Acting The Metaphor The Laban-Malmgren System of Movement Psychology and Character Analysis
Acting The Metaphor The Laban-Malmgren System of Movement Psychology and Character Analysis
Vladimir Mirodan
Professor of Theatre
Central Saint Martins
University of the Arts London
Granary Building
1 Granary Square
London N1C 1AA
Tel. ((PA – Maggie Wilkinson): 020 7514 9363
Tel. (home): 020 7281 3139
[email protected]
0
Acting the Metaphor
influential training method of the post-war era, has helped to shape the thinking and
work of some of the most expressive actors of our time, from Anthony Hopkins to
the Swedish dancer and acting teacher Yat Malmgren (1916-2002). He taught the
System to generations of actors, first at his private Movement Studio in the West
End, then at RADA and the CSSD and from 1963 until 2001 at the school he co-
the world.
The System originated in work developed in the early fifties by Rudolf Laban and one
belief that "words dance", that they could be made to convey psychological
experiences which hitherto had only been intimated through physical sensation.
always accurate, readings in Jung the confluence of two major intellectual traditions -
Jung and Laban - had begun to bear fruit in a planned book relating to a
1
Acting the Metaphor
classification of human types according to their psychology and the way in which this
(Preston-Dunlop 1998, p.263) and Laban, feeling unable to develop the work on his
own, entrusted Yat Malmgren with the notes Carpenter had left behind 2. In the years
developments - notably Uta Hagen), Yat Malmgren struck out on his own, developing
Laban’s and Carpenter’s approach was based on a deceptively simple idea: that
direct correspondence. Jung had argued that the psyche could be described in
also maintained that personalities developed in accordance with the ‘best foot
foremost’ principle: that people recognised early in life that one or the other of these
functions secured approval and success and therefore tended to rely on it and
underplay the others. As a result, personalities are shaped by the “habitual” reliance
on one of the functions (Jung 1971, p.482) and Jung describes a “Sensing type”, a
“Feeling type” and so on. A particular personality also continues to draw on the other
Thus, one person could be said to be primarily a Sensing type, but to use Intuition as
the “auxiliary” function (or vice versa)4. Moreover, following the James-Lange theory
2
Acting the Metaphor
the two. “I regard affect” – Jung writes – “on the one hand as a psychic feeling-state
This was the fundamental thesis Carpenter had absorbed from Jung. He and Laban
then decided to test the theory in practice: they observed the movement patterns of a
group of people undergoing analysis at the Withymead Centre (McCaw 2011, pp.
306-16), a clinic run by two Jungian analysts who, as part of their arts-orientated
analysands were classified using the four elements into which Laban had long
argued all movement could be analysed: the four dimensions or ‘Motion Factors’ of
'chemical analysis'. He studied the various 'molecules' and 'atoms' which made up
a. which part of the body moves and what relationship exists between it and those
b. what is the duration of the movement and in what relationship does it stand to
3
Acting the Metaphor
Laban observed that in order to lift a heavy weight the muscles strain to overcome
the force of gravity. Equally, lifting a light object necessitated only the lightest of
muscle exertions. Thus, Laban declared, one of the four components of movement is
its Weight, which, according to the intensity of the energy deployed, can be either
Strong or Light.
as possible between the raised arm and the nail - the movement is Direct. But a
coachman whipping his horses will raise the whip, move it first to the left of his head,
then to the right and only then crack it forward. The movement is convoluted,
roundabout, Flexible.
