100% found this document useful (1 vote)
414 views31 pages

Acting The Metaphor The Laban-Malmgren System of Movement Psychology and Character Analysis

The Laban-Malmgren System of Movement Psychology and Character Analysis is an influential acting training method developed in the 1950s. It combines Rudolf Laban's ideas on movement with Jungian psychology and Stanislavskian acting techniques. Yat Malmgren took notes from Laban and William Carpenter and developed them into a coherent acting training system. The system links Jung's four psychological functions (Sensing, Thinking, Intuiting, Feeling) with Laban's four dimensions of movement (Weight, Time, Space, Flow) to analyze characters and inform physical acting choices. The system is still taught in conservatories today and influenced many famous actors like Anthony Hopkins and Tom Hardy.

Uploaded by

Jenna Zhu
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as DOCX, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
100% found this document useful (1 vote)
414 views31 pages

Acting The Metaphor The Laban-Malmgren System of Movement Psychology and Character Analysis

The Laban-Malmgren System of Movement Psychology and Character Analysis is an influential acting training method developed in the 1950s. It combines Rudolf Laban's ideas on movement with Jungian psychology and Stanislavskian acting techniques. Yat Malmgren took notes from Laban and William Carpenter and developed them into a coherent acting training system. The system links Jung's four psychological functions (Sensing, Thinking, Intuiting, Feeling) with Laban's four dimensions of movement (Weight, Time, Space, Flow) to analyze characters and inform physical acting choices. The system is still taught in conservatories today and influenced many famous actors like Anthony Hopkins and Tom Hardy.

Uploaded by

Jenna Zhu
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as DOCX, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 31

Acting the Metaphor

Acting the Metaphor

The Laban-Malmgren System


of Movement Psychology and Character Analysis

Theatre, Dance and Performance Training Journal

Vladimir Mirodan
Professor of Theatre
Central Saint Martins
University of the Arts London
Granary Building
1 Granary Square
London N1C 1AA
Tel. ((PA – Maggie Wilkinson): 020 7514 9363
Tel. (home): 020 7281 3139
[email protected]

Not for review

0
Acting the Metaphor

Acting the Metaphor

The Laban-Malmgren System

of Movement Psychology and Character Analysis

The Laban-Malmgren System of Movement Psychology and Character Analysis, an

influential training method of the post-war era, has helped to shape the thinking and

work of some of the most expressive actors of our time, from Anthony Hopkins to

Tom Hardy. The System is an original synthesis of Rudolf Laban’s ideas on

movement, Jungian typology and Stanislavskian physical action, brought together by

the Swedish dancer and acting teacher Yat Malmgren (1916-2002). He taught the

System to generations of actors, first at his private Movement Studio in the West

End, then at RADA and the CSSD and from 1963 until 2001 at the school he co-

founded, Drama Centre London. It continues to be taught in conservatoires across

the world.

The System originated in work developed in the early fifties by Rudolf Laban and one

of his late-career collaborators, William Carpenter 1. Carpenter was inspired by the

belief that "words dance", that they could be made to convey psychological

experiences which hitherto had only been intimated through physical sensation.

Working alongside Laban, he attempted a synthesis between the latter’s ideas on

movement expression and aspects of Jungian psychology, drawing in particular on

Jung’s influential book on Psychological Types. Through Carpenter's diligent, if not

always accurate, readings in Jung the confluence of two major intellectual traditions -

Jung and Laban - had begun to bear fruit in a planned book relating to a

1
Acting the Metaphor

classification of human types according to their psychology and the way in which this

was reflected in movement. However, in June 1954 Carpenter died unexpectedly

(Preston-Dunlop 1998, p.263) and Laban, feeling unable to develop the work on his

own, entrusted Yat Malmgren with the notes Carpenter had left behind 2. In the years

that followed, by linking Laban’s and Carpenter’s work to Stanislavskian thinking on

character and action (‘physical action’, Michael Chekhov, some American

developments - notably Uta Hagen), Yat Malmgren struck out on his own, developing

what Carpenter had conceived as a general ‘movement psychology’ into a narrower,

yet coherent and useable training System for actors.

Laban’s and Carpenter’s approach was based on a deceptively simple idea: that

between certain psychological and certain physically-based concepts there is a

direct correspondence. Jung had argued that the psyche could be described in

terms of four psychological functions: Sensing, Thinking, Intuiting and Feeling 3. He

also maintained that personalities developed in accordance with the ‘best foot

foremost’ principle: that people recognised early in life that one or the other of these

functions secured approval and success and therefore tended to rely on it and

underplay the others. As a result, personalities are shaped by the “habitual” reliance

on one of the functions (Jung 1971, p.482) and Jung describes a “Sensing type”, a

“Feeling type” and so on. A particular personality also continues to draw on the other

functions, so Jung distinguished between a “superior” and subordinate functions.

Thus, one person could be said to be primarily a Sensing type, but to use Intuition as

the “auxiliary” function (or vice versa)4. Moreover, following the James-Lange theory

of affect, Jung also emphasised the physiological foundation of psychological

processes, emotion in particular, and the mutually reinforcing relationship between

2
Acting the Metaphor

the two. “I regard affect” – Jung writes – “on the one hand as a psychic feeling-state

and on the other as a physiological innervation-state, each of which has a

cumulative, reciprocal effect on the other." (1971, p. 412).

This was the fundamental thesis Carpenter had absorbed from Jung. He and Laban

then decided to test the theory in practice: they observed the movement patterns of a

group of people undergoing analysis at the Withymead Centre (McCaw 2011, pp.

306-16), a clinic run by two Jungian analysts who, as part of their arts-orientated

therapeutic programme, encouraged their patients to attend movement classes

(Hodgson 1979, pp.75-78; Stevens 1986, p.151).The habitual movements of the

analysands were classified using the four elements into which Laban had long

argued all movement could be analysed: the four dimensions or ‘Motion Factors’ of

Weight, Time, Space and Flow (1950/1971, p.22).

In order to determine the dimensions of movement Laban undertook a sort of

'chemical analysis'. He studied the various 'molecules' and 'atoms' which made up

movement with the aid of four 'instruments', four fundamental questions:

a. which part of the body moves and what relationship exists between it and those

parts of the body which remain still?

b. what is the duration of the movement and in what relationship does it stand to

other movement durations around it?

c. how much muscular power is exerted in effecting the movement?

d. which direction in space is the movement leading towards or away from?

