Grevenstein 2019 EVOS Resilience
Grevenstein 2019 EVOS Resilience
net/publication/332268544
CITATIONS READS
6 284
4 authors, including:
Corina Aguilar-Raab
Universität Heidelberg
60 PUBLICATIONS 145 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
Die MUSED-Studie: Musiktherapie bei Depressionen: Eine randomisiert kontrollierte Studie zur Evaluation der Wirksamkeit von Musiktherapie bei depressiven Frauen
View project
All content following this page was uploaded by Dennis Grevenstein on 14 May 2019.
PII: S2212-6570(17)30120-4
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mph.2019.200160
Article Number: 200160
Reference: MHP 200160
Please cite this article as: Dennis Grevenstein , Matthias Bluemke , Jochen Schweitzer ,
Corina Aguilar-Raab , Better Family Relationships – Higher Well-Being: The Connection Between
Relationship Quality and Health Related Resources, Mental Health & Prevention (2019), doi:
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mph.2019.200160
This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service
to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo
copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please
note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and
all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1
Psychological Institute, University of Heidelberg
T
2
GESIS – Leibniz Institute for the Social Sciences
IP
3
Institute of Medical Psychology, University Hospital Heidelberg
CR
US
AN
Word count: 8695
M
ED
PT
Acknowledgment: This research was in part supported by a grant from Heidehof foundation.
CE
Thanks go to Magdalena Hornung and Christina Baacke for help with the data collection.
Abstract
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Family relationships form very important social relationships. They provide the social context
enabling the development for a healthy personality and fostering social competencies and the
capacity for social adjustment. Several constructs constitute a complex sample of health
beneficial attributes, such as resilience, sense of coherence, self-compassion and others, that
haven’t been investigated in connection with perceived quality of family relationships and
T
relationships – assessed with a relatively new measure: the Evaluation of Social Systems
IP
Scale – was associated with these advantageous health-related qualities, additionally
CR
confirming EVOS’ construct validity. In study 1 (N = 207) and 2 (N = 305) university
US
heterogeneous sample took part in an online-survey. Controlling for participants’ age and sex,
AN
better family relationships were associated with reduced psychological distress (r = .30 to
.37), more satisfaction with life (r = .40), stronger resilience (r = .37), sense of coherence (r
M
= .37), self-compassion (r = .33), optimism (r = .32), general self-efficacy (r = .27), and self-
esteem (r = .34). Results highlight the importance of the family environment not only for
ED
psychological health and quality of life, but also for individual adaptation and well-being. In
PT
future research, this should be especially addressed in designing and providing preventative
well-being.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1. Introduction
social relationships assist people to cope with stress and stay well (Berkman, 2000; Landstedt,
Hammarström, & Winefield, 2015; Uchino, 2006). In longitudinal studies, good social
relationships predicted mental health, interpersonal functioning (Paradis et al., 2011), and
even longevity (Kern, Della Porta, & Friedman, 2014). Strong social relationships, indicated
T
by good social integration, increase the likelihood for survival by 50% (Holt-Lunstad, Smith,
IP
& Layton, 2010). Loneliness and social isolation can be understood as risk factors for poor
CR
health outcomes leading to a reduction of longevity, especially for those under 65 years of age
(Holt-Lunstad & Smith, 2012; Holt-Lunstad, Smith, Baker, Harris, & Stephenson, 2015).
Hampson, 2000; Cohen, 2004; Epstein, Bishop, Ryan, Miller, & Keitner, 1993; Epstein,
M
Bishop, & Levin, 1978; I. Miller, Ryan, Keitner, Bishop, & Epstein, 2000; A. K. Ryan &
Willits, 2007; Umberson & Karas Montez, 2010). Social bonds between children and their
ED
parents can be regarded as social capital – including norms and values – which may be as
important as financial and human capital for health and well-being (Parcel & Bixby, 2016).
PT
have been associated with better psychological health and individual flourishing (Barber &
Harmon, 2002; Dunn, McLaughlin, Slopen, Rosand, & Smoller, 2013; Finchham & Beach,
AC
2010). A need to belong to and to be related to other people forms a basic motive in human
psychological need (R. M. Ryan & Deci, 2000). Complementing, rather than opposing
relatedness, a need for autonomy is considered to be a desire to be a causal agent of one's life
and a preference to choose for oneself. In the best possible way, these basic needs can be met
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
in a flourishing and high-quality relationship context (Deci & Ryan, 2000; Lavigne,
Family is a social system and at the same time provides a context for development
(Henry, Sheffield Morris, & Harrist, 2015). It is often thought to shape an individual’s life
& Turkewitz, 2012). Family systems, their members and their relational ties, are reciprocal in
T
nature and are characterized as dynamic and interdependent (Henry et al., 2015; Masten &
IP
Monn, 2015). Not only health and adaptation, but personality itself is thought to develop
CR
under the influence of family members from early childhood on (Bornstein, 2006; Pomerantz
US
forming an individual’s identity on the one hand and building relatedness as well as
AN
nourishing relationships on the other hand (Diehl, Elnick, Bourbeau, & Labouvie-Vief, 1998).
Family members offer regulative functions. In healthy contexts, parents co-regulate their
M
children by being sensitive, responsive and caring. They structure, validate and stabilize
within the family during childhood and adolescence on relationships to significant others in
PT
later life (Donnellan, Larsen-Rife, & Conger, 2005). Based on a cohesive child-parent
CE
relationship, feelings of support and closeness foster greater social competencies and social
adjustment (Allen, Moore, Kuperminc, & Bell, 1998; Smetana, Campione-Barr, & Metzger,
AC
2006), and are associated with higher well-being in adolescence (Jose, Ryan, & Pryor, 2012).
