0% found this document useful (0 votes)
176 views

The Chore Curriculum

This document discusses the concept of a "chore curriculum" - the informal process by which children in many societies learn essential work skills through participation in age-appropriate chores. It begins by noting the lack of focus on children's work in anthropology compared to children's play. The author then defines chores as mandatory tasks all boys or girls are expected to master by a certain age, such as fetching water or caring for livestock. The document goes on to describe how children's participation in realistic play and make-believe enactments of work serves as a "pre-school" stage as they observe and rehearse chores before performing them for real. Examples are given of play progressively mirroring gender divisions of labor. Overall

Uploaded by

pat rodriguez
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
176 views

The Chore Curriculum

This document discusses the concept of a "chore curriculum" - the informal process by which children in many societies learn essential work skills through participation in age-appropriate chores. It begins by noting the lack of focus on children's work in anthropology compared to children's play. The author then defines chores as mandatory tasks all boys or girls are expected to master by a certain age, such as fetching water or caring for livestock. The document goes on to describe how children's participation in realistic play and make-believe enactments of work serves as a "pre-school" stage as they observe and rehearse chores before performing them for real. Examples are given of play progressively mirroring gender divisions of labor. Overall

Uploaded by

pat rodriguez
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 25

Utah State University

From the SelectedWorks of David Lancy

January 2012

The chore curriculum

Contact Start Your Own Notify Me


Author SelectedWorks of New Work

Available at: http://works.bepress.com/david_lancy/119


The chore curriculum.

David F. Lancy

Chapter In Working and Learning Among Africa’s Children. Gerd Spittler and
Michael Bourdillion (Eds). (pp. 23-57), Berlin: Lit Verlag.

Introduction

While claims that anthropology has ignored children (Hirschfeld 2002) have been
successfully countered (Lancy 2008, 2011a), children’s work has undoubtedly been
slighted. The anthropological study of children’s play has yielded an enormous amount of
scholarship—an annual conference, newsletters, journals and an entire shelf of scholarly
volumes. The anthropological study of children’s work has been limited, by comparison.
This imbalance does not reflect any grounded reality, children in most societies spend as
much time doing useful work as they do playing (Harkness and Super 1986). Rather, it
reflects an inherent bias on the part of anthropologists whose culture provides no
legitimate role for children as workers. And in the vast annals of developmental and child
psychology, “work” as a category of children’s experience—worthy of study and analysis
— is virtually absent. Hence, this volume, growing out of the Berlin conference
organized by Professor Spittler, should be seen as a contribution towards redressing this
unfortunate omission.

In my first attempt to review and synthesize the material on children’s work, distinctive
patterns emerged that crystallized into the expression ‘chore curriculum’ (Lancy 1966:
149). In the second survey, further analysis refined and enhanced the value of the concept
(Lancy 2008: 235-242). I hope to take it one step further. The term “chore” is somewhat
self-evident, except that in contemporary society, we use it to characterize relatively
minor tasks, completely peripheral to the “important” work that is done exclusively by
adults. However, in the archives of ethnographers and historians we find that children are
expected to assist in a variety of critical areas of domestic and corporate production
including the care of infants, gardening, herding and foraging. There is a widely
acknowledged distinction between the work that all children are expected to do
(contingent upon age and gender) and realms of endeavour that are optional or,
contingent upon the child’s interest and aptitude. A chore is any task that all boys or all
girls should master by a roughly agreed upon age and carry out willingly and efficiently.
Among the Kpelle, these include running errands, fetching water and firewood, tending
younger siblings, weeding in the fields, caring for livestock, daubing mud on the walls of
houses under construction, sweeping out the compound and so on (Lancy 1996). The
chore inventory is mandatory and corresponds, in the parlance of contemporary (US)
discussions of children’s education, to the “core” that all pupils are expected to master. In
my study of Kpelle children, I found that they may also elect to pursue non-mandatory
skills such as weaving, basketry (see also Köhler, this volume, page X) and becoming a
blacksmith. These are not part of the chore inventory and the pattern of skill acquisition
and mastery is somewhat different (Lancy and Grove 2010).

The term “curriculum” in chore curriculum conveys the idea that there is a discernible
regularity to the process whereby children attach themselves to, learn, master and carry
out their chores. While the academic or core curriculum found in schools is formal and
imposed on students in a top-down process, the chore curriculum is informal and emerges
in the interaction of children’s need to fit in and emulate those older, their developing
cognitive and sensorimotor capacity, the division of labour within the family and the
nature of the tasks (chores) themselves. In the remainder of this chapter, my goal is to
delineate and illustrate the common elements of the chore curriculum.

A Note on Methodology

The methodology used in this chapter involves canvassing the ethnographic record,
assembling illustrative cases and teasing out broad, “culturally invariant” patterns. It is
referred to as ethnology. In a recent exemplar, MacDonald describes her review of the
ethnographic record on boys learning to hunt as a “cross–cultural synthesis” leading to an
analysis that is of necessity, “qualitative, as the relevant evidence from the ethnographic
literature is either anecdotal or generalized and includes limited quantitative data” (2007:
390). The current survey of children’s work grew out of a comprehensive review of the
ethnographic record pertaining to childhood (Lancy 2008). In that review, approximately
1350 published and unpublished reports were used, and since publication, an additional
250 sources have been found and added to the corpus. The material is comprehensive
with respect to geography and subsistence patterns. This corpus is canvassed for all
descriptions of children’s work, however brief, and then this smaller but still extensive
corpus of cases is systematically searched and organized around common themes, such as
the child’s acquisition of “sense” (see below).

The Pre-school or Play Stage of the Chore Curriculum

In Becoming a blacksmith in Gbarngasukwelle I described children’s amazingly detailed


and faithful replication of the blacksmith’s forge in an episode of make-believe. The
blacksmith’s compound was a happening place in the village, consistently attracting a
crowd of enthralled spectators and gossips, young and old (also true in a Kuba village, cf,
Binkley 2006: 106). Children could watch the action of the smiths and eavesdrop as
village affairs were retailed. They thus built up a stock of script material that could be
woven into their make-believe play. The boy playing the smith, in particular, had
obviously absorbed a great deal of the processes, both technical—he constructed
reasonable replicas of bellows, anvil, tongs—and social—assigning the roles of novice,
wives and helpers to his playmates. The terminology for tools, actions and relationships
used in the “script” was also a faithful rendition (Lancy 1980a).1 The town chief’s court

1
In using this vivid example, I must acknowledge that blacksmithing is not
a chore. In fact, becoming a blacksmith requires an apprenticeship. But, as I
proved a similar magnet with similar engagement by children as spectators and later,
dramatists (Lancy 1980b; see also Read 1960: 84).

Ethnographic descriptions of work activity enacted in make-believe and play with objects
are rich and varied and even include archaeologists’ reconstructions of childhood based
on the discovery of miniature or crudely made artefacts assumed to be toys (Park 2006:
56-7). In the domestic sphere, we see children playing at food preparation, cooking and
feeding the family. In these vignettes we occasionally get a glimpse of development over
time in children’s make-believe:

When [very young Kaoka] pretend to keep house they make no sexual distinction
in the allocation of the tasks. Boys and girls together erect the shelters, plait the
mats, cook the food, and fetch the water. But within a year or so, although they
continue to play in company, the members of each group restrict themselves to the
work appropriate to their sex. The boys leave the cooking and water carrying to
the girls, who, in turn, refuse to help with the building (Hogbin 1969: 38).