3
Acting the Metaphor

Laban observed that in order to lift a heavy weight the muscles strain to overcome

the force of gravity. Equally, lifting a light object necessitated only the lightest of

muscle exertions. Thus, Laban declared, one of the four components of movement is

its Weight, which, according to the intensity of the energy deployed, can be either

Strong or Light.

Movements also have a direction in Space. Someone hammering will go as cleanly

as possible between the raised arm and the nail - the movement is Direct. But a

coachman whipping his horses will raise the whip, move it first to the left of his head,

then to the right and only then crack it forward. The movement is convoluted,

roundabout, Flexible.

Movements take place in Time. Here Laban pointed out that the important factor was

not whether the movement was slow or quick, but how slow or quick in comparison

with other movements around it, either of parts of the same body or of different

objects. Thus a movement is said to be Quick or Sustained not in an absolute way,

but in comparison with its surroundings.

Finally, effective movements involve a degree of co-ordination: they are either free

and easy or halting and tight. This Laban defined as the Flow of movement, which

could be either Free or Bound.

These components of movement rearrange themselves constantly: now one, now

the other occupies the foreground, while the others take a supporting role. When a

movement is expressive, the components are "in harmony" (Laban 1951, p.6). For

4
Acting the Metaphor

Laban, movement was a psychophysical event, incorporating both physical motion

and the psychological processes associated with it (1950/1971, p.22). The harmony

of the components of physical movement was therefore an indication of the harmony

of the inner being. Conversely, an inability to give physical expression to one's

innermost feelings and sensations was a sign of inner conflict. In extreme cases this

could lead to an "un-balancing" of the personality (Kosterlitz 1950, p.6; Laban in

McCaw 2011, p.320). But, rather than look for specific gestural or postural

manifestations of psychological disturbance, as some psychoanalysts working in the

same period attempted to do (see, for example, Deutsch 1952; Reich 1933/1945;

Lowen 1958), Laban and Carpenter linked the component elements of movement,

their Motion Factors, with the 'elements' of the psyche, Jung's four psychological

functions, which they called their ‘Mental Factors’. They argued that:

Sensing corresponds to Weight

Intuiting corresponds to Time

Thinking corresponds to Space

Feeling corresponds to Flow.

These connections were purely experiential. No proof existed, apart from that offered

by their observations, which told them that a person's 'weighty presence' was

associated with her sensuous engagement with the world; that a 'thinker' tended to

go 'in and out' of his ‘inner space’ in order to solve a problem. Indeed, Laban’s

journey to this conclusion was fraught with hesitations (Laban 1947, pp. 57-9;

1950/71, pp. 126-7) and even Carpenter sounds a little defensive on this topic when

he writes:

5
Acting the Metaphor

When we are asked 'How can you prove that, say, a movement in Space

reveals ‘Thinking and Attention', we must reply that our proof is empirical and

that it is based on long continued observations. Psychological theory is based

equally on empiricism and is not invalidated thereby (1954, p.3).

Elsewhere Carpenter elaborates a little on the nature of the observations which had

led Laban to make this significant leap:

In our research into this relationship between bodily movements and inner

emotion, we have been aided by students who had had an Analysis for a

relatively long period under the Freudian or Jungian methods before joining us

for training in Movement. In every instance the information obtained by the

Movement Analyst from a study of the student's movements, has

corresponded with the findings of the Psycho-Analyst... It is mainly through

the comparison of such coupled findings that we arrived at the coordination of

Flow & Feeling, Time & Intuition, Space & Thinking and Weight & Sense

Perception (1954, p.19).

While Carpenter’s focus had been on the therapeutic application of Laban’s ideas,

Yat Malmgren sensed their potential as an approach for actor training. Building on

Carpenter’s foundation and incorporating other important concepts from Laban’s

published work - Working Actions, Subconscious Motifs, Externalised Drives, etc. –

Malmgren constructed a System designed to sensitise actors to the links between

physical activity and mental and emotional states. In this general overview of this

6
Acting the Metaphor

complex System, I only propose to focus on two of its salient features: the links

described above and the classification of theatre characters flowing from it.

As we have seen, the System in founded on the observation that not only can one

perceive which of the mental functions is dominant in an individual personality

through the way in which a person moves, but that the more a person moves in a

certain way, the more that movement pattern reinforces the personality bias. Thus,

the System is founded on the mutuality of the Mental and Motion Factors within what

Laban was already calling the “body mind”. This interdependence has since been

reasserted both philosophically (Sheets-Johnstone 2005, p. 379 for a

phenomenological approach) and by recent neuropsychological research (Damasio

1999, p.86; Edelman and Tononi 2000), enabling theatre scholars to speak of a

“reflexive feedback loop” (Kemp 2012, p.61) of embodiment, in which gesture,

verbalisation and impulse form an inseparable whole.

Laban and Malmgren think of this unit as a form of integrated psychological and

physical ‘movement’ and the System describes it as a tempo 5, a ‘cocktail’ of Motion

and Mental Factors through which one can both conceive and perceive a character.

The System describes this psychophysical process using the terminology of Jung’s

types. In so doing, the System simplifies Jung's ideas considerably: Jung had divided

the functions along a major fault line, into rational (Thinking and Feeling) and

irrational (Sensing and Intuiting). These were said to be incompatible with one

another: a personality which overemphasised its rational functions suppressed the

irrational and vice versa. The Laban-Malmgren System disregards this

incompatibility and assembles its theatre character typology by combining

7
Acting the Metaphor

psychological functions across the rational/irrational divide. By considering that any

of the functions can be the superior function and combining it with any of the other

three, the System arrives at a total of six broad types of theatre character. These

Laban named his six Inner Attitudes. They are usually presented as pairs of

opposites:

 A Sensing/Thinking6 character type called Stable

 An Intuiting/Feeling character type called Mobile

 A Sensing/Intuiting character type called Near

 A Thinking/Feeling character type called Remote

 A Sensing/Feeling character type called Adream

 A Thinking/Intuiting character type called Awake.

