PEACE UNIT 2 Tubadocx
PEACE UNIT 2 Tubadocx
In 1932 Albert Einstein wrote a letter to the father of psychoanalysis, Sigmund Freud, asking
if this new science could offer insights that might deliver humankind from the menace of war.
In his response to Einstein, Freud1 expressed little hope for an end to war and violence, or the
role of psychoanalysis in changing human behaviour beyond the individual level. Even
though some analysts such as Jacob Arlow have found indications of cautious optimism in
some of Freud’s writings, Freud’s general pessimism was mirrored by many of his followers.
This has played a key role in limiting the contributions psychoanalysis has made to
international relations in general and in finding more peaceful solutions for conflicts between
enemy groups in particular.
Freud gave up the idea that the sexual seduction of children came from the external world,
and instead focused on the stimuli that comes from the child’s own wishes and fantasies for
formation psychopathology. Since early psychoanalysts followed this tradition, classical
psychoanalysis accepted this de-emphasis on actual seduction coming from the external
world when considering the developing child’s psyche and generalized it to include de-
emphasis on the role of traumatic external events. This de-emphasis included traumatic
international events as they impact the mental health of individuals affected by them. The
Holocaust is a prime example. For a long time, psychological studies of the Holocaust were
too painful to be carried out, and the whole topic of its psychological impact on those who
were affected directly and on the human psyche in general was avoided. (Despite of some
studies on this topic, in general, a “denial” of the psychological plight of Holocaust survivors
strangely persisted for decades after the World War II – a defense that, astonishingly,
extended even to Israel. In a November 2, 1995 story, an Israeli television station reported
that even the Jewish state had long neglected the trauma undergone by Holocaust survivors.
After their arrival in 1940s, survivors had been immediately treated for depression and other
mental disorders in psychiatric hospitals. Incredibly, however, many of these patients’ official
files did not even mention that they were Holocaust victims.)
Freud
The Disillusionment of the War:
Freud has taken it upon himself to help the general civilian population understand and come
to terms with their mental distress. He believes that the two major factors contributing to this
distress are the disillusionment and altered attitude towards death which the war has brought
about.
According to Freud, within the warring nations, high social standards induced much self-
restraint on the individual. These social standards were the basis of the civilized state’s
existence. Freud asserts, however, that there is a disagreement between the warring state’s
moral standards and its actions. The state acts in the way that it openly condemns in the
individual, including deliberately lying and using deception (112). It keeps secrets from its
citizens while expecting utmost loyalty. However, the state cannot prevent itself from wrong-
doing, because that would put the nation at a disadvantage. This relaxation of moral demands
has the same effect on individuals. When society cannot rebuke citizens for the sake of
hypocrisy, suppression of base passions is relieved, and men commit acts of treachery,
cruelty, fraud, and barbarity. Samuel Weber, however, criticizes Freud’s use of
[4]
psychoanalysis to explain the violence of the war as insufficient. According to Weber, the
attempt to account for the phenomenon of war in terms of a theory of “drives,” developed
through the interpretation of the behavior of individuals, runs the risk of being dismissed as
completely psychologistic. Louis Breger’s disagreement with Freud on this point is aimed
more at psychoanalysis in general. According to Breger, psychoanalysis was not able to
predict the multitude of citizens who suffered from lasting mental illness as a result of the
[5]
war.
The disillusionment results not only from the discrepancy in moral relations exhibited by the
state and its expectations, according to Freud, but also from the cruelty shown by individuals.
The inmost essence of human nature consists of elemental forces, which include those that
society considers evil. Civilization is the fruit of renunciation of instinctual satisfaction and
causes instincts to be transformed into altruistic and social ends. Society strains the moral
standard to the highest possible point, which forces citizens to diverge even further from their
instincts.
Our mortification and disillusionment about the barbaric behavior of our fellow humans in
this war is unjustified. In reality, Freud argues, they have not sunk as low as previously
thought, because they have never risen so high as society believed them capable of doing.
They are merely taking a break from the oppressive moral constraints of society (Freud 121).
