Hart Cognitive Poetics
Hart Cognitive Poetics
F. Elizabeth Hart
today, ironically, we may have at our disposal more instruments than ever
before with which to explore how human beings communicate.
Philosophers and literary scholars on both sides of the great Theory Divide
are wondering alike, inevitably: What's next? How might we proceed in the
wake of poststructuralism's rearrangement and even discrediting of the for-
malist and structuralist strategies that once provided our (admittedly foggy)
windows onto communicative acts and the texts that encode them?
To the authors and editors of these books, the tools literary scholars need
not merely to dispense with the older paradigms but actually to surpass them
in their ability to teach us about meaning-making are most readily and sen-
sibly found in the cognitive sciences. These studies culminate (though in a
limited way that I will discuss shortly) roughly twenty years of eavesdropping
by a small contingent of literary scholars on conversations taking place with-
in fields like cognitive psychology, hnguistics, psychohnguistics, computer
science, the cognitive neurosciences, cognitive evolution, evolutionary psy-
chology, and others that fall under the widely interdisciphnary label "cogni-
tive science" (and that generally tend to make those of us in the humanities
cringe—^with honest intimidation if not distaste). Literary scholars intrigued
with recent advances in learning ahout the human brain/mind (learning
spurred on by the invention of imaging technologies such as CAT scan,
MRI, and OIS that allow scientists to view live brains in action) have been
bringing to their roles as literary theorists and critics a conviction whose
time has surely come: Simply put, they beheve that our approaches to liter-
ary questions can and should be enriched by an acknowledgment of how the
enabling and constraining behaviors of brains and minds contribute to liter-
ary experience.
The diversity of tides now associated with this belief testifies to the
expansion and increasing maturity of cognitive literary studies. To cite some
of the major titles from just the past five years, including monographs, essay
collections, and theater as well as literary studies: Alan Palmer, Fictional Minds
(2004), Alan Richardson and Ellen Spolsky (eds.). The Work of Fiction:
Cognition, Culture, and Complexity (2004), Patrick Colm Hogan, The Mind and
Its Stories: Narrative Universals and Human Emotion and Cognitive Science,
Literature, and the Arts: A Guide for Humanists (both 2003), David Herman
(ed.). Narrative Theory and the Cognitive Sciences (2003), Bruce McConachie,
American Theater in the Culture of the Cold War: Producing and Contesting
Containment, 1947-1962 (2003),JosephTabbi, Cognitive Fictions (2002), David
Lodge, Consciousness and the Novel: Connected Essays (2002), Mary Thomas
Crane, Shakespeare's Brain: Reading With Cognitive Theory (2001), Ellen
Spolsky, Satisfying Skepticism: Embodied Knowledge in the Early Modern World
(2001), and BlakeyVermeuele, The Party of Humanity: Writing Moral Psychology
F. Elizabeth Hart 227
volume as a whole. But something else that each essay offers, which might
be indispensable to the scholar or student wanting to delve deeper into cog-
nitive hterary theory, is an introductory perspective on Fauconnier and
Turner's "cognitive blend" theory, the aspect of cognitive hnguistics that I
believe wOl have the most lasting impact on literary studies. Before I proceed,
however, with my brief analysis of Fauconnier and Turner's The Way We
Think, let me summarize my comments about the Stockwell and Gavins and
Steen textbooks as follows: Overall, these books provide a sincere, pedagog-
ically considerate introduction to a new field, albeit one that runs the risk of
making poetics seem even more difficult for students than it already does
owing to the density and complexity of its theory and readings. But the
problem of inaccessibility is not these authors' fault since, in truth, the diffi-
culty, including the challenges of the science, cannot be helped. It is the
nature of this particular interdisciplinary beast because, as cognitive science
itself is now showing us, our minds' interactions with both the everyday
world and the art that responds to it are astoundingly complex, perhaps mak-
ing them impossible ever to analyze satisfactorily. Teachers and students who
engage with these textbooks must accept the difficulty and also, one hopes,
actively embrace it, taking these texts slowly and in the spirit of exploration.