These categories are in effect clusters of psychophysical characteristics,

engendered and shaped by the character’s Super-Objective. A character whose

primary motivation revolves around sensuous desires, whose main concern is its

own physical well-being (Moliere's malade imaginaire, for example) belongs to

materialistic Near. On the other hand, characters whose lives are dominated by

emotion (Ophelia, say, or Hedda) are considered Adream. These decisions do not

represent restrictions on the actor's freedom of interpretation any more than

choosing a Super-Objective does. An actor playing Iago could choose as his primary

source of motivation the character's resentment at being overlooked for promotion,

his sexual jealousy or class and racial enmity - all Sensing-rooted desires typical of a

8
Acting the Metaphor

Near character. But should he decide to find his main motivation in Iago's emotional

dependence on Othello, on unacknowledged love for the Moor and hatred of himself,

the character will be considered Adream.

Here a further level of complexity intervenes. In the practice of expressive

movement, Laban considered his Motion Factors as points on a continuum between

what he called their “Yielding” and “Contending” Elements or extremes:

Weight moves along a spectrum of Light to Strong

Space moves along a continuum of Flexible to Direct

Time moves along a continuum of Sustained to Quick

Flow moves along a continuum of Free to Bound.

The actor developing an Inner Attitude can therefore choose Variations which are

combinations of these elements. At any given moment, a Stable character, for

example, inhabits one of the following Variations:

Light/Flexible

Light/Direct

Strong/Flexible

Strong/Direct

The System thus analyses the tempi of characters in terms of Weight, Space, Time

and Flow. Character traits described by adjectives such as 'dynamic', 'relaxed',

9
Acting the Metaphor

'powerful', 'indecisive' and a myriad others, are ultimately defined in terms of Light or

Strong, Sustained or Quick and so on.

Yat Malmgren then extends this analysis from the realm of Character to that of

Action and subdivides the latter into an Inner and an Outer Action.

The Inner Action denotes the thought-process of the character, its 'stream of

consciousness'. It is therefore a psychological process, an internal layer through

which the impulse passes before emerging into physical gesture.

The Outer Action consists of the 'doings' of the character. These include the

psychological as well as the physical Activities the character plays on other

characters. They are the means by which the character achieves its desires - the

Objectives.

Inner and Outer Actions are described with the same terminology as the Inner

Attitudes. An Inner Attitude cannot express itself through an Action Attitude which is

its direct opposite (Stable through Mobile; Adream through Awake, etc.), but has at

its disposal the two remaining pairs as Inner and Outer Actions. Thus, a Stable

character might play an Adream (Feeling/Sensing – an inner monologue of cold

rage, perhaps, or of intense, altruistic concern) Inner Action, covered by an Awake

(Thinking/Intuiting) Outer Action - the visible expression of both Character and Inner

Action in a cool, poised and perhaps manipulative attitude towards the other

characters in the scene. A kind of ‘model of the atom’ structure emerges, with the

10
Acting the Metaphor

Inner Attitude (the character essence) constant yet hidden at the centre and the

Action Attitudes revolving at speed around it.

Malmgren’s way of looking at the relationship between Character and Action

constituted a substantial departure from Laban’s original concept of Inner Attitudes.

For Laban the Inner Attitudes referred to different states of mind or inclinations within

the same, real-life, person. They were supposed to generate movement in an

unmediated way: a certain Inner Attitude would find expression through its

corresponding Motion Factors; changing the Motion Factors would result in

psychological changes, with therapeutic outcomes. In applying these concepts to

acting, Yat Malmgren separated Character from Action and described a dialectic

relationship between Inner Attitude and Action Attitudes and their expression in

movement:

The Inner Attitude is revealed through:

a. posture

b. shadow moves7.

The Inner Action is revealed through:

semi-voluntary yet recurring moves, akin to Michael Chekhov’s “psychological

gesture”.

The Outer Action is revealed through:

a. (conscious) psychological activities

b. (consciously designed) physical activities.

11
Acting the Metaphor

Pedagogically, most students of the System are introduced to its concepts by means

of practical exercises known as ‘scenarios’. Exercises in dramaturgy as well as

acting imagination, scenarios are short monologues devised in order to embody one

or the other Inner Attitude and its respective Actions and are adaptations of a

performance style made famous by the American actress Ruth Draper (Zabel 1966).

A major performer from the twenties to the fifties, Ruth Draper (1884-1956) has

faded from all but the most assiduous of theatrical memories, yet a quarter of a

century after her death John Gielgud was still describing her as “the greatest

individual performer that America has ever given us” (Draper 1979, p. xi). Draper’s

speciality was the solo character performance, the creation alone on stage and by

the simplest theatrical means (a table, chairs, a sofa set against a plain velvet

curtain; a modest dress, at times a shawl or a carefully chosen hat) of strongly

projected characters and – miraculously – of the worlds in which these existed.

Through her reactions she told the audience everything they needed to know about

the children, animals, lovers, servants, husbands of her characters. Her show, Ruth

Draper and Her Company of Characters, for which she created more than sixty

characters, was performed all over the world. It was on one of these tours that Yat

Malmgren met her in May 1940; he later adopted her use of the monologue as the

means by which his students assimilated the main features of his System.

Monologue is perhaps misleading, because – although actors are alone on stage –

the dramaturgical structure inherited from Draper requires them to surround

themselves with a host of ‘invisible partners’- imaginary characters whose presence

12
Acting the Metaphor

the viewer infers from the protagonists’ reactions. The text we hear is therefore part

of a dialogue or dialogues of which we only have access to one side. The spectator’s

imagination is rewarded; the actors in turn concentrate on honing their reactions so

as to express with as much clarity as possible the underlying Inner Attitude and the

specific Actions to which this gives rise.

The earliest scenarios focus on one of the Variations of an Inner Attitude, drawing on

simple experiences from the student’s daily life (arguing with a bank manager for an

overdraft; putting a recalcitrant sibling to bed; etc.).They provide a sensation of what

it is to be in, say, Light/Sustained mode, as opposed to Strong/Sustained or any of

the other tempi described by the System. As the course progresses, scenarios

became more complex and abandon the mundane for sophisticated, imagined

worlds. Indeed, student scenarios are at their worst when their matter is the

exploration of private angst or adolescent resentments, against parents, authority in

general, lovers. They are at their best when, like the accomplished models in Ruth

Draper’s work, they transport us to a different realm and display a strong feel for

what Yat Malmgren called “the third circle”: a layer of meaning which transcends the

internal worlds of the actors and their immediate relationships (the “second circle”)

and extends to matters of social or political import - a Nazi doctor maintaining the

purity of his ideals even after the fall of the Reich; a Rwandan lay preacher come to

extract his wayward sister from a Paris brothel. These complex, though concise

(usually around five minutes’ long) ‘Character Scenarios’ access the full complexity

of an Inner Attitude and its four Variations, as well as deploying a full panoply of

Action Attitudes.