It is not until all these vicissitudes to which instinctual impulses are subject have been
surmounted that what we call a person's character is formed, and this, as we know, can only
very inadequately be classified as 'good' or 'bad'. A human being is seldom altogether good or
bad; he is usually 'good' in one relation and 'bad' in another, or 'good' in certain external
circumstances and in others decidedly 'bad'. It is interesting to find that the pre-existence of
strong 'bad' impulses in infancy is often the actual condition for an unmistakable inclination
towards 'good' in the adult. Those who as children have been the most pronounced egoists
may well become the most helpful and self-sacrificing members of the community; most of
our sentimentalists, friends of humanity and protectors of animals have been evolved from
little sadists and animal-tormentors.
The instinctual impulses of other people are of course hidden from our observation. We infer
them from their actions and behaviour, which we trace back to motives arising from their
instinctual life. Such an inference is bound to be erroneous in many cases. This or that action
which is 'good' from the cultural point of view may in one instance originate from a 'noble'
motive, in another not. Ethical theorists class as 'good' actions only those which are the
outcome of good impulses; to the others they refuse recognition. But society, which is
practical in its aims, is not on the whole troubled by this distinction; it is content if a man
regulates his behaviour and actions by the precepts of civilization, and is little concerned with
his motives.
Primitive man was violent, as evidenced by the murderous history of man which children
learn in school. However, the sense of guilt that humans feel can be traced back to primitive
man’s unease about killing humans. Contemplation of a loved one’s corpse prompted early
man to feel guilt and sorrow. The corpse not only influenced the development of the soul, but
also the first inkling of a moral sense—Thou shalt not kill. According to Freud, this final
development is no longer experienced by civilized man (128).
When the unconscious is probed, Freud finds that it has the same view of death as the
primitive man. There are two antithetical attitudes: that which acknowledges it as the
destruction of life, and the other which denies its success in that endeavor. Towards the
stranger, our minds are murderously-minded. Our own death is inaccessible to us, and loved
ones evoke an ambivalent or divided mind-set. The war strips us of civilization’s effects on
our minds and causes us to regress to the primitive man within us.
Freud ends with an adaptation of an old saying: If you would endure life, be prepared for
death (133). Weber finds this conclusion to be ironic. According to Weber, everything that
Freud said earlier in the article only proves how difficult it is to follow his parting words of
[7]
advice. Freud’s ending, however, shows how far modern culture and society are from even
acknowledging that the task of preparing for death might be necessary.
Volkan
His theory of collective violence begins with the chosen trauma. A chosen trauma is the
shared mental representation of an event in a large group’s history in which the group
suffered a catastrophic loss, humiliation, and helplessness at the hands of enemies. When
members of a victim group are “unable” to mourn such losses and reverse their humiliation
and helplessness, they pass on to their offspring the images of their injured selves and the
psychological tasks that need to be completed. This process is known as the
transgenerational transmission of trauma. All such images and tasks contain references to
the same historical event. As decades pass, the mental representation of such an event links
all the individuals in the large group. Thus, such a mental representation of a historical event
emerges as a significant large-group identity marker.
Chosen Trauma
A chosen trauma reflects the “infection” of a large-group’s mourning process. A reactivation
of a chosen trauma serves to link the members of a large group. Such reactivation can be used
by the political leadership to promote new massive societal movements, some of them deadly
and malignant. The defeat of Serbs by Turks at the battle of Kosovo in 1396 was the battle
cry in the 1990’s for ethnic cleansing of the Moslems. Although the defeat occurred six
hundred years ago, it lives on in the minds and hearts of Serbians. A chosen trauma is the
shared mental represtation of an event in a large group’s history in which the group suffered a
catastrophic loss, humiliation and helplessness at the hands of enemies. When members of a
victim group are unable to mourn such losses and reverse their humiliation and helplessness,
they pass on to their off spring the images of their injured selves and the psychological tasks
that need to be completed. This process is known as the transgenerational transmission of
trauma. Political leaders may initiate the reactivation of chosen traumas in order to fuel
entitlement ideologies.