Similarly, Goody (1992) describes a continuum from make-believe to “for real” food
preparation in which older children model for younger ones, real but scaled down pots.
These may substitute for toy pots and if the mother is willing, edible ingredients go into
the pot rather than grass. In a second illustration of this development from play to work, I
was able to observe the “canoe curriculum”. Ifaty village in southwest Madagascar
depends primarily on marine resources and a modest-sized outrigger sailing canoe is the
primary means of accessing such resources as well as marketing them. Virtually all adult
males use such canoes almost daily. On the beach and in the shallows, I observed)(almost
simultaneously): a) a 2-year-old splashing alone in a tide-pool, learning about water; b)
three boys around 5 years old clambering over a beached canoe, learning an agile dance
from thwart to gunwale; c) two boys about 7 years old independently preparing and then
sailing model canoes, making appropriate adjustments to sail angle and rudder; d) two
boys of 8 years playing with an abandoned outrigger in the shallows. They climbed on,
paddled it, capsized it, took turns as captain and mate; e) when two young men began to
rig and prepare to launch a full-size outrigger, the boys paddled over to watch this unfold
and; f) shortly after they sailed away, a boy of about 10 came paddling in to shore in a
half-sized canoe. Based on previous and more thorough studies of the canoe curriculum, I
am confident that these experiences prepare boys to become mariners with little need for
any formal instruction (Wilbert 1976: 318).

When children re-enact activities they have witnessed these are not just generic scenes
but quite precise and thoughtful replications of complex systems. Katz’s (1986: 47-8)
descriptions of Sudanese boys’ make-believe enactment of two contrasting agricultural
systems is a case in point. Aside from farming, children learn critical skills through play

indicate elsewhere, the play stage is found across the task inventory, from chores
to relatively simple crafts to full-fledged, complex crafts requiring an
apprenticeship (Lancy 2011b).
in varied environments. Among pastoralist societies, we find “Dhebar boys… using
camel and sheep droppings to practice herding sheep and lambs” (Dyer and Choksi,
2006: 170). Tuareg boys who will eventually care for substantial camel herds, begin their
lives as herders tending a kid they treat as a playmate (Spittler 1998: 343). Franz Boas
describes Baffin Inuit boys “play-hunting” seal using miniature harpoons fashioned by
their parents (Boas 1901: 111).

Of course, children’s play “pre-school” is not limited to chores but may prefigure
virtually any customary adult activity, including for example the processes involved in
carrying out trance-induced shamanism (Katz 1981). In order for children to take the
initiative and get a head start on learning their culture, chores included, the “culture”
must be an open book. The public nature of most adult activity facilitates children’s
engagement at a safe distance where they are not interfering. Anthropologists often note
adult awareness and sympathy towards children’s mimicry. “When adults are asked about
children’s mimetic play they reply: ‘That is how they learn’” (Fortes 1938/1970: 23).
Another example is drawn from the Sisala. “When a boy first goes to the farm with his
father, he is told to sit in the shade of a tree and observe what his elders are doing. When
he asks to help, someone gives him a hoe with which to play” (Grindal 1972: 29).

John Bock has been the scholar most noted for empirical tests of the hypothesis that play
equals preparation for work. In field studies among peoples in the Okavango with varied
subsistence patterns, he has demonstrated that the likelihood of children mimicking
specific adult activities in play closely tracks the relative importance of those activities in
the local subsistence system. Furthermore, he has carefully demonstrated the process
whereby girls build up skill in grain processing through “play-pounding” (driving a pestle
into a mortar to hull grain but without actual grain) which can then rapidly develop into
the real thing, functioning from the outset at a high level of skill (Bock 2004: 274). We
have evidence then, from a multiplicity of sources, of children playing at the tasks and
production systems that they will later assay in earnest. We also have more limited
evidence that this play activity is “educational” but several disclaimers are in order.

Children’s motivation to engage in make-believe and object play cannot be attributed


solely or even primarily to the desire to learn their culture. The essence of play is its
purposelessness. Still, there is little play that does not echo adult activity. Play with toys
that are uniquely toys (as opposed to miniature or discarded tools) is associated almost
entirely with high civilization and social class. In upper class households in Egypt circa
2300 BCE, one finds an array of professionally made toys that would not look out of
place in a contemporary toy store, such as a wooden crocodile with articulated tail and
jaws (Wileman 2005: 31). This implies an attitude of indulgence: that parents expect
children to play just for fun.

In contrast, village children draw primarily on the scenes from their direct experience
(Power 2000: 272) because unlike bourgeois children they do not have access to
manufactured toys, storybooks, videos and other sources to launch them into more
inventive fantasies (Gaskins, in press). And, in contrast to contemporary bourgeois
society where parents engage with and encourage their child’s fantasy play, villagers do
not engage directly in children’s play (Lancy 2007). “[Sisala] parents regard an interest in
children’s play as beneath their dignity” (Grindal 1972: 25). Lastly, not all chores are
foreshadowed in play. Little girls may care for an infant sibling in lieu of playing with a
doll, for example (Broch 1990: 110) or assist their mother in a market stall in lieu of
pretending to market (Paradise and Rogoff 2009: 113).

Running Errands

Margaret Mead, reporting on her first field study in Samoa, provides one of the earliest
characterizations of the centrality of work in childhood.

The tiniest little staggerer has tasks to perform—to carry water, to borrow fire
brands, to fetch leaves to stuff the pig… learning to run errands tactfully is one of
the first lessons of childhood… these slighter tasks are laid aside for harder ones
as soon as the child becomes strong enough or skilled enough (Mead 1928: 633).

Running errands nicely illustrates key characteristics of the chore curriculum.


Fetching and carrying is inherently staged or laddered. “Very young children (age 3) may
start with one or two sticks of wood, or yams in a carry net, but by age 8 they are carrying
firewood, water, produce and messages” (Zeller 1987: 544). A barely mobile toddler
may be asked to carry a cup from its mother across an evening family circle to its father.
The same toddler will tag along as an older sibling makes a longer delivery excursion, in
effect serving as an understudy. Errands can vary by length and territory, between close
kin and strangers, can involve loads of varying size and fragility, and can include an
exchange of some kind including a market transaction. Adults match their assignments to
the child’s level of skill and size and each new assignment ratifies (and motivates) the
child’s growing competence.

The stalwart little helper publicly advertises the quality of its upbringing and its
worthiness as a potential foster child, enhancing the family’s reputation. On the other
hand, children are favoured as errand runners “because adolescents or adults seen in close
proximity to neighbors’ houses might be suspected of adultery, theft or witchcraft. And
boys are favored because their virtue isn’t as fragile as girls’” (Lancy 2008: 238).
Learning to become an errand runner rarely entails teaching by an adult. Children observe
and replicate the process with only minor guidance from an older, sib role model.

Anthropologists often note with some degree of awe how early the child embarks on the
chore curriculum and how significant is their contribution to the domestic economy.
Ottenberg notes that, among the Afikpo, babies are routinely cared for by their 4 or 5
year-old siblings (1968: 80). Among the Hadza, groups of children forage in the vicinity
of camp while their parents are gone on longer expeditions. From 4 years-of-age they
successfully gather baobab fruits, tubers and small birds and mammals, providing up to
25 percent of their daily diet. Marlowe notes: “foraging simply emerges gradually from
playing” (Marlowe 2010: 156; see also Tucker and Young 2005: 169).
Even with change in the village economy, such as the introduction of cash crops,
children’s work remains crucial. For example, in the coffee industry children make varied
and valued contributions. The youngest plant seeds in small plastic bags to germinate
new plants, they also collect coffee cherries that have fallen to the ground at harvest.
Somewhat older children are responsible for weeding and spreading chalk. 12 to 15-year-
old boys are responsible for spraying urea as a fertilizer and assisting with trimming,
which may involve climbing trees to lop off branches with a machete. All help with the
harvest and with sorting the cherries. Agile boys are only too happy to climb into the
branches to collect the fruit that adults cannot reach (Ruiz, 2011: 169-171).

