A History of The Behavioral Therapies (O'Donohue Et Al., 2018)
A History of The Behavioral Therapies (O'Donohue Et Al., 2018)
Table of Contents
Preface
Introduction
Chapter 1
Thomas Nickles
Chapter 2
Roger Poppen
Chapter 3
3
B. F. Skinner’s Contribution to Therapeutic Change: An
Agency-less, Contingency Analysis
Julie S. Vargas
Chapter 4
Paul T. Mountjoy
Chapter 5
Sidney W. Bijou
Chapter 6
June 1953-1965
Ogden R. Lindsley
4
University of Kansas and Behavior Research Company
Chapter 7
Arnold A. Lazarus
Chapter 8
Albert Bandura
Stanford University
Chapter 9
Albert Ellis
Chapter 10
5
From Psychodynamic to Behavior Therapy: Paradigm
Shift and Personal Perspectives
Cyril M. Franks
Rutgers University
Chapter 11
Leonard Krasner
Stanford University
Chapter 12
W. Stewart Agras
Chapter 13
Walter Mischel
Columbia University
Chapter 14
6
A Small Matter of Proof
Donald M. Baer
University of Kansas
Chapter 15
Todd R. Risley
University of Alaska
Chapter 16
Montrose M. Wolf
University of Kansas
Chapter 17
Gordon L. Paul
University of Houston
7
Chapter 18
Gerald C. Davison
8
Dedication
This book is dedicated to the loving memory of Janet Bijou, a
true friend of behavior therapy, and whose intelligence and
kindness reflect the humanitarian spirit guiding us all.
9
Distributed in Canada by Raincoast Books
Oakland, CA 94609
www.newharbinger.com
Epub ISBN:9781608825882
p. cm.
June, 1999"—Pref.
10
ISBN 1-878978-40-3
616.89’142’09—dc21
2001047081
11
Preface
This book is based on a conference held at the University of
Nevada, Reno in June, 1999. The editors organized this
conference in order to provide an opportunity for us and
others to better understand the development of the behavioral
therapies and to capture a part of the historical record before it
was lost forever. Most importantly, we wanted to honor the
founders of the behavioral therapies and to watch as this
group of approximately 20 individuals interacted — most of
them as old friends, but also perhaps for the last time, at least
in this large of a group.
The chapters the arise from this conference show the unique
properties of these unique individuals. While participants had
an outline of topics to address, each chapter reflects the topics
that the presenters felt best revealed their intellectual history
and the context and content of their contribution. The
difference between chapters in toen and approach could not
realistically be eliminated without muffling the very voices
we wanted to hear. In the end, we made the conscious
decision to give these leaders of the field the freedom to tell
their story in their own way.
12
were told. We would also like to thank them for sharing the
stories of their lives and their work. These stories were often
quite moving, and sometimes very funny. What these
individuals showed us is that the story of the development of
behavior therapy is not a dry story of purely intellectual
commitments and technical developments. Rather it is clearly
a story of deeply held values, caring, compassion, conflict,
fate, and, at times, personal tragedy. This book is dedicated to
these (and other) founders of our discipline.
13
We would also like to thank supporters of this conference.
Dean Robert Mead and Vice President Ken Hunter were
particularly helpful. Their generous help made this conference
possible.
William T. O'Donohue
Deborah A. Henderson
Steven C. Hayes
Jane E. Fisher
Linda J. Hayes
14
Participants in the Nevada Conference
on the History of the Behavior
Therapies
15
Bottom row (from left to right): Roger Poppen, Jerry
Davison, Julie Vargas, Leo Reyna, Len Krasner, Albert
Bandura, Linda Hayes
16
Introduction
17
of general psychological principles” (Krasner & Ullmann,
1965, p. 244).
18
methodologies (e.g., single subject vs. group designs)? What
constitutes a legitimate problem for the behavior therapist?
What constitutes improvement and how is this best to be
measured? How important are cognitive factors, and how are
they to be accounted for within a behavioral paradigm?
19
common to all — For if you look at them you will not see
something that is common to all, but similarities,
relationships, and a whole series of them at that…Are they all
“amusing”? Compare chess with naught and crosses. Or is
there always winning and losing, or competition between
players? Think of patience. In ball games there is winning and
losing; but when a child throws his ball at the wall and
catches it again, this feature has disappeared. Look at the
parts played by skill and luck; and at the difference between
skill in chess and skill in tennis. Think now of games like
ring-aring-aroses; here is the element of amusement, but how
many other characteristic features have disappeared! And we
can go through the many, many other groups of games in the
same way; can see how similarities crop up and disappear.
And the result of this examination is; we see a complicated
network of similarities overlapping and criss-crossing;
sometimes overall similarities, sometimes similarities of
detail (p. 31-32).
20
similarities but the field as a whole does not share one general
paradigm.
21
Why should a history of the behavioral therapies be
attempted? Coleman (1988 p. 3-4) provides an interesting set
of reasons for studying history:
22
Sixth, studying history is like getting an immunization shot.
Knowledge of the great pendulum swings that have occurred
in psychology and of the fads and orthodoxies that have come
and gone can reduce the persuasive power of new fads.
23
are common coin in literary and conversational context of the
larger world of ideas: even the specialist might wish to
become better acquainted with such material.
24
Origins often interest us and they often relate to other
interesting and sometimes watershed events. In this history of
the behavioral therapies we see the influence of the Great
Depression; World War II; American ascendancy in the 20th
century; the Cold War and its arms race; the rise of
technology; the economic prosperity of the latter part of the
century at least in the West; as well as the rise and spread of
the research university.
25
state that is closer to what Kuhn calls normal science. Clinical
problems such as enuresis, chronic skills deficits of the
developmentally disabled, and certain anxiety problems were
demonstrated to be remediated, for the first time in history.
Before this psychotherapy was based on less sophisticated
epistemologies. However, this is not to say that before
behavior therapists came on the scene no one was taking a
scientific approach. Clearly this was not the case. The
Minnesota school was taking sophisticated approaches to test
development and validation. The Rogerians, and others, were
doing outcome research. And in fact some of the key
publications of our discipline were short on experimental
rigor. Many were case studies demonstrating the possibilities
of efficacy. Some were single subject designs with no
replications. Some were simply persuasive manifestos.
Although we do not have space here it would be interesting to
trace developments in the quality of the evidence during the
development of behavior therapy. This project could also
attempt to examine if philosophies of science can faithfully
capture these developments. Were behavior therapists
attempting to falsify their commitments, as Popper would
claim constitutes good science? Were they extending
problem-solving exemplars, as Kuhn would suggest? Were
they opportunistic Dadaists, permissively following
Feyerabend’s recommendation, “Anything goes”? Were they
none of these?
26
2. Properties a, b, c … accounted for the success of
behavior therapy.
3. Now behavior therapy has lost or reduced properties
a. b, c, …
4. Therefore if behavior therapists want to increase their
success, they ought to reorient to properties a, b, c,…
Obviously the soundness of this kind of argument depends
upon the accuracies of the historical claims contained in the
first premise. Glib histories can be useful to someone wanting
to make certain points. Glib, superficial histories are plentiful
and the reader ought to be on guard against believing these.
Smith (1986) has nicely shown that glib histories of
behaviorism falsely associated it with logical positivism
instead of characterizing it as associated with indigenous
psychological epistemologies.
27
hard, often as a small minority fighting a powerful and
entrenched majority, because they would not accept as
satisfactory what they saw. What they saw was much human
suffering that was not being effectively treated by the
standard interventions of the day. And what they also saw
was that the alleged evidential basis for claims surrounding
these interventions was problematic. They had a vision that
things could be better and they were willing to pay the price
to attempt to make things better. Their efforts, we believe, are
responsible for bettering the lives of many individuals —
most importantly the clients who experience more relief from
their pain; but these efforts also paved the way for better
opportunities and careers for the second and third generation
behavior therapists, as journals were launched; professional
associations were formed; and inroads to hiring this strange
new breed of therapists and researchers were made. To
paraphrase Isaac Newton, we see farther because we stand on
their shoulders.
Historiography
Behavior therapists tend to be explicitly concerned with
method. Writing a history calls for a historical method.
Historiography is the study of the method of writing history.
As such it concerns itself with a meta-question: How should
history be properly studied? Any history attempts to fairly
capture part of the story knowing that the story also
legitimately could be told in other ways. Because of the
complexity of the tale that the historian is attempting to tell,
the would-be historian makes hard choices. A key choice is
what method is to be used to capture and explicate a slice of
28
the historical record. The historian is constrained by a lack of
knowledge. Much is lost in the mists of the past.
Historical Methods
29
are seen as good or important. Those that hindered or are seen
as irrelevant to what is presently seen as correct are then
depicted as bad or unimportant. Whiggish accounts are
sometimes referred to as “presentist” accounts.
30
more “alive” account. However, there are at least two major
limitations to the autobiographical approach that also need to
be recognized: 1) the biases introduced by allowing
individuals to tell their own stories; and 2) errors introduced
by the particular sample of individuals chosen.
31
Becker, Brady, Eysenck, Ferster, Geer, Gewirtz, Goldiamond,
Greenspoon, Kalish, Kanfer, Keller, Lan, London, Lovaas,
Malott, Marks, Michael, Patterson, Rachman, Salzinger,
Sidman, Staats, Stolz, Ullmann, Yates, among others are not
told in this volume, but if these were a fuller account would
have been made. Another history of the second generation of
behavior therapists would also be key to further understand
the history of behavior therapy. It should be notes that we
largely tell the story of the rise of behavior therapy in the
United States, although it also had important roots in Great
Britain and South Africa. Part of the South African story is
told here in the biography of Wolpe and the autobiography of
Lazarus. However, the British story is admittedly slighted,
mainly due to the logistics of travel. Finally, a more complete
account of the history of the behavioral therapies would track
the influences noted by Krasner and Ullmann (1965):
32
learning theory as a respectable base for clinical
work.
• Classical conditioning as the basis for explaining and
changing normal and deviant behavior (Pavlov, 1928)
interactionism, social psychology and sociology.
• Theoretical concepts and research studies of social
role learning and research in developmental and child
psychology which emphasized vicarious learning and
modeling (Bandura, 1970; Jones, 1924).
• Social influence studies of demand characteristics,
experimenter bias, hypnosis, and placebo (Frank,
1961).
• An environmental social learning model as an
alternative to a disease model of human behavior
(Bandura, 1969; Ullmann & Krasner, 1965).
• Dissatisfaction with psychotherapy and the
psychoanalytic model.
• The development of the clinical psychologist as
scientist-practitioner.
• A group of psychiatrists emphasizing human
interaction (e.g., Adolph Meyer, 1948; Harry Stack
Sullivan, 1953).
• A utopian stream emphasizing the planning of social
environments to elicit and maintain the best of man’s
behavior (e.g., Skinner’s 1976 Walden Two).
We chose first generation behavior therapists as we wanted to
tell the story of the beginning of behavior therapy. We
defined first generation as individuals we found critical to the
development of behavior therapy in the 1950’s and early
1960’s. We chose these particular individuals as they have
been the most influential. Others were excluded either due to
33
their early deaths (e.g., Hans Eysneck, Charles Ferster), space
limitations, or to their personal decision not to participate.
Object Lessons
34
scholarship contained in Ullmann and Krasner’s (1969) early
writings has largely been unmatched. Currently we see a more
narrow technological focus in behavior therapy. The larger
intellectual perspective is largely lost and the behavioral
therapies are not enriched by the broader ideas and
developments in psychology and other fields.
35
assimilated into the power structure; it is also apparent that
we have made compromises. We no longer see radical
critiques of the medical model; we often use the DSM system;
we design treatment packages and research protocols that will
get funded by federal agencies; we eclectically adopt
assessment or treatment methods from other schools; our
ethical challenges to suboptimal treatment are less frequent
and more muted; and our goals have become more modest —
we seek no longer to change institutions or society but rather
to make a living. The boldness, zeal and courage to question
radically and to profess one’s convictions even when
unpopular have largely given way to a discipline which is
more conservative and conventional. Part of a mature
discipline is to be derivative — to do normal science; but in
this we also have lost some of the creativity of the early years.
Where are our revolutionaries that fight against the
problematic compromises or who see the radical possibilities
within our set of commitments and remind us that we are to
go in a vastly different direction? Should we be reminded of
the ideals of the founders and look again whether our
comittments to evidential standards could be improved;
whether our extrapolations from basic psychology could be
better; whether we seek to properly intertwine process and
outcome research; whether behavior therapy is profiting from
corrupt larger intellectual developments?
36
References
Bandura, A. (1969). Principles of behavior modification. New
York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
37
Jones, M. C. (1924). The elimination of children’s fears.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 7, 382-390.
38
O’Donohue, W. & Kitchener, R. (1999). Handbook of
behaviorism. San Diego: Academic Press.
39
Wittgenstein, L. (1958). Philosophical investigations. New
York: Macmillan.
40
Chapter 1
41
Curie aside, it was increasingly difficult to understand
scientific breakthroughs as the achievement of great,
Romantic geniuses working in isolation, against scientific,
religious, or political orthodoxy.
42
But beyond warning that individual biography, including
psychobiography of any kind, is considered old fashioned in
some quarters, my purpose today is not to belabor these
differences among the science studies disciplines. The point
of bringing up science studies is that its emphasis is on
20th-century science, especially recent science and
technology and science policy as they have been transformed
by World War II and postwar developments. The historian of
technology Derek Price (1963) marked this difference as a
distinction between “little science” and “big science.”
43
exponentially from the time of the scientific revolution until it
began leveling off in the 1970’s. It is sobering to think that
about 80% of the scientists who have ever lived are still alive
today. There is therefore some urgency in studying the history
of recent science before the participants die.
44
naïve, an instance of the intentional fallacy and the whig
fallacy, both of which I shall explain later.
45
A second reason for taking case-studies seriously derives
from a more radical suggestion, one that goes to the heart of
learning theory as applied to scientists themselves, that is, to
the very idea of scientific method. The basic idea of this
deflationary conception of method is that method itself is
better construed as a set of exemplary cases than as a set of
rules. This suggestion is at least implicit in The Structure of
Scientific Revolutions. In the next two sections, I proceed to
contrast two approaches to scientific method in this light.
46
well-founded before proceeding to the next step. For them the
source of all meaning was the data language, and the source
of all justification was the empirical data themselves. The
result was a one-dimensional, indeed, one-directional
conception of justification, a cumulative account of scientific
development, both doctrinal and conceptual, and an
empiricist, “building-block” theory of language learning and
concept formation, whereby each theoretical term could be
learned only in terms of less theoretical language and that
language in terms of still less, and so on back to the
observation language.
“White?,” he asked.
“Swan?”
“Curved?”
47
Compare the now-dated behaviorist joke: Two behaviorists
meet on the street. One greets the other: “You feel fine. How
do I feel?”
48
century, was actually an anti-methodologist. Although his
one-time colleague, Feyerabend, was notorious for his book,
Against Method (Feyerabend, 1975), Kuhn himself was
against method in his own way.
And yet Kuhn also has a positive message. We can read him
as propounding the view that the sciences operate by means
of a very different sort of method than the standard,
rule-based one. This is not only an alternative method: it is
also an alternative learning theory (including an account of
concept formation), indeed, an alternative treatment of what it
is to engage in rational inquiry. Like Ludwig Wittgenstein
before him and the cognitive psychologist Eleanor Rosch
(e.g., Rosch, 1973) and her associates after him, Kuhn
rejected the view that a concept is defined in all-or-nothing
fashion in terms of a set of necessary and sufficient conditions
or rules. Rather, concepts are based on a resemblance or
similarity relation, and hence graded.
49
What this amounts to, I would claim, is a rhetorical turn in
our conception of human cognition, of learning and inquiry, a
turn away from logic and toward rhetoric. For rhetoric is
concerned with simile, metaphor, analogy, and such tropes
rather than with sets of logically necessary and sufficient
conditions. Insofar as Kuhn is right, logical rules, where they
function at all in the process of inquiry, are derivative from
exemplars. In my next section I will sketch how this view
extends to a case-based as against a rule-based conception of
scientific inquiry.
50
Chomskian linguistic theory and in standard artificial
intelligence.
51
true hypotheses, by refusing even to entertain them seriously
because they went far beyond currently available evidence.
Nothing ventured, nothing gained. Science is inherently a
risky business. In managing risk, whether in everyday life,
business life, or science, we need to consider the utilities, the
possible payoffs, as well as the epistemic probabilities.
Accordingly, James stressed the importance of the fertility of
a hypothesis. Don’t worry so much about where it came from
but look to where it might lead.
52
or not this claim is certifiably true or this practice completely
unproblematic, does it open up new avenues of inquiry that it
would be fruitful to pursue?
53
e. flourishes on case-based thought and practice over
thoroughgoing rule-based inquiry.
54
‘rule-based reasoning’ (CBR and RBR) are today most
commonly used in artificial intelligence (AI), the basic ideas
have a long history dating back to debates between medieval
logic, with its concern with universality, and rhetoric, with its
concern for exemplum — and even back to Socrates’s attempt
to defend rule-based definition over examples. Although I
think that Kuhn exaggerated the incompatibility of the two
approaches and underestimated the role that rules of various
kinds can play, I shall, for present purposes, continue to draw
the distinction sharply.
55
‘computational’ means and requires. In the heyday of expert
systems research, or knowledge-based computation,
“knowledge engineers” attempted to elicit knowledge-laden
heuristic strategies, in the form of rules, from experts in the
particular field of the AI application. A major difficulty of
this approach was the so-called “knowledge-elicitation
bottleneck” or “Feigenbaum’s problem.” Many experts
claimed not to be using rules at all; and when they did offer
rules that fit the current problem, they often violated these
supposed rules when given new problems. The rules were not
uniformly projectable onto new cases.
Now back in the 1960’s, Kuhn had made exactly the same
prediction of philosophers who tried to reduce scientific
practice to rules. In today’s terminology these philosophers
treated methodology of science either as a rule-based “general
problem solver” (truncated by the logical positivists to a logic
of justification only) or as a kind of content-laden, rule-based,
expert system; whereas, for Kuhn, scientific methodology
(insofar as that enterprise can be defended at all) is a
case-based rather than a rule-based system. Thus AI’s
experience with the knowledge-elicitation bottleneck
confirms Kuhn’s claim that sharing a common practice does
not entail sharing a common, rule-based theory of that
practice (Hoyningen-Huhne, 1993, p. 137). Interestingly, the
“expert systems” philosophers were not only the positivists
and Popperians. They prominently included the new,
historical philosophers of science, who attempted to extract
methodological rules from historical cases. However, the
latter were more interested than the positivists were in
heuristic rules that possess real, problem-solving power.
56
Another difficulty with rule-based reasoning (RBR) is that
rule-based systems do not scale well. Indeed, they typically
become slower and clumsier instead of faster as more
knowledge-based rules are added. They do not degrade
gracefully. Conflicts among rules are hard to avoid, since no
one can see all the implications of present rules or of adding
new rules. (The unavoidable failure to recognize all the
deductive consequences is one aspect of the Meno problem.)
Moreover, in order to be reliable, rule-based systems must be
pretty complete; but that makes them relatively static and
unable to learn from experience, including learning from their
mistakes. These and several other difficulties have dampened
the early enthusiasm for simple, rule-based expert systems as
a model of human scientific inquiry.
57
form of adaptive problem solving. A case-based approach
can, in principle, handle difficult, nonlinear problems in this
fashion, because it does not require a Cartesian
decomposition of a problem or complex system into its
simplest logical components. Hence CBR represents a major
departure from the traditional method of logical analysis and
synthesis. Even when we are dealing with logic problems,
write Rumelhart et al. (1986, 44), “The basic idea is that we
succeed in solving problems not so much through the use of
logic, but by making the problems we wish to solve conform
to problems we are good at solving.”
58
exemplary cases show us how to proceed. In fact, drawing
attention to the applicability to new problems of off-the-shelf
exemplars is probably the most important facet of heuristic
appraisal.
4. Various Difficulties
However, not everything here is sweetness and light. A
case-based account of problem solving faces difficulties of its
own. What is to count as a case? How do we produce the very
first exemplars of a new field? At the other end of this
spectrum, which of the zillion possible cases are to be
retained in the case library? How can relevant cases be
indexed and retrieved? By what mechanism is a new case
recognized to be relevantly similar to one case but not to
another? In scientific research what and where is this case
library, anyway? Is it internal to the individual investigator or
does it reside externally in the informational resources of the
community, e.g., in real libraries or distributed over
communities of investigators? Clearly, retaining in accessible,
episodic memory all instructive, problem-solving experiences
as distinct cases is an impossibility. It is too piecemeal.
59
We must ask similar, skeptical questions about Kuhnian
exemplars and the processes by which they are constructed,
indexed, retrieved, and activated. His own system of
case-based reasoning turns out to be too simple. In fact, a
CBR perspective already reveals some shortcomings of
Kuhn’s account of exemplars. For example, Kuhn’s
exemplars are all positive achievements, whereas, typically,
some of the most exemplary lessons are negative. Janet
Kolodner (1995) stresses that we need to include the notable
failures also — the “war stories.” Kuhn 1974 does note the
need to learn dissimilarities among things when acquiring the
similarity relations, but his scientific exemplars all seem to be
achievements rather than failures (Hoyningen, 1993). In
neglecting what we learn from negative exemplars, Kuhn’s
position is too far from Popper’s!
60
how present problems and commitments shape our recall of
past work. The scientist qua scientist is necessarily a bad
historian (Nickles, 1992). We should expect a mutual
adjusting, a mutual fitting, of old exemplars to new problems,
not a one-way influence. Hence, Kuhn’s account is
dynamically inadequate. This point is directly relevant to the
concerns of this conference on the history and future of
behavioral therapy.
61
backward-looking (or, rather, forward-looking from the point
of view of some time now past). Moreover, with each major
success, indeed with each grant proposal, scientists rewrite
the previous history of their subfield in order to make their
present work look like a logically plausible, if not inevitable,
continuation of previous work. Scientists, as such, use history
to clarify and advance their current projects. In this respect,
good science is bad history, and vice versa (see Nickles,
1992).
62
be a struggle for authority over the historical record. Are
professional historians or the interested participants
themselves the authorities?
63
(i.e., history as a list of established facts and dates), but also
over history as the late-career reflections of practicing
scientists themselves.
64
framework of the few cases already available. This is the
mistake, if your only tool is a hammer, of seeing every
problem as a nail.
65
shaped directly by the contingencies of direct experience.
Here I am invoking something like Skinner’s distinction
between contingency-shaped versus rule-governed learning
(Skinner, 1989). In any case, Kuhnian exemplars cannot be
fully conveyed by verbal instruction.
66
communication” (Kuhn, 1962). For recognition of
problem-solving progress is now more a matter of one’s
rhetorical tradition than of logic-plus-empirical data. Science
turns out to be a highly cultural activity!
References
Boring, E. (1929). A history of experimental psychology. New
York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
67
Hoyningen-Huene, P. (1993). Reconstructing science:
Thomas Kuhn’s philosophy of science. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.
68
Latour, B., & Woolgar, S. (1986). Laboratory life (2nd ed.).
Princeton: Princeton University Press.
69
social dimensions of science (pp. 85-129). Notre Dame:
University of Notre Dame Press.
70
Rumelhart, D., Smolensky, P., McClelland, J., & Hinton, G.
(1986). Schemata and sequential thought processes in PDP
models. In J. McClelland & D. Rumelhart (Eds.), Parallel
distributed processing (Vol. 2). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
71
Notes
1
I am indebted to the U. S. National Science Foundation for
research support on heuristic appraisal and problem solving,
and to the conference participants for helpful discussion.
Section 3 and parts of section 4 are borrowed from Nickles
(2000).
72
Chapter 2
73
An old proverb states, “As the twig is bent, so grows the
tree.” The course of Joseph Wolpe’s intellectual development
can be traced not only to his childhood, but even before that
to his family history. His grandparents emigrated from
Lithuania at the end of the 19th century, a time of reprisals
against its Jewish population, and settled in Johannesburg,
South Africa. Lithuanian Jews had a long tradition of
scholarship and respect for learning, and this was true in
Joseph’s own family. His parents, while not religious,
maintained traditional values of self-discipline, hard-work,
and learning. As the oldest of four children, young Joseph did
not disappoint them. He was a precocious and avid reader,
fond of sports and sports stories, but was not a gifted athlete
himself. Rather, he entered and won numerous scholastic
competitions, winning prizes in a wide variety of subjects.
Thus, early on a competitive repertoire was shaped, a
repertoire that persisted throughout his professional career.
74
As a compromise between his wishes to be a chemical
researcher and his parents’ wishes for a respectable career,
Joseph chose to study medicine, following the British model
of a six-year university curriculum after high school.
University provided new intellectual avenues for him to
explore. He discovered the joys of discussion and debate of
philosophical issues with other young medical students. He
was interested in epistemology, finding the empiricist
philosophers to be more satisfactory in explaining the origins
of knowledge and the operations of the mind.
75
disappointing. Few young soldiers were returned to adaptive
functioning as a result of their treatment. As he did earlier,
Wolpe participated in vigorous discussion with his peers. But
rather than broad questions of epistemology, these discussions
focused on the causes and cures of neurosis.
76
Freud’s theories of conflicting unconscious forces, and saw in
Hull’s system an alternative to both. Just how this might take
form was unclear, but it provided him a direction for further
study.
77
general paradigm. One feature was fairly consistent, however;
once it occurred, experimental neurosis was very difficult to
get rid of. This seeming permanence was responsible for
Pavlov’s suggestion of neurologic damage, a point Wolpe
was not willing to concede.
78
anxiety functioned as both a response and a drive. It was
evoked during acquisition by the unconditioned stimulus, and
during maintenance by the conditioned stimulus. When the
stimulus terminated, the anxiety drive was reduced,
reinforcing the anxiety response. Thus even though the
primary aversive stimulus no longer occurred, anxiety
continued to be reinforced through its own reduction, making
it remarkably persistent.
79
branch. This explained the food refusal in anxious animals
and it also suggested a means of overcoming anxiety — by
feeding. If feeding could be made to occur in a situation that
evoked anxiety, then anxiety would be inhibited in that
situation and its bond to that stimulus weakened. This set the
stage for the second part of Wolpe’s cat experiment,
eliminating the experimental neurosis by feeding in the
presence of attenuated anxiety-evoking stimuli.
80
materialistic stance, opposed to mysticism and mentalism. His
study of medicine reinforced this position, adding a
physiologic reductionist point of view. From Freud he took
the idea of anxiety as the core of maladaptive behavior, and
also the idea that anxiety was acquired and could be removed
— though not in the ways that Freud proposed. From Pavlov
came the idea that neurosis could be studied experimentally in
the laboratory, its factors teased apart and examined in a
quantitative fashion. Pavlov also advanced the idea of a
neurologic substrate of behavior, though Wolpe did not
accept the mechanisms he suggested. Hull provided the
specific stimulus-response-reinforcement framework to
organize the study of behavior; Wolpe extrapolated these
principles from rats in a runway to people trapped by
crippling fears. Finally, Sherrington’s principle of reciprocal
inhibition suggested how to counteract destructive arousal
and replace maladaptive with adaptive behavior.
81
(1952a) published a similar critique, with the added feature of
describing the success of his therapy procedures. Eysenck’s
article seemed to kick open an anthill and traditional therapy
proponents swarmed to the counterattack. But the challenge
was not simply to prove effectiveness compared to base rates
of recovery; the comparison now was with this newfangled
therapy based on learning theory.
82
provided much of the ammunition in these early battles. In
addition, his competitive juices were stirred by Eysenck’s
combativeness, and he carried on as a leader on the American
front.
83
principles were not relevant for the emotional problems that
characterized neurotic behavior. For their part, most behavior
analysts did not address the types of clinical problems that
Wolpe dealt with and did not consider his physiologic
reductionist approach to be consistent with functional
analysis.
84
and, most notably, the work of Steve Hayes and his
colleagues on Relational Frame Theory and Acceptance and
Commitment Therapy (Hayes & Wilson, 1995). It will be
interesting to see if such insurgencies are the beginning of a
counter-counterrevolution.
Therapy as Education
85
An educational approach also requires assessment. Wolpe
proposed to measure the problematic behavior of the client in
response to the environment in which it occurred. As with
treatment procedures, some of the assessment methods he
helped develop are currently useful, such as the Life History
Questionnaire and the Fear Survey Schedule. He advocated
what he initially called a “stimulus-response analysis”
(Wolpe, 1969) and later termed “behavior analysis of case
dynamics” (Wolpe, 1990), that is consistent with what many
now term a “functional analysis.” The goal is to determine the
specific needs of the client which, in turn, determines the
therapy procedures to be employed.
Desensitization Research
86
comprised a huge pool of potential subjects with whom
methods of anxiety reduction could be investigated. Jerry
Davison’s (1968) dissertation, employing this population, was
a paragon of experimental rigor that demonstrated the
effectiveness of desensitization while controlling for
extraneous factors. Gordon Paul’s (1966) dissertation,
comparing desensitization with insight therapy in college
students with public speaking anxiety, was an immediate
classic. Hundreds if not thousands of studies followed in the
next decade, investigating the variables and parameters that
comprised desensitization and, to a lesser extent,
assertiveness training, along with alternative methods and
explanations. Other procedures, such as flooding and
modeling, were investigated in a similar fashion. The net
result was an immense bulwark of empirically validated
procedures that formed the identifying characteristic of
behavior therapy and distinguished it from traditional
psychotherapies.
Clinical Trials
87
revolutionary in the 1950’s, was to rate the outcome of all
patients he had seen who had met certain diagnostic and
treatment criteria. Wolpe (1952a, 1954, 1958) reported 90%
cured or much improved, in contrast with the two-thirds
base-rate recovery reported in Eysenck’s (1952) review.
Traditional psychotherapists were challenged to prove
Eysenck wrong, while behavior therapists took up the
challenge to prove Wolpe right. Analogue research promoted
more sophisticated designs than Wolpe’s retrospective case
summarization, and controlled prospective studies were soon
undertaken with clinical populations as well.
88
others were not content to settle for “equality,” they wanted
clear superiority. Closer reading of the data indicated a
definite edge in favor of behavior therapy (e.g. Giles, 1983;
Poppen, 1976), but the “equal effectiveness” conclusion
prevailed.
The same controversy was played out a few years later when
nearly 500 therapy outcome studies were evaluated with the
statistical technique of meta-analysis (Smith, Glass, & Miller,
1980). These authors included the full spectrum of analogue
and clinical studies, identifying three major classes of therapy
(Behavioral, Verbal, and Developmental), made up of six
subclasses, in turn comprised of 18 individual types. The
largest contributor to their data pool was desensitization
research. Behavioral therapies, at all levels of groupings,
showed consistently greater effects than verbal therapies, and
both were much superior to developmental counseling. But
Smith et al. downplayed the effectiveness of behavior therapy
by attributing the differences to measurement bias,
recommending that the “benefit of the doubt should be
granted to theories that lack technologies readily applicable to
outcome evaluation” (p. 31). Traditional psychotherapists felt
that meta-analysis research upheld the equal effectiveness
notion, while Wolpe and others criticized the meta-analysis
procedure, or the studies included in them, or reanalyzed the
data to show the superiority of behavior therapy.
89
out such projects, his contributions have been immense. His
claims of behavior therapy superiority spurred the showdown
studies that led to grudging acceptance of its equality and
final recognition as the treatment of choice for many
disorders.
90
one, and the student would be well advised to compare the
issues then and now; what has changed and what issues
remain?
91
Professional Training
92
Wolpe’s theories and methods were subjected to rigorous
investigation. Variations, additions, and alternatives arose
from cognitive and operant learning theories. These
challenged the benchmarks established by Wolpe and greatly
expanded the scope of behavior therapy. Progress is achieved
through competition, and we have all benefited from Wolpe’s
willingness to take controversial positions.
Significant Publications
Wolpe’s publishing career did not begin auspiciously. He at
first was determined to be a Hullian learning theorist.
However his efforts in this direction were not well received.
Only when he published the results of his clinical work did he
begin to be noticed. Thus the reaction Wolpe received to his
initial publications very much influenced the development of
behavior therapy.
93
for such a work. Interestingly, the following year saw the
publication of Personality and Psychotherapy by John
Dollard and Neil Miller (1950), and Learning Theory and
Personality Dynamics by O. H. Mowrer (1950). As two of the
most prominent students of Hull, Miller and Mowrer had
conducted basic research on anxiety conditioning in the
1940’s and in these works extended Hullian theory to account
for human neuroses. Both also were influenced greatly by
Freudian theory and offered nothing other than
psychoanalysis, couched in learning theory terminology, as
treatment. It should be remembered that, at this time, Wolpe
himself had not developed any new clinical procedures based
on his learning principles. Any therapeutic applications were
purely speculative and it is perhaps fortunate that his
manuscript was not published before he had done the hard
work of translating principles into practice.
94
exercises in logic. The recent surge of interest in “neural
network learning theory” (Tryon, 1993) might find it
profitable to revisit these neglected papers of Wolpe.
95
work to date, including his neural theory of conditioning (a
reprise of his “neuropsychological view” papers), a critical
review of the experimental neurosis literature, and a
recounting of his cat experiment. He added a theoretical
account of the acquisition of various types of human
neuroses, including pervasive anxiety, hysteria, and
obsessional behavior, providing illustrations from his patients.
The last half of the book gave specific details of his therapy
procedures. He described the assessment interview and
advocated the use of the Willoughby Personality Inventory,
both as a means to discover information and as an assessment
device to measure therapeutic change. He described his
therapeutic procedures in detail, presenting individual cases to
illustrate their use. Finally he supplemented his earlier
outcome reports with an additional 88 cases, reporting an
overall success rate of 90%.
