The Revised NEO Personality Inventory - Clinical and Research Applications PDF
The Revised NEO Personality Inventory - Clinical and Research Applications PDF
PERSONALITY
INVENTORY
CLINICAL AND RESEARCH
APPLICATIONS
THE PLENUM SERIES IN
SOCIAL/CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY
Series Editor: C. R. Snyder
University of Kansas
Lawrence, Kansas
AGGRESSION
Biological, Developmental, and Social Perspectives
Edited by Seymour Feshbach and Jolanta Zagrodzka
A Continuation Order Plan is available for this series. A continuation order will bring
delivery of each new volume immediately upon publication. Volumes are billed only
upon actual shipment. For further information please contact the publisher.
THE REVISED NEO
PERSONALITY
INVENTORY
CLINICAL AND RESEARCH
APPLICATIONS
RALPH L. PIEDMONT
Loyola College in Maryland
Baltimore, Maryland
Figures 3.1-3.4, 4.1-4.7, 5.1-5.7, and the appendices on pages 111 and 157 are
reproduced by special permission of the Publisher, Psychological Assessment
Resources, Inc., 16204 North Florida Avenue, Lutz, Florida 33549, from the
NEO Personality Inventory-Revised, by Paul Costa, and Robert McCrae,
Copyright 1978, 1985, 1989, 1992 by PAR, Inc. Further reproduction is
prohibited without permission of PAR, Inc.
vii
viii FOREWORD
NEO PI-R interpretations. Instead, the author makes every effort to pro-
vide the reader with a context and a foundation for understanding how
and why the instrument was developed, and how and when it should be
applied. The wide-ranging coverage of the research literature in ensuing
chapters illuminates the versatility of the NEO PI-R in differing contexts
that emphasize clinical, applied, cross-cultural, and psychometric consid-
erations. I believe that this book really hits its mark and that it will prove
to be an effective resource for anyone who wishes to develop a deeper un-
derstanding of the area of personality assessment. The book is delightfully
reader-oriented and user-friendly. Dr. Piedmont has a flair for presenting
technical issues in clear, readable prose that should be both accessible and
engaging to a wide audience of both students and professionals.
FOREWORD ix
All of the forces just mentioned are moving the mental health field to-
ward a recognition of the need for better documentation of clinical effi-
cacy. The mental health field needs to provide support for what it does
[quantitatively] in an objective, substantive manner. The technology best
suited for providing this kind of information is psychological assessment.
Traditionally, measures of clinical practice have been the purview of psy-
chology. Psychologists usually receive extensive training in the application
and interpretation of psychological measures. In fact, many clinical as-
sessment instruments are usually restricted to psychologists who have had
detailed graduate training in testing. Therefore, many nonpsychologists
believe that they do not have access to any types of psychological measures
and thus do not employ testing as part of their practice, nor do they see it
as part of their clinical identity. This is unfortunate, because there is a wide
range of nonclinical psychological measures that are appropriate for
nonpsychologists and that would also be relevant to, and helpful for, their
practice. Being unaware of these measures deprives such therapists of im-
portant sources of information that could be useful for client assessment,
for documenting clinical efficacy, and for obtaining valuable clinical feed-
back.
The measures to which I refer are instruments designed to assess nor-
mal personality qualities. Such scales are less restricted by test publishers.
Usually, a graduate course in psychological assessment and/ or some su-
pervision in their use is all that is required to gain access to these materials.
Personality questionnaires can be very useful in telling a therapist about a
wide range of client dispositions, needs, and motivations. Although they
may not be diagnostically revealing in their own right, these individual-
difference variables set important parameters for any therapeutic interac-
tion. For example, a person who is closed and rigid, who does not
experience a wide range of emotions, and who requires much structure
may not benefit from insight therapy. Conversely, an individual who is
open and has very permeable inner boundaries may keenly experience
whatever negative affect is dominant. By assessing a client's personality, a
therapist can gain insight on how the client sees the world and copes with
it. Depending on the constructs being measured, a therapist may also be
able to anticipate therapeutically relevant outcomes. For example, a client
high on Machiavellianism may have trouble in establishing trust, while a
client high on Anxiety may need a great deal of reassurance and support.
There is a wide range of personality questionnaires that span numer-
ous constructs. These measures vary from assessing specific constructs
(e.g., Fear of Fat Scale) to more global, multidimensional inventories (e.g.,
the California Personality Inventory). Personality measures are usually
easy-to-use scales that, with some reading and practice, can be used by
PREFACE xiii
In the writing of any book, there are numerous people who contribute to
the project even though there may be only one author. In this case, I am in-
debted to a number of colleagues and friends who contributed information
and material, as well as moral support. First and foremost, I would like to
thank my wife, Rose, who read and reread the manuscript several times to
check structure and grammar. I would also like to thank Gail Worrall, who
edited the entire manuscript. The manuscript was certainly made more
"user friendly" because of the efforts of Rose and Gail. I would also like to
express my gratitude to Drs. Joseph Ciarrocchi and Thomas Rpdgerson,
who shared with me some of their clinical experiences using the NEO PI-R.
Special thanks go to Reg Watson, who created the NEO PI-R profile forms
used in this book. I would also like to thank those individuals who con-
sented to have their NEO PI-R profiles included as examples throughout
the book. I would like to thank Dr. Robert Wicks, my colleague and friend,
who provided the initial inspiration for doing this book. Finally, I would
like to thank Paul T. Costa, Jr. and Robert R. McCrae for all they taught me
about the NEO PI-R in particular, and about good science in general.
xv
CONTENTS
Chapter 1
Personality and Its Assessment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
Chapter 2
Psychometric Overview of the NEO PI-R . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35
Outline of Scales. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35
Reliability. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36
Factor Structure. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40
xviii CONTENTS
Chapter 3
Interpreting the NEG PI-R . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79
Chapter 4
Profile Analysis Using the NEG PI-R. . . .. . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . .. . . . 113
Chapter 5
Applications of the Rater Version of the NEG PI-R . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 165
Chapter 6
Research Applications with the NEG PI-R . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 209
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 255
PERSONALITY AND
ITS ASSESSMENT
Every time I teach a course in personality theory, I begin the class by ask-
ing the students, "What is personality?" Given the newness of the class sit-
uation and the perennial need of college students to remain anonymous to
teachers, this question is always followed by a long silence. In order to
jump-start this social interaction, I follow up with a request. I ask students
to raise their hands if they believe they have a personality. Invariably, there
are muffled coughs, eyes begin to dart around the room, and perhaps two
or three people will, grudgingly, raise their hands. "Good," I say. "It seems
that some people are certain that they have a personality but others are not
as sure as to whether they have one or not. Perhaps some people may have
had a personality at some time in the past, but do not have one currently,
while still others may be engaged in an ongoing search to find one."
On a good day, these comments will elicit not only several good chuck-
les, but some hands will be raised in an attempt to define the term "person-
ality." The variety of definitions I get for the term clearly tells me that we all
know personality when we see it, but we cannot put our fingers on what ex-
actly we mean by "it." Personality is something that defines who we are as
people, yet some believe that it is always changing. Some people are said to
have "personality"; others seem to lack it. Some believe that personality is the
aggregate of our behaviors and attitudes, yet others see personality as some-
thing more fundamental: It is the "why" to our behavior. No doubt a course
on personality theory is exactly what these students need! Personality is a
central concept in the social sciences because it speaks about people: Who
they are, how they come to be, and where they are heading in their lives. It is
the foundation for building theories of psychopathology and treatment.
1
2 CHAPTER 1
WHAT IS PERSONALITY?
needs that arise from within and without the individual. Further, there is
a developmental aspect to personality. Just as we are not born physically
mature, so, too, personality is something that begins in a simple form and
becomes progressively more sophisticated over time. These changes also
follow a lawful, orderly path.
This definition is saying there is something inside us that emerges
over our lifetimes that comes to define us as unique individuals. This inner
system helps to orient us toward the world and enables us to adapt to the
demands of our environment. This inner structure has an inherent lawful-
ness to it, in that different personalities will lead individuals to follow dif-
ferent life paths. Further, the process by which personality unfolds will
also follow certain regularities that can be known and understood. Thus
the process of change is complex and there is a need to differentiate be-
tween changes that reflect superficial, adaptive modifications and changes
that reflect more fundamental, structural shifts.
this writing he is still alive and in his 80s), and he was still wearing that
tight jumpsuit! He is still energetic, still works out and stays fit. If you were
to see him today, you would most certainly say that he is a muscular man.
But there is no doubt that Jack's body today is quite different from what it
was 40 years ago. His body has experienced the inevitable changes of
aging: There is more body fat, muscles are not as "tight" as they once were,
and the skin appears looser around the body. But despite these very obvi-
ous changes, Jack is still described as a muscular man. The reason for this
consistency in labeling is that we recognize that what is meant by "mus-
cularity" is different at different stages of development. Phenotypically he
is different, but genotypically he has not changed at all.
The same can be said for personality. We can think of many different
qualities, but these qualities may mean different types of behaviors at dif-
ferent ages. For example, being an extravert in college may mean going to
many different parties and staying out late. It may mean being "rowdy"
and loud. However, at age 40 this quality may take on a quite different
form. Rather than the raucous college parties, it may be hosting formal sit-
down dinner parties for friends and coworkers. It may mean getting in-
volved in community activities. Clearly extraversion may take a more
conventional form later in life. But there is no doubt that the person re-
mains an outgoing, socially active person.
It is important when we apply measurement instruments to assess in-
dividuals that we have some understanding of the level of analysis being
assessed. Measures of phenotypic behavior may be very prone to changes,
either over short periods of time or even across different situations. For ex-
ample, measures of state anxiety (e.g., State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, Speil-
berger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970) or of depressive symptomatology (e.g.,
Hamilton Depression Inventory, Reynolds & Kobak,1995) are designed to
detect fluctuations in very specific aspects of functioning over relatively
short time intervals. They are useful for understanding treatment impact or
individual responses to specific situations. Changes on this level, although
adaptively important for specific situations, may be less diagnostic of more
fundamental, broad-based change. Genotypic measures, on the other hand,
aim at uncovering the basic psychological strata of the person. A measure
such as the NEO PI-R is designed to access a person's more fundamental
temperaments, those qualities that drive, direct, and select behaviors.
Scores on this type of scale are more likely to remain constant over time and
across situations. However, changes on this level may indicate more pro-
found shifts in personality. Thus, change needs to be evaluated carefully.
Another important realization related to this is the diversity of human
behavior through which genotypes can be expressed. People have devel-
oped extensive behavior repertoires for fulfilling their needs. They adapt
PERSONALITY AND ITS ASSESSMENT 5
improving the mental health of your clients. To this end, each client is as-
sessed on a mental health inventory when entering therapy. Ahigh score in-
dicates mental distress. After completing therapy each client completes the
mental health inventory again. After treating about 100 clients, you "crunch"
the numbers; you correlate Time 1 scores on the inventory with Time 2
scores. If you were successful in treating your clients, what would you ex-
pect to find? Should there be (a) a high positive correlation (i.e., high scores
at Time 1 continued to be high at Time 2), (b) a high negative correlation (Le.,
high scores at Time 1 became low scores at Time 2), or (c) should there be no
correlation (Le., no relationship between scores at the two time periods)?
Finding a zero correlation would not be helpful for drawing any types
of inferences. Such a result could be the product of many influences, in-
cluding the lack of reliability of your assessment instrument. A negative
correlation seems to be a desirable outcome, with those scoring high at first
becoming much lower at the end of treatment. However, the reverse
equally applies: Those who were low at Time 1 (indicating a degree of men-
tal health) scored higher at Time 2 (indicating that the treatment made them
less healthy). The most desirable result would be to find a positive correla-
tion. This would indicate some reliability in your assessment instrument.
But if you found a high positive correlation between the two sets of
scores would that mean you were not as successful as you thought in help-
ing your clients? That scores are basically the same at the end of treatment
as they were at the start? Not necessarily. In evaluating a correlation, it is
important to appreciate what that correlation tells you about your data.
Correlation is a measure of rank order stability. That means that if you obtain
a high positive correlation, the ranking of scores at Time 1 are similar to the
ranking of scores at Time 2. In terms of our example, those who were more
mentally distressed upon entering therapy were also among the more men-
tally distressed when leaving therapy. The term "more" is a relative term.
More distressed in comparison to whom? Well, the others in the study. How
distressed were they? That question is not answered by correlation.
Remember that rank order is an ordinal level of measurement. It tells
you position but not magnitude. Thus, the person who ranked number
one in mental distress at Time 1 could have improved greatly over treat-
ment, resulting in a much lower score at Time 2. However, if everyone else
in the sample improved at a similar rate over treatment, then this person
may again be ranked number one at Time 2. If everyone else also keeps the
same rank order, then the correlation between these two sets of scores will
be perfect (Le., +1.0), even though there were significant decreases in the
amount of psychological distress. Therefore, it is possible to obtain a high
positive correlation between Time 1 and Time 2 mental health scores even
when there are large changes in scores.
PERSONALITY AND ITS ASSESSMENT 7
OVERVIEW
chotherapy available to more individuals than ever before, the price for
this new availability is a level of cost consciousness heretofore unexperi-
enced in the social sciences. Third-party payers are expecting therapists to
do more, in less time, with greater impact. In common parlance, these ser-
vice providers are looking for greater "bang for the buck."
The result of this process has been a greater emphasis on short-term
therapies. Under the traditional system it would not be uncommon for the
first 6 months of therapy to focus on getting to know client and problem.
Today, in 6 months the therapy should be coming to an end. Therefore,
clinicians do not have the time for understanding the client that they once
enjoyed. New technologies are needed to expedite the evaluation and di-
agnostic process, and measures of personality can help fill this need.
There are numerous assessment devices that have recently appeared
aimed at uncovering important aspects of clients. These new-generation
instruments represent a high level of psychometric sophistication and can
be very useful for prOViding clinically relevant insights. But this you al-
ready recognize; after all, that is why you are reading this book. The NEO
PI-R can be very useful in a clinical context because it can make you aware
of the types of needs clients seek to satisfy, their interpersonal styles, value
systems, and levels of ongoing emotional distress. The NEO PI-R can also
be helpful in identifying therapeutic interventions that may be particularly
effective. For example, you would not want to put a social introvert in
group therapy, or try insight therapy with an individual who has little or
no personal insight or empathy. This book will provide you with a first
step in learning to use the NEO PI-R in this way. But there is another value
to personality assessment that is frequently overlooked.
As professionals we have an ethical responsibility to both ourselves
and our clients to provide them with high-quality, effective interventions.
As therapists, we need to have feedback on our interventions, information
about what therapies work and with what type of client. To determine the
amount of improvement we are able to bring about. This information is
also useful to our clients, who need to be informed about the efficacy of
our services in clear, concrete ways. Psychological assessment provides a
straightforward method for documenting treatment efficacy. Such mea-
sures provide a standardized, quantifiable means for determining the na-
ture and degree of change. It is important that people have access to
therapists, but it is even more important that people have access to thera-
pists who will actually improve their mental status.
You can ask any therapist whether the clients who come to see them
benefit from their services, and in almost every instance the reply will be
"Of course!" What other answer is there? After all, even therapists have
to maintain some sense of personal efficacy and professional value. If a
10 CHAPTER 1
therapist thought that he or she was not helping clients, the therapist
would certainly not stay in that line of work. But the real question is, "Do
your clients get better?" Just taking a therapist's word on the matter is in-
sufficient. Some kind of empirical documentation of benefit is needed. By
how much do clients improve? In what ways do clients improve? These
are important questions that health care providers, clients, and therapists
themselves are increasingly asking. The answers will need to be in a lan-
guage that is objective and universally understood: a quantitative one.
In this cost conscious, highly competitive environment it becomes cost
effective not only to identify effective therapists (individuals who can de-
liver some therapeutic clout in a short period), but also to be able to match
clients with therapists who are particularly adept with the kind of problem
the client is facing. Therefore, health care networks are now beginning to
conduct their own clinical outcome studies. Eventually, they will begin to
demand from their therapists some evidence of their own effectiveness.
Obtaining such evidence will require the use of standardized tests.
The paradigm for this type of study is quite straightforward. Simply give
a test(s) to clients when they enter therapy and again when they exit. In
this way it can be shown how much change there is over the identified di-
mensions. Combining these data with clinician ratings of effectiveness
(simple Likert scale ratings have been developed and are frequently used),
one can identify those personality characteristics of the individual noted at
the beginning of therapy that are linked with ratings of treatment efficacy
(these issues and methods to address them are discussed in more detail in
Chapter 6).
The implications of this type of research are legion. The value they serve
for improving therapy efficacy and developing new models of intervention
cannot be overstated. Although many bemoan the new health care behe-
moth, there may be a silver lining to this dark cloud. In developing a facil-
ity with assessment technologies now, you will give yourself an edge in this
developing health care landscape. When you begin to use the NED PI-R
along with other clinically relevant indices, you will not only be gathering
important information about your client that can be directly applied to your
therapeutic work, but you will also be creating a clinical database. This in-
formation can provide you with important feedback on the quality of your
work and important documentation of your psychotherapeutic effect.
be done with both husband and wife. However, in the second scenario it
is clear that the problems are situationally induced. There is a transient
stressor that is turning a usually goodnatured person into an ogre. Cer-
tainly when the stressor subsides many of these problems may disappear
as well. Treatment may wish to focus on developing short-term coping
skills and a complete remission of symptoms is likely.
Personality instruments like the NEO PI-R can be helpful in outlining
the underlying dispositions of clients and alerting you to the presence of
internal motivations that may be fueling current problems.
The second clinical value concerns differential diagnosis. Although the
NEO PI-R is not designed to assess pathology, it can be useful for provid-
ing information relevant to making a diagnosis or for ruling one out. For
example, a high score on extraversion, one of the major domains assessed
by the NEO, would rule out schizoid personality disorder. A high level of
emotional dysphoria is a feature of all clinical patients, but the presence of
only high Self-Consciousness (a facet of the NEO) would suggest a social
phobia rather than depression.
Personality inventories can serve as a basis for thinking meaningfully
about the kinds of diagnoses that may be relevant in a particular case. As-
sessing the underlying dispositions of the client leads one to see the moti-
vational sources of behavior, which in tum can help clarify the etiology of
the presenting problem. Later chapters evaluate the relations between the
dimensions measured by the NEO PI-R and both DSM-IV (American Psy-
chiatric Association, 1994) Axis I and Axis II clinical phenomena. These
empirical relations can be very useful diagnostically.
The third value of personality assessment is empathy and rapport. The
insights into clients that are provided can help promote understanding of
and empathy with the client. A better understanding of the person's con-
flict, struggles, and aspirations helps to present a more nuanced portrait of
the issues that surround his or her seeking of treatment. Unlike a more
clinically focused scale and its emphasis on pathology, the NEO PI-R pro-
vides a more holistic, person-centered orientation that captures clients'
growth potential as well as their growing edges.
Feedback and insight represents another important value of personality
assessment. Providing clients with feedback on their NEOs can help pro-
vide a voice for the client to articulate his or her issues: the patient can feel
understood. Many times clients feel distress or conflict but do not have the
language to fully articulate their difficulties. Oftentimes, clients may feel
that their problem is something unique, not understood, and therefore not
treatable. Once clients receive feedback on the NEO PI-R and begin to see
how their own personalities are accurately captured, they have a sense of
PERSONALITY AND ITS ASSESSMENT 13
hope. New words are learned that carry some of the nuances of their own
special psychological experiences.
This new language can then be shared with the therapist and a com-
mon language for working in therapy is then established. Presenting prob-
lems, clinical issues surrounding treatment goals, problems and issues,
and resistance (to name only a few) can all be framed in the dimensions of
the NEO PI-R. This language can also be used by clients to monitor their
own sense of change and growth throughout the therapeutic process.
These insights can readily be shared with the therapist, because both part-
ners have a common framework.
The NEO PI-R is one of a few instruments that provide a computer-
generated feedback report that can be given to clients. This report is pre-
sented in easily interpreted, jargon-free terms that give the client a broad
understanding of their personality. These reports are readily accepted by
clients and contain little objectionable material. One of the advantages of
giving feedback is that it can provide a springboard to meaningful dia-
logue between therapist and client.
Anticipating the course of therapy is the next value. Successful psychother-
apy depends not only on a therapist'S skill, but also on the client's coopera-
tion, motivation to work, and capacity for therapeutic change. No matter
what clinical school one works from, each client brings unique characteristics
to the session that need to be accommodated in treatment. The NEO PI-R can
provide important insights into these aspects of the individual.
For example, very low Agreeableness scores indicate a client who may
be skeptical about the entire therapeutic process and expect the clinician to
prove him- or herself at every tum. Such individuals may appear resistant
and mistrustful. Conversely, very high Agreeableness scores can indicate
an overly compliant client who may easily become dependent on the ther-
apist. The client may smile and nod in agreement but inside be completely
rejecting everything the therapist is saying. Or such a client may uncriti-
cally accept whatever the therapist says without critically judging its mer-
its for his or her particular situation. In such situations, the treatment fails
because the client does not struggle enough to tailor the intervention to his
or her own needs.
Because personalty scales provide information about adaptive styles,
the therapist can anticipate potential issues that may emerge in treatment
and take preemptive action. Scores on the various personality scales can
also be useful for identifying issues that were not part of the presenting
problem but may need to be explored.
Finally, matching treatments to clients is perhaps the most important
contribution personalty assessment can provide to practitioners. It has
14 CHAPTER 1
long been known that certain clients benefit more from certain types of
therapies than others. Personality measures can facilitate this matching
process. For example, individuals high on Extraversion, who are sociable
and talkative, will find therapies that require interpersonal interactions
more helpful than do introverts, who may prefer and benefit from behav-
ior therapy or Gestalt approaches. Individuals high on introspectiveness,
who are more willing to consider novel ideas and tryout unusual prob-
lem-solving approaches, may prefer Gestalt, psychoanalysis, or Jungian
analysis. Individuals who are closed to internal experiences will prefer di-
rective psychotherapies that offer sensible advice, behavioral techniques
that teach concrete skills, or client-centered therapies that provide emo-
tional support.
By obtaining high-quality information about the basic dispositions of
the client, therapists can think more precisely about the kinds of treat-
ments that would complement the client's style. Personality assessment
can help therapists better appreciate the uniqueness of clients and to think
flexibly about how to intervene. As clinical outcome research progresses,
we hope gains will be made in our ability to link specific treatments to par-
ticular types of individuals.
There is no doubt that measures of normal personality characteristics
provide a lot of "bang for the buck." Obtaining a comprehensive sampling
of personality traits can provide a pool of information that is as flexible in
its application as it is informative in its descriptions. The next section out-
lines some of the major models of personality constructs that dominate the
way personality psychologists conceptualize temperament. It also outlines
the problems these approaches create. The NEO PI-R is an outgrowth of
one of the solutions to these problems.
QUALITIES OF PERSONALITY
acteristics that are the most salient for understanding underlying motiva-
tions. There is a rich literature in personality theory.
Modem scientific thought about personality finds a strong beginning
with the work of Sigmund Freud, who saw two major personality dimen-
sions: Eros (the life instinct) and Thanatos (the death instinct). For Freud
these two qualities were the basic sources of all personal motivations.
Every behavior or characteristic of an individual could be psychologically
parsed into some combination of these two forces.
Later theorists noted many limitations to Freud's analyses of tem-
perament and began to elaborate on other qualities of the person. Areview
of these theorists and their philosophical foundations is beyond the scope
of this book, but Table 1-2 outlines a few of the more modem major per-
sonality theorists whose ideas continue to influence current personality
theory and measurement. These theorists are merely a small sampling of
personality psychologists. They were selected because they represent a
spectrum of approaches to conceptualizing personality and also because
the instruments that are based on their approaches are multidimensional
inventories claiming to represent a comprehensive sampling of personal-
ity constructs. These theorists were selected largely because they see many
more qualities of the individual as important than Freud did.
The individuals listed in Table 1-2 represent various perspectives on
normal personality. Henry Murray provided a motivational approach,
while Cattell, a trait theorist, based his work heavily on quantitative meth-
ods. However, the reader should be aware that there are other personality
models that exist for explaining abnormal personality functioning. There
are the biosocial model of Millon (e.g., Millon, 1990), the psychobiological
model of Cloninger (e.g., Cloninger, 1987; Cloninger, Svrakic, & Przybeck,
1993), and a model semantically derived from the Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders itself (e.g., Harkness & McNulty, 1994). There
are numerous variables here that also need to be understood not only in re-
lation to each other but also with respect to those constructs that define
normal functioning.
Two issues emerge from Table 1-2. First, it is quite obvious that these
few theorists have identified a number of personality characteristics; there
are 80 constructs identified in the table alone. Yet, there are many other
psychologists who have identified more circumscribed, putatively impor-
tant constructs that define important aspects of personality and predict
salient life outcomes. Such constructs as cynicism, hostility, hopelessness, shy-
ness,fear of success, fear offailure, origenence, intellectence, critical parent, adult,
free child, nurturing parent, altruism, well-being, coping ability, burnout, empa-
thy, self-esteem, stress experience, and authoritarianism are but a few of liter-
ally thousands of such scales that occupy the psychological literature. The
16 CHAPTER 1
all seem to have something in common. But one should never rely on scale
names to identify the personological content of the scale. It is entirely possi-
ble that two scales having the same name measure very different constructs.
Conversely, two scales having different names may in fact be measuring the
same quality. I give an example of this issue in the next chapter.
But here lies the real crux of the issue. There really is no established
framework for evaluating all these diverse scales or for linking them to-
gether conceptually. As things stand now, assessors need to select scales
that appear to them to be appropriate for their particular needs. However,
many of these scales and inventories proceed from very different theoreti-
cal traditions. For example, if one is interested in motivational needs, then
measures of Murray's constructs are appropriate. If one does not prefer
such a motivational perspective, then the more empirically based con-
structs offered by Cattell may be preferred. Then again, one may wish to
take the "folk" approach offered by Gough and use constructs he deems as
being important concepts for describing important interpersonal behaviors.
In each instance, though, the selection is one of theoretical preference.
Even though one's identification of a theoretical preference is based on a
serious consideration of both the heuristic value of the theory and the rel-
evant research data, unfortunately there is no larger, objective paradigm
for critically comparing and evaluating the merits of these many ap-
proaches. Therefore, the field is left in a chaotic state, in that there are many
different measurement models each with its own jargon and terminology
but with no conceptual or empirical linkage among them. When one sur-
veys the field of assessment instruments, it appears almost as a Tower of
Babel: so many constructs, so little understanding.
What is needed is a taxonomy of personality characteristics. Such a
framework would allow one to compare and contrast personality mea-
sures in terms of important, defining qualities. Ideally, this framework
would not work out of any particular theoretical tradition. Rather, it would
represent a more objective, preferably empirically based, system of classi-
fication. A taxonomic model would be useful in organizing the many per-
sonality constructs that exist and help in identifying areas of redundancy
and uniqueness. This would make the task of developing a comprehensive
assessment battery much easier.
WHAT IS A TAXONOMY?
objects that may be discovered. Measuring new items in terms of the ex-
isting dimensions of the taxon allows one to determine whether the new
element represents something distinct from existing elements, or overlaps
them in specific ways. It is this "parsing" feature of taxonomies that can be
the most value for psychological assessment.
For the most part in personality psychology, the major resources for
identifying important dimensions of personality were the experiences and
insights of individual personality theorists. Although these theories have
important theoretical and clinical value, the fact remains that they are in-
herently subjective. Ellenberger (1970) has advanced the idea that many of
these influential theories find their basis in the "creative illnesses" of their
PERSONALITY AND ITS ASSESSMENT 21
who will help us from the ones who will hurt us. We need to discover
whom can we depend on and whom we should select as a mate. In short,
to survive in the human community, one needs to be able to effectively an-
ticipate the motivations and behaviors of others. Because the personal
characteristics of others play a significant role in our own survival, it
stands to reason that over time our language has accrued terms that reflect
or describe these important aspects of people. Thus, the lexigraphic hy-
pothesis asserts: "Those individual differences that are most salient and
socially relevant in people's lives will eventually become encoded into
their language; the more important such a difference, the more likely is it
to become expressed as a single word. The analysis of the personality vo-
cabulary represented in a natural language should thus yield a finite set of
attributes that the people in the language community have generally
found to be the most important" (John, Angleitner, & Ostendorf, 1988,
p. 174). The view of language as a means for our species to codify impor-
tant dimensions of our environments is the central element of the lexi-
graphic hypothesis.
The rationale for the lexical hypothesis is relatively straightforward,
although there are three important limitations that need to be appreciated.
First, how words come to be developed and included in a lexicon is
not well understood. Further, words that do appear may not necessarily be
of importance scientifically. It is possible that some qualities of personal-
ity may not be apparent to lay observers and therefore overlooked in the
language. Also, some words that are developed may be more evaluative
and expressive than descriptive. Thus, language serves more than just a
descriptive function.
Second, the personality attributes that are defined in a lexicon are spe-
cific to the culture that generated them. Different cultures may emphasize
certain qualities over others, or may not exhibit various characteristics that
are apparent in other cultural contexts. Thus, word-based models may lack
generalizability.
Finally, although words do have a shared meaning within a culture,
there is a broad range of descriptive and explanatory convenience for any
given term. According to Allport (1961), words represent "ratbag cate-
gories," or broad ranges of qualities. For example, we can refer to two peo-
ple as being dominant, but that does not mean that their levels of
dominance are the same. Rather, the same term implies some level of gen-
eral agreement in temperament; how much is not known. Thus, the same
label may be applied to describe a wide array of behaviors. This lack of
precision makes it difficult to isolate the phenomena described.
These three issues raise important questions that need to be addressed
empirically if any lexically based taxonomy is to be found useful. For now,
PERSONALITY AND ITS ASSESSMENT 23
report), five different factors would emerge, and the content of these fac-
tors remained very similar. Norman (1963) concluded from both his own
work and a review of the literature that these five factors seemed to con-
stitute an "adequate taxonomy of personality" (p. 582).
This was a pretty powerful conclusion from these data. Norman was
arguing that underlying the ratings on these personality variables were
only five dimensions, and no more. Many were skeptical that such a small
number of dimensions could adequately describe the entire realm of per-
sonality (e.g., Digman, 1996, p. 10). But, aside from a philosophical distrust
of only five factors, there was a very real limitation to the studies done by
Norman and..others. Simply put, they relied almost exclusively on the
scales identified and employed by Cattell in his early research: 35 bipolar
rating scales. Given that Allport and Odbert (1936) identified close to
18,000 adjectives, 35 bipolar ratings (70 adjectives) seemed hardly repre-
sentative of the original pool of items. Certainly there could be other fac-
tors residing among those remaining 17,883 adjectives.
The reason why such a small number of adjectives were selected in
the first place was that factor analysis is a very complicated and intricate
statistical tool. In the years before computers, individuals had to rely on
hand calculations. It was not uncommon for someone to apply for a re-
search grant to fund such an analysis and it was equally not uncommon
for such a process to take the efforts of dozens of individuals several
months to complete. Therefore, having only 35 to 50 items to factor analyze
was a major project. Today, with the easy availability of powerful, high-
speed computers and user-friendly statistical software, it may take a single
person about several moments to "do" a factor analysis. It is ironic that it
takes longer to print out the results of a factor analysis than it does for the
computer to actually do all the calculations. Nonetheless, the issue of the
representativeness of the original selection of adjectives was a valid criti-
cism and conceptual limitation.
In response to this, Norman (1963) went back to the original pool and
identified an additional 2,800 adjectives which he was able to rationally sort
into the five obtained factor dimensions. Picking up where Norman left off
was Lewis Goldberg who, starting in the early 1980s, began an impressive
program of research aimed at empirically evaluating the linguistic structure
of these adjectives. Over the next decade using large sets of ratings based on
an extensive pool of adjectives, Goldberg discovered that the five-factor
structure obtained in the early research continued to be observed in these
larger samplings of adjective ratings (Goldberg, 1981, 1982, 1990). By the be-
ginning of the 1990s, after nearly four decades and uncounted computer
time, it was becoming clear to most researchers that underlying the adjec-
tives identified originally by Allport and Odbert really were five large fac-
tors (Digman, 1990; Goldberg, 1993; McCrae & John, 1992).
PERSONALITY AND ITS ASSESSMENT 27
than others? Or that some factors have been around human psychological
functioning longer than others, therefore having more of an opportunity to
emerge into more diverse synonym clusters? The answer to the second
question is certainly "No." All five factors have been shown to be psycho-
logically meaningful for understanding personality. Each domain is rela-
tively equal in size in terms of its capacity to explain a diverse range of
psychological phenomena. The answer to the third question is less clear
and more open to debate and discussion. As noted earlier when discussing
the lexigraphic approach, one drawback is that we do not understand how
words come to enter and leave the lexicon, or the factors that influence the
development nf new terms.
It would be interesting to perform lexigraphic analyses of the English
language as it developed over time. There are distinct periods of language
development (e.g., Old English, Middle English, Modern English); one
could determine the relative prevalence of adjectives representing the five
dimension at each stage. It may be that interpersonal behavior, a founda-
tion of human psychological and physical development and survival, is
most preeminent. As cultures develop, both socially and technologically,
new pressures are placed on humans to adapt and with this evolutionary
impetus come new terms to describe the emerging personality qualities
being expressed. One could formulate a number of hypotheses along this
line and the results of such investigations would be interesting testimoni-
als to human psychological development. But this takes us away from our
current theme. Suffice it to say that in working with adjectives, one needs
to recognize that the sheer volume of adjectives does not connote the sig-
nificance of a factor.
But the fourth question that emerges from the reality of an uneven dis-
tribution of adjectives over the five factors highlights one limitation to the
use of adjectives: "How many adjectives are necessary before one considers
a factor to exist?" Certainly, if there is a personality quality that has adap-
tive significance to humans, then many adjectives would be developed to
capture this quality and as a result, a factor would emerge representing
these numerous related terms. But there are replicable, smaller factors that
have been found beyond the "Big Five" that have not received much atten-
tion. Are these qualities as personologically trivial as their low numbers
suggest? The lexical approach does not establish any quantitative index for
determining whether a factor is large enough to warrant attention.
A second limitation to the use of simple adjectives is that there may be
qualities of people that are too complex to be reduced to a single word; they
may require several words, or a phrase or sentence in order to nuance
the quality. For example, Goldberg (1990) obtained smaller personality di-
mensions in his data when more than five factors were rotated, but they
PERSONALITY AND ITS ASSESSMENT 29
Costa, 1984). Therefore, this type of scale was avoided. Certainly, future re-
search may be able to discern new specific personality constructs that are
relatively independent of the existing facet scales for a particular domain,
but yet belong in that personological space. This would argue for the in-
clusion of more facet scales.
Although the ultimate structure of the NEO model may not have been
settled on, the current instrument-the NEO PI-R-contains a large
amount of personological information: 30 separate facet scales combining
to assess the five major dimensions of personality. The NEO PI-R is
rapidly becoming one of the most popular measures of normal personality
in the research literature. Psychometric reviews of the instrument are uni-
formly favorable (e.g., Botwin, 1995; Juni, 1995; Piedmont, 1997b), citing
the robust empirical nature of the instrument as well as its impressive pre-
dictive validity in a number of applications (e.g., physical and psycholog-
ical health, job success, well-being, coping ability).
But given the empirical strength of the instrument, there is still a basic
question that needs to be asked: "With so many different personality
inventories available, why should one use the NEO PI-R?" There are sev-
eral responses to this query. First, as noted earlier, the five-factor model
of personality represents a comprehensive, empirically based taxonomy of
personality traits. The NEO PI-R is the only commercially available in-
strument designed explicitly to measure those dimensions. Although there
are a number of marker scales available in the general literature for as-
sessing these domains <e.g., Goldberg, 1992; John, 1990; Saucier, 1994), the
NEO PI-R is the only instrument that also provides more specific facet
scales within each domain. This allows for a more precise and differential
evaluation of a person's place within each personality dimension. For
example, the Extraversion domain has the facet scales of Warmth, Gregar-
iousness, Assertiveness, Activity, Excitement Seeking, and Positive Emo-
tions. It is possible for someone to score high on Extraversion, but possess
only the interpersonal component of the domain <e.g., Warmth, Gregari-
ousness, and Assertiveness) and none of the qualities relating to personal
energy or tempo <e.g., Activity, Excitement Seeking, and Positive Emo-
tions). Global scores miss these important distinctions.
Another reason for preferring the NEO PI-R is that the five-factor
model is rapidly becoming the preeminent measurement paradigm for
personality in a number of contexts, both clinical and applied (Barrick &
Mount, 1991; Costa, 1996; Goldberg, 1993). The empirical basis and con-
ceptual clarity afforded by the model makes it an ideal measure for talking
about individual differences that crosses subdisciplines in the social sci-
ences. Further, these major dimensions of personality also have been
shown useful for describing both normal and clinical populations.
32 CHAPTER 1
The purpose of this book is to provide you with a document that pre-
sents the technical and interpretive aspects of the NEO PI-R. The follow-
ing chapters provide basic psychometric information about the instrument
and interpretive guidelines for evaluating NEO PI-R profiles. Also cov-
ered are topics relating to the application of the instrument in a research
context. After reading this book I hope that you will gain an appreciation
for the potential value of the NEO PI-R for your own work. By becoming
familiar with the concepts of the five-factor model you will develop a lan-
guage for discussing individual differences that will enable you to dia-
logue with professionals from other assessment areas. The five-factor
model is the direction that personality assessment is moving toward and
in the future will serve as the basis for all conversations about personal dis-
positions.
To help you gain this expertise, many interpretive examples are pre-
sented for both individuals and couples. It is hoped that these materials
will illustrate the interpretive nuances represented by each of the scales.
Most important, efforts have been made fo perform profile interpretations
of the NEO PI-R. Profile interpretations rely on an understanding of how
several different scales combine to describe one's personality. Although
work examining specific NEO PI-R configural patterns is just beginning,
what is available demonstrates the exciting interpretive potential the in-
strument possesses.
As you read through the case histories you will begin to see how in-
terpretations are made. As you begin to develop a fluency with the termi-
nology and its application, try to interpret the case history profiles before
reading the evaluation. In this way you can compare your own insights
with those presented. It is certainly possible for you to see things that were
PERSONALITY AND ITS ASSESSMENT 33
here and is recommended for use. In this way, individuals wishing to join
the ongoing dialogue on the psychometric integrity of the NEO PI-R can
do so in a language that is the most relevant and persuasive for those who
already use the instrument.
Overall, this book should provide a useful first step to professionals
interested in developing a basic competence in the interpretation and ap-
plication of the NEO PI-R. Many of the materials presented here are pro-
vided in an attempt to facilitate your efforts at finding new ways of
applying both the instrument and the underlying measurement model.
Many of the interpretive strategies reflect preliminary results from a few
research studies. As such, you should be aware that most of the knowledge
about the NEO PI-R still needs to be revealed-this book should be con-
sidered a work in progress. Ultimately, it will be up to you, the reader, op-
erating in diverse contexts, to provide the additional information to help
fill out our understanding of this instrument and to document its utility to
our field.
CHAPTER 2
PSYCHOMETRIC OVERVIEW
OF THE NEO PI-R
OUTLINE OF SCALES
The NEO PI-R consists of 240 items that clients answer on a (1) strongly dis-
agree to (5) strongly agree Likert-type scale and they are balanced to control
for the effects of acquiescence. The items are simple sentences describing
specific behaviors or attitudes. The NEO PI-R measures the five major do-
mains of personality and within each domain there are six facet scales that
are designed to assess more specific aspects of each domain. Alisting of the
facets is presented in Table 2-1.
In designing these 'facet scales every attempt was made to create
scales that were as nonredundant as possible while still assessing the
same overall dimension. This was done using a technique called validi-
max factoring (McCrae & Costa, 1989b). This procedure establishes the
structure of a scale by its pattern of correlations with external criteria. In
this manner, one can have confidence that information presented in one
facet is minimally redundant with information contained in other facets.