Work and Identity

It is not enough to acknowledge that children are kept busy doing chores or that being
helpful makes them “fit in”. The very identity of the child may be largely defined in
terms of the work that he or she does. Throughout human history children have been seen
as an investment. Parents have them and raise them so that their labour can support the
household, and eventually provide an old-age pension or social security (Clark 1994;
Reynolds 1991). Among the severely impoverished Papel in Guinea-Bissau, infant
mortality is extremely high: roughly one-third of all children born alive will die before
they reach the age of five. In spite of these dire statistics, and the fact that women work
extremely hard to eke out a bare survival, Jonina Einarsdottir found that mothers were not
interested in reducing their fertility, on the contrary, they wanted as many children as
possible. They operated on the assumption that the more you have, the more likely there
will be a few who survive and “help with work, contribute to their emotional well-being,
take care of them in old age, secure them a respectable funeral, and ease their entrance
into the afterlife” (Einarsdottir 2004: 86). A Kpelle mother asserts: “What makes a child
good? If you ask her to bring water, she brings water. If you ask her to cook she cooks, if
you tell her to mind the baby, she does it. When you ask her to plant rice she doesn’t
complain” (Lancy 1996: 76). Given these widely shared views, it should not be surprising
that for the Nuer (and many others), it is only when:

the boy tethers the cattle and herds the goats… cleans the byres and spreads the
dung to dry and collects it and carries it to the fires [that] he is considered a
person (Evans-Pritchard 1956: 146, emphasis added).

In some societies the association between chores and identity is made even more explicit.
Among the Giriama, a two to three-year-old is labelled, in effect, “water carrier”. An
eight-year old girl is called a “maize pounder”, a boy of the same age is muhohomurisa
or, “herd boy” (Wenger 1989: 98). In pre-modern Russia “our plowboy,” “our herd boy,”
and “our nanny girl” were habitual terms parents used to address their children
(Gorshkov 2009: 15). And among the Tchokwe’, “children are identified through the roles
they assume [for example] kambumbuare children… who help parents in the field or with
fishing and hunting” (Honawana, 2006: 41-42). On the other hand, an adult seen carrying
out chores normally assigned to children would be an object of scorn and pity and
Russian children:

who could not learn how to do work appropriate to their age were subjected to
mockery… a girl who could not learn how to spin by a certain age was called a
“no spinner” (nepriakha); if a girl could not weave cloth by age fifteen, she was
called a “no weaver” (netkakha). Boys who had not learned how to make bast
shoes were called “shoeless” (Gorshkov 2009: 23).

Aside from nomenclature that marks the child’s developmental progression as a worker,
we find numerous examples of adults turning these transitions into minor rites of passage.
These include: a Kaoka boy’s first pig (Hogbin 1969: 39); a Vlach 6-year-old being given
his first shepherd’s crook (Campbell 1964: 156); a Kutenai boy’s first bow and arrows
(Grinnel 1923: 115); a Netsilik girl’s first caught salmon, her brother’s first goose
(Balikci 1970: 45); and “When [an Mbuti] boy kills his first “real animal,” he is
immediately acclaimed as a hunter…[and honoured by cicatrization]…an operation
performed… by one of the ‘great hunters’” (Turnbull 1965: 257). On the other hand,
certain areas of work are considered inappropriate for children (just as certain tasks are
reserved for males, others females). The Kwoma prevent children from prematurely
assuming more difficult chores because tasks are associated with rank, and moving to a
more challenging assignment is tantamount to a promotion (Whiting 1941: 70). Koori
boys are not permitted to touch “real” hunting weapons (toy or scaled-down are OK) as
that would demean the adult hunters who have earned the privilege (Basedow 1925: 86).

Chores also play a major role in the differentiation of gender—especially before the
appearance of secondary sex characteristics.2 Among the Kel Ewey Tuareg, goats are
tended by boys and girls, but only boys tend camels (Spittler 1998). In West Africa,
weaving is the province of women among Akwete Igbo, and 3-year-old girls pretend to
weave, eventually picking up the skill through watching and helping their mothers. For
the Baulè, males weave and boys acquire the skill through a lengthy, formal
apprenticeship (Aronson 1989). Stereotypically, among Hadza foragers girls forage and
boys hunt (Marlowe 2010). Almost universally girls are preferred as caretakers for
younger siblings and sons are conscripted for this chore only when a daughter is
unavailable (Ember 1973: 425-6).3 Indeed, it is common to see two clusters of children in
open areas of the village or farm. Girls gather together in a corner with their young
charges playing in a subdued fashion. Boys, on the other hand, roam more widely, engage

2
Iris Köhler has written (personal communication 25/7/11): “Within the
society I explored, I can validate the connection between chore curriculum and
identity building: Very small children are allowed to play and 'work' tasks of both
sexes. People say that they ‘don't know yet who they are’ and how to act their
age.”
3
A significant exception to this generalization occurs in a few, highly
egalitarian, foraging societies, the Central African foragers known as pygmies in
particular (Henry, Morelli, and Tronick 2005: 200).
in a panoply of games, some quite noisy and boisterous, and seem generally carefree
(Read 1960: 82).

The Kerkenneh Islands are typical in transitioning girls into responsible roles well before
boys. “By the age of four most little girls have some real responsibility in the household.
Little boys are babied longer and are discouraged from imitating their mothers because
that is women’s work” (Platt 1988: 282). Indeed, boys in many societies are defined as
much by their freedom from work—relative to girls—as by the specific work they do
(Pope-Edwards 2005: 87). In many cases, a boy or man doing “women’s work” would be
the object of ridicule.

The onset of middle childhood marks an important transition in the child’s identity. As
Edel describes, Chiga children have been busy play/working and learning to do a variety
of useful things. But by around seven or eight (earlier in some societies) the child falls
into a routine of predictable, competent, essential work. Earlier, “any assumption of adult
ways and attempts at adult skills or responsibilities is praised and applauded” (Edel
1957/1996: 178). Now they can be relied upon to do their chores without guidance,
instruction, scolding or praise. Girls, in particular, can largely replicate and substitute for
their mothers.

The child has reached a point in its development where he or she starts to “get noticed”
(Lancy and Grove 2011) due to their evident intelligence or common sense, referred to
among the Kipsigis as ng’omnotet (Harkness and Super 1985: 223); wijima among the
Sisala (Grindal 1972: 28). “Intelligence in the village is associated with qualities like self-
sufficiency, obedience, respect towards elders, attention to detail, willingness to work and
effective management of younger siblings and livestock” (Lancy 2008: 168; see also,
Wober 1972). Most, if not all, of these signs of maturity, relate to the child as worker.

The transition from child to adult is often marked by a rite of passage or initiation, seen
as a precursor to marriage and adult standing. However, work may play a critical role in
this transition. “Fully adult Hadzamen are referred to as epeme men. When a male is in
his early 20s and kills a big-game animal, he becomes an epeme or fully adult male”
(Marlowe 2010: 57). Among the Warao, a prospective bridegroom is judged on his
ability to garden, hunt, fish and above all, make a canoe: any deficit is a bar to marriage
and adult status (Wilbert 1976: 327). Contemporary anthropological accounts describe
many cases of a breakdown in the chore curriculum resulting in a failure to marry and
establish an adult persona. Among the Bumbita, a breakdown in traditional socialization
practices due to missionary interference and temporary out-migration of youths has
denied them knowledge of how to cultivate yams. A young man, who cannot grow yams,
cannot support a family (Leavitt 1998: 178). For many, the obligation to attend school
pulls them out of the chore curriculum. Youth from foraging societies may be particularly
vulnerable as they miss out on hunting and gathering expeditions and fail to learn the
local ecology. For Cree children:
By the time they finished their schooling, they had become foreigners to Cree
tradition, not only by failing to acquire skills and knowledge of the land but also
by lacking an appropriate attitude for life on the land. Thus, formal schooling led
to the weakening of the existing social system (Ohmagari and Berkes 1997: 207).

