CFA Jamovi
CFA Jamovi
A DISSERTATION
SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF THE
UNIVERSITY OF MINNESOTA
BY
Alyssa A. Schardt
May 2019
©Alyssa A. Schardt, 2019
i
Acknowledgements
Graduate school, and the dissertation process in particular, has been arduous and
only possible because of the support and guidance I have received from so many
individuals. First, I would like to whole-heartedly thank Dr. Faith G. Miller, my academic
experience has helped shape who I am professionally. I am forever grateful she endorsed
the vision I had for my research line, and encouraged my curiosity. I also wish to
acknowledge the continued support of my committee chair, Dr. Amanda Sullivan, who
also like to thank my committee members, Drs. Timothy Piehler and Annie Hansen-
Burke. Dr. Piehler’s input on my methodology and analyses has been invaluable, and Dr.
graduate school. Dr. Hansen-Burke’s words of wisdom and dedication to her students’
completion continues to inspire me. In addition, I would like to thank Dr. Scott
own path.
friends over the past five years. Our mutual support for one another created an
environment that allowed us to flourish and find joy in stressful times (and set a high
expectation for applause). I would also like to formally thank Jordan Thayer who has
fortunate for my family and their continued support. Specifically, I want to express my
appreciation for my sister, Lauren Mann, for always taking the time to put a smile on my
face (even from across the country), and my parents, Paul Schardt and Donna Brenner
Schardt, who could not have been more encouraging throughout every step of my
journey.
Lastly, I gratefully recognize the time and involvement from the students and
teachers who participated in this study. I can only hope I continue to work with educators
career.
iii
Abstract
Both engagement and motivation have been identified as constructs that are critical to
student success and are linked with later academic achievement. However, the multitude
of conceptualizations around these constructs and how they relate to one another has
become a point of contention within the field. The primary aim of the current study is to
engagement wheel (MEW), using the Motivation and Engagement Scale (MES; Martin,
2009; Martin, Ginns, & Papworth, 2017) and replicating the model structure with a more
expand the MEW by examining an adapted model structure that included alternate
Disaffection with Learning-teacher and student reports and Patterns of Adaptive Learning
Scale. Participants included 270 students in 3rd - 6th grade (predominately African
American, 67.4%), from an urban area in the Midwestern United States. Confirmatory
factor analysis (CFA) techniques were used to compare the higher order structure across
which best align with the MEW, demonstrated best fit across both the replication and
adaptability models. Thus, this study provided additional support for the structure of the
maladaptive motivation
iv
Table of Contents
Acknowledgements .................................................................................................. i
References ............................................................................................................. 56
v
List of Tables
Table 2: Comparing Means and Cronbach’s alphas for Subscales of MES Across
Samples ..................................................................................................... 64
Table 3: Descriptive Statistics for Subscales of MES, EvsD, and PALS .............. 65
Motivation ................................................................................................. 72
................................................................................................................... 73
Motivation ................................................................................................. 74
Engagement............................................................................................... 75
Table 12: Standardized Item Factor Loadings and Errors for Model 4 ............... 76
Table 14: Standardized Item Factor Loadings and Errors for Model 8 ............... 79
vi
List of Figures
2007). ........................................................................................................ 84
................................................................................................................... 85
traits........................................................................................................... 86
engagement. .............................................................................................. 87
loadings ..................................................................................................... 93
loadings ..................................................................................................... 94
viii
List of Appendices
Appendix F: Patterns of Adaptive Learning Scales: Student Survey Items ....... 107
1
CHAPTER 1
Introduction
Engagement and motivation in school make it possible for students to learn in the
classroom, and therefore are foundational to academic success (e.g., Greenwood, Terry,
Marquis, & Walker, 1994). To this end, both motivation and engagement have been
identified as academic enablers (DiPerna & Elliott, 2002). While variation exists in how
engagement aligns more to actions while motivation aligns more to thoughts. Throughout
academic achievement (e.g., Finn & Rock, 1997). Academic engagement is positively
correlated with standardized test scores and grades (e.g., Marks, 2000; Singh, Granville,
& Dika, 2002), and has been established as a protective factor against school risk factors
(Finn & Rock, 1997). Alternatively, a lack of engagement has been associated with
school dropout (Archambault, Janosz, Morizot, & Pagani, 2009). This is true for students
with and without exceptionality; if engagement is low, students are less likely to
complete high school (Reschly & Christenson, 2006). Additionally, early difficulties with
academic engagement have been linked to a long-term negative impact on grades and test
major problem across all grade levels, regardless of experience levels of the teachers
Students’ engagement is also a concern for teachers; when a child is not academically
engaged it not only contributes to their own lack of learning, but also has the potential to
negatively impact the teacher and other students in the classroom. According to one
survey, 36% of teachers reported losing more than two hours of instruction time per week
due to behavioral concerns (Walker, Ramsey, & Gresham, 2003). Findings were even
more drastic for teachers in urban areas—Walker and colleagues reported 21% of
teachers in urban settings lost more than four hours per week because of behavioral
Over the past several decades, the impact engagement and motivation have on
academic success has become well-established in the research literature, with the benefits
& Wylie, 2012; DiPerna & Elliott, 2002; Finn & Rock, 1997). Indeed, to date over eight
theories have been developed to help explain the phenomena of motivation and
theory is that engagement and motivation are generally considered to be closely related
this body of research also reveals inconsistencies with how motivation and engagement
are conceptualized, measured, and defined. Thus, while there is agreement on the concept
of motivation and engagement broadly and their academic benefits, there is still
disagreement in the field regarding operationalization of these constructs and how they
3
relate to one another. From some theoretical perspectives, engagement and motivation
than two separate factors. This was the case for the original motivation and engagement
wheel, a model of engagement and motivation established by Martin (2007), which has
since been revised. However, many researchers posit engagement and motivation are
closely related, but separate constructs—a more prominent and current perspective in the
field (e.g., DiPerna, Volpe, & Elliott, 2002; Martin, Ginns, & Papworth, 2017; Reschly &
Christenson, 2012). Given these differing perspectives, the conceptual and theoretical
relationship between engagement and motivation has become a point of contention in the
field (Reschly & Christenson, 2012). Consequently, the current state of the science lacks
coherence and consensus. The primary aim of this research study was to add clarity to
understand the relationship between these broad constructs. These perspectives were
examined in the current study through comparing model fit for models that represent a
single combined engagement and motivation latent construct and other models that
separated engagement and motivation into multiple latent constructs, replicating models
associated with the updated version of the motivation engagement wheel (MEW; Martin
et al., 2017).
Recently, Martin and colleagues (2017) proposed an updated model for the MEW
inclination, energy, emotion, and drive relevant to learning, working effectively, and
achieving,” while engagement is defined as “the behaviors that reflect this inclination,
4
energy, emotion, and drive” (Martin et al., 2017, p. 150). In this most recent study,
Martin and colleagues (2017) used confirmatory factor analysis to compare four higher-
engagement and motivation were a single construct, a two-factor higher-order model that
motivation. Fit indices indicated that the four-factor model had the best fit. Thus, within
this updated model, engagement is broken into adaptive and maladaptive behaviors, and
motivation is also separated into adaptive and maladaptive cognitions (see Figure 1).
While this model holds promise, several gaps and limitations exist that require further
study.
Rationale
The MEW model was chosen for replication over other models of engagement
and motivation for a variety of reasons. First, the MEW holds promise because it posits a
aspects from a variety of conceptualizations. While some models propose that motivation
drives engagement (i.e., DiPerna & Elliott, 2002) and others suggest that engagement
influences motivation (i.e., Reeve & Lee, 2014), the MEW posits a cyclical relationship
Chapter 2). Another reason the MEW was chosen in the current study is the explicit
5
inclusion of both positive and negative components of engagement and motivation within
the model. While inhibitors and facilitators of these constructs are universally accepted
and measured, the MEW provides a parsimonious conceptualization and is the only
model that parses apart adaptive and maladaptive factors into their own constructs. In
other words, most models will measure both positive and negative components on the
same scale, but Martin’s model provides additional information by separating adaptive
provide information about adaptive engagement (e.g., task management). Thus, more
cognitions and behaviors, interventions could be better targeted to address the underlying
behavioral therapy, while maladaptive behaviors may be better served through a self-
monitoring intervention. Therefore, the current study aimed to replicate Martin and
colleagues’ (2017) MEW model and assess the adaptability of their model using
engagement and motivation, there are number of limitations associated with their
research. First, neither the original nor the updated model has been replicated outside of
the original research group despite being widely accepted and used over the past 10
years. Replication is particularly essential with confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) due to
the influence of sample on factor analytic techniques (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2013).
6
Additionally, the model relies solely on the Motivation and Engagement Scale (MES)
latent variables. Maruyama (1998) suggested using multiple measures for each construct
is the only defensible way in which to create viable models. Additionally, utilizing
measures that are not directly derived from the MEW will permit further validation of the
model, allowing for the possibility to better understand how the MEW bridges multiple
students in the United States. In addition, although the MES was developed and has been
validated for students nine years of age and older, the higher order structure of the MEW
has only been tested with a secondary sample. Thus, there is a need to further examine
Purpose. The primary purpose of this study was to examine the relationship
between adaptive and maladaptive engagement and motivation by replicating the models
presented by Martin et al. (2017) with a more diverse, and elementary aged American
sample to confirm their four-factor higher-order model. Secondarily, this study aimed to
capture additional components that make up these constructs beyond those included in
engagement that include components of these constructs beyond those currently assessed
engagement and motivation beyond what is captured by the measure created by Martin
and colleagues.
Research questions. Based on prior research and theory and using the MEW as
the conceptual base and the MES, Engagement Versus Disaffection with Learning-
teacher and student reports (EvsD), and Patterns of Adaptive Learning Scale (PALS) as
the measurement base, this study aimed to answer the following research questions:
as determined by CFA?
Based on findings from Martin and colleagues, it was expected that the four-
factor structure of the MEW would also fit a more diverse, American, elementary aged
motivation, that capture additional components of these constructs (e.g., interest and on-
task behavior; see Appendix A), would fit within the adaptive motivation, adaptive
in Martin’s (2017) updated MEW, thus upholding the four-factor higher order structure
of the model.
8
CHAPTER 2
Literature Review
is important to first contextualize the intricacy and complexity of doing so. For example,
Berliner, 1985) or, more commonly, multidimensional (e.g., including internal and
engagement, differences even exist with regard to the number and nature of proposed
al., 2012; Fredricks et al., 2004), the operationalization of the individual subtypes of
To further complicate matters, the term motivation is often not defined (e.g.,
something;” Ryan & Deci, 2000, p.54). It is generally agreed amongst researchers that
orientation). To add to the confusion, similar terms are used to describe vastly different
concepts and vastly different terms are used to describe similar concepts for both
9
engagement and motivation. For instance, both affective engagement and emotional
engagement is often defined in terms of on-task behaviors, but also used to describe
perspectives and how engagement and motivation are defined. Depending on the
interchangeable single construct (e.g., Martin, 2007; Skinner & Belmont, 1993) or closely
related, but separate constructs (e.g., DiPerna et al., 2002; Martin et al., 2017; Reschly &
Christenson, 2012). Given the influence engagement and motivation have on academic
achievement, it is of particular interest to better understand these constructs and how they
relate to one another from both theoretical and applied perspectives. This chapter outlines
prominent theories of engagement and motivation, discusses the impact these theories
have on the conceptualization of engagement and motivation, and describes how they
relate to one another in order to synthesize findings from theoretical literature and
increase conceptual clarity of these constructs to better inform research and practice. For
the purpose of the current study, theories were operationalized as underlying ideas used
while models are specific to populations and measures utilized; b) theories are abstract
conceptualizations, while models are statistically driven; and c) theories are based on
manipulation.
the basis of AGT focuses mainly on the construct of motivation without explicitly
activity, concept of self, and the task outcome determine if success is of intrinsic or
extrinsic value (Ames, 1992). Across researchers, the name for the opposing processes
differ; however, the theory remains aligned to orienting towards internal gains, such as
acquiring knowledge, versus orienting towards external gains, such as grades or money.
Maehr and Nicholls (1980) refer to these processes as task-involvement versus ego-
involvement goals, while Ames and Archer (1988) utilized the terminology of mastery
versus performance goals. Similarly, Elliott and Dweck (1988) preferred the terms
mastery goals all refer to success as determined by internal gains. Ego-involved and
performance goals, on the other hand, refer to success as determined through external
markers.
