Behavioral and Psychological Implications of Body Dissatisfaction: Do Men and Women Differ?
Behavioral and Psychological Implications of Body Dissatisfaction: Do Men and Women Differ?
3/4, 1988
The degree to which people are satisfied with their bodies may have pro-
found implications for their self-perceptions and behaviors. A relationship
between body satisfaction and self-esteem has been documented for both men
and women (Lerner, Karabenick, & Stuart, 1973; Lerner, Orlos, & Knapp,
1976; Rosen & Ross, 1968). Importantly, however, recent research suggests
that men and women may differ in two key aspects of body image satisfac-
tion: (1) the dimensions that underlie body image satisfaction and (2) the
direction of body image dissatisfaction.
First, the dimensions underlying body image satisfaction may differ for
men and women. Franzoi and Shields (1984) factor analyzed the Body Es-
teem Scale (BES), a measure derived from the Body Cathexis Scale of Secord
and Jourard (1953). The BES assesses satisfaction with a wide range of aspects
of the body. The factor analysis revealed different dimensions of body satis-
faction for men and women. The three factors of body esteem that emerged
for men were as follows: "physical attractiveness," which assesses satisfac-
tion with the face and physique that determine how "good-looking" a man
is; "upper body strength," which measures satisfaction with body parts and
functions that can be changed through exercise and that contribute to the
ideal, V-shaped male figure; and "physical condition," which taps satisfac-
tion with the body's functional aspects related to how "well the body works."
For women, the three factors comprising body esteem were as follows: "sex-
ual attractiveness," which assesses satisfaction with physical attractiveness
and sexuality; "weight concern," which taps satisfaction with body parts that
pertain to body size; and "physical condition, ''2 which measures evaluation
of physical qualities such as stamina and strength. This line of work sug-
gests that research on the relationship of body dissatisfaction to other varia-
bles should incorporate a multidimensional perspective on body esteem.
A second gender difference emerges from research that has examined
the direction of dissatisfaction with body size. Underweight appears to have
a different meaning for the two sexes. Underweight men seem unhappy with
their body weight, whereas underweight women appear satisfied (Cash, Win-
stead, & Janda, 1986). Studies comparing weight dissatisfaction of men and
women are needed that take into account the direction of the weight dis-
satisfaction. An interesting but unresearched question is whether the impli-
cations for self-esteem are comparable for someone who wants to be heavier
and someone who wants to be thinner.
At the core of the phenomenology of body image dissatisfaction are
two images: the perceived self and the ideal self (Silberstein, Striegel-Moore,
& Rodin, 1987). A failure to match the internalized ideal is likely to prompt
self-criticism and to damage self-esteem. Research on the relationship be-
tween body satisfaction and self-esteem in the two genders has produced con-
flicting results. Some studies have reported that women's body image
satisfaction is more highly correlated with self-esteem than is men's (Lerner
et al., 1973; Secord & Jourard, 1953), whereas other studies have found the
reverse to be true (Franzoi & Shields, 1984; Mahoney, 1974). A more differen-
tiated examination of body image may help illuminate this issue. From their
review of the literature on body image, Cash and Brown (1987) conclude
that investigators should include multiple body image measures in their
research and should recognize that body image is a multidimensional con-
struct.
It has been argued that, in contemporary society, weight is a domain
of central importance to women (Rodin, Silberstein, & Striegel-Moore, 1985).
METHOD
Subjects
weight of males was 167.06 pounds (SD = 25.4) and of females was 127.90
pounds (SD = 15.7). Compared to weight norms for their age and height
(Metropolitan Life Foundation, 1983), males were +6.4% (SD = 11.57)
and females were - . 7 7 % (SD = 9.92).
Procedure
ing from never to always.4 An overall score was obtained by summing over
all 26 items, with higher scores suggesting more disturbed attitudes toward
food, weight, and eating.
Reasons for Exercise. A Reasons for Exercise Inventory was developed
for this study. Items were chosen to represent seven general domains: exer-
cising for weight control, for fitness, for health, for improving body tone,
for improving overall physical attractiveness, for improving one's mood, and
for enjoyment. O f an initial 28 items, 4 items were dropped because o f in-
sufficient item-total correlations. The 24-item Reasons for Exercise Inven-
tory is provided in the appendix.