Frequent and positive family involvement, emotional self-disclosure, and a loving expression
of affection create a positive family atmosphere, which has been related to reduced symptom
development (O'Brien et al., 2006). Although family life is characterized by highly fluctuating
challenges and tasks – as the developing child has ever-changing needs – social support,
collective decision making, and frequent communication and interactions buffer against stress
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
and the development of psychopathology (Ackard, Neumark-Sztainer, Story, & Perry, 2006;
Laursen & Collins, 2004; Phillips-Salimi, Robb, Monahan, Dossey, & Haase, 2014). Taken
together, high family relationship quality and good family functioning have shown numerous
health benefits in the past. Family resilience is fostered by a process of flexible, collective
adaption. As a system, families contribute to development of health and may constitute both a
protective factor as well as a potential vulnerability (Cicchetti, 2013; Masten & Monn, 2015).
T
Still, the connection between a resilient family with flourishing family relations and well-
IP
adapted individual needs further clarification.
CR
1.2. Resilience and Individual Adaptation
Individual characteristics that promote health and well-being are generally described
US
as inner strength and resilience (Patterson, 2002). There is no definite consensus about what
AN
exactly contributes to a resilient disposition, yet a number of psychological constructs drawn
from diverse theoretical perspectives have been proposed over the years. These are supposed
M
to reflect how some individual’s seem to be spared the effects of hardships and critical life
events on health. Among the most prominent are resilience itself (Wagnild & Young, 1993),
ED
sense of coherence (Antonovsky, 1987), dispositional optimism (Carver, 2014; Carver &
Scheier, 2014), general self-efficacy (Schwarzer, 2014), self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1965), and
PT
effects of stress. It promotes adaptation to stressful situations (Wagnild & Young, 1993;
AC
Windle, 2011). Family and individual child resilience are interconnected, and lead to an
overall capacity to maintain functionality in the light of adverse life experiences. A number of
scales aim to measure resilience. Windle and colleagues reviewed nineteen different scales
and concluded that ―the conceptual and theoretical adequacy of a number of the scales was
questionable‖ (Windle, Bennett, & Noyes, 2011). One of the most widely used measures is
the Resilience Scale by Wagnild and Young (1993). This conception of resilience
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
reliance, the trust in oneself and one’s capabilities; and meaningfulness, the view that life has
purpose and the valuation of one’s contributions; and existential aloneness, the realization that
each person’s life path is unique. Contrasting many other health beneficial construct,
resilience is often thought to stem from the successful coping with negative experience
T
(Windle, 2011). Resilience is therefore often connected to the concept of post-traumatic
IP
growth (Jayawickreme & Blackie, 2014). Nonetheless, some individuals possess the ability to
CR
cope with stress without having endured significant negative experiences. The idea of the
―invulnerable children‖ reflects that some people appear to be able to cope with any stressful
US
event. Fonagy and colleagues argued that resilience is established by early childhood
AN
experiences and interactions (Fonagy, Steele, Steele, Higgitt, & Target, 1994). Thus,
regardless of traumatizing experiences in later life, the foundation for resilience may already
M
resistance resource. Sense of coherence promotes health when people are faced with stressful
PT
life events. It encompasses three major facets: Comprehensibility denotes a person’s tendency
CE
to perceive situations and events as clear and structured. Manageability represents the belief
to possess the necessary skills to deal with life challenges. Meaningfulness describes an
AC
individual’s conviction that the demands and challenges of life are worthy of investment and
social support, but also wealth, education, or even intelligence (Antonovsky, 1987).
denotes a person’s ability to use them (Lindström, 2001). In the past, sense of coherence has
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Jungaberle, 2016).
positive attitude towards life and its challenges (Scheier & Carver, 1985). Optimists tend to
T
expect positive outcomes and often cope better with failure. As a self-regulatory resource,
IP
optimism promotes persistence in the face of challenges and more adaptive and active coping
CR
with stress. It is related to motivational processes in engaging in health-promoting behaviors.
Current research suggests that dispositional optimism also leads to stronger commitment in
US
social relationship for example in terms of more constructive problem solving (Carver &
AN
Scheier, 2014).
judgmental and receptive state of mind, which fosters a clear perception of challenging
ED
situations and one’s own thoughts and emotions, ultimately leading to a better mental state
(Keng, Smoski, & Robins, 2011). Beyond that however, self-compassion describes the
PT
tendency not only to be open and moved by one’s own suffering, but also to confront life
CE
and lead to feelings of caring and kindness towards oneself. Additionally, an individual
AC
should perceive her own experience in the context of common human experience.
self-efficacy across a broad range of challenging situations that require effort and
perseverance and thus goes beyond specific situations (Schwarzer & Jerusalem, 1995). It is an
important concept for health development in youth. For example, Di Giunta et al. (2018)
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
showed that lower self-efficacy in adolescents was associated with greater internalizing and
externalizing problems. Furthermore, current research indicates that the development of self-
efficacy is domain-sensitive and that girls more often than boys rely on social sources in this
of one’s own worth. Global self-esteem describes a general judgment of oneself and includes
T
positive and negative evaluations of one’s competencies, abilities, and other aspects
IP
connected to the self (Rosenberg, 1965).
CR
The presented constructs constitute a complex sample of health beneficial attributes.