13
Acting the Metaphor

Ruth Draper was famous for not drawing on people from her direct circle of

acquaintances when devising her characters; in the same way, student scenarios

shine when they are acts of vivid imagination, furthest removed from autobiography

as well as direct observation. In all cases, the entry point into the tempi of the

character portrayed is a small gesture, performed repeatedly, until an altered sense

of self materialises8. The actor can similarly manipulate posture or silhouette, gait

and dominant gesture patterns, thus accessing the psychological make-up of the

character by way of the physical. The actor Michael Mears offers an example of the

way in which this approach translates into professional practice (personal

communication). In the nineties Mears played Mr Brisk, a character in The Double

Dealer by Wycherley9. As his name indicates, Mr. Brisk is a busy-body, a buffoon

obsessed with fashion, surface and making an impression. Responding intuitively to

the writing, Mears considered Mr Brisk’s defining Working Actions to be Dabbing and

Flicking. He then took as his point of departure the image of a young boy trying to

keep a balloon in the air - a never-ending series of Dabs. From this simple gesture

came a series of Shadow Moves. Shadow Moves, Mears observes, "suggest

themselves, you notice them, then you begin to exaggerate a little, pull back,

eventually incorporate them consciously into the physical life of the character."

Occasionally, two or three such Shadow Moves may be distilled into “significant

gestures” - gestures expressive of the character essence. From these arose a tempo

which was eventually stitched into the character’s patterns of speech. Sooner or

later, says Mears, the physical tempo must affect the psychology of the character.

The actor and director Simon Callow also quotes playing a character that "came to

him" as someone who traces convoluted, delicate round Gliding shapes with his

fingers. This gesture arose spontaneously; Callow then consciously refined it into a

14
Acting the Metaphor

deliberate expression of the essence of the character, to the point where he felt able

to say, quoting Michael Chekhov, that "the gesture was the character"(personal

communication).

In Simon Callow’s production of Les enfants du paradis10 the part originally played by

Arletti in Jean-Louis Barrault's seminal film was taken by Helen McCrory. Director

and actress are both graduates of the Drama Centre and were able to discuss the

character using the terms of the System. Surprisingly, Callow recalls, by analysing its

Action Attitudes they reached the conclusion that the character’s Inner Attitude was

Stable (sensuous and cunning). On the page as well as in Arletti's interpretation, the

character had presented Action Attitudes in the pairs Adream/Awake (the loving,

erotic Outer Action hiding an inner element of calculation) and Near/Remote (the

street urchin with a hidden loneliness and a melancholy sense of self-

sufficiency).This pointed, Callow reasoned, to the Inner Attitude of Stable, with the

character constantly distancing itself from the temptation of Mobility, of succumbing

to emotion. Not an obvious interpretation for one of the most romantic figures of

twentieth century drama, yet one which imposed itself upon consideration within the

terms of the System.

An articulate account of this type of analysis was also offered by the actress Kate

Garthside, another former student of Yat Malmgren at the Drama Centre (personal

communication). Garthside played the character of Gervaise in an adaptation of

Emile Zola's L'assommoir11 and her initial impulse, responding to the character's

tragic emotional life, was to consider it to be Adream. Testing this initial reaction by

using the System, Garthside reconsidered the character's Inner Attitude by asking

15
Acting the Metaphor

what its conscious Super-Objective was and took the view that the character was

defined by her social limitations, her working class status. Gervaise was a tragic

character indeed, but unaware of her tragedy, thus lacking one of the defining

aspects of Adream, awareness of the heightened emotional state. Garthside's

conclusion was that the character was in fact Near; emotion played a part, but was

not intrinsic to the character essence.

The difference in Inner Attitude came to the fore when Garthside rehearsed the high

point of the role’s trajectory: Gervaise is faced with the loss of her laundry, the

source of her livelihood and status, and after a long silence during which she has

been listening to the men around her discussing her plight she screams: "I hate the

fucking lot of you" and walks out banging the door. That single line had to express

the way in which the character deals with her feelings at a moment of major

significance. At first Garthside felt her emotion was "flying out" and was "acted",

"over-indulgent" - an Adream, self-conscious display, expressed vocally and

gesturally as an elongated Slash. However, in Near characters, Garthside

remembered from her experience of Near scenarios, emotion is “halted”, strangled at

birth by the 'spade's a spade' attitude of the character. The tempo of the line, coming

from a Near Inner Attitude, now changed to a Punch. From this sensation, arrived at

through analysis of the Inner Attitude and amplified by the physical work deriving

from it, the actress was able to build a character defined by the characteristics of

Near. Garthside considers that she might have reached the same conclusion even

without her training, intuitively, but that without the vocabulary of the system she

would not have arrived at "the clarity of thought" which gave her intuition a

16
Acting the Metaphor

"definition" necessary to construct characters such as Gervaise consciously and

methodically.

The application of the System is thus predicated on two interdependent processes,

familiar from Stanislavskian practice:

 An analytic phase, during which the actor collects biographical, sociological

and historical data from the dramatic text and its contexts; draws inferences

from these regarding the psychological traits of the character; then gives

these characteristics physical expression

 A synthetic phase, which is reached at the point where the analysis has been

integrated into a tempo which expresses the Inner Attitude as well as serving

as a shortcut for accessing the character.

