A Meta-Analytic Investigation of The Within-Person
A Meta-Analytic Investigation of The Within-Person
net/publication/264479041
CITATIONS READS
153 18,073
2 authors:
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
A Meta-Analysis of Self-Regulated Learning in Work-Related Training and Educational Attainment: What We Know and
Where We Need to Go View project
From Occupations to Individual Outcomes: Mediating Role of Occupational Personality Heterogeneity View project
All content following this page was uploaded by Gillian Yeo on 11 October 2017.
GILLIAN YEO
University of Western Australia
Since its inception 35 years ago, self-efficacy has become the most
frequently studied construct in the self-regulation domain (Vancouver &
Day, 2005). Self-efficacy is defined as people’s beliefs regarding their
capability to succeed and attain a given level of performance (Bandura,
We sincerely thank the researchers that contributed data to this meta-analysis. This work
was supported by Australian Research Council grants DP0984782 (Chief Investigators
Gillian Yeo and Shayne Loft) and DP120100852 (Chief Investigators Andrew Neal, Gillian
Yeo, and Hannes Zacher).
Correspondence and requests for reprints should be addressed to Traci Sitzmann, Univer-
sity of Colorado Denver, 3920 Perry St., Denver, CO 80212; [email protected].
C 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. doi: 10.1111/peps.12035
531
532 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
1
Note that this negative effect is expected to occur within a goal level; that is, during
the goal striving phase when discrepancy reduction processes are active. When goal setting
processes are active, self-efficacy is expected to have a positive effect on goal level and
performance as a function of discrepancy creation (Vancouver, More, & Yoder, 2008).
SITZMANN AND YEO 535
2
Although some studies conducted prior to 2001 collected repeated measurements of
self-efficacy and performance (e.g., Bandura & Jourden, 1991; Bandura & Wood, 1989), the
relationship between self-efficacy and performance was examined at the between-persons
level of analysis. These studies were not included in the meta-analysis because we were
unable to obtain access to the data or within-person correlation matrices.
TABLE 1
Studies Included in the Meta-Analysis With Study Information and Effect Sizes for the Within- and Between-Persons
536
Self-Efficacy/Performance Relationship
(2012a)
Daniels et al. (2010) No report NA Exams in a statistics class 354 .16, †, .39 Yes −.71* Unipolar Field Unpub
Day et al. (2007) No NA Space Fortress scores 465 †, .69, .59 No .88* Likert Lab Pub
DeShon et al. (2004) No NA TANDEM performance 687 .36, †, .33 Yes .48* Likert Lab Pub
Feltz et al. (2008) No NA Back dive performance 320 .52, .62, .82 No .60* Likert Field Pub
Gilson et al. (2012) Yes PositiveLT, PP Weight athletes could squat 345 .08, −.12, −.03 No .40* Unipolar Field Pub
Heggestad & Kanfer No NA Stimulus word pair reaction 1,110 .45, .57, .65 No .78* Unipolar Lab Pub
(2005) time
Hepler & Feltz (2012) No NA Baseball decision making 650 −.04, −.06, −.10 No −.01 Unipolar Lab Pub
continued
TABLE 1 (continued)
Report SE/ N SE/Perf,
w/in- Perf positive, (participants PP/SE, Self-
person negative, Performance × repeated) between- Goal Perf efficacy Lab or Publication
Study SE/Perf? or null? measure measures) persons r setting trend scale field status
Jundt (2009) No NA TANDEM performance 3,372 .21, .35, .32 No .37* Likert Lab Unpub
McLean & Yeo (2006) No NA Air traffic control decision 2,813 .42, .63, .74 Yes .50* Unipolar Lab Unpub
points
Neal & Yeo (2003) Yes NegativeLT, PP Air traffic control penalty 355 −.23, .29, −.11 No .33* Unipolar Lab Unpub
Study 1 points (reverse scored)
Richard et al. (2006) Yes NullLT Exams in a statistics class 344 .10, .38, .49 No −.26* Unipolar Field Pub
Study 1
Richard et al. (2006) Yes NullLT or Positive Chemical reactor 1,036 .31, .20, .17 No .59* Likert Lab Pub
Study 2 simulation scores
Schmidt & DeShon Yes NegativePP Mastermind scores 590 −.09, .37, .55 No .07 Unipolar Lab Pub
(2009)
Schmidt & DeShon Yes NullLT Anagram scores 285 −.26, .34, .47 Yes .27* Unipolar Lab Pub
(2010)
Seo & Ilies (2009) Yes PositiveLT, PP Stock market simulation 1,870 .56, .50, .61 Yes .07* Unipolar Field Pub
scores
SITZMANN AND YEO
Sitzmann (2012) No NA Exams in a Microsoft Excel 828 .27, .07, .31 No −.40* Likert Field Pub
