10 When Is A Word A Word
10 When Is A Word A Word
AND
LORRAINE McCUNE
Rutgers University
ABSTRACT
INTRODUCTION
In studies of early child language the word plays a pivotal role. Recognition
of the 'first word' preoccupied clinical practitioners long before the advent
of contemporary psycholinguistic research (McCarthy, 1954), while interest
continues to focus on the size and typology of the earliest vocabularies of
more or less rapidly advancing infants (Goldfield & Reznick, 1990;
[•] This research was supported by funding from the National Science Foundation (BNS-
8209695 and 85 20048). We would like to thank Fumiko Arao (Stanford University),
Catherine Durand (C.N.R.S., Paris), and Liselotte Roug-Hellichius and Ingrid Landberg
(Institute of Linguistics, Stockholm University) for their help in applying our criteria to
crosslinguistic data; we also thank Charles A. Ferguson for his thoughtful and con-
structive comments. Address for correspondence: Marilyn May Vihman, Department of
Special Education, Southeastern Louisiana University, P.O. Box 879, Hammond, LA
70402, USA.
517
VIHMAN & MCCUNE
Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk, Seltzer & Lyons, 1991). Such current debates
as the relative precocity of early language in the oral vs. the gestural mode
(Orlansky & Bonvillian, 1988; Petitto, 1988; Meier & Newport, 1990) and
resolution of the issue of the ' nominal insight' and its relation to referential
word use (McShane, 1979; Harris, Barrett, Jones & Brookes, 1988) depend
crucially on recognition of the child's very first words and on establishing the
developmental significance of different categories of early words. Yet the
problem of word definition and identification has received relatively little
attention among influential works addressing the onset of word use. For
example, neither Halliday's (1975) diary study nor Bates, Benigni,
Bretherton, Camaioni & Volterra's (1979) broad-based study of 25 Italian-
and English-learning infants provided any discussion of the problem.
When explicit mention is made of this central methodological issue, two
complementary criteria are characteristically invoked: (a) resemblance of
phonetic form to an adult word and (b) situational consistency in use (e.g.
Lewis, 1936; Leopold, 1939; Nelson, 1973; Ferguson & Farwell, 1975;
Greenfield & Smith, 1976). The recognition of words via the broad criterion
of conventional form with a sound-meaning link is far from simple, however,
particularly in the earliest period of potential word production, when
children make a gradual transition from babbling to adult-like word use,
producing a variety of vocalizations of differing degrees of 'wordiness'
(Menn, 1978; Bates et al. 1979; Vihman & Miller, 1988). Meanings may be
unconventionally narrow or limited, on the one hand, or global and diffuse,
on the other (Rescorla, 1980; Griffiths, 1986; Nelson, 1988); forms may be
indistinguishable from concurrent babble (Labov & Labov, 1978; Vihman,
Macken, Miller, Simmons & Miller, 1985), and both the sound and meaning
consistencies may be idiosyncratic to a given child (Halliday, 1975; Dore,
Franklin, Miller & Ramer, 1976; Ferguson, 1978).
Balanced attention to form and function combined with detailed speci-
fication of the problems involved or the criteria used is rare. To allow
comparability across studies investigators would need, at a minimum, to
address such specific questions as the degree and type of phonetic resem-
blance to an adult word and the nature of the factors which they take to
constitute evidence for the existence of a usage pattern which is situationally
consistent. One problem in comparing infant words to adult language is the
essentially syntactic character of the latter. Some authors have considered the
single word to be a sentence or HOLOPHRASE (e.g. de Laguna, 1927; see also
Dore, 1985), while others have been unwilling even to accord the term
'word' to presentational verbalizations (e.g. Werner & Kaplan, 1963, who
coined the term VOCABLE).
Different categories have been proposed for children's productions in the
single word period. Nelson (1973) categorized children's words as reported
by mothers partly on the basis of their apparent function for the child (e.g.