My more serious complaint about both volumes, as mentioned earlier,
has to do with the narrow self-conceptualization and presentation of this
"cognitive poetics" thatfi-anklyignores companion scholarship by others in
cognitive literary studies. For instance, I find it strange that in his final chap-
ter Stockwell represents poststructuralist problems such as discourse and ide-
ology as still largely unexplored fi-om a cognitive-literary standpoint; when,
in fact, these have been the subjects of a number of studies pubhshed in
recent years by Spolsky, Crane, Zunshine, and others. (I'm tempted to spec-
ulate that this is a sign of typical U.K.-U.S. disjunction, except that
StockweU cites some American researchers in his introduction, and several
of the Gavins and Steen contributors are American.) While such an over-
sight might have been unintentional, nonetheless, its effect is to hide more
than a decade of careful, related work, which amounts—in my mind, at
least—to misrepresentation.
Gilles Fauconnier and Mark Turner's The Way We Think summarizes, in
their first monograph together, the long collaboration between a cognitive
linguist (Fauconnier) and a literary critic with considerable experience in
cognitive studies (Turner). The two have published numerous articles that
have alternately targeted scientific and literary audiences (you can usually tell
which by the arrangement of their by-lines). There are signs that this mono-
graph is clearly intended for a cognitive science readership: its by-line
arrangement, publisher's categorization, even the fact that Turner's literary
F, Elizabeth Hart 233
ism are similarly suggestive, as the blends we will find in postcolonial texts
surely encapsulate long histories of struggle between cultures whose overlap
has been conceptual as well as geographic. This list could continue, of course,
to include the many "-isms" of our postmodern literary milieu.
As with the other two studies considered in this review, the challenge for
hterary scholars is largely technical. The science will be daunting for many,
and Fauconnier and Turner's diagrams of cognitive blends begin to blur after
a while, reaching an almost absurd level of detail and what may seem like
over-thought. To their credit, however, Fauconnier and Turner have made
every effort to write a text that is as friendly to the lay reader as it is to their
fellow scientists, using crisp prose and accessible examples and maintaining
their focus on the same set of examples throughout (though this in itself may
contribute to the blur factor).
The reason,finally,to feel intimidated by this study is not so much its
difficulty but the vastness of its imphcations for literary studies, a vastness that
the researchers have left it up to others to explore. Fauconnier and Turner
offer an endorsement for this exploration near the end of their study, where
they ruminate on that singularly evolved human capacity for double- or
multiple-scope cognitive blending: Such blending, they write—the differ-
ence that, in the evolutionary scenario, determined which form of human
would finally win the title of "human"—is also the hallmark of hterary and
other forms of artistic creativity. In other words, what hterary scholars do,
what we study and what we strive to articulate for our students, is under-
stood within this model as realizing the most sophisticated of all human
capabilities. And that's saying something for the humanities.
Works Cited
Crane, Mary Thomas. 2001. Shakespeare's Brain: Reading with Cognitive Theory.
Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Gibbs, Raymond W. 1994. Poetics of Mind: Figurative Thought, Language, and
Understanding. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Herman, David, ed. 2003. Narrative Theory and the Cognitive Sciences. CSLI Pubs.
Hogan, Patrick Colm. 2003a. The Mind and Its Stories: Narrative Universals and Human
Emotion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
. 2003b. Cognitive Science, Literature, and the Arts: A Guide for Humanists.
Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
LakofF, George, and Mark Turner. 1989. More Than Cool Reason: A Field Guide to
Poetic Metaphor. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lodge, David. 2002. Consciousness and the Novel: Connected Essays. Cambridge:
Harvard University Press.
McConachie, Bruce. 2003. American Theater in the Culture of the Cold War: Producing
and Contesting Containment, 1947-1962. Iowa City: University of Iowa Press.
F. Elizabeth Hart 237