Understanding Memory Dysfunction: Andrew E. Budson, MD
Understanding Memory Dysfunction: Andrew E. Budson, MD
From the *Center for Translational Cognitive Neuroscience, Geriatric Research The core of the episodic memory system is the medial temporal
Education Clinical Center, Bedford VA Hospital, Bedford, MA; *Boston
University Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Department of Neurology, Boston lobe and hippocampus. It is worth examining these structures more
University School of Medicine, Boston, MA. closely. The medial temporal lobes may seem simple but they are
Reprints: Andrew E. Budson, MD, Bldg 62, Rm B30, Bedford VA Hospital, 200 actually neuroanatomically complex structures with multiple regions
Spring Road, Bedford MA 01730. E-mail: [email protected]. and subregions. In Figure 3 you can see the medial temporal lobe
Copyright © 2009 by Lippincott Williams & Wilkins
ISSN: 1074-7931/09/1502-0071 structures, including the parahippocampal gyrus, presubiculum, subic-
DOI: 10.1097/NRL.0b013e318188040d ulum, and hippocampus proper, including its subregions.
Although we do not completely understand how the medial riences an episode of their life, such as having breakfast that
temporal lobes store and retrieve memories, our current understand- morning. The cortically distributed patterns of neural activity rep-
ing from cognitive neuroscience is as follows. An individual expe- resenting the sights, sounds, smells, tastes, emotions, and thoughts
half-way down the aisle on the left (Fig. 7a). On the second day, the car
is parked in the Blue area, on the 2nd floor, and it was not such as good
day. Nonetheless, this distinct pattern of neural activity allows a unique
hippocampal index to form that enables one to remember that the car
was parked in the Blue area, on the 2nd floor, and half-way down the
aisle on the right (Fig. 7b). On the third day, the car is also parked in the
Blue area, on the 2nd floor, and it again was not such a good day (Fig.
7c). Although one might wish that a hippocampal index will form to
FIGURE 4. Areas of the cerebral cortex, including sensory
enable one to remember that the car was parked in the Blue area, on the
areas, are connected bidirectionally to the parahippocampal
2nd floor, and all the way down the aisle on the left—there is a problem.
region, which is in turn bidirectionally connected to the hip-
When there are completely overlapping patterns of neural activity, a
pocampus. (Adapted with permission from Eichenbaum,
separate hippocampal index cannot form. Instead, there is a single
1997; permission granted by Science Magazine).
hippocampal index that forms for both days 2 and 3 (Fig. 7d). This
hippocampal index is strengthened for the common aspects of the 2
memories: parking in the Blue area, on the 2nd floor. But, this index
will also contain divergent aspects of the memory: half-way down the
aisle on the right and all the way down the aisle on the left. Thus, on day
3, it will be easy to remember that the car is parked in the Blue area on
the 2nd floor, but it will be difficult to remember if it is parked half-way
down the aisle on the right or all the way down the aisle on the left.
the medial temporal lobes. Dysfunction of the frontal lobes may memory dysfunction attributable to Alzheimer disease versus depression.
cause a variety of memory problems, including distortions of epi- Patients with Alzheimer disease have a dysfunctional “recent memory file
sodic memory and false memories, such as when information be- cabinet,” whereas patients with depression have a dysfunctional “file clerk.”
comes associated with the wrong context18 or incorrect specific The time course of the patient’s episodic memory deficit is often
details.19 Extreme memory distortions are often synonymous with extremely helpful in distinguishing different disorders. Disorders of
confabulations, which occur when “memories” are created to be episodic memory may be transient, such as those due to concussion,
consistent with current information,18 such as “remembering” that seizure, or transient global amnesia. Static disorders, including trau-
someone broke into the house and rearranged household items. matic brain injury, hypoxic or ischemic injury, single strokes, surgical
A clinically useful analogy can be used to help conceptualize the lesions, and encephalitis, are typically maximal at onset, improve, and
dysfunction in episodic memory that occurs due to damage to the medial then become stable. (Note that in static disorders the onset may last
temporal lobes (and Papez’s circuit) versus damage to the frontal several days, and the period of improvement may last 2 years or more.)