RESULTS
4The response indicating the most severe disturbance is scored 3, the adjacent response a 2,
the next response a 1, and the remaining responses are scored 0 (Garner et al., 1982).
5t Values of the gender comparisonson the individualBES items are availablefrom the authors
upon request.
224 Silberstein et al.
tracting the ideal figure from the perceived figure. Unless otherwise indicat-
ed, the absolute value of the discrepancy was used in further analyses in order
to ensure that dissatisfactions in one direction (e.g., wanting to be heavier)
did not cancel out dissatisfactions in the other direction (e.g., wanting to
be thinner). As Table I shows, men and women did not differ on the abso-
lute values of their perceived-ideal discrepancy scores.
The percentages of men and women choosing each figure as their per-
ceived and ideal are presented in Fig. 1. Virtually identical percentages of
women (22.7070) and men (21.7%) indicated that their perceived figure
matched their ideal. The majority of both male (78.2070) and female (77.3070)
subjects selected an ideal figure different from their perceived figure. Whereas
twice as many women (26.7°70) as men (12.8070) selected an ideal that was
two or more figures apart from their perceived figure on the BSD continu-
um, this gender difference in the degree of perceived-ideal discrepancy was
not statistically significant [x2o) = 2.01, p = .16].
When the direction of the perceived-ideal discrepancy was examined,
a strong gender difference emerged. Men were as likely to express a desire
to be heavier (43.4070) as to be thinner (34.8070). In contrast, only 1 female
subject (2.307o) wanted to be heavier, whereas 75070 of the females chose an
ideal figure that was thinner than their perceived figure [X2(l) = 23.06, p <
.OOll.
Implications of Body Dissatisfaction 225
1 2 3 4 5 0 7 8 9
relationship between body esteem and self-esteem. The three BES subscales
and the BSD were entered simultaneously as independent variables. The over-
all regression coefficient was significant for females (R ~ = .37, F = 5.10,
p < .01). The Sexual Attractiveness factor (b = .37, p < .05) and Physical
Condition factor (b = .43, p < .01) were significantly related to self-esteem
for women, whereas the Weight Concern factor (b = - . 3 6 , n.s.) and BSD
(b = - . 0 3 , n.s.) did not emerge as significant predictors of self-esteem.
For men, the regression coefficient was not significant. However, the
failure to find a significant regression coefficient for the relationship between
the various body satisfaction measures and self-esteem may be due to the
high intercorrelation among the body satisfaction measures for men revealed
in Table II. The zero-order correlations of self-esteem with the BSD, the BES
total score, and the BES subscales were all significant except for Physical
Attractiveness.
M a t c h i n g One's B o d y Ideal vs. Experiencing a Discrepancy. After hav-
ing explored the relationship between the degree of body dissatisfaction and
self-esteem, we next examined the relationship between the direction of body
dissatisfaction and self-esteem. In order to examine the hypothesis that for
men the effects on self-esteem are comparable for body dissatisfaction in
either direction, men were divided into three groups: those whose ideal BSD
figure was larger than their perceived (n = 16), those whose ideal BSD figure
was smaller than their perceived (n = 16), and those whose perceived figure
matched their ideal (n = 10). An analysis of variance was performed on the
self-esteem scores of the three groups IF(2, 39) = 4.52, p < .05]. Student
Newman-Keuls post hoc comparisons showed that the self-esteem scores of
the men whose ideal and perceived figures matched (M = 46.80, S D = 4.13)
were significantly higher than the self-esteem scores of men who desired to
be thinner (M = 41.93, S D = 7.04) and those who wished to be heavier
(M = 38.56, S D = 7.77). The self-esteem scores of men who wanted to be
heavier did not differ from those who wished to be thinner.
A comparable division into groups was done for women. However, only
one woman had a perceived figure that was smaller than her ideal, and there-
fore was not included in this analysis. Self-esteem scores of women who want-
ed to be thinner (n = 34, M = 40.66, S D = 6.17) were compared to those
of women whose ideal body matched their perceived figure (n = 10, M =
38.67, S D = 6.48). The scores of the two groups did not differ significantly
[t(42) = - . 8 5 ] .
