Children at Risk - An Evaluation of Factors Contributing To Child Abuse and Neglect
Children at Risk - An Evaluation of Factors Contributing To Child Abuse and Neglect
AN EVALUATION OF
FACTORS CONTRIBUTING TO
CHILD ABUSE AND NEGLECT
CHILDREN AT RISK
AN EVALUATION OF
FACTORS CONTRIBUTING TO
CHILD ABUSE AND NEGLECT
Edited by
ROBERT T. AMMERMAN
Western Pennsylvania School for Blind Children
Pil/sburgh, Pennsylvania
and
MICHEL HERSEN
University of Pil/sburgh
School of Medicine
Pillsburgh, Pennsylvania
vii
viii CONTRIBUTORS
ix
x PREFACE
Robert T. Ammerman
Michel Hersen
Pittsburgh , Pennsylvania
CONTENTS
PART I. INTRODUCTION
Chapter 1
Research in Child Abuse and Neglect: Current Status and an
Agenda for the Future. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3
Robert T. Ammerman and Michel Hersen
Introduction. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3
The Problem of Definition 6
Consequences of Maltreatment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8
The Search for Risk Factors 10
Treatment and Prevention 14
Summary 16
References 16
Chapter 2
The Epidemiology of Child Maltreatment 23
Raymond H. Starr, [r., Howard Dubowitz, and Beverly A. Bush
Introduction. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23
Basic Epidemiological Concepts 24
xi
xii CONTENTS
Chapter 3
Trends in Legislation and Case Law on Child Abuse
and Neglect . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 55
Randy K. Otto and Gary B. Melton
Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 55
Justification for State Intervention 56
Parens Patriae Power 56
Orientation and Scope of Intervention 56
Criminal and Civil Adjudication of Child Abuse
and Neglect . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59
Legislative Responses 63
The First Wave of Legislation: Child Abuse
Reporting Statutes 63
Child Abuse Prevention and Treatment Act of 1974 66
Other Federal Legislation 68
Children's Trust Funds 69
Procedural and Evidentiary Reforms Related to Adjudication 70
The Impact of Testifying upon Child Witnesses 71
Children's Competency to Testify . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72
Hearsay Exceptions 73
Closed Courtrooms 74
Special Courtroom Procedures 75
Psychological Testimony about Abuser and Victim Profiles 76
Discussion 77
Summary 78
References 78
CONTENTS xiii
Chapter 4
Research Directions Related to Child Abuse and Neglect . .. . 85
Roy C. Herrenkohl
Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85
Research Issues . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 86
Incidence and Prevalence of Abuse and Neglect 86
Research Directions 87
Causes of Abuse and Neglect 89
Research Directions 92
Consequences of Abuse and Neglect 92
Physical Abuse 93
Emotional Abuse 94
Sexual Abuse . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 94
Child Neglect 94
Research Directions 95
Treatment of Abuse and Neglect 96
Research Directions 98
Prevention of Abuse and Neglect 100
Research Directions 100
Research Results and Policy Considerations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 101
Intergenerational Transmission . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 101
Antisocial Behavior 101
Adoption 102
Family Dissolution 102
Summary , . .. . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . ... ... . . . . . . .. . .. 104
References 105
Chapter 5
Social and Emotional Consequences of Child Maltreatment .. 109
Lise M . Youngblade and Jay Belsky
Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 109
Issues in the Study of Child Abuse and Neglect 111
Overview 114
Social and Emotional Consequences during Infancy
and Toddlerhood 114
Toddler-Peer Relations 121
Social and Emotional Consequences during Childhood 126
Family Interaction in Maltreating Households 127
xiv CONTENTS
PART III. RISK FACTORS ASSOCIATED WITH CHILD ABUSE AND NEGLECT
Chapter 6
Sociological and Ecological Factors 149
Joan I. Vondra
Introduction 149
The Multiple Determinants of Parenting 151
A Model of Influences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 151
Factors within the Family . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 151
Childrearing History 151
Adult Psychological Resources. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 154
The Marital Relationship . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 155
Child Characteristics 158
Factors Outside the Family . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 160
Social Network Support 160
Socioeconomic Considerations 161
The Sociocultural Milieu . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 162
Summary 163
References 164
Chapter 7
Parental Psychopathology and High-Risk Children 171
David C. Factor and David A . Wolfe
Introduction 171
Child-Rearing Patterns and Their Suspected Influence
on Child Development 172
Parental Disorders and Child Adjustment: An Overview
of the Literature 174
Parental Immaturity 175
Parental Criminality 176
CONTENTS xv
Chapter 9
Prevention Programs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 225
Maxine R. Newman and John R. Lutzker
Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 225
Prevention Levels : . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 226
Tertiary Pre vention . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 226
xvi CONTENTS
PART V. CONCLUSIONS
Chapter 12
Future Directions 291
James Garbarino
Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 291
Expanding Definitions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 292
Polarization of Family Experiences 293
Proliferating Linkage 294
Community Responsibility for Children 295
Psychological Maltreatment 297
Summary 297
References 297
INTRODUCTION
CHAPTER 1
INTRODUCTION
It is common to decry the relative paucity of empirical research in newly
emerging fields, and the area of child maltreatment is no exception. Yet,
during the past decade, we have seen an explosion of investigative
activity that has thrust research in child abuse and neglect to higher
levels of understanding and sophistication. Indeed, the growth in
knowledge has been so extensive that recent endeavors have brought
the field to something of a watershed. Therefore, it is timely to assess
the gains that have been achieved and review the changes in investiga-
tive practices and theoretical formulations that have thus far charac-
terized the field . From a research perspective, a transition has occurred
from unidimensional design approaches to multivariate strategies (e.g.,
ROBERT T. AMMERMAN • Western Penn sylvania School for Blind Children , Pitt sburgh,
Pennsylvania 15213. MICHEL HERSEN • Department of Psychiatry, Western Psychiatric
Institute and Clinic, Univer sity of Pittsburgh School of Med icine, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
15213.
3
4 ROBERT T. AMMERMAN and MICHEL HERSEN
Ammerman, Cassisi, Hersen, & Van Hasselt, 1986; Friedrich & Ein-
bender, 1983).
The literature in the early and mid-1970s was dominated by the
search for causative variables in maltreatment. Kempe et al. (1962)specu-
lated that parental psychopathology was the most important determi-
nant in the etiology of abuse and neglect. Subsequently, in their review,
Spinetta and Rigler (1972) concluded that psychopathology and psychi-
atric disorder in parents were crucial factors in the development of abuse
and neglect. In turn, this review resulted in a burgeoning of more well-
controlled empirical studies examining the characteristics of abusive par-
ents. However, findings from these investigations failed to support
Spinneta and Rigler's (1972)initial conclusions. Rather, although current
data indicate that most abusive parents exhibit deficits in a variety of
areas of functioning, they rarely suffer from severe psychiatric distur-
bances (Wolfe, 1985).
On the other hand, Gelles (1973) proposed that societal factors are
the primary causes of abuse and neglect. Within this framework, it is
posited that maltreatment stems from stress engendered by poverty,
educational disadvantage, and cultural sanctioning of physical punish-
ment. Moreover, Gelles (1973) addressed the intergenerational transmis-
sion of child abuse and outlined factors that led many abused children to
grow up to become abusive parents. This model remains a major contri-
bution to the understanding of the causes of child abuse and neglect,
and has received compelling empirical support (e.g., Garbarino, 1976).
However, it is clear that societal elements alone are neither necessary nor
sufficient to bring about child maltreatment. Evidence for this derives
from the fact that most families experiencing economic hardships do not
engage in abuse and neglect. Similarly, child maltreatment occurs at all
levels of socioeconomic status. Furthermore, the extent of intergenera-
tional transmission of maltreatment does not appear to be as extensive
as was previously posited (Kaufman & Zigler, 1987).
The next significant theoretical development in the field was Parke
and Collmer's (1975) treatise hypothesizing that maltreatment was best
understood within the context of the parent-child relationship. Specifi-
cally, they contended that maltreatment was most likely to occur in
particular situations (e.g ., parent-child conflict) in which both parent
(e.g ., poor behavior management skills) and child (e.g ., oppositionality)
characteristics contribute to abuse. Thus, the interaction of parent, child,
and situational variables combine and interact to result in domestic vio-
lence. A large body of data has accrued supporting the social-situational
approach (see Wolfe, 1987) and forming the basis of subsequent models
of maltreatment.
Parke and ColImer's (1975) model suggests that the child can playa
6 ROBERT T. AMMERMAN and MICHEL HERSEN
CONSEQUENCES OF MALTREATMENT
Elmer and Gregg's (1967) seminal investigation suggested that the
psychosocial effects of maltreatment on children are severe and per-
vasive. Since that time, extensive research has been conducted which
has examined the sequelae of abuse and neglect. These studies have
documented a variety of deficits and dysfunctions in maltreated chil-
dren, including insecure attachment formation (Egeland & Sroufe,
1981), depression (Kazdin, Moser, Colbus, & Bell, 1985), anxiety (Green,
1978), conduct disturbance (McCord, 1983), poor peer relations (Bousha
& Twentyman, 1984), academic underachievement (Morgan, 1979), and
intellectual deficits (Hoffman-Plotkin, & Twentyman, 1984). In general,
maltreated children are heterogeneous in terms of type and in severity
of psychopathology. No syndrome has been identified that is specific to
abused or neglected children, and no symptom is common to all victims
of domestic assault (with the possible exception of insecure mother-
infant attachment in maltreated infants). Although it is evident that the
RESEARCH : CURRENT STATUS AND THE FUTURE 9
SUMMARY
It is encouraging that both the public and the research community
have recently taken an active interest in child abuse and neglect. The past
25 years have seen tremendous growth in scientific investigation, and this
has led to a complementary increase in our understanding of the anteced-
ents and consequences of maltreatment. Although plagued by methodo-
logical impediments, research in child abuse and neglect has elucidated
the contributing factors in etiology. In addition, promising developments
in the treatment of perpetrators and child victims have emerged. The
future holds promise, providing that researchers attend to the multivari-
ate nature of maltreatment, and that they will direct their efforts toward
the development, implementation, and empirical evaluation of compre-
hensive assessment, treatment, and prevention strategies.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
REFERENCES
Ainsworth, M. D. (1980). Attachment and child abuse. In G. Gerber, C. Ross, & E. Zigler
(Eds .), Childabuse: An agenda foraction (pp. 35-47). New York:Oxford University Press.
RESEARCH: CURRENT STATUS AND THE FUTURE 17
Am erican Humane Association . (1984). Highlights of official child neglect and abuse reporting
1982. Den ver, CO : Author.
Ammerm an , R. T. (1989). Child abuse and neglect. In M. Hersen (Ed .), Innovations in child
behavior therapy (p p. 353-394). New Yor k: Springer.
Ammerman, R. T. (1988). Preve ntio n of mother-child problem s in families with young
mul tihandicapped children . International Journalof Rehabilitation Research, 11,416-417.
Amme rma n, R. T., Cassisi, J. E., Hersen , M. , & Van Hasselt, V. B. (1986). Con sequences of
ph ysical abus e and neglect in children . Clinical Psychology Review, 6, 291-310.
Amme rma n, R. T., Hersen, M., & Van Hasselt, V. B. (1987). The Child Abuse and Neglect
Interview Schedule(CANIS). Unpublish ed manuscript, Western Pennsylvania School for
Blind Children , Pittsburgh , Pennsylvani a.
Ammerman, R. T., Van Hasselt, V. B., & Hersen, M. (1988). Maltr eatment in handicapped
children: A critical review. Journal of Family Violence, 3, 53-72.
Ammerman, R. T., Hersen , M., Van Hasselt, V. B., McGon igle, J. J., & Lubetsky, M. (1989).
Abu se and neglect in psychiatrically hospitalized multihandicapped children. Child
Abuse and Neglect, 13, 335-343.
Barone, V. J., Green, B. v., & Lutzker, J. R. (1986). Home safety with families being treated
for child abuse and ne glect. Behavior Modification , 10, 93-114.
Belsky, J. (1980). Child maltreatment: An ecological integration. American Psychologist, 35,
320- 335.
Besharov, D. J. (1982). Toward bett er research on child abu se and neglect : Makin g defini-
tional issues an explicit methodological concern. Child Abuse and Neglect, 5, 383-390.
Bou sha, D. M., & Twen tyman, C. T. (1984). Mother-child in teractional sty le in abuse,
neglect , and con trol gro ups : Naturalistic observations in th e home. Child Development,
93, 196-114.
Brassard, M. R., Germain, R., & Hart , S. N. (Eds .). (1987). Psychological maltreatment of
children and youth. New York: Pergam on Press.
Burgess, R. L., & Draper, P. (1988). A biosocial theo ry of family violence: The role of
natural selection, ecological instability, and coer cive in terpe rso nal contingencies. In L.
Ohl in & M. H . Ton ry (Eds .), Crime and justice-an annual review of research: Family
violence. Chicago : Univers ity of Chicago Press.
del.issovoy, V. (1979). Towa rd the definition of "abuse pro voking child ." Child Abuse and
Neglect, 3, 341- 350.
Egeland, B., Breitenbucher, M., & Rosenberg, D. (1980). Prosp ective study of significance
of etiology of child abu se. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 195-205.
Egeland, B., & Brunnquell, D. (1979). An at-risk approach to th e study of child abuse .
Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 18, 219-236.
Egeland, B., & Sroufe, L. A. (1981). Att achment and early maltreatm ent. Child Develop-
ment, 52, 44-52.
Egeland, B., Sroufe, L. A., & Erickson , M. (1983). The developmental conse quences of
different patterns of maltreatment. Child Abuse and Neglect, 7, 459-469.
Elmer, E. (1977). Fragile families, troubled children: Theaftermath of infant trauma . Pittsburgh :
University of Pitt sburgh Press.
Elmer, E., & Gregg, G. S. (1967). Developmental characteristics of abu sed children. Pedi-
atrics, 40, 596-602 .
Fantuzzo, J. W., [urecic, L., Stovall, A., Hightower, A. D., Goins , C,; & Schachtel, D.
(1988). Effects of adult and peer social initiations on the social behavior of withdrawn,
malt reated presch ool children . Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 34-39.
Fantuzzo, J. W., & Twen tyman, C. T. (1986). Ch ild abu se and psychotherap y research:
18 ROBERT T. AMMERMAN and MICHEL HERSEN
Lutzker, J. R., & Rice, J. M. (1987). Using recidivism data to evaluate Project 12-Ways: An
ecobehavioral approach to the treatment and prevention of child abuse and neglect.
Journal of Family Violence, 2, 283-290.
MacMurray, B. K., & Carson, B. A. (in press). Legal issues in violence toward children. In
R. T. Ammerman & M. Hersen (Eds.), Case studies in familyviolence. New York: Plenum
Press.
McCord, J. (1983). A 4O-year perspective on effects of child abuse and neglect. Child Abuse
and Neglect, 7, 265-270.
Morgan, S. R. (1979). Psycho-educational profile of emotionally disturbed abused children.
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 8, 3-6.
Morgan, S. R. (1987). Abuse and neglect of handicapped children. Boston: Little, Brown.
Parke, R. D., & Collrner, C. W. (1975). Child abuse: An interdisciplinary analysis. In E. M.
Hetherington (Ed.), Review of childdevelopment research (Vol. 5, pp. 509-590). Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Plotkin, R. c., Azar, S., Twentyman, C. T., & Perri, M. G. (1981). A critical evaluation of
the research methodology employed in the investigation of causative factors of child
abuse and neglect. Child Abuse and Neglect, 5, 449-455.
Rosenberg, M. S., & Reppucci, N . D. (1985). Primary prevention of child abuse. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 576-585.
Spinetta, J. J., & Rigler, D. (1972). The child abusing parent: A psychological review.
Psychological Bulletin, 77, 296-304.
Starr, R. H., Jr. (1988). Physical abuse of children. In V. B. Van Hasselt, R. L. Morrison, A.
S. Bellack, & M . Hersen (Eds .), Handbook of family violence (pp. 119-155). New York:
Plenum Press.
Starr, R. H., Dietrich, K. N. , Fischhoff, J., Ceresnie, S., & Zweier, D. (1984). The contribu-
tion of handicapping conditions to child abuse. Topics in Early Childhood Special Educa-
tion, 4, 55-69.
Walker, C. E., Bonner, B. L., & Kaufman, K. L. (1988). The physically and sexually abused
child: Evaluation and treatment. New York: Pergamon Press.
Wolfe, D. A. (1985). Child abusive parents: An empirical review and analysis. Psychological
Bulletin, 97, 462-482.
Wolfe, D. A. (1987). Childabuse: Implications for child development and psychopathology. New-
bury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
PART II
GENERAL ISSUES
CHAPTER 2
THE EPIDEMIOLOGY OF
CHILD MALTREATMENT
RAYMOND H. STARR, JR., HOWARD DUBOWITZ, AND
BEVERLY A. BUSH
INTRODUCTION
Articles about child maltreatment appear daily in almost every metro-
politan newspaper. It seems that children are being injured, molested,
and even killed at an alarming rate. In an attempt to understand child
maltreatment, the first question to ask is what do we really know about
the extent of the problem? Answering that question is the purpose of
this chapter.
The first part of this chapter deals with the critical need to define
child maltreatment. Epidemiology cannot be considered independently
of the definition of differing types of abuse and neglect. The next section
of the chapter summarizes the results of three different types of study:
(1) analyses of reported maltreatment cases, (2) examinations of both
reported and unreported cases known to professionals, and (3) surveys
of maltreatment among the general public. The implications of these
data are discussed in the final section of the chapter.
23
24 RAYMOND H. STARR, JR., et al.
PHYSICAL ABUSE
PHYSICAL NEGLECT
SEXUAL ABUSE
SUMMARY
"Strictly speaking, we cannot discuss incidence and prevalence with regard to reported
cases . Incidence figures may be inflated by reports of maltreatment of children already
known to child protective services personnel who do not represent new cases. Similarly,
pr evalence cannot be determined based on reports, only on the total number of cases
currently considered maltreated. For consisten cy, we use the term s rate and incidence in
anal yzing reporting practices in this chapter.
EPIDEMIOLOGY OF MALTREATMENT 29
The American Association for Protecting Children (AAPC) and its par-
ent agency, the American Humane Association, have been funded since
1974 to prepare annual summaries of child maltreatment reports submit-
ted to child protection agencies. The data submitted to the AAPC in-
clude the total number of reports, the data source, and the charac-
teristics of the reporting system. In addition, many state and local
agencies provide case level data.
The use of reported cases to examine child maltreatment incidence
has inherent problems. Foremost among these is reporting bias . For
example, poor and minority families are more likely to be reported as
maltreating (Hampton & Newberger, 1985; O'Toole, Turbett, & Nalepka,
1983; Pelton, 1977) due, in part, to their increased contact with social
service providers. Further evidence suggests that only about 40% of
maltreatment cases are reported to CPS (NCCAN, 1988). Other studies
have examined reporting practices in more detail (e.g., Adams, Barone,
& Tooman, 1982; Gelles, 1982; Knudsen, 1988; Morris, Johnson, &
Clasen, 1985; Newberger, 1983).
Results of the most recent AAPC data-for 1986-indicate that over
two million children (2,086,000) were reported as maltreated to protec-
tive services agencies, a rate of 33 per 1,000 children (AAPC, 1988).
Reports were filed on 1,335,000 families with a mean of 1.6 children per
family reported. These data indicate an 8% increase in reported cases
from 1985 and a 212% increase in the past decade, an average increase of
13% per year. However, these AAPC data need to be interpreted with
caution, because the figures may be overestimates due to the inclusion
of duplicate cases if more than one maltreatment report was filed for a
child in a given calendar year. Alternatively, they may be underesti-
mates because reports on Native Americans are not included by some
states. Regardless of the accuracy of these data, most investigators do
agree that reported cases are only the "tip of the child abuse iceberg."
The AAPC (1988) has also analyzed available data by type of mal-
treatment in those cases in which an investigation indicated that mal-
treatment did indeed occur a. Fluke, personal communication, Novem-
ber 22, 1988) (see Table 1). Most, but not all, of these indicated cases are
substantiated. The AAPC differentiates between indicated and substan-
tiated cases with the former representing a slightly larger class. How-
ever, only some states differentiate between indicated and substantiated
cases.
The AAPC data are based on extrapolations from those states that
provide computerized records of maltreatment reports and that used
similar definitional criteria for the various forms of maltreatment. Phys-
ical neglect (deprivation of necessities) comprised a majority of cases.
30 RAYMOND H. STARR, JR., et aI.
Maltreatment type
All Phy sical Sexual Psychological
maltreatment abuse Fatalities abuse Neglect maltreatment
«Adapted from AAPC (1988). Indicated cases are tho se where abuse is deemed to have occurred as a
a.
result of inve stigation. Most , but not all, indicated cases are substantiated Fluke, personal com-
munication, November 22, 1988).
neglect and least likely to sexually abuse their child. However, other
relatives were involved in almost a quarter of the cases of child sexual
abuse. Males were typically the perpetrator of sexual abuse. Females
were more likely to neglect, psychologically maltreat or murder a child
due to, at least in part, their greater contact with children. Single females
particularly tended to be more neglectful and psychologically maltreat-
ing. Unemployed, single females were at the highest risk for neglect.
This latter finding is not surprising. Such women are at risk for in-
creased reporting because of greater stress, poverty, and depression; not
to mention the bias introduced by negative professional stereotypes and
greater monitoring by social services agencies.
Other reporting trends were also found . Young children from black
families were disproportionately more likely to be reported as abused or
neglected (AAPC, 1988). Overall, 43% of reports in 1986 were for chil-
dren less than 6 years old, with a mean child age of 7.2 years, compared
to a national mean of 8.6 years for all children. Whites were underrepre-
sented in reported cases. They constituted 81% of all U.S. children but
only 66% of maltreatment reports. These reporting trends have existed
since the first analyses of reports in 1976 (AAPC, 1988).
There are still more significant trends in the AAPC (1988) report.
With the increase in recent years in the reporting of sexual abuse there
has been a corresponding decrease in the percentage of all maltreatment
reports involving boys. In 1986, 48% of all reports were for boys, a
decline from 50% ten years earlier. Males were increasingly less likely to
be the caretaker of the reported child (39%) but were disproportionately
more likely to be the perpetrator of maltreatment (44%). The decline in
male caregivers is positively correlated with changes in the percentage
of reports for single-parent, female-headed families (32% in 1986). Al-
most half of all reports (49%) in 1986 were for families who received
public assistance, although they comprise only 12% of families in the
United States. A more detailed analysis of confirmed maltreatment re-
ports from one state's central maltreatment registry supports these find -
ings (Rosenthal, 1988).
Relying on reported, rather than substantiated or indicated, cases to
examine the incidence of child abuse is questionable because allegations
frequently are not substantiated upon CPS investigation. Data for 26
states showed that between 40% and 42% of reports were held to be
valid by the reporting CPS agencies (AAPC, 1988). Using the 40% sub-
stantiation rate and applying it to the 1986 reporting rate of 3311,000, a
substantiated maltreatment rate of approximately 13/1,000 results,
which is similar to the rate of indicated cases of 12/1,000 (see Table 1).
Substantiation rates varied from state to state in 1986 with a range of
32 RAYMOND H. STARR, JR., et at.
23% to 64%, suggesting not only wide variability in definitions but also a
relationship between case validation and number of reports. Nationally,
74% of states have substantiation policies. As might be surmised, these
differ from state to state. Moreover, policies are used in a uniform way in
only one half of the states that have them (Trainor, cited in Russell &
Trainor, 1984). Other factors further influence substantiation rates. For
example, overburdened caseworkers are less likely to classify a given
case as one of maltreatment than are workers with lighter caseloads
(NCCAN, 1981).
Secondary analyses of the American Humane Association and the
AAPC data bases for 1980 and 1983 have also been performed (Max-
imus, Inc ., 1986a,b,c). These support the above conclusion that between
40% and 45% of child abuse and neglect cases are substantiated (Max-
imus, Inc ., 1986a). If the assumption is made that about 42% of abuse
and neglect cases are substantiated, then slightly more than 737,000
children were classified as maltreated in 1986.
Another type of reported case concerns children who are fatally
abused or neglected. Because fatalities are more likely to be officially
reported, data concerning their incidence has been used to analyze tem-
poral changes in child maltreatment. Results of one recent survey indi-
cate that the number of fatalities increased from a projected 899 in 1985
to 1,181 the next year. There was a slight decline to 1,132 in the following
year, 1987 (Daro & Mitchel, 1988). National data were extrapolated from
reports of fatalities from between 34 and 39 states, the number varying
each year.
Again, the counting of fatalities is not so simple as it may seem at
first glance (Mitchel, 1987), because many fatalities due to physical abuse
are misclassified as accidental deaths or sudden infant death syndrome.
The actual incidence of fatalities may therefore be much higher than the
suggested estimates of up to 5,000 a year (Christoffel, Liu, & Stamler,
1981; Mitchel, 1987). In general, studies have found few demographic or
case differences between families in which a child is maltreated but not
fatally injured and those in which the maltreatment is deadly (Mitchell,
1987). Fatalities that are due to maltreatment are a serious, and in all
likelihood an increasing, social problem. It is hoped that better pro-
cedures for securing accurate fatality data will be developed in the near
future so that the exact magnitude can be determined more accurately.
Research also has focused on reporting among more specific sam-
ples . Results of such studies complement the findings of larger, more
comprehensive investigations (Pelton, 1981). In one such study, report-
ing patterns for a single Indiana county over a 20 year period (1965-
1984) were examined (Knudsen, 1988). Demographic characteristics
EPIDEMIOLOGY OF MALTREATMENT 33
were not significantly different from national data for poverty level,
education, occupational status, median age, and sex ratio. The overall
rate of reporting and the percentage of substantiated reports increased
over time and was greater for children less than six years of age. Knud-
sen considers this increase to be the result of a broadening of the defini-
tion of and to an increase in the actual incidence of maltreatment, and
not just the result of increased reporting. The rate of maltreatment sub-
stantiation also increased over time, which would be the case if less
severe forms of maltreatment were being reported. Reports of less se-
vere maltreatment were seen as less likely to be substantiated. In 1984,
the incidence rate for substantiation on a first report of suspected mal-
treatment was more than 10 per 1,000 children. In the last year of his
study, 38% of all reports were for abuse and 62% for neglect. Most
reports were for lack of supervision (38%), followed by physical abuse
(20%), physical neglect and sexual abuse (14% each), other forms of
neglect (10%), and psychological abuse (4%).
A detailed analysis of reported cases in one state, over an 8-year
period, provides further information (Rosenthal, 1988). Although con-
firming other evidence showing that girls were more likely to be sexually
abused than boys, Rosenthal also concluded that boys were more likely
to be severely injured than girls . However, some age differences were
present. For example, when the age of the child was considered, girls
from 13 to 17 years old were more likely to be maltreated, even when
sexual abuse was removed from the analyzed data. In addition, males
were more likely to be the perpetrator with older children, and females
the perpetrator with you nger children. This latter finding is interpreted
as evidence that male teens, who are likely to fight back when struck,
are less likely to be abused and that, when abuse does occur, it is proba-
bly perpetrated by a relatively stronger adult male.
Still other analyses have examined what have been termed the "eco-
logical correlates" of maltreatment (see a review by Zuravin, 1989). Eco-
logical studies, based on the theoretical views of Bronfenbrenner (1977)
and Bronfenbrenner, Moen, and Garbarino (1984), emphasize the rela-
tion of community and environmental characteristics to maltreatment.
In one study, Zuravin (1989) examined the ecology of child maltreatment
in an urban area using reported cases of abuse and neglect as the depen-
dent variable and neighborhood characteristics for individual census
tracts as the independent variable. Eliminating duplicate reports on fam-
ilies, she found that the average abuse incidence for 1983 and 1984 was
23/1,000 families with children. The corresponding figure for neglect
was 26/1,000. The strongest correlates of both abuse and neglect were
low income and the rate of vacant housing in the neighborhood. AI-
34 RAYMOND H . STARR, JR., et al.
though this correlation does not imply a cause and effect relationship, it
does seem likely that poverty and maltreatment are related (Pelton,
1977). The work of Zuravin and others (e.g., Garbarino & Crouter, 1978;
Spearly & Lauderdale, 1983) suggests that adopting an ecological per-
spective should provide information of value in understanding the epi-
demiology of child maltreatment.
In summary, relying on analyses of reported cases to provide reli-
able and valid information on the incidence of maltreatment presents
problems-and foremost, the securing of accurate reports. Unfortunate-
ly, at present it is safe to conclude that relying on substantiated reports
yields an underestimate of the true incidence of abuse and neglect.
Investigators are currently evaluating other approaches to examining
the incidence and prevalence of child maltreatment.
Original definitions
Maltreatment Percentage of change 1986
category 1979-1980 1986 1979-1980 to 1986 Definition
Total abuse 336,600 (5.3) 580,400 (9.2) +72 (+74) 675,000 (10.7)
Physical 199,100 (3.1) 311,200 (4.9) +56 (+58) 358,300 (5.7)
Sexual 42,900 (0.7) 138,000 (2.2) +222 (+214) 155,900 (2.5)
Psychological 132,700 (2.1) 174,400 (2.8) +31 (+33) 211,100 (3.4)
Total neglect 315,400 (4.9) 498,000 (7.9) +58 (+61) 1,003,600 (15.9)
Physical 103,600 (1.6) 182,100 (2.9) +76 (+81) 571,600 (9.1)
Psychological 56,900 (0.9) 52,200 (0.8) -8 (-11) 223,100 (3.5)
Educational 174,000 (2.7) 291,100 (4.6) +67 (+70) 292,100 (4.6)
«Adapted from NCCAN (1988). Numbers in parentheses are for incidence (rate/I ,OOO children) and for
change in incidence.
36 RAYMOND H. STARR, JR., et al.
from those found for the incidence study as a whole. The children
detected by hospitals, compared with cases reported by other agencies,
were more likely to: (1) live in urban areas (66% vs . 42%), (2) be younger,
(3) have younger parents, (4) be black (25% vs. 16%), and (5) have been
physically abused. Multivariate analyses of the data for cases that were
and were not reported to CPS indicated four variables that were the key
predictors of hospital-based reporting: (1) type of maltreatment, (2) fam-
ily income, (3) maternal role in maltreatment, and (4) race or ethnicity.
Psychologically abused children from white families with above average
incomes, in which the mother was deemed responsible for the maltreat-
ment, were least likely to be reported. Physically abused children from
lower income, minority families, in which the alleged perpetrator was
not the mother, were more likely to be reported. These results support
the argument that a report is more likely to be made when there is a
demographic difference between the reporter and the maltreating family
(O'Toole et al., 1983).