According to Volkan traumatized groups may evolve two kinds of leadership; the
“reparative” type uses the traumatic event to unite the group and solidify its identity without
harming another group. The “destructive type” uses the “chosen trauma” to increase a sense
of victimization, vilify a real or imagined enemy and to resurrect dormant ideologies. These
ideologies typically claim exaggerated privilege and endorse revenge. This construct provides
an excellent psychological framework to analyze the phenomenon of violent entities such as
ISIS. Here, ISIS propaganda videos targeting Iraqi audiences repeatedly evoke the “Shia
regime” of Nouri Al-Maliki. The solitary speech of Abu Bakr Al-Baghdadi uploaded on
social media contained repeated references to excesses by Maliki and spoke of the
“ideological war” that must be fought with Shias. The videos aimed at Syrians speak of the
atrocities perpetrated by theAlawite Bashar-ul-Assad on Sunni civilians.
The “chosen trauma” paradigm is thus utilized by violent extremist leadership throughout the
Muslim world and provides them with some powerful modus for imposing draconian statutes
as well as evoking fear and hatred. It allows them to justify persecution both within the
ingroup as well as towards outgroups designated as enemies. The crucial role that the
vicarious trauma of graphic videos plays in fostering hate on all sides cannot be stressed
enough. These videos are viscerally disturbing and evoke fear and loathing which they are
designed to do. While their transmission on popular websites is protected by free speech laws
some self-censorship is certainly called for. The tendency of some sources to seek out the
most gruesome carnage and put it on display for all may enhance popularity but benefits
ultimately only those who feed on hatred and schism.
The next step is the failure to mourn for the losses sustained in the chosen trauma. That is
why the trauma lives on. Then comes the feeling of entitlement to revenge. Rather than
facing the anguish of mourning and self-examination, a group can find distraction in self-
righteous hostility and aggression against a purported enemy.
Then there is collective regression. Under the pressure of fear/anxiety, a majority regress to
early childhood mentality: mixtures of good and bad are unknown. One’s parents and leaders
are good, and enemies are bad. This mentality views violence as the only alternative, since
we are completely good, the enemy is not only bad, but evil. Collective regression of the kind
described by Volkan has less direct effect on the conduct of one’s daily life than it does on
large scale matters at a distance.
Large groups also mourn. Since a large group is not one living organism with one brain, its
mourning over the loss of loved ones, lands, and prestige after a war or war-like situation will
appear in large-group processes on a societal level. For example, after a major shared trauma
and loss at the hand of enemies, a political ideology of irredentism —a shared sense of
entitlement to recover what had been lost—may emerge that reflects a complication in large
group mourning and an attempt both to deny losses and to recover them.
But we need to integrate all four emotions (grief, anxiety/dear, shame, anger) into a wider
consideration of emotional/relational worlds. These worlds, although next to invisible in
modern societies, probably play an important part in generating either public support or
opposition to collective violence.
Despite its theoretical basis as psychoanalysis, the book is not laden with heavy terminology,
but rather written in a simple style and a personal tone which is another merit in terms of ease
of reading. The author defines precisely and lucidly such key terms as the ‘echo’ and
‘accordion’ phenomena, and ‘mini conflicts’, and makes some valuable suggestions about the
process of Track II diplomacy, namely informal and unofficial diplomacy. He uses the
metaphor of the accordion to describe the ‘rhythmic alternation of togetherness and
distancing’ (p. 46) of opposing groups, and argues that ‘agreements between enemies are
most stable if they are made when the accordion is at neither extreme’
In addition, he points out the negative impacts of an external event over the unofficial
diplomatic dialogue which is defined as the ‘echo phenomenon’ and suggests that facilitators
should not ‘minimize and deny it and force delegates to follow the scheduled agenda’ (p. 47).
Similarly, he coined the term ‘mini conflict’ to refer to the occurrence of a crisis usually at the
beginning of the meeting which is to be resolved by the facilitator.
Volkan makes a point of analysing international conflicts based on large groups, which refers
to the collective identity of a nationality, ethnicity, race, clan, tribe, religion, class or a
political ideology, and in connection to massive traumas. Accordingly, in analysing, for
example, the Israeli–Palestinian conflict, he identifies the psychodynamics of two large
groups, of which Palestinians are characterized as displaying ‘masochistic (victimized) large-
group narcissism’ (pp. 86–7), while Israelis are interpreted as in need of having ‘enemies to
externalize elements that cannot be synthesized’ (p. 87). If a sense of suffering makes large
groups feel morally superior, he contends that there is a sort of competition for ‘victimization’
between Israelis and Palestinians, both of which suffer from humiliation which is linked with
the massive traumas they have undergone.