Movements take place in Time. Here Laban pointed out that the important factor was
not whether the movement was slow or quick, but how slow or quick in comparison
with other movements around it, either of parts of the same body or of different
Finally, effective movements involve a degree of co-ordination: they are either free
and easy or halting and tight. This Laban defined as the Flow of movement, which
the other occupies the foreground, while the others take a supporting role. When a
movement is expressive, the components are "in harmony" (Laban 1951, p.6). For
4
Acting the Metaphor
and the psychological processes associated with it (1950/1971, p.22). The harmony
innermost feelings and sensations was a sign of inner conflict. In extreme cases this
McCaw 2011, p.320). But, rather than look for specific gestural or postural
same period attempted to do (see, for example, Deutsch 1952; Reich 1933/1945;
Lowen 1958), Laban and Carpenter linked the component elements of movement,
their Motion Factors, with the 'elements' of the psyche, Jung's four psychological
functions, which they called their ‘Mental Factors’. They argued that:
These connections were purely experiential. No proof existed, apart from that offered
by their observations, which told them that a person's 'weighty presence' was
associated with her sensuous engagement with the world; that a 'thinker' tended to
go 'in and out' of his ‘inner space’ in order to solve a problem. Indeed, Laban’s
journey to this conclusion was fraught with hesitations (Laban 1947, pp. 57-9;
1950/71, pp. 126-7) and even Carpenter sounds a little defensive on this topic when
he writes:
5
Acting the Metaphor
When we are asked 'How can you prove that, say, a movement in Space
reveals ‘Thinking and Attention', we must reply that our proof is empirical and
Elsewhere Carpenter elaborates a little on the nature of the observations which had
In our research into this relationship between bodily movements and inner
emotion, we have been aided by students who had had an Analysis for a
relatively long period under the Freudian or Jungian methods before joining us
Flow & Feeling, Time & Intuition, Space & Thinking and Weight & Sense
While Carpenter’s focus had been on the therapeutic application of Laban’s ideas,
Yat Malmgren sensed their potential as an approach for actor training. Building on
physical activity and mental and emotional states. In this general overview of this
6
Acting the Metaphor
complex System, I only propose to focus on two of its salient features: the links
described above and the classification of theatre characters flowing from it.
As we have seen, the System in founded on the observation that not only can one
through the way in which a person moves, but that the more a person moves in a
certain way, the more that movement pattern reinforces the personality bias. Thus,
the System is founded on the mutuality of the Mental and Motion Factors within what
Laban was already calling the “body mind”. This interdependence has since been
1999, p.86; Edelman and Tononi 2000), enabling theatre scholars to speak of a
Laban and Malmgren think of this unit as a form of integrated psychological and
and Mental Factors through which one can both conceive and perceive a character.
The System describes this psychophysical process using the terminology of Jung’s
types. In so doing, the System simplifies Jung's ideas considerably: Jung had divided
the functions along a major fault line, into rational (Thinking and Feeling) and
irrational (Sensing and Intuiting). These were said to be incompatible with one
7
Acting the Metaphor
of the functions can be the superior function and combining it with any of the other
three, the System arrives at a total of six broad types of theatre character. These
Laban named his six Inner Attitudes. They are usually presented as pairs of
opposites:
primary motivation revolves around sensuous desires, whose main concern is its
materialistic Near. On the other hand, characters whose lives are dominated by
emotion (Ophelia, say, or Hedda) are considered Adream. These decisions do not
choosing a Super-Objective does. An actor playing Iago could choose as his primary
his sexual jealousy or class and racial enmity - all Sensing-rooted desires typical of a
8
Acting the Metaphor
Near character. But should he decide to find his main motivation in Iago's emotional
dependence on Othello, on unacknowledged love for the Moor and hatred of himself,
The actor developing an Inner Attitude can therefore choose Variations which are
Light/Flexible
Light/Direct
Strong/Flexible
Strong/Direct
The System thus analyses the tempi of characters in terms of Weight, Space, Time
9
Acting the Metaphor
'powerful', 'indecisive' and a myriad others, are ultimately defined in terms of Light or
Yat Malmgren then extends this analysis from the realm of Character to that of
Action and subdivides the latter into an Inner and an Outer Action.
The Inner Action denotes the thought-process of the character, its 'stream of
The Outer Action consists of the 'doings' of the character. These include the
characters. They are the means by which the character achieves its desires - the
Objectives.