lobes.20,21 The frontal lobes are analogous to the “file clerk” of the Degenerative diseases, such as Alzheimer disease,22 dementia with
episodic memory system, the medial temporal lobes to the “recent Lewy bodies, and frontotemporal dementia, begin insidiously and
memory file cabinet,” and other cortical regions to the “remote memory progress gradually. Disorders that affect multiple brain regions, such as
file cabinet” (Table 3). Thus, if the frontal lobes are impaired, it is multiple sclerosis and vascular dementia, generally progress in a step-
difficult— but not impossible—to get information in and out of storage. wise manner. Some disorders of memory can have a more complicated
For example, getting information into storage may require stronger and variable time course, including memory dysfunction attributable to
encoding, and getting information out of storage may require stronger tumors, hypoglycemia, medications, and Korsakoff syndrome.
cues from the environment. Additionally, when the frontal lobes are When a disorder of episodic memory is suspected due to inabil-
impaired the information stored in memory may be distorted due to
ity to remember recent information and experiences accurately, further
“improper filing” that leads to an inaccurate source, context, or se-
evaluation is warranted. A detailed history of the memory dysfunction
quence. If, on the other hand, the medial temporal lobes are impaired,
should be taken, with particular emphasis on the time course of the
it may be impossible for recent information to be stored. This will often
memory disorder. Speaking with a caregiver or other informant is
lead the patient to ask for the same information again and again—perhaps
20 times in an hour. Older information that has been consolidated over usually critical, since the patient with memory dysfunction will invari-
months to years is likely stored in other cortical regions and will therefore ably not remember important aspects of the history. A history of other
be available for retrieval even when the medial temporal lobes or Papez’s cognitive deficits (such as deficits in attention, language, visuospatial,
circuit are damaged. To illustrate this analogy we can compare the episodic and executive function) should be obtained. Medical and neurologic
examinations should be performed, searching for signs of systemic
illness, focal neurologic injury, and neurodegenerative disorders.
TABLE 3. A Filing Analogy of Episodic Memory Brief cognitive testing may be performed by asking the patient to
remember several words or a short story, or by using tools such as
Brain Structure Analogy Mini-Mental State Examination,23 the Blessed Dementia Scale,24 the
Frontal lobes File clerk Three Words-Three Shapes memory test,2 the word list memory test of
Medial temporal lobes Recent memory files the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer disease,25 the
Other cortical regions Remote memory files Drilled Word Span Test,2 and the Seven-Minute Screen.26 To help
distinguish episodic memory dysfunction attributable to impairment
of the frontal lobes versus impairment of the medial temporal lobes, Almost any disorder that can disrupt the anterior and infero-
difficulties in the encoding and retrieval of information should be lateral temporal lobes may cause impairment of semantic memory,
contrasted with a primary failure of storage. When information including traumatic brain injury, stroke, surgical lesions, encepha-
cannot be remembered even when multiple rehearsals have maxi- litis, and tumors (Table 2). Patients with semantic dementia (the
mized encoding, and retrieval demands have been minimized with temporal variant of frontotemporal dementia) exhibit deficits in all
the use of a multiple-choice recognition test, a primary failure of functions of semantic memory, such as naming, single-word com-
storage is present. In complex cases, a formal neuropsychological prehension, and impaired general knowledge (such as the color of
evaluation should be obtained. common items). Other aspects of cognition, however, are relatively
The history, examination, and cognitive testing will suggest a preserved, including components of speech, perceptual and nonver-
differential diagnosis, which in turn will determine which laboratory bal problem-solving skills, and episodic memory.40
and imaging studies are indicated. Treatment depends upon the specific Although naming difficulties (particularly with proper nouns)
disorder identified. Cholinesterase inhibitors have been approved by the are common in healthy older adults, naming difficulties may also be
Food and Drug Administration (FDA) to treat Alzheimer disease27 and a sign of a disorder of semantic memory. When a disorder of
Parkinson disease dementia28; these medications have also been used to semantic memory is suspected, the evaluation should include the
treat vascular dementia29 and dementia with Lewy bodies.30 Meman- same components as the evaluation for episodic memory disorders.