WHEN IS A WORD A WORD?
consulted external evidence, both from the mother's report and from the
longitudinal record, before making a judgement. We returned periodically to
the data in context to ask how strong an impression of ' wordiness' we could
gain from the textually embedded vocalization itself. A final decision was
eventually made in each case, although we remain keenly aware of the
inherent ambiguity of child behaviour which, like adult behaviour, is subject
to the progressive evolution and expansion or re-definition of meaning in the
course of use. In short, although the procedures described here make explicit
the rationale for including each accepted word and thus provide a useful
meta-analysis for comparison with previous work and for the design of future
methodologies, one theme of this paper is that judgement is necessarily at the
core of studies of the earliest words.
METHOD
prepare for the sessions in such a way as to encourage the child to produce
any new words or other behaviours of interest (e.g. by bringing in a bowl of
flowers when the child had been using the word flower over the previous
week). Stanford mothers frequently engaged the children in book-reading
and some of them elicited imitations as a way of demonstrating new words.
The one observer typically present interacted verbally with the mother from
time to time and responded to the child's advances but did not normally
initiate activities with the child.
For the analyses reported here, one session per child, at i;s or i;6, was
transcribed phonetically from the videotapes for each of the 20 children by
a single transcriber, with contextual information regarding child activity,
gestures, and direction of gaze in connection with vocalizations, and also
relevant maternal or observer actions and talk directed to the child. In
addition, five to seven more monthly sessions per child were transcribed from
the Rutgers videotapes and the Stanford audiotapes, supplemented for the
Stanford sample by contextual video notes or, for a few sessions, full
videoscripts.
Reliability of phonetic transcription from the video-recordings was
checked for three of the Stanford samples against full earlier transcriptions
of the audio-recordings of the same sessions by the same transcriber (cf.
Vihman et al. 1985, for discussion of intertranscriber reliability based on
audio only). Agreement as to length in syllables and complete consonant
identity for all supraglottal consonants, disregarding differences in voicing
and in place of articulation for sibilants, averaged 83-6% (based on 805
vocalizations and 498 consonants). A partial retranscription of a single
Rutgers videotape by the same transcriber, after a two-year interval, yielded
81-7% agreement as to length in syllables and identity of supraglottal
consonants.
Word candidates were then evaluated for specific evidence of word status
using the following criteria:
Each word candidate was rated for the presence or absence of each type of
evidence, yielding a word status profile which provided the framework for
the dialogic process of evaluation, discussion and re-evaluation. Since a
variety of irrelevant factors may influence (and even bias) the initial screening
- such as the observer's familiarity with the child, the child's volubility and
apparent intelligibility, or maternal evaluation and level of interaction with
the observer - the word status profile and decision-making based on it
maximize comparability in word identification across subjects.
The word identification criteria are intended to provide an explicit record
of the factors entering into a decision regarding the word status of each word-
like vocalization, or word candidate, as opposed to such relatively loose
procedures as ' [determining] recognition [of a word] by global characteristics
of the sound sequence and, inevitably, by appropriateness of the conditions
of usage' (Plunkett, 1993). A team of coders could independently apply our
criteria to a subset of data as a test of inter-observer reliability in judging
word status. Alternatively, decisions for one or more subjects could be made
by a single observer, with use of the word identification criteria as docu-
mentation. In either case, identification would be based on an explicit
procedure, but a final pass would also be needed in which the subjective
impression of the observer/transcriber was compared against the mechanical
results of applying criteria. Each team of investigators would ultimately have
to make decisions based on a complex understanding of the material, and
then try to articulate and illustrate those decisions as we do here, especially
when dealing with the earliest words.
II. STANFORD
Molly 433 5° 49
Sean 368 38 27
Deborah 264 38 26
Timmy 239 32 23
Emily [1; 3] .85 15 12
Thomas 162 18 12
Andrew 165 12 7
Jonah 126 7 5
Camille iS° 5 4
Susie [1; 3] 334 5 3
Mean 2426 205 168
Targets: flower tea yum no down outside sky berries yellow spoon
Shapes: [p'adi] [ti:] [?m] [nae] [ta] [Jja] [ks:] [bebij] [hawa]
N uses 3 i i i 4 i i 3 i
Decision Y Y N Y" Y N N Y N
Criteria
Det. ctxt
Mat. id.
Mult, use
Mult.eps.