Inner and Outer Actions are described with the same terminology as the Inner
Attitudes. An Inner Attitude cannot express itself through an Action Attitude which is
its direct opposite (Stable through Mobile; Adream through Awake, etc.), but has at
its disposal the two remaining pairs as Inner and Outer Actions. Thus, a Stable
(Thinking/Intuiting) Outer Action - the visible expression of both Character and Inner
Action in a cool, poised and perhaps manipulative attitude towards the other
characters in the scene. A kind of ‘model of the atom’ structure emerges, with the
10
Acting the Metaphor
Inner Attitude (the character essence) constant yet hidden at the centre and the
For Laban the Inner Attitudes referred to different states of mind or inclinations within
unmediated way: a certain Inner Attitude would find expression through its
acting, Yat Malmgren separated Character from Action and described a dialectic
relationship between Inner Attitude and Action Attitudes and their expression in
movement:
a. posture
b. shadow moves7.
gesture”.
11
Acting the Metaphor
Pedagogically, most students of the System are introduced to its concepts by means
acting imagination, scenarios are short monologues devised in order to embody one
or the other Inner Attitude and its respective Actions and are adaptations of a
performance style made famous by the American actress Ruth Draper (Zabel 1966).
A major performer from the twenties to the fifties, Ruth Draper (1884-1956) has
faded from all but the most assiduous of theatrical memories, yet a quarter of a
century after her death John Gielgud was still describing her as “the greatest
individual performer that America has ever given us” (Draper 1979, p. xi). Draper’s
speciality was the solo character performance, the creation alone on stage and by
the simplest theatrical means (a table, chairs, a sofa set against a plain velvet
Through her reactions she told the audience everything they needed to know about
the children, animals, lovers, servants, husbands of her characters. Her show, Ruth
Draper and Her Company of Characters, for which she created more than sixty
characters, was performed all over the world. It was on one of these tours that Yat
Malmgren met her in May 1940; he later adopted her use of the monologue as the
means by which his students assimilated the main features of his System.
12
Acting the Metaphor
the viewer infers from the protagonists’ reactions. The text we hear is therefore part
of a dialogue or dialogues of which we only have access to one side. The spectator’s
as to express with as much clarity as possible the underlying Inner Attitude and the
The earliest scenarios focus on one of the Variations of an Inner Attitude, drawing on
simple experiences from the student’s daily life (arguing with a bank manager for an
the other tempi described by the System. As the course progresses, scenarios
became more complex and abandon the mundane for sophisticated, imagined
worlds. Indeed, student scenarios are at their worst when their matter is the
general, lovers. They are at their best when, like the accomplished models in Ruth
Draper’s work, they transport us to a different realm and display a strong feel for
what Yat Malmgren called “the third circle”: a layer of meaning which transcends the
internal worlds of the actors and their immediate relationships (the “second circle”)
and extends to matters of social or political import - a Nazi doctor maintaining the
purity of his ideals even after the fall of the Reich; a Rwandan lay preacher come to
extract his wayward sister from a Paris brothel. These complex, though concise
(usually around five minutes’ long) ‘Character Scenarios’ access the full complexity
of an Inner Attitude and its four Variations, as well as deploying a full panoply of
Action Attitudes.
13
Acting the Metaphor
Ruth Draper was famous for not drawing on people from her direct circle of
acquaintances when devising her characters; in the same way, student scenarios
shine when they are acts of vivid imagination, furthest removed from autobiography
as well as direct observation. In all cases, the entry point into the tempi of the
of self materialises8. The actor can similarly manipulate posture or silhouette, gait
and dominant gesture patterns, thus accessing the psychological make-up of the
character by way of the physical. The actor Michael Mears offers an example of the
the writing, Mears considered Mr Brisk’s defining Working Actions to be Dabbing and
Flicking. He then took as his point of departure the image of a young boy trying to
keep a balloon in the air - a never-ending series of Dabs. From this simple gesture
themselves, you notice them, then you begin to exaggerate a little, pull back,
eventually incorporate them consciously into the physical life of the character."