11
While AGT does not explicitly discuss engagement, there is a focus on motivation
underlying belief that hard work leads to success, and failure can be remedied by effort
(Ames, 1992). Motivation is not overtly defined within this theory. Instead, AGT
provides the framework that informs motivational patterns (Ames, 1992). Specifically,
mastery goals are aligned to a motivation to learn, which is associated with a higher
quality of participation that will likely sustain across settings (Ames, 1992; Brophy,
performance goal lens, achievement with little effort is perceived as a greater success
orientation leads to a motivational pattern that is associated with avoiding failure. Thus,
performance oriented individuals are more likely to take on less challenging tasks in
While sometimes categorized as its own theory (e.g., Martin et al., 2017),
include perceptions of success and failure within mastery and performance goal
case of poor performance. It is important to note that neither success-oriented nor failure-
avoidant orientations is associated with a specific goal-type, but rather both mastery and
When AGT was originally conceptualized, goal orientations were often thought of
as more stable traits rather than a changeable perspective within an individual. However,
performance goal orientations, individuals with fixed mindsets believe that, if you have
the ability, then effort is unnecessary. In contrast, individuals with growth mindset value
effort and are more likely to take risks in achievement activities; thus, growth mindset
parallels mastery goal orientation. Dweck also emphasized that fixed versus growth
mindsets are not traits, but perspectives that can be taught. In other words, according to
the growth mindset perspective, educators could help facilitate learners’ understanding
that goals can be achieved through effort and persistence through challenges.
orientation has on motivational patterns. Students who are more aligned to the
performance orientation care less about the process of gaining knowledge, but are
with a motivation to learn. Thus mastery orientated individuals believe effort and success
are linked, and are therefore more likely to persist through challenges. AGT has been
versus success oriented perspectives, and has even driven much of the research on growth
dynamics (e.g., concepts of self), actions, and environmental factors (Bandura, 1986).
Based in this theory, goal setting, causal attributions, self-regulation, and self-efficacy are
considered key constructs that underlie both engagement and motivation (Eccles &
Wigfield, 2002; Schunk & Mullen, 2012). Similar to AGT, neither motivation nor
engagement are explicitly defined within this theory. Instead, it is the reciprocal
relationship between the personal factors, actions, and environment that influence both
influences task choice, effort, persistence, and use of learning strategies (i.e., behaviors;
Schunk & Mullen, 2012). Conversely, these actions lead to increased learning, which
This process is often discussed using the term self-regulation within engagement and
motivation literature (Eccles & Wigfield, 2002). Self-regulated students are viewed as
motivationally and behaviorally active in their own learning process and in achieving
their personal goals. Thus, goal setting also plays an important role within the SCT
14
(1981), the intervening process of goal setting and self-evaluation affects self-motivation,
with self-efficacy being associated with greater achievement gains. Thus, unlike AGT
Self-Determination Theory
“energized or activated towards” a task (Ryan & Deci, 2000, p. 54). Thus the primary
humans are driven to maintain an optimal level of stimulation while balancing a need for
competence (Eccles & Wigfield, 2002). In other words, individuals seek tasks that they
are successful in completing, but are not overly simplistic. According to Deci and Ryan
separate outcome (e.g., getting a good grade; Ryan & Deci, 2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic
motivation are maintained when individuals feel both competent and self-determined
(Ryan & Deci, 2000). Instead, individuals who are truly self-determined transfer external
15
the behavior), and integrated regulation (internal based on one’s own values; Ryan &
Deci, 2000). For example, consider the possible progression of a student in school –
initially a student may complete homework for an extrinsic reason (e.g., a grade), but
later may recognize that good grades are expected from his or her parents (introjected
regulation). Later that student may realize that good grades will lead to a better college
(identified regulation), and eventually the student may begin to value the act of learning
persistence, a more positive sense of self, and higher quality engagement in achievement
Additional Theories
efforts (Weiner, 1985). In other words, their explanation for success or failure will not
only drive their engagement in an activity, but also their future motivation for
achievement. Ability, effort, task difficulty, and luck are some examples of achievement
attributions, which can be categorized under locus of control, stability, and controllability
(Eccles & Wigfield, 2002). Locus of control is represented by internal versus external
believed causality. Stability refers to whether causes can change with time (e.g., effort
can change over time). Lastly, the controllability dimension refers to causes that can and
cannot be controlled (e.g., skill versus luck; Weiner, 1985). Similar to SCT and AGT,
16
attribution theory also suggests goal setting as an essential, but not the solitary
expectancy to attain that goal are central to motivational beliefs (Eccles & Wigfield,
2002).
motivation as the immediate subjective experience that occurs when people are engaged
to immediate reason for behavior, Eccles and Wigfield (2002) suggest that these theories
reflect a different perspective of the same point. Flow theory is also closely aligned to
of action and awareness, lack of self-consciousness, and a feeling of control over both
actions and the environment—is experienced only when an individual is fully engaged
emotionally (Nakamura & Csikszentmihalyi, 2009). Also, flow can only be achieved
when skill and challenge level are matched, which aligns with SDT’s optimal
performance perspective.
One limitation of flow theory is the fact that it primarily describes optimal levels
and aversive situations; relaxation and flow are both intrinsically rewarding, while
overwhelming demands and apathy are aversive situations. Thus, flow theory describes
17
Interest theories. While there is not a single theory for interests’ role in
motivation and engagement, there was an upsurge in this theoretical framework during
the 1990’s (Eccles & Wigfield, 2002). Across these theories, individual versus situational
interests are differentiated; individual interests refer to stable orientations toward domains
activity. Individual interests can be broken down even further into feeling- (feelings
associated with a task) and value-related (personal significance) valences (Eccles &
Wigfield, 2002). Specific to learning, both individual and situational interest are
associated with deeper levels of learning (Schiefele, 1991). Thus, interest, which is
merely implied from other theoretical perspectives, may be an important factor missing
that autonomy, competence, and relatedness are fundamental motivational needs that lead
expansion of SDT, but also incorporates some components of interest theories. SDT was
(decisions are beyond own control; Deci & Ryan, 1985), which parallel those represented
and motivation, self-system model assumes motivation is a necessary, but not sufficient,
18
suggest that students who have opportunities aligned with these motivational factors (e.g.,
task choice, scaffolded learning opportunities) will be more engaged in the classroom.
amount of effort an individual puts forth to reach a goal is a function of their expectancy
to reach the goal with effort and the value they place on reaching said goal (Brophy,
1983). Brophy argued that motivation to learn must not be maximized, but rather
optimal level. Despite attribution theory being founded from expectancy X Value theory,
it also closely aligns to SDT’s perspective that humans are motivated to maintain an
optimal level of stimulation and competence. Additionally, flow theory also emphasizes
and motivation it is clear that the relationship between these constructs can be described
in a number of ways. While some (e.g., SCT & SDT) view engagement and motivation as
mutually reinforcing of one another, others (e.g., AGT) view motivation as an underlying
Models, on the other hand, are statistical representations based on theory. Therefore, a
limited number of models that describe how these constructs relate to one another, using
enablers as a set of attitudes and behaviors that allow students to benefit from school.
Within their model, engagement and motivation, along with study and interpersonal
skills, were identified as the constructs that enable students to learn. In this model,
motivation was defined as a person's internal desire to complete a task based on interest,
persistence, and approach regarding academic subjects (DiPerna & Elliott, 1999).
Engagement, on the other hand, was defined as attention and active participation in
classroom activities (DiPerna & Elliott, 2002). Similar to the self-system model of
motivation, DiPerna and colleagues (2002) found that motivation leads to engagement. In
other words, motivation is foundational to engaged behaviors. However, unlike the self-
system model of motivation, the model of academic enablers does not clearly align to any
For their models, DiPerna et al. (2002) relied on the Academic Competence
Evaluation Scales (ACES), which is a measure they created (DiPerna & Elliott, 1999).
While an ACES – Student Rating exists for grades 6th through 12th, the model relied on a
single-informant using the ACES – Teacher Rating for kindergarten through 12th grade.
The ACES provide subscale ratings for both motivation and engagement. With regard to
was between .88 and .97 for teachers. Internal consistency fell between .97 - .98 for
teachers. Regarding validity, concurrent validity between academic performance and the
teacher ACES rating fell between .31 and .87. The ACES demonstrates adequate
psychometric properties for measuring academic enablers and skills, but narrowly aligned
and motivation. Unlike the self-system model or the model of academic enablers, which
suggest motivation is necessary for engagement, Reeve and colleagues hypothesized that
engagement could influence motivation. Their hypothesis was founded in SDT, which
posits that individuals who are self-determined transition from external to internal
regulation (Ryan & Deci, 2000). The progression of internalization can be explained
requirement), identified (internal based on the usability of the behavior), and integrated
regulation (internal based on one’s own values; Ryan & Deci, 2000). Thus, increasing
engagement could help facilitate internalization. In other words, while a student may start
off engaged because of external expectations within the classroom, eventually that
Similar to DiPerna and Elliott, Reeve and Lee (2014) relied on a single-informant.
Additionally, Reeve and Lee relied on multiple measures of motivation and engagement
within their model. In order to measure engagement, they used the behavioral
engagement subscale of the EvsD. While the EvsD reports adequate psychometric
properties for the full measure, it is important to note that the whole measure was not
utilized. Regarding the measurement of motivation, PALS and the mastery goals scale
from the Achievement Goal Questionnaire (AGQ) were used. PALS has an internal
consistency that fell between .60 and .84, and a test-retest reliability between .34 and .61.
21
Additionally, convergent validity has been reported with goal questionnaires as falling
between.63 and .67 (Fredricks et al., 2011; Skinner, Kindermann, & Furrer, 2009).
Lastly, the mastery goal subscales of the AGQ has reported internal consistency between
.74 and .76, and discriminant validity between .3 and .51 (Finney, Pieper, & Barron,
2004). Reeve and Lee (2014) utilized multiple measures and theoretical perspectives to
create their model; however, only particular subscales were used rather than full
MEW. Martin’s (2007) original MEW was a model made up of 11 factors that
behaviors and impeding cognitions. The adaptive cognitive dimension was made up of
self-efficacy, mastery orientation, and valuing of school, while the adaptive behavioral
dimension was made up of persistence, planning, and study management. Anxiety, failure
avoidance, and uncertain control were factors within the impeding cognitive dimension.
beliefs and emotions that influence and guide behavior, while engagement is described
through behavioral, emotional, and cognitive lenses (Way, Reece, Bobis, Anderson, &
Martin, 2015). Despite defining these constructs, motivation and engagement are used
interchangeably within this original model, treating them as the same construct.
Over the past decade Martin’s MEW has undergone a series of modifications. In
the most recent model, Martin and colleagues (2017) parsed apart engagement and
motivation within the wheel. They replaced terminology of the four quadrants with
engagement rather than considering positive and negative behavior and cognitions. Thus
engagement is broken into adaptive (i.e., persistence, planning, and task management)
is also separated into adaptive (i.e., self-belief, learning focus, and valuing) and
maladaptive (i.e., anxiety, failure avoidance, and uncertain control) cognitions (see
Figure 1).
et al. (2017) used confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to examine four possible higher
order factors. The main sample consisted of 5432 secondary students (grades 7th through
12th) from 12 different independent high schools. Just under half of the participants were
female (43%) and 57% were male. The mean socioeconomic status of the participants
was higher than the national average, with 8% of the sample from a non-English speaking
background. For this population, it was determined that the four-factor structure, most
aligned with the MEW, had superior fit (χ2(885) = 8357, CFI = 0.92, RMSEA = 0.039,
SRMR = 0.060) in comparison to the positive and negative two-factor, the engagement
models. According to fit indices, both the four-factor and the positive and negative two-
factor models yielded acceptable fit as demonstrated by CF > 0.90, RMSEA < 0.08, and
SRMR < 0.08. However, the four factor higher order model had the best fit,
0.001). While this study demonstrated that engagement and motivation are unique
relied on a single-informant through a single measure that was created by their own
research team. The MES was used to measure students’ perception of their engagement
and motivation in all models of the MEW. The MES has demonstrated test-retest
reliability between .61 and .81, and internal consistency between .70 and .87, as well as
criterion related validity between .40 and .63 for achievement and academic outcomes
(Fredricks et al., 2011; Liem & Martin, 2012). Thus, the MES appears to demonstrate
adequate psychometric properties for measuring engagement and motivation, but similar
The MEW encompasses multiple theoretical perspectives that most closely align
to AGT and SCT. AGT, which focuses on goal orientation, aligns to the learning focus
and failure avoidance factors found within the motivation quadrants. SCT on the other
hand, focuses on the relationship between personal dynamics, environmental factors, and
causal attributions. Within the MEW self-belief, persistence, anxiety, and uncertain
control all fall within the SCT perspective. However, according to Martin et al. (2017)
the MEW was also influenced by attribution theory, expectancy X value theory, and
SDT. Thus, unlike other models of engagement and motivation that outline the direction
MEW posits a reciprocal relationship between the two constructs. Therefore, the MEW
was utilized in the current study because of the broad conceptualization of engagement
Many of the models presented offer promising explanations for how motivation
and engagement relate to one another in an educational context. However, there are a
further research to explore our understanding of motivation and engagement and how
One limitation to existing models is the lack of replication. To date, the models
presented above have not been replicated outside of the original research teams.