Complex
Exact
Prosodic
Imitated
Invariant
No inapprop.
Total
Y = yes, accepted; N = no, rejected.
a
Context suggests that the child is rejecting a suggestion by the mother, but is too vague to
be credited as Determinative context. Both maternal report and earlier recorded use
confirm word status.
b
Child vocalizes as he reaches for the spoon, after previously picking up the coffee pot.
Maternal report confirms word status.
points will yield the final 'yes' or ' n o ' decisions actually arrived at, however,
any more than any one set of 'necessary and sufficient' criteria would
adequately specify the construct 'word' in adult language (cf., for example,
Matthews, 1974). Instead, we tested the 'count' of applicable criteria against
our subjective impression of wordiness; when the two failed to agree, we
reconsidered, using external criteria (as specified in the notes to Table 2) as
well as a return to the transcripts or even the videotapes (as discussed below).
Example a: baby
ADULT FORM: # 'b e i b i
SUBJECTS CHILD FORMS
Alice [beibi] + + + + +
Deborah [p'e:bi] + + (+) + +
Vido [bobap] + [o] + [a] [p]
Example b: cock-a-doodle-doo
ADULT FORM : # k a k 3 d u d a 1 'd u: #
SUBJECTS CHILD FORMS
Aurie [kakijali::] + + + [i] [CV] [CV]
Jonah [skakekia] [a] + + + M [CV]
Sean [dalodalu::] [d] + [1]
Matching segments indicated as +, feature-match (but not full segment) as (+). Mismatching
segments set off by square brackets; matching syllable-count but mismatching segments
indicated as [CV]. (All of these illustrative words were accepted and are included in the
children's 1; 4 word lists given in the Appendix.)
[1] For a more complete discussion of the criteria used for word identification or of the
procedure for determining degree of phonetic match, together with examples drawn from
the data reported here, please write to the authors.
527
VIHMAN & MCCUNE
and vowel (e.g. Sean: [?ij] horse; Timmy: [s: ae] fish).2 In such cases no
phonetic criteria were taken to aid in word identification; contextual criteria
were strong enough to lead to acceptance of the word.
which we found necessary to account for the children's word uses. When a
word was produced spontaneously in varying contexts its use was considered
referential, whereas production in limited contexts was taken to reflect use as
[2] Acceptance of the plausibility of such 'global' matches depends on familiarity with the
phonetic patterns used by a particular child. For further discussion of Timmy's incipient
phonological system, which supports recognition of ' fricative + low vowel' as an attempt
at fish, for example, see Vihman, Velleman & McCune (1994). Compare also Waterson
(1971), who cites such globally matching forms as [wae] forfly,[oj] for vest, for her son
at i ; 5 .
528
WHEN IS A WORD A WORD?
Rutgers Stanford
Flexible nominals 45 39 43 26
Relationals 10 9 9 5
Context-bound 41 36 7' 42
Imitations 18 16 45 27
Total 114 168
529
VIHMAN & MCCUNE
I. Rutgers sample
Flexible nominals — 2 3 3 16 20 44
Relational — I 2 5 14
Context-bound 2 3 IO 10 12 24 28 28
Imitations 3 i 6 2 S 5 9 19
Total S 6 20 12 21 47 62 i°5
No. of subjects 3 3 6 4 7 7 9 10
Mean no. of words i-7 2O 33 30 30 67 69 105
per subject
II. Stanford sample
Flexible nominals — I — 1 4 13 17 47
Relational — — i — 3 3 6 8
Context-bound — 5 14 17 39 33 46 84
Imitations 2 4 11 7 21 25 37 46
Total 2 IO 26 25 67 74 106 185
No. of subjects 2 4 7 6 7 8 10 10
Mean no. of words ro 25 3'7 41 95 93 106 i8'5
per subject
Raw totals are given for each month. The 'number of subjects' refers to the subjects with
identifiable words in each month sampled.
40
20
10 11 12 13 14 15 16 10 11 12 13 14 15 16
Age in months
Fig. i. Word use over time in two samples. D, Flexible; +, other spontaneous; *, imitated.
N = io each.