class
Sitzmann & Ely (2010) No NA Exams in a Microsoft Excel 776 .36, .07, .25 No −.31* Likert Field Pub
class
Sitzmann & Johnson No NA Exams in a Microsoft Excel 2,392 .12, .08, .24 No −.15* Likert Field Unpub
(2011) class
Sitzmann & Johnson No NA Exams in a Microsoft Excel 862 .24, .10, .36 No −.18* Likert Lab Unpub
(2012) class
Vancouver & Kendall Yes NullLT or NegativeLT Exams in an industrial- 296 −.15, .27, .54 Yes .30* Unipolar Field Pub
(2011) organizational
psychology class
537
continued
TABLE 1 (continued)
538
Note. SE = self-efficacy; Perf = performance; PP = past performance; NA = not applicable; Pub = published; Unpub = unpublished. Report w/in-person
SE/Perf? indicates whether the report includes information on the within-person self-efficacy/performance relationship. SE/Perf positive, negative, or
null? indicates whether the report concludes the self-efficacy/performance within-person relationship is positive, negative, or null. SE/Perf, PP/SE,
between-persons r indicates the within-person self-efficacy/performance and past performance/self-efficacy correlations as well as the between-persons
correlation without covariates in the analysis.
LT
indicates the study controlled for the linear trajectory.
PP
indicates the study controlled for past performance.
† indicates there were insufficient data to either examine the within-person effect of self-efficacy on performance or past performance on self-efficacy.
*p < .05.
SITZMANN AND YEO 539
The linear trajectory and lagged dependent variable are critical co-
variates in repeated measures designs. However, primary studies differ
regarding which (if any) covariates are included in the analyses (see
Table 1), which may in part explain why they reached different conclu-
sions regarding within-person self-efficacy effects. A primary contribu-
tion of this meta-analysis is we examine whether within-person reciprocal
relationships between self-efficacy and performance are affected by co-
variates and provide recommendations to ensure consistency in this line
of research. In the following sections, we clarify why the linear trajectory
and past performance/past self-efficacy should be included as covariates
and provide an overview of the debate regarding whether self-efficacy
should be residualized from past performance.
Linear trajectory. Linear trajectory is called different things across
studies, including exam order (Vancouver & Kendall, 2011), trial (Seo &
Ilies, 2009), and training module (Sitzmann, Ely, Bell, & Bauer, 2010).
For example, Yeo and Neal (2006) controlled for practice, which was
coded 0, 1, 2 . . . 28, 29 to represent the order of the 30 practice trials.
Regardless of the title applied, linear trajectory is operationalized as the
order of the repeated measurements.
Statisticians Singer and Willett (2003) suggest that the linear trajectory
is the single most important predictor in repeated measures analyses and
should be included as a predictor in every study of change. Yet, in the self-
regulation domain there has been a surge of within-person research with
little discussion of whether or why it is necessary to control for the linear
trajectory. Controlling for the linear trajectory is important for within-
person self-efficacy research because studies often focus on contexts in
which self-efficacy and performance increase (e.g., skill acquisition tasks)
or decline (e.g., tasks that get progressively difficult) across trials. When
both constructs trend in the same direction, they share variance as a
540 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
Performance
Self-Efficacy
r = .63 r = .82
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
Trial Number or Linear Trajectory Trial Number or Linear Trajectory
r = .48 r = –.07
Self-Efficacy 0
Self-Efficacy Deviations
Regression line
Moderators
Method
Meta-Analytic Methods
Results
performance residualized
Past performance/self-efficacy with covariates
Linear trajectory 36 30,733 .29 .30 .00 .03 4.08 .22 .35 .09 .52
Past self-efficacy 34 29,078 .30 .32 .00 .03 3.52 .24 .37 .09 .55
Linear trajectory & past 34 29,078 .30 .32 .00 .03 4.40 .24 .36 .11 .52
self-efficacy
Note. k = the number of studies included in the analysis; ρ = mean corrected correlation; Var (e) + Var (a) = sampling error variance plus variance due
to unreliability in self-efficacy measures; LL = lower limit; UL = upper limit.