The facets for each domain were selected on the basis of their being psy-
chologically relevant and descriptively diverse (see Costa & McCrae,
1995).
Each facet scale is composed of eight items and domain scores are cal-
culated by summing scores over the six facet scales. As will be shown later,
the brevity of the scales does not compromise their reliability.
The NEO also has a rater version, Form R, which contains the same
items phrased in the third person. There are different versions for rating
men and women. Not many personality assessment instruments have this
35
36 CHAPTER 2
capability; the NEO PI-R's provision of a rater form has several real ad-
vantages. One advantage is the ability to gather five-factor model infor-
mation on an individual when a self-report cannot be trusted. Second,
rater data can be used to provide another perspective to the assessment of
a client. Finally, rater forms enable one to evaluate groups and their inter-
personal dynamics. These issues are discussed later in this book.
RELIABILITY
TABLE 2-2 Reliability Coefficients for the NEe-PI in an Adult, Mixed-sex Sample
NEG-PI Scale Alpha reliability" Retest reliability"
NEUROTICISM .92 .87
Anxiety .78 .75
Hostility .75 .74
Depression .81 .70
Self-consciousness .68 .79
Impulsiveness .70 .70
Vulnerability .77 .73
EXTRAVERSION .89 .82
Warmth .73 .72
Gregariousness .72 .73
Assertiveness .77 .79
Activity .63 .75
Excitement seeking .65 .73
Positive emotions .73 .73
OPENNESS TO EXPERIENCE .87 .83
Fantasy .76 .73
Aesthetics .76 .79
Feelings .66 .68
Actions .58 .70
Ideas .80 .79
Values .67 .71
AGREEABLENESS .86 .63'
Trust .79
Straightforwardness .71
Altruism .75
Compliance .59
Modesty .67
Tender-mindedness .56
CONSCIENTIOUSNESS .90 79'
Competence .67
Order .66
Dutifulness .62
Achievement .67
Self-discipline .75
Deliberation .71
'N= 1,539.
°
'Six-year stability coefficients for N, E, and domains and facets, 3-year coefficients for A and C domains,
all data from NEO-PI. When adjusted for attenuation, all coefficients are greater than .90. Ns range from
63 to 127 for various subsampJes of N, E, 0, A, and C.
'Retest coefficients for A and C are from NEO-PI original domain scales.
ings, the lower the retest correlations. The passage of time attenuates the
ability of a scale to correlate over two administrations. All things being
equal, the minimum acceptable retest reliability values are between .5 and
.6. Values between .6 and .7 are considered to be very good, between .7 to
.8 are excellent, and above .9 are impreSSive.
38 CHAPTER 2
The values presented in Table 2-2 are not 6-week coefficients nor are
they 6-month coefficients rather, they are 6-year values. As can be seen,
most are in the .7 to .8 range, suggesting very good stability over time. It
should also be pointed out that these values were obtained from data per-
taining to the NEO PI, the previous version of the NEO PI-R. The facet
scales for Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Openness are essentially the
same as in the current NEO PI-R. The NEO PI did not have facet scales for
the Agreeableness and Conscientiousness domains; scores for these two
domains are based on global I8-item scales. Data for these facets are still
forthcoming. In any event, adjusting these correlations for attenuation re-
sults in reliability estimates over .90!
What these data say is that there is impressive stability in these per-
sonality domains over time for adults (i.e., individuals over age 30). Costa
and McCrae (1994) provide longitudinal data on these dimensions that in-
dicate impressive stability, both in terms of rank order and mean level: 25-
year retest coefficients for these five major personality dimensions show
about 80% of the variance as stable, while 50-year estimates indicate about
60% of the variance remaining stable. Based on this great stability, Costa
and McCrae believe that after 30 years of age personality is "set like plas-
ter." From this point forward, an individual's personality has crystallized
and, all things being equal, will not change. A person begins now to select
his or her own environments and creates a direction for his or her life to
follow. Looking into this finding certainly raises a number of important
issues.
First, it seems almost counterintuitive to believe that personality is un-
changing in adulthood, suggesting that people are "locked" into their
lives. Any adult can review the past decade of life and see many areas of
personal development that have reconfigured his or her "personality."
How, then, can personality be destiny?
To answer this question, we need to revisit the concepts of genotype
and phenotype. Genotype refers to the basic underlying composition of
our nature. It is a trajectory through time, a pathway that our development
will follow. Phenotype, on the other hand, refers to the expression of the
genotype at any given moment in time. Phenotype refers to a dynamic and
lawful progression of change over time. One's underlying genotype re-
mains constant, but its expression over time will change. For example, Ex-
traversion represents a genotypic quality, but what Extraversion means, or
how it is expressed, will change over the course of one's lifetime. Being an
extravert in college may mean going to parties and staying out late, dating
many people, and being loud and boisterous. However, extraversion in
middle adulthood is something very different. Instead of going to many
parties and being loud, it could be hosting formal sit-down events, vaca-
PSYCHOMETRIC OVERVIEW 39
FACTOR STRUCTURE
The 30 facet scales of the NEO PI-R constitute five independent per-
sonality dimensions. The six facets for each domain were selected so that
there would be a sufficient number of defining variables to reliably evi-
dence each of the domains in later factor analyses of the instrument (Costa
& McCrae, 1995a). Although having six factors for each domain appears
very "neat" and artificial, there really is nothing magical about the num-
ber. There are certainly other aspects of personality that arguably could be
PSYCHOMETRIC OVERVIEW 41
other ways, these secondary patterns do make some intuitive sense. Indi-
viduals who are in control of their impulses and cope well with stress are
also able to compete and succeed. Individuals who experience a high de-
gree of positive affect are open to their inner, emotional worlds. People
who are warm and nonthreatening also appear very agreeable and com-
passionate.
Despite this conceptual overlap, it should be noted that the magni-
tude of these secondary loadings creates correlations that are relatively
small (averaging about .20). Thus, measures obtained from each domain
can be considered, for the most part, quite independent. However, truly
orthogonal domain scores can be calculated from factor score weights pre-
sented in the manual. These weights are applied to each of the 30 facets
and result in domain scores that are mutually independent.
For all practical purposes, the NED PI-R provides a factor structure
that it is quite rob~st. Minor overlaps notwithstanding, these five dimen-
sions have consistently emerged over instruments, raters, and even cul-
tures (McCrae & Costa, 1987; Piedmont, 1994; Piedmont & Chae, 1997).
Users of the NED PI-R can be confident that the information obtained
from the five dimensions represents a relatively comprehensive selection
of personological material.
language plus any additional factors that have been discovered by the so-
cial sciences.
and important to note that this omnibus test does not provide adequate
coverage of this personality dimension.
It is also interesting to note that attitudinal qualities, such as voca-
tional interests, are also related to the five-factor model. That the types of
jobs that interest us are related to personality underscores the contention
that the dimensions of the five-factor model represent genotypic qualities
that establish trajectories that guide the general directions and patterns of
our lives. This complements a growing body of research that illustrates the
usefulness of these large personality dimensions for predicting job success
and job satisfaction (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Mount, Barrick, & Strauss,
1994; Piedmont, 1993; Piedmont & Weinstein, 1994).
Recognizing the empirical overlap between scientific conceptions of
personality and the dimensions of the five-factor model provides two im-
portant pieces of information. First, we can have increased confidence that
the five-factor model is indeed a comprehensive description of individual-
difference variables. Second, given its comprehensiveness, this modei pro-
vides an opportunity for better understanding the personological content
of any psychological measure. By examining a construct's relations with
these dimensions (which, as shown previously, are clear, univocal quanti-
ties), we can derive a better understanding of the kinds of dispositions and
motivations reflected in scores from the scale. Further, we can also deter-
mine similarities and differences among scales, regardless of their theoret-
ical origin or label, by comparing their patterns of correlations with these
same five dimensions. We now turn to this major value of the five-factor
model.
These terms refer to, respectively, the tendency to see scales as being simi-
lar, or different, on the basis of their label rather than on any empirical ev-
idence. The former sees convergence where none may exist, and the latter
allows useless redundancy to develop. Perhaps the least useful place to
look for a scale's meaning is its name. But these "fallacies" too often char-
acterize the field of assessment. Without any useful, larger framework
within which to evaluate scales, there is no way to disentangle the person-
ological content of scales.
An example of this arose in my own work during graduate school.
During my first years of school, I was interested in evaluating those per-
sonality variables that predicted performance on cognitive tasks. My first
set of variables included measures of anxiety and achievement motivation
(see Piedmont, 1988b). In selecting measures of achievement, I was very
much interested in using Murray's need for achievement. Being the in-
dustrious graduate student I was, I wanted to make sure that I was doing
good science by ensuring that I had multiple measures of my constructs.
Thus, I used two need achievement scales: one from the Edwards Personal
Preference Schedule (EPPS; Edwards, 1959) and another from the Adjec-
tive Check List (ACL; H. B. Gough & Heilbrun, 1980).
These scales were engaging because they were both established mea-
sures of Murray's need-press theory. Working from Murray's earlier work,
Edwards constructed carefully worded definitions for each need scale and
then selected items that clearly reflected those dimensions. H. B. Gough
and Heilbrun (1980) were also interested in developing measures of Mur-
ray's needs and they used Edwards's original definitions and items as
guidelines for selecting their adjectives. In short, these two scales share not
only a common theoretical pedigree, but are methodological cousins as
well. If there were any two scales that I would expect to be related mea-
sures of the same construct, these were the two. Thus, once my data were
collected, my first "validity check" was to correlate these two scales to
make sure that I found the high positive correlation that putatively should
be there. When I performed the correlation, I was horrified to find that
these two scales had a correlation of "0.00"! After several attempts at
rechecking my scoring, data entry, and computer commands, I realized
that this value was accurate. A review of the literature showed why.
As early as 1968, researchers had noted that these two scales were not
related and that they should not be considered equivalent measures
(Bouchard, 1968; Megargee & Parker, 1968). Yet despite this glaring lack of
construct validity, professionals continued to use widely both of these in-
struments. In fact, the ACL and the EPPS were listed by Buros (1978)
among the top 10 tests cited in the research literature, and the EPPS was
one of the most popular tests used in both clinical (Lubin, Larsen, &
48 CHAPTER 2
Matarazzo, 1984) and counseling settings (Watkins & Campbell, 1989). De-
spite the empirical evidence and calls to the contrary (e.g., Entwisle, 1972),
both researchers and clinicians widely employed these measures and rou-
tinely interpreted them in ways consistent with their putative definitions.
There are many reasons for this, but perhaps the most salient for me
was that researchers at the time had no way to resolve this issue. Empiri-
cally both these scales predicted achievement-related outcomes, but they
failed the test of convergent validity. One or both of these scales was in-
valid. Yet there was no independent, empirical criterion to which an appeal
could be made in order to determine which one was the culprit. Although
rational expla_nations were possible (e.g., Piedmont, DiPlacido, & Keller,
1989), they lacked the kind of persuasiveness that only data can provide.
The five-factor model offers such an empirical framework for person-
ologically parsing scales. The dimensions of the model are clear and struc-
turally robust. As the data in the previous section have shown, the model
is conceptually comprehensive, capable of organizing information from di-
verse theoretical orientations. These empirical realities provide a useful
medium for evaluating scales. Correlations between a scale and these five
factors can help to conceptually locate the measure within this well-
defined space. Second, the pattern of correlations can serve as the scale's
"fingerprint," so to speak, its unique blend of personological material.
From here, the similarity of the scale to other measures can be determined
by an inspection of each construct's pattern of correlates. Two scales can be
said to be related to the degree to which they share common correlations to
the five factors. Separate patterns of five-factor correlates would be evi-
dence of discriminant validity.
Locating scales within the five-factor space provides an opportunity
for overcoming the jingle and jangle fallacies by helping to identify areas
of overlap among scales as well as areas that have been overlooked. Given
the fact that some dimensions are better represented in the natural lan-
guage than others (i.e., Agreeableness is most represented, Openness is the
least; Goldberg, 1990, 1992; McCrae, 1990; Peabody & Goldberg, 1989), it is
possible that the development of personality scales has followed a similar
course. This would mean that some aspects of personality are better as-
sessed than others, construct names notwithstanding. Correlations with
the five-factor model would help to determine whether, and to what de-
gree, such a distinct pattern exists.
Ozer and Riese (1994) argued that determining a scale's pattern of
correlates with the five-factor model will become an essential part of de-
termining its construct validity. As they stated, "[those] who continue to
employ their preferred measure without locating it within the five-factor
model can only be likened to geographers who issue reports of new land
but refuse to locate them on a map for others to find" (p. 361).
PSYCHOMETRIC OVERVIEW 49
TABLE 2-4. Joint Factor Analysis of the NED PI Domains and the
Adjective Check List
Factor
Scale N E 0 A C
NEO-PI Factors
Neuroticism (N) .80
Extraversion (E) .70
Openness (0) .79
Agreeableness (A) .68
Conscientiousness (0 .75
ACLScaies
Unfavorable -.59 -.61
Favorable .57 .57
Communality .44
Achievement .51 .73
Dominance .79 .44
Endurance .91
Order .87
Intraception .54 .63
Nurturance .83
Affiliation -.40 .66
Heterosexuality .79
Exhibition .82
Autonomy .45 -.71
Aggression .58 -.65
Change .59 .48
Succorance .49 -.61
Abasement -.67 .40
Deference -.52 .67
Self-control -.71
Self-confidence .77 .45
Personal Adjustment .62 .50
Ideal Self -.48 .50
Creative Personality .43 .71
Military Leadership .80
Masculinity .58 -.41
Femininity -.64
Critical Parent -.80
Nurturant Parent .57 .65
Adult -.40 .80
Free Child .80 .42
Adapted Child .50 -.71
Welsh'sA-1 -.50
Welsh'sA-2 .47 -.40 -.48
Welsh'sA-3 .70
Welsh'sA-4 .SO
Note: N = 244. Only Loadings> 1.40 I are given.
PSYCHOMETRIC OVERVIEW 51
tion with Extraversion also shows high scorers to be energetic and domi-
nant. In a group they would like to be leaders and may exude positive
"vibes" that others may find inviting and charismatic. Individuals high on
these two dimensions are described by others as being ambitious, proud,
persistent, active, and competitive (Hofstee, de Raad, & Goldberg, 1992).
It is also interesting to note the pattern of correlates found for the Dom-
inance scale. It, too, loads on both Extraversion and Conscientiousness, al-
though it emphasizes the surgency while the Achievement scale highlights
the ambition aspects of personality. But, despite their very different labels,
these two scales are measuring redundant aspects of personality.
Similar evaluations can be done for each of the ACL scales. Each time,
an evaluation of their construct validity and personological uniqueness
can be made. For example, the Adapted Child scale seems not to be what
it claims. The high loading on Neuroticism and the low association with
Conscientiousness portrays an individual who is emotionally distressed,
selfish, and needy of others. This pattern of correlates hardly reflects
"adapted" qualities. Thus, the name for this scale is misleading. Again,
scale names should not be relied on for interpreting scale content.
Finally, the pattern of correlations shows that many of the ACL scales
correlate with the Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness do-
mains. Fewer correlations are found for Neuroticism and Openness. The
ACL does provide a comprehensive description of personality, but does
not evidence equal personological weight across the different domains.
One may wish to complement the ACL with additional measures of these
domains in order to ensure an adequate sampling of all relevant personal-
ity dimensions.
These factor analytic results are similar to the findings of other studies
using different instruments. The studies in Table 2-3 show that the five-fac-
tor model is indeed comprehensive: Personological material from many
different theoretical positions can be substantively organized under the
umbrella of the five-factor model. As Ozer and Riese (1994) suggested, the
correlations of scales with these domains goes far in outlining the scales'
construct validity. However, the comprehensiveness of the five-factor
model should not be construed as suggesting that these domains are all that
are necessary for understanding personality: The five-factor model and the
NEO PI-R are not intended to supplant other measures of personality. Their
inclusion in an assessment battery can ensure that a broad, comprehensive
description of personality is obtained, but other scales may be necessary to
provide more precise measures of specific psychological phenomena.
By way of completeness, I should also add that I conducted a similar
joint factor analysis of the NEO PI with the EPPS in order to establish those
scales' relations to the five-factor model (Piedmont, McCrae, & Costa,
52 CHAPTER 2
1992). It was interesting to note that the achievement scale correlated with
low Agreeableness. This is not the kind of association one would expect to
find. To some degree this result was due to the item content, where high
scorers sought high-status positions so they could influence others. This is
consistent with low Agreeable individuals, who are manipulative and con-
trolling. Yet it was also believed that this unexpected finding was due to
the ipsative nature of the instrument (ipsatization is a relatively complex
mathematical process that puts the test results into a certain form-one
that is characterized by the sum of all scores adding to constant value). But
whatever the reason, it became clear why these two instruments did not
correlate-they were assessing independent dimensions of personality!
Only the presence of a larger, taxonomic structure could provide a way of
understanding why these two achievement scales relate to each other in
the ways they do. Further, the correlations of the EPPS with the NEO PI
provide a framework for reinterpreting the previous literature employing
this instrument.
The data presented have shown that the dimensions of the NEO PI-R
are internally consistent, structurally robust, and stable over time. Further,
these dimensions do in fact represent salient individual-difference qualities;
they are present not only in our general language but also among the con-
structs developed by social scientists. However, the majority of the data
presented so far rely on self-reports. It could be argued that the overlap
demonstrated in the research just described is entirely the result of self-re-
ported distortions. In other words, measures of the five factors overlapped
with other personality measures not because of some substantive relation-
ship but rather because of the presence of correlated error. The dimensions
of the five-factor model may represent only individuals' implicit personal-
ity theories, internal fictions used to help explain the behaviors of others.
These dimensions may not have any real correspondence to actual psycho-
logical dynamics (e.g., Mischel, 1968).
Determining whether the dimensions of the five-factor model repre-
sent a cognitive fiction or are real psychological quantities requires appeal
to multiple information sources.
SELF-PEER CONGRUENCE
Peers (N = 145)
Neuroticism .41' .10 .03 .03 -.14
Extraversion -.03 .4"- .00 .11 -.11
Openness .07 .10 .71' .01 -.02
Agreeableness .01 -.24~ -.01 .52' -.22~
Conscientiousness .05 -.13 -.05 -.08 .46'
'Based on factors from Table 2-4. Based on "Adjective Check List Scales and the Five-Fac-
tor Model," by R. L. Piedmont, R. R. McCrae, and P. T. Costa, Jr., 1991,/ournal of Personal-
ity and Social Psychology, 60, p. 630-637.
'p< .05
'p < .001
54 CHAPTER 2
OVERVIEW
mal functioning. In fact, several recent volumes have appeared that at-
tempt to outline theoretically the expected linkages between traits and
clinical nosology (Costa & Widiger, 1994; Strack & Lorr, 1994).
Widiger and Trull (1992) suggested four ways in which traits may be
related to Axis I disorders: (a) they may predispose individuals to a disor-
der; (b) they may be a result of the disorder; (c) they may affect the expres-
sion of the disorder; or (d) they may share a common etiology with the
disorder. McCrae (1994c) contended that the five-factor model can provide
a framework for evaluating person by treatment interactions (see also
Miller, 1991). Costa and McCrae (1992a) have also argued that the assess-
ment of these normal personality dimensions can provide valuable, nonre-
dundant information to clinical assessment protocols that rely solely on
measures of symptomatology. Finally, the five-factor model is increasingly
being perceived as a dimensional alternative to the current categorically
based classification system (Costa & Widiger, 1994).
There are four major questions that need to be addressed when con-
sidering the utility of the NEO PI-R in a clinical context. First, does the
psychometric integrity of the instrument continue to be evidenced when
used clinically? Second, what are the relations between these five domains
and Axis I symptomatology? Third, what are the relations between the
five-factor model and Axis II symptomatology? Finally, what is the incre-
mental validity of the NEO PI-R? Does it provide any additional, clinically
relevant information over what is already available with current instru-
ments? Each of these questions is discussed in tum.
gree of similarity between two sets of factor loadings. Using critical values
provided by McCrae, Zonderman, Costa, Bond, and Paunonen (1996), it
was determined that the obtained factor structure can be considered iden-
tical to the normative structure. This tells us that the NEO PI-R in a clini-
cal context continues to manifest the same underlying factor structure that
it does with nonclinical volunteers.
Retest reliability coefficients were also evaluated. Six weeks after ad-
mission to the outpatient program, participants completed another NEO
PI-R as part of their discharge process. In evaluating these values, one
needs to keep in mind that the respondents had just gone through a treat-
ment program designed to impact their ways of perceiving, interpreting,
and responding to the world. Correlations between scores taken pre- and
postcounseling were quite high for both the domains (range .52 for Neu-
roticism to .79 for Openness) and facets (range .42 for Order to .72 for Aes-
thetics, median = .58), although these values were lower than normative
values. This may have been a result of real individual-difference changes
among the participants as a consequence of their treatment.
Two other studies have evaluated the reliability of the NEO PI in a
clinical context. Trull and colleagues (1995) found 3-month retest coeffi-
cients averaging .85 for a sample of university-based clients receiving out-
patient services. Similar findings were reported by Bagby and colleagues
(1995) in a sample of depressed outpatients; they found retest reliabilities
averaging about .73. In all studies, mean level changes on the domains
were noted. The Piedmont and Ciarrocchi (in press) study evidenced large
(about one-half standard deviation) changes on all five domains. The other
two studies showed smaller and more circumscribed changes. These dif-
ferences may have been a function of the type of sample and intensity of
the treatment. Nevertheless, these three studies show that the NEO scales
remain structurally valid and reliable in a clinical context.
TABLE 2-6. Correlations between the MMPI-PD Scales and the Five
Domains Scores of the NEO PI
Personality
NEO PI Domain Score
Disorder
Symptoms N E 0 A C R
Paranoid .46' -.06 .04 -.48' -.05 .61'
Schizoid .00 -.72' -.27" -.19 .02 .73'
Schizotypal .45b -.49' -.21 _.40b -.12 .66'
Obsessive-compulsive .52' -.29" .03 -.27 -.14 .58'
Histrionic -.01 .61' .39b -.14 -.14 .71'
Dependent .64' -.21 -.01 -.08 -.33" .67'
Antisocial .29" -.22 -.02 -.42& -.27" .50"
Narcissistic -.30" .59' .43& -.06 .20 .73'
Avoidant .55' -.63' -.27 -.16 -.19 .78'
Borderline .61' .13 .18 -.45& -.24 .75'
Passive-Aggressive .56' -.19 .11 -.45& -.37& .68'
mUltiple R .82' .80' .54 .66& .66b
'p<.05
&p < .01
'p < .001, two-tailed
Note: From "OSM-III-R Personality Disorders and the Five-Factor Model of Personality," by T. J.
Trull, 1992, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, p. 557. Copyright 1992 by the American Psycholog-
ical Association. Adapted with permission.
N = 54. All correlations are two-tailed. Correlations are based on raw NEO PI scores and raw
MMPI-PD scores. R's in the bottom row indicate the correlations between the 11 personality dis-
order scores and each NEO PI domain. R's in the last column indicate the correlation between the
five NEO PI domain scores and each personality disorder. MMPI-PD = Minnesota Multiphasic Per-
sonality Inventory-Personality Disorder.
Another observation from Table 2-6 is that each of the personality dis-
order scales correlates with at least one of the five personality domains,
demonstrating that personality qualities underlying the personality disor-
ders are related to those dimensions that describe normal functioning. The
degree of overlap is quantified by the multiple Rs that are presented in the
last column and row of the table.
The multiple Rs in the last column were obtained by using each MMPI
PD scale score as the dependent variable in a multiple regression analysis.
The five personality factors of the NED-PI were entered as predictors. The
resulting multiple R indexes the degree of relationship between each per-
sonality disorder and the five-factor model. Squaring these values will
provide the amount of shared variance. As can be seen, each of the MMPI
PO scales is strongly associated with the personality domains, sharing be-
tween 25% and 61 % of their variances.
The multiple Rs in the last row index the degree to which each of the
personality domains is involved in the expression of deviant charactero-
logical functioning. These values were obtained by using each NEO-PI
PSYCHOMETRIC OVERVIEW 67
distress that fuels an active suspicion and distrust of others. The Histrionic
personality reflects qualities of high Extraversion and high Openness.
Such an individual can be described as being theatrical, intense, dramatic,
expressive, and eloquent.
It is clear that both the five-factor model of personality and the NEO
PI-R have something important to contribute to our understanding of Axis
II functioning. Those personality qualities that define normal functioning
are the same as those that describe abnormal functioning. As to where one
may draw the line, that is a question for future research. At this point there
is no cutoff score that clearly demarcates the boundary between the two
levels. Scores. on the NEO PI-R reflect only normal personality dynamics.
An extreme score on any dimension does not indicate the presence of any
type of maladaptiveness. It may be necessary to develop additional mea-
sures that capture individual-difference qualities that are more germane
diagnostically. Nl)netheless, scores on the NEO PI-R can be used to ad-
dress Axis II-type issues in two ways. First, as Costa and McCrae (l992a)
have argued, scores on these scales can be useful in making differential di-
agnoses. An individual high on Agreeableness is not likely to have an An-
tisocial Personality Disorder. Relatedly, high scores on Neuroticism can
help rule out a Schizoid disorder. Second, an individual's pattern of scores
on both the domains and facets can be compared to patterns of NEO PI-R
profiles obtained from individuals known to possess various personality
disorders. An empirical index of profile agreement can then be calculated.
Very high congruence coefficients may suggest the presence of a personal-
ity disorder, or, at the least, indicate the presence of certain character styles
similar to those whose disordered profile signature is being mimicked. The
computer report generated for the NEO PI-R will conduct such analyses,
systematically comparing the obtained profile to prototype profiles for
each of the personality disorders. In this way, clinicians can be alerted to
the presence of salient character traits that could significantly impact the
therapeutic process.
In any event, the research findings make it clear that Axis II function-
ing may be better captured by a dimensional model of personality than by
the current categorical system. As the research evidence mounts, future re-
visions of the DSM may move in that direction. In the meantime, the NEO
PI-R has much to contribute to the clinical context; it is a very useful com-
ponent of the assessment process. As the research presented in this section
has shown, scores on the NEO PI-R overlap significantly with a number of
clinical measures. The next question to be addressed is, liTo what degree
does the NEO PI-R provide information about clients that cannot be ob-
tained from standard clinical instruments?" It is to this question of incre-
mental validity that we now tum.
PSYCHOMETRIC OVERVIEW 69
linked together. As Miller (1991) has noted, "the five-factor model can re-
late patient personality, presenting complaint, treatment plan, and treat-
ment outcome to each other in a reasonable, systematic way, without loss
of empathy or compassion for the patient" (p. 432).
The data presented throughout this chapter clearly show that the five-
factor model of personality is a sound paradigm for assessing individual
differences. Although originally derived from the English language, the
constructs represented by this model are reflected in numerous instru-
ments that were derived from diverse theoretical orientations. The NEO
PI-R provides a psychometric articulation of these constructs that is quite
robust, useful in describing both normal and abnormal groups. However,
the model is not without its critics (J. Block, 1995; McAdams, 1992).
J. Block (1995) criticized the five-factor model and NEO PI-R for their
non theoretical basis; the model and the instrument were founded in fac-
tor analysis. Block believed that the model, as a statistical tool, is unable to
capture the subtleties of personality. Further, as with any statistical analy-
sis, what you put into it very much determines what you pull out. If you
put in sufficient information for five dimensions, then the factor analysis
will retrieve only five dimensions. Broadening the inclusion criteria may
result in the retrieval of additional factors. There are those who believe that
there are more than five factors (e.g., Benet & Waller, 1995; Hahn & Com-
rey, 1994; Hogan & Hogan, 1992; Hough, 1992; Tellegen, 1993). Thus, J.
Block (1995) believed that the five-factor model does not provide as com-
prehensive a sampling of personological content as it argues.
McAdams (1992) saw a number of limitations to the five-factor model.
The bottom line here is that because of its trait-based nature, the five-fac-
tor model provides a shallow and superficial description of personality.
McAdams believed that the five-factor model is essentially a "psychology
of the stranger"; it provides a broad-band description of people that is use-
ful when you do not know anything else about them.
Critics of the five-factor model argue that, at best, this paradigm cap-
tures superficial, relatively descriptive aspects of the individual that, taken
at face value, can provide some useful information. However, the model
lacks depth and interpretive insight. It overlooks many of the more im-
portant qualities of the individual that drive and direct the ongoing course
of a person's life. In response to some of these issues, the next sections at-
tempt to demonstrate the value and depth of the model. Rather than being
surface descriptors, the five dimensions reflect fundamental dispositions
72 CHAPTER 2
HERITABILITY
CROSS-CULTURAL GENERALIZABILITY
The five-factor model finds its origins in the English language. These
dimensions certainly represent important personality qualities for those
individuals who speak English and share in its derivational experiences.
This leaves the door open for the criticism that these dimensions are cul-
turally specific and may not even generalize to other cultural contexts. If
this is so, then the five-factor model represents circumspect, superficial
qualities of the individual. However, if these dimensions do generalize
cross-culturally, then this would be strong evidence that these dimensions
address human functioning at a much more basic, fundamental level.
In addressing issues of cross-cultural generalizability, there are two
major approaches: emic and etic. The first, ernie, refers to identifying cul-
ture-specific constructs. The second, etie, is concerned with identifying
similarities among cultures, to identify universal aspects of human func-
tioning. Research has shown that the dimensions of the five-factor model
represent discernible constructs in a variety of societies and can be useful
for understanding culture-specific phenomena (Capara, Barbaranelli, Bor-
gogni, & Perugini, 1993; Heaven et at., 1994; John, Goldberg, & Angleitner,
1984; Paunonen et at., 1992).
Numerous researchers have shown that the dimensions of the five-
factor model generalize quite well to a number of different cultures, in-
cluding European (Italian-Capara et al., 1993; Capara, Barbaranelli, &
Comrey, 1995; German-Borkenau & Ostendorf, 1990; Finnish and Polish-
Paunonen et at., 1992; Spanish-Avia et at., 1995), Indian (Narayanan,
Shanka, & Levine, 1995), and Asian (Japanese-Bond, Nakazato, & Shi-
raishi, 1975, Isaka, 1990; Chinese-Bond, 1979; Korean-Piedmont & Chae,
1997; Filipino-Katigbak, Church, & Akamine, 1996). Research continues to
document the presence of these five factors in both self-report and rater
data. It is of particular interest to note that the five-factor model is recov-
erable in languages that do not share a common derivational or experien-
tial history with English (see McCrae & Costa, 1997). The diversity of
cultures that have developed and applied personality dispositions identi-
cal to Western-based ones is exciting evidence of the unity of human psy-
chosocial functioning.
An even more exciting fact has been the finding that these domains
operate maturationally in similar ways across cultures (McCrae, Costa,
74 CHAPTER 2
Piedmont, et ai., 1996). It has long been known that adolescents and young
adults (ages 17-21) have higher levels of Neuroticism, Extraversion, and
Openness than older (age 30 and over) adults and lower levels of Agree-
ableness and Conscientiousness. There are real changes in personality that
occur over the early adult years. These maturational changes have also
been noted in several other cultures where the NED PI-R has been trans-
lated (e.g., Korea, Italy, and Croatia). Finding similar developmental pat-
terns across diverse cultures mutes criticisms that these factors are merely
structural artifacts that emerge from the questionnaires themselves. If this
were so, it would be unlikely that changes in the levels of these domains
over the life span would follow similar forms in different cultures.
Rather than superficial descriptive qualities or statistical artifacts, the
five-factor model represents broad, generalizable qualities of individuals.
Measuring people on these constructs enables one to anticipate important
behavioral outcomes in a variety of cultures. These data support the view
that there is tremendous parsimony to human personality. That much of
personality can be described by a finite set of constructs sets the stage for
the development of a more inclusive and complete understanding of indi-
vidual differences.
THEORETICAL FOUNDATION
McCrae and Costa (1996) have outlined a general theoretical frame-
work within which they place the five-factor model of personality. The
value of this model is that it attempts to put forth the underlying, motiva-
tional role these five dimensions play in shaping personality. Although they
note that their model is only one of a large number of theories that are con-
sistent with research on the five-factor model, its value is that it provides a
series of testable hypotheses that can help extend research in this area.
There are five components of this model and they are outlined in Figure
2-1, The first is Basic Tendencies. This refers to the fundamental raw material of
personality, the "stuff" with which we are born and from which the individ-
ual emerges. This material is genetically received and not shaped by envi-
ronmental forces (e.g., Plomin & Daniels, 1987). Rather, it is internally driven
through its development, with final form reached in adulthood. The content
of these basic tendencies includes the five broad dimensions of personality:
Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscientious-
ness. These domains are hierarchically organized, with more specific facets
defining more specific levels of organization. There are other aspects of func-
tioning located here as well, including physical characteristics (e.g., physical
appearance, gender, health), cognitive capacities (e.g., perceptual styles, gen-
eral intelligence), and physiological drives (e.g., need for food and oxygen).
PSYCHOMETRIC OVERVIEW 75
Objective
Biography
External
Inftuences
Cullund_.
LIIe _ _:
SlluetJon
B.slc
Tendencle. hi:!:=~
NIIIIIOIiciIm.
Extra-*,.
Openneu.
~.
-
CCJIIIC:ienIIcMMu
.
FIGURE 2-1. General Theoretical Framework for the Five-Factor Model of Personality.
Note. From "Toward a New Generation of Personality Theories: Theoretical Contexts for the
Five-Factor Model," by R. R. McCrae and P. T. Costa, Jr., 1996. In J. S. Wiggins (Ed.), The Five
Factor Model cfPersonality: Theoretical Perspectives (p. 73), New York: Guilford Press. Copyright
1996 by Guilford Press. Reproduced with permission.
79
80 CHAPTER 3
SOCIAL DESIRABILITY
Perhaps one of the more influential books written in the area of per-
sonality assessment was Edwards's (1957) tome on social desirability. His
argument that responses on personality questionnaires were influenced to
a large extent by the social desirability of the item rather than by any per-
sonological content was quickly embraced by the measurement field and
has had a lasting impact. The Edwards Personal Preference Schedule
(EPPS; Edwards, 1959), with its aim of eliminating biasing effects due to
social desirability, represented a major shift in test construction. Even
today, many test developers continue to find it noteworthy to demonstrate
their scales' independence from this bias by correlating scores on their
scale with measures of social desirability (Linehan & Nielsen, 1983).
One of the first efforts at uncorking the genie in the social desirability
bottle surrounded the MMPI. Edwards and Heathers (1962) argued that
the first factor to emerge from factor analyses of the MMPI represented a
social desirability factor. The implication of this assertion was that the ma-
jority of explainable variance in MMPI responses was attributable to re-
sponse distortions rather than to any kind of substantive psychological
phenomenon. However, the specter of the MMPI as providing nothing
more than systematic error was laid to rest with J. Block's (1965) cogent
and empirically compelling analysis of the problem. He was able to
demonstrate that scores on the first factor did, in fact, represent charac-
terological aspects of the individual.
Nonetheless, the MMPI possesses a validity scale, K, that is designed to
detect socially desirable responding by test takers. Several of the content
scales (Hs, Pd, Pt, Sc, and Ma) are mathematically adjusted, or corrected, for
K. The logic of this process is that mathematically partialing out social de-
INTERPRETING THE NEO PI-R 81
sirability from a test score increases its validity. However, such an assump-
tion does not seem to be empirically justified. McCrae et al. (1989) have
shown that K corrected MMPI scores are less valid than their uncorrected
versions suggesting that rather than being only a suppressor variable, so-
cial desirability represents a substantive aspect of personality. Anumber of
studies have shown that adjusting measures of normal personality for so-
cial desirability will compromise rather than enhance the scale's validity
(Hsu, 1986; Kozma & Stones, 1987; McCrae, 1986; McCrae, & Costa, 1983b;
Piedmont, McCrae, & Costa, 1992; Silver & Sines, 1962). Although the issue
of social desirability as an index of response distortion has been largely re-
futed (see Nicholson & Hogan, 1990), interest remains keen among psy-
chometricians to develop measures better suited for detecting response
distortions. Instead of developing items whose content would reflect ten-
dencies to manipulate one's self-presentation, efforts have focused on "con-
tent-free" validity scales. Such measures rely on response styles rather than
on item content and represent the next generation of validity scales.
each pair if both items are scored in the same direction. For example, one
such pair from the MPQ is "When I work with others I like to take charge"
and "I usually do not like to be a 'follower.'" Responding True to one item
and False to the other item in the pair would be scored as one inconsistent
response. High scores on the VRIN scale would indicate an individual
who is responding to the questions in an indiscriminate manner.
The TRIN scale is made up of item pairs that are opposite in content.
For example, one such item pair from the MPQ is "I would rather tum the
other cheek than get even when someone treats me badly" and "When
people insult me, I try to get even." Inconsistency is scored when an indi-
vidual answers True to both items or False to both items. High scores on
TRIN indicate a tendency to answer True indiscriminately (an acquies-
cence effect) while low scores indicate a tendency to answer False ("nonac-
quiescence" or "nay-saying"). In both cases, extreme scores would reflect
a protocol that may be invalid, uninterpretable, or both. Tellegen and
Waller (in press) have shown that the VRIN and TRIN scales are uncorre-
lated with each other and maintain low associations with the content
scales of the MPQ. Thus, these scales are unconfounded with substantive
personality and are claimed to be able to identify invalid protocols that
would not be detected by the more familiar validity scales.
Interestingly, even though the NEO PI-R does not contain any valid-
ity scales, several have been developed (Schinka, Kinder & Kramer, 1997;
Goldberg, personal communication, January 25,1995). These scales assess
the degree to which a subject is willing to claim positive and negative qual-
ities as characteristic. Others evaluate the degree of consistency in re-
sponding to the items. Regardless of the content of these validity scales,
their purpose is to identify protocols that are likely to be invalid. Put an-
other way, scores on these validity scales determine the likelihood of a
scale's validity. Scores on the validity scales moderate the validity of the
substantive scales.
In a series of analyses conducted by Piedmont and McCrae (1998), we
evaluated the validity of a number of validity scales, both those developed
for the NEO PI-R and those on the MPQ. From all the analyses that were
done in this study it was determined that the validity scales showed no
consistent, significant pattern of effects. Scores on these response-distor-
tion scales did not moderate the validity of either self-reports or observer
ratings. Their value for use among volunteer, nonclinical samples is seri-
ously questioned. Of course, further research is needed in this area to eval-
uate the utility of validity scales in clinical, forensic, and job selection
samples. Until a complete answer to the value of validity scales is ob-
tained, a reliance on self-report data will continue. In order to use such in-
formation well, some consideration of its value is necessary.
INTERPRETING THE NED PI-R 83
stereotypes), these biases do not overlap with the errors that are inherent
to self-reports (McCrae, 1982). Thus, convergence between peer and self
ratings cannot be attributable to correlated error but to a reliable effect (see
Wiggins, 1979). Because self-peer agreement seems to be the rule in mea-
suring the FFM, disagreements between these two sources of information
may be indicative of some type of distortion. McCrae (1994c) provides a
methodology for using both self and observer ratings on the NEO PI-R.
But if a self-report cannot be trusted, observer data can be a very helpful
stand-in. Data do support the validity of peer ratings in predicting useful
outcomes such as job success and marital satisfaction (Kosek, 1996a;
Mount, Barri<j<, & Strauss, 1994).
Believing that self-report data are relatively sound, Costa and McCrae
(1992c) developed the NEO PI-R without any type of validity scales (see
Costa & McCrae, 1992a). The only "validity" scale is the presence of three
short items at the end of the NEO PI-R that ask the subject if he or she has
completed all items in the proper way. Although such direct items may not
capture subtle attempts at distortion, a "No" to any of the items would
clearly indicate a questionable protocol.
NEUROTICISM
1993). The six facets for this domain, and th.eir definitions are from Costa
and McCrae (1992c). Adjective descriptors for each facet are taken from a
variety of sources (McCrae & Costa, 1992; Piedmont & Weinstein, 1993).