B o d y E s t e e m a n d t h e E A T . The study was designed in part to examine
how body esteem related to disordered eating. We were forced to limit our
analyses to the female subjects only, because of the highly skewed distribut-
ed of EAT scores for male subjects, as discussed above. Significant correla-
tions were obtained between the EAT and the BES total score (r = - . 3 4 ,
228 Silberstein et al.
DISCUSSION
when people are portrayed in the media, in art work, or in research subjects'
drawings, males tend to be presented in head-and-shoulders views, whereas
females tend to he displayed in full figure (Archer, Iritani, Kimes, & Bar-
rios, 1983). The ways in which men and women are portrayed in our society
may contribute to the differential sources of body dissatisfaction for each
gender: face for men, body for women.
As expected, women scored higher than men on a measure assessing
behaviors and attitudes associated with disordered eating. Furthermore, wom-
en more than men reported that they exercised for weight control, and for
women, exercising for weight control was significantly related to scores on
the EAT. The pervasive incidence of dieting and weight concerns among fe-
males can be considered a "normative discontent" for women today (Rodin
et al., 1985). The current data confirm a view that, for many women, exer-
cising may be part of the complex of attitudes and behaviors that comprise
normative discontent.
Upon examining the interrelationships among the body satisfaction
measures, we found that the BSD and the weight dissatisfaction index were
highly correlated with each other for both men and women. Whereas for
men all three subscales of the BES were highly correlated with the BSD, for
women only the Weight Concern factor of the BES was related to the BSD.
Thus, it appears body size dissatisfaction is related to the multiple dimen-
sions of body esteem for men, but only to weight dissatisfaction for women.
These results suggest that it is not necessary for future research to include
both the BSD and the weight dissatisfaction measure. However, for women
in particular, the BES does add information not tapped by the BSD or weight
dissatisfaction measure.
A central question of the current study was the relationship of self-
esteem to the dimensions under investigation. For men, the regression of BES
subscales and BSD onto self-esteem did not reach statistical significance,
which may reflect the high intercorrelations of the independent variables.
The BES total score was significantly correlated with self-esteem for men.
The high intercorrelations of the BES subscales raises the question of the
usefulness of the subscale scores, particularly for males, suggesting that the
total score may be sufficient in future research. When examining the rela-
tionship of body size dissatisfaction to self-esteem for men, we found that
a man's self-esteem was affected by the degree of his dissatisfaction and was
equally influenced by his being heavier or thinner than his body ideal. The
impact on men's self-esteem of deviating from their ideal body size suggests
that weight may be a variable that is central to men's sense of self.
For women, the BES factors Sexual Attractiveness and Physical Con-
dition, but not Weight Concern, related to self-esteem. This counterhypothe-
sized finding replicates results reported by Franzoi and Shields (1984), who
utilized the same self-esteem measure as we did, and is consistent with find-
230 Silberstein et al.
REFERENCES
Archer, D., lritani, B., Kimes, D. D., & Barrios, M. Face-ism: Five studies of sex differences
in facial prominence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1983, 45, 725-735.
Cash, T. F., & Brown, T. A. Body image in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: A review
of the literature. Behavior Modification, 1987, 11, 487-521.
Cash, T. F., Winstead, B. A., & Janda, L. H. The great American shape-up. Psychology To-
day, April 1986, pp. 30-37.
Drewnowski, A., & Yee, D. K. Men and body image: Are males satisfied with their body weight?
Psychosomatic Medicine, 1987, 49, 626-634.
Fallon, A. E., & Rozin, P. Sex differences in perceptions of body shape. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 1985, 94, 102-105.
Franzoi, S. L., & Herzog, M. E. The Body Esteem Scale: A convergent and discriminant valid-
ity study. Journal of Personality Assessment, 1986, 50, 24-31.
Franzoi, S. L., & Shields, S. A. The Body Esteem Scale: Multidimensional structure and sex
differences in a college population. Journal of Personality Assessment, 1984, 48, 173-178.
