shelly, ‘the round one takes longer to hit the ground.’
‘Did you use several measurements?’
‘Yes.’
14 1 What Is Our Purpose?
Fig. 1.4 Shelly determines the drop time for a square, plastic parachute
In Parachute, Shelly and Katie were engaged in learning that involved designing
experiments and determining independent variables, dependent variables and con-
trolled variables. MELT is one way of representing the sophisticated thinking in this
story. The simple analysis of Parachute below foreshadows the six facets of MELT
that are introduced more thoroughly in Chap. 2.
Shelly is determined to compare square and round parachutes, whereas Katie
looked at cotton versus plastic, the teacher’s example. This is where each decided
1.3 Parachute 15
what to embark on for their experiment, as well as beginning the complex process
of ongoing clarification of purpose.
Early on, Shelly worked to craft two plastic parachutes of different shape but
the same size, and realised that she needed to use geometry to achieve this. She
created the round parachute, measured it and generated its surface area from pi r
squared. Then she used the formula for perimeters of a square to calculate its sides—
generating a number that dwarfed the circle. Shelly found needed resources (plastic, a
calculator and maths formulas from memory) and generated data using an empirical
methodology.
Shelly realised there was a problem, and her evaluation correctly suggested that
there was insufficient material to make a square big enough to match her calculation. I
was surprised that she did not notice the huge discrepancy in size between her actual
circle and her proposed square. It seems she implicitly trusted the mathematical
calculation, even when the discrepancy was huge, evidencing little reflection at that
point. Shelly sought help from me for more materials. However, instead of sourcing
more, I prompted her to consider that her calculation of the size of the square was
wrong. At this point, Shelly and I tried to determine whether she knew an appropriate
formula to determine the amount of material she would need for a square parachute.
When she struggled remembering a correct equation for the area of a square, I
provided her numerous cues until I virtually told her area equals the length of one
side squared.
Once she applied a formula that gave a more sensible answer, she produced a
square parachute of comparable size to the round one, and began to time parachute
drops. She wrote, erased and rewrote results into her results table. This organisation
of results was accompanied by the evaluation of data as somehow wrong and in
need of re-recording. All the while, Shelly had the huge pressure of managing her
dwindling time to set up the entire experiment, which was novel, in that no one else
was contrasting shape, while others were following the teacher’s example procedure.
Shelly analysed her data and found that the round one took longer to fall. Her
synthesis was the overall finding that ‘shape does matter to drop time, given the same
surface area’.
Throughout, Shelly listened to and talked with her desk partner, me and the teacher,
while she also wrote and recorded, demonstrating multiple modes of communication.
She applied remembered and prompted knowledge (formulas for the area of a circle
and of a square) to the experiment, as well as her evolving knowledge of experimental
design, relating this to her existing knowledge and to others in the classroom.
The story is set in a school science laboratory, but Shelly engaged in sophisti-
cated thinking that is as relevant to hunter-gatherers as it is to learners in virtual
worlds. Lab learning, hunting and online gaming differ in many ways and the fun-
damental neuronal wiring of the brains of children brought up engaging heavily
online maybe quite different to that of hunter-gatherers. However, the story contains
elements of the sophisticated thinking that we crave as teachers and such thinking
spans 100,000 years. Like most learners throughout human prehistory and history,
Shelly experienced frustrations, setbacks and unresolved tensions, as well as some
successes.
16 1 What Is Our Purpose?
The in-class learning of Shelly, involving exploring and risk taking, and of Katie,
involving emulating and safety, demonstrated a lot of the potential and pitfalls con-
nected to learning autonomy. Shelly attempted to work on her own questions about
square and round parachutes, showing much more autonomy in learning than Katie
and others in the class by initiating in many MELT facets, and improvising in the
rest. Katie was primarily emulating the teacher’s example. Thus, there were differ-
entiated experiences along the continuum of learning autonomy in the same room,
at the same time, given the same parameters by the teacher. The task in Parachute
had the scope of open-ended inquiry, but because the teacher provided modelling
that students were permitted to imitate, most students took the safer, easier way of
emulating.
Shelly’s higher autonomy had associated risks, exemplified by her extreme mis-
calculation. Shelly had possibly applied the formula for the area of a square dozens
of times in her schooling, and this time she was way off any sensible number. While
there is potential for huge learning here, the situation is also an example of a huge
learning curve. Where that curve is too steep, some students may experience enough
demotivation and discouragement to put them off trying. However, Shelly was not
deterred by her miscalculation, in part because she did not perceive the discrepancy
in the actual size of the round parachute and the calculated size of the square one.
Instead of recalculating, she sought more material to match her calculation. From
a Problem Based Learning (PBL) perspective, if Shelly recognised the discrepancy
between calculation and size, this could have led to deep learning [32]. However,
within the constraints of the classroom, she had little time to complete and submit
her work. Practicalities of teaching, such as lesson length, frequently dictate what
actually happens in formal education regardless of theory informing the lesson. Ulti-
mately, Shelly demonstrated in the eighty minutes before lunch the sophisticated
thinking that comprises MELT’s six facets on the higher end of the continuum of
learning autonomy. Often these facets are not deployed consciously and, much like
an unopened parachute, learners may have a hard landing when conceptual difficulties
emerge.
In Parachute, Shelly’s own research question created a need for mathematical
calculation and specific mathematical equations. This maybe contrasted with Place
Value, where children required no specific knowledge to play around the tree, and
in their classroom, the focus was on content knowledge inculcation or construction.
Shelly, a student of thirteen, had experienced seven years of schooling in maths, and
it’s reasonable to assume that she would have learned about calculating the area of
a square several years earlier. Place Value represented learning experiences in two
different contexts and each elicited a very different learning autonomy. However,
Parachute showed some of the tensions that maybe evident within a single context,
and highlighted the intimate connection between content knowledge and inquiry
learning. This tension between an appropriate knowledge base and learning through
discovery has been evident throughout human prehistory. The questions of engaged
learning and teaching are as pertinent to ancient Homo sapiens as they are to contem-
porary humans whether they are inhabiting real or virtual environments. The wiring
of our brains may be or become quite different, but the questions of learning are the
same.
1.3 Parachute 17
Through our prehistory, humans proved to be highly adaptable because our brains
were not programmed to ‘get stuck’ on one mode of learning. But ironically for a
discipline centred on learning, the practice and research of formal education have
tended to become ‘stuck’. That is, researchers have often prioritised researching the
forms of learning that seem optimal to them, and looking at the learning gains by
contrasting performance, e.g., that of Direct Instruction versus discovery learning.
The findings of studies, of course, have always depended on what was valued as a
learning gain in the measurements. But a mentality which emphasises measurement
has possibly diminished educators’ awareness of the potential range of ways to learn,
with a whole spectrum of possibilities being minimised for the sake of theory and
parsimony. Search for a grand theory that underpins all learning undermines the
potential of humankind to engage in diverse and enriching learning and teaching.
To maximise the efforts of educators and students, it is necessary to pull together
the disparate threads in education, in a way that reflects how humans were geared to
learn 100,000 years ago and how they are currently geared to learn. In educational
practice and theory, MELT connotes a fluidity, rather than being stuck in one place,
in keeping with our learning as a species.
Humans are the only known animals to use systematic, descriptive labels for living
things, including ourselves. And while we labelled our cousins with systematic and
descriptive species names such as Homo erectus and Homo habitus, we chose for
ourselves the more interpretive title of Homo sapiens, or ‘wise man’. If sentient ani-
mals had a vote, would they describe modern humans as wise? We are certainly a
learning animal, but many animals are that too. What about our learning is distinc-
tively sapiens? We have big brains, proportionally, but Neanderthals had a bigger
brain volume [33] and still died out.
As noted early in this chapter, Homo sapiens have been very efficient at solving
problems whose solutions created new problems which emerged days, years or cen-
turies later. This is highly disturbing, albeit probably inevitable. The disturbing side
of the process whereby solutions beget problems is obvious, with rates of species
extinction unprecedented since the meteor that led to the demise of the dinosaurs,
degradation of water, air, soils and society, and massive migrations, alienations and
competition between cultures, religions, political ideologies and nations.
The inevitable side of the process by which human-made solutions generate new
problems may come as a surprise. To demonstrate this inevitability, I present a
comparison between humans and beavers—the pre-eminent engineers of the ani-
mal world, for more than 20 million years [34] up until 12,000 years ago. The reason
for this comparison is that those early engineer-beavers were more instinctive than
18 1 What Is Our Purpose?
The massive engineering projects requiring complex social behaviour, up until rel-
atively recently in the earth’s history, were conducted by beavers. It is only in the
recent past, starting about 12,000 years ago [35], that human engineering projects
were conducted on a scale more massive than beavers’ dams, even though the human
physique’s capacity for engineering has exceeded the beavers’ physical capacity for
200,000 years.
So, how is it that beavers were more able to conduct massive engineering projects
than humans for over 180,000 years? In short, because it took a whole lot of learning
about materials, physics, chemistry, maths, nature, and ourselves, including through
artistic expression, creative imagining and philosophising, for humans to take top
spot on massive-scale engineering. Critically, what is important to, and reflective of,
MELT, is that this human learning was diverse in mode.
A curious feature of beaver dam-building know-how is that the majority of it is
in their genes [36, 37]. Beavers can learn over time to build better dams, but their
fundamental damming behaviour is hard-wired. So, even if you could challenge a
beaver to build some other massive structure (such as a bridge) it would not have
the cognitive capacity to do so. Beavers are smooth-brained (lissencephalic) whereas
humans and other apes have wormy brains featuring gyri and sulci, which allow for
more neurons and synapses to be packed into the same space. Pre-programed know-
how only gets you so far, and beavers can’t generalise their learning from dams to
other engineering projects.
As with beavers, Homo sapiens have genetically programmed know-how: we can
suck, chew and crawl, and maybe some humans can walk and run without examples.
But we can’t talk, write, read or do anything like build a dam without learning to
do so, whether by watching and listening, with a teacher or through trial and error.
Beavers’ engineering knowledge is pre-packaged, whereas ours developed over the
course of tens of thousands of years, and needs to be culturally transmitted rather
than genetically transmitted.
Perhaps because we have so little genetic know-how, human babies are called
‘sponges’, as they appear ready to soak up and imitate almost anything they see and
hear. As a tragic example, orphans in Romania who were deprived of care by adults
[38] mimicked the movements of cranes that they could see through the window.
Engaged learning is the norm for a baby in three modes: being taught, observing,
and experimenting through trial and error. All these three modes are vital for normal
development, and the second and last are ubiquitous for babies.
Being taught, however, requires someone to facilitate the learning. This could
involve correcting a child’s pronunciation, instructing on the number of protons in
1.4 100 Billion Brains: Learning from Human Prehistory … 19
Another good reason to know about beaver learning is to deepen our understanding
of human learning and its consequences. As beavers evolved increasing capacity,
physically and cognitively, to build dams, they not only built bigger and better dams,
but they changed entire ecosystems through the flooding of valleys to make lakes.
They changed the course of evolution for untold species of plants, animals, fungi
and bacteria. However, this process took millions of years, and entire ecosystems
20 1 What Is Our Purpose?
adapted slowly to beaver-induced changes [39]. Many animals wreak major changes
quickly on the landscape, such as swarming insects or defoliating elephants, but few
have had an impact on the scale of beaver technology [40].
Compared to the elaborate engineering projects of beavers, we Homo sapiens
were very simple builders for most of our evolution. However, as noted earlier,
simple toolmaking eventually gave rise to a compounding [8] of tool use, where
one tool enabled other tools: a chipped rock made sharp for killing an animal, for
example, could also be used to smooth the haft of a spear. Animals use what’s at their
disposal, and humans were no different. The pace of technology compounded, and
while initially glacial, it became exponential: single-edge stone tools maintained the
same design from 200,000 to 120,000 BCE [8], but once further adaptations emerged,
tool use and development took off—literally—like a rocket!
This ratcheting up of problem-solving capacity and technology is possible because
human brains are learning-versatile compared to beaver brains. A major element of
human learning before the establishment of cities was that our brains had the capacity,
much more so than beavers, to engage in didactic-reproductive learning, generation
after generation. At the same time, we also had the capacity, used when the cir-
cumstances warranted it, to engage in the development of new knowledge through
research-like processes of discovery. When competition came, when the environment
changed, when an important prey animal died out or humans moved into different
ecosystems, the learning of established ideas through reproductive modes was inad-
equate by itself. Adaptation was required which, in turn, demanded a different order
of learning. Human brains are adaptive and plastic, and adults could shift their think-
ing somewhat, rewiring neural networks. However, for the young with developing
brains who moved with their extended family into new challenges, an upbringing of
upheaval geared their brains for discovery mode; they neurologically wired to inquire
[41, 42]. A challenge for educators in the twenty-first century, then, is to work out
what modes of learning are pertinent in emerging local and global circumstances and
what balance can be struck across different modes of teaching and learning, from
more didactic to more discovery-oriented. The MELT facets and continuum of learn-
ing autonomy provide the what and how of Homo sapiens’ transmission-oriented and
innovative learning over the course of the last 1000,000 years, as we have always,
and need still, to emulate, improvise and innovate.
Because humans are adept at solving problems, and because our solutions cause
more problems, we and the planet are on a destructive trajectory. Education systems
that replicate our current successes will also magnify our current problems. Instead,
we need educators with broadened perspectives that cope with, and even enjoy, a
range of teaching and learning modes. MELT articulate the human capacity to learn
1.5 One Billion Brains More: The Problems … 21
didactically and to learn through discovery in ways that reinforce each other. While
some educators, parents, researchers and students take a polarised position towards
one particular teaching and learning approach, MELT suggest that we need learning
environments that explicitly value every point on the learning autonomy continuum
This is, arguably, our best hope to move from an inevitable problem-solving/problem-
causing loop to a less predetermined future for the Earth.
We need to be able to cover every point on the continuum of teaching and learning
approaches, because we need brains that are wired to swing adaptively from learning
established knowledge to constructing fresh knowledge. We need brains that are
wired to memorise and recall and wired to inquire and delve [41]. Education systems
need to draw on young children’s natural curiosity, promote the acquisition of the
massive canon of modern knowledge, and be more purposive in how they move
between these two.
How can parents, caregivers and early childhood educators help young children’s
brains to wire in such a way that they love to learn existing knowledge, as well as
to discover knowledge on their own? How can these loves be nurtured throughout
primary and secondary schooling? What will facilitate the further development of
such engaged learning with undergraduates and Master’s students, including those
who are enrolled in teaching degrees? What difference will this make to Ph.D. stu-
dents and other researchers when addressing the problems of the Earth? And what
will this mean for teachers who engage in their own active and systematic learning,
such as action research and ongoing professional development, to improve their stu-
dents’ learning? Further, how can we best nurture a love of moving fluidly across the
continuum of learning autonomy, rather than prioritising one of the ends?
The above fluidity is one where our genetically-determined neuronal architecture
and its adaptive responsiveness to environmental cues [42] forms our brains with not
merely the capacity to learn new things, but the capacity to rewire. MELT can inform a
rewiring for the next billion brains that is both knowledge-savvy and inquiry-oriented
(Fig. 1.5).
Humans have the greatest number of neurons at about seven months’ gestation,
typically two months before birth [42]. Then apoptosis—healthy, natural cell death—
is genetically programmed to remove the less used neurons, so that newborn babies
and adults alike have substantially less neurons than we had when we were in the
womb [42] where the adult brain has around 80 billion neurons. Typically, those neu-
rons that synapse—or connect—with other neurons survive. These neurons continue
the complex process of developing neural networks, with any one neuron connected
up to 1,000 other neurons [42] in a mind-boggling array that is the powerhouse of
sophisticated thinking.
Most amazing and relevant for MELT is that the number of our synapses—the
connections between neurons—goes through the roof early in life, so that by age
three we have trillions. But after the age of three, our healthy genetic programming
begins to reduce those networks in a process called ‘neural pruning’ [42]. Our healthy
brains go through this natural pruning process where if we don’t use it, we lose it:
a synapse connection in which two neurons rarely communicate tends to be pruned.
The saying is that neurons that fire together wire together [42], meaning that if two
22 1 What Is Our Purpose?
synapsed neurons communicate regularly, they will stay connected and maintain
their part of the network. After the age of three, we begin to lose billions of synapses
each year, and this process continues through different parts of the brain until the
twenties [42]. This sounds bad because learning equates to synapse formation and
maintenance. However, neurons and synapses require a lot of energy, and an overly
complex network is less efficient and effective.
But here’s the thing: what we are sensing, doing, thinking and saying from birth
and beforehand (e.g., tasting mother’s amniotic fluid as an embryo) influences our
synapse formation, and what is pruned [42]. So children who spend the majority
of their time using information and communications technology (e.g., gaming) are
using their brains in such a way that some neural networks are enhanced and others
are limited. As a result, their neuro-architecture—their actual brain’s wiring—will
reflect their environment [42], just as has always been the case for human brains.
The brain’s wiring is a complex mix of genetic determinants and environmental
influences. As an extreme example, a baby reared in the dark will not be able to see
even when taken into the light at age one [42]. The development of optical pathways
in the brain is not only genetically determined but requires environmental stimulus;
use it or minimise it.
Learning in the human brain is not compartmentalised into ‘cognitive’ and ‘affec-
tive’. Long-term memory is closely associated with the part of the brain called the
hippocampus, which itself is also associated with emotions [43]. Correspondingly,
a longstanding finding from psychology is that we learn that to which we attend
1.5 One Billion Brains More: The Problems … 23
[44], and so motivational elements are vital for learning [43], frequently in multi-
modal ways that reinforce learning [44]. MELT explicitly articulate the cognitive
dimensions of engaged learning in concert with the affective dimensions. The beaver
in each cartoon at the beginning of each chapter calls out affective-oriented words
that connect to the more cognitive facet titles, and this simultaneous connection of
cognitive and affective domains is central to MELT, because the connection is true
and vital for human learning.
A full range of educational experiences, including face-to-face, hands-on, online
and virtual learning, is needed for the education of the next billion brains on the
planet. This range of experiences needs to be full not just in terms of subject matter,
but in terms of how this subject matter is learned. In MELT terms, this especially con-
cerns the continuum of learning autonomy. A full range of educational experiences
would embrace teacher-directed immersion of students in content knowledge and in
key concepts, as well as student ownership of learning and inclusion in curriculum
decisions, where students would be given scope for high levels of autonomy in their
learning through investigation, problem-solving and discovery. For the MELT, auton-
omy ebbs and flows from levels of low learning autonomy, with prescribed teaching,
through to high levels of learning autonomy with teacher boundaries removed, and
back again to low learning autonomy. Such shuttling, where students emulate, impro-
vise, initiate and flow back to emulate over time, is suggestive of spiralling learning
autonomy throughout formal education from early childhood through primary and
secondary school, to employment, technical study and university, whether a student
completes compulsory education, undergraduate study or proceeds to Master’s level
and Ph.D.
This book’s seven-chapter structure mirrors the seven core components of the MELT,
the six facets and learning autonomy. Each chapter has as its title one of the seven cen-
tral questions of MELT on a title page comprising a cartoon by Dr. Aaron Humphries
[45]. Each cartoon has three characters: a young child, Albert Einstein, and a beaver,
each saying something emblematic of the sophisticated thinking pertaining to a par-
ticular facet. The child sings a line that represents a facet from the song Research
Mountain [21] except in Chap. 7 she says a line from a research article. Einstein says
something for each facet that connects to sophisticated thinking from the Nobel prize-
winning end of the learning spectrum. And the beaver calls out the affective aspect
of each facet. In each cartoon’s banner is the facet process, such as embark and clar-
ify, and all facet processes are double-edged, comprising two strong, interdependent
verbs for learning.
This introductory chapter asked, ‘What is our purpose?’ for student learning, for
teaching and for the book itself. This chapter elaborated on the need for, and the
possibility of, a coherent solution to the problems associated with an education in
which all the parts are not well connected.
24 1 What Is Our Purpose?
Chapter 2 is titled ‘What should we use?’ and this question is asked in consid-
eration of our educational purposes. Chapter 2 provides a deep sense of the MELT,
explaining in detail the six facets of sophisticated thinking and a consideration of
how much guidance students need in terms of learning autonomy. Chapter 2 does
this by delving into each facet in turn, interpreted in research stories from primary
school (Place Value in the Preface), high school (Parachute in this chapter, below)
and the account of a university graduate (Silver Fluoride in Chap. 2). Chapter 2
details the educational literature that informed the creation of MELT, and notes that
much of that literature is descriptive in nature and lacks a theoretical underpinning.
Chapter 3 of this book, ‘How do we arrange?’, provides numerous examples
of teachers using MELT to arrange and prompt more sophisticated thinking across
the educational trajectory. These examples help to introduce others’ pedagogical
interpretations of what form MELT had needed to take on and how it needed to be
implemented in order for it to work in each of their contexts.
Chapter 4 ‘What do we trust?’ pulls competing theories together in productive
tension and places them as the underpinning of MELT. By providing the theoretical
underpinning of MELT, the chapter specifically positions competing theories together
on the learning autonomy continuum, with the aim of arousing awareness and choice
of where to operate on or across this continuum.
Chapter 5 is titled ‘What does it mean?’ and provokes a consideration of seminal
and recent learning theories and what they mean for contemporary educational prac-
tices and teacher action research in light of MELT. Unpacking and placing learning
theories on MELT’s continuum of learning autonomy is very practical for teacher
understanding of theories and for their application of these to classrooms and online
learning. Chapter 5 shows the connections between four contemporary learning theo-
ries, some of which are perceived to be in direct opposition, and the ways that teacher
action research can bring theory to life. The chapter contains a chilling warning of
continuing to treat educational theories as competitive rather than complementary,
as Machine Teaching comes to the fore.
Chapter 6 asks ‘How do we relate?’ in regard to humanity’s relationship with itself
and the planet, and why things seemed to have panned out in a way that leads us
inevitably to environmental devastation and social upheaval. The chapter proposes
ways in which MELT may be part of a solution that doesn’t cause more problems.
Chapter 7, ‘How much guidance?’ addresses the scaffolding of student learning,
using MELT’s consideration of learning autonomy. The chapter considers those
involved in education and the amount of guidance that may be needed to make
MELT work in various settings. Autonomy in MELT is a relationship word, and is
intimately connected to ‘ownership’ of teaching and learning. The need for ownership
and empowerment is a major factor when considering how much structure and what
sort of guidance is needed by students and teachers alike. The coherence of student
learning journeys maybe possible through teachers, parents, schools and universities
pulling together in the same direction, as directed by policy, but policy is not so
good at motivating that pursuit in the long-term. In order to promote ownership and
empowerment, it’s good for teachers to be autonomous. But students exposed to a
series of overly autonomous teachers may find that their education feels broken and
incoherent. How much guidance do we need for teachers and for students?
1.6 Structure of This Book 25
In summary, this chapter explains the need for the MELT, Chap. 2 details them,
and Chap. 3 provides examples from ECE to Ph.D. level. Chapter 4 provides the
theoretical underpinning, Chap. 5 considers contemporary learning theories in light of
MELT, and Chap. 6 draws together the relationships that MELT may forge. Chapter 7
concludes with considerations for operationalising MELT.
The billion human brains that will be born between 2023 and 2030 need something
different from the learning and education that has occurred so far across 100,000 years
of human history. That billion will inherit the leadership of the earth somewhere from
2040, with all of the accumulated problems caused by humanity until that time. Those
billion need diverse learning environments that resonate with their complex learn-
ing capacities, that connect to multiple educator perspectives and theories, and that
enable them to address local and global issues in ways that do not cause more prob-
lems than they solve. The complexities of human learning demand an expansive and
encompassing conceptualisation of learning that mirrors different disciplines, learner
ages, teaching theories, learning throughout prehistory and educational research. The
billion need to be those whose knowledge, skills, attitudes, values, creativity and
discernment are so powerful that they can anticipate problems caused by proposed
solutions, and forge solutions that don’t cause more problems. The facets and their
elaboration across the continuum of learning autonomy of the MELT are proffered
in the next chapter as a conceptualisation that can help address this educational need.
References
1. Einstein Bubble in Cartoon: Isaacson, W. (2007). Einstein, his life and universe. London: Simon
and Schuster.
2. Dewey, J. (1904/1974). The relation of theory to practice in education. In R. Archambault (Ed.),
John Dewey on Education: Selected Writings (pp. 315– 338). Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
3. Bundy, A. (Ed.). (2004). Australian and New Zealand Information Literacy framework: Prin-
ciples, standards and practice (5th ed.). Adelaide, Australia: Australian and New Zealand
Institute for Information Literacy.