In a regressed society political, legal or traditional borders begin to symbolize the canvas of
the large group tent. In other words, borders become highly psychologized and people,
leaders, and official organizations become preoccupied with their protection. Since there is a
realistic danger “out there,” obviously borders need to be protected and because of this, it is
difficult to study the psychological aspects of this preoccupation.
When an ethnic, national, religious large group regresses it primarily becomes involved in
certain large group processes that serve to maintain, protect and repair the large-group
identity. Since large groups have their own specific characteristics that are built upon a
centuries-old continuum and shared mental representation of history and myth, the
examination of signs and symptoms of their regression should also include psychological
processes that are specific to such large groups. When Freud (1921) wrote about this
phenomenon he did not say that he was referring to regressed groups. Robert Waelder (1930)
brought to our attention the fact that Freud was describing regressed groups. Sometimes the
members of a large group continue to rally around a leader for decades and remain
“regressed’ in order to modify the existing characteristics of their large-group identity. In this
situation what we observe is similar to an individual’s “regressing in the service of
progression and creativity.” When a large group is in a regressed state, the personality and the
internal world of the political leader assumes great importance concerning the manipulation
of what already exists within the large-group psychology.
Regression
Two types of splitting are also signs of large-group regression. First, a splitting between “us”
and “them” (the enemy outside the regressed large group) becomes very strong and the
“other” becomes a target for dehumanization. Second, in regressed large groups, following
the initial rallying around the leader, a severe split occurs within the society itself, especially
when the leader cannot maintain hope and cannot tame shared aggression.
The Study of Mind and Human Interaction (CSMHI) evolved a process to deal with the
unfolding of large-group regression and conflicts between large groups. the “Tree Model”
(Volkan, 1999) to reflect the slow growth and branching of a tree. • this methodology has
three basic components or phases: (1) psychopolitical diagnosis of the situation, (2)
psychopolitical dialogues between influential delegates of opposing groups, and (3)
collaborative actions and institutions that grow out of the dialogue process.
SOCIAL-PSYCHOLOGICAL APPROACH
KELMAN
His work over more than three decades has focused on the development and application of
interactive problem solving: an unofficial, scholar-practitioner approach to the resolution of
protracted, deep-rooted, and often violent conflicts between identity groups—particularly
ethnonational groups—which is derived from the pioneering work of John Burton and
anchored in social-psychological principles (Kelman 1999c; 2002). His primary focus over
the years has been on the Israeli Palestinian conflict, but his students and associates have also
applied the approach in a number of other arenas of ethnonational conflict, including Cyprus,
Northern Ireland, Sri Lanka, Colombia, and South Africa.
The central distinction in our work, following John Burton, has been between settlement and
resolution of conflict. In contrast to the negotiation of a political settlement, a process of
conflict resolution goes beyond a realist view of national interests. It explores the causes of
the conflict, particularly causes in the form of unmet or threatened needs for identity, security,
recognition, autonomy, and justice. It seeks solutions responsive to the needs of both sides
through active engagement in joint problem solving. Hence, agreements achieved through a
process of genuine conflict resolution—unlike compromises achieved through a bargaining
process brokered or imposed by third parties—are likely to engender the two parties’ long-
term commitment to the outcome and to transform their relationship
Assumption
His work starts with the assumption that the nonviolent termination of conflicts between
identity groups requires a process of conflict resolution of the kind that I have briefly
described.
Conflict settlement
Conflict settlement can be described as a process yielding an agreement that meets the
interests of both parties to the extent that their respective power positions enable them to
prevail. Third parties—outside powers or international organizations—often play a role in
brokering or even imposing an agreement, using their own power by way of threats or
inducements. The agreement may be supported by the publics on the two sides because they
are tired of war and have found the status quo of continuing hostility and uncertainty
increasingly intolerable. Such support of the agreement does not rest in any particular change
in public attitudes toward the adversary. The settlement process is not especially designed to
change the quality of the relationship between societies . The stability of a political settlement
ultimately depends on surveillance—by the parties themselves, in keeping with their
deterrent capacities, by outside powers, and by international organizations.