Occasionally, two or three such Shadow Moves may be distilled into “significant
gestures” - gestures expressive of the character essence. From these arose a tempo
which was eventually stitched into the character’s patterns of speech. Sooner or
later, says Mears, the physical tempo must affect the psychology of the character.
The actor and director Simon Callow also quotes playing a character that "came to
him" as someone who traces convoluted, delicate round Gliding shapes with his
fingers. This gesture arose spontaneously; Callow then consciously refined it into a
14
Acting the Metaphor
deliberate expression of the essence of the character, to the point where he felt able
to say, quoting Michael Chekhov, that "the gesture was the character"(personal
communication).
In Simon Callow’s production of Les enfants du paradis10 the part originally played by
Arletti in Jean-Louis Barrault's seminal film was taken by Helen McCrory. Director
and actress are both graduates of the Drama Centre and were able to discuss the
character using the terms of the System. Surprisingly, Callow recalls, by analysing its
Action Attitudes they reached the conclusion that the character’s Inner Attitude was
Stable (sensuous and cunning). On the page as well as in Arletti's interpretation, the
character had presented Action Attitudes in the pairs Adream/Awake (the loving,
erotic Outer Action hiding an inner element of calculation) and Near/Remote (the
sufficiency).This pointed, Callow reasoned, to the Inner Attitude of Stable, with the
to emotion. Not an obvious interpretation for one of the most romantic figures of
twentieth century drama, yet one which imposed itself upon consideration within the
An articulate account of this type of analysis was also offered by the actress Kate
Garthside, another former student of Yat Malmgren at the Drama Centre (personal
Emile Zola's L'assommoir11 and her initial impulse, responding to the character's
tragic emotional life, was to consider it to be Adream. Testing this initial reaction by
using the System, Garthside reconsidered the character's Inner Attitude by asking
15
Acting the Metaphor
what its conscious Super-Objective was and took the view that the character was
defined by her social limitations, her working class status. Gervaise was a tragic
character indeed, but unaware of her tragedy, thus lacking one of the defining
conclusion was that the character was in fact Near; emotion played a part, but was
The difference in Inner Attitude came to the fore when Garthside rehearsed the high
point of the role’s trajectory: Gervaise is faced with the loss of her laundry, the
source of her livelihood and status, and after a long silence during which she has
been listening to the men around her discussing her plight she screams: "I hate the
fucking lot of you" and walks out banging the door. That single line had to express
the way in which the character deals with her feelings at a moment of major
significance. At first Garthside felt her emotion was "flying out" and was "acted",
birth by the 'spade's a spade' attitude of the character. The tempo of the line, coming
from a Near Inner Attitude, now changed to a Punch. From this sensation, arrived at
through analysis of the Inner Attitude and amplified by the physical work deriving
from it, the actress was able to build a character defined by the characteristics of
Near. Garthside considers that she might have reached the same conclusion even
without her training, intuitively, but that without the vocabulary of the system she
would not have arrived at "the clarity of thought" which gave her intuition a
16
Acting the Metaphor
methodically.
and historical data from the dramatic text and its contexts; draws inferences
from these regarding the psychological traits of the character; then gives
A synthetic phase, which is reached at the point where the analysis has been
integrated into a tempo which expresses the Inner Attitude as well as serving
While the dramatic action – people, events, circumstances - of the narrative retains
phenomenological (Hayes 2010). Overall, actors trained in the System cite one
feature as its enduring legacy: the way in which as students it created a systematic
way of linking physical activity with mental and emotional states and thus
Yat Malmgren's work …addresses itself directly to the very nature of acting:
not 'What it is for?' or 'What are the conditions which give rise to it?' It
17
Acting the Metaphor
Yat… [put] psychological concepts into space, into action, into the physical
(http://www.firth.com/articles/03backstagewest.html)
How does one account for this perception of an “enormous amount of sense” about
the “very nature of acting”? I have suggested elsewhere (Mirodan 1997, p. 262) that
the utility as well as the fascination of the Laban-Malmgren System resided not in its
roots in a branch of psychology, now much contested 12, but in the fact that it
provided a sui generis language, a vocabulary describing its key concepts and a
‘syntax’ determining the relationship between these. I have also come to look at it as
a complex of metaphors.
