Stereopsis: Jump To Navigationjump To Search
Stereopsis: Jump To Navigationjump To Search
Contents
1Distinctions
o 1.1Coarse and fine stereopsis
o 1.2Static and dynamic stimuli
o 1.3Research on perception mechanisms
2Prevalence and impact of stereopsis in humans
3History of investigations into stereopsis
4Human stereopsis in popular culture
5Geometrical basis
6Interaction of stereopsis with other depth cues
7Computer stereo vision
8Computer stereo display
9Stereopsis tests
o 9.1Random dot stereotests
o 9.2Contour stereotests
10Deficiency and treatment
11In animals
12See also
13References
14Bibliography
15External links
Distinctions[edit]
Coarse and fine stereopsis[edit]
There are two distinct aspects to stereopsis: coarse stereopsis and fine stereopsis, and
provide depth information of different degree of spatial and temporal precision.
Stereopsis was first explained by Charles Wheatstone in 1838: “… the mind perceives
an object of three dimensions by means of the two dissimilar pictures projected by it on
the two retinæ …”.[34] He recognized that because each eye views the visual world from
slightly different horizontal positions, each eye's image differs from the other. Objects at
different distances from the eyes project images in the two eyes that differ in their
horizontal positions, giving the depth cue of horizontal disparity, also known as retinal
disparity and as binocular disparity. Wheatstone showed that this was an effective depth
cue by creating the illusion of depth from flat pictures that differed only in horizontal
disparity. To display his pictures separately to the two eyes, Wheatstone invented
the stereoscope.
Leonardo da Vinci had also realized that objects at different distances from the eyes
project images in the two eyes that differ in their horizontal positions, but had concluded
only that this made it impossible for a painter to portray a realistic depiction of the depth
in a scene from a single canvas.[35] Leonardo chose for his near object a column with a
circular cross section and for his far object a flat wall. Had he chosen any other near
object, he might have discovered horizontal disparity of its features.[36] His column was
one of the few objects that projects identical images of itself in the two eyes.
Stereoscopy became popular during Victorian times with the invention of the prism
stereoscope by David Brewster. This, combined with photography, meant that tens of
thousands of stereograms were produced.
Until about the 1960s, research into stereopsis was dedicated to exploring its limits and
its relationship to singleness of vision. Researchers included Peter Ludvig
Panum, Ewald Hering, Adelbert Ames Jr., and Kenneth N. Ogle.
In the 1960s, Bela Julesz invented random-dot stereograms.[37] Unlike previous
stereograms, in which each half image showed recognizable objects, each half image of
the first random-dot stereograms showed a square matrix of about 10,000 small dots,
with each dot having a 50% probability of being black or white. No recognizable objects
could be seen in either half image. The two half images of a random-dot stereogram
were essentially identical, except that one had a square area of dots shifted horizontally
by one or two dot diameters, giving horizontal disparity. The gap left by the shifting was
filled in with new random dots, hiding the shifted square. Nevertheless, when the two
half images were viewed one to each eye, the square area was almost immediately
visible by being closer or farther than the background. Julesz whimsically called the
square a Cyclopean image after the mythical Cyclops who had only one eye. This was
because it was as though we have a cyclopean eye inside our brains that can see
cyclopean stimuli hidden to each of our actual eyes. Random-dot stereograms
highlighted a problem for stereopsis, the correspondence problem. This is that any dot
in one half image can realistically be paired with many same-coloured dots in the other
half image. Our visual systems clearly solve the correspondence problem, in that we
see the intended depth instead of a fog of false matches. Research began to
understand how.
Also in the 1960s, Horace Barlow, Colin Blakemore, and Jack
Pettigrew found neurons in the cat visual cortex that had their receptive fields in
different horizontal positions in the two eyes.[38] This established the neural basis for
stereopsis. Their findings were disputed by David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel, although
they eventually conceded when they found similar neurons in the monkey visual
cortex.[39] In the 1980s, Gian Poggio and others found neurons in V2 of the monkey brain
that responded to the depth of random-dot stereograms.[40]
In the 1970s, Christopher Tyler invented autostereograms, random-dot stereograms
that can be viewed without a stereoscope.[41] This led to the popular Magic Eye pictures.