tine has been approved to treat Alzheimer disease, with or without One of the first aspects of the history and cognitive examination that
concomitant treatment with cholinesterase inhibitors.31 should be ascertained is whether the problem is solely one of
difficulty in recalling people’s names and other proper nouns (com-
mon in healthy older adults) or to a true loss of semantic informa-
Semantic Memory tion. Patients with mild dysfunction of semantic memory may show
only reduced generation of words in a semantic category (for
example, the number of grocery items that can be generated in 1
minute), whereas patients with a more severe impairment of seman-
Semantic memory refers to our store of conceptual tic memory usually show a 2-way naming deficit: they are unable to
name an item when it is described, and they are also unable to
and factual knowledge that is not related to any describe an item when it is named. General knowledge is also
impoverished in these more severely affected patients. Treatment
specific memory. will depend upon the specific disorder identified.
Procedural Memory memory tests.47,50 Other causes of damage to the basal ganglia or
cerebellum including tumors, strokes, and hemorrhages may also
disrupt procedural memory. Patients with major depression also
show impairment in procedural memory tasks, perhaps because
depression involves dysfunction of the basal ganglia.51
Procedural memory refers to the ability to Disruption of procedural memory should be suspected when
learn cognitive and behavioral skills and patients show evidence of either substantial difficulties in learning
new skills (compared with their baseline) or the loss of previously
algorithms that operate at an automatic, learned skills. For example, patients may lose the ability to perform
automatic, skilled movements, such as writing, swinging a tennis
unconscious level. racket, or playing a musical instrument. Although these patients may
be able to relearn the fundamentals of these skills, explicit thinking
becomes required for their performance. As a result, patients with
damage to the procedural memory system lose the automatic effort-
Procedural memory refers to the ability to learn cognitive and lessness of simple motor tasks that healthy individuals take for
behavioral skills and algorithms that operate at an automatic, un- granted. The evaluation of disorders of procedural memory is similar
conscious level. Procedural memory is nondeclarative and implicit. to that of disorders of episodic memory; treatment depends upon the
Examples include learning to ride a bike or play the piano (Table 1). specific disease process. Lastly, it is worth noting that patients
Because procedural memory is spared in patients who have severe whose episodic memory has been devastated by a static disorder,
deficits of the episodic memory system (such as those who have such as encephalitis, have had successful rehabilitation by using
undergone surgical removal of the medial temporal lobes), it is clear procedural memory (and other nondeclarative forms of memory) to
that the procedural memory system is separate and distinct from the learn new skills.52
episodic memory system.32,47
Functional imaging research has shown that a number of Priming
brain regions involved in procedural memory become active as a
Priming occurs when a prior encounter with a particular item
new task is learned, including the supplementary motor area, basal changes the response to the current item (Table 1). Because this
ganglia, and cerebellum (Fig. 8).48 Convergent evidence comes from phenomenon occurs even if the individual does not consciously
studies of patients with damage to the basal ganglia or cerebellum remember encountering the prior item, priming is another example
who show impairment in learning procedural skills.49 Because the of an implicit and nondeclarative form of memory. Priming is often
basal ganglia and cerebellum are relatively spared in early Alzhei- divided into perceptual priming, which is modality specific (eg,
mer disease, despite their episodic memory deficit these patients auditory, visual) and does not benefit from elaborate encoding when
show normal acquisition and maintenance of their procedural mem- materials are being learned, versus conceptual priming, which is not
ory skills. modality specific and shows enhancement with increased encoding.