Specifically for the MEW, which is the theoretical basis of the present study, Martin and
colleagues have researched the factor structure of their model over the past decade.
Despite the fact that they have run extensive studies across multiple countries, replication
outside of their research team has not occurred. According to Tabachnick & Fidell
(2013), replication is particularly important within CFA, the analyses used to compare
models and their underlying factor structure. Beyond CFA, replication is an area of
critical concern within the field and should be viewed as an essential aspect of science
Another limitation is the fact that many of these models rely on a single-informant
Maruyama (1998), using multiple measures for each construct is the only defensible way
minimum number of recommended items. Martin and colleagues, for example, relied
solely on their MES to build their model of motivation and engagement, while DiPerna
25
and Elliott relied on their ACES. Thus, violating Maruyama’s recommendation for
creating a defensible model. Relying on the MES is also problematic because it was
created by Martin’s team. Thus, the MES was created based on the MEW model rather
than relying on measures of motivation and engagement to inform the construction of the
model. For example, the MES includes items that are aligned to SDT (e.g., valuing), but
the MES does not include any questions aligned to interest (another aspect of SDT). It is
possible that interest still fits within their model under adaptive motivation, but has not
been assessed due to relying solely on the MES when examining the structure of the
MEW. Therefore, there is a need to not only replicate the MEW but also examine the
perspective of engagement (i.e., including both internal and external factors). The model
patterns and engagement. Similarly, key components and terminologies associated with
motivation and engagement varied across theories. Based on the theories and models
presented, motivation can be described through goal setting, concept of self, desire,
reward, interest, and expectancy of success. These components align to the terminologies
outlined in a review conducted by Murphy and Alexander (2000), which included goals
(e.g., AGT), intrinsic and extrinsic factors (e.g., SDT), interest (e.g., interest theories),
26
and concept of self (e.g., SCT) which were described as central terminologies associated
with motivation. The current study aimed to capture these central components of
engagement and motivation that are not assessed in the MES (e.g., desire, on-task
behavior) in order to determine if they align with the four-factor higher-order structure of
the MEW.
there are a number of overlapping constructs across theories and models. For instance,
individuals who are more aligned to mastery goal orientation tend to have higher self-
efficacy, relating AGT and SCT. This makes sense given the reciprocal relationship
individuals are more likely to take on and persevere through challenging achievement
tasks. Thus, the behaviors of mastery oriented individuals fuel the reciprocal relationship
described in SCT increasing self-efficacy. Similarly, SDT, flow theory, and the
expectancy X value model all emphasize the optimal level of performance based on
motivation. Specific to the various interest theories, Eccles and Wigfield (2002) suggest
that flow falls within the feeling-valence dimension of individual interest. Additionally,
intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, which are primarily discussed in SDT, align to the
seen as interchangeable (e.g., the original MEW; Martin, 2007). Other theories include
separate from one another (e.g., SCT; SDT). SDT is of particular interest because it
27
engagement. While other theories explain how motivation impacts engagement (e.g.,
AGT; attribution theory; Reschly & Christenson, 2012), SDT posits the concept that
engaging tasks actually drive motivation (Reeve, 2012). Similarly, SCT discusses the
reciprocal nature of self-efficacy and behaviors. Through this lens, motivational patterns
self, environmental factors, and actions, which influences both engagement and
motivation, which aligns to the updated MEW (Martin et al., 2017). The current study
within educational literature that cannot fully be answered at the present time. First, there
across theoretical perspectives. However, Martin’s updated MEW holds promise in that it
incorporates multiple theoretical perspectives and 11 factors that make up adaptive and
theoretical perspectives, but there is little consensus across researchers as to the essential
essential to academic success (DiPerna & Elliott, 2002). However, how can educators
28
and researchers intervene to help foster growth in these areas if we continue to lack clear
Additionally, how do motivation and engagement truly relate to one another? The
two constructs are often discussed in tandem, but are researched separately; there are
separate interventions that focus on engagement and motivation, and separate measures
for engagement and motivation. While multiple researchers have aimed to answer how
motivation and engagement are related, there is little to no replication, with each research
team reaching a different conclusion. For instance, DiPerna and Elliott (2002) used
structural equation modeling in their model of academic enablers and concluded that
motivation leads to engagement. Conversely, Reeve and Lee (2014) found that
engagement can actually alter motivation. Most recently, Martin and colleagues (2017)
concluded that engagement and motivation actually influence one another. In addition to
the lack of replication, many of these studies relied on a single measure of engagement
and motivation that was created by the same researchers; DiPerna and Elliott relied on the
ACES, while Martin and colleagues utilized the MES. If motivation and engagement are
constructs to help prevent the loss that occurs throughout adolescence (e.g., Archambault
Summary
also chaotic. Despite clear associations between engagement, motivation, and academic
and motivation. Operational definitions of these constructs not only differ across theories,
29
but often are not explicit. However, in synthesizing various theories and models of
motivation and engagement, patterns emerge; both engagement and motivation are
conceptualized to include both internal and external components, while motivation is best
no single underlying component that fully captures motivation or engagement, but instead
Thus, there is a need for additional research that not only replicate previous findings, but
also utilizes multiple measures to better understand the relationship between motivation
and engagement. The primary purpose of the current study was to examine the higher
order structure of the MEW, a model that encompasses multiple theoretical perspectives,
by replicating the models presented by Martin et al. (2017) with a more diverse, and
additional measures of these constructs, beyond what was used by Martin and colleagues.
30
CHAPTER 3
Method
Setting
through sixth) in an urban area within the Midwestern United States. According to 2017-
2018 school data for the first school, there were 338 students, in which 85% were African
American, and 15% were Hispanic. For the second school, which had 381 students, 37%
of students were African American, 31% were White, 13% were Asian, and 10% were
Hispanic. Additionally, 96% of the students at the first school were identified as coming
free or reduced price lunch. For the second school, 66% of students were identified as
coming from a low-income background and 12% had limited English proficiency.
Participants
Participants were recruited at the classroom level. Thus, teachers of 3 rd through 6th
grade students were first recruited for participation. After teachers were recruited,
students in their class were then recruited for participation. To this end, parents first
received a passive consent form, which determined which students were eligible to
participate. Parents who signed and returned the opt-out form if they did not want their
child to participate. Students whose parents opted-out of participating were provided with
an alternative activity (e.g., reading time, work-sheet) while the class completed the
questionnaire. Finally, prior to the start of the study, student assent was obtained from
those students for whom parental passive consent was obtained. Students were not given
31
any incentive for participating in this study. However, participating teachers received a
$10 gift card as compensation for their time completing study measures.
Thus, there were 360 potential participating students and 16 teachers initially
recruited for study participation. However, one teacher did not agree to have her class
participate (potential student n = 340), 14 parents waived consent, seven students did not
assent, 16 discontinued after starting, 15 were absent, and there were 18 technological
errors. Thus, 270 students and 15 teachers were included in the final sample. See Table 1
Procedures
for in-depth analysis of both children and teachers’ perspectives of academic engagement
and motivation. Data were collected at a single time point during the spring of 2018. The
items per student, while student questionnaires included three measures (Motivation and
Engagement Scale [MES], EvsD-student report, and Patterns of Adaptive Learning Scale
[PALS]) consisting of a total of 129 items. The order of the measures within the student
completed the measures at the same time on iPads. The student questionnaire took
administered the student questionnaires and read the items out loud in order to help
students comprehend each item and respond to the best of their abilities. Teachers were
order. Teacher questionnaires were completed while the graduate students administered
Measures
Student demographic information was obtained through the district online portal,
level of education and number of years teaching (see Appendix B). Four measures of
motivation and engagement were used to capture students’ engagement and motivation:
MES, EvsD – teacher report and student report, and PALS. Each of these measures has
been validated with upper elementary aged students and captures both adaptive and
model theoretically.
MES. Martin’s (2016) MES – Junior School is a student self-report measure for
that are grouped under four scales—self-belief (e.g., “If I try hard, I believe I can do my
schoolwork well”), learning focus (e.g., “I feel very pleased with myself when I really
schoolwork at first, I keep going over it until I understand it”), planning (e.g., “Before I
start an assignment I plan out how I am going to do it”), and task management (e.g.,
33
“When I study, I usually study in places where I can concentrate;” adaptive engagement);
anxiety (e.g., “When exams and assignments are coming up, I worry”), failure avoidance
(e.g., “Often the main reason I work at school is because I don’t want to disappoint my
parents”), and uncertain control (e.g., “I’m often unsure how I can avoid doing poorly at
school;” maladaptive motivation); and self-sabotage (e.g., “I sometimes don’t study very
disengagement (e.g., “I often feel like giving up at school;” maladaptive engagement; see
Appendix C for the full measure). Students provided a rating for each item on a Likert
sale (1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree). The MES provides scores for the
eleven subscales and four scales, where higher scores are better for the adaptive factors
and lower scores are better for the maladaptive factors. The MES – Junior School takes
approximately 10 minutes to administer and has been validated for students between the
ages nine and 13 (Martin, 2016). Research has demonstrated adequate test-retest
reliability (.61-.81) and internal consistency (.70-.87), as well as criterion related validity
(.40-.63) for achievement and academic outcomes (Fredricks et al., 2011; Liem & Martin,
2012).
EvsD. In addition to the MES, teachers and students completed the EvsD. The
EvsD includes four subscales for both the teacher and student reports: behavioral
engagement (e.g., “I try hard to do well in school”), behavioral disaffection (e.g., “When
something in class, I get involved”), and emotional disaffection (e.g., “When we start
something new in class, I feel nervous;” see Appendices D and E for the full measures).
For each item, respondents used a 4-point Likert-type scale: ranging from 1 (not at all
34
true) to 4 (very true). The EvsD, which was designed for students between the ages of
eight and 13, has demonstrated strong internal consistency for the teacher report (.81-
.87), but more variable internal consistency for the student report (.61-.85). Test-retest
reliability was also higher for the teacher report (.65-.82) compared to the student report
demonstrated for both the teacher report (.50-.81) and the student report (.34-.61;
PALS. Students also completed the PALS student self-report. The PALS Personal
Achievement Goal Orientations includes three scales: Mastery Goal Orientation (e.g.,
looking like I have trouble doing the work”). The PALS Academic–Related Perceptions,
Beliefs, and Strategies includes 8 scales: Academic Efficacy (e.g., “I am certain I can
master the skills taught in class this year”), Academic Pressure (e.g., “When I’ve figured
try hard in class. Then if they don’t do well, they can say it is because they didn’t try.
How true is this of you?”), Avoiding Novelty (e.g., “I would prefer to do class work that
is familiar to me, rather than work I would have to learn how to do”), Cheating Behavior
(e.g., “I sometimes cheat on my class work”), Disruptive Behavior (e.g., “I sometimes get
into trouble with my teacher during class”), Self-Presentation of Low Achievement (e.g.,
“One of my goals in class is to avoid looking smarter than other kids”), and Skepticism
35
About the Relevance of School for Future Success (e.g., “My chances of succeeding later
in life don’t depend on doing well in school;” see Appendix F for the full scales). PALS
uses five point Likert-type scales: items are anchored at 1 = "Not at all true,” 3 =
"Somewhat true,” and 5 = "Very true." The PALS has been validated with students
between the ages of six and 18. It has demonstrated adequate internal consistency (.60-
.84), and test-retest reliability (.34-.61). Additionally, convergent validity has been
were conducted using R version 1.0.136 and Jamovi version 0.9.5.15 to examine
potential problems with outliers and account for skewed distribution. Additional analyses
matrices for all subscales were calculated in order to create measurement models for the
additional measures of engagement and motivation (i.e., EvsD teacher and student
performed through MPlus 8.1 on the four measures presented above. CFA were
conducted following Rindskopf and Rose’s (1998) method of comparing nested models.
Models were tested by comparing the fit of least to most restrictive competing models.
