There S Something in My Head But It S No
There S Something in My Head But It S No
Doctorate of Philosophy
i
DECLARATION
This work is original and wholly the work of the candidate, Jan Golembiewski, including
Dr. Rena Archer (FADP, U. Syd.), Dr. Michael Robertson (Medical Ethics and the Law,
Dr. Branka Spehar (Psychology, UNSW) and Dr. Richard White (Psychiatry, U. Syd.), Dr.
Martes Alison.
ii
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
CONTENTS
CONTENTS ............................................................................................................................................. 3
ABSTRACT ............................................................................................................................................. 8
QUESTION 1: SPECIALIST PSYCHIATRIC FACILITIES TO HELP PEOPLE WITH SCHIZOPHRENIA .............. 35
QUESTION 3: THE ROLE OF THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT IN THE AETIOLOGY AND SYMPTOMS OF
QUESTION ONE: HOW CAN SPECIALIST PSYCHIATRIC FACILITIES IMPROVE THE WAY
START MAKING SENSE: APPLYING A SALUTOGENIC MODEL TO ARCHITECTURAL DESIGN FOR
SO YOU’RE GOING TO DESIGN A MENTAL HEALTH FACILITY? HOW TO MAKE IT FUTURE-‐PROOF. ....... 97
DETERMINISM AND DESIRE: SOME NEUROLOGICAL PROCESSES IN PERCEIVING THE DESIGN OBJECT.
.............................................................................................................................................................................. 126
A TAXONOMY OF THE NEURO-‐CORRELATES OF PERCEPTION, ACTION AND DESIRE. ............................. 129
Affordances; the heart of perception and the automation of action. ................................... 130
Behaviour-‐settings: the context of affordances and the automation of inhibition. ........ 132
Creativity and awareness: the deliberate extension of affordances and behaviour-‐
4
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
MOVING FROM THEORY TO PRAXIS ON THE FLY: INTRODUCING A SALUTOGENIC METHOD TO EXPEDITE
THE RIDDLE OF PSYCHOTIC PERCEPTION RESOLVED: THE INTEGRATED FINDINGS OF AN IN-‐DEPTH
Appendix III: Notes on data sources for table 3. ............................................................................ 293
3. QUESTION THREE: WHAT IS THE ROLE OF THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT IN THE
LOST IN SPACE: THE ROLE OF THE ARCHITECTURAL MILIEU IN THE AETIOLOGY AND TREATMENT OF
5
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
ARE DIVERSE FACTORS PROXIES FOR ARCHITECTURAL INFLUENCES? A CASE FOR ARCHITECTURE IN
COMMON PSYCHOTIC SYMPTOMS CAN BE EXPLAINED BY THE THEORY OF ECOLOGICAL PERCEPTION.
.............................................................................................................................................................................. 358
INTRODUCING THE CONCEPT OF REFLEXIVE AND AUTOMATIC VIOLENCE: A FUNCTION OF ABERRANT
6
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
John Soan for Tomas Storey Kirkbride, a Quaker in the mid nine-
teenth Century who advocated the moral treatment of the insane. This
7
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
ABSTRACT
This thesis explores schizophrenia and the dynamics it has with the built environment.
In doing this, three research questions are addressed. The answers are all delivered in
Question one: ‘what can architects do with the design of specialist psychiatric facilities
schizophrenia than is available in print. My need to know not only what schizophrenia
cogent integrative meta-hypothesis, spanning and integrating datasets and ideas from
Findings in this research led to speculation on the possible role of the built environment
beyond the treatment of the syndrome - possibly even as an aetiological factor. As such,
the thesis finishes with the question three: What is the role of the built environment in
8
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Q.1) Designing with a view to fostering a sense of coherence promises that design
decisions will firstly ‘do no harm’. Further, by building opportunities for patients to
and top-down perception becomes over stimulated and over focused. This combination
gives rise to eight syndromes. These syndromes can explain all the symptoms of
schizophrenia.
significant as any other major known epidemiological factors. But the many functions
of the built environment are complex, leaving several possibilities worthy of future
exploration.
9
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
I must dedicate this thesis to my father, Olek (Alex) Golski because a thesis must
explore a gap and his death forced me to learn about impossible gaps and have the
My father knew gaps like few others. As a young boy he was cast into one of the most
fabled gaps of history: one of Hitler’s concentration camps. The gap I have chosen to
explore is also fabled and for many of the same reasons. Like the holocaust,
schizophrenia is better described as an abyss than a gap. And like the holocaust,
schizophrenia has claimed untold numbers of lives to a madness that few of us have the
It is not just a poetic metaphor – it’s a central premise of this thesis that the edge of a
cliff demands a leap. I discovered this precipice in 2006 while designing a new mental
health facility for the New South Wales Health Department and I was unsatisfied with
the regulatory guidelines that drove the project and still drives similar projects. Like
many other architects faced with similar jobs, I intuitively felt that there is a relationship
between mental illness and the built environment. But it was a commercial necessity to
ignore that relationship because knowledge is time consuming and difficult to come by.
10
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Little did I know then that it’s not psychiatrists or psychologists but architects that do
almost all the research that ever goes into the design of better psychiatric facilities, and
if we have a problem with the guidelines, it’s ours (as a profession) to fix. I did my best
then, but no sooner had the project come to a close, and I returned to university and to
The research component of my Master’s degree was far from sufficient. To return to my
metaphor, the research I did for the Master’s was really just scouting around the edge.
My leap occurred only when I arrived at Sydney University at the beginning of 2009, to
I have a lot of people to thank for this privilege. Nobody could have been more kind and
supportive than Dr. Bem Le Hunte, my wife. Without her constant support, her training
and her pressure to complete this project in good time (‘have you finished yet?’ became
a daily chant, well before the thesis was even due), I would have never made this leap,
I also have to thank my supervisor, Dr. Peter Armstrong. In a supervision meeting, very
early on, Peter and I were discussing the scope of this project and the time it may take in
achieving my goal. Peter leaned back and said, “It’s an awfully big gap isn’t it?” Once I
admitted that neither he, nor I knew the half of it, he added. “Well, if I’ve ever met a
man who can pull a rabbit out of a hat, it’s you.” I don’t know, Peter, if this thesis is a
rabbit exactly. To me it resembles a jumbo-jet, but whatever – you deserve thanks for
11
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
the continual support you have given me and for helping me through the trials I’ve had
on the way. I’ve always suspected that you kept me on a loose rein because you (like
me) intuitively felt that there is another horizon, and that the world could be a better
place if I find it. Only recently, schizophrenia claimed your sister’s life. I know it’s
presumptuous but I felt, throughout the entire period, that it was her experience that you
reflected on when reading and hearing about my discoveries. I hope, that in some way,
this thesis helps you better understand your sister’s trial – her gap – the great
12
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Thanks
My mother, Kathy Golski, for feeding and helping me when I’ve been pressed for time.
My supervisors – Dr. Peter Garth Armstrong, my principle supervisor from the outset.
There are many times when I may have looked well out of my depth. Thanks for
Dr. Branka Spehar, Dr. Arne Dietrich and Dr. Michael Robertson. All of you had deep
insight into the mechanisms of the human mind and were able to steer me out of the
Dr Rena Czaplinska Archer, Thank you for finding me a place in the faculty. With a
project as peculiar as mine, it was difficult to find someone willing to say “I’ll do it,”
Dr. Richard (Dick) White and Dr. Anthony Harris. Thanks for taking the time to read
through my work and be of assistance, even for the short periods you were available.
13
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
International Research Society, The International Academy for Design and Health and
the countless other researchers elsewhere who provided the stepping-stones I needed to
My kids, for all the time I should have been doing stuff with them – and especially
Taliesin who, when I’ve been stuck was always great at explaining basic scientific
The university staff; Joanna, Penny, Kim, Suzanne, Jennifer and others who beaver
away in secret, sorting out all the paperwork that proliferates at the university. The
librarians: Tom, Rosemary, Bill, Tina and others for teaching me Endnote, finding
obscure documents deep in storage and for locating books in faraway places. Bruce for
the hundreds of times he’s helped me out when I’ve lost my keys, and to Leslie, Ben,
To other students and academics, both at Sydney and other universities, who have kept
me on track and inspired. Some deserve a special mention – those who have reviewed
my work (often doubly blinded) and who have always given valuable feedback.
boy, Peter Muller took it upon himself to be my mentor and to teach me the basic
principles of architecture. I was only in my mid teens when he taught me how to cut
14
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
sections and draw plans. This work is much more theoretical than those practical
origins, but in everything I write, I hope that its applicability to praxis is always present.
15
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
This thesis was written to be useful. It was to have broad and practical application but
this came at the expense of the tight and cogent narrative structures that PhD theses
traditionally aspire to. The thesis is largely composed of separate articles, each with its
I wanted a PhD degree, but a tome gathering dust in the Fisher Library like a medieval
viva voce as my colleagues have done in Cambridge and Berkeley, but that’s not
methodology and another on my findings, but this goal is not particularly practical, nor
is it very likely to succeed. Every article needs its own literature search, methodology
and findings – otherwise they have little value and won’t get published, and even if they
did, their usefulness would be questionable. What good is a literature review that covers
a gap as broad as mine? Instead the thesis became a collection of mini-theses. And
there’s the possibility that it’ll be read that way – as a fractured document that says
many things rather than honing in on the archetypal single gap and approaching it with
The perception that the thesis might be fractured is also driven by a second reason – it
set out to cover a cavernous knowledge gap. Thus some articles were conceived as
scaffolding for others: (Golembiewski, in review-d) provides support for other articles
such as (2013), for example. Sure this logic is linear, but a reader can equally trace other
linear tracks through the thesis to come to other conclusions. (2010b) Provides a
scaffolding and a methodology for (2012e), but more complex still, the basis of (2010b)
for (2010b). As jumbled as it all sounds, each of these threads is coherent. But this
thesis can be read any number of other ways – and should be, depending on what a
Instead of tracing a singular narrative from the front to the back, the logic of my thesis
is more like an academic book than a fairy-tale. It’s intended to be read in parts, entered
and exited at any point, by people who are interested in the subject matter and want to
find out more but not in material that is of peripheral interest, indeed it’s intended to be
read principally as separate articles. Thus the thesis structure is in a fundamental way
quite rhizomic, as Deleuse and Guittari proposed that academic writing should be.
But the Deleusian model doesn’t apply in full. I’m not a post-modernist and the thesis
can also be taken as a whole. In its completeness it is more than the sum of its parts. The
thesis is thus also arbourescent, and its structure is ultimately very simple – although
still non-linear. Each of the three research questions make a section, and these are
Part 4: Question Three: What is the Role of the Built Environment in the
symptoms that follow an imbalance within the perceptual system. But it’s really a
coincidence that my aims and findings actually link up well. At the top level, the thesis
sufferers are not disingenuous but are genuine reactions to a very odd take on the world.
This is particularly important in the architectural context because the built environment
is usually all that is left once the social environment is depleted (as is always the case in
18
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
criteria.)
This is a very important reading of the thesis, and it would be lovely if academics and
practitioners would take note and change their perspectives because of it. But I’m
sceptical about whether academics and practitioners ever consult theses, much less
change their opinions because of what they read therein. Even published books are a
little old fashioned, and it’s my belief that people are more likely to read journals – so
Realistically, how the thesis is to be read depends on what (and how much) a reader
needs to know and why. If you are a health architect, you’ll find it really useful to know
how patients tick, and what kinds of interventions may be useful for given situations.
You may also want to know the implications for healthy people. For you, question 1
will be recommended reading. In this case the papers in the later sections of the thesis
provide only empirical support, which you’d only bother with if you were sceptical. If
would be your target. If you wanted to understand patient behaviour, question 3 might
also be useful. An epidemiologist would start with question 3 and might go no further.
The result is, that the reading of the thesis is a very personal experience. The writing
was too. The process consumed me for several years, and there can be no denying my
passion for the subject and my findings. This is reflected in my writing style. Criticise it
19
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
as you may, it’s a fallacy that the academic can be truly extricated from their work. So I
don’t hesitate to insert my personal viewpoint into my articles nor this exegesis. Where
there is no first-person voice, you can assume that reviewers have been through and
20
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
If asked, what is the single most important contribution this thesis makes to knowledge,
I would like to assert that ‘the Riddle…’ (from page 204) is the only comprehensive
hypothesis for schizophrenia. It’s an outlandish claim, considering that the subject has
been scrutinised for a century or so by thousands of researchers the world over - the
keyword ‘schizophrenia’ turns up in over 6000 citations per year in a Medline search
(see p. 206).
I never anticipated pulling such a body of data together. In fact, I never even realised
that I was even trying to do so. It’s just that I naïvely thought that there must be
someone who already had, and I thought I was trying to locate something that had surely
been cognised before – some pearl of wisdom, while I write up what I find.
I knew that schizophrenia hadn’t been understood to the level where the effect of the
environment meant, and I saw that as my job: a relatively appropriate step to take as a
PhD project. But another Medline review of nearly 2000 articles carrying the keywords
“schizophrenia AND hypothes*” revealed that at best, authors would focus on just one
side of the syndrome (either the negative signs or just the positive symptoms) and
ignore the other, yet in the schizophrenia diagnosis both sides of the syndrome are
21
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
ubiquitous, even if they are not directly observed by a clinician (American Psychiatric
Association, 1994, p. 277). More often, authors hone in on a single gene or neuron and
offer no cogent explanation why save saying that ‘it was implicated in schizophrenia.’
(Eg. Welch et al., 2011) Notable exceptions such as Gray, Feldon, Rawlins, Hemsley
(1991), Fletcher and Frith (2009) and Kapur (2003) were inevitably also profoundly
skewed, but at least all of these made some attempt to pull themselves from the mire of
partially digested data and explain schizophrenia more holistically. It is like this small
number of authors apparently spent enough time staring at the broader field of data
(like some great random dot autostereogram) to discern some really relevant patterns,
but still they missed the big picture. Like the autostereogram, the data on schizophrenia
is apparently plagued by random data with produces definable patterns but no clear
picture. It’s only after staring at it and going wall-eyed (that is seeing everything in
In three years of reading the most notable hypotheses and literally thousands of other
findings, I suddenly saw that every one of the thousands of articles I read, as well as
every diagnostic criteria and other symptom actually does make sense – and not only in
a scattered way, but with a singular cogence. It was a bit like seeing the big image in the
theory approach, but not situated among people, but ideas and data. Like in a
Hollywood movie, it happened not during my studying hours, but during a vivid dream.
22
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The exploration was intended to provide for better questions about the relevance of the
built environment in schizophrenia, not to be the heart of the thesis, but there it was. If
ever I needed to advance new knowledge, here was my opportunity. I had discovered
something quite unexpected. As an architect I should have ignored it – the aetiology and
diamonds you find in a gold mine, just because you’re a gold-miner? I very nearly
jumped boat and abandoned the hunt for ‘gold’ and made this question alone my PhD
topic, but I was restricted by formal constraints. I was in the Faculty of Architecture,
Design and Planning and my supervisor was an architect with no more than a flirting
interest in schizophrenia. So I had to plough on and pursue the question of the built
environment.
Even so, the model for schizophrenia that I present is quite literally the heart of the
than being based on original empirical studies. The theory I developed was that
perceptual stimulation must either make the condition more severe or incrementally
better.
Fortunately this finding was very relevant to the built environment because the built
In this context, the built environment (on all scales) reflects a concentration of many
potentially active percepts, with each individual building offering a range perceptual
opportunities – some of which are good, others harmful. Effectively this makes the built
an innocent agent, except as it carries dangerous psycho-agonists: like water for cholera.
The two other sections effectively emerged from the second: the first (question 1, from
page 62) is how tailored environments should be shaped to mitigate the effects of
schizophrenia. These articles are not about aetiology, but are about treatment.
The articles in the third section are about the culpability of designed objects,
architecture and the urban environment in the aetiology of schizophrenia and related
mental illnesses. This section (from page 296) finds the stimulation provided by the
built environment culpable twice over. Firstly, for a lack of consideration of how it
stimulates the formation and maintenance of delusions and hallucinations. But perhaps
more importantly there’s the other side – the failure of architecture to stimulate where it
24
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
PURPOSE STATEMENT
Ultimately I want this work will reveal new architectural innovations and affordances
that will assist: a) in urban planning to avoid schizophrenia onset and b) to assist in the
25
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
POTENTIAL SIGNIFICANCE
Schizophrenia is a debilitating and deadly illness that can strike in any social context
variation (Kirkbride, Fearon, et al., 2007). The normative opinion among researchers is
that schizophrenia not uncommon but reliable figures on schizophrenia incidence show
only that it is subject to extreme variation. The fact is that 0.2% of Australia’s
from reported figures (Carr et al., 2002) and the Australian population at the same time
schizophrenia
Schizophrenics lose their ability to interact meaningfully with society. They lose their
somatogenic perception, meaning the connection with the senses that inform them about
pain, hunger, comfort and other essentials is lost or unusually poor. People with
schizophrenia also lose their abilities to manage day-to-day tasks. (Osmond, 1966;
Searles, 1960; S. Williams, 2002b). Finally, and most famously, the ability to perceive
26
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
the outside world is hampered by interference from delusions, internal projections and
schizophrenics die by suicide (Harris & Barraclough, 1997; Palmer, Pankratz, &
Correia, Brandt, Ekbom, & Sparén, 2000). An estimated 2% die from catatonic
complications (Wyatt, Alexander, Egan, & Kirch, 1988) and many others die from
comorbid illnesses that would be treatable in healthy people but are difficult to treat in
Comorbidities lower life expectancy by about 9 years (Dembling, Chen, & Vachon,
1999). Overall, diagnosed schizophrenics have a life expectancy that is much lower
than the general population, with two thirds of excess mortality being from ‘natural
causes’ including heart disease and poorly managed diabetes (Auquier, Lançon,
27
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Medication, the main mode of treatment, is well recognised as being ineffective in about
30% of cases (H. M. Jones, 2004), and when it is effective, it only treats some
perfect fit for patients and it causes an incredible array of undesirable side effects, some
useless for schizophrenics (S. Williams, 2002a) although clinicians sporadically report
good results with various forms of talk therapy (Searles, 1960, 1965), CBT (cognitive
behavioural therapy), physical therapy (Vancampfort et al., 2012) and even placebo
Although it has been suspected for a long time that modifications to the environment
will improve the well-being of the schizophrenics (Foley & Lacy, 1967; Osmond,
1966), very little research has been done to discover what those changes could be. This
area of research has more or less vanished with the growth of other areas of focus - most
28
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Incidence rates
Schizophrenia can develop anywhere, in any population and demographic. Despite this,
its incidence is far from even. The most commonly reported figure for the worldwide
average incidence is 1% over the course of a lifetime, but the reality is that the figure
seems to be far lower, although in some locations at some points in history the figures
have crept up and even exceeded this figure. The 1% is a ‘strategic’ number,
presumably intended to bring attention to this very important cause, in the tradition of
deaths and bring attention to her new methods of healthcare (H. Woodbury, 2008). A
meta-analysis of 1,458 rates taken from 33 countries suggests that the incidence spans a
distribution curve running from 7.7 to 43.0 cases per 100 000 people (i.e., 0.0077% to
But the heterogeneity of rates is far more extreme than normalization curves would have
criteria and paradigms (even before on social and physical environment are considered).
At the moment, the sober figure mentioned by McGrath et al is modified by the removal
2003; Boyle, 2002; Hoffer & Osmond, 1963). As many as 1% of the population were
were estimated to make up 3% of the entire population of the USA in 1957 (Campbell,
1958).
population-wide levels at that time, this opinion was based on assumptions rather than
methodological empiricism (Campbell, 1958) and even on much smaller scales, the
variation of incidence was already known to be enormous (Faris & Dunham, 1939).
Since then, incidence patterns must have changed radically. The removal of nosological
conditions from the schizophrenia diagnosis had marked huge changes in distribution of
incidence patterns. Until the mid twentieth century, pellagra was common in the USA
for instance. Especially in rural areas of the southern states where the diet (being based
on maize) had no natural vitamin A. It’s possible that the inner city prevalence of
schizophrenia at the time was also a product of diet – as poor people were removed
although by the time of Faris and Dunham’s study, the cause of scurvy was well known
and should not have been reflected in the data they collected.
increase with exposure to urban settings. Against this backdrop, there is only one known
Corbusier, in the state of Punjab, (India) which has lower incidence than its surrounding
rural areas (Varma et al., 1997; Wig et al., 1993). The reason for this, like anything
about schizophrenia, is unknown and has not been formally hypothesised. Chandigarh is
an interesting case however, because it is exception it’s the exceptions that explain
bigger patterns.
exceptional case.
Other known exceptions are high incidence rates among rural populations (their
relationship with the nearest cities has not yet been tested to my knowledge). These
occur in the north west of Ireland, in rural Scandinavia (especially in the north), and in
Istria, a peninsula jutting into the Adriatic, currently part of Croatia, on the Italian and
Slovenian border.
The placement of Istria is traditionally in the warpath of just about every major
European conflict. And being in close proximity to Italy, Slovenia, Austria, Germany,
Hungary and Bosnia-Herzegovina, its population has never recovered from the wars of
31
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
the twentieth Century and most of its buildings are still in ruins. Rather than mark an
32
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Socio-economic costs
Schizophrenia represents a huge social and financial burden to society – but not evenly
distributed over the whole of society. Rather the strain is patchy and uneven because of
inconsistent models of care, and because the greatest burden is usually handled by the
families of those who suffer (Beecham & Knapp, 2001). Incidence and recovery rates
also vary from region to region. But as a whole, the cost to Australia in 2001 alone was
estimated to be $1.85 billion (about 0.5% of the GDP). Over a third of this was borne by
families, and another $661 million in direct costs to the healthcare system and finally
the indirect costs at $772 million in lost income and increased morbidity etc. (Carr et al.,
2002).
To estimate the potential financial impact on this figure by the research that was
conducted for this thesis will be a futile process because we cannot guess at the impact
that my work may have. This thesis affects three domains: urban planning (where
design (where outcomes are intended to improve treatment) and in the general
understanding of the syndrome (which promises more abstract, but high-level impacts).
Just how much does it help to have someone understand the syndrome? Will that
improve medical and social treatment? It is conceivable. Will this understanding also
help hone the broad study of schizophrenia to more practical outcomes? Once again, it’s
RESEARCH QUESTIONS
34
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
schizophrenia
Originally “How can specialist psychiatric facilities be improved in the way they
help people with schizophrenia?” was to be my one and only research question. An
extraordinarily naïve one too – loaded with assumptions that somewhere someone may
not only know what schizophrenia was, but also may know what may help.
I started out by researching suitable models to understand how innovation in the built
environment may improve the wellbeing and health outcomes of schizophrenic patients.
I had previously worked on mental healthcare design and had been frustrated by the
scarcity of tools for designers who are tackling the problems that come with designing a
psychiatric facility – unless they are designing a dementia facility, in which case there’s
a fair amount of quality information available (largely due to the efforts of John Zeisel,
Ian Forbes and a few others) but Alzheimer’s and other old age dementias don’t share a
lot with schizophrenia, certainly not on a neurological level. Where dementia is defined
‘unexpected’ coupled with intense awareness of expected events (see the section on
35
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
tool that would be easy and fast to use and would generate better outcomes regardless of
theoretical concerns about the relationship between schizophrenia and the built
environment.
Other design researchers pointed the way. In the first decade of the 21st century, a
(Bahrs et al., 2003; Dilani, 2008; Edvardson, Sandman, & Rasmussen, 2005; Eriksson
& Lindsrom, 2008; Eva Langeland et al., 2005; Eva Langeland, Wahl, Kristoffersen, &
social model for understanding the parabolic continuum that spans the theoretical states
of perfect health and death (Antonovsky, 1987, 1996). These researchers used the term
‘salutogenics.’ They also roughly sketched out the principles of salutogenesis and its
benefits over other models of psycho-supportive design. But I turned the theory into a
36
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
There are any number of studies that demonstrate the relative strength of salutogenic
theory, but how the theory works is relatively obscure. Antonovsky did explore this
question, but he did so in the mid 1970’s to the mid 1980’s and what was acceptable
then (in terms of psychological explanation) seems weak now. At any rate, I had to dive
deeper than the theory of salutogenics. And in order to find substance to make my terms
of reference deeper, I had to look at schizophrenia itself. How could I know how the
environment affects schizophrenia (and vice versa) until I knew what schizophrenia
actually was? As it happens, I was eventually led to make associations between the
in review-d) (Starts on page 204). This is a paper that returns to salutogenic theory to
find neurological causes: ‘The neural basis for the salutogenic method of healthcare
In the outset, the possibility that salutogenic theory may present answers to what
perception in Lang’s textbook on theoretical drivers for architecture (1987). It was one
of my associate supervisors, Dr. Spehar who directed me back to it, suggesting it may
37
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
have relevance. This theory, whilst relatively well known among perceptual
relevant. Already some fringe areas of medicine were picking up on the theory to
explain the ‘mirror neuron’ phenomena that was being observed in interpersonal
perception is direct and directly stimulates action and such a model could explain a lot
that cognitive models of perception can’t. It seemed plausible that schizophrenia – when
perception. I have explored this possibility in several papers, most notably ‘The Riddle
Because of these lines of enquiry, and because of my dissatisfaction with the dismal
My work on the nature of schizophrenia is where my deepest research has taken place,
and where my breakthroughs have been most profound. I thus consider my work on
schizophrenia itself to be the heart of my thesis. I would have been happy to let my
findings about the nature of schizophrenia be the topic and substance of this thesis.
rather than scientific training. Architects must address wicked problems on a regular
38
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
basis, and they learn to be comfortable with the unknown and unresolved. Furthermore,
there are reasons why many scientists would not be able to approach the subject of
they have common normative values and frequently researchers emerge from under a
factors affected me. In effect I was freed by my independence and lack of awareness of
significant works.)
It was the demand from healthcare architects planners and facility managers have made
it impossible to put questions about psychiatric design off until I finished my PhD – in
39
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
effect it was industry that has kept me focused on my original question. It was little
things that kept me to my original question – the need for designers to know more, the
and the opportunities I was given such as a paper on how my neurological findings may
be applied generally and deterministically to any design project, from object design, to
page 126).
(Golembiewski, 2011b). This opened a floodgate, and the pressure for me to produce
more and more of this material is evident, several more offers of plenary addresses at
conferences have accelerated the interest in this material. I started to get approached by
the health departments around Australia and by several prominent architecture firms
who work in this space; HASSELL Healthcare, Bates Smart, BVN, Woods Bagot and
MAAP (UK). I have been asked to guest edit an edition of Facilities on the subject and
In short, I had little choice but to return to the space I had started out in: health design.
40
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The great algebra of what schizophrenia is and what may cause it has dominated the
three years of my PhD research. Originally it was because the depth of answers I needed
(about the relationship between schizophrenia and the environment) could only emerge
perhaps have spent time with a sizable number of schizophrenia patients and conducted
an ethnographic methodology, but this approach never felt right, and besides, I never
contexts. Professor Perkins (in review) relates his experience doing just that. The
findings that he and his students and colleagues discovered are very reasonable and
offer great depth to his field of enquiry (landscape design for psychotic patients). The
principles Perkins et al. devised are intimately related to the salutogenic methodology I
published and present in this thesis, but the issue of the life-world of schizophrenic
patients is one that Perkins et al. wisely skirted around. Indeed, his work makes no great
In any case, I did not choose an ethnographic methodology but a speculative enquiry
through literature. It was not going to be sufficient to rely on other’s hypotheses, simply
because the vast bulk of them just didn’t ring true. And besides, many were useless for
understanding how schizophrenia interacts with the built environment and vice-versa.
41
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Schizophrenia is not an illness as such, but a diagnosis based on loose but cohesive sets
published diagnostic system (such as the DSM-IV and the ICD-9), and on a finer level
there is a range of different sets of symptoms apparent even within a single diagnosis. It
is hardly surprising, then, that the established knowledge base about schizophrenia is
At a minimum I had expected to find that research and practice might be unified by a
common theory – or at least a common hypothesis. But what I found was that every one
of tens of thousands of researchers and clinicians harbour their own heuristic ideas. Of
these, few reach far beyond the very limited scope of their own very limited areas of
permission to break the illness into component parts and address the bits, while
dismissing the holistic presentation of the illness. The pet interests of the theorists only
exacerbate the problem; the very ephemerality of the question means that answers
appear to be skewed to match or prove a-priori agendas. Examples include beliefs that
human consciousness is a delusion (Taylor, 2010), or that heeled shoes have a knock-on
effect that causes mental illness (Flensmark, 2004), or that thin people are have a
territorial nature (Kellett, 1973). If I had tailored practical solutions to such limited
hypotheses, it is unlikely that they would be of much use. An analogy that we can all
understand it that the schizophrenia researchers of the world are apparently attempting
to draw a 1:1 scale map of the world of schizophrenia, but are ignoring the mountains,
So I persevered, placing a great deal of interest in the hypotheses that focused on what I
figured may ‘bridge’ the individual mind and the world we all live in. This meant I
prioritised hypotheses that focused on perception such as (Fletcher & Frith, 2009; Gray,
Feldon, Rawlins, Hemsley, & Smith, 1991; Kapur, 2003). As useful as they were, these
perception, developed by James J Gibson (1979). The discoveries I have made, with the
43
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
symptoms of schizophrenia
Contrary to the views of many researchers, to me it seems obvious that the aetiology of
reason for this: firstly because schizophrenia is a diagnosis and not a nosological
discovered that cause these symptoms (or similar ones), they are automatically
symptomatologies have thus been excluded. They include pellagra, scurvy, syphilis,
The other reason that the aetiology and symptomatology of schizophrenia cannot be
disentangled is that genetic and environmental factors that come into play do so because
they create a predisposition for schizophrenia and a context into which schizophrenia is
expressed: the environmental and genetic factors thus moderate and mediate the
44
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
and thus also in diagnosis. Like genetics, the environment cannot be considered as
express what it means to experience schizophrenia) “how would you like to have square
eyes?” at this point Searles realised that the patient was being quite literal. The
external physical environment. The square eyes the patient complained of were the
The very inability to disentangle schizophrenia from the environment is very interesting
from the perspective of an architect because this viewpoint involves the built
urban syndrome. The incidence of schizophrenia grows with exposure to urban centres
(Kelly et al., 2010). This was first observed nearly a century ago (Faris & Dunham,
1939). But there are few hypotheses that lend themselves to understand this
phenomenon. Some say that cities are places of greater social anxiety (Selten & Cantor-
Graae, 2005, 2007), places of social ghettoization and stigma (Kirkbride, Fearon, et al.,
2007; Kirkbride et al., 2006; Kirkbride, Morgan, et al., 2007; March et al., 2009), places
where noise and air pollution are rife (Halpern, 1995), others have suggested that one of
45
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
the symptoms of schizophrenia is urban drift, and that people end up in the cities in
search of cheaper accommodation (Evans, 2003; D. Freeman, 1994; Lapouse, Monk, &
Terris, 1956), some arguments have been rejected by cross-sectional studies (the noise
and air pollution proposals for example) but for most, the jury is still out (March et al.,
2008).
My third question is: “what role does the built environment itself play in the
aetiology and epidemiology?” The strong relationship between the built environment
and schizophrenia means that this question remains valid, even if the popular
hypotheses (that don’t point to the physical environment as an aetiological factor) are
found to be true. Minimally, the built environment makes up the context within which
schizophrenia is experienced.
Importantly to me, a designer, the question has a suffix: what does the correlation
between schizophrenia and place mean for us, the designers of the built environment –
whether we design psychiatric units, cities, or the buildings that they are built from?
46
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
It is the central premise of this thesis that the relationship between psychotic conditions
The reason is an argument in logic: Both schizophrenia and bipolar-1 are diagnoses
Psychiatric Association, 1994). This effectively means that these psychoses cannot be
47
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
behaviour is especially unusual), and behaviour occurs within a common context. All
behaviour reflects the context in which it is expressed, both by interaction and also by
sitting on the floor of a Japanese room with tatami mats and sitting in the muck of a
a schoolyard, but not in church. All behaviour is, at least in equal part, environmental.
But logic aside, epidemiological studies consistently find the 'urban milieu' to account
for about 30% of all incidence, even once all other known factors are accounted for
If this evidence is insufficient, then the Camberwell walk study can be called in; a study
of 17 paranoid psychiatric patients undertook a PANSS test before and after a 10-
minute walk through the busy Camberwell High Street of London. The test showed that
paranoia and other psychotic scales were increased after this short 'dose' of urban life
(Ellett, Freeman, & Garety, 2008). Even without going into my PhD research into why
this may be so, the evidence is amply triangulated. The environment is an inextricable
factor in psychosis and we, as the designers and planners of the environment are
culpable.
48
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Just as a building has to have foundations on the earth, psychosis is an expression of the
mind as it meets experience. And for most of us, most of the time, that experience is of
Now that we know how implicated the environment is in psychotic conditions, it is time
to look back at the environments we prescribe for psychotic patients. Could they be
better? How would I feel if I were 'on the edge' and in this space? Are there any
provisions to improve a patient's sense of coherence? (See the research question on p.35
49
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
METHODOLOGY
My methodology has been unorthodox. What started as naïvely pursuing very diverse
research questions – over a period became my guiding methodology. Over the last three
years, I have discovered that one line of questioning gives perspective to the other.
Typically scientific enquiry is approached in very small steps and with close guidance
from a supervisor who is doing research in a closely related field. The resulting research
findings will be published in a small collection of papers with the supervisors name on
the right and the student as the first or second author. The process taken by a PhD
student called Melissa Green typified this trajectory, right here at Sydney University.
Her topic was specific, doable, and likely to produce publishable results: ‘Facial affect
study of delusion formation’ (Green, 2001). The research questions addressed little gaps
and solidly filled them with new findings using established lab-based methodologies. If
I had been in the psychology department (as Green was) my thesis question would not
have been accepted, much less my far more expansive research questions. But in the
faculty of Architecture, Planning and Design, my supervisor wasn’t to know that I had
50
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
But I’m not at all upset that I took this route because it yielded results. My approach
meant that I had to come to understand schizophrenia using very broad-brush strokes,
and then to refine the knowledge I acquired. This is the opposite of the normative
approach, which is to learn first-up that the solution is not in sight and that a researcher
has to focus on one little piece of the puzzle. Forget schizophrenia – look at delusions.
The researcher then reads everything about delusions, but focus their own studies on a
single piece of that puzzle. There’s no doubt – that method works, and it’s far less risky,
Architects are practical people, yet people that are able and trained to address wicked
problems (Kunz & Rittel, 1972) and find practical solutions. This is, in fact possibly the
most useful talent architects possess and there is a whole research school in Stanford
(The D School) to study ‘Design Thinking’ (R. Buchanan, 1992). Architects are trained
to find the simple bits of information that are needed allow them to make lateral
innovations that solve many problems. My approach to PhD research followed this
practical approach, only I didn’t ask simple questions. At first I just wanted to know
what schizophrenia was, so I could establish solid design guidelines for an incredibly
hypothesis after another. As and when they became coherent, I sent them off to be
published. Inevitably, most were rejected, but rarely without some sound advice: look at
this or that. Sometimes my associate supervisors gave similar advice: look at this or
that. As they got more refined, my papers started being reviewed, and usually still
knocked back. The reviewers’ advice was always well considered and to the point. But
51
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
difficult issues, I integrated disparate seams of knowledge and my hypothesis for what
But it seems that there aren’t many people who do approach schizophrenia research
be expansive and an ideal question to consider for three to six years. In the last few
decades there have been very few comprehensive hypotheses for the illness – and all
have somehow lacked the integrative holism that I sought. Out of all of them, Gray,
Feldon, Rawlins, Hemsley, & Smith was in 1991 (Gray et al., 1991). It was by far, the
hypothesis with the broadest reach, and it has also remained remarkably good over the
last 20 years, but since it was published, another 75 000 or so related articles have come
off the press (MacDonald & Schulz, 2007). In many ways, my own hypothesis
comes to the argument from a completely different route, and is naturally more up-to
date on its sources. Gray et al. 1991 was an uncharacteristic leap, stretching far further
than any hypothesis since. Subsequent broad stroke hypotheses include (Kapur, 2003),
synthesis’ (H. M. Jones, 2004), the author himself defends it as a hypothesis for no
more than ‘psychosis within schizophrenia’ (Kapur, 2004). Far from the reach of Gray
et al, this putatively explains but a handful of symptoms from the ‘complex
magnum on schizophrenia was Fletcher and Frith’s 2009 Bayesian hypothesis (Fletcher
52
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
& Frith, 2009), that studiously avoided mention of Kapur 2003, yet didn’t reach far
beyond it. But typically the authors marched knowledge forward incrementally; they
Friston (2003).
My method was two pronged, and indeed, the rigor and substance I was able to bring to
the theory of design from my scientific discoveries has been well received, and as I
write, have been accepted into press or published in several international journals. My
scholarship has also been awarded with many prizes and even a guest editorship in
Facilities Journal.
In terms of formal methods, I’m not ashamed to say that all my research has been
argumentation (Groat & Wang, 2002). Exactly the same method was used by (Gray et
al., 1991) in their landmark thesis. The decision to take this approach was based on the
advice of Dr. Peter Armstrong (my supervisor) and Professor Hemsley (in personal
conversation, at the IoP in 2011). Despite this heritage, the method is sure to attract
negative attention. Once again, there is a normative expectation (at least in the sciences)
that PhD theses will generate new empirical data. Here there is another great benefit in
being in architecture. Architecture is not a science, and so the requirement for new data
can be dismissed without further thought. But even so, I feel compelled to refute this
expectation. I did take (and methodologically analyse) a dataset, only it was not one of
my making.
53
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Although there is a cavernous gap of knowledge around the spatial and material needs
for psychiatric patients, there is too much knowledge about schizophrenia, and these
opposites are complimentary. In 2007 there were 4400+ articles and other publications
of schizophrenia related issues published. That’s up from about 4200 in 2006 and 3700
in 2005. This steady upward climb has continued since at least 1987 (MacDonald &
Schulz, 2007). The data isn’t up to date, but at the same rate of growth, estimates are
that this year will see about 7000 – about 52 articles a week. If there ever was a data
It was from my emersion in this data that I noticed patterns and brought them to light.
Methodologically this is very similar to a Grounded Theory method (Corbin & Strauss,
1990; Strauss & Corbin, 1990), except that instead of grounding myself in a social
milieu, I grounded myself in published data. Where a Grounded Theorist typically takes
noted on observed behaviour until saturation has been achieved, I took notes on data.
Where a Grounded Theorist diligently ignores what people say they are doing (to
observe the reality of their actions), I ignored what authors said their findings were, to
Inevitably my methodology will not please everyone and for those who remain critical
about it, it’s worth remembering what a methodology is for. It is a routine, an accepted
road-map to approach a gap in knowledge. Methodologies are not only useful for
pointing a researcher the right way, so they make their discoveries efficiently, they are
particularly useful for justifying failures. If a PhD student were to finish their tenure
54
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
without discovering anything, they had better be able to explain what they did, that they
could find so little. If their approach was systematic (such as taking half the rooms in a
mental health unit, painting them in different colours and comparing the in and out
PANNS scores over time), and still the study found nothing conclusive, then the
researcher could safely write: ‘the colour of rooms appears to make no difference to the
PANNS scores of patients over time.’ It is thus safe, and useful, even when an
grounded research within a mental health unit, ethics permission allowing it), but I
didn’t because I started making discoveries about the time I started applying for ethic
permission. As I was aware that I would not need to justify a failure post-hoc because
my findings looked like they would be ample, I knew that I wouldn’t need to undertake
55
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Among first-rank universities around the world, it is now de rigeuer that a PhD can be
granted by publication and a viva voce. Unfortunately University of Sydney hasn’t made
this step, and instead, a PhD may be given to a series of publications that are
accompanied by an exegesis.
What an exegesis is, and what form this PhD including publication takes is all but clear.
Professor Adrian Snodgrass once said, “all I want to see in an exegesis is one short
sentence: ‘It’s all in the papers.’” On the other hand, some still feel there is value in the
traditional tome.
There are advantages either way. The ‘by papers’ route exposes a candidate to the
papers as they are produced and the candidate (me in this case) gets to work on their
advice. At the same time the candidate builds a portfolio of publications and a
reputation in their field before the PhD is even granted – these benefits serve the
university too.
56
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The disadvantage of this approach, as I see it, is that when papers are collected into a
tome (as mine have), they can sometimes be a bit repetitive: sometimes the same
arguments are employed in two or more papers, and if one paper draws on another, they
may recap on findings. Furthermore, the thesis is likely to lose its monolithic narrative
structure. If you as a reader find these problems in the published works of this thesis,
please forgive me. Rather than re-write the papers to fit the tome, I have kept them
much as they were published, excepting only changing the language to The Queen’s
Another thing I have noticed is that ideas do move on. Data emerges that is
capricious, and an idea that has enough validity to be published need not be future-
proof. Although publishing gives knowledge an aura of finality, it doesn’t stop it from
being falsified. Indeed this has happened in my own papers. My finding that negativity
(Golembiewski, in review-d) (also reprinted here from page 204). The reasons are
detailed on page 356, but in a traditional tome contradictions like these would probably
not be acceptable. Once again, here I must present my papers much as they are being
read by others, not with an apology (I shouldn’t need to apologise for publishing after
all) but with a note to explain the odd U-turn or logical contradiction over a series of
Finally the exegesis with publications doesn’t follow a logical linear structure that one
may expect from a thesis. Obviously since Nietzsche, Wittgenstein and with
postmodernity, the days of the singular narrative are a bit passé anyway, but I do see the
value in in-depth linear analysis, and for all those who are inclined to agree, the
enclosed paper “The Riddle…” (from page 204), is nearly 20 000 words, plus another
5 000 if citations are included, you are welcome to take that as the thesis and ignore the
rest!
58
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
NOT INCLUDED
schizophrenia. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry (2010) 44, A51.
Design for disability lecture for the Masters studio, Faculty of Architecture, Design and
We are our architecture. Science and Non-duality Conference. San Rafael, California,
There’s something in my head (but it’s not me): The complex relationship between the
60
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
61
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Abstract
Purpose
This paper aims to look into the significance of architectural design in psychiatric care
illness, and also between the patient and his environment. As such, even minor design
therefore, that a psychiatric milieu is sympathetic and does not exacerbate the psychosis.
Design/methodology/approach
This paper analyses the architectural elements that may influence mental health, using
better health results from a state of mind which has a fortified sense of coherence.
62
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Findings
salutogenic theory is presented as a practical method for making design decisions (in
praxis) when evidence is not available. As demonstrated, the results appear to reflect
what evidence is available, but real evidence is always desirable over rationalist
speculation. The method suggested here cannot prove the efficacy or appropriateness of
Practical implications
The design of mental health facilities has long been dominated by unsubstantiated
policy and normative opinions that do not always serve the client population. This
63
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Originality/value
The paper will prove to be helpful in several ways. First, salutogenic theory is a useful
framework for improving health outcomes, but in the past the theory has never been
applied in a methodological way. Second, there have been few insights into how the
architecture itself can improve the functionality of a mental health facility other than
There is growing evidence that the design of a healthcare facility will directly affect the
health outcomes of patients (Ulrich, 2006). But designing a healthy psychiatric unit is a
difficult task as the patients are prone to distorted perceptual systems that make them
tactile, acoustic, temporal, olfactory or visual stimulus (E. T. Hall, 1975). This article
analyses various aspects of design, ranging from apparently minor interior choices such
doing this we should be able to see how design decisions may affect patients’ health
outcomes. To establish these links, the analysis looks at the transactional nature of
recommendations drawn from literature) for psychiatric health facilities. It is hoped that
this methodology will be useful for making informed decisions in circumstances where
64
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
made regardless.
There is a reasonable body of literature about the design of hospitals and related
buildings, and even about mental healthcare facilities, but the material is severely
limited when compared to the complexity of problems that an architectural team faces.
Without specific studies into the specific questions that an architect has to face hundreds
of times a day, how are architects and other team members to know that the choices he
or she has made are the best for the subject group? There are no studies on the
psychological benefits for basic design elements that are mandatory in mental health
facilities. And it is the moral obligation of all facility planners’ to question any details
that may not be in the best interests of the facilities reason d’être, which is to assist
patients in recovery. How do architects know how a space with cushioned vinyl walls
and floors will affect the wellbeing of patients inhabiting that space?
As it happens, the architect is often given little choice about the layout and finishes of
standard rooms within a programme as standards and codes have already pre-empted
any decision-making. “The Australian Health Facility Guidelines” (HCAMC & CHAA,
2007) for example, specifies the layout and finishes of most of the spaces within a
Facility planning teams will also have to resolve occasional contradictory findings or
opinions – many of these will not be impartial. One study that observes the benefits of
65
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
opening up nurses’ stations and other spaces in mental wards (Whitehead, Polsky,
Crookshank, & Fik, 1984), is contradicted by another article calling for the same wards
to be ‘locked down’ and made more secure (Osmond, 1958; Sine, 2008)1. It’s important
that the lead architect is proactive and has perspective; architects have to pose a
question to others on the design team; “Who are we designing for, why, and how will
this decision help to do that job well?”2 A strong guiding theoretical hypothesis makes
rational decision-making easier where there are no relevant empirical studies to provide
support. For this reason salutogenics – a theory gaining interest in recent literature
reviews for linking the environment to health outcomes (Dilani, 2008; Schweitzer,
Gilpin, & Frampton, 2004) may prove to be useful for formulating robust principles for
1
Mr. David Sine, the author, is president of Safety Logic Systems, a company that may
governments and other involved people are also very important, but great care must be
taken that their needs do not come before those of the patients. (Osmond, 1958) These
negotiations won’t be easy, as empowering the patients will to some degree mean
wrestling control from staff some of who may well be part of the planning team.