96
brief descriptions of procedures that he later identified as
instances of “cognitive” procedures — if one chose to use that
terminology — namely “clarifying misconceptions” and
“thought-stopping.” These took up about 1% of his first three
books. His two final editions included chapters on “cognitive
therapeutic techniques” and presented critiques of the
“cognitive revolution” in behavior therapy, taking up about
17% of his last book. Finally, Wolpe first recognized operant
conditioning in 1969, where he included a brief chapter in an
effort to be inclusive of the entire field of behavior therapy.
However the topic comprised only 2-3% of this and
subsequent editions. In the final analysis, these books reflect
the increasing battles Wolpe had with cognitive approaches,
his rather cursory recognition of behavior analysis, and his
continued faith in the efficacy of his own approach to
behavior therapy.
Effectiveness of Therapy
97
others’ case reports, has evolved into multimethod-multitrait
assessment batteries involving direct observation,
physiological measurement, self-report, significant-other
report, and medical records, in longitudinal, long-term
follow-up, blinded clinical trials. The sophistication and
expense of outcome research has grown and will continue to
do so. However, in his own work and his editorship of
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry,
Wolpe supported the innovative case report and small-n
study. This approach continues to provide the seeds for new
and more effective procedures, and both large and small-scale
studies are necessary for continued development.
98
necessity of concurrent behavioral intervention, such as the
combination of behavioral procedures and the nicotine patch
for treating smoking. However, these approaches do not
address the factors that may make drugs useful in one case
and not in another. Nor do they solve the problem of how to
make a profit delivering a behavioral program whether or not
drugs are involved. “Behavioral health” programs are a
definite trend, and their control over treatment is an important
issue for the future.
99
Wolpe opened the door of the consulting room and clearly
described desensitization, assertiveness training, sexual
therapy, and other procedures. Another important step
involved the search for “common factors” across various
therapy approaches. Jerome Frank (1961) is known for this
line of investigation, but early in his career Wolpe (1958, p.
193) recognized that “the various special points of procedure
that the different therapists regarded as so vital to success
were not vital at all, and that the effective factor must have
been something that all the therapeutic situations generated in
common.” Wolpe, and many since, have speculated on what
those common features are and the mechanism of their action
(Arkowitz, 1992). This issue is also likely to continue into the
future.
100
designated as “best practice” to be employed for particular
disorders. Managed care companies are interested in best
practices and treatment guidelines, and it is likely this trend
will markedly increase in the future.
101
outcome data on current methods are good, there is still much
room for improvement and it would be premature to close the
book — or the manual — at this point.
102
No theory has yet provided an overarching account of all
methods at all steps that is acceptable to all of us. This
conference may be helpful in that regard. It shows us that
behavior therapy has progressed along the scientific path that
Wolpe was one of the first to point out. Science is a
cumulative and corrective process, an evolution born of
competitive behavior and professional disputation. Perhaps
there is a young Wolpe in the audience who is saying, “I can
do better than that,” a young person who sees how things fit
together in ways that we currently do not, and who can take
behavior therapy to yet a higher level of effectiveness or
theoretical integration. That would be a fitting legacy to this
great champion of behavior therapy.
References
Arkowitz, H. (1992). Integrative theories of therapy. In D. K.
Freedman (Ed.), History of psychotherapy: A century of
change (pp. 261-303). Washington, DC, American
Psychological Association.
103
Eysenck, H. J. (Ed.). (1960). Behaviour therapy and the
neuroses. London: Pergamon Press.
104
Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (3rd ed.,
pp. 627-670). New York: Wiley.
105
Paul, G. (1966). Insight versus desensitization in
psychotherapy. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
106
Salter, A. (1949). Conditioned reflex therapy. New York:
Creative Age Press.
107
Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
108
109
Chapter 3
B. F. Skinner’s Contribution
to Therapeutic Change: An
Agency-less, Contingency
Analysis
Julie S. Vargas
110
consequences of individual actions. Changes in the rate of bar
pressing occurred through a process of selection by
consequences, not through the pairing of antecedent stimuli.
His functional analysis showed how behavior is related to,
and thus can be controlled by, factors in an individual’s
environment, without appealing to psychological processes in
the “mind” or physiological processes in the brain. Practices
incorporating Skinner’s discoveries rippled throughout
society, impacting particularly education and behavior
therapy. How did a small-town Pennsylvania lad come to
make such an impact on the twentieth century?
Intellectual Background
Skinner grew up in a typical family of four in a small railroad
town in Pennsylvania. Life was good. His father’s law
profession was growing and the family could afford the newly
appearing fruits of science and industrialization — electricity,
the radio, the telephone, silent movies, and when Skinner was
six years old, the automobile. Skinner attended the local
public school. Early on he began challenging conventional
thinking. In his autobiography, Skinner tells of an incident
that happened in eighth grade. His English class was studying
Shakespeare’s As You Like It. At home, the young Fred had
heard someone question the authorship of the plays and he
announced to his class that Shakespeare didn’t write the play
they were reading. His teacher, Miss Graves, told him he
didn’t know what he was talking about. As Skinner puts it,
“that afternoon I went down to the library and found a copy of
Sir Edwin Durning-Lawrence’s Bacon is Shakespeare which I
read in great excitement. The next day, to Miss Graves’s
dismay I knew only too well what I was talking about.” His
111
teacher permitted discussion and Skinner continued to gather
evidence for his case, reading “biographies of Bacon,
summaries of his philosophical position, and a good deal of
the Advancement of Learning, the Essays, and Novum
Organum” (Skinner, 1976, p. 129).
112
took a job at a bookstore in Greenwich Village in New York,
living the bohemian life and subscribing to literary magazines
like the Dial. It was in the Dial that Skinner read a review by
Bertrand Russell in which Russell called Watson’s
Behaviorism “massively impressive.” Skinner bought
Watson’s book and also Russell’s Philosophy. The latter
book, Skinner says, “begins with a careful statement of
several epistemological issues raised by behaviorism
considerably more sophisticated than anything of Watson’s”
(Skinner, 1979, p. 10). Years later, Skinner told Russell that
his book Philosophy had converted him to behaviorism.
Russell exclaimed, “My God, I thought I demolished that
view.” But Skinner had stopped reading at chapter eight
where Russell talks of that nature of the physical world, and
thus missed the last third in which Russell criticizes
behaviorism. Towards the end of his Greenwich Village stay,
Skinner decided to go back to school to study psychology.
Following advice from an old Hamilton professor about
where to go, he applied to Harvard. His training was about to
begin in earnest.
Graduate Training
113
bang against a block of wood. Boring’s presence extended to
every psychology classroom in the form of piped compressed
air for producing low whistles for students to compare. This
was not what Skinner planned to study. But fortunately, when
Skinner arrived, Boring was away on Sabbatical finishing his
book, the History of Psychology. Skinner looked for courses
to take and found one in the biology department that looked
promising. To his delight, the text discussed Pavlov’s work!
The course was taught by Hudson Hoagland, a young
instructor brought in by William Crozier, chair of a newly
created Department of Physiology. Crozier had studied with
Jacques Loeb. (It was a small world. Loeb had corresponded
with Mach and had taught a course that Watson took at the
University of Chicago.) Like Loeb, who was said to “resent
the nervous system,” Crozier had no use for explanations that
appealed to inner processes. Explanation consisted of finding
functional relationships between the manipulations of the
experimenter and the resulting behavior of the organism —
the same platform Watson had espoused. As E. Vargas put it,
the match between Crozier and Skinner was “ a professional
marriage made in heaven: Crozier — caustic, hard-driving
and hard-drinking, impatient, contemptuous of what he called
organ physiology, an advocate of Loeb in biology and Mach
in philosophy; and Skinner — sarcastic, radical and
rebellious, impatient, contemptuous of compromisers, and
eager to put the investigation of behavior on an independent
scientific footing” (Vargas, 1995, p. 108). In his second
course in physiology Skinner was assigned research with a
more senior student on Crozier’s favorite subject, tropisms.
This resulted in Skinner first published research article
(jointly with T. Cunliffe Barnes). Crozier was an editor of the
Journal of General Psychology, and the Barnes and Skinner
article “The Progressive Increase in the Geotropic Response
114
of the Ant Aphaenogaster,” appeared in that journal (Barnes
& Skinner, 1930). Predictably, it references Crozier and Loeb.
115
and then giving a bottle contingently when the baby uttered
“da.” He analyzed the process, however, as Pavlovian
conditioning to the “da” stimulus preceding responding
(Watson, 1924 p. 182). Watson’s procedure of presenting
stimuli in “trials” also made it difficult to see relationships
between actions and consequences independently of the role
of preceding stimuli.
116
ordering empirical knowledge with respect to some
specifiable domain of events” (Skinner, 1999, p. 108). To
Skinner, this was the reverse of Bacon’s dictum that books
should follow science, science should not follow books. The
“method” of science is what scientists do, not a set of rules or
procedures. Skinner had already published an article in which
he argued against the methodology of statistics and
hypothesis testing, offering his own behavior to illustrate how
discoveries come about. He submitted a revised version of
that article for the book. The only rules provided, such as
“When you run onto something interesting, drop everything
else and study it,” or “Some people are lucky,” are serious
suggestions, though phrased whimsically as if in contempt for
rules of science.
117
Parthenon and built a platform on piano wires to measure the
motion of baby rats when pulled increasingly by the tail.
Extending the work to adult rats and abandoning attachments
to the animals, Skinner build an eight-foot runway in a tunnel,
again recording the movements of the rats as clicks were
sounded. Note that he was still investigating reflexes in a trial
format — the responses of the rats to an antecedent click.
118
experimenter produces each stimulus to which the organism is
to respond, making rate meaningless. In Skinner’s apparatus,
the rat could produce a pellet at any time without waiting for
any antecedent stimulus. In a letter to his parents he explained
that he had demonstrated that “the rate in which a rat eats
food, over a period of two hours, is a square function of the
time. In other words, what heretofore was supposed to be
”free” behavior on the part of the rat is now shown to be just
as much subject to natural laws as, for example, the rate of his
pulse” (Skinner, 1979. p. 59). But though freed from the
restrictions of “trials”, in the publication of his experiments
Skinner still talked of “eating reflexes”. Time since the
beginning of the eating period served as the antecedent
stimulus. He had not yet broken free of the stimulus-response
tradition. Skinner reported on his work correlating rate of
eating as a function of time in two more articles. The first,
submitted in July 7, 1931, describes apparatus with a door
that the rat pushes open to get at pellets. In the article Skinner
grapples with the problem of lack of consistency between
stimuli and responses that you should get in a reflex: “If it is
in fact true that a rat’s approach to a bit of food is reflex, why
is the response not always evoked by the appropriate
stimulus?” (Skinner, 1932a, p. 32). He appeals to a “third
variable”, conditions such as deprivation of food imposed by
the experimenter to take the place of what other psychologists
would call “drive”, thus avoiding any need to appeal to
internal physiology. In the second article, submitted three
months later, he describes a “problem box” with a lever —
the first mention of an operant chamber in Skinner’s
published works. In this article, submitted three years after
arriving at Harvard, Skinner talks of “eating behavior” as a
chain of reflexes, still in an S-R format: “The stimuli for the
119
initial members of this sequence of reflexes emanate from the
food or the food tray.” (Skinner, 1932b).
120
The central relationship Skinner discovered was that of the
operant, a class of behaviors defined by a common effect on
the environment. The bar press in the operant chamber was
defined by the closing of a switch, not by the form of the
motions of the rat. Skinner realized that even if the rat falls
upon the lever, for example when slipping from climbing up
the corner of the box, the depression of the lever is recorded
as a bar press. Such data are just as legitimate as a press by a
paw, but until bar presses are brought under experimental
control of consequences, the actions that close the switch are
not considered part of the operant of bar pressing. Unlike
respondent conditioning, operant conditioning is controlled
not by the stimuli that precede actions, but by those that
follow. Operant behavior is a two-term relationship in which
no identifiable stimulus need precede a response.
121
approach to how behavior originates. Reinforcing
consequences select behavior, making it more likely to occur
again under similar circumstances. The process of selection
not only increases a particular response, it shifts a whole
gradient of properties of responding, gradually producing
novel responses, much as new species are created by the
selection of individuals. Just as Darwin’s analysis provides
the mechanism through which the extraordinary variety of
species could arise, Skinner’s analysis gave a plausible
account of the origins of novel or unusual behavior in people
including many of the kinds of behaviors with which
therapists deal. Interestingly, Skinner didn’t draw the
selection analogy until very late in his career. It appears in the
title of a 1981 article in which Skinner talks of three kinds of
selection affecting what people do, natural selection, operant
conditioning, and cultural evolution (Skinner, 1981). Of
course antecedent stimuli come to gain control over operant
behavior, too, but only by being present when
action-consequence relationships occur.
122
as how behaviors could be changed. Direct manipulation of
consequences of client behavior was all that was needed. To
directly shape behavior, however, the therapist had to interact
with patients as behavior occurred. The therapist who sees a
client only in office visits is not present to set up
consequences following specific problem behaviors of a
client. But in institutions, more continuous interactions were
possible. Thus it was in institutional settings that operant
conditioning as a therapeutic technique first occurred.
123
problems as diverse as stuttering (Flanagan, Goldiamond, &
Azrin, 1958, p. 177), multiple tics (Barrett, 1962/66), and
wearing or glasses by a child with autism (Wolf, Mees, &
Risley, 1964/66). By 1964, Lindsley could report that “more
than 100 applications of free-operant methods to human
behavioral pathology have been published (Lindsley, 1966, p.
167).
124
therapist must rely, therefore, not on direct observation and
shaping of behavior, but on patient report. Thus verbal
behavior enters strongly into the standard therapeutic
relationship.
125
with all three categories, a mediator serves both to reinforce
specific verbal responses and to provide a discriminative
stimulus for general aspects of verbal behavior, such as
starting to speak or the language spoken. Complex
relationships such as those involved in “grammatical
construction” were also addressed in the book.
126
Influence of Skinner’s Analysis of
Verbal Behavior on Therapy
The distinction between mand and tact has led to
technological improvements in the field of special education.
Traditionally, children with autism were taught first to name
objects. When they learned to say “milk” when shown milk, it
was assumed that they “knew the word,” and could ask for
milk if they wanted it, even if milk was not currently visible
to them. Mark Sundberg and his colleagues showed that, just
as Skinner had predicted in Verbal Behavior, the two “milks”
were separate operants (Skinner, 1957). Teaching a child to
tact did not necessarily enable that child to mand. The same
form of response (saying “milk”) under one set of controls did
not always occur under a second set of controls. When
different operants are typically learned far apart in time, as
with speaking and reading, everyone recognizes the
difference in controls: No one would expect a child who can
say “milk” to automatically then be able to read that word.
Reading has to be taught separately. Similarly mands and
tacts may need to be taught separately to children with
autism. Since the mand is maintained by getting the specified
object or event, while tacts are maintained by generalized
reinforcement such as social approval, Sundberg and his
associates found that it was easier to teach mands than tacts.
Children learn much more quickly to “ask for what they
want,” than to “name objects”. Where it could take literally
months to teach a rudimentary tact vocabulary of four to five
terms, by reversing the order and teaching mands first,
children more rapidly learned a basic vocabulary. (See the
127
journal, The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, for additional
applications of Skinner’s analysis.)
128
Internal Feelings
129
pain often describe it as feeling “like a red hot poker”. Lastly
much behavior is learned at the overt level, and then reduces
to the covert. We learn to read at first aloud, but gradually the
pronunciation of words recedes to the covert level, with, in
some readers, a slight movement of the lips revealing the
origin of the behavior. (It is also interesting that when the
covert level is inadequate, as when reading very difficult
directions or in a noisy environment, the behavior is likely to
reappear at the overt level.) In a similar way, a person may
tact a physical reach for a cigarette in the pocket where she
used to keep cigarettes, then only start to reach, and finally
reach at such a small magnitude that only the ex-smoker can
identify each incident as an “impulse.” In researching the
origins of dozens of terms describing internal feelings, (such
as “anxiety”) Skinner found that they originated as terms
describing overt behaviors. “Anxiety,” for example originated
in the word for the overt action “choke.” Gradually the
word’s meaning shifted to describing events inside the skin.
Still, a verbal community taught its new members to describe
“feeling anxious.”
Awareness or Consciousness
130
of other steps in stairs you encountered. The phrase
“conscious of the number of steps” would than be applied to
your behavior. The same process occurs when the
reinforcement is less conspicuous, such as social approval.
Thus the community of people with whom clients interact
determines awareness. One part of that community is the
therapist. In therapy sessions, the therapist can increase a
client’s awareness of aspects of his or her life by the kinds of
statements to which the therapist attends. The only drawback
to working primarily with verbal behavior is that what clients
say may not accurately reflect critical components of a their
life.
131
By extending the principles discovered in the laboratory to
verbal behavior and to an analysis of how covert behavior
comes about, Skinner opened the way for therapists to base
therapy on covert behaviors and covert reinforcement. This
made it possible to bring a client’s problems into the
therapist’s office, since scenarios, visualized actions, and
imagined consequences are not restricted to a particular place.
In the 1950’s Wolpe used visualization to desensitize patient
fears in a respondent conditioning format. If respondent
extinction through imaging of antecedent stimuli could help a
patient, why couldn’t imagery be used in an operant
paradigm? In 1966, Joe Cautela began using imagery with
operant behaviors. He described his “covert conditioning”
procedures as “the modification of a behavior by imagining
particular consequences to influence the behavior in the
desired directions” (Cautela, 1986, xii). The “particular
consequences” were reinforcement, extinction, and
punishment, taken directly from Skinner’s work. Covert
conditioning has been used successfully in treating a wide
variety of patient problems, including anxiety, athletic
performance, pain, depression, aggression, and sexual
disorders (Cautela & Kearny, 1993). Skinner’s assertion that
verbal and covert behaviors obey the same laws, and that they
interact with each other and with overt behaviors has been
born out in therapeutic applications.
132
into play. Training may guide the initial interview — whether
inventories are given or not, and which ones, or what
questions are asked. But as interactions continue, other factors
enter in. Current cultural practices affect therapeutic practice.
Sometime back, therapists would try to change the sexual
orientation of a homosexual client. Today that is not
necessarily the accepted approach. Standards of ethical
conduct also change according to the times. But ultimately,
like the experimenter shaping behavior in a rat, chimpanzee,
pigeon, or any other convenient platform, and like a teacher
shaping behavior of a baby, child, or adult, the therapist, in
order to be maximally effective, must come under control of
change in behavior of the client. While the precision obtained
by cumulative records may not be feasible for tracking daily
behavior, direct continuous measurement of the rate of
relevant behaviors is the goal to approximate.
133
last talk, Skinner warned against “cognitive” approaches,
drawing an analogy between them and “creationism.” Only
by abandoning internal causal agents will therapists turn full
attention to the contingency relationships responsible for the
behaviors they treat.
Summary
Therapy was not Skinner’s field. But, in one sense he was a
therapist. For he contributed an analysis of behavior which,
combined with the conditioning procedures worked out by
Pavlov, encompass all behavior. Skinner discovered the
critical role of postcedent events both in selecting actions that
become part of an individual’s repertoire and in determining
how antecedent events, through pairing with selective
processes, gain control over behavior. He spent thousands of
hours in research, changing every conceivable aspect of
contingencies and noting the effects on the probability of
behavior, measured as rate of actions. By working out the
complexities of how the rate of operant behavior is a function
of environmental variables, Skinner showed how maladaptive
behaviors could have originated, and how alternative
repertoires could be established to take their place.
134
make their own beds, get themselves to meals on time, and
even to enjoy social events — all by changing contingencies.
135
illness or self concept. At worst, statements that sound like
explanations hinder further inquiry. By excluding agency
entirely, behavior can be functionally related to
environmental events — the same events that an agency
analysis must eventually consider. Until Skinner’s discovery
of the role of consequences in selecting behavior, and the
hundreds of experiments investigating the relationship of
contingencies to the probability of particular actions, the
functional relationship between behavior and environmental
events was not well understood. With the science of
contingent relationships between behavior and environmental
events, the therapist has a comprehensive, well-researched,
and effective approach upon which to base practice.
References
Ayllon, T., & Michael, J. (1959). The psychiatric nurse as a
behavioral engineer. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
Behavior, 2, 323-334.
136
Flanagan, B., Goldiamond, I., & Azrin, N. (1958). Operant
stuttering: that control of stuttering behavior through
response-contingent consequences. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1, 173-177.
137
Skinner, B. F. (1956/1999). A case history in scientific
method. In Cumulative record: Definitive edition (pp.
108-131). Acton, MA: Copley Publishing Group.
138
Footnotes
1
Skinner called this category “verbal behavior under control
of verbal stimuli” and used “intraverbal” for one special case.
E. A. Vargas suggested using “intraverbal” for the whole
category and in a private conversation Skinner agreed that
such designation would have been better. “But,” he said, “it
would be too hard to change now.” Some of us are still trying.
139
Chapter 4
140
contributions has been limited, and I hope to redress that
balance to some extent here.
141
his daughter (Helene Juliet Kantor — ”Bobby”), and in this
period created some of his most important works (e.g.,
Kantor, 1981). (See Kantor, 1976; Mountjoy & Cone, in
press; Mountjoy & Hansor, 1986; and Wolf, 1984, for
additional details). During his retirement he traveled and
lectured frequently, including several visits to Mexico, on the
first of which he received a standing ovation and a gold medal
(Ribes, 1984). Death came in February of 1984, seemingly in
the midst of his creative and active life, since he had
published one book and two journal articles in that year, as
well as having prepared a book chapter which was published
posthumously. A Festschrift was presented (Smith, Mountjoy,
& Ruben, 1983) only a few months prior to Kantor’s death.
142
taught that although light could be slowed slightly by the
medium through which it passed, it was for all practical
purposes constant at about 186,300 miles per second in a
vacuum. Hau, Harris, Dutton, & Behrooz (1999) announced
that with the use of a Bose-Einstein condensate (BEC), they
had slowed light to a velocity of 17 meters per second
(approximately 38 miles per hour). This was accomplished by
shining a “coupling” laser into the opaque BEC, then also
firing a second laser beam into the BEC so that the two beams
interacted in a process called electromagnetically induced
transparency. Only 25% of the luminous energy passed
through the formerly opaque BEC, and it was slowed to only
17 meters per second. They expect the technique to further
allow them to slow light down to 37 meters per hour. Quite an
unusual and unexpected finding.
143
person who is behaving within a certain space-time
framework (setting, or context), and who has a unique
interbehavioral (learning, or behavioral) history. This history
consists not only of the specialized professional training
received in both undergraduate and graduate school, but is
also comprised of both the post-training self-learning features
of academic-scientific-research activities, and the cultural
shaping procedures of pre-professional training. After all, we
are usually reasonably law-abiding (follow the rules of our
larger cultural group, and the many cultural sub-groups to
which we belong), but we all are shaped to be politically
conservative or liberal, and some of us are trained to be
religious in various degrees, etc. This baggage of our early
life does follow us about as we attempt to do the scientific
job, and it does to some extent influence our scientific
behavior. However when these social preconceptions intrude
too much we can have a scientific problem. In other words,
science is composed of two complementary and necessary
factors: One is empirical investigation, and the second is the
logical analysis, by means of which we examine among other
things, the influence of cultural factors upon scientific work
(Kantor, 1958/1959, 1963-1969). Throughout the history of
science we see the constant interplay of these two essential
components of our scientific activities (see, e.g., Burtt, 1924/
1932; Dampier, 1929/1966).
144
and it is possible for any person to be trained to apply
scientific methodology to the observation and interpretation
of those events.
145
work of Benzer, for demonstrating the genetics of complex
human behavior with drosophila.)
146
There is a cautionary tale among historians of science which
runs something like this. A biologist was investigating diets
for cattle at a research station. His superior was a physicalistic
reductionist. One day the biologist showed his superior two
lists of chemical elements. This sort of thing: Al = x%, C =
y%, H = z%, and so forth. The biologist said that these
represented the analyses of two diets for cattle, and asked
which one was better. The superior stated he could see no
difference. The biologist replied that this was strange because
while one represented food intake, the other represented the
outflow of residual metabolic matter.
147
temporal mechanistic chain of smaller events as explanatory,
while the non-reductionistic attitude treats total events with
multiple factors which comprise the complete causal
description.
148
biologists are members of the reductionistic school, as
described in Wilson’s (1998) Consilience, which proclaims
the desirability, possibility and feasibility of reducing all of
sociology (read psychology) as well as all of biology to
genetic (i.e., internal) determinants. However, competing
traditions do persist, and actually remain healthy and
influential. The non-reductionistic approach, which is to be
found in Darwin’s (1859/1964) Origin of Species (along with
the opportunity for reductionistic development), also gave rise
to the ecological movement (Brewer, 1960). The most easily
available scientific and philosophical accounts of the status of
non-reductionistic biology are Meyr (1982, 1997) and Rose’s
(1999) Lifelines. Rose, incidently is an active researcher in
the molecular mechanisms of memory, while Wilson is also
famous for his ecological work. We scientists always strive
for consistency, but all too often fail to achieve it.
149
life, but enriched my social life as well. In this presentation I
shall use some of the technical vocabulary of each, as well as
the common language. Please bear with me as I do not do this
to confuse, but: (1) Because there is so little agreement
regarding technical psychological terminology; and (2),
Because all three of these men were constrained by the nature
of psychological events to address the same issues, even
though they did so with somewhat different terminology and
constructs.
150
instrument aspect of the functionalist school of James (1890).
Bentley’s (1895) definition may be regarded as one early step
toward the elimination of the purported mental explanatory
fiction.
151
us have fallen into the error of simply substituting the
conceptual nervous system (CNS) for the
non-spatio-temporal, and purported, rather than actually
isolatable, explanatory device of the Soul or Mind.
152
many recent reports have upheld his statements of some 52
years ago. For example: Vining, et al. (1997) reported on 58
children who had been hemispherectomized in order to
control life threatening grand mal episodes. Their behavior
was quite normal for such severely injured organisms, and
included remarkable voluntary control of the arm and leg
contralateral to the missing hemisphere.
153
form of economic system for scientific progress is capitalism.
So, in a definite sense I am, as was Kantor, a true descendant
of the eighteenth century enlightenment.]
154
1936, 1953, 1977, as well as Skinner (1957), and many others
too numerous to cite here).
155
Kantor was teaching us, I was never able to escape the forms
of the English language to think, speak and write on the
Kantorian theoretical plane (p. 184).
Walker did come close to the point here. To Kantor, there was
no problem of a “real world.” The so-called problem of
whether there is “an external reality” is a pseudo-problem
created by that weird, but venerable, politico-religious
assumption of a psycho-physical universe. There is no need to
assume a dual universe, and to become entangled in problems
of “internal” and “external” realities. The simplest solution
seems to be to ignore the dual universe assumption, since it
leads to scientifically insoluble problems and is itself
inherently self-contradictory. In addition, it is not derived
directly from scientific investigation, but only indirectly
through considerations of social control and cultural
cohesiveness. What is needed are other assumptions which
will allow the scientist to proceed in a clear, coherent, and
productive manner. To the development of these other
assumptions, and the explication of their consequences,
Kantor was to devote the largest portion of his professional
career.
156
a philosophy suitable for the pursuit of a natural science of
psychology, and to this endeavor he remained true throughout
his life. For him there were human beings (organisms) who
interacted with their surroundings. One adjustment technique
was called science, and this was the most satisfactory of all
interactions for producing rules which resulted in more and
more effective types of interactions with things and events.
He elected to: (1) observe what scientists actually did; and
(2), develop a set of rules; which (3), if followed, would result
in better scientific work. That is, he argued that if one
observed scientists at work, and then accurately described the
behaviors they performed, and then reacted both critically and
constructively, the inevitable result would be better, and more
naturalistic science. Up to the point in time at which Kantor
began to study there was excess pseudo-scientific verbiage
which was not really based upon observation, and which
therefore just got in the way of the scientific worker since the
job of the scientist, properly conceived, was to observe and
describe things and events. The point was discovery. And to
discover most effectively the worker must not approach with
notions which were not derived from previous observations.
Political and religious concepts of a dualistic (half Mental and
half Physical) world had been developed to organize and
control cultural groups. That may have been a legitimate
behavior for people in the distant past, whose aim was to
produce a coherent and cohesive society with a high
probability of survival in a hostile environment, but it was not
scientific work. The scientist does things differently than the
priest or politician. And the behavior of priests, politicians,
and even scientists, was a proper domain of psychological
events which are important objects for observation and study
by the psychological natural scientist (see Kantor, 1963-1969
157
for explication of his natural science approach to the
interbehaviors of scientists with their data).
158
psychopathology.” In addition, he treated abnormal behaviors
in both of his introductory textbooks (Kantor, 1933a; Kantor
& Smith, 1975). Thus, we can see that his early interest in
psychological maladjustment was maintained throughout his
scholarly life and career. Other publications which support
this generalization include his 1923 paper on personality
(which certainly influenced Lundin’s treatment of personality
(1961, 1969), and which was reiterated, for example, in his
introductory texts (1933a, 1975).
159
earth, for example, has existed as an entity for approximately
four and one-half billions of years. Now, since we human
beings interact with other things (or events), and produce
behavior products which may, as in the case of writing,
endure for many years, it seems useful to speak of the most
effective of these interactions as science.
160
a cultural group (Christians) which already so believes. Now,
if I could discern the religious origin of this bit of
so-called-science as an undergraduate, why is it that Ph.D.’s
in anthropology still have problems? Well, in brief, they have
not totally rejected the psychophysical protopostulate (I love
how that phrase rolls off the tongue).
161
science is itself an example of working as a natural scientist
(Kantor, 1963-1969, 1976).
Formal Students
162
appreciating his own critical approach to the commonly
accepted cultural mythology of a psychophysical universe. In
my opinion, there was a common core emphasis upon a
problem solving approach which was shared by all of the
faculty in the psychology department at Indiana University
when I was in residence between 1949 and 1954. I assume
that Kantor played a role in producing that approach among
his colleagues. At any rate, the department did produce a
number of individuals who were achievers within psychology.
163
Pronko, (1949), Bucklew (1941, 1943, 1958), Bucklew &
Hafner (1951; Lundin (1961, 1965, 1969); Mountjoy (1957,
1976, in press a, in press b), Mountjoy & Cone (1995, 1997,
in press); Mountjoy & Ruben (1983); Pronko (1946, 1980, ),
Pronko & Hill (1949), Pronko & Bowles (1951), Pronko &
Herman (1982); Herman et al. (1957); Hill (1944a, 1944b,
1945); and Wolf (1958a, 1958b). The major contributions of
Mountjoy are not well represented by the references cited
above for him. In fact, his editorial work on The Record, and
especially his participation in the development of the
behavioral graduate program at Western Michigan University,
constitute contributions which will impact the scientific
culture some years into the future.
164
prevented from further academic work with Kantor by his
retirement in 1959. My criteria for inclusion of the above list
are both personal communications and publications that I
have been able to discover.
Colleagues of Kantor
165
many other examples may be found in Smith, Mountjoy and
Ruben (1983) and Smith (1990/1993).
166
Koziol, Stout, & Ruben 1993). Although references could be
cited for Shook, it appears most appropriate to indicate that
the Society for the Advancement of Behavior Analysis
presented a twenty-five year career award for Public Service
in Behavior Analysis to him at the 1999 convention of The
Association for Behavior Analysis in Chicago, IL. This award
was bestowed primarily for his work on developing testing
and legislative procedures for National Certification of
Behavior Therapists.
167
Interbehavioral Research
Kantor not only encouraged laboratory work by his students;
he also regarded experimentation as essential to scientific
work and progress (Kantor, 1978, 1959/1960). In so far as he
was concerned, each and every thing and event was subject to
experimental manipulation and scrutiny in principle. He
himself had clinical skills, and actually was the hypnotist in
the Davis and Kantor (1935) report on changes in
physiological measurement associated with hypnosis. Thus it
seems appropriate to cite reports which are either directly
related to the interbehavioral framework and/or at least
compatible with it. One example which rewards attention is
Kantor’s analysis of the experimental analysis of behavior
(1960).
168
am confident that he would have found them to be
praiseworthy.
169
Summary and Conclusions
Robert Kantor gave us a way of looking at (perceiving) the
world which allowed us to bypass the hoary traditions of
nearly two millennia of conceiving reality as having two
levels of existence: The tangible world of science and its
exact linguistic opposite, the purportedly intangible world of
spirit, which is the ultimate and absolute cause not only of
behavior, but indeed of everything.
170
in, the event. We can categorize these in a general way as (1)
an organism or biological entity, (2) a stimulus object, (3) the
surroundings and limits, and (4) the history of interactions of
that Organism with that Stimulus Object.
171
construct of a desired outcome is based upon the expectations
of the social/cultural group(s) to which the client belongs, not
necessarily only those to which the therapist belongs! (See M.
Meyer, 1922, 1927 re: “the other one,” which is his
mnemonic for the necessity of an observer external to the
psychological event.)
Postscript
The evidence discussed above leads me to conclude that
Kantor played a reasonably important role in the development
of the theoretical foundations for behavior therapy through his
efforts at construction of a system for a natural science of
psychology. In addition, he devoted a remarkable amount of
energy to the dissemination of supportive research activities
with his founding and support of The Psychological Record,
and his teaching activities. Even after retirement, until his
hearing impairment made it impossible to react to questions
from the audience, he continued to travel and lecture. Indeed,
he devoted his life to the pursuit of a natural science of
psychology. What better epitaph could there be?