While the dramatic action – people, events, circumstances - of the narrative retains

through analysis a prescriptive control on the behaviour of the character, both

learning and application of the System are fundamentally experiential and

phenomenological (Hayes 2010). Overall, actors trained in the System cite one

feature as its enduring legacy: the way in which as students it created a systematic

way of linking physical activity with mental and emotional states and thus

enabled them to give words to their sensations. They retain an enduring

perception of a phenomenological level of truth in the propositions put forward by the

System. Simon Callow writes:

Yat Malmgren's work …addresses itself directly to the very nature of acting:

not 'What it is for?' or 'What are the conditions which give rise to it?' It

17
Acting the Metaphor

attempts to say what it is. It amounts to a praxis of character in action, an

account of the physical embodiment of character and impulse (1984, p.39,

emphasised in the original).

And Oscar-winner Colin Firth:

Yat… [put] psychological concepts into space, into action, into the physical

world…. I found that after a couple of years of it, it started to make an

enormous amount of sense; it came as close as anything anybody really can

to teaching acting... It made sense to me, and I still use it."

(http://www.firth.com/articles/03backstagewest.html)

How does one account for this perception of an “enormous amount of sense” about

the “very nature of acting”? I have suggested elsewhere (Mirodan 1997, p. 262) that

the utility as well as the fascination of the Laban-Malmgren System resided not in its

roots in a branch of psychology, now much contested 12, but in the fact that it

provided a sui generis language, a vocabulary describing its key concepts and a

‘syntax’ determining the relationship between these. I have also come to look at it as

a complex of metaphors.

In certain cases, as the neuro-linguist George Lakoff and the philosopher Michael

Johnson have argued, metaphors are not only a function of language, but integral to

consciousness. In traditional ‘faculty psychology’ our ability to reason - to form and

use abstract concepts – is separate and distinct from our ability to perceive and to

move. This tradition considers that, as Lakoff and Johnson put it: “perception may

inform reason, and movement may be a consequence of reason, but…no aspect of

18
Acting the Metaphor

perception or movement is part of reason” and that consequently “there is assumed

to be an absolute dichotomy between perception and conception” (1999, p.37, italics

in original). In their influential work on Conceptual Metaphor Theory, Lakoff and

Johnson proposed an alternative perspective, that of “embodied reason” (1999, p.

555 for a summary). According to this view, concepts and their properties are an

inevitable and necessary consequence of the way in which our bodies and brains are

structured, as well as of our interactions with one another and the physical world

around us (1999, p.37). We conflate abstract concepts (emotions, ideas) with other

concepts of which we have a direct, physical experience (spatial orientation, objects,

etc.) and the two categories may be integrated at the neural level (Lakoff and

Gallese 2005, p. 456).This, I believe, is what Carpenter had intuited when he

asserted that “words dance”. While from a strictly cognitive science point of view his

and Laban’s association of psychological descriptors with physical dimensions may

be considered arbitrary13, they nonetheless give the actor a systematic way of

describing the type of “experiential gestalt” of which Lakoff and Johnson speak:

“coherent organizations of our experiences in terms of natural dimensions”

(1980/2003, p. 117)

When given expression in language, these conflations lead to what Lakoff and

Johnson describe as “metaphoric thinking”. We speak, for example, of a warm

welcome and of people we are close to; or of rising political tensions and falling

prices. Such metaphors are “based on cross-domain correlations”: a source domain

(the sensorimotor experience through which we perceive temperature, distance,

gravity, etc.) is conflated with a target-domain, that of subjective judgment (intimacy,

emotions, justice, etc.) (1980/2003, p. 244). According to Johnson’s theory of

19
Acting the Metaphor

conflation, such associations are formed automatically and unconsciously in early

childhood: thus, “for an infant the subjective experience of affection is typically

correlated with the sensory experience of warmth, of being held” (1999, p. 46);

equally, repeated observations in early childhood that liquids or piles of objects rise

as they increase and drop as they decrease link the subjective judgment of quantity

(more/less) with the sensorimotor experience of verticality (up/down). Such

conflations give rise to what Lakoff and Johnson call “primary metaphors”:

AFFECTION IS WARMTH14; MORE IS UP; IMPORTANT IS BIG; CHANGE IS

MOTION, etc. (1999, pp. 50-54)15.

In the Laban-Malmgren System the actor learning to associate Space

(Direct/Flexible) with Thinking starts with the sensations of ‘towards’ and ‘away from’,

as well as of direct and flexible movements. Thoughts are conceptualised as being

either grasped, captured (fixed in space) or drifting. Complex thinking is revealed

physically in the concave body of a listener doubting the speaker’s assertions (“Let

us sit crooked and think straight” as Brecht says).Thought is perceived as a journey:

the THOUGHT IS A JOURNEY experiential gestalt tells us that to accept or relate to

an idea is to move closer to it; to doubt or examine it is to distance oneself from it:

Thought and Space become inextricably linked. Similarly, linking Flow (Free/Bound)

to Feeling is rooted in the sensation of emotions flowing or being blocked through the

body, expressed in metaphors such as the adrenaline coursed through my veins;

love flowed from his eyes; her heart stopped in her mouth; a wave of jealousy.

The System is built on a “foundational” (Lakoff and Johnson 1999, p. 542) metaphor:

PSYCHOLOGICAL ACTION IS PHYSICAL ACTION. Psychological functions are

perceived as movements: feelings run high and are jarring; thought goes (straight) to

20
Acting the Metaphor

the heart of the issue or around the problem; intuitions are quick off the mark or slow

to catch on; sensations are connected to gravity – heavy/strong smell; light touch.

Our psychological experiences are structured in terms of physical forces.

Metaphors such as FEELING IS FLOW or THINKING IS SPACE are also defined by

the primacy of the physical experience: THINKING IS SPACE makes experiential

sense; SPACE IS THINKING does not. The relationship between source and target

domains is “asymmetrical” - “inferences only flow from the sensorimotor domain to

that of subjective judgment”” (Lakoff and Johnson 1999, p. 55), so to express the

idea that a character is highly emotional students of the System tend to say “he is

Mobile” or “he has a lot of Flow”, never “he is Feeling” or “Intuiting”; the original

source domain for Mobile (Feeling/Intuiting linked to Flow/Time) has been forgotten;

only the target domain remains – the association of intense emotion with agitation.

This makes this approach particularly attractive for actors whose starting points are

movement and physical sensation: the conflation of Mental Factors with Motion

Factors gives the analysis of character and action, of necessity involving abstract

concepts, “the qualitative feel of sensorimotor experience” (Lakoff and Johnson

1999, p. 128). Terms such as Remote, Stable, Mobile, etc., have immediately

accessible physical connotations. Thus, when directing, Simon Callow tends to use

the terms of the System even with actors who are not familiar with their specialised

meanings: "For example, I use Remote” Callow says, “because the word has a

suggestive value in itself, even without the specific images created in training."