(Searles, 1960).
66
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
merely pleasant, but the best room of any ward (Osmond 1958). The
rooms he describes have hardly changed since 1958 and are better
vulnerable patients. This room was designed according all the rec-
67
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Spaces not dissimilar from this have been shown in repeated tests to
Salutogenics is a psychosocial study of what keeps people healthy, starting from the
perspective that illness and health are different points on the same continuum. Where
treatment may be required when one is ill, a supportive environment is always required
to assist and maintain good health. The primary premise of salutogenic theory is that a
internal and external environments are predictable and that there is a high probability
that things will work out as well as can reasonably be expected (Antonovsky, 1987;
68
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
{
Sense of coherence derived from:
Meaning, comprehensibility and manageability
and other treatment Generalised resistance deficits (GRD’s)
Active forces
DEATH LIFE
State of well being
The idea that the environment has a direct effect on health through psychology is both
intuitive and well supported in research. As Ulrich writes ‘the concept of stress provides
a credible departure point for understanding why design should affect health
outcomes…’ (Ulrich, 1997). Ulrich and others have created a model associating
psychological stress to poor health outcomes; the maxim is that anything likely to
increase stress levels is to be avoided in health design. The stress model is simple and in
most instances it is appropriate for healthcare design and especially for mental health.
69
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
But the stress model is not comprehensive nor specific, and it does come under criticism
by Antonovsky who points out that there are times when stress can have a salutogenic
effect; that is when a person is subjected to stressors whilst receiving high levels of
the environment (Antonovsky, 1987; Bahrs et al., 2003). Lawton and Nahemow support
this viewpoint by observing that an environment that lacks challenges leads to atrophy,
but then too much challenge can be damaging (Lawton & Nahemow, 1973). They add
that patients have a reduced capacity to adapt when they are ill or infirm. Aside from
this caveat, the salutogenic model seems an appropriate broad framework in which to
locate the stress model because it supports the stress model with much needed substance
for its readability4. From the point of view of mental health design, the salutogenic
3
Being able to influence the surrounding environment is the basis of manageability.
4
Readability is an architectural simile for comprehensibility.
70
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
What is interesting about salutogenics from the psychiatric point of view is that in this
model the relationship between a patient and the environment is understood as being
transactional, not fixed; the environment effectively changes according to the subject’s
sensory and perceptual abilities and conditioning (E. T. Hall, 1990). Perception is a
and yet it is the only channel for receiving new information of any sort5. It has been
postulated that a great deal of mental health problems occur because of imbalances and
and delusions. Thus, the act of perception itself can trigger psychotic events (E. T. Hall,
1975, 1990; Maeissner, Perry, Dorr, & Rowan (Ed.), 1965; Osmond, 1957, 1958, 1966;
5
It is conceivable that old information (memory) is stored and retrieved through non-
71
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Knowledge (epistemology)
a b
Perceptual Ability
Sensation
Memory
Culture
(sight, smell etc.)
Experience
Object Object as
experienced
Subject
people as trees he is not suggesting that people are trees or that peo-
ple are like trees but that, for this woman, people are experienced as
Searles, 1965)
72
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
M. Woodbury & Woodbury, 1969), and in the case of psychiatric patients, this may
mean making sense of a living nightmare (M. Woodbury & Woodbury, 1969). It is
therefore important that all decision makers in design teams for psychiatric wards
understand that hallucinatory experience is very real to those who suffer from it and that
Hallucinations are stressful and often spiral into a vicious cycle of disorienting
1969). There may be cases when there is a well informed psychologist within the team
who will take responsibility for relevant design decisions, but it cannot be assumed that
those on the client side will always understand the sensitivity of this issue or the
synthetic, (there are arguments either way, but there is little doubt that sensory ‘data’
must at some point be translated onto neurochemicals and electrical impulses, implying
Psychology Research Center, 1955; Collerton, Perry, & Ian, 2005; Grossberg, 2003a;
Ullman, 1980)) then the relationship between comprehensibility and the environment
that we draw experience from is personal. Not all people will gather the same
information from the same experience (E. T. Hall, 1990; Searles, 1960). However, in
normal circumstances, the ‘gap’ between a subjective experience and the reality of the
73
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
schizophrenics the disjuncture between the experiential reality and objective reality is
confusing for the patient (Searles, 1965; Weckowicz, 1957). For this reason it is
distortion. Size perspective, for example, is a ‘natural’ effect whereby the apparent size
episode is illustrated by just how much the patient thinks an object has shrunk rather
than moved backwards in space (Weckowicz, 1957). The distortion caused by size
(Osmond, 1957).
There are a number of other perceptual functions that are also subject to distortion,
misreading or loss. These functions include very basic skills such as the understanding
of distance, relative dimensions, mass, spatial orientation, the passage of time etc. (E. T.
Hall, 1990). Cognitive scientists have counted at least thirteen distinct ways that people
comprehend the relative depth of space visually and several other ways when we
include the various tactile, thermal and acoustic perceptual systems. Whilst I shall not
discuss them in any depth here6 some provision can be embedded into the environment
to assist patients in these tasks. Hall notes that where one sensory ability might fail,
6
A handy summary is found in the appendix of Hall (1990) and in more depth in James
74
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
there are others that will provide support (Osmond, 1957). Textured surfaces assist with
perspective8. Various objects (outside; the presence of trees, landscape features; and
inside; pieces of furniture, paintings, rugs, light fittings etc.) assist with size
perspective9, the various perspectives of parallax10 and other perspectives11 (E. T. Hall,
1990; Osmond, 1957, 1958, 1966; Searles, 1960). As all these perspectives support the
same cognitive function, that is the measuring and comprehension of space, the more
provisions for these functions the better for reducing the likelihood of misreading and
hallucination.
7
The density of texture appearing to increase as space recedes.
8
Parallel lines appear to converge to a vanishing point.
9
Relative size apparently decreasing with distance.
10
The angular difference as seen from one eye and the other; (binocular vision) and
motion parallax where distant objects appear to move slowly, but objects up close move
11
Including blur perspective, aerial perspective, relative upward location in the visual
field, textural shifts when one object is placed before another, completeness of outline
75
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
where, not just because there are no windows to connect the interior
to the exterior, but also because the corridor is typical of these spaces
difficult to tell the walls from the floor from the ceiling. Bare drab
it is hardly any wonder such spaces bring out the worst in mental
76
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
what objects are and what they are for is also of great importance. But the purpose of a
through the filters of memory, culture and a pre-existing epistemology (E. T. Hall,
1990; Searles, 1965). Thus we understand the environment through association with
reinforce messages that aren’t likely to increase stress levels or paranoid delusions. For
this reason Osmond, Elliot and Bayes Friba recommend that the typology of a
ubiquitous and desirable; a cosy and safe home (Elliot & Bayes Friba, 1972; Linebaugh,
Searles points out that all mental illnesses affect perceptual cognition although
schizophrenia does so most dramatically (Searles, 1960). It’s therefore imperative that
the design team considers visual, acoustic, haptic, temporal and olfactory sensibilities in
their designs, not just to avoid excessive sensory pollution (such as street sounds and
kitchen smells,) but to avoid distortion generally. Echoes have been found to be
disturbing to patients who hear voices anyway. Excessive or repetitive noise can also be
disturbing, especially for patients with neurotic complications. A sense of real time to
treat temporal distortion can be promoted by including elements that track time such as
a broad range of mental illnesses (Halpern, 1995). Though there haven’t been a great
deal of studies into this effect in mental institutions12, problems have been found in how
patients interpret the symbolism inherent in nurses’ uniforms, which have on occasion
been found to bring back wartime memories and other paranoid delusions (Richardson,
1999). When designing a mental facility, aesthetic choices and forms need to be
12
Halpern’s observations were in the wider community.
78
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Care still has to be taken to ensure the space is readable. The red
stripes, texture-less and colourless surfaces and cut ceilings don’t re-
late to any traditional forms and could prove very confusing to a pa-
79
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
(release granted)
Objects should look, sound and feel like whatever they are (except, of course for
institutions, which should have charm and personality and should be the functional
‘door-like’ and should have a comfortable thud when closed (Osmond, 1958). Needless
to say, there are a range of other details that have not been tested and therefore where an
about whether walls have to be solid and run perpendicular to the ground or whether
they should have coved skirtings can be tested against salutogenic criteria; in this case,
perceptual abilities then the team might consider other available options. The choice of
sliding glazed doors for example. Because sliders are walls, doors and windows all at
the same time this could pose a problem to patients with categorisation impairments (a
that could come under scrutiny for comprehensibility might be the material palette. Can
13
Technically a symptom is positive and refers to an unusual phenomenon.
normal ability.
80
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
the team avoid the use of ambiguous materials? Could materials appearing to be
something other than what they are such as veneer or printed timber patterns are
confusing to some patients? Perhaps there are other advantages still for choosing real
exposed timber, stone, natural carpet and quality pressed brick; being ubiquitous the
world over, they will resonate with a wider range of cultural backgrounds. Natural
materials are also replete with textures to assist with difficulties in perceptual cognition.
A design team that adopts salutogenics as a guiding theory might find the framework at
but do not necessarily need to be abandoned altogether. These are decisions the team
will have to make. Dr. Sivadon found that the practice of gradually exposing
schizophrenic patients to more and more complex social environments was an effective
treatment (Baker, Llewelyn, & Sivadon, 1959; E. T. Hall, 1975). The medical
specialists on the team may propose that something similar is done with the built
environment.
Other fashions are much clearer in terms of intentions and can also be considered with
care; Modernist chic tends to plainness and mechanical functionality, both of which
Psychiatric Association, 1994). Woodard Smith recognises this and points out that
advances in engineering have enabled plasticity of form, giving bad designers scope to
81
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
extend worst practice past the restrictions of traditional construction techniques. When it
comes to form, mental institutions benefit from the strong structural grid and small
Architectural form itself can amplify or deny power. If one questions the validity of this
major psychological effect, then consider the effect of Albert Speer’s Reich
Chancellery, related in The Edifice Complex. Sudjic tells how it was the disempowering
‘architectural stage set’ of Nazi architecture in 1939 that caused Emil Hácha,
Czechoslovakia’s president, to hand over his state to Germany without even engaging
the ‘well equipped Czech army with modern artillery, technologically advanced aircraft
and Skoda tanks’ that were waiting to defend Czechoslovakia’s border. From the broad
The second of the principles of salutogenics is manageability. The feeling that a person
is in control of his or her environment and life circumstances is very fortifying. And, as
illustrated by Sudjic, the feeling that you are totally out of control is absolutely
disempowering. All patients are subject to loss of control in the hospital system, with
those who forfeit control being seen by staff as ‘good patients’ and those who struggle
to maintain control as being ‘bad’ (Sloan Devlin & Arneill, 2003). This is even more
true for psychiatric patients than any other group; as patients are overtaken by mental
illness, control (manageability) is one thing that is lost entirely, not just because of
82
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
pressure from the hospital staff or the disempowering nature of the hospital
environment, but also because they lose trust in their own perceptions, memories, their
own selves. With this loss goes all social support; patients frequently lose their old
lives, their jobs, their sense-of-self and their perceptual abilities (Searles 1960). For this
reason it is imperative that the shreds of control that patients still have left are
There are a host of things facility planners and architects can do to nurture a patient’s
sense of control and ability to be effective. Right up on top of the list is keeping unit
numbers small and making arrangements so that numbers are never bigger than those of
the archetypal human community; the nuclear family. More than five or six patients
should not have to come into contact if they don’t wish to (Osmond, 1966). The
tendency of institutions to put people together in large dayrooms, dining rooms, living
rooms and other spaces is well known for exacerbating psychotic symptoms, because
the number of human interactions compounds with every extra person present. And at a
time when the capacity to relate to one another is hampered anyway, this equates to a
direct loss of control (Osmond, 1958). Consider that control in a social setting relies on
an awareness of your ‘place’. In which case, two people will have one relationship. In
other words, they only have to maintain an awareness of one another. Three people will
have three dynamics. They maintain awareness of each other and of a single new
dynamic, which is the relationship between the other two. Four people already have six
dynamics to maintain an awareness of, and seven people have twenty. By the time there
are fifty people, a setting plays host to one thousand two hundred and twenty five
83
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
delusions of the patients, which will make even a simple set of relations potentially
14
It is normal for schizophrenic patients to attribute non-human qualities to themselves
84
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
ness of your “place” among all the possible one – to – one relation-
ships in the milieu: two people will have a single dynamic; that is they
three dynamics. They are aware of one another and each is aware of a
single new dynamic, which is the relationship between the other two.
Four have 20. By the time there are 50 people, a setting plays host to
The problem of overcrowding leads to one of the classic issues that define mental health
problems in the public imagination: the ‘madman in the cupboard.’ E. T. Hall (1975)
“…In one of the violent ‘back’ wards, where most of the communication was
spatial… the ‘currency’ of the ward was space. Woodbury observed that the
organization of the ward was territorial rather than social. In terms of hierarchy
patient were two patients, each of whom could move freely in his own half of the
ward. Each of these dominated the territory of other patients who were
patient who slept under a bench and was not permitted even to use the spit hole
in the centre of the floor. His so-called ‘incontinence’ was a function of the fact
that the toilet was not in his territory and therefore he was not permitted to use
taught everything: how to wash, how to cook, clean, or use a toilet (Osmond, 1957). For
this reason provisions for these basic tasks should be very simple. It should be very easy
to maintain personal hygiene and for a patient to clean up if mistakes are made
(Osmond, 1957). The relearning of these ordinary tasks is now generally considered as
part of the therapeutic process and is both empowering and essential for life outside of
an institution. For this reason one of the beneficial innovations over the last half-century
86
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
the outside world, such as kitchens, laundries, baths, telephones15 etc. ADL (Activities
of Daily Living) facilities (as they are called,) are now a part of the normal programme
for a new institution and there are recommended guidelines for their design (Osmond,
1957). Unfortunately the ADL facilities are usually in locked rooms and don’t actually
serve real-life functions; that is, they are only for structured lessons or diagnosis.
Planning teams should consider placing the ADL kitchens centrally and having them
open (even if the ovens etc. have to be locked to prevent accidents,) to replicate a
domestic environment as recommended by Elliot and Bayes Friba (1972). In the same
spirit there are many other tools that are present outside of institutions that people
should be able to exercise their control over, lest patients’ abilities atrophy. Opening
windows and adjustable heating and cooling are very obvious examples. Such features
will assist in maintaining successful control of the environment and will have the
It has been observed that sports such as table tennis have a salutogenic effect for mental
patients, (not just for the players, but for the greater patient populations) as the events
15
ADL (Activities of Daily Living) services are usually used as teaching and diagnostic
87
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
perceptual abilities and tackling apathy and boredom, both of which lead to skills
atrophy (1972).
There are other ways to humanise mental hospitals and make them more manageable
avoided as they exacerbate the worst aspects of schizophrenia (Osmond, 1957). Physical
retreats must be provided (Elliot & Bayes Friba, 1972; Osmond, 1957, 1958, 1966);
Furniture should be movable, but solid enough to feel secure (Osmond, 1957). Of
course there are many other details that will also be useful but are more simply
recognised with a basic understanding of salutogenic principles rather than being listed
here.
1963) and is therefore the most significant ingredient of a sense of coherence. And
while meaning is found in the environment, it is illusive and difficult to provide for, as
rather than to the simple cause and effects so desirable in the physical sciences. Of all
the sources of environmental meaning, there is little doubt that it is primarily found in
the social environment – in love and communication, in family, friendship and in sexual
relations. But the one thing that is common to all mental patients with no exceptions – is
alienation from the greater community and they are to a greater or lesser extent socially
88
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
isolated (Osmond 1957). Even so, good facilities for receiving social support must be
considered to enable recovery The current practice of affording space meet up with
family and old friends (HCAMC & CHAA, 2007) is very important and, in the interests
of fostering meaning, might even be extended with the provision of extra facilities for
friends and family to stay over. (Gutkowski, Ginath, & Guttmann, 1992; Osmond, 1966;
Whitehead, Ellison, Kerpen, & Marshall, 1976; Whitehead et al., 1984; M. Woodbury
A salutogenic perspective means that affordances for pets may be considered. They
have been shown to radically improve mental well-being (Searles, 1960, 1965, 1986;
Wells, 2007) and while affordances for cats and dogs may be too difficult, too
dangerous and raise any number of health issues including the possibility of allergies, it
must be remembered that relationships with pets are often of more significance to the
mentally ill than relationships with other humans and are often important stepping
stones for re-establishing human relationships and other milestones for recovery such as
When interpersonal relationships have broken down the material environment can be of
life saving significance (Searles 1960). Mental patients regularly place huge importance
and feel very emotional about things and places, with schizophrenics frequently
confusing their environments and themselves. Searles relates dozens of anecdotes about
One patient spoke poignantly during therapy about having lost herself. During the
89
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
session it emerged that the ‘self’ she was referring to was the home she used to live in.
Her identity was inextricably linked her childhood home and all it meant to her and it
was when she left the house she started experiencing mental problems. Another case
Searles reported was of critical melancholia; a man who preferred to spend his life in
bed, not doing anything lest he should see someone caring for a garden. He was
overwhelmed with grief about the loss of his closest companion: his own garden. The
same man refused to leave therapy sessions. It turned out that Searles’ telephone
Whilst these fixations might sound trivial to people who don’t suffer from psychosis, it
experiences are very real, not poetic interpretations and they are symptomatic of a very
deep and painful ontological crisis (Searles, 1960, 1962, 1965) A designer must be
aware of how meaning and its inverse (meaninglessness) may be structured into the
environment. The negative effects an ugly and dehumanising space can have on such a
patient cannot be underestimated (Osmond, 1966). And neither can the reassurance that
comes with a space that is highly refined and aesthetically considered. As De Botton (a
popular philosopher) suggests, architecture really only comes into its own when we
have to plumb life’s deepest questions; when we have to deal with pain, grief and
confusion, and it is at these times that no pill can help (De Botton, 2006).
Ulrich has made some very important associations between accessibility of the natural
environment and better health outcomes. Whilst it hasn’t been explicitly noted in his
90
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
papers, it is likely that the health benefits derived from access to a natural landscape
occurs because meaning is so easily found in nature (Ulrich & Parsons, 1990).
order and aesthetic considerations (Bachelard & Orion Press (Tr.), 1958, 1964 (Tr.)).
The more afflicted the patient, the more significant it is that the spaces they use are truly
beautiful. Obviously such a term raises all kinds of prickly issues, (like who is to be the
final arbiter of taste,) but the point, (raised by Osmond,) is not so much about the
aesthetic of the final solution, but that the ‘usual’ drabness of hospital architecture is
than a ‘nice’ home because it is only when patients’ expectations are exceeded that they
will feel a sense of ease (Osmond, 1958). Currently there is a long way to go; the
dungeons, and more recently as asylums (Osmond, 1966; Sine, 2008). These
Bare, drab spaces directly affect perception (even of healthy individuals,) in a very
impairment of organised thinking, oppression and depression, even for healthy people
(Hebb, Heath, & Stuart, 1954; Osmond, 1966; Searles, 1965; Solomon, Leiderman,
Mendelson, & Wexler, 1957) and the inverse; a multi-sensory environment that is rich
system functions (Schweitzer et al., 2004; Woodard Smith, 1959). To this end, Osmond
suggests that spaces for psychiatric care should be exceptionally generous in the way
they’re decorated and finished (Huntoon, 1959; Osmond, 1957, 1958), even going so far
as recommending fresh cut flowers in the private spaces. Any fears about patients’
scatological and autoerotic behaviour (presumably the reason for vinyl surfaces in the
first instance,) can be largely allayed. According to Osmond, a patient is only likely to
resort to ‘apsophilic activities, touching the staff and painting the walls with faeces’
when left with no other more acceptable sensory gratification (Osmond, 1958).
can also be a very important transitional fixation in the process of re-engagement with
the outside world. It is very important that patients aren’t separated from their pre-
existing lives, as this completes a disjuncture that is already a serious problem for
mental health. Patients should be able to bring in photos and to stick them on their walls
(Osmond, 1966; Searles, 1960). Ideally they will have some kind of music system in
their rooms also, so they can listen to their favourite tunes (Osmond, 1958). Of course
these things can give rise to melancholy, but they can also be restorative as they remind
a patient that there is meaning in life; be it love, desire, friendship or something else
92
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
cal behaviour in such a sensory void, painting the walls with faeces
and masturbating are the only available options for sensory gratifica-
tion. Such acts are the last resort in preventing further mental deterio-
93
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Conclusion
Under normal circumstances people have a great deal of ability to adapt to new
surroundings – even in stressful situations – and such changes can actually support the
general sense of coherence – when meaning, control and comprehensibility are lost –
resistance to disease weakens and perceptual difficulties are exacerbated, often creating
It is imperative, therefore, that health facility planning teams carefully scrutinise plans
for anything that may not be in the patient’s interests. A basic understanding of what
makes an environment supportive in order to assist and maintain good health will
provide a useful framework for this critique. To this end, Salutogenic theory is a
application.
Essentially the theory proposes that a ‘sense of coherence’ (SOC) is an integral part of
the natural healing process and that a strong SOC is supported by feelings of
16
A hypothesis about the specific effects of each decision can be established with
relative ease.
94
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
ramifications. Of course the use of a salutogenic framework does not mean that no
further research is required and nor does it insure that all the choices the design team
makes will be the very best decisions in the given circumstances, but in providing a
basis for making purposeful decisions about any aspect of the design on the fly, it
means that the architectural teams are empowered to design the very best facilities they
can, given restricted time and budgets and a the general paucity of useful empirical
evidence.
There are many things that can be done with the architecture of a facility that may
alleviate mental symptoms, lessen the likelihood of future psychotic episodes, alleviate
stress and assist with basic cognitive functions. These innovations can be broken into
1. Comprehensibility; making sure that perceptual cues are present to assist percep-
tual processes. These include attention to texture and materiality, controlling the
size of spaces and the numbers of patients and expressing environmental fea-
ronment, details such as opening windows and the provision of ADL and sport-
ing facilities.
95
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
considerations as well as providing good spaces for visitors special personal be-
All of these approaches come together to create an architecture that really serves the
needs of mental health patients, fortifying their overall sense of coherence and mental
96
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Published in: World Health Design Scientific Review, (2012); Psychiatric Design: Using
a salutogenic model for the development and management of mental health facilities.
5(2), 74-79.
97
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Abstract
The prevailing model of psychiatric design (the world over) does not fulfil its potential
in supporting the healing process. In order to design for future usability, design teams
must have a vision beyond current paradigms and understand the direction healthcare is
going. More importantly still, models of care that will actually improve mental health
To create this vision, a methodological salutogenic approach can be employed for the
project development and management phases – from design of the buildings through to
the design of the models of care. This approach advocates taking an interdisciplinary
and collaborative approach to actively improve a sense of coherence for all users
including patients and staff. This can be done at every decision point by choosing to
The current paradigm of the design of psychiatric facilities has a long history. But many
historical approaches to the treatment of mental illness were not supportive to healing
process. Even today vestiges of the ancient traditions of imprisonment and punishment
of psychiatric patients can still be found in the buildings that healthcare designers are
presenting today – and these are for current models of care and for units that are
98
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
there is a lot of value to be retained in existing models of care and paradigms of design.
The same cannot be said for mental healthcare. To move into the future, we have to
This paper does not take aim at the worst aspects of psychiatric care. It isn’t necessary
to be explicit about the shortfalls of seclusion and other compromising methods that are
magistrates, nursing staff, clinicians, politicians and local community groups – the
problem areas of the psychiatric milieu are quite obvious and need no elaboration.
Instead, this paper outlines a challenge and a methodology for achieving this
appropriate goal – to design a mental health facility that is appropriate for the task and
in-depth study of the psychiatric milieu, I have seldom met anyone who doesn’t already
feel the challenge tapping away at their consciences. The challenge is to bring
humanity, aesthetics, love and meaning back into the psychiatric milieu to address the
This is the methodology that medicine has to adopt in order to become sustainable.
Obscure, as it is now, the salutogenic method will become universal over the years to
99
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
unsustainable. Even in places that are highly dependent on private health insurance,
government subsidies to the healthcare industry are a major economic problem. The
reason is because it is very expensive to treat an illness once it has become critical. The
cost of keeping a person alive when they are suffering from the failure of a major organ
is enormous.
(Antonovsky, 1987) pp. 71-72), meaning the fall from a state of health accelerates. The
more entrenched illness becomes, the more energy is required to arrest that fall. For this
reason 80% of a country’s health budgets are spent trying to prevent the inevitable – the
hospitalisation and treatment costs that arise in the last year of life from preventable
With limited resources we must reverse the entropy of disease much earlier, while it is
still affordable. In many cases, this can take place before disease ever occurs. Not only
is early intervention much cheaper, but the benefits are not only economic but social
and environmental also. Worldwide vaccination programmes and similar massive scale
interventions have already tested this approach and found them to be spectacularly
successful.
100
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
interventions can improve health outcomes and shorten hospital stays. There have been
hundreds of such studies. These studies identify views of nature, passive plants, hand-
other interventions (Ulrich, 2006). Even in mental health, small things like the décor of
a unit reduce stays by 25% (Vaaler, Morken, & Linaker, 2005). But can such
interventions occur earlier still? Before a patient ever gets sick by preventing the risk
Antonovsky shows that they can – and it’s never too late, although the best time is
Salutogenics
The theory of salutogenics is critical of the current model of illness. An illness doesn’t
occur with the development of a distinct pathology, but well before, with any slippage
from an idealised state of health. In this model the state of health is a continuum, with
an idealised state of perfect health and well-being at one end, and with illness at the
other. The only point of definition is at the far end of illness, and it is death – the point
of no return (see
101
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
{
Sense of coherence derived from:
Meaning, comprehensibility and manageability Generalised resistance deficits (GRD’s)
and other treatment There are no absolute GRD’s.
Active forces
DEATH LIFE
State of well being
Figure 8).
{
Sense of coherence derived from:
Meaning, comprehensibility and manageability Generalised resistance deficits (GRD’s)
and other treatment There are no absolute GRD’s.
Active forces
DEATH LIFE
State of well being
Two sets of forces that affect the state of well-being compete, with
102
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Frankl, 1963).
This continuum has competing forces working in either direction. The forces driving a
person toward health are called generalised resistance resources (GRR’s) and those that
drive toward illness and ultimately to death are generalised resistance deficits (GRD’s).
Both GRR’s and GRD’s are the same thing – they are life events. But it is not what they
are – it is how they are dealt with that is important to designers and facility staff. A
stressor may push one person over the edge, and another may not notice the stressor at
The difference lies in what Antonovsky called the sense of coherence (SOC). “The
confidence that, as in the past, things by and large, work out well” (Antonovsky, 1987),
p. 133). Three engines power the SOC: Manageability, comprehensibility and meaning.
The GRD’s on the other hand, reflect inabilities in dealing with situations, paralysis in
A strong SOC provides motivation for action and an understanding of the situation at
hand, but a weak SOC is paralysing. There is no impetus to act, nor knowledge of
what’s at stake or what action to take in any case. Thus very similar circumstances have
very different effects on different people. In Antonovsky’s time this was difficult to
prove. Questionnaires like the ones used by Antonovsky could show no more than
103
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
correlations, because individual circumstances could never exactly be the same and
variables in real life situations are impossible to control - even inside a lab. To get
around this, some scientists created a virtual reality experiment where people were
given a virtual experience of a trip on the London Underground (D. Freeman &
Freeman, 2008). In this way, the expressions and behaviours of every passing stranger
could be guaranteed to be identical, and strictly neutral. The virtual reality experiment
showed that even healthy people interpreted the same circumstances very differently
and behaved differently accordingly. Where one subject said things like “It was nice –
much nicer than a real experience. I thought they (the virtual commuters) were pretty
friendly,” another subject said, “there was something dodgy about that guy. Like he was
about to do something – assault someone, plant a bomb, say something not nice to me,
be aggressive (D. Freeman & Freeman, 2008) pp. 71-72). These reactions depend
Very recent reviews of all the mental health units in New South Wales (Australia) and
the Australian Health Facilities Guidelines (CHAA, 2009) found that the decisions
embodied into existing units and even the guidelines themselves were based on little or
no evidence at all. In many cases, the units and the guidelines contrasted starkly to
result) the Department of Health Infrastructure in New South Wales made a statement
that the guidelines are “only guidelines” and that the reliance on them is being “rolled
back” (Rust, 2012). This will improve the healthcare design, because it liberates design
104
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
17
teams to employ evidence-based design (where it exists – for a brief review see
(Shepley, Pasha, & Huffcut, in review). But perhaps more importantly, it allows the
salutogenic method for psychiatric healthcare design, which is currently the only,
As every architect knows, a hundred decisions have to be made every day. Some of
these will be critical – like in writing the functional program; but most of the decisions
will be small; which cornice, which colours, textures, finishes and specs. As the virtual
reality experiment above demonstrates, it isn’t only critical things that people with a low
SOC will react to (everybody reacts to those!) How can we make even these tiny
The answer isn’t as difficult as it may seem. But before we go into the specifics of the
salutogenic method, we must acknowledge that the patients who are going to use the
facilities we design are likely to be there because in some ways, it’s too late to rely only
17
The term evidence-based design (EBD) is often regarded as a marketing tool and
certification programme – the term ‘research-based design’ does not have these
105
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
on salutogenics, because many of those patients are going to be suffering acutely before
Customisation
Most mental disorders don’t have known pathologies, and for these there are no
behavioural therapy (CBT) and other therapies help patients to get themselves on their
feet – but none offer a cure. This means that a lot is left for the environment to do and
A very important starting point is to understand the treatment profile of the patients who
are expected to use the facility. I recommend dedicated units for specific psychotic
Those who are depressed, with dementia and mood disorders; and those who are hyper-
Institutional resistance
introduction of new ideas – especially if they are the foundational ones that are seldom
habit. But there’s reason to be positive that change will be received well. In the years I
have studied the design of mental health facilities, the only people who are happy with
106
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
the current paradigm are those who sell anti-ligature hardware, vandal-proof windows
and other products that are customised for the current paradigm (eg, Sine, 2008). The
list of those who know that structural changes are needed include everybody from
To some extent the fear that a new paradigm may be mistaken is mitigated by the reality
that mistakes are already well entrenched, and short of a reversal to total barbarism,
Negativity and positivity are both processed differently, and in different regions of the
brain. This is possibly why early judgements are very slow to reverse, so first
perceptions are pivotal on how a new unit will be accepted. A positive initial
experience will mean that users will be forgiving of a few mistakes. A bad start, on the
other hand, will mean that a unit may never be loved – despite having brilliant vision
and innovations (Golembiewski, 2012a, 2012c) (Articles are included on pages 358-
and 159-.)
A positive first experience is essential for all new users of a new facility, whether they
Although negativity and positivity are processed differently in the brain, they both use
107
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
dopamine pathways. Here we run into one of the most difficult complications in
central to the psychopathology of most mental illness – and the inhibition of dopamine
transmission is the central task of all anti-psychotic medications (Ginovart & Kapur,
Being one of the four central neurotransmission systems of the mammalian physiology,
dopamine serves many functions. One of these, and possibly the most relevant in this
context, is that the dopamine receptors moderate attention. This means that the subject
dysfunctional in most mental illness (Golembiewski, in review-d) (p. 204- q.v.) The
focus of mental health patients is frequently bizarre and unpredictable. Having said this,
the diagnoses of mental illnesses are very useful for drawing generalisations, because
the standardised diagnoses are largely based on symptoms that reflect the specifics of
The attentional biases of various mental disorders can be understood in simplified terms
according to Table 1.
Top-down Bottom-up
Superfluity Hyper-arousal, mania, paranoid psychosis Hyper-arousal
Deficits Depression Flat affect
Forgetfulness Poor self-awareness
108
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Neutral perception
As the virtual reality experiment of Freeman and Freeman (2008) above demonstrates,
there’s no such thing as a neutral perception. This is a reflection of the fact that affective
positivity and negativity are processed differently on a neurological level (also above).
as negative when neutrality occurs in a negative context. With healthy people, all
default. This bias is reversed in psychotic conditions (other psychiatric conditions have
psychiatric conditions (Golembiewski, 2012a) (See article on p. 358 q.v.) The result is
that everyone and the environment must work hard to counter this bias. For psychiatric
patients, ambiguity is much harder to resolve and is much more likely to be taken badly.
109
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
the polarisation of perception, because it touches the sorest points of paranoid thinking
(Chadwick, 1992; Freeman & Freeman, 2008). Psychiatric patients are often bizarrely
unaware of their surroundings unless they relate in some small way to the underlying
narratives of their delusional scripts. At which point, they become highly attuned. If
communication will not go unnoticed, even if it is very subtle. It is easy to scoff at the
idea that patients will pick up on any subtleties at all, when the circumstances of their
existence deny the fact so completely. Schizophrenic patients are frequently oblivious to
the fact that their shoes are on the wrong feet, and the fact that they sleep in between six
lanes of traffic, so how could they pick up on nuances that most of us would miss?
Several studies demonstrate that schizophrenic patients are an order of magnitude more
sensitive to expected stimuli than healthy controls, especially when those expectations
relate to delusional ideas (Brennan & Hemsley, 1984; Dakin, Carlin, & Hemsley, 2005;
(1996); Gottman, McCoy, and Coan (1996) are able to identify ten common negative
communication behaviours within three minutes of any conversation. It’s important that
staff are trained to identify their own negative words and actions, lest negativity is
present in communication with patients, because if they are, the architecture cannot
110
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
come to the rescue. The common negative communications are: contempt, disgust,
fear. There is ample evidence that communication failures frequently lead to the use of
2004). One of the recommended ways to dispose of these facilities is to, “Try to gain
trust: suit the action to the word and the word to the action (and…) try to get in touch
with patients by talking to them and showing genuine interest… Don’t shy away from
physical contact with patients.” (Hoekstra et al., 2004, p. 282) In other words, to train
Many healthcare units are high reliability organisations where a small practitioner error
can have sudden and catastrophic consequences. In these circumstances the fit between
spatial design and the routines established in clinical praxis are critical to the
A major shift in models of care will be required before equivalent spaces in the mental
healthcare milieu can be developed because these facilities are essentially domestic. The
main treatment rooms in a mental health facility include bedrooms, courtyards, living
rooms and rooms that can be used to retrain patients in the use of normal activities of
daily life (ADL) including kitchens, bathrooms, and laundries. Other clinical spaces also
serve normal human functions and thus are best if they are human in scale and layout:
consultation and assessment rooms, rehabilitation gyms, and spaces for other person-to–
111
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
person treatments. The exact details of these seldom matter, aside from good acoustic
The spaces in existing units that are possibly the most design sensitive are seclusion and
medication rooms. Both are useful only when models of care have failed.
Consultation is an attempt to engage interest groups in the design process. How this
works depends a lot on the all those involved, but it’s rare that consultation is a two-way
conversation – unless those on the design side see the opportunity for a PR exercise.
Certainly it’s rare that a genuine dialogue evolves that requires an on-going
commitment from those that are consulted. Collaboration, on the other hand, requires
that both problems and solutions be shared, and with a deep level of responsibility. As
counter-intuitive as it sounds, we should not seek opinions and ideas of the stakeholders
to guide the project, but rather seek out representatives of the interest groups who are
willing to take the whole journey, sharing the responsibility for both successes and
failures. With this approach, you’ll find people who are going to understand – at a
depth, why things turn out one way or another. This is where people are engaged just
enough that they feel they have been heard. They have handed over their opinions and
ideas, but most people have no idea just how complex architectural problems are, and
that some concepts must get lost in the design and building process. When the finished
product inevitably doesn’t reflect their input, people feel deceived and unheard, and this
breeds negativity – even if the opinions and ideas were worthless or not implementable.
112
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
It’s important that everyone knows that the project is aiming to be the best and future-
proof, which means it cannot rely on current models of care or design paradigms. But
this short paper is not the place to lay down a methodology for effective collaboration
and project development. Consider engaging a change management expert and see for
In 2010 a method was published especially for addressing the salutogenesis of mental
health patients to give a good basis for decision making, wherever evidence is hard to
locate or simply doesn’t exist (Golembiewski) (Article on p.62 q.v.) The method aims to
make every decision support the SOC in some way. Generally speaking these will have
To do this all decisions, however minor, should be subjected to close scrutiny for how
they assist the ability to manage, comprehend or find meaning. They should then be
checked for how they might erode the sense of coherence by taking away
that the critical decisions can be ratified from other perspectives (Plsek & Wilson,
2001).
113
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
tinuum, with death at one end and a more meaningful, more fulfilled,
more connected life at the other. The sense-of-self in this model is the
greater world, but beliefs are plastic and changeable to suit any situa-
tion – or delusion. Only beliefs that are grounded in reality are non-
delusional.
114
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
sibility.
“The absence of the things that make life manageable has obvious consequences,
although they are not as significant as we tend to assume. Lack of food, water
and shelter will be a source of stress that will make outcomes worse, but with
their meaning and comprehensibility needs looked after; people can go a long
time without basics. As Frank Lloyd Wright famously said, “give me the
Not all interventions and decisions are equal. There is a distinct hierarchy of meaning,
with meaning and connections with society and the greater world as the foundational
basic needs. This inverts Maslow’s well-known logic that basic needs are pivotal for
maintaining human life and that self-actualisation a final luxury once all other needs are
in place.
The mental health unit is also a forum for the competing needs of the various users: the
clinical, legal, non-clinical, patients and visitors. Each has a claim on limited resources
such as space, sunlight and proximity to the centre of the building/s. But more
115
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
importantly still, clinical and legal staff can claim the basic freedoms of patients. These
freedoms are essential to the maintenance of a sense of meaning, yet all too often
psychiatric facilities are designed to make manageability for the staff extremely
efficient, while taking power from patients. This is all too easy with architecture, which
has a long tradition of amplifying power to some and denying it to others (Jan
works against the very raison d’ etre of the unit: to empower and enable patients
enough that they can handle life on their own once they leave. Great care should be
taken to make sure that the patient’s well-being and sense of meaning is always
protected. In the context of the mental health facility, this principle harks back to the
Hippocratic oath “I will apply measures for the benefit of the sick according to my
ability and judgment; I will keep them from harm and injustice.” (Edelstein, Temkin, &
Temkin, 1987), p. 6)
Manageability
This is what hospital architecture does best. Support for manageability means providing
the basics to support life; food, shelter, medication, security, activities to occupy the
mind etc. (Golembiewski, 2012b) (See article on p.183- q.v.) it is also the focus of a lot
of the treatment; managing pain, managing patients etc. But at every step, the role of the
unit in managing on behalf of patients should be rolled back to allow patients to manage
on their own. It may be impractical, but giving the less critical patients access to a
kitchen and groceries could be an important step in enabling independence. “The feeling
that a person is in control of his or her environment and life circumstances is very
116
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
fortifying” (Golembiewski, 2010, p. 107) (In this thesis, p.82). Things that support
manageability for patients will include provisions for their health, security and comfort,
and even more importantly, provisions for the patients to make decisions for themselves
Comprehensibility
information of every kind. In a mental health facility context, that means knowing why
they are there, how to negotiate the facility and its routines and how to do things,
including how to leave. It also means knowing about the diagnoses and medications
they’re being given, their rights etc. and the context of the greater world, including the
cope with adversity if people can understand their circumstances: if people know what
Psychiatric patients often have a very hard time understanding the basis of how things
work and why things are like they are. In fact, it is widely hypothesised that attempts to
rationalise beliefs about how things work are the basis of delusions. (D. Freeman,
Garety, Kuipers, Fowler, & Bebbington, 2002; Garety & Freeman, 1999; Garety,
Kuipers, Fowler, Freeman, & Bebbington, 2001; Startup, Freeman, & Garety, 2008)
117
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Things that may improve comprehensibility for patients are clear way-finding, simple
decision making and as little ambiguity as possible. Ambiguity may be fun for healthy
Meaning
starvation, pain, illness and the worst demonstrations of human antipathy and feel
confident, that in the long run everything will turn out for the best (Antonovsky, 1987;
ever release, resolution and recovery (Clarkson, 2006), thus it is reasonable to assert
that the fostering of meaning is the single most important role of the mental health
facility. It is also the hardest task to accomplish because efforts cannot be prescriptive.
One of the great problems of health facility design and management is the problem of
suicide. The creation of meaning is the only way suicidal ideation can be reliably
treated. If anti-ligature fittings, CCTV and constant monitoring work at all, it is only
while a patient is locked into a unit. As long as patients are free to leave, suicidal
patients can always throw themselves in front of a passing car. When a patient discovers
something to live for, suicidal thoughts will lose their power to turn into action.
118
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
anything that is of greater importance than the individual self; friends, family, society,
the planet, pets – all these are likely areas of concern on which meaning can be fostered.
Religion can also fit into this group, but belief is infinitely plastic – in other words,
grounded and validated by some level of external reality, it loses its purpose and is as
worthless as any delusion. What belief is useful for is to create real and meaningful
links with the greater society, world and cosmos. Remember that one of the
fundamental reasons that the principles of salutogenics work, is because they enable
action rather than paralysis. If I believe God loves me because I care for the
environment, I will actively care for the environment, and those positive actions form
the basis of an affectively positive connection with the environment. A basis for
meaning.
behaviours. If I were to believe that it doesn’t matter, I can’t make a difference anyway;
I would be suffering from a passive delusion. If I were to believe that a random shopper
was evil, and the best thing I could do is to slay her (such an event took place recently in
p.373 q.v.)
External reality is, for the best part, very unforgiving. People can be very savage –
especially to people who behave strangely like many mental patients do (Goffman,
119
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
1963). But there are ways that patients can connect with external reality meaningfully,
This is not the place to detail all the ways of encouraging meaningful connections to the
Many patients with mental illnesses have pets – especially dogs, and in the context of
mental health recovery, pets should be considered as more important than any
Beck, 1983; Searles, 1960). When designing a unit, it is wise to provide at least some
Arts
The arts form important and traditional associations – they can be a meaningful way of
linking mental patients with society. This is particularly relevant because there is a
common perception that mental patients are frequently geniuses. Regardless of whether
or not this is true, everything that could encourage artistic and literary endeavours
performance are all wonderful for promoting a sense of meaning. And if you are
concerned (like many others are) about handing patients carving tools, bear in mind that
the use of woodcarving tools are increasingly common even in forensic mental health
units (yes, where patients are sectioned because of extreme psychiatric violence) in New
120
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Zealand, and the impacts on improved mental health and behaviour are really good. I’ve
also heard concerns that patients will paint on walls and write obscene and threatening
material. My answer to those concerns it to let them, and even to encourage it, but give
the patients washable materials! That way the invective and obscenities can be washed
Meaningful activity
It’s hard to find meaningful activity for patients who cannot even look after themselves,
but an effort should be made. One of the most meaningful things a patient can do is
share their recovery by helping out the more critical patients (Alomes, 2009). There was
a short period between 1850 and 1860 when less critical tasks used to be routinely given
to patients. Patients used to milk cows and work gardens, and the products of their
labours used to end up on the tables in the evening, by all accounts, it was a
Meaning takes time and effort to build, but can vanish in minutes. The thing that is most
reason that someone may want to hate the world and society: cruelty, meanness, broken
fit into this category. This is not to say anti-psychotics are bad. In most cases they are
essential. But the use of sedation as a management tool puts the management needs of
121
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Conclusion: an example of how a finished salutogenic unit might look and function.
facility will be different, but to conclude this article, I would like to present the
Frank is brought in a police van. It’s 2am. He’s been through this before because the
past few years he has spent more time in short stay mental health facilities than out. He
has been diagnosed with chronic Bipolar (type I) with frequent substance induced
psychotic episodes. Frank is aware that he was smoking cannabis earlier in the night,
but he’s furious because he didn’t do anything wrong – until the police arrived. It’s just
that someone tried to steal his car and beat him up and stripped him as a final
humiliation.
When the van door opens, Frank hurls abuse at his captors, the police. But he steps out
somewhere unexpected. It’s not a prison cell, nor a mental health facility; it’s nice. It
looks like the back veranda of a country home. The lights are low, and there are a
couple of people sitting, chatting on a sofa having a drink and a cigarette. One gets up
and meets Frank at the bottom of the steps. “Hi. I’m Lloyd,” He says.
122
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
“When we picked him up, he was vandalising a car in a parking lot…” one of the
policemen start to report. Frank doesn’t realise that the staff at the salutogenic unit have
all the information the police can give already. They were contacted twenty minutes
ago, and that’s why they are out here on the veranda at 2am!
“Put it in all your report, and kindly leave. This feller is naked, unhappy, and needs to
relax.” Lloyd says to the policeman, who is slightly affronted but gets back into the van,
and drives off. Lloyd leads Frank onto the veranda. “Do you want a dressing gown? A
drink or a cigarette?” Lloyd asks. Veranda, nice person, dressing gown, cigarette,
drink? Have I gone to heaven? Frank thinks to himself as he volunteers an arm for
Over the next fifteen minutes, Frank debriefs to Lloyd and Zaha on the sofa, with a
glass of lemon, lime and bitters (not his personal choice – but that’s what was there)
while he is introduced to the e-cigarette for the first time. “…Not allowed to smoke real
ones here,” Zaha explains, handing Frank the e-cigarette. “But this is much the same.