Study results also clarify some of the issues involved in interpreting
the analyses of CPS reports discussed above (AAPC, 1985, 1986, 1987,
1988; American Humane Association, 1984; Russell & Trainor, 1984). Of
particular importance is the elimination of the counting of duplicate
reports in the NCCAN incidence studies. National Incidence Study data
suggest that the increased recognition of maltreatment cases by sources
other than CPS workers has not been reflected in an increased incidence
of confirmed CPS cases . There are two likely explanations for this find-
ing. First, the surveyed professionals may fail to report cases to CPS.
Second, CPS may fail to confirm as legitimate maltreatment those cases
that are reported. If the former is correct, efforts need to be made to
increase the recognition of maltreatment and to convince professionals
of the value of reporting it. If the latter view is correct, efforts need to be
made to secure more investigative resources for CPS agencies. In reality,
it is likely that both explanations contribute to this discrepancy.
Physical Abuse
Some of the earliest estimates of the incidence of physical abuse
were based on surveys of nationally representative samples. Gil (1973)
conducted a survey in 1965 in which 1,520 adults were asked about their
personal knowledge of families who had injured "a child, not by acci-
dent, but in anger or deliberately" (p. 49). The results showed that 3% of
the sample knew of at least one such incident. Allowing for error vari-
ance, between 2.5 and 4.1 million children were injured in the year prior
to the survey-a figure that seems high even today. However, Gil's
methodology has been criticized (Light, 1973). When Light controlled for
errors in the Gil analysis, the incidence of physical abuse decreased to
500,000 children a year.
Straus, Gelles, and their colleagues conducted national surveys of
the incidence of physical abuse in the general population in 1975 and in
1985 (Gelles, 1978; Gelles & Straus, 1987, 1988; Straus et al., 1980; Straus
& Gelles, 1986). Physical abuse was defined as the parental use of certain
violent acts toward a child as measured using the Conflict Tactics Scale
(Straus, 1979). This questionnaire asks family members about how they
resolve conflicts. There were slight differences in the scales used in the
two surveys. The original, 1975 scale asked about throwing objects;
pushing, grabbing, or shoving; slapping or spanking; kicking, biting, or
hitting with a fist; hitting or trying to hit with an object; "beating up";
and threatening with or using a knife or gun (Straus & Gelles , 1986).
Questions concerning scalding or burning were added in 1985.
Child abuse was defined as kicking, biting, punching, "beating up,"
and threatening with or using a knife or a gun (Gelles & Straus, 1987).
Other violent acts-including hitting with an object and threatening
with a weapon-were excluded. They were considered as variations of
normal discipline, rather than abusive, or were deemed unlikely to lead
to actual injury.
For the 1985 study, telephone interviews were conducted with a
nationally representative sample of 1,428 families containing a male-
female couple or one adult over 18 years old where there was at least
one child from 3 to 17 years old . Overall, 84% of contacted families
agreed to participate in the survey. Data are available for only two-
parent families. The 1975 study was conducted using a different method
(Gelles, 1978; Straus & Gelles, 1986). First, in-person rather than tele-
phone surveys were conducted. Second, families were given the alter-
40 RAYMOND H . STARR, JR., et al.
violent acts included in the survey was quite limited. Many other acts,
such as burning and poisoning, result in physical abuse. For example, 5
in 1/000 families in the 1985 survey reported burning or scalding their
child (Gelles & Straus, 1987). This finding suggests that the rate of child
abuse is probably higher than the reported 19/1/000. Third, the surveys
included onl y children over 2 years of age despite the fact that younger
children were more likely to be physically abused than older ones (ap-
proximately 7% for 3- to 4-year-olds, and 4% for older children) (Gelles,
1978). The presence of an inverse relationship between child age and
physical abuse is supported by other studies. For example, 79% of phys-
ical abuse reports in Arizona and 85% in Louisiana were for children less
than four years old (Maximus, 1986b). The latter figures, in turn, are
probably overestimates. Young children are more subject to scrutiny by
professionals and are more likely to represent unconfirmed cases of
physical abuse (Iason, Andereck, Marks, & Tyler, 1982). A fourth reason
the Gelles and Straus findings are likely to be underestimates of physical
abuse is that results are available for intact families even though only
single-parent families are under greater stress and, as such, are more
likely to be violent. In a reanalysis of National Incidence Study data,
Miller (1984) found that teenage mothers, who are likely to be single
parents, also were more likely to physically abuse their children than
were older mothers. Furthermore, retrospective evidence suggests that
single parents are more punitive toward their children (Sack, Mason, &
Higgins, 1985). Thus, the Gelles and Straus data only suggest the inci-
dence of physical abuse in the general population.
The Gelles and Straus (1987) data indicate that approximately 1.5
million children were subjected to potentially abusive violence in 1975
compared to about 750/000 children in 1985. Moreover, they suggest that
the decline in incidence is most probably because of some combination
of attitudinal and behavioral change rather than differences in survey
methodology. Only further research can yield a conclusive answer about
the validity of these alternative explanations.
The results of the 1975 survey do reveal something about the types
of families that direct high levels of violence toward children. Examining
the Conflict Tactics Scale variables that were used to determine child
abuse (Straus & Gelles, 1986)/ approximately 4% of fathers and 6% of
mothers were abusive (Gelles, 1978). This heightened level of maternal
abuse may be due to the greater amount of time mothers spend with
their children, to the greater degree to which children interfere with
maternal daily activities (Straus et al., 1980)/ or to other factors related to
family functioning. Boys were more likely to be subjected to physical
42 RAYMOND H. STARR, JR., et al.
Sexual Abuse
The preceding sections have noted the dramatic increase in public
awareness concerning sexual abuse over the past decade. Given this
recognition, it is not surprising that many studies have revealed its
prevalence, including general population surveys and studies of special
populations that are believed to have a high likelihood of sexual abuse
(e.g., Gruber & Jones , 1983; Silbert & Pines, 1981).
It is important to remember that all the data in this section are for
the prevalence of sexual abuse during childhood as measured retro-
spectively in adult samples. These data indicate how many adults were
sexually abused during childhood. The rates reported are thus consider-
ably higher than would be the case if the incidence per 1,000 cases per
year (the measure used for all other data in this chapter) were used as a
measure of problem magnitude.
There is considerable variation in study methodologies. Studies use
dissimilar definitions, consider childhood to end at various ages, have
different criteria about the inclusion of cases where there was consent to
the maltreatment, use diverse question framing techniques, and inter-
view samples with different characteristics using a variety of interview
techniques.
As was indicated earlier, definitions are a major factor influencing
the outcome of incidence and prevalence studies. Key variables are
whether physical contact was involved, the age of the victim, the age
differential between the victim and the perpetrator, and whether the
EPIDEMIOLOGY OF MALTREATMENT 43
Geographic surveys
Finkelhor Kercher & McShane Russell Wyatt
(1984) (1984) (1983, 1984) (1985)
«This figure may be 55% depending on how response rate is mea sur ed (Haugaard & Reppucci. 1988).
EPIDEMIOLOGY OF MALTREATMENT 45
the subject's race and education level. Taking all of this into considera-
tion, Wyatt and Peters (1986b) conclude that not only is more research
needed to clarify the exact extent of sexual abuse but that future studies
should use in-person interviews so that the issue of sexual abuse can be
embedded within a study described as having a broader purpose. They
also suggest (1) matching subject and interviewer race, (2) using care-
fully trained project personnel, (3) ensuring interview confidentiality,
and (4) paying subjects for the time and expense incurred.
What do these studies tell us about the prevalence of sexual abuse
in addition to the fact that study design plays an important role in
determining study outcome? A major conclusion, unfortunately, is that
the prevalence of child sexual victimization is much higher than would
have been predicted a decade ago . Although we cannot cite one preva-
lence figure as giving a single, valid indicator of the extent of child
sexual abuse, we can state with certainty that females are at significantly
greater risk. However, it is important to remember that, relying on the
high prevalence figures cited by D. E. H. Russell (1983) and Wyatt
(1985), less than 5% of women have been sexually abused by a parent or
parent-figure. Since these studies examined prevalence rates and did
not include males, the actual annual incidence of sexual abuse for the
overall child population will be significantly lower. In spite of this cave-
at, sexual abuse is a major problem and most cases never reach the
attention of authorities.
is to compare the incidence and prevalence data from studies of the first
four levels of maltreatment: (1) cases known to protective services, (2)
those that have been reported to other investigative agencies, (3) mal-
treated children who have been detected by other professionals and
agencies, and (4) cases in which other individuals, including per-
petrators and victims, know of the maltreatment.
A second answer to the above question is that much is known about
the epidemiology of child maltreatment. Indeed, there is much data,
much more than we have been able to discuss in this chapter. The
problem is how to interpret and systematize the available findings . Table
4 summarizes data for the different maltreatment levels by the type of
maltreatment.
The data in Table 4 indicate a wide variation in estimates of the
incidence and prevalence of child maltreatment. The two data sets that
are most similar are those for reported cases (AAPC, 1988) and for the
National Incidence Study of cases that have either been reported or are
known to agencies or professionals (NCCAN, 1988). Although it would
be expected that the National Incidence Study data would show a high-
er incidence of maltreatment-because a broader sample of cases were
included-this is not consistently the case. Thus, the data for the Na-
tional Incidence Study indicate a lower incidence of sexual abuse than
the AAPC data suggest. But the AAPC data are inflated because of the
only for determining incidence and prevalence, but also for case finding,
treatment, and prevention.
Third, progress has been made since 1975 when Cohen and Sus-
sman stated "the only conclusion which can be made fairly is that infor-
mation indicating the incidence of child abuse in the United States sim-
ply does not exist" (cited in Gelles, 1978, p. 582). But, much still remains
to be done. For example, research groups could come together to agree
on standardized definitions to be used in epidemiological studies. If a
group liked a given definition better than the standard, agreed upon
one, they could incorporate both definitions in their study. This was
done in the most recent National Incidence Study to allow comparability
of the 1979-1980 and 1986 data (NCCAN, 1988). Furthermore, better
information is needed about the prevalence of neglect and psychological
maltreatment at Level 4 of the National Incidence Study categorization
of level of awareness (NCCAN, 1988). Ideally, a study could be con-
ducted using a national interview sample in which the presence of all
types of maltreatment is evaluated. Although planning and conducting
such a study would be difficult, it would be a major step forward in
obtaining a better idea of the actual dimension of the problem of child
maltreatment in all its forms.
Another way to increase knowledge about the epidemiology of
child maltreatment is to improve analyses of reported case data. The
American Association for Protecting Children receives annual funding
sufficient for only partial analysis of state report data. Annual data anal-
yses are not needed. It would be a better investment to have detailed
analysis of data every 5 years in order to obtain a fuller examination of
reporting and case substantiation trends-the very data policy makers
need to make informed decisions. It is unfortunate that existing data do
not permit making a firm conclusion about the magnitude of child mal-
treatment and whether our prevention and treatment efforts are having
a meaningful impact in reducing the extent of the problem. Perhaps
future research and analyses of reporting trends will provide the an-
swers to these as yet unanswered questions.
REFERENCES
Adams, W., Barone, N. , & Tooman, P. (1982). The dilemma of anonymous reporting in
child protective services . Child Welfare, 61, 3-14.
American Association for Protecting Children. (1985). Highlights of official child neglect and
abuse reporting 1983. Denver: American Humane Association .
EPIDEMIOLOGY OF MALTREATMENT 51
American Association for Protecting Children. (1986). Highlights of official child neglect and
abuse reporting 1984. Denver: American Humane Association.
American Association for Protecting Children. (1987). Highlights of official child neglect and
abuse reporting 1985. Denver: American Humane Association.
American Association for Protecting Children. (1988). Highlights of official child neglect and
abuse reporting 1986. Denver: American Humane Association.
American Humane Association. (1984). Highlights of official child neglect and abuse reporting
1982. Denver: Author.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development. Amer-
ican Psychologist, 32, 513-531.
Bronfenbrenner, U., Moen, P., & Garbarino, J. (1984). Child, family, and community. In R.
Parke (Ed.), Review of childdevelopment research (pp . 283-328). Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.
Christoffel, K. K., Liu, K., & Stamler, J. (1981). Epidemiology of fatal child abuse: Interna-
tional mortality data . Journal of Chronic Diseases, 34, 57-64.
Daro, D., & Mitchel, L. (1988). Child abuse fatalities remain high: The results of the 1987 annual
fifty statesurvey (Working Paper No .8). Chicago: National Committee for Prevention of
Child Abuse.
Dubowitz, H. (1987). Child maltreatment in the United States : Etiology, impact, and prevention.
Washington, DC: Office of Technology Assessment.
Finkelhor, D. (1979). Sexually victimized children . New York: Free Press.
Finkelhor, D. (1984). Child sexual abuse: New theory and research . New York: Free Press.
Finkelhor, D., & Hotaling, G. T. (1984). Sexual abuse in the National Incidence Study of
Child Abuse and Neglect: An appraisal. Child Abuse and Neglect, 8, 23-33 .
Fritz, G. S., Stoll, K., & Wagner, N. N. (1981). A comparison of males and females who
were sexually molested as children. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 7, 54-59.
Fromuth, M. E. (1986). The relationship of childhood sexual abuse with later psychological
and sexual adjustment in a sample of college women. ChildAbuseand Neglect , 10, 5-15.
Fromuth, M. E., & Burkhart, B. R. (1987). Childhood sexual victimization among college
men: Definitional and methodological issues. Violence and Victims, 2, 241-253 .
Garbarino, J.• & Crouter, A. (1978). Defining the community context for parent-child
relations: The correlates of child maltreatment. Child Development, 49, 604-616.
Garbarino, J., & Gilliam, G. (1980). Understanding abusive families . Lexington, MA: Lex-
ington Books.
Garbarino, J., Guttman, E., & Seeley, J. W. (1986). The psychologically battered child: Strategies
for identification, assessment, and intervention. San Francisco: [ossey-Bass .
Gelles, R. J. (1978). Violence toward children in the United States. American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 48, 580-592 .
Gelles, R. J. (1982). Problems in defining and labeling child abuse. In R. H. Starr, Jr. (Ed.),
Child abuse prediction: Policy implications (pp . 1-30) . Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.
Gelles, R. J., & Straus, M. A. (1987). Is violence toward children increasing? A comparison
of 1975 and 1985 national survey rates . Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 2, 212-222 .
Gelles, R. J., & Straus, M. A. (1988). Intimate violence. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Gil, D. G. (1973). Violence against children: Physical childabuse in the UnitedStates. Cambridge:
Harvard University Press.
Giovannoni, J. M., & Becerra, R. M. (1979). Defining child abuse. New York: Free Press.
Gruber, K. J., & Jones, R. J. (1983). Identifying determinants of risk of sexual victimization
of youth: A multivariate approach. Child Abuse and Neglect, 7, 17-24.
Hampton, R. L., & Newberger, E. H. (1985). Child abuse incidence and reporting by
52 RAYMOND H. STARR, JR., et al.
hosp itals: Significance of severity, class, and race. American Journal of Public Health, 75,
56-60.
Hau gaa rd, J. J., & Reppucci , N . D. (1988). The sexual abuse of children: A comprehensiveguide
to current knowledge and intervention strategies. San Franci sco: Jossey-Bass.
Jason, J., Andereck, N. D., Mark s, S., & Tyler, C. w., Jr. (1982). Child abuse in Georgia: A
method to evalua te risk factors and rep orting bias. American Journalof Public Health, 72,
1353-1358.
Kercher, G. A., & McShane, M. (1984). The pre valence of child sexua l abuse victimization
in an adult sample of Texas residents. Child Abuse and Neglect, 8, 495-501.
Kinsey, A. C. ; Pomeroy, W. B., Martin, C. E., & Gebhard, P. H. (1953). Sexual behavior in the
human female. Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders.
Knudsen , D. D. (1988). Child maltreatment over two decade s: Change or continuity?
Violence and Victims, 3, 129-144.
Korbin, J. E. (1987). Child abu se and neglect: The cultural context. In R. E. Helfer & R. S.
Kempe (Eds.), The battered child (4th ed. , pp. 23-41) . Chicago : Univ ersity of Chicago
Press.
Last, J. (1983). A dictionary of epidemiology. New York: Oxford Univer sity Press.
Light, R. (1973). Abused and ne glected children in America : A stu dy of alternative pol-
icies. Harvard Educational Review, 43, 556-598.
Mau sn er, J. S., & Kramer, S. (1985). Epidemiology: An introductory text. Philadelphia: W. B.
Saunders.
Maximu s, Inc. (1986a). Child maltreatment: A secondary analysis of the American Humane
Association data base. Washington, DC: Author.
Maxirnus, Inc. (1986b). Child maltreatment: A secondary analysis of the American Humane
Association data base: Exhibits. Washington , DC: Au thor.
Maximu s, Inc. (1986c). Assessingalternative statistical estimation procedures for determining the
incidence of child maltreatment: Appendix A: Reporting, substantiation, and confirmation
rates, 1980 and 1983 (Final rep ort, Task Order U, BOA 105-84-8103, prep ared for the
Office of Program Development, Office of Human Developm ent Services). Wash-
ing ton, DC: Author.
Miller, S. H. (1984). The relation sh ip between ad olescent childbearin g and child maltreat-
ment. Child Welfare, 63, 553- 557.
Mitche l, L. (1987). Child abuse and neglect fatalities: A review of the problem and strategies for
reform (Working Paper No. 838). Chicago : Na tional Committee for Prevention of Child
Abus e.
Morri s, J. L., Johnson, C. F., & Clasen, R. W. (1985). To report or not rep ort: Physician's
attitudes toward d iscipline and child abu se . American Journal of Diseases of Children,
139, 194- 197.
National Center on Child Abu se and Neglect. (1981). Study findings: National study of the
incidence and severity of child abuseand neglect (DHHS Publication No . OHDS 81-30325).
Washington, DC: U. S. Government Printing Office.
National Center on Child Abu se and Neglect. (1988). Study findings: Study of national
incidence and prevalence of child abuse and neglect: 1988. Washington, DC: U. S. Depart-
ment of Health and Human Services.
Newberger, E. H. (1983). The help ing hand strikes again: Unintended cons equences of
child abuse reporting. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 12, 307-311.
O'Toole, R., Turbett, P., & Nalepka, C. (1983). Theories, prof essional knowledge, and
diagnosis of child abu se. In D. Fink elhor, R. J. Gelles, G. T. Hotalin g, & M. A. Strau s
(Eds .), The dark side of families : Current family violence research (pp. 349-362). Beverly
Hills, CA: Sage .
EPIDEMIOLOGY OF MALTREATMENT 53
Pelton, L. H. (1977). Child abuse and neglect: The myth of classlessness. American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 48, 608-617 .
Pelton , L. H. (1981). Introduction. In L. H. Pelton (Ed.), The social contextof child abuse and
neglect (pp . 11-21). New York: Human Sciences Press.
Polansky, N., & Williams, D. (1978). Class orientations to child neglect. Social Work, 23,
397-401.
Radda Barnen. (1980). The ombudsman and child maltreatment. Stockholm, Sweden: Radda
Barnen.
Risin, L. I. , & Koss, M. P. (1987). The sexual abuse of boys : Prevalence and descriptive
characteristics of childhood victimizations. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 2, 309-323 .
Rosenthal, J. A. (1988). Patterns of reported child abu se and neglect. Child Abuse and
Neglect, 12, 263-271.
Russell , A., & Trainor, C. M. (1984). Trends in childabuse and neglect: A national perspective.
Denver: American Humane Association.
Russell, D. E. H. (1983). The incidence and prevalence of intrafamilial and extrafamilial
sexual abuse of female children. Child Abuse and Neglect, 1987, 133-146 .
Russell, D. E. H . (1984). The prevalence and seriousness of incestuous abuse: Stepfathers
vs. biological fathers. Child Abuse and Neglect, 8, 15-22 .
Sack, W. H., Mason, R., & Higgins, J. E. (1985). The single-parent family and abusive child
punishment. American Journalof Orthopsychiatry, 55, 252-259 .
Sedney, M. A., & Brooks, B. (1984). Factors associated with a history of childhood sexual
experience in a non-clinical female population. Journal of the American Academy of Child
Psychiatry, 23, 215-218 .
Silbert, M. H. , & Pines, A. M. (1981). Sexual child abuse as an antecedent to prostitution.
Child Abuse and Neglect, 5, 407-411.
Spearly, J. L., & Lauderdale, M. (1983). Community characteristics and ethnicity in the
prediction of child maltreatment rates . Child Abuse and Neglect, 7, 91-105 .
Starr, R. H., Jr. (1988). Physical abuse of children. In V. B. Van Hasselt, R. L. Morrison, A.
S. Bellack, & M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of family violence (pp. 119-155). New York:
Plenum Press .
Straus, M. A. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence : The Conflict Tactics (CT)
Scale. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41, 75-88.
Straus, M. A., & Gelles, R. J. (1986). Societal change and change in family violence from
1975 to 1985 as revealed by two national surveys. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 48,
465-479 .
Straus, M. A. , Gelles, R. J., & Steinmetz, S. K. (1980). Behind closed doors: Violence in the
American Family. Garden City, NY: Anchor Books.
Valentine, D. P., Acuff, D. 5., Freeman, M. L., & Andreas, T. (1984). Defining child
maltreatment: A multidisciplinary overview. Child Welfare, 63, 497-509 .
Whiting, L. (1976). Defining emotional neglect. Children Today, 5, 2-5.
Wyatt, G. E. (1985). The sexual abuse of Afro-American and white-American women in
childhood. Child Abuse and Neglect, 9, 507-519 .
Wyatt, G. E., & Peters, S. D. (1986a). Issues in the definition of child sexual abuse in
prevalence research. Child Abuse and Neglect, 10, 231-240.
Wyatt, G. E., & Peters, S. D. (1986b). Methodological considerations in research on the
prevalence of child sexual abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect , 10, 241-251.
Zuravin, S. J. (1989). The ecology of child abuse and neglect: Review of the literature and
presentation of data. Violence and Victims, 4, 101-120.
CHAPTER 3
INTRODUCTION
The recognition of child abuse and neglect as a significan t social problem
in the United States is a relatively recent de velopment. Although most
states had passed specific child maltreatment laws by the earl y 1920s, it
was not until publication of a 1962 article describing the "battered-child
syndrome" (Kempe, Silverman, Steele, Droegenmuller, & Silverman,
1962) that legislators and health care professionals paid considerable
attention to the problem of child abuse and neglect. Since then, there
have been several waves of legislation and judicial activity that have
been nearly universal in American jurisdictions but that seldom have
had unequivocally positive effects.
RANDYK. O TTO • Department of Law and Mental Health , Florida Mental Health Institute,
University of South Florida, Tampa, Florida 33612-3899. GARY B. MELTON' Dep art-
ment of Psychology, Unive rsity of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, Nebraska 68588-0308.
55
56 RANDY K. OITO and GARY B. MELTON
[t]he fundamental liberty interest of natural parents in the care, custody, and
management of their child does not evaporate simply because they have not
been model parents or have lost temporary custody of their child to the state.
Even when blood relationships are strained, parents retain a vital interest in
preventing the irretrievable destruction of their family life. (p. 753)
agreement about either the specific circumstances that justify child pro-
tective jurisdiction or the level of coercive intervention that is desirable.
One school of commentators, emphasizing the deleterious effects of
abuse and neglect, argues that the fundamental value placed on family
privacy undermines the state's ability to protect children from abuse and
neglect at the hands of their caretakers (e.g ., Bourne & Newberger, 1977;
Feshbach & Feshbach, 1976; Garbarino, 1977, 1982; Garbarino, Gaboury,
Long, Grandjean, & Asp, 1982). This group perceives children as partic-
ularly vulnerable and in need of special protection by the state. "Family
privacy" is considered merely to shield abusive families from public
scrutiny, rather than protect them from unnecessary or unjustified state
intervention. Accordingly, this group advocates minimal restrictions on
the state in its attempts to identify and intervene in cases of suspected
abuse. Such "child savers" support adoption of low standards for invok-
ing state intervention and aggressive, high levels of intervention in cases
of suspected abuse or neglect.
By contrast, two other schools of thought share the belief that state
intrusion into family life is rarely advisable, even though they disagree
why families should be better insulated against state intervention in
cases of suspected abuse and neglect. One particularly influential family
law scholar, Michael Wald (1975, 1976, 1982; Wald, Carlsmith, & Leiber-
man, 1988) has relied primarily on utilitarian arguments to support fami-
ly integrity and privacy. Given the potential harm and lack of clear
benefit frequently associated with state intervention in cases of alleged
abuse (see, e.g., Children's Defense Fund, 1987; Clark Foundation, 1985;
Mnookin, 1973), Wald has advocated clearly defined, strictly limited
bases for state intervention. In short, he has concluded that more harm
usually will be done by intervention than would have occurred if the
state had ignored possible child maltreatment. Accordingly, Wald con-
tends that coercive intervention should occur only when there is clear
evidence that serious harm will result from inaction or a relatively unin-
trusive intervention.
An important step toward adoption of Wald's view carne with the
publication of the Juvenile Justice Standards Relating to Child Abuse
and Neglect (Institute of Judicial Administration/American Bar Associa-
tion [IJA/ABA], 1981). The Standards generally would limit state inter-
vention to "situations in which there are findings that a child has suf-
fered, or is at substantial risk of suffering, serious harm, and ...
intervention is necessary to protect the child from being endangered in
the future" (Melton et al., 1987, p. 311). Although the Standards Relating
58 RANDY K. OTIO and GARY B. MELTON
to Abuse and Neglect, unlike most of the other volumes of the Juvenile
Justice Standards, have not been adopted by the ABA as its official
policy, the Standards remain an important reference for critics of the
child welfare system.
A second school argues for family privacy and greater protection
from state intrusion on the grounds that such intervention, insofar as it
threatens children's perceptions of their "psychological parents," can
have extremely deleterious effects (Goldstein, Freud, & Solnit, 1973,
1979). Like Wald (1975, 1982), these commentators would set a high
threshold before state intervention could take place (indeed even higher
than Wald advocates), but they differ from Wald in that they support
aggressive intervention (even immediate termination of parental rights)
once serious abuse or neglect is substantiated. Goldstein et aI. believe
that the best interests of the child (or, to use their term, the "least
detrimental alternative") are best ensured by minimization of uncertain-
ty and unpredictability and promotion of children's belief in the om-
nipotence of their parents. Their approach involves forceful, immediate
measures, but only when it is clear that serious abuse or neglect has
occurred. Otherwise, Goldstein et al. advocate virtually unfettered def-
erence to parental autonomy.
With such a difference of opinion among experts in the fields of
child mental health and family law, it is not surprising that the approach
that states should adopt in cases of suspected abuse and neglect is
controversial. Unfortunately, there is no reason to believe that this lack
of consensus will be remedied in the immediate future.
Even if a consensus about the nature of child maltreatment and the
appropriate policy responses to it is lacking, it is clear that the general
direction since the 1960s has been toward more expansive concepts of
abuse and neglect, accompanied by correspondingly increased interven-
tion, despite the paradoxical evolution of constitutional law on family
privacy during the same period (Melton, 1987c). The impression held by
legislators and the public alike is that there is a need for broad standards
for state intervention. For example, the scope of maltreatment for legal
purposes has expanded to include emotional abuse and neglect, even
though application of such concepts in a manner that is not arbitrary or
discriminatory may be impossible (Melton & Thompson, 1987). Accom-
panying expanded definitions have been calls for greater use of criminal
sanctions, especially in regard to sexual abuse. In that respect, in recent
years, legislatures in every American jurisdiction have adopted pro-
cedural and evidentiary reforms designed to make prosecution easier
(Bulkley, 1985), despite questions of the efficacy of a "get-tough" strat-
TRENDS IN LEGISLATION AND CASE LAW 59
Criminal Adjudication
Although criminal prosecution has been rare until recent years,
remedies for child abuse and neglect have been available in both the civil
and criminal arenas for a considerable period of time (Davidson & Horo-
witz, 1984). All 50 states provide potential criminal sanctions for child
maltreatment. As a matter of practice, though, criminal prosecution is
generally reserved for sexual and the most serious cases of physical
abuse. Data gathered by Midonick (1972) are illustrative, although some-
what out of date. Examining New York records, Midonick found that
fewer than 10% of all reports of child abuse found their way to family
court, and fewer than 10% of those cases adjudicated were referred to
criminal court.
A philosophical argument can be made that child abuse and neglect
should be adjudicated criminally because to do otherwise minimizes the
seriousness of the behavior. "Decriminalizing" what is otherwise crimi-
nal behavior is considered to send a subtle message to the public. And as
with other crimes, criminal adjudication is considered by some to pro-
mote specific and general deterrence (Chisolm, 1978; Davidson, Horo-
witz, Marvell, & Ketcham, 1981).
Advocates of criminal adjudication argue that it best ensures the
safety of children in serious cases by incapacitating the abuser when
necessary (Davidson, 1981). Even with the use of restraining orders, civil
adjudication cannot guarantee that the abuser will not have further con-
tact with the abused child (or other children).
Additionally, use of criminal prosecution is advocated because it is
considered to better ensure compliance with sanctions that may be im-
posed by the judge. Compliance with criminal court-ordered treatment
is thought by some to be more likely than compliance with civil court-
ordered treatment (Urzi, 1981).
Criminal adjudication is also advocated on the grounds that law-
enforcement officials will investigate allegations more aggressively if
they believe that a criminal prosecution will result (Urzi, 1981). Criminal
prosecution, because of the greater due process requirements and bur-
60 RANDY K. OTIO and GARY B. MELTON
den of proof, is also more protective of families' privacy rights than civil
adjudication.
Finally, recent work suggests that criminal adjudication may be able
to reduce recidivism in criminal behaviors traditionally considered to be
psychosocial problems. Like child abuse, spouse abuse has traditionally
been managed through noncriminal interventions (Costa, 1983). More
recently, however, states have begun to respond to spouse abuse using
the criminal justice system. This approach has met with some success
and may have implications for the management of child abuse.
In a demonstration project conducted in Minneapolis, Sherman and
Berk (1984) found that individuals arrested for assaulting their spouse
showed a somewhat lower recidivism rate than those who were either
counseled by, or simply separated from their spouse by the responding
police officer. Certainly, these preliminary findings are open to in-
terpretation but they do suggest that criminal adjudication may be indi-
cated with some types of behavior traditionally considered to be psycho-
social problems and amenable to treatment.