Parkinson disease is the most common disorder disrupting Perceptual priming depends upon a perceptual representation
procedural memory. Patients in the early stages of Huntington system, involved in processing information regarding the form and
chorea and olivopontocerebellar degeneration also show impaired structure of items but not their meanings.53 Converging evidence
procedural memory while performing nearly normally on episodic suggests that posterior cortical regions involved in processing of
sensory information are important for perceptual priming. A patient der, obsessive compulsive disorder, depression, and schizophrenia,
with bilateral occipital lobe lesions demonstrated normal episodic can also impair working memory.64 – 66
memory and conceptual priming while failing to show perceptual Disorders of working memory may present in several differ-
priming.54 Neuroimaging studies of visual perceptual priming using ent ways. Often the patient will exhibit an inability to concentrate or
PET and fMRI show changes in activation of visual peristriate pay attention. Impairment in performing a new task with multistep
cortex.55 By contrast, neuroimaging studies of conceptual priming instructions is frequently seen. Interestingly, a disorder of working
typically show changes in left prefrontal regions.55 Most studies have memory may also present as a problem with episodic memory,
shown that patients with early degenerative diseases that do not affect because information must first be “kept in mind” by working
the sensory association cortices, such as Alzheimer, Parkinson, and memory in order for episodic memory to encode it.13 Such cases will
Huntington diseases, demonstrate normal perceptual priming.56 For therefore show a primary impairment in encoding.
conceptual priming, however, many studies have found these groups to The evaluation of disorders of working memory is similar to
be impaired.47 that of disorders of episodic memory. Treatment depends upon the
underlying cause. Stimulants, approved by the FDA for the treat-
Working Memory ment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder,67,68 will often be
helpful in disorders of working memory.
CONCLUSION
Working memory refers to the ability to temporarily Although traditionally, memory has been viewed as a simple
concept, converging and complementary evidence from patient stud-
maintain and manipulate information that one ies and more recent neuroimaging research suggest that memory is
needs to keep in mind. composed of separate and distinct systems. Improved understanding
of these different types of memory will aid the clinician in the
diagnosis and treatment of the memory disorders of their patients.
As more specific therapeutic strategies are developed for the treat-
ment of diseases that cause memory dysfunction, this knowledge
Bringing together the traditional fields of attention, concen- will become increasingly important.
tration, and short-term memory, working memory refers to the
ability to temporarily maintain and manipulate information that one
needs to keep in mind. Requiring active and conscious participation, ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
working memory is an explicit and declarative memory system. This work was supported by National Institute on Aging grant
Working memory has traditionally been divided into 3 components: P30 AG13846. This material is also the result of work supported
one that processes phonologic information (eg, keeping a phone with resources and the use of facilities at the Edith Nourse Rogers
number “in your head”), one that processes spatial information (eg, Memorial Veterans Hospital in Bedford, MA.
mentally following a route), and an executive system that allocates
attentional resources.57 REFERENCES
Studies have demonstrated that working memory involves a
1. Newman SD, Carpenter PA, Varma S, et al. Frontal and parietal participation
network of cortical and subcortical areas, which differ depending on in problem solving in the Tower of London: fMRI and computational
the particular task.58 Participation of the prefrontal cortex, however, modeling of planning and high-level perception. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:
is involved in virtually all working memory tasks (Fig. 8).13 The 1668 –1682.
network of cortical and subcortical areas typically includes posterior 2. Mesulam MM. Principles of Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology. 2nd ed.
brain regions (eg, visual association areas) that are linked with New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2000.
prefrontal regions to form a circuit. Research suggests that spatial 3. Ally BA, Waring JD, Beth EH, et al. Aging memory for pictures: using
high-density event-related potentials to understand the effect of aging on the
working memory tends to involve more regions on the right side, picture superiority effect. Neuropsychologia. In press.
and phonologic working memory tends to involve more regions on 4. Schacter DL, Wagner AD, Buckner RL. Memory systems of 1999. In:
the left side of the brain. Bilateral brain activation is observed, Tulving E, Craik FIM, eds. The Oxford Handbook of Memory. New York:
however, in more difficult working memory tasks, regardless of the Oxford University Press; 2000:627– 643.