172
Immediately prior to his fatal seizure, Robert was preparing a
never to be completed paper for The American Psychologist,
and his daughter later found a handwritten note on his desk
which may have been intended as a title within the text, or
perhaps a general concluding statement for that paper. I quote
it in full as it does summarize his life work in one pithy
statement (Mountjoy and Hansor, 1986, p. 1297):
References
With some annotations. Certain of these references may be of
antiquarian interest only, but others are quite seminal. This
list is, of course, historically incomplete as it centers upon my
discovery of items which are by and/or relatable to J. R.
Kantor. For a more complete, though, of course now dated by
the inevitable passage of relentless time, list of references
concerning behavior therapy, see Kazdin (1978). I must admit
that my selection criteria might be argued to have been not
always consistent. At times a selection was made based upon
its early date, and at other times I considered content as more
important. For some individuals, I listed more works in order
to characterize their career. In my own defence, I state that I
used my judgement as to what criteria would serve this
audience best, and still produce a reference list of a
manageable size. The annotations serve as a substitute for a
fuller discussion of the work of many of the individuals cited
due to constraints upon space available. Some might object to
my inclusion of “popular” works, written for the lay audience,
173
however, I regard the education of mankind into the nature of
science to be essential for all citizens. After all, they vote for/
against the people who provide the monies to allow us to do
our work.
174
of emphasis upon rehabilitation, social adaptive living, and
community reintegration.)
175
Bentley, A. F. (1954). Inquiry into inquiries. Boston: Beacon
Press. (Contains complete list of his publications.)
176
concerning his later career, although I know that he occupied
a special place in Kantor’s life. Tears came to the eyes of
Bobby when she spoke to me about Briones after her father’s
death. Briones may have returned to his native land to teach.)
177
7, 219-224. (Is this the first approximation toward behavior
therapy? Perhaps. The date is right, and the content awaits
analysis by a competent behavior therapist who is interested
in the history of psychology.)
178
Davis, R. C., & Kantor, J. R. (1935). Skin resistance during
hypnotic states. Journal of General Psychology, 13, 62-81.
(Kantor was the hypnotist in this study.)
179
Frazier, K. (Ed.). (1999, July-August). Skeptical Inquirer,
23(4), 1-84. This special issue on science and religion is cited
here by the name of the editor, and contains papers by many
scientists, philosophers, and popular writers.
Gould, E., Beylin, A., Tanapat, P., Reeves, A., & Shors, T. J.
(1999). Learning enhances adult neurogenesis in the
hippocampal formation. Nature Neuroscience, 2, No. 3,
260-265.
180
Grossberg, J. M. (1981). Comments about cognitive therapy
and behavior therapy. Journal of Behavior Therapy &
Experimental Psychiatry, 7, 25-33.
181
Hill, H. (1944a). Stuttering: I. A critical review and
evaluation of biochemical investigations. The Journal of
Speech Disorders, 9, 245-261.
182
Kantor, J. R. (1918). Conscious behavior and the abnormal.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 13, 158-167.
183
Kantor, J. R. (1945; 1950). Psychology and logic (Vols. 1-2).
Bloomington, IN: Principia Press.
184
Kantor, J. R. (1977). Psychological linguistics. Chicago, IL:
Principia Press.
185
three gorillas, whose motor cortices were mapped by
electrical stimulation, portions ablated, and then allowed to
recover from the surgery. Upon re-examination, recovery of
function was recorded. All experimental subjects were quite
young, which is congruent with the report of Vining, et al
(1997) concerning recovery of function in young human
beings following hemispherectomy.)
186
Marr, M. J. (1990). Behavioral pharmacology: Issues of
reductionism and causality. In J. E. Barrett, T. Thompson, &
P. B. Dews (Eds.), Advances in behavioral pharmacology.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.
187
Morris, E. K. (1982). Some relationships between
interbehavioral psychology and radical behaviorism.
Behaviorism, 10, 187-216.
188
Mountjoy, P. T., & Hansor, J. D. (1986). Jacob Robert Kantor
(1888-1984). American Psychologist, 41, 1296-1297.
189
Parrot, L. J. (1984). J. R. Kantor’s contributions to
psychology and philosophy: A guide to further study. The
Behavior Analyst, 7, 169-81.
190
Ratner, S. C. (1957). Toward a description of language
behavior: I. The speaking action. The Psychological Record,
7, 61-64.
191
Ruben, D. H. (1984b) Major trends in interbehavioral
psychology from articles published in The Psychological
Record 1937-1983). The Psychological Record, 34, 589-617.
192
Sherrington, C. S. (1906). The integrative action of the
nervous system. New Haven: Yale University Press.
193
Smith, N. W., Mountjoy, P. T., & Ruben, D. H. (Eds.).
(1983). Reassessment in psychology: The interbehavioral
alternative. Washington, DC: University Press of America.
(This is a Festschrift for Kantor, and includes contributions
both by his students and also by individuals who were not
students in the formal sense, but were influenced by his
works, and sometimes engaged in correspondence and other
personal interactions with him. Although marred by numerous
typographical errors due to hasty production, for which
Mountjoy takes full responsibility, it was delivered into
Kantor’s hands by Mountjoy and Ruben late in 1983, shortly
before Kantor’s death in early 1984.)
194
Van Praag, H., Kempermann, G., & Gage, F. H. (1999).
Running increases cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the
adult mouse dentate gyrus. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 266-270.
195
works describes Seymour Benzer’s reduction of complex
behavior to genetic components.)
196
indicate the important roles of environmental conditions upon
the behaviors of brain injured, aphasic, patients.)
197
Acknowledgments
My long term friends and colleagues Dr. Donna M. Cone, Dr.
Dennis Delprato, and Dr. Noel W. Smith have read an early
draft of this paper and their comments have contributed
greatly to its current status. All errors remain, of course, my
own responsibility. Dr. Howard E. Farris, Chair, Department
of Psychology, Western Michigan University, and Dr. R. R.
Hutchinson, President of The Foundation for Behavioral
Resources, have provided logistical support in terms of office
space, computer access, photocopying and mailing privileges.
198
Chapter 5
Introduction
In keeping with the purpose of this conference, I will focus
my remarks on the early history of child behavior therapy
considered in its broadest sense. To this end I will present
them in terms of the four topics suggested by William
O’Donohue: (1) my intellectual biography, (2) my perception
of the important developments in the rise of child behavior
therapy, (3) detailed case studies of selected publications, and
finally, (4) possible object lessons learned for the future.
Intellectual Biography
In reviewing my rather long history, I find that my intellectual
biography with respect to the early history of child behavior
therapy easily falls into two phases. The first pertains to my
training and experience as a clinical psychologist, the second,
to my training and experiences as a child psychologist
focusing on the psychopathologies.
199
Phase One
200
Incidentally, Uri Bronfenbrenner, known particularly for his
ecological child development (e.g., Bronfenbrenner, 1992),
grew up in the environment of Letchworth Village where his
father was a well-respected research pathologist.
201
After receiving my degree in 1937, I took a position as
clinical psychologist at the Delaware State Mental Hygiene
Clinic on the campus of the Delaware State Hospital, near
Wilmington. The Clinic provided diagnostic services to adult
patients in the hospital and to children and adults with mental
health problems throughout the small state of Delaware. My
job was to administer psychological tests and prepare reports
for the psychiatrists to help them make diagnoses and
recommendations.
The upside of the appointment was that for the first time I was
exposed to patients of all ages and covering the entire range
of mental health problems, since the Clinic served all the
social and educational agencies in the state. I administered 33
different types of psychological tests to over 2000 patients.
The downside was similar to that of the clinical psychologists
at Letchworth Village: my duties were largely limited to
testing and report writing.
202
found to be a valuable screening measure for young children
nationwide when the Head Start program was launched.
203
relationships among the variables were correlational rather
than functional. My interest was on functional relationships.
Besides, I was losing interest in Lewin’s theory. I was
beginning to believe, through the intensive learning course
with Spence, that the learning theories of Hull, Tolman, and
Guthrie held more promise than Lewin’s for the future of
psychology as a natural science.
204
was working with many children and adults with speech
problems.
205
There was an interlude of about five years — from 1941 to
1946 — between receiving my degree and my first academic
appointment. During that time, I spent two years as a research
psychologist at the Wayne County Training School in
Northville, Michigan, a residential school for high-grade
retarded children, and three years in the military service, first
administering intelligence tests at Induction Centers, then Air
Force aptitude tests for selecting pilots, bombardiers, and
navigators and finally, supervising psychological services at
Convalescent Centers in resorts throughout the country.
Phase Two
206
Indiana was as intellectually exciting as Iowa, although in a
different way. Here, frequent heated discussions took place
among faculty and students over the merits and limitations of
the Skinnerian, Hullian, and Kantorian views. There was a
Skinner-Kantor, standing-room-only, joint seminar plus
informal competing seminars by the students of Skinner and
Hull. At the same time, ground-breaking research was being
done by students, such as Bill Estes, Norman Gutman, Paul
Fuller, and Joel Greenspoon.
207
which consisted of his telling stories, some related to
learning, some not. When students raised questions critical of
his theory, he would laugh and counter with one even more
devastating. The students obviously enjoyed his stories.
Incidentally, the undergraduates good-naturedly referred to
the psychology department as offering only two courses: An
introductory course on the behavior of cats in a puzzle box
and a developmental course on the behavior of kittens in a
puzzle box.
208
individual children. On the basis of a review of the literature,
I decided to carry out a study which had implications for the
frustration-aggression hypothesis (Bach, 1945). The study
would be concerned with the relationship between aggression
in a social situation and fantasy aggression as manifested in a
doll-play situation (fantasy aggression). In planning the
experiment I realized that I would have to obtain teachers’
ratings of children’s aggressiveness, prepare a doll house
simulation of a preschool, find or make a teacher doll and
three children dolls, one being of the opposite sex of the
subject, train two observers to record data from behind a
one-way glass, then analyze their protocols in terms of
frequency, direction and latency of aggression, and train a
student to perform the task of an “experimenter,” the person
who interacts with the subject in the course of an experiment.
All these requirements made me realize that laboratory
research with children in the Hullian tradition was quite
different from animal research which was relatively simple
and included good control over the antecedent and consequent
conditions of an objectively recordable response. The study
was never done. Instead I turned to the only other procedure I
knew — Skinner’s operant conditioning model.
209
Figure 1. The ball dropping apparatus.
210
principles scrupulously. Because the ball dropping response
on occasion created recording problems (e. g., a child would
miss the hole and go scrambling to retrieve it ), I substituted a
lever (an O-Cedar sponge mop handle) for a response. In
addition, two sophisticated commercial instruments
(Gerbrands) replaced my home-made devices: One for
recording responses and stimulus events, the other for
delivering trinkets as well as other small objects.
211
Figure 3. Cumulated responses of the first, third, and fifth
sessions of two four-year old boys. Sequence A is on a
fixed-interval schedule of reinforcement. Sequence B is on a
fixed-ratio schedule of reinforcement.
212
4, that could be easily towed to any nursery school in the
Seattle area where additional studies could be carried out
(Bijou, 1958a).
213
under Howard Hunt; Montrose Wolf who studied remedial
reading with Arthur Staats at Arizona State University; Jay
Birnbrauer who carried out learning studies at Indiana
University, and Robert Orlando who did discrimination
learning studies on children with retardation under David
Zeaman at the University of Connecticut.
214
extinction after fixed-interval schedules (1958b), on
discrimination as a baseline for individual analysis of young
children (1961); by Bijou, Lovaas, and Baer on experimental
procedures for analyzing the interaction of symbolic social
stimuli and children’s behavior (1965); by Bijou and Oblinger
on responses of normal and retarded children as a function of
the experimental situation (1960); by Bijou and Orlando on
single and multiple schedules of reinforcement with retarded
children (1960, 1961); and by Bijou and Sturges on positive
reinforcers for experimental studies with children —
consumables and manipulables (1959).
215
development of motor skills (Hart, Allen, Buell, Harris &
Wolf, 1964; Harris, Wolf, & Baer, 1964: Hart, Reynolds,
Baer, Browley, & Harris, 1968); Johnston, Kelley, Harris, &
Wolf, 1966; Sloane, Johnston, & Bijou, 1967).
216
In 1961 I spent a sabbatical year at Harvard with Skinner on a
Senior National Institute of Mental Health Fellowship. I set
up a laboratory at the Fernald State School near Cambridge to
study complex discriminative behavior in retarded children.
Using a match-to-sample apparatus developed by James
Holland, I explored the programming of antecedent stimuli to
facilitate the learning of right-left visual discrimination
(Bijou, 1968). I also audited Skinner’s large undergraduate
lecture course which was the basis of his Science and Human
Behavior (Skinner, 1953) and his weekly meetings of the
“Pigeon Staff”, an informal seminar for psychologists in and
around Harvard for displaying and discussing data from their
research with pigeons. Each session ended on a high note
with a round of beer. And I spent considerable time in
Skinner’s Teaching Machine Project (Skinner, 1968), learning
principles and techniques of programmed instruction.
217
actually enjoyed coming to class (Bijou, Birnbrauer, Kidder,
& Tague, (1966). After the project was terminated, the
reading program, now known as the Edmark Reading
Program, was adapted for use with a computer touch- screen
and is distributed by IBM ( International Business Machine
Company).
218
Parsons, Ely Rayek, Emilio Ribes, Howard Rosen, Grover
Whitehurst, Barbara Wilcox, and Koaru Yamaguchi.
219
1980, Yamaguchi arranged for me to attend a meeting of the
Personnel Committee of the Asian Conference on Mental
Retardation which was concerned with plans for the future
treatment of mentally retarded children. With Yamaguchi’s
support, I advised the Committee to invest their countries’
limited resources to training teachers in the Portage Project, a
behaviorally based teaching method, so that they would be
able to train parents to treat their young retarded children in
their homes or in community centers rather than dissipate
their funds on building large institutions. Yamaguichi himself
took action by forming the Japan Portage Association,
translating and the Portage Program into Japanese, and
adapting it for his culture. The Association now has 34
chapters throughout the country. Among them, 28 chapters
provide individualized teaching of mothers for about 300
retarded infants and young children; the other six chapters
carry out parent training in small groups called Day Care
Centers. In Tokyo, about 70 handicapped infants and young
children and their parents come to the headquarters facility
once or twice a week for individualized instruction and
Portage teachers home-visit about 40 families and teach
parents of about 60 special children in Day Care Centers.
220
Important Developments in the Rise of
Child Behavior Therapy
Looking back over my experiences, I believe there were
several indications of the rise of child behavior therapy. The
first was a study by a graduate student at Indiana University
(Fuller, 1949) showing that the behavior of a profoundly
low-functioning institutionalized patient, who, according to
the hospital staff, was unable to learn, could in fact learn to
increase his arm movements when they were followed by a
mixture of warm milk and sugar and to decrease them when
the contingency was withheld. This simple demonstration
impressed not only the hospital staff but also many
psychologists because never before had operant principles
been shown to be effective with a human being.
221
could learn simple responses to vending-type machines in a
laboratory setting. The fourth indicator was the previously
mentioned clinical study by Wolf, Risley, and Mees (1964) in
which operant principles were applied to the treatment of a
hospitalized three-and-a-half-year-old “schizophrenic” boy
with a serious visual handicap. This was indeed a remarkable
study in that the team had not only to create the treatment
programs but also had to train the attendants, nurses, and
parents to carry out the programs and to record data in a form
amenable to a quantitative presentation. This study pointed
out the route to the future that child behavior therapy would
take.
222
In addition to the development of a field methodology, these
studies had two powerful side effects. One was that the staff’s
over-arching view of child development and child
management was changed from the Freudian-Rogerian
position to the behavioral approach (Allen & Goetz, 1982;
Goetz & Allen, 1983). The other was that these studies served
to redefine the activities of the staff: They were not only
teachers; they were now researchers as well. One of the
nursery school teachers, Betty Hart, was so inspired by the
research that after taking a Ph. D. degree at the University of
Kansas, she carried out a series of studies on the role of
verbal behavior in the development of children and finally
embarked on a multi-year, pioneering study with Todd Risley
on how children learn to talk in their home settings (Hart &
Risley, 1995, 1999).
223
neonatal, and infancy stages, and the third to the early
childhood stage (Bijou, 1976). To our regret, we learned
through a review of the literature that we could not extend the
series to middle childhood and beyond because of the paucity
of studies amenable to a functional analysis at those levels.
We therefore supplemented the series with a book of readings
to show the application of principles in others areas related to
child development (Bijou & Baer, 1967).
Volume One was revised twice. The first revision (Bijou &
Baer, 1978) emphasized that the relationship between
behavior and stimulating conditions was mutual and
reciprocal, and extended the analysis to complex behavior,
such as self-management and problem-solving. The second
revision (Bijou, 1993) included an analysis of verbal behavior
and elaborated on the meaning of setting factors.
224
oversight is understandable. Believing that the parents were
either the cause of the problem or part of it, early-day
therapists tended to exclude parents from treatment on the
assumption that they would either delay or retard the child’s
treatment program.
Summary
The early advances in child behavior therapy have been
nothing short of spectacular. A review of therapeutic
techniques for children (Bijou, 1954) described five
approaches, three of which were offshoots of psychoanalysis:
225
Child analysis (A. Freud, 1946; Kline, 1949), the briefer
analytic child therapies (Newell, 1941), and the expressive
therapies (Levy, 1939). Of the other two, one was based on
the Rankian psychology (Taft, 1933; Allen, 1942), the other
on Rogers’ client-centered approach (Axline, 1947). There
was no mention of learning or behavior therapies. An update
of that review, 12 years later (Bijou & Sloane, 1966),
included both classical and operant conditioning child
therapies. The classical conditioning therapies emphasized the
systematic desensitization of children with phobias (e.g.,
Bentler, 1962; Lazarus, 1959), whereas the operant
conditioning therapies stressed a range of problems from
autism (Wolf, Risley, & Mees, 1964) to everyday “normal”
problems (Harris, et al., 1964) to the rehabilitation of simple
motor skills (Johnston, Kelley, Harris, & Wolf, 1966). So
rapidly had the field expanded during the next ten years that
the editor of the 1975 edition of American handbook of
psychiatry saw fit to include a separate chapter on the child
behavior therapies (Bijou & Redd, 1975).
References
Allen, F. H. (1942). Psychotherapy with children. New York:
W. W. Norton.
226
Axline, V. M. (1947). Play therapy. New York: Houghton
Mifflin.
227
Bentler, P. M. (1962). An infant’s phobia treated with
reciprocal inhibition therapy. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 3, 185-189.
228
Bijou, S. W. (1957b). Methodology for an experimental
analysis of child behavior. Psychological Reports, 3, 243-250.
229
Bijou, S. W., & Baer, D. M. (Eds.). (1967). Child
development: Readings in experimental analysis. Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
230
(Vol. 2, pp. 63-78). Palo Alto, CA: Science & Behavior
Books.
231
Ferster, C. B., & DeMyer, M. K. (1961). The development of
performances in autistic children in an automatically
controlled environment. Journal of Chronic Diseases, 13,
312-345.
232
Hart, B. M., Allen, K. E., Buell, J. S., Harris, F. R., & Wolf,
M. M. (1964). Effects of social reinforcement on operant
crying. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1,
145-153.
233
Johnston, M. S., Kelley, C., Harris, F. R., & Wolf, M. M.
(1966). An application of reinforcement principles to the
development of motor skills of a young child. Child
Development, 37, 379-387.
234
Mussen, P. H., Conger, J. J., & Kagan, J. (1956). Child
development and personality (1st ed.). New York: Harper &
Row.
235
Rheingold, H. L., Gewirtz, J. L., & Ross, H. W. (1959).
Social conditioning of vocalizations in the infant. Journal of
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 52, 68-73.
236
aggressive fantasy play by management of contingencies.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 8, 217-226.
237
Wolf, M. M., Risley, T. R., & Mees, H. (1964). Application
of operant conditioning procedures to the behavior problems
of an autistic child. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1,
305-312.
Footnote
1
Many thanks to my wife, Janet, for her careful and thorough
editing of the manuscript and my son, Bob, for preparing the
figures.
238
Chapter 6
The Beginning
I was born on August 11th, 1922 in Jane Brown Hospital,
Providence, Rhode Island. My father was a young Harvard
Law School graduate practicing corporate law in Edwards and
Angel, Providence’s most prestigious law firm. My mother
had dropped out of Wheaton College at the end of her junior
year to marry my father upon his graduation from law school.
We wintered at 282 Wayland Avenue on the upper income
East Side of Providence. We summered on the west shore of
Narragansett Bay in Quidnessett, Quonset Point, Rhode
Island, on our 365-acre farmstead with a house built in 1804
and its own Allen’s harbor.
239
his genius were family legends. He sailed to London,
England, to design looms to weave hair cloth royal coat of
arms fabric to cover chair backs and seats in Buckingham
Palace. He spent endless hours inventing in a shop behind his
house. He invented a very fast early bicycle, and a machine to
roll cigars. There was a wonderful oil painting of him in long
mustache and beard over the large mahogany Stella music
box in grandmother Lindsey’s home. The music box stood 37
inches from the floor and 24 inches from the wall. When I
was so small that I had to reach up to the edge of the music
box, I would pull with both hands standing tippy-toe to peer
over the top at Isaac Lindsley’s face. I had been given his
brass microscope with glass slides in a handsome wooden box
with an engraved brass nameplate which read, “Isaac
Lindsley, Inventor.” I wanted to grow up to be like him!
We had two rooms and a bath on our third floor attic. One
was Helen, our maid’s bedroom, and my mother, Mildred
Flagg Monroe Lindsley, made the other into a little school
room with three desks, a chalkboard, a table, and book cases
containing first and second grade public school books. I still
have some of those books: the McCall Speller -Intermediate,
Mother Westwind’s Children, the Hiawatha Primer, The
Elson Readers Book Three, Stories of American Discovers
for Little Americans. Every afternoon my mother taught me
school up there. Often one or two of my playmates came over
to be taught by my mother. It was wonderful, she was so kind
and had such a great sense of humor. Most of the time we
were learning and laughing. I learned so much that when it
came time for school, the principal at Moses Brown Lower
School had me skip first grade so I went into second grade at
six years of age. We wore little navy blue short pant suits
with white Eaton collars, and little navy blue caps with
240
MBLS, for Moses Brown Lower School, embroidered in
white. We had to walk home through some streets where
tough poor kids from Fox Point walked home from Nathan
Bishop Junior High School. The tough teens chased and
terrorized us, throwing rocks and horse chestnuts at us and
yelling “Momma’s Baby Lemon Sucker,” referring to the
MBLS on our caps. For a while we had police escorts until
the Fox Pointers gave up.
241
instantly, his body totally crushed. My mother had multiple
fractures; her hips were fractured in five places and she was in
the hospital almost a year-long enough for the guppy fish pair
that my brother and I gave her to have multiplied, filling a
bowl in almost every room in the hospital!
242
My grandmother and mother could not agree where to bury
my father, so for a while he was in a tomb in Swan Point
Cemetery, the ritziest one in Providence. I sneaked over to his
tomb on a still fall night and built a nice wet fire with lots of
smoke beside the tomb and waited for signals from my dead
father. Suddenly I felt a hand on my shoulder! A male voice
boomed out, “What are you doing here, Lad!” I shook with
fright, turned around and saw a large, scowling, uniformed
Providence cop. I blurted out, “I’m keeping warm. My dead
father is in there!” The cop beamed and said in an Irish
brogue, “Sure and the fine young lad is holding a wake for his
dear departed father!” He took me to McDonald’s drugstore,
bought me ice cream, and then to the East Side police station
on Sessions Street. At the station he told all the cops what a
brave little man I was sitting awake by his father’s tomb. I did
not have the heart to break his myth and tell him that I was
trying to communicate with the dead!
We lost all of our money. The shoe polish company did not
pay my father’s salary from January to May 1935, when it
was customary for executives to get the full year’s salary for
the year in which they died. A partner in rental housing did
not honor the partnership and we received no money from
that source. It was the depth of the depression. My father did
not believe in insurance, saying he was smarter than insurance
people and could better invest his money. My mother had a
giant policy drawn up on my father that was to have sent me
to Switzerland for prep school if he died. But he had been too
busy cornering the shoe polish market to take the physical
examination, so this life insurance policy was not in force.
We moved to our summer home, the farm in Quidnesset,
full-time and became country boys. I put my arm around my
little brother’s shoulders and said “Don’t cry, Brad. I will take
243
care of you!” I never teased Brad or hit him again. I never
cried after my father’s death, and I assumed his role. I
dropped the “junior” from my name and stopped people from
calling me “Sonny.” I became “Ogden,” or “Oggie.”
244
In 1937 my horse, Prince, got a thorn in his head and
developed a severe throat infection. It was before penicillin,
so he was rapidly dying. I had to walk him about a quarter
mile down to our sandpit, where it was easy digging, and dig
his grave. Then I injected Prince with the strychnine that the
vet had given me to kill him. It was one of the hardest things I
had to do up to that point in my life. Having my father killed
was hard, but having to kill Prince was even harder!
245
had to walk about 12 miles home through blown over trees
across the highway. Refuse from broken Oceanside homes
and boats littered our front lawn and pasture. My brother and
I found three dead bodies washed up on our side of the
harbor. We called the National Guard who had declared an
emergency and sealed off our area to prevent looting.
246
(Analytical Geometry) or the lowest (Advanced Calculus). If
I loved it, I exceeded. If I hated it, I flunked. There was no
middle ground! The war escalated in Europe. High school
friends joined the merchant marine on the Murmansk run.
Others joined our U.S. Marines. One was a Navy flier. I
thought about joining the war effort every day. I envied
uniforms and combat ribbons and wings! My heart was in
flying a spitfire as an American volunteer for the Royal Air
Force over England and chasing Messerschmitts back to
Germany. I would rather die in air combat, a hero, than sit
here in Providence in a lecture seat.
In January 1942 the U.S. Army Air Force dropped its age
requirement for cadets from 21 to 18 years. I immediately
joined and was in Class 42J Aviation Cadets at Maxwell
Field, Montgomery, Alabama. I went through preflight at the
top of my class, but washed out in June on a flight physical
eye exam with prism divergence to exceed six diapers. The
medics giving the physicals urged us to erase the check marks
on the examination forms we carried from exam post to exam
post. Some of the cadets did erase physical problem checks. I
chose not to erase my prism divergence check. I was so
young, naive, and moral that I thought the physicians knew
more than I did. I did not want to take nine men on my crew
to their death because I wanted silver wings. So I left the
check mark on my form, and was washed out. The
ophthalmologist said my eyes were not good enough to be a
navigator or bombardier. My eyes were not even good enough
for glider pilot school! I was discharged from Cadets. I was
ashamed to go home as a civilian, so I enlisted in the Army
Air Corps as a private at Maxwell field. In July the Air Corps
found there were not enough perfect eyes in the country to
fight the war, so they dropped mission requirements for
247
flying. Prism divergence dropped from six diopters to 12!
Mine was eight diopters! I was so young and inexperienced
that I did nothing about it. I let them assign me to the military
police and be a guard at the main Maxwell field gate. Because
I had been top in my cadet preflight class I really could polish
brass, buttons and buckles, stand rather ramrod straight, and
salute smartly.
248
scores, my year-and-a-half college engineering experience,
and the shooting got me sent to airplane mechanics school at
Keesler Field, Biloxi, Mississippi. I studied hard and became
a student instructor who went to class an hour before the other
students, and then taught a squad of about 15 students the
topic of the day. I graduated first in a class of about 800 and
was awarded my certificate and engineers badge by the major
general in front of the entire school student body massed at
attention. As a reward for being top in the class, I was being
sent to Helicopter Engineering Officer School at Twenty Nine
Palms while all the other graduates were off to gunnery
school at Tyndall Field, Florida, to become combat flight
engineers. The first sergeant came running out of the orderly
room yelling, “Lindsley, we’ve got to cut you a new set of
orders! We have to re-stencil your barracks bags! You can’t
go to Twenty Nine Palms! You’re on detached service from
the 831st Military Police Company and have to go back to
Maxwell!”
249
on B24 bombers. I worked up to be crew chief for Major
Buttman on Army 00, The Flying Goose, the squadron
commander’s ship. Soon our squadron was transferred to pilot
transition training at Smyrna Field, Tennessee. An instructor
pilot and an engineer took up two rookie pilots just out of
twin-engine flight school, shot a few landings by each student
pilot, then the instructor got out. The flight engineer stayed
while the student pilots learned to fly the heavy four engined
B24 bomber. We would shoot 30 to 60 landings a day!
Landing and takeoff accidents were common. I was flying
every day, but not in a spitfire over England chasing
Messerschmitts back to Germany.
250
suits and a truck raced back to the hangars to get them. Just
then there was a terrible swoosh and a wave of hot air as the
plane went up in flames from the spilled 120 octane gas.
251
After parachuting out over the North Albanian Alps we were
traded by partisans to the Croatians for guns and then handed
over to the Germans in Dubrovnick, Yugoslavia. We were
interrogated by the Hungarian Gestapo in Pestvideki Prison in
Budapest and imprisoned in Gross Tychow, Pomerania,
between Danzig and Berlin. In January of 1945 our guards
marched us from Luft IV to Hamburg across northern
Germany to keep us away from the advancing Russian
armies. We had only two bowls of hot soup in 63 days. I went
from 175 to 114 pounds. I sneaked through the wires into the
French prisoners’ compound as my fellow Air Force POW’s
were marched back into Germany. A few days later, dressed
as a French POW, I escaped from a wood picking-up detail in
the forest with two French POWs. We went through the
German front lines to the Queen’s Regiment, British 2nd
Army. I escaped rather than be marched back into Germany
with the rest from Stalag Luft IV. I was afraid that when
Germany was only a few hundred kilometers wide, there
would be no room for hated prisoners of war, and we might
all be machine gunned as we marched.
252
major in experimental psychology and histochemistry. I had
been influenced by Flanders Dunbar’s book, “Psychosomatic
Medicine.” I planned to personally solve the mind-body
problem by becoming expert in both. I became president of
Zeta Psi fraternity and help fill the house with returned
combat veterans. Notable among them was Dick Check,
former chief quartermaster on the aircraft carrier Bunker Hill.
I was also proud of getting the first Jewish man into our
chapter by blackballing the whole delegation until his box
passed with all white balls. With Ben Latt, whose dad was a
union organizer, I started the Lincoln Society with its motto
of “Fellowship without Fences.” I became interested in liberal
causes and folk music, and learned to sing and play a
six-string guitar. I graduated with Highest Honors in
Psychology in June 1948. I did not make Phi Beta Kappa or
Cum Laude because Brown averaged in my grades and
incompletes (which had become E’s) from before the war!
253
apparatus and threw him across the room. That was the last
time that I angered at slow learners.
I admired Lord Adrian for his creativity from a story that Carl
had told me about Adrian’s first recording of the electrical
discharge from an eel’s eye. It was in the days before reliable
electronic amplifier tubes. Adrian physically amplified the
electrical signal by using a light and a movable mirror. He
went into the basement of the longest cathedral he could find
and set up his equipment. In the dark a small mirror moved a
miniscule distance when the eel eye was stimulated with light.
At the other end of the hundred foot long dark cathedral
photographic paper was pasted on the wall which recorded
the amplified signal. This classy, simple, physical solution
fascinated me. I wanted to create similar simple solutions to
my own psychological research problems.
254
moment you stimulated and took as many pictures as you
could before it also died. This took 3 to 4 hours, most of a
Saturday, and a lot of patience. About 1 in 5 or 1 in 10
operations got a single nerve fiber dose response curve! Here
I learned scientific discipline and patience.
255
B grade at Brown. To this day Greg does not believe he
taught me anything, but I know how much he changed me.
256
with preference to Mayflower descendants at Harvard, and
Galambos accepted me as a student. Our family — by now
Mary Elizabeth and Deborah Melinda had been born —
moved to a housing project in Watertown, Massachusetts in
June 1950. I wanted an early start at Harvard to learn the
ropes and to learn micro-electrode recording from Galambos
before classes started in September.
257
you’ve blown it! I’m going to Walter Reed in 2 weeks.” I
said, “Wow! And you’re taking me with you?” Galambos
said, “Nope! You are a graduate student and your fellowship
is for here!” I said, “But my fellowship is to study micro
electrode recording with you!” He said, “Yes, but I have not
asked Walter Reed to support you.” Such was the life of a
graduate student at Harvard. So different from Brown.
258
After several sessions of pairing the clicks with the pellet
dispenser the rats were clicker trained, running back to the
place where they were last clicked. The four rats were soon
stepping over the meter stick lying on edge. When I raised the
meter stick up on the ring stands, one rat kept pulling down
on one end. I immediately put him back in his home cage and
built him a different apparatus, pivoting and putting an
adjustable sliding weight on the meter stick. I was going to
teach this rat to lift weight. I put a ring handle on the end of
the meter stick, because his paws kept slipping off. Within a
week of daily training, Samson Rat was pulling down a
weight equal to his own body weight. He jumped up, hung on
the handle with both fore paws, and tried to bounce down the
weight. One time his back feet swung forward and his toe
nails stuck into the plywood wall and with this leverage he
was able to pull down a weight heavier than his own weight. I
put him in his home cage, ran to the Harvard Coop across
Harvard Square, getting there just before closing at 5:00 pm. I
bought a rubber stair tread to use as an exercise mat on the
wall of Samson’s weight lifting apparatus, so his back feet
would not slip. I was in a hurry to see if Samson’s exercise
mat would help him lift more weight. Now, what to use for
fast drying glue? I remembered the very, very expensive
Ambroid, non conductive electrode cement, in the psycho
acoustic electro physiological operating room. It cost about
$40 a quart but dried in an instant. I smeared the back of the
stair tread with Ambroid, ran to the pigeon and rat labs in the
other end of Memorial Hall basement, and stuck the tread on
Samson’s wall. I rushed Samson from his home cage back to
his lifting apparatus with its new non-slip mat. Samson leaped
up, swung on his ring, planted both hind feet on his rubber
mat, arched his back, and pulled the weight all the way down.