The parallel to child abuse and neglect is clear. Criminal sanctions
may be effective in reducing child abuse and neglect, at least with a
segment of the population. However, research comparing recidivism
rates among abusive and neglectful parents who have been processed
through the criminal justice and civil systems needs to be conducted.
It appears then that there are specific advantages associated with
criminal adjudication. These include the provision of considerable pro-
cedural protections to families accused of abuse, ability to manage and
control individuals who present a continuing, serious threat to their
victims and other children, and the potential impact of general and
specific deterrence by way of threatened imposition of criminal sanc-
tions.
Civil Adjudication
Although criminal prosecution has become more common in child
maltreatment, civil intervention long has predominated in such cases.
The first child protective agency was established in New York in 1875,
and by the mid-1920s all states had laws prohibiting child maltreatment
(Besharov, 1983).
Perhaps foremost, civil adjudication of abuse and neglect has been
based on the belief that these behaviors are psychosocial problems that
can be differentiated from criminal behavior (Fontana & Besharov, 1979;
Rosenberg, 1975; Urzi, 1981). Abusing one's child often is considered to
be a mental health problem in itself, or at least indicative of underlying
TRENDS IN LEGISLATION AND CASE LAW 61
Discussion
The debate about the relative merits of criminal and civil interven-
tion does not result in a clear answer. Each form of intervention has
advantages and disadvantages of varying importance in particular cases,
and both civil and criminal sanctions may be sought in some instances.
Some (e.g., Chisolm, 1978) have argued that the courts and child protec-
tive workers should choose the judicial intervention accordingly, with
the child's best interest being of paramount concern. Such a principle
TRENDS IN LEGISLATION AND CASE LAW 63
LEGISLATIVE RESPONSES
that are responsible for abuse investigation, name those individuals re-
quired to report abuse, and list penalties for failure to report. Addi-
tionally, these statutes generally contain clauses overriding professional
privileges (with the general exception of the attorney-client privilege)
and granting immunity from civil liability for good faith reporting.
Initially, only physicians were statutorily mandated to report cases
of suspected abuse, presumably because they were considered to be in a
unique position to identify such cases (Isaacson, 1975; Paulsen, 1967).
Gradually, however, legislators expanded reporting statutes to the point
that most child professionals and many laypersons are required to re-
port suspected abuse (see Myers, 1986, for a review of the 50 state
reporting statutes).
This first wave of legislation had a considerable impact on the
number of suspected abuse cases reported to child welfare and law-
enforcement authorities. Besharov (1983) noted that there was an
eightfold increase in the number of cases of suspected abuse reported
between 1966 (150,000) and 1980 (1.1 million).
Reporting statutes have certainly not been the panacea that some
might have hoped. The most common criticism of these statutes is that
they are vague and overbroad (Besharov, 1983, 1984; Davidson & Horo-
witz, 1984; Flicker, 1977; Melton, 1987a; Melton & Thompson, 1987).
Representative of states' abuse laws is New York's reporting statute,
which mandates that health care and law-enforcement professionals re-
port abuse when "they have reasonable cause to suspect" abuse (New
York Social Services Law Sec. 413, McKinney, 1988). Exactly what the
professional needs to see or believe in order to report abuse and thereby
comply with the reporting statute is not made clear in most statutes.
There may also be difficulty reaching a consensus about the behav-
ior that falls within the scope of child maltreatment. Research has shown
that there is considerable disagreement between professionals regarding
what constitutes sexually abusive behavior on the part of caretakers
(Atteberry-Bennett, 1987; Atteberry-Bennett & Reppucci, 1986). It can
even be difficult to reach agreement about the boundaries of physical
abuse (Giovannoni & Becerra, 1979). There is considerable disagreement
about the circumstances under which spanking and other, less severe
forms of corporal punishment should be viewed as abusive (Alvy, 1975).
The matter becomes still more complicated when neglect or "failure
to provide" is considered. All states consider parents' failure to provide
their children with basic care and necessities to be neglectful (Davidson
& Horowitz, 1984). Nonetheless, the scope of "basic care and neces-
sities" is not self-evident. Although some state statutes requiring par-
ents to provide adequate shelter, food, and care have been struck down
TRENDS IN LEGISLATION AND CASE LAW 65
in 1973, the year before passage of Public Law 93-247, only $507,000 of
Title IV-B monies went toward child abuse activities nationally (House of
Representatives Report No. 685, 1974).
Noting that state efforts largely went toward reporting and that
there was inadequate follow-up, the Child Abuse Prevention and Treat-
ment Act attempted to establish a research agenda and prevention and
treatment programming to be coordinated through the newly estab-
lished National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect (NCCAN). The act
authorized the director of NCCAN to compile, analyze, and publish
research results regarding child abuse; develop and maintain an infor-
mation clearinghouse; compile and publish training materials; provide
technical assistance (through grants and contracts) to assist public or
private nonprofit organizations in the planning, development, and im-
plementation of prevention and treatment programs; and conduct re-
search regarding causes and prevention of child abuse.
To be eligible for CAPTA funding, states had to meet certain criteria,
most of which were related to reporting and investigation procedures.
In order for states to receive CAPTA funding they had to have a system
in place for abuse reporting and investigation, provide criminal and civil
immunity for good-faith reporting, ensure that abuse reports and rec-
ords were kept confidential, and provide guardians ad litem for all chil-
dren. CAPTA also required states to maintain their funding of abuse
programming and extend preferential treatment to parental organiza-
tions organized to prevent and treat child abuse.
Presumably in response to some of the criticisms presented above,
there was discussion of the difficulty in defining such terms as neglect
and mental or emotional injury in the House report. However, the bill, as
passed, offered the following definition: "This section defines the term
'child abuse and neglect' as the physical or mental injury, sexual abuse,
negligent treatment, or maltreatment of a child under the age of eighteen
by a person who is responsible for the child's welfare" (House of Repre-
sentatives Report No. 685, 1974; emphasis added).
The Child Abuse Prevention and Treatment Act of 1974 has been
amended several times so that its scope is now somewhat broader. A
number of important amendments were passed in 1984. Public Law
98-457 extended the definition of abuse and.neglect to include the sexual
exploitation and the withholding 'of medically Indicated treatment (the
"Baby Doe" amendment), and expanded CAPTA so that abuse and ne-
glect occurring in residential and out-of-home placements was also cov-
ered by the act .
An amendment passed in 1986 (Public Law 99-401) directed the
Secretary of Health and Human Services to establish demonstration
68 RANDY K. OTIO and GARY B. MELTON
Since 1980, advocates for abused and neglected children have estab-
lished Children's Trust Funds in 44 states (National Committee for the
Prevention of Child Abuse [NCPCA], undated). These funds establish a
permanent funding mechanism for child abuse and neglect prevention
and treatment programs at the community level.
Children's Trust Funds are designed to create continuing funding
mechanisms that promote prevention programming for child abuse and
neglect at the community level (NCPCA, undated). Revenues are gener-
ated from various methods, including: surcharges on marriage licenses
and birth certificates, checkoffs on state income tax forms, increases in
divorce filing costs, increased fees for death certificates, and heirloom
birth certificates. This approach allows for the funding of traditionally
neglected prevention programs and insulates them from budget cuts
when state monies become scarce.
The governing body of the trust fund creates a public/private part-
nership. In some states, existing bodies that include representatives
from state agencies, the legislature, and the private sector have adminis-
tered the fund. In other states, public advisory councils consisting of
public and private sector representatives have been created.
Trust fund boards oversee program development and implementa-
tion, hire program staff, review program proposals, and disburse funds.
The major thrust of trust fund programs to date has been in promoting
and funding primary and secondary prevention programs at the com-
munity level. These include, but are not limited to pre- and postnatal
support programs for first-time and recent parents (especially "at risk"
70 RANDY K. orro and GARY B. MELTON
have been controversial and often may have increased children's limbo in
the criminal justice system by opening the door to appeals. Considerable
appellate litigation has resulted from many of the procedural and evi-
dentiary reforms enacted in recent years, because they typically threaten
the defendant's sixth-Amendment rights to a public trial and confronta-
tion of witnesses, his fourteenth amendment right to due process,
and/or the public's first amendment right to access (through the press)
to the trial process (Melton, 1987a).
HEARSAY EXCEPTIONS
CLOSED COURTROOMS
television. Noting that, at the time, 25 states provided for such closed
circuit procedures and 33 allowed the presentation of videotaped testi-
mony of child victims, O'Connor wrote that
nothing in today's decision necessarily dooms such efforts by state legisla-
tures to protect child witnesses. Initially, many such procedures raise no
substantial confrontation clause problem since they involve testimony in the
presence of the defendant. (p. 2804)
O'Connor's concurrence in Coy suggests that the Court may not be
as hostile to special procedures in the future . It is possible that O'Con-
nor, White, and Kennedy (who took no part in the decision) will join the
Coy dissenters (Blackmun and Rehnquist) to form a majority more ac-
cepting of special procedures designed to protect witnesses, providing
that they are not mandatory and that they allow for "face-to-face" con-
frontation between the defendant and witness. The Supreme Court will
address this issue more specifically in Craig v. Maryland, which the court
had yet to hear at the time this chapter went to press.
presented in order to create the inference that the child has or has not
been abused, depending on whether he or she displays the relevant
behaviors. Although such testimony is usually offered by the prosecu-
tion in order to buttress allegations of abuse, the defense may some-
times attempt to refute abuse allegations by showing that the child does
not fit the abused child profile .
The majority of appellate courts considering this issue have ap-
proved of the admission of opinions about the nature of the abuse
syndrome, providing that the expert does not offer an opinion about the
credibility of the alleged victim. Melton and Limber (1990) argue against
admission of such testimony on numerous grounds. First, they note that
there is little empirical support for a "sex abuse syndrome," and the
purported syndromes are based mainly on clinical intuition. Moreover,
many abuse victims display no symptoms at all, and many of the
"symptoms" of child abuse are common to other clinical populations
(Browne & Finkelhor, 1986; Haugaard & Reppucci, 1988).
Secondly, Melton and Limber (1990) note that introduction of such
testimony may mislead the decisionmaker. Even if it could be deter-
mined through introduction of expert testimony that the child was
abused, it does not prove that the child was abused by the defendant.
The decisionmaker may be predisposed, though, to hold the defendant
responsible for the abuse.
Finally, Melton and Limber (1990) argue against admission of such
testimony on the grounds that it, in some ways, puts the victim "on
trial." Such a line of inquiry may result in intrusions into the victim's
privacy through extensive evaluations by both the prosecution and
defense.
DISCUSSION
SUMMARY
The legislature and judiciary have paid increasing attention to the
problems of child abuse and neglect over the past 25 years. Numerous
reforms have been implemented in an attempt to increase reporting of
suspected cases and to facilitate prevention and treatment programs.
Additionally, judicial reforms have been instituted in order to facilitate
adjudication of sexual abuse and more serious cases of physical abuse.
These efforts have produced mixed results for a variety of reasons.
Because no consistent orientation exists among child advocates regard-
ing the nature of child maltreatment, reforms have been based on emo-
tional responses to the problem of abuse as much as empirical analysis
or a theoretical basis . Many reforms have been instituted rather hastily,
with little foresight and planning. Accordingly, we are left with pro-
cedures that are of questionable utility, some of which are also of ques-
tionable constitutionality.
The opportunities presented to psychologists are numerous, given
the current state of affairs. Most importantly, psychologists must ad-
dress definitional issues and study the effects of different policies and
procedures that are now in use . Additionally, psychologists should ap-
ply their expertise and assist child victims and judicial system, while at
the same time acknowledging their limitations.
REFERENCES
Alvy, K. T. (1975). Preventing child abuse. American Psychologist, 30, 921-928.
Atteberry-Bennett, [. (1987). Child sexual abuse : Definitions and interventions of parents
TRENDS IN LEGISLATION AND CASE LAW 79
Child abuse and neglect litigation: A manual for judges (pp . 145-146). Wash ington , DC:
Department of Health and Human Services.
Davidson , H . A., & Horowitz , R. M. (1984). Protection of children from family maltreat-
ment. In R. M. Horowitz & H . A. David son (Eds.), Legal rights of children (pp. 262-312).
Colorado Springs: Shepard's/McGraw-Hill.
David son, H . A., Horowitz, R. M., Marvell, T. B., & Ketcham, O. W. (1981). Childabuseand
neglect litigation: A manual for judges . Washington, DC: Department of Health and
Human Service s.
DeConey, J. J. (1975). The legal pro cess: A positive force in the interest of child ren . In
American Humane Association (Ed .), Fourth nationalsymposium on child abuse (pp . 62-
69). Den ver : Author.
Dickens, B. M. (1978). Legal resp on ses to child abuse. In J. M. Eekelaar & S. N . Kat z (Eds. ),
Fam ily violence: An international and interdisciplinary study (pp. 338- 362). Toronto:
Butt erworths.
Eatman, R., & Bulkley, J. (1986). Protecting child victim/witnesses: Sample laws and materials.
Washington, DC: American Bar Asso ciation .
Feshbach, N. D., & Feshbach, S. (1976). Punishmen t: Parent rites versus children's rights.
In G. P. Koocher (Ed.), Children's rights and the mental health professions (pp. 149-170).
New York: Wiley.
Finkelhor, D. (1984). Child sexual abuse: New theory and research. New York: Free Press.
Flicker, B. D. (1977). Standards for juvenile justice: A summaryand analysis. Cambridge , MA :
Ballinger.
Fontana , V. J., & Besharov, D. J. (1979). The maltreated child (4th ed .). Sp rin gfield , IL:
Ch arl es C Thomas .
Fraser, B. G. (1979). A glance at the pa st, a gaze at the pr esent, a glimps e of the future: A
critical analysis of th e deve lopment of child abus e rep orting statu tes . Chicago-Kent Law
Review, 54 , 641-686.
Garbarino, J. (1977). The pri ce of privacy in the social dynamics of child abuse . Child
Welfare, 56, 565-575.
Garbarino, J. (1982). Children and families in the social environment. New York: Aldine .
Garbarin o, J., Gaboury, M. T., Long, F., Grandjean, P., & Asp, E. (1982). Wh o own s the
children? An ecological perspective on public poli cy affecting children. In G . B.
Melt on (Ed .), Legal policies affecting child and youth services (p p. 43-63). New York:
Haworth.
Gelles, R. J. (1982). Probl em s in defining and labeling child abuse . In R. H . Starr (Ed .),
Child abuse prediction: Policy implications (p p. 1- 30). Cambri dge, MA : Ballin ger.
Giovannon i, J. M., & Becerra, R. M. (1979). Defining child abuse. Ne w York: Free Pre ss .
Globe Newspaper Co . v. Superior Court, 457 Ll.S. 596 (1982).
Goldstein, J., Freud, A. , & Solnit, A. J. (1973). Beyond the best interests of the child. New York:
Free Pre ss .
Goldste in, J., Freud, A. , & Solnit, A. J. (1979). Before the best interests of the child. New York:
Free Press .
Goodman, G. S. (1984). The child witness: Conclusion s and future direction s for resea rch
and legal practice. Journalof Social Issues, 40, 157-175.
Goodman , G. S., & Helgeson , V. S. (1985). Child sexual assault: Ch ildren's memory and
the law. In J. Bulkley (Ed. ), Papers from a nationalpolicy conferencein legal reforms in child
sexual abuse cases (p p. 41-60). Wash ington, DC: Am erican Bar Association , Na tional
Legal Resource Center for Child Advocacy an d Protection .
Graham, M. H . (1985). Child sex abuse persecutions: Hearsay and confront atio n clause
iss ues. In J. Bulkley (Ed .), Papers from a national policy conference on legal reforms in child
sexual abuse cases (pp. 159-208). Washington, DC: Am erican Bar Association .
TRENDS IN LEGISLATION AND CASE LAW 81
Haugaard, J. J., & Reppucci, N. D. (1988). Thesexual abuse of children. San Francisco: [ossey-
Bass.
Hou se of Representatives Report No. 685, 93d Cong ., reprinted in 1974 U.S. Code Congres-
sional & Administrative New s 2663.
Idaho v. Wright, _ _ U.S. _ _, 110 S. Ct. 883 (1990).
In re Freiberger, 153 Mich. App. 251, 395 N .W.2d 300 (Mich. Ct. App. 1986).
Institute of Judicial Administration/American Bar Association (1981). Standards relating to
abuse and neglect. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.
Isaacson, L. B. (1975). Child abu se reporting statutes: The case for holding ph ysicians
civilly liable for failure to report. San Diego LAw Review, 12, 743-777 .
Kalichman, S. E., & Craig , M. E. (in pre ss). Victims of incestuous abuse: Mental health
professionals' attitudes and tendency to report. Victimology: An International Journal.
Kalichrnan , S. E., Craig, M. E., & Follingstad , D. R. (1987, August). Factors contributing to
mental health professionals' reporting of child abuse. Paper presented at the meeting of the
America n Psychologica l Association, New York.
Kelly, D. P. (1987). Victims . Wayne LAw Review, 37, 69-86.
Kempe, C. H., Silverma n, F. N ., Steele, B. F., Droegenmueller, w., & Silver, H. K. (1962).
The battered child syndrome. Journal of the American Medical Association, 181, 17-24.
Kentucky v. Stincer, 107 S. Ct. 2658 (1987).
Kilpatrick, D. G., & Otto, R. K. (1987). Constitutiona lly guaranteed participation in crimi-
nal proceedings for victims: Potential impact on psychological functioning. Wayne LAw
Review, 37, 7-28.
King, M. P., Hunter, W. M., & Runyan , D. K. (1988). Going to cour t: The experience of
child victims of intrafamilial sexual abuse. Journal of Health Politics, Policy, and LAw, 13,
705-721.
Lucht, C. L. (1975). Providing a legislative base for reporting child abu se . In American
Humane Association (Ed.), Fourth national symposium on child abuse (pp . 49-60) . Den-
ver: Author.
Maney, A., & Wells, S. (Eds.). (1988). Professional responsibilities in protecting children: A public
health approach to child sexual abuse. New York: Praeger.
Maryland v. Craig, u.s . ,100 S. Ct. 834 (1990).
Melton, G. B. (1982). Children's right s: Where are the children? American Journal of Ortho-
psychiatry, 52, 530-538 .
Melton, G. B. (1983a). Toward "personhood" for adolescents: Autonomy and privacy as
values in public policy. American Psychologist, 38, 99-103 .
Melton, G. B. (1983b). Decision making by children: Psychologica l risks and benefits. In G.
B. Melton, G. P. Koocher, & M. J. Saks (Eds.), Children's competence to consent (pp . 21-
40). New York: Plenum Press.
Melton, G. B. (1987a). Special legal prob lems in the protection of handicapped child ren
from parental maltreatment. In J. Garbarino, P. E. Brookhouser, & K. J. Authier (Eds .),
Special children-special risks: The maltreatment of children with disabilities (pp . 179- 193).
New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Melton, G. B. (1987b). Children's testimony in cases of alleged sexua l abuse. In M.
Wolraich & D. K. Routh (Eds.), Advances in developmental and behavioral pediatrics (pp.
179-203). Green wich, CT: JAI Press.
Melton, G. B. (1987c). Law and random event s: The sta te of child mental heal th policy.
International Journal of LAw and Psychiatry, 10, 81-90.
Melton, G. B., & Corson, J. (1987). Psychological mal trea tment and the schools: Problems
of law and professional responsibility. School Psychology Review, 16, 188-194.
Melton, G. B., & Thompson, R. A. (1987). Legislative approaches to psychological mal-
treatment: A social policy anal ysis. In M. R. Brassard , R. Germain, & S. N. Hart (Eds .),
82 RANDY K. OTIO and GARY B. MELTON
Psychological maltreatment of children and youth (pp . 203-216). New York: Pergamon
Pre ss.
Melton, G. B., & Limber, S. (1990). Psychologists' involvement in case s of child maltreat-
ment: Limits of role and expertise. American Psychologist, 44, 1225-1233 .
Melton, G. B., Petrila, J., Poythress, N . G., & Slobogin, C. (1987). Psychologicalevaluations
for the courts: A handbook for mental health professionals and lawyers. New York: Guilford
Press.
Midonick, M. L. (1972). Children, parents and the courts: Juvenile delinquency, ungovernability
and neglect. New York: Practis ing Law Institute.
Missing Children Assistance Act 42 U.S.c. § 5771-5777 (1982, Supp. 1986).
Mnookin, R. H. (1973). Foster care : In wh ose best int ere st? Harvard Educational Review, 43,
599-638.
Muehleman , T., & Kimmons, C. (1981). Psych ologists' views on child abuse rep orting,
con fidentiality, life, and the law: An explorat ory study. Professional Psychology, 12, 631-
638.
Myer s, J. E. B. (1986). A survey of child abuse and neglect reporting statutes. Journal of
Juvenile Lnw, 10, 1-72.
National Center for State Courts. (1988). The changing face of child abuse registries.
National Center for State Courts Report, 15(7), 2.
National Committee for th e Prevention of Child Abuse (undated). Children's trust funds.
Chica go: Author.
Nelson , B. J. (1984). Making an issue of child abuse: Political agenda setting for social problems.
Ch icago: Univ ersity of Ch icago Pre ss.
Ne w York Social Services Law Sec. 413 (McKin ney, 1988).
Ohio v. Roberts, 448 U.S. 56 (1980).
Parental Kidnapping Prevention Act 28 U.S.C.S § 1730A (1979, Cum. Supp. 1988).
Paulsen, M. G. (1967). Ch ild abuse reporting laws: The sha pe of th e legislation . Columbia
Lnw Review, 67, 1-49.
Pierce v. Society of Sisters, 268 U.s. 510, 45 S.Ct. 571 (1925).
Prince v. Massachusetts, 321 U.S. 158 (1944).
Roe v. Wade, 410 u.s. 113 (1973).
Rogers, C. M. (1982). Child sexual abuse and the court: Preliminary find ings. In J. Conte &
D. A. Shore (Eds .), Social work and child sexual abuse (pp. 145-1 53). New York: Haworth
Pre ss.
Rosenberg , A. H. (1975). The law and child abuse . In N. B. Ebeling & D. A. Hill (Ed s.),
Child abuse: Intervention and treatment (pp . 161-170). Acton , MA: Publi shing Sciences
Group.
Rosenberg, M. S., & Hunt, R. D. (1984). Child maltreatment: Legal and mental health
issues. In N . D. Reppucci, L. A. Weithom, E. P. Mulve y, & J. Monahan (Eds .), Chil-
dren, mental health, and the law (p p. 79-101) . Beverly Hills: Sage .
Runyan, D., Everson, M., Edelsohn, G., Hunter, W., & Coulter, M. L. (1988). Impact of
legal intervention on sexually abused children. Journal of Pediatrics, 113, 647-653.
Santosky v. Kramer, 455 U.S. 745 (1982).
Senate Rep ort No . 123, 99th Congo reprinted in 1986 Ll.S, Code Congressional & Adm in-
istrative New s 1967.
Sherman, L. w., & Berk, R. A. (1984). The Minnesota domestic violence experiment. Wash-
ington, DC: Police Foundation.
Swoboda, J. S., Elwork, A., Sales, B. D., & Levine, D. (1979). Knowledge of and com-
pliance with privileged communication and child-abuse reporting laws. Professional
Psychology, 10, 448-457.
TRENDS IN LEGISLATION AND CASE LAW 83
Thibaut, J., & Walker, L. (1978). A theory of procedure. California Law Review, 66, 541-566.
Urzi, T. (1981). Cooperative approaches to child protection: A communityguide. Excerpted in H .
A. Davidson, R. M. Horowitz, T. B. Marvell , & O. W. Ketcham (Eds .), Childabuse and
neglect litigation: A manual for judges. Wash ington, DC: Department of Health and
Human Services .
United States v. Iron Shell, 633 F.2d. 77 (8th Cir. 1980).
Wald, M. (1975). State interventions on behalf of "neglected" children: A search for real-
istic standards. Stanford Law Review, 27, 985-1040 .
Wald, M. S. (1976). State intervention on behalf of "neglected" children: Standards for
removal from their homes, monitoring the status of children in foster care, and termi-
nation of parental rights. Stanford Law Review, 28, 625-706 .
Wald, M. S. (1982). State intervention in behalf of neglected children: A proposed legal
response. Child Abuse and Neglect, 6, 3-45.
Wald, M. S., Carlsmith, J. M., & Leiberman, P. H. (1988). Protecting abused and neglected
children. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Whitcomb, D. (1985). Assisting child victims in the courts: The practical side of legislative
reform. In J. Bulkley (Ed.), Papers from a national policy conference on legal reforms in child
sexual abuse cases (pp. 13-29). Washington, DC: American Bar Association, National
Legal Resource Center for Child Advocacy and Protection.
Wisconsin v, Yoder, 406 U.S. 205 (1972).
CHAPTER 4
RESEARCH DIRECTIONS
RELATED TO CHILD
ABUSE AND NEGLECT
Roy C. HERRENKOHL
INTRODUCTION
Future directions for research on child abuse and neglect depend on
goals for the general area and the role to be played by research in
achieving these goals. The goals proposed here are to provide: (1) treat-
ment to abusive families to reduce or remove the likelihood of recur-
rence, and (2) services to abused children to ameliorate the conse-
quences of abuse and to develop prevention strategies to reduce its
incidence. The role of research in meeting these objectives is to obtain
information about the incidence and prevalence of abuse, to examine its
causes, and to evaluate the effectiveness of treatment and prevention.
Policymakers, the third party to the challenges posed by child
abuse, use results from research and the experience of service providers
to inform their decisions concerning the provision of resources for treat-
ment, prevention, and research. After two decades of research, the
nature and extent of the problem in a descriptive sense is clearer, but
Roy C. HERRENKOHL • Center for Social Research, Lehigh University, Bethlehem, Penn-
sylvania 18015.
85
86 ROY C. HERRENKOHL
key questions for policy (i.e., the causes of abuse and neglect, effective
treatment and prevention strategies) remain largely unclear. The inade-
quacy of relevant findings means that policymakers must develop and
implement plans based on informed judgment rather than more objec-
tive evidence.
RESEARCH ISSUES
Five research issues concerning child abuse are considered below:
incidence/prevalence, causes, consequences, treatment, and preven-
tion. These are interrelated in that what is learned about one often has
implications for one or more of the others. There also are important
distinctions, depending on which of the four types of abuse (physical,
emotional, sexual, or neglect) is involved. These often are mentioned
together but are seldom studied or analyzed concurrently. In practice
they sometimes occur singly or in combination (E. C. Herrenkohl & R.
C. Herrenkohl, 1981). This poses a problem of research strategy. Should
research focus on each type of maltreatment separately or on types in
combination? The answer may differ depending on the issue under
study.
The discussion that follows examines the current state of and possi-
ble future directions for research in the aforementioned five areas. Only
illustrations from the different types of abuse are offered. For a more
comprehensive review of specific projects, the reader is referred to
Finkelhor, Hotaling, and Yllo (1988).
RESEARCH DIRECTIONS
renee of and the relationship between the different types of abuse have
not been examined.
Finally, how is the information on incidence and prevalence to be
coordinated so that services can be provided to abusive families, pol-
icymakers can learn the extent of the problem, and the information can
be used in research? This problem points to the need for better coordina-
tion and cooperation among service providers, policymakers, and
researchers.
RESEARCH DIRECTIONS
physical trauma of the abuse. The other concerns the longer term, gener-
ally psychosocial, consequences of maltreatment.
The medical effects of physical abuse and neglect have been the
focus of many studies reported primarily in medical journals. These
studies should be catalogued and evaluated for use by those who must
determine the etiology of childhood injuries. Such a cataloguing might
also provide the basis for indexing injuries with reference to severity
which, in turn, could be related to potential for longer term conse-
quences.
Several researchers (e.g., Gray & Kempe, 1976) have described
physically abused children's psychological state soon after they were
identified as having been abused. Such information can be used to index
the degree of psychological trauma manifested by the child at the time
the abuse was identified. For example, the short-term consequences of
sexual abuse have been described (Wolfe, Wolfe, & Best, 1988) in largely
anecdotal terms. Similarly, neglected children's physical and psycholog-
ical status have been reported by several authors (Polansky et al., 1981;
Oates, 1986). Such reports should be coordinated and evaluated. In the
case of emotional abuse, however, the short-term consequences have
not been differentiated from longer term effects .
The longer term consequences of abuse have been studied by a
small group of researchers (e.g ., Cicchetti & Rizley, 1981; Egeland &
Sroufe, 1981; Elmer, 1977; E. C. Herrenkohl & R. C. Herrenkohl, 1981;
Lynch & Roberts, 1982; Martin, 1976; Oates, 1986). To varying degrees,
their studies have considered physical abuse, emotional abuse, and ne-
glect. On the other hand, studies of the longer term consequences of
sexual abuse have only recently been conducted (Wolfe et al., 1988).
Most of this research is longitudinal in nature, although it differs consid-
erably in terms of the time period covered. The reader is referred to
more comprehensive reviews of these investigations by Augoustinos
(1987) and Toro (1982).
PHYSICAL ABUSE
EMOTIONAL ABUSE
SEXUAL ABUSE
CHILD NEGLECT
RESEARCH DIRECTIONS
form of abuse. Further, because some children experience more than one
type of abuse, a model or models of how the four types interrelate in
various combinations to effect the child are needed.
One strategy to examine how abuse influences psychosocial func-
tioning is to compare abused children who do well developmentally
with those who do not. For example, Garmezy (1983) has used this
approach to determine why some children who are developmentally at
risk do well and others, similarly at risk, do not. It may prove helpful in
identifying features in an abusive family or characteristics of an abused
child that serve to buffer the child against the impact of abuse (Mrazek &
Mrazek, 1987). For example, the abused child who receives nurturance
from a parent or parent surrogate or who has sufficient insight to per-
ceive the abusive behavior as due to the perpetrator's problems may not
succumb to the most damaging consequences of abuse.
Finally, abuse is related to such factors as poverty, marital discord,
and social isolation, and each of these may have an effect on the child's
development similar to those stemming from abuse. How to differenti-
ate the contribution of abuse from the effects of other factors with which
abuse covaries is an additional research problem that is particularly
problematic, because factors, such as poverty, may have a more far-
reaching effect than, say, physical abuse, especially, if the physical inju -
ries are endured in the less-severe range.
RESEARCH DIRECTIONS
RESEARCH DIRECTIONS
skills? Second, if there is evidence that the presumed cause was altered,
was there evidence that the incidence of abuse was reduced among
those who were the target population of the intervention (Rosenberg &
Reppucci, 1985)?
