Validating The Diagnosis of Sensory Processing Disorders Using EEG Technology
Validating The Diagnosis of Sensory Processing Disorders Using EEG Technology
KEY WORDS OBJECTIVE. This study tested the assumption of sensory integration theory that states that a relationship
• behavior exists between brain function and the behavioral manifestations of sensory integrative dysfunction.
• brain METHOD. Electroencephalographic measures were used to examine brain processing in 28 children with
• electroencephalography (EEG) sensory processing disorders (SPD) and 25 children who were typically developing, ages 5–12 years.
• event-related potential (ERP) RESULTS. Children with SPD demonstrated less sensory gating than children who were typically develop-
• pediatric ing. A significant relationship between sensory gating and age was found in children who were typically devel-
oping but not in children with SPD. Brain activity correctly distinguished children with SPD from children who
• sensory gating
were typically developing with 86% accuracy.
• sensory integration
CONCLUSION. These results present empirical evidence that children with SPD display unique brain pro-
• sensory processing cessing mechanisms compared to children who are typically developing and provide external validity for the
• sensory processing disorder (SPD) diagnosis of SPD.
Davies, P. L., & Gavin, W. J. (2007). Validating the diagnosis of sensory processing disorders using EEG technology. Amer-
ican Journal of Occupational Therapy, 61, 176–189.
Patricia L. Davies, PhD, OTR, is Associate Professor, Jean Ayres’s theory of sensory integration has generated more research and
Department of Occupational Therapy, 219 Occupational
Therapy, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, CO
A. controversy than any other theory developed by an occupational therapist
(Bundy & Murray, 2002). These fervent controversies have emphasized how par-
80523; [email protected].
ticularly important it is to find more precise methods to study the phenomenon of
William J. Gavin, PhD, is Research Scientist/Scholar, sensory integration and the treatment of children with sensory processing disorders
Department of Occupational Therapy, Colorado State
University, Fort Collins.
(SPD). One approach yet to be pursued in the study of sensory integration is to
directly test the assumptions of the theory itself. Bundy and Murray (2002) artic-
ulated five assumptions of the sensory integration theory related to the neural and
behavioral bases of sensory integration (pp. 10–12). The assumptions that are most
germane to validating the theory of sensory integration relate to the relationship
between brain maturation or function and behavioral manifestations of sensory
integrative dysfunction, which was eloquently stated by Short-Degraff:
Sensory integration theory assumes that the brain is immature at birth and also is
immature [or dysfunctional] in some individuals with learning problems. The goal
of sensory integration therapy is to provide stimulation that will address certain brain
levels (primarily subcortical), enabling them to mature [or function more normally],
and thereby assisting the brain to work as an integrated whole. (Short-Degraff, 1988,
p. 200) [Bracketed material added by Bundy & Murray, 2002, p. 11]
Of these assumptions, two can be postulated as hypotheses that can be
directly tested using a brain imaging technique commonly used by neuroscien-
tists. First, Ayres’s (1972, 1989) theory proposes that behavioral expressions of
sensory integration dysfunction are related to immaturity or malfunction in brain
processing. This assumption leads to the hypothesis that, when presented with
176 March/April 2007, Volume 61, Number 2
Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 12/01/2020 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms
discrete sensory stimuli, the brain activity of children whose the adaptiveness of the brain’s response” (pp. 25–26). She
behaviors are indicative of a sensory processing disorder will further defined the integrative process as filtering, organiz-
differ from the brain activity of children who are typically ing, and integrating sensory information and did not specif-
developing. More important, Ayres’s theory also proposes ically limit it to the combination of two or more senses.
that sensory integrative therapy will change neural mecha- Thus, her definitions of the term sensory integration seem to
nisms. This assumption leads to the hypothesis that the give a much broader representation of processing than just
brain activity of children with SPD observed after thera- the integration of several senses, the definition often used by
peutic intervention will differ from brain activity observed neuroscientists. When conducting neuroscience research on
before intervention. However, before this assumption can children who have difficulties in processing sensory infor-
be tested, evidence for the validity of the first assumption mation, classifying them as having SPD better captures all
should be demonstrated. Interestingly, despite the fact that aspects of the problems observed in these children, espe-
Ayres began writing articles about sensory integration in the cially filtering, organization, and integration as described
1960s (e.g., Ayres, 1964, 1965, 1969), these two hypothe- very early on by Ayres (1972). The electroencephalographic
ses have yet to be directly tested. For example, brain imag- brain imaging techniques described in this article are com-
ing studies that examine the neural processing mechanisms monly used by neuroscientists (Handy, 2005; Luck, 2005)
in children with SPD and children without disorders have and are ideal for measuring the filtering and organization
yet to be conducted to determine whether group differences aspects of sensory processing.