The linkage between social structure and chores is also founded on the idea that “work
builds character”. In a Nahua village, the elders see children working less and showing,
as a result, diminished vigour and toughness (Sánchez 2007: 94); while Russian peasants
believed that if children are not put to work early, they will never develop into
responsible adults (Gorshkov 2009: 14). Zelizer describes the battle that unfolded during
the first quarter of the 20th century between those who felt that labour was harmful to
children and interfered with their schooling, and those who thought that work built the
child’s character and that early employment staved off idleness and deviance (1985: 57,
72).

Role Models

If I were discussing contemporary bourgeois society this section might be labelled


“Teachers”. But as analysis reveals (Lancy 2010a), active, child-centred instruction is
quite rare outside the contemporary elite. Rather, children assume responsibility for
learning their culture, chores in particular, while relying on those more expert to serve
largely in the capacity of models. Again, contrary to contemporary views on what is
“natural” (Lancy 2010b), the model, especially for boys, is as likely to be an older sibling
or peer as a parent. As examples, I cite Amhara boys who are said to trail after young
males “like retainers follow a feudal lord” (Messing 1985: 213). Hadza boys “almost
never go hunting with their fathers, at… 8 or 9 years old, they go hunting for small
animals, usually in twos” (Marlowe 2010: 157). By imitating their sib-caretakers
Marquesan “toddlers learn to run, feed and dress themselves, go outside to urinate and
defecate, and help with household chores” (Martini and Kirkpatrick 1992: 124).

There are several reasons for the choice of peers as role models. From an early age,
children are placed under the care of older siblings who introduce them into the
neighbourhood playgroup e.g. “Mayan toddlers learn primarily by observing and
interacting with their sibling caretakers...” (Maynard 2002: 978). At the threshold of the
chore curriculum, children are far more likely to be in the company of peers than parents.
Weisner notes that Abaluyia “children care for other children (under a mother’s or other
adults’ management) within indirect chains of support” (1996: 308, emphasis added).
That is, toddlers are managed by slightly older siblings who are in turn guided by
adolescents, while adults serve as rather distant “foremen” for the activity, concentrating
primarily on their own more productive or profitable activity. This phenomenon is well
illustrated in Polak’s study of Bamana families engaged in bean cultivation (Polak 2003,
2011).

There is considerable evidence that siblings are more patient and sympathetic mentors
than adults (Maynard 2002). A contrasting pair of anecdotes is very revealing. Raum
observed a Chaga mother and her little daughter cutting grass to take home to feed the
cattle. Tying the stalks into a bundle is difficult but the “mother refuses requests for help
by saying: ‘Haven’t you got hands like me?’”(1940: 199). Now consider a vignette of
Pushtun children gathering and bundling shrubs (buti) to bring home.

Khodaydad, aged about ten years, showed and explained to his younger brother
Walidad (two and a half) how to put buti together: He made up a small pile while
Walidad squatted next to him and watched. Tying them together, he explained
how to do it. Then he untied the bundle and bound it up again to show how it was
done. Walidad then wanted to carry it home. His elder brother helped him
shoulder it and his sister guided him home, and it was obvious that little Walidad
was very proud of being able to accomplish the work (Casimir 2010: 54).

To be sure, adults may intervene strategically in the learning process. For example, the
“knowledge gained from [Meriam] adults about the reef and how to forage remains
limited mainly to what is edible and what is dangerous” (Bird and Bird 2002: 291).
Bamana children may “want to participate in the work done in the compound and the
fields to a greater extent than they are able… they risk causing damage, for instance by
using a hoe the wrong way or losing seeds” (Polak 2003: 126). Hence, parents must
tactfully intervene to guide them to tasks that are within their grasp (Polak 2011: 104-5).
For Warao boys learning to make canoes, the sine qua non of survival, “there is not much
verbal instruction… but the father does correct the hand of his son [and demonstrates]
how to overcome the pain in his wrist from working with the adze” (Wilbert 1976: 323).

Girls may find that their freedom and opportunity to play is curtailed by their mother’s
need for a “helper at the nest”. The mother becomes the model and supervisor of the
girl’s progress through the chore curriculum. As Riesman notes, by the time a young
woman is pregnant, she has had years of watching her mother and others care for infants
and she has herself logged many hours of child care under the watchful (and critical) eye
of her mother and other senior female relatives (1992: 111). A Kpelle girl will
accompany her mother to the garden, carrying a pail containing their lunch. At the field,
she will watch as her mother hoes until given her own small hoe to try. She will match
her mother’s behaviour, learning the moves through repeated practice and observation
(Lancy 1996: 144). If a Mazahua mother has a market stall, her daughter will “pitch in”
to assist her by trimming onions and tying them into bundles, using her mother’s bundles
as a model. She is eager to set up “her” stand and once the mother’s stand is established,
does so using an abandoned piece of cardboard (Paradise and Rogoff 2009: 113).

It is rarer to find boys paired so closely with their fathers. Children are often considered
too boisterous to serve as companions on fishing and hunting expeditions (Broch
1990:85; Puri 2005, 233). A Yanomami boy will not be welcomed on the hunt by his
father until he is in his mid-teens and has already developed considerable expertise at
tracking and knows the forest and its inhabitants intimately (Peters 1998: 90). Fathers are
often absent from the homestead or the nature of their work, felling trees to clear land for
a farm, precludes a youngster from “pitching in”. However, there are a few cases where
adult males (not necessarily the father) serve as critical role models. The boy or youth
will be moved from the home to the exclusive company of men.

“When FulBeMare’en boys… are old enough to herd… they no longer eat with
their mother and sisters but with the men. Through explicit instruction, listening
to conversations, and observation they learn about the general dietary needs of
cattle, which types of grasses appeal most to cattle, the characteristics of each
animal, the dominance hierarchy in the herd, and the genealogy of the herd”
(Moritz 2008: 111).

As girls move out of the realm of everyday chores into more complex tasks, they may
continue in the same mentoring relationship they have established with their mothers.
Conambo mothers are very supportive of their daughter’s fledgling efforts at ceramics,
which is a chore in the sense that all women are expected to make pots. They sometimes
give the girls well-formed pots and invite them to add minor painted designs or they take
the daughter’s ill-formed vessel, re-shape it and hand it back to the child to decorate.
However, they do remain focused primarily on their own production (Bowser and Patton
2008). Köhler (2009, this volume) studied mother-daughter transmission of ceramic skills
among the Nyarafolo. In her report, we see somewhat less engagement by the mothers,
perhaps because pottery-making is optional. The onus is on the girls to take the initiative
and mothers offer almost no verbal instruction even when doing so would accelerate the
girl’s mastery. In my study of the Kpelle, I found mothers even less involved in the sense
that they seemed to take no responsibility for their daughter’s learning to make textiles: at
most they were willing to permit the daughter to work alongside and observe and copy
their efforts. The attitude seemed to be that the child will learn the skill “when she’s
ready” (Lancy 1996: 149). Yet another variation appears in a study of Dii potters: girls
must endure a lengthy and demanding apprenticeship with their mother. It is significant
that pottery-making is mandatory. In fact, “to have an ungifted apprentice or potter in the
family is a disgrace, and every potter is required to reach a certain level of expertise in
order not to depart from the rest of the potter families” (Wallaert 2008: 187). The only
“teaching” consists in sharp verbal rebukes or directives: “Stop wasting clay,” otherwise
the girl learns the way she probably learned her other chores—through observation,
imitation and much practice. At age ten, the process becomes even more restrictive.