Evaluation research has developed rapidly although it has had a
relatively small impact on the child abuse/neglect area . The lack of a
community of child abuse/neglect evaluators who evaluate child abuse
preventive intervention programs and counsel each other on their eval-
uation activities hinders development of more adequate evaluations of
prevention programs. Communities of researchers exist around the
other issues considered in this discussion and these groups have played
an important role recently. So few evaluations of preventive interven-
tions have been made that each starts largely on its own and tends to
operate independently. Campbell (1987), considering evaluations of pre-
ventive interventions in the area of mental health, notes that one well-
trained scientist or team producing one research report does not result
in understanding the effective uses of an intervention. Rather, progress
toward such understanding is achieved by a scientific community that
stays in close communication on a shared puzzle and that promotes
competitive replication and criticism of each other's evaluative activities.
INTERGENERATIONAL TRANSMISSION
ANTISOCIAL BERAVIOR
ADOPTION
FAMILY DISSOLUTION
Unrealistic Expectations
Inadequate Theories
As has been suggested above, our theories and models are inade-
quate to the demand. The pressure for quick results has tended to steer
researchers away from working on conceptual issues and model build-
ing that are fundamental to obtaining more definitive results.
Measurement Procedures
A serious need exists for more adequate measurement procedures.
For example, conceptualization of the different types of abuse suggests a
continuous variable ranging from mild to severe to abusive discipline.
Measures most often reflect categorical indicators that are of question-
able reliability and validity. In addition, continuous operational defini-
tions are currently being developed.
Adequacy of Designs
Many of the general questions asked regarding research on abuse
explicitly or implicitly concern causality. It is generally accepted that
unequivocal evidence about causality cannot be obtained (Cook &
Campbell, 1979), given the limitations of applied research. The primary
aim of research, then, is to come as close as possible to achieve this . In
improving research designs the objective is to reduce the number of
alternative explanations or, as Cook and Campbell (1979) label them,
"plausible rival hypotheses." Much is known about improving quasiex-
perimental designs that has yet to be applied in this area of research. For
example, quasiexperimental designs are subject to selection bias; that is,
a nonequivalence of experimental and control or comparison groups on
some dimension in addition to the experimental influences under study.
104 ROY C. HERRENKOHL
Sample Selection
Research to date has tended to use experimental group samples
based upon convenience (e.g., hospital patients, clinic patients) and to
select control or comparison groups in much the same way. Such sam-
ples are important for developing new research areas. As the research
becomes more precise, however, issues of generalizability of results in-
crease in importance.
Statistical Procedures
The statistical procedures used in many existing studies are some-
times inadequate to the questions being addressed. Again, this is to be
expected in the early stages of developing a research area. However, the
models that depict the causes of abuse are likely to be complex, and
statistical procedures are currently being tested that are more appropri-
ate to the statistical demands these models create. To date, few studies
have used multivariate statistical techniques, even though these are like-
ly to be used with greater frequency in the future. The models proposed
to explain the occurrence of abuse or the consequences of abuse are
multidetermined and involve a variety of constructs (e.g., Belsky, 1984;
Gelles , 1973; Wolfe, 1987). Realistic tests of these models can only be
accomplished by using such multivariate procedures as multiple regres-
sion, multivariate analysis of variance, factor analysis, or structural
equation (LISREL) modeling.
SUMMARY
The preceding discussion has considered current status and future
research directions for five issues in the area of child abuse and neglect:
incidence/prevalence, causes, consequences, treatment, and preven-
tion. Each issue has a number of problems that require increased atten-
tion by the research and clinical community. In particular, major prob-
lems to be addressed include the need for more explicit theories and
hypotheses, improved measurements and design, and more adequate
sampling and statistical procedures. A renewed focus on improved re-
search approaches will significantly enhance our understanding of the
development, impact, and treatment of child abuse and neglect.
RESEARCH DIRECTIONS 105
A CKNOWLEDGMENTS
REFEREN CES
Achenbach, T. M., & McConaughy, S. H . (1987). Empirically based assessment of child and
adolescent psychopathology. Newbury Park, CA: Sage .
Alvy, K. T. (1975). Preventing child abu se . American Psychologist, 30, 921-928 .
Ammerman, R. T. (1990). Child abuse and neg lect. In M. Hersen (Ed.), Innovations in child
behavior therapy. New York: Springer.
Augoustinos, M. (1987). Deve lopmental effects of child abuse: Recent find ing s. ChildAbuse
and Neglect, 11, 15-27.
Bassetti, M. (1974). N utrition . In K . E. Barnard & H . B. Douglas (Eds.), Childhealth assess-
ment (pp. 59-73). (Part I, USDHEW, Publication No. HRA 75-30) . Bethesda, MD :
Department of Health, Education, and Welfare.
Belsky, J. (1984). Determinants of parenting: A proce ss model. Child Development, 55, 83-
96.
Brassard, M. R., & McNeil , L. E. (1987). Child sexual abuse. In H. R. Brassard, R. Germain,
& S. N . Hart (Eds .), Psychological maltreatment of children and youth (pp . 69-88). New
York: Pergamon Pres s.
Burgess, R. L., & Conger, R. D. (1978). Family in teraction in abu sive, neg lected and
normal families. Child Development, 49, 1163-11 73.
Campbell, D. T. (1987). Guidelines for monitoring the scien tific competence of preventive,
intervention research centers: An exercise in the sociology of scien tific validity. Knowl-
edge, Creation, Diffusion, Utilization, 8, 389- 430.
Cicchetti, D., & Rizley, R. (1981). Developmental perspectives on the etiology, intergenera -
tional transmission and sequelae of child maltreatment. In R. Rizley & D. Cicchetti
(Eds.), Developmental perspectives on child maltreatment (pp . 31-55). San Francisco:
je ssey-Bass.
Cohn, A. H . (1979a). An evaluation of thr ee demonstration child abuse and negl ect treat-
ment programs. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 18, 283-291.
Cohn, A. H. (1979b). Essential components of successful child abu se and neglect treat-
ment. Child Abuse and Neglect, 3, 491-496.
Cohn, A. H., & Dare , D. (1987). Is treatment too late: What ten years of evaluation
research tells us . Child Abuse and Neglect, 11, 433-442 .
Con erly, S. (1986). Asse ssment of suspected child sexua l abuse . In K. Macfarlane, J.
Waterma n, S. Conerly, L. Dam on , M. Durfee, & S. Long (Eds.), Sexualabuse of young
children (pp. 30-51). New York: Guilford Press .
Cook , T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979). Quasi-experimentation: Design and analysis issues for
field settings. Boston : Houghton Mifflin.
de Lissovoy, V. (1979). Toward the definition of "abuse provoking child ." Child Abuse and
Neglect, 3, 341-350 .
Egeland, B., & Sroufe, A. (1981). Developmental sequelae of maltreatment in infancy. In R.
Rizley & D. Cicchetti (Eds .), Developmental perspectives on child maltreatment (pp. 77-92) .
San Francisco: [os se y-Bass,
106 ROY C. HERRENKOHL
Egeland, B., [acobvitz, D., & Papatola, K. (1987). Intergenerational continuity of abuse. In
R. J. Gelles & J. B. Lancaster (Eds.), Child abuse and neglect: Biosocial dimensions (pp . 255-
276). New York: A1dine DeGruyter.
Elmer, E. (1977). Fragile families, troubled children. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press .
Elmer, E. (1979). Child abuse and family stress. Journal of Social Issues, 35, 60-71.
Elmer, E. (1981). Traumatized children, chronic illness and poverty. In L. Pelton (Ed .), The
social context of child abuse and neglect (pp . 185-227). New York: Human Sciences Press.
Finkelhor, D., Hotaling, G. T., & Yllo, K. (1988). Stopping family violence: Research priorities
for the coming decade. Newbury Park, CA: Sage .
Garbarino, J. (1976). A preliminary study of some ecological correlates of child abuse: The
impact of socio-economical stress on mothers. Child Development, 47, 178-185 .
Garbarino, J. (1981). An ecological approach to child maltreatment. In L. H. Pelton (Ed .),
The social context of child abuse and neglect (pp . 228-267). New York: Human Sciences
Press.
Garbarino, J., Guttmann, E., & Seeley, J. W. (1986). The psychologically battered child. San
Francisco : Jossey-Bass.
Garmezy, N . (1983). Stressors of childhood. In N . Garmezy & M. Rutter (Eds.), Stress,
coping and development in children (pp . 43-84). New York: McGraw-Hili .
Geffner, R , Rosenbaum, A., & Hughes, H. (1988). Research issues concerning family
violence. In V. B. Van Hasselt, R. L. Morrison, A. S. Bellack, & M. Hersen (Eds .),
Handbook of family violence (pp . 457-481). New York: Plenum Press.
Gelles, R. J. (1973). Child abuse as psychopathology: A sociological critique and reformula-
tion . American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 43, 611-621.
Gil, D. G. (1970). Violence against children: Physical abuse in theUnited States. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Pres s.
Goldstein, J., Freud, A., & Solnit, A. (1973). Beyond the best interests of the child. New York:
Free Press.
Gold ston, S. E. (1977). Defining primary prevention. In G. W. Albee & J. M. Joffee (Eds .),
Primary prevention of psychopathology, Vol. I (pp . 18-23). Hanover, NH : University Press
of New England.
Gray, J., & Kempe, R. (1976). The abused child at time of injury. In H. Martin (Ed .), The
abused child (pp. 57-65) . Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.
Hart, S. N ., Germain, R B., & Brassard, M. R (1987). The challenge: To better understand
and combat psychological maltreatment of children and youth. In M. R Brassard, R
Germain, & S. N . Hart (Eds.), Psychological maltreatment ofchildren andyouth (pp . 3-24).
New York: Pergamon Press.
Herrenkohl, E. C.; & Herrenkohl, R. C. (1981). Some antecedents and developmental
consequences of child maltreatment. In R. Rizley & D. Cicchetti (Eds .), Developmental
perspectives on child maltreatment (pp . 57-76). San Francisco: jossey-Bass,
Herrenkohl, E. C,; Herrenkohl, R. C,; & Toedter, L. (1983). Perspectives on the in-
tergenerational transmission of abuse. In D. Finkelhor, R. J. Gelles, G. T. Hotaling, &
M. A. Straus (Eds .), The dark side of families (pp . 305-316). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage .
Herrenkohl, E. c.. Herrenkohl, R. c.. Toedter, L. , & Yanushefski, A. M. (1984). Parent-
child interactions in abusive and non-abusive families . Journal of theAmerican Academy
of Child Psychiatry, 23, 641-648 .
Herrenkohl, R. C,; & Herrenkohl, E. C. (1988a). Assessing the occurrence of physical and
emotional maltreatment. Unpublished manuscript, Lehigh University, Bethlehem, PA.
Herrenkohl, R. C.; & Herrenkohl, E. C. (1988b). Identifying theoccurrence of maltreatment in
comparison groups . Unpublished manuscript, Lehigh University, Bethlehem, PA.
Herrenkohl, R. c., Herrenkohl, E. c., Egolf, B., & Seech, M. (1979). The repetition of child
abuse: How frequently does it occur? Child Abuseand Neglect , 3, 67-72.
RESEARCH DIRECTIONS 107
Herrenkohl, R. c., Herrenkohl, E. C,; & Egolf, B. (1983). Circumstances surrounding the
occurrence of child maltreatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 424-
431.
Hershorn, M., & Rosenbaum, A. (1985). Children of marital violence . American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 55, 260-266.
Hotaling, G., & Sugarman, D. (1986). An analysis of risk markers in husband and wife
violence : The current state of knowledge. Violence and Victims, 1, 101-124.
Hyman, I. A. (1987). Psychological correlates of corporal punishment. In M. R. Brassard,
R. Germain, & S. N. Hart (Eds.), Psychological maltreatment of children and youth (pp. 59-
68). New York: Pergamon Press .
Jones, D. (1987). The untreatable family. Child Abuse and Neglect, 11, 409-420.
Justice, B., & Justice, R. (1976). The abusing family. New York: Human Sciences Press .
Kadushin, A., & Martin, J. (1981). Childabuse: An interactional event. New York: Columbia
University Press.
Kaufman, J., & Zigler, E. (1987). Do abused children become abusive parents? American
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57, 186-192.
Lane, T. W., & Davis, G. E. (1987). Child maltreatment and juvenile delinquency: Does a
relationship exist: In J. D. Burchard & S. N. Burchard (Eds.), Prevention of delinquent
behavior. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Lusk, R., & Waterman, J. (1986). Effects of sexual abuse on children. In K. MacFarlane, J.
Waterman, S. Conerly, L. Damon, M. Durfee, & S. Long (Eds.), Sexualabuse of young
children (pp. 101-118). New York: Guilford Press .
Lynch, M. A., & Roberts, J. (1982). The consequences of abuse. London: Academic Press.
Martin, H. P. (1973). Nutrition: Its relationship to children's physical, mental and emo-
tional development. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 26, 766-775.
Martin, H. P. (1976). The abused child. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.
McCleer, S. V. (1988). Psychoanalytic perspectives on family violence . In V. B. Van Hassett,
R. L. Morrison, A. S. Bellack, & M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of family violence (pp. 11-
30). New York: Plenum Press .
Milner, J. S., & Wimberley, R. C. (1979). An inventory for the identification of child abuse.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 35, 95-100.
Mrazek, P., & Mrazek, D. (1987). Resilience in child maltreatment victims : A conceptual
exploration. Child Abuse and Neglect, 11, 357-366.
Nagi, S. (1977). Child maltreatment in the United States. New York: Columbia University
Press .
Navarre, E. L. (1987). Psychological maltreatment: The core component of child abuse. In
M. R. Brassard , R. Germain, & S. N. Hart (Eds.), Psychological maltreatment of children
and youth (pp. 45-56). New York: Pergamon Press .
Oates, K. (1986). Childabuse and neglect, whathappens eventually? New York:Brunner/Maze!.
Parke, R. D., & Collmer, C. (1975). Child abuse: An interdisciplinary analysis. Chicago: Univer-
sity of Chicago Press.
Pelton, L. H . (1981). Child abuse and neglect : The myth of classlessness. In L. H. Pelton
(Ed.), Thesocial contextof childabuse and neglect (pp. 23-38). New York:Human Sciences
Press.
Polansky, N. A., Chalmers, M. A., Buttenweiser, E., & Williams, D. P. (1981). Damaged
parents: An anatomyof child neglect. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Rosenberg, M. S., & Reppucci, N. D. (1985). Primary prevention of child abuse. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 576-585.
Russell, D. (1983). The incidence and prevalence of intra familial and extrafamilial sexual
abuse of female children. Child Abuse and Neglect , 7, 133-146.
Schakel, J. A. (1987). Emotional neglect and stimulus deprivation. In M. R. Brassard, R.
108 ROY C. HERRENKOHL
Germain, & S. N . Hart (Eds .), Psychological maltreatment of children and youth (pp . 100-
109). New York: Pergamon Press.
Schneider, C. (1982). The Michigan Screening Profile of Parenting. In R. H. Starr (Ed .),
Child abuse prediction: Policy implications (pp. 157-174). Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.
Spinetta, J. J., & Rigler, D. (1972). The child abusing parent: A psychological review.
Psychological Bulletin, 77, 296-304.
Starr, R. H . (1982). Child abuse prediction: Policy implications. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.
Starr, R. H. (1988). Physical abuse of children. In V. B. Van Hasselt, R. L. Morrison, A. S.
Bellack, & M. Hersen (Eds .), Handbook of family violence (pp . 119-155). New York:
Plenum Pres s.
Steele, B. F. (1976). Violence within the family. In R. E. Helfer & C. H. Kempe (Eds .), Child
abuse and neglect: The family and the community (pp . 3-23). Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.
Sudia, C. E. (1981). What services do abusive and neglecting families need? In L. H. Pelton
(Ed.), The social context of child abuse and neglect (pp . 268-290). New York: Human
Sciences Press.
Straus, M., Gelles, R., & Steinmetz, S. (1980). Behind closed doors: Violence in the American
family. Garden City, NY: Anchor Press.
Tong, L., & Oates, K. (1987). Personality development following sexual abuse. ChildAbuse
and Neglect, 11, 371-383.
Toro, P. A. (1982). Developmental effects of child abuse : A review. ChildAbuse and Neglect,
6,423-431.
Twentyman, C. T., Rohrbeck, C. A., & Am ish, P. L. (1984). A cognitive-behavioral ap-
proach to child abuse: Implications for treatment. In S. Saunders, A. M. Anderson, C.
A. Hart, & G. M. Rubenstein (Eds.), Violent individuals and families (pp. 87-211).
Springfield: Charles C Thomas.
Wald, M. S. (1976). State in terven tion on behalf of "neglected" children: Standards for
removal of children from their homes, monitoring the status of children in foster care,
and termination of parental rights. Stanford Law Review, 28, 623-706.
Wald, M. S., Carlsmith, J. M., Leiderman, P. H ., French, R. de S., & Smith, C. (1985).
Protecting abused/neglected children: A comparison of homeand foster placement. Palo Alto ,
CA: Stanford Center for the Study of Youth Development.
Wolfe, D. A. (1987). Childabuse: Implications for child development and psychopathology. New-
bury Park, CA: Sage .
Wolfe, D. A., Zak , L., Wilson, S., & Jaffe, P. (1986). Child witnesses to violence between
parents: Critical issues in behavioral and social adjustment. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology, 14, 95-104.
Wolfe, D. A., Wolfe, V. V., & Best, C. L. (1988). Child victims of sexual abuse. In V. B. Van
Ha sselt, R. L. Morrison, A. S. Bellack, & M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of family violence
(pp. 157-185). New York: Plenum Press.
CHAPTER 5
INTRODUCTION
More than a quarter of a century ago, Kempe and his colleagues
(Kempe, Silverman, Steele, & Droegemueller, 1962) alerted th e medical
and academic communities to the "battered child syndrome ." Ever
since, research on, and concern for, child maltreatment has proliferated .
Although substantial concern has been directed toward the victims of
abuse and neglect, most research has focused upon the perpetrators (see
Belsky, 1978, 1980, and Parke & Collmer, 1975, for reviews). There are
compelling reasons why etiology rather than consequences of child mal-
treatment have been the principle focus of empirical inquiry, perhaps
the most obvious of which is priorities. The first task of those concerned
with child abuse and neglect is to stop it from occurring again or to
prevent it from happening in the first place . In order for either remedia-
tion or prevention efforts to succeed, understanding of etiology is
essential.
LI SE M. YOUNGBLAD E AND JAY BELSKY· Department of Ind ividual and Family Studies,
College of Health and Human Development, Penns ylvania State University, University
Park, Penn sylvania 16802.
109
110 LISE M. YOUNGBLADE and JAY BELSKY
Our third and final reason for reviewing in this chapter what is
known about the developmental effects of being abused or neglected as
a child is motivated in part by the results of an investigation reported
just a decade ago, indicating that the effects of child maltreatment ap-
pear to be indistinguishable from those of economic deprivation more
generally. In a study that challenged the field, Elmer (1977) compared
three groups of eight-year-olds, two of which has been identified as
victims of abuse or of accidents in their first year of life at time of
admission to Children's Hospital in Pittsburgh. Broad-based assess-
ments of 17 children from each group, plus an additional 17 control
subjects not previously studied, revealed few differences between
abused, accident, and control children-a finding that was true whether
one looked at physical health, language development, self-concept, in-
tellectual standing, school performance, or self-control. What was most
noteworthy was how widespread deficits were in all three samples of
children.
The results of the Elmer (1977) study called into question the mean-
ing of virtually all investigations up to that point in time, most of which
had been conducted by clinicians not blind to the child's rearing history
and without reliance upon appropriate comparison groups (see Aber &
Cicchetti, 1984, for review and critique). Thus, Elmer's findings raised
the very real possibility that the effects of child abuse might be indis-
tinguishable from that of rearing in an economically deprived family and
SOCIAL AND EMOTIONAL CONSEQUENCES 111
ature, we are forced, for the purposes of this review, to employ a broad
definition of the term child abuse to refer to acts of commission, re-
gardless of duration, intensity or frequency, and "child neglect" to refer
to acts of omission, regardless of duration, frequency or intensity.
Where operational definitions are more refined in the research liter-
ature, we will draw attention to distinctions made between different
types of abuse. However, virtually no study enables us to draw distinc-
tions between groups in terms of duration, frequency, or intensity.
Methodological Issues
In terms of research methodology, there are three issues that must
be considered. The first, already alluded to, revolves around the use of
comparison/control groups. Although virtually all the research to be
cited involves contrast groups of some kind, by no means are all groups
(e.g., Gaensbauer, 1982) matched as carefully as were Elmer's (1977).
Second, we must consider the developmental sensitivity of the mea-
sures used. As we will describe shortly, in infancy the measure used
most widely to assess socioemotional "health" involves the assessment
of infant-mother attachment security. Even though this assessment has
shown adequate reliability and validity properties for use with 12- and
18-month-old infants in "normative" samples (see Waters, 1978), in
some of the studies to be reported it has been used to assess maltreated
infants ranging from 11 to 24 months, and with minor (Lamb,
Gaensbauer, Malkin, & Schulz, 1985) and major (Lewis & Schaeffer,
1981) procedural changes. On the other hand, children of older ages are
typically observed in unstructured situations interacting with persons
other than their parents-often their peers. And third, although a
number of reports emanate from longitudinal research investigations of
child abuse in infancy and early childhood-most notably the Min-
nesota Mother-Child Project (Egeland & Sroufe, 1981a,b) and the Har-
vard Child Maltreatment Project (e.g., Cicchetti, Carlson, Braunwald, &
Aber, 1987; Cicchetti & Braunwald, 1984; Gersten, Coster, Schneider-
Rosen, Carlson, & Cicchetti, 1986; Schneider-Rosen, Braunwald, Car-
lson, & Cicchetti, 1985; Schneider-Rosen & Cicchetti, 1984)-the major-
ity of research, in infancy, preschool, and mid- to late childhood, in-
volves cross-sectional research designs (see Aber & Cicchetti, 1984, for
review). All these methodological inconsistencies limit the comparisons
that can be made and inferences that can be drawn from this body of
research.
SOCIAL AND EMOTIONAL CONSEQUENCES 113
Theoretical Issues
A good deal of research on the developmental effects of child mal-
treatment can be regarded as atheoretical, in that it is guided by little
more than the common-sense notion that maltreatment is aversive and
thereby bad for children, and thus that children who have been sub-
jected to it should function more poorly than age-mates reared in more
considerate ways. Even so, much of the research can be cast in terms of
one of two predominant schools of thought-attachment theory (Ains-
worth & Wittig, 1969; Bowlby, 1969, 1980; Sroufe, 1977) and social learn-
ing theory (Bandura, 1977; Bijou & Baer, 1961; Patterson, 1982). The role
that attachment theory plays in guiding empirical inquiry is most evi-
dent in the work done on the youngest children who are hypothesized
to establish mistrusting or insecure affective bonds with abusive and
neglecting parents (e.g., Egeland & Sroufe, 1981a,b). The contribution of
social learning theory is most evident in studies of preschoolers and
older children, particularly in the study of aggressive behavior (e.g .,
Reid, Taplin, & Loeber, 1981), because it is hypothesized that children
who have been physically abused will be aggressive because they are
imitating the behavior they have been subjected to, reinforced for,
and/or are reproducing behavior patterns that they have seen rewarded.
Social learning theory and attachment theory are in many ways
distinct, in that the former, by tradition, has focused principally upon
overt behavior and the role of imitation and rewards and punishment in
the generation and maintenance of behavior patterns, whereas the latter
has been concerned particularly with affective bonds that influence how
individuals view themselves, others, and relationships more generally.
With the emergence of a more cognitively oriented social learning ap-
proach to behavioral development (Bandura, 1977), it is clear that de-
spite different language systems for explaining human development
processes, the two theoretical approaches have much in common
(Youngblade, Burgess, & Belsky, 1988). Noteworthy perhaps is the as-
sumption that ways of relating to others result from interpersonal expe-
rience and that such experiences not only shape what one does but also
what one attends to in the social arena and how social experience is
interpreted. Thus, both see the individual as an active agent who, as a
result of a social experience, develops expectations that guide interper-
sonal activity and shape the processing of interpersonal experience. Al-
though it is true that attachment theorists place more emphasis upon
how social experience fosters "internal working models" or affective -
cognitive processes that affect social functioning than on social skills per
114 LISE M. YOUNGBLADE and JAY BELSKY
OVERVIEW
attachment theory with regard to child abuse and neglect, we will pro-
ceed to summarize the available evidence.
At the most global level, there is general consensus from an em-
pirical standpoint that maltreatment is associated with elevated rates of
insecure infant-mother attachments (Cicchetti & Braunwald, 1984; Crit-
tenden, 1985, 1988; Egeland & Sroufe, 1981a,b; Gordon & Jameson, 1979;
Lamb et al., 1985; Lyons-Ruth, Connell, Zoll, & Stahl, 1987; Schneider-
Rosen, Braunwald, Carlson, & Cicchetti, 1985; Schneider-Rosen & Cic-
chetti, 1984). The strength of this association is most evident when data
obtained from independent samples in separate studies are compiled
and subjected to statistical analysis at the aggregate level (see Table 1).
Although the data presented in Table 1 derive from investigations that
are cross-sectional and longitudinal in design, from studies using stan-
dardized or modified Strange Situations, and from research on children
of varying ages (see Table 2 for description of study characteristics), it is
clear that young children who have been maltreated are far more likely
to be classified as insecure in their attachments to their mothers than are
agemates from economically similar backgrounds who have not been
maltreated. Indeed, this is true even when the data are examined in
terms of distinct age groups (12 months, Table 1-2; 18 months, Table 1-3;
> 18 months, Table 1-4; and mixed ages, Table 1-5).
When it comes to addressing the issue of specificity, that is, whether
particular forms of maltreatment are differentially associated with differ-
ent patterns of attachment, some of the available evidence is consistent
with the proposition that infants subject to physical abuse should be
more at risk for developing insecure-avoidant attachments, whereas
those subject to neglect should be at heightened risk of developing
insecure-resistant attachments. Egeland and Sroufe (1981a,b), for exam-
ple, found that this prediction holds when studying 12-month-olds, and
Crittenden (1985, 1988), who studied young children of various ages,
reported abuse to be associated with avoidance and neglect with re-
sistance. Inconsistent with the specificity proposition, however, are
Egeland's and Sroufe's (1981b) data indicating that by 18 months of age
both abused and neglected children are most likely to be classified as
insecure-avoidant, a pattern also consistent with the cross -sectional
findings of Schneider-Rosen et al. (1985). Exactly why such developmen-
tal changes take place in the expression of insecurity in the Strange
Situation in the case of maltreated children remains unclear.
As it turns out, there is ever increasing evidence that both abused
and neglected children display elevated levels of resistance and of avoid -
ance (Crittenden, 1985, 1988; Lyons-Ruth et al., 1987), a finding which
has led to the emergence of a new attachment classification labeled Ale
(see also Carlson, Cicchetti, Barnett, & Braunwald, 1989). In fact, Crit-
SOCIAL AND EMOTIONAL CONSEQU ENCES 117
2. At 12 months"
Maltreatment Control Total
Insecure 85 57 142
(e = 61) (e = 81)
X2 [1) = 33.01, P < .0000
Secure 37 106 143
(e = 61) (e = 82)
3. At 18 months'
Maltreatment Control Total
Insecure 29 12 41
(e = 20) (e = 21)
x2 (1) = 22.56, P < .0000
Secure 14 32 46
(e = 23) (e = 23)
43 44 87
(continu ed)
118 LISE M. YOUNGBLADE and JAY BELSKY
"In some cases, the same subjects are repor ted at more than one time period , because of the fact that
they were mea sured at mult iple ages.
bWithin each cell, data are tabled such that the top value reflects observed/actual frequenc y and the
bottom value (in paren theses) is the expected value .
<Egeland & Sroufe (1981a,b); Lyon s-Ruth et al . (1987); Schn eider-Rosen et al . (1985); Schneider-Rosen &
Cicche tti (1984); Carlson, Braunwald , & Cicchetti (1984); Crittenden (1985, 1988); Lamb et al . (1985);
Gordo n & Jame son (1979).
dEgeland & Sroufe (1981a,b ); Lyons -Ru th et al. (1985); Schn eider-Rosen et al . (1985).
' Egeland & Srou fe (1981a,b); Schn eider-Rosen et al . (1985).
/Schneider-Rosen & Cicchetti (1984)-19 mos.: Schneider -Rosen et al. (1985)-24 mos. (Note: A sepa rate
sys tem to score 24 mos. olds was dev eloped and validated .)
s Carlson et al . (1984); Crittende n (1985, 1988); Gordon & Jameson (1977); Lamb et al . (1985).
tenden (1988) discovered that without the new classification many mal-
treated infants were classified, apparently falsely, as secure. Such find -
ings raise questions as to whether the data presented in Table 1 might be
even more revealing if all studies included this classification category. In
fact, the absence of such a category may explain why Lyons-Ruth et al.
(1987) failed to find an association between attachment classification and
maltreatment, even though they discerned elevated levels of resistance
and avoidance among infants who were maltreated.