Nl: Anxiety. Anxious individuals are apprehensive, fearful, prone to
worry, nervous, tense, and jittery. The scale does not measure specific fears
or phobias, but high scorers are more likely to have such fears, as well as
free-floating anxiety. Low scorers are calm and relaxed. They do not dwell
on things that might go wrong. Adjectives that describe high scorers on
this facet include tense, fearful, worried, apprehensive. Adjectives that de-
scribe low scorers on this facet include calm, relaxed, stable, fearless.
N2: Angry lWstility. Angry hostility represents the tendency to expe-
rience anger and related states such as frustration and bitterness. This scale
measures the individual's readiness to experience anger; whether the anger
is expressed depends on the individual's level of Agreeableness. Note, how-
ever, that disagreeable people often score high on this scale. Low scorers
are easygoing and slow to anger. High scorers on this facet are described
as being hot-tempered, angry, and frustrated. Low scorers are described as
being amiable, even-tempered, and gentle.
N3: Depression. This scale measures normal individual differences in
the tendency to experience depressive affect. High scorers are prone to
feelings of guilt, sadness, hopelessness, and loneliness. They are easily dis-
couraged and often dejected. Such individuals are described as being
hopeless, guilty, downhearted, and blue. Low scorers rarely experience
such emotions, but they are not necessarily cheerful and lighthearted-
characteristics associated instead with Extraversion. High scorers on this
facet are described as being seldom sad, hopeful, confident, and as feeling
worthwhile.
N4: Self-Consciousness. The emotions of shame and embarrassment
form the core of this facet of Neuroticism. Self-conscious individuals are
uncomfortable around others, sensitive to ridicule, and prone to feelings of
inferiority. Self-consciousness is akin to shyness and social anxiety. Such
individuals are described as being ashamed, feel inferior, and are easily
embarrassed. Low scorers do not necessarily have grace or good social
skills; they are simply less disturbed by awkward social situations. These
individuals are described as poised, secure, and feel adequate.
N5: Impulsiveness. This facet refers to the inability to control cravings
and urges. Desires (e.g., for food, cigarettes, possessions) are perceived as
being so strong that the individual cannot resist them, although he or she
may later regret the behavior. Low scorers find it easier to resist such temp-
tations, having a high tolerance for frustration. The term impulsive should
not be confused with spontaneity, risk taking, or rapid decision time. High
scorers on this facet are described as being unable to resist cravings, hasty,
86 CHAPTER 3
EXTRAVERSION
Costa and McCrae (1985) have defined this domain as representing the
quantity and intensity of interpersonal interaction, the need for stimulation
and the capacity for joy. This domain contrasts sociable, active, person-ori-
ented individuals with those who are reserved, sober, retiring, and quiet.
There are two qualities assessed on this domain: interpersonal involvement
and energy. The former evaluates the degree to which an individual enjoys
the company of others and the latter reflects the personal tempo and activ-
ity level. This dimension has been shown to capture levels of positive affect.
E1: Warmth. Warmth is the facet of Extraversion most relevant to is-
sues of interpersonal intimacy. Warm people are affectionate and friendly.
They genuinely like people and easily form close attachments to others.
Such individuals are characterized as being outgoing, talkative, and affec-
tionate. Low scorers are neither hostile nor necessarily lacking in compas-
sion, but they are more formal, reserved, and distant in manner than high
scorers. Warmth is the facet of E that is closest to Agreeableness in inter-
personal space, but it is distinguished by a cordiality and heartiness that
is not part of A.
E2: Gregariousness. A second aspect of E is gregariousness-the pref-
erence for other people's company. Gregarious people enjoy the company
of others, the more the merrier. They are characterized as being convivial,
having many friends, and seeking social contact. Low scorers on this scale
tend to be loners who do not seek-or who even actively avoid-social
stimulation. These individuals are described as avoiding crowds and pre-
ferring to be alone.
E3: Assertiveness. High scorers on this scale are dominant, forceful,
and socially ascendant. They speak without hesitation and often become
group leaders. Adjective descriptions of high scorers include dominant,
forceful, confident, and decisive. Low scorers prefer to keep in the back-
ground and let others do the talking. Adjective descriptors include unas-
suming, retiring, and reticent.
INTERPRETING THE NEO PI-R 87
E4: Activity. A high Activity score is seen in rapid tempo and vigor-
ous movement, in a sense of energy, and in a need to keep busy. Active
people lead fast-paced lives. They are described as being energetic, fast-
paced, and vigorous. Low scorers are more leisurely and relaxed in tempo,
although they are not necessarily sluggish or lazy. They are described by
others as being unhurried, slow, and deliberate.
E5: Excitement-Seeking. High scorers on this scale crave excitement
and stimulation. They like bright colors and noisy environments. Excite-
ment-seeking is akin to some aspects of sensation seeking. These individ-
uals are described as flashy, seekers of strong stimulation, and risk takers.
Low scorers feel little need for thrills and prefer a life that high scorers
might find boring. These individuals are described as cautious, staid, and
uninterested in thrills.
E6: Positive Emotions. This facet reflects the tendency to experience
positive emotions such as joy, happiness, love, and excitement. High scor-
ers on this scale laugh easily and often. They are seen as cheerful, high-
spirited, joyful, and optimistic. Low scorers are not necessarily unhappy;
they are merely less exuberant and high-spirited. They are described as
unenthusiastic, placid, and serious. Research has shown that happiness
and life satisfaction are related to both Nand E, and that Positive Emotions
is the facet of E most relevant to the prediction of happiness.
OPENNESS TO EXPERIENCE
02: Aesthetics. High scorers on this scale have a deep appreciation for
art and beauty. They are moved by poetry, absorbed in music, and in-
trigued by art. They need not have artistic talent, nor even necessarily
what most people would consider good taste, but for many of them, inter-
est in the arts will lead them to develop a wider knowledge and apprecia-
tion than the average individual. They are seen as valuing aesthetic
experiences and as moved by art and beauty. Low scorers are relatively in-
sensitive to and uninterested in art and beauty. They are described by oth-
ers as being insensitive to art and unappreciative of beauty.
03: Feelings. Openness to feelings implies receptivity to one's own
inner feelings and emotions and the evaluation of emotion as an important
part of life. High scorers experience deeper and more differentiated emo-
tional states and feel both happiness and unhappiness more intensely than
others. Descriptions of high scorers include emotionally responsive, sen-
sitive, empathic, and values own feelings. Low scorers have somewhat
blunted affects and do not believe that feeling states are of much impor-
tance. Descriptions of low scorers include narrow range of emotions and
insensitive to surroundings.
04: Actions. Openness is seen behaviorally in the willingness to try
different activities, go new places, or eat unusual foods. High scorers on
this scale prefer novelty and variety to familiarity and routine. Over time,
they may engage in a series of different hobbies. They are described by
others as seeking novelty and variety and trying new activities. Low scor-
ers find change difficult and prefer to stick with the tried and true. These
individuals are perceived by others as being set in their ways and prefer-
ring the familiar.
05: Ideas. Intellectual curiosity is an aspect of Openness that has long
been recognized. This trait is seen not only in an active pursuit of intellec-
tual interests for their own sake, but also in open-mindedness and a will-
ingness to consider new, perhaps unconventional ideas. High scorers enjoy
both philosophical arguments and brain-teasers. Openness to ideas does
not necessarily imply high intelligence, although it can contribute to the
development of intellectual potential. These individuals are described as
being intellectually curious, analytical, and theoretically oriented. Low
scorers on the scale have limited curiosity and, if highly intelligent, nar-
rowly focus their resources on limited topics. These individuals are de-
scribed as being pragmatic, factually oriented, and unappreciative of
intellectual challenges.
06: Values. Openness to Values means the readiness to reexamine so-
cial, political, and religious values. High scorers on this facet are seen as
tolerant, broad-minded, nonconforming, and open-minded. Closed indi-
viduals tend to accept authority and honor tradition and as a consequence
INTERPRETING THE NED PI-R 89
AGREEABLENESS
gression, and to forgive and forget. Compliant people are meek and mild.
High scorers are characterized as being deferential, obliging, and kind. The
low scorer is aggressive, prefers to compete rather than cooperate, and has
no reluctance to express anger when necessary. Low scorers are character-
ized as being stubborn, demanding, headstrong, and hard-hearted.
AS: Modesty. High scorers on this scale are humble and self-effacing
although they are not necessarily lacking in self-confidence or self-esteem.
These individuals are perceived by others as being humble and unassum-
ing. Low scorers believe they are superior people and may be considered
conceited or arrogant by others. A pathological lack of modesty is part of
the clinical conception of narcissism. These individuals are seen by others
as being aggressive, tending to show off, and tough.
A6: Tender-Mindedness. This facet scale measures attitudes of sympa-
thy and concern for others. High scorers are moved by others' needs and
emphasize the human side of social policies. Adjective descriptors of high
scorers include friendly, warm, kind, gentle, and soft-hearted. Low scorers
are more hardheaded and less moved by appeals to pity. They would con-
sider themselves realists who make rational decisions based on cold logic.
Adjective descriptors of low scorers include intolerant, cold, opinionated,
and snobbish.
CONSCIENTIOUSNESS
Now that definitions of the 30 NEO PI-R facets has been provided,
this section presents three case profiles for interpretation. This provides
the reader an opportunity to apply these definitions to actual scores and
begin to develop a "feel" for how individuals come to be represented by
these scales.
In evaluating a NEO PI-R profile, I find it useful to proceed domain
by domain. I first evaluate a person's standing on the overall domain, say
Neuroticism, to gain an overview of the kinds of global dynamics that
characterize the person. Then I evaluate the facet scales for that domain in
order to isolate those aspects of the larger domain that are the most defin-
ing. Keep in mind that although the facet scales for a domain are all re-
lated, they were designed to be as nonredundant as possible (see Costa &
McCrae, 1995a). This can result in a high degree of interscale scatter within
each personality dimension. It is not uncommon for an individual to score
high (or low) on a domain, but be low (or high) on several of the facet
scales. Facet analysis can be very revealing and provide a more intimate
understanding of a client (Miller, 1991).
In evaluating the elevation of scores on each scale, T-scores between
45 and 55 are considered average or normative. There is usually no real in-
terpretation of such scores, because the individual can be equally likely to
exhibit behaviors characteristic of the high and low poles. T-scores below
45 are considered low and scores below 35 are seen as being very low. Con-
versely, T-scores above 55 are seen as being high and those above 65, very
high. When scores move into these regions, they carry interpretive value;
a respondent begins to reflect more conSistently the characteristics defin-
ing that end of the pole. When T-scores for a facet or domain go above 80
or below 20, this may indicate the presence of real deficits in an individ-
ual's ability to function in his or her environment. Therefore, one needs to
consider the real possibility of the presence of some pathological process.
The 35 scales of the NEO PI-R present a tremendous amount of infor-
mation to process. This is both good and bad. It is good because of the
vastness of insights revealed by the scales. There are many personological
implications for scores' from each scale, presenting a rather fine-grained
analysis of the respondent. The down side is really not that bad. So much
information means that it will take time to develop a working knowledge
INTERPRETING THE NEO PI-R 93
of all the facet and domain scales and what they represent about a client.
Leaming the 30 facet names can be its own challenge. With practice, how-
ever, one can quickly master this task and become quite fluent in the lan-
guage of the NEO PI-R. Aside from facilitating interpretation, the
personality qualities represented by the NEO PI-R will also provide a lan-
guage for conceptualizing and evaluating other personality constructs and
scales. With this in mind, we turn now to interpreting some case profiles.
The cases presented are of actual individuals that have been culled
from several sources. Some are of actual clinical clients; others are from
data files accumulated by the author over several years. Every effort was
made to select indiYiduals with particularly interesting psychosocial his-
tories and profile interpretations.
J:
.2'
J:
~
Q.ll0·
:> :
J:
.121 ~ :
J:
Q)
g>
Q3: 50·
~ ~
~
..J
~
~
of sOCiability and a desire for social contacts, although there is room for
solitary pursuits as well. Her low average score on E6 (Positive Emotions)
indicates a person who appears somewhat serious and may express hap-
piness in a somewhafmuted fashion.
The Openness to Experience domain score is average, suggesting an
individual who has some inner needs for structure and direction as well as
a capacity to explore new avenues of activity. Underlying this person is
some degree of rigidity; there are limits to how much change or novelty
Debbie would tolerate. Again, however, there is quite a degree of scatter
among the facet scales for this domain.
She is high on 01 (Fantasy) and 05 (Ideas). Debbie has an active inner
life composed of imagination and fantasy. No doubt this inner world con-
tributes to and benefits from her fond desire for reading. The characters in
her books are brought to life by her capacity to imagine. Her high score on
05 (Ideas) suggests that Debbie is very much interested in intellectual pur-
suits, theoretical arguments and discussions, and is willing to consider
new and unconventional ideas. These characteristics certainly work well
with her academic abilities and successes.
However, Debbie scores very low on 04 (Actions) and 06 (Values). The
low Actions suggests that Debbie prefers a great deal of structure in her life.
INTERPRETING THE NEO PI-R 95
There is a preference for the known and a dislike for change. As Debbie once
told me, "1 am pretty set in my ways." Her score on this scale indicates some
degree of personal rigidity. Her low score on 06 (Values) indicates that Deb-
bie also holds some very dear personal beliefs and values that are not open
for discussion, evaluation, or change. Low scores here reflect a dogmatic ori-
entation to a value network that may be difficult to penetrate or to change.
She is accepting of authority and conforms to the dictates of her beliefs.
Debbie's average score on 02 (Aesthetics) represents some enjoyment
and appreciation of the arts. This score reflects her interests in reading, al-
though this scale also reflects interests in other aspects of the arts, such as
theater, ballet, and opera, for which Debbie has far less interest. Her aver-
age score on 03 (Feelings) suggests an individual who has some receptiv-
ity to emotional information and may have some sensitivity to the feelings
of others. However, given her low scores on the Neuroticism facets, Deb-
bie may feel much more comfortable on a cognitive level than on an emo-
tional one. Yet, her score is on the average to low cusp, suggesting that
there may be some emotions that Debbie does not like to confront. Feelings
may be less important to her than ideas.
Concerning Agreeableness, Debbie scores again in the average range.
This indicates an individual who is generally warm and trusting, but has a
degree of skepticism that will not allow her to take all people and events at
face value. She scores high on the Al (Trust), A2 (Straightforwardness),
and A3 (Altruism) scales, suggesting an individual who presents herself to
the world in a frank and "up front" manner and believes that others do the
same. She has a desire to help others in distress and can be very generous
and considerate. Debbie scored Iowan AS (Modesty), indicating that she
has no problems in talking about her achievements and may have a feeling
of superiority because of them. This low score in conjunction with her low
score on Values (06) suggests that Debbie may come across to others as
being arrogant or superior. She may project, in word and deed, a strong
sense of righteousness that others may find off-putting.
She scored in the average range on A4 (Compliance) and A6 (Tender-
Mindedness). A4 indicates that although Debbie can be deferential and
compliant, she can also be competitive and confrontational; when pushed,
she may push back. Her score on A6 indicates that she may not always be
moved by emotional appeals from others. Although her A3 score says she
is helpful, such assistance may not always be forthcoming, especially if she
believes that the person may have brought the problem on him- or herself.
Finally, her Conscientiousness score is in the very high range, sug-
gesting that Debbie is a very organized woman with a strong sense of per-
sonal organization and competency. She sets very high standards for
herself and strives hard to reach them. All of her facet scores for this do-
96 CHAPTER 3
main are in the high to very high range indicating that Conscientiousness
is a broad-based definer of Debbie's personality. Cl (Competence) is very
high, indicating that Debbie believes herself to be very capable and com-
petent, and that this sense of self-efficacy generalizes to any context she
may find herself in. C2 (Order) shows her to be very organized and me-
thodical. C3 (Dutifulness) shows her to be dependable and organized; she
will follow through on her commitments. C4 (Achievement Striving)
shows that Debbie sets very high standards for herself and will work very
hard to attain them. C5 (Self-Discipline) shows her to be focused in her ef-
forts and not easily distracted from her goals. Finally, C6 (Deliberation)
shows that Debbie is a thoughtful and careful young woman. She is not
one to make spontaneous decisions; rather, she will weigh her options
carefully and make measured responses.
These very high Conscientiousness facet scores, coupled with the low
facets on Openness (e.g., Feelings and Actions) and the low AS (Modesty)
score may also suggest someone who has a high need for control over her
environment and possibly the people in it. Given her low Neuroticism
scores, this need for control is not defensive in nature (Le., an attempt to
protect herself against perceived emotional vulnerabilities). Rather, it stems
from this young woman's clear sense of direction and purpose in life.
The overall impression that emerges from this profile is one of a
young lady who is very ambitious and competent. Debbie has set some
very high goals for herself (such as getting a full scholarship to college)
and will do what it takes to reach them. What also emerges is a sense that
Debbie may be perceived as somewhat aloof and distant from others. Al-
though active in extracurricular activities at school, these endeavors may
serve the purpose of obtaining recognition and success more than as a
means of making friends and socializing. Her low Openness facets show
Debbie to have some personal rigidity in terms of her belief and value sys-
tems which may serve to isolate her from others who may not conform to
these personal ideals. Further energizing this rigidity is her high Dutiful-
ness (C3) scores, which show her to have a committed sense of right and
wrong. Whether she is involved with others is dependent on whether or
not they conform to her values and goals; her average score on E shows
that she can be comfortable with others or be alone. Her high Assertive-
ness (E3) and low average Compliance (A4) show her ability to be a leader
and manage others in ways that will take her closer to her goals.
N E o A c
N E 0 A C
.<: .
,!ill eo -
:I: .
.
II
I!
~" :
:
.
.
« :
However, this belies the many interesting features that are present in this
case.
The overall N score shows an individual who experiences an average
amount of negative affect in his life. He is as blue and distressed as most of
us. An examination of his facet scales shows Joe to be low on Nl (Anxiety)
and N2 (Hostility), suggesting that he is a rather calm and easygoing person.
This low level of anxiety perhaps allows him to gamble without having to
worry about the future. His high score is on N5 (Impulsiveness), indicating
that Joe is frequently bothered by thoughts of doing things that he feels may
not be appropriate. He experiences strong urges or temptations. His average
score on N6 (Vulnerability) suggests that he can cope with these feelings rel-
atively well, although there may be times when he feels overwhelmed.
A point of interest here. According to the NED PI-R manual (Costa &
McCrae, 1992c), high scores on NS suggest that an individual will give in
to their temptations. I have found from my own usage of the NED PI-R
that individuals high on Impulsiveness are indeed bothered by many
urges, but whether they capitulate to them is a function of their scores on
Conscientiousness, particularly the CS (Self-Discipline) and C6 (Delibera-
tion) facets. If an individual's scores are low on these facets, then it is likely
that the person will give in to the urges. If scores are high, then the indi-
vidual is unlikely to give in.
98 CHAPTER 3
The domain score for Extraversion is in the high range, indicating that
Joe is, overall, a sociable and engaging individual. He likes the company of
others and is very spirited and engaging. E1 (Warmth) is in the average
range, suggesting that Joe is discriminating in his approach to others; he
can be formal and aloof or friendly and approaching, depending on the sit-
uation. A more consistent description is provided by the remaining E
facets, which are all in the high range. Joe is certainly gregarious, wanting
to be with others. When in the presence of others, he enjoys being the
leader or center of attention. He wants others to look to him for direction.
He is high on E4 (Activity), so Joe is energetic and active. He has a fast per-
sonal temp<>to his life, always liking to be busy. High on E5 (Excitement-
Seeking), he enjoys risks and thrills. He wants to be where the action is,
preferably in the center of it. Finally, the high score on E6 (Positive Emo-
tions) shows Joe to have a high degree of elan and spiritedness. He exudes
much personal energy, and others may be attracted to his very upbeat and
positive outlooks. People may consider him to be charismatic.
His average score on the Openness domain shows Joe to have some
flexibility but also to have areas that need more structure. He is low on the
04 (Activity) and 06 (Values) facets. This, like Debbie, portrays an indi-
vidual who is rigid and dogmatic. He has a clear structure that he likes to
follow and a value system that clearly outlines what he needs to do. Joe
scored high on 03 (Feelings), suggesting that he is open to his own feelings
and those of others. He realizes the value of emotions in his life and is com-
fortable working with a wide range of them. Joe's average scores on 01
(Fantasy), 02 (Aesthetics), and 05 (Ideas) suggest that Joe does not have a
developed inner world and may prefer to keep focused on tasks and peo-
ple around him. His average Ideas score indicates that he may concern
himself less with "the big picture" and more with the bottom line. He
would appear direct and pragmatic.
Scores on Agreeableness are in the average range, suggesting that al-
though caring and concerned about the welfare of others, there is a real-
ization that people may not always be what they appear. As such, there is
some caution in joe's evaluation of others. Of note is his low score on A6
(Tender-Mindedness), reflecting a more hard-hearted, intolerant attitude
toward others. Joe is not moved by emotional appeals from others, tending
to interpret situational distress in more rational, cognitive ways. It would
be of interest to evaluate this low A6 with his low 06 score. Perhaps this in-
sensitivity may work out of his own value network. Given his high 03
(Feelings) score, it can be concluded that this coldness does not originate
from some avoidance of emotions or fear of them.
Finally, scores on Conscientiousness are also average, indicating that
Joe has some degree of personal organization and reliability that enables
INTERPRETING THE NEO PI-R 99
FIGURE 3-3. NEO PI-R profile of Frank V. along with his spouse's rating.
PROVIDING FEEDBACK
The NEO PI-R provides a wealth of information about an individual.
The information that is available is intended for interpretation by a pro-
fessional trained in psychological testing. Thus, providing test takers with
their interpretive reports would be inappropriate. However, there are
times when it may be useful therapeutically to share the results of testing
with a client. Feedback from the NEO PI-R may help provide a paradigm
for clients to understand their presenting problems and issues. Sometimes
clients suffer from distress that they are unable to articulate. The five-fac-
tor model can provide a framework for thinking about these personal is-
sues and can facilitate personal insight. 5ecOlld, the five-factor model can
be used as a common language for both therapist and client in discussing
therapeutic issues. The NEO PI-R may help frame the clinical dialogue in
ways that the patient can comprehend and that are meaningful for the
therapist.
To make such a disclosure would require a method of presentation
that, while accurate, would be nonthreatening to the client and under-
standable. The NEO PI-R comes with two methods for sharing results
with the client. The first is "Your NEO Summary," a standardized one-
page form that provides a general overview of the client's standings on the
domains. A reproduction of this form is presented in Figure 3-4. Each of
106 CHAPTER 3
Your
The NEO inventory measures five broad or adjustment. It does, however, give you some idea about
domains, or dimensions, of personality. The responses what makes you unique in your ways of thinking, feeling,
that you gave to the statements about your thoughts, feel· and interacting with others.
ings, and goals can be compared with those of other adults This summary is intended to give you a general
to give a description of your personality. idea of how your personality might be described. It is not
For each of the five domains, descriptions are a detailed report. If you completed the inventory again, you
given below for diffi:rent ranges of scores. The descriptions might score somewhat diffi:rently. For most individuals,
that are cMcked provide descriptions ofyou, based on your however, personality traits tend to be very stable in adult·
responses to the inventory items. hood. Unless you experience major life changes or make
The NEO inventory measures differences among deliberate elfurts to change yourself, this summary should
normal individuals. It is not a test of intelligence or ability, apply to you throughout your adult life.
and it is not intended to diagnose problems of mental health
Compared with the responses of other people, yow- respoDses sugest that you caD be described as:
0 Sensitive, emotional, and 0 Generally calm and able to 0 Secure, hardy, and generally
prone to experience feelings deal with stress, but you relaxed even under stressful
that are upsetting. sometimes experience conditions.
feelings of guilt, anger, or
sadness.
0 Cooscientious and well· 0 Dependable and moderately 0 Easygoing, oot very well·
organized. \bu have high wen·organized. \bu organized, and sometimes
sundards and always srrive generally hive clear goals careless. \bu prefer not to
to achieve your goals. bur are able to set your work make plans.
aside.
the five rows corresponds to one of the major dimensions. When an indi-
vidual scores in the high range for a domain (e.g., T-score from 55 and
above), one simply checks the first box. Average-range T-scores (e.g.,
45-55) are represented in the second box. Finally, the interpretation for low
range T-scores (e.g., 0 to 45) are provided in the third column. These de-
INTERPRETING THE NEO PI-R 107
scriptions are clear and easily understood by individuals not familiar with
psychological terminology. Further, the presentation is nonthreatening, es-
pecially for the more sensitive dimensions of high Neuroticism.
Another way that information can be shared with clients is through the
computer scoring program. This flexible program provides many advan-
tages for clinical use. First, individuals can complete the NEO PI-R at the
computer itself. Information can then automatically be scored and profiles
generated. If this is not possible, then responses can be manually entered.
The computer program provides a number of interpretive options, includ-
ing a section on clinical hypotheses. But for our current purposes, the com-
puter scoring program produces a three-page summary report that can be
given to the respondent. An example of this report is provided in the Ap-
pendix, where the results for Debbie K. are given. As can be seen, this report
provides more information than the "Your NEO Summary" form. It can be
generated easily when the computer scoring program is used.
Of course, for some clients feedback may be inappropriate, and it is
the test user's ultimate responsibility to determine the suitability of the in-
formation for the client's consumption. However, this information can
help reinforce the therapeutic alliance between client and therapist. For the
therapist, the wide range of information provided by the NEO PI-R can
help foster greater understanding and empathy for the client.
Particularly in the short-term therapy situation, NEO scores can be
helpful in facilitating understanding by providing a wider personological
context for evaluating the client's presenting problems. For the client, NEO
scores can provide an opportunity for new insight and personal under-
standing. By being able to more succinctly voice personal issues, the client
may come to feel better understood. Finally, the language of the five-factor
model can provide a common bridge for client and therapist to use in dis-
cussing the issues that emerge over the course of treatment.
likely that the test is incorrect, then you should consider accessing a dif-
ferent source of information about the client, such as an observer rating.
Observer ratings provide a different perspective on the client that may be
revealing. But before concluding that the test is in error, consider the fol-
lowing two possibilities.
First, there could have been an error made in completing the test, not
so much that the respondent was distorting or "lying," but that the indi-
vidual may have made clerical errors in filling out the forms. For example,
responses on the NEO PI-R are arranged from strongly disagree to
strongly agree. It is possible that a respondent may have reversed that or-
dering accidentally. Instead of indicating agreement, the respondent may
have circled the disagreement response. One way of detecting this prob-
lem is to refer to the three simple "validity checks" at the end of the self-
report answer sheet. Individuals are asked if they have completed each of
the items correctly. Someone who answers "Disagree" or "Strongly Dis-
agree" to this item may have made a mistake in responding.
Another possibility is that the person may have inadvertently shifted
answers on the answer sheet, so that the response to question 15 was
placed in the response space for question 16. Therefore, all remaining an-
swers are moved down one. This would serve to skew the entire scoring
process.
However, if such clerical errors can be dismissed, then an examination
of the item content with the respondent may be in order. It may be that the
client filled in the wrong response to several items. However, if this can be
ruled out, then test score variation from one's self-concept may be an indi-
cation that the test has captured something new about the respondent.
Once clerical errors can be ruled out, it may be informative to sit down
with the respondent and review their responses on the scale(s) that seem
in error. It may be that the client does not have a good understanding of
what a particular facet scale represents. Keep in mind that the five-factor
model has very specific meanings for each of its measured constructs. The
usage of these labels may not necessarily correspond to the way the client
is using language. For example, I use the NEO PI-R in a number of reli-
gious and pastoral contexts. Frequently I give the instrument to clergy
members who usually score average to low on the Values (06) facet. Fre-
quently they are taken back by this score, believing that this means they
INTERPRETING THE NEO PI-R 109
have few, if any, values; that they are amoral or immoral. I am frequently
told that they certainly do have values! What they may not understand at
first is that the Values facet does not speak to whether one is moral or not.
Rather, it examines the degree to which an individual has made some fun-
damental commitments to a worldview that he or she is not willing to
compromise. Certainly clergy have made some decisions about how the
world operates, and they feel strongly about those values. Thus the low to
average score on Values makes more sense for this group when it is prop-
erly understood.
Educating the client in the language of the five-factor model is very
important. It can not only clear up any misinterpretations of what scores
represent, but it also provides a clear and concise language for discussing
his or her own personality and the kinds of motivations that influence his
or her behavior. Both client and therapist can use the same language for
communicating.
Another advantage to reviewing the items is that it allows you to dis-
cern the context the client was using in responding to the items. At times,
clients will say that they were evaluating their behavior in terms of their
work personae, or how they are at home. Or they may have understood
the item differently from how it was intended. In a study looking at dis-
agreements between spouses on their NEO PI-R ratings, McCrae, Costa,
Stone, and Fagan (1996) found that the context and referent behaviors used
for interpreting the items were the most frequent reasons for lack of agree-
ment.
A final reason for discrepancy may be that the respondent is doing
things for reasons different than they think. It has been my experience that
when you can rule out all clerical and interpretive reasons for a discrep-
ancy, the most likely explanation is that the individual may not be truly
aware of his or her motivations. The systematic evaluation of a person's
behavior that is afforded by a standardized personality instrument may
provide a different interpretation. For example, a client does very well in
college, always in the top 10% of her class, always excelling in each class,
always wanting to do more and learn more. She may believe that she is
highly achievement motivated, and as such, high on Conscientiousness.
Although Conscientiousness is related to academic achievement, there are
many motivational reasons for excelling in school. If this woman scores
Iowan Conscientiousness, then it may be that her success can be attributed
to other sources of motivation, such as her high level of Openness to Ideas
(05) and Openness to Actions (04). Such individuals are very theoretically
oriented, like new and different adventures, and enjoy new experiences
and new knowledge. So, her success may be due less to her ambitiousness
than to her innate curiosity and enjoyment of novelty. Or she may be high
110 CHAPTER 3
on Compliance (A4) and does well in school because of the high expecta-
tions her parents have set for her.
Although certain behaviors may be thought of as prototypical of par-
ticular dispositions (e.g., Buss & Craik 1980, 1983), the reality is that any
given behavior can express any number of traits, and a given trait can be
expressed through any number of behaviors. We should never take be-
havior at face value. The merit of a personality assessment instrument is
that it can provide insights into the individual that are unobtainable from
other sources. If a test told us only what an individual can verify, then
there would be no reason to give a test. It is exactly this type of discrepancy
that lies at the- heart of evaluation-the opportunity to gain an enhanced
understanding of the individual.
INTERPRETING THE NEO PI-R 111
The Revised NEO Personality Inventory measures five broad domains or factors of per-
sonality, and six more specific traits or factors within each domain. The responses that you
gave to the statements about your thoughts, feelings, and goals can be compared to those of
other college-age respondents to give a description of your personality.
The NEO Personality Inventory measures differences in personality traits. It is not a test
of intelligence or ability, and is not intended to diagnose psychiatric disorders. It does, how-
ever, give you some idea about what makes you unique in your ways of thinking, feeling, and
interacting with others. This summary is intended to give you a general idea of how your per-
sonality might be described. If you completed the inventory again, you might score some-
what differently, and other people might have different views of what you are like.
If you have any questions or concerns about this summary, please feel free to discuss
them with the professional who administered the inventory.
TheEDomain
The E Domain measures traits related to energy and enthusiasm, especially when deal-
ing with people. Low scorers are serious and introverted; high scorers are outgoing extro-
verts. Your total score puts you in the low range on this factor. You are average in your level
of warmth toward others, but you sometimes enjoy large and noisy crowds or parties. You are
as assertive as most women when the circumstances require. You have a low level of energy
and prefer a slow and steady pace. Excitement, stimulation, and thrills have little appeal to
you and you are less prone to experience feelings of joy and happiness than most women.
The 0 Domain
The facets of this domain measure responses to various kinds of experience. Low scorers
are down-to-earth and conventional; they prefer the familiar and the tried-and-true. High
scorers are imaginative and open-minded. You score in the average range. Your responses
112 CHAPTER 3
suggest that you are somewhat open. You have a vivid imagination and an active fantasy life.
You are like most people in your appreciation of beauty in music, art, poetry, and nature, but
your feelings and emotional reactions are muted and unimportant to you. You seldom enjoy
new and different activities and have a low need for variety in your life. You are interested
in intellectual challenges and in unusual ideas and perspectives, but you are conservative in
your social, political, and moral beliefs.
The A Domain
This domain is concerned with styles of interpersonal interaction. Low scorers are hard-
headed and competitive; high scores compassionate and cooperative. Across the six facets in
this domain, you describe yourself as being generally average on this domain. In particular,
you have moderal! trust in others, but are not gullible, recognizing that people can some-
times be deceptive. You are very candid and sincere and would find it difficult to deceive or
manipulate others, and you are reasonably considerate of others and responsive to requests
for help. You can be very competitive and are ready to fight for your views if necessary. You
are quite proud of yourself and your accomplishments, and happy to take credit for them.
Compared to other people, you are average in your concern for those in need, and your social
and political attitudes balance compassion with realism.
TheCDomain
Traits in this domain describe differences in motivation and persistence. Low scorers are
easygoing and not inclined to make plans or schedules. High scorers are conscientious and
well-organized. Compared to other college-aged respondents, your score falls in the very
high range on this factor. You are rational, prudent, practical, resourceful, and well-prepared.
You are very neat, punctual, and well-organized, and you are highly conscientious, adhering
strictly to your ethical principles. You have a high aspiration level and strive for excellence
in whatever you do. You are determined, perSistent, and able to force yourself to do what is
necessary. You are cautious and deliberate and think carefully before acting.
CHAPTER 4
GENERAL
The five domain scores and 30 facet scales found in the NEO PI-R provide
a tremendous amount of information about a client that may appear over-
whelming to the beginning NEO PI-R user. Keeping the names of the
scales at one's fingertips can be difficult, and remembering the meaning of
each scale even more challenging. It is always good practice to use this ter-
minology as frequently as possible, not just when interpreting a profile.
The language of the NEO PI-R is useful for conceptualizing the motiva-
tions of people, and you should try to use these labels for describing the
behaviors of others. The more you use the terminology, the more adept
you will become at both remembering and understanding the constructs.
No doubt when a scale provides a great deal of information, it is help-
ful to organize that information into meaningful, interpretive chunks. Such
aggregation appreciates the naturally occurring covariance among scales,
and enables a broader interpretation of the individual. Rather than focus-
ing on specific traits and qualities, the test interpreter can discern larger
trends and patterns in the respondent's life. Such "chunking" is referred to
as either profile or typological analysis.
A typology is the flip side of the taxonomic coin (Bailey, 1994). A tax-
onomy enumerates the fundamental elements that describe some set of ob-
jects, while a typology identifies constellations of these elements that
co-occur. For example, we can talk about an individual's desire to com-
113
114 CHAPTER 4
pete, high activity level, and proneness to anger; each construct tells us
something about the person and begins to provide a sketching of their per-
sonality. Or we can see each of these traits as being part of some larger psy-
chological organization that, as a whole, may reflect patterns of behavior
and life outcomes that may not be discernible from an examination of each
of the individual traits-say, the Type A personality (Friedman & Rosen-
man, 1959). This well-known typology represents an aggregation of traits
that naturally occur and are easily identified. Most important, though, are
the behavioral and health consequences associated with this pattern of be-
havior. These outcomes are not readily inferred from the individual traits
that comprise this typology.
The typological approach is not uncommon in assessment. Perhaps
the best-known measure of type is the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator
(Myers & McCaulley, 1985). Here responses to questions are used to find a
person's standing on four separate dimensions, which are then used to
classify the individual into one of 16 different "types." Each type provides
information about the person that goes beyond what each of the four
scales reflect. The MMPI is another example. The information provided
from the 10 clinical scales is frequently reported in terms of a "two-point"
or "three-point" code, which reflects the highest two or three scales. These
codes have been developed to provide an expanded clinical picture of the
respondent (Butcher, Dahlstrom, Graham, Tellegen, & Kaemmer, 1989;
Graham, 1990).
Similar research has been done with the five-factor model. Lorr and
Strack (1993) cluster analyzed the NEO PI responses of a group of college
students (cluster analysis is a multivariate procedure that identifies homo-
geneous subgroups of individuals based on similarities across a set of at-
tributes, in this case NEO PI scores. For an expanded discussion of this
technique see Aldenderfer & Blashfield, 1984; L. Kaufman & Rousseeuw,
1990). Lorr and Strack found six orthogonal sets of profiles. Each cluster
contained a different combination of the five domains. For example, Clus-
ter 1 contained emotionally stable, extraverted, and agreeable individuals.
Cluster 2 included high neuroticism, low agreeableness, and high openness
scores. The presence of such cluster types sets the stage for identifying NEO
PI-based typologies; each may have their own psychosocial sequelae.
Hofstee, de Raad, and Goldberg (1992) provided a different approach to
organizing and presenting five-factor data. They combined the five
dimensions into their 10 nonredundant pairs (e.g., Neuroticism and Extra-
version, Openness and Agreeableness, Extraversion and Conscientiousness)
and constructed them as circumplexes. Acircumplex is a circular ordering of
traits around two independent, bipolar dimensions. This type of organiza-
tion permits an evaluation of how the two dimensions blend together per-
PROFILE ANALYSIS 115
the interpretive descriptions presented here are taken from the part of the
circle that is located 45 degrees from these two axes.
The two personality domains that are involved in these matrices are
listed. An individual is considered high on a domain if his or her T-score
is above 55, and considered low if the T-score is below 45. Of course, the
higher (or lower) the scores, the more clearly an individual can be assigned
a category, and the more characteristic the description is of the client's per-
sonality.
Individuals with scores in the mid-range on one or both of the do-
mains are more problematic. If an individual has a single T-score in the av-
erage range (e:g., T-score between 45 and 55), classification is still possible
although the description may be less accurate. If the respondent's other
score is in the high range, then consider the average score to be low. Con-
versely, if the extreme score is in the low range, then the average score
should be considered high. For individuals whose scores are both in the av-
erage range, no real classification is possible for the particular model. For
such respondents, it may be more useful to place emphasis on the facets
for each of the domains.
Interpersonal Functioning
Perhaps one of the most influential modem theories of personality is
the Interpersonal Circumplex (Leary, 1957). Interest in interpersonal rela-
tions has a long history (see Wiggins, 1996, for a brief history), and current
researchers have done much to extend this conception of personality to a
wide range of psychological phenomena, including abnormal behavior,
problems of living, and human social interaction (Horowitz, 1996; Lorr,
1996; Pincus, 1994; Pincus & Wiggins, 1990; Wiggins, 1980).
The circular ordering of interpersonal behaviors is anchored by the
constructs of Dominance versus Submission and Love versus Hate. All of
our interpersonal strivings are a combination of these two motivations.
Given our social nature, these qualities cut across much of human en-
deavor. Wiggins and Trapnell (1996) showed persuasively the utility of
these two dimensions across a wide range of perspectives, including evo-
lutionary psychology, sociology, and anthropology. They argued that
Dominance and Love are expressions of the higher-order constructs of
Agency and Communion (Bakan, 1966).
Given the value of this model, it is not surprising that the two con-
structs that define this circumplex also define the first two factors of the
five-factor model (in factor analytic studies the order in which these di-
mensions emerge is Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neu-
roticism, and Openness). McCrae and Costa (1989c) jointly analyzed
PROFILE ANALYSIS 117
measures of interpersonal behavior and the NEO PI and found that the
domains of Extraversion and Agreeableness corresponded well to Status
and Love, respectively. Domain scores from the NEO PI-R for Extraversion
and Agreeableness can be used to evaluate the interpersonal style of a re-
spondent. Definitions of the quadrants defined by these two domains are
presented in Table 4-1.