Callow considers that the System offers actors, perhaps for the first time16, a

language akin to that available to musicians. He looks with envy at the fact that

musicians can talk of D major, 4/6 or mezzo-forte and know exactly what they mean.

21
Acting the Metaphor

The lexicon provided by the Laban-Malmgren System goes some way to offering a

similar precision to actors (personal communication).

Naming experiences in terms of physical dimensions also enables actors to

“categorise, group and quantify them – and, by this means, reason about them”

(Lakoff and Johnson, 1980/2003, p. 25). Categorisation is not without its problems.

Grouping characters on the basis of personality traits (characteristics) into the six

Inner Attitudes is founded on two much-contested assumptions: that one can speak

of a psychological ‘essence’, of a stable identity ‘underneath’ the appearances of

behaviour; and that one can therefore classify human beings (and by extension

characters) into categories or types based on such immutable essences. These

assumptions go back to an idea proposed by the early psychologist William James:

that human personality contains a multiplicity of ‘me’s’ which change according to

circumstances, but that these are aspects of a core identity, which we as individuals

recognise as our ‘I’ (see Benedetti 2005, p. 79). Fundamental to both Stanislavskian

and Jungian thinking, the notion of a personality essence has caused a great deal of

anxiety in theoretical discourse on acting, as it is suspected of ignoring, even

negating, cultural, political or psychological differences. A detailed analysis of this

criticism is outside the scope of this article, but it needs to be acknowledged, while

also recording that classification is a natural function of our (metaphorical) way of

thinking. Four decades ago Eleanor Rosch (1977) demonstrated on the basis of

experimentation that we categorise objects by referring to culturally-determined

“prototypes” and associating these with similar objects with which they bear a ‘family

resemblance’. Thus, she asserted, the concept bird arouses the “prototypical” image

of small, flying birds, while chickens or penguins, members of the same category,

are marginal to it: “non-prototypical”. Similarly, considering a particular dramatic

22
Acting the Metaphor

character involves perceiving that which is non-typical by relating it to the typical.

“There is a close relationship between the person and the overall pattern in which he

participates”, says the early ethologist E.T. Hall (1959, p.138). An approach which

starts by describing the typical, as the System does, offers a useful set of reference

points, which situate individual characters within a landscape.

“There are few human instincts more basic than territoriality”, Lakoff and Johnson

declare, before arguing that we naturally place boundaries on reality, defining

territories and objects as containers and thus enabling “an act of quantification”

(1980/2003, p.29).The concept Inner Attitude places a boundary, turning individuality

(diffuse) into Character (defined) in ways which we find most satisfying. This in turn

permits an “act of quantification”; allowing us to compute the components of a

character using Laban’s Motion Factors. Does this restricting of the ever-changing

stream of personality and/or action constitute an unwarranted simplification? Only

insofar as saying 'this is Sicily’ or ‘we are in the Rift Valley’ amounts to picking up a

segment of the overall landscape in order to be able to conceive it and interact with

it.

Actors using the System’s templates, Inner Attitudes in particular, are therefore

concerned with creating characters that are recognisably ‘typical’. Their initial focus

is not on immediacy and spontaneity, but on achieving a strong delineation of the

outer ‘skin’, of the silhouette of the character: the character is ‘designed’. This is not

to say that demeanour and action are artificial or ‘stylised’. In fact, the aesthetic

engendered by the System encourages a fundamental tension between individuality

and personality on the one hand and the artificiality of the ‘designed’ character on

the other. Students often have to confront this paradox: the principles of

Expressionist art and dance (abstraction, distortion and exaggeration) hit against the

23
Acting the Metaphor

physical reality of the actor’s body as well as realistic (though not naturalistic) values

in acting. The result is a performance style which might be described as a form of

attenuated expressionism or accented realism: a strongly delineated outer shape

(the ‘shell’ of the character, as seen in its posture and gestures) containing actions

and reactions carried out with spontaneity and immediacy. These are not silhouettes

in the inflated sense of Brecht’s masked characters or in the Lecoq-inspired

performances of companies such as Theatre de Complicité, they are ultimately

realistic portrayals given an expressive edge.

This interplay between deliberate artificiality and ‘natural behaviour’ amounts to an

unusual and specialised form of deception (Trivers, 2011, building on Byrne and

Whiten, 1992/1998). Think - to take but two readily accessible examples - of Anthony

Hopkins playing Hannibal Lecter or Tom Hardy turning himself into Bronson. The

deliberate design of the physical ‘shell’ signals the deception, while natural(istic)

behaviour delivers it effectively across the audience’s counter-deception

mechanisms. We are in a very satisfying ‘play’ in which we know from the outset that

a game is being played, yet we are allowed to involve ourselves fully in it. Kate

Garthside makes the point that characters which respond best to the System’s

approach are those written with "a universal intention", as metaphors for something

greater than the individuality of the person portrayed. The recognition through

physical signals of ‘what the character stands for’ depends on identifying the pattern

or group to which these point. Inner Attitudes inevitably involve considering

individuality in relation to a pattern. On the other hand, the deceiver will do

everything in his or her power to create randomness, the absence of pattern – so as

better to deceive. Spontaneous, reactive stage actions create believability. Two great

24
Acting the Metaphor

impulses underpin the application of the System: to improvise, to engage in random

activities so as to appear ‘truthful’; counteracting our other great instinct, which is to

discern patterns in random activity and rely on them for judgement.

The System often works best for characters which represent "extremes". The very

notion of Inner and Action Attitudes implies concentration: the physical life of the

character, its speech and gestures, are boiled down to essentials; speech is refined

to its rhythms, to its musical patterns; gestures are reduced to their common

denominators and ultimately distilled to their 'spirit' - their tempo. This moves the

character outside the mundane. Kate Garthside gives as an example her approach

to playing Trish in Unsuitable for Adults by Terry Johnson17. The character is "fragile"

- not an individual fragility alone, but the distillation of the fragility of a whole type of

women. Garthside decided to make her more fragile than the bounds of naturalism

permitted: she lightened her voice, adopted a breathy tempo and an etiolated

posture, as if stretched on a rack. This, she says using the terminology of the

System, resulted in an Adream character whose Weight was so Light, and Flow so

Free that she radiated emotion to the point where she touched on Mobile.