4. Delson, E. (2019, July 10). An early dispersal of modern humans from Africa to Greece. Nature
News and Views. Retrieved from www.nature.com/articles/d41586-019-02075-9.
5. Stringer, C. (2016). The origin and evolution of Homo sapiens. Philosophical Transactions of
the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1698), 20150237.
6. Tattersall, I. (2016). The thinking primate: Establishing a context for the emergence of modern
human cognition. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 160(3), 254–265.
7. Scerri, E. M., Thomas, M. G., Manica, A., Gunz, P., Stock, J. T., Stringer, C., et al. (2018). Did
our species evolve in subdivided populations across Africa, and why does it matter? Trends in
Ecology & Evolution, 33(8), 582–594.
8. Koetsier, T. (2019). The ascent of GIM, the global intelligent machine. Cham, Switzerland:
Springer.
26 1 What Is Our Purpose?
9. van Schaik, C. P., Pradhan, G. R., & Tennie, C. (2019). Teaching and curiosity: Sequen-
tial drivers of cumulative cultural evolution in the hominin lineage. Behavioral Ecology and
Sociobiology, 73(1), 2.
10. Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
11. Hasan, R. (1995). On social conditions for semiotic mediation: The genesis of mind in society. In
A. Sadovnik (Ed.), Knowledge and pedagogy: The sociology of Basil Bernstein (pp. 171–196).
Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
12. Ritchhart, R., & Perkins, D. (2008). Making thinking visible. Educational Leadership, 65(5),
57–61.
13. Wenner, J. A., & Campbell, T. (2017). The theoretical and empirical basis of teacher leadership:
A review of the literature. Review of Educational Research, 87(1), 134–171.
14. Willison, J. W. (2018). Research skill development spanning higher education: Critiques,
curricula and connections. Journal of University Teaching and Learning Practice, 15(4), 1.
15. Willison, J., & O’Regan, K. (2007). Commonly known, commonly not known, totally unknown:
A framework for students becoming researchers. Higher Education Research & Development,
26(4), 393–409.
16. Willison, J. (2012). When Academics integrate research skill development in the curriculum.
Higher Education Research and Development, 31(6), 905–919.
17. Wilmore, M., & Willison, J. (2016). Graduates’ attitudes to research skill development in
undergraduate media education. Asia Pacific Media Educator, 26(1), 1–16.
18. Bandaranaike, S., & Willison, J. (2015). Building capacity for work-readiness: Bridging the
cognitive and affective domains. Asia-Pacific Journal of Cooperative Education, 16(3), 223–
233.
19. Bandaranaike, S., Snelling, C., Karanicolas, S., & Willison, J. (2012). Opening minds and
mouths wider: Developing a model for student reflective practice within clinical placements. In
Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Cooperative & Work-Integrated Education
(pp. 1–16). Istanbul, Turkey: Bahçeşehir University. Retrieved from https://www.waceinc.org/
bahcesehir2012/proceedings.html.
20. Shah, S., Missingham, D., Sabir, F., & Willison, J. (2018). Developing and connecting engineer-
ing skills for researching and problem solving. Journal of University Teaching and Learning
Practice, 15(4), 7.
21. Seebohm, M. (2014). Research Mountain Song. Retrieved from https://www.adelaide.edu.au/
rsd/schooling/early-childhood/.
22. Lingam, N., Sharma, L., Qaloqiolevu, J., Ramoce, W., Lal, S., & Rafai, R. (2017). i-Talitali
framework: Developing a model for engaged learning and teaching in the Pacific. In Conference
Proceedings of the International conference on Models of Engaged Learning and Teaching (I-
MELT). Adelaide, Australia: Univesity of Adelaide. Retrieved from https://www.adelaide.edu.
au/rsd/i-melt/papers/LingamIMELT2017paper.pdf.
23. Monash University (2018). The Digital Skills Development (DSD) framework. Retrieved from
https://www.monash.edu/__data/assets/pdf_file/0010/1652437/DSD-document.pdf.
24. Willison, J (2019). The Blended and Engaged Learning Zones (BELZ). Retrieved from www.
melt.edu.au.
25. Paiva, J., Morais, C., Costa, L., & Pinheiro, A. (2016). The shift from “e-learning” to “learning”:
Invisible technology and the dropping of the “e”. British Journal of Educational Technology,
47(2), 226–238.
26. Ain, C. T., Sabir, F., & Willison, J. (2018). Research skills that men and women developed at
university and then used in workplaces. Studies in Higher Education, 1–13.
27. Willison, J., Sabir, F., & Thomas, J. (2017). Shifting dimensions of autonomy in students’
research and employment. Higher Education Research & Development, 36(2), 430–443.
28. Willison, J., & Buisman Pijlman, F. (2016). Ph.D. prepared: Research skill development across
the undergraduate years. International Journal of Researcher Development 7(1), 63–83.
29. Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruc-
tion does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based,
experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 75–86.
References 27
30. Hmelo-Silver, C. E., Duncan, R. G., & Chinn, C. A. (2007). Scaffolding and achievement in
problem-based and inquiry learning: A response to Kirschner, Sweller, and Clark. Educational
Psychologist, 42(2), 99–107.
31. Gray, R. D., Drummond, A. J., & Greenhill, S. J. (2009). Language phylogenies reveal
expansion pulses and pauses in Pacific settlement. Science, 323(5913), 479–483.
32. Willison, J. W. (2000). Classroom factors affecting student scientific literacy: Tales and
their interpretation using a metaphoric framework (Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Curtin University, Perth, Australia). Retrieved from https://espace.curtin.edu.au/handle/20.500.
11937/268.
33. Neubauer, S., Hublin, J. J., & Gunz, P. (2018). The evolution of modern human brain shape.
Science Advances, 4(1), eaao5961.
34. Martin, L. D., & Bennett, D. K. (1977). The burrows of the Miocene beaver Palaeocastor,
western Nebraska, USA. Palaeogeography, Palaeoclimatology, Palaeoecology, 22(3), 173–
193.
35. Kornienko, T. V. (2009). Notes on the cult buildings of northern Mesopotamia in the Aceramic
Neolithic period. Journal of Near Eastern Studies, 68(2), 81–102.
36. Odling-Smee, F. J. (1995). Niche construction, genetic evolution and cultural change.
Behavioural Processes, 35(1–3), 195–205.
37. Patenaude, F. (1984). The ontogeny of behavior of free-living beavers (Castor canadensis).
Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 66(1), 33–44.
38. Latham, N. R., & Mason, G. J. (2008). Maternal deprivation and the development of stereotypic
behaviour. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 110(1–2), 84–108.
39. Jones, C. G., Lawton, J. H., & Shachak, M. (1994). Organisms as ecosystem engineers. In
Ecosystem management (pp. 130–147). New York, NY: Springer.
40. Hugueney, M., & Escuillie, F. (1996). Fossil evidence for the origin of behavioral strategies
in early Miocene Castoridae, and their role in the evolution of the family. Paleobiology, 22(4),
507–513.
41. Willison, J. (2013). Inquiring ape? Higher Education Research and Development, 32(5), 861–
865.
42. Styles, J. (2008). The fundamentals of brain development: Integrating nature & nurture.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
43. Tambini, A., Rimmele, U., Phelps, E. A., & Davachi, L. (2017). Emotional brain states carry
over and enhance future memory formation. Nature Neuroscience, 20(2), 271.
44. Leong, Y. C., Radulescu, A., Daniel, R., DeWoskin, V., & Niv, Y. (2017). Dynamic interac-
tion between reinforcement learning and attention in multidimensional environments. Neuron,
93(2), 451–463.
45. Humphrey, A. (2014). Beyond graphic novels: Illustrated scholarly discourse and the history
of educational comics. Media International Australia, 151(1), 73–80.
Open Access This chapter is licensed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0
International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits use, sharing,
adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate
credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license and
indicate if changes were made.
The images or other third party material in this chapter are included in the chapter’s Creative
Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not
included in the chapter’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by
statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from
the copyright holder.
Chapter 2
What Will We Use?
This chapter provides a deep and nuanced understanding of the Models of Engaged
Learning and Teaching. Together, these models comprise much of what we may
use across education to connect human brain development and education theory to
diverse practice. Each of the models shares the six facets of sophisticated thinking
that were foreshadowed in Chap. 1 through the analysis of Parachute, as well as the
consideration of learning autonomy that is detailed below. The focus of Chap. 1 was
on the role of education as a means of developing learners born from 2023 to 2030,
who will be the problem-solvers, critical thinkers and researchers leading the planet
from 2040. In order to achieve that purpose, the logic of Chap. 1 naturally led to the
need for a conceptual framework that is broadly representative, across many perspec-
tives, of learning and ideas about learning and teaching. Thus, Chapter 2 addresses
this identified need by presenting the MELT as a viable option for a conceptualisation
that we can use to span education and connect disparate parts.
In Sect. 2.2, this chapter presents an account from a university graduate
who developed MELT skills at university and used these after graduation. Then
Sects. 2.3.1–2.3.6 detail the six facets of MELT, including their affective domain,
guiding questions and descriptions, and learning autonomy. Section 2.3.7 considers
sophisticated learning as spiralling, recursive and messy and looks at the ethical
and social dimensions of MELT. Section 2.4 in conclusion, is about engagement,
adaptability, fluidity and ownership.
The MELT don’t provide grounds for a proposal that education should develop
new theorisations or characteristics. Rather, the aim is to consolidate the past
100 years of research and reflect human learning over the past 100,000 years in
the MELT. Taken individually, each MELT facet and the consideration of learn-
ing autonomy is, in many respects, common, unsurprising and even too familiar. To
reduce the effect of ‘familiarity breeding contempt’, the facets and learning auton-
omy are presented in a way that is intended to bring them to life, re-kindling a
sense of their importance and demonstrating their synergistic power when taken as
a whole. In order to achieve this, each facet and learning autonomy level is con-
sidered through the perspective of three educational contexts: primary schooling
(Place Value from the preface), middle years of schooling (Parachute from Chap. 1)
and a volunteering situation of a university graduate (Silver Fluoride, below).
Through these multiple perspectives, the richness and complexity of each MELT
facet and learning autonomy are partially revealed.
However, it is only possible to fully understand the MELT by perceiving the
resonant energy of the set of facets and the continuum of learning autonomy together.
This potential, this energy is more complex to convey. The stories give some idea
of how the facets and learning autonomy work together, functioning as indivisible
parts of one precious jewel. Just as no facet of a jewel exists in isolation, all the facets
of MELT work together to crystallise into something of shape and enduring beauty.
While the set of facets must be understood holistically, it can be helpful to delineate
sophisticated thinking into separate facets. MELT takes what often remains implicit in
education—the skills of sophisticated thinking—and makes them explicit. Learning
autonomy intersects with the six facets of MELT in unpredictable, nonlinear ways.
As noted in Chap. 1, the facets of MELT represent the ‘what’ of learning, while
the continuum of learner autonomy represents the ‘how’, that is, the way in which
the facets are operationalised by teachers and brought to life for students. In MELT,
learning autonomy is considered to be a ‘relationship’ word, connecting learners to
each other, to teachers and to the learning context.
This chapter pays more attention to the facet embark & clarify than to the other
facets. This is because it is difficult to convey the sense of purpose epitomised by
embark & clarify; students often find it hard to work out what their teachers want
them to do, or to work out what they themselves will pursue. This difficulty is partly
down to the fact that there are so many different ways to embark & clarify and the
subject and discipline differences of how to begin often eclipse the similarities of
complex starting processes. When learning something new, it is essential to begin
correctly and to become increasingly clear about one’s direction; the other five facets
of the MELT depend absolutely on getting the purpose right. Except in the case of
the simplest forms of learning, the embarking processes require clarification, often
many times, over extended periods. It can be necessary to re-embark on a project
time and again and re-clarify direction and purpose. The stories Place Value and
Parachute, from primary schooling and secondary schooling, respectively, and the
2.1 Introducing the Models of Engaged Learning and Teaching 31
The MELT began with a blank whiteboard and a question: what are we as teachers
doing when we facilitate sophisticated student thinking? The resources that Kerry
O’Regan and I used to answer this question were the literature and reflections on
our teaching of well-scaffolded inquiry-based learning [2]. The facets of the MELT’s
were informed by the Australian and New Zealand Institute of Information Liter-
acy (ANZIIL) standards [3], the Structure of Observed Learning Outcomes (SOLO)
taxonomy [4] and Bloom’s taxonomies [5, 6]. Bloom’s taxonomies consist of two
separate hierarchical frameworks with fundamentally different premises for the cog-
nitive domain [5] and the affective domain [6]. The values, attitudes and emotions
associated with the affective domain are embedded in each MELT facet. This means
that the MELT comprise one framework with cognitive domain and affective domain
integrated into the facets.
A crucial intentional structural feature of MELT is that it represents the affective
domain but does not elaborate on it. Affective aspects are presented using single-
word adjectives describing a learner, whereas cognitive aspects are described and
elaborated much more richly. This is not because the cognitive aspects are more
important than the affective aspects. Rather, it is because assessing affect is risky [7]:
assessing something so important yet difficult to probe can be counterproductive.
However, the affective aspects are pivotal for whether learning happens or not. For
example, students are more likely to learn if they are willing to take risks, where, for
example, a creative writing student who’s encouraged to experiment is more likely
to come up with an original metaphor.
The design intention for MELT was to enable a conceptualisation that ran from pri-
mary school to post-graduation [8]. Most educational continua represent an increase
from lower ability to higher, from less sophisticated to more sophisticated thinking.
Yet each student potentially engages in thinking that is sophisticated within the con-
text of their current educational level. Rather than describing an incremental, linear
type of improvement, Kerry and I perceived a need to describe a continuum that spi-
ralled so that it could represent learning from ECE to Ph.D. level, flowing throughout
the years of education. This is because any domain with a linear increase will strug-
gle to remain pertinent across the whole educational trajectory, and is therefore not
helpful for forging connections across education.
32 2 What Will We Use?
Table 2.3 Overlap between teacher scope for learning autonomy and the continuum of learning
autonomy
Scope for Learning Autonomy (provided by educators)
Prescribed Bounded Scaffolded Open-ended Unbounded
Emulate Improvise Initiate
Continuum of Learning Autonomy (what students do)
For each facet, consideration can be made for the level of educator-planned learner
autonomy, called the scope for learning autonomy, and the actual autonomy that stu-
dents work with, called simply learning autonomy, shown together in Table 2.3. This
delineation between teacher intention (scope) and actual student learning autonomy
is important for three reasons:
1. It clarifies the difference between teaching intentions and the lived reality of
students.
2. It provides insights into the process of differentiating the curriculum, so that each
student is more able to engage in the learning that they are ready for.
3. It challenges our concepts of assessment, including what is assessed and why.
This is because many assessments place a uniform demand on student learning
autonomy, as a result of which some students are under-prepared and others are
under-challenged.
MELT are able to connect ECE to Ph.D., across disciplinary and transdisciplinary
contexts, because the learning autonomy question is always pertinent across formal
education: ‘how much structure and guidance do students need?
in that country. The version of MELT that Kevin’s degree used was the Research Skill
Development (RSD) framework, and this influenced the terminology that Kevin uses
to describe his sophisticated thinking. Kevin’s account and the earlier stories Place
Value and Parachute are then used together to bring the seven core characteristics of
MELT to life across the years of learning.
Silver Fluoride
Before I left for Cambodia yes—because I actually took a silver fluoride which is a
product that we didn’t even actually come into contact with in the Bachelor of Oral
Health here. When we went for a placement for Canberra there was a tutor there that
was working with us and she used it [silver fluoride] because she trained I think in
Darwin or somewhere out and she was working out in the sticks and so they had that
as a prevention for decay and all that sort of stuff so they were using that product
a lot, but we didn’t come into contact with it because it stains teeth and there’s all
these things that they don’t like aesthetically so that’s why we weren’t using it, but
it’s like an amazing product because I was really looking into that because I thought
that might be really beneficial for Cambodia because they don’t get care often and
they’re considered more rural so I was doing a lot of research with that and I ended
up purchasing some and taking it over with me and I was using it when I was over
there [16].
The stories Place Value and Parachute, along with Kevin’s account in Silver Fluoride,
are used next to provide a deep understanding of each of the MELT facet’s verb
couplets, affective adjective, key question, detail and then a consideration of the
extent of learning autonomy for that facet.
Detail: Students respond to, or initiate their own, direction, and clarify it while
considering ethical, cultural, social and team (ECST) issues.
To embark is to begin the journey, but how do students clarify what their pur-
pose actually is? Do they know their starting point, where they are going and how
they might get there? There are many different ways of commencing. This variety
of starting points is not only due to the fact that different disciplines have different
processes and terminologies, but also because sophisticated thinking is non-linear; it
2.3 Student Experiences of MELT Facets and Autonomy 35
jumps around and is hard to capture. Embarking processes often require other facets
of MELT to be actuated before what is embarked on becomes fully clarified. For
example, in disciplines that start with a research question, where did that research
question come from? Who framed it, and how did that educator, student or team
come to that phrasing? Formal research questions sometimes start as questions of
speculation and wondering about possibilities, or some intellectual dissonance that
makes one curious. Sometimes the questions emerge as a synthesis of the litera-
ture, through conversations, or by clarification through teaching, and these typically
require all the facets of the MELT.
The intentionally general verb embark is the first facet listed here because it is a
logical starting point. However, it is frequently not the first facet that students begin
within their learning endeavours. Sometimes we stumble on an issue or idea, some-
times we are inspired by curiosity about a phenomenon or issue, and sometimes we
immerse ourselves in a lot of reading around a topic. Formal research questions are
typically a ‘re-embarking’. That is, they follow on from a research journey that has
involved much reading and synthesising of literature, observation, piloting, hypoth-
esising, discussing, reflecting and deciding about which of all possible questions is
the best one to pursue. The same is true for the processes of defining problems when
problem-solving, or clarifying what decisions need to be made in evidence-based
decision-making: the process of formulating a point of embarkation and clarifying
takes time. The point of departure is so complex that it commonly requires all six
facets of MELT to clarify and re-embark.
For students, a sense of purpose is vital in the voyage of discovery that we call
learning. However, a sense of purpose is difficult to nurture if learning is prescribed
by teachers, because compulsion or coercion increases the risk of compliance, where
the purpose is stated and injected rather than internalised by the learner themselves.
On the other hand, when students choose the direction of their own learning, there
are many complexities for them and their teachers to deal with if they are to become
clear about their purpose.
The central question for embark & clarify is ‘What is our purpose?’ The verbs
embark & clarify and the question are in the plural form because of the social dynamic
in teaching and learning. However, ‘What is my purpose?’ is the question for a student
to ask themselves if engaged in an individual project. For educators trying to launch
students into sophisticated (but guided) learning, the question is ‘How do I convey
the purpose to students?’ Like all other facets, the nature of this facet can sometimes
be better understood through the key question. Even better, the combination of the
process verbs, description, key question and affective domain descriptor together
provide a well-rounded sense of the parameters of a particular facet. To understand
the mentality behind MELT, it is essential to understand the characteristics of each
facet, for each MELT facet must be set into its context, and adaptations are typically
required. Nowhere is the need for adaptation truer than the word of embark, which
is almost always too general to be of direct use with students.
Embark emerged from the first ANZIIL standard, ‘Recognises the need for knowl-
edge’ [3], which is one of the dozens of ways of commencing complex thinking.
‘Embark’ includes asking, hypothesising, identifying problems or patient needs,
36 2 What Will We Use?
did the ancient Greeks contribute to trigonometry?’ may steer students away from
all the work that happened in the Middle East between the era of the last influential
Greek mathematicians (around 2,200 years ago) [18] and the mathematical concepts’
reception in Europe (about 900 years ago) [19]. The 1,300 years in between saw much
translation, development and addition of concepts by scholars in the Middle East and
India, so they ended up not just with the Greeks’ trigonometry, but also the Arabic–
Hindu number system (that is the same system used currently in the West, including
in Place Value), algebra and sophisticated trigonometry applications, all from the
Middle East and India [19]. A culturally aware question, instead of the one above,
would be “Who contributed to the development of trigonometry, and what were
their contributions?” This leaves the question open, rather than eliminating entire
cultures’ contributions. Of course, the process could be scaffolded: “What did the
ancient Greeks contribute to Trigonometry” could be asked first, then ‘What did the
Middle East contribute to Trigonometry?” Alternatively, different cultures could be
allocated to teams who would be asked to report back to the whole class.
This example necessarily throws up ethical dimensions too: is it ethical to give
no attribution of ideas to their developers? The wrong or incomplete attribution of
ideas could be seen as unethical, a form of plagiarism. Therefore, it makes sense that
ethical and cultural dimensions are located also in communicate & apply, where, for
example, citation issues may be portrayed as ethical and cultural issues, rather than
as a bureaucratic enterprise. So the ethical, cultural and social dimensions need to be
part of the product or outcomes of sophisticated thinking, but especially integral to
the learning processes of communicating & applying and embarking & clarifying.
In Parachute, Shelly decided to explore differences in parachute shapes, while
Katie emulated the teacher’s lead to explore differences in materials. Shelly in effect
self-identified ‘shape’ as her independent variable, and to do so required high levels
of cognitive ability. She also operationalised this independent variable using two
shapes: square versus round parachute. Katie, following a process that had been
modelled by the teacher, faced the difficult prospect of clarifying and understanding
the teacher’s intention in order to answer the question, ‘what is my purpose?’ Shelly
embarked by posing a research question, and then she seemed to formulate a null
hypothesis ‘If the key factors are the same, the parachute should come down at
the same rate’. However, in the simple structures of Year 8 science, students were
asked merely for an aim. Whether embarkation entails developing an aim, a research
question, a hypothesis, a null hypothesis or a combination of these, serious work is
typically required to conceptualise and articulate a way of embarking.
For Kevin in Silver Fluoride, embarking involved clarifying the aims of his visit
to Cambodia around the general idea of providing dental help to people who usually
received none, all on a volunteer’s budget. Embarking for him involved the process of
getting to the question ‘what would happen if I used silver fluoride?’ This question
was prompted by his memory of a placement experience from his degree where
a tutor had mentioned silver fluoride use with people in remote contexts. As his
bachelor’s degree did not cover silver fluoride formally, Kevin needed to be open-
minded in order to consider whether this compound could be relevant in the context
of a developing nation. The ethical and social issues were paramount, as dentists in
38 2 What Will We Use?
developed nations have stopped using silver fluoride due to cosmetic concerns [20].
But Kevin inferred that rural Cambodians would prioritise keeping their teeth over
concerns about stained teeth. Kevin could have been wrong, and this was an ethically,
culturally and socially charged endeavour in which he took a calculated risk.
The children in Place Value were in play mode around the tree, and were driven by
curiosity and mimicry of other children, engaging with the familiar and the unfamiliar.
The children were having fun doing what they wanted; the curriculum was unknown
to me as an observer, but the learning seemed to be substantial. The children were
also driven by an impetus towards togetherness, and this togetherness did not restrict
their individual exploration.
Some of those children were in the classroom the next day, and had to understand
the complex ideas underlying the mathematical concept of ‘place value’. However,
it was difficult for them to read the term correctly, let alone anticipate or deduce the
purpose of the activity. Nevertheless, the children were lively and enjoyed raising
their voices in a chant with the teacher. It is not clear whether students were able to
connect the original chant of ‘place value’ to the sequencing activity they engaged
in. But we will see in later facets how complex the ‘place value’ concept is, and how
the concept itself is a voyage of discovery requiring many points of embarking and
re-embarking before it can be fully grasped.
Embark & clarify, the MELT verb-pair denoting commencement, has proven
to be fit-for-purpose because it is necessarily broad. As such, it encapsulates the
many starting and re-starting points required for engaged learning and teaching, from
clarifying what a teacher wants, to determining what a patient needs, to selecting
independent variables, to wondering what to play with under a tree. You will see
in Chap. 3 that there are many ways educators and students have brought embark
& clarify to life, and that teachers choose verbs that are appropriate, context-based
ways of commencing. Example verbs that educators have used for ‘embark’ include
the following:
• Ask,
• Question,
• Specify aim,
• Determine goal,
• Define problem,
• Define specifications,
• Diagnose need,
• Identify issue,
• Recognise,
• Speculate,
• Immerse,
• Probe.