Longitudinal findings
In order to explore further the interpretation of a training effect, we applied
the same four-way grouping to the words identified across all eight months
sampled. Table 6 shows the results of this analysis; Fig. 1 offers a graphic
representation of the findings.
VIHMAN. & MCCUNE
The longitudinal perspective confirms our interpretation of the lexicons
presented in the Appendix. The two samples begin to diverge, in both
numbers of words produced per subject and in numbers of children
contributing words to the pool, by o; 11 or i ;o. The Stanford sample shows
a sharply ascending curve for both context-bound words and imitations,
while the rising curve for these categories is greatly attenuated in the Rutgers
samples. The trajectory for the acquisition of referential words, on the other
hand, is virtually identical in the two samples. Such words are meagrely
represented in either group until i;2. At that point, both samples show a
sudden increase in the occurrence of flexible nominals - the kind of increase
that might be expected to accompany an abrupt cognitive revelation, such as
the 'nominal insight'. Relational words double in both samples in the
following month, and double again in the Rutgers sample at i;4. In the
Rutgers sample, the shift to flexible words at i; 2 is also accompanied by a
doubling of word production in the 'other spontaneous' category, which
suggests a new level of attention to the linguistic forms available in the
environment.
CONCLUSION
Our data sets were collected independently, using different methods in the
service of different immediate goals, but in the course of years of collaboration
we have developed a common, maximally explicit and maximally inclusive
procedure for identifying early word use. Two consequences of our approach
can be brought out here. First, if the very earliest words-the 'ragged
beginnings' of speech - are to be recognized, close attention to phonetic
patterns is needed, as well as awareness of a variety of types of ways of linking
sound and meaning. Secondly, if we are interested in identifying relationships
between language and early cognitive development, it is essential to separate
out the 'dribble' of earliest, context-bound words used by some children
from the flexible usage which emerges somewhat later. These two kinds of
word, with their differing degrees of symbolic and referential value, continue
to co-exist, as they characterize not only the language of children but the
usage of adults as well, for example such discourse markers as well, with no
'meaning' or reference outside of the immediate pragmatic context
(Schiffrin, 1985), or formulas and conversational routines, which fit globally
into a discourse event, but which, again, cannot be said to bear referential
'meaning' (Ferguson, 1976; Coulmas, 1979). Some children begin context-
bound word production before they are ready for context-flexible language
use; others begin both at the same time.
The notion of a 'sound-meaning link' is far from simple. The value of the
'word' as a unit of form and meaning varies across the world's languages and
even within a single language, given the problematic status of formulaic
expressions and marginal words, for example. Special care and attention to
532
WHEN IS A WORD A WORD?
definition are all the more necessary in recognizing the emergence of word
use in the vocal production of young children. As linguist and psychologist
in collaboration we found it essential to specify phonetic and contextual
criteria in order to clearly identify and communicate our own underlying
assumptions. In recounting our attempts to define and evaluate both formal
and functional criteria ever more closely we have attempted to provide for
other investigators an instrument for assessing the reliability or degree of
confidence that can be assumed in connection with different approaches to
word identification, and thus for selecting methods appropriate to individual
goals and areas of expertise.
REFERENCES
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of
symbols: cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.
Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time. The Hague: Mouton.
de Boysson-Bardies, B., Vihman, M. M., Roug-Hellichius, L., Durand, C , Landberg, I. &
Arao, F. (1991). Material evidence of infant selection from the target language: a cross-
linguistic phonetic study. In C. A. Ferguson, L. Menn & C. Stoel-Gammon (eds), Phono-
logical development: models, research, implications. Parkton, MD: York Press.
Coulmas, F. (1979). On the sociolinguistic relevance of routine formulae. Journal of
Pragmatics 3, 239-66.
Dore, J. (1985). Holophrases revisited: their 'logical' development from dialog. In M. D.
Barrett (ed.), Children's single-word speech. New York: Wiley.
Dore, J., Franklin, M. B., Miller, R. R. & Ramer, A. L. H. (1976). Transitional phenomena
in early language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 3, 13—28.