The co-mingling of these two expressions of insecurity in the re-
union episodes of the Strange Situation is quite unusual in view of
recent find ings indicating that avoidance and resistance tend to charac-
terize two distinct ends of a behavioral continuum and may even have
their origins, to some extent, in temperamental characteristics of the
infant (Belsky & Rovine, 1987; Frodi & Thompson, 1985). In view of the
possibility that some infants may be inclined to express their insecurity
in one form or another (i.e., resistance or avoidance) because of some
temperamental or affective proclivity, the behavior of maltreated infants
suggests that they may be so distressed and disorganized by their rear-
ing experience that they actually run the gamut of affective expression in
the search for a pattern of relating that will prove more acceptable to the
parent. Although some infants might be predisposed to become avoid-
TABLE 2 . Study Characteristics
Carlson, Braunwald , 13-25 29 Abu sed /neglected 16 13-25 SES Standard Stran ge Cross-sectional
& Cicchetti (1984) Situation
Crittenden (1985) 2-24; 17 Abused 13 2- 24; SES Standard Strange Cross-sectional
i = 13.7 21 Neglected i = 13.7 Situation
22 Problematic
Crittenden (1985) 2-48; 22 Abused 29 2- 48; SES Standard Strange Cross-sectional
i = 24 31 Abused/neglected i = 24 Situ ation
20 Neglected
22 Marginally maltreated
Egeland & Sroufe 12, 18 19 Verbally abused 85 12, 18 SES Standard Strange Longitudinal
(1981a) 12, 18 19 Psychologically abu sed Situation
12, 18 24 Neglected
12, 18 24 Physically abu sed
Egeland & Sroufe 12, 18 33 Abu sed /neglected 33 12, 18 SES; but received Standard Strange Longitudinal
(1981b) "excellent" care Situation
Gaensbauer (1982) 12-19 12 Abused/n eglected ; low 20 12 Middle-class sample Noncla ssified Cross-sectional
SES 20 15
20 18
Gord on & Jameson 12-19 12 Non organic failure to 12 12-19 SES; ho sp ital expe- Mod ified Strange Cross-section al
(1979) thr ive rience Situ ation
(continued)
TABLE 2. (Continu ed)
Lamb et aI. (1985) 8-32; 32 Abu sed /neglected 32 8-32; SES Modified Strange Cros s-sectional
i = 18.4 i = 18.7 Situation
Lyon s-Ruth et aI. 12 10 Abused/neglected 28 12 SES Standard Strange Cross -sectional
(1987) 12 18 Non -maltreated high- Situation; and
risk Rating Scales
Schneider-Rosen & 19 18 Abused/neglected 19 19 SES Standard Strange Cross-sectional
Cicchetti (1984) Situa tion
Schneider-Rosen et 12 17 Abused/ne glected 18 12 SES Stan dard Strange Cros s-sectiona l
aI. (1985) 18 24 Abused/n eglected 24 18 SES Situation
24 25 Abused/ne glected 25 24 SES
Sub sample:
12, 18 10 Abused/n eglected 14 12,18 SES Standard Stran ge Longitudinal
12, 18 16 Abu sed /neglected 16 12, 18 SES Situation
SOCIAL AND EMOTIONAL CONSEQUENCES 121
TODDLER-PEER RELATIONS
Abused / Marginally
Classification Abu sed neglect ed Neglected maltreated Contr ol Total
A 43 62 43 18 74 240
(e = 27) (e = 46) (e = 32) (e = 16) (e = 119)
B 22 40 31 24 283 400
(e = 46) (e = 76) (e = 53) (e = 27) (e = 198)
C 12 34 30 7 45 128
(e = 15) (e = 24) (e = 17) (e = 9) (e = 63)
A/C 18 22 6 7 8 61
(e = 7) (e = 12) (e = 8) (e = 4) (e = 30)
Total 95 158 110 56 410 829
X2 (12) = 176.29, P < .0000
2. At 12 months"
Type of maltreatment
Abused / Marginally
Classification Abused neglected Neglected maltreated Control Total
A 16 17 5 4 32 74
(e = 7) (e = 11) (e = 9) (e = 5) (e = 42)
B 8 10 11 8 106 143
(e = 14) (e = 22) (e = 17) (e = 9) (e = 82)
C 4 12 17 1 19 53
(e = 5) (e = 8) (e = 6) (e = 3) (e = 30)
A/C 0 4 0 5 6 15
(e = 2) (e = 2) (e = 2) (e = 1) (e = 9)
3. At 18 months'
Type of maltreatment
Abused /
Classificati on Abused neglected Neglected Control Total
A 12 19 12 22 65
(e = 8) (e = 11) (e = 8) (e = 38)
B 12 12 15 102 141
(e = 16) (e = 24) (e = 18) (e = 83)
C 4 10 3 18 35
(e = 4) (e = 6) (e = 4) (e = 21)
Abused /
Classificati on ne glected Control Total
A 18 5 23
(e = 11) (e = 12)
B 14 32 46
(e = 23) (e = 23)
C 11 7 18
(e = 9) (e = 9)
Tot al 43 44 87
X2 [2] = 16.45, P < .0000
A 15 8 26 14 15 78
(e = 14) (e = 11) (e = 17) (e = 14) (e = 22)
(continued)
124 LISE M . YOUN GBLAD E and JAY BELSKY
TABLE 3. (Continued)
B 2 4 5 16 43 70
(e = 13) (e = 10) (e = 15) (e = 12) (e = 20)
C 4 1 10 6 1 22
(e = 4) (e = 3) (e = 5) (e = 4) (e = 6)
A /C 18 18 6 2 2 46
(e = 8) (e = 7) (e = 10) (e = 8) (e = 13)
Total 39 31 47 38 61 216
X2 [121 = 111. 94, p < . ססoo
«In some cases, the same subjects are reported at more than one time period, du e to the fact that they
were measured at multiple ages .
bWithin each cell, data are tabled such that the top value reflects observe d /actual frequ ency and the
bott om value (in parentheses) is the expected valu e.
<Crittenden (1985, 1988); Egeland & Sroufe (1981a, 1981b); Lamb et at. (1985); Lyon s-Ruth et at. (1987);
Schn eid er-Rosen & Cicchett i (1984); Schneid er-Rosen et at. (1985).
dEgelan d & Sroufe (1981b); Lyons-Ruth et at. (1987); Schn eider-Rosen et al. (1985).
' Egeland & Srou fe (1981b); Schn eider-Rosen et at. (1985).
/Schneider-Rosen & Cicchetti (1984)- 19 mos.; Schneide r-Rosen et at. (1985)-24 mos. (No te: A separate
sys tem was develop ed and validated to score 24 mos. olds .)
s Crittend en (1985; 1988); Lamb et at. (1985) .
Self-Concept
Central to attachment theory is the notion that the child learns more
than just social skills or ways of behaving as a function of the way he or
she is cared for. In fact, from the standpoint of attachment theory, behav-
ior itself is derivative of the child's internal working model, that is, the
child's self-image and views of relationships and the world. From what
we have seen, it can be anticipated that maltreated children should feel
less positively about themselves than do other children. Evidence from a
number of sources provides consistent support for this contention in
showing that maltreated children evince deficits in self-esteem (Kauf-
man & Cicchetti, 1989; Oates, Forrest, & Peacock, 1985), self-adjustment
(Perry et al., 1983; Straker & Jacobson, 1981), and emotional development
more generally (Kinard, 1980).
In an investigation of 37 6- to 14-year-olds admitted to a hospital
with a diagnosis of abuse and 37 matched controls, Oates, Forrest, and
Peacock (1985) found that the maltreated children scored significantly
lower on a measure of self-concept and, in addition, were less ambitious
than nonabused children with respect to occupational goals. Also, they
viewed themselves as having fewer friends . Even though Kinard's
SOCIAL AND EMOTIONAL CONSEQUENCES 129
Peer Relations
The research conducted during the preschool and early childhood
years, like that already summarized during the toddler years, under-
scores the apparent effect of maltreatment on relationships with age
mates. In one of the earliest relevant studies, Reidy (1977) compared 20
physically abused, 16 neglected, and 22 matched control children in a
multimethod assessment of aggressive child characteristics. In play,
abused children showed significantly more aggressive behavior than
neglected or control children, who rarely exhibited aggressive behavior.
Teachers' ratings characterized both abused and neglected children as
more aggressive than controls, though not significantly different from
each other. And, finally, abused children displayed significantly more
fantasy aggression on a projective test than either the neglected or con-
trol children. It must be noted that Straker and Jacobson (1981) could not
replicate the association between maltreatment and fantasy aggression
in their study of 19 abused and 19 control children between 5 and 10
years of age in South Africa.
132 LISE M. YOUNGBLADE and JAY BELSKY
toddlers respond to peers who become distressed, along with the results
of studies across childhood that now consistently link maltreatment and
aggression, lead to the hypothesis that maltreated children do not sim-
ply have difficulty with self-control but, perhaps, are limited in a variety
of affective and perceptual arenas that are likely to influence their peer
interactions. Evidence that this is indeed the case comes from several
investigations highlighting deficits in their understanding of the view-
points of others and in their sensitivity to the affect displayed by others.
Consider in this regard Straker and Jacobson's (1981) finding that abused
5- to lO-year-olds were less empathic than matched controls, Frodi and
Smetana's (1984) discovery that abused preschoolers were less able to
identify and discriminate other people's emotions from picture stories
(though group differences disappeared with IQ controlled), and Baharal
et aI.,'s (1981) report that (even with IQ controlled) abused children were
less able than carefully matched controls to label feelings accurately, to
cognitively decenter, and to understand complex social roles.
Summary
During the preschool and school-age period there is evidence, just
as there was in the case of infants and toddlers, that child abuse in
particular is rather consistently, though not universally, associated with
problems in parent-child and peer relations as well as in attitudes and
feelings about the self. As we have seen, the dysfunctions evident
among maltreated children in all too many studies highlight both exter-
nalizing and internalizing behavior disorders. And as we have noted,
there can be little doubt that although such patterns of behaving are a
result of the quality of care the children have received, they also serve to
maintain problematic social experiences with parents, teachers/ coun-
selors, and agemates. From the perspective of social-learning theory it
would seem that the children have learned maladaptive wa ys of behav-
ing that function to elicit responses that maintain their problematic be-
havioral proclivities. From the standpoint of attachment theory, we can
speak of behavior patterns that serve to evoke from others responses
that confirm internal working models of the self as bad and unworthy of
love. To be sure, we need to acknowledge again that these interpretive
frames are by no means mutually exclusive and indeed are rather com-
plimentary. Even though one stresses social skills and behavior and the
other affective-cognitive processes, both see the maltreated child as a
product of his experience who actively contributes and apparently un-
dermines his continuing behavioral and psychological development.
134 LISE M. YOUNGBLADE and JAY BELSKY
CONSEQUENCES IN ADOLESCENCE
INTERGENERATIONAL TRANSMISSION
tional criteria for "history of abuse" and "current abuse," and rely upon
observers who were not blind to the subjects' maltreatment status (Kauf-
man & Zigler, 1987). Importantly, because these investigations typically
do not employ parents who were maltreated but are now providing
adequate care to their own children, they tend to overestimate the inci-
dence of intergenerational transmission (Kaufman & Zigler, 1987).
Nevertheless, it is also true that more recent, better-designed, pro-
spective investigations provide support for the link between a history of
maltreatment and subsequent maltreatment of one's own children. For
example, in an investigation of 282 economically at-risk parents of new-
borns admitted to an intensive care nursery, 49 parents reported a histo-
ry of abuse and/or neglect at the initial interview. One year later, 10 of
these babies were confirmed as being abused of neglected; nine of the
abusing parents had a history of childhood maltreatment (Hunter et al.,
1978). In an unrelated study, Egeland et al. (1987) solicited information
from nearly 200 impoverished, predominantly single-parent mothers
regarding their childhood histories and current disciplinary practices.
Using a broad definition of abuse (definite plus borderline or suspected
cases), Egeland et al. reported a 70% rate of intergenerational transmis-
sion. Using a more conservative estimate (i.e. , reported cases only), they
found that 34% of the parents who had been abused but only 3% of the
parents who had been emotionally supported were mistreating their
children. These data are consistent with results from investigations
using less extreme samples (see Belsky & Pensky, 1988, for review). For
example, in their follow-up of English girls who had been institutionally
reared as children, Rutter and his colleagues found that, in comparison
to family-reared girls from the same neighborhood, the ex-care girls
were much more likely to show insensitivity to their 2- to 4-year-old
children, were more prone to exhibit irritability and use frequent spank-
ing and, as a consequence, were far more likely to be categorized as poor
parents (Dowdney, Skuse, Rutter, Quinton, & Mrazek, 1985; Quinton &
Rutter, 1985; Quinton, Rutter, & Liddle, 1984; Rutter & Quinton, 1984).
On the other hand, in each of the studies just reported, there also is
evidence that a significant number of parents, at least during the time of
the particular investigation, broke the intergenerational cycle. For exam-
ple, in the Hunter et al. (1978) investigation, 40 out of 49 parents, at least
during infancy, did not mistreat their infants despite their own histories
of maltreatment. How might this have happened? Interestingly, these
nonabusing parents, as well as the nonrepeaters in the Egeland et al.
(1987) sample, reported having more extensive social supports, and
were less likely to have been abused by both their parents as children
(see also Knutsen, Mehm, & Burger, 1984), were more apt to report a
SOCIAL AND EMafIONAL CONSEQUENCES 137
supportive relationship with one parent while growing up/ and were
more openly angry and better able to give a detailed coherent account of
their earlier abuse. Additionally, Egeland et al. (1987) found that involve-
ment with a supportive spouse or boyfriend, fewer current stressful life
events, and a conscious resolve not to repeat a history of abuse charac-
terized the nonabusing mothers.
Again, these buffering effects are consistent with results from stud-
ies using less extreme samples. Rutter and his colleagues, for example,
found that ex-care women who spoke warmly of their spouse and/or
indicated confiding in him were far more likely to be rated as good
parents and far less likely to be rated as poor parents, leading to the
conclusion that "the spouse's good qualities exerted a powerful ame-
liorating effect" on the parental functioning of women known to be at
risk as a result of their developmental history (Quinton, Rutter, & Lid-
dle/ 1984/ p. 115). Similarly, in a study of teenage mothers, Crockenberg
(1987) discovered that those who reported a history of parental rejection
but experienced good partner support were significantly less likely to be
angry and punitive toward their toddlers than those who received com-
parable care as a child but received limited partner assistance.
In summary, it appears that even though limitations of the data base
are widely acknowledged, most reviewers agree that a history of mal-
treatment in one's own childhood places the person at increased risk of
mistreating his or her own offspring (Belsky, 1978; Belsky & Pensky, 1988;
Burgess & Youngblade, 1988; Parke & ColImer, 1975). Importantly, how-
ever/ intergenerational transmission is not inevitable, even if potentially
likely. In a recent review, in fact, Kaufman and Zigler (1987) estimated the
transmission rate to be around 30%/ a figure that underscores the asser-
tion that the focus for researchers and practitioners should not simply be
whether transmission across generations occurs but the conditions under
which one might expect continuity (Burgess & Youngblade, 1988) or
"lawful discontinuity" (Belsky & Pensky, 1988). Although few studies
chronicle the factors that enable individuals to escape the intergenera-
tional cycle (but see Egeland, Jacobvitz, & Sroufe, 1988; Hunter & Kils-
trom, 1979)/ it is generally acknowledged that it is principally in interac-
tion with other etiological factors (e.g., child temperament, marital
quality, social support) that the risk associated with child-rearing history
is or is not "realized" (Belsky & Pensky, 1988; Cicchetti & Rizley, 1981).
Thus, we might conceptualize the risk of perpetuating the abused-abus-
ing cycle as being akin to the latent vulnerability of a brittle bone. In and of
itself, the property of being brittle will not cause the bone to break, but to
the extent pressure is put on the bone, the prospect of breakage increases.
Likewise, to the extent "pressure" (e.g ., no social or spousal support, low
138 LISE M. YOUNGBLADE and JAY BELSKY
income) is put on the parent who experienced abuse as a child, might the
parent succumb to maltreating his or her own child .
SUMMARY
We began this chapter by considering Elmer's (1977) findings indi-
cating that the developmental functioning of impoverished children
who were maltreated was indistinguishable from that of non-maltreated
children from equally impoverished households. Such results clearly
brought into question the assumed negative impact of child abuse and
neglect on child development. Our review of the evidence that has
become available since the publication of the Elmer study, framed as it
was in terms of attachment and social-learning theory, leads us to con-
clude that there are indeed serious socioemotional consequences of
being maltreated in childhood above and beyond those that emanate
from growing up in an economically disadvantaged household. Al-
though clearly not inevitable, we consistently found a history of abuse
and/or neglect to be linked to negative consequences.
More specifically, three relatively coherent and interconnected pat-
terns of socioemotional effects emerged. First, from infancy through
adolescence, we found maltreatment to be accompanied by dysfunc-
tional parent-child relations, marked by the increased likelihood of
forming an insecure attachment in infancy, coercive interpersonal ex-
changes in the childhood years, and chaotic, punishing and enmeshed
family life during adolescence. Second, the effects of child maltreatment
were not limited to familial relations, as there was repeated indication
that maltreatment is associated with dysfunctional peer relations. Sever-
al studies reveal maltreated children to be more aggressive, less pro-
social, and more disturbed in interaction with age mates than are com-
parison children. Particularly noteworthy was the discovery that in
response to displays of distress-as well as prosocial overtures-mal-
treated toddlers were more likely to be aggressive. Moreover, they were
more likely to avoid interpersonal contacts with familiar persons who
have not mistreated them (e.g., preschool teachers) . Third, data from
multiple investigations also indicated that abused and neglected chil-
dren tend to have lower self-esteem and to display significantly more
internalizing and externalizing behavior problems than non-maltreated
children. Most importantly, although such patterns of behaving are al-
most certainly a result of the quality of care the children have received,
they also serve to maintain problematic social experiences with parents,
teachers, counselors, and agemates (see also Johnson & Morse, 1968).
SOCIAL AND EMOTIONAL CONSEQUENCES 139
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
REFERENCES
Aber, J. L. (1984, August). Environmental influences on the socialemotional and cognitive devel-
opment of maltreated children. Paper presented at the 92nd Annual Meeting of the
American Psychological Association, Toronto.
Aber, J. L., & Allen , J. P. (1987). Effects of maltreatment on young children's socioerno-
tional development: An attachment theory perspective. Developmental Psychology, 23,
406-414.
Aber, J. L., & Cicchetti, D. (1984). The socio-emotional development of maltreated chil-
dren: An empirical and theoretical analysis. In H . Fitzgerald, B. Lester, & M. Yogman
(Eds .), Theory and research in behavioral pediatrics (Vol. 2, pp. 147-205). New York:
Plenum Press.
Ain sworth, M. D. 5., Blehar, M. C. ; Waters , E., & WalI, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Ain sworth, M. D. 5., & Wittig, B. A. (1969). Attachment and exploratory behavior of one-
year-olds in a strange situation . In B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinantsof infant behavior (Vol.
4, pp. 113-136) . London: Methuen.
Aragona, J. A., & Eyberg, S. M. (1981). Neglected children: Mother's report of child
behavior problems and observed verbal behavior. Child Development, 52, 596-602.
Arend, R, Gove, F., & Sroufe, L. A. (1979). Continuity of individual adaption from infancy
to kindergarten: A predictive study of ego resiliency and curiosity in preschoolers.
Child Development, 50, 950-959.
Beharal, R , Waterman, J., & Martin, H . (1981). The social-cognitive development of
abused children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 508-516.
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice HalI.
Belsky, J. (1978). Three theoretical models of child abuse: A critical review. ChildAbuse and
Neglect, 2, 27-49.
Belsky, J. (1980). Child maltreatment: An ecological in tegration . American Psychologist, 35,
320-335.
Belsky, J., & Nezworski, T. (Eds .). (1988). Clinical implications of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Belsky, J., & Pensky, E. (1988). Developmental history, personality and family rela-
tionships: Toward an emergent family system. In R Hinde & J. Stevenson-Hinde
(Eds .), The interrelation offamily relationships (pp. 193-217). London: Cambridge Univer-
sity Press.
Belsky, J., & Rovine , M. (1987). Temperament and attachment security in the Strange
Situation: An empirical rapproachment. Child Development, 58, 787-795.
Belsky, J., Rovine, M., & Taylor, D. (1984). The Pennsylvania infant and family develop-
ment project III: The origins of individual differences in infant-mother attachment:
Maternal and infant contributions. Child Development, 55, 718-728.
Bijou, S. w., & Baer, D. M. (1961). Childdeuelopmeni, Vol. 1: A systematic and empirical theory.
New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
SOCIAL AND EMOTIONAL CONSEQUENCES 141
Crockenberg, S. (1987). Predictors and correlates of anger toward and punitive control of
toddlers by adolescent mothers. Child Development, 58, 964-975.
Curtis, G. (1963). Violence breeds violence-perhaps. American Journal of Psychiatry, 120,
386-387.
Disbrow, M. A., & Doeer, H., & Caulfield, C. (1977). Measuring the components of par-
ents' potential for child abuse and neglect. Child Abuse and Neglect, 1, 279-296.
Dowdney, L., Skuse, D., Rutter, M., Quinton, D., & Mrazek, D. (1985). The nature and
qualities of parenting provided by women raised in institutions. Journal of Child Psy-
chology and Psychiatry, 26, 599-625.
Dumas, J. E., & Wahler, R. G. (1985). Indiscriminate mothering as a contextual factor in
aggressive-oppositional child behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 13, 1-18.
Egeland, B., [acobvitz, D., & Papatola, K. (1987). Intergenerational continuity of abuse. In
R. Gelles & J. Lancaster (Eds .), Child abuse and neglect: Biosocial dimensions (pp. 255-
276). New York: Aldine Gruyter.
Egeland, B., [acobvitz, D., & Sroufe, L. A. (1988). Breaking the cycle of abuse. Child
Deoelomeni, 59, 1080-1088 .
Egeland, B., & Sroufe, L. A. (1981a). Developmental sequelae of maltreatment in infancy.
New Directions for Child Development, 11, 77-92.
Egeland, B., & Sroufe, L. A. (1981b). Attachment and early maltreatment. Child Develop-
ment, 52, 44-52.
Elmer, E. (1977). A follow-up study of traumatized children. Pediatrics, 59, 273-279.
Fagot, B. I., Hagan, R., Youngblade, L. M., & Potter, L. (1989). A comparison of the play
behaviors of sexually abused, physically abused, and normal preschool children. Top-
ics in Early Childhood Special Education, 9(2), 88-100.
Frod i, A., & Smetana, J. (1984). Abused, neglected, and nonmaltreated preschoolers' abil-
ity to discriminate emotions in others: The effects of IQ . Child Abuse and Neglect, 8,
459-465.
Frodi, A., & Thompson, R. (1985). Infants' affective responses in the Strange Situation:
Effects of prematurity and of quality of attachment. ChildDevelopment, 56, 1280-1291.
Gaensbauer, T. J. (1982). Regulation of emotional expression in infants from two contrast-
ing caretaking environments. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 21,
163-171.
Galdston, R. (1965). Observations on children who have been physically abused and their
parents. American Journal of Psychiatry, 122, 440-443.
Garbarino, J., & Gilliam, G. (1980). Understanding abusive families . Lexington, MA: Heath
Publishing. .
Garbarino, J., Sebes , J., & Schellenbach, C. (1984). Families at risk for destructive parent-
child relations in adolescence. Child Development, 55, 174-183.
Garbarino, J., Schellenbach, c., & Sebes, J. (1986). Troubled youth, troubled families . New
York: Aldine.
George, c., & Main, M. (1979). Social interactions of young abused children: Approach,
avoidance, and aggression. Child Development, 50, 306-318 .
Gersten, M., Coster, W., Schneider-Rosen, K., Carlson, V., & Cicchetti, D. (1986). A socio-
emotional basis of communicative functioning: Quality of attachment, language de-
velopment, and early maltreatment. In M. E. Lamb, A. L. Brown, & B. Rogoff (Eds .),
Advances in developmental psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 105-151). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Gibbens, T. E. N., & Walker, A. (1956). Violent cruelty to children. British Journal of Delin-
quency, 6, 260-277.
Giovannoni, J., & Becerra, R. (1979). Defining childabuse. New York: Free Press.
SOCIAL AND EMOTIONAL CONSEQUENCES 143
Gordon, A. H., & Jameson, J. C. (1979). Infant-mother attachment in patients with non-
organic failure to thrive syndrome. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry,
18, 251-259 .
Green, A. H. (1978). Psychopathology of abu sed children. Journal of the American Academy of
Child Psychiatry, 17, 92-103 .
Helfer, R E. (1980). Developmental deficits which limit interpersonal skills. In C. H.
Kempe & R E. Helfer (Eds.), Thebattered child (3rd ed ., pp. 36-48). Chicago: University
of Chicago Press.
Herrenkohl, E. C,; & Herrenkohl, R C. (1981). Some antecedents and developmental
consequences of child maltreatment. New Directions for Child Development, 11, 57-76.
Hoffman-Plotkin, D., & Twentyman, C. T. (1984). A multi-modal assessment of behavioral
and cognitive deficits in abu sed and neglected preschoolers. Child Development, 55,
794-802 .
Howes, c., & Eldredge, R (1985). Respon ses of abused, neglected and non-maltreated
children to the behaviors of their peers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 6,
261-270.
Hunter, R., & Kilstrorn, N. (1979). Breaking th e cycle in abusive families . American Journal
of Psychiatry, 136, 1320-1322.
Isabella, R, Belsky, J., & Von Eye, A. (1989). Origin of infant-mother attachment: Examina-
tion of interaction synchrony during the infant's first year . Developmental Psychology,
25(1), 12-21.
Jacobson, R. S., & Straker, G. (1982). Peer group interaction of physically abused children.
Child Abuse and Neglect, 6, 261-270 .
Johnson, B., & Morse , H. A. (1968). Injured children and their parents. Children, 15, 147-
152.
Kaufman, J., & Cicchetti , D. (1989). The effects of maltreatment on school-aged children's
socioemotional development: Assessments in a day-camp setting. Developmental Psy-
chology, 25(4), 516-524 .
Kaufman, J., & Zigler, E. (1987). Do abused children become abusive parents? American
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57, 186-197 .
Kempe , C.; Silverman, F., Steele, 5., & Droegemueller, W. (1962). The battered child
synd rome. Journal of the American Medical Association, 181, 17-24.
Kinard , E. M. (1980). Emotional development in physically abused children. American
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 50, 686-696.
Knutsen, J. F., Mehm, J. G., & Burger, A. M. (1984). Is violence in the family passed from
generation to generation? Unpublished manuscript, University of Iowa , Iowa City.
Lamb, M. E., Gaensbauer, T. J., Malkin, C. M., & Schulz , L. (1985). The effects of child
abuse and neglect on security of infant-adult attachment. Infant Behavior and Develop-
ment, 8, 35-45.
Lamb, M. E., Thompson, R A., Gardner, W. P., Charnov, E. L., & Estes , D. (1984).
Security of infantile attachment as assessed in the Strange Situation: Its study and
biological interpretation. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 127-171.
Lewis, M., & Schaeffer, S. (1981). Peer behavior and mother-infant interaction in maltreat-
ed children. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), Theuncommon child. (pp. 193-223).
New York: Plenum Press .
Lieberman, A. (1977). Preschoolers' competence with a peer: Relationships with attach-
ment and peer experience. Child Development, 48, 1277-1287.
Loeber, R., & Dishion, T. J. (1984). Boys who fight at home and school : Family conditions
influencing cross-setting consistency. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52,
759-768.
144 LISE M. YOUNGBLADE and JAY BELSKY
Lyons-Ruth, K, Connell, D. B., Zoll, D., & Stahl, J. (1987). Infants at social risk: Relations
among infant maltreatment, maternal behavior, and infant attachment behavior. De-
velopmental Psychology, 23, 223-232.
Main, M., & George, C. (1985). Responses of abused and disadvantaged toddlers to dis-
tress in age-mates: A study in the day-care setting. Developmental Psychology, 21, 407-
412.
Mash, E. J., Johnston, C,; & Kovitz, K (1983). A comparison of the mother-child interac-
tions of physically abused and non-abused children during play and task situations.
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 12, 337-341.
Oates, R. K , Forrest, D., & Peacock, A. (1985). Self-esteem of abused children. ChildAbuse
and Neglect, 9, 159-163.
Oldershaw, 1., Waters, G., & Hall, D. K (1986). Control strategies and noncompliance in
abusive mother-child dyads: An observational study. Child Development, 57, 722-732.
Olweus, D. (1980). Familial and temperamental determinants of aggressive behavior in
adolescent boys : A causal analysis. Developmental Psychology, 16, 644-660.
Parke, R., & Collmer, C. (1975). Child abuse: An interdisciplinary review. In E. M.
Hetherington (Ed.), Reviewof childdevelopment research (Vo!. 5, pp . 509-590). Chicago :
University of Chicago Press .
Patterson, G. R. (1976). The aggressive child : Victim and architect of a coercive system. In
E. Mash, 1. Hamerlynck, & 1. Handy (Eds.), Behavior modification andfamilies (pp. 267-
316). New York: Brunner/Maze!.
Patterson, G. R. (1979). A performance theory for coercive family interaction. In R. B.
Cairns (Ed.), Theanalysis of social interactions: Methods, issues, and illustrations (pp. 117-
162). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum .
Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercive family process. Eugene, OR: Castalia .
Patterson, G. R. (1986). The contribution of siblings to training for fighting: A microsocial
analysis. In D. Olweus, J. Block, & M. Radke-Yarrow (Eds.), Development of antisocial
and prosocial behavior: Research, theories, and issues (pp. 235-261). Orlando, FL: Academic
Press.
Patterson, G. R., Dishion, T. J., & Bank, 1. (1984). Family interaction: A process model of
deviancy training. Aggressive Behavior, 10, 253-267.
Patterson, G. R., & Reid, J. B. (1973). Intervention for families of aggressive boys : A
replication study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 11, 383-394.
Patterson, G. R., & Reid, J. B. (1984). Social interactional processes within the family: The
study of moment-by-moment family transaction in which human social development
is embedded. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 5, 237-262.
Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. B., & Dishion, T. J. (in press) . Antisocial boys. Eugene, OR:
Castalia.
Pelton, 1. (1978). Child abuse and neglect: The myth of classlessness. American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 48, 608-617.
Perry, M. A., Doran, 1. D., & Wells, E. A. (1983). Developmental and behavioral charac-
teristics of the physically abused child . Journal of Clinical and ChildPsychology, 12, 320-
324 .
Quinton, D., & Rutter, M. (1985). Parenting behaviors of mothers raised "in care." In A.
Nichol (Ed.), Longitudinal studies in child psychology and psychiatry (pp. 157-210). Lon-
don: Wiley.
Quinton, D., Rutter, M., & Liddle, C. (1984). Institutional rearing, parenting difficulties,
and marital support. Psychological Medicine, 14, 107-124.
Reid, J. B. (1984). Socio-interactional patterns in families of abused and nonabused chil-
SOCIAL AND EMOfIONAL CONSEQUENCES 145
sented at the 18th Annual Convention of the Association for Advancement of Behav-
ior Therapy, Philadelphia.
Waters, E. (1978). The stability of individual differences in infant-mother attachment. Child
Development, 49, 483-494.
Wolfe, D. A., & Mosk , M. D. (1983). Behavioral comparisons of children from abusive and
distressed families . Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 702-708.
Youngblade, L. M., Burgess, R. L., & Belsky, J. (1988, May). Are social skillsenough? A multi-
level view of the ecology of peer rejection. Paper presented at the 5th Biennial Waterloo
Conference on Child Development, Waterloo, Ontario.