Conflict resolution
conflict resolution, particularly if we think of it within an interactive problem solving
framework, goes beyond conflict settlement in many of the ways to which he has already
alluded: It refers to an agreement that is arrived at interactively, rather than imposed or
sponsored by outside powers, and to which the parties therefore have a higher level of
commitment. It addresses the parties’ basic needs and fears and therefore has a greater
capacity to sustain itself over time. It builds a degree of working trust between the parties—a
pragmatic trust in the other’s interest in achieving and maintaining peace—and therefore is
not entirely dependent on surveillance as the guarantor of the agreement (for the distinction
between working trust and interpersonal trust, see Kelman 2005). It establishes a new
relationship between the parties, best described as a partnership, in which the parties are
responsive to each other’s needs and constraints, and committed to reciprocity. It generates
public support for the agreement and encourages the development of new images of the other.
Reconciliation
This is the third, distinct, process of peacemaking: reconciliation, with a primary focus at the
level of identities. He has always argued that an agreement emerging from a process of
conflict resolution within an interactive problem-solving framework and the new relationship
it promotes are conducive to stable peace, mutually enhancing cooperation, and ultimate
reconciliation.
In this spirit, he has proposed that the problem-solving workshops between politically
influential Israelis and Palestinians that his colleagues and he has organized for some years (
Kelman 2002; Rouhana& Kelman 1994) represent tentative steps toward reconciliation,
insofar as participants are encouraged to listen to and to try to appreciate each other’s
narrative and to engage in a process of “negotiating identity” (Kelman 2001).
Whereas conflict resolution refers to the process of achieving a mutually satisfactory and
hence durable agreement between the two societies, reconciliation refers to the process
whereby the societies learn to live together in the postconflict environment. Reconciliation
presupposes conflict resolution of the type that he has described: the development of working
trust; the transformation of the relationship toward a partnership based on reciprocity and
mutual responsiveness; an agreement that addresses both parties’ basic needs. But it goes
beyond conflict resolution in representing a change in each party’s identity.
The primary feature of the identity change constituting reconciliation is the removal of the
negation of the other as a central component of one’s own identity. My main empirical point
of reference in this analysis is the Israeli-Palestinian case, in which mutual denial of the
other’s identity has been a central feature of the conflict over the decades (cf. Kelman 1978;
1999b). Changing one’s collective identity by removing the negation of the other from it
implies a degree of acceptance of the other’s identity—at least in the sense of acknowledging
the validity and legitimacy of the other’s narrative without necessarily fully agreeing with
that narrative. The change in each party’s identity may go further by moving toward the
development of a common, transcendent identity—not in lieu of, but alongside of each
group’s particularistic identity. What is essential to reconciliation, in his view, is that each
party revise its own identity just enough to accommodate the identity of the other. As the
parties overcome the negative interdependence of their identities, they can build on the
positive interdependence of their identities that often characterizes parties living in close
proximity to each other (Kelman 1999b).
New attitudes toward the other can thus develop, not just alongside of the old attitudes, but in
place of the old attitudes. As the new attitudes become integrated into the group’s own
identity, they gradually replace the old attitudes. Working trust can gradually turn into
personal trust. This does not foreclose the possibility that old fears and suspicions will
reemerge, but the relationship is less vulnerable to situational changes. The dilemma is that
the amount and kind of identity change that A requires from B in order to be ready for
reconciliation may be perceived by B as undermining the core of its identity. Insofar as the
other can be demonized and dehumanized, it becomes easier for each party to minimize guilt
feelings for acts of violence and oppression against the other and to avoid seeing itself in the
role of victimizer, rather than only the role of victim.
Thus, in protracted identity conflicts, negation of the other is not a peripheral, marginal
element of each party’s identity that can be easily discarded. His argument is merely that,
from an “objective” point of view, negating the identity of the other is not a necessary
condition for preserving, and indeed enhancing the core of one’s own identity. However, for
conflicting parties to arrive at a point where they can be free to relegate negation of the other
to the periphery of their own identities and eventually discard it requires the hard work of
reconciliation. What is central to that work is the growing assurance that the other is not a
threat to one’s own identity. In that process of assurance, the conditions for reconciliation
play a vital role. It is his contention that reconciliation—especially in cases in which neither
party is prepared to adopt the role of perpetrator—cannot be achieved on the basis of purely
objective criteria of truth, justice, or responsibility, anchored in historical scholarship or
international law, but requires some degree of mutual accommodation in the course of
negotiating the conditions for reconciliation.