Ferguson, C. A. (1976). The structure and use of politeness formulas. Language in Society 5,
137-5'-
(1978). Learning to pronounce: the earliest stages of phonological development in the
child. In F. D. Minifie & L. L. Lloyd (eds), Communicative and cognitive abilities — early
behavioral assessment. Baltimore, M D : University Park Press.
Ferguson, C. A. & Farwell, C. B. (1975). Words and sounds in early language acquisition.
Language 51, 419-39.
Goldfield, B. A. & Reznick, J. S. (1990). Early lexical acquisition: rate, content, and the
vocabulary spurt. Journal of Child Language 17, 171-83.
Greenfield, P. M. & Smith, J. H. (1976). The structure of communication in early language
development. New York: Academic Press.
Griffiths, P. (1986). Early vocabulary. In P. Fletcher & M. Garman (eds), Language
acquisition : studies in first language development. (2nd edn.) Cambridge: C.U.P.
Halliday, M. A. K. (1975). Learning how to mean - explorations in the development of language.
London: Edward Arnold.
Harris, M., Barrett, M., Jones, D. & Brookes, S. (1988). Linguistic input and early word
meaning. Journal of Child Language 15, 77-94.
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M. & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary
growth: relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology 27, 236-48.
Labov, W. & Labov, T. (1978). The phonetics of cat and mama. Language 54, 816-52.
de Laguna, G. A. (1927). Speech: its function and development. New Haven, CO: Yale
University Press.
Leopold, W. F. (1939). Speech development of a bilingual child: a linguist's record, I: vocabulary
growth in the first two years. New York: Northwestern University Press.
Lewis, M. M. (1936). Infant speech : a study of the beginnings of language. New York: Harcourt
Brace.
533
VIHMAN & MCCUNE
McCarthy, D. (1954). Language development in children. In L. Carmichael (ed.), Manual of
child psychology. (2nd edn.) New York: Wiley.
McCune, L. (1992). First words: a dynamic systems view. In C. A. Ferguson, L. Menn & C.
Stoel-Gammon (eds), Phonological development: models, research, implications. Parkton,
MD: York Press.
McCune-Nicolich, L. (1981). The cognitive bases of relational words in the single word
period. Journal of Child Language 8, 15-34.
McNeill, D. (1970). The development of language. New York: Harper & Row.
McShane, J. (1979). The development of naming. Linguistics 17, 879-905.
Matthews, P. H. (1974). Morphology: an introduction to the theory of word structure.
Cambridge: C.U.P.
Meier, R. P. & Newport, E. L. (1990). Out of the hands of babes: on a possible sign
advantage. Language 66, 1-23.
Menn, L. (1978). Pattern, control, and contrast in beginning speech: a case study in the
development of word form and word function. Bloomington, Indiana: Indiana University
Linguistics Club.
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development 38.
(1988). Constraints on word learning? Cognitive Development 3, 221—46.
Orlansky, M. D. & Bonvillian, J. D. (1988). Early sign language acquisition. In M. D. Smith
& J. L. Locke (eds), The emergent lexicon : the child's development of a linguistic vocabulary.
New York : Academic Press.
Packer, M. J. (1985). Hermeneutic inquiry in the study of human conduct. American
Psychologist 40, 1081-93.
Petitto, L. A. (1988). 'Language' in the pre-linguistic child. In F. S. Kessel (ed.), The
development of language and language researchers. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Plunkett, K. (1993). Lexical segmentation and vocabulary growth in early language ac-
quisition. Journal of Child Language 20, 43-60.
Rescorla, L. A. (1980). Overextension in early language development. Journal of Child
Language 7, 321-35.
Schiffrin, D. (1985). Conversational coherence: the role of well. Language 61, 640—67.
Sinclair, H. (1970). The transition from sensory-motor behavior to symbolic activity.
Interchange 1, 119—25.
Veneziano, E. (1981). Early language and nonverbal representation: a reassessment. Journal
of Child Language 8, 541-63.
Vihman, M. M. (1992). Early syllables and the construction of phonology. In C. A. Ferguson,
L. Menn & C. Stoel-Gammon (eds), Phonological development: models, research, implica-
tions. Parkton, MD: York Press.