Affective aspects
Embarking is complex and, like all the facets, involves an interaction between the
cognitive and the affective. Shelly’s willingness to break away from the teacher’s
example was full of risks. Was she driven by some curiosity—provoked by an interest
2.3 Student Experiences of MELT Facets and Autonomy 39
in shapes or a TV show on parachute design? Was she driven by the prospect of getting
higher marks because she did not merely follow the teacher’s example? Whatever
drove her prompted a resilience, because throughout the difficult process, she kept
going.
It is possible that the children playing in Place Value barely noticed their learn-
ing, and it’s not likely that they consciously worked out what they were going to
play. Rather, their decision-making was intuitive, spontaneous and curiosity driven.
Open-slather learning can be bewildering, but somehow young children find it in
themselves to play and learn in very unpredictable ways. In contrast, the children
over seven playing nearby chose a fun activity (volleyball) with known and chal-
lenging parameters. There is a big difference between autonomous play and playing
a game with pre-set rules. In terms of measured outcomes, students may learn more
as a result of direct instruction than play, but that doesn’t mean that the learning
which comes from direct instruction is deeper, more comprehensive or more useful.
It may merely be more measurable. While the ability of students to propose research-
able questions is legitimately assessable, affective aspects of embark & clarify, like
‘curiosity’, are much more difficult to measure. This doesn’t make curiosity less
valuable or important; rather, it means that the complex and vital affective domain
needs to be thoughtfully taken into consideration by educators in all learning design.
In Place Value, the maths learning was at the opposite end of the autonomy spec-
trum to the play around the tree, with a specified curriculum and predetermined
answers. However, the teacher rarely corrected any child, instead gave cues like
pauses in pointing at numbers, and silences. Then she continued, asking, ‘is any
number in the wrong place?’ and gave students time to ponder. Perhaps most impor-
tant, she structured in opportunities for student self-correction later in the sequence of
events. She operated with tact, sensing that students needed feedback, but that feed-
back needed to be sensitive and non-accusatory in nature and, as much as possible,
to come from other students. Terms like ‘maths anxiety’ spring to mind, especially
if a child is struggling with more basic aspects of the lesson, like reading a num-
ber as ‘fourteen’ instead of ‘forty-one’. For the teacher, the affective dimension of
that lesson may have been something akin to ‘soothing’ or ‘reconciling’, rather than
curiosity.
In terms of ethical, social and cultural aspects, the teacher used the powerful
influence of spatial togetherness on the italitali mat and chanting of terminology to
weave a sense of social cohesion and support. For students, to even pronounce the
term ‘place value’ was challenging; initially, the students seemed to be confounded
by another maths term, reading the flashcard as ‘plus value’. Some of their earlier
experiences with pairs of numbers placed side by side would doubtless have involved
addition, which may have led to their confusion. So the knowledge base they needed
to draw on was shifting, becoming more sophisticated and more abstract. The concept
of ‘place value’ changes everything, and the students needed to work out what this
teacher wanted them to do with this place that was supposed to be so valuable, but
which maybe didn’t seem as valuable as their tree place by the water. The question
of what students ‘value’ influences every face-to-face class interaction, including for
students who prefer and value online worlds.
40 2 What Will We Use?
The affective descriptor for embark & clarify needs to be responsive to context.
For Kevin to begin to probe the merits of a medication which was not authorised
in his degree, he was driven by empathy for the people of rural Cambodia. Kevin
and eight other university students who completed the Bachelor of Oral Health were
interviewed one year after completion. Analysis of the interviews showed that for
them, embark & clarify was complex in the affective domain, initiated by numerous
and diverse interactions, and seemed to be much more nuanced and context-sensitive
than any other facet [21]. Sometimes, sophisticated thinking is driven by curiosity,
sometimes by empathy for patient needs and sometimes by passion for an issue [16].
This starting point may be initiated by a teacher, as shown in the class context of
Place Value, and in such cases, the challenge is to clarify what the teacher wants. In
cases where the starting point is initiated by a student, the process of clarification can
end up being even more complex, with students needing to fine-tune the question,
aim or need. In addition to curious, the study in the Oral Health context found other
affective descriptors that impelled graduates to embark: passion, ownership, interest
in learning, implications of not researching, boredom and the need to justify [16].
Therefore research is showing that embarking & clarifying has, to date, the most
complex cognitive and affective domains. An implication is that this facet requires
more context-specific modelling by teachers than currently occurs, more practice in
diverse contexts and more explicit connections between contexts. If that all were to
occur, then the metacognition of students in relation to how they embark will be
enhanced, and they will develop a sense of purpose for their own learning.
Einstein looked to the affective domain to explain his success as a learner: ‘I
am not especially gifted or talented. I am only very, very curious’ [22]. Einstein’s
self-characterisation is one reason that ‘curious’ is used as a placeholder for affect in
MELT. However, other affective descriptors are often more pertinent to the context
and should be chosen carefully by educators or students and used instead. At times,
identifying excess and deficit affect associated with each facet is useful, and for
embark & clarify these are obsessive and disengaged, respectively.
Autonomy
Each facet may place its own demands on students, experience by learning expe-
rience, task by task, assessment by assessment, project by project, including how
much autonomy is provided and how much each student operates with. The two sto-
ries and the graduate account are next compared and contrasted in terms of learner
autonomy in order to elucidate this concept. The children playing under the tree
embarked with a high level of autonomy, with no obvious supervision. They deter-
mined what they would play, where, with whom and for how long. But the next day
in the maths class, three of those same children were grappling with the teacher’s aim
for the lesson. Their embarkation was highly prescribed and required sophisticated
and abstract thinking. The students had an oral familiarity with numbers, but found it
difficult to get the right-to-left orientation correct. It was daunting for them to under-
stand what value a specific place or location gave a numeral compared to another
numeral. The class embarked on learning content knowledge specified by the teacher
and with which few students were familiar, and this required much clarification and
re-clarification.
2.3 Student Experiences of MELT Facets and Autonomy 41
Kevin determined to explore the value of silver fluoride without boundaries set
by others. The potential risks and benefits of the substance were his to weigh up, and
his innovative approach may have led to some severe problems. His decision to act
in such an autonomous way may have been driven by his empathy for the people, his
curiosity about a now-abandoned medical approach, or any number of other affective
factors.
Shelly took the opportunity to pose her own question, about parachute shape,
improvising in a way that was in keeping with the scaffolded scope provided, with
some teacher parameters and occasional guidance. Katie, in the same classroom,
emulated the teacher’s example and stuck to a prescribed question about parachute
material.
Shelly’s method of embarkation resulted in different ramifications to Katie’s.
Shelly’s own choice of independent variable led to unanticipated problems, making
engagement with the other MELT facets substantially more complex than it was for
Katie. The surprising element in Parachute is that Shelly seemed so clear so quickly
about her direction, and never wavered from it. Even though she experienced multiple
problems and hold-ups as she improvised and innovated, she actually submitted
her final report on time, along with others who emulated the teacher’s modelling.
For many students, regardless of educational level, this quick clarification would
not have happened, and they would consequently have failed to submit a report of
any substance. The pressures are often against a student to be too innovative, and
sometimes the perceived or actual risk causes students to emulate instead.
The two school stories and the graduate’s account show the diverse range of stu-
dent autonomy that was evident for embark & clarify. A similarly diverse range
of autonomy was also evident in the case of the other facets, as shown below.
Table 2.4 summarises the highest level of autonomy that was observed for each
facet as demonstrated by participants in the stories and account.
Table 2.4 The highest level of learning autonomy evident for each facet, story by story
Facet Place Value Parachute Silver Fluoride
Children Children in Shelly Katie Kevin
around tree class
Embark & Initiate Emulate Improvises Emulates Initiates
clarify
Find & Initiate Emulate Improvises Emulates Initiates
generate
Evaluate & Initiate Emulate Initiates Emulates Initiates
reflect
Organise & Initiate Emulate Initiates Emulates Initiates
manage
Analyse & Initiate Improvise Initiates Emulates Initiates
synthesise
Communicate Initiate Improvise Improvises Emulates Initiates
& apply
42 2 What Will We Use?
Within the same learning context, different students work at differing points on the
continuum of learning autonomy, regardless of teacher intention, and this is seen in
Parachute. Some students frequently seek out clarification, ask what is next, request
specified processes, and pursue a multitude of ways of working with the autonomy
akin to emulate, regardless of whether the teacher provides a learning task that is
engineered for students to improvise or initiate. In Parachute, Katie emulated the
teacher’s model for all six facets, while Shelly initiated evaluating, organising and
analysing and improvised in the other three facets. Same classroom facilitation, with
two very different student experiences. The same six-year-olds in Place Value were
playing highly autonomously one day and learning with prescribed facilitation the
next. This demonstrates that, in MELT, autonomy is not a characteristic that one
acquires, but something which varies according to the relationship of a learner to
teachers, other learners and the context.
Detail: Students find information and tools, and generate data/ideas using appropriate
methodologies.
The MELT facet find & generate is concerned with using methods to make avail-
able elements that we will use, as dictated by a particular purpose. These ‘elements’
could be the information or data needed to address a research question—tables of
results from published articles, for example, or data from one’s own experiments.
Elements could be physical or electronic tools, equipment or manuscripts. A chemist
once said ‘A couple of months in the chemistry lab can save you a couple of hours in
the library’ [23]. This facetious comment, written before the advent of the Internet,
is a reminder that seeking already-existing information can prevent the unnecessary
wastage of resources required to generate data. Seeking others’ information or gen-
erating one’s own data both require methodologies that are learned over time and
that are context-specific.
Kevin the graduate was prompted by his impending visit to consider whether silver
fluoride may be useful in Cambodian villages. With this purpose in mind, his self-
determined need was to look up product and usage information, find out about side
effects, and consider the costs, packing and customs requirements associated with
taking silver fluoride overseas. When in Cambodia, he also had to find patients whose
needs were well-suited to his treatment and not apply a one-size-fits-all strategy.
Shelly’s task looks more complex, as she needed to generate primary data and a
protocol to achieve this, as well as coming to the realisation that she had to control
the size of the parachute, and therefore use mathematical equations to calculate its
optimal area.
2.3 Student Experiences of MELT Facets and Autonomy 43
The six-year-olds in Place Value had to find the ‘place’ that bestows value on
numerals. For each two-digit number, the six-year-olds had to locate the numeral on
the left and interpret it differently than if it was by itself. This was not at all straight-
forward, as evidenced when the students read ‘forty-one’ as ‘fourteen’, and the group
of eight students inserted forty-one immediately after eleven in the sequence of num-
bers. Finding the right ‘place’ made all the difference in terms of determining the
correct number sequence, and the task was initially beyond the understanding of
almost the whole class. We wouldn’t normally think of reading two-digit numbers
as ‘finding’, but that may be because we forget how difficult the task is the first time
around, when reading numbers of more than one digit requires locating the left-hand
numeral in a two-digit number and assigning that number a special value. Locating
the place of value was potentially bewildering for those who did not know their left
hand from their right.
When three of the six-year-olds were playing around the tree the day before the
maths lesson, they had many shells, seeds and other objects to find. Sometimes, the
objects which they found prompted what they would do—a natural ‘object-based’
learning [24]. This type of learning was less about working out someone else’s
(cryptic) meaning about, for example, where ‘place value is found’. The type of
learning was more about a way of finding that gave way to more finding, as when
a child found a specific shell and suddenly noticed specimens of the same shell all
around them. The potential for finding was rich, with new things constantly being
washed up, blown in or dropped down. As the seasons changed, so did the weather,
the texture of the bark, and the character of nearby flowers and seeds. What was
findable in and under the tree provided a rich learning environment that prompted
the children to engage in multifaceted sophisticated thinking.
Affect
The place-holding affective adjective for find & generate is determined—a deter-
mined student pushing on to find relevant and useful information or to generate data.
Shelly epitomises this, as even in an eighty-minute episode she was fierce in her
determination to generate pertinent data despite the pressures of time, equipment
and lack of support. Shelly showed determination to not generate just any data, but
data that was trustworthy, as shown by her frequent writing, erasure and rewriting of
measurements.
For the children in Place Value, the safety of the tree provided many opportunities
to find and to generate ideas that they could further explore. There is a sense that
the ‘finding’ in this setting was expansive, in contrast with the classroom where
the finding process came across as almost pedantic. Under the tree, some children
moved from one activity to another, and you might call that ‘exploration’. But in
a classroom context, that same way of learning could be called ‘unfocussed’. How
did the children navigate affectively between these two learning worlds on a daily
basis? Did the classroom learning partially impel students, by the time they were
seven-year-olds, to play games with other people’s rules—like volleyball? Or is
the progression from unbounded play to play with boundaries just a natural part of
44 2 What Will We Use?
childhood development? Where the affective domain for find & generate is taken to
an extreme, it becomes pedantic; a deficit in this affect could be called slapdash.
Autonomy
The children in Place Value are again at opposite ends of the learning autonomy
continuum when tree-based finding is compared with classroom-based finding. In
the tree, what the children found was constrained only by what remained, what was
grown and what had recently washed up, dropped down and blown in, and so the
children improvised continuously in terms of find & generate. In the maths class,
their finding was prescribed down to a precise location: the number on the left, which
had a special value. In the classroom, there was no opportunity for the children to
improvise on that convention, with the expectation by the teacher that they would
emulate.
In Parachute, Katie emulated the teacher’s protocol and generated data in keeping
with prescribed learning autonomy. Shelly had some scaffolds set by the teacher,
but also had a high degree of independence within those scaffolds, improvising the
protocol to suit her questions and generating data as she went. Kevin innovated, with
the freedom to determine which product information or application processes he
needed to know about, including how he could get the silver fluoride safely overseas.
Detail: Students evaluate the credibility of sources, information, data and ideas, and
through reflection make their own learning processes visible.
The process of finding information or generating data naturally leads to questions
about the trustworthiness of that information or data. Should students believe others’
information, including the teacher’s, by default? A risk in the Information Age is
that students may become gullible consumers of information, and so the educative
process must include ways for students to learn to evaluate. Likewise, students should
not by default trust their own ideas or data, but should evaluate these and reflect on
the processes they use throughout their own learning, including self-reflection to
determine their own potential biases.
In Place Value, after the children called out ‘fourteen’ in response to the written
number ‘forty-one’, the teacher cued to students that there may be an error, by
repeating the first part of the number: ‘Four…’. A student then self-corrected with
this prompting, calling the number ‘fourteen’. When the class was prompted by the
teacher to indicate whether the initial number sequence was correct, many students
cried ‘no’, but no-one volunteered which number was incorrect until one girl standing
2.3 Student Experiences of MELT Facets and Autonomy 45
in the line of eight students pointed at ‘forty-one’. The teacher asked where ‘forty-
one’ should go. Many children pointed to the next position up, so the teacher prompted
the child holding ‘forty-one’ and the child holding ‘seventeen’ to shuffle and change
places.
It is one thing to know what is wrong, and another thing to make it right. The
evaluation process was incremental—students caused ‘forty-one’ to move to the
right-hand side of seventeen, but not past twenty or twenty-nine. It may have been
initially that the student holding the ‘forty-one’ saw it as ‘fourteen’, and so he was
correct in sequencing it between nine and seventeen. But with the realisation that it
was not fourteen but forty-one, there was no aspect that was correct in the sequence
‘17, 41, 20’. The salient aspect for evaluate is that the teacher prioritised evaluative
thinking by allowing the class to place forty-one incorrectly, knowing the contin-
uation of the exercise allowed for further correction. In other words, the teacher’s
Socratic questioning focused on the process of evaluation, not on getting the correct
answer. This approach can take some of the fear out of learning, if students can
incrementally correct rather than being corrected first-off. The teacher could also
have asked for reflection at the end of that task: ‘What was one number that started
in the wrong place and now is in the right place?’ and ‘What is more important—that
we got it right the first time, or got it right in the end?’ In that way, she would have
been helping students learn to be evaluative and reflective.
Kevin had to decide whether the silver fluoride’s negatives (such as staining of
teeth) were outweighed by the positives of tooth decay prevention. With this in
mind, his cost–benefit analysis for potential patients in Cambodia would be very
different from a cost–benefit analysis for urban dwellers in a developed nation. This
is because evaluation is context-sensitive, and in this case determined by cultural and
social factors, as well as by medical and economic ones.
In Parachute, Shelly engaged in evaluative thinking, measuring, writing, erasing,
re-measuring, rewriting and erasing numbers that represented her measured drop
times. When Mrs Breen asked, ‘Did you use several measurements?’ she was getting
at the underlying repeatability of measurements in science, not relying on a single
measure. This is a kind of incremental approach towards a ‘true’ value, where ran-
dom errors decrease as the number of measurements increases. Shelly answered ‘yes’
truthfully. And with her repeated rubbing-out of numbers, she indicated a dissatisfac-
tion with some of her measurements—an evaluative decision in which she was being
discerning. Perhaps this is because she realised a problem with the measurement,
such as stopping her timer too early or starting too late, or that the parachute didn’t
fully open. If that was the case, she was engaging in an evaluative search for accu-
rate measurements. It is also possible that she knew that measurements shouldn’t be
too spread out, which would indicate a lack of precision, and she wanted to elimi-
nate some recorded numbers that made her experiment seem to lack quality. Given
the rushed timeframe, whether she was evaluating the measures themselves, or the
appearance of her experiment, she demonstrated a high level of evaluative thinking.
Nevertheless, it was Shelly’s evaluative thinking in Parachute that shocked me.
How could a series of maths calculations leave her to critique not the actual numbers
she obtained, but the shortfall of materials (‘there isn’t enough’)? In the complexities
46 2 What Will We Use?
request for reflection on the process. The children in Place Value were probed with
Socratic questioning by their teacher to structure their thinking at the bounded level,
in order to enable their group evaluation. At times, one student’s evaluation became
a modelling for the others. The activity was more bounded than prescribed, because
students had several options when choosing what number was in the wrong place,
and then several options for where to move the numbers. Nevertheless, most students
showed evidence of emulating the teacher or experienced peers in terms of evaluation,
with only a few students improvising. Kevin’s own evaluative standards (prevention
of decay) and his decision to intentionally disregard certain criteria (aesthetics) all
emerged from his innovation and sensitivity to the context.
Detail: Students organise information & data to reveal patterns/themes and manage
teams and processes.
To organise information is to enable its effective use. Where there is a plethora
of information and data, organisational processes and formats help to enable sophis-
ticated thinking, especially analyses. Written, spoken and graphical conventions for
organising information and data enable subject-appropriate ways of communicating.
The management of resources (both physical and electronic), of people in teams and
of time all strongly influence how sophisticated student thinking may become.
The children in and around the tree in Place Value played until after sunset,
doing whatever they wanted in that timeframe. Learning wasn’t pre-planned; it was
managed by them—they organised their time, equipment, spatial arrangement and
the things they found. They could be up the tree, under the tree, or indeed anywhere
else, but perhaps something communal kept them within six metres of each other.
In the maths class, by stark contrast, there was strict management of the students:
the bell would ring one hour after recess; the teacher determined the time allocated
to whole-class tasks and then individual tasks; the space was teacher determined,
whether on the Pacific Island italitali mat, standing in a line in front of the class or at
individual desks. The tasks required pre-specified ways of organising the whole class
and individual activities. Organisation and management, as dictated by the teacher,
were directed towards achieving as much learning about place value as possible.
But how often were the students aware of teacher modelling and their own use of
managing and organising skills?
Shelly’s experience was closer to that of the children when they played outside
than their experience in the maths lesson. While she had about the same amount
of time as the six-year-olds in the classroom, between the teacher explanations,
48 2 What Will We Use?
clarifications and requirements, she had to determine exactly how to manage herself
and the experiment. For some aspects of organising the information, Shelly followed
a standardised structure that she had used before: title, aim, results, with observation
table and bar graph, and conclusion. However, for the table and graph, she had to
organise the data by determining what the table comprised, identifying the x-axis and
y-axis components of the graph, which required a degree of difficulty much higher
than previous work, and all in a rushed timeframe.
Kevin had to work out the constraints of international travel and the village sit-
uation himself, and that these constraints would determine what he could carry and
what he could manage to do. This included weight limits, knowing what goods could
be imported to Cambodia, and the equipment available. Every aspect of organise &
manage he determined and initiated.
Affect
Organise & manage can appear the least affective-oriented facet, but if students find
the whole learning process under-stimulating, tedious or overly complex, then poten-
tial learning gains may not eventuate, no matter the learning design used. Einstein
urged ‘Out of clutter, find simplicity. From discord, find harmony’ [25]. Organising
cluttered information and managing discord requires an affective element: harmonis-
ing. This affective element addresses the question, ‘how do we arrange?’ How do we
take cluttered, incoherent data or information and harmonise it in ways that enable
effective analysis? How do we manage our team to work harmoniously? In Place
Value, there are harmonious arrangements, with the children playing individually and
yet socially in the shared tree-space. The highly prescribed maths lesson presents the
harmonies of children on a pacific island mat chanting and learning together with the
teacher. By calling out eight children to the front of the class, rather than individuals,
the teacher managed the class harmoniously in the context of the cultural setting.
In Parachute, the time pressures brought discord to Shelly’s open-ended experi-
mentation, while Katie’s prescribed experiment was relaxed. Even if Shelly learned
more, what lesson did she walk away with? Given her level of anxiety, would her
learning in this lesson have been less about experimental design and more about
planning future work in which she could emulate the teacher’s example, like other
students?
Kevin was endeavouring to tread a fine line between the help that he hoped to give
and the actual helpfulness of his planning, knowing that success would depend on
how people in Cambodian villages actually reacted to stained teeth. That he used the
silver fluoride a lot when he was over there suggests that he began by anticipating a
high demand for the product, that he took a lot of it with him, and that this harmonised
with the reality of what the Cambodians in villages were willing to do to protect their
teeth.
Autonomy
Shelly struggled to squeeze her self-determined experiment into the 80 min allocated
to the lesson. Her time management was paramount, and at one point she created
for herself a resource management issue that didn’t exist: she decided that she didn’t
2.3 Student Experiences of MELT Facets and Autonomy 49
have enough material. She then spent time talking to me, trying to source more
plastic. Simultaneously, she was organising data into tables and graphs that reflected
her actual fields (time and shape) and measurements of time. Shelly improvised
organisational structures because not all options were fully open. For example, she
was provided with the structure of ‘aim, method, results and conclusion’, and was
aware that her teacher would mark her work according to (unknown?) criteria after
the class. But Shelly managed this experimental process amazingly well, to the extent
that she did finish and submit, albeit without the self-reflective part that her teacher
requested near the end of the vignette.
Manage & Organise were highly structured in the Place Value classroom, with
student bodily emulation of the teacher’s physical management. Innovation was
epitomised by the students by the seashore and by Kevin.
So, from the start of this activity in Place Value, there may have been some maths
anxiety, especially given that English was the second language for all the students,
so that their familiarity with number names would have been lower than if English
was their mother tongue.
Whilst we saw in Sect. 2.3.3 that the six-year-olds evaluated ‘forty-one’ as being
in the wrong place, this didn’t automatically entail that they could locate the right
place for the number. The students seemed to have trouble seeing the trend in numbers
as a whole and being able to place forty-one correctly after twenty-nine—in other
words, they struggled to analyse.
The teacher asked a question that seemed practical, but which actually required
analytical thinking. It was not a question which merely evaluated whether forty-one
was in an incorrect position; it also asked students to project the number to its proper
place: ‘So this forty-one should go where?’ For students to see the numerical trend,
to show analytical thinking, they needed to move forty-one up three places in the
line. However, the students indicated that it should go past seventeen only, and the
boy with forty-one moved up one place: ‘5, 9, 11, 17, 41, 20, 29, 57’.
If the process of evaluating forty-one’s position continued incrementally ‘it’s
wrong here…. It’s wrong here, ah, it’s correct here’, then it would have been a
trial and error process. This would be similar to (but more sophisticated than) the
evaluative thinking required to check whether a square peg fit into a round hole, and
then trying each subsequent hole until it fits. The teacher knew that forty-one was
still in the wrong place, but demonstrated pedagogical awareness that if she kept
on asking ‘are you sure?’ regarding forty-one specifically, she would be cuing that
forty-one was wrongly placed. Such a strategy may not even facilitate evaluative
thinking; it would merely train students to read the teacher’s cues.
The teacher instead asked, ‘Any other number is in the wrong place?’ Here, she
shifted the class focus from forty-one, towards looking at all the numbers afresh
and analysing the trend. One student evaluated ‘twenty-nine’ as being in the wrong
place. The teacher then asked a question about twenty-nine, the same question she
asked about forty-one. Again, in this context, this could have represented an attempt
to elicit analytical thinking: ‘Twenty-nine should go where?’