549
550 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
Moderator Analyses
TABLE 3
Categorical Meta-Analytic Moderator Results
Laboratory 24 25,803 .21 .22 .00 .05 1.89 .10 .32 −.07 .51
Field 13 9,067 .22 .24 .00 .05 2.75 .09 .36 −.06 .53
Publication status
Unpublished 10 12,842 .21 .22 .00 .03 3.05 .04 .37 .01 .43
Published 27 22,028 .22 .23 .00 .07 1.90 .12 .32 −.10 .56
continued
TABLE 3 (continued)
95% confidence 80% credibility
N weighted Var (e) + Pop. % Var due interval interval
k Total N mean r ρ var (a) var to artifacts LL UL LL UL
Moderators of the past performance/self-efficacy relationship
Goal setting
Yes 13 9,575 .48 .51 .00 .02 5.57 .39 .57 .33 .69
No 23 21,158 .34 .36 .00 .05 1.92 .24 .44 .06 .66
Performance trend
Positive 18 19,374 .49 .52 .00 .03 2.89 .38 .60 .30 .74
Nonsignificant 8 5,048 .24 .26 .00 .03 6.05 .12 .36 .05 .46
Negative 10 6,311 .17 .18 .00 .02 8.19 .05 .28 .00 .35
Self-efficacy response scale
Likert 13 11,943 .23 .24 .00 .03 3.90 .11 .35 .03 .46
Unipolar 27 21,556 .44 .46 .00 .04 2.45 .35 .52 .19 .73
Laboratory vs. field study
SITZMANN AND YEO
Laboratory 24 23,474 .42 .44 .00 .04 2.50 .34 .50 .19 .69
Field 12 7,259 .27 .28 .00 .06 2.50 .11 .43 −.04 .61
Publication status
Unpublished 9 11,174 .35 .37 .00 .04 1.98 .22 .48 .11 .63
Published 27 19,559 .40 .42 .00 .05 2.37 .31 .49 .13 .72
Note. k = the number of studies included in the analysis; ρ = mean corrected correlation; Var (e) + Var (a) = sampling error variance plus variance due
to unreliability in self-efficacy measures; LL = lower limit; UL = upper limit.
553
TABLE 4
Weighted Least Squares Regression Moderator Results
554
Note. The column header lists the covariates included in the analyses. The continuous performance trend moderator was used in these analyses.
a
We controlled for past performance when examining the self-efficacy/performance relationship; we controlled for past self-efficacy when examining
the past performance/self-efficacy relationship.
†p < .10. *p < .05.
SITZMANN AND YEO 555
Hypothesis 9, goal setting did not have a significant effect on the self-
efficacy/performance relationship (β ranged from –.05 to .07). However,
the results primarily support Hypothesis 10. The past performance/self-
efficacy relationship was stronger if goals were established when no co-
variates were included in the analysis or either the linear trajectory or past
self-efficacy was the only covariate (β = .22, .32, .31, respectively, p <
.10), but this effect was nonsignificant when both the linear trajectory and
past self-efficacy were included as covariates (β = .19, respectively).
Hypotheses 11 and 12 predict that self-efficacy within-person rela-
tionships are stronger in studies with a more positive performance trend.
Self-efficacy had a more positive effect on performance when the perfor-
mance trend was positive in the analyses with no covariates (β = .67, p <
.05) and after controlling for past performance residualized (β = .49, p <
.05). However, the performance trend did not have a significant moderat-
ing effect when other covariates were included in the analyses (β ranged
from –.07 to .27), providing limited support for Hypothesis 11. The past
performance/self-efficacy within-person relationship was strongest when
the performance trend was positive, and this effect was significant regard-
less of which covariates were included in the analysis (β ranged from .37
to .64), supporting Hypothesis 12.