Initiative and trial and error are now forbidden; every gesture must follow the
mother’s pattern. Corporal punishments (spanking, forced eating of clay) are used
to ensure that rules are respected, and verbal humiliations are very common
(Wallaert 2008: 190-1).

Dii pottery is somewhat unusual in that the child apprentices to a parent. In other
societies it is far more usual that the child apprentices to a more distant kinsman or to a
stranger. For example, “Gonja believe that familiarity breeds contempt and that sons
wouldn't show sufficient respect towards their fathers to learn from them” (Goody 2006:
254).
I want to conclude this section, however, by stressing, that despite the observed
variability in the process of skill acquisition, the onus is on the learner not the model.
Hence, Bolin’s description of learning to weave in the Andes is probably normative.
“Children are not taught to spin or weave. Rather, they observe family members who have
mastered these crafts and imitate them directly” (2006: 99, emphasis added). In other
words, I think that the child likely draws on numerous models for guidance and
inspiration not just a single role model (see also Gosselain 2008: 153; Puri 2005: 280).

Progress in the Chore Curriculum

Two central principles of the chore curriculum are the motivation of the child which
propels them up the learning gradient, and the nature of the task environment which
reduces the severity of that gradient by offering “steps” or stages. In this section, we will
examine those stages and also consider what ensues when the child is not making the
expected progress.

Herding is perhaps the most evidently staged “chore”. The child first learns to care for a
single, juvenile animal as a pet. She or he helps cut and gather fodder and mucks out the
byre. The child’s progress is monitored. “Only after a boy has proved his reliability at
herding goats is he preferred to the work of pasturing cattle” (Raum 1940: 200).The
Tuareg boy progresses from a single kid (at three-years-old) to a herd of goats (at ten) to
a baby camel (at ten) to a herd of camels (fifteen) to managing a caravan on a trek across
the Sahara (in their twenties, see Spittler, this volume).The Ngoni boy faces a challenging
education in cattle lore.

The Ngoni classified their cattle according to age, sex, coloring, size and shape of
horns, whether castrated or not, whether in calf or not. Knowledge of the
extensive series of names used for these “classes” of cattle was part of a herd-
boy's A.B.C. By the time he was old enough to be told to drive certain cattle out
of the kraal, designated by their class, he knew exactly which ones were meant.
He could also use the cattle terminology to be precise in telling an owner about a
beast which has strayed or one that had a sore hoof, or one that was giving an
exceptionally good or poor flow of milk (Read 1960: 133).

Staging in agricultural production ranges from tasks as simple as chasing birds from the
ripening crop (Grindal 1972: 29; Lancy 1996: 146) to ploughing behind a team of oxen
(Polak 2011: 144). Children may be assigned their own garden plot as a gesture of
encouragement, likewise they may given their own scaled-down collecting calabash, hoe
or machete. They will shadow an older relative who serves, as we saw, as a role model.
They observe the model and emulate their behaviour, but the relative (usually an older
sibling) also assists by keeping the child on task and preventing him or her from
damaging the work of others (Polak 2011: 104-5). Some tasks are relatively easy to
master, while others, such as learning to plant millet seeds, may take a couple of years
and require an adult to demonstrate the correct procedure (Polak 2011: 84-6).
In foraging, there is a range of task levels corresponding to the difficulty of the terrain
and the elusiveness of the prey. With respect to terrain, we might contrast the
Ju/’hoansi(!Kung) with the Huaorani. In the former, the terrain is extremely difficult and
foragers face the threat of predation. Children must be left behind in camp and do not
begin their foraging careers until their late teens (Hames and Draper 2004; see also Kogel
et al. 1983). The terrain, in the second case, is somewhat more benign and “nothing is
more cheering for a Huaorani parent than a three-year-old’s decision to join a food
gathering expedition” (Rival 2000: 116). Children on the island of Mer in the Torres
Straits can collect edible marine life from the shoreline and shallows. They can be seen
“spearfishing with toddler-sized spears as soon as they begin walking, using them at first
to spear sardines along the foreshore for bait” (Bird and Bird 2002: 262). However while
some types of collecting can be mastered quickly at an early age, shellfish collecting
takes more strength and mature production levels are achieved later in childhood (Bird
and Bird 2002: 245).4

A précis of hunting development is available for the Western Apache. Children were kept
from roaming beyond the camp until eight-years-old but then quickly learned to shoot
small game with sling or bow and arrow. They joined enthusiastically in communal quail
drives. At twelve their tracking and hunting prowess yielded prey that they were proud to
bring back to camp. For his first deer hunt, at fifteen or sisteen, a boy would be permitted
to accompany an older male relative who acted as role model. One earned the right to tag
along (and observe) by fetching wood and water for the temporary hunting camp
(Goodwin and Goodwin 1942: 475). The pattern with hunting seems to be one where
boys may get an early start on using weapons, tracking and capturing small prey, but
nevertheless, take many years to reach adult levels of competence (MacDonald 2007:
391). Despite this, the larger pattern is quite clear, children master most of the skills
needed to function in their society at an early age.

The chore curriculum is remarkably successful in moving children from a state of


dependency to one where they are both self-sufficient and contributors to the domestic
economy. And, this is accomplished with little intervention from an adult “teacher”.
What happens when a “student” pursuing the chore curriculum “fails”? Families vary in
imposing sanctions on “laggards,” with foraging societies being the most laissez faire.
Penalties range from the very mild, as when Mayan family members speak critically of a
child who is cast in the role of “overhearer”, listening to his or her behaviour being
described critically (de León ND) to more severe variations. An Amhara adult may
encourage a child to do its chores “by throwing clods of dirt or manure at him” (Levine
1965: 266). Aka children who evade responsibility are denied food by their mothers
(Boyette 2008). Many societies prescribe a beating for children who fail to do chores or
do them poorly (Ainsworth 1967). A Sebei mother condemns a “lazy” daughter who is

4
For a thorough discussion of the interaction of physical size and dexterity
versus experience and learning in the child’s movement through the chore
curriculum, see Bock (2001).
not upto the mark by saying “I hope that you have stomach pains and dysentery”
(Goldschmidt 1976: 259).

Overall, however, accounts of children eagerly doing chores are far more frequent than
accounts of laggards,5 but significant change in the chore curriculum can be anticipated.

The Waning of the Chore Curriculum

Earlier we touched on the breakdown of the chore curriculum due to forces affecting
village youth. Among these, schooling stands out. In the 1930s and 1940s, schools were
first introduced to Sisala villages and the emphasis was on preparing a new elite to lead
and staff the colonial and then later the post-colonial administration. This meant “a
relatively small body of educated Sisala gained easy access to a large number of
prestigious government positions” (Grindal 1972: 92). The majority of children, however,
continued to pursue the chore curriculum (for a parallel case in Papua New Guinea, see
Pomponio and Lancy 1986). The increasing monetization of the village economy and the
consequent press to use schooling as a pathway to greater earning potential has, in recent
years, forced parents to re-evaluate their dismissal of schooling (see Spittler, this
volume). Government mandated universal schooling and the competition for scarce jobs
has raised the bar in terms of the minimum level of education required to enter the labour
force (Lord 2011: 102). Hence, schooling competes directly with the domestic economy
for children’s time and allegiance.