It’s still smoking, and it’s still nicotine, only you can use it indoors, and it’s not
cancerous. Have it, it’s yours.” Frank is asked if he has pets or anything he has to collect
123
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Soon Frank is led to his bedroom. The short corridor has a polished hardwood floor,
with a long and deep runner rug to muffle footsteps. Paintings (some donated by an art
school and some by other guests, he is told) hang off a picture rail. His room has a big,
heavy wooden door with a brass handle. Inside, the room is simple. A heavy wooden
bed with old-fashioned sheets and blankets is made up. The window has open louvers
and a mosquito screen. There is a heavy wooden desk, with paper, crayons and pens laid
out on it (they are washable, but John hasn’t been told that). The dimmed wall-mounted
wash lighting casts a warm yellow glow. A fern sits in a small pot on the table. There
are picture and dado rails, and an abstract painting hangs from them. As a precaution
against suicide attempts, a few of the features – the curtains, the picture rail and the
toilet roll holder in the bathroom are suspended by strong magnets, but the unit is still
new, and nobody has tried yet, so these features just look normal, fancy even.
The story continues…so long as the designer is willing and belief can be sustained –
This is a snippet of my vision of how psychiatric units will look and function in the
future: always cognisant that the healing process of mental illness involves humanity,
124
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
125
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Abstract
demand action; a cliff demands a leap and binoculars demand a peek. Behaviour-
settings are ‘places;’ spaces encoded with expectations and meanings. Behaviour-
settings work the opposite way to affordances; they demand inhibition; an introspective
settings are designed, and as such, designers are effectively predicting brain reactions.
automatic neural responses (excitation and inhibition). These, for the best part cancel
each other out. This balancing enables object recognition and allows choice about what
action should be taken (if any). But when excitation exceeds inhibition, instinctive
126
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
action will automatically commence. In positive circumstances this may mean laughter
dementia) have a reduced capacity to balance excitatory and inhibitory impulses. For
these people, environmental behavioural demands increase with the decline of frontal
brain function.
The world around us is not only encoded with symbols and sensory information.
interactions constantly take place at a molecular scale. Every space we enter has its own
special dynamic, where individualism vies for supremacy between the opposing forces
this context, even a small change – the installation of a CCTV camera can turn a circus
to a prison.
127
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Watching children on a 6th floor balcony, I couldn’t help but notice the way they
searched for objects to throw over the balustrade. The promise of such a large fall yells:
“Jump. Jump. JUMP!” It is only when an inhibitory reflex kicks in that a battle with a
coherence, and the urge is diverted. And the children harmlessly throw apples over the
The precipice, the apples – every opportunity the environment provides, calls for action.
But why? This article presents a framework of neurological mediators of perception and
128
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
129
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
We don’t just perceive a compilation of colours and shapes (‘sense data’), then figure
out what it all means, and then act on that knowledge as a serial process. The perception
of the actions that an object allows (Gibson, 1979), occurs with the first phase of
perception, prior even to the recognition of features such as colour and shape (Yantis,
threatened will be the first to be eaten, and action must therefore commence before
perception even reaches a declarative (conscious) stage (Stephen Kaplan, 1992). When
we hear a loud roar, a range of automatic actions take place; our legs may start running
and we may grab a weapon. The blood pressure and heart rate increases, plasma levels
of adrenaline increase and our senses are piqued (Ekeberg, Kjeldsen, Greenwood, &
Enger, 1990). All this occurs even before we know where the roar has issued from.
The instinctive reach for tools and escape paths is just one of many hints informing us
that perception revolves around opportunities to act, for better or for ill. These
opportunities are called ‘affordances’ and are a central premise of Gibson’s Ecological
Theory of Perception (Gibson, 1979; Withagen, de Poel, Araújo, & Pepping, 2012).
Action-opportunities (objects) are the primary stimulants for attention (the first phase of
perception), but even so, they are subject to pre-existing environmental restrictions
130
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
contextual awareness, a sense of what is possible and what is the right thing to do in
most given circumstances, and these are deliberately embodied in the physical milieu.
rarely ‘act now and think later:’ such impulsiveness is unusual and is frequently a sign
that you’d expect in children and adolescents (Quay & Quay, 1965).
131
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
inhibition.
expected to take place there, that is the standing patterns of behaviour (mores,) that take
football field invites the affordance of ball play, but restricts the play to specific rules
(Lhermitte, 1986). Most behaviour-settings are designed to reflect the mores that will
at home. At home you can just pick it up and eat it without breaking with convention.
keeping with the milieu is a primary automatic response and is neither creative nor
individualistic. This doesn’t mean that behaviour-settings are bad – or even good. Just
that they exist ubiquitously and that they modify the potential behaviour that an
132
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Normally healthy adults are particularly good at voluntarily resisting the temptation to
act. This inhibitory phase of perception extends the automatic inhibition provided by the
behaviour-setting. But some people have neither choice to act, nor to resist action. They
must act – even against specific and authoritative instructions. These people include
children, whose frontal lobes are not yet fully developed, but even more so patients with
organic brain damage (Lhermitte, 1986; Lhermitte, Pillon, & Serdaru, 1986).
During a clinical study investigating the effects of organic brain damage on behaviour,
one patient (a 52 year old housewife who had been given a partial frontal lobectomy to
remove a cancerous tumour) attempted to give her doctor an injection, just because a
syringe was handy. Another patient (a 51 year old engineer who had also had a
lobectomy for cancer) picked up a gun, and because it was unloaded, found the
appropriate cartridges. The doctor had to intervene to confiscate the weapon (Lhermitte,
1986). Like the patient with the syringe, the man would have had no choice but to shoot
the doctor. This bizarre behaviour, absolutely lacking in autonomy, is caused by the
involuntary in both the cases mentioned above. They are examples of how the constant
stream of action and thought that we humans are engaged in is completely co-opted by
affordances if the organ that is responsible for inhibiting impulses and enabling alternate
actions ceases to function. The same impulse to act occurs even in healthy individuals,
but what is bizarre about the attempted actions of the lady with the syringe and the man
with the gun, is the absence of normal neural reactivity, a product of the very same
organ that also moderates the functions of self-reference (Northoff & Bermpohl, 2004;
133
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Northoff et al., 2006) and creativity (Dietrich, 2004; Dietrich & Kanso, 2010). In other
134
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
behaviour-settings.
Not all action is automatic, especially for healthy adults. Given a context, we have two
choices; we can act as we please within the tight behavioural limits set for us by
learning. That means we can act on the automatic action cues imbedded within
behaviour-settings (see Figure 10). But we can also break convention and extend those
limits. So if a person has learned to adopt a certain demeanour in church, this will be the
automatic mode of action when in church. But, by an act of wilful defiance, a person
may dance down the isle. This is a creative act, originating in the prefrontal cortex, the
seat of creativity (Golembiewski, 2012a). Creative acts of this kind will extend the
1960); once you have danced down the isle once, it will be hard to return to the demure
135
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
matic responses and the frontal cortex the source of creative repro-
136
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
137
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The levels of perceptual engagement that affordances trigger are uneven. The reason is
that the level of perceptual engagement (activation potential) that they capture is uneven
and subject to competing forces. Live studies on monkeys, where sensors were wired
directly to monkey neurons (for obvious reasons not repeated on humans) isolate three
trigger (Schultz, 1998; Schultz, Apicella, & Ljungberg, 1993; Schultz, Dayan, &
Montague, 1997). All three factors are types of attentional salience; and the higher the
salience, the more likely the attention of a subject will be engaged 18. The three factors
are described below from numbers 2-4. The first factor was discovered by
experimentation on humans.
because it’s an opportunity to act. There may be the attraction of a nice smell, a
beautiful hue, a lovely texture (an individual may find themselves touching, looking,
18
The terms prominence, significance and worth are used in place of the more common,
138
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Prominence: This is the prominence of the physical qualia of the object or setting.
Noisy, sudden sounds, flashing lights and bright colours etc., are highly prominent and
Significance; the importance of the whole concept that is being recalled. There are three
a) anything that helps build meaning, b) anything that contributes to understanding, and
your work demands that you react in a certain way, you do so.
generates a much greater impact than the threat of a negative one (Mirenowicz &
Schultz, 1996; Schultz, 1998). This is the graded pleasure principle. There’s a big
difference in human reaction to bland food and delicious food, even though the two may
be equally healthy.
The presence of an affordance may be informed by any of the above four saliency
factors. For example, someone may pick a rose because it’s there to be picked
(opportunity) it catches their eye (prominence), they want to gift it to someone they love
139
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
range of neurotransmitters and processes that are beyond the scope of this article.
humans have rich taxonomies for every kind of sensation and are easily absorbed in
sensory experiences of all kinds. We hoard opportunities for sensory exploration in the
same way that a squirrel hoards acorns. Sensory opportunities are not a quantitative
this kind are invitations to explore and interact with the world in a sensual way (Gibson,
1979). A sensory opportunity may be found in the quality of a curve in a body (not only
From birth, babies will actively engage with the perceptual environment. They will
imitate expressions (Meltzoff & Moore, 1977), grasp a graspable object, and with age
they will want to touch textures, run fingers down Brancusi’s sculptures, absorb
themselves in colours and scents, feel the curvature and warmth of surfaces, immerse
themselves in tastes, experiment with sounds, etc. They will want to squeeze bubble-
wrap and juice cartons, just to hear them pop. They light sparklers to see the sparks fly
off in all directions; they burn incense to scent the air, and climb snowy mountains, just
to feel the rush of zooming down again. None of these engagements are especially
140
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
useful. But they are enjoyable, and possibly even fortify one’s mental health
limits on how much bubble-wrap one can pop). Anecdotally it appears that sensory
pleasures are renewed unless they are recast in a negative light, so the desire to eat fish,
for example might be repeated, but may be extinguished after a night of food poisoning.
Designers deliberately use sensory affordances all the time, and undoubtedly it makes
their projects more successful. From the crackle of pop-candy to the sweet-water
fountains of Rome, little sensory treats are difficult to resist, but do enrich experience in
a positive way. The architect, Louis Kahn appeared to be aware of this, and made his
a similar way to the way Gibson used affordances, and the term agreement to describe
the congruency of behaviour-setting, affordance and mores. It was his view that the
milieu should be designed in agreement with the mores that would take place there
(Kahn, 1971).
will catch your eye, but if you see them in orange all the time, then they take on
background status (they become less salient). Technically, the dopamine neurons that
141
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
are activated by prominence lose their activation potential with familiarity (Berns,
Cohen, & Mintun, 1997; Mirenowicz & Schultz, 1994). Familiarity is comfortable and
One very important fact about prominence is that, in the dopamine neurons that mediate
saliency, it has a short bursting (phasic) action which temporarily dominates the
underlying rhythmic ‘tonic’ action that moderates other forms of saliency (Jan
2007). Prominent perceptions may therefore displace other perceptions and thoughts
along with all other contents of the working memory (Lavie, 2010; Theeuwes, Kramer,
19
Hahn, & Irwin, 1998) . In real terms this will mean that prominent perceptions take
possession of the mind and generate enormous but temporary interest, in the same way
that a tiger would, if you suddenly discovered one in your home. It is as if the brain only
The importance of prominence for architects and other designers is that prominence gets
a lot of attention quickly. But it is difficult to maintain interest unless other saliency
19
It should be remembered that working memory is a hotly debated construct with
142
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
factors support it. The focus of human attention travels logically from an initial phase of
potential opportunity will also be extinguished, leaving only significance and worth.
This truism has frustrated many an egotistical designer. An architect may design a
prominent and forward-thinking building, only to see it rejected and all of its great
innovations ignored. Big exceptions occur when the project is somehow identified as
being genuinely important. This happens whenever the building serves mores, in which
case prominence is at least remembered as people go about their business. For example,
someone working next to Sydney’s Opera House may see it from their window daily, so
the initial shock of the new – the curves, bright white tiles and stylistic ambition – is
maintained by a generalised engagement with the Opera House. The interest we have in
new and delightful designs will only remain as long as those designs continue to be
Significance and worthiness are essentially two axes of the same concept. In the
commercial world, this is the ‘what’s in it for me?’ question that drives many a
spectrum between good or bad) and significance is what the worthiness qualifies. As an
(worthiness) but what is significant is that it will feed you. Significance is the potential
143
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
for an affordance to enrich or diminish the quality of our lives. ‘What’s in it for me?’
Significance A: Meaning. Things that imbue life with the richness of meaning are
significant even if they don’t serve any direct function as such. These things often
transcend the pedestrian notions of good or bad because they are essential. These are the
things we live and die for. And like all other forms of saliency, can be attributed to both
religious artefacts and places for evidence of the importance of these settings and the
mores that surround them. Take the Temple Mount for the Jews, the Church of the Holy
Sepulchre for many Christians and the Al-Aqsa Mosque for Islam. Places and artefacts
comprehensibility. We engage with things that help us understand the situations we find
ourselves in, for example, some people may find astrology interesting as it gives them
insight into people and behaviour and even into themselves. The desire to find out more
is the basis of a rich personal ontology and provides a foundation for independence
(Antonovsky, 1987).
existence manageable right now may also be significant (Antonovsky, 1987). But here
144
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
we have a caveat; you must have a simultaneous corollary need. Things that enable
manageability alone are in themselves not very enriching, yet they are essential – but in
basic shelter from the weather, food etc. Manageability is frequently the only
(Jan Golembiewski, 2010b). And while things that enable existence are important, they
are of no interest when the need does not present itself (preparations for future needs
all. Yet, in mid-winter it becomes very important indeed as it yells ‘put me on!’
At first glace, affordances that appear to be positive draw more attention than negative
ones. Technically, this appears to be due to one of the functions of the medium spiny
striatal dopamine neurons. Empirical studies of these neurons show that, in healthy adult
populations, they are reliably more easily excited by positive stimulus than negative
(Northoff et al., 2004; Schultz, 1998). The judgement of worthiness is among the first
of a series of automatic reactions. And in general terms, positive affordances are far less
subject to reactionary inhibition. For example, smiles, laughter, warm fuzzy feelings
and cause automatic and unrestricted engagement beyond the a-priori automatic
restrictions imposed by the behaviour-setting. Put another way, the choice to engage in
positive experiences is a normal feature of robust mental health, but so too is the ability
this positive bias is far less apparent or is even inversed in fact ‘affective flattening’ or
Golembiewski, 2012a).
(2012a) and based on data from (Northoff et al., 2004)), shows results
from a clinical study of three groups. The first group: C) were akinetic
and seven with bipolar disorder. The second group: P) had seven par-
146
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
there were ten people who had never been diagnosed with a mental
disorder.
The numbers (graphed) show the net excess of neural excitatory reac-
tions after all concurrent inhibitory reactions has been subtracted. All
stimulus (grey card; grey). As you can see, in the negative condition,
there was such a minor excess of excitation in the healthy group that it
was not chartable (0.63%). The patients however, were unable to bal-
were not so strongly balanced out (although some inhibition had taken
place – not graphed). The healthy group had about double the positive
reaction than the paranoid group and about a third greater positive
Unless negative affordances are very prominent, an individual will react against them,
effectively reducing their activation potential to nearly nothing (here only 21 more
excitatory neurons fire than inhibitory ones. In psychiatric conditions the excitation is
much higher - as much as 1385 among the paranoid group. See Figure 11.)
147
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
An interesting correlate is that when healthy people start perceiving something with a
negative sentiment, they avoid further exploration and continue to reject the affordance,
automatic and therefore requires conscious and creative intervention to reverse. For
example, neighbours may complain about the construction of an absolutely glorious and
useful building because the construction meant cutting down a beautiful forest on the
site. To get the neighbours to appreciate the building is going to be difficult, as they
have decided to explore the ‘affordance’ no further. One very important thing to note
here, is that a very small design change can have a disproportionate effect on the
‘worth’ of an affordance or behaviour-setting. A neutral space can very easily adopt the
148
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
traditionally played a major role in the design industries, but observations have seldom
object will be engaged with is relatively simple; an object is perceived for what it offers.
lighter to be used. The Zippo® may also have significance; it is presentable, meaning it
serves a need for meaning (social status), and also for the occasional function
The main complication with affordances is that they are subject to change given
bench. It says, ‘Pick me!’ when hanging, ripe from a bough. And it says, ‘Buy me!’
149
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Figure 12: Jeff Koons (1985). Three Ball 50/50 Tank (Two Dr. J Sil-
ver Series, One Wilson Supershot). MOMA. New York. Glass, painted
33.6 cm).
In Three Ball 50/50 Tank and in others, Koons explored how af-
But behaviour-settings not only represent complications for affordances, they also
called determinism. For small objects (affordances), this is done intuitively and with
relative success, but for big objects – buildings in particular, the variables are more
150
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The concept of architectural determinism has fallen to disrepute and the term is usually
There are a number of reasons why authors criticise deterministic thought – for the fact
that it privileges the power of the environment and neglects psychology – the individual
Sometimes deterministic failure is put down to normative effects such as beliefs of the
designer:
Determinists are accused of overestimating the value and power of the their own ideas –
for example, the innovative vertical corridors in the Pruitt-Igoe complex were thought
by reviewers to be fabulous, but many of the people who lived in the complex were far
less enthusiastic (Franck, 1984; Lang, 1980). In other words, the mere presence of
opportunity within the environment does not ensure that will be noticed, appreciated or
Determinists are accused of a naïve belief that environmental effects on behaviour are
always direct (Franck, 1984). For example, Hellmuth, Yamasaki and Leinweber (later
complex provided common rooms for suites of several apartments in the belief that they
151
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
would create informal communities. The stark reality is that the provision of a common
room doesn’t create a community. The common room is just a space until mores match
it. And a space without positive mores has a tendency to turn to a negative space
(Jacobs, 1961). In this case, Pruitt-Igoe was famously criticised for a lack of passive
Determinists are accused of believing that people are passive in their interactions with
the environment and are not driven by their own autonomous goals and choices (Franck,
1984).
They are also accused of assuming that the environment is an immutable entity that will
Determinists may also be cynical of knowledge that they don’t have – for example an
architect who doesn’t know anything about colour theory might dismiss it without
There is a possibility that deterministic design may fail because designers employ faulty
phenomenon, but for unknown reasons may not be applicable for predictive design.
Psychological theory is largely derived from empirical studies that are performed in
152
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
controlled circumstances, which may not reflect reality in a holistic way. People don't
Input errors may occur because a designer has embraced part of an unresolved debate.
Sometimes the motivations, values and competence levels of populations are not
properly considered when designing behaviour-settings. This may affect the usability of
All of the above factors play a part in limiting the effectiveness of deterministic design;
after all, there are many ways to judge success or failure and many more ways that
something may be right or wrong about a design. But possibly the most significant
factor is that a behaviour-setting must be congruous with the mores that take place
therein, and whilst architects can design the milieu, they cannot design mores. In
other words, when designing a behaviour-setting, architects may design just one side of
a complex interaction; the other side is largely social and neurological and cannot be
‘designed’ – certainly not in the way that buildings can be. As Louis Kahn may have
said, the building is not in agreement (Kahn, 1971). This is not to say that revolutionary
ideas cannot work. It’s just that any new scheme still needs to be matched with mores in
order to be fully functional. Wherever the customary mode is abandoned, any effort to
153
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
make a scheme engaging will encourage users to make the commitment to adapt to the
Of course both the physical environment and behaviour is subject to adaption. But
transferred to a new but familiar milieu. But if the design is congruent with a major shift
successful radical architecture are matched by equally radical social change. Niemeyer
and Costa’s city of Brasília reflected the major social, industrial and political changes of
Brazil that occurred during the highly popular reformist presidency of Kubitscheck.
Even before the foundation stone was laid, the city had been long awaited. The
decision that had been written into the constitution six generations earlier (Avila, 2008).
Similarly another modernist and innovative city that has been continually praised, is
Chandigarh, planned by Le Corbusier, which was built to re-house the Punjab capital
after Lahore was lost to Pakistan during the partition of India. India’s first Prime
Minister, Nehru, personally oversaw the project seeing it as a symbol of India’s new
independence and self-determination (Lang, 2005). But not all ambitious rationalist
154
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The twentieth century saw a list of great architectural ideas being bulldozed (and
occasionally blown up) there are many reasons for the failure of architectural
determinism, as outlined earlier, but especially when inhabitants’ don’t engage with the
an abstract one.
The archetypal example of failure was the aforementioned Pruitt-Igoe public housing
development in St. Louis. This development was originally praised for its innovations –
especially for the creation of common balcony areas, each servicing twelve apartments
of 1-4 bedrooms each. These would run off the common areas to either side or
neighbourhoods’ within the larger monolithic building (Lopez, 1956). Apart from these
common spaces, there was no common horizontal circulation within the buildings.
The relative successes and failures of Pruitt-Igoe are still hotly debated with fashionable
opinions swinging one-way and then the other (see (Freidrichs, 2011) for comparison).
With a project as iconic and complex as Pruitt-Igoe, it’s hardly a surprise. Even so, it’s
reasonable to assert that projects like this that could benefit from greater insight into the
155
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Conclusions
Affordances and behaviour-settings are at once designed objects and are also designed
automatically trigger inhibitory ones. Beyond these, the range of potential behaviours
that are associated with these design-objects can be expanded or contracted through
choice by engaging the creative centre of the brain. This action has the benefit of
fostering individuality and a sense-of-self, which is incidentally one of the major factors
in combating a range of important mental health problems (Kean, 2009; Sass & Parnas,
Affordances are relatively intuitive to design because they are simple. As long as there
are provisions for one or more forms of salience, the affordance (the object) will be in
the running to engage people. But the design of behaviour-settings is a wicked problem
and is much more difficult. The problems are open ended and you can always ‘do
better’ (Kunz & Rittel, 1972). Furthermore the problem isn’t improved by the
affordances. But it is a mistake to think that people will be content with prescribed
behaviour patterns. The strong correlations between the way people behave and the
milieu where the behaviour takes place does not mean that architecture determines
156
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
behaviour – even if it appears that way. Equally, it is mistaken to say that mores
determine human behaviour. A behaviour-setting still inhibits behaviour when only one
person is present. We don’t stop being social beings just because nobody else is around.
Behaviour-settings will continue to suggest mores when people are not present at all.
When a shop is opened up in the morning, shoppers will behave very much as they did
the day before, even though there was nobody there to continue the behaviour
overnight.
Behaviour-settings are more than the limitations on how a space can be used. A
cathedral may offer enough space to play football, but this behaviour is not suggested
by the architecture (curiously sports stadiums have been used as churches during events
such as World Youth Day, but how these exceptions have been achieved are a study of
their own). Behaviour-settings do not only limit behaviour, they suggest it also.
between designed place, the mores that it enables and the neurology and psychology of
the users. A new setting can change those mores, but only incrementally, as much as
people will individually allow. The adoption process can be sped up if the mores are
already in a state of positive flux. Under these conditions, people are more willing to
change.
157
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
because most of the research into health facility design is limited to studies involving
direct evidence. These studies, though very valuable, only serve to prove or disprove
very specific hypotheses in a very controlled setting. Typically, such a study will
involve the modification of one element of the environment and then records of hospital
stays or consumer satisfaction will be gathered. There is a growing frustration with this
approach. Designers, researchers and commissioning bodies need and want more. They
been difficult to apply such knowledge and even more so to assess outcomes. The in-
across the board. With this in mind, I was invited to present the following paper on
general hospital design as an invited plenary address at the Design and Health
March 2012.
158
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
This paper was received a highly commended award in the 8th World
159
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Abstract
Objectives
The theory of salutogenics has a basis in the empirical testing and ideas of Antonovsky,
which state that health outcomes improve when a sense of coherence is fostered. A
sense of coherence in turn, depends on the net resources that support meaning,
health is obvious, but why abstractions like meaning and comprehensibility are
Methods
Salutogenic abstractions are traced to back to current neurological models to locate the
Results
limbic region and the association cortices; and manageability by the reptilian brain: the
mesencephalon and primary perceptual cortices. In general, the slower, but more
evolved frontal functions take precedence. But the paleomammalian organs continually
narrative, the neomammalian processes are allowed to continue, but if things ‘look
bad,’ instincts regulated by the reptilian brain take over. The result includes a wide
Conclusions
architecture embodies narratives that may either support or work against a state of good
161
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Architecture mediates almost all experience and moderates a great deal of behaviour.
For the best part, the moderation effect that architecture plays is qualitatively even. The
ubiquity of vertical walls and orthogonal layouts with very similar features for similar
typologies guarantees this. Is there a qualitative difference in a right turn over a left
one? Does it mater whether a window is a sash-cord or a casement? Either way the
actions these decisions prescribe are radically different, but on a qualitative level, the
But architecture can be genuinely manipulative, and herein lies the qualitative
how architecture is used as a weapon, to defeat an enemy before a shot has ever been
fired; Snodgrass (1990) describes architecture that is used as a vehicle for religious and
spiritual revelation; and Jencks (1999) makes a solid attempt to relate how architecture
can thrill its visitors. But when it comes to healthcare design, architecture is rarely so
focused on psychology. Somehow architecture all too often loses its considerable
models of care, functional programme, efficiencies, sightlines, infection control and the
needs of a diverse group of users. There’s no question; all these things are critical, but to
162
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
let functional and structural issues lead design is to let the cart lead the horse. So is it
possible to harness the considerable manipulative power of architecture for better health
outcomes?
especially when they are already vulnerable. Sudjic (2006) proposes that the endless
gallery leading to Hitler’s chambers in Albert Speer’s Neuen Reichskanzlei, and the
grandiosity and sheer scale of his suite so amplified the helplessness of the 1939
president of Czechoslovakia, Dr. Emil Hácha, that he not only surrendered his nation,
163
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
by Heinrich Hoffmann.
Reichskanzlei (Figure 13) had anything to do with the state of Dr. Hácha’s heart, but
from a salutogenic perspective, the relationship is un-missable. The same can be said for
don’t mean causation. But architects and healthcare planners should not be dismissed so
164
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
easily. Reviews of healthcare design research show there are now thousands of relevant
studies, many of which have been shown to make a substantial difference in health
outcomes (Ulrich et al., 2008). But these studies invariably focus on single details,
‘stress’. The salutogenic methodology is both more holistic and more specific: for
Antonovsky (1987) the stimulation we call stress isn’t just noise, it’s information. And
our ability to cope with the flood of information – be it good or bad – is ultimately what
The theory of salutogenics has a basis in the empirical testing and ideas of Antonovsky
(1987), which find that health outcomes improve when a sense of coherence is fostered.
A sense of coherence (SOC) in turn, depends on the net resources that support
Manageability: the physical resources needed to keep on going. It’s paradigmatic that
this is virtually all healthcare is obliged to do: to treat the sick and keep them warm, fed
and dry while they recover from a point of critical illness or injury.
information will include facts about the nature of the illness, how long a stay will be,
165
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
what treatments are available, who will be involved and how, information about side
effects and what resources will assist in recovery. In the healthcare sector, things are
improving for comprehensibility and there are a few ‘islands in the sun,’ but
Meaningfulness enriches and gives quality to life. It is the sum of all the best reasons
fostering of meaning to be their role, but when people are beleaguered with anxiety
about their state of health and about the future, it’s hard to imagine that any other
in there and believe that everything is going to be okay in the long-run (Antonovsky,
1987, 1996; Jan Golembiewski, 2010b; Golembiewski, 2012b, 2012e). A caveat is due
more important (in the short term) than meaningfulness whenever ‘things are not
looking good’ for a patient, and they know it - in cases of primary myocardial infarction
(cardiac arrest), for instance (Bergman, Malm, Ljungquist, Bertero, & Karlsson, 2012),
166
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The reason that manageability is important for health is obvious and doesn’t need
further elaboration here, but why abstractions like meaning and comprehensibility are so
The brain can be seen as an organ that perceives, creates and manages meaning. The
most basic level of this is manageability. Even the most simple of creatures need to
manage the needs of survival: sustenance, protection and reproduction. The bulk of the
relevant processes to manage these needs are automatic in most creatures. The areas of
the brain that are wired for manageability are within the reptilian complex; a simple
system that implements a simple formula; if it’s food, eat it, if it’s danger, flee and if it
cognitive, interpretive step. And this presents a problem: how is sense-data interpreted.
The internal representations we have of external objects cannot be true to reality. This is
firstly because reality offers a richness that humans simply cannot experience: humans
don’t even share the same sensory range as one another (some have better high
frequency hearing, others are colour blind etc.), and certainly we have very different
sensory ranges to other animals. Bats can hear pitches of up to 115kHz, and humans
only about 17 kHz, humans can feel light of 300-400 tHz range as heat, but poecilia
reticulata (the common freshwater guppy) can see and taste the same spectrum. But
what humans lack in terms of sensory ability, we make up for in cognitive ability. We
167
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
have an extraordinary ability to identify the differences between just about everything,
and this information is stored in vast lexical ontologies. At a glance we can tell the
difference between a Nokia and an iPhone, between a painting by Modigliani and one
by Picasso or a song by Jimi Hendrix and a religious hymn. Not all this information is
immediately relevant, but it is our nature to store and use such information as needed.
Information becomes critical only when required: “I need medical attention – where’s
The cerebral cortex is used for associations, in other words, the links between pieces of
and knowledge is their retrieval. Although learning takes place over a globally
distributed network (Baars & Franklin, 2007), the main place that somatic (sensual,
physical) associations are paired with meanings is in the primary perceptual cortices
(Grossberg, 2009). A paleomammalian (limbic) organ called the thalamus mediates the
somatic nervous and sympathetic nerve impulses and distributes them to the appropriate
perceptual areas of the brain. These are then associated with one another and more
abstract meanings in the higher perceptual association cortices and with actions in the
Delusional subjects generally have relatively low SOC scores, due to poor
manageability and meaningfulness scores. But recovery from delusional states only
reduces SOC scores further – particularly for comprehension, implying that the
168
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Meaning is an order more complex again, and in this evolutionary step, the anterior
hemisphere of the cerebral cortex swells to make a distinctly neomammalian shape, one
that is far more defined in humans than in any other species – it is the neomammalian
prefrontal cortex (PFC). These organs process choice, insight, creativity, the sense-of-
self and other complex cognitive processes to establish appropriate actions for
situations for which there are no ready responses (Dietrich, 2004; Northoff et al.,
2006). The use of the neomammalian complex definitively separates humans from all
Like other advanced mammals, the folds of the neomammalian complex contain a thick
network of dopamine neurons. What differentiates ours from those of all other animals
is that most of the neural receptors in the human frontal cortex are of the D1 type with
higher densities of D2 type receptors deeper, in the paleomammalian regions (see the top
section of Figure 15). In contrast, D2 receptors remain ubiquitous throughout the entire
20
Note that some neuroscientists question the validity of the free-will concept, but neu-
roscientists who do believe in the freedom of will usually link the concept to creativity,
and the ‘most human’ of organs, the prefrontal cortex (Searle, 2001).
169
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
mesofrontal region in other mammals – from the reptilian middle brain right through to
the prefrontal cortex (Nichols, 2010; Prante, Dörfler, & Gmeiner, 2010). The difference
is not subtle. The D1 receptor is excitatory, meaning that when it fires, it activates the
neo-mammalian complex. This means that some perceptions will activate the centre of
activations in the same region. This enables faster judgements, but at the expense of
consideration, thoughtfulness and creativity. This explains what we all know already;
that humans are naturally more considered in their actions than any other animals.
Each evolutionary iteration of the brain must process increasingly complex information,
but like a computer the more complex information is, the slower it is to work through.
reflexes, the more evolved areas don’t replace the primitive ones. The more developed
layers of the brain fold around the more primitive, and mechanisms have been
developed that allow the primitive/instinctive areas to spring back into action whenever
speed is required. In advanced mammals, reflexes are regulated by the amygdalae (used
to monitor emotional context) and the hippocampi (to monitor schemata and narrative
context). Together these set up thresholds for automatic inhibitory responses and
approximately set for appropriate action in a given situation (Golembiewski, 2013) (see
Figure 14).
170
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
#Automatic#limbic#processes# Affective$/$hedonic$preCjudgement
1a.$Narrative/affective$monitoring – +
Automatic#(mesostriatal)#processes Creative#(frontal)#reprocessing
1b.$Latent$restrictions$from
behaviour$settings$(inhibition) 2.$Expanding$possibilities$of$
behaviour$settings$(excitation)
Behaviour#limits
3.$Opportunities$of
Affordances$(excitation) 4.$Choice$to$react$against
– affordances$(inhibition)
+
}}
5.#Potential#range#of#final#action
(mediated#by#the#striatum)
for narratives that impart an affective or hedonic value (i.e., “it’s good or bad
for me”) the sense of “where I am in this story” roughly corresponds to the
171
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Sometimes surprises are good and welcome – jokes for instance, and the automatic
reactions include laughter, smiles and happy feelings. These are good and don’t need to
be managed. But a negative shock will solicit screaming (sounding an alarm to others)
and even more basic self-protection instincts: the release of stored energy to enable
flight, and the defensive withdrawal of blood from the periphery of the body (to prevent
certain death, should a snake or bear bite a limb). All of these reactions commence in
the reptilian complex, and their activation inhibits the activation of the neomammalian
The withdrawal of peripheral circulation protects the body from physical shocks. It is a
primary function of the hypothalamus, the pineal gland and adrenal cortex. Together,
cortisol (Steptoe & Kivimaki, 2012). It’s fast, but not an ideal solution because cortisol
has a number of undesirable side effects: It overrides healthy limits on blood sugar,
blood pressure and heart rate (the tip of the iceberg: there are other negative side effects
also). In non-emergencies, cortisol synthesis doesn’t occur and essential hormones are
produced from the same primary compound (cholesterol) instead. These maintain
dihydrotestosterone (DHEA) “the joie de vivre hormone” (Gluck & Edgeson, 2010),
testosterone and oestrogen, to name only a few (see lower section of Figure 15).
172
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Chemical
Neuronal-Excitatory
Key
Neuronal-Inhibitory (’shuts down’ target)
Neuronal-Axon (base)
Glu
pathway D1
D2
pathway
Adrenocorticotropic hormone
Hippocampus Oxytocin
Figure 15: The automatic vs. creative action pathways – the thresholds
It is important to note that cortisol is not stress and stress is not cortisol, although the
two are commonly confused. Cortisol can be a useful medicine when used wisely.
173
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Cortisone (synthesised cortisol) is used to suppress the immune system (during allergies
b) People find themselves in situations, which they associate with lurking danger (a
comprehensibility).
hippocampi.
These include instinctive and learned behaviours, but the problematic ones are
perseverance (when in excess, all of these are associated with mental illness)
It begins to become clear that the failure of comprehensibility triggers the reflexes in the
reptilian brain to produce cortisol and other unwanted reactions. This is apparently a
174
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
‘last-ditch effort’ to maintain the last and most primitive of the salutogenic resources:
manageability.
If asked if Dr. Hácha’s experience in the Neuen Reichskanzlei could have caused a
heart attack, there is no doubt that the building was an essential factor in rapidly increas-
ing his vulnerability. The design of the Neuen Reichskanzlei worked (as intended) to
defeat adversaries. The narrative of immense power was written into the masonry. And
when this was combined with the generalised negativity that Dr. Hácha must have
sensed as he approached his nemesis, his creative and adaptive resources would have
been reduced. Instead of using his neomammalian brain to test creative solutions, the
negativity inherent in his situation (reinforced by the architecture and social milieu)
must have triggered alternative automatic behaviour pathways, thereby reducing his
thoughts to testing established fears for himself and his country. It’s easy to imagine Dr.
Hácha thinking thoughts along the lines of: ‘Perhaps they have invaded already, and
they are keeping me here so I’m not there to rule my country in its hour of need?’ (Note
the paranoid thinking). Meanwhile, on a physiological level, Dr. Hácha’s cortisol levels
will have soared, causing his blood pressure to increase. When extreme this reaction can
cause transient myocardial ischemia, an atypical cause of cardiac arrest: the heart stops
because of the sudden swelling of the heart tissues (Steptoe & Kivimaki, 2012).
175
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Figure 16: A patient reads the morning paper on one of the many
ridian Asia Pte Ltd – photo credit CPG Consultants Pte Ltd)
Thus architecture can be a powerful weapon. But can it be an equally powerful ally?
this, the embedded narratives within the environment must be managed. That means
taking great care that the typology and patient experience is recognisably and
indisputably positive. The first step is to distance the style of the new facility from those
ubiquitously seen in medical dramas. That means design teams should look for
176
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
functional alternatives to white walls, vinyl floors, strip lighting, typical services and
even the design of surgical scrubs and nurses’ uniforms (Richardson, 1999). Bring in
masses of glorious plants and fresh air like in the Khoo Teck Puat Hospital in Singapore
(Figure 16), and consciously design the soundscapes, lighting, and patient experience.
Perhaps the most alarming features of modern hospitals are the monitors, which set off
alarms when a patient’s vital signs are irregular. There is no question that these
machines are essential, but the alarms no longer need to sound right by a patient’s
devices that are carried by medical staff. Healthcare lighting should be natural and
electric light should be low -glare, and be of a ‘warm’ temperature (Tammes & Burnett,
in review). Windows should look out to gardens and plants and art should adorn patient
rooms. Many interventions of this kind have already been studied and found to be
useful, even on their own, once again see (Malkin, 2008; Ulrich et al., 2008) for handy
reviews.
177
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
friends, the world around etc. and not only focused on survival. In this
diagram blue represents the locus of attention, and green peripheral at-
tention.
The next step is to enrich meaning. Meaning is to be found whenever humans engage in
concerns beyond their own (even if these are very abstract); the well-being of family
and friends, in the greater good of society, in protecting animals and the environment,
even in protecting the security of the cosmos (Figure 17). Designing environments that
foster meaning isn’t easy because what makes life worth living is intrinsically personal,
but design decisions that should be avoided are more universal. At the top of this list,
178
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
(Golembiewski, 2009b, 2012b; Reach Out, 2009; Searles, 1966). As mentioned earlier,
attack. Meaninglessness must be avoided in all scales from typology to detail. Yet
and on, rooms without windows, machines that make alarming sounds and have
flashing lights (the staff appear to ignore them and never explain what they are for).
Large wards, where patients may feel like objects not people. More important still are
the subtleties of approaches to care: Staff that don’t look you in the eye, don’t seem to
(Gottman, 1996). Unsurprisingly, evidence suggests that the same innovations that
improve health outcomes also improve social relations (L. Larsen, Adams, Deal,
Kweon, & Tyler, 1998). It doesn’t always take much to change these details, and the
179
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
tects (Bates Smart and Billard Leece) up the ante: hospitals can be
It seems ironic, but hospital stays are shortened by better hospital experiences. Thus,
designers are wise to concentrate on making a patient want to stay. The ‘-care’ in the
term: ‘healthcare’ is axiomatic for better health because care supports the amygdaloid/
help and it’s all going to be okay,” then the scene is set for the ideal health-building be-
haviour and thought patterns to kick in. Furthermore, unhelpful by-products such as cor-
180
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
tisol will be reduced, meaning the endocrine system can naturally regularize.
Ultimately, the healthcare designer has to juggle two narrative concerns. On one hand
vigilance is needed to avoid any typologies, symbols and settings that may be associated
with negative outcomes. On the other hand, the team should look for opportunities to
imbed positive experiences for all the facility users. Not only are all good things ex-
pected to improve the outcomes for patients, but to improve the environment for the en-
Already some industry leaders are pushing far beyond the statutes and guidelines to
make exceptional hospitals by focusing on the patient and staff experience. Simple con-
cepts like comfort, cosines, joy and aesthetics have had no place in traditional twentieth
century hospitals, yet they are the psychological bricks and mortar of all healthy build-
ings, whether or not they are healthcare facilities. The architects who designed the Roy-
al Children’s Hospital in Melbourne, Billard Leece Partnership and Bates Smart, put in
a giant touch screen – like a huge iPod, for kids to play with. There’s a multi-level
aquarium, a great adventure playground and even a meercat enclosure. CPG Architects
ensured that Khoo Teck Puat Hospital in Singapore departed from the twentieth century
hospital paradigm by introducing an abundance of greenery, fresh air and 100 varieties
of butterflies. The payoff is that patients feel that things are going to be better than ex-
181
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
After completing ‘Start Making Sense’ (from page 62), I could see that the salutogenic
methodology may have application outside the walls of the mental health facility, even
for treating mental health disorders. To test and see if it was acceptable to the many
people involved in mental health services delivery, I presented the following paper to
the Australian Rural and Remote Mental Health Symposium in Canberra, 2nd
182
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Presented to the Australian Rural and Remote Mental Health Symposium in Canberra,
Abstract
Not a lot is known about most mental illness. Its triggers can rarely be established and
nor can its aetiological dynamics, so it is hardly surprising that the accepted treatments
for most mental illnesses are really strategies to manage the most overt symptoms. But
with such a dearth of knowledge, how can worthy decisions be made about psychiatric
stance (that psychiatric health care is for the betterment of psychiatric patients,) and
guiding evidence, the method identifies reasonable alternatives on the fly, enabling
183
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
(Antonovsky 1987).
An emergency psychiatric response is the sort of time critical intervention that might
not be well informed by enough evidence to proceed quickly but must regardless.
Whatever the nature of the emergency, it is the very nature of catastrophe that they
catch people unprepared; in recent history we’ve seen bushfires, floods, earthquakes,
tsunami’s, storms, volcanoes, landslides and winds the events are diverse in nature, but
184
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Office of the Chief Signal Officer. (1906) National archives and Rec-
It is the nature of emergencies that they are sudden and catastrophic (Galambos, 2005).
A swift turn of events means that some people lose their lives and other lives are put on
tenterhooks. The initial damage tends to be physical and material, but mental health
issues follow close behind. Whatever the emergency event: be it tsunami, bushfire,
storm, flood or earthquake, the initial impact of the disaster only spells the beginning of
the catastrophe. Because of supply interruptions provisions for basic needs such as
communication, shelter, food, sanitation and water as well as health services and social
networks, matters tend to deteriorate after the initial shock. It is at this early stage when
It is a conceit to think of mental and physical health as separate issues. Although they
21
are distinct, the two are intimately related. One will exacerbate the other . That is,
21
This is true in situations where the emergency was an unusual occurrence. In
situations where emergencies are regular and expected, such as in the North East of
NSW, which is subject to frequent flooding, events cause some psychological stress, but
185
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
mental stress tends to make physical illness worse and vice versa. For this reason it is
imperative that service providers address mental issues as quickly and appropriately as
possible after a catastrophic event, just as they will food, shelter and sanitation.
Delays in the provision of all forms of emergency care can be caused by indecision
about what is suitable action. The question of appropriateness always calls for
normative decisions based on subjective opinion – and those opinions are difficult to
justify in life and death situations – especially when providing a limited resource to one
person might mean someone else will go without. The outcome, sadly, is that decisions
sometimes aren’t made and this only makes matters worse. It is for the psychological
comfort of the decision makers that they (we) want to rely on evidence to make
decisions. Evidence based decisions are easily justified and move the burden of decision
making to scientists elsewhere. And fair enough. There’s little doubt that empiricism is
the most suitable way forward in the circumstances where empirical evidence exists and
is available at hand (Galambos 2005). This is, sadly, rarely the case.
The scientific method is very specific and conclusions can only be proven if they are
drawn in isolation from any confounding variables that might otherwise influence the
data. Not just is this clinical approach divorced from reality, but also more significantly,
the findings tend to be incremental and as such there isn’t enough empirical data to
answer most basic real world problems. I am an architect and I specialise in mental
186
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
health design 22. Like emergency services provision, there is a huge onus on evidence
to generate any innovations in this area of the profession. As with emergency services
provision, evidence is time consuming to locate and analyse, and in many cases it turns
out to be irrelevant in any case. There are few clients who are willing to encumber their
design bills with much time for research, and yet, decisions need to be made constantly
because architectural problems are incredibly consequent. One minor change here will
affect something else elsewhere. Like the problems that face providers for emergencies,
the problems we face are incredibly complicated and open ended, often with no clear
solutions. Thus we usually have two ways forward. To continue to do things the way
they have always been done – even if we suspect those methods are dated or plain
wrong. Or we have to take risks and improvise. But I have been faced with these
problems and have tried both methods and found them both deficient – So I developed a
methodology that allows reasonable decisions to be made on the fly. And the same
methodology can be easily adapted to enable quick and appropriate decision making for
22
I also have some experience with emergency services provision; I was part of
Bangun, a UNSW effort to assist survivors of the 2004 Boxing Day Tsunami.
187
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Firstly it is important to understand that every provision means the denial of something
else because resources are always limited in some way. It’s like going and ordering at a
restaurant. Out of the twenty dishes presented you’ll only get to choose one. If the
choice is yours, there’s a fair chance it will be the best choice. If the choice is left to
someone else – your spouse for example, you might be disappointed. The defence that
‘beggars can’t be choosers,’ is unhelpful. It is not the spirit of fine dining to be classed
between sickness and health is indistinct and a person’s level of well-being will be
somewhere on the continuum between death and a theoretical state of perfect health.
Antonovsky found that a person’s state of health reflects a person’s sense of coherence.
The more a person feels they understand and make a contribution to the world around
and meaningfulness (Jan Golembiewski, 2010b) (See p.62 q.v.) Ultimately a SOC
builds a dynamic feeling of confidence that one’s internal and external environments
are predictable and that there is a high probability that things will work out as well as
can reasonably be expected (Bahrs et al., 2003). These categories can be extrapolated to
situation a subject finds him or herself in. In a disaster situation this can mean
information: who, why, what, where, how and when. Manageability, the next of the
an emergency situation it will be the things that enable survival, things like food, water,
communication, shelter and things that can be done to lessen the impact of the disaster.