While maximum likelihood estimation is most commonly used, it is only appropriate for
(WLSMV) estimation was used to estimate the model parameters because it is robust
against smaller sample sizes and variables with floor or ceiling effects and is the best
tested based upon models used by Martin et al. (2017). The hypothesized path diagrams
represent higher-order latent variables and ovals represent first-order factors. Absence of
a line connecting variables implies no hypothesized direct effect, while a curved line
represents a correlation.
order factor, which all 11 first-order factors load on (i.e., self-belief, learning
(Figure 3): This model hypothesized two higher order factors, Adaptive and
Maladaptive behaviors and cognitions. Six first-order factors load on the Adaptive
factor (i.e., self-belief, learning focus, valuing, persistence, planning, and task
management), while five first-order factors load on the Maladaptive factor (i.e.,
This model hypothesized two factors, Motivation and Engagement. Six first-order
factors load on the Motivation factor (i.e., self-belief, learning focus, valuing,
37
anxiety, failure avoidance, and uncertain control), and five first-order factors load
order factors load on the Adaptive Motivation (i.e., self-belief, learning focus, and
uncertain control), while two factors load on Maladaptive Engagement (i.e., self-
were tested based upon models used by Martin et al. (2017), but including alternate
restrictiveness.
order factor, which 19 first-order factors load on (i.e., teacher and student rated
skepticism about relevance of school for future success, teacher and student rated
disruptive behavior).
(Figure 7): This model hypothesized two higher order factors, Adaptive and
Adaptive factor (i.e., teacher and student rated emotional engagement, mastery
relevance of school for future success, teacher and student rated behavioral
behavior).
This model hypothesized two factors, Motivation and Engagement. Eleven first-
order factors load on the Motivation factor (i.e., teacher and student rated
achievement, skepticism about relevance of school for future success), and eight
39
first-order factors load on the Engagement factor (i.e., teacher and student rated
behavior).
order factors load on Adaptive Motivation (i.e., teacher and student rated
order factors load on Adaptive Engagement (i.e., teacher and student rated
relevance of school for future success), and five factors load on Maladaptive
Difference tests and χ2 probability were used to assess model fit. A χ2 that is not
statistically significant suggests that the hypothesized model does not differ from the
population model meaning that the model fits the population of students. Model fit was
1) Comparative fit index (CFI): A ratio of the fit of the estimated model over the
40
null hypothesis model where the closer to 1.0 the better the fit (Brown, 2014).
Values greater than .90 indicate an adequate fit and values above .95 indicate a
good fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999; Tabachnick & Fidell, 2013).
of a good fit in comparison to the ideal model, where the closer to 0 the better the
fit (Brown, 2014). RMSEA values equal to or below .08 indicate an adequate fit,
values equal to or below .06 indicate a good fit, and values equal to or below .05
indicate excellent fit (Browne & Cudeck, 1993, Hu & Bentler, 1999; Schumacker
RMSEA, where the closer to 0 the better the fit. SRMR values equal to or below
.08 indicate close fit, while a value equal to or below .05 indicates excellent fit
Chi-squared difference tests were used to determine if the nested models were a
statistically significant improvement over the baseline model. In the case of χ 2 difference
tests, a statistically significant finding suggests an improved model fit. Models that
resulted in statistically significant improvement over the baseline model were compared
and the final model was determined by the best overall fit indices.
41
CHAPTER 4
Results
Analytic Assumptions
Based on the number of parameters within the models, the initial objective was a
sample size of a minimum of 300 student participants. Despite fewer participants than
anticipated (i.e., n = 270), two separate analyses confirmed the sample size was adequate
for the replication models: Bartlett’s test of Sphericity [X2(946) = 4951, p < .001] and
Kaiser (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy (all items < .6 indicating adequate).
Sample size was smaller for the adaptability models due to the fact that it included
teacher surveys. Based on teacher response, only 185 students were included for the
adaptability models. Thus, KMO indicated some items > .6. However, Bartlett’s
Sphericity still indicted adequate sample size, X2(5995) = 13,529, p < .001. Based on a
Little’s MCAR test that was not statistically significant, data appeared to be missing
coded items (i.e., missing data were coded as 1 while non-missing were coded as 0),
which indicated neither school, grade, race, nor order of measures predicted missingness
(p > .05 across all items). In other words, data were missing with no clear pattern of
clustering based on demographics. Because less than 1% of data were missing, missing
data were left empty and pairwise deletion was utilized, which is recommended with
WLSMV estimation.
Studio and by calculating standardized skewness and kurtosis statistics (see Tables 2-6).
Skewness and kurtosis were present across measures. However, this was expected given
42
the nature of the scales (i.e., it was expected that students would rate their positive
motivation and engagement higher and their negative motivation and engagement lower).
Consequently, WLSMV estimation was used, which is recommended for both smaller
sample sizes and non-normal data. Linearity was assessed using scatterplots and best-fit
Multiple linear regression analyses were used to assess any statistically significant
differences in motivation and engagement, as measured by the MES, EvsD, and PALS,
based on race, grade level, gender, school, and the order measures were presented. Race
( = -0.02, p > .05), grade level ( = -0.02, p > .05), biological sex ( = 0.01, p > .05),
school ( = 0.04, p > .05), and order the measures were presented ( = -0.01, p > .05)
were not statistically significant predictors of engagement and motivation. The overall
Across measures, students rated their Adaptive Engagement and Motivation higher
and their Maladaptive Engagement and Motivation lower. On the MES, which provided
raw scores out of 5 and converted scores out of 100, the average subscale scores and
Cronbach’s alphas from the current sample aligned to Martin’s (2016) sample. On
average, subscale scores differed by only 1.5 for the converted scores out of 100 across
the two samples; the largest average score difference was found in the Valuing subscale
in which the current sample had a Mean of 84 (raw score = 4.20) compared to an average
of 89 (raw score = 4.45) for Martin’s sample, while some subscales demonstrated no
difference across samples (i.e., Self-Belief and Task Management). Differences between
Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were also minimal across samples with alphas ranging
43
between questionable (.6 < < .7) and good (.8 < < .9), with the majority of alpha
coefficients falling in the adequate range (.7 < < .8); see Table 2). Similar results (i.e.,
higher positive and lower negative components of motivation and engagement and
adequate alpha coefficients) were found on the student and teacher EvsD and PALS
measures. For information regarding average subscale and scale scores and Cronbach’s
alpha coefficients across measures see Table 3. For more detailed descriptive statistics by
Tables 8 through 11 present the Pearson’s correlation matrices for the subscales of the
four measures by adaptive and maladaptive motivation and engagement. Most of the
subscales demonstrated statistically significant correlations with one another, with the
exception of some scales from the teacher EvsD. The MES and student EvsD
0.61) across subscales. Additionally, both were weakly to moderately correlated with
replicated from models proposed by Martin and colleagues (2017), best fits an American,
elementary aged population across various measures, we examined eight possible higher
order factor structures using CFA. Evidence of motivation and engagement as distinct
constructs would be indicated by superior fit for the multi-factor higher order models.
Replication models. Four replication models were run to assess the higher order
structure behind motivation and engagement. Model 1, which was a single-factor higher
order model that combined engagement and motivation, was run first in order to allow for
44
model comparisons. Fit indices for Model 1 indicated poor fit, χ2(891) = 2372, CFI =
0.86, RMSEA = 0.08, SRMR = 0.10. Model 2, a two-factor higher order model that
separated adaptive and maladaptive constructs, yielded χ2(890) = 1981, CFI = 0.90,
RMSEA = 0.07, SRMR = 0.09. Model 3, a two-factor higher order model that separated
engagement and motivation yielded χ2(890) = 2354, CFI = 0.86, RMSEA = 0.08, SRMR
= 0.10. Lastly, Model 4, a four-factor higher order model that separates engagement and
motivation into adaptive and maladaptive constructs yielded χ2(885) = 1577, CFI = 0.94,
RMSEA = 0.05, SRMR = 0.08. Despite statistically significant χ2, indicating that the
models do not fit the population, the four-factor and two-factor adaptive and maladaptive
model yielded acceptable fit (CFI > 0.90, RMSEA < 0.08, and SRMR < 0.08) on
additional fit indices. Of these two models, the hypothesized four-factor higher order
model (Model 4) fit best, as determined by a statistically significantly different chi square
(Δχ2 = 175, df = 5, p < 0.001). Standardized factor loadings and errors for Model 4 are
presented in Tables 12 and 13, and Figure 10 presents the final model with standardized
coefficients.
additional adaptability models, with the same higher order structure as the replication
models, were run. Model 5, a single-factor higher order model that combined engagement
and motivation could not converge. Model 6, a two-factor higher order model that
separated adaptive and maladaptive constructs yielded χ2(5865) = 9769, CFI = 0.89,
RMSEA = 0.05, SRMR = 0.14. Model 7, a two-factor higher order model that separated
engagement and motivation could not converge. Lastly, Model 8, a four-factor higher
45
order model that separates engagement and motivation into adaptive and maladaptive
constructs yielded χ2(5860) = 9696, CFI = 0.90, RMSEA = 0.05, SRMR = 0.14. While
two of the models were unable to converge, the four-factor and two-factor adaptive and
maladaptive model yielded estimates and met criteria for adequate fit in at least a single
fit index. Of these two models, the hypothesized four-factor higher order model fit best,
0.001). Standardized factor loadings and errors are presented in Tables 14 and 15, and
CHAPTER 5
Discussion
The primary purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between
presented by Martin et al. (2017) with a more diverse, and elementary aged American
sample to assess the four-factor higher-order model that represents the MEW.
better fit in comparison to models that combined adaptive and maladaptive engagement
and motivation. In order to strengthen the MEW as a theoretical model and our
those included in the current MEW, fit within the four-factor structure proposed by
Of the four models replicated from Martin and colleagues (2017), the two-factor
adaptive and maladaptive model (Model 2) and four-factor model (Model 4) indicated
adequate fit for at least two fit indices. Although χ2 values were statistically significant
across models, indicating models did not fit, χ2 are impacted by several factors (e.g.,
sample size, correlations) and are often criticized for having compromised statistical
significance (Brown, 2014). Thus, additional fit indices for absolute fit (i.e., SRMR),
parsimony correction (i.e., RMSEA), and comparative fit (i.e., CFI) were included. Based
on these additional fit indices, Model 2 demonstrated adequate fit based on RMSEA and
47
CFI, while Model 4 demonstrated adequate fit based on CFI, close fit based on SRMR,
and excellent fit based on RMSEA. In other words, the four-factor model (Model 4)
indicated better fit across the different fit indices. Additionally, χ2 indicated Model 4
was a statistically significantly better fit compared to Model 2. Thus, findings from the
current study replicated Martin and colleagues’ (2017) outcomes, indicating adaptive
Despite demonstrating better fit compared to the single-factor model (Model 1),
constructs (Model 3) did not yield adequate fit. While this corresponded with findings
from Martin and colleagues (2017), it is still thought-provoking. The overlap between
recognized (e.g., Fredricks et al., 2004; Martin, 2012; Martin et al., 2017). However,
conceptualizing these constructs as highly related, but separate factors has become more
prevalent (Reschly & Christenson, 2012). While current findings show support for the
separation of engagement from motivation, as indicated by Model 4’s superior fit, results
also suggest the interconnectedness of these constructs (as indicated by the lack of fit in
Model 3). In other words, it was not until the positive and negative constructs were
separated that the differentiation between engagement and motivation was evident. This
explicitly discuss maladaptive components of these constructs (e.g., SCT and SDT).
Thus, one possible explanation is that the inclusion of the maladaptive components in the
current model interfered with the precision of engagement or motivation as latent factors.
48
In other words, the inclusion of the negative components of engagement and motivation
created constructs that were too expansive, which therefore did not fit as independent
latent factors. It is possible that engagement and motivation would still be best explained
as separate constructs if they were defined and measured in a way that was more
theoretically aligned (i.e., only including positive aspects of these constructs), but the
current study did not explore this further. However, the current study did highlight the
Findings from the replication models, aligned to the first research question, were
important for a number of reasons. First, neither the original nor the updated MEW has
particularly critical with CFA due to the influence of the sample on factor analytic
research. Recently, psychology has been faced with a “replication crisis.” A lack of
replication undermines findings and limits our understanding for whom and under what
conditions conclusions can be drawn. This is the first study that has examined the higher
order relationship between adaptive and maladaptive engagement and motivation for a
diverse sample. Martin et al. (2017) noted that their sample, which was made up of
mostly White upper-class Australians, was a limitation to their findings and replicating
their models with a more diverse population was suggested as a next step for research.
Thus, findings from the current study extend Martin’s findings, allowing us to expand the
The findings from the current study also indicate that the relationship between
49
these constructs may be stable across age. While Martin and colleagues focused on a
secondary population, the current study utilized an elementary aged sample. The fact that
the results replicated is particularly interesting given our current understanding of the
development of both engagement and motivation; engagement and motivation have been
shown to decrease from childhood into adolescence (e.g., Eccles et al., 1993; Greenwood,
2002; Harter, 1981). However, there have not been studies to date that have explored how
from the current study support the notion that engagement and motivation could endure a
similar relationship over time, despite decreases in engagement and motivation across
development.
structure of the MEW, the secondary research question aimed to provide additional
support for the MEW as a theoretical model through supplementing alternative measures
of adaptive and maladaptive motivation and engagement. Of the four adaptability models,
the two-factor adaptive and maladaptive model (Model 6) and four-factor model (Model
8) were the only models that produced estimates; the single-factor (Model 5) and two-
factor engagement and motivation (Model 7) models did not converge. A lack of
misspecified models; Brown, 2014). Thus, it is likely that Models 5 and 7 would have
theoretically and statistically related). However, Models 5 and 7 failed to converge across
50
attempts. Both the two-factor adaptive and maladaptive (Model 6) and four-factor higher
order model (Model 8) had adequate fit based on RMSEA and CFI. Of the two fitted
alignment to the replication models, the four-factor higher order structure best
Theoretically speaking, the components captured by the EvsD and PALS were
well aligned to the underlying theories that are foundational to the MEW. For instance,
Goal Orientation subscales in the PALS—aligns to AGT, SCT, and SDT, which were
adapted measures were statistically significantly correlated with subscales from the
MEW. Thus, it was expected that alternative measures would fit the four-factor structure
of Martin’s MEW model; a hypothesis that was supported by current findings (i.e., Model
The findings from the adaptability models provided insight on the malleability of
the MEW, but should be interpreted with caution. Based on these preliminary findings, it
appears that the four-factor structure of the MEW is upheld even without the inclusion of
the MES measure. This is of interest for a number of reasons. First, the MEW has
previously relied solely on the MES measure, which is potentially problematic because
utilizing multiple measures for each construct is the most defensible way in which to
create viable theoretical models. Thus, while the MEW is referred to as a theoretical
model, it has not previously met the criteria; instead, it is more aligned to a model of the
51
MES measure. Including measures that are not directly derived from the MEW provided
included multiple sources of information; both student and teacher’s responses were
captured in the model. Models can be strengthened by utilizing multiple measures and
multiple sources to represent latent variables. Thus, these findings could enhance our
understanding and application of the MEW, allowing a better understanding of how this
While this study provided additional support for the four-factor higher order
MEW model across various measures of motivation and engagement, there are several
limitations that must be considered and recommendations for future research. First, the
current study ended up with a lower than expected sample size (n = 270 for replication
and n = 185 for adaptability). While this met the threshold of acceptability from some
perspectives, it would be considered lower than advisable based on other rules of thumb
(Brown, 2014). Hence, findings should not be interpreted outside the context of the
sample and methodology used in the current study, especially with the adaptability
with a larger sample size. The current study only included an elementary aged sample.
Therefore, there is a need for further replication with secondary students from researchers
not associated with Martin. Additionally, despite using a racially diverse sample, the
The current study did not examine the directionality of the relationship between
positive and negative engagement and motivation. Future research should utilize
constructs. While other models have explored the impact engagement and motivation
have on each other (e.g., DiPerna or Reeve’s models), the directionality within the MEW
Another limitation was the use of iPads to gather responses across the PALS,
MES, and EvsD measures. While these measures are psychometrically sound, they are
typically completed in paper-pencil format and have not been validated in an online
format. Thus, while measurement equivalence was assumed, it is possible that the use of
an online survey completed on an iPad could have impacted the psychometric properties
of these measures. The international test commission recommends any new version of a
reports and found no statistically significant differences between the formats and that
online versions can be used with confidence (e.g., Carlbring et al., 2007;
Another possible limitation of the current study is the reliance upon self-report to
measure student engagement and motivation. The current study attempted to balance this
the measures were self-report. While self-report forms are the most common measures of
both engagement and motivation, there can be developmental issues depending on the
age range or cognitive ability of respondents, which can negatively impact validity
53
(Fulmer & Frijters, 2009). Although, issues like social desirability may impact responses
to self-report measures (Devellis, 2003), research has indicated that social desirability
bias is not as problematic with low-risk items such as those found in engagement or
motivation questionnaires (e.g., Miller, 2012). However, there are still issues of
interpretation for both self-report and ratings; it is possible that the individual completing
a measure interpreted an item differently from how the researcher or author intended.
While this study replicated and examined the higher order structure of Martin’s
(2017) analysis, it did not examine the first order factors. Many of the observed variables
were strongly correlated. Thus, it is possible that the observed variables could load into
different first order factors. This is especially true for the adaptability models. While
Martin’s research team has extensively researched the first order factor structure, the
current study relied on theoretical alignment to categorize the subscales of the different
measures into higher-order factors. Measurement models were used to support theoretical
alignment, but alternate first-order structures were not explored. Lastly, this study was
largely theoretical in nature and lacked explicit alignment to school based interventions.
Future research should explore the impacts of these different elements of adaptive and
Practical implications
it is of particular interest to better understand these constructs and how they relate to one
another from both theoretical and applied perspectives. Findings from the current study
are of particular interest in practice because it will allow educators to better understand
engagement and motivation to include adaptive and maladaptive components will allow
educators to create more precise and well-matched interventions and strategies for
working with students. For instance, it is possible for a student to have high adaptive
motivation, but also have high maladaptive motivation. Thus, while many educators
more efficient and effective for that individual student. Future research should explore
Conclusion
This study was the first replication, outside of the original research team, of
Martin and colleagues’ (2017) examination of the higher-order structure of the MEW.
Overall, results indicated that the four-factor higher order structure, which separated
motivation into individual constructs, demonstrated the best fit for a diverse, American,
elementary-aged sample, thus replicating findings from Martin. Secondarily, this study
motivation and engagement. Based on the hypothesized adaptability models, the four-
factor higher order structure demonstrated best fit despite not including the MES. This
supports the hypothesis that the MEW can be adapted to include alternate components of
positive and negative engagement and motivation, thus strengthening the notion that the
MEW is a theoretical model. However, there was no way to compare the fit of the
replication and adaptability models because of the method of estimation used for the
current analysis. It is recommended that future research continues to validate the structure
55
and adaptability of the MEW and explore the impacts of various interventions on these
distinct components of motivation and engagement in order to better understand how this
References
Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Horsey, C. S. (1997). From first grade forward:
Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the classroom: Students' learning
267. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.80.3.260
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., & Reschly, A. L. (2006). Measuring
doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2006.04.002
Archambault, I., Janosz, M., Morizot, J., & Pagani, L. (2009). Adolescent behavioral,
Bandura, A., & Schunk, D. H. (1981). Cultivating competence, self-efficacy, and intrinsic
200–215. doi:10.1080/00461528309529274
57
Publications.
Browne, M.W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K.A.
Bollen, & J.S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136-162).
Carlbring, P., Brunt, S., Bohman, S., Austin, D., Richards, J., Öst, L. G., & Andersson,
Christenson, S. L., Reschly, A. L., & Wylie, C. (Eds.). (2012). Handbook of research on
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). The general causality orientations scale: Self-
doi:10.1016/0092-6566(85)90023-6
DeVellis, R.F. (2003). Scale development: Theory and applications (2nd ed). Thousand
DiPerna, J. C., & Elliott, S. N. (1999). Development and validation of the academic
207–225. doi:10.1177/073428299901700302
DiPerna, J. C., & Elliott, S. N. (2002). Promoting academic enablers to improve student
293–297.
DiPerna, J. C., Volpe, R. J., & Elliott, S. N. (2002). A model of academic enablers and
298–312.
Dweck, C. S. (2007). The perils and promises of praise. Educational Leadership, 65, 34–
39.
Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., Wigfield, A., Buchanan, C. M., Reuman, D., Flanagan, C., &
Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual
doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135153
Elliott E. S., & Dweck C. S. ( 1988). Goals: An approach to motivation and achievement.
3514.54.1.5
59
Finn, J. D., & Rock, D. A. (1997). Academic success among students at risk for school
9010.82.2.221
Finney, S. J., Pieper, S. L., & Barron, K. E. (2004). Examining the psychometric
doi:10.1177/0013164403258465
Fisher, C. W., & Berliner, D. C. (1985). Perspectives on instructional time. New York,
NY: Longman.
Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). School engagement: Potential
of the concept, state of the evidence. Review of educational research, 74, 59–109.
doi:10.3102/00346543074001059
Fredricks, J., McColskey, W., Meli, J., Mordica, J., Montrosse, B., & Mooney, K. (2011).
Fulmer, S. M., & Frijters, J. C. (2009). A review of self-report and alternative approaches
219-246.
60
Greenwood, C. R., Terry, B., Marquis, J., & Walker, D. (1994). Confirming a
Harter, S. (1981). A new self-report scale of intrinsic versus extrinsic orientation in the
Hu, L., & Bentler, P.M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure
Liem, G. A. D., & Martin, A. J. (2012). The Motivation and Engagement Scale:
Maehr, M. L., & Nicholls, J. G. (1980). Culture and achievement motivation: A second
look. In N. Warren (Ed.), Studies in cross cultural psychology. San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
61
Martin, A. J. (2009). Motivation and engagement across the academic life span: A
794–824. doi:10.1177/0013164409332214
Martin, A.J. (2016). The Motivation and Engagement Scale (16th Edition). Sydney,
Martin, A. J., Ginns, P., & Papworth, B. (2017). Motivation and engagement: Same or
doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2017.03.013
Martin, A. J., & Marsh, H. W. (2003). Fear of failure: Friend or foe?. Australian
Meece, J. L., Blumenfeld, P. C., Hoyle, R. H. (1988). Students’ goal orientations and
Midgley, C., Maehr, M. L., Hruda, L. Z., Anderman, E., Anderman, L., Freeman, K. E.,
& Urdan, T. (2000). Manual for the patterns of adaptive learning scales. Ann
http://login.ezproxy.lib.umn.edu/login?url=http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx
?direct=true&AuthType=ip,uid&db=aph&AN=87515376&site=ehost-live
Nakamura, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2009). Flow theory and research. Handbook of
Reeve, J., & Lee, W. (2014). Students’ classroom engagement produces longitudinal
540. doi:10.1037/a0034934
Reschly, A. L., & Christenson, S. L. (2006). Prediction of dropout among students with
doi:10.1177/07419325060270050301
Reschly, A. L., & Christenson, S. L. (2012). Jingle, jangle, and conceptual haziness:
Rindskopf, D., & Rose, T. (1988). Some theory and applications of confirmatory second-
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: Classic
67. doi:10.1006/ceps.1999.1020
299–323.
Singh, K., Granville, M., & Dika, S. (2002). Mathematics and Science Achievement:
http://www.jstor.org.ezp2.lib.umn.edu/stable/27542398
64
Skinner, E. A., & Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the classroom: Reciprocal effects
of teacher behavior and student engagement across the school year. Journal of
doi:10.1177/0013164408323233
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L.S. (2013). Using Multivariate Statistics (6th ed). New
Jersey: Pearson.
Walker, H. M., Ramsey, E., & Gresham, F. M. (2003). Heading off disruptive behavior:
How early intervention can reduce defiant behavior—and win back teaching
Way, J., Reece, A., Bobis, J., Anderson, J., & Martin, A. (2015). Improving student
Table 1
Table 2
Comparing Means and Cronbach’s alphas for Subscales of MES Across Samples
M (SD) Cronbach’s
Current Martin’s Current Martin’s
Factor Sub-Factor Sample Sample Sample Sample
Adaptive Self-Belief 86 (16.1) 86 (14.0) .83 .77
Motivation Learning Focus 84 (16.1) 86 (14.1) .82 .82
Valuing 84 (17.4) 89 (12.4) .79 .75
Adaptive Persistence 79( 16.0) 78 (15.2) .74 .77
Engagement Planning 73 (19.5) 74 (18.7) .79 .85
Task Management 79 (19.6) 79 (18.5) .82 .86
Maladaptive Anxiety 60 (20.8) 61 (18.5) .73 .66
Motivation Failure Avoidance 54 (22.7) 56 (23.9) .77 .85
Uncertain Control 48 (18.9) 49 (19.3) .68 .78
Maladaptive Self-Sabotage 40 (18.1) 41 (19.3) .74 .79
Engagement Disengagement 39 (16.6) 36 (15.8) .64 .70
Note. Average scores from the current sample were transformed from scores out of 5 to
converted scores out of 100 (as presented in the user manual).
67
Table 3
Cronbach’s
Factor Measure Sub-Factor M SD Skew Kurtosis
Adaptive MES Self-Belief 4.28 0.80 -1.29 1.45 .83
Motivation Learning Focus 4.21 0.80 -1.28 1.62 .82
Valuing 4.20 0.83 -1.31 1.47 .79
Scale Average 4.23 0.73 -1.27 1.44 .91
S. EvsD Emotional Engagement 3.05 0.74 -0.89 0.33 .80
T. EvsD Emotional Engagement 3.18 0.78 -0.76 -0.15 .95
PALS Academic Efficacy 4.05 0.87 -1.09 0.89 .79
Mastery Goal Oriented 4.30 0.82 -1.47 2.02 .81
Scale Average 3.67 0.68 -0.55 0.88 .86
Adaptive MES Persistence 3.97 0.80 -0.68 0.08 .74
Engagement Planning 3.64 0.97 -0.45 -0.51 .79
Task Management 3.95 0.98 -0.87 0.03 .82
Scale Average 3.85 0.78 -0.57 -0.20 .89
S. EvsD Behavioral Engagement 3.41 0.56 -0.94 0.48 .80
T. EvsD Behavioral Engagement 2.90 0.93 -0.45 -0.89 .95
PALS Academic Pressure 4.01 0.77 -0.86 0.75 .71
Maladaptive MES Anxiety 2.98 1.04 -0.08 -0.69 .73
Motivation Failure Avoidance 2.69 1.13 0.39 -0.71 .77
Uncertain Control 2.40 0.94 0.38 -0.32 .68
Scale Average 2.69 0.81 0.13 -0.14 .82
S. EvsD Emotional Disaffection 2.17 0.61 0.19 -0.33 .83
T. EvsD Emotional Disaffection 1.73 0.70 0.79 -0.03 .93
PALS Performance Avoid 2.56 0.98 0.25 -0.55 .49
Avoid Novelty 2.47 0.94 0.33 -0.33 .70
Low Achievement 2.11 0.77 0.51 -0.40 .63
Skepticism 1.86 0.89 1.17 1.05 .76
Performance Approach 2.66 1.07 0.42 -0.61 .77
Scale Average 2.25 0.62 0.61 0.46 .83
Maladaptive MES Self-Sabotage 2.02 1.17 0.84 -0.38 .74
Engagement Disengagement 1.76 1.07 1.45 1.37 .64
Scale Average 1.98 0.75 0.74 0.54 .78
S. EvsD Behavioral Disaffection 2.10 0.66 0.11 -0.63 .67
T. EvsD Behavioral Disaffection 2.12 0.90 0.27 -1.04 .93
PALS Self-Handicapping 2.18 0.95 0.58 -0.60 .77
Cheating Behavior 1.57 0.84 1.77 3.16 .79
Disruptive Behavior 2.42 1.08 0.46 -0.66 .83
Scale Average 2.06 0.73 0.72 0.12 .84
Note. (S. EvsD) refers to student’s self-ratings; (T. EvsD) refers to teacher’s ratings of
students.
68
Table 4
Descriptive Statistics for MES Items
Frequencies
Strongly Strongly
Factor Sub-Factor Item Missing M SD Skew Kurtosis Disagree Disagree Neither Agree Agree
Adaptive Self-Belief 13 2 4.43 0.91 -1.81 3.09 1.9% 3.0% 8.7% 22.7% 63.6%
Motivation 23 5 4.23 1.04 -1.40 1.44 3.4% 3.4% 14.2% 24.1% 54.8%
33 4 4.19 1.01 -1.37 1.49 3.1% 4.6% 11.5% 32.1% 48.9%
40 4 4.29 0.98 -1.39 1.40 1.9% 4.2% 13.0% 24.8% 56.1%
Learning Focus 2 1 4.23 0.94 -1.30 1.51 1.9% 3.8% 12.5% 33.6% 48.3%
7 1 4.32 1.01 -1.71 2.63 3.8% 2.6% 9.4% 26.4% 57.7%
25 3 4.18 0.97 -1.31 1.72 3.0% 1.9% 15.2% 33.8% 46.0%
26 4 4.10 1.07 -1.15 0.72 3.4% 5.0% 16.4% 28.3% 46.9%
Valuing 4 2 3.88 1.10 -0.82 -0.03 3.8% 8.3% 19.7% 33.0% 35.2%
14 2 4.48 0.94 -2.00 3.49 1.9% 4.5% 6.1% 18.6% 68.9%
34 2 4.18 1.14 -1.45 1.37 6.1% 2.7% 13.3% 23.5% 54.5%
41 3 4.31 1.02 -1.60 2.07 3.4% 3.0% 11.8% 22.8% 58.9%
Adaptive Persistence 1 1 4.04 0.99 -1.17 1.31 3.4% 3.8% 15.1% 41.1% 36.6%
Engagement 9 1 3.99 1.14 -1.08 0.44 5.3% 5.7% 16.6% 29.8% 42.6%
28 4 3.93 1.04 -0.82 0.07 2.7% 7.3% 20.2% 34.0% 35.9%
36 6 3.93 1.09 -0.73 -0.38 2.3% 9.6% 20.8% 27.7% 39.6%
Planning 21 0 3.56 1.28 -0.54 -0.78 9.0% 13.2% 20.3% 28.2% 29.3%
27 2 3.95 1.13 -1.10 0.60 5.7% 5.7% 14.8% 36.0% 37.9%
30 3 3.69 1.28 -0.62 -0.71 7.6% 11.4% 21.7% 22.8% 36.5%
39 4 3.35 1.29 -0.29 -0.95 10.7% 15.3% 26.7% 23.3% 24.0%
Task 3 2 4.00 1.20 -1.08 0.23 6.1% 6.4% 15.9% 25.0% 46.6%
Management 17 2 3.86 1.19 -0.85 -0.17 5.7% 8.3% 19.3% 28.0% 38.6%
32 3 3.91 1.21 -0.98 0.03 6.5% 7.2% 16.7% 27.8% 41.8%
44 4 4.05 1.21 -1.19 0.48 6.9% 4.6% 15.6% 22.9% 50.0%
Maladaptive Anxiety 10 1 2.99 1.35 0.01 -1.18 17.4% 21.5% 23% 20.8% 17.4%
Motivation 19 1 3.2 1.45 -0.23 -1.29 18.9% 14.3% 19.6% 21.9% 25.3%
Continued
69
Frequencies
Strongly Strongly
Factor Sub-Factor Item Missing M SD Skew Kurtosis Disagree Disagree Neither Agree Agree
Maladaptive Anxiety 37 7 2.91 1.36 0.00 -1.17 22.0% 15.8% 26.6% 20.1% 15.4%
Motivation 43 4 2.78 1.44 0.20 -1.27 27.1% 17.9% 22.5% 14.5% 17.9%
Failure 11 4 2.48 1.52 0.55 -1.17 39.3% 18.3% 15.3% 9.2% 17.9%
Avoidance 20 1 2.55 1.47 0.43 -1.22 35.5% 18.5% 17.4% 13.2% 15.5%
31 1 3.21 1.47 -0.21 -1.32 19.6% 13.2% 21.9% 17.4% 27.9%
38 2 2.51 1.40 0.50 -1.01 33.3% 20.8% 21.6% 10.2% 14.0%
Uncertain 6 2 2.63 1.33 0.31 -1.03 26.9% 21.6% 25.0% 15.2% 11.4%
Control 12 1 2.66 1.36 0.29 -1.11 27.2% 20.8% 23.8% 15.5% 12.8%
16 1 2.40 1.34 0.49 -0.96 37.0% 17.4% 23.8% 12.8% 9.1%
18 6 1.91 1.19 1.13 0.10 52.7% 22.3% 10.0% 11.2% 3.8%
Maladaptive Self-Sabotage 5 1 2.02 1.17 0.84 -0.38 47.5% 19.2% 20.4% 9.4% 3.4%
Engagement 24 2 1.94 1.20 1.09 0.14 52.3% 19.3% 15.9% 7.6% 4.9%
35 3 1.97 1.19 1.11 0.24 47.9% 25.9% 12.5% 8.4% 5.3%
42 2 2.06 1.21 0.89 -0.30 45.5% 22.7% 16.7% 10.2% 4.9%
Disengagement 8 0 1.76 1.07 1.45 1.37 56.0% 24.4% 10.5% 5.6% 3.4%
15 3 1.92 1.19 1.13 0.31 52.5% 19.0% 17.5% 5.7% 5.3%
22 0 2.11 1.24 0.95 -0.06 42.5% 24.8% 19.2% 6.0% 7.5%
29 2 1.98 1.22 1.11 0.25 49.6% 22.0% 15.9% 6.1% 6.4%
70
Table 5
Descriptive Statistics for Student EvsD Items
Frequencies
Not at all Not Very Sort of
Factor Item Missing M SD Skew Kurtosis True True True Very True
Emotional 2 0 3.00 0.92 -0.74 -0.20 9.5% 13.7% 44.1% 32.7%
Engagement 9 4 3.02 0.89 -0.67 -0.26 7.3% 16.6% 42.9% 33.2%
13 1 2.99 0.98 -0.79 -0.34 12.6% 10.7% 41.6% 35.1%
22 2 3.21 0.95 -1.05 0.12 8.8% 10.3% 32.2% 48.7%
24 2 3.34 0.81 -1.09 0.58 3.4% 10.7% 34.5% 51.3%
Behavioral 1 0 3.66 0.621 -2.13 5.16 1.9% 2.3% 23.6% 72.2%
Engagement 8 1 3.57 0.64 -1.37 1.50 0.8% 5.7% 29.4% 64.1%
15 2 3.25 0.86 -1.09 0.59 6.1% 9.2% 38.3% 46.4%
18 2 3.36 0.80 -1.27 1.27 4.6% 6.9% 36.8% 51.7%
23 1 3.19 0.84 -0.81 -0.01 4.2% 14.5% 38.9% 42.4%
Emotional 4 1 1.66 0.88 1.02 -0.12 57.3% 22.5% 16.8% 3.4%
Disaffection 5 2 2.46 1.04 -0.10 -1.18 24.5% 21.8% 36.8% 16.9%
6 1 2.79 1.06 -0.41 -1.06 16.4% 19.5% 32.4% 31.7%
7 0 2.41 1.07 0.03 -1.27 26.6% 24.0% 30.8% 18.6%
11 1 2.06 1.04 0.48 -1.04 40.1% 25.2% 23.7% 11.1%
12 1 1.87 1.01 0.78 -0.69 49.6% 22.5% 19.1% 8.8%
16 1 2.18 1.08 0.34 -1.20 35.9% 24.8% 24.8% 14.5%
17 1 1.87 1.02 0.86 -0.52 49.2% 25.2% 15.3% 10.3%
19 2 2.62 1.11 -0.16 -1.30 21.5% 23.0% 28.0% 27.6%
20 1 2.25 1.11 0.30 -1.28 34.0% 25.2% 22.5% 18.3%
26 1 2.23 1.06 0.24 -1.22 33.2% 24.4% 28.6% 13.7%
27 0 1.68 0.97 1.22 0.26 60.1% 20.2% 11.8% 8.0%
Behavioral 3 2 1.63 0.94 1.26 0.32 63.2% 16.9% 13.8% 6.1%
Disaffection 10 0 1.47 0.81 1.68 1.95 69.2% 18.3% 8.7% 3.8%
14 1 2.32 1.14 0.15 -1.41 34.0% 19.5% 26.7% 19.8%
21 0 2.60 1.01 -0.30 -1.01 20.2% 19.0% 41.8% 19.0%
25 1 2.47 1.10 -0.06 -1.32 26.7% 20.2% 32.1% 21.0%
71
Table 6
Descriptive Statistics for PALS Items
Frequencies
Not at all Somewhat Very
Factor Sub-Factor Item Missing M SD Skew Kurtosis True True True
Adaptive Academic 1 0 3.98 1.06 -0.96 0.45 3.9% 4.2% 21.2% 30.9% 39.8%
Motivation Efficacy 11 4 3.57 1.31 -0.50 -0.88 9.4% 12.2% 23.9% 21.2% 33.3%
52 6 4.10 1.25 -1.21 0.31 6.7% 5.5% 16.2% 13.8% 57.7%
56 4 4.42 1.00 -1.68 1.86 1.6% 6.7% 8.6% 14.9% 68.2%
58 6 4.27 1.17 -1.57 1.41 5.5% 4.7% 10.7% 15.0% 64.0%
Mastery Goal 9 2 4.33 1.04 -1.39 0.94 1.9% 5.4% 15.2% 12.8% 64.6%
Orientation 25 2 4.39 1.11 -1.72 1.89 3.9% 4.7% 11.7% 8.6% 71.2%
29 4 4.30 1.11 -1.48 1.19 3.5% 5.1% 14.1% 12.2% 65.1%
38 3 4.00 1.24 -1.00 -0.07 6.3% 6.6% 19.1% 17.2% 50.8%
49 4 4.47 1.01 -2.02 3.42 3.5% 2.0% 11.0% 10.6% 72.9%
Adaptive Academic 6 0 3.83 1.35 -0.87 -0.47 10.0% 6.9% 18.9% 17.8% 46.3%
Engagement Pressure 10 3 4.02 1.19 -0.99 0.02 5.1% 5.9% 21.1% 18.0% 50.0%
15 1 4.21 1.16 -1.34 0.80 4.7% 4.7% 16.3% 13.6% 60.9%
17 2 3.98 1.32 -1.05 -0.13 8.6% 7.0% 15.2% 16.7% 52.5%
19 2 3.98 1.22 -0.97 -0.06 5.8% 6.2% 20.6% 18.3% 49.0%
53 5 4.04 1.19 -1.03 0.08 5.1% 5.9% 20.1% 18.1% 50.8%
57 5 4.07 1.24 -1.14 0.11 5.9% 7.9% 14.6% 16.1% 55.5%
Maladaptive Performance 3 1 3.04 1.64 0.01 -1.60 28.7% 12.8% 18.2% 6.6% 33.7%
Motivation Avoid Goal 33 3 2.08 1.41 1.02 -0.35 53.5% 14.8% 13.7% 6.3% 11.7%
51 3 2.73 1.59 0.28 -1.45 35.5% 12.1% 20.3% 7.8% 24.2%
55 4 2.38 1.50 0.70 -0.95 42.4% 18.0% 16.9% 5.1% 17.6%
Performance 8 1 2.97 1.41 0.06 -1.20 21.3% 15.9% 29.1% 12.4% 21.3%
Approach 26 3 3.19 1.48 -0.17 -1.35 19.5% 14.5% 21.9% 15.6% 28.5%
41 2 2.38 1.46 0.66 -0.93 41.2% 17.1% 19.5% 7.0% 15.2%
45 2 2.4 1.49 0.56 -1.12 40.9% 15.6% 18.3% 8.9% 16.3%
48 5 2.37 1.54 0.66 -1.10 45.7% 14.6% 14.2% 8.3% 17.3%
Avoid 7 0 2.54 1.40 0.46 -1.01 32.4% 19.3% 24.3% 9.7% 14.3%
Novelty 20 4 2.12 1.33 1.00 -0.18 45.9% 22.7% 15.3% 5.9% 10.2%
Continued
72
Frequencies
Not at Somewhat Very
Factor Sub-Factor Item Missing M SD Skew Kurtosis all True True True
Maladaptive Avoid 23 2 2.62 1.42 0.38 -1.07 31.1% 16.7% 27.6% 8.2% 16.3%
Motivation Novelty 35 4 2.63 1.35 0.35 -0.98 28.7% 18.4% 29.4% 10.2% 13.7%
40 3 2.50 1.42 0.48 -1.05 35.2% 17.6% 23.0% 10.2% 14.1%
Self 2 0 1.84 1.27 1.33 0.50 61.5% 13.5% 11.2% 6.9% 6.9%
Presentation 5 2 2.15 1.46 0.87 -0.74 54.5% 9.7% 14.8% 8.6% 12.5%
Low 21 4 1.90 1.37 1.33 0.37 61.6% 12.5% 11.4% 3.1% 11.4%
Achievement 24 2 2.76 1.57 0.23 -1.46 33.9% 13.2% 18.7% 11.3% 23.0%
27 2 2.13 1.40 0.93 -0.48 51.0% 14.0% 17.5% 5.8% 11.7%
37 4 1.97 1.27 1.12 0.11 53.7% 16.5% 16.5% 5.9% 7.5%
46 4 2.04 1.33 1.01 -0.24 52.9% 14.5% 16.9% 6.7% 9%
Skepticism 4 1 1.81 1.26 1.44 0.83 62.4% 14.3% 10.5% 5.0% 7.8%
Relevance 13 1 1.78 1.22 1.48 1.09 63.2% 13.2% 13.2% 3.5% 7.0%
28 2 1.86 1.29 1.33 0.55 61.1% 12.5% 14.0% 3.9% 8.6%
32 3 1.94 1.32 1.23 0.27 57.8% 13.3% 16.0% 3.1% 9.8%
36 2 1.91 1.34 1.32 0.41 59.1% 16.0% 10.1% 4.7% 10.1%
43 2 1.74 1.20 1.56 1.35 65.0% 12.8% 12.1% 3.5% 6.6%
Maladaptive Academic 12 1 2.22 1.44 0.76 -0.83 50.0% 10.5% 19.0% 8.5% 12.0%
Engagement Self 16 1 2.34 1.44 0.67 -0.91 42.6% 15.9% 19.8% 7.8% 14.0%
Handicap 18 1 2.34 1.51 0.66 -1.05 47.3% 10.9% 18.2% 7.8% 15.9%
42 2 2.21 1.48 0.85 -0.74 50.2% 13.6% 15.6% 5.8% 14.8%
44 3 1.95 1.29 1.19 0.26 55.9% 14.8% 16.8% 3.9% 8.6%
47 3 2.04 1.33 1.09 0.01 51.2% 18.0% 16.8% 3.5% 10.5%
Cheating 22 2 1.39 0.85 2.50 6.11 78.2% 10.9% 7.0% 1.9% 1.9%
Behavior 31 4 1.61 1.02 1.79 2.67 65.5% 17.3% 11.4% 2.4% 3.5%
39 2 1.70 1.12 1.63 1.80 63.0% 17.1% 11.3% 3.5% 5.1%
Disruptive 14 1 2.38 1.40 0.63 -0.84 38.4% 18.6% 22.5% 7.4% 13.2%
Behavior 30 4 2.77 1.48 0.25 -1.28 28.6% 16.9% 23.9% 10.2% 20.4%
34 2 2.44 1.42 0.57 -0.92 37.4% 16.7% 24.9% 6.2% 14.8%
50 4 2.41 1.30 0.54 -0.73 34.1% 19.2% 28.6% 7.8% 10.2%
54 5 2.14 1.37 0.92 -0.43 48.4% 17.3% 16.9% 6.3% 11.0%
73
Table 7
Frequencies
Not at all Not Very
Factor Item Missing M SD Skew Kurtosis True True Sort of True Very True
Emotional 6 0 3.12 0.86 -0.65 -0.41 4.3 18.7 37.4 39.6
Engagement 7 0 3.29 0.78 -0.90 0.30 2.7 11.8 39.6 46.0
8 0 3.19 0.83 -0.83 0.07 4.3 13.9 40.1 41.7
9 0 3.12 0.91 -0.81 -0.19 7.0 15.0 36.9 41.2
10 0 3.15 0.89 -0.85 -0.04 6.4 13.9 38.0 41.7
Behavioral 1 0 3.00 1.01 -0.66 -0.70 11.2 17.1 32.1 39.6
Engagement 2 0 3.15 0.94 -0.86 -0.24 7.5 15.0 32.6 44.9
3 0 3.00 1.00 -0.59 -0.82 9.6 20.9 29.4 40.1
4 0 2.52 1.08 0.08 -1.27 19.8 33.7 20.9 25.7
5 0 2.82 1.05 -0.36 -1.11 13.9 24.1 28.3 33.7
Emotional 16 1 2.01 0.98 0.51 -0.88 38.7 29.6 23.7 8.1
Disaffection 17 0 1.96 0.97 0.62 -0.73 41.2 29.9 20.9 8.0
18 0 1.60 0.81 1.22 0.76 56.7 29.4 10.7 3.2
19 0 1.63 0.86 1.21 0.53 58.3 25.1 12.3 4.3
20 1 1.63 0.89 1.31 0.81 58.1 26.3 9.7 5.9
21 0 1.51 0.75 1.48 1.73 62.0 27.8 7.5 2.7
22 0 1.61 0.91 1.37 0.81 62.0 21.4 10.2 6.4
23 0 1.79 0.95 0.97 -0.16 50.8 27.3 14.4 7.5
24 0 1.76 0.95 1.02 -0.03 51.9 27.3 13.4 7.5
25 0 1.82 1.02 0.95 -0.37 51.9 24.1 13.9 10.2
Behavioral 11 0 2.37 1.03 0.01 -1.19 26.7 24.1 34.8 14.4
Disaffection 12 0 1.93 0.98 0.66 -0.71 43.4 28.3 20.3 8.0
13 0 1.94 1.01 0.66 -0.78 44.4 26.2 20.3 9.1
14 5 2.25 1.09 0.23 -1.30 34.1 22.5 27.5 15.9
15 0 2.12 1.03 0.42 -1.04 35.8 28.3 24.1 11.8
74
Table 8
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. Self-Belief – 0.70* 0.73* 0.56* 0.61* 0.52* 0.16*
2. Valuing – 0.69* 0.40* 0.65* 0.57* 0.15*
3. Learning Focus – 0.46* 0.60* 0.59* 0.17*
4. Academic Efficacy – 0.57* 0.38* 0.10
5. Mastery Goal Orientation – 0.54* 0.11
6. Student Emotional Engagement – 0.12
7. Teacher Emotional Engagement –
Table 9
1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Planning – 0.63* 0.55* 0.31* 0.51* 0.11
2. Task Management – 0.63* 0.27* 0.56* 0.12
3. Persistence – 0.30* 0.61* 0.15*
4. Academic Pressure – 0.35* 0.06
5. Student Behavioral Engagement – 0.27*
6. Teacher Behavioral Engagement –
Table 10
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
1. Anxiety – 0.36* 0.40* 0.12 0.22* 0.18* 0.26* 0.04 0.48* 0.10
2. Failure Avoidance – 0.46* 0.32* 0.42* 0.20* 0.40* 0.17* 0.39* 0.10
3. Uncertain Control – 0.17* 0.25* 0.36* 0.48* 0.30* 0.43* 0.27*
4. Performance Approach – 0.47* 0.10 0.21* 0.12 0.17* 0.06
5. Performance Avoid – 0.22* 0.34* 0.18* 0.32* 0.08
6. Avoid Novelty – 0.43* 0.35* 0.31* 0.16*
7. Self Presentation – 0.41* 0.36* 0.12
8. Relevance Skeptic – 0.29* 0.33*
9. Student Emotional Disaffection – 0.25*
10. Teacher Emotional Disaffection –
Table 11
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. Self-Sabotage – 0.52* 0.40* 0.31* 0.32* 0.45* 0.25*
2. Disengagement – 0.35* 0.29* 0.36* 0.45* 0.19*
3. Self-Handicapping – 0.29* 0.39* 0.28* 0.24*
4. Cheating Behavior – 0.42* 0.36* 0.10
5. Disruptive Behavior – 0.48* 0.50*
6. Student Behavioral Disaffection – 0.31*
7. Teacher Behavioral Disaffection –
Table 12
Standardized Item Factor Loadings and Errors for Model 4
Factors
Learning Task Failure Uncertain
Item Self-Belief Focus Valuing Persistence Planning Management Anxiety Avoid Control
13 0.80 (0.03)
23 0.84 (0.03)
33 0.81 (0.03)
40 0.86 (0.02)
2 0.75 (0.03)
7 0.81 (0.03)
25 0.84 (0.03)
26 0.79 (0.03)
4 0.69 (0.04)
14 0.82 (0.04)
34 0.73 (0.04)
41 0.88 (0.03)
1 0.59 (0.05)
9 0.77 (0.03)
28 0.69 (0.04)
36 0.82 (0.03)
21 0.70 (0.04)
27 0.70 (0.04)
30 0.84 (0.03)
39 0.81 (0.03)
3 0.65 (0.04)
17 0.81 (0.03)
32 0.88 (0.03)
44 0.87 (0.03)
10 0.70 (0.05)
19 0.60 (0.05)
Continued
79
Factors
Learning Task Failure Uncertain Self-
Item Focus Valuing Persistence Planning Manage Anxiety Avoid Control Sabotage Disengagement
37 0.77 (0.05)
43 0.64 (0.05)
11 0.77 (0.04)
20 0.89 (0.04)
31 0.58 (0.06)
38 0.75 (0.05)
6 0.54 (0.05)
12 0.69 (0.04)
16 0.70 (0.04)
18 0.76 (0.05)
5 0.69 (0.05)
24 0.76 (0.04)
35 0.78 (0.04)
42 0.70 (0.05)
8 0.75 (0.04)
15 0.69 (0.05)
22 0.51 (0.06)
29 0.76 (0.04)
80
Table 13
Model 4 First-Order Standardized Factor Loadings and Errors on Higher Order Factors
Higher-Order Factors
Adaptive Adaptive Maladaptive Maladaptive
First-Order Factors Motivation Engagement Motivation Engagement
Self-Belief 0.97 (0.02)
Learning Focus 0.92 (0.02)
Valuing 0.93 (0.02)
Persistence 0.96 (0.02)
Planning 0.81 (0.03)
Task Management 0.92 (0.02)
Anxiety 0.47 (0.06)
Failure Avoidance 0.57 (0.06)
Uncertain Control 1.24 (0.08)
Self-Sabotage 0.79 (0.04)
Disengagement 0.95 (0.04)
81
Table 14
Standardized Item Factor Loadings and Errors for Model 8
Factors
Student Teacher Student Teacher Student
Emotional Emotional Academic Mastery Goal Behavioral Behavioral Academic Emotional
Item Engage Engage Efficacy Orientation Engage Engage Pressure Disaffection
Student EvsD2 0.67 (0.05)
Student EvsD9 0.65 (0.05)
Student EvsD13 0.74 (0.04)
Student EvsD22 0.91 (0.04)
Student EvsD24 0.65 (0.07)
Teacher EvsD6 0.89 (0.02)
Teacher EvsD7 0.83 (0.03)
Teacher EvsD8 0.98 (0.01)
Teacher EvsD9 0.98 (0.01)
Teacher EvsD10 0.99 (0.01)
PALS1 0.69 (0.06)
PALS11 0.50 (0.07)
PALS52 0.77 (0.05)
PALS56 0.85 (0.06)
PALS58 0.82 (0.05)
PALS9 0.68 (0.07)
PALS25 0.87 (0.06)
PALS29 0.67 (0.06)
PALS38 0.65 (0.07)
PALS49 0.90 (0.06)
Student EvsD1 0.76 (0.05)
Student EvsD8 0.75 (0.04)
Student EvsD15 0.60 (0.06)
Student EvsD18 0.92 (0.03)
Student EvsD23 0.81 (0.04)
Continued
82
Factors
Student Teacher Student Teacher
Behavioral Behavioral Academic Emotional Emotional Performance Avoid Self Relevance
Item Engage Engage Pressure Disaffection Disaffection Avoid Novelty Presentation Skeptic
Teacher EvsD1 0.92 (0.02)
Teacher EvsD2 0.95 (0.01)
Teacher EvsD3 0.99 (0.01)
Teacher EvsD4 0.92 (0.01)
Teacher EvsD5 0.96 (0.01)
PALS6 0.37 (0.09)
PALS10 0.51 (0.09)
PALS15 0.51 (0.10)
PALS17 0.87 (0.09)
PALS19 0.58 (0.10)
PALS53 0.74 (0.08)
PALS57 0.38 (0.10)
Student EvsD4 0.58 (0.07)
Student EvsD5 0.85 (0.04)
Student EvsD6 0.33 (0.07)
Student EvsD7 0.82 (0.04)
Student EvsD11 0.79 (0.04)
Student EvsD12 0.44 (0.06)
Student EvsD16 0.27 (0.07)
Student EvsD17 0.53 (0.07)
Student EvsD19 0.53 (0.06)
Student EvsD20 0.78 (0.05)
Student EvsD26 0.40 (0.07)
Student EvsD27 0.67 (0.07)
Teacher EvsD16 0.96 (0.01)
Teacher EvsD17 0.95 (0.01)
Teacher EvsD18 0.69 (0.04)
Continued
83
Factors
Student Teacher
Academic Emotional Emotional Performance Avoid Self Relevance Performance
Item Pressure Disaffection Disaffection Avoid Novelty Presentation Skeptic Approach
Teacher EvsD19 0.74 (0.04)
Teacher EvsD20 0.91 (0.02)
Teacher EvsD21 0.90 (0.02)
Teacher EvsD22 0.83 (0.03)
Teacher EvsD23 0.79 (0.03)
Teacher EvsD24 1.00 (0.01)
Teacher EvsD25 1.00 (0.01)
PALS3 0.39 (0.13)
PALS33 0.47 (0.13)
PALS51 0.46 (0.11)
PALS55 0.73 (0.11)
PALS7 0.50 (0.08)
PALS20 0.88 (0.08)
PALS23 0.58 (0.07)
PALS35 0.56 (0.08)
PALS40 0.63 (0.07)
PALS2 0.61 (0.10)
PALS5 0.58 (0.09)
PALS21 0.52 (0.10)
PALS24 0.29 (0.10)
PALS27 0.34 (0.10)
PALS37 0.85 (0.09)
PALS46 0.35 (0.09)
PALS4 0.60 (0.08)