Coming from different theoretical backgrounds, some constructs are broader and some are
US
more narrowly defined. Some are thought to be rather stable and trait-like, whereas others are
AN
considered to be improvable by training or intervention. Still, higher standing on each of these
characteristics has been found to be associated with better psychological health and well-
M
being. For instance, sense of coherence has been consistently associated with various health
outcomes, such as good mental health and health-related behavior (Eriksson & Lindström,
ED
2006), general psychological well-being (Grevenstein & Bluemke, 2015; Nilsson, Leppert,
Simonsson, & Starrin, 2010), depression (Haukkala et al., 2013), and anxiety (Moksnes,
PT
Espnes, & Haugan, 2013). Likewise, general self-efficacy has been related to health and
CE
self-efficacy covers aspects of self-regulation (Geyer, 1997). Low self-esteem has been found
to be a vulnerability factor for the development of depression (Orth, Robins, & Roberts, 2008;
Rieger, Göllner, Trautwein, & Roberts, 2016). Furthermore, resilience has shown negative
associations with depression and anxiety, and positive correlations with life satisfaction as
well (Ahern, Kiehl, Lou Sole, & Byers, 2006). Also, optimism plays an important role in self-
regulation. Optimists not only cope better with failure, but are more persistent in the face of
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
challenges and engage in more adaptive and active coping with stress. Unsurprisingly,
dispositional optimism was found to be related to various positive mental as well as physical
health outcomes (Carver & Scheier, 2014). Finally, much like the other constructs, self-
compassion displayed negative associations with depression and anxiety, and positive
correlations with life satisfaction (Neff, Rude, & Kirkpatrick, 2007). High levels of self-
compassion were also related to a more supportive and positive interaction style in close
T
relationships (Neff & Beretvas, 2013).
IP
1.3. The Present Research
CR
The present research explores the bi-directional relationships between quality of
family relations, health beneficial individual differences, psychological distress, and general
US
well-being. We applied a relatively new, short, and psychometrically sound measure to assess
AN
most relevant aspects of social functioning: Quality of relationship and collective efficacy.
These two aspects represent affective and cognitive facets of social functioning, which are
M
In EVOS, the following dimensions characterize the two aspects: Communication, cohesion,
ED
atmosphere, giving and taking as well as collective aims, resources, decisions, solutions
findings and adaptability. All of them have been advanced in theories describing and
PT
explaining social functioning (Bandura, Caprara, Barbaranelli, Regalia, & Scabini, 2011;
CE
Beavers, 1981; Epstein et al., 1978; Olson, 1986) – those of which are especially important in
family relationships. The domain of collective efficacy encompasses important aspects of how
AC
a system works together in order to pursue goals, handle resources, make decisions, find
solutions to problems and adapt to new challenges. Bandura (2000) aptly described his
influence the types of futures they seek to achieve through collective action, how well they
use their resources, how much effort they put into their group endeavor, their staying power
when collective efforts fail to produce quick results or meet forcible opposition, and their
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
vulnerability to the discouragement that can beset people taking on tough social problems‖
(p.76). Going beyond a simple, context specific idea of self-efficacy, collective efficacy
captures a key element of systemic thinking, as it describe a system's ability to create unique
solutions.
We assumed that satisfying and positive social relationships help to promote high
adaptation in individuals, and possibly vice-versa. Our study will shed more light onto the
T
association between quality of family relationships and collective self-efficacy as well as a
IP
variety of characteristics promoting health and well-being, which have not yet been
CR
investigated together.
US
2. Methods
AN
2.1. Participants and Study Design
The present research includes three separate studies conducted in Germany. All three
M
focused on the quality of family relationships in connection with sense of coherence and
psychological distress. We did not want to overstrain the study participants with regard to the
ED
duration of the survey, but nevertheless we aimed to shed light on different health-relevant
resilience, and psychological distress; Study 2 assessed family relationship quality, sense of
CE
Study 1 included N = 207 (54.1% female) participants with a mean age of 22.56 years
(SD = 4.25; range = 18 - 50). Most participants (91.8%) were German students of Psychology
and Medicine. About 7.2% were employed persons and 1.0% were trainees. Participants in
study 2 were N = 305 (71.5% female) individuals (Mage = 24.40; SD = 9.50; range = 15 - 64).
The majority were university students (84.9%). The others were volunteering employees
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
(10.5%), high school students (1.0%), unemployed persons (1.0%), trainees (0.3%), or
individuals reporting ―other‖ occupations (2.3%). N = 528 (72% female) took part in study 3
(Mage = 31.24; SD =13.30; range = 14 - 76). Even though university students (46.8%) again
constituted the largest subgroup, the sample was much more diverse. Employed persons
(33.5%), unemployed persons (2.5%), trainees (3.8%), high school students (2.3%), or
individuals reporting ―other‖ occupations (11.2%) summed up to more than half of the
T
sample.
IP
Studies 1 and 2 were conducted locally, with participants filling out paper
CR
questionnaires. Participants were predominantly volunteers (students at the local university)
who participated out of curiosity and for the sake of science without compensation. Potential
US
participants were invited and briefed by a researcher or student assistant about what the study
AN
procedures included, stressing that participation was entirely voluntary. Motivated
team, which included formal consent to study participation. To broaden the sample
recruited via social media sites (i.e., Facebook) and local email lists. As a token of
collected. They were informed that answering the questionnaires automatically entails
voluntary consent to study participation. The underlying research project was approved by the
AC
2.2. Measures
relationship quality was assessed in all three studies using EVOS (Aguilar-Raab et al., 2018;
Aguilar-Raab, Grevenstein, & Schweitzer, 2015). Prior research has demonstrated that EVOS
self-report measure in its German and translated English versions (Aguilar-Raab et al., 2015).