Meaning is the most illusive of all the salutogenic principles, yet it is in many ways the
most important. Meaning always comes into question during disasters, yet it is the
hardest issue to address. Meaning is the subject of the big question; “but why?” If
meaning can be sustained, then survival in the most difficult and horrific circumstances
becomes possible (Frankl, 1963). This kind of survival gives others hope. It is the stuff
A salutogenic methodology for emergency care ensures that the best interests of the
victims of disasters are always maintained with a holistic perspective, even when
individual’s needs holistically and in such a way that the effort isn’t likely to interfere
with the psychosocial needs of the subjects. This method is intended to be used by
remember and easy to apply. It’s just a matter of keeping three things at the forefront of
these concepts work on the ground. With more complex projects (such as architecture or
emergency service provision,) each decision can be analysed with respect to the three
189
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
use on the fly and mistakes are okay, to make an error of judgement using a salutogenic
happening. In the case of the Victorian fires, there was a need for information about
where the fires were heading and at what velocity. Information is also needed about the
emergency response. When will help arrive? People need to know who is dead and who
survived. They need to know about the safety of their family, friends, pets and livestock.
They need to know if their homes are in danger or if there is anything left of them.
These are all very profound issues and quality information is an extremely important
tool for the maintenance of a sense of coherence. The inverse is also true. Rumours and
lies are harmful (Freyd, Klest, & Allard, 2005). And so too can be ‘shielding’ survivors
from the truth (although this is a complex issue in itself because the truth is rarely
definitive and hope must also be maintained as much as possible.) It might be really
tough to tell a mother that her child has died, but if it is absolutely and unavoidably true,
there is no protecting her from that fact. Yes, sympathy is called for and it might be ‘the
last straw, ‘ but it is an inevitable last straw, and at the very least it shouldn’t come with
that everything is working out as well as possible. Disappoint on the other hand might
be taken as betrayal. The art of making promises has implications for higher levels of
coherence. When stressed, people might be excused for making unreasonable demands.
They might, for instance, extract promises that are difficult or impossible to keep. Who,
after all, wants to deny someone who is desperate and might have his or her life in
danger? Who wouldn’t prefer to lie and say, ‘don’t worry. Everything will be fine?’ But
a hastily made guess that ‘someone will be there to help in a couple of hours,’ can start
doing damage at 120 minutes and 1 second (Alomes 2009). The reason is because the
promise suddenly becomes questionable, and at his point meaning starts to erode
Whether it is the provision of food, water, shelter, blankets or medical services, most
helping people to help themselves. Its critical, but the obviousness of this area of
concern tends to dominate emergency provisioning. Needs – physical ones are the
squeaky wheel of emergency care. Information and material support enables people to
act. And when people act, and feel that they can do something, their overall sense of
coherence improves. This is good for health outcomes, and having people move from
stunned/disabled mode to being an active participant in the rescue effort means another
The absence if the things that make life manageable has obvious consequences,
although they are not as significant as we tend to assume. Lack of food, water and
191
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
shelter will be a source of stress that will make outcomes worse, but with meaning and
comprehensibility needs looked after, people can go a long time without basics. As
Frank Lloyd Wright famously said, “give me the luxuries of life, and I’ll gladly go
Meaning is the most difficult aspect of the salutogenic theory to understand and to
provide for, but it is nevertheless the most important. Meaning is the glue of life. It
is what makes lives whole and fulfilled. In psychotherapy, it is only when our life’s
narratives are revealed to be meaningful that there is release and resolution (Clarkson
2006). Meaning is the force that binds social groups together and is a major purpose of
religious belief (Durkheim, 1975; Obeyesekere, 1981). All aesthetics, literature, art, and
all other ubiquitous abstracts of human endeavour relate to and contribute to meaning.
Meaning gives people the power to withstand inhuman conditions, starvation, illness,
and extreme conditions (Frankl 1963). There are theories about why and how meaning
gives people such tenacity, but it is not the purpose of this paper to go into these debates
(For more information, see (Golembiewski, 2011a, in review-d) (see article on p.204).
192
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Figure 21: Meaning trumps concerns for hunger and comfort. Source:
Whilst meaning is associated with arts and the complexities of culture, there is no
suggestion that reading poems will be of any use to someone who has just lost their
family in a bushfire (on the other hand, it might happen to be just what’s needed!) In
the subjects. It means concern for life, for the people they care about, and sometimes for
significant cultural constructions like religion, history and tradition. Meaning is what
193
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
creates the desire to stay alive. And the desire to stay alive keeps people alive. Meaning
comes from anywhere and everywhere, but there are some sources that are especially
potent. Sometimes the emergency effort itself can contribute meaning; after all, being
needed is a big one. Emergencies are situations where the people who are needed, are
needed to the extreme. Thus involvement in the rescue operation is a good way to help
people through their grief and hardship. This is true to the extent that fire-fighter’s have
commonly been found to be arsonists, lighting fires, so they can fight them and feel
significant as humans. By fighting fires, the arsonists found camaraderie and intense
Under normal circumstances meaning is a stable constant for most people. It waxes and
associations and distinctions and narratives that are created throughout a person’s entire
life. As things are found to ‘make sense’ they contribute to this holistic body of
knowledge and order. And things that don’t make any sense and cannot be understood
by maintaining a person’s psyche can suddenly come under fire. The elusiveness of
undermines a salutogenic framework like nothing else can. This causes bizarre and
fact, there is every possibility that the atomisation of an ontological framework is the root
cause of every single case of mental illness – especially the psychoses (Golembiewski,
The capacity for the ontology to flip makes the protection of meaning the single most
Maslow (the hierarchy of needs model,) places meaning as a ‘higher’ need that can only
be considered once the stability of ‘lower’ needs such as shelter or food is established.
This truism is false. Rather it is the ontology the ‘highest need,’ that is the foundation
Without a strong ontology, a person loses the capacity to think, feel, or even act. But
people can go without basic needs or make do indefinitely if the struggle is meaningful.
There is meaning in place and even in the narrative of hanging in there against the odds.
Of course the maintenance of someone else’s body of wisdom and experience isn’t
possible because we still have no means to occupy somebody else’s mind. So how can
challenges we can encourage people to find direction when they most need it. Getting
survivors active and involved in the emergency effort when they look like they might
otherwise take a turn for the worse can be a good idea. We can be encouraging, helping
the survivors of disaster understand that all that is humanly possible will be done and
195
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
that the things will work out as well as can reasonably be expected. It is important,
however not to overplay this, as dashed promises or misinformation are often the
tipping point to despair (Alomes 2009). In catastrophes, the survivors must be allowed
to feel that they can trust the providers and care they are getting. Consider the anger
after Hurricane Katrina when misinformation about looting caused the mobilisation of
armed troops with orders to shoot. The McLeod Commission noted that the greatest
failure of the bushfire response was the misinformation that the response team
Frequently ontological shifts mean that old spiritual models will be dismissed. In these
circumstances missionaries for various faiths can do a lot of good, even as they prey on
the victims of disaster. Rigid belief systems offer support when it is most needed, like a
crutch, but have a tendency to fail people down the track (Antonovsky 1987). In
emergencies missionary activity can be a double edged sword of its own – often
missionaries are very experienced and generous caregivers who ask nothing in return,
Culture is a very important source of meaning and is a context for our life’s narratives.
196
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Figure 22: Nero fiddles while Rome burns. Quo Vadis. (1924). (Rights
expired)
When Nero famously played the fiddle while his city burned, was he mad or was he
reaching for the thing that gave him meaning and a sense of control at a time of extreme
and well-accepted theory and information may not apply. Catastrophes both change the
normal order of things and the speed with which decisions have to be made. So it is
197
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
essential that people who are to go out and face emergencies are equipped to make
fine tool for such circumstances, because it is easy to guess how little efforts might be
amplified once projected onto a simple salutogenic framework; how does an action
affect manageability, comprehensibility and the sense of meaning? Is the sum force on
198
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
This second question is largely answered by a single paper “The Riddle of Psychotic
This section should be read with the following however, because the aetiology cannot
be easily separated from the syndrome. Thus the first papers in the third section “Are
199
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The question about the relationships between schizophrenia and the environment are
and also the built environment. Both are complex in their own right, but lines of enquiry
are further complicated by the unknowable common mechanism: the human mind. In
the case of built form, the idea that buildings are form and space created by people out
of material stuff is very tempting but is only half the story – and probably not even half
because architecture is not only to protect people from bad experience (the weather,
referents differ, we cannot experience the same space in the same way. It is common to
discuss the quality of experience among friends, precisely because experience is not
universal and yet a social experience is one that is shared. “It’s beautiful, don’t you
think?”
200
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Even the experience of a single building draws on knowledge that has been gathered on
approach. Short of arriving in a coma, we cannot separate the experience of the interior
spaces from the experiences of the exterior as we draw near. If we arrive with the
sense of what it is and what it will be like when it is still a postage sized representation
on the back of the retina. If we arrive in the back of a paddy wagon, we will be
with the distinct impression that out safety is in immediate jeopardy, because we don’t
Perceptual experience is sequential and composite. Under normal conditions we will see
first, then we will usually see and hear by this stage we will have greater faculties of
sight, perhaps we will get a sense of depth in parallax and perspective. We might also
have a glimpse of some textural qualities. The first sounds will be deep or loud ones.
Later we will smell, then feel and perhaps at some later stage again taste. Although the
opportunities for sight will generally come much earlier than those for smell or taste, the
latter senses do not shut out the first. Perception becomes more complex as it becomes
Physically our perceptual abilities are very limited. Out of the potentially infinite array
of available information, we genuinely perceive only the tiniest sample of the stimulus
201
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
that we are exposed to. Let’s consider the sight, the primary modality for humans. When
we see, we do so over a very limited focal range, depending on how close an object is,
the range might be as short as a couple of centimetres (reference needed). We also only
truly see whatever is in an arc of about 1.5° (E. T. Hall, 1990). This limited information
is restricted to the capacity of the human eye, the fovea allows us to see finest details
but even then we can’t see objects or textures smaller than about 0.1 mm3 without
assistance. The cone sampling rate drops precipitously with increasing retinal
eccentricity (D. Williams, 1986). Furthermore, with increased eccentricity, the lattice
becomes more irregular (D. Williams, 1986). By the periphery, the eye sees very little –
some colour information and a sense of ‘presence’ generally – but not enough detail to
distinguish shapes. Thus the images we receive from the periphery are highly abstract,
and whatever is in the foveal area is specific. Effectively we don’t get to see a lot with
any certainty, but memory and ontology fills the gaps maintaining the sense of
consistency. This reduces the perceptual demands on the brain. We maintain a notional
‘visual image’ of what is behind our heads also. This is maintained by auditory haptic
and olfactory supplementary information. But our other senses aren’t much better. We
hear unevenly – with a better horizontal range than vertical. The human capacity for
hearing tone seldom reaches its limits of 20hz – 17khz. Even then, we can only focus on
one sound at a time. Humans cannot hear volumes less than 1dB and have difficulty
detecting sounds less than 10dB, and even then, those sounds need to be in the middle
202
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Ultimately, the information we gather from the environment has little new information
fits within a normal range of expectations given similar criteria in the past, these
sensory inputs are constructed from meaning, instinct and memory. The mechanism is
identical to the one used to draw meaning from writing and language. A symbolic
representation draws on the ontology to find and express meaning. The sensory inputs
only relate to the physical as a point of reference, like a written word triggers the
meaning response, but doesn’t embody it. (Meaning is only ever an approximation
(Popper, 1970)) Thus architecture, like all else, as it is experienced, exists primarily in
the mind. Occasionally architectural expressions use our relative inability to perceive;
the architecture of adventure rides in theme parks for example. When sight is
environment can shift and change in order trigger false a-priori decisions about the
The sense of concreteness and reality that is associated with architecture obviously has
some reality beyond experience – but my concern is not for what is, but how it is
constructed from our ontological models of physicality, which is confirmed using the
perceptual systems: Constructs of memory, meaning and desire. These intangibles are
203
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The following paper presents a hypothesis for schizophrenia that draws together many
Abstract
After generations of enquiry, and currently around 6150 papers being published
This in-depth review of all the prominent hypotheses of schizophrenia reveals that none
can account for the heterogeneous presentations of the syndrome, especially one that
these hypotheses (including competing ones like the glutamate vs. dopamine hypothesis
rivalry), the reams of data appear to congeal into a remarkably coherent meta-
hypothesis. For the first time since the DSM-IV diagnostic criteria was published, a
204
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
comprehensive and holistic explanation of all the symptoms and signs (as well as other
The role that the striatal D1/D2 heteromers have in modulating saliency and mediating
both bottom-up and top-down perception is expanded, bearing in mind that the
switching of the striatal D1/D2 heteromers from a low affinity state (D2Low), to high
(D2High) is already putatively the root of schizophrenia. Here links are found that
associate top-down attention with presynaptic tonic dopamine synthesis and the function
of D2High. Bottom-up attention, on the other hand, is given to powerful phasic synthesis
All attentional modes are essential to healthy perception and the functions that depend
eight very closely related syndromes can be identified, each with a distinct presentation
and psychogenesis. These converge roughly into the main schizophrenia subtypes.
The meta-hypothesis that emerges from this review is highly triangulated and supported
205
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
of conditions, but when psychotic symptoms are particularly bizarre and enduring, they
have never been resolved and debate abounds from disciplines as diverse as linguistics
and genetics about what mechanisms may be involved. A Medline review reveals 6146
articles since last year alone23. All heuristically use normative hypotheses, but these are
fragmented and their findings ad-hoc: they study one facet of the disorder or another
and produce new hypotheses to explain their findings – the few comprehensive
hypotheses are notable exceptions. These are discussed below but their explanations
invariably fall far short of plausibility considering quite how bizarre, debilitating and
heterogeneous schizophrenia is. This paper traverses several prominent hypotheses and
all the diagnostic criteria listed in the DSM-IV to a single molecular dysfunction.
206
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
perception as an input and behaviour as an output. Psychotic symptoms are thus output
errors, caused by the dysfunction either within perception, working memory, or any
number of other areas of cognition, depending on the model. One class of hypotheses
with an enduring legacy is that symptoms are caused by an inability to filter out
irrelevant observations (Frith, 1979; Gray et al., 1991; Kapur, 2003). More recently, one
known active agent in psychosis, to the front end of the perceptual process, that is
perceptual saliency (Heinz & Schlagenhauf, 2010; Howes & Kapur, 2009; Kapur, 2003;
Kapur, Mizrahi, & Li, 2005; Schwartz, Wiggins, Naudin, & Spitzer, 2005). In the
Kapur posits that the importance or saliency of the event is tagged by the stimulation of
the D2 receptors within the striatal dopamine neurons. The tagging process ensures that
selective attention is directed to the percept that has been tagged as important.
Salient events (those that have been tagged as important) should reflect normal
harness attention independently of cue and context (Kapur et al., 2005). As an example,
Kapur refers to Bowers & Freeman (1966), who recall a patient describing his
symptoms thus:
207
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
similar in many details: Fletcher & Frith’s Bayesian attenuation failure hypothesis
(2009). In this model, normality is a state where familiar events go unnoticed – in other
words they are attenuated. That is, people get so used to normality that they fail to no-
tice it. However, when there is an attenuation failure, normal experiences appear uncan-
ny, and essential cognitive processes are aborted and left unresolved. Essentially Kapur,
While the prima facie evidence appears to bind dopamine, saliency and psychosis into a
tight relationship, these hypotheses are bereft of details and need exploration. This is
especially important because the hypotheses are used to advocate specific treatments
and account for serious conditions such as schizophrenia. But before we can discuss the
healthy system is needed, and for the purposes of this article, four research questions
Firstly, we need to ask exactly what saliency is. How much do we know about the
208
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
information? Only once these questions have been covered, can we ask questions that
Secondly, if aberrant saliency is the basis for florid psychotic symptoms (hallucinations,
delusions, etc.) as both Fletcher & Frith (2009) and Howes and Kapur (2009); Kapur
(2003); Kapur et al. (2005) assert, then what are the deficit symptoms – the distinct lack
of responsiveness that was once thought to be at the core of the schizophrenic diagnosis
Thirdly, must aberrant salience (the mis-tagging of important events) equate to over-
stimulation – or even dopamine oversupply? Certainly, this is what Fletcher & Frith and
Kapur both propose (Fletcher & Frith, 2009; Kapur, 2003; Kapur et al., 2005), yet about
That is, solely to stimulus-led perception, or does it also affect intention-led perception?
In other words, does it only affect attention that is stimulated by bottom-up, unsought
stimuli – or are there other events that may also trigger saliency responses - even when
The answers to these questions will help us not only to better understand schizophrenia,
209
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Healthy salience
irrelevant information (Milstein, Dalley, & Robbins, 2005). In this general framework,
saliency is the measure of the influence that a singleton (a technical term for a single
potential percept, prior to it actually being singled out and declaratively perceived) has
in gaining attention. At most, a single percept can dominate thought. It can, quite
literally, ‘take possession of the mind’ (James, 1890, p. 404). At a minimum, the percept
may remain latent; that is, beyond the threshold of declarative consciousness
(Duckworth, Bargh, Garcia, & Chaiken, 2002; Hassin, Bargh, Engell, & McCulloch,
2009; Hassin, Bargh, & Zimerman, 2009). In between these two points, a singleton
might make a partial incursion on working memory, occupying one or more ‘places’ of
the model for working memory that you find most compelling. Most models allow four
to seven items in active awareness (Cowan, 2005) and others extend working memory
to a further ‘behind the scenes’ implicit level of awareness (Baars & Franklin, 2003;
Saliency refers to those properties of a singleton that attract selective attention – that is
the directed awareness of the perceiver. But saliency is not a unitary concept. In vivo
210
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
studies of monkeys reveal that at least three factors will capture attention
(Golembiewski, 2013) (Article reprinted from p.126). Because all three are referred to
tagged as important:
Prominence
High contrasts of the singleton against the setting (noisy, sudden sounds, flashing lights
and bright colours etc.) are highly prominent and demand reaction (Ljungberg et al.,
1992). As a subject learns about the presence of a prominent object, the initial burst of
saliency immediately begins to wane - eventually to the point of extinction (Berns et al.,
(Carlsson, 1995; Corringer, Changeux, Bronner, Edelstein, & Smit, 2008) these then
The kind of arousal that prominence evokes is highly automated, instinctive and is
central to orientation reflexes (Ward, 2008). This form of salience must be non-
(Laureys, 2005; Schiff et al., 2002) it will also activate both excitatory and inhibitory
synapses of layer 2/3 of pyramidal neurons even in vitro – the result being a rhythmic
211
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Significance
This form of salience is highly cognitive. It is the potential importance of the concept
(schemata) that is being recalled. There are three things that increase the significance of
words, we react to perceptions that are significant concepts, not only prominence.
Worth
Salience has a pre-cognitive affective or hedonic bias (Barrett & Bar, 2009). Monkeys
experience generates a much greater impact than the threat of a negative one
(Mirenowicz & Schultz, 1996; Schultz, 1998). Worth should further be broken down
into affective and hedonic subcategories. Affective worth is how a singleton makes you
Worth appears to be mediated by the amygdala, and its function appears to remain
either intact or slightly under engaged in schizophrenia (Anticevic et al., 2012; Becerril
& Barch, 2010). This contrasts with affective psychoses like bipolar disorder, in which a
large body of literature reports over-engagement of the amygdalae (S. B. Perlman et al.,
2012).
212
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
A fourth saliency factor should also be added to the list. This factor was not identified
using the same series of animal experiments, possibly because it is peculiar to healthy
humans.
Opportunity
This is response to stimuli just for the sake of it, sometimes just because we are active
beings and need to do something. This is engagement – just for sensory variation, for
stimulation, but not necessarily for affective or hedonic gratification. It seems that no
other animals have a go at things without good reason or choose to avoid a favourite
food, just to try something different. Such behaviour is very compelling for humans, and
is therefore undeniably salient on some highly intellectual level, yet it doesn’t fit into
the categories mentioned above. Nor does it behave the same way: opportunity is
engagement with experience; it is not led by saliency cues and is not normally coercive
salience of choice. The choice to try something off-putting, for no reason other than the
The psychological canon allows for two modes of selective attention (see Table 2.)
There is top-down attention, which is the deliberate focus of declarative resources; and
213
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
bottom-up attention, which refers to the attention that is brought to a subject by the
engagements and focus on the rude intruder. What is it? Does it have relevance? How
does it fit into what I know (the ontology)? If the transfer of focused attention to a new
stimulus is automatic and unavoidable, then intent plays no part and is thus called
bottom-up (Theeuwes, Atchley, & Kramer, 2000). This use of the term ‘bottom-up’ is
thus a bit ambiguous, because it refers to the top-down attention that is given to bottom-
up stimuli. To resolve this ambiguity the terms listed in column 2 of Table 2 are used.
and excitatory
oscillations) and if
strong enough, NDMA
triggers the D1
receptors
Bottom- Identification and experience of novelty, Prominence: mismatch
up aberrance, data mismatches. detection (when D1 has
been already activated.)
Worth (negative) – not
dopaminergic
•General engagement, occupation of interest, Opportunity
Top-down/ feed-forward
214
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
canonical terms: bottom-up and top-down both miss the basis of most
Raw attention
If there is such a thing, in its purest form, bottom-up attention must be a non-cognitive
could be salient, because it would completely lack any kind of definition or ontological
prominence, even during vegetative states, anaesthesia, deep sleep, and in vitro
(Feldmeyer, 2010; Laureys, 2005; Schiff et al., 2002) implying that this raw data feed
Bottom-up attention
The cognitive attention given to anything that is not driven by intention, such as
Singletons that are picked up by bottom-up attention are those that stand out as different
from expectations (for example, you crack an egg and it smells bad – you weren’t
expecting it, but you notice). In bottom-up attention, saliency is primarily a response to
215
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
the unexpected, the undefined, misplaced and unanticipated, in other words mismatches
between singleton-data and related schemata; Functionally this means that bottom-up
attention is a response to difference and novelty, and this is how the term is normally
noticed when they don’t fit expectations, and are otherwise ignored (Clark, 2012 - in
press; Friston, 2003; Stephan, Penny, Daunizeau, Moran, & Friston, 2009). In another
body of literature, another feedback mechanism called error related negativity (ERN) is
also proposed. This is a feedback loop specifically for negative affective or negative
hedonic feedback – ERN occurs when an event is worse than expectations (Falkenstein,
Hohnsbein, Hoormann, & Blanke, 1990; Holroyd & Coles, 2002). ERN is Bayesian too
– but it isn’t driven by bottom-up attention because the negative event happens within
the general range of expectations – ERN therefore fits in another category: active
red poppy in a green field or a tiger on your lawn, the former is inconsequential, the
latter critical.
Bottom-up attention isn’t only given to external stimuli; it’s give to any unanticipated
stimuli. Bodily (somatogenic) signals like hunger, tiredness, pain etc. would normally
stimulate endogenous bottom-up stimuli because they are unsolicited: we do not usually
ask ‘am I hungry, tired or in pain?’ because we will feel hungry or tired spontaneously.
216
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Initially these signals are received using raw attention, but are modulated with bottom-
up attention.
therefore while somatic and other bottom-up experience is normally bottom-up, it may
Top-down attention
Top-down attention is the mode of attention where percepts are anticipated. For
example, if I am hunting for truffles I ignore stones, but when I see something that
response. The top-down mode of attention has an exclusive domain of interest: the
known. It is only concerned with recognizing significance saliency and only the
attention, top-down attention is not Bayesian, because of its tight focus. Because top-
down attention assesses only matches against intentions, top-down attention yields only
positive feedback (you don’t have an interest in things that you weren’t looking for –
these can only be noticed by other modes of attention.) This sets up a confirmation bias
(Nickerson, 1998). Things that are actively sought trigger top-down recognition
reactions easily, yet negative outcomes are referred to the bottom-up system, and will
some way.
217
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
There are two levels of top-down attention; focal and incidental. If we want to do
In such cases saliency is only used for positive feedback. On the other hand, the
attention we give simple and well learned tasks is incidental. Here top-down attention
Active attention
distraction. But such encroachments are not regular (and may even be rare, depending
on one’s lifestyle) because they depend on bottom-up stimuli, which may or may not be
present. Despite this, we are not totally driven by intent (as other animals might be). In
real terms, for humans, there is a large and undefined grey-area that separates the top-
detecting prominence, and top-down attention covers only significance, then what of the
rest? What of ERN? When things are noticed when they are worse than expectations but
are generally within the range of unconsidered probability – such as when someone
reaches into a fire and gets burned? Another whole range of experience is awareness on
the edge of latent automaticity, which is nevertheless still noticed. When we do any
routines we know well – such as drive, we do so automatically for the best part, yet we
218
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
There are many opportunities that we choose to engage in, just because they are there.
Likewise, events are noticed but neither because we are looking for them, nor because
they are prominent. Active attention is a term to describe the middle ground between
bottom-up and top-down. It may be environment led, yet without the sharpness of
novelty. Active attention is where top-down intention directs a ‘fuzzy’ search or where
bottom-up attention reveals something that you had an interest in, but had not been
looking for. Active attention is not driven by saliency, and isn’t a mode of attention in
its own right, but complex combinations of bottom-up and top-down processes, which
are led by habits of engagement, by choice and simply by the availability of resources.
And as such, active attention doesn’t sit comfortably with the assumed dynamics of the
Attention is an expansive resource that doesn’t retreat and become passive when it’s not
engaged in any clear intent or with salient environmental stimuli. If anything, the
opposite: Active attention is the awareness that accompanies the continual activity of
the mind and body, on the lookout for something to do, to experience and to think
about. Indeed, studies have demonstrated that when environments are depleted of
natural stimuli and opportunities for active attention, even healthy people start to
hallucinate them (Grassian, 1983; Weckowicz, 1957) or fabricate them using whatever
The behaviour that takes place around active perception is largely automatic and
involves stereotypical actions that are associated with whatever an object suggests – or
219
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
affords the perceiver, thus objects of engagement are called affordances (Gibson, 1979;
Withagen et al., 2012). An example is when you find yourself eating a cookie that you
found when doing something altogether different. It wasn’t an intentional act, but
action and in evolutionary terms, this is the primary role of perception. Simple creatures
must act according to opportunities offered to them, but they don’t have the cognitive
‘From birth, babies will actively engage with the perceptual environment. They will
imitate expressions (Meltzoff & Moore, 1977), grasp a graspable object, and with age
they will want to touch textures, run fingers down Brancusi’s sculptures, absorb
themselves in colours and scents, feel the curvature and warmth of surfaces, immerse
themselves in tastes, experiment with sounds, etc. They will want to squeeze bubble-
wrap and juice cartons, just to hear them pop. They light sparklers to see the sparks fly
off in all directions; they burn incense to scent the air, and climb snowy mountains, just
to feel the rush of zooming down again.’ (Golembiewski, 2013) (p. 126 q.v.)
220
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Worth
Top-down Working
Intention attention memory
✓
Feedback
! Prominence Active
awareness
★ Significance Working
memory
BU
☺ Opportunity TD
Attention
Worth + –
221
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Perception is the most tangible link between the shared reality and personal experience.
A lot depends on it and this brief section cannot even address all the facets of the psyche
that are affected by attentional dysfunction, much less do them justice. Even so, it’s
sense-of-self and learning are needed because the most characteristic symptoms of
The ontology
(schemata)
Sense-
Automaticity Learning
of-self
Perception
Hebbian antiHebbian
(‘dumb’) (considdered)
learning learning
Raw
perception
using a ‘dumb’ but efficient Hebbian processes ‘cells that fire together
wire together’ (Shatz, 1996). New knowledge isn’t learned this way,
Perception
Even with robust attentional salience, perception is never perfect. Raw perceptual data
is filtered and interpreted by our gross perceptual mechanisms. The sensory world is
experienced through the available senses, so it is neither directly nor fully experienced.
790 tHz) of vibrations, 1-400 tHz is experienced as warmth (Gibson, 1979; Matlin,
1988), and the 17Hz-17kHz range as sound and pressure (Moore, 1997), but bats hear
frequencies up to 115kHz (Heffner, 1983) and guppy fish can see light down to 300tHz
(E. J. Smith et al., 2002). To sense these and other frequencies, humans have to use
frequencies into our native sensitivity range of visible light and audible sound.
The next task ‘of the brain… can seem impossible: it must discover information about
the likely causes of impinging signals without any form of direct access to their source’
(Clark, 2012 - in press, p. 6). Sensations must be iteratively interpreted and experienced
through cortical processes. Information from the sensory organs travels to a series of
223
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The later cortices (such as V4 and V5) are globally integrated to enable the processing
of unknown, unpredictable and abstract stimuli. All the senses have lower-order
processing areas and share the higher order ones – although even primary inputs are
(including the body), but these stimuli can only be known and used in translation; that is
through the top-down process of learning: decoding and recoding, creating and
remodelling schemata. The heavy reliance on top-down processes doesn’t mean that
what we experience isn’t real – but it does mean that experience is highly mediated and
a partially inferred best guess (Sanders, 2004). The implications of this are broad:
1. The fact that all perception is in some way translated into electrical and chemical
‘hallucination’ is the departure from common and grounded sense of reality, and
dysfunctional.
224
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
‘Ideas that may appear to be delusional in one culture (e.g., sorcery and
religious experience (e.g., seeing the Virgin Mary or hearing God's voice). In
linguistic variation in narrative styles across cultures that affects the logical
4. The imperfect match between perception and experience means that all people
have the capacity to be fooled by what they believe they perceive. This leads to
the illusion-hallucination continuum (eg, Brébion et al., 2000; Frith & Done,
1988; Harvey, 1987; Johns et al., 2001). While direct studies of this
therefore a possible role in psychosis, the effect size is still too small to explain
225
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
5. The higher the reliance on top-down and active attentional processes are (that is,
the higher the top-down vs. bottom-up sampling rate is), the more likely that
perception will be distorted from reality, because sampling loses its fact-
attentional system will cause hallucinations. These models don’t acknowledge the
possibility of direct perception. Intellectualist models only outline the higher order
Current intellectualist models argue along the following lines: The brain is modelled as
an organ that assigns meaning to raw perception. On one hand, raw perception is
schemata within ontology. Clark (2012 - in press) and Fletcher and Frith (2009)
efficiency. In their models, singletons are only noticed when they are aberrant, at which
point learning must take place. Grossberg (2009) is interested in the cytoarchitecture of
perception. His model is Hebbian and distinctly not Bayesian, but is no less
226
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
intellectualist. He proposes that the very laminar structure of the neocortical neurons
match raw data to schemata by matching predictive representations of the world with
raw bottom-up stimuli. The model Grossberg proposes is that neither top-down
intentions nor raw bottom-up stimuli will trigger a neuronal excitatory reaction
two inputs: a tonic top-down intention and the other, a raw bottom-up singleton come
together, that a saliency signal is transmitted horizontally (from one neuron column to
another) through dendrites and vertically (to other brain regions) through axonal
connections. In this model, intent quite literally works in a top-down fashion, because it
stimulates the first layer of the neocortex. This tonic stimulation primes perception to
receive appropriate input. Bottom-up perception works the opposite way around, from
the sixth layer upwards. This tonically primes the neuron too, but the excitatory
potential of the neuron is only achieved when both top-down and bottom-up stimulation
occurs.
On their own, intellectualist approaches are limited. A distinction is not made between
knowing and experience. Intellectualist approaches must face a very valid criticism; ‘I
think is not I am, unless by thought I can equal the world’s concrete richness.’ (Merleau
Ponty, 1943), This is not a critique of the reductive efficiency of Descartes, Bayes,
Hemholtz or their inheritors (like Clark, Fletcher, Frith, Friston or Grossberg), but a
227
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
ceases, even in conditions such as a vegetative coma (Laureys, 2005; Schiff et al., 2002;
Zeman, 2003), when sleep-walking (Plazzi, Vetrugno, Provini, & Montagna, 2005) or
even in vitro (Feldmeyer, 2010). This means that raw perceptual processes are
integrates the two. In this model, raw awareness provides a non-cognitive backdrop to
perception, raw sense data is not disposed of, just because it’s understood, nor is it
disposed of merely because it is not. On the contrary, these conditions are when we may
really start to enjoy experience. When we look at a painting and discover the subject,
the richness in how the figures are presented doesn’t evaporate. The active-attention
model accepts the intellectualist models of perception, but also the concept of direct
perception, thereby lowering the threshold for experience. The active attention model
engaged with and acted on iteratively. At the most fundamental (pre-cognitive) level,
action is automated, not necessarily by attention, but by habits that have formed direct
associations with perceptual activations and actions: given familiar singletons, it isn’t
necessary to recognize them before actions commence. We do not see a flat, hard
surface and think; ‘hey – I can walk on that.’ We just walk (Bargh & Dijksterhuis,
2001; Gibson, 1979). The factors of salience will determine how much attention a
228
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
experience) is a factor. But other factors (significance, worth and opportunity – see p.
212-213) also capture attention, and all are important. The mechanics of opportunity are
action, but our awareness of such engagement may be implicit (Hassin, Bargh, Engell,
et al., 2009).
The associations that intellectualist models emphasize are plastic and changeable and
thus are intrinsically vulnerable. Grossberg recognizes this and insists that a resonance
between the raw data of reality and intentions is essential to create stability of meaning,
to prevent it from being re-written with every new perception (Grossberg, 2003b).
Meaning cannot survive (long) when sensory cortical inter connectivity is lost, even if
causes the singletons of perception to vanish, leaving behind nothing but a full, but
dementia in the higher visual cortices cannot even distinguish between the categories of
visual information – between colours, lines and shapes, much less pick out contents of a
view, even though the raw visual information is still omnipresent (Sacks, 2010).
The sense-of-self
perception, but this viewpoint is difficult to defend. All the modes of attention serve a
229
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
single perspective, and this is experienced as the locus of the sense-of-self; I am the
centre of my experience. There are many possible reasons for this, including (at a
minimum):
1) Because all the organs of perception report to a single brain (although nominally to
3) Because the actions perception triggers all occur within a single physiology (Braund,
in preparation);
4) Because all of these functions unite a single sense of identity and ego (Shoemaker,
1968).
230
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The holism these interconnected and interdependent systems generates normally taken
for granted, but it is vulnerable. The person (as philosophically conceived here) is an
ego-centric composite of a body, perceptual system and ontology, and for the best part,
the sense-of-self doesn’t atomize when these constructs do – blindness, injury and
ignorance are all undoubtedly risk-factors, but for the best part, the phenomenological
the visual first person perspective was challenged, in this study the participants’ eyes
were masked and all sight was channelled through monitor glasses with a wireless
closed circuit video feed. The camera was put at a fixed angle in the corner of the room,
creating the illusion that ‘my body is there – not here.’ Even in these circumstances, the
sense-of-self was generally maintained; when participants wrote, the writing faced the
camera, not the body, but the participants had a choice – they could also choose to
engage their proprioception rather than visual modality, meaning that their writing faced
their bodies (Mizumoto & Ishikawa, 2005). This suggests that the self-perspective is
maintained by all the senses and can survive major disruptions. What is more vulnerable
is the ability to reflect on the narratives and actions that we associate with the constructs
because I am not y’ (Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959). They are sometimes affirmative: ‘I
am x because my actions are x-like’ (Bem, 1967). Both arguments rely on perception for
the basic information to know what x and y are, and on narratives to contextualize x and
231
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
I contend that both positions are valid, certainly in terms of contemporary neuroscience,
because the more deliberate actions are, the more resistance: neural excitation and
inhibition occur together, except during highly automated tasks, which are far more
parsimonious, and don’t necessarily engage attention at all (Bargh & Dijksterhuis,
2001). Sometimes excitation and inhibition are in equal measure, in which case the
balance suggests that no actions take place, but the greater the excess of excited
neurons, the more spontaneous action occurs (Golembiewski, 2012a) (see p.358 q.v.) It
maintain a sense-of-self.
Learning
The human search for comprehensibility and meaning appears to be endless. Every
unexpected singleton is met with top-down questions about why, who, what and when –
even when dreaming we are constantly trying to figure things out. This process involves
linking singletons to the lexical and motor schemata that are most challenged by the
new singleton (robin fits with birds, superhero side-kicks; with rhymes bobbin and
coffin; with names and other people called Robin like Robin Hood, with similar names
such as Robert and so on.) This is a logical process that has been adopted to simulate
intelligence in computing and has been adapted for very diverse models of learning
(Baars & Franklin, 2003; Clark, 2012 - in press; Grossberg, Carpenter, & Ersoy, 2005;
232
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
McClelland, & UCSD PDP Research Group, 1986). If concepts defy knowing, they
may still be placed into abstract schemata. Concepts like God, love and infinity are
unknowable, but nevertheless have many associations across the global neural network.
Abstract schemata are sometimes less grandiose but no less vague. If a singleton doesn’t
Regardless of whether their nature is specific or abstract, concepts and their associations
become schemata and are useful for defining other singletons (Johnson Abercrombie,
associations and creates finer and finer distinctions within the ontology. But there is also
‘dumb’ learning, which follows Shatz’ (1996) aphorism to describe Hebb’s rule of
synaptic plasticity; ‘cells that fire together, wire together.’ Where real learning creates
associations, Hebbian learning reinforces them through long term potentation (ie,
usage). The mechanisms are highly complex, but recent reviews link automatic
functions to the strength of associations in the striatal association area (Ashby, Turner,
& Horvitz, 2010). Under normal circumstances, Hebbian and antiHebbian systems work
together. Whereas the anti-Hebbian system is primarilly used for initial learning (as
described by Clark and others), the associations that are generated are strengthened by
Hebbian processes, the oppositeis also true; disuse leads to synaptic loss and sometimes
dementia.
233
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Schemata that have been committed to memory for long periods are known to lump
together with similar concepts, if they can be recalled at all. This is demonstrated when
people are given several stories and asked to relate them after only a short period and
again after a long one. The phenomenon does not occur if subjects consider the stories
regularly; instead, the stories develop details and a new character of their own (Johnson
Abercrombie, 1960). This can be taken as preliminary evidence that the longer elements
of the ontology are left without focal attention, the more tacit they become. The more
focus they receive, on the other hand, the more explicit (Gulick, 2006). Tacit knowledge
is difficult to express because meanings are personal and hermetic, and the
example, someone who has forgotten their primary school science may say ‘light works
because of electricity.’ The person is not wrong, but the information lacks specificity
and isn’t very useful. Without regularly scanning knowledge for errors using a system
similar to the one described by Clark or Friston (Clark, 2012 - in press; Friston, 2003),
Tacit awareness of well known schemata and routines appears to be an ideal level of
Automation
Attention isn’t needed to trigger automatic actions such as well-learned motor behav-
iours. No deliberate attention needs to be given to walking, unless you are still a baby,
234
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
and the skill is new. Having said that, intention (a distinct top-down form of attention)
can both trigger and inhibit automations. Automation is mentioned here because atten-
can cause ‘choking,’ the unintentional inhibition of that activity (Baumeister &
actions and cognitive processes while the singleton is attended to. It will shortly be
demonstrated that there are two pathways that moderate attention: one is directly
inhibitory and the other excitatory – yet, because of their functional routes, they both
levels associated with the troughs in tonic dopamine activity and associated with active
attention (see Figure 27), is likely to be the ideal level of awareness to enable automatic
tasks. This latent level of awareness allows automatic actions to be so efficient (Ashby
et al., 2010). Greater awareness, particularly bottom-up awareness (the phasic bursts in
Figure 27), as it invades working memory inhibits (that is, interrupts or even
terminates) actions whether they are automatic or not, by exciting the possibilities that
the action may precipitate and a range of alternatives (Dijksterhuis, Aarts, Bargh, & van
Knippenberg, 2000). This is presumably a function of the ACC and frontal cortex
through the activation of NMDA/D1 excitatory afferents (more about that shortly). But
even too much top-down attention can do the same (the peaks of tonic action in Figure
27), not because the ACC and frontal cortex are excited but the opposite: because the
235
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
lifting awareness out of the ideal latent state. The classic example is with professional
sports players – when they ‘think too much’ they ‘choke’ – the automatic fluidity of
2010), but for the best part, automatic actions are excited directly by affordances. This
sometimes even occurs in states of very low arousal as mentioned above; for instance
sleep-walkers will engage in automatic behaviours that relate to real and hallucinated
objects and environments: they have been known to drive, cook and occasionally act in
violent ways, especially if disturbed (Plazzi et al., 2005). When imaged using single
self awareness in this state (Bassetti, Vella, Donati, Wielepp, & Weder, 2000).
The typical sleepwalking patterns suggest a relevant issue for this argument; automatic
effectively bypassing all cognitive architecture (including the frontal cortex and ACC).
The implication is that intent is implicit within knowledge and that once knowledge is
knowledge that an apple is edible prompts an intention to eat it (Bargh & Dijksterhuis,
2001). Any attention given to automatic actions has an inhibitory effect, but this is
236
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
processed in parallel. For healthy people, inhibition balances latent ‘intentions’, thereby
The same cannot be said for schizophrenic or bipolar (I) patients, who don’t sufficiently
(Golembiewski, 2012a) (reprinted here from p.358). For this reason, the inhibitory
The attention that healthy people give to their environmental context, automatically
unless they have not learned the behavioural associations associated with the milieu
(babies for instance). This effect is known in environmental psychology as the influence
of the behaviour setting (Barker & Wright, 1954; Golembiewski, 2013). It is this latent
present. A person will automatically assess the context, and will act only within the
24
. The inhibitory effect of the behavior setting should not be confused with latent inhi-
237
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The neural processes involved in attention have been of interest for some time and
scientists have identified a range of systems and subsystems that may be involved.
Within this general field, the very lateralization into two dissimilar hemispheres has
been identified (Toga & Thompson, 2003). Four neurotransmission systems are also of
is also a lot of research into specialized neural architecture, including larger scale region
analysis and the study of neurons and their components. All of these specialist interests
are growing fields, except perhaps lateralization, which, for all its promise, no longer
attracts the interest it did twenty five years ago (McGilchrist, 2010). In this chorus of
opinions, the striatal dopamine system is consistently singled out as the force that shifts
attention from one thing to another (Coull, 2005). Striatal dopamine is also linked to
salience perception (like in the above hypotheses (Fletcher & Frith, 2009; Howes &
Kapur, 2009)), to bottom-up attention (Hickey, Chelazzi, & Theeuwes, 2010; Holroyd
& Coles, 2002) and to reward salience – the way that attention is drawn to rewarding
stimuli (Berridge & Robinson, 1998, 2003; Bromberg-Martin & Hikosaka, 2009;
Mirenowicz & Schultz, 1994). The striatal dopamine pathways have also been
empirically linked to attention system more broadly and this will be discussed in further
depth shortly.
238
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
inhibiting activity that is not task specific (especially somatosensory inputs) and by the
moderation of baseline levels of arousal (Coull, 2005), the cholinergic system, on the
other hand, appears to sustain interest beyond the initial turning of selective attention
(Aston-Jones, Rajkowski, & Cohen, 1999; Coull, 2005) It also serves a very important
along with glutamate (Carlsson, 1995). While all these processes are highly relevant to
the topic of attention, only dopamine, acetylcholine and glutamate appear to be active in
the early phases of perception. Because our interest here is particularly in the earliest
phase of attention - selective attention, the other transmission systems will not be
A viable functional model is that salience is not unitary, and that most somatosensory
inputs are moderated by glutamate, acetylcholine or both and that these moderate
not by receptors, but by association organs – the left and right amygdalae.
trigger the N-methyl D Aspartate (NMDA) receptors (Kleckner & Dingledine, 1988). At
this point the dopaminergic (cognitive) attention system becomes engaged in bottom-up
attention. People are profoundly unaware of glutamatergic activity until the NMDA
239
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
receptors are activated, at which point the ‘winner takes all’ and working memory is at
least temporarily dominated by this new stimuli (Ward, 2008). The activation of the
NMDA receptors requires both a surge of voltage and particular connections, meaning
the receptor gates any activity that continues below the receptor ionization threshold
(Kleckner & Dingledine, 1988). Until this point, glutamatergic activity will continue
receptors can even be used to induce non-associative anaesthesia (Marek et al., 2010).
mosaics (Cepeda, André, Jocoy, & Levine, 2010; Fuxe, Marcellino, Guidolin, Woods,
& Agnati, 2010; Gerfen, 1992; Gerfen, Baimbridge, & Thibault, 1987; Shen &
simplified terms because mosaic patterns are often inconsistent and their distribution
patterns are heterogeneous (Gerfen, 1992). But within the mosaics, there are specific
receptors are collocated (whether they are on separate neurons or the same doesn’t
include NMDA/D1 and D1/D2. These are of further interest because some have been
found to be up- or downregulated in a broad range of mental illnesses (Pei et al., 2010).
As we shall see, the D1/D2 heteromers are almost certainly involved in the moderation
240
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
There are a number of dopamine receptor types and these fall into two families: the D1
(excitatory) and D2 (inhibitory). But the ones that have been singled out as being
responsible for saliency tagging are the striatal D2 receptors, although the literature
rarely differentiates one type of saliency against another. The reasons for identifying the
striatal D2 receptors, according to Ginovart and Kapur (2010), are because psychotic
patients read far more significance into the environment than is evident to the greater
majority, and it is primarily these D2 receptors that are blocked by all anti-psychotic
Dargham, 1999).
Seeman and others have also identified a particular formation of heteromers that render
the D2 receptors far more reactive. These are the D2High receptors. These are thought to
be responsible for psychosis for two reasons: not only are there more D2High receptors in
animal models of psychosis and in vitro studies of psychosis, but these receptors are
very easily stimulated and require very little dopamine to fire (Ginovart & Kapur, 2010;
Samaha, Seeman, Stewart, Rajabi, & Kapur, 2007; Seeman, 2010; Seeman & Kapur,
2000; Seeman et al., 2006). The other formation of the same receptors is D2Low. This
The conversion between D2High and D2Low (and vice versa) is caused by the presence of
other neurons especially NMDA gated glutamatergic ones which are sensitive to the
same stimulants (Cepeda & Levine, 2000; Novak, Seeman, & Foll, 2010; Seeman,
2010; Seeman, Guan, & Hirbec, 2009). If the D1 receptor is stimulated before the
coupled D2, then the D2 receptor is in a D2Low state. In other words it is far less
susceptible to stimulation. If the D1 receptor does not fire before the D2, then the state is
D2High at the time it fires. It would appear that in psychosis, the D1 receptor is not
There are a number of tell-tale signs that the D1 receptors are not properly activated in
limited, but it has been known for some time that the axons of the human prefrontal
(PFC) and anterior cingulate cortices (ACC) are particularly abundant in D1 receptors.