There are indirect threats to the chore curriculum as well. Chief among these is the loss of
sibling caretakers or their transformation into “teachers”. Mayan “children who had been
to school often tried the school model of teaching from a distance with much verbal
discourse until they realized that that model was not resulting in the younger child’s
compliance in doing the task” (Maynard 2004: 530). Many villagers may remain
ambivalent about the value of schooling. In Fez, where there are a range of craft
traditions that are still economically viable, schooling is seen as waste of time, or worse,
that a schooled child will not follow the strict dictates of his/her role model in learning
the craft (Schlemmer 2007: 114). The children are also ambivalent and anthropologists
report cases of children eager to abandon what are seen as arduous and unpleasant chores
for the relative ease of the classroom (Anderson-Levitt 2005: 988), as well as cases where
“Staying at home or in the fields is preferable to the despotic, sometimes boring, and
often a bit cruel school reality... (Alber, this volume, p X).” However, most communities
try to strike a balance, “Family decisions in rural West Africa are based on the notion that
it might be wise to send some of the children going to school and others to continue
peasant’s work” (Alber, this volume, p Y).

5
Just as a boy may be forced to do “girl’s work” if sisters are unavailable,
children may be forced to remain at a very menial level in the chore curriculum
because that is the level at which their contribution is most critical.
While the chore curriculum has retained its viability in many villages, in another sector of
society, it has disappeared altogether. There has been a flood of studies recently of
children’s chores or contributions to the household among middle-class families.

• In West Berlin “parents alone are responsible for… the reproduction of daily
life… the child is the recipient of care and services” (Zeiher 2001: 43; see also
Wihstutz 2007: 80).

• In case studies from Los Angeles, a parent spends a lot of time cajoling/guiding a
five-year-old into making her bed. It becomes a big dramatic production after she
initially refuses, claiming incompetence. In a case from Rome, the father does not
even bother trying to get his eight-year-old daughter to make her bed, he does it
himself, while complaining that her large collection of stuffed animals and
decision to move to the top bunk make his task much harder (Fasulo, et al. 2007:
16-18).

• In a related study in Los Angeles of 30 families “no child routinely assumed


responsibility for household tasks without being asked… the overall picture was
one of effortful appeals by parents for help [who often] backtracked and did the
task themselves… [a father becoming, in effect] a valet for the child” (Ochs and
Izquierdo 2009: 399-400).

• Genevan children “use the vociferous defeat strategy. They comply with what is
asked of them but… cry, scream, bang doors, lock themselves up in their rooms to
sulk and so on… Some… agree to submit if their parents can prove their demands
are well-founded… [some] agree to render a service to their parents in exchange
for permission to go out… One boy mentioned employing a kind of ‘terrorism’”
(Montandon 2001: 62).

In contrast to these cases, Orellana’s research on Central American migrant families in


Los Angeles shows children—in particular girls—making a large contribution to the care
and well-being of the household (and attending school). Not only do they do household
chores, they do odd jobs and bring in needed cash, unlike middle class white children
whose earnings are not shared with the family (Bachman, et al. 2003: 301), and assist
their parents and older relatives around the barriers of culture and language (Orellana
2001).

Contemporary society is not supportive of the chore curriculum. When children become
cherubs, rather than chattel (Lancy 2008: 77, 99) parents and society at large no longer
view them as helpers, contributing significantly to the domestic economy. And children
absorb these values trading a willingness to emulate and assist for an eagerness to express
needs and preferences. The culture is no longer an open book; much of what is important
to learn is hidden, because it is valuable, dangerous or complex. Teaching has all but
replaced observation and imitation as the primary means of transmitting culture to the
descending generation. School learning is highly staged but it is extremely difficult for
the child to proceed from level to level at her own pace, without explicit instruction. The
immediate feedback afforded by mastering the small steps leading to competent work is
now very elusive. The child must be content with often meaningless (“Good Job!”)
praise, grades and smiley faces. The role model is unable to instruct via demonstration
alone. Indeed, when the child tries to emulate a desired activity (cooking, carpentering,
driving) it may be rebuffed because the possibility of damage to the child, the materials,
the project or the tools is great. Toy tools are unsatisfactory because you cannot make
anything truly useful. Where the chore curriculum thrives, it is a thing of beauty fitting
the maturing child into its society like a key into a lock.

Ainsworth, Mary D. 1967: Infancy in Uganda: Infant Care and the Growth of Love.
Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins Press.

Anderson-Levitt, Kathryn M. 2005: The schoolyard gate: Schooling and childhood in


global perspective. In Journal of Social History, 38: 987-1006.

Aronson, Lisa 1989: To weave or not to weave: apprenticeship rules among the Akwete
Igbo of Nigeria and the Baulè, of the Ivory Coast. In Coy, Michael W. (Ed.),
Apprenticeship: From Theory to Methods and Back Again. Albany, NY: SUNY Press,
149-162.

Bachman, Jerald G., Safron, Deborah J., Sy, Susan Rogala and Schulenberg, John E.
2003: Wishing to work: New perspectives on how adolescents’ part-time work intensity
is linked to educational disengagement, substance use, and other problem behaviours. In
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27(4): 301-315.

Balikci, Asen 1970: The Netsilik Eskimo. Garden City, NY: The Natural History Press.

Basedow, Herbert 1925: The Australian Aboriginal. Adelaide, Ausralia: F. W. Preece and
Sons.

Binkley, David A. 2006: From grasshoppers to Babende: the socialization of Southern


Kuba boys to masquerade. In Playful Performers: African Children’s Masquerades.
Simon Ottenberg and David A. Binkley (Eds.), London: Transaction Publishers, 105-115.

Bird, Douglas W. and Bird, Rebecca Bliege 2002: Children on the reef: Slow learning or
strategic foraging? In Human Nature, 13: 269-397.

Boas, Franz (1901). The Eskimo of Baffin Land and Hudson Bay. In Bulletin of the
American Museum of Natural History 15, Part 1:1-370.

Bock, John 2002. Learning, life history, and productivity: Children’s lives in the
Okavango Delta, Botswana. In Human Nature, 13: 161-197.
Bock, John 2004: Farming, foraging, and children’s play in the Okavango delta,
Botswana. In Anthony D. Pellegrini and Peter. K. Smith (Eds), The Nature of Play: Great
Apes and Humans, New York: Guilford, 254-281.

Bolin, Inge 2006: Growing Up in a Culture of Respect: Child Rearing in Highland Peru.
Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.

Bowser, Brenda J. and Patton, John Q. 2008: Learning and transmission of pottery style:
Women’s life histories and communities of practice in the Ecuadorian Amazon. In
Miriam T. Stark, Brenda J. Bowser and Lee Horne (Eds.) Cultural Transmission and
Material Culture: Breaking Down Boundaries. Tucson, AZ: University of Arizona Press,
105-129.

Boyette, Adam 2008: Scaffolding for cooperative breeding among Aka foragers, paper
presented at the annual meeting of The American Anthropological Association,
November 19th-23rd, San Francisco, CA.

Broch, Harald B. 1990: Growing up Agreeably: Bonerate Childhood Observed.


Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii Press.

Campbell, John K. 1964: Honour, Family, and Patronage: A Study of Institutions and
Moral Values in a Greek Mountain Community. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Casimir, Michael J. 2010: Growing Up in a Pastoral Society: Socialization Among Pashtu


Nomads. Köln, Germany: Kölner Ethnologische Beiträge.

Clark, Gracia 1994: Onions are My Husband: Survival and Accumulation by West
African Market Women. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

de León, Lourdes ND: Socializing attention: Directive sequences, participation, and


affect in a Mayan family at work. Submitted to Ethos, March.

Dyer, Caroline and Choksi, Archana 2006: With god’s grace and with education, we will
find a way: Literacy, education, and the Rabaris of Kutch, India. In Caroline Dyer (Ed.),
The Education of Nomadic Peoples: Current Issues, Future Prospects. Oxford: Berghahn
Books, 159-174.

Edel, May M. 1957/1996: The Chiga of Uganda, 2nd ed. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction
Publishers.