Vihman, M. M., Ferguson, C. A. & Elbert, M. (1986). Phonological development from
babbling to speech: common tendencies and individual differences. Applied Psycholin-
guistics 7, 3—40.
Vihman, M. M., Macken, M. A, Miller, R., Simmons, H. & Miller, J. (1985). From babbling
to speech: a re-assessment of the continuity issue. Language 61, 397—445.
Vihman, M. M. & Miller, R. (1988). Words and babble at the threshold of lexical acquisition.
In M. D. Smith & J. L. Locke (eds), The emergent lexicon: the child's development of a
linguistic vocabulary. New York: Academic Press.
Vihman, M. M., Velleman, S. L. & McCune, L. (1994). How abstract is child phonology?
Toward an integration of linguistic and psychological approaches. In M. Yavas (ed.), First
and second language phonology. San Diego: Singular Press.
Waterson, N. (1971). Child phonology: a prosodic view. Journal of Linguistics 7, 179-211.
Werner, H. & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation : an organismic-developmental approach to
language and the expression of thought. New York: Wiley. (Reprinted 1984. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.)
534
WHEN IS A WORD A WORD?
APPENDIX
Word forms at i;4, organized by lexical category. The most common
spontaneous phonetic variant is given or, if variants are equally distributed,
the variant best matching the adult form. Frequency of use is given in
parentheses. Three subjects are sampled at i; 3 as indicated below. One
Rutgers subject produced no words.
I. RUTGERS SUBJECTS
Alice
Nominate: flexible limited specific
apple (4) [?ae] bang (1) [pai] Grandpa (3) [p'a]
baby (7) [beibi] elephant (4) [5:IJA] mommy (10) [ma:ni]
belly (1) [vei] nose (1) [n:ae]
blanket (1) [k'et]
bottle (14) [badj]
bunny (3) [bAnn:i]
daddy (2) [da:di]
duck (1) [tae?]
egg (8) [?ei]
eye (4) [?ai]
flower (3) [p'adi]
hat (2) [?a]
iron (2) [?aiji]
key (1) [ci]
lady (4) [je:ji]
man (1) [ma;:]
meat (1) [mi?]
milk (1) [mms]
Oscar (= puppet) (6) [?a?]
plate (4) [p'ei]
shoe (1) [ci]
tea (1) [p:]
Relational
down (2) [dau] up (2) [?A:p]
no (1) [nae] clean (2) [ti:ni]
Event
shiny (3) [ta:ji]
Social
bye (1) [bai]
Imitation
hello (1) [lou]
535
VIHMAN & McCUNE
Aurie
Nominate: flexible limited
apple (2) [Paebi] cock-a-doodle-doo sound (6) [kakijali::]
book (1) [buk:ts]
dolly (2) [?adi]
shoe (2) [tsi]
Relational
bye (1) [baebaei] up (5) [?ap]
peek-a-boo (2) [p'iljhu:]
Event
ow (1) [?aeo] yum sound (2) [vAm:i]
Social
hello (12) [hA:o] hi (4) [haei]
Routine/game
open (2) [thA:ppu] tickle-tickle (1) [tikatik]
Imitation
orange (1) [Pawis] stuck (1) [tae]
out (1) [Paut] uh-oh (1) [hA?u]
pretty (1) [pli] whoop (1) [wu::]
yoohoo (5) [huho]
Danny
Deictic
aha (8) [?&hsb] that (6) [da::]
Jase
Nominals: flexible limited specific
ball (1) [bo::a] woof sound (5) [wAp] mommy (7) [mam]
dog (4) [du:]
Grover (puppet) (3) [go:]
juice (4) [cfeis]
Relational
more (5) [mo]
Social
no (2) [nou]
Kari
Event
car sound (5) [?m] up (1) [Pap]
Social
uh-oh (1) [?A?a]
Imitation
no (1) [nA]
536
WHEN IS A WORD A WORD?
Rala
Nominate: flexible
baby (5) [p'epi] comb (8) [hekom]
ball (10) [bo] mouth (4) [mao]
beads (2) [?ebi] shoes (2) [aja:/]
bottle (6) [hAbau] spoon (5) [hebu:]
car (7) [[hek'a]
Relational
uh-oh (7) [?A?]