These are 4-7 times more abundant in this region than any other receptor types and in
humans more so than in any other animals (Goldman-Rakic, Castner, Svensson, Siever,
& Williams, 2004; Nichols, 2010; Okubo et al., 1997). But these neurons are
1999; Okubo et al., 1997). The anterior dopamine pathway (the ACC and frontal cortex)
and other types of psychosis (MacDonald et al., 2005). These findings consistently
et al., 2004; Javitt, 2009; Okubo et al., 1997). Another signature of under-activation of
has not been adequately measured in schizophrenic patients and controls, but in animal
models of psychosis they are found to increase by 250% for amphetamine induced
psychosis 180% for phencyclidine induced states and up to 900% for certain gene
But where do concepts of attention fit into this neurological model? I speculate that the
D2Low receptors mediate bottom-up attention because they only fire occasionally (how
often do we see an aberration in the environment – a tiger on the front lawn?) and
because the activated D1 receptors enable the ACC and frontal areas to consider matters
25
insightfully, as one must to understand the significance of aberrant events . D2Low is
excitatory, so its reaction is stronger – perhaps strong enough to turn attention from a
top-down focus to a prominent but possibly unimportant singleton. Finally D2Low draws
25
. Unfortunately, the exact dynamics of the frontal activation is complex and
unclear (Dietrich & Kanso, 2010) so here the abstract paradigm of generalized
frontal activation (areas along the rostral axis from the striatum – notably the ACC
and PFC) as have been shown in the majority of the studies on insight) has to be
243
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Salient cue
Layer I (dendrite) D2
Striatal Excitation
La
cell Bottom-up
ye
Learning
r II
D1 Neocortical cell attention
/III Layer VI (axon)
D2Low configuration
Salient cue
Layer I (dendrite) D2 Inhibition
Striatal Familiar routines Active and
cell top-down
D1 Neocortical cell attention
D2High configuration
the other hand, limits the same awareness, enabling the conditions for
244
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Consequently, the D2High receptors are probably responsible for top-down and active
attention, because these modes of attention are continuous, and usually follow well-
established routines. These are the modes of attention that are used constantly in normal
life (we’re always active, playing with the sugar, seeking out something, etc.). As a
result, they require less dopamine to fire, but they must fire more or less constantly.
In-vivo studies of macaca fascicularis monkeys and a number of rat species were
the striatum. Regardless of species, the studies repeatedly demonstrate two ranges of
dopamine firing in the striatum, in keeping with the above hypothesis. On one hand,
there is the occasional phasic burst, which is activated by unexpected rewards (Grace et
al., 2007); and a low level rhythmic tonic action which remains more or less active
unless it is interrupted (Grace, 2000; Schultz, 1998) (see Figure 26 and Figure 27).
Logically, the phasic burst must be modulated primarily by D2Low receptors because
they fire only occasionally, and only when the stimulus is unexpected. The tonic action,
on the other hand, fires in a continual but low amplitude rhythmic pattern (between 7-
30hz) (Grace et al., 2007; Seamans & Durstewitz, 2008). The implication is that tonic
245
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
1. Unexpected reward
Reward
2. Conditioned reward
Pavlovian
Conditioning
Time (Seconds) 0 1 2
Heinz, Romero, Gallinat, Juckel, and Weinberger (2003); Heinz and Schlagenhauf
(2010) and Grace (1991); Grace et al. (2007) have noticed that both tonic and phasic
dopamine activation patterns are affected by saliency, but their findings vary from paper
246
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
to paper. What is striking is that all their studies appear to be sound. It seems the
reported inconsistent findings reflect the non-unitary nature of salience, not poor
decreased in tonic actions and vice versa, notwithstanding the interrelationships that
There can be no doubt that tonic and phasic mechanisms are very interconnected: a)
They occupy the same neural pathways and stimulate the same neurons (Schultz, 1998).
They are thus only distinguishable by amplitude and oscillatory frequency (Grace, 1990,
supply.
There are also patterns that do the opposite of phasic bursts. These are called tonic
depressions. They are gaps in the otherwise regular rhythmic tonic activity, and they
occur whenever a time-related event is conditioned and set to occur but doesn’t happen.
phasic burst. In condition 2, the monkey is given a reward but only after a Pavlovian
conditioning signal, which the monkey has already learned means a reward is coming
next. In condition 3, the monkey is given the same signal, setting up an expectation, but
the reward never appears. Instead, there is a gap in the tonic waveform exactly when the
247
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
reward was due (Schultz, 1998). Similar findings have been widely replicated using
It appears that tonic depressions are mismatch induced; meaning that tonic dopamine
also occasionally processes mismatches, but only occur when they are between top-
down expectations and how those expectations pan out in reality. Expectation
mismatches in a top-down process cause tonic depressions (condition 3); just as positive
b
f
Awareness
h
d e
a c
Low
D2High D2Low g
posed as the mediator of tonic dopamine wave patterns, and D2Low, the
background).
248
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
frequencies).
NMDA receptors.
1998).
249
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Kapur et al. (2005, p. 61) hypothesize that ‘the normal process of context-driven novelty
and salience to stimuli… without cue or context.’ This line of thought attempts to escape
the obvious fact that these saliency signals occur within specific neurons, and that they
bring attention to the singletons that are associated with those neurons. It seems
axiomatic that in schizophrenia there is some confusion about what is salient and what
Hebbian learning processes that the ontology is constructed; ‘Cells that fire together,
wire together’ (Shatz, 1996, p. 604). If this aphorism of Hebb’s rule of neural plasticity
is valid, then the connections that are stimulated by a saliency event is not random at all,
anyone but a single psychiatric patient. Meanings need not be rational for instance: they
250
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
A compelling model for how neurons are specifically associated with meanings
described by Grossberg (2009) and confirmed by Feldmeyer (2010). The specifics both
authors relate are somewhat different, but this is only to be expected because when
heterogeneous reality (Gerfen et al., 1987). The concept is that the outer dendritic layer
(I) of the neocortex process perceptual stimulus, whereas the inner axonal layer (VI)
processes top-down associations. As these match, the arbors of the middle layers (II-III,
IV, V) connect to other cells horizontally and diagonally across a distributed network
(Baars, 2005). Grossberg asserts that under normal circumstances, neither top-down, nor
cell, but when both top-down and bottom-up stimuli match, an event potential spreads
(identified as aberrant by Kapur et al, 2005), is no different. The phonological loop and
Baddeley, 2003) are hypothesized to use the same mechanics, and largely in the same
Even with the most basic level of bottom-up perception, perception requires an
to represent an external sensation (see perception, p.223). This means that the most
251
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
endogenous vs. exogenous are not dissimilar. They’re only differentiated by whether
they are the subject or object of observation (and this permits confusion if the sense-of-
self were to unravel, as it frequently does in schizophrenia (Kean, 2009; Sass & Parnas,
2001)). The difference is relative – where the proxy for the object is processed in the
the axons.
The higher-order our awareness becomes, the more abstract. Indeed, we can even
become aware of our intentions, meaning that even intentions may be regarded as a
Both Fletcher and Frith (2009) and Howes and Kapur (2009); (Kapur, 2003; Kapur et
al., 2005) agree that the upregulation of dopamine and the correlated upregulation of
26
perceptual salience causes the positive symptoms of schizophrenia . Frith didn’t
always have this view. In 1978, Frith first floated the idea that schizophrenia may be
26. Technically Fletcher and Frith use a different terminology: they write of attenuation
failure rather than aberrant salience, but these are the same.
252
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
stunts the cognitive component of perception (Frith, 1979; Joseph, Frith, &
Waddington, 1979). But because an empirical basis for the hypothesis was weak and
further studies only produced equivocal results, the hypothesis was subsequently
withdrawn (Frith, 1987; Frith & Done, 1988). Even so, the idea continued to find
(Gray et al., 1991) and the attention deficit model for recurrent complex visual
hallucinations (Collerton et al., 2005). In keeping with Frith’s reversal, the aberrant
salience hypothesis and the Bayesian attenuation failure hypothesis do not take the view
that attention in schizophrenia is deficient, rather, that it is in surplus (Fletcher & Frith,
Distinctions between modes of attention allow for both a deficit and a surplus to co-
exist. This may partly explain the division in the schizophrenia diagnostic criteria,
where on one hand, diagnosis by the more florid of the so-called positive symptoms
attentional superfluity, whereas the negative signs (Symptom A5: absences of normal
behaviours – akinesia etc.) appear to be caused by attentional deficits, and others such
functional surplus of top-down and active attention due to a presynaptic and subcortical
253
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
(postsynaptic) dopamine.
receptors (Seeman, 2008b), this model directly explains another prominent paradox. It is
yet no significant losses of glutamate are found in schizophrenia (losses range from 0%
This hypothesis also solves another question that has plagued researchers for decades:
the combined tonic surpluses when added to the phasic deficits would explain why
episode chronic, and catatonic schizophrenia showed that the average upregulation of
dopamine synthesis in schizophrenia was a moderate 14%, but with a telling variation –
at the lowest, three datasets showed very insignificant upregulation over the normal
254
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Converging evidence also points to the same finding: tonic D2 receptor activity is
would mean that patients were matched to controls), yet these increases are not
ubiquitous (Howes et al., 2012). The heterogeneity in the data suggests that in some
medication.
Finally this hypothesis will also explain why anti-psychotic medications are ineffective
in about 30% of patients with schizophrenia (H. M. Jones, 2004). (Note that while this
level 0%, 0.02% and 0.05% (Av =101.3) all were for cohorts with catatonic
schizophrenia. On the other end of the spectrum, upregulation reached as much as 40%
in one study, but this outlier was found to be non-significant during post-hoc analysis.
The other studies showed an average upregulation of 15.4% over a total of 81 cases of
28. Data from a meta-analysis of SPECT and PET studies of drug-naive patients with
matched controls.
255
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
observation lends support for the argument that schizophrenia is two syndromes (Crow,
1980). This logic is untenable because the functional surplus of striatal D2 receptors
To take this hypothesis further, the above outline of what (little) we know about the
healthy attentional system should prove a good starting point. The next step is to drill
down to the specifics of salience, to make informed predictions about what aberrant
256
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
So far this paper has discussed the molecular biochemistry, and how it affects attention
through neuronal interactions (See level A of Table 3). These interactions are then
257
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
active. See level B). The next section of the paper explores the psychological syndromes
and dependent cognitive processes generate (Level C). A number of important cognitive
processes are directly affected including: attentional focus, perception, the ontology
Finally this hypothesis is putatively held to explain behavioural symptoms and signs of
Association, 1994, pp. p. 274, 277), thereby linking the effect of molecular
Which came first is a chicken and egg argument, and it is still difficult to say which
came first. Some symptoms correlate with an up-regulation of top-down and active
attention and others because of the down-regulation bottom-up processes, but at its most
parsimonious, all the symptoms; positive and negative, acute or chronic and rare or
common are caused by confluence of these two interdependent dysfunctions and eight
interdependent but distinct syndromes this confluence causes. The patterns equate
roughly to the three main schizophrenia subtypes. There is the paranoid type (295.30)
with bottom-up attentional deficits; and there is the catatonic type (295.20), which
(Cantor-Graae, Nordström, & McNeil, 2001; Davidson & Heinrichs, 2003; Goldman, Hien, Haas, & Sweeney, 1992; J. A. McGrath
259
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Putative explanation
Paranoid Disorganized Catatonic
Positive feedback
attention deficits
Confirmation bias
Diagnostic tool
Degradation of
DSM page
automaticity
Automaticity
Tunnel focus
Undermined
Subcortical
bottom-up
Behavioral
Frequency
negativity
Reference
distortion
Sample
arousal
Notes
Symptom
General criteria
D,E,F B A,C
Referential; environmental cues (press etc.) are directed at the patient. 275-276 78% 1091 McGrath etal. DSMIII/DSMIV 1 1 1 2 1 0 0 0
Somatic; Loss of control over mind and body 275-276, 279 25% 1031 McGrath etal. 4 DSMIII/DSMIV 3 2 3 2 2 1 1 2
Grandiose or religious 55% 1103 McGrath etal. DSMIII/DSMIV 1 0 1 0 2 0 0 0
Distortions or exaggerations of perception (A2: hallucinations) 274-275 73% 980 Castle etal. DSM-III/DIP 2 3 3 3 1 1 2 0
Auditory 82% 1175 McGrath etal. DSMIII/DSMIV 2 0 2 0 1 2 3 3
Two or more voices or running commentary 0 0 0 0 1 2 3 3
Visual 41% 1067 McGrath etal. DSMIII/DSMIV 0 0 0 0 1 2 3 3
Multimodal no data 2 0 2 0 1 2 3 3
Monomodal no data 0 0 0 0 2 0 1 0
Olfactory or gustatory 15% 1079 McGrath etal. DSMIII/DSMIV 0 0 2 0 2 3 1 0
A2
Derailment no data 2 2 2 1 1 1 2 0
Tangenitality no data 3 3 2 1 1 1 3 0
loose associations to incoherence spectrum 53% 887 McGrath etal. DSMIII/DSMIV 3 3 2 1 1 1 3 0
Distortions behavioral monitoring (A4: grossly disorganized or catatonic behavior)274-275 58% 923 McGrath etal. DSMIII/DSMIV 3 2 0 1 1 1 3 3
Childlike silliness no data 0 2 3 1 1 1 3 0
Unpredictable agitation no data 0 3 0 1 1 1 3 2
Difficulties in goal-directed action, Activities of Daily Living no data 3 1 0 1 1 1 2 0
Deshevelled appearance no data 2 2 3 3 2 1 1 0
dress in an unusual manner (e.g., wearing too much on a hot day) no data 2 2 1 3 2 3 1 0
inappropriate sexual behavior (e.g., public masturbation) no data 2 0 3 0 3 0 2 1
A4:
Alogia (manifested in brief, laconic, empty replies) 275, 277 34% 923 McGrath etal. DSMIII/DSMIV 3 0 0 3 3 1 1 0
Initiation of goal-directed behavior (avolition, apathy) 275, 277 69% 935 McGrath etal. 0 0 0 3 3 1 1 0
Associated features and disorders (Unspecified, not for diagnosis)
gross impairment in reality testing (loss of insight) 50% 980 Castle etal. DSM-III/DIP 0 0 0 0 2 1 1 0
Loss of ego boundaries 75% 151 Parnas etal. 1 Unspecified 0 0 3 1 2 1 1 3
Depersonalisation no data 0 0 3 1 2 1 1 3
Derealisation no data 0 0 3 1 2 1 1 3
Attention dysfunction (Poor concentration) 29% 980 Castle etal. 7 DSM-III/DIP 3 2 3 2 2 2 1 0
Anhedonia (loss of interest or pleasure) 44% 1031 McGrath etal. DSMIII/DSMIV 0 0 0 3 2 1 1 0
Dysphoric mood (depression, anxiety, or anger) 73% 980 Castle etal. DSM-III/DIP 2 3 0 3 3 1 1 0
Disturbance to sleep patterns no data 2 0 3 0 0 1 1 3
Lack of interest in eating as a consequence of delusional beliefs no data 1 3 0 3 1 0 1 0
Psychomotor abnormalities (e.g., pacing, rocking, or apathetic immobility) no data 0 3 0 3 3 1 2 1
Confusion, disorientation, memory impairment no data 2 2 0 3 2 1 1 0
Suicide risk 45% 1139 McGrath etal. DSMIII/DSMIV 1 3 1 3 1 2 1 2
Comorbidity with substance abuse 40-60%
Metaanalysis Cantor-Graae etal. 9 Mixed 2 0 2 1 3 0 2 2
260
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
the symptom.
261
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
maximum of 40% and an average of 14% higher levels than healthy controls (Fusar-Poli
activations, because D2Low consumes about ten times more available dopamine, so when
the heteromers are switched to a high affinity mode, there will be a natural surplus of
dopamine, of D2High and a deficit of D2Low. Many environmental conditions may cause
this switch, including the deactivation of interconnected cortical NMDA receptors, the
receptors. As these fire, they engage in the mechanisms of the striatum: the well-learned
habits, schemata and routines of active attention as well as positive top-down saliency
cues. Some of these naturally follow intentions. But the rest take place without any real
intentions to match them against. Both create symptoms that are particularly noticeable
The following syndromes: tunnel focus, undermined automaticity and confirmation bias
are all common in paranoid type schizophrenia: and these have the best overall
prognosis (Deister & Marneros, 1994; Kobayashi, 2001) this is because all these
syndromes maintain intent, and by doing this, a thread of meaning (however bizarre) is
kept. This prevents the deterioration into the disorganized type or catatonic subtypes,
262
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
which occur when meaning is abandoned (see the specific subheads dealing with these
Tunnel focus
Where intentions exist, the attention they harness is intensified, especially as bottom-up
and in paranoid psychosis, and whilst it is very common, is not unique to schizophrenia.
Top-down superfluity creates a single but intense range or ‘tunnel focus’ of interest that
2012).
Undermined automaticity
Dijksterhuis & van Knippenberg, 2000; Willingham, 2001) Only active attention
completely spares automatic functions. When top-down intention brings too much
focus to activities, intended regular routines will become more difficult, frustrating, time
consuming and prone to error (Semkovskaa, Bédard, Godboutc, Limogec, & Stip,
2004). Too much attention to functions that should be automatic, such as speech and
behaviour can make them seem mildly disorganized. A phenomenal example is familiar
to healthy people when trying to remember (a top-down process) a word or name that is
263
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
‘in the tip of the tongue’. An example is taken from a first person account: ‘… I was left
with a nagging, pulling sensation in the middle of my brain…’ (Hayne & Yonge, 1997,
p. 316)
Confirmation bias
saliency events. If there are any intentions, these ‘bingo,’ ‘this is important’ and ‘just as
Griffiths, Le Pelley, & Weickert, 2012), patients will readily learn irrelevant
associations. The results of this nonsense become part of the automatic ontology and are
These new associations only make tunnel focus worse, because the weakest points of
evidence will be enough to constitute proof. And a deficit of bottom-up attention (due to
The habit of jumping to conclusions has been found (in a meta-analysis) to occur in 40-
70% of studies of delusional patients (Garety & Freeman, 1999). This symptom gives
rise to paranoid and grandiose delusions (A1), and when it gets severe, it can present as
264
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The over-focus on prior expectations has been found in a series of experiments that
and others occurred. The studies (which I won’t detail) found that patients relied so
heavily on prior expectations that they failed to regard obvious sensory evidence.
Predictably, it was noticed, that this effect correlated strongly with the severity of the
delusions:
‘A little girl… looked at me and then said to her mother, “Is that man possessed
by the Devil Mummy?” Her mother also looked at me and replied, “Yes dear”.
This coincidence just when I was thinking this very thought, was enough to
Derailment – a symptom of disorganized speech can also begin to occur due to intense
tunnel focus. The following account about Phil; a patient known to have a delusion
involving Stephen Hawking and bikies. This is a third person recount account of a
‘So, could you, please, just tell me what’s been troubling you over the last
couple of days?’
265
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Phil was slow to answer. ‘Nothing at all… They can stay out of trouble… If they’re in
time.’
‘Sorry,’ Grace said. ‘When you say “They”, who do you mean? Who is it that
‘Bikies.’ Phil was silent for ten seconds, then finished with, ‘everyone really.
‘Yes, Stephen King,’ he nodded. He paused, then continued, ‘He’ll put the bikies
In this account, Phil’s attention has narrowed to the point that he can only focus on
subjects that relate to his delusion. This is principally a symptom of tunnel focus. The
questions posed by Grace are unable to steer Phil from his rut-like delusional course. An
association that would normally be very loose, the relationship between Stephen King
and Stephen Hawking appears to be indisputable and absolutely settled. For Phil, they
are one and the same because top-down saliency cues are triggered by the phonic
similarity (jargonaphasic) between the names. Here you can see the effect of a
266
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
confirmation bias. To anyone else, this evidence would be far from acceptable they
apparently share no more than similar names and careers as famous authors.
Pridmore’s recount also gives some insight into the nature of paranoia. Paranoia, once
associations. If negative ideas are the subject of over-intense focus, these will lead to a
state of paranoia, because evidence to prove that people are conspiring against you will
become ubiquitous and thought patterns will inevitably lead eventually to catastrophe
(D. Freeman & Freeman, 2008; D. Freeman et al., 2002; D. Freeman, Pugh, & Garety,
2008).
attention has not completely atrophied. This may present as grandiosity and religiosity.
Because the causal mechanisms of all of these syndromes are very similar, it is difficult
to perfectly distinguish one syndrome from another. Certainly this makes distinguishing
the schizophrenia subtypes complex. Not all disorganized symptoms are caused by the
267
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Whenever there are no intentions, but there is still a surplus of dopamine and D2High, the
confirmation bias will still continue, but the ‘bingo’ signals (recognition salience) will
The outcome is that the quality of the ontology deteriorates, and habits will become
entrenched.
bizarre. On the contrary, it is taken to be highly salient because of the confirmation bias
and the decline of negative feedback. The conditions that are created are exactly
analogous to the Larsen effect (the bizarre positive feedback patterns that occur when an
amplifier amplifies it’s own signal) (See Figure 28). This distortion amplifies
stereotypical thought patterns, behaviours and even physical actions. (Even motor
functions are maintained in the greater ontology (Donchin & Shadmehr, 2002)). The
results are genuine thought and gross motor disorders (A3, A4). Normally there are
three mechanisms to prevent this positive feedback pattern, and the D2Low channel
moderates all of them. The first is the bottom-up enrichment of the ontology with new
understanding. Another is that the D2Low channel balances positive feedback with
268
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
adequately reflects raw reality (Holroyd & Coles, 2002). The last mechanism is that the
particularly gambling; a gambling habit will be fed by the expectation of wins and the
failure to notice losses (Fletcher & Frith, 2009). Gambling is a common co-morbidity in
schizophrenia (Desai & Potenza, 2009), but it is largely mitigated by a loss of interest in
profits because of the advance of avolition and anhedonia and by confusion about the
As this positive feedback loop develops, the lexical ontology (speech and thought) will
start to take on skewed and hermetic meanings and conventions. Nuances may well
profoundly important but deeply personal experiences a patient has. Associations may
lateralize, meaning that the associations of such irrelevancies as rhyme or meter may
become more important than the traditional target concept (e.g. Rail may be taken to
mean jail, simply because they rhyme) (Chaika, 1977, 1982; Chaika & Lambe, 1989).
Jargonaphasia
The effects of positive feedback distortion can be seen in the Oberrealschul Student case
(presented by Kraeplin):
269
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
‘The patient I will show you today has almost to be carried into the room, as he
walks in a straddling fashion on the outside of his feet. On coming in, he throws
off his slippers, sings a hymn loudly, and then cries twice (in English), “My
father, my real father!…” The patient understands perfectly, and has introduced
many phrases he has heard before into his speech… He speaks in an affected
way’ (Spitzer, Gibbon, Skodol, Williams, & First, 2004, pp. 495-497).
Input
intent
& habit
intent
intent
Hebbian Hebbian
Hebbian
O O O
! AntiHebbian
(-) feedback
A A
hypothesis checking A
Figure 28: Raw perception (R) is ultimately our primary link with the
itive salience cue fires and our attention (A) is brought to the fact
there isn’t much need for fact checking, but bottom-up perception
patterns.
Subcortical confinement
When D2Low receptors are really depleted, a patient may effectively loose all frontal and
functions.
Excitatory receptors are needed to trigger event potentials in the parts of the brain that
are used to process ideas (normal arousal), and the deficit of excitatory dopaminergic
receptors (D2Low) in schizophrenia means this won’t happen, especially as the ACC is
actions, the main cause of awareness (the phasic surges of bottom-up attention) must be
quickly inhibited. This is done through the same critical pathway – the striatal D2High
receptors. When these fire, they do create a limited awareness – particularly if they
follow top-down intentions, but they also limit the engagement of the higher-order
Importantly, a large number of neurons are activated nevertheless, because rather than
have a small amount of excitatory neurons branch laterally to activate the frontal cortex
and ACC, a much larger amount of inhibitory neurons shut it down. This creates the
opposite of the ‘flight of ideas,’ inflated ego, euphoria, dysphoria and other signs of
large amount of activations still creates an event potential of sorts, although wholly
The striatum isn’t used for insight or to make the abstract leap from symbols to
meaning. This involves higher-order areas; the ACC and areas of the frontal cortex
(Dietrich & Kanso, 2010) the striatum is, however strongly implicated in automaticity
and the other structural elements of thought: it is better connected to manage the well-
learned routines of lexica, grammar, schemata and other procedural resources than more
rostral areas are. The striatum has inputs and outputs to the sensori-motor cortex for
behavioural responses; the inferior temporal visual cortex which mediate the visuo-
spatial sketchpad of working memory; the ventral striatum (including the nucleus
272
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
accumbens) that receives input the amygdala and hippocampus: these modulate
It is conceptually bizarre that awareness can shift to focus on the building blocks of
thought, without awareness of meaning, because for most of us the opposite is the only
way we think. For most people meaning comes first, and the means to communicate
theory, the opposite must be the case – because if people stop to consider an impending
danger, they may well just get eaten (Dijksterhuis & van Knippenberg, 2000).
More than any other syndrome, this syndrome addresses the inability of severely
identity and first person accounts report that even their own thoughts become
dysfunctions) are already of interest and are well known to occur in schizophrenia and
autism spectra disorders. Until now, no hypothesis has been presented that makes sense
of these dysfunctions (H. Gallagher & Frith, 2003). In support of this hypothesis, ToM
studies universally find activations of the most anterior region of the ACC – the
paracingulate cortex (H. Gallagher & Frith, 2003) or, in the case of severely regressed
A lot can be gleaned from first person accounts into this bizarre syndrome, and so the
insights from just spending dedicated time with patients is telling. Searles (an old-
273
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
school psychiatrist who sat with his patients for an hour a week, for years on end)
observed: ‘I had worked with schizophrenic patients for several years before I came to
that something that we would call a new concoction of fantasy, a new product of his
imagination enters his awareness, he perceives this as being actual and undistinguished
from the world around him.’ (Searles, 1962, p. 37) This passage pre-empts and distils
this hypothesis beautifully because it is the imagination – the ability to think abstractly
When you know what you are looking for, direct examples abound. When reading the
following extract, you cannot help but be aware of the lack of meaning; despite of a
‘…Speeds up the metabolism. Makes your life shorter. Makes your heart bong.
Tranquilizes you if you've got the metabolism I have. I have distemper just like
cats do, ‘cause that's what we all are. Felines [pause]. Siamese cat balls. They
stand out. I had a cat, a manx, still around somewhere. You'll know him when
you see him. His name is G-I Joe. He’s black and white. I had a little goldfish
274
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
too like a clown [pause]. Happy Halloween Down…’ (Chaika & Lambe, 1989,
pp. 411-412)
Sometimes the inability to raise ones awareness from the processes and schemata that
underlie thought and action are noted by patients with rare flashes of lucidity29:
‘I’m concentrating so much and trying to listen to what they are saying and I
the beach. A physical sensation, it arose in the back of my head and drifted
forward in a pleasant way, like a light gentle wash of sea froth. The waves fell,
disappeared into the sands, and left on the beach a thought. I remembered
suddenly the purpose of traffic signals and what the red and green lights meant.’
275
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
{
1 x D2Low
Synthesis of 10 Normal
DA molecules + Activates
more cells =
arousal
Synthesis of 10
10 x D2High { ~
DA molecules - More cells
activated
Atypical
= arousal
the location of these cells and what they do. In D2Low conditions, exci-
tation is in the ACC (and other more rostral areas), whereas in the
intervention has been effective in consuming excess dopamine, and in other cases, time
has taken its toll and hyperdopaminergia has naturally settled. When this happens, the
symptoms that are driven by excess dopamine will also settle and the distinct absences
of normal behaviours become more obvious. But because these symptoms are caused by
276
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
deficits of D2Low and bottom-up attention, they are expected to underlie all cases of
schizophrenia, even if they are subtle not immediately obvious alongside other more
florid symptoms.
The decrease of bottom-up attention will mean a decreased ability to notice things
including attention to somatic impulses, affect and social connection. Not only are these
1991), it is also a feature of other psychoses including bipolar type I disorder and the
‘The patient sits with his eyes shut, and pays no attention to his surroundings…
In the hospital he was almost dumb, was cataleptic, gave his hand stiffly and
jerkily, and almost entirely refused to eat. His expression was generally
indifferent, though sometimes cheerful, and visits from his relations made no
It is hard to ascertain from the third person perspective why patients like the
Oberrealschul Student may have such an indifference to other people, the environment
or even somatic needs (such as hunger). This inability is not because of distracting
‘noise’ as many theorists propose (eg, Corlett, Frith, & Fletcher, 2009; Heinz &
277
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Schlagenhauf, 2010; Howes & Kapur, 2009; Juckel et al., 2006)(See appendix II, p.
289), but because unsought perceptions will not attract attention and therefore they must
attention is grossly deficient. This condition becomes so extreme that patients may fall
over obvious obstacles and the instinct to block the fall may not be triggered (McDiven,
2011).
Because bottom-up attention also mediates negative feedback (Clark, 2012 - in press;
Holroyd & Coles, 2002) any unsolicited feedback (from other people or from the
perceptual errors can compound, causing experience itself to diverge from the common
30
experience of reality. Many authors speculate that this or similar ideas (Collerton et
al., 2005; Friston & Frith, 1995; Grossberg, 2000, 2003b; Stephan, Friston, & Frith,
30. A related concept is the corollary discharge error (CDE) model for
hallucinations, which that was first described by Feinberg (1978), and further
popularized by Frith (1979- and onwards) This model claims that an individual
carries a record; ‘the efference copy’ of all endogenous actions, and once actions are
hallucinations. The problem with this model is that it attempts to explain bizarre
278
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
2009) can explain hallucinations, but not for schizophrenia because schizophrenic
hallucinations can become very bizarre in a very short period and this cannot be
hypotheses.
obliviousness of social cues often results in the habit of invading other people’s
personal space.
deficits. These are usually tests of auditory bottom-up attention. In mismatch negativity
is interspersed with aberrant notes, etc. A meta-analysis identifies (on an average taken
medications don’t appear to improve this inability to notice aberrant notes (Michie,
2001).
The reason that anti-psychotics cannot be used to treat mismatch negativity deficits or
279
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
already requires far more dopamine to fire, meaning that less available dopamine will
either make no difference, and may even make this symptom worse. Although D2Low has
a much lower affinity for dopamine, its affinity to known antagonists are equal
(Seeman, 2008a; Seeman et al., 2006). Nicotine, on the other hand is known to stimulate
both D1 and prime the NMDA gateway receptors that gate its activation (Aramakis &
The bottom-up deficit syndrome will also present as a decline of emotions and the
overall sense of holism. Somatogenic, affective and hedonic impulses will all recede,
basic and obviously disagreeable experiences; they may dress in many layers in hot
weather, go naked in the cold, eat putrid food and express other signs of anhedonia,
until the associated urges become overwhelming. Most of these symptoms are well
observed in schizophrenia and they fall under the general diagnostic categories that best
somatogenic awareness also. The cliché, ‘taking time to smell the roses,’ refers to a
280
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
known correlation between bottom-up attention and feelings of joy. When talking of
family or friends, most healthy people subtly perk up (Pridmore, 2010). But this
anhedonia (S. Williams, 2002a; Wise, 1982). This stands to reason, because, once again,
When bottom-up attention is debilitated over an extended period, the deficit signs – the
don’t recognize their body parts and bodily processes (Coltheart, Langdon, & McKay,
2007). Symptoms like this are distinctly bizarre, and may be classed as somatic
depend on bottom-up attention (hunger, tiredness etc.) will go unnoticed until they have
most obvious among schizophrenic patients who live rough (Pridmore, 2010). A meta-
analysis of studies of schizophrenic homeless people suggests that about 11% of all
schizophrenic patients live on the streets (Folsom & Jeste, 2002). This figure, derived
largely from US data, is 11-12 times higher than the US national average (including the
disorganization, but also because bottom-up events are so much more intense ‘in the
rough’ and this could be an attempt to maintain some form of bottom-up awareness.
Behavioural negativity
inhibition of automatic functions and routines, resulting from excesses of focal attention
inhibiting them. But automaticity has a complex relationship with attention, and while
symptom.
behaviour setting (see automation, p. 234). But the awareness of the behaviour setting is
bottom-up, and this will be diminished in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and some
other illnesses31. Healthy people always limit their behaviour to some degree – but they
8.
This symptom occurs in schizophrenia, but is more marked in bipolar psychosis
because the amygdala, the organ that monitors positivity and negativity is overactive
in this disorder (S. B. Perlman et al., 2012) in this condition, negative affordances
282
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The kinds of negative behaviours that would be stopped in healthy people, but not in
psychosis include stereotypy, perseveration, and other unwanted habits and negative
routine thinking patterns. When exposed to negative stimulus, healthy controls show
balanced inhibition against activation (meaning no automatic action) and far less
inhibition than psychiatric patients did for positive stimuli, when the total inhibitory
activations were subtracted from the excitatory ones. The resulting activation of
excitatory and inhibitory neurons in each area was recorded and published (Northoff et
al., 2004) and analysed, post hoc by (Golembiewski, 2012a) (see article from p.358).
When patients become aware of these automatic actions, they won’t always recognize
283
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
addressed: How can a singular psychogenesis be discovered for something that cannot
even be defined, bearing in mind that the set of observables used for diagnosis are so
broad that at some point in the past, syphilis, scurvy, pellagra and hyperthyroidism have
been included (Berrios et al., 2003; Boyle, 2002; Hoffer, 1970)? Even the DSM-IV
notes that the diagnostic guidelines are insufficient tools to tackle the task. To lead
clinicians toward grounds for consensus, the DSM-IV stipulates a number of explicit
taxonomic conditions and exclusions (criteria C-F). But this is not enough: the DSM-IV
instructs ‘individuals with appropriate clinical training’ to reach beyond the categorical
definitions, and to recognize the ‘constellation’ of schizophrenia features, that is, not to
It is tempting to ignore this advice and deal with schizophrenia in parts. But the
applicability of such hypotheses are limited to the features they address. The hypothesis
presented in this paper is not intended to dismiss the others, but to follow through on
284
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
this paper has addressed each diagnostic criterion within the DSM-IV. And has done so
doing so, a strict adherence to the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994)
was essential because schizophrenia is not defined by its pathology, but by the
correlations between its presentation and the diagnostic criteria. Without the DSM-IV
each other, against every one of the diagnostic criteria listed in the DSM-IV and against
other observations that are otherwise largely held to be ubiquitous. These include many
symptoms and signs that have (despite the avalanche of papers being published) hitherto
285
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The DSM-IV, currently divides delusions (A1) and hallucinations (A2) into bizarre and
although both occur in schizophrenia. The DSM also notes that disorganized speech
(A3) can be relatively normal, provided it is not ‘severe enough to substantially impair
symptoms into bizarre and non-bizarre is likely to change next year with the release of
DSM-V (Tandon & Carpenter, 2012). This is unfortunate, because the hypothesis for
nia, but not in the continuum of psychotic experience, and therefore an important diag-
The psychotic continuum into healthy populations is well known (Hanssen, Bak, Bijl,
Vollebergh, & van Os, 2005; Johns et al., 2004; Nuevo et al., 2012; van Os, Hanssen,
Bijl, & Vollebergh, 2001; van Os, Linscotta, Myin-Germeys, Delespaula, &
a mean 3.1% report psychotic symptoms (van Os et al., 2009), but this finding is subject
to enormous variance. In Nepal, for instance, 45.8% of 8822 subjects have at least one
psychotic symptom (Nuevo et al., 2012). Hay (1994) reports numbers that are higher
286
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
these are specifically excepted by DSM-IV criteria (Saver & Rabin, 1997). When
compared with the experiences of schizophrenic patients, these numbers are low, but
this is due in part to a fallacious quantification: Few people (with or without mental
illness) believe that their experiences are hallucinatory or delusional, and neither are
they necessarily be aware of their speech disorders. Because inductive methods (self-
reporting) are typically the method used to assess psychotic spectra symptoms in the
community, it’s fair to assume that the data gathering takes place while the subjects are
feeling comfortable, secure, well and with their consent. In contrast, the diagnosis of
symptoms during the first meeting with a clinician is going to be deductive, enforced,
While this should be a concern for the authors of the DSM-V and for clinicians, it is
mentioned here to point out that the symptoms listed in the DSM-IV as not being psy-
chotic per sé. Delusions are extremes of belief and hallucinations employ the mecha-
nisms of normal perception (such as the phonological loop and visuo-spatial sketchpad).
It is not the presence, but the nature of the symptoms that should be a concern for clini-
cians, because the difference between psychotic experiences and those of the communi-
ty (and also from those of other mental illnesses) are quantitative and qualitative and not
absolute. In dementia with Lewy bodies for instance, hallucinations are very common,
possibly more so than in schizophrenia, yet the underlying pathology is putatively with
quite distinct from schizophrenic ones; they tend to be visual and are rarely threatening
(Collerton et al., 2005) whereas schizophrenic patients tend to suffer a multisensory and
287
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
the patient (Chadwick, 1992). Schizophrenic delusions may be both bizarre and non-
bizarre, but they are usually polythematic. The monothematic delusions common in oth-
er conditions are rare in schizophrenia. This means that schizophrenics will believe a
diverse range of things (John Nash, a Nobel laureate mathematician, believed that he
would become Emperor of Antarctica, that he was the left foot of God on Earth, and that
his name was really Johann von Nassau), but dementia patients or patients with a cere-
bral lesion is more likely to have monothematic delusions like Capgras’ (the belief that
emotionally negatively charged (Garety et al., 2001). A study by Honig et al. (1998)
with 18 schizophrenic patients and 15 controls, point out that negativity is ubiquitous
(100% vs. 53%), fear is very common (78% vs. 0%) and interference with daily life is
also ubiquitous (100% vs. 20%). Another difference is closely related: the feeling of
being out of control. All healthy subjects report that they maintain control during
difference is that half the schizophrenic patients experience voices in the third person –
288
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
heuristic belief that symptoms are a product of neural noise (Fletcher & Frith, 2009;
Heinz & Schlagenhauf, 2010; 2009; Rolls, Loh, Deco, & Winterer, 2008). The
hypothesis presented here locates a preoccupation with internal stimuli, but rejects the
notion that this is a distraction preventing focused attention. In fact distraction possibly
plays no greater part in schizophrenic perception (at least for paranoid subtypes) than it
‘Noise’ hypotheses are apparently well supported by evidence, but this reflects a
common flaw in experiment design and analysis and is not an inclusive picture of all
of attention to irrelevant stimuli (E.g., Martins Serra, Jones, Toone, & Gray, 2001;
Moran, Owen, Crookes, Al-Uzri, & Reveley, 2008; Oades, Zimmermann, & Eggers,
1996). When questioning these findings, a similar experiment ruled out the possibility
that this effect was caused by learning difficulties – only strengthening the distractibility
hypotheses (Morris et al., 2012). But no studies addressed the possibility that attention
given to irrelevant stimuli may have been deliberate – and caused by an admixture of
289
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
psychologically tested. Both are natural products of tunnel focus (q.v.) To clarify this
issue, other experiments demonstrate that the undue attention given to distracters in the
Indeed, not all abilities are subject to loss in schizophrenia. While bottom-up attention
tags mismatches, top-down attention tags matches, whether they are true or not. If tasks
are well defined, avoid unexpected (bottom-up) challenges and complications that will
require creative and insightful solutions, tasks should be easier for patients with
increased top-down attention than for controls, after all, paranoid schizophrenics should
be less distractible than controls. A task was given to a group of schizophrenia patients
and two other control groups; a clinical control group, which included patients with bi-
polar, personality and severe affective disorders (some of them also showed psychotic
symptoms); and another healthy control group. The task was simple: to match two
patterns, where distracting fuzzy marks surrounded the target. Subjects were instructed
on the task and told to ignore the distracter stimuli. In experiment test 87% of the
schizophrenic cohort outperformed the best result shown by the combined controls.
Meanwhile, the differences between the two other control groups were insignificant
(Dakin et al., 2005). In another experiment, pairs of words were given in succession;
some had no correlation, others were genuine pairs. Paranoid schizophrenic patients
performed especially well at picking the pairs, particularly when the words were
Hemsley, 1984; Hemsley, 1987). Once again, those with positive symptoms maintained
exceptional focus.
to assess and match physical force, by reciprocating the force applied by a lever. In this
study, all subjects underestimated their own efforts, but schizophrenic patients
Not only do these results contradict the abovementioned distractibility studies, but these
results also appear to be in conflict with widely cited studies that demonstrate deficits in
attention drawn from Stroop tests and Wisconsin Card sorting tests. These tests measure
a combination of top-down and bottom-up attention and cognitive ability, rather than
measuring top-down attention alone. Naturally almost all the results show schizophrenic
patients lagging far behind controls in performance and accuracy (Chan et al., 2010;
Zihl, Gron, & Brunnauer, 1998). The Stroop test involves presenting the name of a
colour (RED) in dissimilar (green) ink, and assessing performance of subjects with a
battery of measures including time, accuracy and neuroimaging. The Wisconsin Card
Sorting Test similarly offers a range of contradictory triggers. In this test, the patient
must arrange cards, which can be sorted by any of four criteria, but with every match,
there is also a mismatch. They are not instructed on a correct method, although they are
told whether they are right or wrong. In other words, both tests are puzzles that assess
higher-order processing (Damasio, 1994; Goldman-Rakic, 1991; Zihl et al., 1998). And
291
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
292
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
(Table 3 is on p.257)
2. The nature of delusions typically changes in different cultural milieus (Ndetei &
Vadher 1984).
is putatively thought to cause the other. The higher occurrence of delusions than the
hallucinations that are thought to cause them may reflect the difficulty clinicians have in
4. It is not known how much crossover or under-reporting there is with this symptom
5. These values are combined because it is impossible to assess these values separately
(Chika, 1982).
6. How this was assessed was not reported. Several studies show better concentration
293
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
abuse in the community, and cultural and other factors including increased negative
9. The authors found that the group with bizarre delusions was the same group that had
other severe positive symptoms, and yet interrater confidence in what constituted
bizarreness was not as high. For this reason the authors recommended against inclusion
A lot of the behaviour that is most feared by psychiatric clinical staff is opportunistic.
The first, ‘All common psychotic symptoms can be explained by the theory of
ecological perception.’ Has been published in Medical Hypotheses, (2012: 78, 7-10.
doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2011.09.029)
294
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The third research question looks at aetiological issues and at symptoms, as they are an
If the above hypotheses and evidence aren’t enough to convince sceptics of the
relationship between schizophrenia and the environment, then the following arguments
may.
295
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
296
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Abstract
environment plays both causal and therapeutic roles in schizophrenia, but what are the
environment and psychotic illness, using primary research from disciplines as diverse as
antagonist for the underlying processes that present as psychosis. The built environment
is implicated through several means: Through the opportunities it provides. These may
negative, and others positive. The built environment is also an important source of
This paper focuses on the role the built environment plays in psycho-environmental
dynamics, in order that negative effects can be avoided and beneficial effects
Limitations and implications: The findings presented are based on research that is
largely translated from very different fields of enquiry. Whilst findings are cogent and
297
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Social implications: The WHO claims that schizophrenia destroys 24 million lives
worldwide, with an exponential effect on human and financial capital. Because evidence
implicates the built environment, architectural and urban designers may have a role to
cause of mental illness. This paper was presented to the Symposium of Mental Health
Facility Design, and is essential reading for anyone involved in designing for improved
mental health.
298
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
theoretical discussion. It draws on the evidence that links the designed and constructed
environment to psychosis – and translates this evidence in a way that helps to inform
Mental illnesses aren’t homogenous. They share some similarities, but also have very
disorder may do the opposite in another (Pei et al., 2010). In fact, not only are mental
disorders heterogeneous, the symptoms that they present are, too (American Psychiatric
Association, 1994). Hallucinations, for example, range from very normal experiences
through to utterly bizarre and frightening ones. Hay (1994) reports that as many as 60%
of people will have experiences that could be classed as hallucinatory if religious and
with dementia are often meaningful and are usually experienced in a single modality – a
person may see or hear a deceased relative or angels for instance, but they rarely see
and hear. Shizotypal experience, on the other hand, is far less common: it’s usually out
of control, malevolent and is usually multimodal –the visual and aural experience of
299
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
having God appear on the TV to command them to commit suicide, for example
Despite the complexity of mental illness, the current evidence available to direct design
decision-making is mundane, predictable and offers little insight into the psychological
hasn’t had much attention since (Elliot & Bayes Friba, 1972; E. T. Hall, 1975, 1990;
Osmond, 1958, 1966). But since then, designers haven’t kept up with the explosion of
Other papers on the design of psychiatric design take a few distinct methodologies, and
all have limitations, particularly when it comes to understanding findings. Some are
based on post-occupancy studies of units that have been renovated (Eg. Hurst, 1960;
Sloan Devlin, 1992; Vaaler et al., 2005). These cannot extend beyond the working
principles of the design team and are further limited by the evaluation criteria of the
identifiable empirical support (Eg. Davis, Glick, & Osow, 1979; Foley & Lacy, 1967;
Gross, Sasson, Zarhy, & Zohar, 1998; Gutkowski et al., 1992; 1957, 1958, 1966).