PART III
SOCIOLOGICAL AND
ECOLOGICAL FACTORS
JOAN 1. VONDRA
INTRODUCTION
Understanding how child maltreatment evolves-whether it is charac-
terized by violent confrontations between parent and child, inattention
to a child's needs for adequate nutrition or supervision, chronic emo-
tional belittlement and/or withdrawal of affection, or some combination
of these-is inherently a task of integration. Knowledge from develop-
mental psychology about what children need for healthy psychological
development, knowledge from clinical psychology about the origins and
manifestations of child and/or adult psychopathology, knowledge from
family disciplines about the dynamics that underlie day-to-day family
functioning and crisis situations, and knowledge from sociology about
social and economic forces that foster or undermine the well-being of the
family, each make a vital contribution to our understanding of both
normal and dysfunctional parenting. Thus, the study of parental care
and its effects on child development is, or should be, by its very nature
149
150 JOAN 1. VONDRA
A MODEL OF INFLUENCES
CHILDREARING HISTORY
CHILD CHARACTERISTICS
The role that the child plays in eliciting patterns of parental care is
accorded special attention in the case of child maltreatment. Neverthe-
less, a growing body of research exists that demonstrates relations be-
tween child factors and parental care within the general population as
well . Factors during infancy, such as gender, prematurity, congenital
handicaps, and temperament, have all been cited as correlates of the
quality of care received. With age, differences in care are increasingly
related to the cognitive status and to observable behaviors of the child .
However, the bidirectional nature of these relations is apparent even at
birth. Parents shape their children's behavior and development which,
in turn, influences subsequent caregiving, and so on .
One finding within the child maltreatment literature is that mal-
treated infants tend to be ill more often (Sherrod, O'Connor, Vietze, &
Altemeier, 1984) and that their mothers experience somewhat more
pregnancy and birth complications (Egeland & Brunnquell, 1979).This is
tempered by the fact, however, that abusive mothers have been found to
express more negative feelings about their pregnancy, to be delivering
more unplanned and unwanted infants, and to have made fewer prepa-
rations for the infant in terms of prenatal and infant care than low-
income comparison mothers (Altemeier et al., 1982, 1984; Egeland &
Brunnquell, 1979). The extent to which these latter findings also reflect
unstable and/or conflicted relations with the father of the child has not
been examined.
Similarly, although neglected infants have been observed to with-
draw socially and affectively in response to their social environment
(Fraiberg, 1980; Gaensbauer & Sands, 1979; Provence & Lipton, 1962),
maltreating mothers have been found to respond adversely to vid-
eotapes of smiling or crying infants, and to exhibit greater hostility and
intrusiveness, and less responsivity, in their interactions with their in-
fants during the first year of life (Crittenden, 1981; Dietrich, Starr, &
Kaplan, 1980; Frodi & Lamb, 1980; Lyons-Ruth, Connell, Zoll, & Stahl,
1987). Using prospective observational data from six infants later identi-
fied as victims of extreme physical punishment, Engfer and Gavranidou
SOCIOLOGICAL AND ECOLOGICAL FACTORS 159
(1988) were able to document that, as newborns, these infants did not
differ in irritability or social responsiveness from demographically
matched controls. As early as 8 months, however, they found these
youngsters to be significantly unhappier and more negative in mood
and, by 33 months, less cooperative and compliant than comparison
children. Importantly, the mothers of these children appeared less sen-
sitive in their interactions even on the maternity ward, and later ap-
peared more negative, angry, and coercive with their children. Further-
more, they described their youngsters as more difficult at every age of
assessment, ratings found to correlate more with characteristics of the
mothers themselves than with observed infant behavior. There is cer-
tainly reason to suspect that the majority of behavioral differences ob-
served in maltreated children have their foundation in the poor care
and/or disturbed relationships the children share with their caregivers.
The same may be noted for older children. Maltreating parents
report that their school-aged children exhibit a clinical rate of behavior
problems, and investigators observe that maltreated preschoolers en-
gage in less "positive self-directed activity" in the home, exhibit low
self-esteem, ego-control, persistence, and compliance in the laboratory,
more behavior problems, dependency, and psychopathology in pre-
school, and perform more poorly on intelligence tests than low-income
comparisons (Aragona & Eyberg, 1981; Barahal, Waterman, & Martin,
1981; DiLalla & Crittenden, 1987; Egeland, Sroufe, & Erickson, 1983;
Estroff et al., 1984; Green, 1978; Sandgrund, Gaines, & Green, 1974).
This is countered by findings that abusive parents are less positive and
more negative and hostile, and that neglecting parents are less positive,
more critical, and more controlling in social interactions with their chil-
dren (Aragona & Eyberg, 1981; Burgess, Anderson, Schellenbach, &
Conger, 1981; DiLalla & Crittenden, 1987; Mash, Johnston, & Kovitz,
1983). In addition, maltreating parents provide, in general, a poorer
quality home environment as a context for development than do low-
income controls (Rosario et al., 1987; Trickett & Susman, 1988).
Obviously, there is a problem with untangling cause and effect in
these observations. Starr, Deitrich, Fischhoff, Ceresnie, and Zweier
(1984) discuss this same issue in reference to the finding that maltreated
children are more likely to be physically or mentally handicapped. Char-
acteristics that make maltreated infants and children less appealing and
more difficult to care for very likely evolve, at least in part, from the
quality of care received . . . even in the prenatal period. By the time
they reach preschool and school age, maltreated youngsters are actively
contributing to a destructive cycle of aversive interactions (Burgess &
Conger, 1978; Trickett & Kuczynski, 1986). Trying to assign responsibil-
160 JOAN 1. VONDRA
SOCIOECONOMIC CONSIDERAnONS
SUMMARY
Given the multiple and interacting factors that contribute to inade-
quate and abusive parental practices, it is apparent that anyone parent-
ing outcome may be the result of very different patterns of resources and
stressors. A single, adolescent, black mother raising two children in her
mother's small, inner-city apartment-already shared by married and
unmarried siblings-may display some of the same forms of maltreat-
ment as the wife of an abusive husband, enlisted in the military, who
moves his wife and four children from base to base in rapid succession.
In each case, different ecological factors are likely to be contributing to
family dynamics. In both cases, however, the balance of supportive
resources to undermining stressors is insufficient to sustain adequate
parental care.
Crockenberg (1987) found that it was only when low-SES mothers
felt rejected by their own mothers in childhood and reported a lack of
support from their partners that they acted in an angry, punitive way
toward their toddlers. Carroll (1977) reported that it was the combination
of low family warmth and high parental punishment during childhood
that predicted family violence in adult clinic patients. Egeland, Breiten-
bucher, and Rosenberg (1980) noted that high life event stress predicted
child maltreatment only when it was experienced by mothers who exhib-
ited a cluster of personality deficits indicating poor coping skills . And
Sameroff and Chandler (1975) have demonstrated that prematurity only
predicts developmental delay and/or disability when child-rearing oc-
curs in the context of family social and economic impoverishment.
Belsky (1984; Belsky & Vondra, 1989) conceptualized this balance in
terms of "stresses and supports," describing parenting as a "buffered"
system. Cicchetti and Rizley (1981) referred to "potentiating and com-
pensatory factors" of short- and long-term duration that jointly contrib-
ute to parenting outcomes. Both argue that maltreatment is not the
164 JOAN 1. VONDRA
REFERENCES
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C,; Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum .
Altemeier, W. A., Vietze, P. M., Sherrod, K. A., Sandler, H. M., Falsey, S., & O'Connor, S.
M. (1979). Prediction of child maltreatment during pregnancy. Journal of the American
Academyof Child Psychiatry, 18, 205-218.
Alteme ier, W. A. , O'Connor, S., Vietze, P. M., Sandler, H. M., & Sherrod, K. B. (1982).
Antecedents of child abu se . Journal of Pediatrics, 100, 823-829.
Aragona, J. A., & Eyberg, S. M. (1981). Neglected children: Mothers' report of child
behavior problems and observ ed verbal behavior. Child Development, 52, 596-602.
SOCIOLOGICAL AND ECOLOGICAL FACTORS 165
Barahal, R. M., Watennan, J., & Martin, H . P. (1981). The social cognitive development of
abused children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 508-516.
Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A proc ess model. Child Development, 55,
83-96.
Belsky, J., & Nezworski, T. (Eds.) . (1988). Clinical implications of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Belsky, J., & Vondra, J. (1989). Lessons from child abuse: The determinants of parenting. In
D. Cicchetti & V. Carlson (Eds.), Child maltreatment: Research and theory on the conse-
quences of abuse and neglect (pp. 153-202). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Billings, A. G., & Moos, R. H. (1983). Comparison of children of depressed and non-
depressed parents: A social-environmental perspective. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy-
chology, 11, 463-486.
Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1980). The role of ego-control and ego-resiliency in the organiza-
tion of behavior. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology (Vol.
13). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Bradley, R. H ., & Caldwell, B. M. (1984). 174 children: A study of the relationship between
home environment and cognitive development during the first five years. In A. W.
Gottfried (Ed.), Homeenvironment and early cognitive development. New York: Academic
Press.
Bretherton, I. (1985). Attachment theory: Retrospect and prospect. Monographs of the Society
for Research in Child Development, 50 (1-2, Serial No . 209).
Bromet, E. J., & Comely, P. J. (1984). Correlates of depression in mothers of young chil-
dren. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 23, 335-342.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1958). Socialization and social class through time and space. In E. E.
Maccoby, T. M. Newcomb, & E. L. Hartley (Eds.), Readings in social psychology. New
York: Henry Holt .
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development. Amer-
ican Psychologist, 32, 513-530.
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Crouter, A. C. (1982). Work and family through time and space. In
S. B. Kammennan & C. D. Hayes (Eds.), Families that work: Children in a changing world.
Washington, DC: Nati onal Academy Press, 1982.
Brunnquell, D., Crichton, L., & Egeland, B. (1981). Maternal personality and attitude in
disturbances of child rearing. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 51, 680-691 .
Burgess, R. L., & Conger, R. D. (1978). Family interaction in abusive, neglectful, and
normal families . Child Development, 49, 1163-1173.
Burgess, R. L., Anderson, E. S., Schellenbach, C. J., & Conger, R. D. (1981). A social
interactional approach to the study of abusive families . In J. P. Vincent (Ed.), Advances
in family intervention, assessment, and theory: An annual compilation of research (Vol. 2).
Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Carroll, J. C. (1977). The intergenerational transmission of family violence: The long-term
effects of aggressive behavior. In Aggressive Behavior (Vol. 3, pp . 289-299). Alan R. Liss,
Inc.
Cicchetti, D., & Olsen, K. (1990). The developmental psychopathology of child maltreat-
ment. In M. Lewis & S. Miller (Eds .), Handbook of developmental psychopathology. New
York: Plenum Press.
Cicchetti, D., & Rizley, R. (1981). Developmental perspectives on the etiology, intergenera-
tional transmission, and sequelae of child maltreatment. In R. Rizley & D. Cicchetti
(Eds .), Developmental perspectives on child maltreatment. San Francisco: Iossey-Bass .
Cochran, M., & Brassard, J. (1979). Child development and personal social networks. Child
Development, 50, 601-616.
166 JOAN I. VONDRA
Colletta, N . D. (1983). At risk for depression: A study of young mothers. Journal of Genetic
Psychology, 142, 301-310.
Cotterell, J. L. (1986). Work and community influences on the quality of childrearing. Child
Development, 57, 362-374.
Crittenden, P. M. (1981). Abusing, neglecting, problematic, and adequate dyads: Differ-
entiating by patterns of interaction. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 27, 201-218.
Crittenden, P. M. (1985). Social networks, quality of childrearing, and child development.
Child Development, 56, 1299-1313.
Crittenden, P. M., & Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Attachment and child abuse. In D.
Cicchetti & V. Carlson (Eds .), Childmaltreatment: Research and theory on the consequences
of abuse and neglect (pp . 432-463). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Crnic, K. A., Greenberg, M. T., Ragozin, A. S., Robinson, N. M., & Basham , R B. (1983).
Effects of stress and social support on mothers and premature and full-term infants.
Child Development, 54, 209-217.
Crnic, K A., Greenberg, M. T., Robinson, N . M. , & Ragozin, A. S. (1984). Maternal stress
and social sup port: Effects on the mother-infant relationship from birth to eighteen
months. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 54, 224-234.
Crockenberg, S. (1987). Predictors and correlates of anger toward and punitive control of
toddlers by adolescent mothers. Child Development, 58, 964-975.
Cummings, E. M., Zahn-Waxler, C,; & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1981). Young children's re-
sponses to expressions of anger and affection by others in the family. Child Develop-
ment, 52, 1274-1282.
Dietrich, K N., Starr, R H., & Kaplan, M. G. (1980). Maternal stimulation and care of
abused infants. In T. M. Field, S. Goldberg, D. Stern, & A. M. Sostek (Eds .), High-risk
infants and children: Adult and peer interactions. New York: Academic Press.
DiLalla, D. L., & Crittenden, P. M. (1987). A factor analytic study of maltreated children's home
behavior. Research presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in
Ch ild Development, Baltimore , April.
Dubow, E. F., Huesmann, L. R, & Eron , L. D. (1987). Childhood correlates of adult ego
development. Child Development, 58, 859-869.
Egeland, B., & Brunnquell, D. (1979). An at-risk approach to the study of child abuse:
Some preliminary findings. Journal of the American Academy of ChildPsychiatry, 18, 219-
235.
Egeland, B., Breitenbucher, M., & Rosenberg, D. (1980). Prospective study of the signifi-
cance of life stress in the etiology of child abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 48, 195-205.
Egeland, B., Sroufe , L. A., & Erickson , M. (1983). The developmental consequence of
different patterns of maltreatment. Child Abuse and Neglect, 7, 459-469.
Elder, G. H ., [r., Liker, J. K, & Cross, C. E. (1984). Parent-child behavior in hard times and
the life course: A multi-generational perspective. In P. Baltes & B. Brim (Eds.), Lifespan
development and behavior (Vol. 6). New York: Academic Press .
Elder, G. H ., [r., Nguyen, T. V., & Caspi, A. (1985). Linking family hardship to children's
lives. Child Development, 56, 361-375.
Elder, G. H ., [r., Caspi , A., & Downey, G. (1986). Problem behavior and family rela-
tionships: A multigenerational analysis. In A. Sorensen, F. Weinert, & L. Sherrod
(Eds .), Human development and the life course. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Engfer, A., & Gavranidou, M. (1985, January). Antecedents and consequences of observed
maternal sensitivity: A longitudinal study. Paper presented at the International Sym-
posium on Psychobiology and Early Development, Free University, Berlin, West
Germany.
SOCIOLOGICAL AND ECOLOGICAL FACTORS 167
Engfer, A., & Cavranidou, M. (1988, June) . Prospective identification of violent mother-child
relationships . Paper presented at the Third European Conference on Developmental
Research, Budapest.
Engfer, A., & Schneewind, K. A. (1982). Cause s and consequences of harsh parental
punishment. Child Abuse and Neglect, 6, 129-139.
Erickson, M. F., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (1985). The relationship between quality of
attachment and behavior problems in preschool in a high-risk sample. Monographs of
the Society for Research in Child Development, 50 (Serial No . 209).
Estroff, T. W., Herrera, C,; Gaines, R., Shaffer, D., Gould, M., & Green, A. H. (1984).
Maternal psychopathology and perception of child behavior in psychiatrically referred
and child maltreatment families . Journal of the American Academy of ChildPsychiatry, 23,
649-652 .
Field, T. M. (1984). Follow-up developmental status of infants hospitalized for nonorganic
failure to thrive. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 9, 241-256 .
Fraiberg, S. (1980). Clinical studies in infant mental health: The first year. New York: Basic
Books.
Fraiberg . S., Adelson, E., & Shapiro, V. (1975). Ghosts in the nursery: A psychoanalytic
approach to the problems of impaired infant-mother relationships. Journal of the Ameri-
can Academy of Child Psychiatry, 14, 387-421.
Frodi, A. M., & Lamb, M. E. (1980). Child abusers' responses to infant smiles and cries.
Child Development, 51, 238-241.
Gaensbauer, T. [., & Sands, S. K. (1979). Distorted affective communications in abused/
neglected infants and their potential impact on caretakers. Journal of the American
Academy of Child Psychiatry, 18, 236-250.
Garbarino, ]. (1977a). The human ecology of child maltreatment: A conceptual model for
research. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 39, 721-727 .
Garbarino, ]. (1977b). The price of privacy in the social dynamics of child abuse. Child
Welfare, 56, 565-575.
Garbarino, ]., & Asp, E. (1981). Successful schools and competent students. Lexington, MA:
Lexington Books.
Garbarino, L, & Gilliam, G. (1980). Understanding abusive families. Lexington, MA: D.C.
Heath.
Garbarino, ]., & Sherman, D. (1980). High-risk neighborhoods and high-risk families : The
human ecology of child maltreatment. Child Development, 51, 188-198 .
Garbarino, j., Stocking, S. H., & Associates (1980). Protecting children from abuseand neglect:
Developing and maintaining effective support systems for families . San Francisco, CA:
]ossey-Bass .
Garbarino, j., & Vondra,]. (1987). Psychological maltreatment of children and youth. In M.
A. Brassard, R. Germain, & S. N. Hart (Eds.), The psychological maltreatment of children
and youth (pp . 25-44) . New York: Pergamon Press .
Gecas, V. (1979). The influence of social class on socialization . In W. R. Burr, R. Hill, F. I.
Nye, & I. L. Reiss (Eds .), Contemporary theories about thefamily (Vol. 1). New York: Free
Press.
George, C,; & Main , M. (1979). Social interactions of young abused children: Approach,
avoidance, and aggression. Child Development, 50, 306-318 .
George, c., & Main, M. (1980). Abused children: Their rejection of peers and caregivers. In
T. M. Field (Ed.), High-risk infants and children: Adult and peer interactions. New York:
Academic Press .
Gil, D. (1970). Violence against children: Physical childabuse in the United States. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
168 JOAN 1. VONDRA
Lewis, M., Feiring, C.; McGuffog, C,; & jaskir, J. (1984). Predicting psychopathology in six-
year-olds from early social relations. Child Development, 55, 123-136.
Lyons-Ruth, KI ., Connell, D. B., Zoll, D., & Stahl, J. (1987). Infants at social risk: Relations
among infant maltreatment, maternal behavior, and infant attachment behavior. De-
velopmental Psychology, 23, 223-232.
Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (1984). Predicting rejection of her infant from mother's represen-
tation of her own experience: Implications for the abused-abusing intergenerational
cycle. Child Abuse and Neglect, 8, 203-217.
Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (1985). Adult attachment classification system. Unpublished manu-
script, University of California, Berkeley.
Mash, E. J., Johnston, C.; & Kovitz, K. (1983). A comparison of the mother-child interac-
tions of physically abused and non-abused children during play and task situations.
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 12, 337-346.
Melnick, B., & Hurley, J. (1969). Distinctive personality attributes of child-abusing moth-
ers . Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 746-749.
Moen, P., Kain, E. L., & Elder, G. H. , Jr. (1981). Economic conditions andfamilylife: Contempo-
rary and historical perspectives. Paper presented for the National Academy of Sciences
Assembly of Behavioral and Social Sciences, Committee on Child Development Re-
search and Public Policy, December.
Mortimer, J. T. (1974). Patterns of intergenerational occupational movements: A smallest
space analysis. American Journal of Sociology, 79, 1278-1299.
Mortimer, J. T. (1976). Social class, work, and the family : Some implications of the father's
career for familial relationships and sons' career decisions. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 38, 241-256.
Mortimer, J. T., & Kumka, D. (1982). A further examination of the "occupational linkage
hypothesis." The Sociological Quarterly, 23, 3-16.
Newberger, E. H ., Reed, R. B., Daniels, J. H., Hyde, J. N ., & Kotelchuck, M. (1977).
Pediatric social illness: Toward an etiological classification. Pediatrics, 60, 175-185.
Pianta, R. C.; Egeland, B., & Hyatt, A. (1986). Maternal relationship history as an indicator
of developmental risk . American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 56, 385-398.
Piotrkowski, C. S., & Katz, M. H . (1982). Indirect socialization of children: The effects of
mothers' jobs on academic behaviors. Child Development, 53, 1520-1529.
Provence, S., & Lipton, R. (1962). Infants in institutions. New York: International Univer-
sities Press.
Ricks, M. H. (1985). The social transmission of parental behavior: Attachment across gener-
ations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50 (1-2, Serial No .
209).
Rohner, R. P., & Rohner, E. C. (1980). Antecedents and consequences of parental rejection:
A theory of emotional abuse. Child Abuseand Neglect, 4, 189-198.
Rosario, M., Salzinger, S., Feldman, R., & Hammer, M. (1987, April) . Home environments of
physically abused andcontrol school-age children . Research presented at the Biennial Meet-
ing of the Society for Research in Child Development, Baltimore.
Rosenbaum, A., & O'Leary, D. (1981). Marital violence: Characteristics of abusive couples.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 63-71.
Rutter, M., Quinton, D., & Liddle, C. (1983). Parenting in two generations: Looking
backwards and looking forwards . In N . Madge (Ed.), Families at risk (pp . 60-98).
London: Heinemann.
Sameroff, A. J. (1975). Early influences on development: Fact or fancy? Merrill-Palmer
Quarterly, 21, 267-294.
Sameroff, A. J. & Chandler, M. (1975). Reproductive risk and the continuum of caretaking
170 JOAN 1. VONDRA
PARENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY
AND HIGH-RISK CHILDREN
DAVID C. FACTOR AND DAVID A. WOLFE
INTRODUCTION
Interest in the relationship between parental behavior and children's
developmental outcome has long been a fundamental concern of social
scientists. This is a fact regardless of the direction of a person's basic
developmental orientation-genetic or environmental-because the
two orientations place significant weight on parental characteristics and
influence. Furthermore, it is now widely recognized that parents and
children influence each other in a reciprocal fashion (e.g ., Bell & Harper,
1977), rather than in a unidirectional, parent-to-child fashion as was
originally assumed by many investigators. Understanding of the man-
ner in which parental psychological characteristics can affect the devel-
oping child's emotional and behavioral adjustment has grown immense-
ly over the past decade, and these recent findings form the foundation
for the discussion in this chapter.
Numerous explanations have been given as to why children of psy-
chologically disturbed parents may be affected by their parents' psycho-
pathology. These arguments range from the heritability of specific disor-
DAVID C. FACTOR· TRE-ADD Program, Thistledown Regional Center for Children and
Adolescents, Rexdale, Ontario, Canada M9V 4L8. DAVID A. WOLFE· Department of
Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada N6A 5B8.
171
172 DAVID C. FACTOR and DAVID A. WOLFE
PARENTAL IMMATURITY
PARENTAL CRIMINALITY
Chronic illness on the part of one or both parents has also been
associated with greater risk of developmental impairment. Blackford
(1988) argued that such children have a greater incidence of depression
and psychosomatic disorders, and that antisocial behavior and poor
school performance are common associated features. Interestingly,
females of chronically ill parents appear to be less noticeably affected by
this situation than are males, a finding that Blackford (1988) and Rutter
(1971) explained in terms of the development of increased sensitivity to
PARENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY AND HIGH-RISK CHILDREN 177
others and the greater resistance to stress among girls than among boys.
Although it is accepted that chronic parental illness is linked with unde-
sirable developmental outcomes in the children, several authors do dif-
ferentiate between loss of a parent through natural events (i.e., death,
divorce) and the diminution of child-rearing responsibilities, as evi-
denced by chronic illness and other impairments affecting family func-
tioning; that is, a loss of the parent does not necessarily lead to impair-
ments in the child's development, but that developmental outcome can
be predicted more or less on the basis of the presence of other compen-
satory factors in the child's family, such as the support of family mem-
bers, availability of the other parent, and the absence of conflict and
discord in the child's presence (Emery, 1982; Hetherington & Martin,
1986).
Along these same lines, several researchers have looked more gen-
erally at family functioning and its relationship to children's develop-
ment. Drawing from Olson, Sprenkle, and Russell's (1979) work with
family cohesion and adaptability, Smets and Hartup (1988) explored the
family systems in which child behavior problems seem to emerge. "Co-
hesion" is defined as the "connectedness of relationships within the
family or the extent to which family members are 'bonded' to one an-
other," whereas adaptability refers to "the capacity of the family system
to change its power structure, role relations, and rules in response to
situational and developmental stress" (p. 239). According to family sys-
tems theory, a ''balanced'' family system is described as one in which
members are both moderately cohesive and adaptable, rather than fall-
ing at an extreme of one or both dimensions. In their review of previous
studies, Smets and Hartup (1988) highlighted the finding that families of
juvenile offenders often score more frequently in the extreme regions of
family functioning (i.e., showing either too high or too low cohesion
and/or adaptability). Similar results are found among families who have
referred their children to clinics because of problematic child behavior.
These researchers concluded, on the basis of previous work as well as on
the basis of their own study involving 120 clinic families, that extreme
forms of family functioning tend to be associated with abnormal child
behavior. These findings concur, as well, with the studies by Minuchin
(1974; Minuchin, Rossman, & Baker, 1978) involving psychosomatic
families.
with a particular diagnosis implies that the child will have a similar or
predictable diagnostic pattern. Most of the literature to date exploring
specific psychiatric diagnostic categories has focused on children of ei-
ther thought-disordered parents (i.e., schizophrenic adults) or children
of parents with major affective disorders (i.e., uni- or bipolar depres-
sion). To illustrate the psychological processes involved, as well as the
developmental implications for the child, we now turn to the expansive
literature on children of depressed parents.
highly correlated, yet this was not found for the adolescents on either
measure. However, they discovered that maternal depression increased
in association with an increase in marital conflict, a finding that mirrors
the literature on wife battering and maternal effectiveness (e.g., Wolfe,
Jaffe, Wilson, & Zak, 1985). That is, marital conflict and violence are
suspected to influence negatively maternal effectiveness and depressive
symptomatology directly, and these impairments in turn have an indi-
rect impact on parenting abilities. Thus, the study by Forehand et al.
(1988) has several implications for the type of measures used to assess
parental as well as adolescent depressive symptoms, and for the assess-
ment of critical situational events in the family, such as marital discord
and violence.
In summarizing these studies on behavioral, cognitive, and affec-
tive problems among children of depressed parents, the common find-
ing should be emphasized that there is no simple relationship between
parental depression (maternal or paternal) and affective disorders in the
offspring. Instead, studies are discovering a much more complicated
pathway whereby children may become at a greater risk for a wide array
of psychiatric problems when either or both parents suffers from depres-
sion. Based on the few studies conducted to date, differences in the
behavior of children of unipolar versus bipolar depressed parents do not
appear to be of clinical significance; rather, both subtypes appear to
elevate the probability of receiving a psychiatric diagnosis during child-
hood or adolescence. Furthermore, the vast majority of studies to date
have focused on maternal depression, and thus the role of paternal
psychiatric illness is poorly understood.
We now turn to a closer look at the interactions between depressed
parents and their children in order to see how their child-rearing meth-
ods and communications to their children may be responsible for devel-
opmental impairments.
pression is more common in women, who also carry the bulk of the
child-rearing responsibility in most families . Although the directionality
issue cannot be easily resolved (i.e., do difficult infants bring about
greater maternal depression, or do depressed mothers give rise to more
difficult infants?), several important aspects of the daily interactions
between depressed mothers and their infants or young children provide
some clarity to the issue of how parental depression plays a pathogenic
role in child development.
Weissman and her colleagues (1979; Weissman, Paykel, & Klerman,
1972; Weissman et al., 1987) concluded from their observations of in-
teractions between depressed mothers and their offspring that these
children were deprived of normal involvement with their parents. For
example, during play these parents were unenthusiastic and provided
little involvement or guidance for the child; as the children grew older,
the parents continued this pattern by showing a lack of interest in the
child's school activities, social events, or peers. Decreased involvement
on the part of the parent took other forms as well, such as paying little
attention to the child's physical health or appearance. The researchers
further noted that the children were not as often encouraged to discuss
their feelings or to discuss their daily activities, compared to comparison
children from nondepressed families. Not only were the parent/child
interactions marked by greater disinterest and less involvement, they
observed that acutely depressed parents behaved in a more hostile fash-
ion toward their children, a finding that has important implications as
well for the child's development of a sense of self and self-mastery.
Overall, these researchers interpreted their findings to highlight the four
primary areas of parental dysfunction that can be identified in families
with one or more depressed parents: involvement and disinterest, com-
munication, affection, and hostility. All four of these areas (when
phrased in positive terms) are deemed critical to healthy child develop-
ment (Maccoby & Martin, 1983), and therefore the mechanisms by
which parental depression plays a role in developmental psycho-
pathology are becoming more apparent.
Additional studies have focused on the critical period of attachment
between mother and infant during the first months of life, and have again
revealed a pathogenic pattern among depressed parents of low involve-
ment and responsivity toward their offspring. Cohn and Tronick (1983)
used an experimental design to manipulate the presence or absence of
maternal depressed mood while interacting with their 3-month-old in-
fants (12 female and 12 male). Mothers in the study were instructed to
interact normally or to interact with a simulated "depressed expression"
with their infants. Results indicated that those mothers assigned to the
188 DAVID C. FACTOR and DAVID A. WOLFE
SUMMARY
We have reviewed a number of areas of parental psychopathology
that have implications for child development. Because our attention was
focused on the major psychosocial factors associated with parental disor-
ders, we drew from the expanding literature on children of depressed
parents to highlight the suspected processes involved in the transmis-
sion or transaction of developmental psychopathology in offspring. De-
spite the growing knowledge in this area of parental psychopathology,
we again remind the reader that the studies covered in this review were
limited primarily to situational and psychological variables. Interested
readers will have to find elsewhere additional theoretical explanations,
methodological procedures, and causal or correlational relationships as-
sociated with genetic, biological, and psychiatric fields of study.
Other important psychosocial variables that were not the focus of
the present review merit some mention in closing. Parental alcoholism,
for example, is known to interfere with normal child-rearing and the
development of a healthy parent-child relationship. Consequently, it
comes as no surprise that alcoholism in parents has been found to be
associated with adjustment problems in children, especially males
(Adler & Raphael, 1983; West & Prinz, 1987). Alcoholic parents appear to
fit a pattern of inconsistent and unpredictable childcare that has been
similarly discovered in many of the studies of parental disorders re-
viewed. In addition, we need to look more carefully at the cultural,
194 DAVID C. FACTOR and DAVID A. WOLFE
familial, and social support factors that play a role in mediating the
negative impact of poverty and disadvantage of children, because chil-
dren from lower socioeconomic backgrounds tend to be more at risk for
school problems (especially among minority children; Felner, Gillespie,
& Smith, 1985).