The examples above point to the raison d'être of the System: transformation. The

system offers a route to the deliberate design of character, first and foremost as a

physical process. Posture, gait, gestural range, vocal qualities (pitch, timbre, tone,

resonance), accent/dialect, breathing rhythms, etc., are translated into Michael

Chekhov’s “imaginary body”, a psychophysical tempo perceptively different from that

of the ‘daily’ actor. However, one would be mistaken to assume a mechanical

application of the System in the practice of professionals. In the conservatoires in

which it is taught, the System sits alongside other approaches, as one aspect of a

25
Acting the Metaphor

complex curriculum, and actors assemble their personal methodologies from all

these influences. Nor do most actors using its tools expect it and its implied aesthetic

of transformation to dominate their work. They are aware that they might find

themselves in the TV studio in the morning, playing a role close to their ‘daily’

physical shape and accent; and that very evening appear unrecognisable on stage;

indeed, according to the requirements of the role find themselves at any number of

points on the transformation ‘scale’.

Nonetheless, those trained in the System find it difficult to discard altogether its core

teachings. The theoretical apparatus underpinning the System is complex, yet the

movement-based, experiential approach through which it is taught goes beyond

theory or technique, to a form of embodied learning which “shapes the subject”

(Downey 2010, p. S2318). By creating a language of acting and through it bringing

sensation to consciousness, the Laban-Malmgren System contributes to the

development of what the French call disponibilité, having a keyboard of acting

possibilities and fingers nimble enough to be able to play it. The System is

concerned, as Simon Callow puts it, "with making the word flesh, literally".

[6791 words]

26
Acting the Metaphor

REFERENCES

Benedetti, R. 2005. The Actor at Work (9th edn.), Boston: Pearson

Bull, N. 1951. The Attitude Theory of Emotion, New York: Nervous and Mental
Diseases Monographs (Coolidge Foundation)

Byrne, R. and Whiten, A. (eds).1992. Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise


and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes and Humans, Oxford: Clarendon
Press

Callow, S.1984. Being An Actor London: Penguin Books

Carpenter, W. undated. “Conflict and Harmony Between Man And Woman (A Study
in Movement Expression)" Addlestone, Surrey: Art of Movement Studio (unpublished
typescript, author’s collection)

Chekhov, M. 1953. To The Actor, On The Technique of Acting, New York:


Harper and Row

Clark, A. 2008. Supersizing the Mind: Embodiment, Action, and Cognitive Extension,
Oxford: Oxford University Press

Damasio, A. 1999. The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making
of Consciousness, London: William Heinemann

De Rivera, J. 1977. A Structural Theory of the Emotions, New York: International


Universities Press

Deutsch, F. 1952. “Analytic posturology”, Psychoanalytic Quarterly 21, 196-214

Downie, G. 2010. “Practice without theory: a neuroanthropological perspective on


embodied learning”, Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 16, S22-S40

Draper, R. 1979. The Letters of Ruth Draper 1920-56 (A Self-Portrait of a Great


Actress), ed. N. Warren, London: Hamish Hamilton

Edelman, G. and Tononi, G. 2000. A Universe of Consciousness: How Matter


Becomes Imagination, New York: Basic Books

Ekman, P. and Friesen, W.V. 1969. “Non-verbal leakage and clues to deception”,
Psychiatry 32, pp. 88-106

Gainotti, G. 2004. “A metaanalysis of impaired and spared naming of different


categories of knowledge in patients with a visuo-verbal disconnection”.
Neuropsychologia 42, 299-319

27
Acting the Metaphor

Gallese, V. and G. Lakoff . 2005. The Brian’s Concepts: The Role of the sensory-
motor system in conceptual knowledge, Cognitive Neuropsychology 22, issues 3-4,
455-79

Hall, E.T. 1959/1980. The Silent Language, New York: The Greenwood Press

Hayes, J. 2010. The Knowing Body: Yat Malmgren's Acting Technique, Saarbrucken:
VDM Verlag Dr Muller

Hodgson, J. 1979. Mastering Movement: The Life and Work of Rudolf Laban,
London: Methuen

Jung, C.G. 1968/1990. Analytical Psychology, Its Theory and Practice (The
Tavistock Lectures), London: Ark Paperbacks

Jung, C.G. 1971. Psychological Types, trans. H. G. Baynes, vol. 6 of The Collected
Works, London: Routledge & Kegan Paul

Kemp, R. 2012. Embodied Acting, What neuroscience tells us about performance,


London: Routledge

Kosterlitz, R. 1950. "The Application of Movement to the Treatment of


Psychological Disorders", Laban Art of Movement Guild Newssheet, no. 5

Laban, R. 1947. Effort, London: MacDonald & Evans

Laban, R. 1950/1971. The Mastery of Movement, London: Macdonald and Evans

Laban, R. 1951. "The Psychological Effects of Movement", lecture given by


R.L. at the Modern Dance Holiday Course, August 1950, reported by H. Irene
Champernowne in Laban Art of Movement Guild Magazine, No. 6

Laban, R. 1960. "Light-Darkness" Laban Art of Movement Guild Magazine, no. 25

Laban, R. 1961. "The Laban Lecture", Reprint of a 1939 speech. Laban Art of
Movement Guild Magazine, No. 26

Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M. 1980, second edition 2003. Metaphors We Live By,
Chicago: University of Chicago Press

Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M. 1999. Philosophy in the Flesh, The Embodied Mind and
Its Challenge to Western Thought, New York: Basic Books

Lamb, W. and Watson, E. 1979. Body Code, The Meaning in Movement, London:
Routledge and Kegan Paul

Lowen, A. 1958. Physical dynamics of character structure: body form and


movement in analytical therapy, New York: Grune & Stratton

Luria, A. R. 1973. The Working Brain, London: Penguin Books

28
Acting the Metaphor

McCaw, D. (ed.) 2011. The Laban Sourcebook, Abingdon and New York: Routledge

Mirodan, V. 1997. “The Way of Transformation (The Laban-Malmgren System of


Dramatic Character Analysis)” Dissertation for PhD, Royal Holloway, University of
London. uk.bl.ethos.285166