The four "types" presented here speak for themselves. They describe
very clear styles of interacting. Each has its strengths and weaknesses; no
one style should be considered superior. Again, the more extreme a re-
spondent's scores are on the Extraversion and Agreeableness domains, the
better these brief snapshots define the person's style. Finally, this table, like
the others to be presented, provides only broad descriptions of the re-
spondent. If more specific information about interpersonal style is needed,
then one should rely on inspection of the facet scales for these two do-
mains. In any event, these tables are best considered as hypotheses about
a client's interpersonal style that are in need of substantiation through the
Emotional Well-Being
Psychological or emotional well-being is an important aspect of psy-
chological functioning that researchers have been interested in examining
for decades (Andrews & Withey, 1976; Cantril, 1965). Many important ques-
tions revolve around this issue: What makes people happy? How does ill-
ness and disease impact levels of well-being? Do good things improve our
emotional well-being, while traumatic events reduce it? Does well-being
decrease over the life span? All of these questions speak to central issues of
concern for all of us. Research in this area has provided several important
insights into how our sense of well-being emerges. First, subjective well-
being is very different from our objective life situations (Costa & McCrae,
1984). One would think that poorer health, less money, loss of sexual vigor
and physical attractiveness would all contribute to making our lives less
"happy," but this is not substantiated in the literature. Increasing age is not
associated with lower levels of well-being (Costa, McCrae, & Zonderman,
1987; Larson, 1978). What the research literature does show is that levels of
well-being seem to be quite stable over the adult life span (Costa, McCrae,
& Zonderman, 1987), and that life's ups and downs, although impacting us
in the short run, have little to do with our long-term levels of happiness
(Costa, McCrae, & Norris, 1981; McCrae & Costa, 1988).
Given the stability of our levels of well-being, it is not surprising that
this life view is strongly linked to personality. Research has shown that
subjective well-being is a function of levels of Neuroticism and Extraver-
sion (Costa & McCrae, 1980a; McCrae & Costa, 1983a). These two dimen-
sions play the major role in how we perceive the world we live in and cope
with the stressors we face. In short, Neuroticism reflects the amount of
negative affect we experience in life, while Extraversion is linked to levels
of positive affect. Note that positive and negative feelings are not opposite
ends of the same continuum. Rather, they define independent affective ex-
periences (see Watson & Clark, 1992). Although at any given moment one
or the other affect can dominate our feelings, over the course of life we can
certainly experience strong negative and positive emotions simultane-
ously.
The domains of Extraversion and Neuroticism also influence our ex-
perience of stress and our ability to cope with it (Costa & McCrae, 1980b,
1987, 1989b). Specifically, individuals high on Neuroticism tend to expe-
rience more somatic complaints although they do not experience higher
levels of disease (e.g., S. Cohen et ai., 1995; Costa & McCrae, 1987; Zon-
PROFILE ANALYSIS 119
-
0
CIl
0::
placid, and unexcitable. These
individuals may not respond
to situations with much
of life. Descriptors include
self-critical, insecure, nega-
tivistic, fearful, self-pitying,
w emotionality. nervous, and fretful.
>
<: These individuals have a strong These individuals experience a
0:: sense of wen-being. They are wide range of affects. There
E-
hardy and adaptive, look are high levels of BOTH posi-
>< forward to what life has to tive and negative affect. Life
w
offer. Adjective descriptors may be experienced as a series
High for this category are coura- of emotional ups and downs.
geous, strong, assured, con- Sometimes their outgoingness
fident, hearty, buoyant, may mask their own inner pain.
uninhibited, and bold. Descriptors for this category
include explosive, extravagant,
excitable, and volatile.
derman, Costa, & McCrae, 1989). Individuals who are high on Extraver-
sion appear to be quite hardy and happy and are able to cope well with
stress. The combination of these two domains and their psychological sig-
nificance are presented in Table 4-2.
Aclient's standing on these two domains can provide insights into the
general happiness of the respondent, his or her levels of subjective well-
being, and his or her ability to cope with stress. Clinical clients tend to
have very high levels of Neuroticism. This makes sense; individuals who
are experiencing a great level of psychological distress tend to seek out
counseling for relief of symptoms. Interestingly, individuals high on both
Neuroticism and Extraversion may not appear in need of treatment; the
positive emotions characteristic of the extraverted individual may mask
his or her need for treatment. Nonetheless, therapists with clients high on
Neuroticism need to keep in mind that their efforts with such clients may
impact only current stress levels. More intense, long-term therapy may be
needed to bring about a more broad-based impact on the client's overall
120 CHAPTER 4
Competitive Orientation
With the publication of their seminal work on achievement motiva-
tion, McClelland, Atkinson, Clark, and Lowell (1953) provided a new per-
spective on individuals and their aspirations. The construct of need for
achievement was found to have great heuristic and empirical value for un-
derstanding why some individuals and societies succeed and others fail
(Bendig, 1958; Edwards & Waters, 1983; H. B. Gough & Hall, 1975; Pied-
mont, 1988b; Piedmont & Weinstein, 1994; Schmeck & Grove, 1979; Steers,
1975). Need for achievement is clearly located on the Conscientiousness
domain, where personal organization and reliability reflect a capacity of
the individual to delay immediate gratification so that larger, long-range
goals can be met (Digman, 1989: Digman & Takemoto-Chock, 1981).
However, with time, research began to show that achievement moti-
vation alone was insufficient to explain or anticipate competitive perfor-
mance, especially for women (e.g., Homer, 1968, 1972; see Tresemer, 1976,
for a review). New constructs were added in order to enhance predictabil-
ity, for example, Fear of Success, Fear of Failure, and Test Anxiety. These
variables addressed the ability of the individual to manage the inevitable
stressors that any competitive situation involves and the sense of personal
confidence and esteem necessary to support any failure that is encoun-
tered. This ability to manage threats to self-confidence and negative affec-
tive arousal is the domain of personality named Neuroticism. Both of these
constructs were seen as essential for understanding competitive perfor-
mance (Piedmont, 1988a, 1995). The Neuroticism by Conscientiousness
matrix is presented in Table 4-3.
Adding the domain of Neuroticism (specifically, the facet scale N4:
Self-Consciousness) to predicting competitive outcomes provides a new
window for understanding how individuals approach and interpret
achievement situations. It helps to explain why some individuals who are
capable and have high aspirations fail, while others who are capable suc-
ceed. In the former situation there is the individual who is high on both
Neuroticism (N4) and Conscientiousness. These individuals may aspire in
an attempt to compensate for actual or perceived personal weaknesses.
Their competitive endeavors are characterized as being very intense and
focused. Although ambitious, their inability to manage their own feelings
of inadequacy may undermine their ability to reach long-range goals, or to
function well under circumstances of close observation.
In the latter situation are those who are low on Neuroticism (N4) and
high on Conscientiousness (particularly the facet C4: Achievement Striv-
PROFILE ANALYSIS 121
-
0
E-
tive, easy-going, and relaxed.
-
~
U
cJ)
ers: Emotionally stable, capable
with a heightened sense of com-
petence and a drive to succeed.
bitious and competitive, but
worry about the outcome of their
efforts. They set high standards
They set high standards for for themselves because they may
Z themselves and have the per- believe that reaching them will
0 High sonal organization to attain be an outward sign that they are
U those goals. They may appear to not as inadequate as they feel on
be overly dispassionate. Some the inside. Obstacles to obtaining
descriptive adjectives for this their goals are responded to with
category are: robotic, objective, anger and hostility. Adjective de-
poised, concise, mechanical, me- scriptors include particular, com-
thodical, and self-diSciplined. petitive, sore losers, intense, and
highly focused.
ing). These are the prototypical achievers-those who set goals and work
hard to attain high, socially meaningful standards of success. The effort
these individuals bring to achievement situations can oftentimes com-
pensate for weaknesses in ability. Their lower levels of Neuroticism (N4)
indicate that they have high levels of self-esteem and can cope well with
whatever setbacks they may encounter. Thus, failure may not be per-
ceived as much of a discouragement, but rather as a barometer of how far
they have progressed. They persist until they are able to overcome their
challenges.
Scores on these two dimensions can be useful in assessing clients with
job selection issues. Challenging environments attract those who have
high aspiration levels. To succeed in such a context necessitates a tem-
perament that can withstand the many stressors that arise, whether they
are time constraints, task demands, or managing the inevitable failures
122 CHAPTER 4
Character
Perhaps one of the more established terms in personality is the con-
cept of "character." In one of the first textbooks on personality (Stagner,
1937), character is described as a subset of the entire personality that re-
lates to moral or ethical activity. Allport (1937) captured this idea of ethical
admirability in his review of the concept: "character is the aspect of per-
sonality that engenders stability and dependability, that is responsible for
sustained effort in the face of obstacles, or works for remote ends rather
than those that are nearer in time but of less worth .... Character enters the
situation only when this personal effort is judged from the standpoint of some
code" (p. 51; italics in original).
There are two components of this definition. The first is the effort to
work toward long-range, socially valued and respected goals that have
higher "payoffs," to delay gratification, and to overcome immediate ob-
stacles. This quality certainly describes the personality domain of Consci-
entiousness. This dimension reflects an individual's personal organization
and reliability, his or her capacity to delay gratification and aspire toward
socially valued goals. Individuals high on Conscientiousness can be
counted on to follow through on their obligations and commitments.
The second element centers on the notion of social evaluation of ef-
forts. The struggle and the means by which the individual pursues the goal
are marked by ethical and moral features. Such qualities may include a re-
spect for others and a belief in higher principles of justice (either of a reli-
gious or patriotic nature). Such personal features usually constitute what
is referred to as a philosophy of life. The five-factor domain relevant to
these qualities is Agreeableness, which impacts our social attitudes and
values (Costa, McCrae, & Dye, 1991).
One may have thought that Openness would be the relevant domain;
after all, it has a facet scale for Values (06). But an important distinction
needs to be drawn here. Openness refers to our inner permeability, the de-
PROFILE ANALYSIS 123
gree to which outer events impact our inner lives and the ability of our inner
dynamics to find expression in our outer behavior. Individuals high on
Openness have value systems that are available for evaluation and modifi-
cation; the inner world is always being "updated" as new information be-
comes available. Closed individuals have a system that is more rigid and
fixed; the commitment to tradition and respect for authority restricts oppor-
-
0
f-
These are individuals who have
gent, and superficial.
-
"'-l set high standards for them- als who are very concerned and
selves and are ambitious. These involved in the plight of others.
U individuals may have intemal- They are responsive to the
CIl
ized positive values and are needs of others, and will re-
Z willing to fight for what they be- spond in helpful ways. When
0 lieve. They can be very deter- moved to action, this group
U High mined and focused on reaching may not respond in confronta-
their goals. There is much pas- tive ways. Rather, they are will-
sion in their efforts and they ing and able to "work through
may be confrontative and direct the system" in order to reach
when resistance is encountered their goals. They have the per-
in reaching their goals. Such in- sistence and diScipline to see
dividuals may love humankind, their efforts bear fruit. Adjec-
but distrust specific humans. tives that describe this category
Descriptive adjectives for this include moral, respectful, rever-
category include strict, deliber- ent, polite, considerate, sincere,
ate, stem, and rigid. and understanding.
124 CHAPTER 4
-
0
tTl
~
that focus on inner dynamics,
such as Freudian or Jungian
analysis, Gestalt therapies, or
hypnotherapy.
t.t.l
> These are individuals who enjoy These individuals also have a de-
~
~
the company of others, but may veloped inner world, and are
E- not be comfortable with express- focused on ideas, feelings, and
>< ing inner feelings, or difficult emotions. However, they also
t.t.l emotional topics, such as anger bring a strong need for social-
and sex. There may be a mora lis- ization. They like talking about
tic or self-righteous quality about these feelings with others. They
these individuals. Adjective de- are into sharing and working
High scriptors include verbose and with others. Descriptive adjec-
pompous. Such individuals tives include Inquisitive, elo-
may benefit from a more goal- quent, worldly, witty, dramatic,
directed, problem-focused ap- expressive, spontaneous, and
proach to treatment. Group adventurous. Group environ-
therapy may be useful, but more ments that are oriented toward
as a support network than any personal revelation such as en-
type of encounter group. counter groups or even psy-
chodramas may provide an
ideal therapeutic medium.
In this section, several case histories are presented along with their
NEO PI-R results. Before turning to the text interpretation provided, I en-
courage you to try to evaluate the profile. In this way, you can apply and
stretch your own interpretive skills and then compare your results with
mine. Of course, you may find insights into these cases that are not dis-
cussed in the interpretations; the evaluations provided here should not be
seen as exhaustive. Rather, every effort was made to capture only the most
salient features. In making the interpretations, we start with a considera-
tion of scores in each domain, always moving from the domain score to the
facets for Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness, Agreeableness, and Con-
scientiousness, in that order. Then we proceed to interpreting the individ-
ual's scores on each of the five matries noted earlier.
people. His low score on N6 shows that he copes very well with whatever
stressors he encounters, both from within himself and in his environment.
His low N6 score shows that he can tolerate frustration well.
Levels of Extraversion are just in the low range, suggesting that Robert
prefers small social circles and may prize his solitude. His high scores on E3
(Assertiveness) and E4 (Activity) show that he exhibits much energy, having
a quick personal tempo and being in the forefront of any group activities: He
likes to lead and has the energy to invest in leadership. He scores lower on
the affiliative aspects of this domain: E1 (Warmth) and E2 (Gregariousness).
He may appear to others to be formal and distant, and he prefers to do
things either alone or with small groups. He would not consider himself to
be a "people person," although he may do well in social circles and be seen
as quite surgent and charming. However, Robert would find such social en-
deavors to be difficult or energy expending activities. After much interper-
sonal contact he may seek quiet time in order to "recharge" himself.
Robert scores quite high on Openness overall, although there is some
important scatter on the facets for this domain. High scores on 01 (Fan-
tasy),03 (Feelings), and 05 (Ideas) show an active inner world, both emo-
tionally and intellectually. The high 05 is not uncommon among
PROFILE ANALYSIS 129
with Borderline Personality Disorder. But note the extreme level of func-
tioning on these two dimensions; facet scores on these two domains are all
in either the "very high" or "very low" range.
Overall her levels of Extraversion are quite low, suggesting that An-
gela prefers to be alone. She has few social contacts and may feel isolated
and detached from others. She has no one to tum to for help, and therefore
seeks more solitary pursuits for self-gratification (e.g., going on spending
sprees). Note that the very low levels found with the facet scales reflect a
defect in her ability to establish social relationships and bonds. Scores in
this extreme high or low range are indicative of an aberrant level of func-
tioning, especially when all the facets in the domain are extreme. Such ex-
tremity reflects more than just a strong tendency to express a particular
trait; there are real defects in functioning.
Concerning Openness, she again scores in the low range. Interestingly,
she scores high on 01 (Fantasy), suggesting that she has an active inner
life. This inner world may serve as an escape for her from the pain she ex-
periences. It may be here that she is able to strike back at those whom she
feels have injured her in the past by acting out her anger on her mind's
stage. Her low scores on 04 (Actions), 05 (Ideas), and 06 (Values) shows
PROFILE ANALYSIS 133
her to possess a high degree of rigidity. She prefers structure and familiar-
ity. She is concrete in her thinking with a strong set of values which she ad-
heres to strongly.
Agreeableness is in the low average range, but this score may be due to
the very high AS (Modesty) facet score. She is certainly not trusting of oth-
ers (A1: Trust) and does not think warmly of others (A6: Tender-Minded-
ness). She is also just in the low range on A4 (Compliance), indicating that
she likes to do things her own way. The combination of low Compliance
and low Assertiveness (E3) presents the possibility of some type of passive-
aggressive behavior. Individuals with this combination like to follow their
own lead and have things done their way. However, being low on As-
sertiveness means that the individual may not have the capacity to speak
up for herself in the presence of others. Because the unassertive person likes
to take a back seat to others, when the group moves in a direction that the
individual does not like, there is little recourse but to resist in noncon-
frontational ways.
Angela's high Modesty (AS) score suggests that she may present her-
self in a nonassuming manner. Given her high scores on Neuroticism, such
modesty may be used to protect her already impaired sense of self-esteem
and value. Her modesty may help to keep others from looking critically at
her and protect her from any additional psychological insults.
Angela's Conscientiousness was discussed previously. Such very low
scores over the entire domain suggest a high degree of impulsiveness and
self-seeking behavior. Individuals low on this domain jump at any oppor-
tunity that is convenient and holds the promise of gratification.
In examining Angela's-scores in the context of the matrices, many ad-
ditional insights emerge. Concerning the Interpersonal Matrix, she would
certain fall in the low Extraversion quadrants, but given her average level
of Agreeableness I would assign her to the "high" category. In many ways
this category captures her behavior. She is certainly passive and shy. She
may present as being timid and bashful. In a group situation she would
prefer to have a very peripheral role. Although outwardly she may acqui-
esce to requests made of her, her low Compliance indicates that if she does
not want to move in that direction, she will resist in nonconfrontational
ways. The likelihood is that Angela would oppose trying new experiences
given her low scores on the Openness facets (e.g., 04, 05, and 06). This
pattern of results shows that Angela has a highly structured world with
clear preferences. She is not likely to modify them or experiment with
other options. Unless one approaches her through her narrow confines, it
is unlikely that she would be cooperative.
On the Emotional Well-Being Matrix she can unambiguously be clas-
sified in the high Neuroticism and low Extraversion category. These are
134 CHAPTER 4
ing deceit and manipulativeness in acquiring credit cards and then consis-
tently exceeding her limits.
Finally, on the Treatment Outcome Matrix, Angela falls squarely into
the low Openness, low Extraversion category. She may respond well to a
direct, nonemotional approach. However, given her emotional dysphoria
and self-centered approach, she will certainly be a therapeutic challenge.
In measuring therapeutic success, one may need to use a very modest mea-
sure. At one point in her treatment, she came"in actively hallucinating and
delusional. She went into a psychiatric hospital, but treatment there was
not working well. She was transferred to a substance abuse program for
about 3 weeks. She nearly died during withdrawal. When she came out of
this program she was reasonably stable for about 1 to 2 weeks, but then re-
verted to her initial behavior.
Another interesting point about this profile centers on the substance
abuse. As noted in the previous chapter with Joe W., compulsive substance
abusers usually score high on NS, ES, and low on either CS or C6, or both.
This pattern was not found for Angela; she was high on N5 and low on
both C5 and C6. Her ES score was very low (although it was much higher
than her other facet scores for this domain). That she does not conform to
the typical substance abuse pattern casts some interesting light on the role
drugs have played in Angela's life.
The NS, ES, C5/C6 pattern represents an individual who is impulsive
and uses drugs as a thrill-seeking adventure. The drugs provide a
"charge" for the person, a source of stimulation that offers a distraction
from the more mundane qualities of the person's world as well as a source
of pleasure and sensual gratification. Angela's personality pattern lacked
the excitement-seeking aspect of this profile. Rather than a distraction or
source of adventure, drugs may have been used as a tool for self-medica-
tion, an attempt to soothe and anesthetize the high levels of emotional pain
she felt inside. The drugs may also have served as an immediate outlet for
her self-destructive impulses. Despite heavy drug use and addiction, An-
gela may not be like the typical drug abuser.
N E o A c
N E 0 A C
~
.2'
J:
~
~ 7D:
tacts. She projects a stoical image to others, having few positive feelings or
emotions. Interestingly, Barbara scores very high on E4 (Activity), indicat-
ing a rapid personal tempo. She likes to stay on the move doing things.
The combination of very high Neuroticism and very low Extraversion in-
dicates an individual with a rather poor sense of well-being. She feels in-
secure and vulnerable; she finds little support and comfort from her life.
No doubt there is a strong feeling of emptiness and despair.
Scores on Openness are also low overall, suggesting a degree of per-
sonal rigidity. Barbara scores low on Fantasy (01), Actions (04), and Val-
ues (06). This indicates an individual who very much desires structure
and form. She has a clear sense of right and wrong, possibly appearing to
others as conventional and conservative. There is little imagination, but
more of a down-to-earth, black-and-white orientation. It is interesting to
note her high score on Activity (E4). She likes to stay busy, but the low
score on Actions (04) indicates that she invests that energy in a very cir-
cumscribed routine. Such a restricted range of activity may help bind the
high levels of negative affect that she experiences.
Barbara scores in the high range on Aesthetics (02) and Feelings (03).
The latter score indicates that Barbara is open to a wide range of feelings,
PROFILE ANALYSIS 141
and would suggest that she experiences her negative affect with a greater
level of sensitivity than someone who is low on this facet. The Aesthetics
score suggests a further ability to be emotionally moved by artistic experi-
ences. The imagination and creativity suggested by a high score on this
facet stand in contrast to her low scores on Values and Actions. This com-
bination of scores suggests that Barbara, although appearing formal and
staid, can be moved by certain imagery, perhaps of a religious nature.
Barbara's score on Agreeableness is in the high average range, indi-
cating that she does feel some compassion for others, but these feelings of
care are certainly guarded. She scores low on Trust (AI) and Tender-Mind-
edness (A6), indicating a suspiciousness of the motives of others. She be-
lieves that the pain others may experience is likely to be a product of their
own misplaced efforts. She scores high on Straightforwardness (A2), indi-
cating that Barbara tends to be frank and sincere toward others. It is inter-
esting that her Assertiveness (E3) score is so low. This suggests that
Barbara is not proactive in sharing her thoughts. She is rather shy and ret-
icent in social situations; one may have to directly query her to find out
what her feelings are. Once asked, she would be forthcoming, but an invi-
tation needs to be offered.
The combination of high scores on Straightforwardness (A2) and Al-
truism (A3) and the low score on Assertiveness (E3) may mean that Bar-
bara is vulnerable to exploitation. She may wish to reach out to others less
out of a sense of social responsibility than out of compassion. She may feel
compelled to reach out altruistically to others, even though she knows that
they may take advantage. Perhaps her high levels of Neuroticism may pre-
vent her from appropriately establishing boundaries to her helping be-
havior. She may be seeking her own personal sense of solace from helping
others, and the strength of her needs for reassurance and comfort may
override her natural suspiciousness.
Barbara also scores high on Modesty (AS), indicating that she is mod-
est and unassuming. Again, the picture that is emerging of Barbara is of a
woman who is quiet and reticent. She prefers to stay in the background
and not attract any attention to herself. If offended, she may not respond
directly. Instead, she may wait for some invitation to speak. Then she may
voice her issues. In short, one should not mistake her outward quiet and
calm for assent. There may be many issues percolating within her that are
not readily apparent. This is evidenced in the therapeutic situation where
the most pressing issue for her was the apparent sexual discrimination she
experienced at her job. These events distressed her greatly, yet it was some
time into the treatment process before she brought it up. Some of this reti-
cence may be a function of her low Trust; she needs to feel safe and secure
before she shares her intimate feelings.
142 CHAPTER 4
lenting quality to her endeavors, given the high E4 (Activity) and high C4
(Achievement Striving) scores. As noted in the description of her case, she
is very duty-bound, leaving little or no room for her own needs and desires.
She was able to thwart her suicidal issues by thinking of her children. No
doubt, she was reminded of what a "good mother" should be like.
An interesting combination in this profile is the high score on N4 (Self-
Consciousness) and the high score on C1 (Competence). The former indi-
cates a low level of self-esteem and feelings of inferiority. Yet she scores
high on Competence, suggesting a good sense of self-efficacy. Such a com-
bination of scores usually suggests an individual who has found a partic-
ular niche in which they feel very comfortable and competent, often
receiving much recognition for his or her performance. However, these in-
dividuals may be very reluctant to move from this environment to another,
new one. Increased responsibilities, new job reporting structures, and so
on, can all induce a high degree of anxiety and feelings of inadequacy. Ex-
pertise in one situation is not perceived as generalizable to new situations.
These individuals feel very uncomfortable with the change, but over time
will recapture their feelings of competence in this new niche, until the next
time they have to move (the Impostor Phenomenon is a related construct;
see Clance, 1985; Clance & Imes, 1978).
Another point of interest in this profile is the combination of high Self-
Consciousness and high Achievement. This is a very powerful combina-
tion of constructs. High achievement is associated with individuals setting
higher personal standards for themselves. They aspire to reach goals that
are socially valued. High achievers can become quite absorbed in their ef-
forts and ambitions and find success to be quite satisfying and rewarding.
However, a corresponding high score on Self-Consciousness adds another
dimension to these aspirations. Because of the underlying feelings of in-
adequacy, the competitive strivings become an attempt by the individual
to find redemption for their perceived inadequacies. The very high stan-
dards such individuals set for themselves are in compensation for the
weaknesses they feel.
As was noted in the "Competitive Performance" section, individuals
with high N4 and high C4 perceive success as bringing relief from their
inner pangs of deficiency, but this respite is only temporary. Soon the need
for new successes rises and the quest to achieve begins again. In this
process, failure becomes an awful experience to be avoided because it pro-
vides some external confirmation of the weaknesses kept hidden within.
This is similar to Horney's (1950) notions of the "search for glory" (p. 23),
the neurotic's attempt to reach the comprehensive neurotic solution-that
fantasied image that, when attained, will solve all the problems. But this
image of success is unattainable. So the individual high on these two facets
144 CHAPTER 4
pursues his or her goals with the affective intensity of a desperate individ-
ual. Barbara may be looking for validation and approval through her ef-
forts to live up to high, socially commendable criteria of success. Being
"the best" is more than just personal development; it is an effort to find
personal justification. Her successes provide an emotional high point, her
failures an affective nadir.
From a therapeutic standpoint, treatment needs to consider that such
high levels of affective dysphoria may not be entirely ameliorated. There
may always be high levels of negative affect, whether depression, anxiety,
self-consciousness, or anger. These feelings are characteristic of Barbara,
and treatment should focus on techniques that can help her deal better
with her internal distress. The high levels of Conscientiousness are an asset
for the therapist. Barbara will work hard toward her therapeutic goals and
can be counted on to follow through with the treatments on her own time.
Her lower levels of Openness suggest that a more focused, behavioral path
may be most effective. Although she is open to experiencing her emotions,
whether she feels comfortable speaking about them may be a function of
her values and level of trust and comfort with the therapist. Marital ther-
apy may be ideal for Barbara because it gives her a much needed interper-
sonal context with an individual with whom she should have some level
of trust and comfort.
H should be pointed out that despite the presence of so much affective
and personal distress, Barbara is able to function in her environment. Al-
though one may be tempted to see much more impairment in such ex-
treme scores, it is important to remember that the NEO PI-R is a measure
of normal personality dynamics. High scores on these domains are not di-
rect indicators of psychopathological functioning. Although most of the
cases presented here are clinically oriented and therefore such high scores
are not surprising, it must be remembered that one can have high scores on
these domains and not have any type of discernible pathology.
N E 0 A C
N E 0 A
f
:I:
as'
·m
""
:I:
:- ~
.f
:
.
iil
CD
." i
if
<
i
has some values that are important to her and toward which she is firmly
committed. However, there are other aspects of her belief system that are
much more flexible and open to evaluation and change. She is low on 04
(Actions), indicating a desire for structure and clarity. She prefers to follow
an established routine and does not do well in unstructured situations. She
is also low on 01 (Fantasy), which indicates that Beverly prefers to keep
her mind on the tasks at hand.
She scores in the very high range on Agreeableness, indicating a very
prosocial, compassionate orientation toward others. She is very trusting
of others, tending to see the best in them. She is direct and honest in re-
vealing information about herself; she tends to tell others how she feels.
However, like Angela, her low Assertiveness score indicates that she may
not be very proactive in making her ideas or wishes known to others. She
would prefer to wait to be asked rather than to take the lead and initiate
a conversation. She is very Altruistic, ready to help others when called
upon. The key term here is "called upon." She may not initiate a response.
High on Modesty and Tender-Mindedness, Beverly is self-effacing and
easily moved by emotional appeals. She is friendly, soft-hearted, and gen-
tle, never overbearing or arrogant. She scores in the average range on
Compliance, suggesting that at times she likes to have things done her
own way. Given her low score on Assertiveness, she may resort to
passive-aggressive strategies.
The domain score on Conscientiousness is in the very high range,
indicating an individual who is very ambitious, organized, reliable, and
self-controlled. She is high on all facets except Achievement (C4). She
has a good sense of Competence (Cl), although given her N4 score,
these feelings may be experienced within a certain well-defined niche.
She may have difficulty in generalizing these feelings to other situa-
tions. She is Orderly (C2) and Dutiful (C3); she is efficient and orga-
nized, and can follow through on commitments that she makes. Scores
on the C5 (Self-Discipline) and C6 (Deliberation) scales, along with an
average N6 score, show that Beverly is not likely to act out impulsively
on any of the negative feelings she experiences. She has enough per-
sonal control and discipline to manage these dysphoric feelings in ap-
propriate ways.
It is interesting to note that Beverly's avoidant personality exists de-
spite her very high Agreeableness score. Although she is afraid of being
with others, she sees them as basically good. Her fears of interpersonal
contact may stem from very deep feelings of inadequacy and real fear of
social ridicule. She may feel truly conflicted, liking people and possibly
wanting to be with them, but afraid that she may not live up to their stan-
dards or may falter interpersonally. The high level of Neuroticism is con-
PROFILE ANALYSIS 147
sistent with the Avoidant Personality Disorder diagnosis and not with the
Schizoid Disorder.
Therapeutically, Beverly's high Openness and Conscientiousness
scores augur well for treatment. She is capable of thinking in new ways
about herself and will work hard at reaching the treatment goals. Given
her scores on the Agreeableness facets, it should be relatively easy to es-
tablish a therapeutic alliance with her. Her relatively low N6 score is an ad-
ditional resource that Angela W. does not have. Beverly already has
developed an important sense of being able to cope with the difficulties
that confront her. Despite her fear about being with others, she was still
able to take on, and perform well, a pOSition as, of all things, a receptionist.
This job certainly puts her in a position to meet and work with others, a
natural outlet for someone high on Agreeableness. Yet she is able to man-
age the terror this position provokes in her. This is an important founda-
tion on which therapy can build.
him to have some degree of charisma. However, despite this personal" en-
ergy," Tom may lack a good sense of personal well-being. His levels of neg-
ative affect may overwhelm any positive feelings he may have.
Openness to Experience is in the average range, suggesting some de-
gree of personal flexibility, creativity, and spontaneity, although there are
aspects that call for some need for structure and conventionality. His high-
est scores are on the facets 02 (Aesthetics) and 03 (Feelings). The Aesthet-
ics facet relates to one's interest in art and beauty. Tom certainly can be
emotionally and physically moved by theater, poetry, and music. His high
Feelings score indicates an individual who is open to a wide range of emo-
tions within himself, and is capable of responding empathically to the af-
fective needs of others.
It is interesting to note that although he is a born-again Christian fun-
damentalist, he does not have the low 06 (Values) score that one may ex-
pect from someone who adheres to such a conservative ideology. Tom's
average level score on this facet suggests that the type of clarity and struc-
ture he wants in his religious values does not generalize to other aspects of
his life (e.g., political views). In some areas he may be very tolerant of di-
vergent viewpoints, and even enjoy some philosophical debates on those
PROFILE ANALYSIS 149
nization and can be counted on, to some degree, to follow through on her
obligations. In working with the interpretive report, keep in mind that the
process of evaluation is working from the broad to the specific. If there are
any apparent inconsistencies, rely on the facet interpretations to be more
accurate than the domain evaluations.
Some of the psychosocial implications of the scores are on page 7 of
the computer report. Included in this discussion are sections on Coping
and Defense styles, Somatic Complaints, Psychological Well-Being, and In-
terpersonal Characteristics, Needs and Motives, and Cognitive Style. All of
this information was taken from the extensive construct validity research
that has been done using the NEO PI-R. Also included in this report is a
section concerning Axis II clinical hypotheses. Although the NEO PI-R is a
measure of normal personality, as shown in Chapter 2, there are strong
linkages between these dimensions and abnormal personality functioning.
What these associations mean is that high (or low) scores on any of the
NEO PI-R scales do not indicate pathology, but individuals with various
characterological disorders have distinct NEO PI-R profiles.
When dealing with a clinical client, this section may be useful for di-
agnostic formulations. When dealing with a nonclinical client, this section
may help outline salient characterological styles of a nonpathological na-
ture. Erica's profile is similar to those characteristic of the Histrionic and
Dependent Personality Disorders. This does not mean that Erica can be di-
agnosed as having either or both of these disorders. Rather, it should be
interpreted that she has an interpersonal style that seeks attention and suc-
corance from others. The final section provides some treatment implica-
tions for this profile, how the individual is likely to appear in therapy, and
issues that are likely to emerge.
The last page of the report (not reproduced here) provides a statement
of the answers recorded for each item. This is useful in case there is a dis-
agreement between the stated interpretation and any perceptions, either
by the interpreter or the client. When such disagreements arise, the first
thing to do is check the accuracy of the responses. Either the client made a
mistake in recording his or her answer, or the person entering the data into
the computer made a mistake. Another strategy is to examine the response
summary at the bottom of the page. This tells the percentage of scores for
each response category. Someone who answers "Strongly Agree" or
"Agree" to all or most of the items (an acquiescence effect) can be easily
identified. The number of items not responded to is also presented. A high
percentage here may invalidate the profile (no more than 40 items, 17%,
should be missing). In the case of Erica, the response summary suggests a
valid protocol, with no items left blank.
152 CHAPTER 4
Our final case history for this section is for Sally Ines, a 36-year-old
married female. She has two young children and is employed full-time in
the banking industry. She has an MBA degree and is currently a mid-level
manager for a large financial institution. She is not seeking treatment for
any problems. Her profile is presented in Figure 4.7.
As can be seen, Sally has a low level of Neuroticism, suggesting a
calm, emotionally secure, and hardy disposition. If there is any type of
negative affect in her life, it is likely to be either Anxiety (Nt) or Self-Con-
sciousness (N4). She is certainly not very Impulsive (N5) and copes very
well with the stressors she encounters in her world (low N6).
Overall she is average on Extraversion, suggesting a more flexible
need for interpersonal interaction. Her low Excitement-Seeking score (E5)
shows that she does not like thrills or risks. Sally scores in the high range
on both the Gregariousness (E2) and Assertiveness (E3) facets. This sug-
gests that Sally enjoys the company of others, and may prefer doing things
in groups. Her high E3 score shows that when with others she prefers to be
the one moving the group. She likes to be the leader rather than a follower.
The combination of low Neuroticism and relatively higher Extraversion
N E 0 A C
domain scores suggests that Sally feels a sense of personal well-being and
overall life satisfaction.
Interestingly, although Sally sees herself as being gregarious, she in-
dicates that she may have a more formal style in dealing with others (low
average Warmth score, El). People may not always find her to be easily ap-
proachable, and this may at times lead Sally to experience a sense of dis-
tance from others. She may enjoy being with people, but finds it difficult
that others may not immediately "warm up" to her.
On Openness Sally scores in the average range overall. This suggests
that although capable of being flexible and receptive to ideas, feelings, and
activities, Sally may evidence some degree of conventionality. An exami-
nation of her facets shows that the Aesthetics facet (02), is very low; that is
responsible for the average overall score. A score on 02 at this level sug-
gests an individual who is not concerned with art and beauty.
Sally scores high on 01 (Fantasy) and 04 (Actions). The former sug-
gests a developed inner world and an active fantasy life. This is consistent
with her strong interest in reading romance novels, which she feels pro-
vides her with a way of relieving tension and finding solace and enjoy-
ment. Her high Actions score indicates an ability to work in unstructured
situations and a desire for novelty and variety. As she likes to tell others at
her work, "Justifying doing something on the basis that it is the way we
have always done it is not acceptable."
Sally's score on 05 (Ideas) is in the low average range, suggesting a
bottom-line-oriented attitude. Although on one level this seems consistent
with someone who is employed as an accountant (and even adaptive),
usually individuals with advanced degrees score higher on this facet. Sally
may not be a "big-picture" thinker, but she is certainly concerned with the
nuts and bolts of any process, and focuses her energies on creating tangi-
ble results.
Sally's score on Agreeableness is in the low range, suggesting a more
suspicious, competitive orientation toward others. Sally can be skeptical
and critical of the intentions and motivations of others. Most of her facet
scales are also in the low to very low ranges. Al (Trust) is low, indicating
that Sally does not always see others as being honest or helpful. The low A2
(Straightforwardness) score indicates someone who is guarded in express-
ing her feelings. She may use flattery and craftiness to manipulate others. In
some ways this is an adaptive quality for a manager to have. Managers
need to be able to motivate individuals to reach established goals. Some
people may need a pat on the back, others may need to be patted a little
lower; some need cajoling, others confrontation. A good manager is able to
look past his or her own feelings about an individual and provide the kind
of feedback and incentives that will be useful to the other person.
154 CHAPTER 4
CONCLUSIONS
The eight case histories described should provide the reader with
some exposure to the kinds of issues and concerns involved in making
NEO PI-R interpretations. There are, no doubt, many nuances involved in
making any interpretation, and only practice and experience can provide
156 CHAPTER 4
that level of expertise. However, there are a few issues that one needs to
keep in mind about making interpretations.
First, never assume pathology in the profile, especially when dealing
with clinical clients. The NED PI-R is a measure of normal personality; it
highlights personological issues that are characteristic of everyday levels
of functioning. Extreme scores are not necessarily indicators of pathology.
The only way to determine whether pathology is present is to employ an
instrument designed to measure such. It is important to develop an inter-
pretive skill that enables you to appreciate what various levels of a con-
struct represent personologically. How does a low Openness individual
appear and what type of environment do they gravitate toward? Being
able to appreciate how these constructs are expressed in everyday living
will provide you with a better interpretive sense of the instrument.
Second, remember that there are adaptive and maladaptive aspects of
each pole of the five factors. Do not immediately assume that it is inher-
ently better to be Agreeable or Conscientious. Personality is an adaptive
structure that individuals develop to help them manage their environ-
ments. So the question is never whether a personality is "good" or "bad";
rather the issue is whether someone with a certain disposition would func-
tion well in a new environment. Always appreciate the context within
which the respondent functions. Then, personality assessment can be use-
ful for highlighting potential issues the person may experience and iden-
tifying resources that can be drawn on.
Finally, always strive to incorporate multiple sources of information
in your assessment battery, and look for convergence over these diverse in-
struments. Never rely on a single scale or test to provide definitive an-
swers. Rather, psychological tests are designed to generate hypotheses
(about a client) that are in need of further experimentation. Evidence can
be gleaned from other instruments, clinical interviews, and observer re-
ports. Finding convergence for a hypothesis over diverse information
sources provides a high degree of confidence in its accuracy. There is no
substitution for good information and no test is perfect-error is every-
where!
PROFILE ANALYSIS 157
- CLIENT INFORMATION-
Results For Erica J.
Age 38
Sex Female
Test Form S
Test Date 07/31/96
Prepared For Ralph L. Piedmont, Ph.D.
The following report is based on research using normal adult samples and is intended
to provide information on the basic dimensions of personality. The interpretive information
contained in this report should be viewed as only one source of hypotheses about the indi-
vidual being evaluated. No decisions should be based solely on the information contained in
this report. This material should be integrated with all other sources of information in reach-
ing professional decisions about this individual. This report is confidential and intended for
use by qualified professionals only; it should not be released to the individual being evalu-
ated. "Your NEO PI-R Summary" provides a report in lay terms that may be appropriate for
feedback to the client.
158 CHAPTER 4
Page 2
I
I I.