Discussion
Major advances in multilevel research were not attained until the 1980s
(Hitt, Beamish, Jackson, & Mathieu, 2007), which was after Bandura de-
rived self-efficacy theory in 1977. Thus, initial research nearly universally
focused on self-efficacy at the between-persons level of analysis. With the
benefit of hindsight, it is clear that self-efficacy should be examined as
both a within- and between-persons construct because about 25% to 35%
of its variance lies at the within-person level of analysis (Beck & Schmidt,
2012b). In the following sections, we review key research findings re-
garding within-person self-efficacy effects, study limitations, directions
for future research, and implications for the science and practice of peo-
ple at work.
and moderator analyses. Controlling for the linear trajectory and/or past
self-efficacy reduced the past performance/self-efficacy within-person
relationship, but the magnitude of the reduction was less than for
the self-efficacy/performance relationship. Moreover, past performance
and self-efficacy maintained a moderate and significant within-person
relationship—sharing 9% of their variance—after accounting for the co-
variates.
Theory has traditionally focused on moderators of the self-efficacy/
performance relationship, ignoring the fact that there may also be
boundary conditions for the past performance/self-efficacy relationship
(Bandura, 2012; Bandura & Locke, 2003; Vancouver & Kendall, 2011).
Moreover, the vast majority of moderator analyses have focused on the
between-persons level of analysis (e.g., Sitzmann & Ely, 2011; Stajkovic
& Luthans, 1998), which confounds these two relationships. As such, we
know little about the impact of moderators on the past performance/self-
efficacy component of this reciprocal relationship.
Disentangling these two effects suggests that established modera-
tors affect the within-person effect of past performance on self-efficacy
rather than vice versa. The performance trend moderated the past
performance/self-efficacy within-person relationship in the same man-
ner as the self-efficacy/performance relationship, and the effect remained
significant for the past performance/self-efficacy relationship after con-
trolling for other moderators and both the linear trajectory and past
self-efficacy. Bandura claimed that self-efficacy will only exert a ben-
eficial effect on performance when learning has occurred (Bandura, 2012;
Bandura & Locke, 2003). The current findings suggest that this claim
needs to be reversed to indicate that past performance will exert its
strongest effect on self-efficacy in contexts that enable learning. In such
contexts, the cumulative experience of performance improvements should
strengthen the belief that an increase in performance will lead to future
success.
Three other moderators are potentially important for understanding the
past performance/self-efficacy within-person relationship: goal setting,
the self-efficacy response scale, and the research setting. Past performance
had a more positive within-person effect on self-efficacy when goals were
set (than when goals were not set), which may be attributed to the fact that
performance feedback is more meaningful in these contexts. Goals serve
as the comparator in control theory; without goals, performance feedback
is rendered meaningless (Bandura, 2012; Carver & Scheier, 2000). Thus,
feedback has a more powerful effect on confidence when people are aware
of the level they are attempting to achieve.
The past performance/self-efficacy within-person relationship was
also stronger when self-efficacy was assessed with a unipolar rather than
558 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
controlling for the trajectory. Thus, some overlap in the constructs was
spurious, resulting in Richard et al.’s conclusion that self-efficacy does
not have a motivational effect on performance.
We also heeded Bandura’s (2012) suggestion and examined the impli-
cations of controlling for past performance with self-efficacy residualized.
Bandura proposed that controlling for raw past performance is an over-
correction because it removes some of the true effect of self-efficacy on
performance, and residualizing past performance from self-efficacy per-
mits an examination of the true effect of self-efficacy on performance.
In contrast, Heggestad and Kanfer (2005) suggest that the residualization
procedure removes excessive variance from the past performance measure,
beyond that accounted for by self-efficacy, resulting in self-efficacy having
an artificially inflated effect on performance when examined in concert
with residualized past performance. Consistent with both perspectives,
self-efficacy had a stronger within-person effect on performance when
controlling for residualized than raw past performance. However, this dif-
ference was not statistically significant and neither effect was significantly
different from the main effect without covariates.
In combination, our findings suggest that the linear trajectory is the
most important covariate in this line of research, and controlling for past
performance/past self-efficacy may not influence the substantive inter-
pretation of results. It would be impossible to draw these conclusions
from a qualitative review of the literature because there is inconsistency
in whether covariates are accounted for and little justification for such
decisions. To bring consistency to this research stream, we recommend
controlling for the linear trajectory in every study at the within-person
level of analysis, which is consistent with the recommendation of Singer
and Willett (2003; see also Bliese & Ployhart, 2002). Researchers should
also carefully consider whether to control for the lagged dependent vari-
able and provide a rationale for their decision, including a discussion
of whether their decision affects the substantive interpretation of results.
This is consistent with Becker’s (2005, p. 284) conclusion that “a clear
and convincing statement regarding why certain variables are controlled
is an essential hallmark of good science.”