Some researchers ask the patients when seeking design solutions. This approach is
effective, with qualifications. Valid questions hover over the usefulness of patient
opinions when patients are typically confused about basic reality. Also, all patient
cohorts cannot be represented – the more critical patients who have ‘lost touch with
unlikely to give meaningful answers at all, whereas it may be hard to contain the
opinions of patients with manic disorders. For this reason, patient questionnaires are
rare. Where useful findings can be uncovered using this approach, they still lack insight
into the dynamics of the environment on the syndrome’s aetiology. Instead they tend to
gauge patient satisfaction with one choice or another (Middelboe, Schjødt, Byrsting, &
Gjerris, 2001) (Eg. Barnhart, 1996; L. S. Larsen, 1992; Perkins, in this issue). As
examples, Barnhart (1996) found that schizophrenic patients generally prefer garden
settings than constructed ones and L. S. Larsen (1992) found that schizophrenic patents
preferred garden settings that were extremely naturalistic, highly enclosed by shrubbery
and extremely complex in contrast to controls who preferred more open gardens, less
Another approach in the literature is to inform design through models of health, illness,
stress and psychosocial needs. These methods provide a welcome richness for designers
because they focus on broad principles. But these methods are still limited to the
Zeisel’s studies on the environments for Alzheimer’s patients (Zeisel, 2005, 2007;
Zeisel & Raia, 2000; Zeisel et al., 2003) cannot be naïvely superimposed. Even so,
some of the eight principles and sixteen dimensions of Alzheimer’s care of (Zeisel)
remain relevant. This draws us toward another approach: the development of design
principles that transcend the specifics of illnesses, and are based on models of sickness
301
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
and health. Examples include: Jan Golembiewski (2010b); (2012e, 2012f); S. Kaplan
(1995); Lawton and Nahemow (1973), but where focusing on specific disorders may be
The approach taken here is not to address problem behaviour (ie. boredom, getting lost,
wandering etc.) but to identify specific aspects of the built environment that appear to
Background
schizophrenia (Fletcher & Frith, 2009; Kapur, 2003; Kapur et al., 2005; Searles, 1960).
diagnoses (Castle et al., 2006) and by a general lack of responsiveness in others (69%,
environments are not uncommon but are usually guided by naïve models of perception.
Changing finishes does not change the way things are perceived. A green-coloured
green paint on the walls of a day-room cannot change the walls in a meaningful way,
even if it does make the place noticeably more cheerful to a healthy visitor. Yes,
replicated evidence does suggest that interior decorations, if significant enough, may
302
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
improve mental health outcomes, lower vandalism rates, and shorten stays (Hurst, 1960;
Sloan Devlin, 1992; Ulrich & Parsons, 1990; Vaaler et al., 2005). These outcomes are
welcome, and these innovations may guide designers to make better choices, but
because superficial approaches are unlikely to affect the psychogenesis that underlies
It’s heretical to suggest that the built environment has a causal effect on schizophrenia,
but the facts are bare: when other known factors are discounted – ‘urbanicity’ at the
the null hypothesis) once genetic factors have been discounted (Kelly et al., 2010).
that all other factors are even. Data from meta-analyses suggest that urbanicity at any
time of life correlates with a 48% increase in schizophrenia incidence. This number is
one of the highest and most stable epidemiological factors for schizophrenia (van Os,
2004). Ultimately, this builds a compelling argument that the urban environment has a
The idea that urbanicity is psycho-toxic has been tested, but the reasons are elusive.
Ellet et al (Ellett et al., 2008) conducted ‘the Camberwell Walk Study,’ which found
that even a few minutes of exposure to the urban environment has a significant negative
impact on psychotic patients. A ten minute walk along the dilapidated but busy
303
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
(anxiety t (14) = –3.57 (p = 0.003); and paranoia t = –2.69 (p = 0.017, negative scores
represent deterioration).
The finger points squarely at the urban environment, but what features of the
environment are culpable? Is the urban environment itself a problem, or is it a proxy for
something else? Can ‘urbanicity’ represent particular social mores, the prevalence of the
built environment, for indoor living? For the psychic pressure of too many people? For
richness of opportunity?
influences are multiplied by environmental interactions (van Os, Kenis, & Rutten, 2010;
van Os, Rutten, & Poulton, 2008) or social dynamics (Selten & Cantor-Graae, 2007).
Collip, Myin-Germeys, and van Os (2008) suggests that the urban environment may
exposure to whatever it is that a patient is sensitised to. This approach seems wise, but it
still fails to identify anything specific. The key to identifying environmental psycho-
agonists (at the level of detail required by designers) is in the relationship between
304
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Ecological perception:
Mental illnesses are not defined by pathology but by behavioural symptoms. If the
mind and reflect natural responses to perceived stimuli, whether they are hallucinatory
McGrath et al., 2009)) are best understood as being very genuine expressions of a state
of mind that is informed by a perceptual bias (Garety & Freeman, 1999). But how can
perception be so disordered that a patient can, in all honesty, mistake people for trees or
perception. Even a lot of scientific literature makes the mistake of assuming that
perception is a process where colour, shape, texture and other qualia (sensory
information; colours smells etc.) are separated and recombined in the mind to form the
objects of knowledge, but this is not a tenable position. We simply don’t have the
computing power to recognise every perception from all available data (Clark, 2012 - in
press). Certainly such a process must occur in healthy perception wherever objects are
unrecognisable, but in most instances, perception is direct and active. One of the best
Perception. This theory doesn’t immediately promise all the answers to the complex
questions surrounding psychotic illness, but it does provide valuable insight into
phenomenology – the presumed basis for psychotic experience. The principle is that we
305
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
ways (Bargh & Dijksterhuis, 2001). The cue-behaviour dynamic occurs in a very
mechanical way – much as a transplanted heart will automatically start thumping when
exposed to warm blood. We notice (and involve ourselves with) things we can directly
recognise, manipulate and use (For empirical evidence, see Gibson, 1979). Meanwhile,
colours, sounds and other raw sense data are easily missed or immediately forgotten.
Superficial changes do not affect the opportunities a space provides. People act when
they recognise affordances, and while colour and shape information may make an
affordance more or less recognisable, people don’t act on qualia (sense information) as
such. The primary task of perception is to initiate action (Bargh & Dijksterhuis, 2001;
Gibson, 1979). There are different types of affordances and each has its own lexica of
action-responses. Although not all are relevant to architecture as such, they all become
306
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Emotional affordances
Some perceptions have direct emotional (affective) meaning. This kind of engagement
is very important for humans. Humour automatically solicits laughter, friendship solicits
happiness, betrayal solicits anger, and disappointment solicits resentment etc. But
psychotic conditions often prevent emotional affordances from eliciting a normal range
of responses. This is particularly so for those with affective psychoses such as bipolar
technical term that does not necessarily denote the presence of paranoia as such
This was found using an image study. The subjects were 17 psychotic patients
schizophrenia, n=3; and type 1 bipolar 1, n=7) and ten healthy controls. All subjects
had their frontal cortices scanned using functional magnetic resource (fMRI) imaging
methods at the same time as they were shown a series of pictures. Some pictures were
generically negative and others positive. One finding was that the psychiatric patients
showed very aberrant activation and inhibition patterns when compared to the controls
(Northoff et al., 2004). Another was more specific: where the healthy controls perfectly
balanced their neural excitations and inhibitions in response to the negative images, the
patients’ neural reactions were out of control. Aberrant excitation correlates with
307
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
excessive thought and activity. And where healthy controls showed some control for the
positive images, the psychiatric patients showed excessive inhibition, indicating that
they were unable to ‘let go’ and enjoy a healthy experience. These dysfunctional
Emotional affordances are, by and large embodied in the social environment (including
pets), but they are also to be found within the arts. The emotional affordances are the
product of aesthetic qualities such as beauty and sublimity. Since antiquity beauty has
been recognised as a quality of architecture (Vitrivius, circa 15BC) – although the last
century has largely disposed of this legacy. Other arts, including the visual arts and
alcohol – and behaviours; gambling, sex, and sometimes theft and violence. In the
context of health facilities, violent behaviour is a significant issue, but other habitual
Among healthy people, the moderation imposed by neural inhibition renders many
hedonic affordances acceptable but without healthy neural inhibition, the use of hedonic
stimulants becomes a mental illness in its own right, commonly known as an abuse or
308
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Physical affordances
A very common symptom of schizophrenia is that patients will drink when a glass is
offered, even if they aren’t thirsty. The symptom seems innocuous, but the subsequent
overconsumption of water has been associated with acutely low sodium levels and there
have been published hypotheses suggesting that this may be the cause of schizophrenia
(American Psychiatric Association, 1994; Reeves, 2004; Wyatt et al., 1988). While this
hypothesis is simplistic and untenable, the symptom draws attention the lack of control
presence of a syringe solicits a jab, a gun solicits a shot and flowers solicit smelling and
picking (Lhermitte, 1986). Louis Kahn also observed that building materials have
that might be a good thing. But if people are deprived of positive affordances, not only
is a beneficial opportunity lost, but the absence of positive affordances could also
309
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Identification affordances
Another critical area where the designed environment plays a part in schizophrenia is in
the formation of the sense-of-self. There is no consensus about what creates a holistic
sense-of-self; but whatever it is, one of the most alarming phenomena in schizophrenia
is that the sense-of-self is lost (Kean, 2009; Sass & Parnas, 2001; Searles, 1966).
those choices (Eg. Bem, 1967; Deci & Ryan, 1991; Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959).
establishing a sense-of-self. But the sense-of-self is absent when choices are automatic.
You don’t choose how to bring a cup to your lip, nor do you peg your identity on this
action. You don’t choose how to drive either (once these basic functions have been
learned), and if you did, it would be a signal to others that the way you drive is an
important expression of self. While automatic choices may define others’ impressions of
you, your sense-of-self is defined by the choices you choose to make and by those you
The designed environment is possibly the most prominent context for personal choice
making, in this regard it is likely to eclipse the social environment in importance – after
310
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
all, the designed and constructed environment is ubiquitous, whereas the social
environment comes and goes. The constructed environment can therefore be understood
as being an important domain for self-expression and therefore for the establishment of
a sense-of-self. Choices abound in the negotiation of the built environment and many of
them are definitively associated with the sense-of-self. Many of these choices revolve
around affordances. What we choose to do, given the opportunity. The major product is
choice to sit in front of the television usually has little to do with choice, if there’s
nothing else to do. Television may even be harmful because it trains passivity in the
face of virtual opportunities and also a sense of unlikely narrative through the storylines
of television shows, and this may structurally reinforce delusional patterns of thought
(see below).
311
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Narrative affordances
Paranoid psychotic patients (the most common subtype of the psychotic spectrum) have
them in which they are the unwitting protagonist (S. Gallagher, 2007). These delusional
narratives hold that all events ultimately relate to the self. Exposure to any place with
strong symbolic loadings and omens of impending disaster or evil must stimulate these
delusions – and such omens will be concentrated wherever people and their symbols are
312
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Figure 30: The odd name of a street may be enough to trigger para-
Some environments might be particularly loaded with affordances that lend themselves
to delusional narratives. This was demonstrated by the Camberwell Walk Study (Ellett
et al., 2008) described on page 303. Camberwell is known for its extremely high
incidence of schizophrenia – as much as 9 times the incidence of areas that are nearby
(Kirkbride et al., 2006). My methodology was not scientific and cannot be reported as
such, but I retraced the area where the study had taken place. My route took me across
313
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
an Orpheus Street (Figure 30), past the Pre-Loved store (Figure 31), and went past
several signs from the Black Katz real estate agent (Figure 32). In one place there was a
gathering of at least thirty police. These examples are just a few of many more odd and
suggestive narrative cues that I experienced in my ten-minute walk. Other notable non-
verbal omens I saw included stray dogs, graffiti, pits in the street, ladders leaning over
walkways, Zeus brand motorbike helmets, and ominous posters for games and
314
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Exactly what triggers a delusional quest shall not be discussed here, but once it has
commenced, there’s little doubt that the urban environment will contain more salient
stimuli in which emotional, physical and narrative cues are present. Without robust
explanation see Golembiewski, 2012a). It has yet to be tested, but this alone seems
increase in urban areas as compared to rural ones. A rural lifestyle is simply going to be
315
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Given the richness in urban narrative subject matter, it seems that a rural setting could
agonists. The theory that the country is healthier than the city is one that dates back two
hundred and fifty years, and has taken form in some archetypal asylums: the York
Retreat and the Kirkbride units that were constructed in the nineteenth Century (Yanni,
2007).
appear to be attracted to the city. Healing though the country may be, schizophrenic
patients show a tendency to drift into the 24/7 red light areas of the big metropolises,
where they sleep rough in doorways, traffic islands and in areas often recognised for the
highest levels of substance induced violence. Psychiatric migration isn’t huge; but
schizophrenic people tend to flow to the centre rather than into the more calming
environments of small towns and the country. A traditional (but still current) argument
links this psychiatric migration to poverty (Read, 2010). But where city centres were
once cheap, the opposite is usually the case now – yet wanderers still roll in. So another
hypothesis is proposed: that the frenetic action of inner city life may provide comfort,
even though quietness and security comforts most other people. And although frenetic
city life may indeed increase the symptoms, it may also provide relief to a particularly
troubling symptom: the loss of the ability to experience anything other than troubling
decisions a person makes, rather than following some sort of unconscious process. But
to understand how the hustle and bustle of city life might provide relief from
316
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
hallucinations and delusions, it’s necessary to look at another aspect of perception: this
time at selective attention theory. This theory also explains another gamut of psychotic
Selective attention
The tendency to live rough in noisy and uncomfortable places suggests that
schizophrenic patients may want or need more external stimulation. As just noted, this
could provide relief from relentless delusional thought patterns because it stimulates
bottom-up attention, one of the two kinds of attention in the perceptual psychology
canon: Bottom-up attention is drawn to all stimuli that are unexpected and unsought.
Affordances are automatic actions that follow a known pattern of behaviour. For
example, a glass of water affords the familiar action of drinking. Bottom-up attention
can’t trigger behaviour, as its stimuli is not yet associated with set behavioural
responses. For example, we recently had a morning in Sydney where the air was red as
if it had been dyed (Figure 33). Nobody knew how to respond, so instead we enquired
(turning on the radio, etc). Enquiry (rather than action) is triggered by bottom-up
draw bottom-up attention beg answers, not actions. Unusual opportunities, awe and
317
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Figure 33: In 2009 Sydney awoke to a bright red sky. Nobody seemed
to know why or what it meant. People asked one another, tuned to the
radio and news and invented theories. Some alarmist people suspected
ke, Magnus.
Where top-down attention is stimulated very easily and requires little stimuli, bottom-up
mismatches with normal expectations, sudden contrasts and jolts from prominent sense
318
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Parkinson’s disease, depression and most other mental illnesses. There are two different
types of dopamine dysfunction. Put simplistically, too much dopamine causes an excess
Psychiatric Association, 1994). A deficit of dopamine (in the simplest terms) causes
symptoms associated with bottom-up attention failure – the so called ‘negative’ signs
such as the noticeable absences of normal behaviour that are very difficult to treat
For reasons that can’t be discussed here, dopamine is sometimes in short supply in one
part of the brain and in surplus elsewhere, meaning that positive symptoms and negative
What this means in schizophrenia is that prominent, awesome or unexpected things tend
to go unnoticed, even when they are plainly obvious. Bottom-up attention deficits are
not easy to observe as an outsider, but may be the most severe and troubling symptoms
of schizophrenia and other mental illnesses. These deficits present a whole host of
problems, not least of all a tendency to not to notice the glaringly obvious (Broome et
al., 2007). The inability to engage with awe (even if only occasionally) is possibly
and contract the perception of time, so that it always feels like it’s running out (Rudd,
Vohs, & Aaker, 2012). Other bottom-up perception abilities are even more critical; the
319
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
ability to listen to internal messages that tell us we’re hungry, tired, happy or miserable:
the signals that guide us socially and monitor whether our behaviour is appropriate or
not, etc.
Deficits of bottom-up attention seem to give rise to very worrisome symptoms. Social
dysfunction, isolation, hunger and confusion can all be traced to this deficit. This
hypothesis proposes that schizophrenic migration to the most stimulating parts of town
may occur in order to stimulate bottom-up attention. To force real feelings – to have real
experience.
Facility designers should be aware that patients with bottom-up attention deficits will be
especially prone to accidents – they are prone to get lost and fail to notice aesthetic
and faculty managers may want to restrict movement and behaviour to prevent accidents
and harm (See Chrysikou, - in this issue). But equally so, environments that don’t
required – improved safety measures must be balanced with aesthetic generosity, and
opportunities for discovery and to act in a fulfilling and positive way (See Perkins, - in
this issue). Decision makers may want uniform and bright lighting, but should be
perceptions (Chadwick, 1992; D. Freeman & Freeman, 2008) while missing contrary
facts. This phenomena, known as a confirmation bias, is very common even outside of
mental illness, but in paranoid states it is ubiquitous (Broome et al., 2007; Nickerson,
1998). Most people with face-to-face experience of schizophrenic and bipolar type I
patients recognise this syndrome and recall patients who may be standing there, wearing
two left shoes, claiming that they have re-cognised Einstein’s theory of relativity. The
experience’). At the same time their bottom-up deficiency makes them oblivious to the
Where a patient’s delusions and hallucinations are often a worry for other people, they
patient is sure about what’s going on and why (even when they’re wrong) (Bergman et
al., 2012). This knowledge may not be much use to negotiate the world, but it does add
and delusions should not be treated as a problem, but as a coping strategy when
caveat is required. Any changes to safety, security and operational systems need to
reflect and parallel changes to the model of care. Furthermore, support from all
321
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
stakeholders should also be in place (Plsek & Wilson, 2001). There can be no doubt that
security and operational features are essential for the function of a psychiatric facility
and arguments against their removal will include fears that opportunities for self-harm,
violence and vandalism may increase. Having said this, the possibility that safety
concerns are in conflict with health issues is also real (L. Bowers, Banda, & Nijman,
2010; Chrysikou, in this issue), and in the interests of good practice, must be discussed
with all stakeholders in a frank and open way. Some patients are not at risk of suicide,
and others are. Patients are individuals and so are their circumstances (For a review of
specific factors see L. Bowers et al., 2010). Some provisions that are essential for one
patient will be detrimental for another, and as such, a diversity of spaces and provisions
should be designed for. Alternatively, units should be customised for the individual
322
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
deficit signs is “at best modest… results have been largely disappointing” (Buckley &
Stahl, 2007, p. 93). This leaves a huge onus on alternative treatments, including
Converging lines of evidence suggest deficits of bottom-up attention are partly caused
by the built environment, but there’s little doubt that the same symptoms diminish an
architect’s toolkit to deal with it. Bottom-up attentional deficiencies mean that design
features may simply go unnoticed, and if they are, top-down attentional surpluses mean
that features may only be noted only because they feed on-going delusions. For
example, a red feature may be noticed, not because of the prominence of the colour in
the context, but because red is interpreted to mean power or some other such delusion
(Reina, 2010).
specifics of causal relationships that the built environment have with schizophrenia, but
evidence is already strong enough to justify assuming causality. In the same way, WHO
advises caution with mobile phones because they are linked to the recent glioma
epidemic, although the mechanism has not yet been identified (WHO & IARC, 2011).
323
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
There is still every possibility that the removal of psychosis agonists and provisions
intended to counteract deficit signs will not reverse schizophrenia, but because even
approach should be tested because it may yield remarkable results. Interventions may
324
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Unfortunately, there’s no easy guideline that will ensure good and beautiful results, and
just about any attempt to do so will incur extra costs in construction. This objective will
assess (Theeuwes et al., 2000) as are aesthetics, which are subjective. The objective of
aesthetic appeal in architecture has a long tradition – perhaps older than man’s ability to
build. It is the third of Vitruvius’ classical qualities of good building; “firmitas, utilitas,
venustas,” that is, the importance of the delight that architecture can evoke (Vitrivius,
circa 15BC).
325
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
tient reads the paper in one of the many abundantly green spaces at
by CPG).
326
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Environmental generosity
When bottom-up attention atrophies, synaptic plasticity is reduced. In other words, the
spatial cognition, information and logic handling, as well as other cognitive functions
that lead to dementia (Schultz & Dickinson, 2000; Stephan, Friston, et al., 2009).
Similarly, a socially and materially deprived environment has also been shown to cause
synaptic atrophy – at least in rats, which are easier to observe and test ethically than
humans (F. Hall et al., 1998). Whether these losses occur for humans or if they are
associated with the psychogenesis of mental illnesses is still unknown, but should be
considered. And any positive or neutral enrichment of the environment should improve
synaptic plasticity. If this proves to be the case, environmental richness will directly
assist recovery – once again, this is a hypothesis that is well worth testing.
327
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Figure 35: Boxed for native birds are attached to trees and the walls
328
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
spaces with positive affordances. Top-down attention is a mode that is concerned only
for engagement. For this reason, designers should focus on what positive activity and
entertainment the space can provide and they should avoid the opposite – negativity and
passive environments.
329
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
room, and the ones around it look into a meercat enclosure. A good
The biggest scale of architectural intervention will be at the level of typology. Typology
house, a bank that looks like a bank etc.) the main function of typology is information:
it informs people about the nature of the place they are visiting. As Kahn puts it, “rooms
suggest their use without a name” (Kahn, 1971). Strong typology simplifies
understanding, orientation and way-finding (Lynch, 1992). But not all typology is equal.
be given to types that may have negative meanings. Negative typologies may include
prisons, hospitals, schools, courts, psychiatric facilities, seclusion rooms and other
how people navigate and use the space. This becomes increasingly important as skills
and cognitive abilities atrophy. For best effect, space should be logical, non-repetitive
Opportunities for engagement also exist at a smaller scale. We don’t regularly engage in
walls, and although we do constantly engage with the floor, it’s only because the ground
beneath us is as ubiquitous as the prevalence of gravity. For the best part, we use the
to engage in architectural elements if they offered us something more, like if they have
to be negotiated in some way. Aalto had people engage with the floor by bringing it up
closer to the eye level of a seated person – he created step-down living spaces. Walls
would come alive with washable crayons and licence to scribble. It’s true that given a
chance, some paranoid patients will use the opportunity to write threats and draw
331
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
obscenities. But if these expressions were easily erased, even this kind of expression
may still be helpful – at least it shows engagement and is a distraction from more
We engage with objects, particularly interesting ones (is not engaging a synonym of
interesting?) more than we do with basic building elements. Our bodies are designed to
interact with movable objects and moving parts. Light switches, venetian blinds,
knickknacks and furniture are the sorts of ordinary moving objects in the built
environment that we regularly engage with. Architectural elements like these are
designed for our bodies and actively invite engagement. We fill our homes, hotels and
workplaces with such things – probably because they fortify mental well-being.
Ironically, the few places that are stripped of physical affordances are the places that
need them in abundance - prisons and psychiatric facilities - because such facilities are
designed with safety as a priority over healing. This is exemplified by the overuse of
anti-ligature devices - showerheads that don’t direct the water properly, doorknobs that
can’t be grasped, tap-less basins, and furniture that can’t be moved because it’s blind-
bolted to the floor. Institutional strip-lighting is recessed and secured and blinds are
often secured behind tempered glass. It is not unusual for anything that moves to be
controlled remotely by nurses. The intention behind the installation of these components
is clear; to restrict possible affordances for self-harm, but the result is that the
332
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Top-down attention (the relevant mode for the engagement in affordances) is driven by
et al., 2007; Heinz & Schlagenhauf, 2010). This means that psychotic patients have a
stronger drive to engage than healthy people do, even though the drive may be masked
by catatonic symptoms or deficit signs (Northoff et al., 2004; Sass & Parnas, 2001) The
page 307. In this context, the removal of physical affordances is predicted to only
frustrate patients and aggravate symptoms, if not the underlying pathology of the
disorder. Given the removal of positive affordances, negative intentions are likely to
become only more focused. It is important to note that even healthy people start having
hallucinatory and delusional experiences when all affordances are taken away
(Grassian, 1983; Weckowicz, 1957). To counter this, Osmond (1957, 1958) (an
experienced clinician and one of the leading authors on the subject of the design of the
music, to write etc. Now we should go further: design teams should actively think about
with relatively harmless but fun toys, sports equipment, drawing tools, opening
windows, doors that have a pleasing sound when closed, adjustable lighting, heating and
ventilation etc. Sensory rooms have already been found to be successful in a psychiatric
milieu for engaging patients and helping to manage undesirable behaviour (McGann).
333
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Conclusion
particularly difficult, but it’s subjective and there’s no established scientific method for
such assertions. Moreover, there will always be specific exceptions for particular
neutrality in the design of psychiatric facilities will only serve to amplify the negativity
‘neutral’ engagement – it’s either positive or negative. Thus exactly the same ‘neutral’
affordances can be taken either way, depending on whether circumstances are judged as
The image study described on page 307 used generic emotive photographs to solicit
emotional responses. Designers can also use the same methods to elicit specific
emotional responses – for example, use paintings or images with generically happy
themes rather than resort to the safety of artistic abstractions. Ulrich identified views of
trees as positive (Ulrich, 1991; Ulrich & Parsons, 1990), and current projects that
employ these concepts have been spectacularly successful (see Figure 34). The ability to
engage with plants to touch them and lie under them is likely to be an even stronger
facilities due to a risk of self-harm. Other architects have been experimenting with the
inclusion of animal enclosures in their hospitals, also with wonderful feedback (Figure
35 and Figure 36). But the beneficial potential of architectural care goes far beyond
334
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
trees, animals and emotive pictures. All positive affordances are likely to support the
recovery from all mental illnesses, where negative ones appear to make the conditions
worse. There are affordances everywhere, seats to sit on, apples to eat, windows to
open, books to read. Most of these are positive, but care should be taken, because
symbolic encoding (eg. the thirteenth room along a corridor) or linguistic associations
(eg. the association between Lucifer and Lucite; a brand of transparent acrylic used in
skylights) etc.
Any opportunities that engage in personal choice are particularly important because
they will contribute to a sense-of-self. But the most elusive opportunities are those that
will have the strongest beneficial effect (if any effect can be evoked at all) – sublimity
and abundant beauty. The response to unintended delight is one of the most profound
atrophies in schizophrenia and one, what’s more, that is currently untreatable using
pharmacological interventions.
335
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Abstract
by the search for neurological imbalances and lesions, for genetic factors. The growing
consensus that these directions have failed, means there is a growing interest in
factors.
In the flood of data that is being produced around the schizophrenia epidemic, one of
demographic factors have been discounted, between a quarter and a third of all
incidence can still be traced to urbanicity. This prospect has been taken to threaten the
336
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
This paper explains that the environment is an inextricable factor in all the above
models, and sketches out the arguments for the built environment to be considered as a
valid epidemiological factor. It maps all these models and demonstrates how they
The reason the built environment hasn’t already become a de rigueur area of
epidemiological research is possibly trivial – it just doesn’t attract enough science, and
Ever since John Snow successfully combined statistics and mapping to identify the
Broad Street pump as the source of London’s cholera epidemic of 1854, attempts have
been made to do the same for other illnesses. Like the Broad St. Pump study, the
research of Faris and Dunham into the incidence of insanity in Chicago also found a
locus of concentration – the inner city slum area (Faris & Dunham, 1939). But neither
pump, nor pathogen was found. Instead, the authors pointed to various ‘breeder’ factors:
race, migration status, poverty and access to sunlight. To make sure this was not a case
of the ‘cum hoc ergo procter hoc’ fallacy (for confusing correlation with cause), Faris
and Dunham gave evidence to counter the ‘drift effect ‘ caused by downward (affected)
social mobility. They found that parents of the ‘insane’ are equally likely to inhabit the
337
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
slums. The possibility that upward (unaffected) social mobility out of these areas may
increase concentrations was not raised for many years (H. L. Freeman & Alpert, 1986).
338
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
339
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Living Geographic
Cholera
environment environment
Figure 38: The epidemiology of the Broad St Pump charted the urban
(in this case the public infrastructure; the streets and the pump itself)
The mapped areas are marked here in grey, and the finding – the
lar, but knowledge of the contaminated water table and the way the
340
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
social environment clustered around it was only made clear after the
Nearly a century later, similar methods are still being used to identify similar findings,
albeit with better controls for drift. But even with new methodological approaches,
different cohorts, studies are still identifying lack of sunlight and social dysfunction as
‘risk factors’. The problem is that effect sizes are universally small and therefore cannot
identify causality. The multifactor hypothesis attempts to explain how small effect ‘risk
factors’ will compound to cause the illness (van Os et al., 2010). In some instances,
ecogenetic (genetic x ecological factors (van Os et al., 2008)). This involves the
(age being another factor which is at once social, biological and environmental). All
people have the COMT gene, which is expressed in one of three functional states:
Met/Met, Val/Met and Val/Val. Whichever functional state a person inherits makes very
When isolated, cannabis use also represents a relatively small risk factor for adult
schizophreniform disorders (OR 1.13, CI 95%). But individuals with the Val/Val
polymorphism were found to have a high risk factor (OR 10.9, CI 95%), if they were a
cannabis user during adolescence. This contrasts to the Val/Met variation (OR 2.5, CI
341
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
the disorder (OR 1.1, CI 95%), a risk factor that was statistically insignificantly lower
As the authors of the COMT x cannabis x age study (quoted above) made explicit in
their findings, statistical analyses that are used to identify causal relationships are at risk
of cum hoc ergo propter hoc (correlation or false cause being taken as cause) fallacies
there is an increasing obligation to link data with biological mechanisms that are known
glutamate neurons. Scientists often go further, with animal studies that inevitably
schizophrenia. The fact that there are dozens of such models, and each quite different
should be alarming (Kilts, 2001; Marcotte, Pearson, & Srivastava, 2001). This allows
apparently to prove the hypothesis. These strategies don’t disprove the hypothesis, but
Schizophrenia animal model, which was generated apparently to justify the Vitamin D
McGrath, Saari, et al., 2004). This model manifests “abnormal motor responses to
psychomimetic agents… and (b) cognitive deficits” (Eyles et al., 2009, p. S252).
Attempts to reverse engineer schizophrenia in animals run the risk of complicating data
with comparison fallacies; the diagnosis of human schizophrenia is subtle and great care
342
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
has to be taken that it is not confused with other human psychoses (American
Psychiatric Association, 1994), much less with induced psychoses of other mammals –
even if they are primates. Furthermore, these strategies do not protect against other
biases, because inclusion criteria must be chosen prior to a study being undertaken.
These inclusions will be based on what is already known or suspected, and on what is
readily measurable. This means that larger factors may well be ignored because they
haven’t yet been considered, are difficult to control, to identify, justify or are not prone
One of the most widely replicated data in schizophrenia incidence is the influence of
and when controlled for other known factors, meta-analyses of epidemiological data
show the influence of the urban environment as place of birth turns increases the odds
cases once genetic factors have been factored out (Kelly et al., 2010). This does not
have to be taken as evidence that the urban environment has an effect on schizophrenia
dismiss the automatic scepticism that abounds when this possibility is addressed.
Personally, I hypothesize that the urban environment has no effect on schizophrenia, per
se – but rather that it exerts a negative salutogenic influence – in other words, it fails to
343
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
reason to reject the possibility that all other factors may also be subject to
multiplication. This is speculated to occur in a social context, for instance, where risk
factors for schizophrenia such as low IQ, poor hearing or immigration status are
multiplied by social discrimination (Selten & Cantor-Graae, 2007)32. There are other
areas where multi-factor compounding takes place, which may be obscure because of
information biases. Of these, on occasion when it has been studied, the urban landscape
has consistently and evenly been shown to have a dose-dependent, raised incidence of
schizophrenia (Krabbendam & van Os, 2005). This appears to be causal because urban
birth and upbringing precedes the development of the disorder (Krabbendam & van Os,
2005).
The effects of many urban factors have been noted to be significant; social cohesion
(Kirkbride et al., 2008; Selten & Cantor-Graae, 2007), the quality of urban fabric
(Curtis, 2008), urban density (Curtis, 2008), the ethnic makeup of the area (Coid et al.,
2008), the geographical location (Torrey, Mortensen, Pedersen, Wohlfahrt, & Melbye,
32
Please note that the evidence of a poor IQ is weak – a recent meta-analysis appears to
the development of the syndrome – 93% of 1007 subjects had good/fair high school
344
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
2001) etc., but like genetic factors on their own, each factor has a relatively small effect
The urban environment is a melting pot for many external factors, implying that multi-
factor relationships take place in the urban milieu. But the living environment (home)
provides the principal filter for the ecological effects of urbanicity and the factors that
urbanicity draw together: society, civic services, poverty, atmosphere, geography and
collective identity. The living environment is also – and possibly more importantly, the
melting pot for internal factors because it is the primary milieu for self-development and
the case in schizophrenia. These personal development factors are very difficult to
measure directly, but they have already been implicated in the expression of
‘urbanicity’ is a proxy for the built environment – and whether the etiological influence
of the urban setting may be as it is mediated by the architectures of home, or work and
345
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Historical Atmospheric
pathology environment
346
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Schizophrenia
Figure 40: Factors that are mapped for the social defeat hypothesis
(competition) and personal factors (use of illicit drugs, low IQ10, hear-
ing impediments).
347
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Genetic Personal
factors factors
Biological
Schizophrenia
factors
Atmospheric
environment
Figure 41: Factors that are mapped for the vitamin D hypothesis pin-
ern European winters). They also take account of some genetics (UV
sunlight, the most universal supply of ultra violet-B spectrum of light (UVB), which is
348
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
the precursor for vitamin D synthesis. For this reason, theorists predicted that people
born in the northern winters, where and when there is virtually no sun, would make
them susceptible to schizophrenia later in life. The hypothesis also suggests that the
high incidence of schizophrenia among immigrants from Africa and the Caribbean is
because the skin colour of these immigrants is dark and therefore reflects more UVB. In
addition, in the cold European climates, the reduction of UVB is compounded by the
need to wear heavier clothing. Other factors that affect vitamin D supply are dietary,
that is, they follow the consumption of fish and mushrooms (which is naturally high in
dietary vitamin D,) but also of vitamin D supplements (Kinney et al., 2009).
There’s no doubt, these associations tend to concur with the vitamin D deficiency
hypothesis once outliers are excluded. But despite the linear graphs that Kiney et al
present, the relationships are nonlinear and are marked by exceptions. The exceedingly
high schizophrenia clusters in North West Ireland, where the risks at (17.4/1000) are
among the highest in the world, appears to be one such exception (D. Freeman, 1994;
Torrey, 1987). This population is presumably largely fair skinned and has a strong
fishing industry (The Irish Department of Agriculture Fisheries and Food, 2009). It is
speculated that exceptions like these must be complicated by increased genetic risks of
an unidentified nature (i.e.. not skin colour genes) (Torrey et al., 2001). Other secondary
elaborations that are used to round up the outliers (frequently by some of the same
authors) is exposure to the feline parasite, toxoplasma gondi (Kinney et al., 2009;
Torrey, Bartko, & Yolken, 2012; Yolken, Dickerson, & Fuller Torrey, 2009) and
et al., 2009). Genetics, infections and parasites are just a few of many factors that also
appear to create a predisposition for schizophrenia that have no clear relationships with
vitamin D. Poverty may have a lot to do with vitamin D deficiency, but surely this is
very speculative.
Architectural factors are no less fuzzy. There are many architectural factors that may
seriously as fish consumption, and perhaps more seriously after all, the entire
schizophrenia aetiology and we should look seriously at all factors that show high
incidence rates. What about the quality and tenure of housing, workplace and facilities
for leisure – what direction do buildings face? Do they collect sunlight or reject it? Is
the air quality good or is it ionised or polluted? Are surfaces hard or soft? Are living
places comfortable or not? There are other ecological factors too; Are these
environments prone to fungal or bacterial growth? Are pets, vermin or other animals
present? And the quality of interior light: does the spectrum include UV? Is it bright
enough to trigger cones, or do people get by only using rod-vision? With all of these
other factors, add the effects of varying amounts of daylight, of temperature and
humidity, and the question of how clusters occur become very difficult to resolve.
al. undertook a study of vitamin D deprived rats. The researchers took a population of
pregnant rats and removed Vitamin D from their diets and the diets of their pups. The
350
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
researchers were careful that the lighting was balanced, (light for 12 hours/day, dark for
Other rats were kept in similar conditions, but were fed with vitamin D supplements
(Eyles et al., 2009). The results of this study were impressive, with the rats developing
peculiar behaviours that are speculated to be ‘schizophrenic like.’ But the assumption
ventricle size are not diagnostic criteria. Certainly they are common correlates – and
may even be directly due to vitamin D deficiency, but they, like so many other
correlations are tendencies that are not ubiquitous (Rosa et al., 2010). Psychosis is but
one set of defining symptoms, and one that is excluded if there is no marked decrease in
social function (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). The rats that were subject to
this study had no noticeable social dysfunction (Eyles et al., 2009) and were therefore
definitively not schizophrenic. The study is once again suggestive, but proves only that
The complexity of data can be read a number of ways; either that the aetiology of
by (Muller & Dursun, 2010; Rutten & Mill, 2009; van Os et al., 2010; van Os et al.,
2008), alternatively the same variation may point be because we are close to the target,
351
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
but still a little way off. Surely John Snow also noticed increasing incidence of illness
and death as he approached Carnaby Street (Figure 37)? Compare Figure 40 and Figure
41 with the bigger picture presented in Figure 39. High, but not absolute correlations are
expected from the social environment and from biological factors, but unless they show
epiphenomenal and co-morbid effects. If this is the case, the aetiology of schizophrenia
may still be parsimonious and singular but somewhere that hasn’t been studied properly.
The physical built environment is sufficiently complex to be such an illusive target. Just
because it cannot be as easily unravelled and analysed as the human genome and it can’t
be put into a lab and studied under microscopes doesn’t mean it’s not the bull’s-eye
The effect the physical environment has on human psychology is often overlooked as a
causal factor. The physical environment has a powerful influence on the social milieu
demographic, personal and other ecological influences. In fact, the physical milieu has
an influence on nearly every factor that has ever been targeted in the search for an
aetiology of schizophrenia so far – the primary exception being genetics. This idea has
been scoffed at in the past33, and to this day, receives little scientific attention. The
33
Specifically, Professor John McGrath’s answers to my question after his presentation
352
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
reasons for this are twofold; Firstly it is difficult to control environmental conditions for
reach outside of the traditional domain of psychiatry and into architecture, non-clinical
Evidence of the etiological effect of the environment is in its infancy, but already
preliminary findings are no less coherent than those of alternative hypotheses – and are
possibly better. My paper ‘Lost in space: The role of the architectural milieu in the
aetiology and treatment of schizophrenia.’ (Q.V.) starts to show some correlations and
possible mechanisms.
The statistical variables raised by the vitamin D case can be explained by the physical
to the sun because of being indoors. Even in a brightly lit milieu, light through glass is
filtered of most UV. The idea that vitamin D deficiency is an indicator of environmental
factors supported by other data – statistical data gathered in various boroughs of South
London show extreme variation in the incidence of schizophrenia. From one borough to
another contiguous one, incidence may increase by several times (Kirkbride, Fearon, et
al., 2007). Even accounting for ethnicity and other demographic data, the increased odds
ratio remains extreme (Kirkbride et al., 2008; Kirkbride, Fearon, et al., 2007; Kirkbride
353
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The same data can be used as evidence of a completely different hypothesis. And one
neighbourhoods, with overworked and stressed parents and too much babysitting by the
TV. But in spite of this variation, environmental poverty, like schizophrenia, can be
found even in the most salubrious homes and well-manicured suburbs, in countries
where sunshine – and therefore vitamin D, is bountiful. This paper poses the question:
further implies (but doesn’t mean) a lack of stimulation? This would mean that the
have a close relationship, but like vitamin D, the correlation reflects an impressive
tendency, but still not a one to one relationship. The studies find the children of
migrants have particularly high (Beebe et al., 2005) incidence of schizophrenia in later
life. The data of (J. J. McGrath et al., 2010) suggests that this may be due to maternal
and neonatal Vitamin D deficiency in the ‘new country’ (Eyles et al., 2009). Whilst this
is a very worthy argument, the same data may mean that the kids are brought up with
little more than a TV for company and stimulation while the parents try to get ahead in
354
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Schizophrenia is strongly associated with poor lifestyle, but the question about whether
poor lifestyle is the product of schizophrenia or predisposed people to it dates right back
McGrath asks “Is it time to trial Vitamin D supplements to prevent schizophrenia?” (J.
J. McGrath, 2010) To this question, the answer must surely be yes, but this doesn’t have
to be at the expense of taking care that the environments in which we rear our children
355
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
There is a prevalent normative view that the physical stuff of existence – literally the
bricks and mortar around us has little to do with mental illness. This is a leftover from
Descartes, who argued that the mind and body were intrinsically separated (Bolton,
In retrospect I have one problem with this article: the principle data source I cite
(Northoff et al., 2004), doesn’t differentiate between bipolar and schizophrenia when
the symptoms these syndromes express are the same (i.e.. Paranoia, catatonia). Whilst
this is reasonable, I regret this lack of specificity and strongly suspect that schizophrenic
automatic behaviours will not reflect the same negative bias. My reason is that the
healthy controls (S. B. Perlman et al., 2012; Watson et al., 2012), whereas in
Regardless, the paper is what it is: a useful tool for heuristically understanding the
357
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
10.1016/j.mehy.2011.09.029).
Abstract
The symptoms of psychiatric illness are diverse, as are the causes of the illnesses that
cause them. Yet, regardless of the heterogeneity of cause and presentation, a great deal
to act. It has also been found that perception automatically causes actions and thoughts
to occur unless this primary action pathway is inhibited. Inhibition allows perceptions to
this kind takes place over the entire frontal lobe and it renders action optional. Choice
about what action to take (if any) is the basis for the feeling of autonomy and ultimately
for the sense-of-self. When thoughts and actions occur automatically (without choice)
they appear to originate outside of the self thereby providing prima facie evidence for
358
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
These may not be noticed if they are neutral and therefore unimportant whereas actions
and thoughts with a positive bias are desirable. Responses to negative stimulus, on the
other hand, are always unwelcome, because the actions that are triggered will carry the
negative bias.
Automatic thoughts may include spontaneous positive feelings of love and joy, but
only do these feel like they emerge from elsewhere but they carry a negative bias (they
Automatic positive actions may include laughter and smiling and these are welcome.
Automatic behaviours that carry a negative bias, however, are unwelcome and like
359
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
INTRODUCTION
Psychiatric illness may strike anywhere and in any demographic. And even though
some syndromes are somewhat treatable, they are often utterly debilitating. For
generations researchers and clinicians have been attempting to grapple with these
syndromes. The effort is impressive, and with more than 7000 peer-reviewed articles for
schizophrenia alone being published per year, (Schizophrenia Research Forum, 2011)
there is no shortage of high quality empirical data. But like the data, most hypotheses
relate to a small aspect of a single syndrome – a single symptom perhaps. Very few
diverse conditions. Take imitation behaviour for instance. This is common in conditions
Psychotic disorders:
360
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Ecological perception
Until mirror neurons were first explained using the ecological theory of perception,
(Gibson, 1979; Rizzolatti, Fabbri-Destro, & Cattaneo, 2009) the theory had no traction
at all in neuroscience or medicine, even though the theory has robust support and is
relatively well accepted in the field of perceptual psychology. (Bargh & Dijksterhuis,
2001) But mirror neuron theory remains a relatively isolated curiosity in medicine,
where naïve belief in serial and qualia oriented perception still dominates. As long as
this belief is maintained, it is hard to imagine that the Descartian divide between the
studies of ‘mental’ psychotic states and ‘physical’ stereotypies will be bridged or that
The ecological theory claims that perception is action. (Gibson, 1979) A person doesn’t
just interpret sense-data (qualia) to compile an array of conclusions – the senses work
holistically to excite or inhibit the actions and thoughts that we find ourselves
opportunity to sit, the colour and shape of the chair may never even register.
361
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
transform) perception/action. The ability to choose how we react forms the basis for
autonomy (as much as there is such a thing.) (Searle, 2001) Simple creatures have no
such discretion. For them perception does not suggest nor demand behaviour, it is
indistinguishable from behaviour. A frog has no choice but to eat a moving object of a
certain size, and a barnacle has no choice but to stick to a hard surface, at which point it
Just as a cue for action is automatic, so too is self-control, at least for animals that have
a frontal lobe which is developed enough to enable self-control. (Bargh & Dijksterhuis,
behaviour with an equal opposite. In most cases, reprocessing is so well balanced that
people will not realize when they have ‘acted’ or ‘behaved’ (refrained from action).
People sometimes notice after the fact: for example when they have just swallowed the
strawberry that was meant to decorate a cake. People may also recognize the impulse
‘telling’ them to jump when they reach a cliff’s edge, but equally so, they recognize the
impulse to self-control: the recoil of alarm at the thought of plummeting of a cliff, or the
guilty thought: ‘I couldn’t resist,’ regarding the strawberry for the cake. As Gibson
points out, the ecological theory makes sense of impulses triggered by perception:
“Fruit says ‘eat me’”. (Gibson, 1979, p. 140) And a cliff says, “jump!”
Because the laws of physics govern action, they should also govern ecological
362
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
untransformed actions should reflect these qualities. A raucous positive action may be
social milieu, but the reprocessing of a positive stimulus need only be partial. Likewise
for neutral stimulus – action resulting from positive or neutral stimulation will rarely be
harmful and unwanted. Negative stimulus, on the other hand, needs complete
transformation, lest unwanted, unintended and unmoderated actions and thoughts occur.