Factorial validity and construct validity were supported with regard to convergent and
discriminant validity with overlapping and theoretically distant other self-report measures. In
adult samples EVOS has shown positive correlations with measures of life satisfaction,
psychological distress and functioning (Aguilar-Raab et al., 2018). EVOS has also been
T
validated in youth populations and has comparably shown positive correlations with family
IP
functioning and negative associations with psychological distress (Grevenstein, Schweitzer, &
CR
Aguilar-Raab, 2018). It is based on systems theory and models of functionality and
relationships in families and other contexts (Epstein et al., 1978) as well as Bandura’s concept
US
of collective efficacy (Bandura et al., 2011). Four items assess the quality of a social system –
AN
quality of the relationship focusing the emotional or affective level (e.g., ―For me, the way we
talk with each other, is …‖). Five items are applied for the rather cognitive evaluation of
M
family functioning or collective efficacy of the system (e.g., ―For me, how we adapt to
change, is …‖). Answers on 4-point rating scales range from 0 = very poor to 3 = very good.
ED
Cronbach’s Alpha was .92 in study 1, .93 in study 2, and .93 in study 3.
original Orientation to Life scale in all three studies (Schumacher, Wilz, Gunzelmann, &
CE
Brähler, 2000). The German sense of coherence scale was validated and standardized in a
representative sample (Schumacher, Gunzelmann, & Brähler, 2000; Schumacher, Wilz, et al.,
AC
2000). In a systematic review, Eriksson and Lindström (2005) concluded that the sense of
coherence scale is a reliable and valid measure. It includes five comprehensibility items (e.g.,
―Has it happened in the past that you were surprised by the behavior of people whom you
thought you knew well?‖), four manageability items (e.g., ―Has it happened that people whom
you counted on disappointed you?‖), and four meaningfulness items (e.g., ―Do you have the
feeling that you don’t really care about what goes on around you?‖). Answers were given on
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
7-point rating scales marked from 1 = very often to 7 = very seldom or never. Cronbach’s
2.2.3. RS-13: Resilience. In study 1 we used the 13-item German version (Leppert,
Koch, Brähler, & Strauß, 2008) of the Wagnild and Young Resilience Scale (Wagnild &
Young, 1993). The German version has been validated in terms of factorial validity and
T
concurrent validity of the original scale were supported by the study of Wagnild and Young
IP
(1993). Nine items assess personal competence (e.g., ―When I make plans I follow through
CR
with them.‖) and four items reflect acceptance of self and life (e.g., ―It’s okay if there are
people who don’t like me.‖). Answers were given on 7-point rating scales marked from 1 =
US
no, do not agree at all to 7 = yes, totally agree. Cronbach’s Alpha was .82.
AN
2.2.4. LOT-R: Dispositional optimism. We used a German adaption of the revised
life-orientation-test with ten items in study 2 (Glaesmer, Hoyer, Klotsche, & Herzberg, 2008).
M
The original scale was constructed by Scheier, Carver, and Bridges (1994). It is reported that
it possess adequate predictive and discriminant validity. The scale includes three positively
ED
worded and three negatively framed items. The remaining four are filler items. Following
Carver and Scheier’s (2014) view of optimism as a unidimensional construct, negative items
PT
were recoded and a mean score was computed. Answers were given on 5-point Likert scales
CE
2011) of Neff’s original self-compassion scale in study 2, which was shown to be valid in
terms of positive correlations with mental health outcomes such as higher life satisfaction and
negative correlations with for example neurotic perfectionism (Neff, 2003a). The scale
calculated by first computing mean scores for each subscale. Negatively scored subscales are
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
recoded before aggregating across subscales. Answers were given on 5-point scales marked
from 1 = very rarely to 5 = very often. Alpha across all items of the total scale amounted to
.89.
Schwarzer & Jerusalem in study 3 (Schwarzer & Jerusalem, 1995, 1999). Being a uni-
dimensional and universal construct based on data from 25 countries, the scale has shown
T
sound psychometric properties including a variety of validity aspects (Scholz, Doña, Sud, &
IP
Schwarzer, 2002). It includes ten items such as ―If there are challenges, I can find a way to
CR
succeed.‖ Answers were given on 4-point scales marked from 1 = not true to 4 = completely
US
2.2.7. Self-esteem. A revised German version of the Rosenberg self-esteem scale was
AN
used in study 3 (von Collani & Herzberg, 2003). Its criterion and convergent validity is
reported to be satisfactory (Ferring & Filipp, 1996). It comprises ten items such as ―On the
M
whole, I am satisfied with myself.‖ Answers were given on 4-point scales marked from 1 =
& Fitzpatrick, 2004; Franke, 2002). Several original studies positively evaluated its
CE
predictive, convergent, and construct validity (Derogatis & Unger, 2010). The SCL-90-R is a
general measure of psychopathology and includes ninety items in nine different subscales.
AC
The Global Severity Index (GSI) can be computed as a mean score over all items representing
a measure of overall psychological distress. Participants provided answers on how much they
suffered from various symptoms on 5-point scales marked 0 = not at all, 1 = a bit, 2 =
considerably, 3 = much, 4 = very much. Cronbach’s Alpha for the total scale was .96 in study
psychological distress (Klaghofer & Brähler, 2001). It is a brief 9-item version of the original
SCL-90-R using the same 5-point scale. In its developmental phase, from each of the nine
subscales of the SCL-90-R, one item with the highest item-to-total correlation was selected.
The SCL-K-9 has been presented as a convergent valid measure with a correlation of r = .93
between the short SCL-K-9 and the full SCL-90-R global severity index in a representative
T
survey (Klaghofer & Brähler, 2001) Cronbach’s Alpha was .85 in our sample.
IP
2.2.10. SWLS: Satisfaction with life. In study 3 we used a German adaption of the
CR
Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS). The scale has shown adequate construct validity in the
original and German version as well (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985; Glaesmer,
US
Grande, Braehler, & Roth, 2011). It is a short 5-item measure of a global judgment of
AN
satisfaction with one’s own life including items such as ―I am satisfied with my life‖.