In certain cases, as the neuro-linguist George Lakoff and the philosopher Michael
Johnson have argued, metaphors are not only a function of language, but integral to
use abstract concepts – is separate and distinct from our ability to perceive and to
move. This tradition considers that, as Lakoff and Johnson put it: “perception may
18
Acting the Metaphor
555 for a summary). According to this view, concepts and their properties are an
inevitable and necessary consequence of the way in which our bodies and brains are
structured, as well as of our interactions with one another and the physical world
around us (1999, p.37). We conflate abstract concepts (emotions, ideas) with other
etc.) and the two categories may be integrated at the neural level (Lakoff and
asserted that “words dance”. While from a strictly cognitive science point of view his
describing the type of “experiential gestalt” of which Lakoff and Johnson speak:
(1980/2003, p. 117)
When given expression in language, these conflations lead to what Lakoff and
welcome and of people we are close to; or of rising political tensions and falling
19
Acting the Metaphor
correlated with the sensory experience of warmth, of being held” (1999, p. 46);
equally, repeated observations in early childhood that liquids or piles of objects rise
as they increase and drop as they decrease link the subjective judgment of quantity
conflations give rise to what Lakoff and Johnson call “primary metaphors”:
(Direct/Flexible) with Thinking starts with the sensations of ‘towards’ and ‘away from’,
physically in the concave body of a listener doubting the speaker’s assertions (“Let
an idea is to move closer to it; to doubt or examine it is to distance oneself from it:
Thought and Space become inextricably linked. Similarly, linking Flow (Free/Bound)
to Feeling is rooted in the sensation of emotions flowing or being blocked through the
love flowed from his eyes; her heart stopped in her mouth; a wave of jealousy.
The System is built on a “foundational” (Lakoff and Johnson 1999, p. 542) metaphor:
perceived as movements: feelings run high and are jarring; thought goes (straight) to
20
Acting the Metaphor
the heart of the issue or around the problem; intuitions are quick off the mark or slow
to catch on; sensations are connected to gravity – heavy/strong smell; light touch.
sense; SPACE IS THINKING does not. The relationship between source and target
that of subjective judgment”” (Lakoff and Johnson 1999, p. 55), so to express the
idea that a character is highly emotional students of the System tend to say “he is
Mobile” or “he has a lot of Flow”, never “he is Feeling” or “Intuiting”; the original
source domain for Mobile (Feeling/Intuiting linked to Flow/Time) has been forgotten;
only the target domain remains – the association of intense emotion with agitation.
This makes this approach particularly attractive for actors whose starting points are
movement and physical sensation: the conflation of Mental Factors with Motion
Factors gives the analysis of character and action, of necessity involving abstract
1999, p. 128). Terms such as Remote, Stable, Mobile, etc., have immediately
accessible physical connotations. Thus, when directing, Simon Callow tends to use
the terms of the System even with actors who are not familiar with their specialised
meanings: "For example, I use Remote” Callow says, “because the word has a
suggestive value in itself, even without the specific images created in training."
Callow considers that the System offers actors, perhaps for the first time16, a
language akin to that available to musicians. He looks with envy at the fact that
musicians can talk of D major, 4/6 or mezzo-forte and know exactly what they mean.
21
Acting the Metaphor
The lexicon provided by the Laban-Malmgren System goes some way to offering a
“categorise, group and quantify them – and, by this means, reason about them”
(Lakoff and Johnson, 1980/2003, p. 25). Categorisation is not without its problems.