do indeed exist. Mulligan (2002) stated that, if we are to
establish professional consensus regarding the validity of the
sensory integration theory, support for the basic assump-
Electroencephalography and
tions of the theory are needed. Event-Related Potentials
To examine the association among brain structure, func-
tion, and behavior related to sensory processing abilities,
Sensory Integration and SPD real-time measures of brain activation during the processing
Sensory integration is a therapeutic approach that has been of sensory stimuli provide the most convincing data. Elec-
used for many years by occupational therapists and has a troencephalography (EEG) and event-related potentials
strong potential to enhance occupational performance in (ERPs)—functional neuroimaging methods—are ideal
children. Ayres described sensory integration as an techniques that may offer occupational therapists new
approach used to enhance the brain’s ability to organize sen- strategies for studying SPD. To obtain EEG data, one or
sory input for use in functional behaviors (1972, 1979). more metallic sensors are placed on the scalp to detect very
According to Ayres (1972), the essential principle in sensory small (10–50 microvolts) and continuous voltage changes,
integrative therapy is to provide the child with experiences which are then amplified and digitized.
rich in sensory input, in a guided manner, to produce an Because EEG measures electrical activity of the cortical
adaptive response (i.e., functional behavior) deemed more regions of the brain, it can provide a more accurate assess-
effective than previously observed behaviors. Therefore, the ment of the processing of sensory stimuli by the brain than
central features of Ayres’s sensory integrative theory and can peripheral measures. Although peripheral measures
intervention focus on the fact that the brain organizes sen- such as electrodermal activity and heart rate do have the
sory input in order for the individual to participate in advantage of measuring an individual’s interaction with the
meaningful occupations (Parham, 2002). environment (Stern, Ray, & Quigley, 2001), investigators
Occasionally the terms that professionals use to can only infer from such measures what processes may be
describe the children who receive therapy guided by sensory invoked in the central nervous system to produce the
integration theory can be ambiguous. For example, the changes observed at the periphery. Electroencephalographic
term sensory processing is often used interchangeably with methodologies provide the advantage of directly measuring
sensory integration, and controversy exists over interchanging brain activity.
these terms (Mulligan, 2002). In the neurosciences, sensory ERPs are graphical displays of the brain’s electrical
integration is used to specifically describe the combining of activity typically associated with a specific, defined event.
signals from two or more senses in the central nervous sys- ERPs are obtained by time-locking the EEG to the occur-
tem (Calvert, Spence, & Stein, 2004; Hicks, Molotch- rence of each event (e.g., the onset of a sensory stimulus),
nikoff, & Ono, 1993). In contrast, Ayres (1972) defined creating segments around the event, and then averaging
integration as “the interaction and coordination of two or together the segments of the multiple presentations of the
more functions or processes in a manner which enhances event (Segalowitz & Davies, 2004). Thus, an ERP waveform
The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 177
Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 12/01/2020 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms
provides information about the temporal aspects of infor- occurring about 50 ms after the stimulus presentation. The
mation processing of stimulus events. Two aspects of the N100 denotes the negative deflection in the proximity of
ERP waveform can be measured: amplitude and latency. 100 ms after the stimulus presentation or event.
Amplitude is measured in microvolts (µV) and can be pos-
Studying Sensory Processing Disorders Using EEG/ERP
itive or negative based on a relative baseline of zero. Latency
Methodology
is usually measured in milliseconds (ms) post stimulus
onset. The ERP components are defined as deflections from Sensory processing has been studied using EEG/ERPs for
baseline and labeled with a P for positive deflections and many years in the general adult population and also in sev-
with an N for negative deflections (see Figure 1a for an eral disorders such as schizophrenia (e.g., Boutros, Belger,
example of an ERP and component labels for an auditory Campbell, D’Souza, & Krystal, 1999; Freedman, Adler, &
stimulus). The number in the label for a component indi- Waldo, 1987), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
cates the latency in milliseconds from the time of the stim- (ADHD) (Olincy et al., 2000), and autism (Kemner,
ulus presentation to the peak of the deflection. As illus- Oranje, Verbaten, & van Engeland, 2002). Most of the
trated in Figure 1a, P50 represents the positive deflection studies involving clinical populations have used auditory
1a. This brain response of a child from the control group displays a large difference in the P50 peak amplitude of the first click (solid line) when compared to
the amplitude of the second click (dashed line).
1b. This brain response of a child from the group with SPD shows minimal difference in the P50 peak amplitude of the first click (solid line) when compared to
the amplitude of the second click (dashed line).
Figure 1. ERP waveforms for two children representing their brain response to the sensory gating paradigm. The averaged brain response to
the presentations for the first click sound, the Conditioning click, is shown as a solid line. The second click sound, the Test click, is shown
with a dashed line.
Table 1. Results of t Test Comparisons of the Two Groups of Children on the Short Sensory Profile
Children Who Were Children With Sensory
Typically Developing Processing Disorders Results of t Tests
2a. This brain response of a child from the control group displays similar peak latencies of the early components (i.e., N100 and P200) of the waveforms for
each tone. Note that the amplitudes of the loud tones (70 dB) are larger than the amplitudes of the soft tones (50 dB).
2b. This brain response of a child from the group with SPD shows considerable variability in the peak latencies and peak amplitudes of the early components of
the waveforms for each tone. These waveforms appear to be more disorganized than those shown in 2a for the child from the control group.