‘Besides seventeen’ one student called out—and the student with twenty-nine
shuffled left, passing forty-one and twenty ‘5, 9, 11, 17, 29, 41, 20, 57.
Indeed, this was the first time a student required a number to move two places at
once, and downstream at that. This one student had seen something in the trend (e.g.,
his internal reasoning may have been something like ‘twenty-nine is lower than forty-
one and higher than seventeen, so it should go between these two numbers’). This
process of seeing the trend rather than engaging in evaluative trial and error indicates
analytical reasoning. The sequence as yet was not perfected, but the student’s request
represented a major leap forward, from an incremental, evaluative process to a more
analytical one. The other students could see and hear this, and realised that they too
could do more than say ‘not here’—instead, they could project a number into its
correct location in the sequence. That leap of two places was a leap of insight for the
child, and may have triggered some new ways of thinking for other children.
2.3 Student Experiences of MELT Facets and Autonomy 51
Still, it is hard to see errors, let alone trends in numbers, and when the teacher
asked if there was any other number in the wrong place, the students called out
together, ‘No’. It may seem bewildering that thirty-one bright students ‘missed’ the
‘20’, especially after another number in the twenties (twenty-nine) was just moved.
‘Are you sure?’ she presses.
‘Twenty!’ a boy calls out
‘Right.’ Where should it go?
‘Besides seventeen’
Again, this child’s analysis went beyond seeing that twenty was in the wrong
position, into spotting the trend and placing the number in its correct place.
The students’ process suggested that some of the students could evaluate, on a
case-by-case basis, whether a number was in the right or the wrong position. But
to analyse entailed being able to move a number to its correct position, even if this
required big jumps. At the end of the activity, there were now two students who
showed evidence of being analytical, and they modelled this ability to the class.
In Parachute, Shelly worked out that there was a difference in parachute drop
times, and since she had controlled other factors, she inferred that the difference
was due to the parachute’s shape. She stated to Mrs Breen that ‘the round one takes
longer to hit the ground.’ To reach this analysis, Shelly shifted from one particular
observation (‘the round parachute in each drop took longer’) to a generalisation (‘the
round one [always] takes longer’). For her conclusion, she elaborated on that analysis
to form a full synthesis of her previous and current understanding of the nature of
science experiments, and of the analysis she conducted, to say, ‘If the key factors are
the same the parachute should come down at the same rate’. Because square versus
round parachutes didn’t have the same drop time, she synthesised her conclusion
as a generalisation: ‘the shape does matter to the time’. The finding that drop time
depended on parachute shape represented a substantial synthesis of empirically based
understanding, developed in an eighty-minute lesson by a thirteen-year-old student.
Kevin’s analysis included a conscious consideration of social, cultural and contex-
tual issues, in order to determine whether he was right in thinking that silver fluoride
may have been appropriate. There was also a technical side to his analysis—e.g.,
‘how poisonous is silver fluoride if ingested, or if it makes contact with skin?’ His
analysis may not have stretched to ‘how sustainable will this approach be for each
village?’ His final synthesis included the decision to take the compound, as well as
considerations on how much to take, and how he would store it while travelling and
when in the villages.
Affect: Creative
The Place Value maths activity was social, but frequently depended on an individual
to go against the consensus and call out an error. This may have meant that some
students who either saw a wrongly placed number, or who even saw where that
number should go may have been inhibited to say so, perhaps because of cultural
norms. The analytical learning evident in this activity seemed to rely on brave or
confident individuals. To understand deeply what ‘place value’ meant, students had
52 2 What Will We Use?
to be intuitive; students who were cognitively and affectively engaged in the task had
to be very creative to move from their original understanding to a newly constructed
understanding of the concept. This creativity was evidenced in the leap of imagination
required for one student to send one number multiple places downstream. Three
students from the class showed the creativity to conjure up different games from
scratch each day as they played by the seashore. Shelly’s analysis followed a scientific
protocol, but she creatively adapted it to her parachute experiment. Kevin showed
the creativity needed to think outside the content box of his study program. This
allowed him to consider, from a raft of potential contributions, what he could add to
oral health practice in a rural context.
Autonomy
In the Place Value maths lesson, the students’ analysis and synthesis were scaffolded
by the teacher, who put structures in place to help students analyse the numerical
trend and rearrange the numbers into their correct order. For some or most students,
this was too big a step. In the process, there was no modelling from the teacher. But
the teacher scaffolded the task so that students modelled to other students the process
of analytical thinking. Such peer-to-peer modelling was envisaged by Vygotsky in
the ZPD, and peer teaching is often regarded as more conducive to student learning
than teacher talk [26].
Kevin’s initiation of analysis was unbounded by any supervisory presence, but this
unboundedness didn’t mean ‘anything goes’. This was especially true in terms of his
patients’ health, which was in his silver fluoride-laden hands. If Kevin, for example,
ignored or overlooked a health warning, he may have endangered his patients. If
applied inexpertly, the treatment may have been ineffective or counterproductive.
Even though Kevin may have been working outside the boundaries of a specific health
system, his status as a professional required him to be aware of how ‘unbounded’
his work was, and therefore to redouble caution, checking and rechecking dosages.
Shelly initiated her own analysis & synthesis and this was in keeping with the
teacher’s more open-ended intention, and so the teacher had no preconceived answer
for Shelly’s self-generated question. The teacher could check the process and the
steps which Shelly used to get to her conclusion, but could not rightly correct the
findings themselves.
Detail: Students apply their understanding and discuss, listen, write, perform,
respond to feedback and present processes, knowledge and implications while
heeding ethical, cultural, social and team (ECST) issues and audience needs.
2.3 Student Experiences of MELT Facets and Autonomy 53
This excerpt is cryptic at first glance. But it closely follows the ‘null hypothesis’
strategy used commonly in science, whereby scientists try to show that there is no
statistical difference, after which the discovery of a difference indicates that there
must be a genuine difference in effect due to the independent variable. Shelly merely
took averages, which are very weak descriptive statistics. Nevertheless, she adopted
the persuasive grammatical structures of science to communicate in writing. How-
ever, she had made a similar point earlier when talking to Mrs Breen, in a way that
was much easier to understand: ‘the round one takes longer to hit the ground.’ The
report is much more science-speak than her oral communication, and as her main
communication product, the report goes beyond communicating results and demon-
strates her findings using scientific discourse. In choosing a communication style
that was appropriate to the situation, Shelly did something that is vital in all learning
contexts. The fact that different styles and modes are required in different settings
suggests that ‘communication’ is not a singular skill. Rather, it comes in multiple
forms, each of which must be learned and incorporated into a personal repository of
styles to be drawn-upon as context demands.
When Kevin communicated with Cambodians in the village, he wanted them to
have confidence that what he was offering would work. But his audience may not
have wanted or understood a scientific discourse. In this situation, apply may have
seemed more important to Kevin than communicate: ‘I thought that might be really
beneficial for Cambodia because they don’t get care often.’ He wanted and found
2.3 Student Experiences of MELT Facets and Autonomy 55
something that worked, and was driven to be constructive, to build up. He anticipated
linguistic barriers, but knew that the care he could give would speak volumes.
For Kevin, communication included a willingness to listen to someone with an
experience that differed from his, someone with rural experience, and this included
practice that was contrary to standard practice in his degree. He used the silver
fluoride ‘a lot over there’, implying that he persuaded people in the village to tolerate
stained teeth in order to keep their teeth longer. Clearly, his intention was to be
constructive. This is where the ethical, cultural and social dimensions really kick
in. He applied the idea of silver fluoride, evaluated his use of it with people and
decided to continue to use it a lot. Communication and application were a type of
endpoint for his sophisticated thinking. But they are also a part of the process from
the beginning, including listening, finding out what was currently available and what
was applicable.
Affect: Constructive
The default affective adjective in MELT for this facet is constructive, used in the
colloquial sense of ‘you’re a constructive person’ or ‘that’s a constructive comment’.
A constructive student or graduate desires to build up rather than self-promote in
their applications and communications. Shelly endeavoured to not only be intrepid,
but to craft a quality finding.
Kevin was a constructive graduate who volunteered, prepared and purchased
equipment using his own money, well in advance. He communicated and maybe
persuaded as he went, and applied his knowledge with the intention to add value
to the lives of people who were otherwise unreached by dental services. In doing
so, he personified the end-game of research and problem-solving: ‘go to where the
silence is and say something’ [27]. Of course, it may be that having committed to
silver fluoride, Kevin was doggedly committed to use it or get rid of it. There is the
possibility that he foisted the product on a more vulnerable population, over-riding
their aesthetic concerns. Kevin, and each of us, ethically and culturally must priori-
tise others and not use coercion to peddle wares or to enforce our own values, like a
mini-colonisation.
The children in Place Value were preoccupied with play in the tree, and in that
setting they entertained themselves without needing any adult resources. The eight
children in line applied their understanding of place value and communicated this
to the whole class. Their willingness to do this and to be critiqued enabled an entire
constructive dialogue to be engineered by the teacher.
Autonomy
That communicative line-up in Place Value had predetermined boundaries, and even
with one ultimate correct answer, the scope was there for incremental improvement.
Giving the children some room to move enabled substantial learning. Kevin and
the children under the tree had unbounded autonomy to communicate and apply.
Shelly faced a mixed bag, with the prescribed structure of a lab write-up and her
own innovations to report, whereas Katie stayed within the modelling provided,
successfully emulating the teachers’ lead.
56 2 What Will We Use?
Sections 2.3.1–2.3.6 are arranged in a logical and coherent order and present as a
more linear representation of the MELT facets. However, the reality of sophisticated
learning is that it is messier than a sequence. At times, several facets occur simulta-
neously, or facets like evaluate and reflect are tightly coupled to every other facet.
Simultaneously, purpose drives everything, frequently requiring a return to embark
& clarify. It is to capture the non-sequential nature of sophisticated learning that
MELT uses the word facet: It’s as if there are six faces of one precious learning
jewel, where different facets are emphasised depending on which way one holds the
jewel, yet all co-exist simultaneously.
There are different practical ways that facets may interconnect or be emphasised
in MELT. The ethical, cultural, social and team considerations are evident in embark
& clarify and in communicate & apply, and as noted earlier this is the only repeated
element in the explicit articulations of MELT. This double-mention was seen to be an
essential emphasis that needed to be articulated explicitly in those facets often seen at
the beginning and end of engaged learning. However, when students think in ways that
are sophisticated for them, all facets are enacted continuously throughout the whole
process. Most noticeably this occurs with evaluate, where questions, information,
management and communication are always evaluated, or should be, and at times
analysis is inseparable from evaluative process. The pentagon formulation of MELT
also urges ‘when in doubt, return to the centre’ to embark & clarify ‘what is our
purpose?’ This acknowledges the recursive, messy aspects of sophisticated thinking
and the strong need for learner clarity, or more precisely, for learners to develop
clarity of purpose. However, Chap. 3 provides examples where educators use the
pentagon configuration and place one of the other five MELT facets in the centre,
and this matches their emphasis for sophisticated thinking.
The cognitive and affective are not separated in MELT in the way that they were
in Bloom’s taxonomies. Separating these two sides of the same facet can provide a
licence to prioritise one over the other. However, a challenge of modern education
is to ‘cover’ the essential knowledge and skills for contemporary life, but in a way
that provides at least enough motivation for students to acquire these, a challenge
that requires simultaneous cognitive and affective considerations. I suggest that the
above sections unpack some of the complexities of the processes in each story and
account, and as educators, one of our challenges is to understand the complexity
for students of affectively engaging in sophisticated learning. In all of the research
on MELT implementation to date, student need and employ all six facets in activ-
ities that require anything more than dialogic-reporductive learning, and that such
learning is always multifaceted needs further research. If the facets of MELT are not
explicitly facilitated, at appropriate times, then the risk is under-developed student
thinking being applied across the years of education. Therefore, another challenge is
to facilitate the development of all of the facets that students need, in consideration
of context, content knowledge and diversity of learners.
2.3 Student Experiences of MELT Facets and Autonomy 57
In Sect. 2.2, the more cognitive descriptions of MELT were co-presented with the
un-elaborated affective domain, for these dimensions of learning mutually reinforce
and co-exist. When designing any learning tasks (including assessment), educators
need to consider affect, so that motivational elements in learning tasks are integral to
curriculum and lesson design. But this does not mean the affective domain needs to be
assessed, and the MELT’s single-word affect descriptors are a type of warning: don’t
treat the affective domain in the same way as the cognitive domain. The affective
domain is crucial because it is salient to ethical, cultural and social considerations
for engaged teaching and learning.
As with all MELT terminology, the affective adjectives are context-sensitive, and
should readily be adapted by educators to fit that context. For example, curious may
be, at times, a word that is contrary to ethical and moral imperatives, and a word
like empathetic may be more appropriate in the context of solving people’s problems
or dealing with patients. Kevin may have been curious to see if silver fluoride was
effective, but hopefully his primary driver was empathy for people who had minimal
access to dental care. In applying the affective descriptor for communicate & apply––
constructive––it is important to pay heed to ethical and cultural issues. Some people
may feel that ‘constructive’ contains overtones of colonisation, echoing Rome’s
mission to ‘civilise’ the world. In being constructive, one should avoid foisting one’s
own solutions or morals onto others; the ECST considerations are designed to check
such impulses. If Kevin’s silver fluoride solution proved to be socially or culturally
inappropriate when he was in a village, he would need to heed this rather than
persuading people with a medicine-only orientation, while defending himself as
being constructive.
own learning as a whole. All of this is part of building and internalising their own
personal models of learning, which will become a thinking routine that can guide
them across their education.
Chapter 3 contains diverse examples of the explicit use of MELT. These examples
demonstrate how different groups have taken the ideas presented here, adapted them
and used them to help students understand what is going on in their learning. This
level of fluidity is liberating and demanding. As MELT is not a generic model that
guarantees effective learning through an off-the-shelf solution, but rather requires
significant input to reformulate and re-articulate, it places demands on teachers. This
is why the examples in Chap. 3 are so helpful: there is no need to re-invent the wheel
if you can find and adapt from contexts close to yours. Educators making choices on
how to adapt MELT to fit specific contexts and what to emphasise have a liberty that
defers to their professional judgement. However, they cannot ignore that we all need
to be better informed about how to enable sophisticated, multifaceted thinking.
References
1. Einstein Bubble in Cartoon: Bucky, P., Einstein, A., & Weakland, A. (1992). The private Albert
Einstein. Kansas City, Missouri: Andrews and McMeel.
2. Willison, J. W., & O’Regan, K. (2005). 2020 Vision: An information literacy continuum for
students primary to postgraduation. In Research and development in higher education: Pro-
ceedings of the higher education research and development conference. Sydney, 3–6 July,
2005.
3. Bundy, A. (Ed.). (2004). Australian and New Zealand Information Literacy framework: Prin-
ciples, standards and practice (5th ed.). Adelaide, Australia: Australian and New Zealand
Institute for Information Literacy.
4. Biggs, J., & Collis, K. (1989). Towards a model of school-based curriculum development and
assessment using the SOLO taxonomy. Australian Journal of Education, 33(2), 151–163.
5. Bloom, B. S. (1956). Taxonomy of educational objectives. Vol. 1: Cognitive domain. New York,
NY: McKay.
6. Krathwohl, D. R, Bloom, B. S., & Masia, B. B. (1964). Taxonomy of educational objectives.
Vol. 2: Affective domain. New York, NY: McKay.
7. Pring, R. (1971). Bloom’s Taxonomy: A philosophical critique (2). Cambridge Journal of
Education, 1(2), 83–91.
8. Willison, J., & O’Regan, K. (2007). Commonly known, commonly not known, totally unknown:
A framework for students becoming researchers. Higher Education Research and Development,
26(4), 393–409.
9. Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
10. Willison, J., Sabir, F., & Thomas, J. (2017). Shifting dimensions of autonomy in students’
research and employment. Higher Education Research & Development, 36(2), 430–443.
11. Willison, J., & O’Regan, K. (2005). 2020 Vision: An information literacy continuum for
students primary school to post graduation. In Proceedings from the HERDSA Conference.
Sydney, Australia: Higher Education Research and Development Society of Australasia.
Retrieved from http://www.herdsa.org.au/publications/conference-proceedings/researchand-
development-higher-education-higher-education-121.
12. Dewey, J. (1908). What does pragmatism mean by practical? The Journal of Philosophy,
Psychology and Scientific Methods, 5(4), 85–99.
13. Hackling, M. W., & Fairbrother, R. W. (1996). Helping students to do open investigations in
science. Australian Science Teachers’ Journal, 42(4), 26–33.
References 59
14. Wood, D., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17(2), 89–100.
15. Willison, J., Peirce, E., & Ricci, M. (2009). Towards student autonomy in literature and
field research. In H. Wozniak, & S. Bartoluzzi (Eds.), Proceedings of the Higher Education
Research and Development Society of Australasia National Conference: The Student Experi-
ence (pp. 483–491). Darwin, Australia: Higher Education Research and Development Society
of Australasia. Retrieved from http://www.herdsa.org.au/publications/conference-proceedings/
research-and-development-higher-education-student-experience-72.
16. Willison, J., Zhu, X., Xie, B., Yu, X., Chen, C., Zhang, D, Shashoug, I. & Sabir, F. (in press).
Graduates’ affective transfer of research skills and evidence based practice from university to
employment in clinics. BMC Medical Education. https://digital.library.adelaide.edu.au/dspace/
bitstream/2440/92390/3/hdl_92390.pdf
17. Harari, Y. N. (2018). 21 lessons for the 21st century. New York, NY: Spiegel & Grau.
18. Seidenberg, A. (1961). The ritual origin of geometry. Archive for History of Exact Sciences,
1(5), 488–527.
19. Lyons, J. (2011). The house of wisdom: How the Arabs transformed Western civilization. New
York, NY: Bloomsbury Publishing USA.
20. Crystal, Y. O., Janal, M. N., Hamilton, D. S., & Niederman, R. (2017). Parental perceptions and
acceptance of silver diamine fluoride staining. The Journal of the American Dental Association,
148(7), 510–518.
21. Wilmore, M., & Willison, J. (2016). Graduates’ attitudes to research skill development in
undergraduate media education. Asia Pacific Media Educator, 26(1), 113–128.
22. Isaacson, W. (2007). Einstein, his life and universe. London, England: Simon and Schuster.
23. Crampon, J. E. (1988). Murphy, Parkinson, and Peter: Laws for libraries. Library Journal,
113(17), 37–41.
24. Chatterjee, H. J. (2010). Object-based learning in higher education: The pedagogical power of
museums. University Museums and Collections Journal, 3, 79–81.
25. Brown, P. (1993). Managing your time. Cambridge: Daniels Publishing.
26. Casey, G. (2013). Building a student-centred learning framework using social software in
the middle years classroom: An action research study. Journal of Information Technology
Education: Research, 12, 159–189.
27. Goodman, A. (2010). Independent media in a time of war. Sacred Heart University Review,
24(1), 1.
Open Access This chapter is licensed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0
International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits use, sharing,
adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate
credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license and
indicate if changes were made.
The images or other third party material in this chapter are included in the chapter’s Creative
Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not
included in the chapter’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by
statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from
the copyright holder.
Chapter 3
How Do We Arrange?
One of the most common questions is ‘how do others use the MELT?’ When turning to
the MELT, educators want more than a philosophy; they are looking for a framework
that can give tangible starting points for facilitating sophisticated learning. This
chapter provides examples of the ways that others have arranged MELT, to inspire
educators to adapt the MELT to diverse contexts. Inspiration is provided through the
diversity of contexts and approaches, rather than a narrow range of age- or content-
specific resources.
Specifically, in this chapter shows how educators have used their own adaptations
of the MELT to benefit student learning, with examples and links applicable to
early childhood, primary, secondary, technical education, undergraduate, course-
based master’s, and doctoral programmes. There are also examples that span across
disciplinary learning and transdisciplinary projects, and those that are aligned to
Direct Instruction or to discovery learning.
In terms of modelling the MELT facets, they maybe taught and learned in the
sequence presented in Chap. 2. However, in reality, sophisticated learning is fre-
quently non-sequential, messy and recursive. A linear, sequential approach can be
used early-on with students in highly prescriptive activities, and in contexts where
they have little experience: this is the case whether in primary, middle and secondary
school, undergraduate, Master’s and sometimes the early months of Ph.D. studies.
Once students can begin to make some decisions and display autonomy in their
learning, they will employ the facets non-sequentially.
The figures below are screenshots of resources available online that are presented
not to read in themselves, but to refer to the associated weblink.
For ease of access, the following sections are arranged according to educational
levels. However, approaches used at one educational level maybe pertinent to other
contexts. It is advisable to consider scanning several examples.
The approaches used are for:
• Early childhood—five-year-olds: song, rhythm and movement
• Year/Grade 4/5: problem-based learning pentagon for teacher planning
• Year/Grade 6: interactive introduction to investigation framework for direct student
use in term-long projects, with issues chosen by students
• Year/Grade 7: interactive introduction to investigation framework for student direct
use in guided transdisciplinary projects
• Year/Grade 8: interactive introduction to project-based learning pentagon for direct
student use in an intensive three-week STEM project posed by an industry partner
• Year/Grade 9/10: interactive introduction to MELT for direct student use in three-
term projects with issues chosen by students
• Technical and Further Education
• First year of university: interactive introduction to RSD and use across two
consecutive terms; multiple assignments in terms of marking criteria and feedback
• Second year of university: Human Resource Management
• Honours year: Medical Science
3.2 Many Models Across Educational Levels and Contexts 63
• Master’s year 1: interactive introduction to RSD and use across two consecutive
assignments, in terms of marking criteria and feedback.
• Master’s year 2: student self-assessment using RSD in the early phase of the major
research project (one semester full-time)
• Doctoral studies: self and supervisor assessment of the proposal.
In addition to this example based on a folk song, another ECE version of MELT
in action (based on a nursery rhyme) is on the website at www.melt.edu.au.
In this example, and some of the others below, there is no explicit mention of
autonomy. Autonomy in these examples is an aspect for teachers who apply profes-
sional judgement in considering ‘how much structure and guidance do these students
need?’ and whether to introduce ideas around autonomy or not.
Year 4/5 teachers involved in a Science Technology Engineering and Maths (STEM)
initiative at a government Primary School transformed the MELT into a Problem
Solving Pentagon. The school had been introduced to an engineering design frame-
work, the terminology of which was used to inform the Problem Solving Pentagon.
The teachers used this for their own thinking about design in their lesson planning,
but not explicitly to facilitate student learning. However, the motivation to develop
the model was to facilitate student engagement in intentional learning in STEM
(Fig. 3.2).
Table 3.1 The international baccelaureate’s approaches to learning, mapped onto the MELT facets
(see www.wcpss.net/Page/15023)
Approaches To learning MELT facet
Collaboration skills Explicitly mentioned in embark & clarify, organise & manage,
analyse & synthesise, and communicate & apply
Communication skills Communicate & apply
Organisation Organise & manage
Reflection Evaluate & reflect
Information literacy The six facets of MELT are based on six Information Literacy
Standards []
Critical thinking The six facets, modified as appropriate, represent the breadth of
critical thinking
Media literacy The six facets, modified as appropriate, represent the breadth of
media literacy
Affective Each facet has an affective component that is integral
Creative thinking ‘Creative’ is positioned as an affective side to ‘analyse and
synthesise’
Transfer ‘Apply’ overlaps with this
66 3 How Do We Arrange?
Fig. 3.3 The Inquiry framework (IF), used directly with Year 6 students, so they can improve their
understanding of their learning and communicate their reflective thinking. https://www.adelaide.
edu.au/melt/k-12-education#primary
requested to better assist students to directly engage with, and become increasingly
aware of, their own approaches to learning and how to represent that learning in an
assessable ‘Process Journal’ (Fig. 3.3).
engage in hands-on labs to foster literacy—reading and writing. In effect, the strat-
egy provides a platform where an individual student’s preferences and strengths
maybe used to address areas of weaker ability.
Specifically, writE science resources were developed and used across school
terms, where these worksheets were applied each week to model and scaffold the
skills that students need to gain so they can work towards carrying out an open-
ended inquiry. Initially, writE resources present prescribed interventions early in the
first term. These worksheets guide students through bounded, scaffolded, and then
open-ended activities. In the last three weeks of term, students engage in inquiry
projects.
Subsequently, a similar structure has been used at other levels, because the design
is adaptable and widely-valued. This application has supported learning for first year
university students, master’s students, and Year 2 primary students.