I
70- * -70
H I I
I I
~---+---*----------------------*--------------------------------------------+--
H :
I II
I I I
G60- * * -60
H I * * I
I I
I * * **
---+-*---------------------------------------------------------------------+--
* I
I
i
I
A I
~
RSO-
I * *
* * I
-50
iI * * * * • *. I
---+-----------------------------------------------------------------------+--
I I
L
040-
I I
-40
W * I
I
I * I
~--i---------:----------------------------------------:---------------:----i--
30- -30
~ II *1
W
<26-------------------------- _________________________ --------------------<26
:
N E 0 A C N N N N NNE E E E E E 000 0 0 0 A A A A A Ace C C C C
1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6 123 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6
PROFILE ANALYSIS 159
Page 3
Raw
Scale Score TScore Range
Factors
(N) Neuroticism 55 AVERAGE
(E) Extraversion 65 HIGH
(0) Openness 50 AVERAGE
(A) Agreeableness 47 AVERAGE
(C) Conscientiousness 32 VERY LOW
Neuroticism Facets
(N1) Anxiety 15 49 AVERAGE
(N2) Angry Hostility 16 57 HIGH
(N3) Depression 22 66 VERY HIGH
(N4) Self-Consciousness 13 46 AVERAGE
(N5) Impulsiveness 21 60 HIGH
(N6) Vulnerability 19 70 VERY HIGH
Extraversion Facets
(E1) Warmth 27 59 HIGH
(E2) Gregariousness 24 65 HIGH
(E3) Assertiveness 14 47 AVERAGE
(E4) Activity 21 57 HIGH
(E5) Excitement-Seeking 21 60 HIGH
(E6) Positive Emotions 25 59 HIGH
Openness Facets
(01) Fantasy 19 56 HIGH
(02) Aesthetics 17 47 AVERAGE
(03) Feelings 21 50 AVERAGE
(04) Actions 19 56 HIGH
(05) Ideas 21 56 HIGH
(06) Values 23 57 HIGH
Agreeableness Facets
(AI) Trust 22 51 AVERAGE
(A2) Straightforwardness 15 33 VERY LOW
(A3) Altruism 25 52 AVERAGE
(A4) Compliance 17 44 LOW
(AS) Modesty 22 56 HIGH
(A6) Tender-Mindedness 20 47 AVERAGE
Conscientiousness Facets
(Cl) Competence 17 36 LOW
(C2) Order 18 47 AVERAGE
(C3) Dutifulness 22 47 AVERAGE
(C4) Achievement Striving 12 31 VERY LOW
(C5) Self-Discipline 17 39 LOW
(C6) Deliberation 8 28 VERY LOW
160 CHAPTER 4
Page 4
TM
Client Name : Erica J.NED PI-R
Test Date : 07/31/96 INTERPRETIVE REPDRT
--Validity Indices--
Validity indices (i.e., Aand C questions, total number of items missing, and response set)
are within normal limits.
--Basis of Interpretation--
This report compares the respondent to other adult women. It is based on self-reports of
the respondent.
At the broadest level, personality can be described in terms of five basic dimensions or
factors. NED PI-R domain scores provide good estimates of these five factors by summing
the six facets in each domain. Domain scores can be calculated easily by hand and are there-
fore used on the (hand-scored) Profile Form. More precise estimates of standing on the five
factors, however, are provided by factor scores, which are a weighted combination of scores
on all 30 facets (see Table 2 in the NED PI-R Professional Manual). Factor scores are best cal-
culated by computer.
Because factor scores have somewhat higher convergent and discriminant validity, they
are used as the basis of this report. In general, domain T scores and factor T scores are very
similar; occasionally, however, they differ. In these cases, the factor T score, which incorpo-
rates information from all 30 facets, is usually a more accurate description of the individual.
Factor scores are used to describe the individual at a global level, based on a composite
of facet scale scores. To the extent that there is wide scatter among facet scores within a do-
main, interpretation of that domain and factor becomes more complex. Interpretive statements
at the factor level may occasionally conflict with interpretive statements at the facet level. In
these cases, particular attention should be focused on the facet scales and their interpretations.
PageS
TM
Client Name : Erica J.NEO PI-R
Test Date : 07/31/96 INTERPRETIVE REPORT
This person is average in Agreeableness. People who score in this range are about as
good-natured as the average person. They can be sympathetic, but can also be firm. They are
trusting but not gullible, and ready to compete as well as to cooperate with others.
Finally, the individual scores in the average range in Openness. Average scorers like her
value both the new and the familiar, and have an average degree of sensitivity to inner feel-
ings. They are willing to consider new ideas on occasion, but they do not seek out novelty for
its own sake.
Neuroticism
This individual is occasionally nervous or apprehensive, but no more so than the aver-
age individual. She often feels frustrated, irritable, and angry at others and she is prone to
feeling sad, lonely, and dejected. Embarrassment or shyness when dealing with people, es-
pecially strangers, is only occasionally a problem for her. She reports being poor at control-
ling her impulses and desires and she is unable to handle stress well.
Extraversion
This person is very warm and affectionate toward others and she usually enjoys large
and noisy crowds or parties. She is as assertive as most women when the circumstances re-
quire. The individual has a high level of energy and likes to keep active and busy. Excitement,
stimulation, and thrills have great appeal to her and she frequently experiences strong feel-
ings of happiness and joy.
Openness
In experiential style, this individual is somewhat open. She has a vivid imagination and
an active fantasy life. She is like most people in her appreciation of beauty in music, art, po-
etry, and nature, and her feelings and emotional reactions are normal in variety and intensity.
She enjoys new and different activities and has a high need for variety in her life. She is in-
terested in intellectual challenges and in unusual ideas and perspectives and she is generally
liberal in her social, political, and moral beliefs.
Agreeableness
This person has moderate trust in others, but is not gullible, recognizing that people can
sometimes be deceptive. She is willing at times to flatter or trick people into doing what she
wants, but she is reasonably considerate of others and responsive to requests for help. This
individual can be very competitive and is ready to fight for her views if necessary. She is
humble, unassuming, and uncomfortable talking about her achievements. Compared to other
162 CHAPTER 4
Page 6
TM
Client Name: Erica J.NEO PI-R
Test Date : 07/31/96 INTERPRETIVE REPORT
people, she is average in her concern for those in need, and her social and political attitudes
balance compassion with realism.
Conscientiousness
This individual is sometimes inefficient or unprepared, and has not developed her skills
and talents fully. She is moderately neat, punctual, and well-organized, and she is reasonably
dependable and reliable in meeting her obligations. She has limited aspirations and might be
considered somewhat lackadaisical or lazy. She sometimes finds it difficult to make herself do
what she should, and tends to quit when tasks become too difficult. She is occasionally hasty
or impetuous and sometimes acts without considering all the consequences.
Somatic Complaints
This person likely responds in a normal fashion to physical problems and illness. She is
prone neither to exaggerate nor to minimize physical symptoms and is fairly objective in as-
sessing the seriousness of any medical problems that she might have.
Psychological Well-Being
Although her mood and satisfaction with various aspects of her life will vary with the
circumstances, in the long run this individual is likely to experience the normal course of pos-
itive and negative feelings and generally be happy.
Cognitive Processes
This individual is likely to be about average in the complexity and differentiation of her
thoughts, values, and moral judgments as compared to others of her level of intelligence and
PROFILE ANALYSIS 163
Page 7
TM
Client Name : Erica J.NEO PI-R
Test Date : 07/31/96 INTERPRETIVE REPORT
education. She would also probably score in the average range on measures of ego develop-
ment.
Inperpersonal Characteristics
Many theories propose a circular arrangement of interpersonal traits around the axes of
Love and Status. Within such systems, this person would likely be described as dominant, as-
sured, warm, loving, and especially gregarious and sociable. Her traits are associated with
high standing on the interpersonal dimensions of Love and Status.
Axis II Disorders
Personality traits are most directly relevant to the assessment of personality disorders
coded on Axis II of the DSM-IV. A patient may have a personality disorder in addition to an
Axis I disorder, and may meet criteria for more than one personality disorder. Certain diag-
noses are more common among individuals with particular personality profiles; this section
calls attention to diagnoses that are likely (or unlikely) to apply.
Borderline Personality Disorder. The most common personality disorder in clinical
practice is Borderline, and the mean NEO PI profile of a group of patients diagnosed as
having Borderline Personality Disorder provides a basis for evaluating the patient. Profile
agreement between the patient and this mean profile neither suggests nor rules out a diag-
nosis of Borderline Personality Disorder; it is comparable to agreement seen in normal in-
dividuals.
164 CHAPTER 4
PageS
TM
Client Name : Erica J.NEO PI-R
Test Date : 07/31/96 INTERPRETIVE REPORT
Treatment Implications
Like most individuals in psychotherapy, this patient is high in Neuroticism. She is likely
to experience a variety of negative emotions and to be distressed by many problems, and
mood regulation may be an important treatment focus. Very high Neuroticism scores are as-
sociated with a poor prognosis and treatment goals should be appropriately modest.
Because she is extraverted, this patient finds it easy to talk about her problems, and en-
joys interacting with others. She is likely to respond well to forms of psychotherapy that em-
phasize verbal and social interactions, such as psychoanalysis and group therapy.
Because the patient is low in Conscientiousness, she may lack the determination to work
on the task of psychotherapy. She may be late for appointments and may have excuses for not
having completed homework assignments. Some evidence suggests that individuals low in
Conscientiousness have poorer treatment outcomes, and the therapist may need to make
extra efforts to motivate the patient and structure the process of psychotherapy.
--Stability of Profile--
Research suggests that the individual's personality profile is likely to be stable through-
out adulthood. Barring catastrophic stress, major illness, or therapeutic intervention, this de-
scription will probably serve as a fair guide even in old age.
END OF REPORT
CHAPTERS
165
166 CHAPTERS
The NEO PI-R computer scoring program provides a very useful plat-
form for evaluating observer ratings and offers a number of advantages.
First, not only does the program carry the normative data for a rating, it
also allows one to directly compare a self-report with a corresponding rat-
ing within the same report. The program lists, side by side, results from the
two assessments. The program also highlights the location of significant
differences between any given pair of ratings. Second, it is possible to si-
multaneously plot the results of such tests on a graph. One can, therefore,
see areas of convergence and divergence between the two profiles. Finally,
the computer-generated report provides an empirical index of profile sim-
ilarity, allowing one to determine how congruent the two ratings are.
These values have also been normed, permitting one to assess the likeli-
hood of finding a particular level of agreement.
As noted earlier, ratings provide an independent source of informa-
tion about an individual that is not contaminated by response distortions
characteristic of a self-report. Therefore, when scores from a self-report
RATER VERSION APPLICATIONS 167
converge with observer ratings, one can have confidence that the results
are not the product of any type of response distortion. Such agreement can
have important therapeutic consequences. For example, a college student
presents herself to the counseling center because she is experiencing er-
ratic school performance. The student rates herself high on Conscientious-
ness. Personality ratings provided by the parents also rate her high on this
domain. This agreement would alert the counselor that the problematic
academic performance represents a recent change rather than a long-
standing habit and would influence the direction of further assessment.
Areas of disagreement can also be interpretively valuable, depending
on the context. For example, a client in treatment may see herself as being
very low on Neuroticism, while her family members may see her as being
quite high. Such a disagreement can alert the therapist to a number of pos-
sibilities. The client may be unaware of the level of her emotional distress
or there could be significant family dysfunction. The client could be delib-
erately attempting to hide her issues from the therapist, or she may be in
denial. In either case, the therapist has important information that can be
employed in the therapeutic context.
Observer ratings can also be used to help validate a self-report that
may be questionable for any number of reasons. In Chapter 3 I brought up
the issue of validity scales in assessment. Validity scales are indices devel-
oped by test makers to determine whether some response pattern evi-
dences any type of distortion or bias. Such biases include, but are not
limited to, denial of problems, random responding, trying to create a pos-
itive image of self, trying to create a negative image of self, and acquies-
cence. Such biases tell us nothing about the individual and therefore a
profile contaminated by such distortions cannot be trusted. Numerous
scales have been developed to detect the presence of such errors.
All too often, however, high scores on these validity scales are used to
reject a test protocol. On the one hand, a test interpreter does not want to
make an erroneous conclusion about a respondent, especially when the
stakes are quite high (as they may be in a forensic or job selection environ-
ment). Yet, to reject the protocol out of hand because of a high score on one
or more validity indices may waste resources. Time, energy, and money
were invested in having the assessment completed and scored; there is a
real financial cost to discarding a protocol. Further, asking the individual
about who they are is the most direct and intuitively appealing approach
to doing assessment. To reject what a person has told us undermines many
aspects of the assessment process. For example, do we rely on a structured
interview? If the person distorted information on a assessment question-
naire, what confidence do we have that they will not distort responses in
the interview? After all, interviews are less reliable than self-report
168 CHAPTERS
~ 80-
..
.
I
,
~
%
- "
,, ,,
.,
I. 70-
! 1\ ~ ~ H' ~ ) , ,
:
II •
I
.- /\
• V.
~, 1'1• j'H
W.~ /•1 '\.
I~
,.or
' 60- • I ., ,, "
~. I
•
• .,. • ..~J:',··1 I
, ' "
... • I
" :
,I.
0 ,, ,, Y-" • ',I ,."" "
r
~~ 50- , ,' ,
- ,
I ..• :.:
.,
•
3~ ""
• . .....
I,
, "
I I I
...
,
~ 40-
- •
3
,
~ 30-
~ 20-
-
II
UlOID 04IUl'fn'4 04 IUl 'fn '4 04IU'fn'4 04I'U'fn'4 04 IUl 'fn '4
Factors Z Z Z Z Z Z III III III III III III 0 0 0 0 0 0 C C C C a: a: Cl Cl DDDCl
FIGURE 5-1. NEO PI-R self-reported and composite observer rating for Barbara Nettle.
A visual inspection shows that the two profiles agree quite well. Rat-
ings on the Extraversion, Openness, and Agreeableness domains are all
similar. On the Neuroticism domain both she and the composite rating see
her as high, although there is a difference in magnitude. A noticeable dif-
ference is observed on the Conscientiousness domain, where she rates her-
self in the Very High range while the rating is in the high Average range.
Consistent with this visual similarity, the profile agreement index (which
is generated by the scoring software) between the two sets of ratings is .81,
suggesting an overall agreement that is very high in comparison with the
170 CHAPTERS
world; they are unhappy at work, unhappy in play, and unhappy in love.
Further, such individuals also behave in ways that may provoke negative
reactions from spouses. But relationships are not static arenas in which a
single individual attempts to find personal gratification. Rather, they are
dynamic. There are two individuals working (or competing) to find satis-
faction. Rather than merely imposing our will on the environment, the
presence of an "other" represents a salient stimulus in our environment
that must be attended to, responded to, and accommodated.
One of the limitations of the research mentioned previously is its
reliance on self-report data. Self-reported behaviors are linked to self-
reported personality characteristics or self-reported problems of one indi-
vidual are correlated with the self-reported personality of the spouse. This
type of research, although important for outlining how individuals act in
a relationship, overlooks more of the dynamic aspects of the dyad. Specif-
ically, I am referring to the kinds of internal representations individuals
carry of their spouses, and how these images create expectations about the
partner and the relationship itself. In order to capture this dimension, one
needs to assess not only individuals, but their perceptions of their spouses,
something that can be done only through an observer rating.
Observer ratings provide an opportunity to access an entirely new
level of the relationship through comparisons of a person's ratings of his or
her spouse with the spouse's self-report. Systematically comparing the de-
gree of congruence between these two sets of images may provide a win-
dow into the degree of marital satisfaction experienced by the couple and
the motivational sources contributing to their level of harmony or conflict.
the couple. These patterns may suggest intervention strategies that would
benefit the couple. Taylor and Morrison (1984) provide a good description
of the value of cross-observer analysis:
The test profiles can be effectively used to shift the focus from the imme-
diate complaints to an examination of the influence of the two personal-
ities; as well as to develop an understanding of the interpersonal
dynamics involved. The test results can help the couple objectify their
problems and focus more on the role played by their individual person-
alities and behavior in the overall situation. (p. 17)
The next section provides an overview of what is involved in per-
forming cross-observer analyses of this type, as well as several case histo-
ries that outline the interpretive value of this approach.
Overview
To conduct a cross-observer agreement (COA) analysis, one needs to
have a couple complete the NEO PI-R for both themselves and their part-
ner. This generates four separate profiles for each couple. The NEO PI-R
works well in this scenario because it provides self-report and observer
booklets as well as separate norms. Scores can also be readily translated
into T-scores and the resulting profiles can be jointly plotted on the same
profile sheet. In addition, the computer scoring program has the capacity
to combine a self-report ~nd an observer rating into a single report, and
will statistically compare the two profiles as well as determine whether
any facets are significantly different. The computer scoring program can
also generate a simultaneous plotting of both profiles (as was demon-
strated in Figure 5-1).
Given the four profiles, which ones should be compared? Any combi-
nation would be of interest and value. One can directly compare the two
self-reports. This would provide information about how well the two in-
dividuals complement each other temperamentally. For example, if one in-
dividual is high on Assertiveness (E3) and low on Compliance (A4) and
the other is just the opposite, then this couple may find themselves a good
"fit." However, such comparisons could also indicate potential areas of
conflict. For example, if both individuals score high on Neuroticism, then
one could expect a great deal of emotional controversy and contentious-
ness in the relationship. Finally, a comparison of the two self-report scores
could be the foundation of a discussion of the kinds of life directions each
person is likely to follow and how the life trajectories of the spouse may fit
with the expectations or needs of the self-reporter.
RATER VERSION APPLICATIONS 177
and for gender.) The first order of business was to show that scores on the
CCICL have no correlation with self-rated personality. In other words,
how an individual rates his or her spouse should have nothing to do with
the individual's personality. There were no significant correlations be-
tween the CCICL and self-rated NEO PI-R scores.
The next step was to show that ratings on the CCICL are correlated
with both NEO PI-R observer ratings of the spouse and with the spouse's
self-reported NEO PI-R scores. Table 5-2 presents these results. These re-
sults provide some preliminary validity evidence to the CCICL. First, they
show that an individual's ratings on the CCICL (which indicate the types
of problems the person is having with the spouse) correlate with the per-
son's ratings of the spouse on the NEO PI-R. The pattern of correlations is
consistent with our hypotheses, in that the categories of the CCICL con-
verge with their appropriate dimensional relative on the NEO PI-R. For
example, individuals who are rated as evidencing many emotional prob-
lems on the CCICL are rated as being high on NEO PI-R Neuroticism. A
second interesting piece of validity evidence is that this pattern of conver-
gence between the CCICL and the NEO PI-R extends to the self-report
scores as well, indicating that the behavioral issues people are finding
problematic in their spouses correspond with temperamental dispositions
acknowledged by the spouses themselves. Coupled with the findings that
self-rated personality does not correlate with CCICL ratings, these data
show that the problems acknowledged by individuals are not figments of
their imaginations, or their own self-projections. Rather, they reflect real
problems that are linked to the motivational styles of their partners. Thus,
the CCICL can be considered a valid index for capturing interpersonal is-
sues that are linked to personality dispositions.
With this support in hand, we moved on to the next, and most cen-
tral, question: "Are discrepancies between an observer rating of person-
ality and the corresponding self-report indicative of the kinds of marital
difficulties that the rater is having with his or her spouse?" Although ear-
lier I recommended that subtracting the rating from the matching self-re-
port score was sufficient for determining the actual difference between
scales (15 T-score points was to be considered statistically significant),
such a procedure does not work very well in a research context. The prob-
lem is that difference scores are not as reliable as their constituent ele-
ments <1. Cohen & Cohen, 1983). For example, say that we wish to
perform a COA analysis on the Openness dimension. The reliabilities for
this domain in self-report and observer versions are .87 and .89, respec-
tively. These are quite high reliabilities for personality scales. Costa and
McCrae (1992c) report spouse-self agreement on this domain as .65. Sub-
tracting the self-report score from the rating would result in a difference
RATER VERSION APPLICATIONS 181
TABLE 5-2 Correlations between CCICL Ratings and NED PI-R Self- and
Observer Ratings.
CCICL Category
Personal
Inter- Coopera- Respon-
NEO PI-R Domain Emotional personal Flexibility tiveness sibility
Husband's CCICL Ratings
Husband's rating of wife
Neuroticism .39' .05 .04 -.12 -.04
Extraversion -.15 .2Sh .18 .01 -.10
Openness -.08 .06 .41' .04 .11
Agreeableness -.3Qh -.13 .16 .4'7'" -.10
Conscientiousness -.22" -.11 -.20" -.04 .3'7'"
Wife's self-report
Neuroticisn .24h -.07 -.11 -.12 .12
Extraversion -.15 .24h .22" .04 -.14
Openness -.13 .14 .39' -.04 .04
Agreeableness -.10 .06 .22' .26b -.04
Conscientiousness -.03 .08 -.13 -.15 .17
score that had a reliability of only .66. Quite a drop! Although for clinical
purposes such a simple arithmetic operation will suffice, for empirical
analysis a more efficient procedure is needed that still reflects the dy-
namic of interest.
182 CHAPTERS
TABLE 5-4. Correlations between CCICL Ratings and NEO PI-R Residualized
COAIndex.
CCICL Category
Inter- Coopera- Personal
NEO PI-R domain Emotional personal Flexibility tiveness Responsibility
Husband's CCICL rating
COA index of wife
Neuroticism .31' .13 .14 -.06 -.14
Extraversion -.07 .13 .06 -.03 -.13
Openness -.02 -.02 .24h .08 .11
Agreeableness -.29 h -.18 .07 .40' -.08
Conscientiousness -.23" -.17 -.15 .03 .33'
peets of marital distress and conflict. eOA indices can be useful for deter-
mining the motivational bases of the conflict. These results show that cer-
tain kinds of distortions are particularly associated with lowered levels of
marital satisfaction (e.g., when the person rates the spouse as being higher
in Neuroticism). But all eOAscores, whether they are higher or lower than
the self-report rating, are associated with particular kinds of issues the per-
son is having with the spouse. These issues become more disruptive as the
overall level of marital satisfaction decreases.
Thus, eOAanalysis is a dynamic approach to understanding couples
because it relies less on actual behaviors of the spouse and more on the
subjective impact these behaviors have on the individual. It is these inter-
pretations of the spouse's behavior that playa major role in how people re-
spond in their relationships (Gottman, 1979; D. K. Snyder, 1982). As D. K.
Snyder (1982) pointed out, "a calm discussion to one may represent veiled
hostility to the other" (p. 193). By capturing these subjective interpreta-
tions, eOAcan allow the therapist to plan interventions that are meaning-
ful to the couple. These interventions can take several forms. For example,
in the preceding quotation it is clear that an individual perceives the low
Neuroticism of his or her spouse as low Agreeableness. A therapist may
wish to engage in some cognitive restructuring to help the person gain a
different interpretation of the spouse's behavior, one more consistent with
its underlying motivations. Or, in the case of an individual who perceives
her spouse higher on a dimension that he himself already scores high on, a
therapist may want to help this husband find better ways of expressing his
motivations in this area, ones that are less upsetting to his mate. In either
situation, the information from the eOA analysis is useful for giving cou-
ples a language for talking about issues that they are eager to discuss. This
language can help them to understand better their spouses' needs and can
promote rapport. A successful outcome in marital therapy would be re-
flected by greater cross-observer agreement at the end of treatment than at
the beginning.
The next section presents some case examples of using eOA analyses
to help better understand the dynamics of the relationship. Although there
are three different comparisons that can be made from performing a eOA
analysis (e.g., self versus self, rating versus rating, and self versus rating),
only the self-reports versus the observer rating information are presented.
This provides an opportunity to become familiar with doing eOA analy-
ses. This is also an opportunity to apply the interpretive skills learned in
the previous chapters for evaluating the self-report results. The reader is
also encouraged to explore the interpretive and clinical value of the other
two types of analyses.
186 CHAPTERS
In performing COA analyses, there are two important steps. The first
is to understand the person being rated. Who do they present themselves
as being? What are their distinguishing personality characteristics and
how would these qualities manifest themselves in an intimate relation-
ship? The second step is to examine the points of disagreement between
the self-report and the spouse's rating. On those dimensions where there is
disagreement, it is important to keep in mind that the person providing
the rating has an exaggerated view of those personality qualities. He or she
sees the spouse as particularly evidencing the negative aspects of these
qualities. The caveat here is that the rater is experiencing some level of dis-
satisfaction with the spouse and the greater the dissatisfaction, the more
dysfunctionality can be inferred from the lack of convergence in the rating.
However, as an aside, given the pattern of correlations presented in Table
5-3, it would be interesting to explore what divergence would represent in
a rater who is experiencing satisfaction in the relationship. Perhaps diver-
gence there may reflect sources of satisfaction being experienced.
It should also be kept in mind that distortions in perception are not
always bidirectional. It is possible that only one partner in a relationship is
experiencing distress, and that person's COAshould be low. Yet, the part-
ner may have a higher level of marital satisfaction and evidence better
cross-observer congruence. Thus, COAcan be useful in identifying which
individual is having a problem with what type of issues. The first case his-
tory provides such an example.
difficulty with the relationship than Henry, the results of her COA are pre-
sented in Figure 5-2.
This figure presents Henry's self-report along with Marge's rating of
him. As can be seen, there are several areas of disagreement. The overall
profile agreement coefficient for these two sets of ratings is .34, indicating
a low amount of convergence. Marge rates Henry significantly lower on
Neuroticism than he rates himself, suggesting that she sees him as being
very emotionally stable which is consistent with her belief that he exhibits
too much emotional control. The NEO PI-R computer report indicates sig-
nificant differences on the Neuroticism facets of Nl (Anxiety) and N6 (Vul-
nerability). Again, Marge believes that Henry's ability to deal with distress
in his life is functioning too well; it prevents him from experiencing feel-
ings that he should be sharing with her. This interpretation is consistent
with her ratings on the CCICL's Emotional section: She indicates that
Henry is emotionally bland, too calm, and exhibits too much emotional control.
On Extraversion, Marge sees Henry as being significantly more Gregar-
ious than he does himself. The low Extraversion self-rating is consistent with
someone who is not emotionally demonstrative and may appear to experi-
ence little joy. The El rating reveals what may be fueling the conflict; Marge's
FIGURE 5-2. NEO PI-R self-rating and observer rating for Henry Dunbar.
188 CHAPTERS
belief that Henry spends time with friends he is close to and may be sharing
his feelings with them rather than her; this belief may fuel the conflict. One
may wish to explore whether Marge harbors some jealousy toward Henry's
friends, who are few in number but may be very close. Although there is no
significant difference between these two ratings, the Et (Warmth) difference
is interesting in this context. Henry sees himself as a mostly formal and staid
individual, although Marge believes him to be quite approachable. Given this
perception of him as warm and inviting to others, she may be puzzled as to
why she is being excluded from his inner emotional world.
Marge's feelings of emotional abandonment are also supported by the
divergence on the Openness to Feelings facet (03; his self-rated T-score is
56 while her rated T-score is 43). A low score may indicate Marge's belief
that Henry has a limited affective range as well as a belief that he does not
consider emotions important. Again, this corresponds with her CCICL
Flexibility rating of Henry as lacking emotional depth. There is also a signif-
icant difference on the 02 facet (Aesthetics). This divergence, which may
at first appear odd, makes good sense here. The 02 scale evaluates the de-
gree to which someone is not only interested in art and beauty but is emo-
tionally moved by it as well. Henry's perceived artistic insensitivity may
reflect another aspect of his personality that Marge sees as being unre-
sponsive to external events.
The differences on Agreeableness are noteworthy, because Marge sees
Henry as being much more compassionate, caring, and considerate than
he sees himself. Significant differences emerge on the At (Trust), A2
(Straightforwardness), and A6 (Tender-Mindedness) scales. These ratings
may be double-edged in reference to Marge's issues. On the one hand, the
higher ratings may suggest why Marge wanted them to go to counseling.
After all, she sees him as a very decent person and, perhaps, in many ways
a very good spouse. Thus, she is intereste~ in maintaining the relationship
and giving it a chance to succeed despite the problems. As we noted in
Table 5-3, distortions in this direction are associated with higher levels of
marital satisfaction for the rater. These may be the qualities of Henry that
contribute to Marge's experienced happiness with the relationship. How-
ever, these ratings may be another part of Marge's frustrations as well. She
may question why a person who is trusting, straightforward, and caring
would refuse to discuss his feelings with her, especially when he knows
how important such revelations are to her. Why, then, does he refuse her?
These kinds of perceived inconsistencies may be underlying Marge's con-
flicts with Henry and motivating her to seek therapy: to find the answers
that seem to be eluding her.
Finally, on Conscientiousness, Marge rates Henry as being lower
overall on this domain than he himself does, indicating that she may per-
RATER VERSION APPLICATIONS 189
feelings with his wife? To answer these questions, we need to tum to an ex-
amination of Marge's COA profile, which is presented in Figure 5-3.
Figure 5-3 presents Marge's self-report along with Henry's rating of
her on the NEO PI-R. These two profiles converge quite well, having a
profile agreement coefficient of .75, indicating a high level of agreement.
This is also consistent with Henry's marital dissatisfaction rating of "2" on
the CCICL. Both raters acknowledge that Marge experiences a high level
of negative affect, although she thinks she copes with these feelings better
than Henry thinks she does. Both agree on her overall Openness and the
pattern of the facets is similar, although Marge sees herself as being more
dogmatic and concrete.
Despite this overall level of agreement, there are several areas of dif-
ference that need to be considered. Overall, Henry sees Marge as much
more extraverted than she sees herself, especially when it comes to her
own sense of joyousness (E6). Significant differences are also noted on E2
(Gregariousness) and E5 (Excitement-Seeking). Henry does not see Marge
as interpersonally isolated as she sees herself to be. This may explain
Marge's need for more emotional intimacy from Henry; he does not really
appreciate how few social contacts she has. He may be her sole source of
emotional contact, although he may see more activity than there is, or
Marge may not realize the extent of her own social world.
Marge
Self Report
Henry
ranking Marge ----------------
FIGURE 5-3. NEO PI-R self-rating and observer rating for Marge Dunbar.
RATER VERSION APPLICATIONS 191
to distance himself in order to maintain his own sense of balance and per-
spective. Although Henry is clearly aware of the great amount of ongoing
emotional distress Marge experiences, some exploration of his responses
to these feelings would be welcome. Perhaps helping Henry affirm and
support Marge's personhood more clearly may assuage and comfort her
feelings of inadequacy.
Another area of exploration is the value systems of these two individ-
uals. Both present low self-report scores on 06 (Values) and acknowledge
such in each other. Low values on this facet indicate that the individual has
a very strong network of values, a clear sense of right and wrong. This
value system is also not open to negotiation or modification; it is a rigid set
of beliefs. To what degree do these two sets of values correspond with one
another? What are the expectations that such values create? Is Marge try-
ing to live up to an unrealistic image of a good wife? Has she set standards
too high for their own relationship? In completing the CCICL, Henry
checked only two items, can't say "no" to others and sets unrealistically high
standards for self. These two items may reflect Marge's strong need to be ac-
cepted by others; she looks for validation through serving others and liv-
ing up to high standards of success. Then again, what values does Henry
hold about marriage? Does he believe that a man should not appear emo-
tionally vulnerable? Does he consider discussions about his feelings inap-
propriate to share with his wife? These issues would need to be explored
with this couple.
Overall, this case history provides us with some important concepts.
First, dissatisfaction in a relationship can corne from many sources. In eval-
uating the COA profiles for Marge, we learn that there are three areas po-
tentially contributing to her dissatisfaction. First, her own high levels of
Neuroticism speak to a general, distressed lifestyle. Insecure and anxious
on the inside, she may find all of her relationships tenuous and difficult to
manage. As the data presented earlier showed, individuals high on Neu-
roticism experience less satisfaction with life in general and their marriages
in particular. Second, her perceptions of Henry are indeed skewed in cer-
tain areas, leading to interpretations of his behavior that reinforce her feel-
ings of isolation and emotional abandonment. Finally, Henry's self-report
indicates the presence of various characteristics <e.g., his own lack of emo-
tionality, his interpersonal aloofness) that also contribute to Marge's lack of
satisfaction. Obviously, all three of these areas need clinical attention.
A second insight from this case is that marital dissatisfaction does not
have to be a mutual experience. Clearly Henry does not have any major is-
sues with Marge, and finds the relationship to be meeting his needs. Given
Henry's profile and his tendency to dampen emotional issues, one must
raise the question as to whether Henry is merely denying some important
RATER VERSION APPLICATIONS 193
issues in the relationship. It may also speak to his own need to avoid con-
flict or there may be some degree of secondary gain for him by keeping
Marge feeling insecure in the relationship. These issues need to be ex-
plored as well. One benefit of Henry's high level of marital satisfaction
may be his willingness to accept changes in the relationship in order to im-
prove its quality for Marge.
Even though only one person in this relationship was experiencing
marital distress, an exploration of both COA profiles can provide useful in-
formation about the relationship. I hope this case study begins to illustrate
the usefulness of COA analyses for understanding the dynamics of any
given relationship. This approach should be useful for working with pre-
marital couples as well; they can explore the motivational expectations of
their partners and begin to see the life directions each of their personalities
would wish to move toward. The COA process can help support the rela-
tionship by fostering greater self-insight, greater emotional insight into the
partner, and enhanced communication skills in dealing with intimate topics.
The following cases are presented to help extend your ability to eval-
uate COA profiles and infer useful clinical insights.
FIGURE 5-4. NEO PI-R self-rating and observer rating for Sarah H.
fears: that she is useless and of no value. Thus, there is a strong emotional
edge to Sarah's ambitions. Sarah is seen as competitively climbing the pro-
fessional and social ladder. in order to find self-acceptance.
This interpretation is further confirmed when the facet scales are exam-
ined. Bob rated Sarah significantly higher on Depression, Self-Conscious-
ness, and Achievement. These ratings suggest that Bob sees Sarah as a very
focused and intense person who may have a very low tolerance for failure.
Another area of disagreement is on E5 (Excitement-Seeking). Sarah is
seen as a risk taker and thrill seeker. Coupled with the preceding percep-
tions, this rating may indicate that Sarah enjoys being "where the action
is." Her drive to succeed is attracted by opportunities for high profile in-
volvement in challenging tasks. Again, this perception contrasts starkly
with Bob's professionally lackluster situation.
A final difference of note is Bob's rating of Sarah on Al (Trust). Such a
score indicates a perception of Sarah as being cynical and skeptical, always
assuming that others are dishonest or dangerous. This perceived inability
to recognize the good in others may underscore Bob's belief that Sarah has
no belief in him or his ability to find gainful employment.
Overall, Bob perceives his wife as a very ambitious woman who
values success in herself and others. Distrustful of others, self-assertive,
RATER VERSION APPLICATIONS 195
energetic, and hardheaded (low A6), Sarah is not seen as taking Bob's un-
employment well. Such "failure" may be completely unacceptable to
Sarah, who may see this situation as reflecting negatively on her as well.
Given Sarah's perceived need to present a highly successful face to the
world, Bob's unemployment may be an embarrassment to her that she is
trying to distance herself from. This picture is complemented by Bob's
CCICL ratings of Sarah as feeling inadequate, can't receive criticism, hostile at-
titude toward others, overcontrolling, always wants to be boss, cynical, always
does things her way, and, too neat.
However, Sarah's self-report does not reflect many of these percep-
tions. She does not indicate the levels of Self-Consciousness and Achieve-
ment Striving seen in the rating. She does not see herself as "driven" to
succeed in order to compensate for feelings of inadequacy. Rather, if she
experiences any negative affect, it is anger and frustration. Although she
feels herself quite competent (CI) and self-disciplined (C5), she does not
see herself as having a very high aspirationallevel (C4). Although Sarah
sees herself as being low on Agreeableness, her level of Trust (AI) is not as
low as Bob indicates; she may be skeptical of people's motives at times but
maintains a general positive attitude toward others.
These varying perceptions of Sarah are significant given Bob's current
employment status. He may be very sensitive to his own failures and in-
ability to find employment and may be projecting his own ambitiousness
and self-accusatory ideations onto Sarah. She may be quite frustrated at
the current situation, especially with the financial hardships Bob's unem-
ployment has caused. As a result, she may respond with anger and hostil-
ity, but this negative affect may not stem from her own sense of personal
inadequacy. Bob may interpret this anger not as frustration, but as a puni-
tive attack on him personally. He may even believe that she is gloating
over his loss of employment.
In order to better understand the areas of conflict in this relationship
we need to consider the other half of this profile: Bob's self-report com-
pared with Sarah's rating, which is presented in Figure 5-5. On the CCICL
Sarah also rated a moderate amount of conflict and dissatisfaction in the
relationship. The profile agreement coefficient for these two profiles is -.52,
indicating a very low level of agreement. In fact, this value shows that the
rating is quite the opposite of the self-report. Given the level of conflict in
this relationship, this amount of discrepancy is not surprising.
Bob saw Sarah as being very ambitious and not tolerating his appar-
ent failures very well. Looking at Bob's self-report we can see that he
scores very high on Self-Consciousness (N4) and Achievement Striving
(C4). Therefore, it is he who is ambitious and tries to succeed in order to
stave off his own personal feelings of inadequacy. Failure represents an
outcome that awakens very painful feelings that he may find difficult to
196 CHAPTERS
FIGURE 5-5, NED PI-R self-report and observer rating for Bob H.
manage or even accept. Thus, our earlier hypothesis that Bob is projecting
his own competitiveness and punitive feelings onto Sarah seems sup-
ported here. His extemalization of his pain is placing a high degree of
stress on the relationship. His repeated failures to find employment may
create a number of frustrations (both interpersonal and financial) on Sarah,
who may respond with anger which is interpreted by Bob as an attack on
his adequacy as a person.
In Sarah's perceptions of Bob, there are a number of inconsistencies.
Sarah sees Bob significantly lower on overall Neuroticism, indicating a be-
lief that in general Bob is a rather emotionally stable individual who may
not show his emotions very clearly. This is consistent with her CCICL rat-
ings of unconcerned about impressions made on others and too much emotional
control. On the facets, her rating of him on N4 is significantly lower than
the self-report, suggesting that she may not see how inadequate he feels
about himself. She may not recognize the effect on his self-esteem his un-
employment has had. More than a professional setback, this situation af-
fects his sense of personal adequacy.
One potential area of conflict for Sarah may center on how well Bob is
able to manage the many negative feelings this situation is creating. Bob
RATER VERSION APPLICATIONS 197
feels that he is coping well (N6) while Sarah certainly does not agree. There
may be some issues surrounding the best way for the couple to manage
Bob's unemployment. Looking to Openness, Sarah believes Bob to be
much more closed and rigid than he sees himself, especially on facets 02
(Aesthetics), 03 (Feelings), and 06 (Values). Bob has a set way of wanting
to do things (low 04) and enjoys structure and direction. However, there
may be a strong reluctance on Bob's part to try things (e.g., employment
opportunities) that may not conform to his expectations or that may not
provide as well-defined a job description as he would like.
This may lead Bob to forgo certain opportunities that present them-
selves. Given the length of his unemployment, Sarah may be urging him to
try new types of jobs that may be available, but Bob resists. He may be
holding out for a certain type of position that Sarah may see as unrealistic
or not possible at this time. As such, Sarah may be finding Bob's intransi-
gence and unwillingness to explore new types of positions frustrating.
Given his low Agreeableness scores (which she agrees with), he can be
quite bullheaded and stubborn. This is consistent with her CCICL ratings
of sees the world in black or white terms, too set in his/her ways, unimaginative,
and stubborn.
Another interesting area of divergence concerns Conscientiousness.
Overall, Sarah rates Bob significantly lower on this domain as well as
lower on the facets of C3 (Dutifulness), C5 (Self-Discipline), and C6 (De-
liberation). Bob sees himself as being organized, focused, and motivated.
Sarah believes just the opposite: He is unorganized and unambitious.
CCICL ratings on the Personal Responsibility domain were unorganized
and sloppy/messy. For Sarah, Bob's behavior is more self-centered and
passive. He does not seem to be energetically pursuing new career oppor-
tunities. Sarah's combined ratings of low Neuroticism and low Conscien-
tiousness suggest that she sees Bob as being informal, noncompetitive, and
easygoing. He may not share the same notions of success and material ad-
vancement that she has.
Sarah's image of Bob is one of a man who is not very ambitious or suc-
cess oriented. Rather, he has some very explicit ideas about how he needs
to pursue his goals which he is very committed to, but which may not be
the most efficient or productive from Sarah's perspective. She does not
think that his way of doing things is working well at all, yet he remains
doggedly attached to that process and will not consider any alternatives.