Figure 2. ERP waveforms for two children representing their brain response to the sensory registration paradigm. The averaged brain
responses to the presentations of each of the 4 tones are shown as separate lines.
that consisted of two principal components, maturation analysis revealed that the proposed model—consisting of
and organization of brain responses to auditory stimuli. The measures of maturation and brain processing of simple
maturation component was simply defined as the partici- auditory stimuli presented in the sensory registration
pant’s chronological age. Based on previously reported paradigm—could account for a very large percent (84%) of
research (Marshall et al., 2004), the expected relationship the variability in the P50 T/C ratios observed in sensory gat-
was that older children would show smaller sensory gating ing paradigm. When the regression analysis was performed
T/C ratios than younger children. Organization of brain using data just from the children with SPD, no relationship
response to auditory stimuli was defined as the participant’s between the dependent and the independent variables was
interrelationship of the peak-to-peak amplitudes and laten- found, R 2 = .35 (Adj. R 2 = .002), F (9, 17) = 1.0, p = .47. Inter-
cies of the N100 and P200 components of the average estingly, in contrast to the children who were typically
waveforms for the two loud intensity auditory stimuli of the developing, age accounted for only 1.4% of the variance in
sensory registration paradigm. The operational assumption the children with SPD, F Change(1, 25) = .35, p = .56.
here was that the less variable these components were This discrepancy between the maturation effects
within an individual, the smaller the individual’s sensory observed in children who were typically developing and the
gating T/C ratio would be. This model was tested using a failure to find maturation effects in children with SPD
3-step multiple regression procedure. The predicted depen- might be better understood by examining the zero order
dent variable was the P50 T/C ratio from the sensory correlations between age and T/C ratios of the P50 compo-
gating paradigm. The predictors—the independent nent for each group separately. For the children who were
variables—were age entered in the first step; the N100 typically developing, a statistically significant relationship
amplitudes and latencies of the two loud intensity auditory was found between age and the T/C ratios of the P50, r =
stimuli of the sensory registration paradigm were entered in –.60, p = .001. Thus, the evidence suggests that for the chil-
the second step; and the P200 amplitudes and latencies of dren who were typically developing, sensory gating
the two loud intensity auditory stimuli were entered last. improves (i.e., T/C ratios become smaller) as a function of
When this analysis was performed with data from both being older in age. However, children with SPD did not
child groups, a nonsignificant relationship was found, R 2 = provide evidence of a relationship between age and the T/C
.32 (Adj. R 2 = .17), F (9, 40) = 2.11, p = .051. ratios of the P50 as r = –.08, p = .67. Thus, children with
However, when the regression analysis was per- SPD do not demonstrate improved sensory gating as a
formed using data just from the children who were typi- function of being older.
cally developing, a statistically significant relationship
between the variables was found, R 2 = .84 (Adj. R 2 = .72), Diagnosing Children With SPD
F (9, 13) = 7.41, p = .001. Age accounted for 32% of the vari- To understand how brain processing in children with SPD
ance (F Change(1, 21) = 9.97, p = .005), N100 amplitudes and differs from the brain processing in children who are typ-
latencies accounted for 49% (F Change(4, 17) = 10.82, p = ically developing, we further examined the relationship
.0002), and P200 amplitudes and latencies accounted for between sensory gating and sensory registration measures.
3% (F Change(4, 13) = .56, p = .70). Thus, only for the chil- First, we derived a prediction equation for P50 T/C ratios,
dren who were typically developing, the 3-step regression using the unstandardized coefficients obtained for each of
184 March/April 2007, Volume 61, Number 2
Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 12/01/2020 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms
the variables in the regression analysis performed on chil- Discussion
dren who were typically developing, as reported previ-
ously. Using this equation, predicted P50 T/C ratios were To our knowledge, this study is the first to examine brain
calculated for each child in both groups. The predicted processing in children identified by occupational therapists
ratios were then subtracted from their actual P50 T/C as having SPD and to compare the results to brain process-
ratios obtained in the sensory gating paradigm to produce ing observed in children without disorders. We explored
a difference score for each child. These difference scores two research questions. The first addressed whether the
were then plotted as a function of their corresponding groups differed in EEG measures of brain processing of
obtained T/C ratios. As seen in Figure 3, difference scores auditory stimuli. The second explored whether electro-
of the children with SPD are distributed on either side of encephalographic techniques would be useful in diagnosing
the children who were typically developing. Thus, this SPD. Our results revealed that, as a group, children with
procedure demonstrates that when P50 T/C difference SPD demonstrated less auditory sensory gating than an age-
scores are derived from the prediction equation based on matched peer group of children without disorders (p = .04),
the regression analysis of the children who were typically although not significantly different when evaluated
developing, the children with SPD can be shown to be against the adjusted alpha level. Furthermore, children
hyperresponsive or hyporesponsive in their sensory gating with SPD did not show a significant relationship between
when compared to the gating of the children who were sensory gating and age, although such a relationship was
typically developing. found in children who were typically developing. Finally,
Figure 3. Illustration of the results of the statistical method used to separate the children with SPD from children in the control group
(children who were typically developing) based on brain responses to auditory stimuli. Obtained P50 T/C ratios of each child are plotted
as a function of the difference between the obtained ratios and the predicted ratios.