The following writeE worksheets illustrate how much detail, examples, help and
modelling students may need to learn to be able to observe (generate data), to reflect,
to analyse and to organise. The prescribed end of the autonomy continuum is as
enduringly important for student learning as the unbounded end. As indicated in
the resource below, it is valuable to provide examples of some of the scaffolding
processes from prescribed to open-ended, so that students will be able to improve
and work towards performing a skill.
The left-hand screen grabs are provided as images, not to be read, to give you an
overarching sense of the process used. The resources are available at https://www.
adelaide.edu.au/rsd/schooling/secondary/resources/.
Observational skills are facilitated in this writE lab with a focus on four senses. In
science labs, teachers often ask ambiguous questions, such as ‘what happens to the
popcorn when heated?’ The question is ambiguous because there is a big difference
between observations about ‘what happens to popcorn while it is being heated?’ and
‘what happens to popcorn after it has been heated?’ Requiring students to write down
observations of an initial state helps them generate a baseline from which to compare.
This process goes beyond mere observing; it is the generation of data. Following a
focus on observation skills, write Science sheets are used for students to infer, iden-
tify independent, responding and controlled variables, hypothesise and pose research-
able questions. These resources together develop student sophisticated thinking in
a science lab context towards a culminating lab experience; designing their own
experiment in writE Science 10.
3.2 Many Models Across Educational Levels and Contexts 69
writE 10
In an open-ended group project (writE 10),
students generated their own research
questions as a culmination of their saffolded
learning. This sheet was the last in the
sequence, built on science-specific skills, and
revisited all six facets time and again.
In this case, students were provided with workshops that introduced each school’s
version of MELT, often named the Investigation Framework, for Year 9 or 10. The
introduction required students to reveal the sophisticated thinking they used in a
highly interactive learning task run during the workshop. Based on their own ways
of explaining their thinking, the six facets were introduced and mapped onto their
own thinking, so that the students could connect with the wording (Fig. 3.4).
Another approach provides a MELT pentagon which explains the facets in a
rudimentary way. Then, students are invited to apply the six facets to a scenario,
such as a climactic scene in a widely-viewed movie like Apollo 13. Equipped with
this group practice of mapping MELT to skills used in the movie, students use the
MELT to plan for and reflect on what they will need to learn in order to complete
their sustained transdisciplinary projects.
In Technical and Further Education, the Innovative High Achieving Template for
Enhancing Maths was developed as a tool for enhancing learning and support-
ing numeracy skills (Fig. 3.5), with its consoling motto, ‘Keep calm, and carry a
pentagon’.
3.2 Many Models Across Educational Levels and Contexts 71
Fig. 3.4 Skills that students identify that they use to engage in inquiry mapped onto MELT facets.
Right column—student inventory of skills used during a learning activity in a workshop. Left column
skills are matched with the MELT facets on the left
Fig. 3.5 The innovative high achieving template for enhancing maths. https://www.adelaide.edu.
au/melt/conferences/short-papers-arranged-by-theme#keep-calm-and-carry-a-pentagon
72 3 How Do We Arrange?
3.2.7 Undergraduate
Fig. 3.6 First year human biology diagnostic task: Early in the first semester, students were given
two sources (left) from which they were required to take hierarchically structured notes (right). These
notes were assessed according to the six facets as used in a task-specific marking rubric (Fig. 3.7).
3.2 Many Models Across Educational Levels and Contexts 73
Fig. 3.7 The rubric structure framed by the six MELT facets was used consistently for assessments
throughout two consecutive semesters. The rubric below is specific to a bounded investigation, and
Fig 3.9 is for an open-ended investigation, where the facet similarities allowing students to connect
the skill set they have been building throughout the year due to the consistent use of MELT facets
74 3 How Do We Arrange?
Fig. 3.8 Students engaged in an open-ended group project gather life data from tombstones to
address their own research question
delineating student autonomy. The rubric below is for an open-ended activity, and
so delineates learning autonomy into four levels, in the context of the competence
expected in first year biology (Fig. 3.9).
Outcomes of this style of use of the RSD have shown substantial benefits for
university students in a variety of disciplines and universities [2].
First Year Mechanical Engineering: Optimising Problem Solving Pentagon
In 2014, upper-level undergraduate mechanical engineering tutors (University of
Adelaide) found that their first year students were not enthusiastic about learning to
communicate in a course that focussed on graphic, written and spoken communica-
tion skills. These tutors, themselves second to fourth year undergraduate students,
took the broad MELT parameters and created the first pentagon version, for direct
use with the first year students they sought to support. Since that version of MELT,
many of the models are introduced in a pentagon configuration especially when the
explicit representation of students’ autonomy may detract from the learning priorities
(Fig. 3.10).
Further examples are similar in principle to the human biology rubric above, and
include second year, third year and honours learning activities. Many details relating
to these activities can be found on the RSD website www.rsd.edu.au.
Outcomes of the use of OPS have shown substantial benefits for First Year
Engineering students [7].
3.2 Many Models Across Educational Levels and Contexts 75
Fig. 3.9 MELT facets frame the assessment rubric for open-ended human biology field research
conducted in first year university
The RSD framework, the first of the MELT has been used to assist in evaluating
learning that takes place in industry settings. However, since the terminology of
research may not resonate with most employers, Sue Bandaranaike from James Cook
University adapted the RSD to develop the Work Skill Development (WSD) frame-
work. Sue Bandaranaike envisioned that the WSD would be used with students and
their employers during co-ops, internships, and other work placements—collectively
called Work Integrated Learning. WSD use in employment contexts has led to a num-
ber of benefits for students, especially their capacity to articulate employability skills
[8] (Fig. 3.11).
MELT has been used in assessment orientations, in ways similar to first year
biology examples. Numerous resources, such as examples, tools, descriptions and
peer-reviewed journal articles, are available on a master’s-specific subsite of the
MELT site https://www.adelaide.edu.au/melt-1.dev.openshift.services.adelaide.edu.
au/melt/university-learning#masters-by-coursework.
3.2 Many Models Across Educational Levels and Contexts 77
Two transdisciplinary contexts at the master’s level that use MELT-informed rubrics
include addiction studies and climate change [11]. MELT characteristics provided
advantages for facilitating fluid conversations across disciplinary boundaries.
Monash University adapted MELT to be used across disciplines in terms of Digital
Literacy, and produced the Digital Skills Development framework (Fig. 3.13).
MELT characteristics pertain to a broad range of teaching and learning contexts, from
early childhood to early career research. However, some contexts have requirements
that may fall outside of typical educational settings, but which can still benefit from
applying MELT characteristics. One example, provided by the University of Ade-
laide, consists of a MELT version that has been tailored towards the Peer Assisted
Study Scheme (PASS, developed at the University of Wollongong). PASS leaders
3.3 Outside the MELT Parameters 79
started with MELT, and adapted it to include new parameters which were better-suited
to their sessions than the existing ones. These were organised around supporting stu-
dents dropping in and seeking help for academic purposes and personal development.
One beautiful characteristic of their model, the Pillars in Evaluation (PIE), was the
facet ‘dynamism’, emphasising the centrality of fluidity (Fig. 3.14).
Kevin, the graduate who recounted using Silver Fluoride in Cambodia experienced an
undergraduate degree that used various versions of MELT—including the Research
Skill Development framework and the Clinical Reflection Skills framework—in the
first four semesters of the Oral Health degree. In the final year of the degree, students
were required to engage in open-ended inquiry. Another graduate of the Oral Health
programme said about the development of their sophisticated thinking from the first
year that:
You have to research it, you don’t get fed stuff anymore. You have to go, research
it, sit down, analyse what’s important and what’s not. So yes, it slowly did lead up to
a better research in the third year. I think if we started researching in the third year,
we wouldn’t produce a high-quality piece of work at all (italics added) [5].
This graduate appreciated that, as a student, starting the process of developing
sophisticated thinking skills from first year enabled those skills to slowly build up
and resulted in better research in the final year because of that ongoing, explicit
80 3 How Do We Arrange?
The common framing of MELT adapted context-by-context and over time, enables
students to take the specifics of any given learning activity, assessment task and
individual course and begin to see the big picture by connecting all the parts that
may otherwise seem separate. They will perceive, for example, not separate activities,
assessments, courses or even separate facet development, but a multifaceted ‘mindset
of research’ or other such sophisticated thinking gems. In a similar vein, a student
engaging in the research-oriented fourth year of a Medical Science degree looked
back on the use of the MELT and found:
Since the beginning [of First Year], they have given us assignments based on this criteria.
You might not have liked the assignments, but because they have been consistently applying
this structure to all of our assignments, we have come to think that way for science… [9]
The MELT used as a thinking routine, became for the student a heuristic to think
scientifically.
As MELT expands across years of study, disciplines, and learning contexts, stu-
dents are increasingly likely to be exposed to more than one application of the model.
Two of the big advantages of repeat exposure are that (a) it improves student self-
assessment and peer assessment, where students become attuned to the standards
of the context, and (b) they become better able to work with increasing levels of
competency and autonomy in each context.
References
1. Einstein Bubble in Cartoon: Brown, P. (1993). Managing your time. Cambridge: Daniels
Publishing.
2. Willison, J. W. (2012). When academics integrate research skill development in the curriculum.
Higher Education Research & Development, 31(6), 905–919.
3. Wilmore, M., & Willison, J. (2016). Graduates’ attitudes to research skill development in
undergraduate media education. Asia Pacific Media Educator, 26(1), 113–128.
4. Ain, C. T., Sabir, F., & Willison, J. (2018). Research skills that men and women developed at
university and then used in workplaces. Studies in Higher Education, 1–13.
5. Willison, J., et al., (in press). Graduates’ affective transfer of research skills and evidence-based
practice from university to employment in clinics. BMC Journal of Medical Education.
6. Willison, J. W., Al Sarawi, S., Bottema, C., Hazel, S., Henderson, U., Karanicolas, S., Kempster,
S., et al. (2014). Outcomes and uptake of explicit research skill development across degree
programs.Sydney: The Office of Learning and Teaching. https://digital.library.adelaide.edu.
au/dspace/bitstream/2440/92390/3/hdl_92390.pdf.
7. Missingham, D., Shah, S., Sabir, F. & Willison, J. (2018). Developing and connecting engineer-
ing skills for researching and problem solving. Journal of University Teaching and Learning
Practice, 15(4).
References 81
8. Bandaranaike, S., & Willison, J. (2015). Building capacity for work-readiness: Bridging the
cognitive and affective domains. Asia-Pacific Journal of Cooperative Education, 16(3), 223–
233.
9. Willison, J. & Buisman Pijlman, F. (2016). Ph.D. prepared: Research skill development across
the undergraduate years. International Journal of Researcher Development, 7(1), 63–83.
10. Velautham, L., & Picard, M. Y. (2009). Collaborating equals: Engaging faculties through
teaching-led research. Journal of Academic Language and Learning, 3(2), A130-A141.
11. Venning, J., & Buisman-Pijlman, F. (2013). Integrating assessment matrices in feedback
loops to promote research skill development in postgraduate research projects. Assessment
& Evaluation in Higher Education, 38(5), 567–579.
Open Access This chapter is licensed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0
International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits use, sharing,
adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate
credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license and
indicate if changes were made.
The images or other third party material in this chapter are included in the chapter’s Creative
Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not
included in the chapter’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by
statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from
the copyright holder.
Chapter 4
What Do We Trust?
In the Information Age, students, teachers and learning communities are at risk. The
question of what to trust is central throughout formal and informal education. From
stranger danger to fake news, from misleading websites to scam journals, questions
of trust are tangible. This partially accounts for the frequency of use of the term
‘critical thinking’ in education. Of course, it is not only students that are challenged
by fake information in the Information Age—educators also have to make sense of
competing educational perspectives, such as the debate about whether comparative
testing facilitates or hinders learning. Information that displays markers of credibility
cannot be taken at face value: there are many errors in peer-reviewed journal articles,
even in journals with tight quality controls [2].
Another, more buried, aspect of trust concerns the educational perspectives that
we hold to. Why is it that some educators lean towards modes of instruction that
are highly directed, some towards those that are open-ended, and others somewhere
in-between? This too is a question of trust in regard to pedagogies of choice, and
determines where we put our concerted efforts, what we can and will achieve as indi-
vidual teachers, as a whole school community or as a system. But why do some hold
a more objectivist rationale, while others are geared towards social constructivist
approaches to education? What we trust also determines the types of controversies
and arguments we enter into, as well as the resource allocations that affect our stu-
dents’ learning. Building on the core characteristics of MELT as depicted in Chap. 2,
and the diversity of its use as shown in Chap. 3, the purpose of this chapter is to
use the MELT for reframing the competition between different educational perspec-
tives and dealing with some trust issues. My aim is to show how each perspective may
complement the other through the MELT.
In the following section, I present a vignette about Tara, a Year 8 student who
engages with a canonical science experiment and declares ‘I told you science is stupid
‘cause you don’t know if you’re right.’ There are good reasons why Tara declares
science to be stupid, and something not to be trusted. This chapter applies the MELT
in order to help you ask, ‘what do I trust?’ It is a challenge to suspend judgement,
or at least to give licence to the possibility that educational perspectives other our
own may have some merit. We will use the facets of MELT to interpret Tara’s story
and consider two facets in detail that are crucial to the issue of trust: evaluate and
reflect and analyse and synthesise. We will then use this interpretation to broaden
our look across the educational landscape, and turn the question of this chapter to
‘why do we distrust other educational perspectives so much?’ with consideration for
the implications of what we trust and distrust.
The story is called Shrink. It describes a situation where I engaged with Tara and
her work partner Shannon as a Participant Observer [3] in another teacher’s class.
4.2 Shrink
By the time I sit behind Tara and Shannon on the experimental bench that runs along
the rear wall, Mrs Stuebalm is already asking the class a recollection question: ‘What
happens to a gas when we heat it?’
‘Expands,’ exclaims one of the boys.
Tara calls out, ‘Matures.’
I wonder to myself why Tara uses that word.
‘This afternoon we are doing another experiment, and I want you to predict what
will happen,’ says Mrs Stuebalm. ‘What do you think will happen if we heat a solid?’
‘Expand,’ says one of the boys near the front.
‘Yes. A very good word,’ commends Mrs Stuebalm.
The boy throws his chest out.
‘Metal burns and shrinks when you heat it,’ adds another boy
Mrs Stuebalm seems to avoid his statement, and immediately asks the students to
start writing their predictions in their exercise books.
Mrs Stuebalm refers to a diagram depicting a metal ring. The ring’s inside diameter
is slightly smaller than the outside diameter of an accompanying metal ball.
She explains, ‘What you’re trying to do is put the ball through the hoop. You need
to be careful, because you could burn yourself. Decide what to heat, the ball or the
ring. You have to push it through the ring.’
Tara collects the equipment and sits down next to Shannon at their lab bench.
They light the Bunsen, and start heating the ring. Soon, Tara lowers the ball through
the hole in the ring, and says, ‘We did it!’
She looks very satisfied at their success.
Suddenly, Mrs Stuebalm calls out, ‘OK everyone, pack your equipment away.
Write down the equipment and tell me what happened to your ball and ring.’
I return to Tara to ask her, ‘What was your prediction?’
4.2 Shrink 85
‘Metal would burn and shrink… and that’s what happened. It shrinks to let the
ball through.’
I’m caught by surprise, but Shannon rescues me.
‘No it doesn’t. It expands.’
‘No, the ring shrinks,’ retorts Tara.
‘It expands.’
‘It shrinks, because the ring becomes thinner and thinner, until the ball gets
through,’ explains Tara, getting extremely frustrated with Shannon’s thinking.
Shannon is also annoyed with Tara, and retaliates, ‘The hole has to get bigger to
let the ball through, so the metal must expand.’
Tara and Shannon call in Mrs Stuebalm, but the conversation goes round and
round in similar circles.
Returning to the front, Mrs Stuebalm asks the class, ‘What happens to the solid
when it’s heated?’
‘Expands,’ someone calls out.
Mrs Stuebalm nods confirmation.
Shannon whispers to Tara, ‘I told you.’
Tara sits quietly, looking annoyed. I return to her to try and understand her point,
and she continues her explanation: ‘I mean it shrinks outwards. Like, the metal inside
shrinks towards the outside.’
‘You’re saying the actual metal bit gets smaller?’ I probe.
Tara nods.
‘What actually happens is the metal expands and moves out,’ I explain.
‘I told you!’ triumphs Shannon again.
Tara grows even more frustrated, but presses on: ‘The outside expands to give the
inside room to shrink… see, I said science is stupid, ‘cause you don’t know if you’re
right.’
Fig. 4.1 Two students heat a metal ring, so that its expansion allows a metal sphere to pass through
it
when you’re right’. And the issues for student learning are immense when educators
try and fail to help students gain fundamental understanding.
Tara was working hard conceptually to make sense of the practical, and even
though the classroom consensus was that ‘metals expand when you heat them’, Tara
would not be persuaded. When I interviewed her one month later about this incident
[3], it turned out that she had some good reasons to think metals shrink when heated.
She had previously watched her dad soldering, and noted that the soft soldering
metal ‘shrinks in’. Likewise, throw an empty aluminium can on the fire, she said,
and it shrinks. While both these observations are better explained by other concepts
(capillary action and chemical change, respectively), I could see that she was basing
her current observations on her past knowledge, and that she was striving to make
sense of the phenomena. Tara didn’t just acquiesce to the classroom consensus or
the curriculum; she was willing to challenge both based on what she perceived to be
relevent experiences.
In that willingness to challenge the prevailing classroom knowledge, she epit-
omised the sort of student we are striving to educate—not a gullible consumer of
facts, but a discerning user and generator of information. If we are concerned about
our students blindly accepting clickbait headlines and ‘fake news’ by default, we
also need to be concerned about how they deal with things that just don’t add up
for them in our curricula. Tara judged the classroom content, and her verdict was
‘science is stupid’. It turned out that she was partly right, in that the experiment,
used in science classrooms for a hundred years, is deeply flawed in its design. That
the equipment is constructed in such a way as to provide ambiguous data, making it
open to interpretation, has been known for some time [3].
4.3 MELT Analysis of Shrink 87
All the facets of MELT are present in this story and, as pointed out in Chap. 2,
this seems to be typical of even slighly complex learning activities. Even though
the location of the lesson on the continuum of learning autonomy was prescribed
in terms of aim, method and final answer, Tara demonstrated something unique in
terms of her learning autonomy when engaging with evaluate & reflect and analyse
& synthesise. Shrink involved a prescribed practical which was designed to con-
firm canonical knowledge. However, the complexities and potential sophistication
of student engagement with the practical are laid bare when scrutinising it from the
perspective of MELT’s six facets and autonomy.
Embark & clarify: in the context of the learning topic, the students knew they were
doing something concerned with heating solids. The teacher, Mrs Stuebalm, intended
for students to learn or confirm that metals expand when heated. Perhaps students
other than Tara left the class feeling that they had experienced, constructed and agreed
on a scientific ‘fact’ about metals. And in many ways, Mrs Stuebalm provided a
wonderful learning environment, managing the practical risks (naked flames, red-
hot metals) and allocating time for an experiment, despite the packed curriculum. As
such, the lesson was an ethical and moral endeavour which socialised students into
the conventions of science in a way that they could engage with.
However, there is an enduring question for education in this story: how clear was
the lesson’s purpose to the students? It is difficult for teachers to provide a sense of
purpose to students engaging in a series of learning activities across terms and years.
If students can’t see connections between different activities or develop a sense of
purpose, what are the implications for their learning?
Find & generate: the data generated in this practical came from visual observa-
tion—primarily, students observed that the ball didn’t fit before heating, but did
fit immediately after heating. All students ‘observed’ that the ring’s inside diameter
grew larger, and Tara and Shannon interpreted this observation in contradictory ways.
However, there were no measurements of the ring’s actual dimensions, partly due to
the dangers of measuring a red-hot ring and the difficulty of obtaining the equipment
needed for accurate measurements of the ring’s diameter.
Organise & manage: this practical was very quick to complete, and one where you
could imagine students having the time to think about its implications. However,
for the sake of presenting a succinct story earlier, I didn’t mention that the students
actually performed several heat-and-expand practicals back-to-back, including one
involving liquids, and that these practicals required time to set up, conduct, pack up,
clean up and write up. As in Parachute, students’ ability to develop deep concepts
was determined to some extent by their ability to organise & manage. This classroom
reflected a packed curriculum which, for some students, seemed to impede learning
for the sake of coverage.
Communicate & apply: the girls followed a prescribed practical structure to write up
their experiment: aim, equipment, results and conclusion. They were not required to
draw a diagram, even though a before-and-after diagram would have captured some
of the complexity of their thinking. The ongoing communication process was vital
88 4 What Do We Trust?
to Tara and Shannon’s ability to consolidate understandings, and here it was their
disagreement that sharpened what they understood to have occurred. Throughout
the practical, Tara applied her previous knowledge to the task, making observations
based on that knowledge. Her observations and inferences supported the application
of her personal theory that metal shrinks when heated.
Evaluate and reflect: there was little class time to reflect, but as a participant observer
in the classroom [3], I had that rare opportunity to interview Tara and Shannonn after
the passion of the classroom evaporated. When reflecting on this practical one month
later, Tara still thought she was right, and that the classroom science didn’t make sense.
Her evaluation was quintessential of personal constructivist thinking, and it is rarely
so evident as in this account that a student is constructing and weighing up knowledge:
‘I said science is stupid’. It may be that the classroom knowledge with which she was
presented made her feel stupid, as it didn’t fit with her previous experience. Perhaps
it unsettled her that self-reflection and attempts to self-correct were of no help ‘cause
you don’t know if you’re right.’ There is a sense of frustration created by self-doubt.
Not only could she be wrong in science, but she would not know that she was wrong,
even when she spent time trying to unravel why she was wrong.
Analyse & synthesise: everyone, including Tara, inferred that the hole grew larger
because of the observation that the ball fell through the hole at the time the ring
was heated. However, Tara’s theory-laden observation (‘it shrinks inwards’) led her
to strengthen her theory that metals shrink when you heat them. Her classmates
considered her to be wrong, even though her analysis made sense in the context of
her prior knowledge and her observations. However, instead of being steered into
self-correction like the students in Place Value, Tara ended up in frustration that you
‘never know when you’re right’. This is partly because the practical was prescribed
in terms of content knowledge, understanding and procedure, with no room for any
understanding other than the science canon. Such an approach makes sense insofar
as we don’t really want students to develop a belief that metals shrink when heated.
However, Tara reacted against the correct answer because it conflicted too starkly
with the theory she had built for herself.
Tara’s analytical thinking was shown in her claim that ‘it shrinks outwards. Like,
the metal inside shrinks towards the outside.’
‘You’re saying the actual metal bit gets smaller?’ I probed, and Tara nodded.
‘The outside expands to give the inside room to shrink.’
Tara’s juggling of ‘outside’ and ‘inside’ shows that she was considering the width
dimension. If one thinks of width, then since metals expand when heated, they would
expand in all directions, outwards and inwards. That would make the hole smaller.
Because the hole got larger, the opposite must be true: metals shrink to create a bigger
hole.
4.3 MELT Analysis of Shrink 89
Tara demonstrated ingenious, creative and discerning thinking here to craft her
inference based on her observations and prior knowledge. Too bad the class had to
pack up, debrief quickly and move onto the next topic. Imagine if the teacher had
asked the students to design another experiment to show their ideas, giving them the
chance to test some of these or at least think them through. What is a pity is that Tara
was thinking in terms of analysis & synthesis, but her teacher failed to reward these
cognitive skills. For a student engaging in analysis like Tara, science then becomes
a ‘belief-based subject’ in which she should follow the mandates of the teacher, and
that such emulation is the only satisfactory way of operating when learning in the
classroom. How do those of us with educational responsibilities balance the need to
teach students correct content knowledge with the fact that their analytical thinking
may sometimes lead them to conclusions which contradict the curriculum? This is
another form of the question ‘what do we trust?’, because what we prioritise indicates
what we trust and value.
Within the framework of Bloom’s Taxonomy, the knowledge that metals expand
when heated is fundamental to students developing an understanding of why metals,
liquids and gases expand when heated. This is connected to a fundamental theory of
matter (called ‘kinetic theory’) that is often taught in the first year of high school.
Without a fundamental understanding of this theory, it is impossible to apply it
correctly. Tara could have been guided to consider ‘What will happen if you heat
the metal ball?’ If she had made a prediction consistent with her existing ideas, she
would have said that the ball should pass through the ring, since it would shrink when
heated. And on observing that the ball did not pass through the ring, she would have
been challenged by the phenomena. However, there was insufficient time for this to
happen. In terms of the teacher picking up Tara’s fundamental error, Mrs Stuebalm
did hear Tara’s idea. But instead of engaging with her, she proceeded immediately
to the front of the room to facilitate a classroom conversation in which the only idea
discussed was that metals expand when heated.