Rather than focus on the task at hand, Bob is perceived by Sarah as lashing
out at her and perhaps making her responsible for his inability to find
work. Although Bob sees himself as being assertive and proactive in this
situation, Sarah instead sees him as becoming involved with his own feel-
ings and circumstances and not really dealing with the issues at hand.
198 CHAPTERS
for 51~ months at the time they completed the COA profiles. Because Karen
was the presenting client, her COA profile of Alan is presented first in Fig-
ure 5-6.
On the CCICL Karen indicates a moderate amount of dissatisfaction
and conflict in the relationship. The overall profile agreement between
Alan's self-report and Karen's ratings is .86, indicating a high level of
agreement. Nonetheless, there were 12 scales on which there is significant
difference.
On Neuroticism, Karen and Alan agree on Alan's overall low level.
On the CCICL Emotional domain Karen indicated issues about Alan hav-
ing too much emotional control, never shows concern for the future, and seems to
want to control my emotions/desires. On the CCICL she notes that Alan
"doesn't plan for much-will call at 4:00 P.M. to ask me to get a sitter for 7
P.M. so we can do whatever just came to him. But he won't PLAN a date."
Thus, Karen believes Alan has a wide range of interests that he wishes to
be involved in, and he wants to do it now. Notice Karen's high rating of
Alan on N5 (Impulsiveness) along with the low ratings on C5 (Self-Disci-
pline) and C6 (Deliberation), which are significantly lower than the self-re-
port scores. This pattern suggests a very impulsive approach to life, one
AlanG.
Self Report
Karen
ranking Alan c
N E 0 A 1 2 3 4
:
5
.
6
FIGURE 5-6. NEO PI-R self-rating and observer rating for Alan G.
200 CHAPTERS
the time to devote to his awn passions but does not seem to be interested
in investing in activities that do not center around his own needs. As Karen
also commented on the CCICL "doesn't seem to desire intimacy with chil-
dren-or me, unless he's in need sexually."
Thus Karen's issues may center around her feeling relegated to sec-
ond or third place in Alan's life. He seems to have emotionally disengaged
from their relationship, except for when his needs have to be met. But
Karen may not recognize the value system that Alan is working with. His
very low 06 score indicates someone very much committed to a particular
tradition and he may see marriage in a very different way from Karen.
These value differences would certainly need to be explored. One particu-
lar area of concern would be Alan's low Dutifulness (C3) score, indicating
someone who may be unreliable and undependable. Certainly this lack of
follow-through on Alan's part contributes to a perception in Karen that he
just does not care about, nor give much importance to, those things having
to do with their home life.
In contrast, the COA profile for Karen is presented in Figure 5-7. On
the CCICLAlan indicates a very high level of dissatisfaction and conflict in
his marriage (a rating of 6 out of a possible 7). The profile agreement coef-
ficient comparing the self-report and observer ratings was only .12, indi-
cating a very low degree of agreement. As readily can be seen in Figure 5-7,
Alan perceives his wife as being significantly higher on all aspects of Neu-
roticism. Alan's perception of Karen is as a very emotionally distressed
person who is unable to manage her emotional world. Although Karen be-
lieves herself to be stable and capable of coping with a wide range of stres-
sors, Alan believes her to be overly sensitive to life's issues. As Karen's
NEO PI-R interpretive report, based on these ratings, describes her, "In
coping with the stresses of everyday life, this individual is described as
being likely to react with ineffective responses, such as hostile reactions to-
wards others ... her general defensive style can be characterized as mal-
adaptive and self-defeating."
Much more convergence is seen on Extraversion, although there is a
significant difference on E2 (Gregariousness). Karen is perceived as being
much less sociable than she sees herself. On the CCICL Alan noted that
Karen has poor social skills. These scores portray a woman who is relatively
reserved and quiet, who takes a very passive role in groups. Not particu-
larly cheerful or optimistic, Karen prefers to pursue a more mundane, low-
key lifestyle. It may be that Alan's preferences for spontaneous adventures
prove to be overstimulating for Karen or, given her very low scores on 04
(Actions), may threaten to disrupt her own schedule and routine. Her low
scores on 05 (Ideas) and 06 (Values) suggest an individual who also has a
very clear set of values that guide her perceptions of the world. She is,
202 CHAPTERS
Karen G.
Self Report
Alan
ranking his wife N E 0 A C
N E 0 A
1f
-<::I:
." ~
:I:
.c . .r
:;:'" ,,-
:~
~.
"e'" ~:
~ .
:" .
~
iil
'""
~
.~
1f
-<
~
FIGURE S-7. NEO PI-R self-report and observer rating for Karen G.
herself, quite dogmatic and has established for herself a very clear and
well-delineated world. Alan's behaviors may too frequently disrupt these
cherished patterns.
The Agreeableness domain portrays Karen as being quite considerate
and caring. Of particular interest is her very high score on A4 (Compli-
ance). In conjunction with the low Assertiveness (E4) rating, this indicates
someone who is very passive and accommodating, who wishes to follow
along with the group and do what is expected. This is curious given
Karen's complaints that Alan always wants to do things his own way with-
out much concern for her feelings. Why would such an accommodating,
passive person have so much difficulty with following along with some-
one's requests? The answer to this question may be found in Karen's very
low 04 (Actions) and 06 (Values) scores. She has certain expectations
about how her clearly structured world should operate. Being asked to do
things that fall outside these boundaries may be perceived as threatening
or inappropriate. In response Karen may refuse to acquiesce.
Finally, on Conscientiousness, Karen is perceived as being quite low
on all facets, suggesting a perception of Karen as being self-centered, care-
less, and disorganized. She is not seen as being very ambitious and does
RATER VERSION APPLICATIONS 203
not always think through her situation before acting. She does not feel
very competent, nor does she feel that she is in control of events in her life.
She is not seen by Alan as someone who can be counted on to follow
through on her commitments. Alan notes on his CCICL that Karen is un-
organized.
Overall, Alan perceives his wife as someone who "bitches, moans,
and complains about everything." She is a mostly passive individual with
little drive or gumption, yet does not seem to be very receptive to his sug-
gestions for activity in the relationship. Alan's overtures are perceived by
Karen as being whimsical and unrealistic because he does not appreciate
all the work that needs to be done to do what he wants, especially when it
is demanded on the spur of the moment.
These COA profiles outline some important, self-maintaining areas of
conflict. Alan sees himself as an active, assertive person who likes to try
new and different things. Routines bore and frustrate him, so he likes to
give variety a try. His requests to have Karen join him in these new under-
takings may initiate a negative sequence of events. First, Karen is not a
well-organized woman and is not very task oriented. Thus when Alan sug-
gests that they go someplace different for dinner, he does not recognize
that Karen does not like to have to "pick up the details" to make the sug-
gestion work. She may doubt her ability to do it correctly or may fear mak-
ing a mistake (e.g., forgetting to make the reservations at the restaurant,
not calling the babysitter). Karen appears very sensitive to task require-
ments and feels that she may not have the energy or desire to make the
necessary efforts.
Asecond issue is Karen's desire for structure and routine. If they are to
go out to a restaurant, Karen will want to patronize her "tried and true" lo-
cations. Alan, on the other hand, will want to explore new cuisines and
restaurants. This will be an ongoing source of frustration for both. Once
Karen gets into her routine, she will not like to have to shift things around;
this is uncomfortable for her. Karen's resistance is perceived by Alan as
being an overreaction to his requests. Given Alan's very low score on 03
(Feelings), he is not very sensitive to feelings and emotions; they are mostly
irrelevant to him. Thus, Karen's affective responses may overwhelm him
and he may "tune out" Karen's complaints, demeaning them as just so
much nonsensical ranting. Thus he rates Karen high on Neuroticism.
Not wanting to be locked into a particular pattern, Alan may retreat
into his relationships with others or his personal hobbies to find the stim-
ulation he needs. Although Karen believes that she is able to cope reason-
ably well with Alan's detachment, her normally compliant, externally
oriented nature feels the lack of a directing force. Her attempts to obtain
Alan's attention may only exacerbate the situation.
204 CHAPTERS
discrepancy. Such a dialogue can help each member better understand the
issues his or her partner experiences in the relationship. The five-factor
model provides a clear and common language to use in this discussion.
Each person should come away with a new understanding of their partner
and themselves.
Finally, COAanalysis provides the therapist with important informa-
tion about where and how to intervene with the couple. As we saw in the
previous case histories, the discrepancies between self- and observer rat-
ings indicate specific, salient issues emerging in the relationship. These are
the points at which intervention is necessary. With Alan G., the "where" of
an intervention is with his low Conscientiousness ratings. A therapist may
want to work toward making him appear less self-centered and fickle, and
more sensitive to Karen's emotional needs and the various strategies she
uses in expressing them. The "how" to intervene would want to consider
this couple's low Openness scores. Such individuals may appreciate a be-
havioral approach rather than a cognitive or dynamic one.
COA analysis is a relatively new area. There are few data available
that contrast various personality ratings. More work needs to be done to
establish a better empirical foundation for using test scores in this manner.
The CCICL was useful in this process: It highlighted the specific issues
these couples were experiencing. Such an instrument can pinpoint areas of
conflict. Although such an instrument is useful, it is by no means required
for doing a COA analysis. The data presented here are encouraging and
certainly showcase the heuristic value of this approach.
represents the target's public self or social reputation. For a self-rating, per-
sonality represents the structures, dynamics, and processes inside the per-
son that explain why he or she acts in a certain way. Thus, information
obtained from these two sources convey, to some degree, very different as-
pects of the individual. As Hogan noted, in some circumstances where the
assessment goal is prediction of behavior (e.g., personnel selection), repu-
tation may be a most important quality to measure. After all, the best pre-
dictor of future behavior is past behavior (Wernimont & Campbell, 1968);
one's reputation, which emerges over one's history of behavior with oth-
ers, should be a singularly useful predictive index.
A number of studies have evaluated the predictive utility of observer
ratings. Digman (1972) reported correlations in the .50s between elemen-
tary school teachers' ratings of Conscientiousness and high school grade
point average. Scheier, Buss, and Buss (1976) showed that observer ratings
of personality had greater predictive validity than did self-ratings con-
cerning the level of awareness of one's own aggressive behavior and re-
lated affective reactions. John and Robbins (1993) suggested that under
highly evaluative circumstances an individual may bias his or her self-
report and thus attenuate the validity of the measure. Yet an observer rat-
ing is not susceptible to the ego-involving motivations of the target, and
therefore would not suffer a decline in accuracy. These authors showed
that self-peer congruence declined when the rating was on an evaluative
dimension while peer-peer congruence remained at comparable levels of
convergence regardless of the evaluative nature of the term being rated.
Mount, Barrick, and Strauss (1994) provided the most empirically
compelling support for observer ratings. They gathered a sample of 105
sales representatives who completed a self-rating on the Big Five person-
ality domains. These individuals also had their supervisor and up to five
coworkers and five customers rate them on the same personality domains
as well as on several work performance indices. The results indicated two
important findings. First, the observer ratings of personality were signifi-
cant predictors of performance outcome (only rated Neuroticism did not
correlate with any of the performance criteria). Interestingly, the only self-
report score that correlated with performance was Conscientiousness.
Thus, ratings may provide more criterion-relevant information in this con-
text than self-reports.
The next finding concerned the incremental validity of the observer
ratings. Incremental validity (discussed in more detail in the next chapter)
determines whether a score provides any additional information over and
above that already obtained by existing measures. Hierarchical multiple
regression analyses were conducted in which the performance ratings
were the dependent variable. On the first step of the analysis, self-reported
RATER VERSION APPLICATIONS 207
personality scores were entered. On the second step the observer ratings
were entered. A partial F-test determined whether the variables entered on
Step 2 added any explanatory variance over that already accounted for on
Step 1. The results showed that for the four personality domains of Extra-
version, Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness, the observer
ratings provided additional, explained variance in the performance crite-
ria over and above whatever contribution was made by the self-reports.
These findings highlight the value of observer ratings as predictors in
their own right. They provide useful information about an individual that
is not entirely redundant with a corresponding self-report. This is because
observers have a different perspective on the target than the target has of
him- or herself, and these insights have their own predictive validity. Fur-
ther, an observer rating is not influenced or distorted by the same kinds of
motivations as a self-report. Thus, under certain circumstances one may
prefer to use a rating in place of the self-report.
Concerning validity research, these data emphasize the need to dis-
tinguish between the validity of a construct and the manner in which the
construct is measured. As Mount and colleagues (1994) showed, at times
an observer rating was a stronger predictor of performance than a self-
report. Therefore, determining the actual validity of a particular construct
in predicting some outcome on the basis of only one information source
(e.g., self-report) may underestimate the true strength of the relationship.
As Mount and colleagues showed, the relationship between personality
and performance was significantly stronger when both the self-report and
observer ratings were included in the prediction equation.
Although these data were obtained in a work setting, the lesson
should not be lost for the clinical environment. The best evaluations are
those that rely on multiple sources of information and look for points of
convergence. Obtaining similar diagnostic indicators from diverse mea-
sures and informants increases confidence in the accuracy of the inferences
drawn. Observer ratings are a useful piece of any assessment process and
complement information obtained from self-reports. Under circumstances
where the validity of self-reported data may be questioned, obtaining in-
formation from knowledgeable informants provides a useful substitute.
Certainly more research is needed for examining the utility of ob-
server ratings. Although ratings do not suffer from the same sources of
bias and distortion as self-reports, they do have their limitations and some
evaluation of their sources of bias would be helpful. Observer ratings are
an assessment technology that has been underutilized. The availability of
an established, normed, rating instrument in Form R of the NEO PI-R
should help make this type of data more accessible, and useful, to re-
searchers and practitioners alike.
CHAPTER 6
RESEARCH APPLICATIONS
WITH THE NEO PI-R
209
210 CHAPTER 6
that the NED PI-R can be quite useful. It can generate tremendous
amounts of information about the client's characteristic ways of living that
can help both inform and direct clinical querying and intervening. Thus,
the energy expended in obtaining personality assessment data is rewarded
when treatment moves into its middle and end phases. This chapter pro-
vides some strategies and materials to use in meeting these challenges.
This chapter also outlines some of the ways that the NEO PI-R can be
used as a research tool in areas such as cross-cultural psychology, scale val-
idation, and program evaluation. Given the strong psychometric founda-
tions of the instrument, it has a significant contribution to make in
expanding our knowledge base in any area that concerns personality
structure and development. Perhaps the two greatest potential contribu-
tions of this relatively comprehensive taxonomy is its ability to clarify the
personological content of existing constructs and to point out ways of
identifying new domains of individual-difference variables. This chapter
outlines some of the techniques and strategies for using the NEO PI-R to
realize these goals.
OVERVIEW
not just go away. It provides a real impetus for the social sciences to begin
to put in place a paradigm for the collection of information that can speak
meaningfully to the quality of our interventions and the conditions under
which they can be expected to work. All of this interest in creating greater
therapist accountability has a singular focus on obtaining outcome assess-
mentdata.
Outcome assessment data speak to the very heart of practice, in that
they provide information about how effective our treatments are, and with
what type of clients. Collecting relevant clinical outcome data will (a) help
improve the quality and efficacy of treatment, (b) enhance the empirical
foundation of the clinical sciences, and (c) provide greater accountability
(Barlow, Hayes, & Nelson, 1984). With so many types of professionals
vying for a slice of the mental health pie the emphasis on empirical data
will only grow, enabling those with the data to claim the largest share.
Thus the pressure will increase for clinicians to create a clinical envi-
ronment that includes a significant assessment component. There are
numerous works currently available that outline how relatively easy out-
come assessments can be performed (e.g., Lyons et al., 1997; Ogles et al.,
1996; Stout, 1997). All of these works stress the need for more information
on treatment efficacy. Research has to be seen as our friend. It alone will be
able to stave off the onslaught of managed care and its ubiquitous call for
more efficiency. Only data will be able to speak persuasively about how
much treatment various "problems" will need to receive, and how much
improvement can be expected with different kinds of clients.
This need for more outcome data must be seen as an irresistible force
in our field. This information must be collected and someone will have to
do it; either managed care will collect its own data or we will need to do it
ourselves. If it is us, social scientists, both clinicians and researchers, who
undertake this research challenge, then we will receive an added bonus for
our efforts: We will have the ultimate control of how we define ourselves
professionally. It is the selection of variables and outcomes that will have
the largest influence on marking out the territory we as social scientists call
our own, the constructs we find of interest, and the goals we pursue with
our interventions. It would be to our advantage if we make these selec-
tions rather than having some outside agency impose them on us.
The basic paradigm for conducting outcome research is really quite
straightforward. It may consume some additional time of the client and
therapist, but its payoff is certainly worth the investment. The NEO PI-R
can playa pivotal role in this process. The dimensions assessed speak
meaningfully about the motivations and capacities of individuals. As was
highlighted in Chapter 2, the NEO PI-R facets and domains are very
closely linked to a wide array of clinical dimensions and outcomes. Scores
212 CHAPTER 6
on the NEO PI-R can be used to match treatments to clients, to make dif-
ferential diagnoses, and to formulate expectations of treatment outcome,
to name a few of its potential benefits. But this is only the beginning.
of our discipline and brings forth our own sense of identity, rather than
having it imposed on us from outside sources. Finally, our ability to influ-
ence managed care and its reimbursement strategies will be entirely de-
pendent on the type and quality of empirically based information that we
can bring to bear on the issues of treatment and outcome. Because we are
at the applied frontier of our field, we need to take a more active and in-
volved role in this process.
FACTOR ORTHOGONALITY
server form of the NEO PI-R (see Piedmont, 1994, for a description of the
sample and methods). The purpose of these analyses was to evaluate the
overlap between the Neuroticism and Extraversion domains. As was out-
lined earlier, these two factors are strongly related to experienced Well-
Being and there continues to be some controversy over the relationship
between these two domains. Some argue that Positive and Negative Affect
(Extraversion and Neuroticism, respectively) are correlated (e.g., Chen,
Dai, Spector, & Jex, 1997; Green, Goldman, & Salovey, 1993), while others
believe the two to be truly orthogonal (e.g., Goldstein & Strube, 1994).
The MPQ contains two global domains of interest here, Positive Affect
and Negative Affect. I tested two models using LISREL 8 (Joreskog & Sor-
bom, 1993). The first model is presented in Figure 6-1, and outlines two
latent dimensions, Negative Affect (NEG-AFF) and Positive Affect (POS-
AFF). The former was defined by self-rated measures of Neuroticism
(SELFN) and Negative Affect (NEGAFF) as well as by observer-rated Neu-
roticism (RATEN). The latter latent dimension was defined by self-rated Ex-
traversion (SELFE) and Positive Affect (POSAFF) and observer-rated
Extraversion (RATEE). As can be seen, the estimated correlation between
the two latent dimensions was -.31, suggesting that Positive and Negative
Affect may reflect a singular bipolar dimension rather than two distinct do-
mains. The overall model chi square with 11 degrees of freedom was 25.47
(p = .0078), suggesting a relatively poor model fit, although the Normed Fit
Index (NFI), Goodness of Fit Index (GFI) and Adjusted Goodness of Fit
Index (AGFI) were all over .90, indicating that the model does match the
data well. This apparent inconsistency is a regular problem noted in confir-
matory analyses using personality-based data and is considered later.
A second model was also tested and it is presented in Figure 6-2. Here
two additional latent dimensions were included, one that represented self-
report variance and the other representing observer-rating variance. These
dimensions were added to represent the impact of information source on
scores. The overall model chi square with 4 degrees of freedom was 2.27
(p = .69) indicating a very good fit of the model to the data. Secondary fit
indices were all uniformly close to unity (NFl = .99; GFI = 1.0; AGFI = .98)
This model also fits the data significantly better than the previous one
(Ll = X2 = 23.2, P < .01).
Two conclusions can be drawn from these data. First, by including the
dimensions representing the information source for the ratings (e.g., self
versus observer), we obtain a model that fits the data much better than
when these dimensions are not included (model chi squares of 2.27 versus
25.47, respectively). Second, the estimated correlation between the latent
personality domains drops to a nonsignificant -.18, supporting the con-
tention that these domains are independent. Thus, the observed covari-
216 CHAPTER 6
71.176. .
--1 ~.n SELFN
m.-1 NEGAFF rU
l
~71
n., r'·
RATEE
SELFE
~M
J
u
m.u4 POSAFF ~
FIGURE 6-1. Estimated Confirmatory Factor Analysis Parameters for the Personality Only
Model.
ance among the personality scales noted in these data can be attributed to
correlated method error arising from a single information source (note that
the correlation between rating sources is r =.75 which attests to the cross-
observer validity of these personality dimensions). Thus, two personality
scales will correlate both because they share substantive content and be-
cause it is the same person generating the information. Any distortions in-
troduced in one scale by a person will also be included in all other
measures he or she completes. Hence, all these measures will correlate to
some degree.
These data support my contention that observed correlations among
the five personality domains noted in several studies is the result of corre-
lated error introduced by relying on a single information source. By using
multiple raters and including latent dimensions that represent these
RESEARCH APPLICATIONS 217
n.~ RA~N k~
4.71 SELFN
11.
... ~ HECIAFF K1
n.~,-
...
FIGURE 6-2. Estimated confirmatory factor analysis parameters for the Personality and In-
formation Source Model.
NUMBER OF FACTORS
The issue of how many factors are needed to explain the full spectrum
of personality continues to be debatable in the field. As noted in Chapter 1,
there are numerous personality models advocating any number of per-
sonality domains. Discussions of the merits of these models is beyond the
scope of this book, but the issue of how many factors there are does raise
two concerns for us. The first issue relates to evaluating various factor
structures and the second centers on one's ability to obtain the appropriate
factor structure from NEO PI-R data.
formation that is contained in the NEO PI-R, then usually one can be con-
fident that the smaller model is overlooking important aspects of person-
ality. If it is combining domains, then some empirical considerations need
to be evaluated to determine whether the smaller model is to be preferred
due to its greater parsimony or whether the smaller model is inappropri-
ately combining independent dimensions.
For the three-factor model advocated by Eysenck, both issues emerge.
First, Eysenck's model leaves out the dimension of Openness to Experience.
However, Eysenck (1991) has suggested that Openness is really an aspect of
cognitive ability rather than personality. Although there is data showing
that Openness is not an aspect of cognitive functioning (McCrae, 1993-1994,
1994b), let us finesse this point for now by granting the issue to Eysenck
(see Costa & McCrae, 1995b, for a response). This still leaves us with the
need to evaluate the worth of a system with fewer than five factors.
Several studies have shown that Eysenck's dimension of Psychoticism
is inversely related to the domains of Agreeableness and Conscientious-
ness (e.g., Costa et a/., 1991; Goldberg & Rosolack, 1994) and Eysenck
(1992) has interpreted this to mean that these domains are merely facets of
the larger Psychoticism dimension. However, Costa and McCrae (1992b,
1995b) have argued that Psychoticism represents "a relatively arbitrary
conflation of two independent dimensions" (1995b, p. 310). As noted ear-
lier in this chapter, the question as to whether the five factors should be
considered orthogonal or correlated has important conceptual implica-
tions. If the five factors are correlated, then this leaves open the possibility
of obtaining "high-order factors," super dimensions that are comprised of
two or more of the five. This is what Eysenck claimed. However, if the five
domains are orthogonal, then no such super factors can exist (an identity
matrix cannot be factored) and Costa and McCrae would be correct. Al-
though the data suggest that orthogonal factors have better predictive va-
lidities than oblique (correlated) factors, there are other empirical criteria
that can be applied for evaluating the internal quality of a factor structure.
One important way of clarifying this issue is to evaluate the pattern of
external correlations these elements have. One of the important aspects of
factor analysis is that it identifies homogeneous groupings of items. As in
the NEO PI-R, the six facet scales are more specific articulations of the
larger domain to which they belong. Thus, these scales all correlate in a sim-
ilar way with relevant criteria. For example, all six Neuroticism facets are
related to low psychological well-being (e.g., Costa & McCrae, 1984). There-
fore, aggregating across the facets produces domain scores that are more re-
liable and stable than their constituent elements. If Agreeableness and
Conscientiousness are merely subfacets to Psychoticism, then they should
reveal a similar pattern of correlates to a given criterion and summing these
222 CHAPTER 6
two dimensions together should produce an index that is more strongly as-
sociated with this criterion than either of the domains separately.
Costa and McCrae (1995b) used this strategy in evaluating the relations
between the NEO PI-R and the Eysenck Personality Profiler (EPP). Corre-
lations between Agreeableness and Conscientious and the EPP scales
showed very different patterns of correlates for the two NEO PI-R domains.
Further, when Agreeableness and Conscientiousness were aggregated and
then correlated to the EPP scales, the resulting correlations were lower than
their individual associations. Thus, the Psychoticism dimension appears to
inappropriately combine independent personality qualities. This forced ag-
gregation ultimately impairs external validity, which should be regarded as
an important criterion for evaluating the utility of any personality model.
What should be taken from this discussion is that empirical methods
can and do generate a wide variety of statistical models for describing per-
sonality systems. Any review of the current literature will reveal numerous
taxonomic models ranging from 3 to as many as 16 factors. Often, the
quantitative basis of these models is quite compelling. This can leave one
quite confused about the state of affairs in personality measurement. How
many factors are there? But with a little care and patience, the discerning
reader can begin to ask the appropriate, sophisticated questions that can
provide an answer to that question. Given that the five-factor model has
been shown to be a reliable description of personality variables, it can and
should be the reference point for any critical evaluation of other models.
In the case of a model advocating more than five personality dimen-
sions, the questions that need to be asked include whether the factor struc-
ture can be replicated in a new sample, the independence of the new
factors from the established five, and the personological breadth of the
new domains. When a model advocates fewer than five factors, it needs to
be determined whether the model omits something already contained in
the five-factor model. Such an omission would be sufficient evidence to re-
ject the new model. However, if the model seems to combine two or more
of the Big Five's domains, then some evaluation of the predictive utility of
this global factor needs to be conducted. Does the "super factor" actually
evidence higher external validity than its components? If not, then the new
model does not provide an optimal specification of personality structure.
using EFA techniques. Five factors were extracted and rotated and the re-
sults of this analysis are presented in Table 6-2. Congruence coefficients were
calculated between this solution and factor loadings presented in the NEO
PI-R manual (Costa & McCrae, 1992c). Several findings of interest emerge.
First, the overall congruence coefficient of .82 is significant, suggest-
ing that the general structure is comparable with normative values. In fact,
an examination of each of the five factor congruence coefficients (located at
the bottom of the columns) suggests that all five factors are recovered in a
form comparable to the normative structure. The domains of Neuroticism,
Openness, and Conscientiousness more strongly resemble their English
counterparts than do Extraversion and Agreeableness. However, an ex-
amination of the facet congruence coefficients indicates that none of the
facet scales for the latter two domains is well replicated. The insignificant
coefficients tell us that the pattern of loadings for these scales across the
five domains is different from that found in the normative structure.
Clearly, in using EFA there is a failure to replicate two of the five domains.
But there are several reasons for this apparent failure. One reason is
that the five factors do not operate in a way that was observed in America.
Given the cross-cultural nature of this sample, this different factor structure
may have something important to say about Korean culture and how per-
sonality is expressed through it. A second explanation deals with the fact
that the NEO PI-R does not have perfect simple structure and that subtle
shifts in the facet scales' interrelatedness could impact overall structure.
Given, as noted earlier, that the Extraversion and Agreeableness domains
constitute the interpersonal circumplex, a complex ordering of traits that is
designed to combine various quantities of these two dimensions, this sec-
ond explanation may be likely. The only way to determine which of these
two hypotheses is correct is to conduct an Orthogonal Procrustes rotation
of the results. If such an analysis still could not recover the five domains in
a way consistent with normative data, then this would be strong evidence
that personality may indeed be organized differently in Korean culture.
However, if the normative structure could be better approximated, then the
observed distortions most likely are due to sample specific factors.
These data were then analyzed using the statistical program pre-
sented in Table 6-1. The result of this targeted rotation procedure was the
factor structure presented in Table 6-3. As can be seen, the obtained factor
structure more closely approximates normative values. The overall con-
gruence coefficient is much higher than previously found (.96 versus .82).
The congruence coefficients for the five factors are all quite high, including
the Extraversion and Agreeableness domains, whose values have risen
to .95. Finally, the facet congruence coefficients also show significant
230 CHAPTER 6
section, I expand on the value of using the NEO PI-R in validity research,
especially as it pertains to the development of a scale's construct and in-
cremental validity.
CONSTRUCT VALIDITY
The real question in this area is whether these putatively spiritual con-
structs represent qualities of people that are distinct from established per-
sonality measures or are rather some new combination of them. Also,
without any larger framework for classifying spiritual constructs, the pos-
sibility of redundancy in content is very high. Although the five-factor
model is not religiously based, it can provide a useful starting point for
evaluating the personological content of religious constructs. As I have
noted elsewhere (Piedmont, 1996b), if religious variables have any overlap
with personality, then correlating such scales with the five factors can help
illuminate the underlying personological motivations of the scale as well
as highlight the types of behavioral outcomes to be anticipated. Further,
correlations with the five-factor model can also help to nomologically link
religious constructs with one another.
In order to address this question, I gave the NEO PI-R along with sev-
eral popularly used religious scales to a mixed-sex undergraduate sample
of 493 people. The first scale was the Hood Mysticism Scale (Hood, 1975),
a measure of reported mystical experiences. It contains two factors, a gen-
eral mystical experience dimension and the joyful or noetic qualities asso-
ciated with religious experiences. Also given was the Faith Maturity Scale
(Benson, Donahue, & Erickson, 1993). It reflects "the degree to which a per-
son embodies the priorities, commitments, and perspectives characteristic
of vibrant and life-transforming faith" (p. 3). There are two subscales, the
Vertical, which evaluates the degree to which one has a close relationship
with God, and the Horizontal, which looks at the degree to which one's
faith influences relationships with others. The third measure was the In-
trinsic/Extrinsic Religiosity Scale (Gorsuch & Venable, 1983). Intrinsic Reli-
giosity reflects a religious orientation that is internalized and done for its
own sake. Extrinsic Religiosity reflects an attitude toward religious in-
volvement for the sake of achieving some other goal. The final measure was
the Spiritual Well-Being scale (Ellison, 1983). This scale attempts to measure
spirituality as it speaks to overall well-being. There are two subscales, Reli-
gious Well-Being, which examines the level of satisfaction one experiences
in his or her relationship with God, and Existential Well-Being, which eval-
uates the level of personal meaning one has derived for his or her life. The
correlation of these scales to the NEO PI-R is given in Table 6-4.
Two conclusions can be drawn from these data. First, the number of
significant correlations attests to the relevance of the five-factor model for
understanding religious-oriented constructs. For example, the Intrinsic Re-
ligiosity scale correlated positively with Agreeableness and· Conscien-
tiousness. People who have an internalized view toward religion may be
perceived by others as being moral, reverent, considerate, responsible, and
dependable (Hofstee, de Raad, & Goldberg, 1992). The negative correlation
RESEARCH APPLICATIONS 235
TABLE 6-4. Correlations between Select Religious Constructs and the NEO PI-R
Domains
NEO PI-R Domain
Religious scale N E 0 A C R2 a a-R2 I-a ~R2
Hood Mysticism I" .02 .08 .23' -.04 .02 .06' .90 .84 .10 .001
Hood Mysticism 2h -.04 .13d .20' .01 .09'" .06' .82 .76 .18 .002
Intrinsic Religiosity .00 -.04 -.09' .09'" .11' .03' .71 .68 .29 .007
Extrinsic Religiosity .11' -.04 -.07 -.07 -.09' .02 .69 .67 .31 .000
Existential Well-Being -.51' .34' -.05 .20' .39' .35' .87 .52 .13 .000
Religious Well-Being -.04 .05 -.11' .11' .13d .05' .93 .88 .07 .001
FMS - Horizontal -.06 .08 .17" .25' .16' .09' .74 .65 .26 .12'
FMS - Vertical -.09'" .11' .04 .13d .17" .05' .81 .76 .19 .02d
'General Mysticism Factor
'Joyful Religious Expression Factor
'lp<.01
< .05
With so many scales available in the marketplace today and the very
real question of redundancy in content, it is critical that one evaluate the
unique value of a scale; its "bang for the buck," so to speak. What does a
scale tell us about some outcome that cannot be obtained from already es-
tablished measures? This question is particularly salient when one is in-
terested in developing new personality dimensions. The incremental
validity paradigm is useful in determining the common and unique pre-
dictiveness of various classes of variables. Its ultimate value is that it pro-
vides a direct index of what a scale adds to our predictive ability.
For religious research, incremental validity paradigms would be help-
ful in outlining those individual-difference qualities distinctive to religious
constructs that are predictive of important outcomes (e.g., prosocial be-
havior, altruism, racism, sexual behavior) over and above the more tradi-
tional personality variables as represented by the five-factor model and the
NEO PI-R. In this manner, researchers can begin to articulate more clearly
those aspects of human functioning that are singularly captured by reli-
gious constructs.
At the heart of this approach is stepwise regression analysis. Once a
suitable criterion is selected, measures of the five-factor model (i.e., the do-
main scores from the NEO PI-R) can serve as markers of traditional per-
sonality qualities. They would be entered on step 1 of the analysis. Then,
the religious construct or constructs of interest can be entered on step 2.
This variable would then identify that variance in the criterion not associ-
ated with the personality traits. A partial F-test (J. Cohen & Cohen, 1983)
would then determine whether the variance accounted for by the religious
variable (or the construct entered on step 2) added significantly to that al-
ready accounted for by the personality dimensions entered on the first step.
The data in Table 6-4 provide a systematic evaluation of the unique
variance of each of the religious constructs. The Cronbach alpha (a) for
each scale is given for this sample. This value outlines the amount of reli-
able variance in the observed scores. The column labeled "a - R2" indicates
the amount of unique, reliable variance in the observed scores. This value
subtracts the amount of shared variance between the religious and per-
sonality constructs from the total amount of reliable variance. The larger
this value, the more scores on this scale capture qualities of the individual
not shared by the five-factor model. A. S. Kaufman (1975) and Silverstein
238 CHAPTER 6
(1982) suggest that to conclude a scale has enough reliable variance to war-
rant a separate interpretation, its unique variance component should be
larger than its error (1 - (1) and at least 25% of the total variance. All of
these scales have a pattern consistent with these guidelines. Thus, they
have something to say independent of the five personality dimensions.
However, the most important question is whether this unique, reliable
variance has something meaningful to contribute to the prediction of im-
portant outcomes. This is where the incremental validity paradigm comes
into play. For the data presented in Table 6-4, those subjects also completed
a behavior-based index of prosodal behavior. This variable served as the
outcome for the stepwise regression analysis. The NEO PI-R domain scores
were entered on the first step, and then each of the religious constructs was
systematically entered on step 2. The last column of Table 6-4 (~R2) indi-
cates the increase in explained variance the religious constructs added to
predicting prosodal behavior over personality's contribution. As can be
seen, only the FMS scales added significantly to the explained variance. The
other measures, despite large amounts of reliable, unique variance, had
nothing to contribute uniquely to this outcome. Thus, any association be-
tween these religious constructs and prosocial behavior would be due en-
tirely to variance that overlaps with the more traditional personality scales.
These findings are surprising given the importance of the religious con-
structs. They are measurement staples of religious research, yet any predic-
tive value they may have in regard to nonreligious outcomes may be due to
their overlap with standard personality variables. It should also be men-
tioned that other studies have evaluated the predictiveness of these con-
structs against a wide range of outcomes (e.g., burnout among clergy, racism,
sexual attitudes, quality of life) and the results have been equally lackluster
(Csarny, 1997; Rodgerson, 1994). The one, and to date in my research, only ex-
ception has been the Faith Maturity Scale, which has consistently shown it-
self to possess significant incremental validity (e.g., Chen, 1996).
The incremental validity paradigm provides an empirically sophisti-
cated methodology for evaluating the utility of individual-difference con-
structs in providing additional, nonredundant information about people.
It very directly assesses what a new scale tells us about others over and
above what can be determined with existing scales. The NEO PI-R serves
as the anchoring point in these ventures. Although in this example I used
only the domain scores as the reference point, it is equally appropriate to
enter the facets instead. This will generate a larger portion of variance at-
tributable to personality. Scales that fail to improve predictiveness need to
be reconsidered. As seen in the example data here, many of the religious
constructs failed the incremental validity test. Thus, researchers in the area
of spirituality need to demonstrate the value of many of their constructs;
RESEARCH APPLICATIONS 239
OUTCOME RESEARCH
3. Treatments which seemed most helpful (Place a plus sign (" + ") before all treatments that were used
SUCCESSFULLY; a minus sign (".") before any treatments that were used UNSUCCESSFULLY.
OGestalt o Vocational OSpiritual Direction OCultural Trips
OCfient Centered OGroup SeSsions o Probl.m Solving OJournaling
OCognitive Treatment OR.laxation Sellions DArt Thar.py
Olnsight DSystematic O.sans. DAA/NA Program
4. TlIERAPEUnC SUBPROCESSES USED (Place a plus sign (" +") before all subprocesses that
were used SUCCESSFULLY; a minu sign (" 0") before any subprocesses that were used
UNSUCCESSFULLY.
DSupport and r....ur.nce Dlnt.rpr.tation: Current life oCognitive restructuring
DAdvic., problem solving DProbl.m SOlving .dvlce OF ree association
OExperiencing techniques OStr••s manag.m.nt oPrayer/meditation
oInterpretation: early life techniques oSpiritual "adings
oOther:
---------------------------------------------------------
S. Length of program for client (in weeks):
6. Reason for termination (check only one):
Orelapse a other priorities a violation of progrem rules
Oloss of interest a legal difficulti.s a complet.d program
o Other:
----------------------------
7. How successfully were treatment Ilolls reached? "I" not
successful "4" partially successful "7" fully successful .... 2 3 4 5 6 7
8. What degree of recovery was noted? "I" likely to
relapse "4" no change "7" likely to stay clean . . . . . . . . . . 2 3 4 5 6 7
9. Spiritual development: "I" rigid (willfulness), "4' no
chanlle "7" flexihility (willingness) ................ 2 3 4 5 6 7
10. What was the client's attitude towards vocational
training? "I" resistive "4· complacent "7" proactive ..... 2 3 4 5 6 7
II. How well did the client learn the job searching/
vocational techniques provided? "I" not well, "7" very
well .................................... 2 3 4 5 6 7
12. Overall, how much personal growth (i.e., emotional,
vocational. spiritual) did this person experience? "I" not
much growth "7" a lot of growth . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 3 4 5 6 7
(Continued)
242 CHAPTER 6
The size of this type of list is limited only by the kinds of interventions
that are being used. This type of approach appreciates the reality that most
clinicians are eclectic in their dealings with clients. Thus, rather than trying
to classify therapeutic interventions by the theoretical orientation of the
therapist, this type of list can provide a more detailed insight into exactly
what interventions were applied.
Another useful question asked is why clients dropped out of the pro-
gram. Oftentimes in clinical research one goal is to identify those who
complete from those who fail to complete the course of treatment. This
type of question seeks to identify the reason or reasons for early termina-
tion. Are certain types of individuals more prone to "quit" than others?
But, as this item illustrates, there are numerous possible reasons for some-
one's dropping out. In this example, one reason could be a relapse to using
drugs. Identifying those who are at risk for repeat drug use and those more
likely to stick with the program is of fundamental interest to substance
abuse counselors. However, people left this program for other reasons, in-
cluding a loss of interest, legal problems (such as being arrested for an ear-
lier crime) or life circumstances that took them out of the area. These types
of cases should not be included in any analyses of recidivism.
A similar kind of form can also be given to clients to complete on their
own. Such a questionnaire can access their perceptions of the therapeutic
program and how well each element was received. Clients can also rate how
successful they found the treatment to be. Other questions can be included
about the client's view of a number of useful dimensions, all of which speak
to his or her satisfaction with the services that were rendered. Figure 6-4 pro-
vides an example of such a questionnaire that I have used, although there
are a number of standardized client satisfaction questionnaires already
available (e.g., Larsen, Attkisson, Hargreaves, & Nguyen, 1979).