Practical Implications
Conclusion
For over 2 decades, prominent theorists have been debating when the
direction of the self-efficacy/performance relationship is positive, neg-
ative, or null and whether self-efficacy theory or control theory more
accurately explains this relationship. Our results suggest that conducting
research solely at the between-persons level of analysis has led to mis-
statements regarding the role of self-efficacy in driving life’s successes
as well as the conditions under which self-efficacy’s effects are most
powerful. Examining both components of this reciprocal relationship at
the within-person level of analysis indicates that self-efficacy is not the
driving force compelling higher performance; rather, it is an indicator of
whether people have succeeded in the past.
The main effect, moderator, and covariate analyses provide strong
support for this conclusion. Together the main effect and moderator
analyses suggest that at the within-person level of analysis: (a) self-
efficacy has at best a moderate, positive effect on performance and a
null effect under other moderating conditions; (b) the main effect of
past performance on self-efficacy is stronger than the effect of self-
efficacy on performance, even in the moderating conditions that pro-
duce the strongest self-efficacy/performance relationship; (c) the effect
of past performance on self-efficacy ranges from moderate to strong
across moderating conditions, and the effect is statistically signifi-
cant across performance tasks, contextual factors, and methodological
564 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
moderators. The covariate analyses provide further support for this con-
clusion. Specifically, the within-person self-efficacy/performance rela-
tionship is near zero after controlling for the linear trajectory and past
performance, whereas the past performance/self-efficacy relationship re-
mains moderate and positive after controlling for the linear trajectory
and past self-efficacy. Overall, this pattern of results suggests that past
performance enlightens assessments of confidence rather than confidence
compelling higher performance.
REFERENCES
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis that are
also discussed in the text.
Ackerman PL, Kanfer R, Goff M. (1995). Cognitive and noncognitive determinants and
consequences of complex skill acquisition. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Applied, 1, 270–303.
Bandura A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psycho-
logical Review, 84, 191–215.
Bandura A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Pren-
tice Hall.
Bandura A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. American Psychologist, 44,
1175–1184.
Bandura A. (1991). Self-regulation of motivation through anticipatory and self-reactive
mechanisms. In Deinstbier RA (Ed.), Perspectives on motivation (pp. 69–164).
Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Bandura A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY: Freeman.
Bandura A. (2012). On the functional properties of perceived self-efficacy revisited. Journal
of Management, 28, 9–44.
Bandura A, Jourden FJ. (1991). Self-regulatory mechanisms governing the impact of so-
cial comparison on complex decision-making. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 60, 941–951.
Bandura A, Locke EA. (2003). Negative self-efficacy and goal effects revisited. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 88, 87–99.
Bandura A, Wood R. (1989). Effect of perceived controllability and performance standards
on self-regulation of complex decision making. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 56, 805–814.
*Bauer KN, Orvis KA, Brusso R, Tekleab A. (2011, May). Feedback, goal revision, and
the mediating effects of self-regulatory processes. Poster presented at the Annual
Meeting of the Association for Psychological Science, Washington, DC.
*Beattie S, Lief D, Adamoulas M, Oliver E. (2011). Investigating the possible negative
effects of self-efficacy upon golf putting performance. Psychology of Sport and
Exercise, 12, 434–441.
*Beck JW, Schmidt AM. (2010, April). Overall self-efficacy moderates within-person
effects on performance. Poster presented at the 25th Annual Conference of the
Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Atlanta, GA.
*Beck JW, Schmidt AM. (2012a). State-level goal orientations as mediators of the rela-
tionship between time pressure and performance: A longitudinal study. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 98, 354–363.
SITZMANN AND YEO 565
*Beck JW, Schmidt AM. (2012b). Taken out of context? Cross-level effects of between-
person self-efficacy and difficulty on the within-person relationship of self-efficacy
with resource allocation and performance. Organizational Behavior and Human
Decision Processes, 119, 195–208.
Becker TE. (2005). Potential problems in the statistical control of variables in organizational
research: A qualitative analysis with recommendations. Organizational Research
Methods, 8, 274–289.
Bliese PD, Ployhart RE. (2002). Growth modeling using random coefficient models: Model
building, testing, and illustrations. Organizational Research Methods, 5, 362–387.
Boyer DA, Zollo JS, Thompson CM, Vancouver JB, Shewring K, Sims E. (2000, June). A
quantitative review of the effects of manipulated self-efficacy on performance. Poster
session presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Society,
Miami, FL.