Student evaluation, even though hierachically located at the top of Bloom’s old tax-
onomy [4], and second from the top in the new taxonomy [5], acctually is a frequent
and recurrent necessity of sophisticated thinking. Evaluation occurs almost always
when students are formulating knowledge by internally processing data, information,
details or facts. Some of the students in the class depicted in Shrink already had the
knowledge that metals expand when you heat them, while some may have barely
noticed what happened in the practical and just adhered to the teacher’s explanation.
There may have been some students who had never heard about thermal expansion,
but managed to observe the data and construct a phenomena-based knowledge that
metals expand when heated. Others may have arrived at the correct conclusion by
tuning into the class discourse, relying on the social context, including spoken and
written language. Shannon and I were working hard to help Tara comprehend our
knowledge, and were using a lot of language and pointing to do so, but Tara’s prior
knowledge and theories rendered our words senseless to her. She personally con-
structed and consolidated something that had to make sense to her and, fortunately,
she would not acquiesce, because acquiescence would show that she had given up
on sophisticated learning in science.
90 4 What Do We Trust?
Even in the very simple learning activity of Shrink, the empirical substance
of which took less than five minutes, learning was very complex and the think-
ing required to engage with the phenomena of metal and heat was sophisticated.
Section 4.5 shows how the multifaceted thinking in Shrink renders the perspective
of any individual learning theory incomplete, and not to be fully trusted to guide
the realities of learning and teaching. First, three major theoretical orientations to
teaching and learning are summarised and then used to provide perspective on Shrink.
that employs a one-to-one correspondence between content and what is learned [13].
Direct Instruction and aspects of national assessment regimes are strongly influenced
by objectivism, as is much online learning design and, curiously, the implementa-
tion of ‘constructive alignment’ [14]. ‘Curious’, because the word ‘constructive’
was coupled with ‘alignment’ to connote a constructivist orientation [14]; however,
in use, the phrase often boils down to mean ‘curriculum that is well structured to
achieve its intentions’. The quote above about ‘default epistemology’ is close to the
mark; teachers who haven’t consciously thought through their own ‘epistemology’
or theory of learning will often work from the default setting of objectivism.
Personal Constructivism
Personal constructivism involves internal cognitive processes that build conceptual
understanding, based on the foundations of a student’s prior knowledge. Learning is
a dynamic process, and the way in which learning happens is mediated by internal
factors like language, culture and social context. Some personal constructivists see all
learning, including research, as a process of trying to ascertain and approximate the
nature of objective truth, while others believe that truth itself is internally constructed
and does not exist outside of a learner’s mind [10, 15, 16].
Social Constructivism
Social constructivism pertains to learning that is co-constructed through interpersonal
mechanisms, especially through forms of communication like language. While it is
conceivable that a social constructivist may believe in an objectively real world in
which we can construct a viable (but never true) understanding, most would hold that
the world only is ‘real’ insofar as individuals or groups of individuals understand it
to exist [17, 18].
Shrink is one example in which a teacher employs Direct Instruction using a pre-
scribed practical to engineer understanding of content. All three theories in 4.4 may
shed insight into this one practical. One probing question is ‘Why did Tara and
Shannon make the same observations, but produce such different explanations?’
Their analysis was based on different starting points, including their prior knowl-
edge and experience. In science, the dependence of observation on theory has been
discussed for a long time [19]; often, we see what we expect to see. However, in this
practical, the girls make the same observation that the ball goes through the ring, and
both reason that the hole, therefore, got bigger. Then Shannon says, ‘The hole has
to get bigger to let the ball through, so the metal must expand.’ Metallic expansion,
however, results in a greater volume and surface area of metal. This is the opposite of
most experiences with holes, where to expand a hole, we must take something away.
If you have a hole in a wall, and a picture plug is too big to go in, you get a bigger drill
bit and take away more of the wall. The analysis made by Shannon, Mrs Stuebalm,
92 4 What Do We Trust?
the rest of the class and me represented a consensus, but it was not in keeping with
our general knowledge of holes. That ‘metals must expand’ may have been for some
a recall of the science canon or just a commonly known ‘fact’. Others’ analyses may
have been deductive rather than inductive: ‘metals expand when you heat them. We
heated the metal ring, so the metal expanded to make the hole bigger.’ What type of
reasoning was this experiment, designed in the 1900’s, designed to elicit? [20, 21].
The issue of whether each student trusts the teacher, their own rational thinking or the
social discourse is an important one. Objectivism, personal constructivism and social
constructivism each provide a different basis for teachers, parents, administrators and
researchers to understand, plan for and determine how to ‘measure’ learning. From
the MELT perspective, all three theoretical perspectives are pertinent and valid. All
three can inform the learning that takes place in classrooms [18]. As we saw earlier,
theoretical purity is not the default in the sciences, with competing paradigms such
as relativity theory and quantum mechanics enduring together.
4.5 Understanding the Three Theories Using the Example of Shrink 95
Instead of being used to direct educators into a narrowed perspective, these three
theoretical observations could collectively serve teachers and students to provide
understandings of a rich educational experience that is in keeping with the way that
learning has taken place over tens of thousands of years. To help make connections
between the different perspectives, the next section shows the theory underpinning
MELT, and how this theory enables MELT to connect multiple theories.
Shrink and its analysis are now taken up to consider the theory that underpins MELT.
While Chap. 2 outlined the literature that informed MELT, the MELT comprise a con-
ceptual framework for learning and teaching, not a theory. A conceptual framework,
such as Bloom’s Taxonomy, provides a structure for thinking, and is not by default
theoretical. To a large extent, Bloom’s Taxonomy, the ANZIIL framework, and the
SOLO taxonomy are themselves descriptive. The MELT function at a level that is
explicitly connected to practice. However, there is theory underpinning MELT, and
aspects of epistemology and theory provide a fundamental understanding of the ways
that MELT may provide a conceptual glue between disparate paradigms, theories and
practices.
The MELT were designed to provide bridging points between different educa-
tional ideas, by addressing multiple theories, and this is enabled in practical terms
through the consideration of learning autonomy. The MELT always need to become
fluid through the warmth of conversation or the heat of the argument. In a similar
spirit, Dewey wrote
It is the business of an intelligent theory of education to ascertain the causes for the conflicts
that exist and then, instead of taking one side or the other, to indicate a plan of operations
proceeding from a level deeper and more inclusive than is represented by the practices and
ideas of the contending parties [26].
These conflicts, before the Second World War, were over similar issues to the
issues that are debated today, including the efficacy of inquiry-based learning versus
a focus on content acquisition and mastery [26]. In Dewey’s terms, no ‘intelligent
theory’ for education has emerged that has provided a ‘plan of operations’ that
includes different perspectives, although classroom teachers themselves frequently
take a practical orientation that is ‘inclusive’ in Dewey’s sense. The MELT provide a
plan of operations that is inclusive because it spans the gamut of approaches through
its elaboration of learning autonomy. This section shows how the MELT proceed
from a ‘level deeper’.
The MELT are theoretically underpinned by conceptual metaphor [27], which
allows for the development of an educational perspective from a deeper and more
inclusive level. The idea of conceptual metaphor is that for all but the most con-
crete representations, human conceptual structures resonate with metaphor. From
this perspective, ‘objectivism’ is based on a thing, the object, [6] being real, tangible,
96 4 What Do We Trust?
Clashes between those with differing theoretical referents and metaphors occur when
there is a consideration of how much learning autonomy should be provided to spe-
cific groups of students. No one argues about whether students need to be or become
autonomous learners, but they do argue about when this should happen. Vygotsky’s
use of ‘zone’ in the Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD) [17] connotes a metaphor-
ical region in space, an awareness of a breadth of possibilities. He did not articulate
a PPD, a ‘Point of Proximal Development’, a place where things are ‘just right’, or
a narrow band of performance. In the zone, the part that is closer to the learner is
higher in learning autonomy as it pertains to the capacity of the learner without sup-
port. Further from the learner (towards the outer edge of the zone) is a point beyond
her own capacity, but she can successfully demonstrate intended performance at this
outer border of competence with support from experienced others. That is, she will
demonstrate less learning autonomy at this point, and benefit from more guidance,
emulating others. With learning that is enabled by such support, a learner increases
4.6 Theoretical Underpinning of MELT 97
Table 4.1 Emphases for objectivism, personal constructivism and social constructivism in terms
of the continuum of learning autonomy. (The heavier the shading, the more emphasis)
Prescribed Bounded Scaffolded Open-ended Unbounded
Objectivism
Personal
constructivism
Social
constructivism
in her capacity to perform at a more rigorous, sophisticated level. Over time she
becomes able to self-direct and self-regulate her learning at this hightened level of
competance, improvising or even innovating, that is with higher learning autonomy
and with higher competence than before. However, when new concepts are encoun-
tered, new skills needed or higher levels of rigour are required, a movement back to
the outer edge of the zone, towards lower learning autonomy, with guidance from an
experienced peer of teacher, is often needed again, with its modelling and structure.
Thus, the ZPD suggests movement from lower to higher learning autonomy, from
lower levels at the further reaches of the zone to higher levels closer in to the learner’s
exisitng capacity. Further out, with modelling and guidance, each student learns how
to add sophistication and rigour to their learning, and then when the student applies
this learning by herself, she works with higher learning autonomy in the edge of the
zone that is proximal to the student’s capacity. For this reason, MELT implies the
need for movement from low learning autonomy to high, and back [28], like a tidal
zone in the complex ecosystem of learning.
From an objectivist perspective, learning autonomy should initially be low, pro-
viding structured learning environments in which students can acquire a content
knowledge base and practice the skills of the discipline. From a social constructivist
perspective, autonomy for students should primarily be high, allowing for student
ownership and collaborative action. From a personal constructivist position, students
are primarily engaging in internal sense-making that emphasises evaluation, reflec-
tion, analysis and synthesis. These facets span from the highly prescribed to the
unbounded, with a concentration of effort that is at the scaffolded level, as shown
visually in Table 4.1.
From the MELT perspective, objectivism, personal constructivism and social con-
structivism don’t need to be competing theoretical perspectives but can be treated
as metaphors that mutually support each other. Each brings to the educational table
98 4 What Do We Trust?
its own set of strengths. An objectivist perspective will emphasise the development
of students’ knowledge bases and cognitive skill sets, in keeping with prescribed
and bounded learning autonomy. Objectivists, of course, can and do provide more
open-ended tasks, but this is typically once the student is demonstrably ready with
the required knowledge and skills.
Those who are more closely aligned with personal constructivism will engineer
learning environments that enable students to overcome or skirt misconceptions and
to develop robust understanding. There may be some rote learning and more open
investigation, but there will be much scaffolded learning. Social constructivists will
maximise motivation, social interaction and student ownership of learning, treating
students more as if they are in a learning partnership with teachers, allowing, as often
as possible, for open-ended and unbounded learning autonomy.
Treating each theoretical perspective as a metaphor that connects to parts of the
continuum of learning autonomy may facilitate a more complementary approach to
education than currently exists, as each perspective then merely emphasises a dif-
ferent place on the continuum. Objectivists know that learning the facts and details
of a discipline through discovery modes is inefficient, and that it is easy for stu-
dents to develop misconceptions, and so become frustrated and anxious. Personal
constructivists know that didactic teaching and rote learning do not prompt deep
understanding, that misconceptions developed while learning are substantial and
long-lived, and that frequently there must be structure to learning. And social con-
structivists see that unless students own their learning and engage in social processes,
their motivation to engage and capacity to learn will be diminished. MELT’s contin-
uum of learning autonomy calls for each perspective to speak to and complement
the others, to provide for each student the full experiences of the entire continuum
and a shuttling back and forth across it as they traverse the years of education.
One aspect of research that we have found so far is that educational gains made over
the timeframe of a semester using MELT can be subsequently lost: skills tend to
atrophy unless they are explicitly reinforced after they are learned [29]. Facilitating
learning is a long-term business, where students’ motivation is crucial to what they
attend to long-term. Empirically, MELT requires significant timeframes and repeated
exposure. Because engagement is a long-term process, MELT must be tested over
educationally significant timeframes—a minimum of a term or semester. Neurolog-
ically, long-term memory consolidation is thought to take place over years, not days
or even terms [30].
Of the many MELT, only three have undergone some long-term evaluation—the
RSD [31–33], OPS [34] and the WSD [35, 36]. Conceptual frameworks cannot be
effectively evaluated through any one trial because ways of interpreting and imple-
menting a conceptual framework vary markedly. What is needed are sustained use
by practitioners and substantial, diverse and numerous evaluations in many contexts.
4.7 Trusting the MELT? 99
can nullify each other, but they can also co-exist and conceptually connect through
a consideration of the MELT’s continuum of learner autonomy elaborating as they
do the facets of sophisticated thinking.
References
1. Einstein Bubble in Cartoon: Schwarz, D. (1944, March 12). The Einstein theory of living. The
New York Times Magazine, pp. 16–17.
2. Nuzzo, R. (2014). Scientific method: Statistical errors. Nature News, 506(7487), 150.
3. Willison, J. W. (2000). Classroom factors affecting student scientific literacy: tales and their
interpretation using a metaphoric framework (Doctoral dissertation, Curtin University, Perth,
Australia). Retrieved from https://espace.curtin.edu.au/handle/20.500.11937/268.
4. Bloom, B. S. (1956). Taxonomy of educational objectives. Vol. 1: Cognitive domain. New York,
NY: McKay.
5. Krathwohl, D. R. (2002). A revision of Bloom’s taxonomy: An overview. Theory into Practice,
41(4), 212–218.
6. Chapline, G. (2003). Quantum phase transitions and the failure of classical general relativity.
International Journal of Modern Physics A, 18(21), 3587–3590.
7. Thiemann, T. (2007). Loop quantum gravity: an inside view. In I. Stamatescu & E. Seiler (Eds.),
Approaches to fundamental physics (pp. 185–263). Berlin, Germany: Springer.
8. Jonassen, D. H. (1991). Objectivism versus constructivism: Do we need a new philosophical
paradigm? Educational Technology Research and Development, 39(3), 5–14. Sutton, C. (1993).
9. Sutton, C. (1993). Figuring out a scientific understanding. Journal of Research in Science
Teaching, 30(10), 1215–1227.
10. Roth, W. M., & Roychoudhury, A. (1994). Physics students’ epistemologies and views about
knowing and learning. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 31(1), 5–30.
11. Wodak, D. (2017). Can objectivists account for subjective reasons. J. Ethics & Soc. Phil., 12,
259.
12. Moallem, M. (2001). Applying constructivist and objectivist learning theories in the design
of a web-based course: Implications for practice. Educational Technology & Society, 4(3),
113–125.
13. Davis, N. T., Jo McCarty, B., Shaw, K. L., & Sidani-Tabbaa, A. (1993). Transitions from
objectivism to constructivism in science education. International Journal of Science Education,
15(6), 627–636.
14. Biggs, J. (1996). Enhancing teaching through constructive alignment. Higher Education, 32(3),
347–364.
15. Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. Vol. 1. A theory of personality.
Vol. 2. Clinical diagnosis and psychotherapy. Oxford, England: W. W. Norton.
16. Wadsworth, B. J. (1996). Piaget’s theory of cognitive and affective development: Foundations
of constructivism. White Plains, NY: Longman Publishers USA.
17. Vygotsky, L. S. (1980). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
18. Hayes, C. (2018, March). Podiatric medicine and surgery: Situated learning in simulation
with social constructivism. Presented at the Health Education England North East Quality
Conference, Durham, UK.
19. Schurz, G. (2015). Ostensive learnability as a test criterion for theory-neutral observation
concepts. Journal for General Philosophy of Science, 46(1), 139–153.
20. Cowie, B., Jones, A., & Otrel-Cass, K. (2011). Re-engaging students in science: Issues of
assessment, funds of knowledge and sites for learning. International Journal of Science and
Mathematics Education, 9(2), 347–366.
References 101
21. Willison, J. W., & Taylor, P. C. (2006). Complementary epistemologies of science teaching.
In P. J. Aubusson, A. G. Harrison, & S. M. Ritchie (Eds.), Metaphor and analogy in science
education (pp. 25–36). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer.
22. Trumper, R. (2001). A cross-age study of junior high school students’ conceptions of basic
astronomy concepts. International Journal of Science Education, 23(11), 1111–1123.
23. Tytler, R. (2002). Teaching for understanding in science: Constructivist/conceptual change
teaching approaches. Australian Science Teachers Journal, 48(4), 30–35.
24. Vosniadou, S., Ioannides, C., Dimitrakopoulou, A., & Papademetriou, E. (2001). Designing
learning environments to promote conceptual change in science. Learning and Instruction,
11(4–5), 381–419.
25. Driver, R. (1989). Students’ conceptions and the learning of science. International Journal of
Science Education, 11(5), 481–490.
26. Dewey, J. (1938/1963). Experience and education. New York, NY: Collier MacMillan.
27. Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: the Embodied Mind & its Challenge
to Western Thought. New York, NY: Basic Books.
28. Willison, J., Sabir, F., & Thomas, J. (2017). Shifting dimensions of autonomy in students’
research and employment. Higher Education Research & Development, 36(2), 430–443.
29. Willison, J. (2012). When Academics integrate research skill development in the curriculum.
Higher Education Research and Development, 31(6), 905–919.
30. Styles, J. (2008). The fundamentals of brain development: Integrating nature & nurture.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
31. Willison, J., & Buisman Pijlman, F. (2016). Ph.D. prepared: Research skill development across
the undergraduate years. International Journal of Researcher Development, 7(1), 63–83.
32. Wilmore, M., & Willison, J. (2016). Graduates’ attitudes to research skill development in
undergraduate media education. Asia Pacific Media Educator, 26(1), 1–16.
33. Ain, C. T., Sabir, F., & Willison, J. (2018). Research skills that men and women developed at
university and then used in workplaces. Studies in Higher Education, 1–13.
34. Missingham, D., Shah, S., Sabir, F., & Willison, J. (2018). Student engineers optimising
problem solving and research skills. Journal of University Teaching and Learning Practice,
15(4).
35. Bandaranaike, S. (2018). From Research Skill Development to Work Skill Development.
Journal of University Teaching & Learning Practice, 15(4), 7.
36. Bandaranaike, S., & Willison, J. W. (2015). Building capacity for work-readiness: Bridging
the cognitive and affective domains. Asia-Pacific Journal of Cooperative Education, 16(3),
223–233.
37. Research Skill Development for Curriculum Design and Assessment. (2018). Retrieved from
www.rsd.edu.au.
38. Models of Engaged Learning and Teaching (2017). Retrieved from www.melt.edu.au.
Open Access This chapter is licensed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0
International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits use, sharing,
adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate
credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license and
indicate if changes were made.
The images or other third party material in this chapter are included in the chapter’s Creative
Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not
included in the chapter’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by
statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from
the copyright holder.
Chapter 5
What Does It Mean?
This chapter focuses on what the MELT mean for education theory and practice. A
number of contemporary learning theories (in addition to those discussed in Chap. 4)
sit comfortably on the learning autonomy continuum of MELT. Some of these sit at
the prescribed end, some at the unbounded end and others in the middle. What this
means is that the MELT can function as a kind of conceptual glue that holds these
often-competing theories in tension: by placing them on the same page, the intention
is to open up the conversations between people who hold to each one. Then, based
on this complementary view of educational theories, the second half of the chapter
considers what MELT means for enhancing learning and improving curricula and
pedagogy through teacher action research. This addresses one of the major issues
identified in Chap. 1: how to use MELT to engage with educational theory in ways
that make practical sense to educators.
Threshold Concepts (TCs) are the pivotal learning concepts in each subject or dis-
cipline. Within Meyer & Land’s theory, these concepts are difficult for students to
grasp, making it hard for learners to ‘enter through’ into discipline-specific ways of
seeing the world. For example, in physics, the fundamental concept of inertia (that
objects keep moving at their current speed unless a force slows them down or speeds
them up) seems counter-intuitive to many. Without understanding the concept of iner-
tia, it is impossible to comprehend Newtonian physics, even if students master the
equations associated with motion. TCs are usually difficult or troublesome for learn-
ers, and pedagogical structuring is frequently required to enable students to cross the
threshold of understanding. The learning involved in crossing this threshold may be
frustrating and frightening, and the concept to be learned can seem alien or wrong.
In the case of Tara in Shrink, the correct explanation of phenomena was opposed to
her own interpretation, and so she deemed it ‘stupid’. Sometimes, threshold concepts
are very difficult to understand in ways that can be operationalised, as in the case of
‘controlled variables’ in Parachute. Sometimes they involve arbitrary conventions
represented in absolute ways, such as the left-hand numeral in a two-digit number
being worth ten times its face value (as in Place Value). Once a TC is crossed over,
the concept often seems ‘obvious’ to the learner, after which it is easy to forget the
struggles that such learning requires.
As ‘thresholds’, such concepts are considered non-optional entry points into learn-
ing. Without a fundamental understanding of these concepts, subsequent learning
will be superficial and may eventually collapse. In Shrink, the idea that metals shrink
when heated would ultimately dismay Tara if, for example, she built a brick cooking
place with a metal plate measured to fit snugly between the bricks. She ‘knows’ that
the metal plate will ‘shrink when heated’, leading her to believe that there is plenty
of room for the plate. However, in reality, the plate will expand, buckling or breaking
the brickwork. If Tara were to become a designer working with metals—a jeweller
or architect, for example—her misconceptions could have dangerous and expensive
consequences.
Threshold concepts are by definition conceptually demanding, and this demand-
ingness needs to be interpreted according to the age and experience of the learners.
For example, an MBA student who has twenty years’ industry experience may find
it easy to grasp the concept of ‘distributed leadership’, while MBA students with
no professional experience are likely to find the concept much more difficult. Con-
versely, people with twenty years’ experience of top-down leadership may find the
concept of distributed leadership an even more difficult threshold to cross. In this
latter case, where a TC is counter to a person’s experience, educators may need to
plan for the student to ‘unlearn’ an old idea before learning a new one. If there is
one thing that is clear from the literature, it is that prior understandings are resilient
and difficult to displace [6]. Awareness of TCs helps educators to consider students’
existing preconceptions; as such, educators will be less likely to allow students to
stumble around with incorrect ideas.
5.1 Situating Contemporary Learning Theories/Ideas 105
TCs help to explain why students need more than an internet connection if they
are to become adept at a particular discipline. Accessing information can lead to
a bewildering array of relevant or irrelevant knowledge. Without grasping funda-
mental threshold concepts, students risk building incorrect models. Threshold con-
cepts require knowledge that is frequently best attained through repeated exposure,
experience and educative guidance from an experienced hand.
In terms of MELT, the educative guidance required to cross over thresholds (from
a TC perspective) falls within the prescribed and bounded scope provided by edu-
cators. Here, modelling of and guidance into content and concepts are a part of the
educational experience that students may need before they are ready for self-initiated
discovery. Merely ‘telling’ a student about a threshold concept does not guarantee
that they will internalise it. True understanding of a TC requires more than hard
thinking; it may require every facet of MELT, including the application of current
ideas and observation of where these fall short. Most commonly, this happens in an
environment of low student autonomy, where teachers provide students with a highly
prescribed learning environment and the students emulate. This can be a good way
to help many students move across a threshold, because the teacher will be aware
of conceptual sticking points. However, if a lesson pertains to a threshold concept
that some have already grasped and others have not, there can be frustration. If the
teacher assumes the TC as background knowledge, those who have failed to cross
the threshold will feel left behind. Conversely, if the teacher approaches the TC as
something that all need to learn, those who have already ‘crossed the threshold’ will
feel bored. In such cases, it can be necessary to provide students who understand the
TC with more scope for their learning autonomy.
Once attained, TCs allow students to operate in a whole new way and, in terms of
the MELT, the attained concepts enable students to operate with higher autonomy,
more ownership and empowerment, until the next TC is encountered. MELT’s per-
spective on autonomy, as unpacked in Chap. 2, suggests a shuttling back and forth
between low and high student autonomy. Shuttling back to lower learning autonomy
(where teachers prescribe and/or students emulate) shows a recognition of the need
for students to be guided through new TCs.
visual and aural inputs predominate. CLT theorists talk about the ‘visual scratchpad’,
which has a memory of half a second, and an ‘auditory loop’ which can retain
information for up to thirty seconds. If this information is held temporarily in the
WM, it can be juggled conceptually.