He has identified five conditions that may help groups in conflict arrive at the difficult point
of revising their identity so as to accommodate the identity of the other:
1. Mutual acknowledgment of the other’s nationhood and humanity, which involves
acceptance of the other as an authentic nation and inclusion of the other in one’s own moral
community.
2. Development of a common moral basis for peace, allowing for a peace that both sides
perceive as consistent with the principles of fairness and attainable justice.
3. Confrontation with history, which does not require a joint consensual history, but does
require admitting the other’s truth into one’s own narrative.
4. Acknowledgment of responsibility, expressed in both symbolic and material terms.
5. Establishment of patterns and institutional mechanisms of cooperation, including various
people-to-people activities that are genuinely useful to both parties and based on the
principles of equality and reciprocity.
All five of these conditions for reconciliation are designed to facilitate changes in the
collective identities of the conflicting parties, with particular emphasis on removing the
negation of the other as a key element of each group’s own identity.
Criterion 1: Normative Versus Value-Neutral Research. The goal of SPPR is to reduce and
prevent conflict, and to promote positive relations between groups. In other words, peace
psychological research is, by definition, inherently normative, and not value neutral (Fuchs &
Sommer, 2004). Normative research aims at improvements, which means that it includes
evaluation of the present state of things and of the direction of future development.
Evaluation is only possible from somebody's point of view. It should be noted, however, that
while these values may define the choice of research topics and the direction of interventions,
they do not necessarily affect the scientific evaluation of hypotheses and the interpretation of
data.The value relevance (Parsons, 1949; Weber, 1968) of topics studied in SPPR does not
violate the principle of value neutrality in science (Albert, 1968)
In sum, SPPR addresses the prevention of structural violence and intergroup conflict as well
as the promotion of positive intergroup relations. In exploring these issues, research in this
tradition is, prototypically, (1) normative, (2) contextualized, (3) inclusive of multiple levels
of analysis, and (4) oriented toward the solution of practical problems
Criterion 2: Contextualized Research In line with the focus of the early years of social
psychology (Greenwood, 2004), increasingly more social psychologists argue that human
behavior can only be fully understood in the social context in which it is shaped (e.g., Bar-
Tal, 2004, 2006; Fischer, 2006). Historically and socially contextualized social psychology
(Pettigrew, 1991; Smith, 1983) allows for the study of the complexity, relevance, and
emotional involvement that is central to understanding human experience (Bar-Tal, 2004)
In sum, plenty of research in mainstream social psychology can be characterized by the same
criteria that define the core of SPPR. Rather than contradicting the basic tenets of social
psychology, the study of peace and conflict falls squarely within its scope, and some would
even argue that it is closer to its roots (Bar-Tal, 2004).
Furthermore, the popularity of certain paradigms and topics in mainstream social psychology
is reflected in the body of SPPR as well. The articles aiming at understanding direct and/or
structural violence focus almost exclusively on prejudice and stereotyping.
Similarly, the frequent use of social identity theory and other intergroup relations theories is
in line with a general increasing trend within mainstream social psychology over the last
years (Sherman et al., 1999).
Criticism
SPPR, is biased toward the problems facing societies in North America and Western Europe.
There is a need to broaden the scope of research, the contexts of study, and the range of
topics in order to attain both depth and breadth in knowledge, and consequently to become
more effective in tackling such global issues as peace and conflict.
Another critical issue that arises is whether the criterion of contextualization is sufficiently
satisfied by simply framing research questions in the context of group memberships that are
of political or societal relevance (such as racial or ethnic groups).
Furthermore, although in most cases the research questions are contextualized in meaningful
group memberships, the research itself was usually conducted outside the actual relevant
social context. This pattern becomes apparent in the high proportion of studies conducted in
the laboratory and using self- report instead of behavioural measures.
The most frequently studied context in this body of research, intergroup relations, also
represents how most of the studies met the criterion of including the macro-level of analysis.