Grouping characters on the basis of personality traits (characteristics) into the six
Inner Attitudes is founded on two much-contested assumptions: that one can speak
behaviour; and that one can therefore classify human beings (and by extension
circumstances, but that these are aspects of a core identity, which we as individuals
recognise as our ‘I’ (see Benedetti 2005, p. 79). Fundamental to both Stanislavskian
and Jungian thinking, the notion of a personality essence has caused a great deal of
criticism is outside the scope of this article, but it needs to be acknowledged, while
thinking. Four decades ago Eleanor Rosch (1977) demonstrated on the basis of
“prototypes” and associating these with similar objects with which they bear a ‘family
resemblance’. Thus, she asserted, the concept bird arouses the “prototypical” image
of small, flying birds, while chickens or penguins, members of the same category,
22
Acting the Metaphor
“There is a close relationship between the person and the overall pattern in which he
participates”, says the early ethologist E.T. Hall (1959, p.138). An approach which
starts by describing the typical, as the System does, offers a useful set of reference
“There are few human instincts more basic than territoriality”, Lakoff and Johnson
territories and objects as containers and thus enabling “an act of quantification”
(diffuse) into Character (defined) in ways which we find most satisfying. This in turn
character using Laban’s Motion Factors. Does this restricting of the ever-changing
insofar as saying 'this is Sicily’ or ‘we are in the Rift Valley’ amounts to picking up a
segment of the overall landscape in order to be able to conceive it and interact with
it.
Actors using the System’s templates, Inner Attitudes in particular, are therefore
concerned with creating characters that are recognisably ‘typical’. Their initial focus
outer ‘skin’, of the silhouette of the character: the character is ‘designed’. This is not
to say that demeanour and action are artificial or ‘stylised’. In fact, the aesthetic
and personality on the one hand and the artificiality of the ‘designed’ character on
the other. Students often have to confront this paradox: the principles of
Expressionist art and dance (abstraction, distortion and exaggeration) hit against the
23
Acting the Metaphor
physical reality of the actor’s body as well as realistic (though not naturalistic) values
(the ‘shell’ of the character, as seen in its posture and gestures) containing actions
and reactions carried out with spontaneity and immediacy. These are not silhouettes
unusual and specialised form of deception (Trivers, 2011, building on Byrne and
Whiten, 1992/1998). Think - to take but two readily accessible examples - of Anthony
Hopkins playing Hannibal Lecter or Tom Hardy turning himself into Bronson. The
deliberate design of the physical ‘shell’ signals the deception, while natural(istic)
mechanisms. We are in a very satisfying ‘play’ in which we know from the outset that
a game is being played, yet we are allowed to involve ourselves fully in it. Kate
Garthside makes the point that characters which respond best to the System’s
approach are those written with "a universal intention", as metaphors for something
greater than the individuality of the person portrayed. The recognition through
physical signals of ‘what the character stands for’ depends on identifying the pattern
better to deceive. Spontaneous, reactive stage actions create believability. Two great
24
Acting the Metaphor
The System often works best for characters which represent "extremes". The very
notion of Inner and Action Attitudes implies concentration: the physical life of the
character, its speech and gestures, are boiled down to essentials; speech is refined
to its rhythms, to its musical patterns; gestures are reduced to their common
denominators and ultimately distilled to their 'spirit' - their tempo. This moves the
character outside the mundane. Kate Garthside gives as an example her approach
to playing Trish in Unsuitable for Adults by Terry Johnson17. The character is "fragile"
- not an individual fragility alone, but the distillation of the fragility of a whole type of
women. Garthside decided to make her more fragile than the bounds of naturalism
permitted: she lightened her voice, adopted a breathy tempo and an etiolated
posture, as if stretched on a rack. This, she says using the terminology of the
System, resulted in an Adream character whose Weight was so Light, and Flow so
Free that she radiated emotion to the point where she touched on Mobile.