There are several instances in Place Value where the cognitive load is associated
with conflicting concepts of value accorded to left–right positions of numbers. This
was in evidence when the teacher asked, ‘is there any other number in the wrong
place?’ At first, the whole class called out ‘no’. There were multiple loads on their
working memory, and together, these may have been partly responsible for the group’s
unanimous incorrect answer. Some of the cognitive load at that point relates to the
question asked early in the lesson: ‘Is place value on your right or on your left?’
Within a two-digit number, the digit of higher value is on the left (e.g. in ‘44’, the
left-hand four is worth forty and the right-hand four is just worth four.). However, in
another mathematics convention, numbers of a higher value are placed on the right
in a sequence (e.g. 9, 14, 27). This right–left difference for these two conventions is
hard enough to grasp when students are learning about double-digit place value for
the first time, but much more so when some of these six-year-old students may not
be clear about which side is left. In Place Value, this set of inherent complications is
magnified: students on the italitali mat viewed the number sequence back-to-front—
it decreased from left to right because it was sequenced from the perspective of the
eight students that were facing them. To students on the mat, it appeared as ‘57,
41, 29, 20, 17, 11, 9, 5’. With this extra extraneous load on each student’s WM,
the processing time required for the analysis became longer, increasing the risk of
inaccuracy, confusion and a feeling of dissonance, all of which may provoke early
maths anxiety. In effect, the ‘grammar’ of mathematics, like any language, is complex
and often has internal inconsistencies which can overload WM. Not until students
internalise this grammar can the load on WM be lessened when students are dealing
with two-digit numbers and be more able to, say, add together two-digit numbers.
We can only process small amounts of new information at a time. These small
chunks of information build-up and eventually become part of our personal knowl-
edge base. For short-term memory to be consolidated into LTM requires time,
rehearsal and the application of information.
From a CLT perspective, the problem with providing students with higher levels
of autonomy is that their STM can become overloaded with new bits of information.
Like threshold concepts, CLT provides a rationale for laying down the foundations of
a knowledge base, with minimal distracting elements, and slowly building up oppor-
tunities to apply that knowledge. From the perspective of these theories, teachers
should not consider allowing students to initiate until students have demonstrated a
mastery of the appropriate content. Shelly struggled in Parachute because she did
not have the necessary basic concepts in place, such as the formula to calculate the
area of a square. Kevin’s success in Silver Fluoride is based on the fundamental
knowledge and skill base that he developed during his degree, and which therefore
prepared him to initiate sophisticated learning.
CLT and TCs are contemporary learning theories/ideas that provide an impetus
for learning to be engineered, prioritising the lower end of the learning autonomy
5.1 Situating Contemporary Learning Theories/Ideas 107
continuum, where teachers prescribe and students emulate. But another current and
influential theory sits at the opposite end of the continuum of learner autonomy:
connectivism.
the process of looking back at the indeterminate decisions made through reflection-
in-action, and teaching oneself. Therefore, knowledge external to the learner and
others’ theories are secondary to internal decision-making, which provides a pow-
erful impetus to learn. Together, reflection-in-action and reflection-on-action make
Schön’s Reflective Practitioner a strong learner, able to improve and exercise high
levels of autonomy.
At the same time, not only do each of the theories sit on the continuum of learning
autonomy, but also in their various foci, each requires all six MELT facets. For
example, given MELT’s ancestry of Information Literacy standards, with their focus
on ‘information-seeking behaviour’, the know-where of connectivism requires all six
facets of MELT. As stated earlier, know-where is ‘the understanding of where to find
knowledge needed’, and in many ways it is a form of ‘know-how’. One reason for this
is that ‘where’ is not merely the location of a source, but the level of relevance and
trustworthiness possessed by that source. Likewise, ‘know-where’ cannot connote
something effective for learning if it were to merely ‘relocate’ knowledge from
one position to another. In addition, there must be analysis and synthesis that are
dependent on effective organisation and that are enabled by, and lead to, effective
communication. Across all these MELT facets, the students increasingly need to
clarify their purpose, especially if the beginning is vague due to a lack of clarity
from teacher instructions or due to the student’s own lack of specificity. As noted in
Chap. 2, in the flood of irrelevant information, clarity is power [7]. Einstein makes
the same point on the Chap. 3 title page: ‘Out of clutter bring simplicity’ [8]. To
paraphrase and synthesise both quotes ‘powerful thinking results from learning how
to clarify the clutter’ and this requires all six facets of the MELT. Thus, the MELT
perspective about sophisticated thinking is not so much ‘higher order thinking’ in
keeping with Bloom’s taxonomy, but multifaceted thinking.
The four contemporary learning theories/concepts described above are current and
appealing. However, when we compare them to each other, we find apparent ten-
sions between them in terms of what teachers ought to emphasise. These tensions
may be relieved by placing each of the theories/concepts along MELT’s continuum
of student autonomy. Doing this provides an understanding which propels shuttling
back and forth along the continuum. There are various challenges to each of the four
perspectives above, especially from each other. While two are especially information
and knowledge focused, one of these––Cognitive Load Theory––posits that learning
occurs in an individual’s brain, while the other—Connectivism––defines learning as
something distributed outside any one’s brain. The other two perspectives empha-
sise deep understanding, and where Threshold Concepts prioritise the inculcation
of foundational concepts by teachers, the Reflective Practitioner employs and val-
ues learners’ existing resources and intuitions. Conceptualisations that have overlap,
then, occupy ‘competitive spaces’, and so, in effect entice adherents to warn about the
110 5 What Does It Mean?
Disparate and competing views make the educational enterprise, broadly speaking,
more vulnerable to vested interests and to the emerging new world order, especially
that mediated by machines. Just like the theorists above, humans currently working on
AI and Machine learning are working on issues of epistemology. Machine Learning
has to be on some learning platform or other and the issue of epistemology––or how
learning happens––dynamically influences and is influenced by that platform. At a
mechanical level, some AI learning platforms mimic the neuronal architecture of
the human brain, whereas some are altogether different. Quantum computers will
probably be a key component of AI operating systems, with possibilities including
‘a neural network encoded in the quantum properties of light’ [10]. We do not know
what will comprise the intelligence of AI because the possibilities are numerous and
broad, and include hybrid versions of old-style analogue computing and quantum
computing [10]. With the advances in quantum computing, it is difficult to know
which platforms or combinations will learn best in which situations. At a processing
level, it has been known for a long time that machine epistemology is or can be very
different from human epistemology [11] but because the structure of AI will not
be clear for a long time, the epistemologies of AI will likewise remain unknowable
until they fully emerge or diverge. Well before we can understand Machine Learning,
however, we will have to understand Machine Pedagogy, for this will dynamically
influence student learning in classrooms.
AI used in schooling and university education for teaching will face unresolved epis-
temological questions just like all AI, but it is the pedagogy that machine teachers
5.1 Situating Contemporary Learning Theories/Ideas 111
use on children that will impact first. We do know that the machines that teach chil-
dren will need to choose or have chosen for them learning theories that are suited
to humans not machines. An AI teacher may derive human learning theories itself,
discarding all those that have preceded, and use a grounded theory approach [12]
because, after all, machines will use data from children learning, in order to formulate
their theories of teaching. Alternatively, specific learning theories may be prioritised
by some programmers of teaching machines. For example, one study found that ‘The
results suggest that integration of certain behavioral theories as features in machine
learning systems provides the best predictions’ [13] (italics added). Currently, stu-
dents learning to read, as mediated by reading robots, seem to develop a strong bond
to the robot, and these robots could be programmed with a Social Constructivist epis-
temology where correction may be secondary to connection. A robot programmed
with a behaviourist orientation, however, would favour correction over connection
in order to provide correct stimuli leading to correct response. This raises an issue
that has been endemic in education. What works ‘best’ depends on what you value
in your measurements, with objectivists valuing, and so measuring, quite different
things from constructivists.
In the short to medium term, it is likely that parameters for learning theories
that machine teaching will operate by will be set by human programmers. This
may result in some incredibly consistent teaching, and maybe even highly creative,
varied machine teaching, engaging students within the boundaries set. But it also risks
escalating paradigm wars to new levels, with AI programed to follow parameters that
prioritise one end of the learning continuum or the other. It could result in unequivocal
understandings about the superiority of a theory in a specific set of circumstances,
and/or in vested interests competing for the commercialisation of their AI/paradigm
package.
AI could, of course, be programmed with parameters that allow for and encourage
the full spectrum of theories, interpreted along the MELT continuum of learning
autonomy. The window of opportunity for educators to play a role in determining
such parameters is closing and with late 2023 being when the earth is predicted to hit 8
billion people alive at the same time, that provides 2020–2023 to make decisions that
will impact on the education of the next billion human brains. The main readership
of this book, teachers of young children to supervisors of PhD students, have a little
time to inject a deeper sense of humanity into the debate about Machine Teaching.
Maybe parents and other citizens would like to think too about the ramifications of
narrow or competing sets of learning paradigms for machine teachers. In addition,
all of us have to wonder if we want a future where human teachers are increasingly
irrelevant, and where maybe there is little point of human learners when machine
learners get all the jobs.
Each perspective in 5.1.1–5.1.4 provides a currently useful consideration for
engaged learning and teaching, each can be challenged by the other theories, and
each can be placed on the extent of autonomy continuum. Placing them in such a
way allows each theory to be held tentatively, without considering any of the four
to represent ‘the truth’. Weighing together different educational theories helps us to
understand how they can inform and improve engaged learning and teaching. When
112 5 What Does It Mean?
we bring in Machine Learning and Teaching, the stakes about differences in learning
theories become much higher, mission critical. While we have human teachers, or
maybe to keep teachers human, what are the insights that learning theories can pro-
vide to improving student learning, when viewed in a complementary way through
the MELT?
Using MELT to connect different learning theories has practical implications for
student learning. Firstly, curricula need to have strong conceptual underpinnings.
Such underpinnings help teachers to facilitate student acquisition of a contemporary
knowledge base and investigate areas of interest in ways that develop sophisticated
thinking and rigour. Secondly, curricula themselves need to improve and adapt over
time in order to enhance learning and teaching. By using MELT, educators can hold
a variety of perspectives, such as Direct Instruction and discovery learning, so that
instead of conflicting, the perspectives can work in unison to inform curricula and
their improvement. MELT reduces educators’ obligation to choose one perspective
or theory only, and increases their capacity or willingness to hold several in pro-
ductive tension. Such multiply-informed curricula, taken together as a set over time,
may better scaffold the development of sophisticated thinking when compared to
a set of curricula informed by a narrow range of strategies. This is only true if a
way of connecting, like MELT, enables teachers to conceptually unite a variety of
perspectives so there is a legitimate coherence between them.
Which curricula are best able to provide learning that spans learner autonomy,
that enables the acquiring and construction of students’ own knowledge, and the
simultaneous development of the skills associated with sophisticated thinking? The
answer is not in the curricula, which vary enormously, but in how teachers bring a
particular curriculum to life, improving it over time through planning, implementa-
tion and revision. Ongoing improvement of this kind is here called ‘action research’,
and maybe an imperative to keep teaching human.
Action research entails teachers intentionally enhancing the conditions for learning
through spirals of action and improvement, in contexts for which they have direct
responsibility. Much published educational research comes under this definition of
AR, although such research is frequently labelled as something else. The compo-
nent of the definition from which some people distance themselves is ‘have direct
responsibility for’. One reason for this distancing is that a phenomenon for which
the researchers have teaching responsibility entails a ‘subjective’ engagement rather
than ‘detached’ observation. In terms of standards of objective research, subjectivity
5.2 MELT for Curriculum Design and Improvement 113
Generalisable Transferable
teachers engaging in AR have to deal with aspects that RCT approaches see as
‘confounding’, such as student motivation to engage long-term in the learning.
Action research is powerful because it is fitted to the specific context in which
it is enacted, by the teachers who have responsibility and ownership of the learning
environment. This leads to ownership of the research, a strong interest in positive out-
comes and steering towards the best student learning possible. While the researcher
has a vested interest to confirm that their own approach works in AR (compromising
the study’s objectivity and generalisability), this is also AR’s strength, for positive
learning outcomes provide an endorsement of teacher professional judgement. The
teacher’s ownership of the research speaks to the heart of their educational prowess, a
self-validation but not a generalisation to other contexts. Table 5.1 shows the features
of AR when compared to RCTs.
Analysis of multiple MELT-informed AR studies could inform not only individ-
ual implementers, but assist a move from transferable to generalisable research. A
systematic review of studies about research-based learning found that the amount of
guidance provided by teachers to students was reported in an ad hoc manner. The
review’s authors, therefore, recommended that an a priori framework be used for
reporting [22]. MELT’s continuum of learning autonomy provides a cross-studies
language to describe ‘amount of guidance’, and along with the six facets, it provides
an a priori framework that is both adaptable and readily useable for reporting. Meta-
analyses of AR, if MELT facets and autonomy are used to connect multiple studies
together, may show that these studies have some generalisable findings. MELT as an
a priori framework for analysis and reporting can connect the reportable outcomes
of otherwise non-generalisable AR, providing a trend over many studies.
Action research can begin with low rigour and sophistication, but as AR continues,
it should demonstrate increases in rigour as part of the learning inherent in research
as a process. A practical way of adding rigour is ensuring that AR researchers use and
report on all of the facets of MELT. Increases in rigour can be assisted by reflection-
in-action, and simultaneously by adding to personal, idiosyncratic approaches, those
informed by the literature. As an example of increase in rigour, Table 5.2 considers
pertinent questions, facet by facet, early and later in AR cycles of actions. With MELT
to inform ways to lift rigour of AR processes, and to enable connections between
different AR studies, this gives AR the means to improve student learning in ways
5.2 MELT for Curriculum Design and Improvement 117
Table 5.2 Questions and issues for each facet and level of learning autonomy in relation to
commencing AR and maturing AR
Early cycle of AR Later cycles of AR
Embark & clarify What is my biggest problem? What problems are endemic
What is my purpose? across the years of Science,
English, etc.?
Find & generate Do I have an idea worth trying? What approaches are in the
What do I need? Who can I ask? literature? What data can I collect
that is relevant?
Evaluate & reflect Is this the best option to try? How trustworthy is the data I
What do I trust? collected?
Organise & manage How will I implement this? How do I sustain this with other
How do we arrange? teachers?
Analyse & synthesise How much did this approach How much did this change
What does it mean? improve student learning? colleagues’ understanding about
student learning?
Communicate How do I tell my students? How do I tell parents, school staff
& apply or an educational conference?
How do we relate?
Extent of Autonomy How much guidance do they Can students negotiate their own
need? preferred autonomy?
MELT helps teachers to interpret educational theories by plotting them along the
learning autonomy continuum. This allows them to connect to and use theories as
part of their teaching repertoire. For example, if a teacher needed to choose between
CLT or Connectivism to inform a contemporary curriculum design, each theory
implies a very different design with correspondingly different learning opportunities
for students. However, connecting these theories through MELT using the continuum
of learning autonomy to inform when and how to use each theory can help teachers
design a curriculum which is more holistic and helpful for engaged learning.
118 5 What Does It Mean?
References
15. Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (2016). The constructivist Credo: Yvonna S. Lincoln Egon G.
Guba. In The Constructivist Credo (pp. 25). Routledge.
16. Jacobs, A. M., & Weaver, R. K. (2015). When policies undo themselves: Self-undermining
feedback as a source of policy change. Governance, 28(4), 441–457.
17. Joshua, J. (2017). Economic policies of economic growth and development. In China’s Eco-
nomic Growth: Towards Sustainable Economic Development and Social Justice (pp. 45–80).
Palgrave Macmillan, London.
18. Tracy, S. J., & Hinrichs, M. M. (2017). Big tent criteria for qualitative quality. The International
Encyclopedia of Communication Research Methods, 1–10.
19. Sedgwick, P., & Greenwood, N. (2015). Understanding the Hawthorne effect. British Medical
Journal, 351, h4672.
20. De Coninck, K., Valcke, M., Ophalvens, I., & Vanderlinde, R. (2019). Bridging the theory-
practice gap in teacher education: The design and construction of simulation-based learning
environments. In Kohärenz in der Lehrerbildung (pp. 263–280). Springer VS, Wiesbaden.
21. Baker, M. (2016). Is there a reproducibility crisis? A Nature survey lifts the lid on how
researchers view the crisis rocking science and what they think will help. Nature, 533(7604),
452–455.
22. Lazonder, A., & Harmsen, R. (2016). Meta-analysis of inquiry-based learning: Effects of
guidance. Review of Educational Research, 86(3), 681–718.
Open Access This chapter is licensed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0
International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits use, sharing,
adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate
credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license and
indicate if changes were made.
The images or other third party material in this chapter are included in the chapter’s Creative
Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not
included in the chapter’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by
statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from
the copyright holder.
Chapter 6
How Do We Relate?
This chapter shows that what is needed in formal and informal education is not only
the connections between theories and practice that have been called for so far in the
book but also something that enables people to genuinely relate to each other and
disparate ideas. This chapter asks, ‘How do we relate?’ Like other MELT questions,
we can ask this question in relation to the small scale: within a class, or a small group
investigation before lunch. Or we can ask it in relation to a larger scale: how do we
relate learning from one lesson to the next, from one subject, one term, one year, one
stage of education to the next? Ultimately, the question becomes ‘how do we relate
our thinking about teaching and learning to that of other educators?’ The MELT
provide a conceptualisation that represents these different progressions, people and
perspectives, and that facilitates connected relationships.
This chapter focuses on the problems associated with the relationships between
humans, as well as relationships between humans and the planet. It looks at why
things have proceeded in a direction that was, to a large extent, inevitable in terms of
how human solutions to problems, including technology, caused problems that were
sometimes larger than the ones originally solved. However, now that we have a more
comprehensive view of the planet than ever before, the chapter moves on to consider
whether our future is, or may become, a little less inevitable. As noted in Chap. 5, part
of the solution to entrenched or emerging problems is rethinking what theory does for
education, and taking a complementary rather than a competing perspective of theory.
This will enable more unified efforts towards providing learning environments that
develop thinkers who can solve humanity’s and Earth’s problems.
First in this chapter is a story about cosmonauts and astronauts uniting in space
during the middle of the Cold War. This story is a reminder that connections between
clashing perspectives can be worthwhile and powerful, and that such connections
can be made against all odds. The story provides context for how we might relate to
those with perspectives on important aspects of life (including education) which are
the polar opposite of our own.
Energies of teachers and researchers need to unite around shared, adaptable and
culturally sensitive models for education that graduate students who have the research
mindedness to solve the many entrenched problems, and new problems that will
plague us from 2040, in a way that does not create additional problems. How do
we enable the next billion brains born to become primarily constructive people who
desire to build up society, the environment and each other? Student sophisticated
thinking requires faciliation by professional teachers who are not forced into ‘best
practice’ by others, but have the discernment to be constructive. For students and
teachers alike, to be constructive is not to be persuasive or building-oriented, but
nurturing and full of care.
The communist USSR and the capitalist USA arguably represented the twentieth
century’s most polarised, long-term adversarial positions. But in the Smithsonian
Air and Space Museum in Washington DC, there is an amazing configuration: a
re-enactment of a moment from 1975, when, in the middle of the Cold War, the
USSR and USA cooperated at the highest, most sensitive and most complex levels
(Fig. 6.1).
In the museum is a thirty metre contraption comprising a USSR-era Soyuz craft
docked with a US Apollo module. Despite the two nations’ adversarial politics and
their appearance of extreme competition, especially in space, both realised that the
competition would kill them. To begin to unite on earth, they chose a symbolic act of
Fig. 6.1 Apollo and Soyuz coupled in the air and space Museum, Washington, D.C.
6.1 Soyuz and Apollo: A Story About a Cold War Meeting in Orbit 123
uniting in space. This act is not famous, but the docking of the craft may have been
the beginning of the end of the Cold War.
The Apollo–Soyuz Test Project required both groups, years in advance of 1975, to
share top-secret information on guidance systems, space hardware and software [2].
The two ships were very different. For example, their entry hatch sizes were incom-
patible, so a three-metre ‘docking unit’ needed to be engineered for the event. The
Soyuz normally operated on pure oxygen at 1/3 atmosphere, whereas the US craft
used air at 1 atmosphere [2]; the Americans would have blown up the Russians had
they docked and connected their triple-pressure atmosphere. The two teams had to
share trajectories, launch times, positional information and operational information.
Then the astronauts needed to engage with the cosmonauts socially—they couldn’t
dock and sit in separate capsules! They had to be willing to communicate with those
who had not only a different mother tongue, but a very different ideology. The lan-
guage barrier was perceived to be one of the biggest obstacles, and so the Americans
learned Russian before launch and spoke it in when docked, while the Russians
learned and spoke English [2]. The crews ate a meal together, shared memorabilia,
signed international certificates and hoped, as their respective presidents watched
via a live telecast (alongside millions of their citizens), that ‘our joint work in space
serves for the benefit of all countries and peoples on the earth’ [2].
I am writing this on the eve of the fiftieth anniversary of the first moonwalk, but
that event further escalated the Cold War, whereas the Soyuz–Apollo Project helped
to defrost it. The threat of mutual annihilation provoked by the nuclear and space race
was a sufficient stimulus to prompt changes in the way that the USSR and the USA
related to each other. However, to actually change political and public sentiment is a
highly charged affair, and the Soyuz–Apollo docking was a kind of circuit breaker
that allowed high-voltage differences to leak out over time. It is salient that the event
which generated more tension (the moon-landing) is famous, while the one that
began a genuine connection between warring parties (the Apollo–Soyuz docking) is
almost forgotten. As a species, we tend to prefer winning over cooperating, and this
is food-for-thought for anyone involved in educational disconnections.
Current global deterioration, one would think, should be enough to prompt a
similar response. However, we no longer feel the sense of urgency that came with
the possibility that one button-push could launch the world into a nuclear winter.
Our concerns about nuclear annihilation come and go with the news headlines. Our
biggest current earth-wide issues, however, arguably involve a slow decline of the
planet’s ecosystem, causing habitat destruction that is induced especially by over-
population pressures and the increasing prosperity sought by billions. This slower-
speed issue is hard to resolve without concerted, unified and sustained agreement by
many governments. While the overarching problem of the Cold War was evident, it is
difficult to even identify the problems facing Earth in 2020, and there are now many
more parties involved than the two main governments of the Cold War. Maybe a big
124 6 How Do We Relate?
Humans for 100,000 years have been outstanding problem solvers. However, as
noted in Chap. 1, many of the solutions we have found have resulted in further
problems which are more difficult to solve than the ones we started with. So our
skill at solving problems was, in part, a function of the sheer number of problems
we caused, including the Cold War.
This capacity to solve problems while inadvertently causing more problems does
not mean that humans are wicked and greedy in a way that separates them from the
rest of nature. Rather, the process was inevitable. A species so well-equipped for
sophisticated thinking, with a body that could work in a way that corresponded to
that thinking, was powerfully adaptive to its environment for 100,000 years. Then
Homo sapiens began to adapt the environment to suit it, tens of thousands of years
ago [3]. At first, such adaptations of the environment were small: the intentional use
of fire to manage foliage and grazing [3], or the collection of grass seeds in the fertile
crescent in order to sow it in a specific well-watered location [4].
In addition to achieving what was intended, some problems’ solutions produced
unanticipated effects. Planting grass seeds allowed small populations to remain in one
place for longer, reducing the need to travel and more predictable food supplies made
it possible to establish larger family groups [5]. The consequent rise in population
afforded our ancestors more protection from predators and from competing bands of
humans [6]. Escalation of agricultural technology ensued, providing a competitive
advantage over humans who did not plant seeds [6]. Technology compounded, with
success growing on the back of technological success [7]. But no-one anticipated the
inevitable problems associated with such success. How could Homo sapiens have
anticipated such problems?
Until around 50,000 years ago, nothing humans did compared to the environmental
change wrought by beavers (as noted in Chap. 1). Had beavers been equipped with
learning brains and grasping thumbs, they might have caused far more environmental
degradation than they did, at a rate that would have put humans in the shade. However,
super-specialisation locked them into a niche that was hard to break out of, and even
in 20 million more years, beaver descendants may still be dam engineers. In contrast,
humans were generalists, able to run (slowly), climb (poorly), fight (weakly), build
(badly) and learn adaptively. While we may be slower than cheetahs, weaker than
gorillas and less architecturally intuitive than termites, our learning capacity means
6.2 Inevitable Earth 125
that we will almost inevitably land a human on Mars. That is, unless the compounding
problems associated with our compounding solutions catch up with us first! An Earth
human population which crashes to several million, say, following an environmental
cataclysm, is going to have problems visiting the neighbours on the other side of the
stream [8], let alone getting off the planet.