nature of the material being processed.1 Additionally, an increase in 5. Schacter DL, Tulving E. What are the memory systems of 1994? In: Schacter
the number of brain regions activated in prefrontal cortex is ob- DL, Tulving E, eds. Memory Systems 1994. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT
Press; 1994:1–38.
served as the complexity of the task increases.59
Because working memory depends upon networks that in- 6. Squire LR. Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats,
monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review. 1992;99:195–231.
clude frontal and parietal cortical regions as well as subcortical
7. Wagner AD, Schacter DL, Rotte M, et al. Building memories: Remembering
structures, most neurodegenerative diseases impair working mem- and forgetting of verbal experiences as predicted by brain activity. Science.
ory. Studies have demonstrated that working memory may be 1998;281:1188 –1191.
impaired in patients with Alzheimer disease, Parkinson disease, 8. Brewer JB, Zhao Z, Desmond JE, et al. Making memories: brain activity that
Huntington chorea, and dementia with Lewy bodies, as well as less predicts how well visual experience will be remembered. Science. 1998;281:
common disorders such as progressive supranuclear palsy (Table 1185–1187.
2).60,61 In addition to these neurodegenerative diseases, almost any 9. Papez JW. A proposed mechanism of emotion. Arch Neurol Psychiatry
1937;3:725–743.
disease process that disrupts the frontal lobes or their connections to
10. Ribot T. Les Maladies de la Mémoire. Paris: Félix Alcan; 1881.
posterior cortical regions and subcortical structures can interfere
11. Stickgold R. Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature. 2005;437:
with working memory. Such processes include tumors, strokes, 1272–1278.
multiple sclerosis, head injury, and others.62,63 Because it involves 12. Stickgold R. Neuroscience: a memory boost while you sleep. Nature. 2006;
the silent rehearsal of verbal information, almost any type of aphasia 444:559 –560.
may impair phonologic working memory. Disorders that diminish 13. Fletcher PC, Henson RNA. Frontal lobes and human memory: insights from
attentional resources, including attention deficit hyperactivity disor- functional neuroimaging. Brain. 2001;124:849 – 881.
14. Simons JS, Spiers HJ. Prefrontal and medial temporal lobe interactions in 42. Markowitsch HJ. Anatomical basis of memory disorders. In: Gazzaniga MS,
long-term memory. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience. 2003;4:637– 648. ed. The Cognitive Neurosciences. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press,
15. Petrides M. The mid-ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and active mnemonic 1995:765–779.
retrieval. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2002;78:528 –538. 43. Dimitrova A, Weber J, Maschke M, et al. Eyeblink-related areas in human
16. Johnson MK, Kounios J, Nolde SF. Electrophysiological brain activity and cerebellum as shown by fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;1:100 –115.
memory source monitoring. Neuroreport. 1997;8:1317–1320. 44. Solomon PR, Stowe GT, Pendlbeury WW. Disrupted eyelid conditioning in a
17. Kopelman MD, Stanhope N, Kingsley D. Temporal and spatial context patient with damage to cerebellar afferents. Behav Neurosci. 1989;103:898–902.
memory in patients with focal frontal, temporal lobe, and diencephalic 45. Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H, et al. Double dissociation of conditioning
lesions. Neuropsychologia. 1997;35:1533–1545. and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus in
18. Johnson MK, O’Connor M, Cantor J. Confabulation, memory deficits, and humans. Science. 1995;269:115–118.
frontal dysfunction. Brain Cogn. 1997;34:189 –206. 46. Chu CC, Tranel D, Damasio AR, et al. The autonomic-related cortex:
19. Budson AE, Sullivan AL, Mayer E, et al. Suppression of false recognition in pathology in Alzheimer’s disease. Cereb Cortex. 1997;7:86 –95.