The examples above point to the raison d'être of the System: transformation. The
system offers a route to the deliberate design of character, first and foremost as a
physical process. Posture, gait, gestural range, vocal qualities (pitch, timbre, tone,
which it is taught, the System sits alongside other approaches, as one aspect of a
25
Acting the Metaphor
complex curriculum, and actors assemble their personal methodologies from all
these influences. Nor do most actors using its tools expect it and its implied aesthetic
of transformation to dominate their work. They are aware that they might find
themselves in the TV studio in the morning, playing a role close to their ‘daily’
physical shape and accent; and that very evening appear unrecognisable on stage;
indeed, according to the requirements of the role find themselves at any number of
Nonetheless, those trained in the System find it difficult to discard altogether its core
teachings. The theoretical apparatus underpinning the System is complex, yet the
possibilities and fingers nimble enough to be able to play it. The System is
concerned, as Simon Callow puts it, "with making the word flesh, literally".
[6791 words]
26
Acting the Metaphor
REFERENCES
Bull, N. 1951. The Attitude Theory of Emotion, New York: Nervous and Mental
Diseases Monographs (Coolidge Foundation)
Carpenter, W. undated. “Conflict and Harmony Between Man And Woman (A Study
in Movement Expression)" Addlestone, Surrey: Art of Movement Studio (unpublished
typescript, author’s collection)
Clark, A. 2008. Supersizing the Mind: Embodiment, Action, and Cognitive Extension,
Oxford: Oxford University Press
Damasio, A. 1999. The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making
of Consciousness, London: William Heinemann
Ekman, P. and Friesen, W.V. 1969. “Non-verbal leakage and clues to deception”,
Psychiatry 32, pp. 88-106
27
Acting the Metaphor
Gallese, V. and G. Lakoff . 2005. The Brian’s Concepts: The Role of the sensory-
motor system in conceptual knowledge, Cognitive Neuropsychology 22, issues 3-4,
455-79
Hall, E.T. 1959/1980. The Silent Language, New York: The Greenwood Press
Hayes, J. 2010. The Knowing Body: Yat Malmgren's Acting Technique, Saarbrucken:
VDM Verlag Dr Muller
Hodgson, J. 1979. Mastering Movement: The Life and Work of Rudolf Laban,
London: Methuen
Jung, C.G. 1968/1990. Analytical Psychology, Its Theory and Practice (The
Tavistock Lectures), London: Ark Paperbacks
Jung, C.G. 1971. Psychological Types, trans. H. G. Baynes, vol. 6 of The Collected
Works, London: Routledge & Kegan Paul
Laban, R. 1961. "The Laban Lecture", Reprint of a 1939 speech. Laban Art of
Movement Guild Magazine, No. 26
Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M. 1980, second edition 2003. Metaphors We Live By,
Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M. 1999. Philosophy in the Flesh, The Embodied Mind and
Its Challenge to Western Thought, New York: Basic Books
Lamb, W. and Watson, E. 1979. Body Code, The Meaning in Movement, London:
Routledge and Kegan Paul
28
Acting the Metaphor
McCaw, D. (ed.) 2011. The Laban Sourcebook, Abingdon and New York: Routledge
North, M. 1958. "Scientific Penetration Gives Basis For Guidance And Treatment",
Laban Art of Movement Guild Magazine, No. 21
Stevens, A. 1986. Withymead, A Jungian Community for the Healing Arts, London:
Coventure
Trivers, R. 2011. Deceit and Self-Deception: Fooling Yourself the Better to Fool
Others, London: Allen Lane
Zabel, M. D. 1960. The Art of Ruth Draper, New York: Doubleday & Co. and Oxford
University Press
29
1
For, relatively scarce, material on William (Bill) Carpenter see Valerie Preston-Dunlop 1998, p.260; Mirodan
1997, pp. 26-28
2
For a detailed description of the materials handed to Yat Malmgren, see Mirodan 1997, pp. 33-34. A broad
outline of some Carpenter materials can be found in Hodgson 1979, pp.156-161. See also McCaw 2011, pp.351-
7.