Given human brain capability and our anatomy, it was inevitable that human
capacity got us to this point of compounding problems. An interesting example of
inevitable, compounding problems cropped up yesterday when I attended a public
presentation on quantum computing [9]. The presenter argued that quantum comput-
ers would be able to hack existing digital security protocols within the next five years.
The only remedy he presented for this was the adoption of new quantum computer-
generated security systems. Such a state of affairs is reminiscent of a self-fulfilling
prophecy, where the need for a technology is, in part, created by the existence of
the technology. A reflection on the inevitability of this process may take some of
the pressure off us. We are a self-incriminating species, and it can help to pause and
understand that we are, or were, part of the biosphere—not especially weird or holy
or special.
However, understanding the inevitability of a deteriorating Earth is not an excuse
to say, ‘that’s fine’. We can now see the entirety of the planet and understand our
place in it. Indeed, since Yuri Gagarin’s journey in 1961, we have been able to see
the whole Earth from space [10]. With our information gathering and sharing, we
can now perceive in great detail our impact on the planet, and with that knowledge
we have a chance to make global deterioration a little less inevitable. More than
ever, we are able to see the extreme social stratification and isolation, environmental
degradation and species extinction, as well as escalations in our capacity to annihilate.
But our ability to observe these problems does not guarantee that we will do anything
effective about them.
If we continue in our very intelligent ways of solving problems, then maybe the fate
of the Earth is sealed: inevitable species extinction and a human population crash.
For a model of the scale of crash possible, the Mayan civilisation was thought to
comprise between 15 and 30 million people at its peak, and the population crashed
to thousands in several decades [11]. If a crash of similar severity were to hit the
planet in 2023, this would mean that the population of 8 billion humans would be
reduced to a few million. Such a crash has happened more than once in large and
small human populations, and it could happen on a global scale [11].
There are not currently any palatable solutions to mitigate the problems we face
with a large human population. In 1979 or earlier, China elected to minimise its
126 6 How Do We Relate?
population growth through the one-child policy. This resulted in the ‘prioritisation’
of boys over girls, with estimates of girls ‘missing’ in China varying from 20 million to
160 million [12]. As is common, our solutions often have perverse and unpredictable
consequences that cause more problems.
Therefore, it is no longer enough for us to merely solve problems. We need minds
that can genuinely anticipate problems that will result from solutions and mitigate
these or, even better, look for solutions that ‘first do no harm’. If our education systems
can produce critical thinkers capable of creative solutions that anticipate subsequent
problems, our earth’s immediate future may be a little less inevitable. In order to lead
us to a trajectory where planetary destruction is not assured, these thinkers will need
to be primarily ‘constructive’, rather than self-serving or ideology-based, and have
had a mind-expanding education. With a connected education informed by MELT,
they could prompt a less fated, more evitable earth trajectory with room for hope.
From a MELT perspective, each passionately held theory and approach can help
the community to build a little towards a mind-expanding education. Let educators
and parents with different perspectives talk and, if they are ‘poles apart’, at least
perceive the ground between the poles. For example, a big focus on content acqui-
sition may have some great advantages in terms of discovery learning, if students
have learned some key and pertinent ideas. Likewise, discovery learning might be a
great motivator towards learning content. In Parachute (Chap. 1), Shelly knew about
independent and dependent variables, and she may have acquired these concepts in
a prescribed context. She applied these tricky concepts in a personal instance of
discovery learning that was open-ended. Although she faced many difficulties, she
applied the concepts of experimental research effectively, and conducted research
that demonstrated sophisticated thinking in a science context.
From the MELT perspective, there is no philosophical law against jumping from
facilitating prescribed to open-ended learning nor from unbounded to bounded learn-
ing. But teachers implementing a curriculum need to have discernment and power
to implement their well-reasoned judgements about what is best for each situation.
MELT can enhance the capacity for discernment, because as an analytical tool, it is
functional, addressing the practical questions, ‘what do these students need?’ and
‘how much guidance?’
If we treat theories, by definition, as competitive, then we may continue to have a
problem. Given the complexities of learning and teaching, educational theories may
need to complement each other more and fight less. Seeing theories and perspectives
as more metaphorical and less literal might help educators to at least acknowledge
theories that are a pole away from their own perspective.
6.4 Retheorising Theory in Education, from ‘Competition’ … 127
In Chap. 1, I proposed the enterprise of educating human brains so that they have
the capacity to solve problems without causing unanticipated additional problems.
This educational wiring would involve brains that have a substantial content knowl-
edge base of fundamental concepts. It would also involve brains that take risks,
delve into issues and problems, and are highly discerning. While learning content
and learning through delving can be presented in mutually exclusive ways, in MELT
they belong to the same continuum of learning autonomy. Rather than conflicting
with each other, they are complementary.
Tara’s resilient understanding of content in Stupid was contrary to the scientific
canon. She showed that we cannot merely say, ‘Give students lifelong learning skills.
All the information they need is available, so they just need to know how to access
it.’ This is a very tenuous position to hold, no matter how often it is said. If we don’t
understand ideas, we won’t even know what we are holding, and we certainly won’t
be able to readily synthesise multiple ideas.
This next billion humans born from 2023–2030 may be the make-or-break gen-
eration. They will inherit all the problems of the planet, including those which have
been made, inevitably, by the 100 billion other brains that came before them. They
will enter formal education from 2024 onwards, and most of those billion will com-
plete their compulsory education around 2050. The problems we face and will face
are still improperly identified,hidden or not yet created, and the solutions are out of
our present-brained generation’s league. Overall, MELT is an opportunity to put into
the hands of the billion the sophisticated thinking tools that they will need. These
are the tools of the inquiring ape, because these tools of sophisticated thinking are
the best we have. But now we need to connect disparate efforts and contexts so that
we can ‘rachet up’ our sophisticated thinking to enable us to solve problems with
solutions that first do no harm.
As a species, we named ourselves the ‘wise man’. For an animal that destroyed its
own environment through desertification, salinification, heavy-metal contamination
and warfare, the word ‘wise’ seems a little off-the-mark. Beavers, with their smooth
brains and genetically stored behaviours for dam construction radically altered their
environments, but at such a pace that ecosystems were able to evolve along with the
change. Our learning brain seems to learn too fast for ecosystems to catch up, but
not fast enough to enable us to craft solutions that don’t make things worse. We are
both too smart and not wise enough.
128 6 How Do We Relate?
References
1. Einstein Bubble in Cartoon: Paraphrased from Einstein, A. (1934). On the method of theoretical
physics. Philosophy of Science, 1(2), 163–169.
2. Redmond, C. (2004, July 19). The flight of Apollo-Soyuz. Retrieved from https://history.nasa.
gov/apollo/apsoyhist.html.
3. Harari, Y. N. (2014). Sapiens: A brief history of humankind. New York, NY: Harper.
4. Diamond, J. (1997). Location, location, location: The first farmers. Science, 278(5341), 1243–
1244.
5. Holliday, R. (2005). Evolution of human longevity, population pressure and the origins of
warfare. Biogerontology, 6(5), 363–368.
6. Armelagos, G. J., Goodman, A. H., & Jacobs, K. H. (1991). The origins of agriculture:
Population growth during a period of declining health. Population and Environment, 13(1),
9–22.
7. Van Schaik, C. P., Pradhan, G. R., & Tennie, C. (2019). Teaching and curiosity: Sequen-
tial drivers of cumulative cultural evolution in the hominin lineage. Behavioral Ecology and
Sociobiology, 73(1), 2.
8. Diamond, J. (2013). The world until yesterday: What can we learn from traditional societies?.
London, England: Penguin.
9. Sparkes, B (2019). Quantum of promise. Public presentation. University of Adelaide, Adelaide,
Australia.
10. Faure, G., & Mensing, T. M. (2007). Introduction to planetary science. Dordrecht, Netherlands:
Springer.
11. Diamond, J. (2005). Collapse: How societies choose to fail or succeed. London, England:
Penguin.
12. Kennedy, J. J., & Shi, Y. (2019). Lost and found: The missing girls in rural China. New York,
NY: Oxford University Press.
References 129
Open Access This chapter is licensed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0
International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits use, sharing,
adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate
credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license and
indicate if changes were made.
The images or other third party material in this chapter are included in the chapter’s Creative
Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not
included in the chapter’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by
statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from
the copyright holder.
Chapter 7
How Much Guidance?
One great piece of feedback on the MELT, written on a post-it note during a workshop
introducing the models, was ‘obvious’. That terse slight in many ways reflected that
which the MELT was intended to convey: what educators do when they facilitate
learning effectively. It is common that an educator looks at a version of MELT
and says ‘this is what I’m already doing!’ That is perfect, for MELT provides an
articulation of what educators often do implicitly and makes it explicit. For the
person who said ‘obvious’, the question is whether their teaching approach is as
obvious to their students or colleagues as it is to them. The answer is probably ‘no’.
Educators use MELT for guidance when they perceive a need to move from leaving
understanding implicit to making it explicit, in order to
(1) improve their awareness of their own practice and its connection to theory;
(2) enhance student metacognition, especially students’ regulation of their own
learning;
(3) connect disparate teaching energies, practices, sectors and theories.
As noted in earlier chapters, learning autonomy in MELT is not a characteristic
that increases linearly, but a relationship: between students, between students and
teachers, and between students and concepts. Engaged teachers attend implicitly to
learning autonomy, because it is fundamental to ownership of the learning enterprise.
MELT supports an explicit exploration of learning autonomy.
A lot was made in the previous chapters about the continuum of learning auton-
omy. Chapter 2 discussed the continuum in relation to students and Chaps. 4 and 5
discussed it in relation to theory and action research. To reiterate here, autonomy in
MELT is not an attribute to be attained to or an optimum level of guidance. A con-
tinuum suggests that there is a broad spectrum of possibilities, where all parts of the
continuum are vital for learning: there is nothing inherently superior about initiate on
© The Author(s) 2020 131
J. Willison, The Models of Engaged Learning and Teaching,
SpringerBriefs in Education, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-15-2683-1_7
132 7 How Much Guidance?
this continuum, nor is there anything implicitly lesser about emulate. The important
thing is to ask frequently, ‘how much guidance do these students need?’ Autonomy
in MELT depends on the relationship between learners and what is learned, where it
is learned and who it is learned with. Learning autonomy distinctly ebbs and flows
in a tidal zone, moving from low autonomy to high autonomy and back.
Measuring or pinning down learning autonomy is awkward, because it is a messy
and recursive concept... Yet autonomy is absolutely central to daily teaching and
learning. Because learning autonomy is a ‘relationship term’ in MELT, not an abso-
lute quality, students don’t become autonomous. Rather, they operate at times with
higher levels of autonomy, depending on their familiarity with context and the rigour
needed in the situation, and they frequently cycle back to lower levels of autonomy in
less familiar contexts. This is what we found with employed graduates, who shuttle
back and forth between different levels of autonomy in employment, showing that
autonomy is non-linear and context-specific [2]. If MELT helps educators understand
how much learning autonomy is required by a student, that ‘measurement’ can make
a lot of sense for those who know and understand the context and inform teaching
practices in practical ways. However, to say ‘x% of students were working with high
levels of autonomy’ does not communicate anything meaningful to those without
knowledge of the context.
In practical ways, MELT promotes constructive alignment to engineer quality,
engaged learning. For example, MELT can inspire cognitive and affective learning
outcomes or goals (Chap. 1), inform the design of face-to-face, online and blended
learning environments (Chap. 3), inform the assessment of learning (Chap. 3) and
help structure evaluative elements of course effectiveness (Chap. 5). However, con-
structive alignment only comes to life through the efforts of engaged teachers, for it
requires teacher care and empowerment to realistically engage learners.
The MELT are used in a variety of ways for teaching, learning and researching, and
this necessarily includes the assessment of student learning. Crucially, the continuum
of learning autonomy suggests a student move from reliance on the teacher for
feedback towards self-assessment, and back again in fresh contexts. Assessment
rubrics, each informed by the six facets of MELT but with performance criteria
made specific to a task, provide something in common between assignments. Such
rubrics also provide something specific to that task alone. MELT rubrics straddle the
middle ground between rubrics that are too specific to allow students to generalise
their learning from it [3] and those that are too general and therefore provide little
guidance. Importantly, when students use teacher feedback from one assignment
to improve a subsequent assignment, the connections between assignments due to
MELT make a substantial difference [4]. As students calibrate to teacher expectations
using MELT-informed rubrics, they become more realistic in their self-assessment
according to the rubric, and more able to give useful feedback to peers [5]. As shown
in Chap. 3, an effective technique is to allocate marks to students’ response to peer
feedback, as well as marks for the feedback they give to others, as opposed to students
allocating marks to students.
While this book has emphasised learning autonomy, teachers themselves need to
model different levels of autonomy. Autonomy and ownership is a crucial dimension
7.1 Autonomy: Engaged Learning, Engaged Teaching 133
for teachers, because the MELT only work when educators make the models their
own. If we want students to become increasingly autonomous and take risks in
learning, then teachers themselves must have a license to act autonomously, at times,
in order to show students what autonomy looks like. There is a need, then, to consider
factors that increase or decrease teacher autonomy, because this implicitly affects
teacher engagement and effectiveness.
When teachers do not show initiative or willingness to improvise, this may speak
to the nature of the systems they teach in. This nature is often determined by what
learning institutions reward and what the consequences of non-compliance are. Cur-
riculum developers and policymakers should consider factors that mediate for and
against teacher autonomy and ownership, including curriculum and quality assurance
processes created for course, program and institution level.
MELT’s facets articulated along the continuum of learning autonomy provide
a framing that is ‘sufficiently well structured’ and ‘sufficiently nebulous’, as an
educator said [6]. Another educator likened MELT to a thin silver wire providing
the structure in which shimmery soap bubbles can emerge [7]. As the MELT draws
on 100 years of educational research and practice, it contains nothing of a surprise
in its separate elements. Rather, it is the juxtaposition of the facets with the levels
of learning autonomy that makes the MELT distinctive in structure. But it is the
educators who care, and their students, who make the MELT fluid through human
interactions.
Overall, the MELT help to:
1. Envisage connections by providing a conceptualisation that holds in tension dis-
parate educational practitioners’, administrators, communities and researchers’
perspectives.
2. Join together separate disciplinary and transdisciplinary usage.
3. Map the educational landscape, because they are informed by numerous
educational theories, conceptual frameworks and practices.
4. Spiral through education and become a thinking routine, from primary school
to Ph.D. level.
5. Delineate learning autonomy, unpacking how much guidance learners and
teachers need.
6. Differentiate the curriculum, where the consideration of autonomy promotes an
understanding of how to manage the curriculum for gifted and talented students,
students with learning disabilities, and the full range of ‘average’ students.
7. Couple cognitive and affective learning, foregrounding the relationship between
these domains
8. Prompt ethical, social and culturally minded teaching and learning.
9. Pose seven probing questions to teachers and students that are key across all
educational contexts.
10. Materialise in audience-dependent ways, where educators adapt and articulate
their own MELT, fit to the context and audience, even as the nature of the six
facets and of the continuum of learning autonomy are maintained.
134 7 How Much Guidance?
The characteristics of MELT are based on 100 years of education research and mirror
100,000 years of human learning as experienced by 100 billion Homo sapiens. The
next billion brains born from 2023 to 2030 need something different to those which
have already been born. Existing members of our species brought us to the amazing
creativity, adaptability, and problem-solving that will enable, in all likelihood, a
human population of 8 billion alive in 2023. However, despite the massive problem-
solving endeavour of our species, we are facing the catastrophic consequences of our
solutions.
We need some guidance towards a mode of learning that will help the next billion
brains, at the very least, to do no further damage to our world. We need to earn the
name sapiens. MELT provides this guidance in a conceptual manner, but this guid-
ance should not be represented as a set of rules or mandates. The MELT’s adaptable
structure absolutely requires the breath of creativity to bring it to life in particular
subjects, disciplines and contexts. The MELT are fully dependent on Homo sapiens’
willingness to adapt, to heat solid structures with the warmth of human interac-
tion until conversations become fluid, and then crystallise in new shapes for student
learning. If there are any things that are unique about the MELT, these include the
articulation of the facets of sophisticated thinking along the continuum of learning
autonomy, and the consequent way the models are able to simply connect theories
with theories and theories with practice. The MELT’s shared questions are effective
prompts to engage teaching and learning. They continuously ask teachers and stu-
dents, ‘What will we use? What do we trust? How do we arrange? What does it mean?
How do we relate? How much guidance?’ and especially, ‘What is our purpose?’
Melting, flowing, shaping and crystallising need to happen time and again, so that
each of the MELT suits its context and is renewed when there are changes in that
context. Each adapted MELT model interlinks conceptually with all other MELTs,
7.2 Conclusion: Structure Provided, Creativity Needed 135
and can help teachers and students make the connection across education’s years,
subjects, ideologies, and contexts. The MELT provide the adaptive fluidity we need
to conceptualise the coherent and connected learning journey needed for the billion
brains born from 2023. The children of Kevin, Shelly and Tara could be beneficiaries
of an adept use of MELT, enabling them to experience and perceive their own forest
of engaged learning. If those billion brains experience a coherent and connected
curriculum, they may become sophisticated thinkers who craft solutions that first
cause no harm. As they become the generation to lead us globally, they could pull us
back from inevitable, accelerating, planetary degradation so that we all may enjoy
a more evitable Earth trajectory. Remaining endangered habitats may be preserved
despite increased human population and polar ice might remain frozen, because
education, early childhood to Ph.D. is fluidly connected with the MELT.
References
1. Einstein Bubble in Cartoon: Einstein, A. (2010). The ultimate quotable Einstein. Princeton, NJ:
Princeton University Press.
2. Willison, J., Sabir, F., & Thomas, J. (2017). Shifting dimensions of autonomy in students’
research and employment. Higher Education Research & Development, 36(2), 430–443.
3. Adcroft, A. (2011). The mythology of feedback. Higher Education Research & Development,
30(4), 405–419.
4. Willison, J. (2012). When academics integrate research skill development in the curriculum.
Higher Education Research and Development, 31(6), 905–919.
5. Wu, C., Chanda, E., & Willison, J. (2014). Implementation and outcomes of online self and
peer assessment on group based honours research projects. Assessment & Evaluation in Higher
Education, 39(1), 21–37.
6. Willison, J., & Buisman Pijlman, F. (2016). Ph.D. prepared: Research skill development across
the undergraduate years. International Journal of Researcher Development, 7(1), 63–83.
7. Wisker, G. (2018). Frameworks and freedoms: Supervising research learning and the undergrad-
uate dissertation. Journal of University Teaching and Learning Practice, 15(4), 2.
Open Access This chapter is licensed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0
International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits use, sharing,
adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate
credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license and
indicate if changes were made.
The images or other third party material in this chapter are included in the chapter’s Creative
Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not
included in the chapter’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by
statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from
the copyright holder.
Glossary of MELT Terms
Analyse: to break down into constituent parts and pull together again in ways that
show relationships. In quantitative research, these relationships involve trends.
In qualitative research, these relationships involve themes. There is a close and
recursive relationship between analyse and organise. Moreover, at times, anal-
ysis has strong evaluative elements; however, at other times they are distinctly
different.
Apply: in the context of a learning task, put current knowledge and understanding
into use cognitively, affectively and/or physically. Also put newly-developed
knowledge into use in the context of a future task or problem.
Autonomy: the extent to which students drive the sophisticated thinking processes
in MELT, which is related to the extent to which educators set the learning
environment parameters.
Cognitive Load Theory (John Sweller): Human Working Memory has a small
capacity and easy to overload with incoming information, especially multi-
sensory information.
Communicate: ongoing process from start to finish of own or team’s articulations,
written, spoken and portrayed, with active listening and response. Frequently
this leads to products for an audience, who may provide part of a feedback loop
for ongoing improvement.
Connectivism (George Sieman’s): Information and knowledge are available to a
learner from networks of people, devices and other sources, and often accessed
at time of need.
Clarify: fine-tune one’s purpose through a deepened understanding of sophisticated
thinking.
Constructive: a desire to build up, not merely to persuade or gain self-benefit.
Creative: making something new to oneself and one’s team, whether through the
synthesis of parts or from scratch; not mimicry.
Curious: intoxicated by an issue or phenomenon and inspired to pursue it; not
disengaged.
Determined: continuing until one has an appropriate resolution; not a gathering of
information or generation of data that is slapdash.
© The Editor(s) (if applicable) and The Author(s) 2020 137
J. Willison, The Models of Engaged Learning and Teaching,
SpringerBriefs in Education, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-15-2683-1
138 Glossary of MELT Terms
A D
Affective, 6–8, 19, 22, 23, 29, 31, 34, 35, 38– Determined, 7, 14, 22, 40–43, 45–48, 62, 87,
44, 47–49, 53, 55–57, 65, 130, 131, 131, 135
136 Discerning, 7, 44–47, 83, 86, 89, 126, 136
Analyse, 7, 42, 47, 49–52, 65, 67, 79, 84, 85,
87, 88, 90, 92, 116, 117, 135
Apollo, 70, 122 E
Apply, 7, 36, 37, 42, 43, 53–57, 64, 65, 70, Embark, 7, 8, 15, 23, 30, 31, 34–36, 38–41,
87, 89, 106, 117, 135 53, 56, 65, 70, 87, 117, 136
Autonomy, 5, 8, 16, 23, 24, 30, 32, 33, 39– Empathetic, 57
42, 44, 47, 49, 52, 56, 61, 62, 64, 67, Evaluate, 7, 42, 44, 45, 51, 54, 56, 65, 84,
74, 77, 80, 87, 97, 100, 105–109, 111, 85, 87, 88, 114, 117
112, 116, 117, 129–132, 135, 136 Evitable earth, 19, 123, 125, 127, 133
B H
Beavers Harmonising, 7, 47, 48, 136
dams, 18, 19, 124
learning, 5, 18–20, 23, 124
I
C Inevitable earth, 19, 123
Cambodia, 34, 36, 37, 40, 43, 45, 47, 48, 55,
79
Clarify, 7, 8, 23, 30, 31, 33–36, 38–42, 53, L
56, 65, 70, 87, 107, 109, 117 Learning autonomy, 5, 6, 8–10, 16, 20, 21,
Cognitive Load Theory, 5, 103, 105–109, 23–25, 29, 30, 32–34, 41, 42, 44, 74,
117, 135 77, 87, 95–98, 100, 103, 105, 106,
Communicate, 7, 22, 36, 37, 42, 53–57, 65, 109–111, 116–118, 126, 129–132
66, 74, 87, 117, 123, 130, 135
Constructive, 7, 53, 55, 57, 91, 122, 125, 130
Constructivism M
personal, 90, 91, 93, 95–98 Machine learning, 110–112, 136
social, 90, 91, 94–98, 110 Machine teaching, 24, 110, 111, 136
Creative, 7, 18, 31, 49, 52, 65, 89, 111, 114, Manage, 3, 7, 42, 47–49, 65, 87, 124, 131,
125, 135, 136 136
Curious, 7, 8, 18, 34, 35, 40, 57, 91, 135 Metacognitive, 62
© The Editor(s) (if applicable) and The Author(s) 2020 139
J. Willison, The Models of Engaged Learning and Teaching,
SpringerBriefs in Education, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-15-2683-1
140 Index
T
P
Teaching autonomy, 5, 6, 8–10, 20, 21, 23–
Pacific, 4, 11, 47, 48, 53
25, 29, 33, 63, 96, 98, 100, 103, 110,
111, 117, 129–131, 136
Thinking routines, 3, 10, 58, 80, 114, 131
R
Threshold concepts, 5, 93, 103–106, 109,
Reflect, 7, 17, 22, 30, 33, 42, 44, 46, 56, 65,
136
67, 70, 84, 85, 87, 88, 92, 108, 114,
117, 136
Reflective practitioner, 103, 108, 109, 136
U
Understanding, 1–3, 5, 8, 9, 11, 19, 24, 29,
S 30, 34, 37, 43, 49–53, 55, 66, 70, 85–
Silver fluoride, 11, 24, 30, 33, 34, 36, 37, 89, 91–95, 98, 104, 105, 107, 109,
41–45, 47, 48, 51, 52, 55, 57, 79, 106 111, 117, 125, 126, 129, 131, 132,
Soyuz, 122, 123 135, 136