North, M. 1958. "Scientific Penetration Gives Basis For Guidance And Treatment",
Laban Art of Movement Guild Magazine, No. 21

Preston-Dunlop, V. 1998. Rudolf Laban: an extraordinary life, London: Dance Books

Reich, W. 1933/1945. Character Analysis, translated by Theodore Wolfe, Rangeley,


Maine: Orgone Institute Press

Rosch, E. 1977. “Human Categorization” in Warren, N. ed., Advances in Cross-


Cultural Psychology, vol. 1, New York: Academic Press

Shaughnessy, N. (ed.). 2013. Affective Performance and Cognitive Science, Body,


Brain and Being, London, etc.: Bloomsbury

Sheets-Johnstone, M. 2009. “Animation: The Fundamental, Essential and Properly


Descriptive Concept”, Continental Philosophy Review 42, 375-400

Sofia, G. 2013. “Achieved spontaneity and spectator’s performative experience – the


motor dimension of the actor-spectator relationship” in De Preester E. (ed.) Moving
Imagination. Explorations of Gesture and Inner Movement, pp. 69-85,
Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company

Stevens, A. 1986. Withymead, A Jungian Community for the Healing Arts, London:
Coventure

Trivers, R. 2011. Deceit and Self-Deception: Fooling Yourself the Better to Fool
Others, London: Allen Lane

Wiggins, J.S. (ed.)1996. The Five-Factor Model of Personality: Theoretical


Perspectives, New York: Guilford

Willett, J.1970. Expressionism, London: Weidenfeld and Nicholson

Zabel, M. D. 1960. The Art of Ruth Draper, New York: Doubleday & Co. and Oxford
University Press

Zarilli, P. (ed.). Second edition 2002. Acting (Re)Considered: A Theoretical and


Critical Guide, New York: Routledge

29
1
For, relatively scarce, material on William (Bill) Carpenter see Valerie Preston-Dunlop 1998, p.260; Mirodan
1997, pp. 26-28
2
For a detailed description of the materials handed to Yat Malmgren, see Mirodan 1997, pp. 33-34. A broad
outline of some Carpenter materials can be found in Hodgson 1979, pp.156-161. See also McCaw 2011, pp.351-
7.
3
Throughout this article, initial capitals denote that terms are used in the specific sense given them by the
Laban-Malmgren System and its predecessors: Jung, Laban, Stanislavsky.
4
This is a greatly simplified account of Jung’s ideas on personality. See Psychological Types and for a more
accessible outline, The Tavistock Lectures; and Mirodan 1997, pp.57-83 for details of what the Laban-Malmgren
System took from Jung’s theory of types and the differences between its understanding and Jung’s writing.
5
Defining this holistic conflation has exercised thinkers across disciplines: Warren Lamb, a Laban disciple,
called a similar model the “Posture-Gesture-Merger (PGM)” (Lamb and Watson 1979, p.90); A.R. Luria speaks
of a “kinetic melody” (1973; also Sofia 2013, p.84); Sheets-Johnstone (2009, p. 393) cites Luria but introduces
the Husserl-inspired concept of “primal animation”, a “spatial-temporal-energic whole” (1996); see also Carol-
Lynne Moore in McCaw 2011, p. 319.
6
Here and in what follows, the / indicates that the two elements should be read in either direction. Thus, one
Stable character can be Sensing/Thinking, where the Sensing aspect is dominant, while another Stable
character is Thinking/Sensing, where the Thinking aspect prevails, and so on.
7
"…shadow movements…are tiny muscular movements such as the raising of the brow, the jerking of the hand
or the tapping of the foot, which have none other than expressive value. They are usually done unconsciously
and often accompany movements of purposeful action like a shadow - hence the term" (Laban1950/1971, p.12).
See also North 1958, p.15 and McCaw 2011, p.355 for Shadow Moves revealing the Inner Attitude. For the
capacity of involuntary moves to reveal inner states see Ekman and Friesen 1969.
8
This is a fairly standard process used by Stanislavskian teachers (see, for example, Benedetti 2005, p.22),
echoing some of the exercises described by Michael Chekhov (himself influenced by Laban [1953, p. 13] and, in
turn, an important influence on Yat Malmgren).
9
On a National tour, with Paul Eddington in the lead
10
Royal Shakespeare Company, Barbican Theatre, 1996
11
West Yorkshire Playhouse, Leeds, 1992
12
Jung’s description of personality types is considered obsolete, with the consensus in the psychology of trait
and type now crystallising around the Five-Factor Model (Wiggins 1996).
13
Although Nina Bull’s contemporaneous research on postural attitudes in hypnotised patients (1951) and
Joseph de Rivera’s subsequent analysis of the connections between emotions and movement attitudes (1977)
offer at least some observational support for Laban’s and Carpenter’s intuitive assumptions.
14
Here and below I follow Lakoff’s and Johnson’s convention of using capitals to denote representative
metaphors.
15
Conceptual Metaphor Theory (CMT) is not without its challenges. As Lakoff and Johnson themselves point
out, neurological research supporting it is not yet conclusive (1999 pp. 38, 42; see also Gainotti 2004) although,
working alongside the eminent neurophysiologist Vittorio Gallese, Lakoff attempted to strengthen the argument
(2005).The perceived over-extension of the concept of embodiment has been the subject of sustained criticism:
see, for example, Sheets-Johnstone 2009 for a phenomenology-based critique, especially pp. 386, 397; Clark
2008; and Tribble and Sutton in Shaughnessy 2013, locs. 791-807 for a brief overview. I nonetheless find it a
useful framework, while acknowledging that it remains a fluid hypothesis, not yet fulfilling scientific tests of
falsifiability and repeatability.
16
See Sheets-Johnstone 2009, p. 390 on the limitations of describing phenomenological experience in
language; and Zarilli 2002, p.16 on the specific case of metaphors for acting experiences.
17
Library Theatre, Manchester
18
See also Sofia in Shaughnessy, ed., locs. 3802ff. for recent (and incomplete) experiments designed to
ascertain whether actor training can change cognitive capacities at a fundamental level.

You might also like