Carver CS, Scheier MF. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A control-theory approach
to human behavior. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
Carver CS, Scheier MF. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A
control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 19–35.
Carver CS, Scheier MF. (2000). On the structure of behavioral self-regulation. In Boekaerts
M, Pintrich PR, Zeidner M (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation research (pp. 41–84).
San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Colquitt JA, LePine JA, Noe RA. (2000). Toward an integrative theory of training moti-
vation: A meta-analytic path analysis of 20 years of research. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 85, 678–707.
*Daniels MA, Kain JM, Gillespie JZ, Schmidt AM. (2010, August). Latent profiles of goal
orientation and motivational outcomes. In Schmidt AM (Chair), A lens on the world:
Traits and states influencing motivational processes. Symposium presented at the
annual meeting of the Academy of Management, Montreal, Canada.
*Day EA, Boatman PR, Kowollik V, Espejo J, McEntire LE, Sherwin RE. (2007). Col-
laborative training with a more experienced partner: Remediating low pretraining
self-efficacy in complex skill acquisition. Human Factors, 49, 1132–1148.
*DeShon RP, Kozlowski SWJ, Schmidt AM, Milner KR, Weichmann D. (2004). A multiple-
goal, multilevel model of feedback effects on the regulation of individual and team
performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 1035–1056.
*Feltz DL, Chow GM, Hepler TJ. (2008). Path analysis of self-efficacy and diving perfor-
mance revisited. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 30, 401–411.
Fishbein M, Ajzen I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention and behavior: An introduction to
theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison Wesley.
Fu FQ, Richards KA, Hughes DE, Jones E. (2010). Motivating salespeople to sell new
products: The relative influence of attitudes, subjective norms, and self-efficacy.
Journal of Marketing, 74, 61–76.
*Gilson TA, Chow GM, Feltz DL. (2012). Self-efficacy and athletic squat performance:
Positive or negative influences at the within and between levels of analysis? Journal
of Applied Social Psychology, 42, 1467–1485.
*Heggestad ED, Kanfer R. (2005). The predictive validity of self-efficacy in training per-
formance: Little more than past performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Applied, 11, 84–97.
*Hepler TJ, Feltz DL. (2012). Self-efficacy and decision-making performance on a simu-
lated baseball task. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 83, 55–64.
Hitt MA, Beamish PW, Jackson SE, Mathieu JE. (2007). Building theoretical and empirical
bridges across levels: Multilevel research in management. Academy of Management
Journal, 50, 1385–1399.
566 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
Hofmann DA, Gavin MB. (1998). Centering decisions in hierarchical linear models: Im-
plications for research in organizations. Journal of Management, 24, 623–641.
Huffcutt AI, Arthur W. (1995). Development of a new outlier statistic for meta-analytic
data. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80, 327–334.
Hunter JE, Schmidt FL. (2004). Methods of meta-analysis: Correcting for bias in research
findings (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
*Jundt DK. (2009). Adaptability from a process perspective: Examining the effects of task
change type and a metacognitive intervention on adaptive performance. Dissertation.
Michigan State University, East Lansing.
Kanfer R, Ackerman PL. (1989). Motivation and cognitive abilities: An integrative/aptitude-
treatment interaction approach to skill acquisition. Journal of Applied Psychology,
74, 657–690.
Kozlowski SWJ, Gully SM, Brown KG, Salas E, Smith EM, Nason ER. (2001). Effects of
training goals and goal orientation traits on multidimensional training outcomes and
performance adaptability. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
85, 1–31.
Lipsey MW. (2003). Those confounded moderators in meta-analysis: Good, bad, and ugly.
The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 587, 69–81.
Locke EA, Latham GP. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal theory of
goal setting and task motivation: A 35-year odyssey. American Psychologist, 57,
705–717.
*McLean K, Yeo G. (2006, April). The moderating effects of goal orientations on reactions
to goal-performance discrepancies. Presented at the 33rd Australian Experimental
Psychology Conference, Brisbane.
Mitchell TR, Hopper H, Daniels D, George-Falvy J, James LR. (1994). Predicting self-
efficacy and performance during skill acquisition. Journal of Applied Psychology,
79, 506–517.