Alzheimer’s disease and in patients with frontal lobe lesions. Brain. 2002; 47. Heindel WC, Salmon DP, Shults CW, et al. Neuropsychological evidence for
125:2750 –2765. multiple implicit memory systems: a comparison of Alzheimer’s, Hunting-
20. Budson AE, Price BH. Memory: Clinical disorders. In: Fullerlove G, ed. ton’s, and Parkinson’s disease patients. J Neurosci. 1989;9:582–587.
Encyclopedia of Life Sciences. Vol. 11. London, England: Macmillan Pub- 48. Daselaar SM, Rombouts SA, Veltman DJ, et al. Similar network activated by
lishers Ltd, Nature Publishing Group; 2002:529 –536. young and old adults during the acquisition of a motor sequence. Neurobiol
21. Budson AE, Price BH. Memory dysfunction. N Engl J Med. 2005;352:692– Aging. 2003;24:1013–1019.
699. 49. Exner C, Koschack J, Irle E. The differential role of premotor frontal cortex
22. Solomon PR, Budson AE. Alzheimer’s Disease. Clin Symp. 2003;54: and basal ganglia in motor sequence learning: evidence from focal basal
1– 44. ganglia lesions. Learn Mem. 2002;9:376 –386.
23. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. A practical method for grading the 50. Salmon DP, Lineweaver TT, Heindel WC. Nondeclarative memory in neu-
cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189 – rodegenerative disease. In: Troster AI, ed. Memory in Neurodegenerative
198. Disease: Biological, Cognitive, and Clinical Perspectives. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press, 1998:210 –225.
24. Blessed G, Tomlinson BE, Roth M. The association between quantitative
measures of dementia and of senile change in the cerebral grey matter of 51. Sabe L, Jason L, Juejati M, et al. Dissociation between declarative and
elderly subjects. Br J Psychiatry. 1968;114:797– 811. procedural learning in dementia and depression. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol.
1995;17:841– 848.
25. Welsh KA, Butters N, Hughes JP, et al. Detection and staging of dementia in
Alzheimer’s disease. Use of the neuropsychological measures developed for 52. Glisky EL, Schacter DL. Extending the limits of complex learning in organic
the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease. Arch Neurol. amnesia: computer training in a vocation domain. Neuropsychologia. 1989;
1992;49:448 – 452. 27:173–178.
26. Solomon PR, Hirschoff A, Kelly B, et al. A 7 minute neurocognitive 53. Tulving E, Schacter DL. Priming and human memory systems. Science.
screening battery highly sensitive to Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol. 1990;24:301–306.
1998;55:349 –355. 54. Keane MM, Gabrieli JD, Mapstone HC, et al. Double dissociation of memory
27. Winblad B, Engedal K, Soininen H, et al. A 1-year, randomized, placebo- capacities after bilateral occipital-lobe or medial temporal-lobe lesions. Brain.
controlled study of donepezil in patients with mild to moderate AD. Neurol- 1995;118:1129 –1148.
ogy. 2001;57:489 – 495. 55. Schacter DL, Buckner RL. Priming and the Brain. Neuron. 1998;20:185–195.
28. Emre M, Aarsland D, Albanese A, et al. Rivastigmine for dementia associated 56. Koivisto M, Portin R, Rinne JO. Perceptual priming in Alzheimer’s and
with Parkinson’s disease. N Engl J Med. 2004;351:2509 –2518. Parkinson’s diseases. Neuropsychologia. 1996;34:449 – 457.
29. Moretti R, Torre P, Antonello RM, et al. Use of galantamine to treat vascular 57. Baddeley AD. Recent developments in working memory. Cur Opin Neuro-
dementia. Lancet. 2002;360:1512–1513. biol. 1998;8:234 –238.
30. McKeith I, Del Ser T, Spano P, et al. Efficacy of rivastigmine in dementia 58. Rowe JB, Toni I, Josephs O, et al. The prefrontal cortex: response selection
with Lewy bodies: a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled interna- or maintenance within working memory? Science. 2000;2:1656 –1660.
tional study. Lancet. 2000;356:2031–2036.