Einarsdottir, Jonina. 2004: Tired of Weeping: Mother Love, Child Death, and Poverty in
Guinea-Bissau. Madison, WI: The University of Wisconsin Press.
Ember, Carol R. 1973: Feminine task assignment and the social behavior of boys, In
Ethos, 1: 424-439.

Evans-Pritchard, Edward E. 1956: Nuer Religion. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Fasulo, Alessandra, Loyd, Heather and Padiglione, Vincenzo 2007: Children’s


socialization into cleaning practices: A cross-cultural perspective. In Discourse & Society
18:11-33.

Fortes, Meyer 1938/1970: Social and psychological aspects of education in Taleland. In


John Middleton (Ed.), From Child to Adult: Studies in the Anthropology of Education.
Garden City, NY: The Natural History Press, 14-74.

Gaskins, Suzanne In Press: Pretend play as culturally constructed activity. In M Taylor


(Ed.) Oxford Handbook on The Development of Imagination. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.

Goldschmidt, Walter 1976: Culture and Behavior of the Sebei. Berkeley, CA: University
of California Press.

Goodwin, Grenville and Goodwin, Janice Thompson (1942): The Social Organization of
the Western Apache. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.

Goody, Esther N. 1992. “From play to work: adults and peers as scaffolders of adult role
skills in Northern Ghana.” Paper given at 91st meeting, American Anthropological
Association, San Francisco, December.

Goody, Esther N. 2006: Dynamics of the emergence of sociocultural institutional


practices, In David R. Olson and Michael Cole (Eds.), Technology, Literacy, and the
Evolution of Society. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 241-264.

Gorshkov, Boris B. 2009: Russia’s Factory Children: State, Society, and Law, 1800-
1917. Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press.

Gosselain, Olivier P. 2008: Mother Bella was not a bella: Inherited and transformed
traditions in Southwestern Niger. In Miriam T. Start, Brenda J. Bowser, and Lee Horne
(Eds.), Cultural Transmission and Material Culture: Breaking Down Boundaries. Tucson,
AZ: The University of Arizona Press, 150-177.

Grindal, Bruce T. 1972: Growing up in Two Worlds: Education and Transition among
the Sisala of Northern Ghana. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.

Grinnel, George B. 1923: The Cheyenne Indians: Their History and Ways of Life, 2 Vols.
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Hames, Raymond, and Draper, Patricia 2004: Women’s work, child care, and helpers-at-
the-nest in a hunter-gatherer society. In Human Nature, 15 (4), 319-341.

Harkness, Sara and Super, Charles M. 1985: The cultural context of gender segregation in
children’s peer groups. In Child Development, 56, 219-224.

Harkness, Sara and Super, Charles M. 1986: The cultural structuring of children's play in
a rural African community. In Kendall Blanchard (Ed.) The Many Faces of Play.
Champaign/Urbana, IL: Human Kinetics, 96-103.

Henry, Paula Ivey, Morelli, Gilda A. and Tronick, Edward Z. 2005: Child Caretakers
Among Efe Foragers of the Itruri Forest. In Barry S. Hewlett and Michael E. Lamb,
Hunter Gatherer Childhoods: Evolutionary, Developmental, and Cultural Perspectives.
New Brunswick, NJ: Aldine Transaction, 191-213.

Hirschfeld, Lawrence E. 2002: Why don’t anthropologists like children? American


Anthropologist 104: 611-627.

Hogbin, H. Ian 1969: A Guadalcanal Society: The Kaoka Speakers. New York: Holt,
Rinehart, and Winston.

Honwana, Alcinda (2006): Child Soldiers in Africa. Philadelphia, PA: University of


Pennsylvania Press.

Katz, Cindi 1986: Children and the environment: work, play and learning in rural Sudan.
In Children’s Environment Quarterly, 3 (4): 43-51.

Katz, Richard. 1981: Education is transformation: becoming a healer among the !Kung
and the Fijians. In Harvard Education Review 51: 57-78.

Köhler, Iris 2009: Es Sind die Hände, die die Töpfe Schön Machen.
Töpfernde Frauen und Töpfernlernen bei den Nyarafolo. Berlin: Lit Verlag.

Kogel, Amy, Bolton, Ralph, and Bolton, Charlene 1983: Time allocation in four
societies. In Ethnology, 22: 355-370.

Lancy, David F. 1980a: Becoming a blacksmith in Gbarngasuakwelle. In Anthropology


and Education Quarterly, 11: 266-274.

Lancy, David F. 1980b: Speech events in a West African court. In Communication and
Cognition, 13: 397-412.

Lancy, David F. 1996: Playing on the Mother Ground: Cultural Routines for Children’s
Development. New York: Guilford.
Lancy, David F. 2007: Accounting for variability in mother-child play. In American
Anthropologist, 109, 273-284.

Lancy, David F. 2008: The Anthropology of Childhood: Cherubs, Chattel, Changelings


Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Lancy, David F. 2010a: Learning “From Nobody:” The Limited Role of Teaching
in Folk Models of Children’s Development. In Childhood in the Past 3: 79-106.

Lancy, David F. 2010b: When nurture becomes nature: Ethnocentrism in studies of


human development. In Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33: 39-40.

Lancy, David F. 2011a: “Why Anthropology of Childhood? A short history of an


emerging discipline.” Keynote Speech for: Towards an Anthropology of Childhood and
Children. Ethnographic Fieldwork Diversity and Construction of a Field. Institute of
Human and Social Sciences, University of Liege (Belgium) March 9-11.

Lancy, David F. 2011b: “Apprenticeship: A Survey of Ethnographic and Historical


Sources.” Paper presented at the Fifth International Conference of the Society for the
Study of Childhood in the Past. Theme: Child Labour in the Past: Children as Economic
Contributors and Consumers, Oct. 1st.

Lancy, David F. and Grove, M. Annette 2010: Learning guided by others. In Lancy,
David F., Gaskins, Suzanne and Bock, John (Eds.) The Anthropology of Learning in
Childhood. Lanham, MD: Alta-Mira Press, 145-179.

Lancy, David F. and Grove, M. Annette 2011: Getting noticed: Middle childhood in
cross-cultural perspective. In Human Nature. 22(3):

Leavitt, Stephen C. 1998: The Bikhet mystique: masculine identity and patterns of
rebellion among Bumbita adolescent males. In Gilbert Herdt and Stephen C. Leavitt
(Eds.), Adolescence in Pacific Island Societies. Pittsburgh,PA: University of Pittsburgh
Press, 173-194.

Levine, Donald N. 1965: Wax and Gold: Tradition and Innovation in Ethiopian Culture.
Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Lord, Jack 2011: Child labor in the Gold Coast: The economics of work, education, and
the family in late-colonial African households, c. 1940-57. In Journal of the History of
Childhood and Youth 4.1: 86-115.

MacDonald, Katherine 2007: Cross–cultural comparison of learning in human hunting:


Implication for life history evolution. In Human Nature, 18: 386-402.
Marlowe, Frank W. (2010): The Hadza: Hunter-Gatherers of Tanzania. Berkeley, CA:
University of California Press.

Martini, Mary, and Kirkpatrick, John 1992: Parenting in Polynesia: A view from the
Marquesas. In Jaipaul L. Roopnarine and D. Bruce Carter (Eds.), Parent-Child
Socialization in Diverse Cultures. Norwood, NJ: Ablex, 199-222.

Mead, Margaret 1928: Samoan children at work and play. Natural History, 28: 626-636.

Maynard, Ashley E. 2002: Cultural teaching: The development of teaching skills in Maya
sibling interactions. In Child Development, 7: 969-982.

Maynard, Ashley E. 2004: Cultures of teaching in childhood: Formal schooling and


Maya sibling teaching at home. In Cognitive Development, 19: 517–535.