Event
tea (11) [ti:] X go[es] (6) [Pik'ou]
do (6) [nhto]
Deictic
see (20) [si:]
Imitation
bye (3) [bai]
toe (2) [hethoo]
Rick
Nominals: flexible limited specific
ball (3) [p'o:] beads (1) daddy (2) [daegi]
doggie (2) [go:di] mommy (6) [mam:]
eyes (3) [?ai]
spoon (1) [p'o:p'i]
Relational
no (2) [nou] open (17) [oup'i]
Event
yum sound (1) [?Am]
Social
hi (6) [hai]
Ronny (i;3)
Nominals: flexible specific
baby (7) [p'ebi] mama (1) [mama]
woof sound (1) [wa[3a]
Event
vroom sound (1) [vf:um]
Social
hello (1) [[?al:o]
Routine/game
sit (1) [sit]
Deictic
this (1) [di]
537
VIHMAN & MCCUNE
Imitation
yellow (i) [ijaeb]
Vido
Nominals: limited
baby (2) [bobap]
Social
thank you (8) [de:ide] unh-hunh (5) [[?5h
Imitation
apple (2) [bapba] good (1) [go:n]
daddy (1) [darn] keys (2) [se:s]
flower (3) [fa:|3wa] yellow (1) [deblou]
I I . STANFORD SUBJECTS
Andrew
Nominals: Limited
spoon (2) [p'o]
Relational
up (3) [?aep]
Social
no (1) [ne::]
Imitation
bird (7) [be] mountain (2) [ITIA]
clock (3) [kaek]
tail (4) [t'e]
Camille
Relational
up (7) [Pap]
Event
car sound (3) [djhis]
Deictic
(what's) this (7) [?9zis]
Imitation
hello (5) [haha]
Deborah
Nominals: flexible limited specific
baby (7) [p'e:bi] moo sound (8) [bo::] monkey (1) [hmmae:]
ball (7) [boa]
bird (2) [bwa]
bottle (2) [ba:]
cheese (2) [§i:]
corn (5) [kho:]
538
WHEN IS A WORD A WORD?
539
VIHMAN & McCUNE
Molly
Nominate: flexible limited specific
baby (2) [pebi] bead (8) [pi:] Brett (9) [pat]
block (8) [pak] piano (2) [pau] Graham (9) [kon:i]
book (13) [puk] picture (4) [popo] Grandma (1) [meuwA]
camera (4) [kama] Grandpa (1) [kaepo]
ear (4) [he:] mama (7) [mama]
glasses (29) [kaki] Nicky (2) [m:i]
meow sound (8) [miA] Nonny (10) [nanni]
nose (1) [no:] Ruth (6) [wut]
Relational
stuck (1) [kak]
Event
bang (4) [pan:a] vroom sound (12) [b?Am]
choochoo sound (10) [tutu] walk-walk (8) [wokae]
down (10) [tanna]
Social
hi (4) [?ai:]
Routine/game
baa sound (2) [pae:] snort sound (14) [?e?]
cluck sound (11) three (2) [wi:]
[bA?bA?]
neigh sound (8) [p'A] two (2) [tu]
moo sound (13) [mu:] woof sound (17) [wu]
peek-a-boo (7) [pik]
Imitation
click (11) [kik] open (1) [hopo]
good girl (2) [guga] pig (1) [pik]
green (3) [kyn:i] red (2) [wat]
house (2) [haut] rug (3) [wa:k]
in (1) [ihni] tail (26) [teu]
name (1) [nem:i] that (2) [tat]
oink sound (1) [ho:k] work (6) [hAk]
Sean
Nominals: flexible limited specific
bird (12) [bwi:ts] cock-a- mama (9) [ma:]
block (16) [pak] doodle-doo sound (2) [daludalu::]
book (4) [bik] quack sound (5) [?a?]
bug (2) [mbAkl] vroom sound (3) [(3::]
butterfly (3) [pAJA:] woof sound (2) [wau]
cracker (2) [djak]
dog (3) [tao]
54O
WHEN IS A WORD A WORD?
542