As can be seen in Figure 6-4, clients are directly asked to indicate how
successful they found various aspects of the treatment and how much im-
RESEARCH APPLICATIONS 243
PARTICIPANT'S EVALUAnON
NAME: _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ __ DATE: _ _ _ _ __
As pan %ur evaillOtion o/the tjftctilltrltss o/the CHRP program, we would Ilkt your view o/the
counseling you Milt rtcttvtd. PlellSe answer thefollowlng questions.
I. How helpful did you find your participation in the CHRP program in dealill8 with the problem that
brought you in for treatment?
OVery helpful Osomewhat helpful [Jnot very helpful o not helpful at all
2. In general, how do you feel now compared to how you felt when you began the program?
Omuch better abetter Othe same o worse
3. Do you have more self-esteem and feel better about yourself?
Omuch better Obetter Othe same o worse
4. Do you think you are able to get along better with other people?
Qmuch better abetter Othe same o worse
S. Which aspects of the program seemed the most helpful to you? Please check one response for each
aspect.
very somewhat not
helpful helpful helpful
or don't
know
Gettill8 advice about what to do ................... . Q o Q
Feelill8 accepted by the group . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Q a o
Learning to understand yourself in relation to the God of
your understandill8 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . o a o
Getting reassurance and support ................... . Q a o
Learnill8 about other people . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Q a o
Expressing your feelings .......•................. o o a
Taking responsibility for your life .................. . o o a
The mornill8 dialogue .......................... . a a a
The employment readiness training ................. . Q a a
The spiritual element to the program ................ . o a a
Learning about" Action, Attitude, Awareness" .......... . o a a
Learning about "Serendipity" ..................... . Q a a
If you knew someone with similar problems, would you recommend the program to
them? .................................................... aVe. ONo
FIGURE 6-4. Example of a client satisfaction questionnaire given at the end of treatment.
This type of questionnaire can also provide very useful feedback informa-
tion for therapists. A therapist may believe that a certain intervention is
important or even critical to the process of therapy, but clients may indi-
cate that it is the least useful. Finding out why such discrepancies exist can
be helpful in tailoring a treatment program to better meet the needs of the
clients. It may also provide insights as to why resistance may be develop-
ing in some or all clients at certain points in the program.
The utility of both these questionnaires is that they are easy to admin-
ister and interpret. They do not take much time to complete and can pro-
vide a wide range of valuable information. Given the high face validity of
the items, these measures can be readily interpreted by simply reading
over the responses. Further, these responses can be easily entered into data
programs for later statistical analyses. These scores become very interest-
ing when aggregated over many clients and then correlated to the psycho-
metric information that is collected in tandem with the forms. The next
sections outline some of the kinds of data I have obtained from using
scales like the ones just described.
As we noted earlier in this book, the dimensions of the NEO PI-R re-
main very stable in adulthood; as Costa and McCrae (1994) noted, 25-year
test-retest values hover around .80, and it is estimated that over a 50-year
adult life span 60% of the variance remains stable. These stability coeffi-
cients are complemented by analyses of mean level change which also
show no significant shifts over time. Based on this data Costa and McCrae
noted that personality is pretty much "set like plaster" after age 30.
Such evidence for stability raises a number of theoretical, philosophi-
cal, and ideological questions. How, then, do we define the concepts of
psychological growth and freedom? To what degree do individuals have
the capacity to make choices? Is personality destiny? From an applied per-
spective, what purpose do psychological interventions serve? Should clin-
icians attempt to affect clients by helping them make shifts in their
fundamental psychological structures or through improving their psy-
chosocial instrumentality (e.g., coping ability)? Answers to these questions
certainly address our field at its most fundamental level and force us to
think most seriously about concepts of change and development (these is-
sues were touched on in Chapter 1 in the discussion on genotypic versus
phenotypic change).
But even if adulthood is not characterized by any natural psychologi-
cal metamorphoses, are such transformations possible given the interdic-
tion of certain events? There are three types of events that could arguably
RESEARCH APPLICATIONS 245
ity, those seeking changes may be able to find them. These data indicate
that psychological interventions may be able to induce positive changes in
one's underlying dispositions. The magnitude of these shifts is indeed quite
high (see Trull, Useda, Costa, & McCrae, 1995; Bagby, Joffe, Kalemba, &
Harkness, 1995 for comparisons) and suggests that the more time invested
in treatment, the larger the personological gains experienced by the client.
Of course, further follow-up data is necessary to determine whether
these treatment changes last. Are the noted changes temporary, with the
individuals returning to premorbid levels in time? Also, we need to deter-
mine whether the NEO PI-R scores obtained at posttreatment continue to
be as predictive of life outcomes as found with individuals who do not
show any change. More longitudinal research with clinical samples needs
to be undertaken to answer these questions. However, for our purposes,
two conclusions can be gleaned from these data. First, it appears that the
plaster cast can be broken. Those seeking serious changes in their lives
may be able to find it through therapy. The magnitude of effect for some of
the NEO PI-R domains was indeed quite large, as compared with other
outcome studies using this instrument. But the intervention was quite in-
tense; these individuals experienced 30 hours of therapy a week for 6
weeks. Individual treatment usually is not this focused and does not last
so long. The 180 hours of treatment correspond to about 3 years of tradi-
tional one-on-one therapy. Thus, the more put into therapy, the more ben-
efit that may be received.
The second value of these data is that they suggest that the NEO PI-R
may be a useful measure of psychotherapy outcome. Its ability to change
in response to therapy indicates that it is sensitive to treatment interven-
tions and can be a very useful index for gauging the extent of experienced
change. The fact that clients changed on Extraversion and Agreeableness
makes a powerful statement about the kinds of shifts in interpersonal
style. Not only do these data validate the treatment that was received, but
they also establish very useful expectations for future behavior on the part
of clients. With a more approachable and engaging style that will lend it-
self to developing more mutually satisfying emotional relationships with
others, these people can anticipate a deeper and more fulfilling quality to
their lives. The increased scores on Conscientiousness lead one to expect a
greater capacity on the part of clients to obtain and maintain vocational
opportunities.
By simply collecting NEO PI-R data pre- and posttreatment, one can
obtain a useful description of where clients are at these points. Shifts on the
domains and facet scales can indicate how and to what degree therapy af-
fected clients. As I pointed out in the beginning of this chapter, this type of
information can be very useful in documenting efficacy for third-party
RESEARCH APPLICATIONS 247
Extraversion
High: Comfortable with less Talkativeness can blunt
structured approaches; treatment focus
optimistic and energetic
Low: Comfortable with structured Lacks enthusiasm for
approaches interaction with therapist
Openness
High: Prefers imaginative Excessive curiosity can
approaches scatter resources
Low: Responds well to practical Rigidity and lack of
approaches; Education, curiosity can be mis-
support, behavior therapy understood as resistance
Agreeableness
High: Treatment alliance easily Accepts interpretations
formed uncritically. Need to
please therapist inter-
feres with disclosure of
transference
Low: Assertiveness and clear Hostility and skepticism
thinking about self-interest toward therapist; diffi-
facilitate problem solving cult to form treatment
alliance
Conscientiousness
High: Works hard to benefit from (possibly none)
treatment. Willing to
tolerate discomfort and
fustration
Low: (possibly none) Unlikely to do homework;
likely to reject interven-
tions that require hard
work or toleration of
discomfort
From Miller. T. (1991). The psychotherapeutic utility of the five-factor model of personality: A clinician's
experience. Journal ofPersonality Assessment, 57, 418-419. Copyright 1991 by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,
Inc., Publishers. Adapted with permiSSion.
RESEARCH APPLICATIONS 249
ally induced problems. The task with these people is to find ways of mar-
shaling already existing personal resources to manage the new stressor,
which may in time pass away. No doubt, the more personal resources a
client brings to therapy, the more likely they are to experience a positive
outcome. The real goal is to identify ways to engage clients with fewer psy-
chological resources and still bring about a better clinical result.
Using the evaluation form presented in Figure 6-3, I conducted a pro-
gram evaluation of an outpatient substance abuse program (Piedmont &
Ciarrocchi, in press). Data from this project have been presented through-
out this book. One of the interesting parts of this project was to correlate
NEO PI-R scores at Time 1 (entrance into the program) with counselor rat-
ings obtained at Time 2 (at the end of treatment). Of particular interest
were the correlations with items in questions 3 and 4 in Figure 6-3. These
associations speak to those initial qualities of the clients that seemed to
benefit from specific treatment interventions.
Our results indicated that those high on Neuroticism benefited from
client-centered therapy and systematic desensitization, while problem-
solving advice was not seen as effective with these types of clients. Open-
ness correlated positively with vocational techniques. Those high on
Agreeableness were rated as responding well to the AAand NA programs,
while those low on Agreeableness responded well to relaxation sessions,
art therapy, and joumaling. Those high on Conscientiousness responded
well to Gestalt techniques. Openness did not correlate with any of the
treatment techniques.
These results should not be interpreted as generalizing to all thera-
peutic contexts; more research is needed before that conclusion can be
drawn. Rather, these findings represent patterns of relationships that hold
for a particular program, and this is the data's greatest value. In conducting
outcome evaluations, clinicians are interested in doc\imenting the efficacy
of their particular interventions; for determining which of their specific
clients will respond to the presented treatments. This is why outcome re-
search is so important for individual clinicians. It provides a framework for
better understanding the nuances of their clinical practice. There are so
many factors that influence how a person high on some personality di-
mension responds to a treatment, such as the demeanor of the therapist and
the way the treatment is presented. These unique features of a therapist's
treatment environment are captured in these types of ratings and provide
important feedback on how well various interventions are working.
These data also provide some support for the preference of therapists
for eclectic approaches to treatment. Certainly there are no generic treat-
ment models that work for all people equally well. Therapists need to re-
spond to a myriad of nonspecific factors that clients present in order to
250 CHAPTER 6
course of therapy. Given the interpersonal style of the group, this outpa-
tient program had to build in techniques that would address the blustery
yet insecure and untrusting interpersonal nature of the group.
A second value of these types of data is that they allow clinicians to cre-
ate local norms for their psychological instruments. Knowing the overall
population characteristics, it becomes useful to evaluate each new client in
252 CHAPTER 6
reference to these qualities. Yes, these clients are suspicious and non trust-
ing, but how does this new client compare on these dimensions relative to
others who have come through the program? Is he or she more detached or
less so? These specific questions provide more texture and nuance to psy-
chological evaluations. Developing local norms allows for a more fine-
grained analysis of personality as it applies in a particular treatment context.
The final value of aggregate data works out of the first two: It enables
therapists to conduct a needs assessment of their clients. Given the overall
portrait of the clients, what are the needs they bring to treatment? Certainly,
the overall profile presented here indicates individuals in need of several
things. Interpersonally, they need to learn how to better relate to others; to
be able to initiate and maintain emotionally satisfying relationships. People
cannot always be seen as threatening objects, but rather as possible sources
of support and encouragement. Intrapersonallr- these individuals need to
create better self-images and to develop more efficient coping skills. They
need to become less impulsive and more self-controlled. Treatment with
these individuals needs to proceed on multiple levels simultaneously.
Group data can provide a useful summary statement of the personal-
ity qualities in one's treatment population. Such information can certainly
inform treatment selection and identify potential areas of intervention. The
use of group data can be extended to reflect different subgroups as well.
For example, it may be possible to identify normatively those who re-
sponded well to treatment and those who did not. These aggregated pro-
files can be used in two ways. First, the personality differences between the
two groups can be plotted and interpreted for its clinical significance. Why
do clients high on dimension X and low on dimension Y do poorly, while
those with the opposite pattern do well? Is it something about our center,
or therapist, or treatment modality?
A second way that these group profiles can be used is in the selection
of clients. Some programs, like the substance abuse treatment program dis-
cussed earlier, have limited resources and must evaluate prospective
clients prior to accepting them for treatment. The effort is to identify those
who appear most likely to benefit from the intervention. In such a situa-
tion, the profiles of new clients can then be compared to these aggregated
norms of those who successfully completed the program. The degree of fit
can be used as part of the selection process.
CONCLUSIONS
ments. Rather, one should consider the five-factor model the first step of
scientific inquiry: the accurate description of personality. By providing a
common language for talking about personality-related phenomena, we
lay the foundation for the development of more precise theories and more
powerful empirical tests of the hypotheses these theories generate.
REFERENCES
Aldenderfer, M. S., & Blashfield, R. K. (1984). Cluster analysis. Beverly Hills, CA:
Sage.
Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation. New York: Holt.
Allport, G. W. (1961). Pattern and growth in personality. London: Holt, Rinehart &
Winston.
Allport, G. w., & Odbert, H. S. (1936). Trait names: A psycho-lexical study. Psycho-
logical Monographs, 47(1, Whole No. 211).
Almagor, M., Tellegen, A., & Waller, N. G. (1995). The big seven model: Across-cul-
tural replication and further exploration of the basic dimensions of natural lan-
guage trait descriptors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 300-307.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Andrews, F. M., & Withey, S. B. (1976). Social indicators ofwell-being: Americans' per-
ceptions oflife quality. New York: Plenum.
Arbisi, P. A, & Ben-Porath, Y. S. (1995). An MMPI-2 infrequent response scale for
use with a psychopathological populations: The infrequency-psychopathol-
ogy scale, F(p). Psychological Assessment, 7,424-431.
Avia, M. D., Sanz, }., Sanchez-Bemardos, M. L., Martinez-Arias, M. R., Silva, E, &
Grana, J. L. (1995). The five-factor model-II. Relations of the NEO-PI with
other personality variables. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 81-97.
Bagby, R. M., Joffe, R. T., Parker, J. D. A, Kalemba, Y., & Harkness, V. (1995). Major
depression and the five-factor model of personality. Journal of Personality Dis-
orders, 9, 224-234.
Bailey, K. D. (1994). Typologies and taxonomies: An introduction to classification tech-
niques. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence: Isolation and communion in Western
man. Boston: Beacon.
Bandura, A (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. American Psychologist,
44,1175-1184.
255
256 REFERENCES
Barlow, D. H., Hayes, S. c., & Nelson, R O. (1984). The scientist-practitioner: Research
and accountability in clinical and educational settings. New York: Pergamon.
Barrick, M. R, & Mount, M. K. (1991). The Big Five personality dimensions and job
performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44,1-26.
Barry, W. A. (1970). Marriage research and conflict: An integrative review. Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 73, 41-54.
Ben-Porath, Y. S., & Waller, N. G. (1992). Five big issues in clinical assessment: Are-
joinder to Costa and McCrae. Psychological Assessment, 4, 23-25.
Bendig, A. W. (1958). Comparative validity of objective and projective measures of
need achievement in predicting students' achievement in introductory psy-
chology. Journal of General Psychology, 59, 51-57.
Benet, v., & Waller, N. G. (1995). The Big Seven factor model of personality de-
scription: Evidence for its cross-cultural generality in a Spanish sample. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 701-718.
Benson, P. L., Donahue, M. J., & Erickson, J. A. (1993). The faith maturity scale: Con-
ceptualization, measurement, and empirical validation. Research in the Social
Scientific Study of Religion, 5, 1-26.
Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the
analysis of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588-606.
Bergeman, C. S., Chipuer, H. M., Plomin, R, Pedersen, N. L., McClearn, G. E., Nes-
selroade, J. R, Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R R (1993). Genetic and environmen-
tal effects on Openness to Experience, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness:
An adoption/twin study. Journal ofPersonality, 61, 159-179.
Berne, S. L. (1961). Transactional analysis in psychotherapy. New York: Grove.
Block, J. (1965). The challenge of response sets: Unconfounding meaning, acquiescence,
and social desirability in the MMPI. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Block, J. (1971). Lives through time. Berkeley, CA: Bancroft Books.
Block, J. (1995). A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to personality de-
scription. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 187-215.
Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1980). The role of ego-control and ego-resiliency in the or-
ganization of behavior. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), Minnesota symposia on child psy-
chology (pp. 39-101). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bond, M. H. (1979). Dimensions used in perceiving peers: Cross-cultural compar-
isons of Hong Kong, Japanese, American, and Filipino university students. In-
ternational Journal of Psychology, 14,47-56.
Bond, M. H., Nakazato, H., & Shiraishi, D. (1975). Universality and distinctiveness
in dimensions of Japanese person perception. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psy-
chology, 6, 346-357.
Borkenau, P. (1992). Implicit personality theory and the five-factor model. Journal
of Personality, 60, 295-327.
Borkenau, P., & Liebler, A. (1992). Trait inferences: Sources of validity at zero ac-
quaintanceship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 645-657.
Borkenau, P., & Liebler, A. (1993). Convergence of stranger ratings of personality
and intelligence with self-ratings, partner ratings, and measured intelligence.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65,546-553.
REFERENCES 257
Borkenau, P., & Ostendorf, F. (1990). Comparing exploratory and confirmatory fac-
tor analysis: A study on the 5-factor model of personality. Personality and Indi-
vidual Differences, 11, 515-524.
Botwin, M. (1995). Review of the NEO PI-R In J. Conoley & J. Impara (Eds.), Men-
tal measurement yearbook (12th ed., pp. 862-863). Lincoln: University of Ne-
braska Press.
Bouchard, T. J. (1968). Convergent and discriminant validity of the Adjective Check
List and the Edwards Personal Preference Schedule. Educational and Psycho-
logical Measurement, 28, 1165-1171.
Bouchard, T. J., Jr., & McGue, M. (1990). Genetic and rearing environmental influ-
ence on adult personality: An analysis of adopted twins reared apart. Journal
of Personality, 58, 263-292.
Boyle, G. J. (1989). Re-examination of the major personality-type factors in the Cat-
tell, Comrey, and Eysenck scales: Were the factor solutions by Noller et al. op-
timal? Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 1289-1299.
Brooner, R K, Herbst, J. H., Schmidt, C. W., Bigelow, G. E., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1993).
Antisocial personality disorder among drug abusers: Relations to other per-
sonality diagnoses and the five-factor model of personality. Journal of Nervous
and Mental Disease, 181,313-319.
Buros,o. K (Ed.). (1978). Eighth mental measurements yearbook (Vol. 1). Highland
Park, NJ: Gryphon.
Buss, D. M. (1984). Marital assortment for personality dispOSitions: Assessment
with three different data sources. Behavior Genetics, 14, 111-123.
Buss, D. M. (1991a). Conflict in married couples: Personality predictors of anger
and upset. Journal of Personality, 59, 663-688.
Buss, D. M. (1991b). Evolutionary personality psychology. In M. Rosensweig & L.
Porter (Eds.), Annual review of psychology (pp. 459-451). Palo Alto, CA: Annual
Reviews.
Buss, D. M. (1992). Manipulation in close relationships: Five personality factors in
interactional context. Journal of Personality, 60, 477-499.
Buss, D. M., & Craik, K H. (1980). The frequency concept of disposition: Domi-
nance and prototypically dominant acts. Journal of Personality, 48, 379-392.
Buss, D. M., & Craik, K H. (1983). The act frequency approach to personality. Psy-
chological Review, 90, 105-126.
Butcher, J. N., & Rouse, S. V. (1996). Personality: Individual differences and clini-
cal assessment. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 87-111.
Butcher, J. N., Dahlstrom, W. G., Graham, J. R, Tellegen, A., & Kaemmer, B. (1989).
MMPI 2: Manual for administration and scoring. Minneapolis: University of
Minnesota Press.
Cantor, N. (1990). From thought to behavior: "Having" and "doing" in the study of
personality and cognition. American Psychologist, 45, 735-750.
Cantor, N., Norem, J., Langston, c., Zirkel, S., Fleeson, W., & Cook-Flannagan, C.
(1991). Life tasks and daily life experience. Journal of Personality, 59, 425-451.
Cantril, H. (1965). The pattern ofhuman concerns. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers Uni-
versity Press.
258 REFERENCES
Capara, G. V., Barbaranelli, c., Borgogni, L., & Perugini, M. (1993). The "Big Five
Questionnaire": Anew questionnaire to assess the five-factor model. Person-
ality and Individual Differences, 15,281-288.
Capara, G. v., Barbaranelli, c., & Comrey, A. L. (1995). Factor analysis of the NEO-
PI inventory and the Comrey Personality Scales in an Italian sample. Person-
ality and Individual Differences, 18, 193-200.
Carmichael, C. M., & McGue, M. (1994). A longitudinal family study of personality
change and stability. Journal of Personality, 62, 1-20.
Caspi, A., & Bern, D. J. (1990). Personality continuity and change across the life
course. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality theory and research (pp.
549-575). New York: Guilford.
Cattell, R. B. (1933). Temperament tests: II. Test. British Journal of Psychology, 23,
308-329.
Cattell, R B. (1944). Interpretation of the twelve primary personality factors. Char-
acter and Personality, 13, 55-91.
Cattell, R B. (1947). Confirmation and clarification of the primary personality fac-
tors. Psychometrika, 12, 55-91.
Cattell, R B. (1948). The primary personality factors in women compared with
those in men. British Journal of Psychology, 38, 114-130.
Cattell, R B. (1994). Constancy of global, second-order personality factors over a
twenty-year-plus period. Psychological Reports, 75, 3-9.
Chae, J-H, Piedmont, R. L., Estadt, B. K., & Wicks, R. J. (1995). Personological eval-
uation of Clance's Impostor Phenomenon Scale in a Korean sample. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 65,468-485.
Chen, M. C. (1996). Psychosocial correlates ofprosocial behavior among college students
in Taiwan. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Loyola College, Baltimore.
Chen, P. Y., Dai, T., Spector, P. E., & lex, s. M. (1997). Relation between negative af-
fectivity and positive affectivity: Effects of judged desirability of scale items
and respondents' social desirability. Journal of Personality Assessment, 69,
183-198.
Church, A. T., & Burke, P. J. (1994). Exploratory and confirmatory tests of the Big
Five and Tellegen's three- and four-dimensional models. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 66, 93-114.
Clance, P. R (1985). The impostor phenomenon: Overcoming the fear that haunts your
success. Atlanta, GA: Peachtree.
Clance, P. R., & Imes, S. A. (1978). The impostor phenomenon in high achieving
women: Dynamics and therapeutic intervention. Psychotherapy Theory, Re-
search and Practice, 15,241-247.
Clark, L. A. (1990). Toward a consensual set of symptom clusters for assessment of
personality disorder. In J. Butcher & C. Spielberger (Eds.), Advances in person-
ality assessment (Vol. 8, pp. 243-266). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cloninger, C. R. (1987). Asystematic method for clinical description and classifica-
tion of personality disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 573-588.
Cloninger, C. R, Svrakic, D. M., & Przybeck, T. R. (1993). Apsychobiological model
of temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975-990.
REFERENCES 259
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the be-
havioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cohen, S., Doyle, W. J., Skoner, D. P., Fireman, P., Gwaltney, J. M., Jr., & Newsom,
J. T. (1995). State and trait negative affect as predictors of objective and sub-
jective symptoms of respiratory viral infections. Journal of Personality and So-
cial Psychology, 68, 159-169.
Colvin, C. R, & Funder, D. C. (1991). Predicting personality and behavior: A
boundary on the acquaintanceship effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 60, 884-894.
Costa, P. T., Jr. (1996). Work and Personality: Use of the NEO PI-R in Industrial/Or-
ganizational psychology. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 45,
225-241.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R R (1980a). Influence of extraversion and neuroticism
on subjective well-being: Happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 38, 668-678.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R R (1980b). Somatic complaints in males as a function
of age and neuroticism: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Behavioral Medicine,
3,245-257.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R R (1984). Personality as a lifelong determinant of
well-being. In C. Z. Malatesta & c. E. Izard (Eds.), Emotion in adult development
(pp. 141-157). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R R (1985). Concurrent validation after 20 years: The
implications of personality stability for assessment. In J. N. Butcher & c. D.
Spielberger (Eds.), Advances in personality assessment (pp. 31-54). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R R (1986). Age, personality, and the Holtzman inkblot
technique. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 23, 115-125.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R R (1987). Neuroticism, somatic complaints, and dis-
ease: Is the bark worse than the bite? Journal of Personality, 55, 299-316.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R R (1988a). From catalogue to classification: Murray's
needs and the five-factor model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55,
258-265.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R R (1988b). Personality in adulthood: A six-year lon-
gitudinal study of self-reports and spouse ratings on the NEO Personality In-
ventory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 853-863.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R (1989a). Personality continuity and the changes of
adult life. In M. Storandt & G. R VandenBos (Eds.), The adult years: Continuity
and change (pp. 45-77). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R R (1989b). Personality, stress, and coping: Some
lessons from a decade of research. In K. S. Markides & c. L. Cooper (Eds.),
Aging, stress, and health (pp. 269-285). New York: Wiley.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R R (1990). Personality disorders and the five-factor
model. Journal of Personality Disorders, 4, 362-371.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R R (1992a). Normal personality assessment in clinical
practice: The NEO Personality Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 4, 5-13.
260 REFERENCES
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R R (1992b). Reply to Eysenck. Personality and Individ-
ual Differences, 13,861-865.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R R (1992c). Revised NED Personality Inventory: Profes-
sional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R (1992d). Trait psychology comes of age. In T. B. Son-
deregger (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation: Psychology and aging (pp.
169-204). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R R (1994). "Set like plaster"? Evidence for the stability
of adult personality. In T. F. Heatherton & J. L. Weinberger (Eds.), Can person-
ality change? Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R R (1995a). Domains and facets: Hierarchical person-
ality assessment using the revised NEO Personality Inventory. Journal of Per-
sonality Assessment, 64, 21-50.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1995b). Primary traits of Eysenck's P-E-N system:
Three- and five-factor solutions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69,
308-317.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R R (in press). The revised NEO Personality Inventory
(NEO-PI-R). In S. R. Briggs & J. Cheek (Eds.), Personality measures (Vol. 1).
Greenwich, CT: JAI.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & Widiger T. A. (1994). Personality disorders and thefive-factor model of
personality. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Costa, P. T., Jr., McCrae, R. R, & Norris, A. H. (1981). Personal adjustment to aging:
Longitudinal prediction from neuroticism and extraversion. Journal of Geron-
tology, 36, 78-85.
Costa, P. T., Jr., McCrae, R R, & Holland, J. L. (1984). Personality and vocational in-
terests in an adult sample. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, 390-400.
Costa, P. T., Jr., Zonderman, A. B., McCrae, R R, & Williams, R B., Jr. (1985). Con-
tent and comprehensiveness in the MMPI: An item factor analysis in a normal
adult sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 925-933.
Costa, P. T., Jr., Busch, C. M., Zonderman, A. B., & McCrae, R R. (1986). Correla-
tions of MMPI factor scales with measures of the five-factor model of person-
ality. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50, 640-650.
Costa, P. T., Jr., McCrae, R. R, & Zonderman, A. B. (1987). Environmental and dis-
positional influences on well-being: Longitudinal follow-up of an American
national sample. British Journal of Psychology, 78, 299-306.
Costa, P. T., Jr., Zonderman, A. B., McCrae, R. R, Comoni-Huntley, J., Locke, B.
Z., & Barbano, H. E. (1987). Longitudinal analyses of psychological well-
being in a national sample: Stability of mean levels. Journal of Gerontology,
42,50-55.
Costa, P. T., Jr., McCrae, R R, & Dye, D. A. (1991). Facet scales for Agreeableness
and Conscientiousness: A revision of the NED Personality Inventory. Person-
ality and Individual Differences, 12, 887-898.
Costa, P. T., Jr., Fagan, P. J., Piedmont, R. 1., Ponticas, Y., & Wise, T. N. (1992). The
five-factor model of personality and sexual functioning in outpatient men and
women. Psychiatric Medicine, 10, 199-215.
Creamer, M., & Campbell, I. M. (1988). The role of interpersonal perception in
dyadic adjustment. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 424-430.
REFERENCES 261
Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). Anew scale of social desirability independent
of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349-354.
Csarny, R. J. (1997). The incremental validity of religious constructs in predicting quality
of life, racism, and sexual attitudes. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Loyola
College, Baltimore.
Deniston, W. M., & Ramanaiah, N. V. (1993). California Psychological Inventory
and the five-factor model. Psychological Reports, 73,491-496.
Derogatis, L. R. (1993). Brief Symptom Inventory, manual. Minneapolis, MN: National
Computer Systems.
Digman, J. M. (1972). The structure of child personality as seen in behavior ratings.
In R. Dreger (Ed.), Multivariate personality research (pp. 587-611). Baton Rouge,
LA: Claitor's.
Digman, J. M. (1989). Five robust trait dimensions: Development, stability, and util-
ity. Journal of Personality, 57, 195-214.
Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model.
Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 417-440.
Digman, J. M. (1996). The curious history of the five-factor model. In J. S. Wiggins
(Ed.). Thefive-factor model ofpersonality. New York: Guilford.
Digman, J. M., & Takemoto-Chock, N. K. (1981). Factors in the natural language of
personality: Re-analysis, comparison, and interpretation of six major studies.
Multivariate Behavioral Research, 16, 149-170.
Duijsens, 1. J., & Diekstra, R. F. W. (1996). DSM-III-R and ICD-lO personality disor-
ders and their relationship with the big five dimensions of personality. Per-
sonality and Individual Differences, 21, 119-133.
Edwards, A. L. (1957). The social desirability variable in personality assessment and re-
search. New York: Dryden.
Edwards, A. L. (1959). Edwards Personal Preference Schedule manual. New York: Psy-
chological Corp.
Edwards, A. L., & Heathers, L. B. (1962). The first factor of the MMPI: Social desir-
ability or ego strength. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 26, 99-100.
Edwards. J. E., & Waters, L. K. (1983). Predicting university attrition: A replication
and extension. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 43, 233-236.
Ellenberger, H. (1970). The discovery of the unconscious. New York: Basic Books.
Ellison, C. W. (1983). Spiritual well-being: Conceptualization and measurement.
Journal of Psychology and Theology, 11, 330-340.
Entwisle, D. R. (1972). To dispel fantasies about fantasy-based measures of achieve-
ment motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 77, 377-39l.
Eysenck, H. J. (1991). Dimensions of personality: 16,5, or 3?-Criteria for a taxo-
nomic paradigm. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 773-790.
Eysenck, H. J. (1992). Four ways five factors are not basic. Personality and Individual
Differences, 13,667-673.
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, M.W. (1985). Personality and individual differences. Lon-
don, Plenum.
Fagan, P. J., Wise, T. N., Schmidt, C. W., Ponticas, Y., Marshall, R. D., & Costa, P. T.,
Jr. (1991). A comparison of five-factor personality dimensions in males with
sexual dysfunction and males with paraphilia. Journal of Personality Assess-
ment, 57, 434-448.
262 REFERENCES
Helson, R., & Moane, G. (1987). Personality change in women from college to mid-
life. Journal at Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 176-186.
Hershberger, S. L., Plomin, R., & Pedersen, N. L. (1995). Traits and metatraits: Their
reliability, stability, and shared genetic influence. Journal at Personality and So-
cial Psychology, 69, 673-685.
Hirschfeld, R. M. A., Klerman, G. L., Clayton, P., Keller, M. B., McDonald-Scott, P.,
& Larkin, B. (1983). Assessing personality: Effects of depressive state on trait
measurement. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 695-699.
Hofer, S. M., Hom, J. L., & Eber, H. W. (1997). A robust five-factor structure of the
16PF: Strong evidence from independent rotation and confirmatory factorial
invariance procedures. Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 247-269.
Hofstee, W. K. B., de Raad, D., & Goldberg, L. R. (1992). Integration of the Big Five
and circumplex approaches to trait structure. Journal at Personality and Social
Psychology, 63, 146-163.
Hogan, R. (1991). Personality and personality measurement. In M. D. Dunnette &
L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook at industrial and organizational psychology (2nd
ed., pp. 873-919). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Hogan, R., & Hogan, J. (1989). How to measure employee reliability. Journal at Ap-
plied Psychology, 74, 273-279.
Hogan, R., & Hogan, J. (1992). Hogan Personality Inventory, manual. Tulsa, OK:
Hogan Assessment Systems.
Holden, R. R. (1992). Associations between the Holden Psychological Screening In-
ventoryand the NEO Five-Factor Inventory in a nonclinical sample. Psycho-
logical Reports, 71, 1039-1042.
Hood, R. W. (1975). The construction and preliminary validation of a measure of
reported mystical experience. Journal for the Scientific Study at Religion, 14,
29-41.
Horner, M. S. (1968). Sex differences in achievement motivation and performance in com-
petitive and non-competitive situations. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Uni-
versity of Michigan, Ann Arbor.
Homer, M. S. (1972). Toward an understanding of achievement-related conflicts in
women. Journal atSocial Issues, 28,157-175.
Horney, K. (1950). Neurosis and human growth. New York: Norton.
Horowitz, L. M. (1996). The study of interpersonal problems: A Leary legacy. Jour-
nal at Personality Assessment, 66, 283-300.
Hough, L. M. (1992). The "big five" personality variables-Construct confusion:
Description versus prediction. Human Performance,S, 139-155.
Hough, L. M., Eaton, N. K., Dunnette, M. D., Kamp, J. D., & McCloy, R. A. (1990).
Criterion-related validities of personality constructs and the effect of response
distortion on those validities. Journal at Applied Psychology, 75,581-595.
Hsu, L. M. (1986). Implications of differences in elevations of K-corrected and non-
K-corrected MMPI T scores. Journal at Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54,
552-557.
Isaka, H. (1990). Factor analysis of trait terms in everyday Japanese language. Per-
sonality and Individual Differences, 11, 115-124.
REFERENCES 265
Jackson, D. N. (1989). Basic Personality Inventory manual. Port Huron, MI: Sigma As-
sessment Systems.
Jackson, D. N., Aston, M. c., & Tomes, J. L. (1996). The six-factor model of person-
ality: Facets from the big five. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 391-402.
Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., & Vernon, P. A. (1996a). The genetic basis of personality
at different ages: Across-sectional twin study. Personality and Individual Differ-
ences, 21, 299-30l.
Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., & Vernon, P. A. (1996b). Heritability of the Big Five per-
sonality dimensions and their facets: A twin study. Journal of Personality, 64,
577-591.
Jessor, R. (1983). The stability of change: Psychosocial development from adoles-
cence to young adulthood. In D. Magnusson & V. L. Allen (Eds.), Human de-
velopment: An interactional perspective (pp. 321-341). New York: Academic
Press.
John, O. P. (1990). The "Big Five" factor taxonomy: Dimensions of personality in
the natural language and in questionnaires. In L. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook ofper-
sonality theory and research (pp. 66-100). New York: Guilford.
John, O. P., & Robbins, R. W. (1993). Determinants of inter-judge agreement on per-
sonality traits: The Big Five domains, observability, evaluativeness, and the
unique perspective of the self. Journal of Personality, 61, 521-551.
John, O. P., Goldberg, L. R., & Angleitner, A. (1984). Better than the alphabet: Tax-
onomies of personality-descriptive terms in English, Dutch, and German. In
J. J. c. Bonarius, G. L. M. van Heck, & N. G. Smid (Eds.), Personality psychology
in Europe: Theoretical and empirical developments (pp. 83-100). Lisse, Switzer-
land: Swets & Zeitlinger.
John, O. P., Angleitner, A., & Ostendorf, F. (1988). The lexical approach to person-
ality: A historical review of the trait taxonomic research. European Journal of
Personality, 2, 171-203.
Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: The attribution process
in person perception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy-
chology (Vol. 2, pp. 78-103). New York: Academic Press.
Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1972). The actor and the observer: Divergent percep-
tions of the causes of behavior. In E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E.
Nisbett, S. Valins, and B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the causes ofbe-
havior (pp. 79-94). Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.
Joreskog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. (1993). LISREL 8 user's reference guide. Chicago: Sci-
entific Software International.
Juni, S. (1995). Review of the NEO PI-R. In J. Conoley & J. Impara (Eds.), Mental
measurement yearbook (12th ed., pp. 863-868). Lincoln: University of Nebraska
Press.
Kaiser, R. T., & Ozer, D. J. (1997). Emotional stability and goal-related stress. Per-
sonality and Individual Differences, 22, 371-379.
Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1995). The longitudinal course of marital quality
and stability: A review of theory, method, and research. Psychological Bulletin,
118,3-34.
266 REFERENCES
Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1997). Neuroticism, marital interaction, and the
trajectory of marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72,
1075-1092.
Katigbak, M. S., Church, A. T., & Akamine, T. X. (1996). Cross-cultural generaliz-
ability of personality dimensions: Relating indigenous and imported di-
mensions in two cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70,
99-114.
Kaufman, A S. (1975). Factor analysis of the WISC-R at 11 age levels between 6%
and 161h years. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43,135-147.
Kaufman, L., & Rousseeuw, P. J. (1990). Finding groups in data: An introduction to
cluster analysis. New York: Wiley.
Keller, M. c., Thiessen D., & Young, R. K. (1996). Mate assortment in dating and
married couples. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 217-221.
Kosek, R. B. (1996a). Criss-cross ratings of the Big Five personality dimensions as an
index ofmarital satisfaction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Loyola College,
Baltimore.
Kosek, R. B. (1996b). The quest for a perfect spouse: Spousal ratings and marital
satisfaction. Psychological Reports, 79, 731-735.
Kozma, A, & Stones, M. J. (1987). Social desirability in measures of subjective well-
being: Asystematic evaluation. Journal of Gerontology, 42, 56-59.
Lanning, K. (1994). Dimensionality of observer ratings on the California adult Q-
Set. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 151-160.
Larsen, D. L., Attkisson, C. c., Hargreaves, W. A, & Nguyen, T. D. (1979). Assess-
ment of client/patient satisfaction: Development of a general scale. Evaluation
and Program Planning, 2, 197-207.
Larson, R. (1978). Thirty years of research on the subjective well-being of older
Americans. Journal of Gerontology, 33,109-125.
Leary, T. (1957). Interpersonal diagnosis ofpersonality. New York: Ronald.
Lester, D., Haig, c., & Monello, R. (1989). Spouses' personality and marital satis-
faction. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 253-254.
Levin, J., & Montag, I. (1991). Relationship between the Basic Personality Inventory
and the NEO-Personality Inventory in a nonpatient sample. Psychological Re-
ports, 69, 1176-1178.
Levin, J., & Montag, I. (1994). The five factor model and psychopathology in non-
clinical samples. Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 1-7.
Linehan, M. M., & Nielsen, S. L. (1983). Social desirability: Its relevance to the mea-
surement of hopelessness and suicidal behavior. Journal ofConsuiting and Clin-
ical Psychology, 51,141-143.
Livesley, W. J., West, M., & Tanney, A (1985). Historical comment on the DSM-III
schizoid and avoidant personality disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry,
142,1344-1347.
Livesley, W. J., Jackson, D. N., & Schroeder, M. L. (1992). Factorial structure of traits
delineating personality disorders in clinical and general population samples.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 432-440.
Livesley, W. J., Schroeder, M. L., Jackson; D. N., & Jang, K. L. (1994). Categorical dis-
tinctions in the study of personality disorder: Implications for classification.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 6-17.
REFERENCES 267
Locke, H. J., & Wallace, K. M. (1959). Short marital-adjustment and prediction tests:
Their reliability and validity. Marriage and Family Living, 21, 251-255.
Loevinger, J. (1994). Has psychology lost its conscience? Journal of Personality As-
sessment, 62, 2-8.
Lorr, M. (1996). The Interpersonal Circle as a heuristic model for interpersonal re-
search. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66, 234-239.
Lorr, M., & Strack, S. (1993). Some NEO PI five-factor personality profiles. Journal
of Personality Assessment, 60,91-99.
Lorr, M., Youniss, R P., & Kluth, C. (1992). The Interpersonal Style Inventory and
the five-factor model. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 48, 202-206.
Lubin, B., Larsen, R. M., & Matarazzo, J. D. (1984). Patterns of psychological test
usage in the United States: 1935-1982. American Psychologist, 39, 451-454.