Although organic brain damage (particularly to the frontal lobe) may prevent
moderated by the dopamine system (Fletcher & Frith, 2009) also appears to be in place
behaviour free rein. (Cowan, 2005) Because automatic processes are fast and accurate,
the evolutionary purpose for this bypass function is presumably to allow much faster
aware of their behaviour or not. Because all unmoderated actions originate outside of
the autonomous domain, they will appear to originate elsewhere. If primarily physical,
continual automaticity will erode a sense-of-self, because actions are genuinely not
autonomous. The primary exceptions being cases of severe frontal damage, where the
sense-of-self cannot exist at all (Northoff & Bermpohl, 2004; Northoff et al., 2006), but
neither can choice (Lhermitte, 1986). Aside from these extreme situations, many of the
bizarre beliefs and experiences that are common among psychiatric patients, and
characterise schizophrenia are related to the loss of autonomy of thought and action.
When automatic reactions are predominantly physical, they will be classed as catatonic
action and thought, the paranoid classification is most appropriate (DSM-IV 295·30). If
may be the diagnosis (DSM-IV 295·70). Thus, one solution addresses all the primary
364
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Evidence
Reprocessing of action/perception is thought to occur over the entire frontal lobe of the
brain. This area is subject to decreased connectivity in schizophrenia (Das et al., 2007),
and is also the primary site for the processing of creativity, choice (Dietrich & Kanso,
2010) and the sense-of-self (Northoff & Bermpohl, 2004; Northoff et al., 2006). The
study of fontal lesions showed that 100% eventually eventuated in imitation behaviour
or a more severe disorder involving loss of autonomy (4%, n=1), (n=29). (At the time
the study was conducted, there was one exception, whom presented with headaches.
This patient developed imitation behaviour shortly after the study period.) (Lhermitte et
al., 1986)
(Northoff et al., 2004). In this study, akinetic catatonic patients (DSM-IV 295·20, n=3;
295·30, n3; bipolar 1 DSM-IV 296·54, n7) and healthy controls where exposed to
emotionally positive, neutral and negative stimulus in the form of pictures (from the
International Affective Picture System) while undergoing fMRI scans of their entire
frontal lobes. For positive and neutral stimulus, all subjects showed processing
imbalances (eccentricity) where excitation (+) exceeded inhibitory reprocessing (-). For
negative stimulus, however, only the psychiatric cohorts showed any eccentricity. They
were all unable to balance the negative impact of the stimulus over the areas the frontal
365
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
lobe (the orbitofrontal, anterior cingulate, medial prefrontal, lateral prefrontal, premotor
and motor cortices). In contrast, healthy controls did this nearly perfectly
automatic behaviours, but this does require further testing using experiments based on
the model developed by Northoff and his colleagues (Northoff et al., 2004). At this
experiments, care should be taken to monitor for physical tics and mannerisms. To see
It appears that any dysfunction within the frontal cortex (regardless of specific region)
will cause automatic and unintended action (imitation behaviour or a more severe
disorder also involving the loss of autonomy). But the frontal cortex must still be largely
delusions. Without a functional frontal cortex it is unlikely that self-reflection can occur
at all, although this doesn’t mean that automatic expressions of happiness and sadness
are disingenuous. On the contrary, insincerity is one of the inhibitory functions that are
debilitated. The declarative experiences of patients are likely to be more genuine than
those of healthy individuals who are better equipped to voluntarily moderate their
366
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
behaviour and take ownership of it. In many ways this explains some of the most vexing
questions about psychotic behaviour. When patients blame others for making the world
a bad place, their delusions are very genuine. And a diminished sense-of-self may easily
turn to selflessness and when actions are unpredictably automatic, sometimes the results
Not only does this hypothesis predict much of the bizarre experience and behaviour
inhibitory processes in the frontal lobe are insufficient to balance excitatory impulses, it
would follow that the primary inhibitory neurotransmitter within the frontal lobe (ϒ-
amniobutyric acid – GABA) will reflect this with moderate down-regulation, moderate,
because only negative impulses are properly balanced in healthy adults and also because
evidence seems to point this way, but has hitherto remained unexplained. (Li,
It is hoped that this hypothesis will allow scientists and doctors to understand some of
their science in a more human way. To understand that an excited neuron has a corollary
action, and that the Newtonian laws even apply to the minds we live in.
367
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Symptom Explanation
DELUSIONS
Delusions of control, thought insertion Automatic (non-autonomous) actions, esp.
delusions and other misattribution delusions when experienced alongside hallucinations.
Grandiose delusions Hallucinations with positive bias.
Paranoid delusions Perception with negative bias due to
Somatic delusions improperly reprocessed negative stimulus
(Figure 42 and 43).
Delusions associated with automatic (non-
autonomous) body functions and hallucinated
experience.
HALLUCINATIONS
Voices Automatic (non-autonomous) thoughts, esp.
Visual hallucinations ones with negative bias.
Automatic (non-autonomous) visualisation,
esp. images with negative bias.
DISORGANISED SPEECH
Disorganised thought, derailment, Automatic thoughts interfere with normal
tangentality, communication difficulties, alogia sequence of logic and narrative structure.
GROSSLY DISORGANISED BEHAVIOUR
Disorganised behaviour, attention deficits, Automatic actions and thoughts interfere with
agitation, stereotypy, perseveration, normal sequence of behaviour.
behavioural monitoring peculiarities, agitation,
inappropriateness, silliness
NEGATIVE SIGNS
Affective flattening Attempts to reprocess negative stimulus
Negativity indiscriminately causes flattening. Some
positive automaticity is lost with the negative.
(See the reduced positive reactivity in Figure
42)
Incomplete negative reprocessing causes
negative bias.
CATATONIC SYMPTOMS
Catatonia and other deficit signs Evidence demonstrates that these symptoms
are caused by greater affective impact of
action/perception alongside a strong
reprocessing eccentricity. See figures 42 and
43 (Northoff et al., 2004).
Table 5: Most of the symptoms and signs listed in the DSM-IV for
368
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
1484
Amplitude
-796
-1595 -1463
C P H
Negative stimulus
369
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Excitatory Eccentricity
0
C P H
Neg 1212 1385 21
Pos 598 470 881
Neu 812 539 198
Eccentricity (all stimulus)
Figure 43: Overall eccentricity caused by all stimulus: Note the in-
ative stimulus and are able to ‘express’ positive responses, the oppo-
et al., 2004).
371
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
PSYCHIATRIC VIOLENCE
The concept presented in Medical Hypotheses 2012 can be used to understand not only
bizarre behaviour, but also psychiatric violence. The following paper was written for a
special issue of the APA journal, Psychiatric Violence, on technology and violence. It
was rejected principally because it was the only article that didn’t conceive technology
in terms of computers. The paper was subsequently reviewed by the journal and
returned for changes. I haven’t had the chance to make these changes and try again.
372
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Abstract
The reflexive violence model proposes that perceptual stimuli trigger unwanted actions,
hallucinations and delusions. The model is based on one of the central tenets of the
perceiver) are cues to act. As an apple triggers a desire to eat, a gun triggers a desire to
shoot. These affordances (as they are called) are part of the perceptual apparatus, they
allow the direct recognition of objects – and in emergencies they enable the fastest
possible reactions.
Even under normal circumstances, the presence of a weapon will trigger inhibited
violent impulses. The presence of a victim will also, but under normal circumstances,
affordances of this kind don’t turn into violence because negative action impulses are
concurrently totally inhibited whereas in psychotic illness – and possibly in other mental
373
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
illnesses also, negative affordances are treated as emergencies, and then far exceeds the
inhibition threshold, and what would have been recognition becomes blind, automatic
actions.
lesions can cause the inhibitory impulse to be exceeded. At it’s most innocuous, this
misattributed), but the act of perception may have the power to trigger very serious
violent acts also. This kind of violence is devoid of motives or any planning and is often
374
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
As part of an observational study, two patients were invited (separately) into the
apartment of their doctor, the investigator for the study. The patients were to be exposed
to a few tableaus (a made bed, a sideboard laid out with food etc.) and their behaviour –
housewife, was presented with was a syringe and vial of saline. Without hesitation, she
The other patient, a man this time, was a 51-year-old engineer was presented with a
painting sitting on the ground. A hammer and nail were nearby. The patient used the
tools to hang the painting. The patient was led into a bedroom. He stripped and hopped
into bed. A gun lay out on a tabletop. When the patient spotted it, he headed to it with
an expression of sheer delight. The patient picked it up and spun the barrel. There were
no cartridges, so he searched until he found some, then he loaded the gun… and the
experiment had to be called off. The doctor had to intervene to confiscate the weapon.
The patent was not angry, he had no prior homicidal intentions but would have had no
choice but to shoot the doctor just as the woman has no choice but to inject him with
saline. The gun was ‘telling’ the man to shoot (Lhermitte, 1986). Neither patient
suffered delusions because they lacked the self-reflexive criticality required to consider
their actions. What they did experience, however, were simple ‘non-bizarre’
375
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
hallucinations. Somehow, these inanimate objects were able to speak and with enough
authority to motivate action: their instructions to act came from thoughts that arose
The issues of interpersonal and (self-inflicted) violence are a wicked problems (Kazdin,
solutions for an open number of stakeholders, and any number of other confounding
embed other wicked problems (Kunz & Rittel, 1972). One of these is the problem of
psychiatric violence. At some point violence may become the product of a psychiatric
illness, and not of rationally considered alternatives (Alderman, 1916). This article
attempts to shed light on some of the processes involved and normative explanations for
them so that psychiatric violence can be better understood and identified more readily
and to ascertain to what extent violence is compos mentis (in right mind).
Although the issues of psychiatric violence are definitively wicked, current debate about
focuses on delusional intentions – not only is this a single and relatively contained
perspective, but it is also a case of ignoratio elenchi logic, because it leads nowhere. To
date, there is no cogent and well-accepted hypothesis for delusions, and of the strongest
to be their common source – for a review see (Garety & Freeman, 1999).
376
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The delusory intentions hypotheses are nevertheless relatively well accepted (De Pauw
& Szulecka, 1988; Richard-Devantoy et al., 2008), despite being poorly developed – if
anything there is evidence that delusional patients don’t act on their delusional beliefs
See review (A. Buchanan, 1993), but rather that delusions occur to explain actions that
are otherwise inexplicable (Startup et al., 2008). This is a parallel of Bem’s self-
perception theory: ‘Individuals come to “know” their own attitudes, emotions, and other
internal states partially by inferring them from observations of their own overt
behaviour and/or the circumstances in which this behaviour occurs.’ (Bem, 1972, p. 2).
sense of entitlement; and altruistic narratives, where violence is regrettable, but believed
al., 2009; Volavka et al., 2011). In these paradigms the delusionary motivations for
violence are considered to be a product of psychiatric illness, but the mechanism that
turns perception into action is not questioned. In this paper, the case for violence as a
least concurrent with any subsequent delusional ideation, because the technology is not
just a means, but part of the very hallucinatory experience that drives delusional
narratives.
378
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
There is a long history of debate about how people (and animals) perceive, and models
abound. I refer readers to some of the literature, but will not enter the debate in this
paper. For reviews on the many models and their strengths and weaknesses, see
(Baldwin, 2007; Clark, 2012 - in press; Golembiewski, in review-d) and perhaps a text
book also. Instead, this article hypothesizes about the implications of the ecological
theory of perception (one of the prominent theories of perception) when applied to the
Despite this weakness, the theory has demonstrated strengths for understanding how
recognizable objects and opportunities are identified (Bargh & Dijksterhuis, 2001;
2010).
The concept of ecological perception is that objects and opportunities are recognized by
the actions they enable. An object is recognized by the thoughts that commence as part
of the process of perception; once it is familiar, a syringe is not recognized by its colour
enables. Equally an apple says eat me (Gibson, 1979), and a gun says shoot me. In
humans and animals with more developed frontal cortices, these impulses are not
379
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
and physical contexts as suggested by the milieu (Proshansky, Ittelson, & Rivlin, 1972).
system, but a desire/action management system, and is thus more visceral. It is a stab of
hunger or a proto-desire for a taste that causes the recognition of food – not an analysis
of available sense data. This means impulses will progress into action if a decision to
engage with an object is ever made. In healthy people and more evolved animals,
decisions are made at the moment of recognition whether to engage, and accept the
altogether. Impulses are limited by context (the behaviour setting, and further by
p.373 and p.204 q.v.). Simple creatures such as a barnacle that have no frontal cortex
have no choice but to act according to their perceptions (Bargh & Dijksterhuis, 2001).
perspective (Withagen et al., 2012). Objects that contain such opportunities and thus
demand action are called affordances. (Bargh & Dijksterhuis, 2001; Gibson, 1979).
380
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
continuous, so too are affordances (Clark, 2012 - in press). All animals engage in
affordances because they are always on the lookout to recognize them. One is connected
Affordances will build on one another. In the study above, the doctor offered a
complimentary affordance to the syringe and the gun – because given the context of one
status changed from doctor to target only once the syringe or gun had been
Reflexive violence is hypothesized to occur when the constant stream of action and
thought that we humans are continually engaged in, is completely co-opted by a series
of automatic negative affordances, when either in a setting that allows such action, or
(more likely) when there are deficits of the automatic inhibition imposed by the
substance abuse.
381
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The patients with the gun and the syringe were showing symptoms of a spectrum of
utilization behaviour disorders, all of which erode personal autonomy. These are
one study, these were found to be ubiquitous in cases of frontal lesions; 100% of the 29
patients with frontal lesions that were studied developed these behaviours to some
degree (Lhermitte et al., 1986). 52% (n=15) of these patients developed utilization
cues from the milieu (Lhermitte, 1986); and imitation behaviour, where patients
automatically imitate the behaviour of others, for example, if they are watching
someone else putting on a pair of glasses, they will try to do so themselves, even if they
are already wearing glasses (Lhermitte, 1983). Only one patient at the time of the study
showed none of these syndromes, but he developed them three weeks after the study
was complete. These syndromes, bizarrely lacking in autonomy, are presumably caused
neurons of the frontal cortex have been severed (Golembiewski, in review-b, in review-
extreme cases, functionally identical to the perceptions of a barnacle, except for a much
larger and expandable ontology, and therefore more programmed reflexive actions.
382
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
connectivity is lost. The report of Lhermitte et al. (Lhermitte, 1986) also identifies other
conditions that sometimes cause utilization behaviour – these include various other
conditions. These are more complex than biological lesions, and are thus not strictly
speaking the same. In psychotic disorders at least, the inhibition of perceptual stimuli
appears to depend on the emotive nature of the stimuli, with percepts that have a
positive affective bias being over-inhibited and affectively negative ones being
patients, 10 more common psychiatric patients1 and ten healthy controls. All
participants were shown emotive images while undergoing fMRI scans of their frontal
lobes. The emotional bias of the images was generic and indisputable, regardless of
acculturation. Negative pictures included things like a mangled face, positive ones a
happy baby. A neutral condition was also added – it was a piece of grey card (Northoff
et al., 2004). Among other findings, it was noticed that in the negative condition healthy
controls showed equal inhibitory and excitatory activity, thus cancelling each other out.
Psychiatric patients, on he other hand, showed very low levels of inhibitory activation
circumstances (Golembiewski, 2012a) (see article from p.358- q.v.). Thus patients with
383
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
schizophrenia and other psychoses may also present with automatic and uncontrollable
behaviours, but only when the objects of perception are seen as negative
(Golembiewski, 2012a).
It may be difficult to assess what constitutes negativity, but affective qualities are often
relatively universal, despite the undeniable fact that subjective opinions are based on
culturally encoded schemata. And different schemas will evoke different responses - if
through words and deeds. Social expressions of negativity may include expressions of
whining, sadness, tension or fear (Carrere & Gottman, 1999). It’s important to note that
these affective cues are subtle and function on an automatic and precognitive level (if
cognition strictly speaking plays a role at all), and thus may never be declarative. Social
negative affect, for instance, is typically not declaratively noticed by participants, but
(SPAFF) (Gottman et al., 1996). Furthermore, with automatic actions, the time lag
between impulse and action is virtually non-existent, as such automatic actions cannot
210 homicides committed between 1975 and 2005. Of these 14 were identified as being
potentially psychiatric. Schizophrenic patients committed all, and all shared distinctive
features; none of the 14 homicides appear to have been premeditated, all were
384
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
committed alone, and all used available means (e.g. bare hands) (Richard-Devantoy et
al., 2009).
attacks appeared to occur unpredictably, suggesting that much domestic violence may
also be unpremeditated. The caveat here is that both the author of the study attributes a
motivation to this unpredictability; “Instilling the sense of fear that a physical attack is
possible at any moment is one way battering men control their partners” (Langford,
1996)p. 372). Like the observations of Richard-Devantoy et al. (2009), these attacks
available means – victims were ‘hit with objects such as telephones, lamps, chairs’
(Langford, 1996), p. 374). To link these assailants to the psychiatric ones further,
conditions, such as “psycho eyes” and degraded speech patterns (Langford, 1996), p.
376). Abnormal eye gaze and is not useful for diagnosis of psychotic disorders, but they
are sufficiently common that they have been the subject of many studies and are thus
Association, 1994).
385
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
If an affordance is charged with a negative affect, the chance the perceptual proto-
desires will turn to actions is increased in psychotic conditions. Psychosis doesn’t need
substance abuse (Caton et al., 2005), and in reality substance abuse is single the most
common factor of any violence outside of war (Langevin & Hardy, 1987; Langford,
1996).
It appears that automatic actions somehow bypass whatever it is that tags a sense-of-
self-agency, this may be experienced three ways; as a hallucination, where the action-
amnesia, where events cannot be remembered because they were never properly
rationalized (D. Freeman et al., 2002; Startup et al., 2008); much the same way as
individuals come to develop a self identity based on their own actions, functionally as if
they were outside observers (Bem, 1972). Indeed, detailed first-hand accounts of
delusions:
“A little girl said to her mother, ‘Is that man possessed by the Devil Mummy?’
Her mother also looked at me and replied, ‘Yes dear.’ This coincidence just
386
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
when I was thinking this very thought, was enough to prove (it to me)… I had to
make sense, any sense, out of all these uncanny coincidences. I did it by
Cases of amnesia may be more common than cases of delusions. In one London-based
While this data may have been confounded somewhat by criminals who wish to avoid a
guilty sentence, the high number is also reflected in various conditions that made such
some of these cases, the assailant handed themselves in, knowing they were prone
Unlike the lady with the syringe and the man with the gun, the negative charge provided
purpose, but only after or during the act because the motivation to act was never
intentional. It comes from recognition of the weapon or a victim, and didn’t exist any
earlier, yet actions are very convincing proof of intention even to the assailant. But
murdered, for no reason, one of his children with whom he had been on the best
387
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
schizophrenic, but more likely to be bipolar) grabbed a knife from a supermarket shelf,
and spontaneously decapitated a nearby tourist. He was apparently unprovoked and his
violence was almost certainly not premeditated. The only time the assailant had ever
met the tourist before was just a few moments earlier. The victim had complained to a
security guard about being harassed, and the assailant had been moved on. He
immediately returned to the supermarket and killed the tourist (Neild, 2011). Although
it is far from clear why the tourist was victimized, there’s no doubt that her presence
caused the assailant to become rapidly aroused. In this case, the convergence of the
negative affordance suggested by the by the tourist (‘target me’) and the no less horrific
negative affordance suggested knife (‘kill with me’) came together in a particularly
tragic way. The assailant’s delusions must have implicated the tourist in a very specific
way, because the he took her severed head out to the street and declared; “I am God's
avenger and I come to mete out justice!” The delusional narrative that this quotation
speaks to has a mythological quality – and draws on common culture, but nevertheless
must have developed with – or after, the events it related to –it couldn’t have developed
earlier, because the assailant and the victim had never met.
special (often encoded) meaning for the patient. These build quickly on themselves
(Coltheart et al., 2007). These become delusional narratives, which leave little
388
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
reasonable forensic evidence, even though they make complete sense of the
Random homicides by psychotic strangers are very rare (Nielssen et al., 2011), and for
this reason, data is poor, but in a cross sectional study of homicides between 1978 and
1983, 1418 homicides were analysed and 108 were identified as having been committed
Obviously sudden and impulsive violence doesn’t always involve strangers, but
circumstances that do involve strangers are particular because they usually lack any
(Langevin & Hardy, 1987). Other evidence that suggests that violent impulses are
somatogenic to the assailant come from cases where no reasonable motive can be found:
“…The couple were on the very best of terms. There had been occasional sexual
had seized her by the throat but had relaxed his grip without harming her. She
had agreed to forget the incident and to continue their acquaintanceship. On the
night of the murder they had spent the evening together listening to the radio at
his home… As the girl rose to go he suddenly seized her by the throat and
strangled her, placing the body under the bed and then notifying the police.”
389
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
“He was in love with the girl and wished to marry her… Having met the girl one
felled her with a heavy piece of wood and stabbed her in the neck with his clasp
The events of the Tenerife homicide appear to be qualitatively similar, as all these cases
(and may more) suggest that psychiatric homicides can occur by circumstance rather
than choice. Attacks are rarely remembered by the assailant (O'Connell, 1959; Stafford-
Clark & Taylor, 1949). If events are apparently remembered, what may appear to be a
homicide may well be a case in point as the statement, “I am God’s avenger and I come
to mete out justice!” implies. This statement appears to match the assailant’s actions
(carrying the victim’s head by her hair, while clutching a knife in the other hand), but
not the assailant’s intentions – because he clearly had none. Delusions are like a fire,
kindled out of perceived events, opportunities and objects. The availability of a knife in
the Tenerife case was not premeditated, and neither was picking it up. The opportunity
to use the knife was also involuntary, even as a delusional narrative may have been
The relationship between technology and action is direct. Even the most simple of
action. A barnacle drift in the ocean until it hits a hard surface that it will then attach
itself to (Bargh & Dijksterhuis, 2001). A hard surface is a very limited form of
390
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
technology, but as life becomes more complex, so too does the technology it can
engage. The sea eagle’s uses stones to break eggshells, and there are any number of
animals that will make good use of a hollow tree. Obviously for humans technology is
far more complex again, and human technology enables a potential range of affordances
that may be either specific (such as a custom part of an engine), or incredibly diverse
like a computer. But the complexity of human technology does not make it dissimilar to
a surface for a barnacle. Once the meaning of the object is known to the point that its
use is automated, it triggers action in very much the same way – although the barnacle
which given some conditions, isn’t even ubiquitous in humankind, much less in simple
life. Thus technology, once its function has been learned, is part of the process of
391
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
As the image of the assailant and his words testify, the homicide in
392
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
It appears that the knife used in the Tenerife homicide was not just a weapon. It served
an important role in the psychological process that led to the attack. In premeditated
violence, the technology involved is a tool that is chosen after a decision to commit the
act of violence is made. This presupposes a motive and a desire to cause harm, which
was clearly the case in Tenerife, where the alleged assailant had not met the victim so
much as ten minutes earlier. But technology, firearms in particular, do trigger violence
that otherwise would never have occurred, whether or not there is a psychiatric
condition. In the USA, about 40% of the deaths caused by firearms are accidental and
automatic reflexive inhibition in the frontal cortex is low for children (8-10yrs) and
lower still for adolescents (14-18yrs) (Luna et al., 2001). A picture begins to emerge,
where the perception of the opportunity that enables violence suggests violence. But
suggested by the weapon, but rather by the presence of a vulnerable victim. Violence of
this sort is sudden, unplanned and direct – usually the attack is done with bear hands
(Alderman, 1916; Langevin & Hardy, 1987; Richard-Devantoy et al., 2009). Although
these circumstances are more common, in either case – the violence emerges
393
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Discussion
The issue of violence is a broad subject, and only part of the whole problem will be
caused by reflexive action. This part is expected to be quite small overall, although it is
likely to explain most psychosis related violence. One of the features of schizophrenia
and type 1 bipolar disorder is that deliberation becomes very difficult, as is reflected in
the endless Wisconsin card sorting and Stroop tests that have been performed on
schizophrenic patients (Bora, Yücel, & Pantelis, 2010). The implication is that planning
violent action will be difficult in this condition. Furthermore, the conditions that make
reflexive action likely are present in these conditions (Golembiewski, 2012a). Beyond
this, becomes more difficult to further identify a group that suffers from reflexive
violence, except to say that these symptoms are likely to be far more common in
untreated psychosis, particularly first episode (about 80% of cases, in statistical meta-
analysis), when insight is poor (about 40% of cases) and when associated with self-harm
Several etiological options are presented here that may give rise to reflexive violence.
Of these, all are associated with reduced frontal inhibition, particularly when
forms of violence have been linked to cases of paedophilia, for instance, and in these
cases at least, frontal lesions are clearly evident in positron tomography studies. But it is
regarded as negative, offering some support for the reflexive action hypothesis, but this
support carries no external validity and low numbers of studies and subjects mean that
394
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
even internal validity is notional. Having said this, it is interesting to note that
utilization behaviour was found to be a common comorbidity along with all the
paraphilia’s including paedophilia etc. (Mendez, Chow, Ringman, Twitchell, & Hinkin,
2000).
Ultimately the greatest risk of reflexive violence is not for unknown victims, but for the
patient themselves. The reflex to jump, when at a cliff’s edge is noticeable, even for
many healthy people. And when an affordance to commit violence is observed, the fact
that schizophrenic patients spend most of their time alone means that they are most
395
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
PSYCHIATRIC SUICIDE
The dreams in which I'm dying are the best I've ever had
The issue of suicide in all mental illness is very pertinent to any consideration of the
psychiatric milieu. The suicide rates of schizophrenic patients have a standard mortality
ratio (as calculated from meta-analyses) internationally of around 8.5% of all those with
a diagnosis. This figure has an incredibly high variation: from 0.8 to 115 times the
standard mortality ratio. One of the reasons for variation is in the time the study is
taken. The rate is at its peak at the time of first medication and admission to psychiatric
treatment (Harris & Barraclough, 1997; Palmer et al., 2005). Other psychiatric
conditions are higher – Bipolar disorder at about 15%, and major depression 20.5%
(Harris & Barraclough, 1997). The figure for opportunistic suicide is unknown, but it is
396
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The cost of keeping patients from self-harm is very high. Psychiatric facilities go to
incredible lengths to keep patients physically safe – even if these efforts are at the cost
of the patients’ best interests. Anti-ligature fittings (fittings that people cannot possibly
hang themselves on) are normally standard, windows are fixed and louvers are behind
desperately nasty that they make the lights in any other facilities look positively
pleasant.
discussed in:
volume.)
397
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
FURTHER RESEARCH
I can’t say I left the big questions until last. But I have covered a lot of ground very
quickly, and sometimes the view has been obscured by my dust-cloud. One of the things
I missed during the research for this thesis is grounded research – that is, research
grounded in anything other than literature and the world of my own ideas. I didn’t spend
time with psychiatric patients and to ask them what they thought – well, that’s not
entirely true, but for ethical reasons, I cannot use the data I acquired this way in my
My reasons for not doing grounded research may have been valid: I figured that asking
patients would be unlikely to reveal especially useful information. Perhaps little more
than John Snow’s questions to his dying patients in the Barbican. Just because you are
afflicted, doesn’t mean you have all the answers – just ask someone who is suffering
from an undiagnosed allergy. The question itself can be frustrating! But even so,
398
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
The research itself has opened up several prospects. One of the projects I have
papers I have reviewed for it tells of this exact approach. Professor Nathan Perkins, of
the University of Guelph, in Ontario, Canada, his students and his colleagues did some
studies of facilities that were grounded in patient participation. Quite apart from the
social capital this approach engendered among patients, these studies identified nine
design imperatives, all of which in some way ground the salutogenic paradigm I have
Press). Although I doubt that you would have managed to get such comprehensible
answers from regressed schizophrenic patients, the idea of doing these grounded studies
is quite inspiring.
Looking back, I can see that the Medical Hypotheses and ‘Riddle of psychotic
perception’ articles both start to flesh out a much broader framework for understanding
mental illness than just schizophrenia alone. As such, I’m tempted to pursue the
There’s still a lot to be learned about healthcare settings, and mental health facilities, but
I’ve discovered that the method of approaching a subject laterally seems to work. I think
that a lot could be learned from in-depth research into forensic facilities, because I think
399
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
that a lot of the pain-points of psychiatric facility design are embodied by this quasi-
health and psychiatric facilities that are tailored to specific cultural archetypes. Some
time ago I started a dialogue with Paul Memmott at University of Queensland about
facilities for Aboriginal people with mental illness. Since then the conversation has
grown and others are getting involved. This may turn to something – who knows?
I find the ideas I have been sketching out (included in this thesis) on the aetiology of
schizophrenia deeply interesting and very important for architectural praxis and urban
design too. I’d like to resolve some of the mysteries surrounding this area too.
400
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Glossary of terms
following.
Activities of Daily Living (ADL): A normative set of daily tasks used to assess
patients in psychiatric care using ADL facilities like kitchens bathroms, telephones,
laundries etc.
Active attention: A term to describe the middle ground between bottom-up and top-
down attentional processes. It may be environment led, yet without the sharpness of
novelty. Active attention is where top-down intention directs a ‘fuzzy’ search or where
bottom-up attention reveals something that you had an interest in, but had not been
looking for. Active attention is not driven by saliency, and isn’t a mode of attention in
its own right, but complex combinations of bottom-up and top-down processes, which
are led by habits of engagement, by choice and simply by the availability of resources.
Aetiology (US. Etiology): The causal circumstances for a syndrome or illness beyond
the pathology or psychopathology. For cholera, the aetiology is how water came to be
401
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
infected and how people came to drink that water. The pathology of cholera relates
305-317.
Alogia: ‘An impoverishment in thinking that is inferred from observing speech and
language behavior. There may be brief and concrete replies to questions and restriction
adequate in amount but conveys little information because it is over concrete, over
Amygdala: The twin amygdalae are parts of the limbic system and are pared with the
a circumstance’s relevance to me. The amygdalae remain either intact or slightly under
engaged in schizophrenia (Anticevic et al., 2012; Becerril & Barch, 2010). This
402
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
contrasts with affective psychoses like bipolar disorder, in which a large body of
Anterior cingulate Cortex (ACC): a region of the frontal cortex, between the corpus
callosum and the prefrontal cortex. It is well situated between the striatum and the
prefrontal cortex thought to mediate deliberative action, and attention. The dopamine
receptors in the ACC and PFC are largely of the D1 type in humans.
Anti-ligature (anti-lig): An architectural term for devices that are designed to prevent
Automaticity: Automatic actions, either well-learned or instinctive that take place with
minimal declarative awareness. Examples are actions that take place while sleepwalking
Avolition: ‘An inability to initiate and persist in goal-directed activities. When severe
403
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
from completing many different types of activities (e.g., work, intellectual pursuits, self-
Bayesian: a statistical method where meaning is derived from data that defies statistical
normalization.
Wright (1954) to describe environmental settings that contain pre-set mores and
404
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
including designed objects, buildings, landscaped areas, parks, roads and urban plans.
stupor), certain types of excessive motor activity (apparently purposeless agitation not
Clang: a form of psychotic jargonaphasia where words (and apparently meanings) are
Cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT): A method of talk therapy that avoids the
there’s a notable bias to confirm ideas about perceived events rather than reject them.
with the paranoid subtype, though it is probably ubiquitous in the condition. See page
264.
Corollary discharge error (CDE): a hypothesis for hallucinations, which that was first
405
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
works. This model claims that an individual carries a record; ‘the efference copy’ of all
endogenous actions, and once actions are complete, the record is ‘discharged.’ Errors in
this process are perceived as hallucinations. The problem with this model is that it
attempts to explain bizarre hallucinations as if they were simple illusions, and goes no
D1: A class of excitatory dopamine receptors, which are ubiquitous in the ACC and PFC
D2: A class of inhibitory dopamine receptors, which are ubiquitous in the striatum and
limbic areas in human brains. In rats they are also the most common type in the ACC
D2Low: A dopaminergic heteromer when in its low affinity state (see D2High above).
They are speculated to drive postsynaptic, phasic dopamine activity, and be closely
associated with the high levels of attention that is given to declaratively aware tasks.
D2High: A dopaminergic heteromer when in its high affinity state. These heteromers are
bistable – that is, they modulate two functional outcomes. The D1/D2 heteromer
modulates sensitivity to dopamine, and is subject to change rapidly from a high affinity
406
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
activity, and be closely associated with the low levels of attention that is given to
Delusions: A false belief based on incorrect inference about external reality that is
firmly sustained despite what almost everyone else believes and despite what constitutes
incontrovertible and obvious proof or evidence to the contrary. The belief is not one
ordinarily accepted by other members of the person's culture or subculture (e.g., it is not
overvalued idea (in which case the individual has an unreasonable belief or idea but
Delusions are subdivided according to their content. Some of the more common types
Bizarre: A delusion that involves a phenomenon that the person's culture would
407
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
actions are experienced as being under the control of some external force rather
which the false belief is not as firmly held nor as fully organized into a true
belief.
Persecutory: A delusion in which the central theme is that one (or someone to
against.
408
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Thought broadcasting: The delusion that one's thoughts are being broadcast
Thought insertion: The delusion that certain of one's thoughts are not one's
own, but rather are inserted into one's mind.’ (American Psychological
from hallucinations.)
Determinism (architectural): Maurice Broady (1966) coined the term to describe the
dubious practice of asserting that designs, once constructed will affect people in a
409
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
clinical setting inferences about thought are based primarily on the individual's
speech, the concept of disorganized speech (Criterion A3) has been emphasized
of ways. The person may “slip off the track” from one topic to another
severe disorganized thinking or speech may occur during the prodromal and
maintaining hygiene. The person may appear markedly disheveled, may dress in
hot day), or may display clearly inappropriate sexual behavior (e.g., public
other mental illnesses. The implication stems from the reality that all the medications
that are used to treat psychosis affect dopamine transmission in some way. It is
speculated in this thesis that dopamine is used to moderate attention through two
Epidemiology: the branch of medicine that deals with the incidence, distribution, and
Error related negativity (ERN): This is a perceptual feedback loop specifically for
negative affective or negative hedonic feedback – ERN occurs when an event is worse
than expectations.
Evidence-based design (EBD): an approach to design that relies on empirical data for
411
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
because of the narrowness of its methods and its impracticality when thousands of
Generalised resource deficits (GRD): Part of salutogenic theory. GRD’s are entropic
forces that work against a state of better health. See diagram on page 114.
Generalised resistance resource (GRR): Part of salutogenic theory. GRR’s are forces
that work to build a state of better health. They include anything that contributes to a
Hallucinations: ‘A sensory perception that has the compelling sense of reality of a true
perception but that occurs without external stimulation of the relevant sensory organ.
voices.
412
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
hallucinations are distinguished from real physical sensations and from a tactile
hallucination.
something being under one's skin. The most common tactile hallucinations are
identified in this thesis, where automatic behaviours are relearned wrongly. See page
268.
413
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
they are bizarre, as they do not communicate meaning in the normal way. These include
rhyme, clang, alliteration and other ‘poetic’ devices. See disorganised symptoms.
life liveable on the most basic level. Things like food and shelter. See page 116.
Meaning: The generalised resistance resource in salutogenic theory that enriches life
though engagement with other people, other life and the world and cosmos beyond
ourselves. Things like art and the environment. See page 118.
Mismatch negativity deficits are well observed in schizophrenia, but are less common in
other mental illnesses. In this thesis MMN is taken as a deficit in bottom-up attention.
Models of care: The routines, rules and staffing patterns of a health facility.
Negative Signs: Also known as deficit symptoms: ‘account for a substantial degree of
the morbidity associated with schizophrenia,’ yet these are the most treatment resistant
symptoms. Three negative symptoms used for diagnosis: affective flattening, alogia,
and avolition, but there are also other negative symptoms (e.g., anhedonia). In this thesis
the negative signs are taken to be deficits in bottom-up attention (American Psychiatric
414
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
that have a ‘gated’ effect. Stimulation has to be of significant intensity to activate the
Noise Theory: The heuristic belief that symptoms are a product of excessive neural
noise. The theory is that irrelevant perceptions or internal chaotic neuronal reactions are
preoccupation with internal stimuli, but rejects the notion that this is a distraction
support the dopamine function by selectively inhibiting activity that is not task specific
the apparent position of an object when viewed along two different lines of sight.
Things that are further away seem to mode more slowly due to the effect of parallax.
415
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
tendency to catastrophise and imagine the worst outcomes of a given situation will
Paranoid Type schizophrenia: The most common subtype of the DSM schizophrenia
Association, 1994, 286). See page 262 for explanation according to the hypothesis
receptor. As opposed to a tonic action. The phenomenal effects of phasic and tonic
dopamine phasis regulates bottom-up attention and dopamine tone regulates top-down
hypothesis. The phonological loop is used to manage semantic and aural resources.
where a lack of new knowledge from bottom-up channels causes experience to become
increasingly stereotyped. The conditions that are created are exactly analogous to the
416
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Larsen effect (the bizarre positive feedback patterns that occur when an amplifier
Prodrome: The undefined period prior to a mental illness being officially diagnosable.
The prodrome of schizophrenia is often marked by some symptoms, but not to the level
condition, the term ‘prodrome’ often refers to that period. Although symptoms are
normatively thought to be similar in the prodrome, first person accounts often tell
another story. Where the symptoms of schizophrenia are generally horrific, the
hallucinations and delusions of the prodrome are often extremely exciting (Bowers, &
Psychiatric spectra continuum: A state of psychosis (as loosely defined) marks one
end of a spectrum, with as much as 60% of some populations having numinous and
perceptions normatively only occur in less than 3% of the population over the course of
417
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Psychopathology: The term isn’t used much because it is too easily confused with the
prominent hallucinations, with the hallucinations occurring in the absence of insight into
their pathological nature. A slightly less restrictive definition would also include
Broader still is a definition that also includes other positive symptoms of Schizophrenia
Despite the narrow terms of definition of psychosis in the DSM, the term ‘psychosis’ is
used very liberally. Examples given in (Howes and Kapur 2009; Kapur 2003; Kapur,
Mizrahi, and Li 2005) neither refer to prominent hallucinations nor delusions, nor even
lack of insight, and is thus not even psychosis as the DSM-IV defines the term.
Certainly the application of the broadest definition (all the positive symptoms of
as a cough is to influenza.
418
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Psychotropic: A psychoactive drug that affects the central nervous system. Such drugs
are often used to treat schizophrenia, but they are ineffective for many symptoms and in
cognitive processing. Theoretically, in this raw state, nothing can be salient, because it
must completely lack any kind of definition or ontological associations. Even so,
vegetative states, anaesthesia, deep sleep, and in vitro (Feldmeyer, 2010; Laureys, 2005;
Schiff et al., 2002) implying that this raw data feed bypasses cognition. See page 215.
and events. This is low-level and thought to be driven by tonic dopamine reception. A
Salience: In the literature saliency means a number of very different things to do with
the triggers for attention. From page 210 in this thesis a more specific taxonomy of
salience is proposed.
morels health and illness as a continuum. See the sections starting on the following
419
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Scatology: An obsession with excrement and other bodily wastes. Fear of scatological
rooms, which are also known as quiet rooms or isolation rooms (there are sometimes
variations, depending on the model of care) typically are spaces where the furniture is
fixed to the floor and there are no affordances for patients to harm themselves or others.
This usually means the spaces are very bare. Seclusion is usually used to punish bad
Sense of Coherence: The sum of all generalised resistance resources (the total
70.
Significance: One of the forms of salience in the taxonomy proposed within this thesis.
420
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Striatum: The part of the brain associated with automatic and autonomic functions. The
mental illnesses. It is situated between the mesencephalon (mid-brain) and the larger
where D2Low receptors are severely depleted and a patient effectively looses all frontal
and ACC connectivity, thereby confining their awareness subcortical functions such as
Sublime: The aesthetic experience of an immense order: nature, intense beauty and
religious experience.
receptor. As opposed to a phasic action. The phenomenal effects of phasic and tonic
dopamine phasis regulates bottom-up attention and dopamine tone regulates top-down
421
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Top-down attention: The attention that is given to expected outcomes. These are
generally low level and little to get ‘excited about’ in normal circumstances. Top-down
Tunnel-focus: As the name implies, is the kind of top-down attention that excludes all
language of tectonic form, materials, size and situation. Using the language of typology,
Undermined automaticity: Another new term for a syndrome identified in this thesis,
automaticity occurs when top-down intention brings too much focus to activities and
makes intended routines more difficult and prone to error. Too much attention to
automatic functions, such as speech and behaviour can make them seem mildly
remember (a top-down process) a word or name that is ‘in the tip of the tongue’. See
page 263.
422
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Way-finding: A term from the architectural lexicon, developed by Kevin Lynch (1992)
to understand how people find their way about a landscape using nodes, landmarks,
Wicked problems: Wicked problems are those that abound with open-ended questions,
shifting goalposts, compromised solutions for an open number of stakeholders, and any
wicked problems is that they embed other wicked problems (Kunz & Rittel, 1972).
declarative awareness. WM can be broken down into two essential roles; firstly WM is
the domain and the limit of declarative consciousness (Baars & Franklin, 2003). The
second role is the WM in its capacity as an information processor. This later function is
and a visuo-spatial sketchpad, and to perceptual inputs, desires, needs and learning
controversial topic. On one hand, we are told that poor WM function is a strong
423
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
standard degree’ of variation (Becerril & Barch, 2010; Bora, Yücel, & Pantelis, 2010;
Goldman-Rakic, 1994; Haenschel & Linden, 2010; Klemm, Schmidt, Knappe, & Blanz,
2006; Pantelis et al., 1997; Simon et al., 2007; G. Williams & Goldman-Rakic, 1995b;
H. J. Williams, Owen, & O’Donovan, 2007; Wirgenes et al., 2010). Yet, studies aimed
at the genetic variants that underpin WM processes are sometimes inconsistent and in
other studies, even proven false (Nieratschker et al., 2010). Dramatic differences like
these may be caused by the lack of consensus about what WM actually is (Cowan,
2005).
It is commonly accepted that there are limits on how much declarative information the
WM can simultaneously handle, with different models contradicting one another on the
number of items that can be simultaneously handled. Some also include a ‘non-
declarative’ level of working memory, with an unlimited capacity (Cowan, 2005; Bargh,
1997).
Worth: One of the subtypes of salience defined in this thesis. Worth measures affective
145.
424
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
455,
461
C
Arneill,
73,
454
Camberwell
walk
study,
40,
293,
301,
302,
303
Automatic
action,
226,
341,
351
Cantor-‐Graae,
37,
249,
293,
326,
329,
398,
453
426
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Dunnett, 99, 406 Frith, 19, 35, 45, 101, 197, 198, 199, 215, 216, 217,
Durkheim, 181, 406 228, 242, 243, 257, 261, 265, 266, 277, 279,
Expectation,
238
G
Experience
(see
Phenomenology
also),
175,
189,
Eyles, 324, 332, 336, 408, 437 Garety, 40, 109, 253, 255, 276, 295, 360, 407, 411,
413,
455
F
Gibson,
35,
66,
121,
122,
127,
128,
130,
160,
210,
427
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Hedonic, 298, 395 Kahn, 129, 142, 215, 298, 389, 426
Heinrichs, 249, 404 Kapur, 35, 44, 99, 197, 198, 199, 228, 231, 240,
Heinz, 197, 236, 265, 277, 313, 423, 426 241, 242, 244, 266, 292, 306, 414, 424, 427,
Hemsley, 35, 44, 45, 101, 279, 397, 403, 419, 423, 448, 452
Holroyd, 206, 228, 257, 266, 424 Kirkbride, 7, 18, 31, 37, 303, 304, 326, 335, 402,
Howes, 197, 199, 228, 242, 245, 266, 424 Krabbendam, 40, 274, 326, 430, 460
Learning, 156, 222, 406, 411, 420 Mirenowicz, 128, 130, 201, 202, 228, 439
Lhermitte, 122, 123, 124, 298, 343, 346, 347, 359, Montagna, 218, 444
Light, 312, 456 Morgan, 37, 335, 399, 428, 429, 435
Manageability, 62, 73, 86, 94, 107, 133, 154, 177 Newman, 140, 440
March, 31, 37, 38, 147, 435, 449 Nickerson, 207, 252, 306, 440
Meaning, 79, 109, 110, 112, 132, 133, 149, 157, 225, 229, 230, 232, 239, 244, 250, 268, 390
Meltzoff, 129, 210, 438 Northoff, 124, 134, 135, 157, 271, 297, 313, 315,
429
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Searles,
18,
20,
37,
58,
63,
64,
65,
67,
68,
69,
74,
Questionnaires,
95
79,
80,
81,
82,
111,
144,
167,
184,
262,
292,
299,
451
R
Seeman,
230,
231,
232,
233,
234,
244,
250,
268,
430
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Shergill, 101, 279, 453, 454 Torrey, 326, 331, 458, 461
Significance, 3, 18, 127, 132, 133, 202, 204 Tunnel focus, 247, 251
Smith,
35,
44,
72,
82,
213,
216,
419,
438,
454,
463
U
Walls,
312
Tacit
knowledge,
224
Weckowicz,
63,
65,
209,
313,
461
Tamminga,
244,
457
Weickert,
239,
252,
440,
444
Tandon,
267,
274,
428,
457
Whitehead,
57,
79,
389,
462
Taylor,
34,
371,
372,
417,
421,
455,
457
Wicked
problems,
398
Theeuwes,
130,
204,
206,
228,
310,
423,
457
Wiggins,
197,
450
431
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Williams, 18, 20, 191, 232, 258, 269, 404, 415, Yolken, 331, 458, 465
432
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Akil, Mayada, Pierri, Joseph N, Whitehead, RE, Edgar, CL, Mohila, C, AR, Sampson,
1580-1589.
