Metis 203475
Metis 203475
Ruut Veenhoven
In: Alex C. Michalos (Ed.) Encyclopedia of Quality of Life and Well-Being Research
Springer, Dordrecht, Netherlands, 2014. Springer Reference Series, pp. 3645-3647.
ISBN 978-94-007-0752-8
DOI: 10.1007/978-94-007-0753-5_1669
1 SYNONYMS
2 DEFINITION
Livability is the degree to which a living environment fits the adaptive repertoire of
a species. Applied to human society, it denotes the fit of institutional arrangements
with human needs and capacities. Livability theory explains observed differences in
happiness in terms of need- environment fit.
3 DESCRIPTION
3.1 Assumptions
Livability theory involves the following six key assumptions:
1. Like all animals, humans have innate needs, such as for food, safety, and
companionship.
2. Gratification of needs manifests in hedonic experience.
3. Hedonic experience determines how much we like the life we live (happiness).
Hence, happiness depends on need gratification.
4. Need gratification depends on both external living conditions and inner abilities
to use these. Hence, bad living conditions will reduce happiness, in particular
when its demands exceed human capabilities.
5. Societies are systems for meeting human needs, but not all societies do that job
equally well. Consequently, people are not equally happy in all societies.
6. Improvement of the fit between social institutions and human needs will result in
greater happiness.
Comparison Theories
These theories assume that happiness results from comparisons between notions of
how life should be and how it actually is. The greater the gap between what one
wants and what one has, the less happy one is (Michalos, 1985). In this theory,
“wants” differ from the above mentioned “needs.” Firstly, wants are held
consciously, while one may be unconscious of what one needs. Secondly, wants are
social constructs and as such likely to vary across cultures, while needs are
hardwired and universal. Thirdly, wants tend to be endless, while needs can be
satisfied. This latter point implies that great happiness is not possible. If we always
want more than we have, we will never get any happier. It is for this reason that the
pursuit of happiness has been typified as a “hedonic treadmill” (Brickman &
Campbell, 1971). Variants of this theory emphasize different standard of
comparison and different mechanisms that inflate aspirations. See the lemma on
“contentment” in this encyclopedia.
Trait Theories
These theories hold that happiness is a static characteristic of an individual,
comparable to the color of one’s hair. One variant claims that happiness is largely
genetic, some people are born to be happy, and others to be constitutionally
depressive (e.g., Lykken, 1999). Another variant is that our early experiences
program us to enjoy life or not. Cummins’ (1995) set-point theory assumes that we
are all hardwired to feel reasonably happy (between 7 and 8 on scale 0-10) and that
homeostatic mechanisms keep us around that level, unless extreme circumstances
push us below or above.
3.3 Evidence
Since we cannot yet look in people’s heads, we cannot test these competing theories
as such. Yet we can check some of their implications. One implication of both
comparison theory and trait theory is that average happiness will be about the same
all globally. This is clearly not the case, average happiness on a scale 0-10 is 2,8 in
Togo and 8,2 in Denmark (Veenhoven, 2012a). Another implication is that average
happiness will remain at the same level, even if living conditions deteriorate or
improve in a nation. This appears not to be the case either. Average happiness
dropped dramatically in Russia after the Ruble crisis in the late 1990s, and in the
1990s, happiness also dropped in other postcommunist countries where major
transformations took place. Yet average happiness has gone up in most developed
countries over the last 30 years, and in the last 10 years, it has also increased in the
postcommunist countries (Veenhoven, 2012b). Both the differences in average
happiness across nations and the rise of happiness over time correspond with social
qualities, such as economic development, political democracy, and good
governance.
Follow-up studies at the individual level also show considerable changes in
happiness over time, which are linked to both things that happen in our lives, such
as marriage and bereavement (Headey, 2006).
Still another piece of evidence is that conditions for happiness appear to be fairly
universal. Though there is some variation in what people think will make them
happy, actual correlates of happiness are strikingly similar all over the world
(Veenhoven, 2010b).
All this fits livability theory quite well.
Cummins, R. A. (1995)
On the trait of the gold standard for subjective well-being.
Social Indicators Research, 35, 179-200.
Headey, B. (2006)
Happiness: Revising set point theory and dynamic equilibrium theory to account for
long term change.
DIW discussion paper 607.
Berlin, Germany.
Lykken, D. T. (1999)
Happiness: What studies on twins show us about nature, nurture and the happiness
set- point.
New York: Golden Books.
Michalos, A. C. (1985)
Multiple Discrepancies Theory (MDT)
Social Indicators Research, 16, 347-413.
Veenhoven, R. (1993)
Assessing livability of nations by happiness. Part I in happiness in nations’:
Subjective appreciation of life in 56 nations 1946-1992.
RISBO, Studies in Sociale en Culturele Verandering, nr. 2, p. 364, Erasmus
University Rotterdam, ISBN 90-72597-46.
Veenhoven, R. (2000)
The four qualities of life. Ordering concepts and measures of the good life
Journal of Happiness Studies, 1, 1-39.
Veenhoven, R. (2010a)
Life is getting better: Societal evolution and fit with human nature.
Social Indicators Research, 97, 105-122.
Veenhoven, R. (2010b)
How universal is happiness?
In E. Diener, J. F. Helliwell, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), International differences in
well-being, 2010 (pp. 328-350).
New York: Oxford University Press. Chapter 11, ISBN-13: 978-0-19-973273-9.
Veenhoven, R. (2012a)
Average happiness in nations.
World Database of Happiness, Erasmus University Rotterdam.
Veenhoven, R. (2012b)
Trends in average happiness in nations
World Database of Happiness, Erasmus University Rotterdam.
Cross-References in Encyclopedia:
Happiness
Need Theory