Lyons, J. 5., Howard, K. I., O'Mahoney, M. T., & Ush, J. D. (1997). The measurement
and management of clinical outcomes in mental health. New York: Wiley.
Marsh, H. w., Balla, J. R, & McDonald, R P. (1988). Goodness-of-fit indexes in con-
firmatory factor analysis: The effect of sample size. Psychological Bulletin, 103,
391-410.
Marshall, G. N., Wortman, C. 8., Vickers, R R, Jr., Kusulas, J. W., & Hervig, L. K.
(1994). The five-factor model of personality as a framework for personality-
health research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 278-286.
McAdams, D. P. (1992). The five-factor model in personality: A critical appraisal.
Journal of Personality, 60, 329-361.
McClelland, D. c., Atkinson, J. w., Clark, R w., & Lowell, E. L. (1953). The achieve-
ment motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
McCrae, R R (1982). Consensual validation of personality traits: Evidence from
self-reports and ratings. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 43, 292-303.
McCrae, R R (1986). Well-being scales do not measure social desirability. Journal of
Gerontology, 41, 390-393.
McCrae, R R (1989). Why I advocate the five-factor model: Joint factor analyses of
the NEO-PI with other instruments. In D. Buss & N. Cantor (Eds.), Personal-
ity psychology: Recent trends and emerging directions (pp. 237-245). New York:
Springer-Verlag.
McCrae, R R (1990). Traits and trait names: How well is Openness represented in
natural languages? European Journal of Personality, 4, 119-129.
McCrae, R R (1993). Moderated analyses of longitudinal personality stability. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 577-585.
McCrae, R R (1993-1994). Openness to experience as a basic dimension of per-
sonality. In K. S. Pope & J. L. Singer (Eds.), Imagination, cognition, and personal-
ity: Consciousness in theory-research-clinical practice (Vol. 13, pp. 39-55).
Amityville, NY: Baywook.
McCrae, R R (1994a). The counterpoint of personality assessment: Self-reports and
observer ratings. Assessment, 1, 151-164.
McCrae, R R. (1994b). Openness to experience: Expanding the boundaries of fac-
tor V. European Journal of Personality, 8, 251-272.
McCrae, R. R. (1994c). Psychopathology from the perspective of the five-factor
model. In S. Strack & M. Lorr (Eds.), Differentiating normal and abnormal per-
sonality (pp. 26-39). New York: Springer.
268 REFERENCES
McCrae, R R, & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1983a). Psychological maturity and subjective
well-being: Toward a new synthesis. Developmental Psychology, 19, 243-248.
McCrae, R R, & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1983b). Social desirability scales: More substance
than style. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 882-888.
McCrae, R R, & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1985a). Comparison of EPI and Psychoticism scales
with measures of the five-factor model of personality. Personality and Individ-
ual Differences, 6, 587-597.
McCrae, R. R, & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1985b). Openness to experience. In R Hogan & W. H.
Jones (Eds.), Perspectives in personality <Vol. I, pp. 145-172). Greenwich, CT: JAI.
McCrae, R R, & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1987). Validation of the five-factor model of per-
sonality across instruments and observers. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 52, 81-90.
McCrae, R R, & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1988). Psychological resilience among widowed
men and women: A 10-year follow-up of a national sample. Journal of Social Is-
sues, 44,129-142.
McCrae, R R, & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1989a). Reinterpreting the Myers-Briggs Type In-
dicator from the perspective of the five-factor model of personality. Journal of
Personality, 57, 17-40.
McCrae R R, & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1989b). Rotation to maximize the construct valid-
ity of factors in the NED Personality Inventory. Multivariate Behavioral Re-
search, 57, 17-40.
McCrae, R R, & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1989c). The structure of interpersonal traits: Wig-
gins's circumplex and the five-factor model. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 56, 586-595.
McCrae, R R, & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1992). Discriminant validity of the NED-PIR facet
scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 52, 229-237.
McCrae, R R, & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1994). Does Lorr's Interpersonal Style Inventory
measure the five-factor model? Personality and Individual Differences, 16,
195-197.
McCrae, R R, & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1995). Positive and negative valence within the
five-factor model. Journal of Research in Personality, 29, 443-460.
McCrae, R R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1996). Toward a new generation of personality the-
ories: Theoretical contexts for the five-factor model. In J. S. Wiggins (Ed.), The
five-factor model of personality: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 51-87). New York:
Guilford.
McCrae, R R, & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human uni-
versal. American Psychologist, 52, 509-516.
McCrae, R R, & John, 0. P. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor model and its
applications. Journal of Personality, 60, 175-215.
McCrae, R R, Costa, P. T., Jr., & Busch, C. M. (1986). Evaluating comprehensive-
ness in personality systems: The California Q-Set and the five factor model.
Journal of Personality, 54, 430-446.
McCrae, R R, Costa, P. T., Jr., Dahlstrom, W. G., Barefoot, J. c., Siegler, I. c., &
Williams, R B., Jr. (1989). A caution on the use of the MMPI K-correction in re-
search on psychosomatic medicine. Psychosomatic Bulletin, 51, 58-65.
REFERENCES 269
McCrae, R. R., Costa, 1'. T., Jr., & Piedmont, R. L. (1993). Folk concepts, natural lan-
guage, and psychological constructs: The California Personality Inventory
and the five-factor model. Journal of Personality, 61, 1-26.
McCrae, R. R., Costa, 1'. T., Jr., Piedmont, R. L., Chae, J-H, Caprara, G. v., Barbaranelli,
c., Marusic, 1, & Bratko, D. (1996, November). Personality development from col-
lege to mid-lifo: A cross-cultural comparison. Paper presented at the 49th annual
meeting of the Gerontological Society of America, Washington, DC.
McCrae, R. R., Costa, 1'. T., Jr., Stone, S. v., & Fagan, P. J. (1996, August). Some reasons
for disagreement between spouses in personality assessment. Poster presented at
the 104th annual convention of the American Psychological Association,
Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
McCrae, R. R., Zonderman, A. B., Costa, P. T., Jr., Bond, M. H., & Paunonen, S. v.
(1996). Evaluating replicability of factors in the Revised NED Personality In-
ventory: Confirmatory factor analysis and Procrustes rotation. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 70, 552-566.
Meehl,1'. E. (1978). Theoretical risks and tabular asterisks: Sir Karl, Sir Ronald, and
the slow progress of soft psychology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol-
ogy, 46, 806-834.
Megargee, E. 1 (1972). The California Psychological Inventory handbook. San Francisco:
Jossey-Bass.
Megargee, E. I., & Parker, G. V. C. (1968). An exploration of the equivalent of Mur-
rayian needs as assessed by the Adjective Check List, the TAT, and Edwards
Personal Preference Schedule. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 24, 47-51.
Miller, T. (1991). The psychotherapeutic utility of the five-factor model of person-
ality: A clinician's experience. Journal of Personality Assessment, 57, 415-433.
Millon, T. (1990). Towards a new personology: An evolutionary model. New York: Wiley.
Mischel, W. (1968). Personality assessment. New York: Wiley.
Mooradian, T. A., & Nezlek, J. B. (1996). Comparing the NEO-FFI and Saucier's
mini-markers as measures of the Big Five. Personality and Individual Differ-
ences, 21, 213-215.
Mount, M. K., & Barrick, M. R. (1995). The big five personality dimensions: Impli-
cations for research and practice in human resources management. Research in
Personnel and Human Resources Management, 13, 153-200.
Mount, M. K., Barrick, M. R., & Strauss, J. 1'. (1994). Validity of observer ratings of
the Big Five personality factors. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 272-280.
Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in personality. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Muten, E. (1991). Self-reports, spouse ratings, and psychophysiological assessment
in a behavioral medicine program: An application of the five-factor model.
Journal ofPersonality Assessment, 57, 574-583.
Myers, I. B., & McCaulley, M. H. (1985). A guide to the development and use of the
Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Narayanan, L., Shanker, M., & Levine, E. L. (1995). Personality structure: A culture-
specific examination of the five-factor model. Journal of Personality Assessment,
64,51-62.
270 REFERENCES
Rodgerson, T. E. (1994). The relation between situation, personality, and religious prob-
lem-solving in the prediction of burnout among American Baptist clergy. Unpub-
lished doctoral dissertation, Loyola College, Baltimore.
Russell, R. J. H, & Wells, P. A (1994). Predictors of happiness in married couples.
Personality and Individual Differences, 17,313-321.
Saklofske, D. H., Kelly, I. W., & Janzen, B. L. (1995). Neuroticism, depression, and
depression proneness. Personality and Individual Differences, 18, 27-3l.
Saucier, G. (1994). Mini-markers: A brief version of Goldberg's unipolar Big-Five
markers. Journal of Personality Assessment, 63, 506-516.
Scheier, M. E, Buss, A H, & Buss, D. M. (1976). Self-consciousness, self-report of
aggressiveness, and aggression. Journal of Research in Personality, 44, 637-644.
Schinka, J. A (1985). Personal Problems Check Listfor Adults. Odessa, FL: Psycholog-
ical Assessment Resources.
Schmeck, R. R., & Grove, E. (1979). Academic achievement and individual differ-
ences in the learning process. Applied Psychological Measurement, 3, 43-49.
Sch6nemann, P. H. (1966). A generalized solution of the orthogonal Procrustes
problem. Psychometrika, 31, 1-10.
Schroeder, D. H, & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1984). Influence of life event stress on physical
illness: Substantive effects or methodological flaws? Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 46, 853-863.
Schroeder, M. L., Wormworth, J. A, & Livesley, W. J. (1992). Dimensions of per-
sonality disorder and their relationships to the Big Five dimensions of per-
sonality. Psychological Assessment, 4, 47-53.
Schinka, J. A, Kinder, B. N., and Kremer, T. (1997). Research validity scales for the
NEO PI-R: Developmental and initial validation. Journal of Personality Assess-
ment, 68, 127-138.
Shock, N. W., Greulick, R. C, Andres, R., Arenberg, D., Costa, P. T., Jr., Lakatta,
E. G., & Tobin, J. D. (1984). Normal human aging: The Baltimore Longitudinal
Study of Aging (NIH Publication No. 84-2450). Bethesda, MD: National Insti-
tutes of Health.
Siegler, I. C, Zonderman, A B., Barefoot, J. C, Williams, R. B., Jr., Costa, P. T., Jr., &
McCrae, R. R. (1990). Predicting personality in adulthood from college MMPI
scores: Implications for follow-up studies in psychosomatic medicine. Psy-
chosomatic Medicine, 52, 644-652.
Silva, E, Avia, D., Sanz, J., Martinez-Arias, M. R., Grana, J. L., & Sanchez-Bemardos,
L. (1994). The five-factor model-I. Contributions to the structure of the NEO-
PI. Personality and Individual Differences, 17,741-753.
Silver, R. J., & Sines, L. K. (1962). Diagnostic efficiency of the MMPI with and with-
out the K correction. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 18,312-314.
Silverstein, A B. (1982). Factor structure of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence
Scale-Revised. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 661-664.
Snyder, D. K. (1982). Advances in marital assessment: Behavioral, communica-
tions, and psychometric approaches. In C D. Spielberger & J. N. Butcher
(Eds.), Advances in personality assessment (Vol. I, pp. 169-201). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
REFERENCES 273
Viken, R. J., Rose, R. J., Kaprio, J., & Koskenvuo, M. (1994). A developmental ge-
netic analysis of adult personality: Extraversion and Neuroticism from 18 to
59 years of age. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 722-730.
Wanous, J. P., Reichers, A E., & Hudy, M. J. (1997). Overall job satisfaction: How
good are single-item measures? Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 247-252.
Watkins, C. E., & Campbell, V. L. (1989). Personality assessment and counseling
psychology. Journal of Personality Assessment, 53, 296-307.
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A (1991). Self- versus peer ratings of specific emotional
traits: Evidence of convergent and discriminant validity. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 60, 927-940.
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A (1992). On traits and temperament: General and specific
factors of emotional experience and their relation to the five-factor model.
Journal ofPersonality, 60, 441-476.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A, & Harkness, A R. (1994). Structures of personality and
their relevance to psychopathology. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology, 103, 18-31.
Webb, E. (1915). Character and intelligence. British Journal of Psychology Mono-
graphs, 1(3), 1-99.
Welsh, G. S. (1975). Creativity and intelligence: A personality approach. Chapel Hill:
University of North Carolina, Institute for Research in Social Sciences.
Wernimont, P. F., & Campbell, J. P. (1968). Signs, samples, and criteria. Journal of Ap-
plied Psychology, 57, 120-128.
Widiger, T., & Corbitt, E. M. (1994). Normal versus abnormal personality from the
perspective of the DSM. In S. Strack & M. Lorr (Eds.), Differentiating normal
and abnormal personality (pp. 158-175). New York: Springer.
Widiger, T. A, & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1994). Personality and personality disorders. Jour-
nal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 78-91.
Widiger, T. A, & Frances, A (1985). The DSM-III personality disorders: Perspec-
tives from psychology. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 615-623.
Widiger, T. A, & Frances, A (1994). Toward a dimensional model for the personal-
ity disorders. In P. T. Costa, Jr, & T. A Widiger (Eds.), Personality disorders and
the five-factor model ofpersonality (pp. 19-40). Washington, DC: American Psy-
chological Association.
Widiger, T. A, & Shea, T. (1991). Differentiation of Axis I and Axis II disorders. Jour-
nal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 399-406.
Widiger, T. A, & Trull, T. J. (1992). Personality and psychopathology: An applica-
tion of the five-factor model. Journal ofPersonality, 60, 363-393.
Wiggins, J. S. (1979). Personality and prediction: Principles of personality assessment.
Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Wiggins, J. S. (1980). Circumplex models of interpersonal behavior. In L. Wheeler
(Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 265-293). Beverly
Hills, CA: Sage.
Wiggins, J. S. (1995). Interpersonal Adjective Scales: Professional manual. Odessa, FL:
Psychological Assessment Resources.
Wiggins, J. S. (1996). An informal history of the Interpersonal Circumplex Tradi-
tion. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66, 217-233.
REFERENCES 275
Wiggins, J. 5., & Pincus, A L. (1989). Conceptions of personality disorders and di-
mensions of personality. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 1, 305-316.
Wiggins, J. 5., & Trapnell, P.D. (1996). A dyadic-interactional perspective on the
five-factor model. In J. S. Wiggins (Ed.), Thefive-foctor model ofpersonality: The-
oretical perspectives (pp. 88-162). New York: Guilford.
Winch, R. F. (1958). A study of complementary needs. New York: Harper.
Worthington, D. L., & Schlottman, R. S. (1986). The predictive validity of subtle and
obvious empirically derived psychological test items under faking condition.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 50, 171-181.
Wrobel, T. A, & Lachar, D. (1982). Validity of the Wiener subtle and obvious scales
for the MMPI: Another example of the importance of inventory-item content.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 469-470.
Zonderman, A S., Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1989). Depression as a risk for
cancer morbidity and mortality in a nationally representative sample. Journal
of the American Medical Association, 262,1191-1195.
Zonderman, A 8., Herbst, J. H., Schmidt, c., Jr., Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R.
(1993). Depressive symptoms as a non-specific, graded risk for psychiatric di-
agnoses. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 544-552.
AUTHOR INDEX
Akamine, T.X., 73, 213, 266 Benet, v., 71, 219, 256 Butcher, J. N., 80, 114,257,
Aldenderfer, M.S., 114,255 Ben-Porath, YS., 69, 79, 80, 258,259,262,272
Al1port, G. W., 20, 21, 22, 255,256
23,24~26,27, 122,255 Benson, P. L., 234, 256 Campbell, I. M., 175, 260
Almagor, M., 219, 255 Bentler, P. M., 224, 256 Campbell. J. P., 206, 274
Andres, R., 272 Bergeman, e. 5., 39, 72, 256 Campbel\, V. L., 48, 274
Andrews, F. M., 117,255 Berne, S. L.. 49, 256 Cantor, N., 77, 257, 267
Angleitner, A., 22, 73, 265 Bigelow, G. E., 59, 257 Cantril, H., 117,257
Arbisi, P. A., 79, 255 Blashfield, R. K., 114, 255 Capara, G. v., 73, 213, 258
Arenberg, D., 272 Block, J., 7, 41, 46, 71, 80, Carmichael. e. M., 39, 258
Aston, M.e., 265 213 Caspi, A., 5, 258
Atkinson, J. w., 120, 267 Block, J. H., 134 Cattell, R. B., 15,17,24,25,
Attkisson, e. e., 242, 266 Bond, M. H., 60, 73, 256, 26,257,258
Avia, M.D., 73, 255, 272 269 Chae, J-H, 42, 43, 45, 73,
Bonnett, D. G., 224 213,228,258,269,271
Bagby, R.M., 40, 60, 223, Borgogni. L., 73, 258 Chen, M. e., 238, 258
246,255,270 Borkenau, P, 41. 54, 73, 223, Chen, P. Y, 215, 258
Bailey, K.D., 17, 113,255 256,257 Chipuer, H. M., 72, 256, 271
Bakan, D., 116, 255 Botwin, M., 31, 257 Christal, R. E., 25, 273
Balla, J.R., 224, 267 Bouchard, T. J., 47, 72, 257 Church, A. T., 73, 213, 224,
Bandura, A., 76, 255 Boyle, G. J.. 25, 257 225,258,266
Barbano, H.E., 260 Bradbury, T. M., 171, 172, Ciarrocchi, J. w., 59, 60, 61,
Barbaranelli, e., 73, 213, 265,266 62,245,248,271
258,269 Bratko, D., 269 Clance, P. R., 143,258
Barefoot, J.e., 268, 272 Brooner, R. K., 59, 257 Clark, L. A., 54, 63, 67, 118,
Barlow, D.H., 211, 256 Burke, P. J., 224, 225, 258 258,274
Barrick, M. R., 31, 46, 83, Buros, O. K., 47, 257 Clark, R. w., 120, 267
84,122,155,206,256, Busch, e. M., 61, 260, 268 Clayton, P., 264
269 Buss, A. H., 206, 272 Cloninger, e. R., 15,258
Barry, W.A., 173, 256 Buss, D. M., 72,110,172, Cohen, J., 180,237,259
Bern, D.J., 5, 258 173,178,206,257,267, Cohen, P., 180,237, 259
Bendig, A. w., 120, 256 272 Cohen, S., 118, 259
277
278 AUTHOR INDEX
Colvin, C R., 54, 259, 262 Ellenberger, H., 20, 261 Gwaltney, J. M., 259
Comrey, A. L., 71, 73, 213, Ellison, C W, 234, 261
257,258,263 Entwisle, D. R., 48, 261 Haan, N., 7, 39, 263
Connors, J., 43, 263 Erickson, J. A., 234, 256 Hahn, R., 71, 263
Cook-Flannagan, C, 257 Estadt, B. K., 258 Haig, C, 172, 266
Corbitt, E. M., 57, 274 Eysenck, H. J., 219. 220, Hall, W B., 120,263
Cornoni-Huntley, J., 260 221,261 Hargreaves, W A., 242, 266
Costa, P. T., Jr.. 11. 30, 31, Eysenck, M. W, 219, 261 Harkness, A. R., 15,40, 63,
35,36,38,39.40.41,42, 67,246,255,263,274
43,45,49,51,53,56,59, Fagan, P. J., 62, 109, 260, Hartka, E., 7, 39, 263
60.61,62,63,67,68,72, 261,269 Hartmann. H., 76, 263
73,74,76,80.81,84.85, Fireman, P., 259 Hayes, S. C, 211, 256
86,87.92,97,109,116. First. M. B., 2,15,18,22.24, Heath, A. C, 39, 72, 263
117, 118, 119, 122, 123, 27,31,38.40,46.49,59. Heathers, L. B., 80, 261
157,180,213,214.217, 60.62,68,99,103.105, Heaven, P. C L., 43, 73,
220,221,222,229,244, 107,108.117,156, 166, 263
246,256,257,259,260, 168,175,177,180,182, Heilbrun, A. B., 47, 263
261,268,269,271,272, 192.203,204,206,209, Helmes, E., 63, 263
273,274,275 215,220.221.225,229, Helson, R., 7, 264
Craik, K. H., 110,257 232,234,240,246,249, Herbst, J. H., 59, 61, 84, 257,
Creamer, M., 175, 260 251, 262 275
Crowne, D. P., 81, 261 Fiske, D. W, 25, 262 Hershberger, S. L., 72, 264
Csarny, R. J.. 238. 261 Fleeson. W.. 257 Hervig, L. K., 43, 267
Forsterling, F.. 43, 270 Hirschfeld, R. M. A., 40,
Dahlstrom, W G., 114,257, Frances. A. J., 58, 63, 64, 264
268 262,274 Hofer, S. M., 25, 264
Dai, T., 215, 258 Friedman. M., 114,262 Hofstee, W K. B., 51, 67,
Daniels, D., 74, 271 Funder, D. C, 54, 259, 262 114,115,234,264
Davis, K. E., 55, 265 Furnham, A., 83, 262 Hogan, J., 71, 122,264
Davis, W. W., 262 Hogan, R., 71, 81, 83, 122,
de Raad, D., 51, 114,234, Gerbing, D. W, 262 205,206,264,268,270
264 Goldberg, L. R .. 25, 26, 27, Holden, R. R., 264
Deniston, W. M., 261 28,29,31,48,51,73,82, Holland, J. L., 260
Derogatis, L. R., 61, 70, 239, 114,220,221,234,262, Horn, J. L., 25, 264
261 264,265,270,272 Horner, M.S., 120,264
Diekstra, R. F. W, 63, 261 Goldman, S. L., 215, 263 Horney, K., 143, 264
Digman, J. M., 24, 25, 26, Goldstein, M. D., 215, 262 Horowitz, L. M., 116, 264
120,206,261, 262 Gorsuch, R. L., 4, 25, 59, Hough. L. M., 71, 83, 264
DiPlacido, J., 48. 271 220,225,233,234,235, Howard, K. I., 210, 267
Dodge, K. L., 175, 271 262.263,273 Hoyle, R. H., 223, 270
Donahue, M. J., 234, 256 Gottman, J. M., 185,263 Hsu, L. M., 81, 264
Doyle, W J., 259 Gough, H. G., 17.43,45,47, Hudy, M. J., 240, 274
Duijsens, I. J., 63, 261 49,120,263
Dunnette, M. D., 83, 264 Graham. J. R.. 114.257.263 lmes. S. A., 143, 258
Dye, D. A., 122,213.260 Grana, J. L., 255, 272 Isaka, H., 73, 264
Green, D. P., 215. 263
Eaton, N. K., 83, 264 Grove, E., 120,256,272 Jackson, D. N., 43, 63, 83,
Eaves, L. J., 39, 263 Gruber-Baldini, A. L., 173, 219,263,265,266.270
Eber, H. W, 25, 264 263 Jang, K. L., 63, 72, 266
Edwards, A. L., 47, 80, 261 Guilford, J. P., 24, 263 Janzen, B. L., 61, 272
Edwards, J. E., 120,261 Guilford, R. B., 24, 263 Jessor, R., 7, 265
AUTHOR INDEX 279
Jex, S. M., 215, 258 Locke, B. Z., 260 Millon, T., 15,269
Joffe, R To, 40, 246, 255 Locke, H. J., 179, 267 Millsap, R, 7, 39, 263
John, O. P., 22, 24, 26, 31, Loehlin, J. C, 72, 271 Mischel, W, 52, 269
73,174,206,265,268 Loevinger, J., 213, 267, Moane, G., 7, 264
Jones, E. E., 55, 265, 268 273 Monello, R, 172, 266
Joreskog, J. G., 215, 223, 265 Lorr, M., 59, 114, 116,262, Montag, I., 43, 266
Juni, 5., 31, 265 263,267,268,271,273, Mooradian, T. A., 214, 269
274 Morrison, L. P., 174, 176,273
Kaemmer, B., 114,257 Lowell, E. L., 120,267 Mount, M. K., 31, 46, 83, 84,
Kaiser, R T., 8, 265 Lubin, B., 47, 267 122,155,206,207,256,
Kalemba, v., 40, 246, 255 Lushene, R. E., 4, 273 269
Kamp, J. D., 83, 264 Lyons, J. 5., 210, 211, 267 Murray, H. A., 15, 17,43,
Kaprio, J., 39, 274 47,49,259,269
Karney, B. R, 171, 172, 265, Marlowe, D., 81, 261 Muten, E., 83, 269
266 Marsh, H. W, 224, 267 Myers, I. B., 114,269
Katigbak, M.S., 73, 213, Marshall, G. N., 43, 267
224,225,266 Marshall, R. D., 261 Nakazato, H., 73, 256
Kaufman, A. 5., 237, 266 Martinez-Arias, M. R, 255, Narayanan, L., 73, 269
Kaufman, L., 114, 266 272 Neale, M. c., 39, 263
Keller, M. B., 173,264,266 Marusic, I., 269 Nelson, R 0., 211, 256
Keller, W, 48, 271 Masters, K. 5., 210, 270 Nesselroade, J. R., 256
Kelly, I. W, 61, 272 Matarazzo, J. D., 48, 267 Newsom, J. T., 259
Kendler, K. 5., 39, 263 McAdams, D. P., 71, 267 Nezlek, J. B., 214, 269
Kessler, R C, 39, 263 McCaulley, M. H., 114, 269 Nguyen, T. D., 242, 266
Klerman, G. L., 264 McClearn, G. E., 256 Nicholson, R. A., 81, 270
Kluth, C, 267 McClelland, D. C, 120, Nielsen, S. L., 80, 266
Kobak, K. A., 4, 271 267 Nisbett, R. E., 55, 265
Kosek, R B., 84, 172, 179, McCloy, R. A., 83, 264 Norem, J., 257
266 McCrae, R. R, 11,26,30, Norman, W T., 26, 270
Koskenvuo, M., 39, 274 35,36,38,39,40,41,42, Norris, A. H., 117,260
Kozma, A., 81, 266 43,45,48,49,51,53,56,
Kusulas, J. W., 43, 267 59,60,61, 68, 72, 73, 74, O'Mahoney, M. T., 210, 267
76,80,81,82,84,85,86, Odbert, H. 5., 23, 24, 26, 27,
Lachar, D., 83, 275 87,92,97,109,116, 117, 255
Lakatta, E. G., 272 118,119, 122,123,157, Ogles, B. M., 210, 211, 270
Lambert, M. J., 210, 270 165, 168, 180,213,214, Ones, D. 5., 122
Langston, C, 257 217,220,221,222,224, Ormel, J., 8, 270
Lanning, K., 266 225,226,229,232,244, Ostendorf, E, 22, 41, 73,
Larkin, 8., 264 246,256,259,260,267, 223, 257, 265
Larsen, D. L., 242, 266 268,269,271,272,273, Ozer, D. J., 8,19,48,51,232,
Larsen, R. M., 47, 267 275 265,270
Larson, R, 117, 266 McDonald, R. P., 224, 267
Leary, To, 116, 264, 266 McDonald-Scott, P., 264 Panter, A. T., 223, 270
Lester, D., 172,266 McGue, M., 39, 72, 257, Parker, G. V. C, 47, 269
Levin, J., 43, 266 258 Parker, J. D. A., 40, 223,
Levine, E. L., 73, 269 McNulty, J. L., 15, 263 255,270
Liebler, A., 54, 256 Meehl, P. E., 233, 269 Paunonen, S. v., 43, 60, 73,
Linehan, M. M., 80, 266 Megargee, E. I., 47, 174, 269 269,270
Lish, J. D., 210, 267 MieJ, G. M., 262 Peabody, D., 48, 270
Livesley, W. J., 63, 64, 72, Miller, T., 32, 59, 71, 92, 123, Pedersen, N. L., 72, 256,
265,266,272 126,247,269 264
280 AUTHOR INDEX
Perugini, M., 73, 258 Sher, K. J., 62, 69, 273 Tuley, M. R., 262
Piedmont, R. I., 178, 271 Shiraishi, D., 73, 256 Tupes, E. c., 25, 273
Piedmont, R. L., 29, 31, 40, Shock, N. W., 49, 272
41,42,43,46,47,48,49, Siegler, I. c., 7, 39, 268, 272 Useda, D., 40, 246, 273
51,54,56,59,60,61,62, Silva, E, 213, 255, 272
70,73,74,81,82,85, Silver, R. J., 81, 272 Vassend, 0., 213, 214, 218,
m, 120, 122, 155, 157, Silverstein, A. 8.,237,272 273
165,168,178,213,215, Sines, L. K., 81, 272 Venable, G. D., 234, 263
220,228,234,245,248, Skoner, D. P., 259 Vernon, P. A., 72, 265
258,260,269,270,271 Skrondal, A., 213, 214, 218, Vickers, R. R., 43, 267
Pincus, A. L., 63,116,262, 273 Viken, R. J., 39, 72, 274
271,275 Snyder, D. K., 185, 272
Plomin, R., 72, 74, 256, 264, Snyder, M., 76, 273 Wallace, K. M., 179, 182,
271 Sorbom, D., 215, 223, 265 183,267
Pontic as, Y., 62, 260, 261 Spector, P. E., 215, 258 Waller, N. G., 69, 71,80,82,
Przybeck, T. R., 15, 258 Spielberger, C. D., 258, 259, 219,255,256,273
Ptacek, J. T., 175,271 262,272,273 Wanous, J. P., 240, 274
Spirrison, C. L., 214, 273 Waters, L. K., 120,261
Ramanaiah, N. V., 261 Stagner, R., 122, 273 Watkins, C. E., 48, 274
Reichers, A E., 240, 274 Steers, K. M., 120,273 Watson, D., 54, 63, 118, 274
Reynolds, W. M., 4, 271 Stone, S. V., 109, 269 Webb, E., 24, 274
Riese, S. P., 19,48,51,232, Stones, C. R., 43, 263 Weinstein, H. P., 46, 85, 120,
270 Stones, M. J., 81, 266 122,155,271
Robbins, R. w., 206 Stout, C. E., 211, 273 Wells, P. A., 172, 272
Rodgerson, T. E., 238, 272 Strack,S., 59, 114,262,263, Welsh, G. 5., 49, 274
Rose, R. J., 39, 274 267,271,273,274 Wernimont, P. E, 206, 274
Rosenman, R. H., 114,262 Strauss, J. P., 46, 84, 206, West, M., 64, 223, 266
Rosolack, T. K., 221, 262 269 Wicks, R. J., 43, 258
Rouse, S. v., 80, 257 Strube, M. J., 215, 262 Widiger, T. A, 57, 58, 59, 63,
Rousseeuw, P. J., 114,266 Summerfeldt, L. J., 223, 270 64,67,84,260,262,274
Russell, R. J. H., 172, 272 Svrakic, D. M., 15,258 Wiggins, J. 5., 53, 63, 84, 116,
261,268,271,274,275
Saklofske, D. H., 61, 272 Takemoto-Chock, N. K., Williams, R. 8.,61,260,268,
Salovey, P., 215, 263 120,261 271,272
Sanchez-Bernard os, M. L., Tanaka, J. S., 223, 270 Willis, S. L., 173,263
255,272 Tanney, A., 64, 266 Wise, T. N., 62, 260, 261
Sanz, J., 255, 272 Taylor, R. M., 174, 176,273 Withey, S. B., 117,255
Saucier, G., 31, 269, 272 Tellegen, A., 71, 81, 82, 114, Wohlfarth, T., 8, 270
Schaie, K. w., 263 214,219.255.257,258, Wormworth. J. A., 63, 272
Scheier, M. E, 206, 272 273 Worthington, D. L., 83, 275
Schinka, J. A, 61, 82, 272 Tharp, R. Go, 173,273 Wortman. C. B.. 43. 267
Schlottman, R. S., 83, 275 Thiessen. D., 173, 266 Wrobel, T. A., 83, 275
Schmeck, R. R., 120, 272 Thurstone. L. L., 25, 273
Schmidt, C. w., 59, 61, 84, Tilly, S. M., 262 Young, R. K., 173, 266
257,261,275 Tobin, J. D., 272 Youniss, R. P.. 267
Schonemann, P. H., 225, 272 Tomes, J. L.. 219, 265
Schroeder, D. H., 30, 272 Trapnell, P. D., 116,275 Zirkel, 5., 257
Schroeder, M. L., 63, 266, Tresemer, D., 120, 273 Zonderman, A. B., 39, 60,
272 Trull, T. J., 40, 58, 59, 60, 62, 61,84,117,220,224,
Shanker, M., 269 63,65,69,246,273,274 225,226,232,260,269,
Shea, T., 63, 274 Trzebinski, J., 43, 270 272,275
SUBJECT INDEX
281
282 SUBJECT INDEX
Cl: see Competence Convergent validity, 48, 53-54, 166, 184; see
C2: see Order also Validity
C3: see Dutifulness Coping, 32, 70,118,119,151,209
C4: see Achievement Striving Correlated error, 215-217
C5: see Self-Discipline Couples, see also Cross-observer agree-
C6: see Deliberation ment; Rater version
California Psychological Inventory, 44 Agreeableness and, 172
California Q-Set, 44 COA index, 182-184-185
Categorical model, 63 cross-observer differences, 175-185
Cattell, Raymond, 15, 16,24-26 manipulative tactics, 172
Character, 122-123, 124, 130-131, 134, 138, marital dissatisfaction, 172, 175
154 neuroticism and, 172, 192
Circumplexes, 61, 93-96, 97,114-115, 116- perceptions of spouse, 173-174, 182-183
117, 120-122, 130-134, 138, 140, 143- profile interpretaion, 100-105, 171-176
144,153-154,155; see also specific self-other congruence, 173-176
circumplexes Couples Critical Incidents Check List
Client-therapist relationship, 10, 13-14 (CClCL), 178-185
understanding the client, 9,11-12,70, Cooperativeness, 178, 181,195
249-251 correlations with NEO PI-R, 180, 183,
Clinical assessment, 12,62,66,69-71,207 184
Cloniger's Tridimensional Personality correlations with rater's personality, 180
Questionnaire, 44 Emotional, 178, 181, 187, 195, 196, 199
Cluster analysis, 114 Flexibility, 178, 181, 188, 195, 197
COA index: see Cross-observer agreement Interpersonal, 178, 181, 188, 195
Competence (Cn 90, 96, 102, 131-132, 137, Personal Reliability, 178, 181, 189, 195,
142,146,150,154,236 197
Competitive Orientation, 120-122, 130-131, Relationship Context, 178, 181
143-144, 154 Criss-cross testing, 174
Compliance (A4), 89-90, 102, 129, 142, 154 Cross-cultural generalizability, 73-74, 224-
Compulsive behaviors, 99 232
Computer reports, 149-151, 161-164 Cross-observer agreement (COA), 52-56,
Comrey Personality Scales, 45 73-74, 175,224-232; see also Couples;
Confirmatory factor analysis, 33, 215-218, Rater version
222-225 analyses, 176-177, 183-185
Congruence coefficients, 59-60, 220, 225 convergent validity, 53-54, 166, 184
Conscientiousness, 27, 36-38, 90,131-132, discriminant validity, 54
137, 142,146,149,150,154; see also index, 182
specific facets interfactor correlation, 214-215
and adolescents, 74 reliabi Ii ties, 180-181
in Axis II disorders, 66, 135 residualized rating scores, 182
and CClCL, 181, 188-189, 191, 193, 197, value of, 56, 83-84, 100, 204-205
199,202-203
and Character, 122-123, 124 Deliberation (C6), 91, 96, 97, 99, 102, 131-
and Competitive orientation, 120-122 132,135,137,142,146,154,236
interpretation, 90, 92-93, 95-96,101,102 Dependent personality disorder, 151
and Personal Reliability, 178 Depression (N3), 85, 131-132, 136, 139, 145,
in substance abuse popUlation, 59 147
and treatment selection, 126-127, 144 Differential diagnosis, ]],12,59-62,64-70,
Construct validity, 151, 233-237; see also 146-147
Validity Dimensional model, 63, 68
Discriminant validity, 54; see also Validity
SUBJECT INDEX 283
Domains, 27, 31, 36-38, 40, 41, 213-218; see Factor analysis, 25, 26, 33, 35-41, 49-52,
also Five Factor Model; Personality; 114,212,215-218,222-225
specific domains Factor structure, 40-42, 59-60, 213-222
Dutifulness (C3), 91, 96,102,131-132,137, Faith Maturity Scale, 234-239
138, 142, 146, 149, 154 Fantasy (01). 87, 94, 102, 128, 132, 137-138,
140, 146, 153
El: see Warmth Feedback,9, 11, 12-13, 105-107, 108-110
E2: see Gregariousness Feelings (03), 41, 88, 95,102,128,137,140-
E3: see Assertiveness 141, 148
E4: see Activity Five Factor Model, 17-20, 24-27, 43-48, 58,
E5: see Excitement Seeking 71, 73-74, 212
E6: see Positive Emotions Basic Tendencies, 74-75
Edwards Personal Preference Schedule Character Adapatation, 74-75
(EPPS), 44, 47, 80 comprehensiveness, 43, 58
Emapthy, 11, 12 convergent validity, 48
Emotional Well-Being, 117-118, 130, 131- criticisms of, 71-72, 218-222
132, 133-134, 138, 140, 153 cross-observer convergence, 53, 73-74
English language: see Lexigraphic hypothe- Dynamic Processes, 75
sis External Influences, 74
Excitement Seeking (E5), 41, 59, 87, 93, 98, generalizability, 73-74, 212, 222-232
135, 147, 152 normal personality and, 58-59
Existential Well Being Scale, 234-239 Objective Biography, 74
Exploratory factor analysis, 33, 224-225 personality disorders, 67-68
Extraversion, 7, 9, 12, 13,25,27,36-38,86, robustness, 71
128,136,139,145,147,150,152;see Self Concept, 74
also specific facets taxonomy, 17-20,32
and adolescents, 74 theoretical foundations, 74-77
and Avoidant personality disorder, 65 vocational interests, 46
and Axis II disorder, 66, 136 Folk concepts, 45-46
and CClCL, 181, 187-188, 189, 190,201 Freud, Sigmund, IS, 20
and emotional we II-being, 117-119 Fundamental attribution error, 55
and Histrionic personality disorder, 65,
68 Gambling: see Impulsive behaviors
and interpersonal functioning, 116-117, Genotype, 4, 7-8, 38-39
118 Gough's Folk Concepts, 43, 45
interpretation 86, 92-93, 98, 101 Gregariousness <E2), 86, 98, 102, 128, 152
loading on A, 41, 86, 93, 98, 101-102, Guilford-Zimmerman Temperament Sur-
123 vey, 45
mean level change, 7
and Narcissistic personality disorder, High-order factors, 221-222
65 Histrionic personality disorder, 65, 68, 151
and Schizoid personality disorder, 12, Holden Psychological Screening Inventory,
65, 135 45
and Social phobia, 69 Holtzman Inkblot, 45
and treatment selection, 9,14,123-126 Hood Mysticism Scale, 234-235
Extrinsic Religiosity Scale, 234-235
Eysenck Personality Inventory, 44 Ideas (05), 88, 94, 102, 128, 132-133, 137,
Eysenck Personality Pro filer, 44, 222 145,153
Imposter phenomenon, 143
Facets, 30, 35, 36, 40-41, 84-92; see also spe- Impulsive behaviors, 131,133-135, 142, 146,
cific facets 147-149
284 SUBJECT INDEX
Validimax factoring, 35, 41 Values (06), 88-89, 95, 98, 99, 132-133, 138,
Validity, 48, 53-54, 69-71, 79-84, 151,206- 140,142,148,154,236
207,212,233-237,237-239 VRIN, 81-82; see also Validity
construct validity, 151,233-237 Vulnerability (N6), 41,86,101,127,128,
content free, 81, 82 131-132,136-139,152
incremental validity, 151, 233-237
and self-report data, 83-84 Warmth (El), 41, 86, 102, 145, 153
and social desirability, 80-81 Wiggin's Interpersonal Adjective Scales, 45