The findings presented throughout this chapter support a social
learning explanation of developmental psychopathology, which predicts
a disruption or alteration in development as a function of significant fac-
tors affecting learning opportunities, of which parental depression is but
one. Rather than assuming a one-to-one correspondence between pa-
rental psychopathology and developmental outcome, social learning
theory suggests that events that have a significant influence on the
child's learning environment can lead to changes or deviations in coping
responses, expectations, problem-solving skills, and related develop-
mental events. In this manner, parental depression represents one of the
more visible and dramatic circumstances that can change the course of
normal child development, much the same as do child abuse (Wolfe,
1987)/ parental divorce (Hetherington & Martin, 1986)/ wife battering
(Wolfe et al., 1985)/ and parental criminality (Lewis, Balsla, Shanok, &
Snell, 1976)/ to name only a few. Consistency and predictability of child
care may be the common threads that link many of these deviant forms
of parenting practices to the wide variety of negative developmental
outcomes (Wahler & Dumas, 1987).
Several methodological considerations emerge from the current lit-
erature in this area that justify further attention. Perhaps because of the
inexact nature of our diagnostic systems, the criteria for determining
and agreeing upon the type of problem(s) exhibited by some parents are
often unclear. Furthermore, the choice of assessment instruments and
procedures typically varies from study to study, and therefore it is diffi-
cult to draw comparative conclusions. One major direction that has been
undertaken in this regard is a better understanding of the manner in
which certain pathological conditions in parents affect their ratings of
child behavior (see, for example, Jensen, Traylor, Xenakis, & Davis,
1988; Schaughency & Lahey, 1985). Most importantly, we need more
longitudinal research on this topic in order to draw more firm conclu-
sions about the long-term effects of parental psychopathology and the
course and stability of such changes. Finally, we point to the growing
recognition that a significant number of children survive without any
detectable harm, even in very unhealthy environments (Beardslee &
Podorefski, 1988)/ a reality that may offer assistance in understanding
the plight of children of disturbed parents.
PARENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY AND HIGH-RISK CHILDREN 195
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
REFERENCES
Adler, R., & Raphael, B. (1983). Review : Children of alcoholics. Australian and New Zealand
Journal of Psychiatry, 17, 3-8.
Anderson, C (1987). Depression and families . Journal of Psychotherapy and the Family, 3,33-
48.
Barnett, P. A., & Gotlib , I. H . (1988). Psychosocial functioning and depression : Dis-
tinguishing among antecedents, concomitants, and consequences. Psychological Bul-
letin, 104, 97-126.
Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Developmental Psychology
Monographs, 4 (1, pt. 2).
Beardslee, W. R., & Podorefsky, D. (1988). Resilient adolescents whose parents have se-
rious affective and other psychiatric disorders : Importance of self-understanding and
relationships. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 63-69.
Beard slee, W. R., Bemporad, j., Keller, M. B., & Klerman, G. L. (1983). Children of parents
with major affective disorder: A review. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 825-
832.
Beardslee, W. R., Keller, M. B., & Klerman, G. L. (1985). Children of parents with affective
disorder. International Journal of Family Psychiatry, 6, 283-299.
Bell, R. Q. , & Harper, L. (1977). Child effects on adults. Hillsdale, N]: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Blackford, K. A. (1988). The children of chronically ill parents. Journal of Psychosocial Nurs-
ing, 26, 33-36.
Breznitz, Z., & Sherman, T. (1987). Speech patterning of natural discourse of well and
depressed mothers and their young children. Child Development, 58, 395-400.
Cantwell, D. P., & Baker, L. (1984). Parental mental illness and psychiatric disorders in "at
risk" children. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 45, 503-507.
Christensen, A., Phillips , S., Glasgow, R. E., & Johnson, S. M. (1983). Parental charac-
teristics and interactional dy sfunction in families with child behavior problems: A
preliminary investigation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 11, 153-166.
Cohn, ]. E , & Tronick, E. Z. (1983). Three-month-old infants' reaction to simulated mater-
nal depression. Child Development, 54, 185-193.
Cooper, S. E , Leach, C, Storer, D., Tonge, W. L. (1977). The children of psychiatric
pa tients: Clinical findings. British Journal of Psychiatry, 131, 514-522.
Coyne, ]. C, Kahn, ]., Gotlib , I. H . (1987). Depression. In T. Jacob (Ed.), Family interaction
and psychopathology: Theories, methods, and findings (pp . 509-533). New York: Plenum
Press.
196 DAVID C. FACTOR and DAVID A. WOLFE
Emery, R. (1982). Interparental conflict and the children of discord and divorce. Psychologi-
cal Bulletin, 92, 310-330.
Feiner, R D., Gillespie, J. F., & Smith, R. (1985). Risk and vulnerability in childhood: A
reappraisal. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 14, 2-4.
Forehand, R, Brody, G., Slotkin, J., Fauber, R, McCombs, A., & Long, N. (1988). Young
adolescent and maternal depression: Assessment, interrelations, and family predic-
tors . Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 422-426.
Gold stein, M. J. (1988). The family and psychopathology. Annual Review of Psychology, 39,
283-299.
Green, A. H . (1978). Child abuse . In B. B. Wolman, J. Egan, & A. Ross (Eds .), Handbook of
treatment of mentaldisorders in childhood andadolescence (pp. 430-455) . Englewood Cliffs,
NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Hammen, C ,; Adrian, C.; Gordon, D., Burge, D., Jaenicke, C,; & Hiroto, D. (1987a).
Children of depressed mothers: Maternal strain and symptom predictors of dy sfunc-
tion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 190-198.
Hammen, C, ; Gordon, D., Burge, D., Adrian, c., Jaenicke, C.; & Hiroto, D. (1987b).
Maternal affective disorders, illness, and stress: Risk for children's psychopathology.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 736-741.
Hetherington, E. M., & Martin, B. (1986). Family factors and psychopathology in children.
In H. C. Quay, & J. S. Werry (Eds.), Psychopathological disorders ofchildhood (3rd ed ., pp.
332-390). New York: John Wiley.
Jensen, P. S., Traylor, J., Xenakis, S. N ., & Davis, H . (1988). Childhood psychopathology
rating scales and interrater agreement: I. Parents' gender and psychiatric symptoms.
Journal of the American Academy of Childand Adolescent Psychiatry. , 27, 422-450.
Kandel, E., Mednick, S. A., Kirkegaard-Sorensen, L., Hutchings, B., Knop, J., Rosenberg,
R., & Schulsinger, F. (1988). IQ as a protective factor for subjects at high risk for
antisocial behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 224-226.
Kashani, J. H ., Burk, J. P., Horwitz, B., & Reid, J. C. (1985). Differential effect of subtype of
parental major affective disorder on children. Psychiatry Research, 15, 195-204.
Klein, D. N ., Clark, D. C,; Dansky, L., & Margolis, E. T. (1988). Dysthymia in the offspring
of parents with primary unipolar affective disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97,
265-274.
Lahey, B. B., Conger, R. D., Atkeson, B. M., & Treiber, F. A. (1984). Parenting behavior
and emotional status of physically abusive mothers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 52, 1062-1071.
Lee, C. M., & Gotlib, I. H. (1989). Clinical status and emotional adjustment of children of
depressed mothers. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 478-483 .
Lewis, D.O., Balla, D., Shanok, S., & Snell , L. (1976). Delinquency, parental psycho-
pathology, and paternal criminality: Clinical and epidemiological findings. Journal of
the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 15, 665-678.
Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family : Parent-
child interaction. In P. H . Mussen (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (4th ed .). New
York: Wiley.
Masten, A. S., & Garmezy, N. (1985). Risk, vulnerability, and protective factors in develop-
mental psychopathology. In B. B. Lahey, & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in clinical
child psychology (Vol. 8, pp. 1-52). New York: Plenum Press.
Minuchin, S. (1974). Families and family therapy. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Minuchin, S., Rosman, B. L., & Baker, L. (1978). Psychosomatic families . Cambridge: Har-
vard University Press.
Olson, D. H ., Sprenkle, D. H ., & Russell, C. S. (1979). Circumplex model of marital and
PARENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGY AND HIGH-RISK CHILDREN 197
family systems: I. Cohesion and adaptability dimensions, family types, and clinical
applications. Family Process, 18, 3-28.
Orvaschel, H. (1983). Maternal depression and child dysfunction: Children at risk . In B. B.
Lahey, & A. E. Kazd in (Eds.), Advances in clinical childpsychology (Vol. 6, pp. 169-197).
New York: Plenum Press.
Orvaschel, H., Weissman, M. M., & Kidd, K. K. (1980). Children and depression. Journal of
Affective Disorders, 2, 1-16.
Orvaschel, H., Walsh-Allis , G., & Ye, W. (1988). Psychopathology in children of parents
with recurrent depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 17-28.
Parke, R. D., & Collmer, C. W. (1975). Child abuse: An interdisciplinary analysis. In E. M.
Hetherington (Ed.), Reviewof child development research (Vol. 5, pp. 509-590). Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Phipps-Yonas, S. (1980). Teenage pregnancy and motherhood: A review of the literature.
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 50, 403-430 .
Radke-Yarrow, M., Cummings, E. M., Kuczynski, L. , & Chapman, M. (1985). Patterns of
attachment in two- and three-year-olds in normal families and families with parental
depression. Child Development, 56, 884-893.
Rutter, M. (1966). Children of sick parents: An environmental and psychiatric study.
Institute of Psychiatry, Maudsley Monographs No 16. London: Oxford University
Press.
Rutter, M. (1971). Parent-child separation: Psychological effects on the children. Journal of
Child Psychiatry, 12, 233-260.
Rutter, M. (1979). Protective factors in children's response to stress and disadvantage. In
M. W. Kent & J. E. Rolf (Eds.), Primary prevention of psychopathology: Vol III. Social
competence in children . Hanover, NH : University Press of New England.
Sameroff, A. J., Seiffer, R., & Zax, M. (1982). Early development of children at risk for
emotional disorder Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 47(7
Serial No . 199).
Schaughency, E. A., & Lahey, B. B. (1985). Mothers' and fathers' perceptions of child
deviance: Roles of child behavior, parental depression, and marital satisfaction. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 718-723.
Schore, E. L. (1988). Families, family roles and psychological diagnoses in primary care.
Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 9, 327-332.
Schwartz, B. A. (1979). Adolescent parents. In J. D. Nosphitz, I. N. Berling, & L. A. Stone
(Eds.), Basic handbook of child psychiatry: Prevention and current issues (Vol. 4, pp . 360-
363). New York: Basic Books.
Scott, K. G., Field, T., & Robertson, E. (1981). Teenage parents and their offspring. New York:
Grune & Stratton.
Silverman, W. K., Cerny, J. A., Nelles , W. B., & Burke, A. E. (1988). Behavior problems in
children of parents with anxiety disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Childand
Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 779-784.
Smets, A. C,; & Hartup. W. w., (1988). Systems and symptoms: Family cohesion/ adapt-
ability and childhood behavior problems. Journal of Abnormal ChildPsychology, 16, 233-
246.
Spinetta, J. J. & Rigler, D. (1972). The child abusing parent: A psychological review. Psycho-
logical Bulletin . 77, 296-304.
Susman, E. J., Trickett, P. K., Iannotti, R. I. , Hollenback, B. E., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1985).
Child-rearing patterns in depressed, abusive, and normal mothers. American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 55, 237-251.
Sylvester, C. E., Hyde, T. S., & Reichler, R. J. (1987). The diagnostic interview for children
198 DAVID C. FACTOR and DAVID A. WOLFE
and personality inventory for child ren in studies of children at risk for anx iety disor-
ders or depression . Journal of the American Academy ofChildand Adolescent Psychiatry, 26,
668-675.
Turner, S. M., Beidel, D. C.; & Costello, A. (1987). Psychopathology in the offspring of
anxiety disorders patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 229-235.
Wahler, R. G., & Dumas, J. E. (1987). Family factors in childhood psychology: Towards a
coercion-neglect model. In T. Jacob (Eds.), Family interaction and psychopathology: Theo-
ries, methods, and findings (pp . 581-627). New York: Plenum Pres s.
Webster-Stratton, C.; & Hammond, M. (1988). Ma ternal depression and its relation sh ip to
life stress, perceptions of child be hav ior problems, parenting behaviors, and child
conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 299-315.
Weissman, M. M. (1979). Depressed pa rents and their children: Implications for preven-
tion . In J. D. No shpitz, I. N ., Berlin, & L. A. Stone (Eds .), Basic handbook of child
psychiatry: Prevention and current issues (Vol. 4, pp . 292-299). Ne w York: Basic Books .
Weissman, M. M., Gammon G. D., Joh n, K., Merikangas, K. R., Warner, V., Pru soff, B. A.,
& Sho lornskas, D. (1987). Children of depressed parents . Archives of General Psychiatry,
44 847- 853 .
Weissman, M. M., Paykel , E. S., & Klerman, G. L. (1972). The depressed woman as a
mother. Social Psychiatry, 7, 98-108.
Werner, E. E. (1989). Hig h-ri sk children in young adulthood: A longitudinal stu dy from
birth to 32 years . American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 59, 72-81.
West, M. 0 ., & Prinz, R. J. (1987). Parental alcoholism and childhood psychopathology.
Psychological Bulletin, 102, 204-21 8.
Wolfe, D. A. (1985). Child-abusive parents: An empirical review and anal ysis . Psychological
Bulletin, 97, 462-482.
Wolfe, D. A. (1987). Child abuse: Implications for child development and psychopathology. New-
bury Park, CA : Sage .
Wolfe, D. A., Jaffe, P. J., Wilson, S. K., & Zak, L. (1985). Children of battered women: The
relation of child behavior to family violence and maternal stre ss. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 53, 657-665.
Worland, J., Weeks , D. G., Janes, c. L., & Stro ck, B. D. (1984). Intelligence , classroo m
behavior, and acad emic ach ievement in children at hig h and low risk for psycho-
pathology: A structural eq uation analysis . Journal of Abnormal ChildPsychology, 12,437-
454.
Za hn-Waxler, c., Mayfi eld , A., Radke-Yarrow, M., McKnew, D. H. , Cytryn, L. , & Dave n-
port, Y. B. (1988). A follow-up investigation of offspring of parents with bipolar disor-
der. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 506-509.
CHAPTER 8
INTRODUCTION
Since the original description of the Battered Child Syndrome by Kempe
and his colleagues (Kempe, Silverman, Steele, Droegemueller, & Silver,
1962), there has been a tremendous growth in professional and media
interest in child abuse and neglect. Evidence of expanded awareness is
found in the plethora of specialty journals (e.g., Journal of Family Violence,
Child Abuse and Neglect) and books (MacFarlane, Waterman, Conerly,
Damon, Durfee, & Long, 1986; Oates, 1982; Wolfe, 1987) devoted to this
topic. This increased focus of attention is at least partly attributable to
the dramatic rise in reported cases of child maltreatment in recent years.
For example, there has been an 8% increase in reported cases of child
abuse and neglect from 1985 to 1986 (American Association for Protect-
ing Children, 1988). Although such an elevation in reports is partly
related to greater professional awareness of child maltreatment, most
experts agree that child abuse and neglect currently represent significant
social problems (see Chapter 2 in this volume).
Because of the deleterious physical and psychological consequences
associated with child maltreatment (see Ammerman, Cassisi, Hersen, &
Van Hasselt, 1986; Friedrich & Einbender, 1983), recent investigators
199
200 ROBERT T. AMMERMAN
have directed their efforts toward the prevention of abuse and neglect
(e.g., Lutzker & Rice, 1984). An important component of such efforts is
to identify children who are at high risk for maltreatment in an effort to
curtail the processes leading to abuse or neglect (Parke & Collmer, 1975).
At-risk populations have been targeted, using a variety of factors
thought to be implicated in the etiology of abuse and neglect, including
family demographics, socioeconomic status, parental psychopathology,
parent history of abuse, and substance abuse disorders (see Starr, 1988).
In addition, some theorists have suggested that certain child charac-
teristics may playa role in the development and maintenance of abuse
(deLissovoy, 1979). Thus, based upon this premise, it is possible that
these child characteristics can be subsequently used in screening for
potential child maltreatment or selecting at-risk populations for preven-
tative interventions.
This chapter will review the evidence that child factors contribute to
the etiology and maintenance of physical abuse and neglect. First, con-
ceptual models that describe the process by which children can be in-
volved in the development of maltreatment will be presented. Second,
early childhood characteristics posited to be risk factors for abuse or
neglect will be reviewed. Third, interactional studies describing the co-
ercive relationship between abusive parents and their children will be
discussed. These studies elucidate the ways in which child factors may
contribute to the maintenance of abuse. Fourth, the role of child hand-
icapping conditions in increasing risk for maltreatment will be consid-
ered. Finally, the utility of using child factors in assessing risk will be
examined, and future directions that research might take are outlined.
Child contributions to the etiology of sexual abuse will not be covered,
and the reason for this is that it is widely acknowledged that the dynam-
ics involved in sexual abuse differ greatly from those observed in phys-
ical abuse and neglect (see MacFarlane et al., 1986). Moreover, there is
little support for the contention that children have any causative role in
sexual abuse.
COMMENTS
were dubbed onto the full-term baby's video tape, and vice versa, in
order to control for the influence of physical characteristics on parental
responding. Findings indicated increased emotional and physiological
arousal in response to the premature baby's cry as contrasted with that
of the full-term baby. Furthermore, parents reported less willingness to
interact with the premature than with the full-term infant. Further ex-
amination revealed that parents react negatively to infants labeled as
"premature" or "difficult" regardless of actual birth status (Frodi, Lamb,
Leavitt, & Donovan, 1978). Frodi and Lamb (1980) extended their re-
search on emotional and physiological responses to premature infants to
child abusers. When compared to nonabusive parents, child abusers
evidenced more pronounced increases in autonomic and emotional
arousal when presented with the cries of a premature infant. In addi-
tion, abusive parents showed similar patterns of response to a smiling
infant. Thus, abusive parents appear to view as aversive almost any
social contact with a premature infant.
Although these studies describe the mechanisms through which
prematurity may contribute to abuse, two methodological limitations
prevent drawing firm conclusions from the data. First, the analogue
nature of the aforementioned investigations do not demonstrate that
such processes are found in the natural environment. And second, it is
unclear from these findings that the negative behaviors displayed by
premature infants precede and subsequently elicit abuse from care-
takers. Rather, such aversive characteristics may develop as a function of
abuse, although they also may serve to elicit further maltreatment in the
future (Frodi, 1981).
Similar difficulty delineating cause and effect is encountered in the
study of attachment formation in maltreated infants. Attachment is the
affective and social bond between mother and infant formed via the
unique contributions of parent and child (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, &
Wall, 1978). Attachment is a qualitative construct that can be categorized
as secure or insecure (Sroufe & Waters, 1977). Behavioral deficits in
attachment-promoting behaviors exhibited by the mother and/or the
infant can lead to insecure attachment. Ainsworth (1980), a pioneer in
the empirical examination of attachment formation, has proposed that
insecure attachment related to parental characteristics (e.g ., unrespon-
siveness, inadequate caretaking) or aversive child behaviors (e.g., fre-
quent crying, difficult to calm) can lead to maltreatment. However, no
data are available showing that disruption in attachment leads to abuse
or neglect, but numerous studies have shown disproportionate occur-
rence of insecure attachment in maltreated infants and their mothers
(see Cicchetti, 1987), although it is most likely that such disruptions are a
206 ROBERT T. AMMERMAN
COMMENTS
COMMENTS
INCIDENCE OF MALTREATMENT IN
HANDICAPPED POPULATIONS
COMMENTS
SUMMARY
This chapter began with the premise that child characteristics can
contribute to the etiology of physical abuse and neglect. The framework
for this hypothesis is drawn from recent theoretical formulations (the
216 ROBERT T. AMMERMA N
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
REFERENCES
Ainsworth, M. D. (1980). Attachment and child abuse. In G. Gerber, C. Ross, & E. Zigler
(Eds .), Childabuse: An agenda foraction (pp. 35-47). New York: Oxford University Press.
Ainsworth, M. D. 5., Blehar, M. C.; Waters , E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A
psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
American Association for Protecting Children. (1988). Highlights of official child neglect and
abuse reporting 1986 . Denver: American Humane Association.
Ammerman, R. T. (1989). Child abuse and neglect. In M. Hersen (Ed.), Innovations in child
behavior therapy (pp. 353-394). New York: Springer.
Ammerman, R. T., Cassisi, J. E., Hersen, M., & Van Hasselt, V. B. (1986). Consequences of
physical abuse and neglect in children. Clinical Psychology Review, 6, 291-310.
Ammerman, R T., Lubetsky, M. J., Hersen, M. , & Van Hasselt, V. B. (1988a). Maltreatment
of children and adolescents with multiple handicaps: Five case examples. Journal of the
Multihandicapped Person, 1, 129-139.
Ammerman, R. T., Van Hasselt, V. B., & Hersen, M. (1988b). Maltreatment of handicapped
children: A critical review. Journal of Family Violence, 3, 53-72.
Ammerman, R T., Hersen, M. , Van Ha sselt, V. B., McGonigle, J. J., & Lubetsky, M. J.
(1989). Abuse and neglect in ps ychiatrically hospitalized multihandicapped children.
Child Abuse and Neglect, 13, 335-343.
Bauman, M. K., & Yoder, N. M. (1966). Adjustment to blindness-Reviewed. Springfield, IL:
Charles C Thomas.
Belsky, J. (1980). Child maltreatment: An ecological integration. American Psychologist, 35,
320-335.
Birrell, R , & Birrell, J. (1968). The maltreatment syndrome in children: A hospital survey.
Medical Journal of Australia, 2, 1023-1029 .
Bousha, D. M., & Twentyman, C. T. (1984). Mother-child interactional style in abuse,
neglect, and control groups: Naturalistic observations in the home. ChildDevelopment,
93, 106-114.
Browne, D. H . (1986). The role of stress in the commission of subsequent acts of child
abuse and neglect. Journal of Family Violence, 1, 289-297.
Burgess, R L., & Draper, P. (1988). A biosocial theory of family violence: The role of
natural selection, ecological instability, and coercive interpersonal contingencies. In L.
Ohlin & M. H. Tonry (Eds .), Criminal and justice-An annual review of research: Family
violence. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Cicchetti, D. (1987). Developmental psychopathology in infancy: Illustration from the
study of maltreated youngsters. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 837-
845.
Cicchetti, D. , & Serafica, F. C. (1981). Interplay among behavioral systems: Illustrations
from the study of attachment, affiliation , and wariness in young children with Down's
Syndrome, Developmental Psychology, 17, 36-49.
Crittenden, P. M. (1985). Maltreated infants: Vulnerability and resilience. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 26, 85-96.
218 ROBERT T. AMMERMAN
deLissovoy, V. (1979). Toward the definition of "abuse provoking child." Child Abuse and
Neglect, 3, 341-350.
Diamond, L. J., & [audes, P. K. (1983). Child abuse in a cerebral-palsied population.
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 25, 169-174.
Drotar, D., Baskiewicz, A., Irvin , N. , Kennell, J., & Klaus, M. (1975). The adaptation of
parents to the birth of an infant with congenital malformation: A hypothetical model,
Pediatrics, 56, 710-717.
Egeland, B., & Brunnquell, D. (1979). An at-risk approach to the study of child abu se.
Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 18, 219-236.
Egeland, B., & Vaughn, B. (1981). Failure of "bond formation" as a cause of abuse, neglect,
and maltreatment. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 51, 78-84.
Egeland, B., Breitenbucher, M., & Rosenberg, D. (1980). Prospective study of significance
of etiology of child abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 195-205.
Elmer, E., & Gregg, G. S. (1967). Developmental characteristics of abused children. Pedi-
atrics, 40, 596-602 .
Emde, R. N., & Brown, C. (1978). Adaptation to the birth of a Down 's Syndrome infant.
Grieving and maternal attachment. Journal of the American Academy of ChildPsychiatry,
17, 299-323.
Fontana, V. J. (1973). The diagnosis of the maltreatment syndrome in children. Pediatrics,
51, 780-782.
Fox, N. A. (1988). The effect of prematurity and postnatal iIlness on the mother-infant
interaction: A study of a high-risk dyad. In E. J. Anthony & C. Chiland (Eds.), Thechild
in his family: Perilous development: Child raising and identity formation under stress (pp.
211-222). New York: Wiley.
Friedrich, W. N., & Benskin , J. A. (1976). The role of the child in abuse: A review of the
literature. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 46, 580-590.
Friedrich, W. N., & Einbender, A. J. (1983). The abu sed child : A psychological review.
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 12, 244-256.
Frodi, A. M. (1981). Contribution of infant characteristics to child abuse. AmericanJournal of
Mental Deficiency, 85, 341-349.
Frodi, A. M., & Lamb, M. E. (1980). Child abusers' responses to infant smiles and cries.
Child Development, 51, 238-241.
Frodi, A. M., Lamb, M. E., Leavitt, L., & Donovan, W. (1978). Fathers' and mothers'
responses to infant smiles and cries . Infant Behaviorand Development, 1, 187-198.
Frodi, A. M., Lamb, M., Leavitt, L., Donovan, W., Neff, c., & Sherry, D. (1978). Fathers'
and mothers' responses to the appearance and cries of premature and normal infants.
Developmental Psychology, 14, 490-498.
Gaensbauer, T. J., & Sands, K. (1979). Distorted affective communications in abus-
ed /neglected infants and their potential impact on caretakers. Journal and the American
Academy of Child Psychiatry, 18, 236-250.
Gaines, R., Sandgrund, A., Green, A. H., & Power, E. (1978). Etiological factors in child
maltreatment: A multivariate study of abusing, neglecting, and normal mothers. Jour-
nal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 531-540.
Gallagher, J. J., Beckman, P., & Cross, A. H . (1983). Families of handicapped children:
Sources of stress and its amelioration. Exceptional Children, 50, 10-19 .
Garbarino, J. (1976). A preliminary study of some ecological correlates of child abuse: The
impact of socioeconomic stress on mothers. Child Development, 47, 178-185.
Gath, A. (1977). The impact of an abnormal child upon the parents. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 130, 405-410.
PREDISPOSING CHILD FACTORS 219
Trickett, P. K., & Kuczynski, L. (1986). Children's misbehaviors and parental discipline
strategies in abusive and nonabusive families . Developmental Psychology, 22, 115-123.
Van Ha sselt, V. B., Ammerman, R. T., & Sisson, L. (in press). Physical disability. In A. S.
Bellack, M. Hersen, & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), International handbook of behavior modification
and therapy (2nd ed .), New York: Plenum Pre ss .
Waisbren, S. E. (1980). Parents' reactions after the birth of a developmentally disabled
child. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 84, 345-351.
Warren, D. A. (1977). Blindness and earlychildhood development . New York: American Foun-
dation for the Blind .
Wasserman, G. A. (1986). Affective expression in normal and physically handicapped
infants: Situational and developmental effects . Journal of the American Academy of Child
Psychiatry, 25, 393- 399.
Wasserman, G. A., Allen, R., & Solomon, C. R. (1985). At-risk toddlers and their mothers:
The special case of physical handicaps. Child Development, 56, 73-83.
Wasserman, G. A., Lennon, M. C.; Allen, R. & Shilansky, M. (1987). Contributors to
attachment in normal and physically handicapped infants. Journal of the American
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 9-15.
White, R., Benedict, M. I., Wulff, L., & Kelley, M. (1987). Physical disabilities as risk factor s
for child maltreatment: A selected review. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57, 93-
101.
Wolfe, D. A. (1985). Child abusive parents: An empirical review and analysis. Psychological
Bulletin, 97, 462-482.
Wolfe, D. A. (1987). Childabuse: Implications for childdevelopment and psychopathology. New-
bury Park, CA: Sage.
Wolfe, D. A., & St. Pierre, J. (1989). Child abuse and neglect. In T. H . Ollendick & M.
Hersen (Eds .), Handbook of child psychopathology, (2nd ed ., pp. 377-398). New York:
Plenum Press .
Zirpoli, T. J. (1986). Child abuse and children with handicaps. Remedial and Special Educa-
tion, 7, 39-48.
PART IV
PREVENTION PROGRAMS
MAXINE R. NEWMAN AND JOHN R. LUTZKER
INTRODUCTION
A discussion of the prevention of child abuse or neglect, as an y discus-
sion of prevention, requires definition. In a seminal book Principles of
Preventive Psychiatry (1964), Caplan defined three types of prevention,
some of which were already in use by psychiatrists and psychologists as
early as the mid-1960s: primary prevention, secondary prevention, and
tertiary prevention. In this chapter, we will discuss these three levels of
prevention generically, give a brief historical overview of past efforts to
identify and prevent child abuse or neglect, de fine prevention in the
area of child abuse or neglect, specificall y when the intervention is
intended to: prevent further abuse or prevent first abuse, con sider some
programs designed to prevent further abuse and others which focus on
intervening before abuse occurs, and provide some recommendations
for future prevention programs.
225
226 MAXINE R. NEWMAN and JOHN R. LUTZKER
PREVENTION LEVELS
TERTIARY PREVENTION
SECONDARY PREVENTION
PRIMARY PREVENTION
HISTORICAL OVERVIEW
AFTER KEMPE
closet. They noted the Bittner and Newberger (1981) model for under-
standing child abuse in which predisposing factors to family violence
were described as (1) child-produced stresses (handicapped, retarded,
difficult, hyperactive, foster child); (2) family stresses (poverty, unem-
ployment, isolation, poor housing, relationship difficulties, parent-
child problems, inappropriate child-rearing style); and (3) parent-pro-
duced stresses (low self-esteem, abused as a child, depression, sub-
stance abuse, psychiatric disorder, ignorance of child-rearing, unrealistic
expectations). These factors are then influenced by a triggering situa-
tion, such as a discipline problem, substance abuse, an argument or
family conflict, or some acute environmental problem, followed by mal-
treatment by one family member toward another.
Identifying a similar population, Meredith, Abbott, and Adams
(1986) found that, as the use of physical violence among family members
increased, there were significant decreases in the family perception of
family strengths, marital satisfaction, and parental satisfaction. This
highlights the high risk for abuse or neglect in families who practice
physical violence toward each other.
Primary prevention methods, similar to tertiary prevention mea-
sures, have a community focus. Although as we have noted, tertiary
prevention is geared toward the already diagnosed situations of child
abuse and neglect, primary prevention has a broader goal: To reduce the
rate of new cases in a population over time by counterbalancing adverse
influences, and to reduce the risk of child abuse and neglect for the
entire community. Thus, primary prevention programs for child abuse
and neglect would include political/social action, examination of factors
that produce healthy family behaviors, as well as examination of factors
that lead to family dysfunction, exploration of problems of living, and,
the study of environmental factors that impact family life.