In 1989 Antonio Medina Puerta demonstrated with photographs that retinal images with
no parallax disparity but with different shadows are fused stereoscopically, imparting
depth perception to the imaged scene. He named the phenomenon "shadow
stereopsis". Shadows are therefore an important, stereoscopic cue for depth perception.
He showed how effective the phenomenon is by taking two photographs of the Moon at
different times, and therefore with different shadows, making the Moon to appear in 3D
stereoscopically, despite the absence of any other stereoscopic cue.[42]
Geometrical basis[edit]
Stereopsis appears to be processed in the visual cortex of mammals in binocular
cells having receptive fields in different horizontal positions in the two eyes. Such a cell
is active only when its preferred stimulus is in the correct position in the left eye and in
the correct position in the right eye, making it a disparity detector.
When a person stares at an object, the two eyes converge so that the object appears at
the center of the retina in both eyes. Other objects around the main object appear
shifted in relation to the main object. In the following example, whereas the main object
(dolphin) remains in the center of the two images in the two eyes, the cube is shifted to
the right in the left eye's image and is shifted to the left when in the right eye's image.
The cube is shifted to the right in left The cube is shifted to the left in the
The two eyes converge on the object of
eye's image. right eye's image.
attention.
Stereopsis tests[edit]
In stereopsis tests (short: stereotests), slightly different images are shown to each eye,
such that a 3D image is perceived in case stereovision is present. This can be achieved
by means of vectographs (visible with polarized glasses), anaglyphs (visible with red-
green glasses), lenticular lenses (visible with the naked eye), or head-mounted
display technology. The type of changes from one eye to the other may differ depending
on which level of stereoacuity is to be detected. A series of stereotests for selected
levels thus constitutes a test of stereoacuity.
There are two types of common clinical tests for stereopsis and stereoacuity: random
dot stereotests and contour stereotests. Random-dot stereopsis tests use pictures of
stereo figures that are embedded in a background of random dots. Contour stereotests
use pictures in which the targets presented to each eye are separated horizontally. [49]
Random dot stereotests[edit]
Main article: Random dot stereogram § Random dot stereotests
The ability of stereopsis can be tested by, for example, the Lang stereotest, which
consists of a random-dot stereogram upon which a series of parallel strips of cylindrical
lenses are imprinted in certain shapes, which separate the views seen by each eye in
these areas,[50] similarly to a hologram. Without stereopsis, the image looks only like a
field of random dots, but the shapes become discernible with increasing stereopsis, and
generally consists of a cat (indicating that there is ability of stereopsis of 1200 seconds
of arc of retinal disparity), a star (600 seconds of arc) and a car (550 seconds of
arc).[50] To standardize the results, the image should be viewed at a distance from the
eye of 40 cm and exactly in the frontoparallel plane.[50] There is no need to use special
spectacles for such tests, thereby facilitating use in young children.[50]
Contour stereotests[edit]
Examples of contour stereotests are the Titmus stereotests, the most well-known
example being the Titmus Fly Stereotest, where a picture of a fly is displayed with
disparities on the edges. The patient uses a 3-D glasses to look at the picture and
determine whether a 3-D figure can be seen. The amount of disparity in images vary,
such as 400-100 sec of arc, and 800-40 sec arc.[51]
In animals[edit]
There is good evidence for stereopsis throughout the animal kingdom. It occurs in many
mammals, birds, reptiles, amphibia, fish, crustaceans, spiders, and insects. [1]
See also[edit]
Binocular vision
Stereoscopy
Computer stereo vision
Horopter
Orthoptics
Vectograph
Correspondence problem
Cyclopean image
Epipolar geometry
Stereoblindness
Pupillary distance
References[edit]
1. ^ Jump up to:a b c Howard IP, Rogers BJ (1995). Binocular vision and stereopsis. New York: Oxford
University Press."
2. ^ Howard IP, Rogers BJ (2012). Perceiving in Depth. Volume 3. New York: Oxford University Press."
3. ^ Barry S (2009). Fixing My Gaze: A Scientist's Journey into Seeing in Three Dimensions. New York:
Basic Books. ISBN 9780786744749."
4. ^ Vishwanath D (April 2014). "Toward a new theory of stereopsis". Psychological Review. 121 (2):
151–78. doi:10.1037/a0035233. hdl:10023/5325. PMID 24730596.