Answers were given on 7-point scales marked from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly
M
3. Results
We initially explored if men and woman differed on any of the study variables.
PT
0.47 vs. 0.37, SDs = 0.37 vs. 0.28) and more satisfaction with life in study 3 (t = 2.25, df =
526, p = .03, Ms = 5.04 vs. 4.78, SDs = 1.19 vs. 1.22). There were no differences on other
AC
variables. Descriptive statistics are displayed in Table 1. These results indicated that our
participants used the whole range of most scales and some individuals reported high
We computed partial correlations controlling for participants’ age and sex between
family relationship quality and the other variables. As hypothesized, the quality of family
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
relations was significantly correlated with all criteria. Then, we compared the strength of the
various associations. Differences emerged only in study 3. Family relationship quality was
less strongly related to general self-efficacy than to any other variable, all Zs > 2.03, all ps <
.05. In light of the number of comparisons, this may be a spurious finding. Nonetheless, even
after applying Bonferroni correction (adjusting the significance level to p = .005), two
comparisons still remained significant. Family relations were more strongly associated with
T
life satisfaction than with general self-efficacy (Z = 3.27, p = .001) and more strongly related
IP
to sense of coherence than to general self-efficacy (Z = 2.82, p < .005).
CR
The general pattern suggests that family relationship quality was consistently, and
about equally, associated with all criteria, sharing 7% to 16% of the variance between the
US
predictor and each criterion. General self-efficacy appeared slightly less connected to the
AN
quality of family relationships.
4. Discussion
ED
The present research investigated the association between family relationship quality
and health beneficial individual differences, as well as psychological distress and satisfaction
PT
with life. Our results confirmed the association between family relations and psychological
CE
health. People with better family relations reported less psychological distress and more life
satisfaction. Thus, social interactions indeed appear as an important predictor for health and
AC
well-being (G. Miller, Chen, & Cole, 2009; Rusbult & Van Lange, 2003). The need to belong
or relatedness as one of the three basic needs defined within the framework of the Self-
Determination Theory (R. M. Ryan & Deci, 2000) is an important factor for a healthy
development in a social context of a family: For example, an adolescent child can become
independent in its striving for autonomy only on the basis of a secure bond or attachment
(Mattanah, Lopez, & Govern, 2011; Moretti & Peled, 2004). While distancing itself from
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
parental caregivers its feeling to be autonomous only makes it possible to experience itself as
(self-)effective and competent. Thus, it becomes clear how relatedness, autonomy and
competence are mutually dependent. Self-regulatory mechanisms and health-related traits are
Beyond that, correlations between family relationship quality and aspects of positive
T
adaptation have emerged. As hypothesized better family relations indicated higher sense of
IP
coherence, resilience, optimism, self-compassion, general self-efficacy, and self-esteem.
CR
These associations are far from trivial. For example, family relations in adolescence have
US
because the family context provides meaningful experiences for the general increase in sense
AN
of coherence through puberty and adolescence (Grevenstein & Bluemke, 2017) as well as an
& Jiménez-Iglesias, 2012). More generally, social relationships aid the development of self-
ED
regulation skills in adolescence (Farley & Kim-Spoon, 2014). Complementary factors, such as
a lack of positive interactions and low parental involvement, have been identified as a risk
PT
factor for severe personality dysfunction (Fruzzetti, Shenk, & Hoffman, 2005).
CE
EVOS is a relatively new measure capturing important dimensions relevant for family
relationships. It was created to assess quality of relationship and collective efficacy, both of
AC
which are related to individual health (Aguilar-Raab et al., 2018). The new evidence
presented here supports the construct validity of EVOS (Cronbach & Meehl, 1955).
Specifically, good family relations are clearly related to individual adaptation and resilience.
individual resources and enabling people to cope with adverse life events.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
be rather unspecific, because family relations were correlated to all individual differences and
health variables to a similar extent. The similarity of these associations indicates that future
studies will need to look at more specific outcomes and the detailed mechanisms of the effects
of family relations.
The results presented here have implications for prevention strategies in the family
T
context: In the sense of behavioral preventive aspects, it seems worthwhile to strengthen
IP
families, especially parents, in gaining and maintaining a nurturing, benevolent togetherness,
CR
in which belonging, cohesion and trust are addressed in a special way. The creation of
closeness that also allows for an individual striving for autonomy is particularly important
US
from a developmental psychological point of view. Affective co-regulative mechanisms, in
AN
which the sense of coherence, individual but also collective/familial resilience, self-
compassion and other personality traits are strengthened, seem to be meaningful strategies
M
here. Currently, mindfulness- and compassion-based interventions for parents, caregivers and
families can be considered in this regard pointing towards a promising direction (Duncan,
ED
Coatsworth, & Greenberg, 2009; Gehart & McCollum, 2007; Kirby, 2016).
4.1. Limitations
PT
correlational design of the set of studies. Hence, we can neither declare that family
relationship quality has a direct effect on positive adaptation or health, nor that resilience of
AC
family members determines the quality of family interactions. In addition to the potential for
bidirectional causality, there is the potential of a third factor being causal. Relationships
between family relationship quality, health, and personality might be reciprocal, at least in the
long run (Alferi, Carver, Antoni, Weiss, & Durán, 2001). Therefore, as much as family
nurtures an individual’s characteristics, some people with certain characteristics may also
engage more skillfully in positive social relationships. Only longitudinal data and complex
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
research designs (cross-lagged panel designs; Kenny, 1975, 2014) can estimate the relative
size of the influence of both causal paths. In fact, family systems theory proposes that families
are constituted by highly interdependent relationships between family members with unique
4.2. Conclusions
Better family relationship quality was linked to better health and well-being, higher
T
life satisfaction, and individual salutogenic characteristics. This finding empirically highlights
IP
the importance of the family constituting the social environment for the maturation and
CR
actualization of an individual’s dispositions. It underlines the potential benefits of
preventative strategies and family therapy not only enhancing the quality of relationships but
US
initiating positive change on the individual level, too.