HOORAY! Samson had taught me what he needed! Within
259
another week he was pulling down weight over two and a half
times his own body weight!
Skinner has often said that he did not make free operant
conditioners, the rats did! Samson had just made me a free
operant conditioner. It was the speed of learning, and the
precise control of Samson’s behavior that bowled me over. I
was a laboratory scientist. I had to admit that I had more
control over a whole, free roaming, rat than I ever did over a
single rat nerve in a temperature controlled moist chamber. I
never again did a physiological experiment. I still kept all my
expensive custom designed iridectomy forceps and knives.
260
was what Peter Dews later did. The other was with Walter
Jetter who was a state pathologist and professor of legal
medicine at Boston University medical school and who had a
grant from the atomic energy commission to study the effects
of irradiation on beagle dog’s physiology, exercise and
behavior. Fred said, “I have been receiving a few hundred
dollars a month from him in consultation, which has helped
keep Julie in Putney School. All I have done is design a
hamburger magazine that Ralph Gerbrands has about half
built.” I chose to work with the dogs rather than the pigeons
because they seemed more social and closer to my goal of
researching human behavior. I also knew that dog blood is
closer to human blood than that of other animals, and
preferred by many pharmacologists for research.
261
The well known drug effects provided a comparison. Then the
dogs were taken over to Massachusetts Institute of
Technology (MIT) in small body tight cages where they were
given 300 Roentgen units of total body irradiation (150 units
on each side) from a Van der Graph Generator. That amount
of radiation will kill half of the dogs (Lethal Dose of 50% —
LD50) from Leukopenia (loss of white cells) about 15 days
after irradiation. The survivors gradually recover, taking
another 15 days to regain strength and health. Hunter, named
for Walter Hunter and the quickest to learn of the 63 dogs,
survived and became Fred Skinner’s family dog. Hunter lived
to a ripe old age, although Eve Skinner was convinced that
Hunter’s embarrassing flatulence had been caused by his
irradiation.
262
keeping experimental animals in top physical shape. Our dogs
were examined and treated by a veterinary physician every
day. They had their temperatures and blood sampled every
day. They were washed and combed every day, Their home
cages were hosed down and sterilized every day. They
exercised on a treadmill at 20 miles per hour at 20 percent
grade for 20 minutes every day!
263
Figure 1. Hunter, the fastest learner of 67 beagle dogs,
pressing a panel to get a bit of hamburger delivered up
through the magazine hole to his right. Hunter survived his
dose of 50% lethal total body radiation and became the
household pet of Julie and Debby, B. F. Skinner’s daughters.
264
gorilla to play a piano and simple card games. I ended up
spending eleven and a half years studying psychotics from the
back wards of Metropolitan State Hospital.
Skinner got support from the Milton Fund of Harvard and the
U. S. Office of Naval Research. Harry Solomon, chair of
Psychiatry at Harvard and Commissioner of Mental Health
for Massachusetts agreed to serve as a co-investigator. I
started in June 1953 with $7,500 for the year in “A”
Basement (an abandoned hydro therapy unit) of Metropolitan
State Hospital, Waltham, Massachusetts. We studied both
acute and chronic male and female psychotic patients, autistic
children, and even the most violent patients from the locked
wards (it was before routine drug therapy). We studied
attendants, adult volunteers, and school children as normal
controls.
265
Figure 2. Chronic psychotic patient, diagnosed catatonic
schizophrenic, standing in his usual position in a corridor of
Harvard’s Behavior Research Laboratory waiting for his
daily session in one of the experimental rooms behind the
doors to the left.
266
However the third aspect of fixed ratio behavior, pausing
immediately after reinforcement was deficient.
267
Figure 3. Head banging psychotic on his way from male
violent ward to Behavior Research Laboratory for his 91st
experimental session. The chart at the right shows ten minutes
of his experimental session, which is described in the text.
268
“Harvard Medical School Behavior Research Laboratory”
(Lindsley, Skinner, & Solomon 1955). Strangers would read
it in the telephone book and call up and ask, “Do you do
laboratory research on behavior?” Mary Hall, our secretary,
would laugh and answer, “Of course!” This was the first use
of the name “Behavior Research.”
Operandum Design
Many chronic psychotic patients occasionally become highly
destructive with no advance warning. They throw objects,
smash chairs, and break windows. This is the behavior that
keeps them in the hospital, and this is the behavior we must
study and understand. In order to record the behavior of such
patients while in their destructive episodes, we needed
indestructible rooms, signals (stimuli), operating switches
(operanda), and reward delivery magazines. The operanda had
to be able to be moved easily at frequencies above 300 per
minute so there would be no ceiling on response frequency.
No commercially available switches met these demands, so
we designed and Ralph Gerbrands built, our “Lindsley
Operandum.” Other laboratories later purchased this
operandum from Gerbrands, Inc.
269
broke his chair against the work panel, but did not harm our
indestructible panel.
Symptom Recorders
To record psychotic symptoms we used electrical mats on the
floor to record pacing, and voice operated relays hidden in the
ceiling to record vocal hallucinating (talking and yelling to no
one). These frequencies were recorded minute to minute on
cumulative recorders with electrical counters for the hourly
and daily totals. Three recorders ran through each session, a
manual work recorder, a pacing recorder, and a vocal
hallucinating recorder (Lindsley, 1959, 1963a).
Reinforcer Search
I designed and Gerbrands built universal magazines that
would carry anything from a penny, an M&M candy, a
cigarette, or a slice of apple, to a package of cigarettes, and
deliver them rapidly down a chute into the experimental
room. You can see four universal feeders mounted up the
outside back of the experimental rooms at the left of figure 6.
We never found adequate rewards for several of the patients
(Lindsley 1956a). In vain we tried projected 2 x 2 slides of
various subjects, including nude women for the men. We tried
270
various music selections and movies, but the silent periods in
the image and sound when the frequency of responding
dropped disturbed the viewers.
271
Figure 5. Diagram of experimental room and apparatus for
recording vocal symptoms, manual plunger pulling for
reinforcements, and pacing symptoms.
272
Figure 6. Apparatus Alley, a long corridor behind the six
experimental rooms contained the controlling and recording
apparatus. Details described in the text.
273
hospital keys on a chain and standing while recording some
counts on a standard data card resting on top of the fourth
recorder down.
Conjugate Reinforcers
We designed and built the first conjugate reinforcer to
continuously present narrative movies and music without the
brief pauses that destroy narration and mood. The reward is to
bring the image or sound louder, closer, or more in focus. The
rate of response is directly linked to the intensity of the video
or audio channel. The faster the patient presses one switch the
louder the audio; the faster they press the other switch the
brighter the projected image.
274
studies using the conjugate schedule have since been
reviewed (Rovee-Collieer & Gekoski, 1979).
275
Social Reinforcers
In attempts to see if patients were too guilty to work to reward
themselves, we tried giving them the opportunity to feed a
hungry kitten as a reward (Lindsley, 1956b). We yoked two
experiment al rooms to see whether patients would work to
reward a friend, attractive member of the opposite sex, or a
stranger (Cohen, 1962; Cohen & Lindsley, 1964). We
generated cooperation between children without giving
instructions by using reward contingencies alone (Azrin &
Lindsley, 1956). None of these attempts were effective with
our most depressed inactive patients.
276
Simultaneous learning to discriminate (tell the lights apart)
and differentiate (tell the plungers apart) could be seen
developing on the five recorders. Learning deficits in
nonverbal and violent patients were easily diagnosed and
compared with the learning of normal children and adults
(Lindsley, 1962a).
277
An example shows how this notation system helped us
determine which component of a deficient operant was not
functioning. If a child pulled a plunger on the wall when the
light was on, every tenth pull produced a piece of candy. The
child pulled the plunger and got some candy, but she pulled
when the light was off as much as when it was on. What can
we say, except that the child could not learn to pull only when
the light was on? The operant equation now reads:
Our child only jumps for candy when the light is on! Our
educator has now discovered four operant components and
278
can assign operant functions to the components in the child’s
equation:
Our educator analyst can now use the three components with
proven function to test to see whether the child will work for
tokens:
279
Ten Year Data Histories
Our core group of 50 male chronic psychotic patients
participated in our rooms each weekday for as long as 10
years. Several patients had 25 to 29 day rhythms in their
performance which we tried to relate to phases of the moon,
sun spots, temperature and humidity fluctuations, but none of
these held up. Patients whose 10 year histories were without
rhythms were good for measuring drug effects because we
could rule out mood swings, hospital events, and family
visits.
Figure 8 shows that fifteen minutes into session 746 our nurse
injected 20 mg. of Benactyzine intramuscularly and the
effects appear in the top panel. Fifteen minutes into session
747 our nurse injected a lactose placebo whose effects are
shown in the bottom panel. Notice that the Benactyzine
280
injection produced sustained vocal hallucinating for a about
four hours. During this time the manual work was depressed.
Also notice that the vocal cumulative record is less steep for
the first 2 hours representing a rate of about 12 per minute.
During this time his plunger pulling work fell off from his
normal rate of 160 per minute to 6 per minute. During the
second 2 hours after injection the vocalizing almost doubled
to 21 per minute while his plunger pulling work gradually
returned to normal. Benactyzine produced a psychotic type
episode lasting almost 4 hours. In the placebo session 747 a
short 25 minute and a longer 40 minute psychotic episode
occurred. This patient number 7 displayed 15 to 45 minute
psychotic episodes of this type every few hours on his ward.
281
only a very few brief pacings (See NC 45 in figure 7). The
normals did not stop working during their brief pacing
episodes.
282
Figure 9. Bar diagrams of 7 hour sessions comparing
emission of never rewarded vocalizing (thin top bar), manual
working for rewards (thick middle bar), and never rewarded
pacing (thin bottom bar) for 8 psychotics, 1 retarded child
(RG), and 1 normal child (NC).
283
Except for the vocal hallucinating and pacing symptoms, this
reflex coextension was the first emergent diagnostic item that
we found. All other diagnostic items that we found were
deficiencies, a decrease or absence of some aspect of normal
performance.
284
If we tried a new compound a week and were lucky we might
expect to find a psychotic treatment drug in 300 trials, or 6
years, at the rate of one trial a week. The hooker was that the
drug companies were only producing 5 to 10 per year that
were of human toxicity. That would take 30 years if we were
as lucky as Curie and 300 years if we had Edison’s luck!
Lab Visitors
Our laboratory guest book shows that in the twelve years
from 1953 through 1964 ninety eight university classes from
ten universities with a total of 1857 students spent day long
field trips in our laboratory. Nine Hundred thirty seven
professionals visited our laboratory from the United States
and over seventeen other countries. Visiting psychiatrists and
psychologists included Carl Rogers, Harry Harlow, Roy
Menninger, Piere Pichot, Paolo Nuzzi, Koji Sato, Hudson
Hoagland, Otto Kernberg, Andey Snejnenski, Carl Pfaffman,
Frank Beach, Donald Lindsley, Carl Pribram, Joe Zubin, and
285
Timothy Leary. Visiting behavior analysts included Don
Baer, Harold Weiner, William Morse, Ted Allyon, Dale
Brethower, Matthew Israel, Charlie Catania, Thom Verhave,
Joe Brady, and Charles Ferster. Sidney Bijou visited in April
1957, November 1961, and April 1962. Bijou built a similar
laboratory for children at the University of Washington, as
did Ferster at the University of Indiana Medical School, Azrin
at Anna State Hospital in Southern Illinois, and Barrett at
Fernald State School, Waltham, Massachusetts.
Research Trainees
Larry Fane and Donald Cohen conducted undergraduate
honors theses. Julie Rich and Barbara Morgan conducted their
Master’s theses. Post Doctoral trainees were Nathan Azrin,
Beatrice Barrett, Peter Nathan, Martha Mednick, and Paul
Blachly. Tom Gilbert conducted research in our laboratory
while on a University of Georgia sabbatical.
Awards
In 1962 the American Psychiatric Association awarded one of
our research papers its annual Hofheimer Research Prize
(Lindsley, 1960). In 1964 the American Academy of
Achievement awarded us its Golden Plate Achievement
Award. These twelve pioneering years were cited in two
recent awards, the 1998 Thomas F. Gilbert Distinguished
Professional Achievement Award from the International
Society for Performance Improvement and the 1999 Award
for Distinguished Service to Behavior Analysis from the
Society for the Advancement of Behavior Analysis.
286
Appliers Abandon Frequency
Most of the behavior analysts who visited our laboratory and
then set up studies in clinics, hospitals, and schools did not
record their learner’s rate of response; they recorded percent.
To me this was a crisis because we had proven that frequency
was as much as l0 times more sensitive than percent. I
considered rate of response and the cumulative self-recorder
to be Skinner’s greatest contributions, and both were
discarded by the appliers. Azrin went so far as to say “suit the
metric,” which meant use a different measure for every
behavior you work with. It may have been easier to do and
much easier to sell, but such a loss of measurement standards
rules out real science which requires standard measures
287
student self charting. Clearly education was a larger industry
with far greater market potential than mental health.
References
Azrin, N. H., & Lindsley, O. R. (1956). The reinforcement of
cooperation between children. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 52, 100-102.
288
Barrett, B. H., & Lindsley, O. R. (1962). Deficits in
acquisition of operant discrimination and differentiation
shown by institutionalized retarded children. American
Journal of Mental Deficiency, 67, 424-436.
289
N5-Ori-07662, Office of Naval Research. Waltham MA:
Harvard Medical School, Behavior Research laboratory.
290
Lindsley, O. R. (1963a). Direct measurement and functional
definition of vocal hallucinatory symptoms. Journal of
Nervous and Mental Disease, 136(3), 293-297.
291
Lindsley, O. R., & Conran, P. (1962). Operant behavior
during EST: A measure of depth of coma. Diseases of the
Nervous System, 23, 407-409.
292
Harvard Medical School, Department of Psychiatry,
Metropolitan State Hospital, Waltham, MA, Office of Naval
Research Contract N5-ori-07662, Status Report III, 1 June
1954 - 31 December 1954.
293
Chapter 7
Conditioning Therapy
In South Africa, the forerunners of what came to be known as
“Behavior Therapy,” were Joseph Wolpe (a general medical
practitioner), James Taylor (a university-based psychologist),
Abe Adelstein (an epidemiologist and methodologist),
Cynthia Adelstein (a psychologist), and Leo Reyna, an
American psychologist who served as a senior lecturer at the
University of the Witwatersrand in Johannesburg from 1946
to 1950. The foregoing individuals guided and inspired
Wolpe and were the driving forces behind the well-known
294
experiments that he conducted on cats. Wolpe first submitted
the dissertation that resulted from his animal experiments to
the Department of Psychiatry at the University of the
Witwatersrand. Given that he had never received any formal
training in psychiatry, they turned it down. Similarly, the
Department of Psychology rejected it because Wolpe had
never enrolled in any psychology courses. Finally, given that
he had graduated from the medical school with an M. B., B.
Ch. (Bachelor of Medicine and Bachelor of Surgery), the
Department of Medicine awarded him an M.D. degree in
1948.
295
registered clinical psychologist) inherited his private practice.
I also became the leader of the training group seminar in
Johannesburg that Wolpe had launched. Later, Rachman was
awarded a Ph.D., from the University of London, soon
became Eysenck’s right hand man, and went on to a
distinguished career as a writer, editor and clinical
experimenter.
296
commitment to CT is exemplified in the proceedings of an
important conference held at the University of Virginia that
was published under the title The Conditioning Therapies
(Wolpe, Salter & Reyna, 1964). The conditioning label
remained quite popular. Thus, Franks (1964) edited a book he
called “Conditioning Techniques in Clinical Practice and
Research.” Nevertheless, I had published an article (Lazarus,
1958) in which I put forward the terms “behavior therapy”
and “behavior therapist.” Later, Eysenck (1959)
independently used these terms in print, but I lay claim to
having been the first person to use them in a scientific journal.
(Wolpe, 1968, discovered that in 1953, Skinner, Lindsley and
their associates, working at the Metropolitan State Hospital,
Waltham, Massachusetts had some mimeographed status
reports on operant conditioning with psychotic inmates that
they referred to as “Studies in Behavior Therapy.”)
Behavior Therapy
After Eysenck (1960, 1964) edited two books on behavior
therapy, the term became more widely disseminated and won
out over Conditioning Therapy and other designations that
had been proposed — e.g., “Behavioristic Psychotherapy,”
“Objective Psychotherapy,” and “Reciprocal Inhibition
Therapy.” Subsequently, Cyril Franks founded the
Association for Advancement of the Behavioral Therapies in
1966 (soon afterward called the Association for Advancement
of Behavior Therapy) and he later edited a highly significant
book on behavior therapy (Franks, 1969). In the 1960’s and
1970’s, despite denouncements from critics, skeptics and
detractors, there was a high degree of interest in behavioral
methods among researchers, theoreticians and clinicians. In
297
1963 when Albert Bandura invited me to spend a year at
Stanford University training graduate students in behavioral
theories and methods, enthusiasm for this novel and
promising approach was high. (My favorite student was a
very bright, personable and energetic fellow named G. C.
Davison who, as you well know, has played a major role in
the field.)
298
Rubin, Fensterheim, Lazarus, & Franks, 1971). Populations to
which these new behavioral methods were applied included
such areas as schizophrenia, alcoholism, mental retardation,
geriatrics, school settings, and juvenile delinquency. Indeed,
the proliferation of books, articles, and popular publications
on behavior therapy led Franks and Wilson (1973) to bemoan
the fact that “quantity is accelerating at a far greater rate than
quality” (p. vii). Thus, they launched their Annual Review of
Behavior Therapy to provide an integrated distillation of the
vast literature. They also offered trenchant commentaries that
placed the many developments in perspective. These erudite
800-page tomes were extraordinarily illuminating, and it
always astonished me that Franks and Wilson managed to
address and fulfill all their other academic and clinical
demands and still produce these volumes. Each year from
1973 to 1979 the monumental volumes appeared on time.
Gradually, it became evident that the commentaries by Franks
and Wilson were becoming equal in length to the reprinted
articles. It also became clear that most readers were much
more interested in the commentaries than in the reprinted
material. Consequently, for Volume 8, Kendall and Brownell
were recruited to add their specific areas of expertise to the
enterprise. With the advent of Volume 12 written (not edited)
by Franks, Wilson, Kendall and Foreyt (1990), the question
was posed as to whether there was still a need for these
publications. The cost-benefit ratio seemed disproportionate
and the series ended with Volume 12. At this stage, formal
behavior therapy had been in existence for over 30 years.
299
Cognitive-Behavior Therapy
Some of the personages associated with Cognitive-behavior
therapy (CBT) include Ellis, Beck, Meichenbaum, Goldfried,
Mischel, Davison and Mahoney. My book Behavior Therapy
and Beyond (1971/1996) is arguably one of the first books on
cognitive-behavior therapy. It soon became a Citation Classic.
In the 1977 Annual Review of Behavior Therapy, the
transition from BT to CBT was discussed, and Cyril Franks
used the term “cognitive-behavior therapy” for the first time
in his overview.
300
the process of change had gone from “insights into putative
unconscious complexes,” to “reciprocal inhibition,
counterconditioning, and extinction.” At this juncture, I
started to view cognitive restructuring as one of the primary
psychotherapeutic change agents. I was influenced by Ellis’s
(1962) Reason and Emotion in Psychotherapy, and London’s
(1964) The Modes and Morals of Psychotherapy, and began
to embrace the notion that the power of a person’s beliefs can
often override his or her operant or respondent conditioning.
When I stated that in addition to focusing on behavior,
elegant therapy called for attention to cognitive processes –
beliefs, attitudes, values, and opinions – my behavioral peers
were unimpressed. They saw it as an atavistic regression to
mentalism, and I was wrongly accused of being a closet
psychoanalyst.
A Pivotal Case
301
behavioral techniques consisting primarily of deep muscle
relaxation, imaginal and in vivo desensitization, behavior
rehearsal, and assertiveness training. After 5 months of
therapy she was able to enjoy taking long walks alone,
shopping, visiting and traveling without distress. Important
changes had accrued above and beyond the client’s capacity
to venture out of her home — she was no longer socially
submissive and enjoyed a wider range of social outlets; she
found that relaxation and positive imagery were capable of
quelling any residual fears or anxieties; and her marriage
relationship and sexual experiences were more gratifying.
Nevertheless, although the client was delighted by her
newfound ability to remain anxiety-free while traveling and
engaging in the niceties of social interaction, she continued to
view herself as a worthless person. She referred to herself as
being like a 12-year-old who was now able to cross the street
alone, but was contributing nothing to society. At this
juncture, what is now called 'cognitive therapy” was clearly
indicated, and we launched into an assessment of her more
fundamental attitudes and beliefs. This led her to conclude:
“If you want to feel useful, you have to be useful.”
Consequently, she founded an organization that distributed
basic essentials such as food and clothing to impoverished
people. This behavior, based upon her attitudes and
self-concept led her to view herself as “eminently
worthwhile.” In a follow-up interview she stated: “Thanks to
the fact that I exist and care, thousands of people now derive
benefit,” and she proclaimed herself “eminently worthwhile.”
This case is described in greater detail in Behavior Therapy
and Beyond (Lazarus, 1971/1996).
302
analogy) to take people into the end zone. It became quite
apparent to me that it was often necessary to venture beyond
the customary parameters of behavioral interventions into
such territory as values, attitudes and beliefs. Although the
early books on behavior therapy discussed the need to
“correct misconceptions” (Wolpe, 1958; Wolpe & Lazarus,
1966), the focus was solely on erroneous ideas and did not
address the realm of the client’s self-talk, his or her basic
values, or other cognitive processes. When I emphasized the
need to explore and modify such concerns, and when I drew a
distinction between what I termed “narrow band behavior
therapy,” and “broad-spectrum behavior therapy,” the
reactions from Wolpe and many of my fellow behavior
therapists were less than positive. Eysenck (1970), who was
after all a theorist who had never treated a patient in his life,
wrote a strident criticism and said that my ideas would lead to
“nothing but a mishmash of theories, a huggermugger of
procedures, a gallimaufry of therapies, and a charivaria of
activities having no proper rationale, and incapable of being
tested and evaluated” (p. 145). It took about ten years before
the need to add cognitive interventions to standard behavioral
methods became widely recognized. Goldfried and Davison
(1994) in their updated edition of Clinical Behavior Therapy
(first published in 1976) state: “One no longer needs to argue
for the admissibility of cognitive variables into the clinical
practice of behavior therapy. Indeed, more than two-thirds of
the membership of the Association for Advancement of
Behavior Therapy now view themselves as
“cognitive-behavior therapists’” (p. 282). As an aside, I took
Eysenck thoroughly to task in a chapter I called “On Sterile
Paradigms and the Realities of Clinical Practice,” (Lazarus,
1986).
303
As we move into the 21st Century, the emphasis on
empirically supported methods and the use of carefully
crafted treatment manuals have already begun to clarify the
issue of treatments of choice for specific disorders. They
supply much needed information about the active ingredients
of therapeutic techniques (see Wilson 1995, 1998). Add
various breakthroughs that have occurred in the biological
arena to the greater precision that is now taking place in the
field of cognitive-behavior therapy, and it becomes evident
that quantum leaps are in the immediate offing.
304
took offense at ethnic slurs uttered by clients during a therapy
session. In one case, the client had made an anti-Semitic
remark. The Jewish trainee took exception to it and handled
the matter emotionally, not clinically. In the other instance, a
Hispanic student refused to continue working with a client
who made an ethnic remark about Puerto Ricans. My
recommendation was that, at the very least, these students
should be urged to undergo a course of systematic
desensitization.
References
Bandura, A. (1969). Principles of behavior modification. New
York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
305
Ellis, A. (1962). Reason and emotion in psychotherapy. New
York: Lyle Stuart.
306
Goldfried, M. R., & Davison, G. C. (1994). Clinical behavior
therapy. New York, NY: Wiley.
307
Salter, A. (1949). Conditioned reflex therapy. New York:
Creative Age Press.
308
Chapter 8
Stanford University
309
spawn either irascibility or sweetness. The theory was not
only shrouded in conceptual fog, but the proposed causal
structures were essentially indeterminant and strewn with
loopholes. Such theories were used mainly as post hoc
explanatory devices.
310
The third distinguishing characteristic was the heavy reliance
on the interview as the vehicle of personal change. It was
assumed that by analyzing clients’ reports of their recurrent
conflicts and the problems they reenacted with their therapists
they gain insight into their inner dynamics and develop better
ways of behaving. Self-insight would promote change.
311
of their behavior than to change their behavior enduringly.
For example, alcoholics can be more readily persuaded that
they drink because of fixated orality than to get them to give
up booze. Interview modes of treatment showed limited gains
in actual behavioral functioning, but some benefits on
self-ratings of change. However, such measures exaggerate
the level of behavioral change (Williams & Rappoport, 1983).
On self-ratings even most nontreated controls report gains.
Following the old adage that one should light a candle rather
than curse the conceptual darkness, Dick Walters and I set
forth an alternative view of human behavior in the book,
Social Learning and Personality Development (Bandura &
Walters, 1963).
312
It underscored the influential role of modeling and direct
consequential experiences in the acquisition of behavior
patterns, and their regulation through the complex interplay of
contextual, incentive, and self-regulatory influences rooted in
personal standards.
313
symbolic, vicarious, and self-regulatory mechanisms in
human adaptation and change.
314
manuscript on Psychotherapy By Reciprocal Inhibition to the
Stanford Press for consideration. The Press sent it to me for
advisory evaluation. In my supportive review I predicted that
it will have modest sales at the outset, but continue to garner
significant sales in years to come in the evolving field of
behavior therapy. I invited Arnold Lazarus to Stanford during
my sabbatical leave, which greased his relocation from South
Africa.
315
studies of the determinants and mechanisms of behavioral
functioning replaced content analyses of interviews.
316
professional guidance, practitioners greatly expand the scope
of their impact. If professionals had to implement every
aspect of their treatments, their contribution would be but a
tiny ripple in the vast sea of human problems.
317
inside the organism, namely the implanted history of
reinforcement. Like other internal determinants, ontogenic
history is neither observable nor directly accessible.
318
Intractable phobics, of course, are not about to do what they
dread. Therapists must, therefore, create environmental
conditions that enable phobics to succeed, despite themselves.
This is achieved by enlisting a variety of performance
mastery aids. Threatening activities are repeatedly modeled to
demonstrate coping strategies, and to disconfirm people’s
worst fears. Intimidating tasks are reduced to graduated
subtasks of easily mastered steps. Treatment is conducted in
this stepwise fashion until the most intimidating activities are
mastered. Joint performance with the therapist enables
frightened people to do things they would refuse to do on
their own. Another method for overcoming resistance is to
have phobics perform the feared activity for only a short time.
As they become bolder the length of engagement is extended.
Protective conditions can also be introduced to weaken
resistances that retard change.
319
Moreover, multiple diverse successes serve as a vehicle for
building resilience to the negative effects of adverse
experiences. The capacity of an aversive experience to
reinstate dysfunctions depends, in part, on the pattern of
experience in which it is embedded rather than on its
properties alone. A lot of neutral or positive experiences can
neutralize the negative impact of an aversive event and curtail
the spread of negative effects (Hoffman, 1969). For example,
if after treatment, a dog phobic has no contact with dogs, an
aversive encounter will quickly reinstate the phobia. But if the
phobic had many neutral and positive experiences with
different varieties of dogs, an aversive encounter is likely to
produce a circumscribed effect — avoidance of the
threatening animal without phobic generalization to the other
canine types.
320
was helping her to wash the dishes. Eventually reptiles faded
from her dreams and ruminative activity ceased.
321
change, and the self-directed mastery component enhanced
the generality and durability of the changes (Bandura, Jeffery,
& Gajdos, 1975; Bandura, Jeffery, & Wright, 1974).
Self-Efficacy Theory
Self-efficacy theory was an outgrowth of our research
described earlier designed to build resilience to adversity. In
follow-up assessments, we were discovering that the
participants not only maintained their therapeutic gains, but
made notable improvements in domains of functioning quite
unrelated to the treated dysfunction. Thus, for example, after
mastering an animal phobia, participants had reduced their
social timidity, became bolder in public speaking, expanded
their competencies in various spheres of their lives, and
boosted their venturesomeness in a variety of ways. Success
in overcoming, within a few hours of treatment, a phobic
dread that had constricted and tormented their lives for twenty
or thirty years produced a profound change in participants’
beliefs in their personal efficacy to exercise better control
322
over their lives. They were acting on their belief, putting
themselves to test and enjoying their successes much to their
pleasant surprise.
323
procedure, Weinberg, Gould and Jackson (1979) raised the
self-efficacy beliefs of one group by telling them that they
had triumphed in a competition of muscular strength, and
lowered the self-efficacy beliefs of another group by telling
them that they were outperformed by their competitor. The
higher the instilled illusory beliefs of physical strength, the
more physical endurance the participants displayed during
competition on a new task measuring physical stamina.
Failure in a subsequent competition spurred those with a high
sense of perceived self-efficacy to even greater physical
effort, whereas failure further impaired the performance of
those whose perceived self-efficacy had been undermined.
324
A fifth approach to causality is to introduce a trivial factor
devoid of information to affect competency, but that can alter
perceived self-efficacy. Cervone and Peake (1986) used
arbitrary anchor values to influence self-appraisals of
efficacy. Self-appraisals made from an arbitrary high starting
point biased students’ perceived self-efficacy in the positive
direction, whereas an arbitrary low starting point lowered
students’ appraisals of their efficacy. The higher the instated
perceived self-efficacy, the longer individuals persevered on
difficult and unsolvable problems before they quit.
Mediational analyses revealed that the anchoring influence on
performance motivation was entirely mediated by perceived
self-efficacy.
325
The final way of verifying the contribution of efficacy beliefs
to human functioning is to test the multivariate relations
between relevant determinants and subsequent performances
using structural equation modeling. Such analyses indicate
how much of the variation in performance is explained by
perceived self-efficacy when the influence of other
determinants, including past performance, is controlled. The
results of numerous studies revealed that efficacy beliefs
contribute to performance both directly and through their
impact on other determinants (Bandura & Jourden, 1991;
Locke, Frederick, Lee, & Bobko, 1984; Ozer & Bandura,
1990; Wood & Bandura, 1989).
326
1977; Bandura, Adams, & Beyer, 1977; Bandura, Adams,
Hardy, & Howells, 1980). The results of these microanalytic
studies were consistent in showing that the self-efficacy belief
system is a common pathway through which diverse
interventions effect changes.
Self-Regulatory Mechanism
People are not only knowers and performers. They are also
self-reactors with a capacity to motivate, guide, and regulate
their activities (Bandura, 1986; 1991). Another line of
327
research that I was pursuing was designed to advance
understanding of self-regulatory mechanisms. Self-regulation
operates through a set of psychological subfunctions that must
be developed and mobilized for self-directed change. People
have to learn to monitor their behavior and judge it against an
adopted performance standard. The cognitive comparison sets
the occasion for self-reactive influence. Self-reactions provide
the mechanism by which personal standards regulate
motivation and action.
328
Self-Management Models with Social
Utility
Our knowledge of self-regulatory and self-efficacy
mechanisms was used to devise efficacious self-management
models with high social utility. Applications to health
promotion and disease prevention is but one such example.
329
Efficacy beliefs play an influential role in every phase of
personal change —whether people even consider changing
their health habits; whether they enlist the motivation and
perseverance needed to succeed should they choose to do so;
their vulnerability to relapse; their success in recovering
control after a setback; and how well they maintain the habit
changes they have achieved (Bandura, 1997; 1998).
330
In tests of this system, employees in the workplace lowered
high cholesterol levels by altering eating habits high in
saturated fats (Bandura, 1997). They achieved even larger
reductions if their spouses participated. The greater the room
for dietary change, the greater the reduction in plasma
cholesterol. A single nutritionist implemented the entire
program at minimal cost for large numbers of employees.
Sodium intake is linked to hypertension in people who are
sensitive to this mineral, a sensitivity that increases with age
as the body loses some of its efficiency. West and his
colleagues demonstrated with patients suffering from heart
disease that the self-management system enhances their
self-regulatory efficacy and gets them to cut back on their
level of sodium intake to desired levels and to maintain the
low sodium diet stably over time (West, et al., 1999). At each
successive point in the self-change program, the stronger the
perceived self-regulatory efficacy, the greater the reduction in
sodium intake.
331
arteriosclerosis. They also had fewer hospitalizations for
coronary heart problems, and fewer deaths.
332
with minimal guidance have a high sense of efficacy that they
can get themselves to adopt healthful habits and to stick to
them. Those who distrust their ability to succeed give up
trying when they run into difficulties. They need additional
support and guidance via telephone contact to see them
through tough times. And finally, those who believe that their
health habits are beyond their personal control need a great
deal of guidance in a stepwise mastery program. Graduated
successes build belief in their ability to exercise control and
bolster their staying power in the face of difficulties and
setbacks.
333
The way health care systems deal with clients can alter their
sense of efficacy in ways that support or undermine their
restorative efforts (Bandura, 1998). Clients are, therefore,
taught how to take greater initiative for their health care and
dealings with health personnel. These capabilities are
developed through modeling of self-management skills,
guided mastery practice, and informative feedback.
Macrosocial Applications
Social modeling also plays a paramount role at the
society-wide level in enabling people to improve their lives
and social conditions. Modeling is not only an important
component in most modes of change, but an essential one. It
shortcuts the tortuous process of competency development.