3
Throughout this article, initial capitals denote that terms are used in the specific sense given them by the
Laban-Malmgren System and its predecessors: Jung, Laban, Stanislavsky.
4
This is a greatly simplified account of Jung’s ideas on personality. See Psychological Types and for a more
accessible outline, The Tavistock Lectures; and Mirodan 1997, pp.57-83 for details of what the Laban-Malmgren
System took from Jung’s theory of types and the differences between its understanding and Jung’s writing.
5
Defining this holistic conflation has exercised thinkers across disciplines: Warren Lamb, a Laban disciple,
called a similar model the “Posture-Gesture-Merger (PGM)” (Lamb and Watson 1979, p.90); A.R. Luria speaks
of a “kinetic melody” (1973; also Sofia 2013, p.84); Sheets-Johnstone (2009, p. 393) cites Luria but introduces
the Husserl-inspired concept of “primal animation”, a “spatial-temporal-energic whole” (1996); see also Carol-
Lynne Moore in McCaw 2011, p. 319.
6
Here and in what follows, the / indicates that the two elements should be read in either direction. Thus, one
Stable character can be Sensing/Thinking, where the Sensing aspect is dominant, while another Stable
character is Thinking/Sensing, where the Thinking aspect prevails, and so on.
7
"…shadow movements…are tiny muscular movements such as the raising of the brow, the jerking of the hand
or the tapping of the foot, which have none other than expressive value. They are usually done unconsciously
and often accompany movements of purposeful action like a shadow - hence the term" (Laban1950/1971, p.12).
See also North 1958, p.15 and McCaw 2011, p.355 for Shadow Moves revealing the Inner Attitude. For the
capacity of involuntary moves to reveal inner states see Ekman and Friesen 1969.
8
This is a fairly standard process used by Stanislavskian teachers (see, for example, Benedetti 2005, p.22),
echoing some of the exercises described by Michael Chekhov (himself influenced by Laban [1953, p. 13] and, in
turn, an important influence on Yat Malmgren).
9
On a National tour, with Paul Eddington in the lead
10
Royal Shakespeare Company, Barbican Theatre, 1996
11
West Yorkshire Playhouse, Leeds, 1992
12
Jung’s description of personality types is considered obsolete, with the consensus in the psychology of trait
and type now crystallising around the Five-Factor Model (Wiggins 1996).
13
Although Nina Bull’s contemporaneous research on postural attitudes in hypnotised patients (1951) and
Joseph de Rivera’s subsequent analysis of the connections between emotions and movement attitudes (1977)
offer at least some observational support for Laban’s and Carpenter’s intuitive assumptions.
14
Here and below I follow Lakoff’s and Johnson’s convention of using capitals to denote representative
metaphors.
15
Conceptual Metaphor Theory (CMT) is not without its challenges. As Lakoff and Johnson themselves point
out, neurological research supporting it is not yet conclusive (1999 pp. 38, 42; see also Gainotti 2004) although,
working alongside the eminent neurophysiologist Vittorio Gallese, Lakoff attempted to strengthen the argument
(2005).The perceived over-extension of the concept of embodiment has been the subject of sustained criticism:
see, for example, Sheets-Johnstone 2009 for a phenomenology-based critique, especially pp. 386, 397; Clark
2008; and Tribble and Sutton in Shaughnessy 2013, locs. 791-807 for a brief overview. I nonetheless find it a
useful framework, while acknowledging that it remains a fluid hypothesis, not yet fulfilling scientific tests of
falsifiability and repeatability.
16
See Sheets-Johnstone 2009, p. 390 on the limitations of describing phenomenological experience in
language; and Zarilli 2002, p.16 on the specific case of metaphors for acting experiences.
17
Library Theatre, Manchester
18
See also Sofia in Shaughnessy, ed., locs. 3802ff. for recent (and incomplete) experiments designed to
ascertain whether actor training can change cognitive capacities at a fundamental level.