5. ^ Jump up to:a b Barry SR (17 December 2012). "Beyond the critical period. Acquiring stereopsis in
adulthood". In Steeves JK, Harris LR (eds.). Plasticity in Sensory Systems. Cambridge University
Press. pp. 187–188. ISBN 978-1-107-02262-1.
6. ^ Craven A, Tran T, Gustafson K, Wu T, So K, Levi D, Li R (2013). "Interocular acuity differences alter
the spatial frequency tuning of stereopsis". Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science. 54 (15):
1518.
7. ^ Narasimhan S, Wilcox L, Solski A, Harrison E, Giaschi D (2012). "Fine and coarse stereopsis follow
different developmental trajectories in children". Journal of Vision. 12 (9): 219. doi:10.1167/12.9.219.
8. ^ Giaschi D, Lo R, Narasimhan S, Lyons C, Wilcox LM (August 2013). "Sparing of coarse stereopsis
in stereodeficient children with a history of amblyopia". Journal of Vision. 13(10):
17. doi:10.1167/13.10.17. PMID 23986537.
9. ^ Meier K, Qiao G, Wilcox LM, Giaschi D (2014). "Coarse stereopsis reveals residual binocular
function in children with strabismus". Journal of Vision. 14 (10): 698. doi:10.1167/14.10.698.
10. ^ Fujikado T, Hosohata J, Ohmi G, Asonuma S, Yamada T, Maeda N, Tano Y (1998). "Use of
dynamic and colored stereogram to measure stereopsis in strabismic patients". Japanese Journal of
Ophthalmology. 42 (2): 101–7. doi:10.1016/S0021-5155(97)00120-2. PMID 9587841.
11. ^ Watanabe Y, Kezuka T, Harasawa K, Usui M, Yaguchi H, Shioiri S (January 2008). "A new method
for assessing motion-in-depth perception in strabismic patients". The British Journal of
Ophthalmology. 92 (1): 47–50. doi:10.1136/bjo.2007.117507. PMID 17596334.
12. ^ Heron S, Lages M (June 2012). "Screening and sampling in studies of binocular vision". Vision
Research. 62: 228–34. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2012.04.012. PMID 22560956.
13. ^ Handa T, Ishikawa H, Nishimoto H, Goseki T, Ichibe Y, Ichibe H, Nobuyuki S, Shimizu K (2010).
"Effect of motion stimulation without changing binocular disparity on stereopsis in strabismus
patients". The American Orthoptic Journal. 60: 87–
94. doi:10.3368/aoj.60.1.87. PMID 21061889. S2CID 23428336.
14. ^ Jump up to:a b Wilcox LM, Allison RS (November 2009). "Coarse-fine dichotomies in human
stereopsis". Vision Research. 49 (22): 2653–
65. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2009.06.004. PMID 19520102. S2CID 11575053.
15. ^ Tyler CW (1990). "A stereoscopic view of visual processing streams". Vision Research. 30(11):
1877–95. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(90)90165-H. PMID 2288096. S2CID 23713665.
16. ^ Stidwill D, Fletcher R (8 November 2010). Normal Binocular Vision: Theory, Investigation and
Practical Aspects. John Wiley & Sons. p. 164. ISBN 978-1-4051-9250-7.
17. ^ See the interpretation of statements by Bela Julesz provided in: Leonard J. Press: The Dual Nature
of Stereopsis – Part 6 (downloaded 8 September 2014)
18. ^ Hildreth EC, Royden CS (October 2011). "Integrating multiple cues to depth order at object
boundaries". Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics. 73 (7): 2218–35. doi:10.3758/s13414-011-0172-
0. PMID 21725706.
19. ^ Domini F, Caudek C, Tassinari H (May 2006). "Stereo and motion information are not independently
processed by the visual system". Vision Research. 46 (11): 1707–
23. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2005.11.018. PMID 16412492.
20. ^ For dynamic disparity processing, see also Patterson R (2009). "Unresolved issues in stereopsis:
dynamic disparity processing". Spatial Vision. 22 (1): 83–
90. doi:10.1163/156856809786618510. PMID 19055888.
21. ^ Ban H, Preston TJ, Meeson A, Welchman AE (February 2012). "The integration of motion and
disparity cues to depth in dorsal visual cortex". Nature Neuroscience. 15 (4): 636–
43. doi:10.1038/nn.3046. PMC 3378632. PMID 22327475.
22. ^ Fine I, Jacobs RA (August 1999). "Modeling the combination of motion, stereo, and vergence angle
cues to visual depth". Neural Computation. 11 (6): 1297–
330. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.24.284. doi:10.1162/089976699300016250. PMID 10423497. S2CID 1397246.
23. ^ Coutant BE, Westheimer G (January 1993). "Population distribution of stereoscopic
ability". Ophthalmic & Physiological Optics. 13 (1): 3–7. doi:10.1111/j.1475-
1313.1993.tb00419.x. PMID 8510945.
24. ^ Mazyn LI, Lenoir M, Montagne G, Savelsbergh GJ (August 2004). "The contribution of stereo vision
to one-handed catching" (PDF). Experimental Brain Research. 157 (3): 383–90. doi:10.1007/s00221-
004-1926-x. hdl:1871/29156. PMID 15221161. S2CID 6615928.
25. ^ Elshatory YM, Siatkowski RM (2014). "Wiley Post, around the world with no stereopsis". Survey of
Ophthalmology. 59 (3): 365–72. doi:10.1016/j.survophthal.2013.08.001. PMID 24359807.
26. ^ Biddle M, Hamid S, Ali N (February 2014). "An evaluation of stereoacuity (3D vision) in practising
surgeons across a range of surgical specialities". The Surgeon. 12 (1): 7–
10. doi:10.1016/j.surge.2013.05.002. PMID 23764432.
27. ^ Bauer A, Dietz K, Kolling G, Hart W, Schiefer U (July 2001). "The relevance of stereopsis for
motorists: a pilot study". Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology. 239 (6): 400–
6. doi:10.1007/s004170100273. PMID 11561786. S2CID 6288004.
28. ^ Rubin GS, Ng ES, Bandeen-Roche K, Keyl PM, Freeman EE, West SK (April 2007). "A prospective,
population-based study of the role of visual impairment in motor vehicle crashes among older drivers:
the SEE study". Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science. 48 (4): 1483–91. doi:10.1167/iovs.06-
0474. PMID 17389475.
29. ^ Pineles SL, Velez FG, Isenberg SJ, Fenoglio Z, Birch E, Nusinowitz S, Demer JL (November
2013). "Functional burden of strabismus: decreased binocular summation and binocular
inhibition". JAMA Ophthalmology. 131 (11): 1413–
9. doi:10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2013.4484. PMC 4136417. PMID 24052160.
30. ^ Damian McNamara (2013-09-23). "Strabismus study reveals visual function deficits". Medscape
Medical News.
31. ^ Strabismus, by All About Vision, Access Media Group LLC
32. ^ Bradley A, Barrett BT, Saunders KJ (March 2014). "Linking binocular vision neuroscience with
clinical practice". Ophthalmic & Physiological Optics. 34 (2): 125–
8. doi:10.1111/opo.12125. hdl:10454/10180. PMID 24588530.
33. ^ Sandra Blakeslee: A Defect That May Lead to a Masterpiece, New York Times, New York edition,
page D6, 14 June 2011 (online June 13, 2001; downloaded 22 July 2013)
34. ^ Jump up to:a b Contributions to the Physiology of Vision. – Part the First. On some remarkable, and
hitherto unobserved, Phenomena of Binocular Vision. By CHARLES WHEATSTONE, F.R.S.,
Professor of Experimental Philosophy in King's College, London.
35. ^ Beck J (1979). Leonardo's rules of painting. Oxford: Phaidon Press. ISBN 978-0-7148-2056-9.
36. ^ Wade NJ (1987). "On the late invention of the stereoscope". Perception. 16 (6): 785–
818. doi:10.1068/p160785. PMID 3331425. S2CID 24998020.
37. ^ Julesz, B. (1960). "Binocular depth perception of computer-generated images". Bell System
Technical Journal. 39 (5): 1125–1163. doi:10.1002/j.1538-7305.1960.tb03954.x.
38. ^ Barlow HB, Blakemore C, Pettigrew JD (November 1967). "The neural mechanism of binocular
depth discrimination". The Journal of Physiology. 193 (2): 327–
42. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1967.sp008360. PMC 1365600. PMID 6065881.