The revolutionary advances in the communications
technologies have vastly expanded the power of symbolic
modeling. In modern day life, the electronic media, feeding
off the communications satellites, are shaping lifestyles
worldwide, transforming institutional practices, and serving
as a major vehicle of sociopolitical change (Bandura, 1997;
Braithwaite, 1994).
334
Symbolic modeling is readily applicable to macrosocial
applications through creative use of the electronic media. The
soaring population growth and the environmental devastation
it produces is the most urgent global problem. The world
population is doubling at an accelerating rate. It will seriously
strain the earth’s carrying capacity and degrade the quality of
life if left unchecked (Ehrlich, Ehrlich, & Daily, 1995).
335
the social practices that are enacted and to enhance their
recallability.
336
The greater the exposure to the modeled patterns, the stronger
the effects on perceived efficacy to control family size and
risky sexual practices.
Concluding Remarks
The present article traces the evolution of social cognitive
theory from a chilly tributary to part of the mainstream. The
value of a psychological theory is judged by three criteria. It
must have explanatory power; predictive power and, in the
final analysis, it must demonstrate operative power to
improve the human condition. Well-founded theory provides
solutions to human problems. This brief retrospective report
addresses some of the facets of an agentic sociocognitive
approach to human understanding and betterment, and
documents some of the applications of this theory at both
individual and macrosocial levels.
337
References
Bandura, A. (1961). Psychotherapy as a learning process.
Psychological Bulletin, 58, 143-159.
338
Bandura, A., & Adams, N. E. (1977). Analysis of
self-efficacy theory of behavioral change. Cognitive Therapy
and Research, 1, 287-308.
339
Bandura, A., & Kupers, C. J. (1964). Transmission of patterns
of self-reinforcement through modeling. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 69, 1-9.
340
Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. (1963). Social learning and
personality development. New York: Holt, Rinehart &
Winston.
341
DeBusk, R. F., Miller, N. H., Superko, H. R., Dennis, C. A.,
Thomas, R. J., Lew, H. T., Berger III, W. E., Heller, R. S.,
Rompf, J., Gee, D., Kraemer, H. C., Bandura, A., Ghandour,
G., Clark, M., Shah, R. V., Fisher, L., & Taylor, C. B. (1994).
A case-management system for coronary risk factor
modification after acute myocardial infarction. Annals of
Internal Medicine, 120, 721-729.
342
Holroyd, K. A., Penzien, D. B., Hursey, K. G., Tobin, D. L.,
Rogers, L., Holm, J. E., Marcille, P. J., Hall, J. R., & Chila,
A. G. (1984). Change mechanisms in EMG biofeedback
training: Cognitive changes underlying improvements in
tension headache. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 52, 1039-1053.
343
Lorig, K. (1990, April). Self-efficacy: Its contributions to the
four year beneficial outcome of the arthritis self- management
course. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for
Behavioral Medicine, Chicago, IL.
344
Ozer, E. M., & Bandura, A. (1990). Mechanisms governing
empowerment effects: A self-efficacy analysis. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 472-486.
345
Singhal, A., & Rogers, E. M. (1989). Pro-social television for
development in India. In R. E. Rice & C. K. Atkin (Eds.),
Public communication campaigns (2nd ed., pp. 331-350).
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
West, J. A., Bandura, A., Clark, E., Miller, N. H., Ahn, D.,
Greenwald, G., & DeBusk, R. F. (1999). Self-efficacy predicts
adherence to dietary sodium limitation in patients with heart
failure. Unpublished manuscript, Stanford University.
346
Whiting, J. W. M., & Child, I. L. (1953). Child training and
personality. New Haven: Yale University Press.
347
Wood, R. E., & Bandura, A. (1989). Social cognitive theory
of organizational management. Academy of Management
Review, 14, 361-384.
348
Chapter 9
Biographical Introduction
I can’t say that I originated cognitive behavior therapy
because you can always find some ancient sage who practiced
a form of it many hundreds of years ago. Thus, Gautama
Buddha became enlightened and founded Buddhism more
than 2500 years ago; and to his cognitive teachings were soon
added the behavioral exercises of the Zen Buddhists and other
groups. Hindu Yoga practices are also definitely cognitive
behavioral, as are the combinings of religious philosophies
with various behavioral rituals of the ancient Hebrews and the
early Christians.
349
profound personality changes, humans often have to strongly
and persistently follow these combined methods. Combined
cognitive, emotional, and activity procedures are almost
essential to basic personality change.
350
trainer, Richard Hulbeck, was a training analyst of the Karen
Horney Institute and was also a leading existentialist
(Hoellen, 1993). Moreover, on my own I was opposed to
Freudian analysis, since I thought that Freud (1965) was
exceptionally unscientific, knew very little about the origin of
people’s sex and love problems, and wrote brilliant fiction
which he presented as scientific non-fiction. So I mostly
followed the neo-Freudian views of Alfred Adler (1929),
Franz Alexander and Thomas French (1946), Erich Fromm
(1955), Karen Horney (1950) and Harry Stack Sullivan
(1953).
351
Why was REBT, right from its start, heavily behavioral?
Mainly because I had used behavior therapy on myself before
I even thought of becoming a therapist; and because I had
used it successfully for a dozen years in my specialty of sex,
love, and marriage therapy. I first used it when I was 19 years
of age and had a severe phobia of public speaking. I was the
youth leader of a radical political group, but never dared give
a public speech. But by reading the early experiments of John
B. Watson and his associates (Jones, 1920; Watson, 1919;
Watson & Raynor, 1920), I learned that they deconditioned
young children in a few sessions to overcome their fears of
animals by in vivo desensitization. So I forced myself, very
uncomfortably, to speak and speak in public and within ten
weeks got completely over my phobia — and since that time,
as I frequently tell my workshop audiences, you can’t keep
me away from the public speaking platform!
352
I actually, in one month, sat next to 130 young women —
which I had practically never done in my life before, so afraid
was I of being rejected. Whereupon 30 out of the 130
immediately got up and walked away. But that left me an
even sample of 100 — good for research purposes! Nothing
daunted, I opened a conversation with the remaining 100
women — for the first time, again, in my entire young life. I
spoke about the weather, the birds and the bees, the flowers
and the trees, the book they were reading — truly, about
everything and anything.
353
approaching women for the rest of my life and have espoused
the pickup technique of meeting new partners to hundreds of
my clients over the years.
354
century by realizing that suggestion was not only at the heart
of hypnotic therapy, but also that people’s negative
self-suggestion — or what he called autosuggestion — was a
prime element in creating neurotic disturbance and that they
could consciously choose to replace it with positive
autosuggestion to solve many of their emotive and behavioral
problems. Coué also invented positive visualization to help
disturbed people improve their psychophysical functioning.
So he was definitely a cognitive behavioral therapist.
355
On the contrary, the ancient philosophers were
constructivists. They largely maintained, along with Epictetus
(1899), who wrote in The Enchiridion or Manuel in the first
century, “People are disturbed not by things, but by the views
which they take of them.” This philosophy gives humans
some choice in making themselves disturbed and undisturbed;
and it was solidly reiterated by the existentialist philosophers
— such as Kierkegaard (1953), Heidigger (1962), Sartre
(1968), and Tillich (1953) — in modern times.
356
This was much more specific than the previous cognitive
therapies, and was largely adopted by most of the other
cognitive behavioral systems that originated about a decade
later, such as those of Glasser (1965), Beck (1967),
Meichenbaum (1974, 1977), and Mahoney (1974).
357
1958a, 1958b, 1960, 1962) that it is both highly
emotive-evocative and behavioral; and I changed its name in
1961 from rational therapy (RT) to rational emotive therapy
(RET). In the 1960’s, following the work of Perls (1969) and
Shutz (1967), I also added many experiential exercises to
REBT, including my famous shame-attacking exercises (Ellis,
1973). So, from the start, REBT differed from most other
therapies in its regular use of many cognitive, many emotive,
and many behavioral methods; and in this respect it has
always been, to use Arnold Lazarus’ (1989) term, multimodal
(Kwee & Ellis, 1997).
358
Emotion, like thinking and the sensorimotor processes, we
may define as an exceptionally complex state of human
reaction which is integrally related to all the other perception
and response processes. It is not one thing, but a combination
and a holistic integration of several seemingly diverse, yet
actually closely related, phenomena (Ellis, 1958, p. 35).
359
behavior therapy and seem to be increasingly headed in that
direction. So today, more than ever before, behavior and
cognitive behavior therapy are following REBT and Lazarus’
multimodal therapy in becoming wide-ranging in their
therapeutic procedures.
360
“blaming anyone for anything at any time,” is essentially
identical with Carl Rogers’ principle of unconditional positive
regard. The thoroughness with which they espouse this
principle and its implications together with their respective
ways of effectively implementing it, distinguishes their
systems from other systems. This is the source of their
potency and economy. I consider Ellis’ Reason and Emotion
in Psychotherapy the most important contribution to the field
since Carl Rogers’ contribution (Bone, 1968, p. 174).
361
Harold Greenwald, and Rowena and Heinz Ausbacher. Frank
Richardson said that “It is still perhaps the single best
‘self-help’ book available to lay persons and psychotherapy
clients” (1977, p. 271). Sol Gordon noted, “Still, in my
judgment, the most sensible and usable of the self-help
books” (1980, p. 203).
362
today, no matter what system of therapy they say they follow.
Many of its common procedures — such as cognitive
restructuring and in vivo shame-attacking exercises — are
widely used by many different kinds of therapists.
363
workable methods can also interfere with clients’ using
deeper and more elegant methods of treatment.
References
Adler, A. (1929). The science of living. New York:
Greenberg.
364
Clark, D. A. (1997). Twenty years of cognitive assessment:
Current status and future directions. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 65, 946-1000.
365
Ellis, A. (1958a). Rational psychotherapy. Journal of General
Psychology, 59, 35-49.
Ellis, A. (1960). The art and science of love. New York: Lyle
Stuart & Bantam.
366
Ellis, A. (1996). Better, deeper, and more enduring brief
therapy. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
367
Forel, A. (1922). The sexual question. New York: Physician’s
and Surgeon’s Book Company.
368
Heidegger, M. (1962). Being and time. New York: Harper &
Row.
369
Lazarus, A. A. (1989). The practice of multimodal therapy.
Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins.
370
Rogers, C. R. (1943). Counseling and psychotherapy. Boston:
Houghton Mifflin.
371
372
Chapter 10
From Psychodynamic to
Behavior Therapy:
Paradigm Shift and Personal
Perspectives
Cyril M. Franks
Introduction
The value of an intellectual biography lies in tracing the
connection between events and thoughts. This intellectual
autobiography spans some 50 years, 3,000 miles and the
greatest adventure of all, a journey of the mind from the birth
of behavior therapy to its coming of age on the threshold of a
new millennium. My early years are likely to be of only
historic interest to the general reader and alien to most
373
American psychologists. Nevertheless, a description of these
formative years might clarify the manner in which my
thinking about behavior therapy evolved. The basic
ingredients include: a classical education stressing language
and literature; training in both the technology and
methodology of applied science; my wartime experiences; my
growing disappointment with psychoanalysis, at best a
pseudo-scientific model with a total unawareness of outcome
evaluation, accountability and the like; the cavalier treatment
of psychologists in medical facilities; the self-satisfied
medical establishment’s unawareness that any therapy other
than Freudian could be either possible or necessary. Half a
century ago, most clinical psychologists seemed to tolerate
their exclusion as therapists and see little that was wrong with
this situation. Inspired by the work of Hans Eysenck and his
associates, such factors sparked my desire to establish a new,
and eventually accepted, behavioral paradigm. To the best of
my knowledge, these matters have not been described
elsewhere.
Formative Years
I was born in 1923 in a primarily English — rather than
Welsh — speaking resort town in South Wales. Until war
came my childhood was pleasant and uneventful. As with all
children of promise, my education was fully subsidized from
kindergarten through postgraduate university levels by the
British regional department of education. I attended an elitist
high school for boys from ages 11 through 17 where selection
for a strictly academic education was determined by high
scores on a rigorous screening examination. Intellectually
qualifying girls received a similar education in a separate
374
school. My earliest recollection was of Dylan Thomas, a then
unknown one-of-the-crowd aspiring writer several years
ahead of me in school, striving to instill the elements of
Chaucer into my unappreciative head. In this rarefied setting,
typing and woodwork were out of the question and sports and
physical training were compulsory after-school activities in
addition to a demanding homework schedule. Nevertheless,
morale and teacher/student camaraderie were high despite
continuing academic pressures.
375
to pass all school examinations. But, by then, my technical
interest in both physics and electronics had began to wane. As
the war intensified, at 18 years of age I was directed, because
of my specialization subjects, to become a full-time student at
the nearby University of Wales and complete an intensive,
accelerated four-year degree program in two years, continuing
with the same three high school subjects. It was too late to
change and, in any event, I had no say in the matter. Time
was not on the British side and, despite the Soviet Union’s
valiant war effort, we were still very much alone. Fortunately,
things changed when America entered the war. Two years
later I obtained my expected B.S. degree as planned and,
despite a still declining interest in applied physics and
electronics, once again I was drafted, this time to a top-secret
government facility for electronic “hot shots” who, for
painfully obvious reasons, soon turned out not to include me.
376
wherever I chose, again at government expense. Although by
then vastly more interested in the methodology of science
than the technology of electronics, my love of literature and
writing remained. At the same time, my interest in people and
whatever made them “tick” blossomed. So, as a compromise,
I enrolled in a 16-month teacher-training program at London
University, specializing in teaching applied sciences and
elementary electronics at technical high school levels.
The Search
My first, and last, school job was teaching general science to
Merchant Service cadets at the London Nautical School.
Classes ending in the early afternoon left me ample time to
take numerous university courses in general and clinical
psychology. These under my belt, four years later I was
eligible to apply for admission both to what is now known as
a clinical internship and a Ph.D. program in psychology. Then
came the daunting task of finding an acceptable university
base. Even at this early stage I knew that “acceptable” meant
a broadly behavioral climate and a stringent questioning of
Freudian theory and practice which, even then, I regarded as a
pseudo-science. Freudian psychotherapy went unchallenged
by all mental health practitioners and necessities such as
control groups, validation, outcome evaluation, follow-up and
patient satisfaction were unheard of.
For three months I briefly visited and rejected most of the few
university departments in Western Europe offering training in
clinical psychology at that time. Another requirement, then
thought out of the question for non-physicians, was that
fully-trained clinical psychologists be permitted to serve as
377
therapists in medically controlled settings. I felt that this
situation would never change in the foreseeable future.
Fortunately, I was wrong but, at the time, I had no idea how
change might come about.
378
Maudsley Days
Eysenck, then at the beginning of his career and already
department head, even in those early days, was a favorite
target of criticism. Prominent in psychology circles, Eysenck
had astutely gathered around him a coterie of loyal but, at
times, critical students and junior faculty periodically
reinforced by an infusion of visiting colleagues from around
the world. Our first, and long-term-goal, then regarded by the
mental health community as an impossible dream, was to
supplement the ubiquitous, and then only, model of Freudian
therapy with an, as yet, undetermined data-based learning
theory approach. In effect, this involved a paradigm shift
from a psychodynamic to a behavioral model and in which
the two paradigms were expected initially to co-exist with
gradually diminishing psychodynamic influence.
379
terms of two factorially determined orthogonal dimensions,
first neuroticism and introversion-extraversion followed,
much later, by psychoticism (the latter remaining more a hope
than a reality). To advance these goals a soundproof classical
conditioning laboratory had to be constructed, using primarily
eyeblink and GSR conditioning.
380
training in projective techniques was relaxed soon after even
though they were always used sparingly and with caution.
381
The Dawn of Behavior Therapy
Having agreed that our new approach to therapy would stem
from the notions of Pavlov and Hull, if only because there
was really no feasible alternative, we set out to develop some
behaviorally valid theory-based innovative procedures, a task,
which turned out to be more arduous than anticipated. At first,
laboriously we produced little more than a few novel, but
validated, parent-training reinforcement techniques, some
phobia extinction and anxiety reduction strategies and
unimaginative aversion conditioning training for the mentally
retarded. Not knowing better, initially we focused exclusively
on the presenting problem and none of this could even
remotely be called behavior therapy. This was around the
time that Eysenck (1959) coined the term behavior therapy in
Europe, Wolpe and Lazarus (1958) in South Africa and
Lindsley, Skinner and Solomon in the USA (1958), all
working independently. Regrettably, we overlooked the
operant conditioning of Skinner and his associates, perhaps
because we viewed Skinner as primarily an animal
psychologist and not a clinician.
382
still unworthy of the name behavior therapy, or any therapy
for that matter, failed dismally, as we might have expected.
Slowly, I became more sophisticated and learned from
experience, publishing a few exploratory studies of the
behavioral treatment of alcoholism and achieving no real
success until I updated my thinking. Anticipating the
trail-blazing multimodal therapy of Lazarus (1971), I went
beyond conditioned aversion per se, began to talk to the
patient about seemingly significant concerns in addition to
presenting problems and started to explore multi-level
situations, real life settings and multi-stimulus approaches,
including the development of a conditioned aversion, trying
to modify the patient’s self-defeating life-style. Although our
short-term success rates were modestly better, but far from
spectacular, for me this was the beginning of primitive
behavior therapy (Franks, 1963), a tiny crack in the
physician’s Freudian superstructure.
383
degree at the University of Minnesota where primarily, I
learned that everyone “did his or her own thing.” Few had
heard of behavior therapy whereas, at the University of
Minnesota, everyone knew all about the MMPI!
384
American therapists would offer better opportunities in a
more friendly climate. Once again I was disappointed.
385
AABT. These individuals include such luminaries as Paul
Brady, Edward Dengrove, Andrew Salter, Arthur Staats,
Leonard Ulmann, Leonard Krasner, Arnold Lazarus, Joseph
Cautela and Joseph Wolpe in addition to Dorothy Susskind
and myself. We constituted the initial planning group, with
Stuart Agras and Eysenck offering periodic support from afar.
Sadly, Salter, Wolpe, Eysenck and Cautela are no longer with
us and we continue to miss them. On a happier note, three
founding fathers are still professionally active; Staats,
Lazarus, and myself.
386
mundane chores as stuffing envelopes and folding and
mailing announcements. It was around this period, 1970, that
I became Professor of Psychology at Rutgers University.
387
the occasional pockets of resistance, still encountered from
some physicians, are becoming fewer and fully-qualified
nonmedical behavior therapists are now free to practice in
most psychiatric facilities on almost equal footings—a
remarkable index of progress in a comparatively short time.
Third and fourth are two earlier texts, both “firsts” in their
respective ways. The first, Conditioning Techniques in
Clinical Practice and Research (Franks, 1964), focused on
the limited behavior therapy literature of that time. The
second, an overview of the still limited behavioral literature in
1969, consists primarily of an appraisal of what I then
considered to reflect the overall status of behavior therapy,
written by others and myself. Appropriately, the title is
Behavior Therapy: Appraisal and Status (Franks, 1969).
388
Since the field changes so rapidly, both books are now of
primarily historic interest.
389
unity of behavior therapy was rapidly falling apart. Until the
1990’s my still lingering belief in the unity of behavior
therapy seemed feasible and that, somehow, even the unity of
psychology at large was at hand, so much so that, in 1993, I
became President of Arthur Staats’ now defunct organization
SUNI: the Society for Studying Unity Issues. Now, however,
recent events force me to reconsider my position both with
respect, on the one hand, to the desirability of the notion of
unity in behavior therapy and, on the other hand, the
probability that diversity rather than unity in behavior therapy
adds vitality to our movement. After all, not surprising, if
long-established disciplines such as physics and medicine
continue to fall far short of unity how can the relatively new
discipline of behavior therapy expect to succeed? Regrettably,
Staats’ lifelong attempt to generate a comprehensive system
of clinical/behavioral psychology has not received the
recognition it merits.
390
obvious to me I find this curious notion totally unacceptable.
Behavior therapy and psychodynamic therapy are two
incompatible systems and both, in my book, would probably
fare better to go about their respective ways (Franks, 1984).
391
the West, could legally be neither practiced nor studied
anywhere in the then Communist world regardless of the fact
that my brand of behavior therapy stems largely from the
work of Pavlov. In all Communist countries, behavior
therapy, said the Soviets, is a regressive product of S-R
learning theory built upon a “false and simplistic” 19th
century mechanistic materialism. Patiently, my Soviet
colleagues tried, with no success, to correct my faulty
thinking to reflect a “politically correct” dialectical
materialism rather than my “primitive” mechanistic
materialism.
It was not until very recently that I admitted that, as far as the
differences between mechanistic and dialectical materialism
are concerned, the Soviets had a point, but, of course, I never
could accept the Marxist overtones. Only recently did I begin
to appreciate contemporary developments in operant
conditioning, applied behavior analysis and their, as yet
largely unrealized clinical potentials. Gradually I became
aware of very different behavioral models couched in
functional dynamic, operant conditioning and contextual
terms involving notions still new to me, such as integrative
couples therapy, dialectical behavior therapy and more. It is
time to take into account more than an exclusively
mechanistic behavior therapy, models hitherto neglected by
most therapists, models not, as yet, well-developed clinically
in sharp contrast to their mechanistic, classical
conditioning-based predecessor.
It was not until late 1999 that Hayes and Toarmino clearly
pinpointed two very different, and rarely interacting,
traditions in behavior therapy, thereby sharpening my fuzzy
thinking about such matters, two compartmentalized
392
traditions with few points of interaction, the first popular and
well-established clinically and the second clinically not
well-developed and yet to show what it could accomplish; the
first tradition, still largely mechanistic and based upon
classical conditioning, the second less mechanistic, stemming,
in large part, from operant conditioning, and applied behavior
analysis.
Postscript
In 1970 I became Professor of Psychology at Rutgers
University in its clinical Ph.D. program. From time to time
my positions have included Clinical Director and Director of
post-Doctoral Training. In 1974 the Rutgers Graduate School
of Applied and Professional Psychology came into being,
offering a full-time doctorate in applied and professional
psychology (Psy.D.) for clinicians, the first of its kind
anywhere. My active involvement with both programs
continued until retirement as Distinguished Professor
Emeritus in 1991.
393
The evening of retirement festivities began with a merciless,
but lovingly crafted, “roasting” engineered by half a dozen
longtime associates in the presence of several hundred
colleagues and friends from the USA and abroad. The
evening concluded with a quite different, totally unexpected,
event, the annual “Cyril M. Franks’ Award for Excellence in
Research.” A small committee would meet annually to select
what, in the committee’s opinion, was the most outstanding
doctoral dissertation of the year at Rutgers in professional
psychology. As part of the graduation ceremony, held at the
end of each academic year, the award-winning new doctor is
presented with an appropriately inscribed plaque and his or
her name added to an annual, wall-mounted list. The recipient
also receives a modest honorarium, a procedure followed ever
since.
394
References
Dollard, J., & Miller, N. D. (1950). Personality and
psychotherapy. New York: McGraw-Hill.
395
Hayes, S. C., & Toarmino, D. (1999). The rise of clinical
behaviour analysis. The Psychologist, 12, 105-108.
396
Chapter 11
Cognitive Behavior
Therapy: The Oxymoron of
the Century
Leonard Krasner
Stanford University
Introduction
The topics I plan to cover are the rationale for the topic of the
paper “Cognitive Behavior Therapy – The Oxymoron of the
Century,” a history of “behavior therapy,” “behavior
modification” and the Unabomber, some views and the
joining of the label of “cognitive,” of cognitive therapy, with
“behavior therapy” and a quick touch on the future of
“behavior therapy.”
397
In effect, the contentions of this paper are that both behavior
therapy and cognitive therapy are helpful procedures in
alleviating health and mental health problems, although my
own bias makes me prefer behavior therapy. I am not
precluding a therapist using both procedures, cognitive
therapy and behavior therapy to help a troubled client.
However, the major theme of the paper is that the label
“cognitive behavior therapy” is an “oxymoron.”
Behavior Therapy
As for the history of behavior therapy, I will start with a paper
I wrote on “Behavior Therapy.” It was in volume 22 of the
Annual Review of Psychology, 1971, and it was the first paper
on this topic in the Annual Review.
398
behavior therapy. In preparing the material for this chapter we
have compiled a bibliography of over 4000 items, most of
which should be included in any comprehensive review of
this field. Needless to say, space limitations in this volume
and the finiteness of the author’s life preclude full justice
being done to all.
399
(1958, 1968, 1969), who in turn was influenced by the
psychologists James Taylor and Leo Reyna. Wolpe
introduced the technique of reciprocal inhibition, basing it in
part on the classical conditioning research of Pavlov and Hull.
400
The social psychology laboratory has contributed a
psychology of social influence which is being increasingly
incorporated into a behavior therapy framework.
401
large part, out of dissatisfaction with traditional
psychotherapy techniques.
402
Unless we view behavior therapy in this context, its
applications and implications will be irrelevant.
403
The first use of the term behavior therapy in the literature was
in a 1953 status report by Lindsley, Skinner, and Solomon,
referring to their application of operant conditioning (of a
plunger pulling response) research with psychotic patients.
Lindsley suggested the term to Skinner, based on it simplicity
and linkage to other treatment procedures.
404
combine the two approaches was a controversy of the period.
Eysenck (1969), for example, took the position that bridging
between behavior therapy and psychoanalysis was
undesirable. My own position on this issue is that the bridging
of two paradigmatic models which have historically
developed as clear alternatives to each other would generally
show a misunderstanding of the basic principles of both.
405
which entailed more or less original ways of thinking about
and approaching clinical problems.”
Behavior Modification
Next we move on to the label of “behavior modification” and
its impact on an individual who has become quite infamous in
our society, the Unabomber.
406
Bandura (1969), in a most influential and widely cited book,
placed “the principles of behavior modification” within the
conceptual framework of social learning….By requiring clear
specification of treatment conditions and objective assessment
of outcomes, the social learning approach…contains a
self-corrective feature that distinguishes it from change
enterprises in which interventions remain ill-defined and their
psychological effects are seldom objectively evaluated (p. v).
407
from graduate students saying “watch your mail.” However,
within the next year, Ted Kaczynski was arrested as the
Unabomber and on the list of people he had sent bombs to, in
1985, was James McConnell, a psychology professor at the
University of Michigan, who had written articles on
“behavior modification.” McConnell did not open the
package but his secretary did and was badly hurt.
408
Cognition: “The mental process or faculty by which
knowledge is acquired. That which comes to be known as
through perception, reasoning or intuition; knowledge”
(American Heritage Dictionary).
409
cognitive/disease) model. Behavior therapy has, to a large
extent, been co-opted by and merged into the inner model,
thus it would no longer represent the paradigm in which many
of its early adherents believed. The term “cognition” has
returned to a predominant position in psychology. We view
these developments not as another paradigm shift but rather
as a paradigm lost.
410
right after all, everyone could relax. Mind was back.
Cognitive psychologists like to say that “the mind is what the
brain does,” but surely the rest of the body plays a part. The
mind is what the body does. It is what the person does. In
other words, it is behavior, and that is what behaviorists have
been saying for more than half a century.
411
medical or psychoanalytic model that aims to alter a person’s
behavior indirectly by first altering intrapsychic
organizations.”
412
In effect according to Wolpe, the theoretical and practical
roots of behavior therapy were deviated from, misunderstood,
and misrepresented.
413
and that the goals of BT should be extended to include more
comprehensive and enduring “personality change.” In this
commentary, I discuss the underlying medical model adopted
by cognitive therapists, the empirical data that bear on the
issue of “adding” to BT, and an alternative framework to
evaluate clinical practice. It is suggested that there are more
similarities among therapists, and their therapies, than
differences. Thus, as Charles Ferster (1972) suggested 25
years ago, more effort should be devoted to studying what
successful therapists do, and less to arguing the merits of
therapists’ theoretically informed explanations for success
(Reitman, 1997).
414
efficacious (Eysenck, 1952). Behavior therapy has changed
all that by adding hundreds of controlled treatment trials to
the literature in the intervening years. The major advances in
the treatment of anxiety disorders, depression, eating
disorders, alcohol dependence, and the many applications
within the field of behavioral medicine all attest to the
remarkable success of behavior therapy. The field has brought
experiment to the clinic, has refined the methodology for such
studies, and has developed the standards for determining the
efficacy and effectiveness of psychosocial treatments.
Non-behavioral therapies must now meet the same standards,
and some of them, for example, interpersonal therapy
(Weissman & Markowitz, 1994), are beginning to do so. Such
trends bring us precariously close to a purely empirical
approach to therapeutic behavior change, but that is what is
needed if our aim is to understand the mechanisms by which
all effective psychosocial treatments work in order to
understand how best to change behavior (Agras, 1997).
415
and disagreements that have occurred within the behavior
therapy movement.
References
Bandura, A. (1968). Modeling approaches to the modification
of phobic disorders. In R. Poner (Ed.), The role of learning in
psychotherapy (pp. 201-16). London: Churchill.
416
Bandura, A. (1969). Principles of behavior modification. New
York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
417
Krasner, L. (1971). Behavior therapy. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.),
Annual Review of Psychology (Vol. 22). Palo Alto,
CA:Annual Reviews.
418
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York:
Appleton-Century-Crofts.
419
Wolpe, J. (1969). Basic principles and practices of behavior
therapy of neuroses. American Journal of Psychiatry, 125,
1242-47.
420
Chapter 12
The Development of
Behavioral Medicine
W. Stewart Agras
421
All this may have come to nothing had I not moved to the
University of Vermont, to a newly formed Department of
Psychiatry headed by Tom Boag, a faculty member from
McGill. Until that time, psychiatry had been a division of the
department of medicine, rather than being a department in its
own right. Here, fortuitously, Harold Leitenberg, with a Ph.D.
in experimental psychology from the operant program at
Indiana, arrived as an assistant professor in the Department of
Psychology at the same time. I was looking for an
experimental psychologist to work with, and he was looking
for an entree to the clinic to apply operant procedures.
Together we worked with two different experimental methods
to identify effective therapeutic procedures for the treatment
of phobia. Following Lang’s work using snake phobics as an
analogue of clinical phobia (Lang & Lazovik, 1963; Lang,
Lazovik, & Reynolds, 1965), we examined the hypothesized
therapeutic ingredients in desensitization in randomized
controlled studies, eventually demonstrating that none of the
hypothesized procedures such as a hierarchy or pairing
imagined feared scenes with relaxation, appeared critical to
the outcome (Agras, Leitenberg, Barlow, Curtis, Edwards, &
Wright, 1971; Barlow, Leitenberg, Agras, & Wincze, 1969).
In a complementary approach with phobic patients, we took
an operant approach to phobias of various types, examining
the effects of reinforcement, informational feedback, and
exposure to the feared situation, in a series of single case
controlled research studies. We were fortunate in being able
to admit these patients to the University of Vermont Clinical
Research Center (CRC), where they could be studied using
single case experimental designs, in a controlled environment
free of charge. We were also fortunate to attract an excellent
group of graduate students in psychology, the first of which
was David Barlow. With this group, we examined other
422
disorders in similar ways: anorexia nervosa (Agras, Barlow,
Chapin, Abel, & Leitenberg, 1974; Leitenberg, Agras, &
Thompson, 1968), tics (Agras & Marshall, 1965), hysterical
paralysis (Agras, Leitenberg, Barlow, & Thompson, 1969),
and even delusional speech (Wincze, Leitenberg, & Agras,
1972). In each case demonstrating experimental control over
these difficult to manage behaviors.
423
was observable, frequent checks of the patient’s behavior
were made throughout the study, confirming the accuracy of
their reports. Following a baseline phase in which the patient
was told to walk as far as they could alone, praise for progress
in walking further was given in a shaping schedule. As can be
seen in Figure 1, distance walked increased steadily while
being reinforced, declined dramatically when no
reinforcement was given, showing first a typical extinction
burst, and then quickly recovered when reinforcement was
reinstated, with the patient being able to walk downtown
alone, something she had not done for many years.
424
The first patient was a severe claustrophobic whose phobia
dated back to childhood when she was locked in a small
cupboard by some friends, one of the rare occasions when a
specific trauma is reported as leading to a phobia. The fears
became increasingly incapacitating and were much worsened
when her husband died about seven years before admission to
the CRC. Briefly the experiment consisted in giving the
patient a stopwatch and asking her to record the amount of
time she was able to remain in a small dimly lit room. This
provided her with feedback as to her progress. After 22
sessions in which she was able to increase her time in the
room, the stopwatch was removed, with the excuse that it was
broken. Under this condition her progress slowed and then
picked up again when the stopwatch was reintroduced.
Similar results were found for the second case.
Parenthetically, the close relationship to animal studies is
revealed in this paper by the detailed description of the
experimental environment, e.g. “A room 4 ft. wide and 6 ft.
long, illuminated by a 100W shaded bulb, provided a
situation in which the patients claustrophobia could be
measured.”
425
Five patients took part in these experiments, although only
one of these patients who participated in two studies will be
discussed here (Leitenberg, Agras, Edwards & Thompson,
1970). The patient was a knife phobic. Her problem had
begun some seven years before the experiment. While using a
kitchen knife she suddenly thought that she might kill one of
her grandchildren who were running around the kitchen
making a noise. This very frightening intrusive thought
recurred and during the next year her obsessive thoughts
increased so that she became unable to use a knife. She was
admitted to a psychiatric unit and treated with various
medications and systematic desensitization and was
discharged improved. Eighteen months later her husband
died, precipitating a marked depression that required further
hospitalization and treatment with electroshock therapy, again
improving. However, her fear of knives slowly returned, and
once more she was unable to handle or even look at a sharp
knife, and had been unable to cook or work in her kitchen.
426
treated. Hence, in the next series of trials, psychotherapy was
alternated with exposure practice, with a positive expectancy
engendered for both treatments. Again, however, no progress
occurred during the no exposure (psychotherapy) phases,
while continued progress occurred during exposure trials.
This result was confirmed in our other work using
randomized controlled experiments with snake phobic
individuals (Barlow, Leitenberg, Agras, & Wincze, 1969;
Leitenberg, Agras, Barlow, & Oliveau, 1969; Oliveau, Agras,
Leitenberg, Moore, & Wright, 1969). It was clear that
exposure to the actual phobic situation was far superior to
imagined exposure as would occur in systematic
desensitization.
427
disseminating new psychotherapeutic procedures, a problem
that remains with us today.
428
It should be noted that the introduction of behavioral science
teaching in medical schools began in 1958 at the University
of Kentucky. At the time of my move to Mississippi there
were some 17 Departments of Behavioral Science in schools
of medicine. Hence the need to introduce the findings of
modern behavioral science to medical students and residents
in various disciplines was well recognized. Yet much of this
teaching was not viewed as relevant by medical students and
other physicians in training. This was probably because
nothing was taught about the application of behavioral
science to the treatment of medical conditions. Mainly, of
course, because very little was known about such applications
at the time.
429
medical problems, was one of the important prerequisites for
the later development of behavioral medicine.
430
Psychosomatic Medicine during the same period.
Nonetheless, the basic psychological research stemming from
the psychosomatic medicine field, as it moved back to the
laboratory away from psychoanalytic theory, was an
important prerequisite for the development of behavioral
medicine.
431
Cleary, & Gutman, 1980). In 1975 the Laboratory for the
Study of Behavioral Medicine was founded at Stanford under
my direction. Simultaneously, demonstrating that the notion
of behavioral medicine was in the air at the time, Ovid
Pomerleau and Paul Brady opened a behavioral medicine
program at the University of Pennsylvania (Pomerleau, 1975;
Pomerleau, 1979). Both programs were focused on clinical
research using theories and procedures from the experimental
analysis of behavior and from behavior therapy. At this time
Bandura’s formulation of social learning theory formed the
theoretical background to our work (Bandura, 1965).
Researchers from the Stanford program became consultants to
the Coronary Primary Prevention Project, bringing their
expertise to the recruitment of the large number of
participants needed for this study, and later to the problem of
providing help to the participants in adhering to the
medication regimen in this long-term study. Hence, adherence
research became one focus of the Stanford program, followed
in the next few years by studies on the treatment of obesity,
and the use of non-pharmacological methods to reduce blood
pressure. The program produced basic psychological research
pertinent to the clinical problems being investigated,
identified behavioral risk factors for these disorders, and
carried out both preventive and intervention research. In
addition to the research focus, an outpatient clinic for the
treatment of stress disorders, obesity, and other medical
problems for which behavior change procedures seemed
justified, was opened, followed by an inpatient unit
specializing largely in eating disorders, pain problems, and
other medical disorders with accompanying psychological
problems. The close linkage between research and the clinic
was important because it allowed reciprocal feedback from
432
the two different worlds, on the one hand informing research,
and on the other informing clinical practice.
433
various disciplines involved in behavioral medicine. To this
end the Abstracts of Behavioral Medicine was begun, a
journal that provided selected abstracts of pertinent studies.
The notion was that primary research papers should be
published in the scientific journal most relevant to that
research and not in a specialized behavioral medicine
publication with limited readership. This step was aimed at
better disseminating the results of research from this new
field. The Abstracts was aimed at cross-disciplinary
communication.
434
Each of these procedures led to increased caloric intake and
weight gain in patients with anorexia nervosa and were used
in the treatment of such patients (Agras & Werne, 1977). The
research with bulimia nervosa began somewhat differently,
eventually following the research flow described in more
detail elsewhere (Agras & Berkowitz, 1980). The first step
was to treat a series of cases of bulimia nervosa treated with a
version of cognitive-behavior therapy (CBT) reported by
Fairburn, (1981). The results reported by Fairburn were
replicated with a reasonable proportion of bulimics
recovering by the end of treatment (Schneider & Agras,
1985). The treatment was based on the hypothesis, deriving
from clinical observation, that extreme dieting caused by
severe weight and shape concerns, was the driving force
behind binge eating and purging. Later studies carried out in
our laboratory demonstrated that when bulimics were
deprived of food by not serving them breakfast and lunch, and
were then served a buffet, they would eat more at the buffet
than a non-deprived control group of bulimics. However, the
number of calories eaten over the whole laboratory day were
not statistically significantly different between groups (Telch
& Agras, 1996). This suggests that the caloric regulation of
bulimics is normal. However, because they restrict food
intake and then eat a large amount of food at one sitting, they
perceive this to be a binge. Later studies suggested that mood
was likely to alter the perception of a feeding episode. In a
negative mood bulimics were more likely to classify an eating
episode as a binge (Agras & Telch, 1998).
435
patterns, at to guide the therapist and patient in selecting goals
for treatment, as well as providing monitoring of progress.
Weight and shape concerns and other cognitive and emotional
distortions were addressed with behavioral experiments and
cognitive therapy in the second half of treatment.
436
was necessary, leading to the first multisite study. The
background to the second study was the question as to what
treatment to offer patients who fail CBT. The two potential
candidates were antidepressant medication and IPT both of
which had proved useful in controlled trials. In order to have
sufficient subjects for such a comparison, i.e. treating only the
CBT failures (about 45% of those treated) a large sample size
was again needed, leading to the second multisite trial. The
conduct of multicenter trials is of necessity more complex
than the conduct of a single site trial. There are, however,
many advantages besides the large sample size that such trials
are able to accrue. The involvement of several investigators
leads to improvements in all aspects of the study including:
assessment procedures, therapist training, data acquisition and
analysis, as well as writing up the results of the study.
437
in its therapeutic procedures will attract innovative
researchers who will move the field forward. It may often be
the case that the early findings, seminal to the development of
the field, will later be found wanting. But such studies
provoke new research which is best served if it can utilize
findings and theories from basic science. After all, Wolpe
described animal experiments in his book, but did not provide
any controlled studies with human behavior problems.
But there have been some relative failures along the way. The
notion of prevention has not penetrated clinical medicine as
much as might have been hoped. It is true that the prevalence
of cigarette smoking has dropped as have the rates of lung
cancer. But the prevalence of overweight and obesity has
steadily increased over the years, even among children,
forecasting a rise in the diseases comorbid with obesity. For
behavioral medicine to advance prevention efforts it is clear
that scientists must ally themselves with political activists so
438
that changes can be made at the societal level. Altering the
ways in which cigarettes can be advertised can do more than
individual physicians can do in their efforts to help someone
stop smoking.
439
locales, simpler modes for the solo practitioner, more
complex modes for the specialty clinic, where a particular
kind of case would form a substantial proportion of therapists
time. An example of a simpler therapeutic mode is the use of
self-help manuals with brief therapist support. More
sophisticated self-help treatments are also available in
computerized form, and would be easily accessible via the
internet.
References
Agras, W. S. (1982). Behavioral medicine in the 1980’s:
Nonrandom connections. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 50, 797-803.
440
Agras, W. S., & Berkowitz, R. I. (1980). Clinical research in
behavior therapy: Halfway there? Behavior Therapy, 11,
472-487.
441
Agras, W. S., & Werne, J. (1997). Behavior modification in
anorexia nervosa: Research foundation. In R. Vigersky (Ed.),
Anorexia nervosa. A monograph of the National Institute of
Child Health and Development (pp. 181-195). New York,
NY: Raven Press.
442
therapy, and cognitive-behavior therapy. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 50, 419-428.
443
Leitenberg, H., Agras, W. S., & Thompson, L. (1968). A
sequential analysis of the effect of selective positive
reinforcement in modifying anorexia nervosa. Behavior
Research and Therapy, 6, 211-218.
444
Schwartz, G. E., & Weiss, S. M. (1977). Proceedings of the
Yale conference on behavioral medicine (NIH Publication
No. 78-1424). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health,
Education, & Welfare.
445
Chapter 13
Toward a Cumulative
Science of Persons: Past,
Present, and Prospects
Walter Mischel
Columbia University
446
more closely at the data of the field and at my own research
findings.
Early Hopes
This was a time when, beginning in the mid-1950’s, the field
of clinical psychology and personality had a giant growth
spurt, both as a profession and as a science. It was stimulated
on the one hand by demands for clinical psychologists in new
health care facilities for millions of World War II veterans,
and on the other by the opening of the Sputnik-launched
space race, which fueled federal support for science in the
United States. In this context, a new generation of
clinically-schooled psychologists was trained to dedicate
themselves to two-pronged careers as “scientist/practitioners.”
Most of us tried to straddle both roles; scientists devoted to
invalidating our favorite hypotheses in research, and
clinicians eager to apply the findings of what promised to
become the new psychological science to improve the human
condition.
447
human dispositions. Shared both by trait and psychodynamic
approaches, these traditional assumptions about personality
arose from a self-evidently true observation. On practically
any dimension of human behavior, there are substantial,
distinctive differences in the response of different persons
within the same social situation: Obviously, within the same
objective stimulus situation, there often are also large
differences between individuals. Second it is assumed that
individuals are characterized by stable and broadly
generalized dispositions that endure over long periods of time
and that generate consistencies in their social behavior across
a wide range of situations. With this belief, assessors tried to
predict behavior in many domains and contexts from a variety
of personality indicators or “signs” from which they inferred
these dispositions.
448
(which also were highly intercorrelated), and these were
aggregated into a multiple scale criterion. To illustrate the
major findings, simple self-reports and self-ratings, as on
anxiety-relevant items and attitudes to authority, yielded
modest but often statistically significant correlations with
outcome criteria, accounting for small but significant amounts
of variance. On the other hand, more global and indirect
measures failed even to reach significance. For example,
behavior in the interviews, one of the favorite global methods,
predicted total criterion performance in Nigeria with a
correlation of .13, accounting for a trivial percentage of the
variance.
449
1968 Challenges to Classic
Dispositional Assumptions
Findings like these were startling at a time when even small
samples of behavior had seemed to promise a diagnostic
X-ray to illuminate the core of personality — to allow rapid
inferences from a few subtle signs observed by experts to
broad generalizations about what the individual was like “on
the whole,” and then from these inferences about generalized
global dispositions to predict specific outcomes. Were such
predictive failures anomalies?
450
highly resistant to change. Self-concepts, and the impressions
of other people including clinical judgments — the theories
that we have about ourselves and each other — these
phenomena and many more of the same type were found to
have consistency and even tenacious continuity (Mischel,
1968). Indeed, our constructions about other people are often
built quickly and on the basis of little information (e.g.,
Bruner, Olver, & Greenfield, 1966), and soon become
difficult to disconfirm. An impressive degree of continuity
and consistency also was found for another aspect of
cognition: namely, cognitive or information-processing styles.
These styles are often closely related to measures of
intelligence and cognitive competence and, like “intelligence”
itself, they tend to have higher consistency relative to more
social dimensions of behavior (e.g., “conscientiousness,”
“honesty,” “friendliness”).
451
more dissimilar the evoking situations, the less likely they are
to produce similar or consistent responses from the same
individual. Even seemingly trivial situational differences may
reduce correlations to zero. Response consistency tends to be
greatest within the same response medium, within self-reports
to paper-and-pencil tests, for example, or within directly
observed nonverbal behavior. Intraindividual consistency is
reduced drastically when dissimilar response modes are
employed. Activities that are substantially associated with
aspects of intelligence and with problem solving
behavior—like achievement behaviors, cognitive styles,
response speed—tend to be most consistent (Mischel, 1968,
p. 177).
452
The Psychodynamic-Clinical
Alternative
At the same time that global traits were challenged in their
core assumptions, I — along with many others (e.g., Peterson,
1968) — also questioned, but on different grounds, the
dispositional paradigm of psychodynamic approaches. In
contrast to the neglect of situational variables for which
classic trait approaches were being criticized, psychodynamic
approaches to personality dispositions had long recognized
both the specificity and complexity of behavior, rejecting the
idea of broad, overt behavioral consistencies across situations
at the “surface” level. They believed that the observed
inconsistencies in the individual’s overt behavior could be
understood as merely superficial diversities that masked the
fundamentally consistent, underlying dispositions and
dynamics (Mischel, 1971a, p. 153). Thus, psychodynamic
theories at an abstract level could readily deal with the facts
of inconsistencies in the person’s behavior. But they were
subject to another problem. The embarrassment for them was
in the failure to provide compelling empirical evidence that
the inferences they generated about the underlying or
genotypic dispositions were useful either for the prediction of
behavior or for its therapeutic modification, especially when
compared to simpler, less inferential, and less costly
alternatives.
453
came to doubt the value both of the psychodynamic and of the
trait-dispositional portraits to which they were devoting most
of their effort (e.g., Peterson, 1968; Vernon, 1964).
Skepticism about the utility of such global assessments arose
not from any lack of interest in the client’s dispositions nor
from a neglect of individual differences. Instead, it arose from
a growing anxiety that psychodynamic and trait “personality
diagnostics,” too often generated without close attention to
the clients’ own views of their lives and specific behaviors,
might be exercises in stereotyping that missed the uniqueness
of individuals and pinned the persons instead on a continuum
of clinician-supplied labels, as George Kelly (1955) had
charged years earlier.
454
of a patient’s file folder and the incidence of rehospitalization.
As results like these illustrate, even a simple measure of a
person’s past can sometimes powerfully predict relevant
aspects of the future, in sharp contrast to more complex,
indirect, costly efforts. It should be equally evident that the
simple fact that one cannot predict well from some previously
favored measures and strategies denies neither the importance
of individual differences nor the potential value of all sorts of
assessments for all sorts of purposes (e.g., Mischel, 1983). It
does not mean that individual differences are necessarily
unpredictable, but it does indicate that the nature and locus of
that predictability may be quite different from what had been
assumed.
455
been exceeded by more psychometrically sophisticated
personality tests, by combining tests into batteries, by
assigning differential weights to them, or by employing more
complex statistical analyses involving multiple-regression
equations. The conclusions for personality measures apply, on
the whole, to diverse content areas including the prediction of
college achievement, job and professional success, treatment
outcomes, rehospitalization for psychiatric patients, parole
violations for delinquent children, and so on. In light of these
findings it is not surprising that large-scale applied efforts to
predict behavior from personality inferences have been
strikingly and consistently unsuccessful. . . . (Mischel, 1968,
p. 145-146).
456
The critiques of traditional dispositional approaches that
emerged at this juncture were first read by many as
“situationist” attacks on personality itself and as unjustified
denials of the importance of individual differences. Reactions
of this type were understandable, given a long tradition of
dichotomizing the person and the situation and contrasting the
relative importance of the two sources of variance, rather than
clarifying how they interact psychologically. But while the
challenge to traditional dispositional paradigms called
attention to the significance of situations or contexts in the
study of persons, my basic message was not a negation either
of personality as a field or of individual differences as a
phenomenon. On the contrary, the focus was on the
idiographic nature of each person interacting with the specific
contexts of his or her life and on the need to revise some
favorite assumptions of traditional personality theories to take
those unique interactions into account seriously. Far from
denying individual differences in personality, the criticisms
were largely motivated to defend individuality and the
uniqueness of each person against the tendency, prevalent in
1960’s clinical and diagnostic efforts, to use a few ratings or
few behavioral signs to categorize people into categories on
an assessor’s favorite nomothetic trait dimensions. It was
common practice to assume in the 1960’s that such
assessments were useful to predict not just “average” levels of
individual differences, but a person’s specific behavior on
specific criteria as well as “in general.” It was not uncommon
to undertake decision making about a person’s life and future
on the basis of a relatively limited sampling of personological
“signs” or “trait indicators.” It was this type of practice that I
challenged:
457
Global traits and states are excessively crude, gross units to
encompass adequately the extraordinary complexity and
subtlety of the discriminations that people constantly make.
Traditional trait-state conceptions of man have depicted him
as victimized by his infantile history, as possessed by
unchanging rigid trait attributes, and as driven inexorably by
unconscious irrational forces. This conceptualization of man,
besides being philosophically unappetizing, is contradicted by
massive experimental data. The traditional trait-state
conceptualizations of personality, while often paying lip
service to man’s complexity and to the uniqueness of each
person, in fact lead to a grossly oversimplified view that
misses both the richness and the uniqueness of individual
lives (Mischel, 1968, p. 301)
458
Aftermath of the Challenge: Toward a
Theoretical Integration
These challenges fueled a period of prolonged and heated
controversy about personality dispositions and the construct
of personality itself that dominated thinking in the area
throughout the 1970’s and early 1980’s (e.g., reviewed in
Magnusson & Endler, 1977). The debate was multifaceted —
engaging many segments of the field, spilling into adjacent
areas, and spanning from one extreme that exaggerated the
dilemma to another that trivialized it. The claims ranged from
contentions that personality was a largely fictitious
construction in the mind of the perceiver (e.g., Shweder,
1975), to counterarguments intended to prove that global
dispositions as traditionally conceptualized were “alive and
well” if one simply employed a more reliable measurement
strategy to find them (e.g., Epstein, 1979). In the same period,
social psychologists amassed evidence for the power of
situational variables, and proposed that humans have a
persistent tendency to invoke dispositions as favorite (albeit
erroneous) explanations of social behavior (e.g., Nisbett &
Ross, 1980; Ross & Nisbett,1991). In that sense, Skinner’s
focus on the importance of the situation and of stimulus
control in the regulation of social behavior, although never
acknowledged within social psychology, was at last echoed
within it.
459
confusion. The continuing challenge, however, still awaited
answers: how to reconceptualize dispositions to take such
interactions into account incisively, a priori in the form of
specific predictions, and not just in post hoc attempts to deal
with unpredicted and perhaps basically unpredictable
higher-order interactions after they are found in the data. My
efforts to address this challenge and to provide a theoretical
framework for understanding and predicting individual
differences in the interactions of persons and situations have
been spelled out in detail over the years elsewhere (e.g.,
Mischel, 1973, Mischel & Shoda, 1995, 1998).
460
The cognitive-social learning approach to personality
(Mischel, 1973) shifted the unit of study from global traits
inferred from behavioral signs to the person’s cognitions,
affect, and action assessed in relation to the particular
psychological conditions in which they occur. The focus thus
changed from describing situation-free people with broad trait
adjectives to analyzing the interactions between conditions
and the cognitions and behaviors of interest. In the 1960’s
much personality research on social behavior was undertaken
to study the processes of cognition and social learning
through which potential behaviors are acquired, evoked,
maintained, and modified (e.g., as reviewed in Bandura,
1969; Mischel, 1968). Less attention had been devoted to the
psychological products within the person of these processes in
the course of development. The cognitive-social learning
reconceptualization of personality was intended to identify a
set of interrelated person variables that capture these
“products” of the individual’s psychological history and that
in turn mediate the manner in which new situations are
interpreted.
461
psychological processes relevant to understanding the
operations of that variable and its psychological meaning.
Thus, each variable has both a structural and a functional
aspect in an emerging theory of personality. More recent
versions of this approach address the specific nature of the
organization of the person variables, their interactions within
the mediating system (the Cognitive Affective Personality
System, or CAPS), and the stimulus features in the social
environment that activate them. These interactions have been
analyzed and modeled in detail, including as computer
simulations (Mischel & Shoda, 1995; Shoda & Mischel,
1998).
A Conditional Approach to
Dispositions
Historically, the failure to find strong support for
cross-situational consistency at the behavioral level, given the
widespread assumption that personality consists of traits,
expressed across many different situations as generalized,
global behavior tendencies, was read originally as a basic
threat to the construct of personality itself. As noted above,
the effect was an unfortunate and prolonged “person versus
situation” debate, and a paradigm crisis in the area.
462
personality (e.g., as discussed in Mischel & Shoda, 1995,
1998; Pervin, 1994). The most widely accepted strategy used
by the classic dispositional or trait approach currently
acknowledges the low cross-situational consistency in
behavior found from situation to situation: It then
systematically removes the situation by aggregating the
individual’s behavior on a given dimension (e.g.,
“conscientiousness”) over many different situations (e.g.,
home, school, work) to estimate an overall “true score,”
treating the variability across situations as “error.”
463
Evidence for the conditional (contextualized) expression of
dispositions. Many of the results of my research program on
the structure of consistency in social behavior were based on
behavior observed intensively and extensively at a
well-controlled field laboratory site. The long-term, intensive
observational field laboratory developed in this research
program was located within a 6-week summer residential
camp setting and treatment program for troubled children,
called Wediko (e.g., Mischel, 1990; Shoda, et al., 1993a,
1993b, 1994). This setting provided an exceptional
opportunity to examine behavior in vivo as it unfolds across
situations and over time under unusually well-controlled
research conditions that assured the reliability and density of
measurement.
464
the underlying social cognitive and affective processes and
system that generate it (e.g., the individual’s construals, goals,
and motivations).
465
The key for achieving generalizability, therefore, is to identify
psychological features of situations that (1) play a functional
role in the generation of behaviors, and (2) are contained in a
wide range of nominal situations.
466
distinctive and stable if/then contingencies for the individual
are defined in relation to the basic psychological ingredients
or features that occur in many different nominal situations,
then it may be possible to understand and predict behaviors in
a novel situation given that its psychological ingredients are
known. Moreover, to the degree that particular sets of such
active ingredients or psychological features for an individual
(or for a personality type) are imbedded in diverse nominal
situations (e.g., at woodworking in camp, on playground at
school, at mealtime at home), it may become possible to
predict behavior across those seemingly different situations
and contexts, allowing much broader predictability even for
quite specific behavioral manifestations (Mischel & Shoda,
1995, 1998; Shoda, et al., 1994).
467
To test this proposition, my studies of the consistency
paradox (Mischel & Peake, 1982, 1983) were conducted with
a sample of Carleton College undergraduates who
volunteered to participate in extensive self-assessments
relevant to their conscientiousness and friendliness. Directly
contradicting Bem’s prediction, we found that the students’
perceptions of their own overall consistency or variability on
conscientiousness were not related closely to the observed
cross-situational consistency of their actual behavior directly
observed as it occurred in vivo across diverse daily situations.
Although inter-judge agreement was greater for those
students who saw themselves overall as consistent in
conscientiousness, their average behavioral consistency
across the measures was not significantly greater than that of
students who saw themselves as variable. In short, in this
study, people who saw themselves as consistent on a
dimension were seen with greater interjudge agreement by
others, but their overall behavior was not necessarily more
consistent cross-situationally.
468
We hypothesized that the impression that a person is
consistent with regard to a trait is not based mostly on the
observation of average cross-situational consistency in all the
potentially relevant behaviors (e.g., punctuality for classes,
punctuality for appointments, desk neatness, etc.). Instead, we
proposed that when people try to assess their variability (vs.
consistency) with regard to a category of behavior, they scan
the temporal stability of a limited number of behaviors that
for them are most relevant (prototypic) to that category. That
is, it was hypothesized that the impression of consistency is
based extensively on the observation of temporal stability in
those behaviors that are most relevant to the prototype. No
relationship was expected between the impression of high
consistency versus variability and overall cross-situational
consistency. The results supported these expectations
(Mischel & Peake, 1982). Those students who saw
themselves as highly consistent in conscientiousness were
significantly more temporally stable on these prototypic
behaviors than were those who viewed themselves as more
variable from situation to situation — an effect that was
replicated in the domain of friendliness by Peake within the
same sample of students (Peake, 1982). In contrast to the
clear and consistent differences in temporal stability for the
prototypic behaviors, the self-perceived low and high
variability groups did not differ in mean temporal stability for
the less prototypic behaviors. Finally, also as expected,
self-perceived consistency and behavioral cross-situational
consistency were unrelated in Mischel and Peake’s (1982)
study.
469
cross-situational consistencies in their behaviors on the
whole. Intuitions about one’s consistency seem to arise,
instead, from the observation of temporal stability in
prototypical behaviors. This would certainly not be a
fictitious construction of consistency. The “error” simply
would be to confuse the temporal stability of key behaviors
with pervasive cross-situational consistency, and then to
overestimate the latter.
Condition-Behavior Stabilities: In
Search of Local Predictability
The classic trait strategy essentially treats situations as if they
were error, and seeks to cancel their effects by aggregating
across them to eliminate their role and to demonstrate stable
individual differences. The approach my associates and I
favor, in contrast to the classic route, seeks consistencies by
linking the behavior of interest to a circumscribed set of
contexts, thus pursuing consistency on a more local,
condition-bound, contingent, and specific (rather than global)
level. Both approaches seem to accept the fact that average
levels of consistency in behavior from situation to situation
tend to be modest, even after aggregation over multiple
occasions. Advocates of the traditional trait strategy propose
circumventing this constraint by abandoning attempts to
predict behavior from situation to situation altogether.
Instead, they confine their predictive efforts to aggregates
over multiple situations (e.g. Epstein, 1979). Such a strategy
can enhance the resulting coefficients dramatically (as the
Spearman-Brown formula has long recognized). But it
bypasses rather than resolves the classic problems found in
470
the search for coherences from situation to situation by
“averaging out” the situation rather than predicting behavior
in it. And, of course, it places a low ceiling on the accuracy
possible for predicting behavior in specific situations.
471
dispositionally relevant behaviors aggregated across a wide
range of situations. It calls explicit attention to inextricable
specific links between conditions and actions in determining
the implications of people’s behavior for dispositional
judgments about them (e.g., Mischel & Shoda, 1995, 1998,
1999; Shoda, Mischel, & Wright, 1989). It equally highlights
the interactive nature of the person-situation relations that
characterize social behavior.
472
Beyond Stimulus Control: Why I
Became a Cognitive Social
Psychologist
The fact that the situation plays an enormously powerful role
in the often automatic activation and regulation of complex
human social behavior was of course central to my critique of
traditional approaches to clinical and personality psychology.
I saw the power of the situation convincingly in my own work
on the willingness and ability of young children to delay
gratification by continuing to wait for two little treats later as
opposed to settling for one right now. We found that such a
seemingly trivial change in the situation as whether the
rewards remain exposed on the plate facing the preschool
child or are placed under it can change the average delay time
from less than a minute to more than ten (e.g., Mischel, Shoda
& Rodriguez, 1989). Thus whether or not the young child
finds delay of gratification excruciatingly difficult or easily
achievable hinges on the subtleties of the situation, and when
these IFs are properly understood and introduced the THENs
that follow can become highly predictable.
473
facing the subject, it was its mental representation, as primed
by suggestions on how to think about the rewards, that
controlled the delay of gratification behavior, regardless of
the actual rewards present in the situation. Namely, when
these representations focused on the “hot” consummatory
features of the stimulus (e.g., “while you’re waiting you can
think about the pretzel’s salty, crunchy taste”) the frustration
of continued delay of gratification became unbearable for
most children even when the external stimulus facing the
subject was completely controlled. Conversely, when the
mental representation focused on the “cool,” informative cue
properties (e.g., “you can think about the pretzels as if they
were little sticks”) , sustained, goal-directed delay of
gratification and “willpower” became manageable, again
regardless of the external stimulus in the situation (Mischel,
1974, 1996; Mischel, Shoda & Rodriguez, 1989). Thus the
way the child represented the stimuli cognitively during the
delay period profoundly transformed their impact: the power
thus resides in the head, not in the external stimulus.
474
long-term goals (e.g. Mischel, Shoda, & Peake, 1988; Shoda,
Mischel, & Peake, 1990). For example, in laboratory
situations in which individual differences in such strategies
were activated, those 4-year-old children who delayed longer
became more socially and cognitively competent young
adults, also achieving higher levels of scholastic performance,
as reflected in their SAT verbal and quantitative scores (e.g.,
Shoda, Mischel, & Peake, 1990). In short, behavior in the
delay situation was a function not only of the characteristics
of the situation but also of the individuals in it. The fact that
these distinctively human efforts also require the strategic
utilization and support of situations undermines neither the
role of the person nor of the situation, but requires attention to
their reciprocal interaction. These interactions have been
conceptualized more recently in a theoretical two-system
“hot/cool” framework that takes account both of the
automatic, stimulus-response aspects of functioning and of
the more reflective, cognitive mediating system, focusing on
the specifics of their interplay (Metcalfe & Mischel, 1999).
475
by distracting themselves and focusing on other aspects of the
situation while continuing in their goal-directed behavior.
476
References
Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological
interpretation. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
477
Hartshorne, H., & May, A. (1928). Studies in the nature of
character: Studies in deceit (Vol. 1). New York: MacMillan.
478
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Peake, P. K. (1988). The nature of
adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of
gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
54, 687-699.
479
Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1982). Beyond Déjà vu in the
search for cross-situational consistency. Psychological
Review, 89, 730-755.
480
Newcomb, T. N. (1929). Consistency of certain
extrovert-introvert behavior patterns in 51 problem boys.
New York: Columbia University, Teachers College, Bureau
of Publications.
481
Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., & Wright, J. C. (1993). The role of
situational demands and cognitive competencies in behavior
organization and personality coherence. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1023-1035.
482
Wright, J. C., & Mischel, W. (1988). Conditional hedges and
the intuitive psychology of traits. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 55, 454-469.
483
Chapter 14
University of Kansas
Intellectual Biography
My parents were immigrants who had to work rather than
finish high-school educations. Appraising their new country,
they saw two ways up for their children: business and
education. They sent me to school with very clear
instructions: It was the most important part of my life; I
should learn everything that was taught; and I should cause no
trouble. I was a compliant child; it would be a long time
before I disobeyed any of those instructions.
484
could prove about how some part of the world worked. I
found that something called “understanding” or “explanation”
was already one of my most potent reinforcers, and I was
taught implicitly that it always came wrapped in experimental
proof.
485
major terms were verbal inventions; by definition, they were
immune to direct proof or disproof. That put proof in its
properly small place, which was to confirm or disconfirm any
observable deductions from the inventions. Confirmations
meant the inventions were good; disconfirmations meant the
inventions were even better, because disconfirmation was
much too harsh a term for this event. It really was a rich
encounter, one that allowed the theorist to “discover” certain
details in the inventions not appreciated before. Given an
apparent disconfirmation, theorists did not drop their
disconfirmed theory and build a better one; instead, they
“discovered” that there was more to the original theory than
they had appreciated before. Explaining away
disconfirmations let them claim an even greater
“understanding” than before.
486
An epiphany led me to behaviorism. I was studying
mathematics and physics in graduate school, and not liking
them. One day a friend in psychology asked me to explain an
equation in one of his textbooks. The equation was Weber’s
Law. I was astounded that psychology could use differential
calculus. My undergraduate brush with psychology had
offered it as psychodynamic, and the media I had consumed
equated it to the rather bizarre forms of insight that allowed a
happy ending.
487
apparently valued proof more than any other approach, so I
joined.
488
conceptual, effective, and capable of producing appropriately
generalized behavior changes.
Applied
489
meaning into two kinds of reasons for changing behavior: one
was to find out how it could be changed; the other was that
someone did not like the behavior the way it was, and did like
some other way it could be. The first connoted basic science;
the second connoted application, depending on the social
importance of that someone.
Behavioral
490
Analytic and Conceptual
Technological
491
complete descriptions of the parameters necessary and
sufficient for them to work reliably. That requires a lot of
proof. Whatever else the development of a technology is, it is
the result of a great deal of proof. Applied behavior analysis
tried to develop and accumulate a behavioral technology, so it
had to prove what was necessary to yield a recipe that should
always work, and then prove the extent to which it did.
Effective
By 1987, effectiveness also meant that the goal was worth the
cost of attaining it the way we had attained it. Cost meant the
costs of the procedures that accomplished the goal, and any
costs of everything else those procedures caused, if anything.
Both were problems in proof, because it was safer to prove
what were and what were not the systematic consequences of
the procedures, than to infer that things must be better now
and that nothing could have gone wrong.
492
But 20 years had shown a use for clients’ statements that
things were or were not better now. Sometimes behavior was
changed in ways the clients or other audiences did not like.
That could result in countercontrol, which severely
diminished anything that could be called effectiveness, and
was bad for our reputation. We began asking all relevant
audiences if they liked the procedures, their outcomes, and
the personnel; we meant to assess something called the social
validity (Kazdin, 1977; Wolf, 1978) of our programs. We did
that not just for the pleasure of positive answers, but also
because negative answers might predict countercontrol. The
old problem of proof arose even more severely: If we allowed
unhappy audiences to lie to us about liking our procedures,
we would fail to predict the subsequent countercontrol, and
effectiveness and reputation would be lost. Experience
suggests that audiences easily lie about what has just
happened to them. Suddenly we were in the ironic position of
needing truthful talk about behavior from the very people we
earlier had not trusted to talk truthfully about behavior.
493
1968, it was sufficient to prove how we accomplished
appropriate generalizations; in 1987, it was imperative to
prove as well that our choices of “appropriate” were in fact
appropriate. It also seemed reasonable to acknowledge that
“generalization” was a pragmatic term, and “stimulus control”
was its analytic obverse.
Overview
494
Important Developments in the Rise of
the Behavior Therapies
For those of us who saw applied behavior analysis primarily
as the acid test of the power and generality of behavior
analysis, the decades after 1968 were very informative.
Behavior analysis passed instance after instance of this test,
and continues to pass more instances of it today. Most of us
saw that some problems of people and society arose from the
behavior of those people and their society. The final
significance of applied behavior analysis might well be that it
would show how to solve some of those problems.
495
parts. They did not prove anything about their inferred
internal behaviors, because they could not; they merely
inferred that these internal behaviors mediated their
observable success, and thereby explained it.
496
show when those procedures would be effective and when
they would not. When the cognitive-behavior therapies gave
primacy to cognitions instead, they allowed, and perhaps
encouraged, a neglect of the crucial details of environmental
control.
497
environments. Another booklet is needed to describe how to
change those schedules to make and keep the time-out
contingency effective. Another booklet is needed to describe
how to record everything relevant and graph it, so as to reveal
whether progress is being made, and how much; and if no
progress is being made, to explain how to best choose the
next procedures. Another booklet is needed to explain
convincingly to the potential user of time out that the user’s
behavior of using it, even ineffectively, is subject to quite
powerful reinforcement, especially when time out frees the
user for a while from living with the behavior of the
timed-out person. Another booklet is needed to explain that
no behavior problem is solved merely by eliminating an
undesirable behavior: The behaver needs desirable ways of
gaining the reinforcers and avoiding the punishers that the
undesirable behavior gained and avoided. Those desirable
ways may not already exist in the behaver’s repertoire. Then
they will have to be taught. Many booklets are needed to
describe the relevant teaching processes well enough to get
them done effectively.
498
the parents’ report of how well it worked. Like all behaviors,
parent report is subject to control by many contingencies.
Sometimes one of those contingencies is to reassure the
therapist that the problem has been solved, not because it has,
but because the parents want a graceful way to leave this
therapist and find a different one. When you give up proof,
you give up knowing.
499
organism (even as they agree that its now far-distant early
origins probably were in the external environment).
500
processing of the solution and its problem. In this chain, there
is very little to be gained by targeting the cognition; it is not
the cause of the solution, but the result of the solution. (3) A
third chain is that a problem is presented, and it
simultaneously evokes a solution and cognitive processing of
the problem. In this chain, the cognitive processing and the
solution have no relation to each other; each is an independent
response to the problem. Then targeting the cognition will
again have little value in promoting a solution.
501
the question is, in the problems with which we all work, how
prevalent is each of these three causal chains?
502
the procedures our science said should have been the effective
agents? Perhaps this question has been neglected by applied
behavior analysis so long because we quietly suspect that we
are very bad theater. We put causation in the environment,
when almost every client knows it surely must well up from
within. Thereby, we are bad theater. In that context, what
cognitive-behavior therapies add to applied behavior analysis
looks, at least, exactly like much better theater: Cognitions
are processes welling up from within! There is a therapy
worth the ticket price.
503
the behavior-change procedures of the package, and ask if the
cognitive techniques add anything to them. I argue here that
the procedures of applied behavior analysis are well known to
me as behavior-change procedures, whereas the procedures of
cognitive reorganization seem to me to be much less
dependable, as I look back on my own education. Thus I will
for myself phrase the null hypothesis as if applied behavior
analysis had the priority: my null hypothesis is that the
addition of cognitive procedures to behavior-analytic
procedures does not alter the effectiveness of the
behavior-analytic procedures. Anyone else may recast the null
hypothesis to give priority to the cognitive procedures: the
alternative null hypothesis is that the addition of behavioral
procedures adds nothing to the effectiveness of cognitive
procedures. Either way, the problem for proof is much the
same; we shall either compare cognitive-only to
cognitive-plus-behavioral, or compare behavioral-only to
behavioral-plus-cognitive. (If we are well funded, which we
never are, we can compare cognitive-only, behavioral-only,
and cognitive-plus-behavioral.)
504
requirements for the comparison of any Therapy A to any
Therapy B. Here is a summary of what they have shown the
problem requires:
505
of these reactions across the two groups could bias
the outcome.
• Assessment of what therapies, if any, each case has
had previously, and with what effectiveness, so as to
analyze the possibility that Therapy A or Therapy B
does best if preceded by some Therapy X or worst if
preceded by some Therapy Y, and so as to analyze
the degree to which any unequal distribution of these
histories across the two groups could bias the
outcome.
• Assessment of the correlates of attrition of cases from
each group, so as to analyze the degree to which an
unequal distribution of attrition, or of reasons for
attrition, across the two groups could bias the
outcome.
This list of the requirements for an accurate comparison of
some Therapy A to some Therapy B is not complete; it is only
a good beginning.
506
We might better simply note that each discipline has found an
evolutionary niche in which it prospers and reproduces, a
little, and that each probably will continue to do so for many
years to come. What we cannot afford to prove about our
variety can always be observed, not to see what is true, but to
see what survives. And if survival (rather than correctness) is
of interest, then we might well remember an old rule of
evolution: A population with some diversity has a better
chance of surviving some sudden change in the survival
contingencies than a population with little diversity.
507
References
Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some
current dimensions of applied behavior analysis. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 91-97.
508
Chapter 15
University of Alaska
509
• with experimental designs that go with observation
and recording over time.
But their most important “breakthrough” contribution was the
demonstration that naively simple immediate things were
actually powerfully important in the real lives of people. You
see, at the time we were all talking about the principles of
learning and behavior but we thought they would be actually
expressed only in complex, multiply-interactive combinations
in the ongoing actions of people in real life. (In this respect,
we were all “Hullians.”) We assumed that their role could
only be isolated and analyzed after carefully designed
histories in specially arranged settings — in other words, in
laboratories. And laboratory work, both ‘neohullian’ and
‘human operant,’ was flourishing in the early 1960’s. It was
certainly flourishing at the University of Washington. (My
master’s thesis research [Risley, 1964] was an example.)
(At this point, we will leave Ayllon to his own affairs, with
the reminder that he was recruited by Nathan Azrin, an
established leader in experimental psychology at the time and
the most prolific contributor of good research on many topics
to the experimental analysis of behavior. At Anna State
Hospital Azrin and Ayllon started [Ayllon & Azrin, 1964],
and Azrin developed, arguably the most productive and
important program of problem-solving field research that has
yet existed [cf. Azrin, 1977]).
510
laboratory at the Institute of Child Development, (ICD) and
had established a research lab at Rainier State School. He had
recruited Donald Baer and Jay Birnbrauer as new assistant
professors in developmental psychology and filled the
post-doc position of child clinic director with successively,
Ivar Lovaas, Ralph Wetzel, and Robert Wahler. On his
research and training grants, Bijou had recruited and
supported a cadre of graduate students including me. (I had
finished my undergraduate work at San Diego State, where
Virginia Voeks, who had studied with both Hull and Guthrie,
taught the learning and child psychology courses. She sent me
to the University of Washington in 1960 where I was offered
an assistantship at ICD.)
511
were being documented only with field notes (excepting the
hospital studies of Ayllon and associates, 1959, 1962, 1963).
512
demonstrated its power with a reversal design. Thirty-five
years later, and now recommended by the American
Academy of Pediatrics (1998), half the parents and teachers in
the United States use this nonviolent practice and call it
“time-out,” which makes it a social invention unmatched in
modern psychology.
That work and related work over the next year (Risley &
Wolf, 1964, 1967; Wolf, Risley, Johnston, Harris, & Allen,
1967) are also noteworthy in that they introduced the direct
reinforcement of verbal imitation and the shaping of
meaningful speech. It is amusing to recollect the exclamations
of amazement from professionals — even behavioral
professionals working with children with autism — at the
sight of Wolf or me matter-of-factly using bites of food to
shape functional speech with children with autism and mental
retardation. (Ferster gave us credit for our procedures, but did
not use them. Lovaas used them.)
513
for the child to initiate, before prompting a more elaborate
request — the germ of the procedure Betty Hart and I later
elaborated into “incidental teaching.”
514
The Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis
Now, let us skip ahead a few years to 1966, after Baer and
Richard Schiefelbusch had recruited some of us together
again to the University of Kansas. The combination of field
research methods and problem-solving strategies that Ayllon
and Wolf modeled for us had now evolved in sophistication
and example and had proliferated across the country (except
peculiarly in those places where operant laboratory research
was strongest). By this time, books of readings (such as
Ullman & Krasner, 1965, or Ulrich, Stachnik, & Mabry,
1966) were not enough to handle the studies being generated.
We needed a journal. Wolf campaigned for a journal and Sage
and Academic Press each responded favorably. But by then
we had convinced the Society for the Experimental Analysis
of Behavior to sponsor an applied companion to the Journal
of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior (JEAB). With the
wise guidance of Azrin, the Society selected Montrose Wolf
as the first editor, and he designed and named the Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis (JABA).
515
methodologies. Underlying all the suggestions in that article
were the dimensions that most concerned Wolf and me:
516
human affairs were highlighted in their introduction and
discussion sections but absent in their procedures and results
sections.
517
My Version of Applied Behavior
Analysis
Within Applied Behavior Analysis there was (and is) a
diversity of emphases from religious to philosophical to
empirical: for some, B. F. Skinner was the final word and
their task was to defend and interpret his writings; for others,
Radical Behaviorism was a philosophical worldview for
logical analysis, explanation and debate; for many of us,
Methodological Behaviorism was an attempt to import the
simplest precepts of natural history and experimental analysis
into the slow, incremental process of observing, accounting
for, and improving human affairs. If allowed to oversimplify
it might be said that Bijou and Baer were mostly interested in
explaining the world, and Wolf was mostly interested in
fixing the world. I think I was mostly interested in exploring
the world.
518
Murray Sidman’s Tactics of Scientific Research (1960) gave
me an experimental, inductive model of science that assumed
that one should strive to discover things and invent
technology in the largely uncharted world of behavior. And
Donald Campbell (1957, 1963, 1969) gave me both the
mission and the methodology to pursue reforms as
experiments.
519
operations and contingencies of social and material
reinforcers across all participants, and provide predictable
curricula of models, prompts, instruction and reinforcement
criteria for each participant. The organizations in which a
person participates provide the infrastructure of predictable
schedules, discriminative stimuli, contingencies and
reinforcers of most of her or his daily life. People suffer when
their family, social, community and work organizations are
disrupted or incompetent.
520
The following is an account of the human organizations and
settings where I have taken responsibility for people in some
significant aspects of their lives — with the intent of making
their lives better and contributing knowledge and technology
for use for others. I have omitted several projects that are too
complicated or sensitive to describe, and many more where
aspects of peoples’ lives were not at stake (see endnote 1 for
some of these), or where someone else was primarily
responsible (endnote 2 names most of those people). In each
of the following projects I was personally at social, political
and professional risk for the conduct of the project, and
ethically responsible to ensure benefits to the people being
served. Although there were several other participants in each
project (partially reflected in the authorship of resultant
publications), I have named only those persons who shared
the risks and responsibilities of starting a project. The term
‘created’ indicates a service where none previously existed;
and ‘begun’ indicates the conversion of an existing service. In
the references, the publications are listed by project.
521
in 1965, it served 200-250 children and closed in 1981 when
our longitudinal study of children’s everyday language
experience (Hart & Risley, 1992, 1995, 1999) began. It was
funded by NICHD grants. Seventeen publications were
generated.
522
organized to decide on community rules and operate a
community security patrol. Created in 1970 with Edward
Christophersen, it served about 1500 residents living in 420
apartments and ended in 1974. It was funded by NIMH
grants. Three publications were generated.
523
institution, for a group of people who were profoundly
retarded and non-ambulatory. Begun with Jim Favell in 1971
with a 2-week working retreat by me with the team of
graduate students and post-docs from my Kansas projects, the
project continued to serve the 16 residents of the Roadrunner
‘cottage’ until 1982. With Judy Favell, portions of the
Toddler Day Care manual were also adapted and
institution-wide quality assurance systems were developed to
improve services to the other 300 institutionalized residents
during that time. It was funded by the State of North Carolina
and NICHD grants. Ten publications were generated.
524
any one time, and closed in 1986. It was funded by USOE and
Maternal and Child Health grants and parent fees. Eight
publications were generated.
525
The Nursing Home Operations Project where we assumed
responsibility for the daily care routines of a 100-bed private
nursing home and the meal service of a 100-bed county
nursing home; adapting the engagement measures and the
staff management and nutrition systems from our child day
care centers. Begun in 1975 with a one-week working retreat
by me and my students working as nursing aides, it served
about 300 fragile elderly people over four years. It was
funded by USPHS Health Services Research grants. Three
publications were generated.
526
NIMH, ADD, and VR grants and State of Alaska general
funds. Two publications were generated.
527
that crankstarted and ran somewhat dependably. With a stick,
she would fish out the washed clothes, garment by garment,
and feed each through the motor-driven wringer into a first
and then into a second tub of rinse water beside the washing
machine. After a final wring, she would clothespin each
garment on an outdoor clothesline and take it down when it
was dry or when it began to rain, whichever came first. In the
winter the clothes would freeze stiff as boards, but would
slowly dry anyway even if it snowed. (We used cloth diapers
then so I was probably toilet trained early.) My mother
weighed 90 pounds so her approach to the heavy labors of
homestead living was of small loads and many trips all day
long. (But mother also read for self-improvement every
evening by lamplight and talked to me of ‘bookish’ things.
With me in tow, she would stop by to see her “school marm”
friends whenever she arranged a ride into town for groceries,
or to the Sunday afternoon gathering of school teachers and
other educated people at the government doctor’s house to
listen to classical music on his victrola. And I would hear talk
of foreign things like wine or sidewalks and foreign lands like
France or California.)
528
that we had ‘walking water’ that was slowly carried by a boy
with his nose in a book and his head in the clouds. My fourth
grade teacher wrote to me last year: “What a dreamer you
were. When we had your attention you did excellent work.”)
529
References
Turner House Language Development Preschool
530
Hart, B. M., & Risley, T. R. (1978). Promoting productive
language through incidental teaching. Education and Urban
Society, 10, 407-429.
531
Risley, T. R. (1977). The social context of self-control. In R.
B. Stuart (Ed.), Behavioral self-management: strategies,
techniques and outcome. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
532
modification: The human effort. Palo Alto: Science and
Behavior Books.
533
Montes, F., & Risley, T. R. (1975). Evaluating traditional day
care practices: An empirical approach. Child Care Quarterly,
4, 208-215.
534
Lawrence Infant Day Care Center
535
Nursing Home Activities Project
Roadrunner Project
536
Evaluation of social programs in community, residential and
school settings. Champaign IL: Research Press.
537
Jones, M. L., Favell, J. E., Lattimore, J., & Risley, T. R.
(1984). Improving independent engagements of
nonambulatory multihandicapped persons through systematic
analysis of leisure materials. Analysis and Intervention in
Developmental Disabilities, 4, 313-332.
538
Favell, J. E., & Risley, T. R., et al. (1982). The treatment of
self-injurious behavior (AABT Task Force Report.) Behavior
Therapy, 13, 529-554.
539
Herbert-Jackson, E., & Risley, T. R. (1977). Behavioral
nutrition: consumption of foods of the future by toddlers.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 407-413.
540
Johnny Cake Child Study Center
541
Domash, M. A., Schnelle, J. F., Stromatt, E. L., Carr, A. F.,
Larson, D., Kirchner, R. R., & Risley, T. R. (1980). Police
and prosecution systems: An evaluation of a police criminal
case preparation program. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 13, 397-406.
542
Individualized Services for People with Mental Health or
Developmental Disabilities
543
Ayllon, T., & Azrin, N. H. (1964). Reinforcement and
instructions with mental patients. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 7, 327-331.
544
family-child interactions observed in natural home
environments. Developmental Psychology, 28(6), 1096-1105.
545
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New
York: Macmillan.
Wolf, M. M., Risley, T. R., Johnston, M., Harris, F., & Allen,
E. (1967). Applications of operant conditioning procedures to
the behavior problems of an autistic child: A follow-up and
extension. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 5, 103-111.
546
Chapter 16
Application of Operant
Conditioning Procedures to
the Behavior Problems of an
Autistic Child: A 25-Year
Follow-Up and the
Development of the Teaching
Family Model
Montrose M. Wolf
University of Kansas
The first thing that I would like to describe is how I first got
into the field of behavioral psychology. Jack Michael arrived
at the University of Houston in 1957. The first description of
Jack by a fellow student was, “He obviously can’t be a very
good teacher because he dresses so badly.” I had also heard
the student rumor that Jack had been asked to leave Kansas
University because he was teaching Skinner. Then Jack
arrived in his strangely colored short pants and flip-flop
sandals. And, to make matters worse, Jack announced that the
course would concentrate on animal research! I began looking
through my timetable for a course more in line with my
547
neo-Freudian interests. But, Learning Theory was a required
course for psychology majors. However, I did arm myself
with drop slips in case the course became too painful. I was
already thinking about changing my major to philosophy,
anyway.
548
institutions for other populations. Finally, others also became
members of the group which met in Jack’s living room and
planned how to save the world through behaviorism,
including John Mabry, Pat Corke, Leland Johnson, Jerry
Short, Lee Meyerson, Nancy Kerr, Lloyd and Polly Brooks
(both deceased), and Sam Toombs (deceased).
549
We also read a very interesting theoretical analysis by Charles
Ferster (1961). He discussed how the behavior problems of
childhood autism might be due to subtle positive
reinforcement, punishment, and extinction contingencies.
Ferster and DeMyer (1961) also published a very important
laboratory study about autistic children. They showed that
neutral stimuli could be conditioned in the laboratory.
550
Washington. Jerman Rose wanted us to work with a child
with the following characteristics.
551
Sid kept replying that we should wait and see what was up
before making a final decision.
And Sid was right, of course (Wolf, Risley, & Mees, 1964;
Wolf, Risley, Johnston, Harris, & Allen, 1967).
552
Todd has visited with Dicky in person and on the phone.
Since high school he has lived independently and has had a
series of jobs.
553
Behavior Analysis Award for Distinguished Service to
Behavior Analysis for 1998, and the American Psychological
Association, Division 25, Award For Outstanding Applied
Research — For innovative and important research on
applications of behavioral principles to address socially
significant human behavior, 1998.
554
Such feedback helps us to continue improving the quality of
the always evolving Teaching-Family model (p. 11-12).
References
Allen, K. E., Hart, B., Buell, J. S., Harris, F. R., & Wolf, M.
M. (1964). A study of the use of reinforcement principles in a
case of “isolate” behavior. Child Development, 35, 511-518.
555
Ferster, C. B., & DeMyer, M. K. (1961). The development of
performances in autistic children in an automatically
controlled environment. Journal of Chronic Diseases, 13,
312-345.
Hart, B. M., Allen, K. E., Buell, J. S., Harris, F. R., & Wolf,
M. M. (1964). Effects of social reinforcement on operant
crying. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1,
145-153.
556
Wolf, M. M., Kirigin, K. A., Fixsen, D. L., Blase, K. A., &
Braukmann, C. J. (1995). The Teaching-Family model: A
case study in data-based program development and
refinement (and dragon wrestling). Journal of Organizational
Behavior Management, 15 (11-68).
557
Chapter 17
University of Houston
558
of what I now do, but I do not call myself a behavior analyst.
The fact is that I abhor the categorical restrictions, frequent
cult-like trappings, and pigeonholing of any “school”
approach. This position has been personally costly in many
ways. For example, I belong to neither the Association for the
Advancement of Behavior Therapy (AABT) nor the
Association for Behavior Analysis (ABA)—and some of my
most respected friends have been key players in these
organizations. Beyond missing the camaraderie, my failure to
regularly network through such groups has resulted in fewer
people being familiar with my work and that of my coworkers
than otherwise would have been the case.
559
For more than 30 years my work has emphasized
institutionalized populations, observational assessment, and
treatment of psychoses (e.g., Mariotto, Paul, & Licht, 1995;
Paul, 1987b; Paul, 2000; Paul & Lentz, 1977, 2001). These
efforts have drawn more heavily on the work of people
identified with applied behavior analysis than did my earliest
work on anxiety-related problems.
Rather than focus on more recent work, I selected for the case
study the publication reporting an examination of insight
versus desensitization in psychotherapy (Paul, 1966). The
investigation was completed for my Ph.D. dissertation in
1964. This publication was chosen because of its
developmental importance for me as well as for the field.
Following the case study of the selected publication, I return
to more autobiography with developmental significance
before offering reflections and recommendations based on
these experiences.
Intellectual Autobiography
560
over abstract truth seeking as a goal. All emphasized
pragmatic over foundational-epistemological selection of
problems for study. All made heavy use of heuristic rather
than just epistemic appraisal of knowledge. All showed a
forward-looking prospective focus rather than a
backward-looking historical one. Science for this line of my
academic ancestry was clearly an enterprise that should make
a difference to societal problems and to the lives of real
people. It is for me as well.
561
single-subject behavior analysis, which would be considered
“rule-based inquiry.”) This portrayal may characterize the
majority of James’s own work. However, the remainder of the
group appears to have used both case-based and rule-based
inquiry, depending on whether the task involved the context
of discovery or the context of justification (Reichenbach,
1938). Within the context of discovery, ideas and hypotheses
were derived from many sources, including traditional case
studies. Such case studies have been used for generating
novel concepts, for teaching, for clarification, and for casting
doubt on widely accepted practices. (Ullmann & Krasner,
1965, p. 44-49, provide an excellent exposition on such use of
case studies.) However, within the context of justification, my
academic ancestors and I seem to be in agreement that data
are an absolute requirement. Evidence from rule-based
inquiry is necessary to separate facts from opinions and
speculations.
Developmental Prelude
My academic genealogy shows a line of influence that is part
of the public record. The more important aspects of an
intellectual autobiography, of course, are not yet in the public
domain. Newly entering professionals often seem to believe
that the senior people in any discipline were born with a
grand plan for accomplishment. That grand plan is further
presumed to be one that was based on the senior person’s
current conceptual approach or research program. Senior
people may be viewed as having always been as they
currently appear. Obviously such notions are not true.
562
A broad range of developmental experience influences each
person’s intellectual approach, accomplishments, and ultimate
status. These include nonacademic life experiences, chance
events, choices based on irrelevant factors, and lucky or
unlucky timing as well as formal academic influences.
Ultimately, consequences interactively shape the approach
that becomes a basis for some rational professional action.
The following excursion is an attempt to summarize a number
of these experiences that now seem important to my
professional development. 1
563
afford. Family and friends were the most important
developmental influences during this period.
564
and it was fun to be with him. Friendships were far more
important to him than career advancement or financial gain.
He was an anti-snob for whom “being one of the guys” meant
refusing elected offices and promotions to management
positions. Explicit advice from my father came in a talk at the
time of my parents’ divorce. I was a sophomore in high
school. “Always remember that your word is your bond,” and,
“Do whatever you want to do, but be the best damned one in
town.” I have tried to follow his advice in those regards.
Unfortunately, we only became close after I could relate to
him as an adult — just before his death in 1954.
565
family had its share of folks who could be called
“philanderers” and those who could be called “prudes.” Large
family reunions were enjoyable events that provided
demonstrations of tolerance as well as multiple examples of
conflict resolution. They also showed selective avoidance of
problem topics, based on respect for others.
566
obligation and furthering my career in music. I majored in
music, minored in business, and discovered psychology as an
avocation during my 4-year enlistment as a contract musician
in the U. S. Navy. My last tour of duty helped to change
career focus from music to psychology.
567
school. Everyone called it the “ivory tower.” The student
performed prepared pieces to demonstrate virtuosity,
followed by demonstration of whatever the examiners might
request — such as transpositions, sight-reading, or riffs.
Examiners included the commandant of the school and heads
of the section (e.g., reeds) and groups (jazz, symphonic) as
well as each student’s individual instructor.
568
I was able to obtain one of the best assignments for anyone
planning the musical career that Jo and I had envisioned. It
was with the band assigned to the commander of cruisers and
destroyers, Pacific fleet. This band was known for its
top-quality jazz musicians and for its great USO show
schedule. During my tenure with the group, musicians were
recruited on discharge directly to the Lawrence Welk
Orchestra (considered a sell-out) and to the Stan Kenton
Orchestra (the epitome for big band jazz). Even though the
assignment counted as sea duty, the admiral, his staff, and the
band transferred among flagships such that sea duty involved
a trip to Hawaii every 6 months. Otherwise, everyone lived on
dry land in San Diego, CA. Jo and I lived in navy housing
where there was an active social life. Both of our sons were
born there.
569
official American band at the Olympics — just a year before I
was to be discharged — was such a great opportunity for our
future in the music business that I could not refuse. I violated
the well-known military taboo and volunteered for temporary
duty on that flagship.
After all the effort, I never did get to the Olympics. The duty
radioman, a friend whose shipboard hammock was in the
same area as mine, awaked me one night with some
disturbing news. A military crisis had resulted in the cruiser’s
reassignment to the Suez Canal theatre of operations. Rather
than serving as the flagship for the Australian Olympics, the
cruiser was to depart for the Suez hotspot within 48 hours. I
570
quickly arranged to terminate my temporary duty and
returned to the admiral’s band in San Diego.
The last year in the navy was spent at Great Lakes with a
show band whose schedule regularly involved 3-week
traveling stints. We lived in an off-base apartment in a “Little
America” ghetto in Waukegan, IL. The band was great, but
the travel schedule was not conducive to a good family life
with Jo and our two sons. As this was a typical schedule for
civilian bands as well, I again volunteered to try something
different — the job of bandmaster for the Service School
Command. In addition to training officers in parade
procedures, this job entailed organizing 100 non-professional
musicians to perform concerts and parades. I imagined that
this would be very similar to teaching high school and college
bands. That experience made it clear that a satisfying career in
music for me was not to be found outside of big band jazz.
Ten years as a professional musician was thus put on the
shelf.
571
married student housing in “tin-hut” converted barracks.
Tuition, rent, and utilities were cheap enough to be covered
by the GI Bill. Jo would need to earn only enough to cover
incidentals, such as food, clothing, and transportation. Iowa
City was also close to our extended families. On these
grounds — with no clue as to the nature of the psychology
department — I applied to the University of Iowa and was
accepted with junior-level standing.
572
by the senior professors at Iowa had a paradoxical effect on
my intellectual development. It solidified my commitment to
the predominant psychodynamic approaches of the time and
to a clinical career, in psychiatry or clinical psychology,
rather than research.
573
the saxophone. It was not much related to psychological
theory, but the findings had some practical use (musical
arrangers should be careful where they score trills).
574
research. The decision became, should it be psychiatry or
clinical psychology? Both could take as long as 7 years before
earning a living above the poverty level. We now had three
children and only the GI Bill as a firm financial resource.
575
research in these two departments had an applied focus with
people. Ron Wilson concurred that both had good reputations.
I applied to those two programs and was accepted at both
places. Both also offered support with a tuition waiver for the
first year. Continuing support would be dependent on my
performance.
576
that determined this choice was the quality of inexpensive
married-student housing. Although still renovated barracks,
instead of the Iowa-style tin huts with 6-foot high window
sills, Illinois housing had frame walls with windows through
which Jo and the kids could actually look. After Iowa City,
ordinary windows and doors had become important.
577
stressful year. He noted a few “empirical facts.” (1) Graduate
students should become immersed in psychology as a
discipline — successful students typically devoted at least 80
hours per week to classes, work, and study in psychology. (2)
It had been documented that people could survive on 4 hours
of sleep per night for the period of time we should plan to be
in graduate school. (3) Two-thirds of the entering class would
likely flunk or leave before earning doctorates. (4) Students
were unlikely to be successful in both graduate school and in
marriage. Clearly, this was going to take real effort in the
interpersonal as well as academic arena.
578
guidelines for the amount of material to be assigned. Each
faculty member attempted to cover everything that was
important in his own specialty. These assignments often
exceeded 2000 pages of reading a week, with testing so
comprehensive as to require recall of specific footnotes. The
expected attrition in the first year class did occur, either from
failure or from people’s unwillingness to tolerate the required
life style. Two students left to be admitted to acute psychiatric
units. Unnecessarily stressful, indeed!
579
Cronbach, Don Dulany, Charles Eriksen, Fred Fiedler, Marty
Fishbein, Ray Frankman, Bob Grice, Harry Hake, Lloyd
Humphreys, Joe Hunt, Will Kappauf, Sam Kirk, Joe
McGrath, Bill McGuire, Hobart Mowrer, Larry O’Kelly,
Charles Osgood, Don Peterson, Hal Rosen, Don Shannon,
Ivan Steiner, Larry Stolurow, Garth Thomas, Harry Triandis,
Ledyard Tucker, Mort Weir, and Jerry Wiggins. Merle
Ohlsen, Cecil Patterson, and Fred Proff taught courses in the
educational psychology counseling program, where I
completed a minor in group counseling and therapy.
580
The clinical facilities, supervision, and training were as rich
as advertised. I overbooked to maximize my clinical
experience. This included extra practica, special topics, and
formal supervision for the clinical aspects of research
assistantships as well as paid clinical service jobs. By
tradition, students continued to carry clients in the
departmental clinic throughout their tenure in the clinical
program — even while collecting data and writing
dissertations.
581
graduation in the program. This was sufficient to successfully
petition to bypass the predoctoral internship requirement and
go directly to postdoctoral internship training.
582
methods and research design, from both Iowa and Illinois,
had prepared me to critically evaluate the literature. Previous
studies, I believed, had simply not involved variables or
designs that were strong enough to demonstrate the
phenomena of learning without awareness in a laboratory
setting. I wanted to ensure that the quality of the research
design was so strong that no alternative explanations could
sully the conclusion that unconscious learning occurred.
583
The production of non-herpetic skin blisters by hypnotic
suggestion should be a natural demonstration. I did a critical
review of that literature, again drawing on my previous forced
methodological training. Once more, my own efforts found
better explanations for the phenomena, not support for the
psychodynamic principles (Paul, 1963). This added another
chink in my beliefs.
I was not the only one who was disturbed by the lack of
evidence to support our practical clinical training. Several of
the faculty and students were as well. Much ferment was
present among advanced clinical students as well as among
the remaining members of my class. A group of us convinced
Erik to offer a seminar in which we could seriously examine
Eysenck’s (1952, 1961) outrageous claims that
insight-oriented psychotherapy did not work and that this new
approach called “behavior therapy” did. The behavior therapy
literature was sparse enough at that time to be covered in a
single semester. We concluded that Bandura’s (1961) review
article was essentially correct regarding the promise for that
approach. However, we did not believe that the literature
demonstrated ineffectiveness of the psychodynamic approach.
We concluded that there was simply no good evidence
regarding the effectiveness of insight-oriented psychotherapy
— only a lot of flawed studies. Nevertheless, the absence of
evidence to support my preferred approach added another
disquieting chink in beliefs.
584
unconscious conflicts between contradictory impulses and
defensive forces. Once I personally observed the superficial
nature of client response to desensitization, I thought, I could
comfortably discard Wolpe’s uncomplicated
counter-conditioning model for the more comprehensive
psychodynamic approach.
585
I arranged to do a small comparison with 11 students who
requested treatment for test anxiety, following their
participation in a validity study of an anxiety scale (Paul &
Eriksen, 1964). I treated five with individual systematic
desensitization and compared their outcomes to six equated
but untreated classmates. In contrast to the untreated group,
the entire treated group showed improvement on self-reported
anxiety and on course exam performance. We never
published this study because of the obvious within-class
confounding of therapist characteristics and lack of control
for nonspecific treatment effects. Nevertheless, the findings
were disquieting. They added another chink in my beliefs.
586
Background and Preparation
I had to complete a dissertation study anyway. If that amount
of work was to be done, it ought to provide some answers to
questions that were personally important. My thoughts were
as follows. It was clear that previous real-life treatment
studies were badly flawed. Both the “known,” but
undemonstrated, effectiveness of insight-oriented
psychotherapy and the early relapse and symptom substitution
expected from systematic desensitization should appear in an
investigation with really good research design. An ideal
design was needed.
587
effectiveness for any psychotherapeutic approach. The
resulting principles and concepts were later expanded in two
publications, where the “ultimate clinical question” was
explicated. A paper on the strategy of outcome research in
psychotherapy was published in the Journal of Consulting
Psychology (Paul, 1967b). The recommendations were further
elaborated in a chapter on design and tactics in Cyril Franks’
edited book (Paul, 1969a). Before detailing this material in
print, however, I had formulated what was needed to design a
comparative study of treatment effectiveness that would allow
unambiguous conclusions. My forced training in research
methods at Iowa and Illinois again proved useful.
588
At that time, a course in public speaking was a graduation
requirement for liberal arts students. Many found
performance anxiety to be a major impediment to their
successful completion of this requirement. People often
delayed the course until their senior year. Large numbers
sought treatment and a very large percentage was reported to
simply drop out without graduating. Here was a population of
young adults who should be essentially psychiatrically
normal, but with a serious emotional problem that they were
highly motivated to overcome. An ideal group, it seemed,
with which to arrange multiple treatment and control
conditions within the necessary partial-factorial research
design.
589
monitored in their performance. Five of the area’s most
highly regarded insight-oriented psychotherapists agreed to
participate: Joe Becker, Alice Jonietz, Merle Ohlsen, Fred
Proff, and Don Shannon. All were doctoral practitioners who
had been previous supervisors of mine.
590
students, concurrently enrolled in a course in public speaking,
and because actual treatment contacts were time-limited to
five sessions within a 6-week period.
591
sessions, based on their consensus of the usual number
needed to treat the focal problem (given that identification
and assessment of the problem occurred in advance).
Treatment sessions took place in the therapists’ own clinical
offices, in one of three “real clinical-service facilities,” rather
than in a laboratory setting. Finally, treatment procedures
were applied as they would have been in ordinary practice,
except for being monitored by audiotapes to ensure fidelity.
592
the pretreatment and follow-up batteries with the entire class
population. People in this group were unaware of their
participation in a treatment study — providing a base for
evaluation of possible improvement resulting from the
additional attention, practice, and anticipation of treatment in
group TC.
593
A total of 380 people requested treatment following
administration of the pretreatment battery. I selected the 96
who were the most debilitated by anxiety for participation in
the study (68 males, 28 females). People in the resulting
sample were “good bets” for psychotherapy, being young,
intelligent, middle-class, highly motivated, and with strong to
severe interpersonal-performance anxiety of 2-20 years
standing. In most cases, anxiety was reported in nearly any
social, evaluative, or interpersonal context — being most
severe in the public speaking situation. Their scores on the
focal anxiety scales were, in fact, higher than those of most
people applying to community clinics with similar problems.
The selected sample also scored significantly higher than the
broad student population on both general anxiety and
emotionality as well as scoring significantly lower on
extroversion. The severity of the problem was further evident
among those not selected for participation, although they had
requested treatment. Even though the latter group scored
lower on anxiety scales than those selected for participation,
32% dropped out without completing the speech course. None
of the treated students dropped out.
594
(with reliabilities exceeding r = .95). Additional
stress-condition measures were obtained just before each
person presented his or her test speech. These included a
self-report Anxiety Differential and two measures of
physiological arousal — pulse rate and palmar sweat. At the
end of the pretreatment test speech, each participant was
scheduled for an interview with me.
595
People assigned to group TC were told that time was not
available for experienced therapists to see everyone that
semester. Rather than ask anyone to work with a less
competent therapist, names had been “picked from a hat” to
determine who would have to wait until the following
semester to receive treatment. I assured them that treatment
would be provided for those who wanted it (and it was). It
still would be necessary, however, for them to return for
another evaluation speech in a few weeks, as our measures
would be meaningless without them.
596
back for posttreatment stress-condition assessments. All
treated clients returned for this evaluation, but 7 of 29 in the
wait-list condition were “no shows.” These no shows were
among the most anxious of that group at pretreatment. The
same measures obtained before and during the pretreatment
test speech were repeated for the posttreatment test speech. I
also added blinding and other procedures to avoid carryover
biases from the pretreatment test speech.
Results
Group and individual differences on all data were evaluated
by analyses of variance, multiple comparisons of the
difference between differences, regression analyses, and
individually significant changes on each measure for each
client. The results were surprisingly clear and consistent, with
converging evidence from all information sources. The
superiority I had hoped to demonstrate for insight-oriented
psychotherapy was no where to be found. The
attention-placebo treatment was, in fact, as effective as
597
insight-oriented psychotherapy! Clients treated by systematic
desensitization were, unmistakably, the most improved of all
groups at treatment termination. In fact, systematic
desensitization was remarkably effective on an absolute level.
The 6-week follow-up demonstrated a similar pattern of focal
treatment effects. For treated clients, only a few additional
changes, beyond the focal effects, appeared at the 6-week
follow-up. There was no evidence of symptom substitution. A
few trends even suggested generalization of positive effects.
The following highlights the major findings.
The second and third bars of each set in Figure 2 reflect the
percentages of improved cases treated, respectively, by
insight-oriented psychotherapy and attention-placebo
treatment. Both of these treatments produced greater rates of
improvement than the no-treatment controls, but the insight
and attention-placebo groups did not differ in effectiveness
from one another on a single measure! The bottom two bars
of each set represent improvement rates for the no-treatment
598
control group. The differing numbers reflect the “no shows”
on the second assessment for those in the wait-list condition.
Improvement rates that include the entire no-treatment control
group, with no-shows treated as “no change,” are in the
lowest bars. The next to the lowest bar of each set shows
improvement rates that include only those with complete data.
599
attention, stress-condition assessments, and promise of
treatment had produced some minimal benefits. However, the
strength and clarity of results among the treatment groups left
no doubt of the superiority of systematic desensitization over
competing approaches, or of the essential equivalence of the
insight and attention-placebo treatments.
600
generalization of focal improvements, with naturally
occurring reinforcement in the posttreatment environment.
601
Focal improvement among treated clients over the 2-year
follow-up period was relatively reliable (r = .78) and
predictable from improvements on stress condition
assessments 2 years earlier (r = .61). The set of bars at the
bottom of Figure 3 shows that systematic desensitization
maintained its higher rate of significant improvement over the
other two treatment groups. Insight and attention-placebo
treatment continued to show no differences in improvement
rates from one another. As noted at the top of Figure 3, there
was no evidence of relapse for any treated client. No matter
what treatment they had received, none showed an increase in
self-reported anxiety in situations that involved public
speaking. Of the positively biased group of untreated controls,
11% showed significant increases in focal anxiety that would
qualify as relapse and 22% showed decreases that qualified as
focal improvement.
602
The middle set of bars in Figure 3 shows evidence of
generalized improvement, with the desensitization group
showing more generalization of positive effects than other
groups. The insight and attention-placebo groups failed to
differ from each other, once again. As reflected in the top set
of bars in Figure 3, changes that could provide evidence of
possible symptom substitution occurred at exceptionally low
rates. None of the groups, including controls, differed from
chance-level changes and there were no differences among
groups.
603
desensitization and using it in their own practices well before
analyses of the objective data. Informal knowledge of the
findings spread rapidly through local psychology, speech, and
counseling departments as I analyzed the results.
Word-of-mouth proliferation quickly extended beyond the
university once committee members read the completed
dissertation.
604
programs, and engaging clients and their physical-social
environments in the ongoing process of reeducation and
change is simply harder work. The increased effort is justified
by routine improvements in clients’ functioning.
The field was in ferment and clearly ready for change. Most
people were interested in the clinical outcomes. Methodology
became of interest as stakeholders in competing theoretical
camps sought to embrace or dismiss the findings. The
timeliness of work in this area is further reflected by the
interest in the focal publication (Paul, 1966) and in the
chapters in Cyril Franks’ book that reviewed systematic
desensitization studies (Paul, 1969d, 1969e) and
605
methodological design and tactics (Paul, 1969a) — all of
which became citation classics.
Intellectual Autobiography:
Afterwards
My incorporation of the utility criterion resulted in
consolidation and expansion of the new paradigm following
completion of the focal study. The approach was strengthened
during a postdoctoral year in California and through
interactions with colleagues and further research at the
University of Illinois, after I returned as faculty. These
experiences also contributed to my later focus on psychoses,
mental hospitals, and mental health systems in Illinois and
Texas.
606
The willingness of my major professors to nominate me for
faculty positions at top universities was very much
appreciated. It was with some trepidation that I chose not to
follow their advice. But, there were many reasons to pursue
postdoctoral clinical training instead.
607
rotations at the VA hospital, I also did some paid consulting
and spent a few hours each week with private clients. As
noted earlier, I was also active on the talk circuit during this
period. VA trainees were treated to weekly seminars with
Stanford University faculty, Mental Research Institute staff,
and visiting professionals as well as some excellent VA staff.
Influential instructors, beyond my direct supervisors, included
Al Bandura, Walt Mischel, Ernest Hilgard, John Vitali, Don
Jackson, Paul McReynolds, and Bob Weiss. Jerry Davison
and I overlapped on the same ward for the last 6 weeks.
608
behavioral practices were rare there. Many psychologists
were content to restrict their activities to testing.
Chemotherapy predominated. Most professionals endorsed
psychoanalytic or related psychodynamic orientations,
providing the opportunity to argue and clarify differing
positions. This helped consolidate my conversion.
609
hallucinogens as well as an investigator of their use in
treatment. To our surprise, Jack and I often gave identical
advice to folks on the ward, although we arrived there from
different conceptualizations. His non-disease, “myth of
mental illness” approach (Szasz, 1961) was quite compatible
with my own.
610
result of new experiences. The California climate was great. I
enjoyed living on the edge of the “hip” culture, while
maintaining more traditional values. However, Jo and I did
not like the ideals that our children were developing. We
decided that it would be better to raise the children in a
community with more traditional mores than we observed in
the bay area, especially with its high rate of broken homes.
611
the policy against hiring the department’s own graduates. I
was also ambivalent about returning. The department
supported the three activities that I sought, but engaging in all
of them would entail 75-80 hour workweeks. Did I want to
continue working that hard? Not really! I was also a bit
concerned about switching from student to faculty roles.
However, discussions with the major players convinced me
that this would not be a problem. The Illinois climate was
lousy but the community was nearly ideal for child rearing
and socializing in ways that we all enjoyed. Jo and the kids
thought returning to Champaign-Urbana would be going
home.
612
Kanfer & Phillips, 1970; Krasner & Ullmann, 1965; Patterson
& Gullion, 1968; Ullmann & Krasner, 1965; Wolpe &
Lazarus, 1966).
613
related problems (Evans & Paul, 1970; Paul, 1969b, 1969c,
1969f; Paul & Trimble, 1970).
614
psychoanalytic practices. However, the utility criterion still
called for empirical answers in those areas of practice where
firm evidence was lacking. Traditional practices were clearly
inadequate in the treatment of psychoses, especially for
people who were chronically institutionalized. This attracted
and maintained my interest. In fact, my work has primarily
focused on institutionalized populations, observational
assessment, and treatment of psychoses for more than 30
years.
615
The expansion to inpatient populations drew heavily on the
work of colleagues who shared their ideas and innovations
through personal interactions. Len Ullmann, Wes Becker,
Don Peterson, Bob Peterson, Jerry Wiggins, Lloyd
Humphreys, Sid Bijou, Lew Kurke, Bernie Wagner, Joe
Williams, and John Nolte all contributed ideas to the
development of inpatient assessment and treatment
procedures. My coworkers and I sought guidance from Elaine
Cumming and Alan Kraft on milieu practices. I had direct
experience with the inpatient programs developed by Ted
Ayllon and Nate Azrin, Jack Atthowe and Len Krasner, and
Bill Fairweather. In the design of treatment programs, my
coworkers and I incorporated procedural innovations from the
work of all of these investigators as well as those developed
by Ogden Lindsley, summarized in this volume. I am pleased
to acknowledge other contributors to this volume — Don
Baer, Monte Wolf, and Todd Risley — for providing the
seminal technologies and templates with children that we
adapted for use in developing effective treatment programs
with adults.
616
Theobald, Bob Paden, Carolyn Paden, Paula Griffith, Peggy
Maynard, and Ralph Trimble.
617
psychosocial and biomedical procedures and, ultimately, to
the operation of treatment facilities and entire systems of
service (Paul, 1986a, 1986b; Paul & Lentz, 1977, 2001).
618
funding to build a few prominent departments, and
psychology was targeted as one of them. More than half of
the faculty was newly recruited into a department that was to
explicitly focus on solving applied problems. A Cullen
Distinguished Chair and a group of stellar colleagues were
part of the offer. Early on, the clinical program counted Dale
Johnson, John Vincent, Roger Maley, Marco Mariotto, Lynn
Rehm, and Len Ullmann among the faculty working on adult
problems, with interests in the seriously mentally ill. The
strongest incentive was the promise of establishing a
combined service-research-training-demonstration center at
Austin State Hospital in which to continue our work and
disseminate findings (see Paul, 1990). Jo was willing to
assume the secretarial duties of our clinical-research project.
Houston had no snow. The kids were grown. It all looked
very promising.
619
Program. Marco Mariotto, Joel Redfield, Mark Licht, Chris
Power, and I completed the theoretical analyses that underlies
our science-based assessment approach (Paul, 1986b). We
continued data analyses and development of the observational
assessment technology with the assistance of our graduate
students. The data set contains information on more than 1200
inpatients and 800 clinical staff in 35 different treatment
units. It has served as a rich source of data for theses and
dissertations.
620
System have been published (Paul, 1986b, 1987a, 1988). At
the time of this writing, the developmental group led by Mark
Licht is upgrading the TSBC/SRIC System computer
programs. Coworkers in the group include Paul Stuve, James
Coleman, Will Newbill, and Susan Hall. They are converting
programs to a more powerful and user-friendly database
system before we finish the final version of implementation
materials (Paul, 2001a, 2001b). The TSBC/SRIC System has
even more potential to improve ongoing practices than just
having an effective treatment program for previously
untreatable clientele. It will allow treatment facilities to offer
ongoing services that are not only “new and improved” but
“ever improving” (Paul, Stuve, & Cross, 1997).
Reflections
I am struck by the degree to which my nonacademic life
influenced my professional work. Family values, emotional
621
experiences, and good and bad models of deportment all play
the expected role. However, chance events and decisions
based on irrelevant factors appear more influential than I
imagined. Long-range planning is risky. Things change. It
seems best to keep options open for as long as possible.
622
— the real behavioral revolution — did not happen in those
instances.
623
Evidence-based practices. I endorse the past decade’s
initiatives to identify and promote empirically validated
treatments as well as the fledgling work on empirically
validated assessments. More generally, science-based practice
and policy guidelines and evidence-based practices should be
the best corrective to “things that still go ‘bump’ in the night.”
624
journals. The psychology task force later included journal
follow-ups and summaries of these studies as valid evidence.
However, psychiatric groups continue to exclude these works,
often on the basis that treatments were not specific to a single
DSM diagnosis. Psychiatric guidelines are mostly for
psychotropic drugs. It would be better if science-based rules
of evidence played a greater role in these undertakings.
Recommendations
My major recommendation for all workers in the area is
captured in the old adage, “Anything worth doing is worth
doing well.” Whether the activity involves learning new
things, conducting research to solve problems, or engaging in
clinical work to help others — do it right. Be responsible. Be
thorough. Be rigorous. Do not take the easy way if a hard way
is required.
625
provide some bases for suggestions to those who are just
entering the field — students, young investigators, and young
practitioners.
626
vanish under careful scrutiny. In most areas, a series of
programmatic studies will probably make a greater
contribution than isolated ones. However, investigations
undertaken as part of degree requirements must be completed
in a timely fashion. It is generally better to begin sequential
investigations, where findings build on one another, than to
try to answer all questions in a single study.
627
as well as those that are historically important — not just the
new findings and technologies from recent publications. Do
not rest on your laurels. Continue to evaluate the evidence.
Continue to learn. Never fully retire.
628
My final recommendation is this. Everyone should use the
ultimate question for guidance, but add the “ultimate” answer
as well. That should help to maintain focus on the
interactional complexities of clinical phenomena. What is the
ultimate answer? “It depends!”
References
Atthowe, J. M., & Krasner, L. (1968). Preliminary report on
the application of contingent reinforcement procedures (token
economy) on a “chronic” psychiatric ward. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 73, 37-43.
629
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 2,
225-237.
630
Hayes, S. C. (1991). Pursuing the ultimate clinical question:
An interview with Gordon L. Paul. The Scientist-Practitioner,
1(3), 6-16.
631
Paul, G. L. (1967a). Insight versus desensitization in
psychotherapy two years after termination. Journal of
Consulting Psychology, 31, 333-348.
632
Paul, G. L. (1969e). Outcome of systematic desensitization II:
Controlled investigations of individual treatment, technique
variations, and current status. In C. M. Franks (Ed.), Behavior
therapy: Appraisal and status (pp. 105-159). New York:
McGraw-Hill.
633
staff functioning. In D. R. Peterson & D. B. Fishman (Eds.),
Assessment for decision (pp. 145-203). New Brunswick, NJ:
Rutgers University Press.
634
Paul, G. L., & Bernstein, D. A. (1973). Anxiety and clinical
problems: Treatment by systematic desensitization and
related techniques. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.
635
Applied and Preventive Psychology: Current Scientific
Perspectives, 6, 193-204.
636
Footnote
1
Preparing the prelude to my intellectual autobiography
involved revisiting aspects of my youth that are laden with
emotions. Some involve people and experiences that I simply
had not recently thought about — at least, not regarding their
impact on my own development. Others involved painful
experiences that I had diligently worked to suppress. The
redintegration of strong emotional reactions, both positive and
negative, that I experienced in considering these aspects of
my history — even while writing this chapter — suggests that
I have, indeed, selected relevant material for inclusion.
637
Chapter 18
638
the formative influences in our professional lives, discuss a
publication that we believe has had some importance in
behavior therapy, and reflect on the nature of that influence
on the field. This paper is an effort to fulfill this unusual and
intriguing assignment.
639
German, Russian, political science, biology, western
civilization, and other general education topics that were
supposed to enable me to declare a major (or “concentration,”
the term favored by Harvardians). By the beginning of my
sophomore year, however, I had managed only to reject
political science as a major and to locate myself in the
German department, where I found myself intrigued more by
the characters in the novels I was reading than by the
language or whatever else it was that a literature major was
supposed to find interesting.
640
listen to the first lecture of the introductory Soc. Rel. course.
And my life changed.
641
signified a wish on his part to rub up against the body of his
brother-in-law. The Radcliffe students in the class —
“Cliffies,” we called them with a mixture of envy and
resentment since most of them were smarter and more verbal
than the Harvard students — were especially vocal in this
rendition of the dream.
642
Fulbright Scholarship to study for a year in Germany
following graduation in June 1961.
643
And now another unexpected event. Because my mother was
worried that I would not eat well in California, she’d
prevailed on me to join a meal plan for at least my first
quarter at Stanford. This found me eating dinner each evening
in the graduate dining hall with other first year students in
psychology. Some of these were clinical students (Stanford
had an APA-approved clinical program at the time), and they
often talked about a professor named Bandura and something
called “behavior therapy.” The basic notion was that all the
stuff I’d been learning as an experimental psychologist had
relevance for understanding and treating abnormal behavior.
This was a new notion for me. None of my professors at
Harvard had ever mentioned this viewpoint — and recall that
Wolpe’s classic book had been published in 1958, while I was
a sophomore. Recall also that Skinner was at Harvard and had
published a couple of papers with Ogden Lindsley on operant
conditioning of regressed schizophrenics. But these
new-fangled notions had not found their way into the
ego-analytic, psychodynamic stronghold of Emerson Hall.
644
very heterogeneous. Precisely, he said, but one thing they had
in common was that all the people were clinical
psychologists. Clinical psychology, he said with obvious
relish, is a bastard discipline. And that’s what makes it
exciting and promising. That conversation with London was
pivotal.
645
research, and practice in what we then called social learning
approaches to psychotherapy, or sometimes just behavior
therapy. Lazarus began to see private patients at a greatly
reduced fee and permitted a few of the clinical students to sit
in with him. I must have spent at least 10 hours a week during
my second year, from September 1963 to June 1964,
watching Lazarus work with patients. Somewhere along the
line the conceptual introduction I had received from Bandura
and Mischel came to life in my sessions with Lazarus, such
that behavior therapy had a completely different meaning for
me at the end of that incredible year than it had had in the
beginning.2
646
internship year would have been like had it not been for
Paul’s calm and skillful introduction into the sometimes
surreal world of the VA mental hospital.
It was a heady time. Here was a program that, well before the
empirically supported treatments movement, elected to focus
on assessment and intervention that enjoyed some measure of
empirical support, eschewing unvalidated approaches and
procedures without a concern that our students would be
unable later on to obtain clinical internships. True, we were
narrow, but the Krasner-Kalish vision was to specialize in
something that we all believed had more promise than the
traditional clinical fare.
647
oneself rather than to a drug leads to greater maintenance of
therapeutic change (Davison & Valins, 1969). Second was
getting together with John Neale to write our abnormal
psychology textbook, the first edition being published in
1974. It was an instant success, and we recently completed
our 8th edition (Davison & Neale, 2001). And finally there
was Clinical Behavior Therapy with Marv Goldfried,
published in 1976 and reissued in 1994 in an expanded form
(Goldfried & Davison, 1976, 1994). This book helped bring
research and theoretical abstractions to life, contributed to the
cognitive trend in behavior therapy, and made a case for
trying to integrate ideas and procedures from the
non-behavioral psychotherapies. I was very fortunate to have
had such talented colleagues as these as well as other Stony
Brook faculty.
648
It was my impression that some of the students had a very
personal interest in the subject matter, but most of the seminar
members were involved in the subject more from an
intellectual than from a personal or political point of view. A
couple of colleagues mentioned to me a few years afterwards
that they wondered if these might have been the first courses
taught in a psychology department with the focus primarily
on homosexuality.
649
Of more interest to me, though, was the brief against sexual
reorientation treatment. Simply stated, why spend so much
time and effort developing, evaluating, and providing
change-of-orientation therapies when they are aimed at a
“problem” that is socially defined?
650
Charles Silverstein, a recent Ph.D. from the Rutgers clinical
program. I recall Chuck sitting in the meeting room with an
interested and fairly friendly expression on his face,
occasionally asking questions about why I was involved in
this sort of scholarship and application. My answer, which
was the standard response of behavior therapists at the time,
was that I would never impose such conversion treatment on
an unwilling homosexual patient, but that I saw it as
appropriate and, indeed, inherent to my professional role to
make such reorientation interventions available to gay and
lesbian patients who asked for sexual reorientation. He never
seemed quite satisfied with my answer but he didn’t push the
issue. Not during the workshop.
651
myself with a couple of extra hours, and then Silverstein’s
symposium came to mind. With no one in particular to talk to
and deciding it would be more interesting to spend the extra
time at the convention as it was ending rather than cooling my
heels elsewhere, I found the room where the radicals were to
hold forth.
652
Being President of AABT in 1973-1974
During my presidential year, I initiated a motion in the AABT
Board of Directors, which passed the following resolution at
its meeting of May 11-12, 1974. It was supported by an
overwhelming vote of the membership later that spring:
653
The American Dream who saw fit to engage in such activities
as circulating the home addresses of fascists like myself,
Israel Goldiamond, and other putative enemies of the people.
Since Goldiamond was a featured presenter at the 1974
convention, we had to arrange for plainclothes as well as
uniformed police to ensure the orderliness and safety of the
proceedings.
654
In an unusual statement accompanying the signing of the bill,
the governor expressed the hope that this legislation would
contribute to greater social acceptance of Protestantism as a
legitimate, albeit unconventional, religion.
655
the time the very naturalness of and familiarity with our
therapeutic practices blind us to the nonempirical biases that
affect how we construe the patient’s problems and the goals
we regard as acceptable to work towards. Better to be aware
of and own up to our biases than to pretend that we have
none.
656
No Cure Without a Disease
Clinicians devote effort to developing and analyzing
therapeutic procedures only if they are concerned about a
problem. Until the 1980s behavior therapists spent a good
deal of time and effort reducing homosexual attraction and
increasing heterosexual attraction in homosexuals (and for the
most part, the target population was men only). Again, until
recently little if any time — and none at all when I first made
my remarks — was spent by mainstream therapists
encouraging health professionals to change their biases
against homosexuality and foster gay-affirmative attitudes
and behavior in patients who happened to be homosexual.
The question for me was and still is the following: How can
therapists honestly speak of nonprejudice when they
participate in or tacitly support therapy regimens that by their
very existence and regardless of their effectiveness condone
the societal prejudice and perhaps also impede social change?
As Begelman pointed out many years ago (1975), sexual
reorientation therapies
657
homosexual “problems” indicates otherwise (p. 180, emphasis
in original).
658
functional analysis, a set of ideas of what is wrong, what the
controlling variables are, and what might be done to alleviate
the suffering and maladaptation. My argument, then, is that
psychological problems are for the most part constructions of
the clinician. Clients comes to us in pain, and they leave with
a more clearly defined problem or set of problems that we
assign to them.
659
some of my friends and colleagues I still hold to, is that
mental health professionals have a responsibility not to be
co-opted by the societal pressures that, sometimes subtly,
channel our clinical problem-solving and decision-making
into narrowly defined domains that result in a maintenance of
a status quo that, in official pronouncements, we say we do
not support.
660
But legal pressures are not the whole story. Research supports
the view that gays and lesbians are discriminated against in all
kinds of ways and that this discrimination takes a particularly
heavy toll on their emotional well-being. So-called “hate
crimes” highlight this problem. A hate crime (sometimes
referred to as a bias crime) is an assault that is based primarily
or solely on a person’s (perceived) membership in a group
against which the perpetrator is prejudiced. The ultimate
modern-day hate crime was, of course, the Holocaust in
Germany and other parts of Europe prior to and during World
War II. The Nazis sought out for imprisonment and execution
millions of Jews and hundreds of thousands of gypsies,
Communists, and homosexuals. The more recent “ethnic
cleansing” in Bosnia and Kosovo and in many other parts of
the world shows us that humankind has not learned much
from the Holocaust experience. But hate crimes as well as
hurtful discrimination are carried out every day in less
organized and less dramatic fashion.
661
As compared to non-hate crimes, bias crimes and verbal
assaults may create more psychological distress, perhaps
because they are an attack not just against the person as a
physical being but against the person’s very identity (Garnets
Herek, & Levy, 1990). Furthermore, such crimes may impart
to the victim a pervasive sense of danger and even loathing of
an aspect of the self that might otherwise be a source of
pleasure and pride.
662
the major factor in gay and lesbian people suffering
particularly high levels of depression (Herek et al., 1996).
663
Long ago, Perry London (1969) warned of an unappreciated
danger in behavior control technology, namely clinicians’
increasing ability to engineer what we have tended to regard
as free will on the part of our patients. In his view, therapists
are capable of making patients want what is available and
what they believe their patients should want. Moreover, even
if therapists assert that they do not work against the will of
their patients, this does not free them from the responsibility
of examining those factors that determine what is considered
free expression of intent and desire on the part of our patients.
Indeed, I would argue that the therapist sets the goals in
therapy more than does the patient.
664
Not Can but Ought
As mentioned below in my discussion of a critique by Sturgis
and Adams (1978), there is an important and oft-overlooked
distinction between being able to achieve a goal and whether
it is proper to try to do so. Empirical evidence as to whether
we can change sexual orientation is not relevant to whether
we ought to — except that we ought not to engage in a given
change effort when there is no evidence that we can actually
do so. This may well be the case with conversion therapies.
The ethical argument against an ineffective treatment is that
patients are bound to be disappointed and likely to feel even
worse and “sicker” if they have made an effort to alter
something that cannot be changed. The patient has not only
failed to achieve a goal that has been set forth by the therapist
as important but is likely to come away from the unsuccessful
therapy continuing to believe that their behavior is bad and
that they are really hopeless and unworthy.
But the two domains — empirical and ethical — are best kept
separate.
665
problems examines those values and ideologies that guide the
decision-making of a society. Individual therapy work, in
contrast, assumes that society is benign and that
psychological suffering can best be alleviated by helping the
patient adjust to prevailing values and conditions. My
underlying assumption is that issues surrounding therapy for
homosexuality should be addressed at an institutional level,
and that greater societal acceptance of homosexuality as a
normal variation of human sexuality rather than as a problem
that needs to be fixed will, in fact, redound to the benefit of
the individual by reducing the discrimination and oppression
described earlier that, I firmly believe, accounts for the
distress that can be associated with homosexuality and
ultimately the desire of some homosexual individuals to seek
sexual reorientation.
666
homosexuals at all.) Indeed, the implication of my thesis is
that therapists consider seriously the problems in living
experienced by people who happen to prefer members of their
own sex as sexual partners. For example, while a gay person
may be depressed because his sexual orientation is mocked or
attacked and he feels insecure about standing up for himself,
gay people also get depressed because their professional
aspirations are thwarted by circumstances having nothing to
do with their sexual orientation. And it would be nice if
alcohol abusers who happen to be homosexual could be
helped to reduce their excessive drinking without having their
sexual orientation questioned. Freed of the inclination of
trying to alter a homosexual’s sexual preferences, therapists
will find many other ways that they might help that individual
lead a more fulfilling life.
667
she just wanted to thank me. I’ve been told that other people
who have been personally affected by conversion efforts and
their promulgation reacted similarly, albeit privately. I have
found these reactions very gratifying, especially as the years
have gone by and I have seen the argument become, if not
universally accepted, at least more mainstream and one that
can no longer be ignored.
It may or may not have been assumed by some that I was gay.
Besides some occasional heterosexist kidding from a
colleague or friend, I’m not aware of this consequence of
which I’d been forewarned (not that it mattered to me one
way or the other). And of course this admonition assumes that
only a gay person would hold the point of view against sexual
conversion therapies that I’d articulated — a position that I’ve
always seen as a strategy, perhaps unconsciously employed,
to denigrate the message by denigrating the messenger.
668
conversion treatment was not of “general enough interest” to
an organization like APA.
669
Clearly my belief was and is that we should not. So I told
Maher that I would not be an appropriate reviewer because I
would have to reject the manuscript out of hand as not
relevant. His response was that he wanted to publish the paper
provided I write a rebuttal (instead of the review he had asked
me to write). This seemed a very sensible editorial decision,
and I agreed to do so. Basically “Not Can But Ought: The
Treatment of Homosexuality” (Davison, 1978) responded to
Sturgis and Adams (1978) in the aforementioned fashion, that
is, that their paper was irrelevant to my argument. I don’t
believe my rebuttal was convincing to the authors, but I found
it interesting some years later to be told by Sturgis that she
had changed her views on the matter and now agreed with my
position. Adams, on the other hand, continued to believe that
therapists have an obligation to change people’s sexual
orientations if they seek such treatment. Interestingly, he and
his students conducted some very interesting and ingenious
research on homophobia, a focus that I was delighted to see
for his considerable research skills.4
670
Empirical versus Ethical Questions
I think my paper, and the rebuttal to the Sturgis-Adams
critique, have contributed to a clearer understanding of the
difference between what we as psychotherapists can or think
we can do and what we ought to be doing. It is surprising to
me how difficult it is for some folks to see this essential and
simple difference. In my teaching I sometimes use an
intentionally bizarre example to make the point. I tell students
that I have a one-session cure for any mental/emotional/
behavioral problem. In fact, it works in much less than one
session. Indeed, it works in much less than one minute. It is a
bullet in the head of the patient. With death comes an end to
all the person’s psychological suffering and/or maladaptive
behavior. No more panic attacks, no more depression, no
more disordered thinking, no more shy withdrawal, no more
non-assertiveness, no more autistic aloneness, no more
psychopathic finagling, no more aggression. All gone in an
instant.
671
The Therapist as Secular Priest
Related to this point are Perry London’s writings on moral
issues in psychotherapy (e.g., London, 1964). This influence
from my graduate school days did not show up fully until I
became obsessed with the sexual conversion issue. As
indicated earlier, his concept of therapist as secular priest
defines our role as inherently moral, whether we like it or not.
Especially behavior therapists unabashedly try to shape the
patient in ways that they believe will benefit the patient and
not infringe on the rights and sensibilities of others. But we
also are good at engineering what the patient ends up
wanting, as Halleck said so eloquently in his 1971 book. I
believe that my article has helped sensitize people to the
issue, regardless of how they think about it. As a teacher it is
enough for me to know that I may have helped frame the
debate and made it legitimate, if not actually necessary, to
consider the influence that therapists have on their patients,
even when therapists think of themselves as hands-off when it
comes to therapeutic goals. I just don’t believe that patients
don’t get shaped in this way. At the very least, I think it is
better to assume this shaping rather than, as we have been
doing, assume its absence.
672
therapies. I am not in a position to know this, but friends and
colleagues have suggested this to be the case. Certainly my
own “conversion” in 1973-1974 took place at a time that
changes in organizational viewpoints were occurring. I cannot
help but be pleased if the position I took was at all
instrumental.
673
Psychosocial Interventions as Part of
Social Institutions
As I argued in my original paper, an institutional perspective
is important in understanding the conduct of psychotherapy.
As private and walled-off-from-the-world as outpatient and
some inpatient mental health intervention is, therapists and
patients do not work together in a social vacuum. As Halleck
argued in 1971, the decisions made in the consulting room
reflect and have effects on the politics and social fabric of the
place and time in which therapy is conducted. Therapists’
behavior is constrained by multiple factors — from
theoretical orientation, to personal taste, to religious values, to
legal requirements and strictures, and most recently to
reporting requirements and treatment decisions from
insurance companies. Patients’ behavior is also influenced by
multiple factors, and the emphasis in my writings on
homosexuality is on the manner in which societal prejudices
and biases shape the very way people come to understand
what is wrong and what is right about themselves, what they
might wish to change and what they might prefer to leave
alone. I continue to focus on the specific issue of
homosexuality because so many people have been and
continue to be hurt by prejudice and discrimination. But as I
hope is clear, the issues are much more general, going to the
heart of how researchers and clinicians set their professional
agendas, which in turn affect what they learn and the
decisions they make. I believe and hope that the position I
took in 1974 has contributed to the debate.
674
References
Bandura, A. (1969). Principles of behavior modification. New
York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
675
Davison, G. C. (1976). Homosexuality: The ethical challenge.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44, 157-162.
676
Fassinger, R. E., & Richie, B. S. (1997). Sex matters: Gender
and sexual orientation in training for multicultural
competency. In D. B. Pope-Davis & H. L. K. Coleman (Eds.),
Multicultural counseling competencies: Assessment,
education and training, and supervision (pp. 83-110).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
677
Herrell, R., Goldberg, J., True, W. R., Ramakrishnan, V.,
Lyons, M., Eisen, S., & Tsuang, M. T. (1999). Sexual
orientation and suicidality: A co-twin control study in adult
men. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 867-874.
678
Wilson, G. T., & Davison, G. C. (1974). Behavior therapy
and homosexuality: A critical perspective. Behavior Therapy,
5, 16-28.
Footnotes
1
For helpful comments on an early draft of this paper, I thank
Asher Davison. This article is dedicated to the memory of one
of my mentors and a best friend, Perry London.
2
This brings to mind something that Jerome Bruner said in a
lecture in his cognitive psychology course back in 1959. He
was discussing concept formation and how, once we have
attained a concept of something, it is hard to recall what life
was like before that understanding. I think his example was
that we look at a chair, consider what it is, and try to
remember what it looked like before we knew it was a chair.
In an analogous fashion, I came away from my yearlong
clinical apprenticeship with Lazarus with a new
understanding of behavior therapy, different from what I had
had before seeing him in action with patients.
3
In those days behavior modification encompassed — in the
view of many laypersons like Senator Ervin’s committee —
psychosurgery and electroconvulsive shock therapy. The
reason was that these and other techniques modified behavior.
This was the kind of misconception we were dealing with at
the time.
679
4
Sadly, Hank Adams died a few months before the present
paper went to press.
680
681