Messing, Simon D. 1985: Highland Plateau Amhara of Ethiopia. New Haven, CT:
Human Relations Area Files.

Montandon, Cleopatre 2001: The negotiation of influence: Children’s experience of


parental educational practices in Geneva. In Leena Alanen and Berry Mayall (Eds.),
Conceptualizing Child-Adult Relations. New York: Routledge,54-69.

Moritz, Mark 2008: A critical examination of honor cultures and herding societies in
Africa. In African Studies Review, 51: 99-117.

Ochs, Elinor and Izquierdo, Carolina 2009: Responsibility in childhood: Three


developmental trajectories. In Ethos, 37: 391-413.

Ohmagari, Kayo and Berkes, Fikret 1997: Transmission of indigenous knowledge and
bush skills among the Western James Bay Cree women of Subarctic Canada. In Human
Ecology, 23: 197-222.

Orellana, Marjorie F. 2001: The work kids do: Mexican and Central American immigrant
children’s contributions to households and schools in California. In Harvard Educational
Review, 71(3): 366-389.

Ottenberg, Simon 1968: Double Descent in an African Society: The Afikpo Village-
group. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press.

Paradise, Ruth and Rogoff, Barbara 2009: Side by side: Learning by observing and
pitching in. In Ethos. 37: 102-138.

Park, Robert W. 2006: Growing up north: Exploring the archaeology of childhood in the
Thule and Dorset cultures of Arctic Canada. In Archaeological Papers of the American
Anthropological Association, 15: 53-64.
Peters, John F. 1998: Life Among the Yanomami: The Story of Change Among the
Xilixana on the Mucajai River in Brazil. Orchard Park, NY: Broadview Press.

Platt, Katherine 1988: Cognitive development and sex roles of the Kerkennah Islands of
Tunisia. In Jahoda, Gustav and Lewis, Ioan M. (Eds.). Acquiring Culture: Cross Cultural
Studies in Child Development. London: Croom Helm, 271-287.

Polak, Barbara 2003: Little peasants: On the importance of reliability in child labour. In
D’Almeida-Topor, Hèléne, Monique Lakroum and Gerd Spittler (Eds.) Le Travail en
Afrique Noire: Représentations et Pratiques à L’époque Contermporaine. Paris: Karthala,
125-136.

Polak, Barbara 2011: Die Könige der Feldarbeit. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation,
Kulturwissenschaftlichen Fakultät der Universität Bayreuth. Bayreuth: Germany.

Pomponio, Alice and Lancy, David F. 1986: A pen or a bush knife: School, work and
personal investment in Papua New Guinea. In Anthropology and Educ. Qtrly, 17: 40-61.

Pope-Edwards, Carolyn 2005: Children’s play in cross-cultural perspective: A new look


at the Six Culture Study. In Felicia F. McMahon, Don E. Lytle, & Brian Sutton-Smith
(Eds.), Play: An Interdisciplinary Synthesis. Lanham, MD: University Press of America,
81-96.

Power, Thomas G. 2000: Play and Exploration in Children and Animals. Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Puri, Rajindra K. 2005: Deadly Dances in the Bornean Rainforest: Hunting Knowledge of
the Punan Benalui. Leiden, Netherlands: KITLV Press.

Raum, Otto F. 1940: Chaga Childhood. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Read, Margaret 1960: Children of their Fathers New Haven: Yale University Press.

Reynolds, Pamela 1991: Dance Civet Cat: Child Labour in the Zambezi Valley. Athens,
OH: Ohio University Press.

Riesman, Paul 1992: First Find Yourself a Good Mother. New Brunswick, N.J.: Rutgers
Univ. Press.

Rival, Laura 2000: Formal schooling and the production of modern citizens in the
Ecuadorian Amazon. In Bradley A.U. Levinson (Ed.), Schooling the Symbolic Animal:
Social and Cultural Dimensions of Education. Pp.108-122. Lanham, MA: Rowman &
Littlefield,
108-122.
Ruiz, Luisa F. M. 2011: Coffee in Guatemala. In G. Kristoffel Lieten (Ed.), Hazardous
Child Labour in Latin America. London: Springer, 165-189.

Sánchez, Martha A. R. 2007: ‘Helping at home:” The concept of childhood and work
among the Nahuas of Tlaxcala, Mexico. In Betrice Hungerland, Manfred Leibel, Brian
Milne, and Anne Wihstutz (Eds.), Working to Be Someone: Child Focused Research and
Practice with Working Children. London: Jessica Kingsley, 87-95.

Schlemmer, Bernard 2007: Working children in Fez, Morocco: Relationship between


knowledge and strategies for social and professional integration. In Betrice Hungerland,
Manfred Leibel, Brian Milne, and Anne Wihstutz (Eds.), Working to Be Someone: Child
Focused Research and Practice with Working Children. London: Jessica Kingsley, 109-
115.

Spittler, Gerd. 1998. Hirtenarbeit. Köln: Rüdiger Köppe.

Tucker, Bram and Young, Alyson G. 2005: Growing up Mikea: Children’s time
allocation and tuber foraging in Southwester Madagascar. In Barry S. Hewlett and
Michael E. Lamb, Hunter Gatherer Childhoods: Evolutionary, Developmental, and
Cultural Perspectives. New Brunswick, NJ: Aldine Transaction, 147-171.

Turnbull, Colin M. 1965: The Mbuti Pygmies: An Ethnographic Survey. New York:
American Museum of Natural History.

Wallaert, Hélène 2008: The way of the potter’s mother: Apprenticeship strategies among
Dii potters from Cameroon, West Africa. In Miriam T. Start, Brenda J. Bowser, and Lee
Horne (Eds.), Cultural Transmission and Material Culture: Breaking Down Boundaries.
Tucson, AZ: The University of Arizona Press, 178-198.

Weisner, Thomas S. 1989: Cultural and universal aspects of social support for children:
Evidence from the Abaluyia of Kenya, In Deborah Belle (Ed.) Children’s Social
Networks and Social Supports. New York: Wiley, 70-90.

Wenger, Martha 1989: Work, play and social relationships among children in a Giriama
community. In Deborah Belle (Ed.) Children’s Social Networks and Social Supports.
New York: Wiley, 91-115.

Wihstutz, Anne 2007: The significance of care and domestic work to children: A German
portrayal. In Betrice Hungerland, Manfred Leibel, Brian Milne, and Anne Wihstutz
(Eds.), Working to Be Someone: Child Focused Research and Practice with Working
Children. London: Jessica Kingsley, 77-86.

Whiting, Beatrice B. and Pope-Edwards, Carolyn P. 1988: Children of Different Worlds:


The Formation of Social Behavior. Cambridge: MA Harvard University Press.
Whiting, John W.M. 1941: Becoming a Kwoma New Haven, CT: Yale University Press

Wilbert, Johannes 1976: To become a maker of canoes: An essay in Warao enculturation.


In Johannes Wilbert (Ed.), Enculturation in Latin America. Los Angeles: UCLA Latin
American Center Publications, 303-358.

Wileman, Julie 2005: Hide and Seek: The Archaeology of Childhood. Gloucester, UK:
Tempus Publishing.

Wober, Mallory M. 1972: Culture and the concept of intelligence: A case in Uganda. In
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 3: 327-328.

Zeiher, Helga 2001: Dependent, independent, and interdependent relations: Children as


members of the family household in West Berlin. In Leena Alanen and Berry Mayall
(Eds.), Conceptualizing Child-Adult Relations. London: Routledge, 37-53.

Zelizer, Viviana A. 1985: Pricing the Priceless Child: The Changing Social Value of
Children. New York: Basic Books.

Zeller, Anne C. 1987: A role for children in Hominid evolution. In Man, 22: 528-557.

You might also like