While this focus by no means excludes structural variables (such as economic inequality,
social class, power, and other characteristics of societal structure), these are only rarely
actually studied. More frequently, individual manifestations of structural violence (such as
discrimination and racism) are examined.
In sum, current SPPR still neglects bidirectional effects of structural variables on the
individual in favor of focusing on individual factors leading to structural violence (see also
Pettigrew, 1996, 2006). To account for the complexity of topics SPPR deals with, it is
essential that the principle of triangulation is followed more, combining experimental with
other methodologies.
Social psychology journals, on the other hand, integrate their findings more often into an
established theoretical framework. Following this model, an integration of research questions
in the field of peace psychology with existing social psychological theory would transfer
explanatory power and stimulation from basic research to the field of peace psychology,
leading to a more efficient and quicker integration of basic and applied research.
Psycho-cultural approach
Theories of ethnic conflict explain the origin, persistence, and course of ethnic disputes. The
most commonly cited social science theories of ethnic conflict are structural, emphasizing
ways in which competing interests and overt conflicts arise from the structure of society or
relationships between societies (Ross, 1993a). Interest-based theories suffer from at least two
important weaknesses -explaining how groups come to define certain interests as critical in
the first place, and why ethnic conflicts attain an intensity and intransigence which many
suggest are often not commensurate with the substantive interests the parties contend are at
stake.
Psycho-cultural interpretation theory, in contrast, offers answers to these questions and in so
doing provides a strong challenge to earlier anti-psychological views of social and political
conflict. In addition, Psycho-cultural interpretation theory answers earlier criticisms that
psychological explanations of collective behavior simply view society as little more than the
family writ large, as Freud did, offering a far richer view of the social world than did early
psychoanalytic formulations. The result is a socially rooted psychoanalytic theory and a
language in which to talk about psychological dynamics in ways that are far more compatible
than earlier work with the theories, empirical concerns, and interests of social scientists. The
great interest in interpretive approaches in the social sciences has made us aware of the
critical importance of understanding how impersonal and cultural frameworks, not just
objective conditions, shape social action. Psycho-cultural interpretation theory provides an
important addition to this perspective and articulates an important set of connections between
individual developmental processes and collective behavior.
Psycho-cultural interpretation theory gives a central role to culturally rooted social and
psychological processes which produce dispositions-shared images, perceptions of the
external world, and motives for individual and group behavior (Ross, 1993a; 1993b). When
conflicts develop, these dispositions are invoked to construct interpretations of the world. In
intransigent ethnic conflicts, deep seated threats to identity and security fears serve as
powerful barriers which prevent groups from addressing the competing substantive interests
which divide them.
The key concepts of Psycho-cultural interpretation theory are derived from contemporary
psychoanalytic formulations, especially object relations theory, which stress that interpretive
frameworks rooted in early social relationships strongly influence how individuals and
groups understand and respond to each other (Greenberg & Mitchell, 1983; Mitchell, 1988).1
Internalized images of the world, established in family contexts, provide a template for later
relations involving larger social aggregates, such as those involving ethnic identities and are
called upon later when high-anxiety events, such as communal conflicts, occur. Shared
objects of identification and common frames of reference facilitate the development of
culturally sanctioned collective responses within ethnic communities (Barnes, 1987)
ROSS
PSYCHOCULTURAL INTERPRETATION THEORY
Contemporary psychoanalytic theory provides a socially rooted model of behavior built on
the work of Klein, Fairbairn, Sullivan, Kernberg and others which evolves structure from the
individual's relations with other people, in contrast to older drive-based theories which give a
preeminent role to instinctual drives (Greenberg & Mitchell, 1983; Mitchell, 1988). Not only
does contemporary theory employ a very different language than earlier psychoanalytic
formulations, but it places a greater emphasis on social relationships in pre-Oedipal
development, a question not well developed in classical theory. What this work does in
emphasizing relational aspects of the human experience is to provide a prominent role for
social and cultural processes in psychological development, giving ethnic group (and other)
attachments a central role in the dynamics of inter-group cooperation and conflict (Fornari,
1975; Barnes, 1987; Volkan, 1988).2 In this section I want to highlight three key elements of
this contemporary psychoanalytic perspective relevant to ethnic conflict and conflict
management: the psychological construction of social worlds, the need to link Psycho-
cultural interpretive process.