*Neal A, Yeo G. (2003). Does self-efficacy help or harm performance? An examination
of the direction of the relationship between self-efficacy and performance at the
within-person level of analysis. Australian Journal of Psychology, 55 (Supplement
1), 138.
Ouellette JA, Wood W. (1998). Habit and intention in everyday life: The multiple processes
by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 54–74.
Parker SK. (1998). Enhancing role breadth self-efficacy: The roles of job enrichment and
other organization interventions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 835–852.
Phillips JM, Hollenbeck JR, Ilgen DR. (1996). Prevalence and prediction of positive dis-
crepancy creation: Examining a discrepancy between two self-regulation theories.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 498–511.
Powers WT. (1973). Behavior: The control of perception. New York, NY: Hawthorne.
Powers WT. (1978). Quantitative analysis of purposive systems: Some spadework at the
foundations of scientific psychology. Psychological Review, 85, 417–435.
Powers WT. (1991). Commentary on Bandura’s “human agency.” American Psychologist,
46, 151–153.
Raudenbush SW, Bryk AS. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data
analysis methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Raudenbush SW, Bryk AS, Congdon R. (2004). HLM 6 for Windows [Computer software].
Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International, Inc.
*Richard EM, Diefendorff JM, Martin JH. (2006). Revisiting the within-person self-efficacy
and performance relation. Human Performance, 19, 67–87.
Salas E, Cannon-Bowers JA. (2001). The science of training: A decade of progress. Annual
Review of Psychology, 52, 471–499.
SITZMANN AND YEO 567
Scherbaum CA, Vancouver JB. (2010). If we produce discrepancies, then how? Testing a
computational process model of positive goal revision. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 40, 2201–2231.
*Schmidt AM, DeShon RP. (2009). Prior performance and goal progress as moderators of
the relationship between self-efficacy and performance. Human Performance, 22,
191–203.
*Schmidt AM, DeShon RP. (2010). The moderating effects of performance ambiguity on
the relationship between self-efficacy and performance. Journal of Applied Psychol-
ogy, 95, 572–581.
Schmidt AM, Dolis CM. (2009). Something’s got to give: The effects of dual-goal dif-
ficulty, goal progress, and expectancies on resource allocation. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 94, 678–691.
Schneider W, Shiffrin RM. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information process-
ing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1–66.
*Seo M, Ilies R. (2009). The role of self-efficacy, goal, and affect in dynamic motivational
self-regulation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 109, 120–
133.
Shantz A, Latham GP. (2012). Transfer of training: Written self-guidance to increase
self-efficacy and interviewing performance of job seekers. Human Resource Man-
agement, 51, 733–746.
Shiffrin RM, Schneider W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information process-
ing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological
Review, 84, 127–190.
Singer JD, Willett JB. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis: Modeling change and
event occurrence. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
*Sitzmann T. (2012). A theoretical model and analysis of the role of self-regulation in the at-
trition process from voluntary online training. Learning and Individual Differences,
22, 46–54.
Sitzmann T, Brown KG, Casper WJ, Ely K, Zimmerman R. (2008). A review and meta-
analysis of the nomological network of trainee reactions. Journal of Applied Psy-
chology, 93, 280–295.
*Sitzmann T, Ely K. (2010). Sometimes you need a reminder: The effects of prompting
self-regulation on regulatory processes, learning, and attrition. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 95, 132–144.
Sitzmann T, Ely K. (2011). A meta-analysis of self-regulated learning in work-related
training and educational attainment: What we know and where we need to go.
Psychological Bulletin, 137, 421–442.
Sitzmann T, Ely K, Bell BS, Bauer KN. (2010). The effects of technical difficulties on
learning and attrition during online training. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Applied, 16, 281–292.
*Sitzmann T, Johnson SK. (2011). Delving into the realm of the unconscious: Examining
the cyclical causes and effects of unconscious affect in online training. Unpublished
manuscript.
*Sitzmann T, Johnson SK. (2012). When is ignorance bliss? The effects of inaccurate
self-assessments of knowledge on learning and attrition. Organizational Behavior
and Human Decision Processes, 117, 192–207.
Snijders T, Bosker RJ. (1994). Modeled variance in two-level models. Sociological Methods
& Research, 22, 342–363.
Stajkovic AD, Lee DS. (2001, August). A meta-analysis of the relationship between col-
lective efficacy and group performance. Paper presented at the 61st Annual Meeting
of the Academy of Management, Washington, DC.
568 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY