Learning Through Play 2008 USA
Learning Through Play 2008 USA
Topic
Play
Introduction
We define play, review the main types of play and their developmental benefits in
various areas.
As children invest time and energy in play, and there are opportunities for learning
when they do play, there seems to be a need for play. This is true of young mammals
generally, although other mammals show much less variety of play forms than human
children. These findings suggest that play has developmental benefits. Benefits might
be immediate, long-term, or both. However, the exact role of play in learning is still
debated. A prevailing “play ethos”3, 4 has tended to exaggerate the evidence for the
essential role of play. Nevertheless, correlational and experimental evidence suggest
important benefits of play, even if some benefits can also be obtained in other ways.
Locomotor play, including exercise play (running, climbing, etc.), involves large body
activity and is generally thought to support physical training of muscles, for strength,
endurance, and skill. Exercise play increases from toddlers to preschool and peaks at
early primary school ages, when the neural and muscular basis of physical
coordination and healthy growth is important, and vigorous play obviously provides
good opportunities for this;5 later, it declines. There is evidence that active,
playground-type breaks can help young children concentrate better at subsequent
sedentary tasks,1 consistent with the cognitive immaturity hypothesis that the “need to
exercise helps young children to space out cognitive demands for which they have less
mature capacities.”6
Social play refers to playful interactions between children and parents or caregivers in
children up to 2 years old, but increasingly with other children as social play increases
dramatically from 2 to 6 years of age. At first, playing with one partner is complex
enough, but by 3 or 4 years old a play group can consist of three or more participants,
as children acquire social coordination skills and social scripts. Parallel play,
common in 2- and 3-year-olds, is when children play next to others without much
interaction. Some play is solitary.7 This type of play can be physical, incorporate
objects or language, be pretend, or include all of these aspects. Rough-and-tumble
play, including play fighting and chasing, can look like real fighting, but in play
fighting children are often laughing, kicks and blows are not hard or do not make
contact, and it is usually done with friends.
Object play refers to playful use of objects such as building blocks, jigsaw puzzles,
cars, dolls, etc. With babies, this play is mouthing objects and dropping them. With
toddlers, this is sometimes just manipulating the objects (e.g., assembling blocks), but
sometimes involves pretend play (e.g., building a house, feeding a doll). Play with
objects allows children to try out new combinations of actions, free of external
constraint, and may help develop problem solving skills. Any benefits of object play
need to be balanced against those of instruction, bearing in mind the ages of the
children, the nature of the task, and whether learning is for specific skills, or a more
general inquisitive and creative attitude. The more marked benefits may be for
independent and creative thought,8 though the evidence is equivocal.9
Language play At around 2 years old, toddlers often talk to themselves before going
to sleep or upon waking up. This is playful, with repetition and sometimes laughter.
Children use language humorously at 3 and 4 years old. (“I’m a whale. This is my
tail.” “I’m a flamingo. Look at my wingo.”) Language skills--phonology (speech
sounds), vocabulary and meaning (semantics), grammar (syntax), and pragmatics
(using language appropriately in social situations)--are rapidly developing in the
preschool years. Some phonological skills can be developed in the solitary
monologues when children babble to themselves in their cot, but most benefits of
language learning probably come in sociodramatic play.
Pretend play involves pretending an object or an action is something else than it really
is. A banana is a telephone, for example. This play develops from 15 months of age
with simple actions, such as pretending to sleep or putting dolly to bed, developing
into longer story sequences and role play. Sociodramatic play, common from around 3
years of age, is pretend play with others, sustained role taking, and a narrative line. It
can involve understanding others’ intent, sophisticated language constructions, and
development of (sometimes) novel and intricate story lines. Children negotiate
meanings and roles (“You be daddy, right?”) and argue about appropriate behavior
(“No, you don’t feed the baby like that!”).
Many learning functions have been advanced for pretend and especially sociodramatic
play.10 One hypothesis is that it is useful for developing preliteracy skills, such as
awareness of letters and print, and the purpose of books.11, 12, 13 The narrative structure
of sociodramatic play sequences mirrors the narratives of story books. For these
benefits, some structuring by adults is helpful (in maintaining a story line, having
suitable materials including plastic letters, books, etc.).
Another hypothesis is that pretend play enhances emotional security. A child who is
emotionally upset, for example, by parents arguing or the illness or death of someone
in the family, can work through the anxieties by acting out such themes in pretend
play, with dolls for example. Play therapists use such techniques to help understand
children’s anxieties; and most therapists believe that it helps the child work towards a
resolution of them.14
A related area of concern has been war play (play with toy guns, weapons, or combat
superhero figures).18 Carlsson-Paige and Levin19 contrasted a developmental view that
play including war play is a primary vehicle for children to express themselves, with a
sociopolitical view that children learn militaristic political concepts and values
through war play. There is not a large research base on which to make informed
judgments about whether the concerns are justified. Dunn and Hughes20 found that 4-
year-old, hard-to-manage children showed frequent violent fantasy and the extent of
this was related to poorer language and play skills, more antisocial behaviour, and less
empathic understanding at the age of 6 years. This does suggest concerns for the
effects of such play on disturbed children.
Implications
In contemporary societies, adults are usually involved in children’s play, providing
play environments and toys. Preliteracy benefits of play can be enhanced by providing
paper, crayons, and plastic letters. Exercise benefits of play can be enhanced by
Nursery staff can work with children to structure their play and give it more
educational value by including activities such as jigsaw puzzles, color and pattern
matching games, and materials like water, sand, and clay that children can manipulate
and by enhancing sociodramatic play.10 Such play tutoring involves providing suitable
props (play house, clothes for role play, hospital equipment, etc.), taking children on
visits to stimulate their imagination (to a hospital, zoo, etc.), and suggesting play
themes and helping children to develop them. Play training can be one enjoyable and
effective way of improving skills in language development, cognitive development,
creativity, and role-taking .21
Most experts in play research believe that a balanced approach is best. There should
be good opportunities for genuine free play. Also, there should be some active
involvement of adults in structuring some play, as in play tutoring. And, increasingly,
as children get older, there is a need for direct instruction. The balance between types
of play is a matter of continuing debate. As all types of play provide different
opportunities, a blended program in preschool, with plenty of opportunities for free
and structured play, is likely to be best for children and to provide them with a happy
and stimulating environment in which they can flourish.
REFERENCES
1. Pellegrini AD, Smith PK. Physical activity play: The nature and function of a
neglected aspect of play. Child Development 1998;69(3):577-598.
2. Burghardt GM. The genesis of animal play: Testing the limits. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press; 2005.
3. Smith PK. Children’s play and its role in early development: A re-evaluation
of the ‘Play Ethos’. In: Pellegrini AD, ed. Psychological Bases for Early
education. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.; 1988: 207-226.
4. Smith PK. Children and Play. New York, NY: J. Wiley. In press.
5. Byers JA, Walker C. Refining the motor training hypothesis for the evolution
of play. American Naturalist 1995;146(1):25-40.
6. Bjorklund D, Green B. The adaptive nature of cognitive immaturity. American
Psychologist 1992;47(1):46-54.
7. Parten M. Social participation among preschool children. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology 1932;27:243-269.
8. Bruner JS. The nature and uses of immaturity. American Psychologist 1972;
27(8):687-708.
9. Pellegrini AD, Gustafson K. Boys’ and girls’ uses of objects for exploration,
play, and tools in early childhood. In: Pellegrini AD, Smith PK, eds. The
Nature of Play: Great Apes and Humans New York, NY: Guilford Press;
2005: 113-138.
10. Smilansky S. The effects of Sociodramatic Play on Disadvantaged Preschool
children. New York: Wiley; 1968.
11. Pellegrini A, Galda L. Ten years after: A reexamination of symbolic play and
literacy research. Reading Research Quarterly 1993;28(2):163-175.
12. Roskos K, Christie J, eds. Play and Literacy: Research from Multiple
Perspectives. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2007.
13. Zigler EF, Singer DG, Bishop-Josef S.J, eds. Children’s Play: The Roots of
Reading. Washington, DC: Zero to Three Press; 2004.
14. Porter ML, Hernandez-Reif M, Jessee P. Play therapy: A review. Early Child
Development and Care. In press.
15. Dunn J, Cutting AL. Understanding others, and individual differences in
friendship interactions in young children. Social Development 1999;8(2):201-
219.
16. Smith PK. Social and pretend play in children. In: Pellegrini AD, Smith PK,
eds. The Nature of Play: Great Apes and Humans. New York, NY: Guilford
Publications; 2005:173-209.
17. Pellegrini AD. The rough play of adolescent boys of differing sociometric
status. International Journal of Behavioral Development 1994;17(3):525-540.
18. Holland P. We don’t Play with Guns here: War, Weapon and Superhero Play
in the Early Years. Maidenhead, UK: Open University Press; 2003.
19. Carlsson-Paige N, Levin DE. The War Play Dilemma. New York, NY:
Teachers College Press; 1987.
20. Dunn J, Hughes C. “I got some swords and you’re dead!”: Violent fantasy,
antisocial behavior, friendship, and moral sensibility in young children. Child
Development 2001;72(2): 491-505.
21. Smith PK, Dalgleish M, Herzmark G. A comparison of the effects of fantasy
play tutoring and skills tutoring in nursery classes. International Journal of
Behavioural Development 1981;4(4): 421-441.
Smith PK, Pellegrini A. Learning through play. In: Tremblay RE, Boivin M, Peters RDeV, eds.
Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online]. Montreal, Quebec: Centre of Excellence for
Early Childhood Development and Strategic Knowledge Cluster on Early Child Development; 2008:1-
6. Available at: http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/documents/Smith-PellegriniANGxp.pdf. Accessed
[insert date].