39. ^ Hubel DH, Wiesel TN (January 1970). "Stereoscopic vision in macaque monkey. Cells sensitive to
binocular depth in area 18 of the macaque monkey cortex". Nature. 225 (5227): 41–
2. Bibcode:1970Natur.225...41H. doi:10.1038/225041a0. PMID 4983026. S2CID 4265895.
40. ^ Poggio GF, Motter BC, Squatrito S, Trotter Y (1985). "Responses of neurons in visual cortex (V1
and V2) of the alert macaque to dynamic random-dot stereograms". Vision Research. 25 (3): 397–
406. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(85)90065-3. PMID 4024459. S2CID 43335583.
41. ^ Tyler CW, Clarke MB (1990). "The autostereogram, Stereoscopic Displays and Applications". Proc.
SPIE. Stereoscopic Displays and Applications. 1258: 182–
196. Bibcode:1990SPIE.1256..182T. doi:10.1117/12.19904.
42. ^ Medina Puerta A (February 1989). "The power of shadows: shadow stereopsis". Journal of the
Optical Society of America A. 6 (2): 309–
11. Bibcode:1989JOSAA...6..309M. doi:10.1364/JOSAA.6.000309. PMID 2926527.
43. ^ Ogle KN (1950). Researchers in binocular vision. New York: Hafner Publishing Company. [page needed]
44. ^ Buckthought A, Kim J, Wilson HR (March 2008). "Hysteresis effects in stereopsis and binocular
rivalry". Vision Research. 48 (6): 819–
30. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2007.12.013. PMID 18234273. S2CID 17092588.
45. ^ Fender D, Julesz B (June 1967). "Extension of Panum's fusional area in binocularly stabilized
vision". Journal of the Optical Society of America. 57 (6): 819–
30. doi:10.1364/josa.57.000819. PMID 6038008.
46. ^ Piantanida TP (1986). "Stereo hysteresis revisited". Vision Research. 26 (3): 431–
7. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(86)90186-0. PMID 3727409. S2CID 7601967.
47. ^ Duwaer AL (May 1983). "Patent stereopsis with diplopia in random-dot stereograms". Perception &
Psychophysics. 33 (5): 443–54. doi:10.3758/bf03202895. PMID 6877990.
48. ^ Levanda R, Leshem A (2010). "Synthetic aperture radio telescopes". IEEE Signal Processing
Magazine. 27 (1): 14–
29. arXiv:1009.0460. Bibcode:2010ISPM...27...14L. doi:10.1109/MSP.2009.934719. S2CID 4695794.
49. ^ Stereoacuity testing, ONE Network, American Academy of Phthalmology (downloaded 2 September
2014)
50. ^ Jump up to:a b c d Lang stereotest in Farlex medical dictionary. In turn citing: Millodot: Dictionary of
Optometry and Visual Science, 7th edition.
51. ^ Kalloniatis, Michael (1995). "Perception of Depth". The Organization of the Retina and Visual
System. PMID 21413376. Retrieved 9 April 2012.
52. ^ "vision therapy". The Canadian Association of Optometrists.
53. ^ Levi DM (June 2012). "Prentice award lecture 2011: removing the brakes on plasticity in the
amblyopic brain". Optometry and Vision Science. 89 (6): 827–
38. doi:10.1097/OPX.0b013e318257a187. PMC 3369432. PMID 22581119.
54. ^ Xi J, Jia WL, Feng LX, Lu ZL, Huang CB (April 2014). "Perceptual learning improves stereoacuity in
amblyopia". Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science. 55 (4): 2384–91. doi:10.1167/iovs.13-
12627. PMC 3989086. PMID 24508791.
Bibliography[edit]
Julesz, B. (1971). Foundations of cyclopean perception. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press
Steinman, Scott B. & Steinman, Barbara A. & Garzia, Ralph Philip
(2000). Foundations of Binocular Vision: A Clinical perspective. McGraw-Hill
Medical. ISBN 0-8385-2670-5.
Howard, I. P., & Rogers, B. J. (2012). Perceiving in depth. Volume 2, Stereoscopic
vision. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-976415-0
Cabani, I. (2007). Segmentation et mise en correspondance couleur – Application:
étude et conception d'un système de stéréovision couleur pour l'aide à la conduite
automobile. ISBN 978-613-1-52103-4
External links[edit]
Middlebury Stereo Vision Page
VIP Laparoscopic / Endoscopic Video Dataset (stereo medical images)
What is Stereo Vision?
Learn about Stereograms then make your own Magic Eye
International Orthoptic Association