Aleman, André, Böcker, Koen B. E., Hijman, Ron, de Haan, Edward H. F., & Kahn,
10.1016/s0920-9964(03)00060-4
Alomes, Vyv. (2009). Alive and Well; A new and total approach to suicide prevention
and community well being in rural areas. Paper presented at the Australian Rural and
433
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Anticevic, Alan, van Snellenberg, Jared X., Cohen, Rachel E., Repovs, Grega, Dowd,
Antonovsky, Aaron. (1987). Unravelling the Mystery of Health. San Francisco: Jossey-
Bass Inc.
434
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Ashby, F.G., Turner, B.O., & Horvitz, J.C. (2010). Cortical and basal ganglia
Aston-Jones, G., Rajkowski, J., & Cohen, J. (1999). Role of locus coeruleus in attention
10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00140-7
Auquier, P., Lançon, C., Rouillon, F., & Lader, M. (2007). Mortality in schizophrenia.
http://www.abs.gov.au/.
Government.
435
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Avila, Fabio. (2008). Paisaens Construidas. Nova Lima (Brazil): Empressa Das Artes.
10.1016/S0079-6123(05)50004-9
Baars, Bernard J., & Franklin, Stan. (2003). How conscious experience and working
6613(03)00056-1
Baars, Bernard J., & Franklin, Stan. (2007). An architectural model of conscious and
unconscious brain functions: Global Workspace Theory and IDA. Neural Networks,
Bachelard, Gastion, & Orion Press (Tr.). (1958, 1964 (Tr.)). The Poetics of Space; the
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nat
Bahrs, O. , Heim, S., Kalitzkus, V. , Matthiessen, P., Meister, P. , & Müller, H. (2003).
436
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Baker, A., Llewelyn, R. , & Sivadon, P. (1959). Psychiatric Services and Architecture.
Barker, Rodger Garlock, & Wright, Herbert F. (1954). The Midwest and its Children;
Company.
437
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Barnhart, Steven Karl. (1996). Examining the relationship between behavioural and
Barrett, L.F., & Bar, M. (2009). See it with feeling: affective predictions during object
Bassetti, C., Vella, S., Donati, F., Wielepp, P., & Weder, B. (2000). SPECT during
effects: Choking under pressure in sports and mental tests. European Journal of Social
10.1093/schbul/sbq009
Beck, Alan M., & Katcher, Aaron Honori. (1983). Between pets and people : the
438
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Beebe, L. H., Tian, L., Morris, N., Goodwin, A., Allen, S. S., & Kuldau, J. (2005).
10.1080/01612840590959551
Bem, Daryl, J. (1972). Self Perception Theory (Vol. 6). New York, London: Academic
Press.
Bergman, Eva, Malm, Dan, Ljungquist, Birgit, Bertero, Carina, & Karlsson, Jan-Erik.
(2012). Meaningfulness is not the most important component for changes in sense of
doi: 10.1016/j.ejcnurse.2011.05.005
439
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Berns, Gregory S., Cohen, Jonathan D, & Mintun, Mark A. (1997). Brain regions
10.1126/science.276.5316.1272
Berridge, Kent C, & Robinson, Terry E. (1998). What is the role of dopamine in reward:
hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Research Reviews, 28(3),
3(2), 111-140.
Bleuler, Eugen. (1950). Dementia praecox; or, The group of schizophrenias. New
Bolton, Derek. (2008). What is mental disorder? an essay in philosophy, science, and
440
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Bora, Emre, Yücel, Murat, & Pantelis, Christos. (2010). Cognitive Impairment in
10.1093/schbul/sbp093
Bowers, Len, Banda, Tumi, & Nijman, Henk. (2010). Suicide Inside: A Systematic
Review of Inpatient Suicides. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 198(5), 315-
Routledge.
Awareness.
Brébion, Gildas, Amador, Xavier, David, Anthony, Malaspina, Dolores, Sharif, Zafar,
441
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
10.1111/j.2044-8260.1984.tb00649.x
Broady, Maurice. (1966). Social Theory in Architectural Design. Arena, 81(898), 149-
154.
preference for advance information about upcoming rewards. Neuron, 63(1), 119-126.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.06.009
Broome, M.R., Johns, LC, Valli, I., Woolley, JB, Tabraham, P., Brett, C., . . . McGuire,
PK. (2007). Delusion formation and reasoning biases in those at clinical high risk for
Brune, M., Lissek, S., Fuchs, N., Witthaus, H., Peters, S., Nicolas, V., . . . Tegenthoff,
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.023
442
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Buchanan, R. (1992). Wicked problems in design thinking. Design issues, 8(2), 5-21.
Buckley, PF, & Stahl, SM. (2007). Pharmacological treatment of negative symptoms of
115(2), 93-100.
Cantor-Graae, Elisabeth, Nordström, LG, & McNeil, TF. (2001). Substance abuse in
10.1097/00004850-199509000-00004
443
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Carrere, Sybil, & Gottman, John Mordechai (1999). Predicting divorce among
newlyweds from the first three minutes of a marital conflict discussion. Family Process,
57(10), 1117-1127.
Castle, DJ, Jablensky, A., McGrath, JJ, Carr, V., Morgan, V., Waterreus, A., . . .
Caton, C.L.M., Drake, R.E., Hasin, D.S., Dominguez, B., Shrout, P.E., Samet, S., &
Cepeda, Carlos, André, Veronique M, Jocoy, Emily L, & Levine, Michael S. (2010).
444
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Cepeda, Carlos, & Levine, M.S. (2000). Dopamine and N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor
AHIA.
Chadwick, PK. (1993). The stepladder to the impossible: a first hand phenomenological
10.3109/09638239309003769
Chaika, Elaine. (1977). Schizophrenic Speech, Slips of the Tongue, and Jargonaphasia:
A Reply to Fromkin and to Lecours and Vanier-Clément. Brain and Language, 4, 464-
445
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Nicolas, & Farrer, Chlöé. (2011). Mentalizing under influence: abnormal dependence on
10.1093/brain/awr306
Chan, Raymond C. K., Huang, Jia, Guo, Lan, Cao, Xiaoyan, Hong, Xiaohong, & Gao,
10.1016/j.psychres.2009.07.014
Clark, Andy. (2012 - in press). Whatever Next? Predictive Brains, Situated Agents, and
446
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Clarkson, John Dr. (2006). The Dream Not Yet: a journey into the not so mystic wold of
whispers …”: empirical data support the call to formally teach handover to
prequalification doctors. Quality and Safety in Health Care, 18(4), 267-271. doi:
10.1136/qshc.2008.029983
Coid, Jeremy W., Kirkbride, James B., Barker, Dave, Cowden, Fiona, Stamps,
REbekah, Yang, Min, & Jones, Peter B. (2008). Raised Incidence Rates of All
Psychoses Among Migrant Groups; Findings From The East London First Episode
Collerton, Daniel, Perry, Elaine, & Ian, McKeith. (2005). What People see things that
are not there: a novel Perception and Attention Deficit model for recurrent complex
10.1017/S0140525X05000130
Collip, Dina , Myin-Germeys, Inez, & van Os, Jim (2008). Does the concept of
"Sensitization" Provide a Plausible mechanism for the Putative Link Between the
447
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Coltheart, Max, Langdon, Robyn, & McKay, Ryan. (2007). Schizophrenia and
10.1093/schbul/sbm017
Corbin, JM, & Strauss, A. (1990). Grounded theory research: Procedures, canons, and
Corlett, PR, Frith, Christopher D, & Fletcher, PC. (2009). From drugs to deprivation: a
206(4), 515-530.
Smit, Guus. (2008). Nicotinic Acetylcholine Receptors. Scholarpedia, 3(1), 3468. doi:
10.4249/scholarpedia.3468
Inc.
Cowan, Nelson. (2005). Working Memory Capacity. New York, Hove: Psychology
Press.
448
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Crow, TJ. (1980). Molecular pathology of schizophrenia: more than one disease
Freeman & S. Stanstead (Eds.), The Impact of the Environment on Psychiatric Disorder
Dakin, Steven, Carlin, Patricia, & Hemsley, David. (2005). Weak suppression of visual
10.1016/j.cub.2005.10.015
Damasio, Antonio R. (1994). Descartes' error : emotion, reason, and the human brain.
Das, P, Kemp, AH, Flynn, G, Harris, AWF, Liddell, BJ, Whitford, TJ, . . . Williams,
LM. (2007). Functional disconnections in the direct and indirect amygdala pathways for
449
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Davis, Charles, Glick, Ira D., & Osow, Irving. (1979). The Architectural Design of a
De Pauw, KW, & Szulecka, TK. (1988). Dangerous delusions. Violence and the
10.1192/bjp.152.1.91
1990, Lincoln.
Deister, Arno, & Marneros, Andreas. (1994). Prognostic value of initial subtype in
9964(94)90072-8
Dembling, B.P., Chen, D.T., & Vachon, L. (1999). Life expectancy and causes of death
in a population treated for serious mental illness. Psychiatric Services, 50(8), 1036-
1042.
450
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Dietrich, Arne, & Kanso, Riam. (2010). A Review of EEG, ERP, and Neuroimaging
10.1037/a0019749
Dijksterhuis, Ap, Aarts, Henk, Bargh, John A., & van Knippenberg, A. (2000). On the
Dijksterhuis, Ap, & van Knippenberg, A. (2000). Behavioral indecision: Effects of self-
10.1521/soco.2000.18.1.55
the design of the physical environment. Design and Health Scientific Review, 1(2), 47-
55.
451
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
systems, 1, 197-204.
Duckworth, K. L., Bargh, John A, Garcia, M., & Chaiken, S. (2002). The automatic
9280.00490
Edelstein, Ludwig, Temkin, Owsei , & Temkin, C. Lilian (1987). Ancient Medicine.
Edvardson, J. David, Sandman, Per-Olof, & Rasmussen, Bergit Holriz. (2005). Sensing
452
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Edwards, G. (1972). Murder and gun control. The American Journal of Psychiatry,
28(7), 811-814.
Ekeberg, Ø., Kjeldsen, S. E., Greenwood, D. T., & Enger, E. (1990). Correlations
doi:10.3109/00365519009089186
Ellett, L., Freeman, D, & Garety, PA. (2008). The psychological effect of an urban
Elliot, James, & Bayes Friba, Kenneth. (1972). Room for Improvement; a better
environment for the mentally handicapped. London: King Edward's Hospital Fund for
London.
Eriksson, M., & Lindsrom, B. (2008). Promoting mental health - Evidence of the
Evans, Gary. (2003). The Built Environment and Mental Health. Journal of Urban
453
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Eyles, D. W., Feron, F., Cui, X., Kesby, J. P., Harms, L. H., Ko, P., . . . Burne, T. H. J.
Falkenstein, M., Hohnsbein, J., Hoormann, J., & Blanke, L. (1990). Effects of errors in
choice reaction tasks on the ERP under focused and divided attention. In C. Brunia, A.
Gaillard & A. Kok (Eds.), Psychophysiological brain research (Vol. 1, pp. 192-195).
Faris, R.E.L., & Dunham, H.W. (1939). Mental disorders in urban areas: an ecological
Feinberg, Irwin. (1978). Efference copy and collorary discharge: the implications for
doi:10.1093/schbul/4.4.636
Feldmeyer, Dirk. (2010). Neuroscience: Signals from far and away. Nature, 464(7292),
454
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Flensmark, Jarl. (2004). Is there an association between the use of heeled footwear and
Foley, A.R., & Lacy, Bill N. (1967). On the need for Intraprofessional Collaboration:
Folsom, D., & Jeste, DV. (2002). Schizophrenia in homeless persons: a systematic
10.1034/j.1600-0447.2002.02209.x
Foucault, Michel. (1977). Discipline and punish: The birth of the prison. (A. Sheridan,
Foxe, John J., Wylie, Glenn R., Martinez, Antigona, Schroeder, Charles E., Javitt,
455
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Franck, KA. (1984). Exorcising the ghost of physical determinism. Environment and
Freedman, David H. (2010). Lies, Damned Lies, and Medical Science. The Atlantic
(11).
Freeman, D, & Freeman, Jason. (2008). Paranoia the 21st Century Fear. Oxford, New
Freeman, D, Garety, PA, Kuipers, E, Fowler, D, & Bebbington, PE. (2002). A cognitive
doi: 10.1348/014466502760387461
456
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Freeman, D, Pugh, K., & Garety, PA. (2008). Jumping to conclusions and paranoid
10.1016/j.schres.2008.03.020
Freyd, JJ, Klest, B, & Allard, CB. (2005). Betrayal trauma: Relationship to physical
Friston, K. (2003). Learning and inference in the brain. Neural Networks, 16(9), 1325-
457
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
10.1192/bjp.153.4.437
Fuxe, K, Marcellino, Daniel, Guidolin, Diego, Woods, Amina, & Agnati, Luigi. (2010).
Gallagher, HL, & Frith, Christopher D. (2003). Functional imaging of 'theory of mind'.
458
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Garety, PA, & Freeman, D. (1999). Cognitive approaches to delusions: a critical review
of theories and evidence. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38, 113-154. doi:
10.1348/014466599162700
model for the positive symptoms of psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 31, 189-195.
doi: 10.1017/S0033291701003312
2236(92)90355-C
Gerfen, C.R., Baimbridge, K.G., & Thibault, J. (1987). The neostriatal mosaic: III.
459
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Ginovart, N, & Kapur, Shitij. (2010). Dopamine Receptors and the Treatment of
Gluck, Marion, & Edgeson, Vicki. (2010). It must be my hormones. Australia: Penguin.
Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: The management of spoiled identity. New York: Simon &
Schuster Inc.
Gold, G. (2009). Dementia with lewy bodies: Clinical diagnosis and therapeutic
Goldman, D., Hien, D.A., Haas, G.L., & Sweeney, J.A. (1992). Bizarre delusions and
and the Brain; The 79th Annual Meeting of the American Psychopathological
460
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Goldman-Rakic, PS, Castner, S.A., Svensson, T.H., Siever, L.J., & Williams, G.V.
Golembiewski, Jan. (2009a). Moving from theory to praxis on the fly; Introducing a
Golembiewski, Jan. (2009b). We are our architecture. Paper presented at the Science
10.1108/02632771011023096
461
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Golembiewski, Jan. (2011b). Lost in Space: The role of the architectural milieu in the
aetiology and treatment of schizophrenia. Paper presented at the Mental Health Facility
Golembiewski, Jan. (2012a). All common psychotic symptoms can be explained by the
10.1016/j.mehy.2011.09.029
Golembiewski, Jan. (2012b). Moving from theory to praxis on the fly; Introducing a
Golembiewski, Jan. (2012c). The neural basis for the salutogenic method of healthcare
design. Dopamine, perception and the need for aesthetic engagement. Paper presented
at the Design and Health Australasia: Global Perspectives, Local Identities, UTS,
Sydney.
Golembiewski, Jan. (2012d). The neural basis for the salutogenic method of psychiatric
healthcare design. Dopamine, perception and the need for aesthetic engagement. Paper
presented at the 8th World Congress for Design and Health, 2012, KLCC, Kuala
Lumpur.
462
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Golembiewski, Jan. (2012e). Psychiatric design: Using a salutogenic model for the
Golembiewski, Jan. (2012f). Salutogenic design: The neural basis for health promoting
perceiving the design object. International Journal of Design in Society, 6, (in press).
Golembiewski, Jan. (in review-a). Are diverse factors proxies for architectural
Golembiewski, Jan. (in review-b). Introducing the concept of reflexive and automatic
Golembiewski, Jan. (in review-c). Lost in Space: the role of the environment in the
Golembiewski, Jan. (in review-d). The riddle of psychotic perception resolved; an in-
463
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Gottman, J.M. (Ed.). (1996). What predicts divorce: The measures. New York: Taylor
and Francis.
Gottman, J.M., McCoy, K., & Coan, J. (1996). The specific affect coding system
M. Gottman (Ed.), What predicts divorce: The measiures (pp. 112-195). New York:
generating regions of rat mdbrain dopamine neurons recorded in vitro. Brain Research,
Grace, A A. (1991). Phasic versus tonic dopamine release and the modulation of
Grace, A A. (2000). The tonic/phasic model of dopamine system regulation and its
implications for
464
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
j.1360-0443.95.8s2.1.x
Grace, A A, Floresco, S.B., Goto, Y., & Lodge, D.J. (2007). Regulation of firing of
Gray, Jeffrey A., Feldon, J, Rawlins, J N P, Hemsley, D., & Smith, A. D. (1991). The
10.1017/S0140525X00065055
Green, Melissa Jayne. (2001). Facial affect processing in delusion-prone and deluded
Groat, Linda, & Wang, David. (2002). Architectural Research Methods. New York:
Gross, Raz, Sasson, Yehuda, Zarhy, Moshe, & Zohar, Joseph. (1998). Healing
465
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Grossberg, S. (2000). How hallucinations may arise from brain mechanisms of learning,
583-592.
Grossberg, S. (2009). Cortical and subcortical predictive dynamics and learning during
10.1121/1.3589258
466
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Gulick, W.B. (2006). Signals, Schemas, Subsidiaries, and Skills: Articulating the
Gutkowski, Silvio, Ginath, Yigal, & Guttmann, Flor. (1992). Improving psychiatric
Psychiatry, 43(9).
Hall, Edward Twitchell. (1975). Mental Health Research and out of Awareness Cultural
Systems. In T. W. Maretzki & L. Nader (Eds.), Cultural Illness and Health (pp. 97-
Hall, Edward Twitchell. (1990). The hidden dimension. New York: Anchor Books.
Hall, FS, Wilkinson, LS, Humby, T., Inglis, W., Kendall, DA, Marsden, CA, &
Robbins, TW. (1998). Isolation rearing in rats: pre-and postsynaptic changes in striatal
Halpern, D. (1995). Mental Health and the Built Environment; More Than Bricks and
Hanssen, Manon, Bak, Maarten, Bijl, Rob, Vollebergh, Wilma, & van Os, Jim. (2005).
467
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
10.1348/014466505x29611
Harris, E.C., & Barraclough, B. (1997). Suicide as an outcome for mental disorders. A
Hassin, R. R., Bargh, John A, Engell, A. D., & McCulloch, K. C. (2009). Implicit
10.1016/j.concog.2009.04.003
Hassin, R. R., Bargh, John A, & Zimerman, S. (2009). Automatic and flexible: The case
10.1521/soco.2009.27.1.20
Hay, David. (1994). 'The Biology of God': What is the Current Status of Hardy's
Hypothesis? International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 4(1), 1-23. doi:
10.1207/s15327582ijpr0401_1
468
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Hayne, Y., & Yonge, O. (1997). The lifeworld of the chronic mentally ill: Analysis of
10.1016/S0883-9417(97)80004-0
HCAMC & CHAA. (2007). Australian Health Facilities Guidelines Vol. B. CHAA
Hebb, Donald O., Heath, E.S., & Stuart, E. A. (1954). Experimental Deafness.
Heffner, H.E. (1983). Hearing in large and small dogs: Absolute thresholds and size of
7044.97.2.310
Heinz, Andreas, Romero, B., Gallinat, J., Juckel, G., & Weinberger, DR. (2003).
469
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
10.1093/schbul/sbq031
Häfner & W. F. Gattaz (Eds.), Search for the Causes of Schizophrenia (pp. 179-188).
vision via the anterior cingulate. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(33), 11096. doi:
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1026-10.2010
Hoekstra, T., Lendemeijer, H., & Jansen, M. (2004). Seclusion: the inside story. Journal
Hoffer, A, & Osmond, Humphry. (1963). Scurvy and schizophrenia. Diseases of the
Holroyd, Clay B., & Coles, Michael G. H. (2002). The neural basis of human error
470
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
00009
Howes, Oliver D., Kambeitz, Joseph, Kim, Euitae, Stahl, Daniel, Slifstein, Mark, Abi-
Dargham, Anissa, & Kapur, Shitij. (2012). The Nature of Dopamine Dysfunction in
Schizophrenia and What This Means for Treatment: Meta-analysis of Imaging Studies.
10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.169
Howes, Oliver D., & Kapur, Shitij. (2009). The dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia:
Version III - The final common pathway. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35(3), 549-562. doi:
10.1093/schbul/sbp006
Huntoon, Mary. (1959). Art for Therapy's Sake. Ment. Hosp. (The American Psychiatric
Jacobs, Jane. (1961). The death and life of great American cities. New York: Random
House.
471
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
James, William. (1890). The Principles od Psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Henry Holt.
Johns, LC, Cannon, M., Singleton, N., Murray, R.M., Farrell, M., Brugha, T., . . .
the British population. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 185(4), 298-305. doi:
10.1192/bjp.185.4.298
Johns, LC, Rossell, S., Frith, Christopher D, Ahmad, F., Hemsley, D., Kuipers, E., &
10.1017/S0033291701003774
Johnson Abercrombie, ML. (1960). The Relation between Inside and Outside Worlds:
472
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Jones, Raya A. (2010). "Talking Brought Me Here": Affordances of Fiction for the
Kahn, Louis. (1971). The 1971 AIA Gold Medal Address: The Room, The Street and
Kahn, Louis. (1982). 1973: Brooklyn, New York. Perspecta, 19, 89-100.
473
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Kapur, Shitij, Mizrahi, R, & Li, M. (2005). From dopamine to salience to psychosis--
Katcher, Aaron Honori, & Beck, Alan M. (1983). New perspectives on our lives with
474
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Kelly, B.D., O'Callaghan, E., Waddington, J.L., Feeney, L., Browne, S., Scully, P.J., . . .
Morgan, M.G. (2010). Schizophrenia and the city: A review of literature and
116(1), 75-89.
Keshavan, M. S., Tandon, R., Boutros, N. N., & Nasrallah, H. A. (2008). Schizophrenia,
"just the facts": what we know in 2008 Part 3: neurobiology. Schizophrenia Research,
Kilts, C.D. (2001). The changing roles and targets for animal models of schizophrenia.
Kinney, D. K., Teixeira, P., Hsu, D., Napoleon, S. C., Crowley, D. J., Miller, A., . . .
consumption, infant mortality, and skin color: a role for prenatal vitamin d deficiency
475
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Kirkbride, James B., Boydell, J., Ploubidis, G. B., Morgan, C., Dazzan, P., McKenzie,
K., . . . Jones, Peter B. (2008). Testing the association between the incidence of
schizophrenia and social capital in an urban area. Psychological Medicine, 38(8), 1083-
Kirkbride, James B., Fearon, P, Morgan, C, Dazzan, P, Morgan, K, Murray, RM, &
Syndromes Findings From the 3-Center AESOP Study. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 63(3),
250-258.
Kirkbride, James B., Morgan, C, Fearon, P, Dazzan, P, Murray, RM, & Jones, Peter B.
10.1126/science.2841759
476
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Russegger, H., Pachinger, T., & Schwaiger, J. (1998).
Induced alpha band power changes in the human EEG and attention. Neuroscience
Krabbendam, L, & van Os, Jim. (2005). Schizophrenia and urbanicity: a major
795.
Kunz, Werner, & Rittel, Horst. (1972). Information science: on the structure of its
Lang, Jon T. (1980). The built environment and social behavior: Architectural
Lang, Jon T. (2005). Urban Design: A typology of procedures and products. Oxford,
Kristoffersen, Kjell , & Wahl, Astrid K. . (2005). The effect of salutogenic treatment
477
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Langeland, Eva, Wahl, Astrid K., Kristoffersen, Kjell, & Hanestad, Berit R. (2007).
Promoting coping: salutogenesis among people with mental health problems. Issues in
Langevin, Ron, & Hardy, Lorraine. (1987). Stranger homicide in Canada: A national
sample and a psychiatric sample. The journal of Criminal Law & Criminology, 78(2),
of predicting battering men's violence. Scholarly Inquiry for Nursing Practice, 10(4),
371-385.
Lapouse, R., Monk, M.A., & Terris, M. (1956). The drift hypothesis and socioeconomic
Large, Matthew M., & Nielssen, Olav. (2011). Violence in first-episode psychosis: A
10.1016/j.schres.2010.11.026
478
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Larsen, Larissa, Adams, Jeffrey, Deal, Brian, Kweon, Byoung Suk, & Tyler, Elizabeth.
(1998). Plants in the Workplace. Environment and Behavior, 30(3), 261-281. doi:
10.1177/001391659803000301
Laruelle, M., & Abi Dargham, A. (1999). Dopamine as the wind of the psychotic fire:
new evidence from brain imaging studies. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 13(4), 358-
Laureys, S. (2005). The neural correlate of (un) awareness: lessons from the vegetative
Lavie, Nilli. (2010). Attention, distraction and cognitive control under load. Current
Lawton, M. Powell , & Nahemow, Lucille (1973). Ecology and the Aging Process. In C.
Eisdorfer & M. Powell Lawton (Eds.), Social Environment of Aging (pp. 619 - 673).
479
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Leonard, Carly J., Kaiser, Sam T., Robinson, Benjamin M., Kappenman, Emily S.,
Hahn, Britta, Gold, James M., & Luck, Steven J. (2012). Toward the Neural
Lhermitte, F. (1983). 'Utilization Behavior' and its relation to leisons of the frontal
Lhermitte, F. (1986). Human autonomy and the frontal lobes. Part II: Patient behavior in
Lhermitte, F., Pillon, B., & Serdaru, M. (1986). Human autonomy and the frontal lobes.
Li, Yan-Chun, Kellendonk, Christoph, Simpson, Eleanor H., Kandel, Eric R., & Gao,
10.1073/pnas.1109718108
480
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Linebaugh, Kelly. (2002). Psychiatric Nurses Views on Architecture and the Design of
Ljungberg, T., Apicella, P., & Schultz, Wolfram. (1992). Responses of monkey
67(1), 145.
Lopez, F G. (1956). Four Vast Housing Projects for St. Louis; Hellmuth, Obata and
Lubow, Robert E. (1973). Latent inhibition. Psychological Bulletin, 79(6), 398-407. doi:
10.1037/h0034425
Luna, B., Thulborn, K.R., Munoz, D.P., Merriam, E.P., Garver, K.E., Minshew, N.J., . .
MacDonald, Angus, Carter, Cameron S., Kerns, John G. , Ursu, Stefan , Barch, Deanna
481
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
http://www.schizophreniaforum.org/whatweknow/default.asp
Maeissner, Ilse, Perry, Ellen, Dorr, Maude, & Rowan (Ed.), Jan C. . (1965). (Dr.
Malkin, Jane. (2008). A Visual Reference for Evidence Based Design. Concord, CA:
March, Dana, Hatch, S. L., Morgan, C., Kirkbride, James B., Bresnahan, M., Fearon, P.,
& Susser, E. (2008). Psychosis and place. Epidemiologic Reviews, 30, 84-100. doi:
10.1093/epirev/mxn006
482
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
March, Dana, Kirkbride, James B., Veling, Wim, & Golembiewski, Jan. (2009). The
Future Directions.
Marcotte, E.R., Pearson, D.M., & Srivastava, L.K. (2001). Animal models of
Marek, G. J., Behl, B., Bespalov, A. Y., Gross, G., Lee, Y., & Schoemaker, H. (2010).
10.1124/mol.109.059865
Martins Serra, A, Jones, S. H., Toone, B, & Gray, Jeffrey A. (2001). Impaired
latent inhibition and the Kamin blocking effect. Schizophrenia Research, 48, 273-289.
doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00141-9
Matlin, Margaret W. (1988). Sensation and Perception (2 ed.). Boston: Allyn and
Bacon.
McClelland, James L., McNaughton, Bruce L., & O'Reilly, Randall C. (1995). Why
483
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Insights From the Successes and Failures of Connectionist Models of Learning and
McDiven, Chris (2011, 16 November, 2011). [With meagre possessions and basic
necessities Matthew lives a simple, yet complicated and confusing life; the story of
Restraint. An Expert Interview With Janice Adam, RN-BC, and Timothy Meeks, BSN,
McGrath, John A., Avramopoulos, Dimitrios, Lasseter, Virginia K., Wolyniec, Paula S.,
10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.56
McGrath, John J. (2010). Is it time to trial vitamin D supplements for the prevention of
0447.2010.01551.x
484
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
McGrath, John J, Eyles, D. W., Pedersen, Carsten B, Anderson, Cameron, Ko, Pauline,
McGrath, John J, Saari, K., Hakko, H., Jokelainen, J., Jones, Peter B., Järvelin, M.R., . .
. Isohanni, M. (2004). Vitamin D supplementation during the first year of life and risk
245.
A systematic review of the incidence of schizophrenia: the distribution of rates and the
influence of sex, urbanicity, migrant status and methodology. BMC medicine, 2(1), 13.
McLeod, Ron. (2003). Inquiry into the Operational Response to the January 2003
Meltzoff, Andrew N., & Moore, M. Keith. (1977). Imitation of Facial and Manual
Mendez, M. F., Chow, Tiffany , Ringman, John , Twitchell, Geoff , & Hinkin, Charles
485
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Meyer, BC. (1969). Truth and the physician. Bulletin of the New York Academy of
Michie, Patricia. (2001). What has MMN revealed about the auditory system in
10.1016/S0167-8760(01)00166-0
Milstein, Jean A., Dalley, Jeffrey W., & Robbins, Trevor W. (2005).
Mirenowicz, J., & Schultz, Wolfram. (1994). Importance of unpredictability for reward
486
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
dopamine neurons by appetitive rather than aversive stimuli. Nature, 379(6564), 449-
Miyake, Akira, & Shah, Priti (Eds.). (1999). Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms
12(7), 3-19.
Moore, Brian C. J. (1997). An introduction to the psychology of hearing (4th ed.). San
Moran, P. M., Owen, L., Crookes, A. E., Al-Uzri, M. M., & Reveley, M. A. (2008).
Morris, R., Griffiths, O., Le Pelley, M. E., & Weickert, T. W. (2012). Attention to
487
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Muller, Norbert, & Dursun, Serdar M. (2010). Schizophrenia genes, epigenetics and
Neild, Barry. (2011, 14 May, 2011). Woman beheaded in Tenerife had sought aid
Newman, Oscar. (1996). Creating Defensible Space. Washington DC: U.S. Department
Receptors. In K. A. Neve (Ed.), The Dopamine Receptors (pp. 75-100). NY: Humana
Press (Springer).
Nielssen, O., Bourget, D., Laajasalo, T., Liem, M., Labelle, A., Hakkanen-Nyholm, H., .
Northoff, G, & Bermpohl, F. (2004). Cortical midline structures and the self. Trends in
488
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Northoff, G, Kötter, R., Baumgart, F., Danos, P., Boeker, H., Kaulisch, T., . . . Witzel,
Novak, Gabriela, Seeman, Philip, & Foll, Bernard Le. (2010). Exposure to Nicotine
doi: 10.3109/00207454.2010.513462
Nuevo, Roberto, Chatterji, Somnath, Verdes, Emese, Naidoo, Nirmala, Arango, Celso,
& Ayuso-Mateos, José Luis. (2012). The Continuum of Psychotic Symptoms in the
doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq099
489
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Oades, R. D., Zimmermann, B., & Eggers, C. (1996). Conditioned blocking in patients
Okubo, Yoshiro, Suhara, Tetsuya, Suzuki, Kazutoshi, Kobayashi, Kaoru, Inoue, Osamu,
Ösby, U., Correia, N., Brandt, L., Ekbom, A., & Sparén, P. (2000). Mortality and causes
2), 21-28.
Osmond, Humphry. (1957). Function as the Basis of Psychiatric Ward Design. Mental
Hospitals, 8, 23-27.
9, 18-19.
490
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Palmer, Brian A., Pankratz, V. Shane, & Bostwick, John Michael. (2005). The Lifetime
Parnas, J, Handest, P., Jansson, L., & Sæbye, Ditte. (2005). Anomalous Subjective
Pei, Lin, Li, Shupeng, Wang, Min, Diwan, Mustansir, Anisman, Hymie, Fletcher, Paul
J., . . . Liu, Fang. (2010). Uncoupling the dopamine D1-D2 receptor complex exerts
Perkins, Nathan. (In Press). Including patients, staff and visitors in the design of the
Perlman, Susan B., Almeida, Jorge R. C., Kronhaus, Dina M., Versace, Amelia,
LaBarbara, Edmund J., Klein, Crystal R., & Phillips, Mary L. (2012). Amygdala
491
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
faces distinguish remitted and depressed mood states in bipolar disorder. Bipolar
Perlman, W.R., Webster, M.J., Kleinman, J.E., & Weickert, C.S. (2004). Reduced
glucocorticoid and estrogen receptor alpha messenger ribonucleic acid levels in the
amygdala of patients with major mental illness. Biological Psychiatry, 56(11), 844-852.
Plazzi, G., Vetrugno, R., Provini, F., & Montagna, P. (2005). Sleepwalking and other
10.1007/s10072-005-0486-6
Popper, K. (1970). Normal science and its dangers. Criticism and the Growth of
Knowledge, 4, 51-58.
492
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Prante, Olaf, Dörfler, Miriam, & Gmeiner, Peter. (2010). Dopamine Receptor Subtype-
Selective Drugs: D2 Like Receptors. In K. A. Neve (Ed.), The Dopamine Receptors (pp.
Tasmania.
Proshansky, H.M., Ittelson, W.H., & Rivlin, LG. (1972). Freedom of Choice and
Quay, Herbert C., & Quay, Lorene C. (1965). Behavior Problems in Early Adolescence.
http://au.reachout.com/find/articles/after-someone-has-died
Read, John. (2010). Can poverty drive you mad? 'schizophrenia', socio-economic status
and the case for primary prevention. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 39(2), 7-19.
10.1176/appi.ajp.161.2.376-b
493
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Reina, Aaron. (2010). The Spectrum of Sanity and Insanity. Schizophrenia Bulletin,
Richard-Devantoy, S., Chocard, A.S., Bouyer-Richard, A.I., Duflot, J.P., Lhullier, J.P.,
Gohier, B., & Garre, J.B. (2008). Homicide et Psychose: Particularites Criminologiques
and Melancolics: a review of 27 Cases. (Tr.). L' Encelephale, 34(4), 322-329. doi:
10.1016/j.encep.2007.06.006
Richard-Devantoy, S., Duflot, J. P., Chocard, A. S., Lhuillier, J. P., Garré, J. B., &
issus d’une série de 210 dossiers d’expertises psychiatriques pénales pour homicide.
10.1016/j.amp.2009.08.007
Rizzolatti, G., Fabbri-Destro, M., & Cattaneo, L. (2009). Mirror neurons and their
10.1038/ncpneuro0990
494
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Rolls, Edmund T., Loh, Marco, Deco, Gustavo, & Winterer, Georg. (2008).
Rosa, Pedro G. P., Schaufelberger, Maristela S., Uchida, Ricardo R., Duran, Fabio L. S.,
Lappin, Julia M., Menezes, Paulo R., . . . Busatto, Geraldo F. (2010). Lateral ventricle
Rudd, M., Vohs, K.D., & Aaker, J. (2012). Awe Expands People’s Perception of Time,
School of Business.
Rumelhart, David E., McClelland, James L., & UCSD PDP Research Group. (1986).
Rust, Robert. (2012). Opportunities for the Private Sector in Health Infrastructure
Delivery. Paper presented at the Design and Health Australasia 2012, UTS, Sydney.
495
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Rutten, Bart P. F., & Mill, Jonathan. (2009). Epigenetic Mediation of Environmental
10.1093/schbul/sbp104
Samaha, A. N., Seeman, Philip, Stewart, J., Rajabi, H., & Kapur, Shitij. (2007).
doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5416-06.2007
Sanchez, J. A., & Barach, P. R. (2012). High reliability organizations and surgical
Sanders, John T. (2004). Retinae don't see. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(6), 887-
Some research findings and directions. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 8, 347-
496
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Saver, J.L., & Rabin, J. (1997). The neural substrates of religious experience. Journal of
Schiff, Nicholas D., Ribary, Urs, Moreno, Diana Rodriguez, Beattie, Bradley, Kronberg,
Eugene, Blasberg, Ronald, . . . Plum, Fred. (2002). Residual cerebral activity and
behavioural fragments can remain in the persistently vegetative brain. Brain, 125(6),
postedToSRF=666
Schneider, Kurt. (1959). Clinical psychopathology (H. MW, Trans. 5th ed.). New york:
dopamine neurons to reward and conditioned stimuli during successive steps of learning
497
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Schultz, Wolfram, Dayan, P., & Montague, P.R. (1997). A neural substrate of prediction
10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.473
Schwartz, MA, Wiggins, OP, Naudin, J, & Spitzer, M. (2005). Rebuilding reality: a
Schweitzer, M., Gilpin, L., & Frampton, S. (2004). Healing spaces: elements of
491-514.
498
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Searles, Harold F. (1986). My Work with Borderline Patients (1990 ed.). Northvale,
Seeman, Philip. (2008a). All psychotic roads lead to increased dopamine D2High
10.1016/j.schres.2007.11.007
499
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
In K. A. Neve (Ed.), The Dopamine Receptors (pp. 1-21). New York: Humana Press
(Springer).
Seeman, Philip, Guan, HC, & Hirbec, H. (2009). Dopamine D2High receptors stimulated
Seeman, Philip, & Kapur, Shitij. (2000). Schizophrenia: More Dopamine, More D2
10.1073/pnas.97.14.7673
10.1002/syn.20303
Correlates with D2High States, Implying Many Paths to Psychosis. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(9), 3513-3518. doi:
10. 1 073/pnas.04097661 02
500
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Selten, Jean Paul, & Cantor-Graae, Elizabeth. (2005). Social defeat: risk factor for
Selten, Jean Paul, & Cantor-Graae, Elizabeth. (2007). Hypothesis: Social Defeat is a
risk factor for schizophrenia? The British Journal of Psychiatry, 191(51), s9-s12.
10.1016/j.schres.2003.07.005
Shepley, Mardelle , Pasha, Samira , & Huffcut, Jamie C. . (in review). Evidence-based
501
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
(2003). Two Eyes for an Eye: The Neuroscience of Force Escalation. Science,
Sine, Davd M. (2008). The Architecture of Madness and the Good Paternalism.
Sloan Devlin, Ann. (1992). Psychiatric Ward Renovation: Staff Perception and Patient
Sloan Devlin, Ann, & Arneill, Allison B. (2003). Healthcare Environments and Patient
502
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Smith, E.J., Partridge, J.C., Parsons, K.N., White, E.M., Cuthill, I.C., Bennett, A.T.D.,
& Church, S.C. (2002). Ultraviolet vision and mate choice in the guppy (Poecilia
Snodgrass, Adrian. (1990). Architecture, time and eternity : studies in the stellar and
Prakashan.
Solomon, Philip, Leiderman, P. Herbert, Mendelson, Jack, & Wexler, Donald. (1957).
Spitzer, R.L., Gibbon, M., Skodol, Andrew E, Williams, J.B.W., & First, M.B. (2004).
First (Eds.), Cases From DSM-IV-TR® Casebook and Its Treatment Companion
10.1176/appi.books.9781585622665.30041
Stafford-Clark, D., & Taylor, FH. (1949). Clinical and electro-encephalographic studies
503
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Startup, H., Freeman, D, & Garety, PA. (2008). Jumping to conclusions and persecutory
Stephan, Klaas E., Penny, W.D., Daunizeau, J., Moran, R.J., & Friston, K. (2009).
Bayesian model selection for group studies. Neuroimage, 46(4), 1004-1017. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.025
Steptoe, Andrew, & Kivimaki, Mika. (2012). Stress and cardiovascular disease. Nat Rev
Strange, Philip D. (1992). Brain Chemistry and Brain Disorders. Oxford New York,
Strauss, AL, & Corbin, JM. (1990). Basics of qualitative research: Grounded theory
Sudjic, Dejan (2006). The Edifice Complex: How the Rich And Powerful and Their
504
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Sylvan, Louise. (2012). Address of the CEO of the Australian National Preventative
Health Agency. Paper presented at the Design and Health Australasia 2012, UTS,
Sydney.
Tammes, Andre, & Burnett, Deborah. (in review). Light and Lighting in the
Tamminga, C. A., Holcomb, H. H., Gao, X. M., & Lahti, A. C. (1995). Glutamate
pharmacology and the treatment of schizophrenia: current status and future directions.
10.1097/00004850-199509000-00005
Tandon, Rajiv, & Carpenter, William T. (2012). DSM-5 Status of Psychotic Disorders:
10.1093/schbul/sbs048
Taylor, JG. (2010). A Neural Model of the Loss of Self in Schizophrenia. Schizophr
Bull.
The Irish Department of Agriculture Fisheries and Food. (2009). Ireland's response to
the Commission's Green Paper on the Reform of the Common Fisheries Policy. Dublin.
505
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Theeuwes, Jan, Atchley, P., & Kramer, Arthur F. (2000). On the time course of top-
Theeuwes, Jan, Kramer, Arthur F., Hahn, Sowon, & Irwin, David E. (1998). Our eyes
do not always go where we want them to go: Capture of the eyes by new objects.
Toga, A.W., & Thompson, P.M. (2003). Mapping brain asymmetry. Nature Reviews
Torrey, E. Fuller, Bartko, John J., & Yolken, Robert H. (2012). Toxoplasma gondii and
Other Risk Factors for Schizophrenia: An Update. Schizophrenia Bulletin, sbs043. doi:
10.1093/schbul/sbs043
Melbye, Mads. (2001). Risk factors and confounders in the geographical clustering of
9964(00)00081-5
506
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Ullman, S. (1980). Against direct perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3(3), 373-
Ulrich, Rodger S. (1991). The Effects of Interior Design on Wellness; Theory and recent
Scientific Research. Paper presented at the Journal of Health Care Interior Design;
Ulrich, Rodger S. (2006). Evidence Based Health-care Architecture. The Lancet, 36(B),
538-539.
Ulrich, Rodger S., & Parsons, R. . (1990). Influences on Passive Plants in Individual
Well-Being and Health. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the National Symposium
DC.
Ulrich, Rodger S., Zimring, C., Zhu, X., DuBose, J., Seo, H., Choi, Y., . . . Joseph, A.
507
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Vaaler, AE, Morken, G, & Linaker, OM. (2005). Effects of different interior
van Os, Jim. (2004). Does the urban environment cause psychosis? British Journal of
van Os, Jim, Hanssen, M., Bijl, R.V., & Vollebergh, W. (2001). Prevalence of psychotic
van Os, Jim, Kenis, Gunter, & Rutten, Bart P. F. (2010). The environment and
van Os, Jim, Rutten, B. P., & Poulton, R. (2008). Gene-Environment Interactions in
508
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Multidisciplinary Care Approach for People With Schizophrenia. Physical Therapy, 92,
11-23.
Varma, VK, Wig, NN, Phookun, HR, Misra, AK, Khare, CB, Tripathi, BM, . . . Susser,
with onset, early manifestations and typology. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 96(6),
431-438.
Volavka, Jan , Large, Matthew , Torrey, E. Fuller, Fazel, Seena , Negro, Paulo ,
http://www.schizophreniaforum.org/for/live/detail.asp?liveID=81
10.4249/scholarpedia.1538
509
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Watson, D. R., Bai, F., Barrett, S. L., Turkington, A., Rushe, T. M., Mulholland, C. C.,
& Cooper, S. J. (2012). Structural changes in the hippocampus and amygdala at first
9141-4
Welch, K. A., Stanfield, A. C., McIntosh, A. M., Whalley, H. C., Job, D. E., Moorhead,
people at familial high risk of schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 199, 386-
Wells, D.L. (2007). Domestic Dogs and Human Health; An overview. British Journal of
Whitehead, Clay, Ellison, Gail, Kerpen, Stephen, & Marshall, David. (1976). The Aging
Whitehead, Clay, Polsky, Richard H., Crookshank, Carol, & Fik, Edward. (1984).
of Psychiatry, 141(5).
510
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
for Research on Cancer, Trans.) (Vol. No. 208): World Health Organisation.
Wig, NN, Varma, VK, Mattoo, SK, Behere, PB, Phookan, HR, Misra, AK, . . .
Williams, DR. (1986). Seeing through the photoreceptor mosaic. Trends Neurosci, 9(5),
193-198.
511
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Wise, R.A. (1982). Neuroleptics and operant behavior: the anhedonia hypothesis.
Wistow, Gerald. (2012). Still a fine mess? Local Government and the NHS 1962-2012.
Withagen, Rob, de Poel, Harjo J., Araújo, Duarte, & Pepping, Gert-Jan. (2012).
10.1016/j.newideapsych.2011.12.003
Withagen, Rob, & van Wermeskerken, Margot. (2010). The Role of Affordances in the
10.1177/0959354310361405
512
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Group. Paper presented at the Chestnut Lodge Symposium, Rockville, Maryland, USA.
Woodward, Todd S., Menon, Mahesh, & Whitman, Jennifer C. (2007). Source
Wyatt, RJ, Alexander, RC, Egan, MF, & Kirch, DG. (1988). Schizophrenia, just the
facts. What do we know, how well do we know it? Schizophrenia research, 1(1), 3.
Yanni, Carla. (2007). The Architecture of Madness; Insane Asylums in the United
513
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Yolken, R. H., Dickerson, F. B., & Fuller Torrey, E. (2009). Toxoplasma and
3024.2009.01131.x
Zeisel, J. (2007). Creating a Therapeutic Garden That Works for People Living with
10.1300/J081v21n01_02
Zeisel, J., & Raia, P. (2000). Nonpharmacological treatment for Alzheimer's disease: A
Zeisel, J., Silverstein, Nina M., Hyde, Joan, Levkoff, Sue, Lawton, M. Powell, &
514
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE BUILT ENVIRONMENT
Zeman, A. (2003). What is consciousness and what does it mean for the persistent
affective disorders: evidence for a final common pathway disorder. Acta Psychiatrica
515