Garner, D. M., Olmsted, M. P., Bohr, Y., & Garfinkel, P. E. The Eating Attitudes Test: Psy-
chometric features and clinical correlates. Psychological Medicine, 1982, 12, 871-878.
Garner, D. M., Rockert, W., Olmsted, M. P., Johnson, C., & Coscina, D. V. Psychoeduca-
tional principles in the treatment of bulimia and anorexia nervosa. In D. M. Garner &
P. E. Garfinkel (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy for anorexia nervosa and bulimia.
New York: Guilford, 1985.
Harter, S. Processes underlying the construction, maintenance and enhancement of the self-
concept in children. In J. Suls & A. Greenwald (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on
the self: Vol. 3. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbanm, 1985.
Johnson, C. L., Stuckey, M. R., Lewis, L. D., & Schwartz, D. M. Bulimia: A descriptive sur-
vey of 316 cases. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 1982, 2, 3-16.
Lerner, R. M., Karabenick, S. A., & Stuart, J. L. (1973). Relations among physical attractive-
ness, body attitudes, and self-concept in male and female college students. Journal of
Psychology, 1973, 85, 119-129.
Lerner, R. M., Orlos, J. B., & Knapp, J. R. Physical attractiveness, physical effectiveness, and
self-concept in late adolescents. Adolescence, 1976, 11, 313-326.
Mahoney, E. R. Body cathexis and self-esteem: Importance of subjective importance. Journal
of Psychology, 1974, 88, 27-30.
Implications of Body Dissatisfaction 231
Mahoney, E. R., & Finch, M. D. Bodycathexis and self-esteem:A reanalysis of the differential
contribution of specific body aspects. Journal of Social Psychology, 1976, 99, 251-258.
McGuire, W. J., & McGuire, C. V. Significant others in self-space: Sex differences and de-
velopmental trends in the social self. In J. Suls (Ed.), Socialpsychologicai perspectives
on the se~ Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbanm, 1982.
Metropolitan Life Foundation. Statistical Bulletin, 1983, 64, 2-9.
Rodin, J., Silberstein, L., & Striegel-Moore, R. Women and weight: A normative discontent.
In T. B. Sonderegger(Ed.), Psychology and gender: Nebraska symposium on motiva-
tion, 1984. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press, 1985.
Rosen, G. M. & Ross, A. O. Relationship of body image to self-concept. Journal of Consult-
ing and Clinical Psychology, 1968, 32, 100.
Rosenberg, M. Societyand the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: University Press, 1965.
Secord, P. F., & Jourard, S. M. The appraisal of body-cathexis: Body cathexis and the self.
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1953, 17, 343-347.
Silberstein, L. R., Striegel-Moore, R. H., & Rodin, J. Feeling fat: A woman's shame. In H.
B. Lewis(Ed.), The role of shame in symptom formation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1987.
APPENDIX
R e a s o n s f o r Exercise I n v e n t o r y 6
People exercise for a variety o f reasons. When people are asked why
they exercise, their answers are sometimes based on the reasons they believe
they s h o u l d have for exercising. What we want to know are the reasons peo-
ple actually have for exercising. Please respond to the items below as honestly
as possible. To what extent is each o f the following an important reason that
you have for exercising? Use the scale below, ranging from 1 to 7, in giving
your answers. (If you never exercise, please skip this section.)
1 2 3 4 5 7
M o o d (~t = .79)
8. To cope with sadness, depression
9. To cope with stress, anxiety
10. To increase my energy level
I1. To improve my mood
Health (ct = .73)
12. To improve my cardiovascular fitness
13. To improve my overall health
14. To increase my resistance to illness and disease
15. To maintain my physical well-being
Attractiveness (~ = .78)
16. To improve my appearance
17. To be attractive to members of the opposite sex
18. To be sexually desirable
E n j o y m e n t (t~ = .67)
19. To meet new people
20. To socialize with friends
21. To have fun
Tone (t~ = .74)
22. To redistribute my weight
23. To improve my overall body shape
24. To alter a specific area of my body