AN
Declarations of conflict of interest:
References
Ackard, D. M., Neumark-Sztainer, D., Story, M., & Perry, C. (2006). Parent-child
Aguilar-Raab, C., Grevenstein, D., Gotthardt, L., Jarczok, M. N., Hunger, C., Ditzen, B., &
Schweitzer, J. (2018). Changing me, changing us: Relationship quality and collective
T
efficacy as major outcomes in systemic couple therapy. Family Process, 57, 342–358.
IP
doi:10.1111/famp.12302
CR
Aguilar-Raab, C., Grevenstein, D., & Schweitzer, J. (2015). Measuring social relationships in
different social systems: The construction and validation of the Evaluation of Social
US
Systems (EVOS) Scale. PLoS ONE, 10(7). doi:e0133442
AN
Ahern, N. R., Kiehl, E. M., Lou Sole, M., & Byers, J. (2006). A review of instruments
doi:10.1080/01460860600677643
Alferi, S. M., Carver, C. S., Antoni, M. H., Weiss, S., & Durán, R. E. (2001). An exploratory
ED
study of social support, distress, and life disruption among low-income hispanic
women under treatment for early stage breast cancer. Health Psychology, 20(1), 41-46.
PT
doi:10.1037/0278-6133.20.1.41
CE
Allen, J. P., Moore, C., Kuperminc, G., & Bell, K. (1998). Attachment and adolescent
8624.1998.tb06220.x
Andersson, L. M. C., Moore, C. D., Hensing, G., Krantz, G., & Staland-Nyman, C. (2014).
General self-efficacy and its relationship to self-reported mental illness and barriers to
care: A general population study. Community Mental Health Journal, 50(6), 721-728.
doi:10.1007/s10597-014-9722-y
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Antonovsky, A. (1987). Unraveling the mystery of health: How people manage stress and
Bandura, A. (2000). Exercise of human agency through collective efficacy. Current directions
Bandura, A., Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Regalia, C., & Scabini, E. (2011). Impact of
family efficacy beliefs on quality of family functioning and satisfaction with family
T
life. Applied Psychology, 60(3), 421-448. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2010.00442.x
IP
Barber, B. K., & Harmon, E. L. (2002). Violating the self: Parental psychological control of
CR
children and adolescents. In B. K. Barber (Ed.), Intrusive parenting: How
psychological control affects children and adolescents (pp. 15-52). Washington, DC:
Beavers, W. R., & Hampson, R. B. (2000). The Beavers Systems Model of family
Berkman, L. F. (2000). Social support, social networks, social cohesion and health. Social
(Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol 4. Child psychology in practice (6th ed.,
Bowen, M. (1986). Family therapy in clinical practice. Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson Inc.
Butz, A. R., & Usher, E. L. (2015). Salient sources of early adolescents' self-efficacy in two
doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2015.04.001
Carver, C. S. (2014). Self-control and optimism are distinct and complementary strengths.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2014). Dispositional optimism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
past, present, and future perspectives. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Cohen, S. (2004). Social relationships and health. American psychologist, 59(8), 676-684.
T
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.59.8.676
IP
Cronbach, L. J., & Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests.
CR
Psychological Bulletin, 52(4), 281-302. doi:10.1037/h0040957
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The ―what‖ and ―why‖ of goal pursuits: Human needs and
US
the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11(4), 227-268.
AN
doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01
Derogatis, L. R., & Fitzpatrick, M. (2004). The SCL-90-R, the Brief Symptom Inventory
M
(BSI), and the BSI-18. In M. E. Maruish (Ed.), The use of psychological testing for
treatment planning and outcomes assessment (3 ed., Vol. 3, pp. 1-41). Mahwah, NJ:
ED
Derogatis, L. R., & Unger, R. (2010). Symptom Checklist‐ 90‐ Revised. In I. B. Weiner &
PT
Di Giunta, L., Iselin, A.-M. R., Lansford, J. E., Eisenberg, N., Lunetti, C., Thartori, E., . . .
doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2018.01.009
Diehl, M., Elnick, A. B., Bourbeau, L. S., & Labouvie-Vief, G. (1998). Adult attachment
styles: Their relations to family context and personality. Journal of Personality and
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction With Life
doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13
Donnellan, M. B., Larsen-Rife, D., & Conger, R. D. (2005). Personality, family history, and
T
Duncan, L. G., Coatsworth, J. D., & Greenberg, M. T. (2009). A model of mindful parenting:
IP
Implications for parent-child relationships and prevention research. Clinical Child and
CR
Family Psychology Review, 12(3), 255-270. doi:10.1007/s10567-009-0046-3
Dunn, E. C., McLaughlin, K. A., Slopen, N., Rosand, J., & Smoller, J. W. (2013).
US
Developmental timing of child maltreatment and symptoms of depression and suicidal
AN
ideation in young adulthood: Results from the National Longitudinal Study of
Epstein, N. B., Bishop, D., Ryan, C., Miller, I., & Keitner, G. (1993). The McMaster Model
view of healthy family functioning. In F. Walsh (Ed.), Normal Family Processes (pp.