PALS13 0.78 (0.08)
PALS28 0.81 (0.06)
PALS32 0.60 (0.07)
PALS36 0.69 (0.06)
PALS43 0.73 (0.07)
Continued
84
Factors
Student Teacher
Self Relevance Performance Behavioral Behavioral Disruptive
Item Presentation Skeptic Approach Disaffection Disaffection Self-Handicap Cheating Behaviors
PALS8 0.49 (0.09)
PALS26 0.43 (0.08)
PALS41 0.73 (0.06)
PALS45 0.80 (0.06)
PALS48 0.93 (0.06)
Student EvsD3 0.66 (0.07)
Student EvsD10 0.78 (0.07)
Student EvsD14 0.31 (0.07)
Student EvsD21 0.75 (0.05)
Student EvsD25 0.72 (0.05)
TeacherEvsD11 0.89 (0.02)
Teacher EvsD12 0.80 (0.03)
Teacher EvsD13 0.96 (0.01)
Teacher EvsD14 0.87 (0.02)
Teacher EvsD15 0.98 (0.01)
PALS12 0.61 (0.08)
PALS16 0.83 (0.06)
PALS18 0.65 (0.06)
PALS42 0.54 (0.09)
PALS44 0.79 (0.06)
PALS47 0.61 (0.08)
PALS22 0.80 (0.07)
PALS31 0.93 (0.05)
PALS39 0.86 (0.06)
PALS14 0.74 (0.05)
PALS30 0.83 (0.04)
PALS34 0.78 (0.04)
PALS50 0.77 (0.04)
PALS54 0.75 (0.05)
85
Table 15
Model 8 First-Order Standardized Factor Loadings and Errors on Higher Order Factors
Higher-Order Factors
Adaptive Adaptive Maladaptive Maladaptive
First-Order Factors Motivation Engagement Motivation Engagement
Student Emotional Engagement 0.58 (0.05)
Teacher Emotional Engagement 0.88 (0.04)
Academic Efficacy 0.50 (0.05)
Mastery Goal Orientation 0.51 (0.05)
Student Behavioral Engagement 0.57 (0.06)
Teacher Behavioral Engagement 0.66 (0.06)
Academic Pressure 0.24 (0.05)
Student Emotional Disaffection 0.58 (0.04)
Teacher Emotional Disaffection 0.77 (0.04)
Performance Avoid 0.39 (0.07)
Avoid Novelty 0.46 (0.05)
Self Presentation 0.46 (0.06)
Relevance Skeptic 0.54 (0.05)
Performance Approach 0.21 (0.06)
Student Behavioral Disaffection 0.73 (0.04)
Teacher Behavioral Disaffection 0.92 (0.02)
Self-Handicapping Behavior 0.46 (0.05)
Cheating Behavior 0.43 (0.06)
Disruptive Behaviors 0.68 (0.03)
86
Figures
Valuing Persistence
Learning Planning
Focus
Self- Task
Belief Management
Disengagement Anxiety
Failure
Avoidance
Self- Uncertain
Sabotage Control
Figure 1. Motivation and Engagement Wheel (reproduced from Martin et al., 2007).
87
Figure 9. Higher-order four-factor model of adaptive and maladaptive motivation and engagement.
95
Figure 10. Four-factor replication model (Model 4) with standardized factor loadings
96
Figure 11. Four-factor adaptability model (Model 8) with standardized factor loadings
97
Appendices
Appendix A
Appendix B
Gender: Male
Female
Race: White
Black or African-American
American Indian or Alaska Native
Asian
Pacific Islander
Other: _______________
Appendix C
2. I feel very happy with myself when I really understand what I’m taught at school
4. I’m able to use some of the things I learn at school in other parts of my life
5. Sometimes I don’t try hard at school so I can have a reason if I don’t do well
6. When I don’t do well at school I don’t know how to avoid that happening next time
7. I feel very happy with myself when I do well at school by working hard
11. The main reason I try at school is because I don’t want people to think that I’m dumb
12. When I get a good mark I often don’t know how I’m going to get that mark again
16. When I get a bad mark I don’t know how to avoid that happening next time
20. The main reason I try at school is because I don’t want people to think bad things
about me
101
22. I’m not involved in things like class activities and class discussion at school
24. I sometimes don’t work very hard at school so I can have a reason if I don’t do well
25. I feel very happy with myself when what I learn at school shows me how something
works
26. I feel very happy with myself when I learn new things at school
28. When I’m taught something that doesn’t make sense, I spend time to try to
understand it
30. I have a plan for how to do my homework or projects when I start them
31. The main reason I try at school is because I don’t want to disappoint my parents
35. I sometimes waste time the night before a test so I can have a reason if I don’t do well
36. I’ll keep working at difficult schoolwork until I’ve figured it out
38. The main reason I try at school is because I don’t want my teacher to think bad things
about me
42. I sometimes leave homework until the last moment so I can have a reason if I don’t
do so well
Appendix D
Behavioral Engagement
Emotional Engagement
3. Class is fun.
Behavioral Disaffection
Emotional Disaffection
Note. Adapted from Wellborn (1991). The items added to the Emotional Disaffection
Appendix E
Behavioral Engagement
Emotional Engagement
Behavioral Disaffection
1. When we start something new in class, this student thinks about other things. (–)
3. When faced with a difficult assignment, this student doesn’t even try. (–)
5. When we start something new in class, this student doesn’t pay attention. (–)
Emotional Disaffection
5. a. When I explain new material, this student doesn’t seem to care. (–)
Note. Adapted from Wellborn (1991). The items added to the Emotional Disaffection
Appendix F
1. I'm certain I can master the skills taught in class this year.
2. I would avoid participating in class if it meant that other students would think I know a
lot.
4. Even if I do well in school, it will not help me have the kind of life I want when I grow
up.
5. If other students found out I did well on a test, I would tell them it was just luck even if
6. When I’ve figured out how to do a problem, my teacher gives me more challenging
7. I would prefer to do class work that is familiar to me, rather than work I would have to
8. It’s important to me that other students in my class think I am good at my class work.
11. I'm certain I can figure out how to do the most difficult class work.
12. Some students fool around the night before a test. Then if they don’t do well, they can
13. My chances of succeeding later in life don’t depend on doing well in school.
16. Some students purposely get involved in lots of activities. Then if they don’t do well
on
their class work, they can say it is because they were involved with other things. How
17. When I’m working out a problem, my teacher tells me to keep thinking until I really
understand.
18. Some students look for reasons to keep them from studying (not feeling well, having
to
help their parents, taking care of a brother or sister, etc.). Then if they don’t do well on
their class work, they can say this is the reason. How true is this of you?
19. My teacher doesn’t let me do just easy work, but makes me think.
21. I wouldn’t volunteer to answer a question in class if I thought other students would
think I
was smart.
23. I prefer to do work as I have always done it, rather than trying something new.
24. If I did well on a school assignment, I wouldn’t want other students to see my grade.
26. One of my goals is to show others that I’m good at my class work.
27. It’s very important to me that I don’t look smarter than others in class.
28. Doing well in school doesn’t improve my chances of having a good life when I grow
up.
109
32. Getting good grades in school won’t guarantee that I will get a good job when I grow
up.
33. One of my goals is to keep others from thinking I’m not smart in class.
35. I like academic concepts that are familiar to me, rather than those I haven’t thought
about before.
37. If I were good at my class work, I would try to do my work in a way that didn’t show
it.
39. I sometimes copy answers from other students when I do my class work.
40. I would choose class work I knew I could do, rather than work I haven’t done before.
41. One of my goals is to show others that class work is easy for me.
42. Some students let their friends keep them from paying attention in class or from doing
their homework. Then if they don’t do well, they can say their friends kept them from
43. Doing well in school won’t help me have a satisfying career when I grow up.
44. Some students purposely don’t try hard in class. Then if they don’t do well, they can
say
45. One of my goals is to look smart in comparison to the other students in my class.
46. One of my goals in class is to avoid looking smarter than other kids.
47. Some students put off doing their class work until the last minute. Then if they don’t
do
well on their work, they can say that is the reason. How true is this of you?
51. It’s important to me that my teacher doesn’t think that I know less than others in
class.
52. I can do almost all the work in class if I don't give up.
53. My teacher makes sure that the work I do really makes me think.
55. One of my goals in class is to avoid looking like I have trouble doing the work.