59. Jaeggi SM, Seewer R, Nirkko AC, et al. Does excessive memory load
31. Tariot PN, Farlow MR, Grossberg GT, et al. Memantine treatment in patients attenuate activation in the prefrontal cortex? Load-dependent processing in
with moderate to severe Alzheimer disease already receiving donepezil: a single and dual tasks: functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuro-
randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2004;291:317–324. image. 2003;1:210 –225.
32. Corkin S. Lasting consequences of bilateral medial temporal lobectomy: 60. Calderon J, Perry RJ, Erzinclioglu SW, et al. Perception, attention, and
clinical course and experimental findings in H. M. Semin Neurol. 1984;4: working memory are disproportionately impaired in dementia with Lewy
249 –259. bodies compared with Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry.
33. Vaidya CJ, Zhao M, Desmond JE, et al. Evidence for cortical encoding 2001;70:157–164.
specificity in episodic memory: memory-induced re-activation of picture 61. Gotham AM, Brown RG, Marsden CD. ‘Frontal’ cognitive functions in patients
processing areas. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:2136 –2143. with Parkinson’s disease ‘on’ and ‘off’ levodopa. Brain. 1988;111:299–321.
34. Damasio H, Grabowski TJ, Tranel D, et al. A neural basis for lexical retrieval. 62. Kubat-Silman AK, Dagenbach D, Absher JR. Patterns of impaired verbal,
Nature. 1996;380:499 –505. spatial, and object working memory after thalamic lesions. Brain Cogn.
35. Perani D, Cappa SF, Schnur T, et al. The neural correlates of verb and noun 2002;50:178 –193.
processing. A PET study. Brain. 1999;122:2337–2344. 63. Sfagos C, Papageorgiou CC, Kosma KK, et al. Working memory deficits in
36. Price JL, Morris JC. Tangles and plaques in nondemented aging and “pre- multiple sclerosis: a controlled study with auditory P600 correlates. J Neurol
clinical” Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol. 1999;45:358 –368. Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2003;4:1231–1235.
37. Lidstrom AM, Bogdanovic N, Hesse C, et al. Clusterin (apolipoprotein J) 64. Egeland J, Sundet K, Rund BR, et al. Sensitivity and specificity of memory
protein levels are increased in hippocampus and in frontal cortex in Alzhei- dysfunction in schizophrenia: a comparison with major depression. J Clin Exp
mer’s disease. Exp Neurol. 1998;154:511–521. Neuropsychol. 2003;25:79 –93.
38. Balota DA, Watson JM, Duchek JM, et al. Cross-modal semantic and 65. Klingberg T, Forssberg H, Westerberg H. Training of working memory in
homographic priming in healthy young, healthy old, and in Alzheimer’s children with ADHD. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2002;24:781–791.
disease individuals. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1999;5:626 – 640. 66. Purcell R, Maruff P, Kyrios M, et al. Cognitive deficits in obsessive-
39. Green JD, Hodges JR. Identification of famous faces and famous names in compulsive disorder on tests of frontal-striatal function. Biol Psychiatry.
early Alzheimer’s disease. Relationship to anterograde episodic and general 1998;43:348 –357.
semantic memory. Brain. 1996;119:111–128. 67. Elia J, Ambrosini PJ, Rapoport JL. Treatment of attention-deficit-hyperac-
40. Hodges JR. Frontotemporal dementia (Pick’s disease): clinical features and tivity disorder. N Engl J Med. 1999;340:780 –788.
assessment. Neurology. 2001;56:6 –10. 68. Mehta MA, Goodyer IM, Sahakian BJ. Methylphenidate improves working
41. Pavlov IP. Conditioned Reflexes: An Investigation of the Physiological memory and set-shifting in AD/HD: relationships to baseline memory capac-
Activity of the Cerebral Cortex. London: Oxford University Press; 1927. ity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45:293–305.