ED
Epstein, N. B., Bishop, D. S., & Levin, S. (1978). The McMaster model of family
PT
0606.1978.tb00537.x
Eriksson, M., & Lindström, B. (2005). Validity of Antonovsky’s sense of coherence scale: a
AC
doi:10.1136/jech.2003.018085
Eriksson, M., & Lindström, B. (2006). Antonovsky's sense of coherence scale and the relation
Reviewing the role of parent, peer, friend, and romantic relationships. Journal of
Ferring, D., & Filipp, S.-H. (1996). Messung des Selbstwertgefühls: Befunde zu Reliabilität,
Finchham, F., & Beach, S. (2010). Of memes and marriage: Toward a positive relationship
T
science. Journal of Family Theory & Review, 2, 4–24. doi:10.1111/j.1756-
IP
2589.2010.00033.x
CR
Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Steele, H., Higgitt, A., & Target, M. (1994). The Emanuel Miller
Memorial Lecture 1992: The theory and practice of resilience. Child Psychology &
US
Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 35(2), 231-257. doi:10.1111/j.1469-
AN
7610.1994.tb01160.x
Fruzzetti, A. E., Shenk, C., & Hoffman, P. D. (2005). Family interaction and the development
ED
García-Moya, I., Moreno, C., & Jiménez-Iglesias, A. (2013). Understanding the joint effects
CE
913-923. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.07.007
García-Moya, I., Rivera, F., Moreno, C., Lindström, B., & Jiménez-Iglesias, A. (2012).
doi:10.1177/1403494812449924
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Gehart, D. R., & McCollum, E. E. (2007). Engaging suffering: Towards a mindful re-
visioning of family therapy practice. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 33(2),
214-226. doi:10.1111/j.1752-0606.2007.00017.x
Geyer, S. (1997). Some conceptual considerations on the sense of coherence. Social Science
Glaesmer, H., Grande, G., Braehler, E., & Roth, M. (2011). The German Version of the
T
Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS). European Journal of Psychological Assessment,
IP
27(2), 127-132. doi:10.1027/1015-5759/a000058
CR
Glaesmer, H., Hoyer, J., Klotsche, J., & Herzberg, P. Y. (2008). Die deutsche Version des
US
Zeitschrift für Gesundheitspsychologie, 16(1), 26-31. doi:10.1026/0943-8149.16.1.26
AN
Grevenstein, D., Aguilar-Raab, C., & Bluemke, M. (2018). Mindful and resilient? Incremental
validity of sense of coherence over mindfulness and big five personality factors for
M
doi:10.1007/s10902-017-9901-y
ED
Grevenstein, D., Aguilar-Raab, C., Schweitzer, J., & Bluemke, M. (2016). Through the
tunnel, to the light: Why sense of coherence covers and exceeds resilience, optimism,
PT
Grevenstein, D., & Bluemke, M. (2015). Can the Big Five explain the criterion validity of
sense of coherence for mental health, life satisfaction, and personal distress?
AC
Grevenstein, D., & Bluemke, M. (2017). Longitudinal factor analysis and measurement
prediction of substance use frequency and mental health. Health and Quality of Life
Grevenstein, D., Schweitzer, J., & Aguilar-Raab, C. (2018). How children and adolescents
evaluate their families: Psychometric properties and factor structure of the Evaluation
doi:10.1007/s10826-018-1254-6
T
Harris, J. R. (1995). Where is the child's environment? A group socialization theory of
IP
development. Psychological Review, 102(3), 458-489. doi:10.1037/0033-
CR
295X.102.3.458
Haukkala, A., Konttinen, H., Lehto, E., Uutela, A., Kawachi, I., & Laatikainen, T. (2013).
US
Sense of coherence, depressive symptoms, cardiovascular diseases, and all-cause
AN
mortality. Psychosomatic Medicine, 75(4), 429-435.
doi:10.1097/PSY.0b013e31828c3fa4
M
Henry, C. S., Sheffield Morris, A., & Harrist, A. W. (2015). Family resilience: Moving into
Holt-Lunstad, J., & Smith, T. B. (2012). Social relationships and mortality. Social and
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., Baker, M., Harris, T., & Stephenson, D. (2015). Loneliness
CE
and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspectives
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., & Layton, J. B. (2010). Social relationships and mortality risk:
doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316
Hupfeld, J., & Ruffieux, N. (2011). Validierung einer deutschen Version der Self-Compassion
Scale (SCS-D). Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie, 40(2), 115-
123. doi:10.1026/1616-3443/a000088
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Jose, P. E., Ryan, N., & Pryor, J. (2012). Does Social Connectedness Promote a Greater Sense
T
Keng, S.-L., Smoski, M. J., & Robins, C. J. (2011). Effects of mindfulness on psychological
IP
health: A review of empirical studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(6), 1041-1056.
CR
doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2011.04.006
US
Bulletin, 82(6), 887-903. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.82.6.887
AN
Kenny, D. A. (2014). Cross-Lagged Panel Design. In N. Balakrishnan, T. Colton, B. Everitt,
Online.
Kern, M. L., Della Porta, S. S., & Friedman, H. S. (2014). Lifelong pathways to longevity:
ED
484. doi:10.1111/jopy.12062
PT
doi:10.1017/s0954579412000715
AC
Kirby, J. N. (2016). The role of mindfulness and compassion in enhancing nurturing family
doi:10.1111/cpsp.12149
Klaghofer, R., & Brähler, E. (2001). Konstruktion und teststatistische Prüfung einer Kurzform
49, 115-124.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Landstedt, E., Hammarström, A., & Winefield, H. (2015). How well do parental and peer
T
Lavigne, G. L., Vallerand, R. J., & Crevier-Braud, L. (2011). The fundamental need to
IP
belong: On the distinction between growth and deficit-reduction orientations.
CR
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37 (9), 1185-1201.
doi:10.1177/0146167211405995
US
Leppert, K., Koch, B., Brähler, E., & Strauß, B. (2008). Die Resilienzskala (RS) -
AN
Überprüfung der Langform RS-25 und einer Kurzform RS-13. [Resilience Scale -
Evaluation of a long (RS-25) and a short version (RS-13)]. Klinische Diagnostik und
M
Masten, A. S., & Monn, A. R. (2015). Child and family resilience: A call for integrated
AC
doi:10.1111/fare.12103
Mattanah, J. F., Lopez, F. G., & Govern, J. M. (2011). The contributions of parental
Miller, G., Chen, E., & Cole, S. W. (2009). Health psychology: Developing biologically
plausible models linking the social world and physical health. Annual Review of
Miller, I., Ryan, C., Keitner, G. I., Bishop, D. S., & Epstein, N. B. (2000). The McMaster
T
Moksnes, U. K., Espnes, G. A., & Haugan, G. (2013). Stress, sense of coherence and
IP
emotional symptoms in adolescents. Psychology & Health, 29(1), 32-49.
CR
doi:10.1080/08870446.2013.822868
Moretti, M. M., & Peled, M. (2004). Adolescent-parent attachment: Bonds that support
US
healthy development. Paediatrics & Child Health, 9(8), 551-555.
AN
doi:10.1093/pch/9.8.551
Neff, K. D., & Beretvas, S. N. (2013). The role of self-compassion in romantic relationships.
PT
Nilsson, K. W., Leppert, J., Simonsson, B., & Starrin, B. (2010). Sense of coherence and
O'Brien, M. P., Gordon, J. L., Bearden, C. E., Lopez, S. R., Kopelowicz, A., & Cannon, T. D.
Olson, D. H. (1986). Circumplex model VII: Validation studies and FACES III. Family
Orth, U., Robins, R. W., & Roberts, B. W. (2008). Low self-esteem prospectively predicts
T
Psychology, 95(3), 695-708. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.95.3.695
IP
Paradis, A. D., Giaconia, R. M., Reinherz, H. Z., Beardslee, W. R., Ward, K. E., &
CR
Fitzmaurice, G. M. (2011). Adolescent family factors promoting healthy adult
US
16(1), 30-37. doi:10.1111/j.1475-3588.2010.00577.x
AN
Parcel, T. L., & Bixby, M. S. (2016). The ties that bind: Social capital, families, and children's
Patterson, J. M. (2002). Integrating family resilience and family stress theory. Journal of
Phillips-Salimi, C. R., Robb, S. L., Monahan, P. O., Dossey, A., & Haase, J. E. (2014).
adolescents newly diagnosed with cancer and their parents. International Journal of
CE
Personality: Theory and Research (3rd ed., pp. 351-374). New York, NY: Guilford
Press.
Rieger, S., Göllner, R., Trautwein, U., & Roberts, B. W. (2016). Low self-esteem
Orth, Robins, and Roberts (2008). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Rusbult, C. E., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (2003). Interdependence, interaction and relationships.
T
doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145059
IP
Ryan, A. K., & Willits, F. K. (2007). Family ties, physical health, and psychological well-
CR
being. Journal of Aging and Health, 19(6), 907-920. doi:10.1177/0898264307308340
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic
US
motivation, social development, and well-being. American psychologist, 55(1), 68.
AN
doi:10.1037110003-066X.55.1.68
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health: Assessment and
M
doi:10.1037/0278-6133.4.3.219
ED
Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from
Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1063-1078.
CE
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.1063
Scholz, U., Doña, B. G., Sud, S., & Schwarzer, R. (2002). Is general self-efficacy a universal
AC
doi:10.1027//1015-5759.18.3.242
Schumacher, J., Gunzelmann, T., & Brähler, E. (2000). Deutsche Normierung der Sense of
1924.46.4.208
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Schumacher, J., Wilz, G., Gunzelmann, T., & Brähler, E. (2000). Die Sense of Coherence
Schwarzer, R. (2014). Self-efficacy: Thought control of action. New York, NY: Routledge.
T
Wright, & M. Johnston (Eds.), Measures in health psychology: A user’s portfolio.
IP
Causal and control beliefs (pp. 35-37). Windsor, United Kingdom: NFER-Nelson.
CR
Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1999). Skalen zur Erfassung von Lehrer- und
US
Wissenschaftlichen Begleitung des Modellversuchs Selbstwirksame Schulen [Scales
AN
for the assessment of teacher and student charecteristics]. Berlin, Germany: Freie
Universität Berlin.
M
doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190124
Umberson, D., & Karas Montez, J. (2010). Social relationships and health: A flashpoint for
AC
doi:10.1177/0022146510383501
von Collani, G., & Herzberg, P. Y. (2003). Eine revidierte Fassung der deutschsprachigen
Wagnild, G., & Young, H. (1993). Development and psychometric evaluation of the
Windle, G. (2011). What is resilience? A systematic review and concept analysis. Reviews in
Windle, G., Bennett, K. M., & Noyes, J. (2011). A methodological review of resilience
T
doi:10.1186/1477-7525-9-8
IP
CR
US
AN
M
ED
PT
CE
AC
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table 1
Descriptives and partial correlations with family relationship quality for study measures.
with family
relations (EVOS)
Study 1
T
Family relationship quality (EVOS) 2.29 0.59 0.23 – 3.00 -
IP
Sense of Coherence (SOC-13) 5.15 0.88 2.67 – 6.77 .37***
CR
Resilience (RS-13) 5.50 0.75 1.92 – 7.00 .37***
Study 2 US
AN
Family relationship quality (EVOS) 2.23 0.64 0.00 – 3.00 -
Study 3
sex.