Paisley (1987) discussed legislative actions to prevent child abuse
and neglect enacted by states and, in particular, the legislation efforts in
North Carolina. She described the role of the school counselor and
noted that primary prevention programs should be geared toward re-
ducing the parent's unrealistic expectations of child and adolescent be-
haviors, caretaker responsibilities, long-term costs of inappropriate par-
ent skills, and family isolation. She recommended programs that en-
hance parent-child bonding, emotional ties, and improved communica-
tion skills. She further suggested programs that would help parents
increase coping skills, especially when under stress. Peer support, ac-
cess to social services and medical/health resources, and parental home-
management/child-management skills are also recommended.
Hodson and Skeen (1987) reviewed the research and theories of
236 MAXINE R. NEWMAN and JOHN R. LUTZKER
child sexual abuse and found that family life education could make a
significant contribution to the prevention and treatment of sexual abuse.
Thus, family life educators and other professionals who routinely work
with children and their families must be supplied with information on
the prevention and detection of childhood sexual abuse, including such
issues as (1) the theoretical explanations of childhood sexual abuse, (2)
the characteristics of abusers, (3) the characteristics of victims, (4) the
means of detecting abuse, and (5) the prevention and response interven-
tions.
In his presidential address for the International Society for Preven-
tion of Child Abuse and Neglect, Ferrier (1986) suggested that, even
though the need for tertiary prevention exists because "accidents always
occur," he urged that professionals agree that cases of abuse and neglect
should only be accidents and "rare ones at that (p. 280)." He noted the
difficulty in secondary prevention because though the identifying of
high-risk groups is attractive it may be, "fraught with the danger of self-
fulfilling prophecy (p. 281)." Ferrier (1986) stated that his "personal bias
is primary prevention." He recommended using education of the gener-
al public to create a change in current attitudes, values, and beliefs about
the use of violence as an acceptable method of child-rearing as well as
heightening societal awareness of the "devastating effect of rejection or
verbal abuse." He supported the position of the American Academy of
Pediatrics in its condemnation of the lack of censorship in television to
ban the most "viciously realistic forms of violence" presented for home
viewing. Finally, he suggested that the further study of those children
who survive abuse or neglect and overcome their circumstances com-
pared with those children who are unable to overcome the abuse or
neglect, would "boost our morale and lift our spirits."
Nelson (1984) analyzed four arenas of political action on behalf of
children: the U.S. Children's Bureau, the media, state legislators, and
Congress. She found that child abuse is a "consensual issue"; that is,
everyone agrees that child abuse is a problem. It also appears to be
relatively high on the agenda of those who make policy decisions. How-
ever, she noted that such decisions are often made from the heart rather
than on the basis of empirical information or data-based studies. Thus,
although made with the best of intentions, state response often unwit-
tingly offers the least effective response.
School sexual abuse prevention programs may produce some unin-
tended consequences and dilemmas (Trudell & Whatley, 1988). This pri-
mary prevention strategy often involves the use of classroom teachers
and other elementary school personnel as instructors or guides for stu-
dents. Although child sexual abuse is a complex issue that is likely to be
PREVENTION PROGRAMS 237
cut across all such programs: (1) there were not enough appropriate
comparison groups; (2) outcome measures were poorly chosen; and (3)
there was a "failure to measure proximal programmatic objectives and
distal prevention goals." They concluded that although the programs
studied offered some exciting possibilities in the area of primary preven-
tion of child abuse and neglect, many have not proved that they could
actually accomplish this goal.
PREVENTION PROGRAMS
Tertiary prevention programs are directed toward an already diag-
nosed population and have, as their goals, the rehabilitation of the per-
petrator, keeping the family intact, and the prevention of further abuse .
Wolfe, Kaufman, Aragona, and Sandler (1981) developed an inter-
vention model for child abusers that consists of a series of step-by-step
procedures to teach abusive parents the skills required in managing
their children's activities, problem-solving and conflict resolution, anger
and impulse control, and building and maintaining positive social rela-
tionships outside the home. Wolfe and his colleagues (1981) presented a
treatment and assessment program and a series of evaluative studies.
The Kansas Child Abuse Prevention Trust Fund Program, which
was founded in 1980 (Poertner, 1987), offers programs to abusive parents
that include parenting education, public awareness (a primary preven-
tion program), home visits, and special programs for latchkey children
and children from homes in which spousal abuse has been reported.
Secondary prevention programs target an at-risk population and
have as their goals the prevention of first-time abuse or the prevention
of further abuse.
Wolfe, Edwards, Manion, and Koverola (1988) evaluated an early
intervention program for parents who were at-risk of abusing or neglect-
ing their children. The subjects were women, aged 16-25, who were
living on welfare, with young children aged 9-60 months. Even though
many of the women had male friends who visited them for different
lengths of time, only three were married or involved in permanent rela-
tionships. These women and their children were assigned to one of two
intervention groups: an information-only program offered by the child
protection agency, or a behavioral parent-training program in addition
to the other program. Although both groups showed improvement in
their child-rearing environments and child behaviors, only the women
who received the behavioral parent-training package showed significant
improvements in parenting risk and child-behaviors problems at post-
PREVENTION PROGRAMS 239
RECOMMENDATIONS
The traditional mental health concept of prevention, as we have
shown, includes three seemingly distinct levels of interventions. How-
ever, Rosenberg and Reppucci (1985) argued that tertiary level programs
that have as their target population individuals whose disorders are
already identified are not, in the strictest sense, prevention programs at
all. On the other hand, we suggest that prevention of further abuse is a
valid perspective.
In searching the literature, we found very few large-scale communi-
ty programs that had as their goal the rehabilitation of all members of the
diagnosed population. We included Project 12-Ways (Lutzker & New-
man, 1986) in our section on secondary prevention because it addresses
many of the risk factors in child abuse and neglect; however, it is an
example of how a tertiary prevention program might succeed. For exam-
ple, Project 12-Ways is community-based; it provides in-home, in-situ
services with the goal of keeping the family intact, where feasible, in-
creasing the parenting and living skills of caregivers, decreasing dys-
functional child behaviors, and decreasing abuse and/or neglect.
PREVENTION PROGRAMS 243
SUMMARY
There are three levels of child abuse or neglect prevention: tertiary,
secondary, and primary. Tertiary prevention in the area of child abuse
and neglect refers to large-scale community programs, often institution-
based, whose goal is to prevent further abuse or neglect in already cited
families. Secondary prevention in the area of child abuse and neglect is
concerned with reaching those groups or individuals who appear to be
at risk for abuse and/or neglect, either because of parental charac-
teristics, child characteristics, or environmental characteristics. The goal
of such programs is to prevent further abuse or first-time abuse. Primary
prevention in the area of child abuse and neglect considers the entire
population as its target. Media campaigns, bibliotherapy, school-based
programs, films, and social and political action are components of these
interventions. Although much work has been done that has proven of
value in the area of prevention programs for child abuse and neglect,
there is still much more to be accomplished in the future.
REFERENCES
Adelson, L. (1969). Slaughter of the innocents. New EnglandJournal of Medicine, 264, 1345-
1349 .
American Humane Association. (1963). Child abuse: Preview of a nationwide survey.
Denver, CO: Author.
Azar, S. T., Barnes, K. T., & Twentyman, C. T. (1988). Developmental outcome in phys-
ically abused children: Consequences of parental abuse on the effects of a more gener-
al breakdown in caregiving behaviors. The Behavior Therapist, 11, 27-32.
Bakwin, H. (1956). Multiple skeletal lesions in young children due to trauma. American
Journal of Pediatrics, 49, 7-15.
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Barmayer, G. H., Alderson, L. R., & Cox, W. R. (1951). Traumatic periostitis in young
children . American Journal of Pediatrics, 38, 184-190.
Barone, U. J., Greene, B. F., & Lutzker, J. R. (1986). Home safety with families being
treated for child abuse and neglect. Behavior Modification, 10, 93-114.
Bittner,S. , & Newberger, E. H. (1981). Pediatric understanding of child abu se and neglect.
Pediatrics in Review, 2, 197.
Boardman, H. E. (1961). A project to rescue children from inflicted injuries. Social Work
(Am . Ed.) 1, 43-51.
PREVENTION PROGRAMS 245
Browne, D. H. (1986). The role of stress in the commission of subsequent acts of child
abuse and neglect. Journal of Family Violence, 1, 289-297.
Caffey, J. (1946). Multiple fractures in the long bones of infants suffering from chronic
subdural hematoma . American Journal of Roentgendogy Radium Therapy and Nuclear Medi-
cine, 56, 163-174.
Campbell, R. V., Twardosz, S., Lutzker, J. R., & Cuvo, A. J. (1983, May). Comparison study
of affectionate behavior across status of abuse neglect and non-abuse neglect. Paper presented
at the annual convention of the Association for Behavior Analysis, Milwaukee,
Wiscon sin .
Campbell, R. V., O'Brien, S., Bickett, A. D., Newman, M. R., & Lutzker, J. R. (in press).
Migraine headaches and marriage and family happiness: An ecobehavioral approach.
In S. Diamond & M. Maliszewski (Eds.), Sexuality and headache: A medical-behavioral
overview. New York: International Universities Press.
Caplan, G. (1964). Principles of preventive psychiatry. New York: Basic Books.
Carstens, C. C. (1911-1912). The prevention of cruelty to children. Proceedings of the Acade-
my of Political Science, 2, 616-617.
Car stens, C. C. (1921). The development of social work for child protection . Annals of the
American Academy of Political and Social Science, 98, 137-141.
Child Protection Report. Vol. 2, January 14, 1976.
Cohn, A. A. (1986). Preventing adults from becoming sexual molesters. Child Abuse and
Neglect, 10, 559-562 .
Conte, J. R., Rosen, c., Saperstein, L., & Shermack, R. (1985). An evaluation of a program
to prevent the sexual victimization of young children. ChildAbuse and Neglect, 9, 319-
328.
Cowen, E. L., Dorr, D., Clarfield, S., Kreling, B., McWilliams, S. A. , Pokracki, D., Pratt,
M., Terrell, D. , & Wilson , A. (1973). The AML: A quick screening device for early
identification of school maladaption. American Journal of Community Psychology, 1, 12-
35.
Cumming, J., & Cumming, E. (1957). Closed ranks . Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Dachman, R. S., Halasz, M. M., Bickett, A. D., & Lutzker, J. R. (1984). A home-based
ecobehavioral parent-training and generalization package with a neglectful mother.
Education and Treatment of Children, 7, 183-202.
Elmer, E. (1960). Abused young children seen in hospitals. Social Work,S, 98-102 .
Englert, Y., Marneffe, C.; Soumenkoff, G., & Hubinont, P. O. (1985). Institutional abuse:
Tubal sterilization in a population at risk of ill-treating their children. ChildAbuse and
Neglect, 9, 31-35.
Fantuzzo, J., Azar, S. T., & Twentyman, C. T. (1985, November). Child abuse and psycho-
therapy research:Mergingsocial concerns andempirical investigation. Paper presented at the
annual convention of the Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Phila-
delphia, Pennsylvania.
Ferrier, P. E. (1986). Pres idential address: The International Society for Prevention of Child
Abuse and Neglect. Child Abuse and Neglect , 10, 279-281.
Fisher, S. H . (1958). Skeletal manifestations of parent ind uced trauma in infants and
children. Southern Medical Journal , 51, 956-960.
Gambrill, E. D. (1983). Behavioral intervention with child abuse and neglect. In M. Hersen,
R. M. Eisler, & P. M. Miller (Eds.), Progress in behavior modification: Vol. 15 (pp . 1-56).
Orlando, FL: Academic Press
Garbarino, J. (1987). Children's response to a sexual abuse prevention program: A study of
the Spiderman comic. Child Abuse and Neglect, 11, 143-148.
Ha genhoff, C.; Lowe , A., Hovell, M. F., & Rugg , D. (1987). Prevention of the teenage
246 MAXINE R. NEWMAN and JOHN R. LUTZKER
Pediatric Hospital: A program of training, research, and seroice. U.S. Department of Health
and Human Seroices. Rockville, MD: Public Health Service, Alcohol, Drug Abu se, and
Mental Health Adm inistration, National Institute of Mental Health.
Willems, E. P. (1974). Behavioral technology and behavioral ecology. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 7, 151-165.
Wolfe, D. A. , Edwards, B., Manion, C,; & Koverola, C. (1988). Early intervention for
parents at risk of child abuse and neglect: A preliminary investigation. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 40-47.
Wolfe, D., Kaufman, K., Aragona, J., & Sandler, J. (1981). The child management programfor
abusive parents: Procedures for developing a child abuse interoention program. Florida : Anna
Publi shing.
Wurtelle, S. K., Marrs, S. R., & Miller-Perrin , C. L. (1987). Practice makes per fect? The role
of participant modeling in sexual abu se prevention programs. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 599-602.
Zuravin, S. J. (1986). Residential density and urban child malt reatment: An aggregate
analysis. Journal of Family Violence, 1, 307-314 .
CHAPTER 10
INTRODUCTION
In most clinical settings, treatment for child abuse rarel y focuses solely
on th e victim, which is inherently sensible since the perpetrator of abuse
must also be treated if the risk for re-abuse is to be sign ificantl y reduced.
Particularly in the area of physical abuse, treatment has largely focused
on abusive parents, whereas little has been written about the treatment
of child victims. (For a discussion of treatment of the child abuser, see
Chapter 11 in this volume.) More recently, however, our understanding
of how to treat abused children ha s increased as data have begun to
accumulate regarding the impact of abuse on victims.
This chapter will address a number of issues related to the treat-
ment of abused children. First, we will present a discussion of defini-
tion al issues, and methodological concerns that have impeded our
efforts to develop appropriate and effective treatment modalities fol-
lowed by discussions of treatment methods and clinical themes relevant
to physically and sexually abused children. We will not address the
concept of emotional abuse in this chapter because we believe that this
type of abuse is so unspecified and ill-defined that a section on treat-
ment devoted to it is simply not possible.
249
250 ANTHONY P. MANNARINO and JUDITH A. COHEN
DEFINITIONAL PROBLEMS
METHODOLOGICAL PROBLEMS
elimination of further abuse has been the major treatment goal. Accord-
ingly, there has been a natural tendency to gear treatment toward par-
ents who perpetrate abuse against their children-an approach that has
been somewhat short-sighted, though, because it fails to recognize that
children may continue to suffer from emotional problems caused by
earlier abuse.
Other reasons may exist why treatment has not focused on child
victims. As Williams (1980) pointed out, child physical abuse has com-
monly been perceived to be in the professional domain of the physician
and not the mental health practitioner. Accordingly, there has been a
major concern about physical injuries and not necessarily the emotional
trauma caused by the abuse. Moreover, from a societal-legal perspec-
tive, some pressure has been focussed on abusive parents who are per-
ceived as needing treatment or punishment.
Recently, an increased emphasis was seen in the emotional and
behavioral difficulties that children experience as a result of having been
physically abused. Several studies have pointed to anxiety, aggression,
and social skills deficits, as being significantly correlated with physical
abuse in children (Mask, Johnson, & Kovitz, 1983; Reid, Taplin, &
Loeber, 1981; Wolfe & Mosk, 1983). With this added focus on the prob-
lems experienced by victims, a more serious interest has developed in
the kinds of interventions that would be most appropriate and effective
for this population.
Although assessment is not the focus of this chapter, it is important
to underscore that any potentially effective treatment for physically
abused children must be based on careful assessment. Walker et aI.
(1988) have stressed the significance of a thorough evaluation in order to
develop a method of intervention that is tailored to the individual needs
of each child. This issue cannot be overemphasized. Appropriate clinical
decisions should be made only after each case has been comprehen-
sively evaluated. More informed judgments can then be formed regard-
ing what kinds of treatment would potentially be most effective for the
child, parent, or family.
pulsivity (Kendall & Braswell, 1985). This model could be employed with
abused children who display any of these types of "externalizing" prob-
lems in response to physical abuse (see Spivack, Platt, & Shure, 1976,
and Meichenbaum, 1977, for specific clinical instructions on how to use
problem-solving techniques with children). In a similar light, systematic
desensitization and other relaxation techniques with documented effec-
tiveness could be used to treat anxiety and fears in children who have
been abused or neglected (Walker et al., 1988). Finally, social skills train-
ing (Michelson, Sugai, Wood, & Kazdin, 1983; Mannarino, Christy, Dur-
lak, & Magnussen, 1982) could be the treatment of choice for abused
children with interpersonal deficits .
None of the aforementioned clinical interventions have been sys-
tematically evaluated as to their potential utility with abused children.
Furthermore, empirical studies need to be conducted that examine how
the effectiveness of these clinical procedures is affected by the child's
developmental status, family situation, or other variables. Nonetheless,
the important point that we are trying to make is that clinical child
psychology and child psychiatry have a number of treatment techniques
(primarily behavioral or cognitive-behavioral) with documented clinical
efficacy and that clinicians working with abused children should not
hesitate to utilize these interventions as part of their overall treatment
strategy.
GENERAL CONSIDERATIONS
of child physical abuse, almost all of these are based on clinical rather
than empirical information. It is important to recognize that no study
exists to date that examines empirically the efficacy of any of these
therapeutic interventions in a methodologically sound manner. Thus,
although many of the clinical approaches to sexuall y abused children
seem inherently logical, there is no objective evidence that they are
indeed effective . This should be born in mind throughout the following
discussion.
In designing treatment programs, it would seem essential to define
what precisely is to be treated. One of the most important issues in the
field of child sexual abuse has been determining just what kind of prob-
lems these children experience. For many years, no one examined this
question empirically, because it was assumed that, based on clinical
experience, psychodynamic theory, or methodologically weak retrospec-
tive studies, sexually abused children had such problems as depression,
anxiety, poor self-esteem, dysfunctional families, impaired ability to
trust others, and poor assertiveness skills. Although these ideas often
seemed to fit with theoretical concepts of child sexual abuse and repre-
sented the best information available at the time , recent empirical stud-
ies have challenged some of those assumptions.
Researchers have demonstrated almost uniformly that sexually
abused children do not necessarily display significant symptoms of de -
pression, anxiety, or low self-esteem as measured by standardized self-
report measures (Cohen & Mannarino, 1988;Tufts New England Medical
Center, 1984; Einbender & Friedrich, 1989). No studies have empirically
examined such issues as tru st or assertiveness in the se child ren, no doubt
in part because such variables are difficult to measure objectively. Em-
pirical studies of family functioning of sexuall y abused children are also
lacking. Even studies that have demonstrated significant ps ycho-
pathology in sexually abused children (as measured by parental ratings
on standardized instruments), have noted that about one half of the
abused subjects were not rated as having any significant pathology
(Friedrich, Urquiza, & Beilke, 1986; Tufts New England Medical Center,
1984). Thus, it is not always obvious what problems need to be treated in
children who have been sexually abused. Also there seems to be great
variability in the type and severity of symptoms experienced.
This is not surprising if sexual abuse is conceptualized as a life event
(or series of events) rather than as a discrete psychiatric syndrome. In
this way, it is similar to experiencing a divorce or a death, and its impact
can vary enormously depending on many factors, some of which have
yet to be determined. Sexual abuse is a very diverse phenomenon. It
may be intra- or extrafamilial, vary in type of abu se, frequency, and level
258 ANTHONY P. MANNARINO and JUDITH A. COHEN
of force used. The context in which it occurs may greatly affect the
impact it has on a child. Abuse may occur in the context of a supportive
cohesive family who believe the child, or in a chaotic dysfunctional
family who either blame the child for the abuse or disbelieve him or her
altogether. It may happen to a well-adjusted socially skilled child, or to a
child who exhibited multiple behavioral, developmental, or emotional
problems prior to the abuse. Some children may be removed from the
home and go through criminal court proceedings, whereas others will
have no involvement in the criminal or child protective systems. Certain
aspects of the child's temperament, such as general adaptability to stress
and cognitive style, may influence the impact of abuse as well.
Because of the above factors, it stands to reason that child sexual
abuse would have variable effects on children. Thus, in planning treat-
ment it is not helpful to conceptualize sexual abuse as a unitary clinical
syndrome with certain constant behavioral and emotional features . Each
child and family must be evaluated carefully and individually to deter-
mine what issues are relevant and what problems need to be addressed
in therapy, rather than categorizing the child as a "sexual abuse victim"
and planning treatment around that label.
Individual Psychotherapy
The case histories of sexually abused children, which have ap-
peared in the psychoanalytic literature for over half a century, have been
of theoretical interest and may have provided insight into the dynamic
issues of some of these children. Unfortunately, because of their focus
on the analytic process, the general applicability of such studies has
been limited.
With the advent of the women's movement in the late 1960s, rape
TREATING THE ABUSED CHILD 259
Group Therapy
Many therapists have described group treatment approaches for
child sexual abuse victims. The idea of group therapy has been appeal-
ing not only for its relatively high cost-effectiveness, but also because of
the observation that many of these victims feel different from other
children after having been abused; consequently, group treatment al-
lows them to meet and interact with other abuse victims and feel more
"normal." None of the following descriptive studies included outcome
260 ANTHONY P. MANNARINO and JUDITH A. COHEN
measures, but they have expanded the kinds of approaches that could
be attempted with this population of children.
Lubell and Soong (1982) described group therapy with sexually
abused children who had been placed in foster care (presumably be-
cause the perpetrator remained at home). They noted the importance of
addressing the feelings of loss that these children experienced as a result
of removal from the parental home. They also focused on the sense of
isolation the children felt because abuse had made them feel "different"
from peers. Another issue addressed in this model was the children's
anger toward the perpetrator, their families, and the system. They
stressed the utility of having co-therapists lead these groups, not only
for the additional sense of support it provided the clients but also to give
support to the therapists.
Berliner and Ernst (1984) described a group program that high-
lighted issues of self-protection, victims' acknowledgment that abuse
had occurred, and appropriate attribution of responsibility to the per-
petrator. They used a variety of therapeutic activities, including art proj-
ects, educational exercises, and focusing on appropriate intragroup in-
teractions.
Sturkie (1983) provided a structured 8-week therapy group for la-
tency-aged children who had been sexually abused. In this format, one
major treatment theme was addressed during each session, including
believability (i.e, the importance of telling the truth about abuse until
someone believes you), guilt and responsibility, body integrity and pro-
tection, secrecy and sharing, anger, powerlessness, other life crises and
tasks, and court attendance . They stressed the value of using role play-
ing to model and practice appropriate behaviors and expression of
feelings.
Damon and Waterman (1986) designed a parallel group treatment
model for treating abused children (aged 8 and younger) and their moth-
ers in concurrent groups. They provided a clear and detailed description
of their therapeutic interventions. Thirteen modules were presented to
the children and parents, carefully coordinated, so that the mothers
would be sensitive to and prepared for what the children had addressed
each week in therapy. Some of the issues focused on included the right
to say no, the emphasis that private parts are private, whom to tell if
abuse occurs, fault and responsibility, anger and punishment, and sex
education. These authors offered a unique wealth of clinical materials,
such as stories and activities, that are particularly useful with younger
children, as well as many practical suggestions. In sum, Damon and
Waterman have provided an excellent descriptive account of an inno-
vative group therapy model for sexually abused children.
TREATING THE ABUSED CHILD 261
Family Therapy
great deal of attention, largely because of its very low (1%) reported
recidivism rate. This project primarily treats father-daughter incest
cases, and is based on work with over 4,000 such families. Treatment
consists of a joint effort involving professionals in the mental health,
criminal justice, and child protective service systems. There are self-help
components (Parents United, Daughters and Sons United) that provide
support and other ancillary services. Therapy focuses on mother-
daughter counseling to overcome their mutual alienation. A variety of
services are provided, including group, family, and individual therapy.
Giarretto's study is unusual in that it provides follow-up data. Ninety
percent of the victims were reunited with their families, and the re-
ported recidivism rate was less than 1%, a result that is very promising,
but it must be noted that these statistics do not necessarily reflect
healthy outcomes. Children returned home were not necessarily free of
psychopathology. Also, the lack of reported reabuse does not neces-
sarily imply that it did not recur. It is possible that many victims or
families, after having gone through extensive interventions, including
the child's removal from the home, may have been more hesitant to
report abuse a second time.
The major limitation of this study, however, is that 90% of the
offenders in this program took full responsibility for the sexual abuse.
This fact is unrepresentative of sexual abuse cases in general, where the
overwhelming majority of perpetrators deny either responsibility for the
abuse or that it happened at all. This suggests that Giaretto's program
treats a highly selected population, and raises questions of how applica-
ble it would be to the majority of incestuous families . Nevertheless, it is
an impressive program for many reasons, including the high level of
cooperation among agencies.
Zimmerman, Wolbert, Burgess, and Hartman (1987) described a
modified family group treatment method, used in cases in which multi-
ple children are abused by the same offender. This model makes use of
artwork within a group of intrafamilial peers, with a great deal of atten-
tion paid to the attributions the children form about the abuse. Specifi-
cally, the authors used attributional questions to examine the victims'
causal beliefs about the abuse. This model also used peer/family support
to prepare the children for court appearances.
In many families in which the nonabusive members respond
promptly and appropriately, the main issues may be in dealing with the
stress of the disclosure, possible feelings of guilt regarding the abuse,
the loss of the perpetrator from the family, and the subsequent legal
proceedings that occur with regard to custody, visitation, and criminal
charges. Porter et al. (1982) pointed out that ambivalent feelings are very
TREATING THE ABUSED CHILD 263
Educational Interventions
Many programs have included victim and family education as part
of the treatment, including such interventions as teaching self-protective
skills, assertiveness training and the right to say no to intrusive behavior
(Berliner & Ernst, 1984; Damon & Waterman, 1986; Sturkie, 1983), differ-
entiating between appropriate and inappropriate touching (Damon &
Waterman, 1986; Sturkie, 1983), what to do if abuse occurs again
(Damon & Waterman, 1986), and education about legal procedures and
going to court (Sturkie, 1983; Zimmerman et al., 1987). Parents as well as
victims often need information about the complex criminal justice sys-
tem and support with regard to court appearances. The kinds of educa-
tional tools available have increased greatly. Coloring books about recog -
nizing and reporting sexual abuse, card and board games teaching how
to avoid potentially abusive situations, and children's books and vid-
eotapes about going to court are now commonplace in centers that treat
sexually abused children. Some of these are also used in sexual abuse
prevention programs. Although the efficacy of such interventions has
yet to be demonstrated, clinicians have frequently found these aids very
helpful.
But because of the complex systems involvement in many sexual
abuse cases, often the therapist is obliged to take on the task of coordi-
nating the various services available, including making a referral for a
physical examination (to rule out or treat possible sexually transmitted
diseases or traumatic genital injuries), educating the family about, and
possibly accompanying the child to, various legal proceedings (juvenile
or criminal court hearings for charges pressed against the perpetrator,
family court hearings to resolve custody and visitation issues), and re-
maining in close contact with child protective services workers to keep
mutually informed about progress and recommendations regarding the
child's situation. Many victims' centers provide advocates who assume
some of these responsibilities, allowing the therapist to concentrate on
treatment. However, any therapist working with sexually abused chil-
dren should have a thorough understanding of the systems involved
and be prepared to spend considerable time on case management and
264 ANTHONY P. MANNARINO and JUDITH A. COHEN
Attribution Theory
Zimmerman et al. (1987) devoted a great deal of time in treatment to
the attributions children make about sexual abuse. Attributional style
has received increased attention recently among researchers. It appears
that this may be one of the important mediating factors in determining
how symptomatic a child may become following sexual abuse. At the
present time, however, there is no empirical evidence to support or
challenge this idea.
Theoretically, two related attributional factors may be involved. The
first pertains specifically to the abuse: To what does the child attribute
the abuse? Some children place full responsibility on the perpetrator,
whereas other victims feel the abuse was entirely their own fault. Many
children fall somewhere in between, believing that the abuse was basi-
cally the perpetrator's fault, but that some facets of the victim (such as
being handsome or pretty, being friendly, or being too weak to fight
back) also contributed in some degree to the abuse. The second factor is
the child's general attributional style: To what does the child attribute
typical life experiences, such as failing an exam or making a new friend?
Is the child's style to attribute such occurrences to aspects of himself or
herself, or to aspects of the outside world, or to some combination?
Future studies could focus on whether either of these attributional
factors affect significantly the development or avoidance of psycho-
pathology, and whether there is an attributional style that is optimal for
recovery from the abuse. If so, cognitive therapy approaches could be
utilized to alter the child's attributions in a way that could positively
affect outcome . Clearly, more research is needed before conclusions can
be drawn about the role of attributional style; however, it is a promising
area for future treatment designs.
Traumagenic Dynamics
Other possible directions for treatment have been suggested by
Finkelhor and Browne (1985) who discuss four concepts that are poten-
tial foci for therapy: traumatic sexualization, stigmatization, betrayal,
and powerlessness. They described these concepts as "traumagenic dy-
TREATING THE ABUSED CHILD 265
namics" that alter the child's cognitive and emotional orientation to the
world, and distort the child 's self-concept, world view, or affective abili-
ties. This conceptualization suggests that possible intervention strat-
egies could be designed to correct these cognitive and emotional distor-
tions.
Behavior Therapy
Berliner and Wheeler (1988) proposed a conceptualization that child
sexual abuse results in conditioned anxiety and socially learned mal-
adaptive responses. They suggested that effective therapy may involve
the use of established modalities, such as systematic desensitization,
relaxation training, and problem-solving training.
Finally, behavior modification programs may be useful in control-
ling many of the problematic symptoms displayed by some sexually
abused children. Aggressive behavior, sexually provocative or overt sex-
ual behavior, and enuresis are all potentially responsive to behavioral
interventions that could be implemented concurrently with other thera-
peutic strategies. Although this approach has been used by some clini-
cians/ it seems to be dismissed by many others who believe that focusing
on behavioral symptoms will obscure the underlying psychological is-
sues. It is not suggested that these psychological, abuse-related issues
are less important to address. However, it does seem critical to reduce
these types of maladaptive behaviors quickly, in order to prevent sec-
ondary problems, such as loss of peers through aggressive behavior, or
ostracism or re-abuse because of inappropriate sexual behavior.
The future direction of treatment for sexually abused children de-
pends to a large degree on the availability of empirical information. In
order to choose effective treatment approaches, there must be more
systematic data gathered with regard to the impact of abuse, and the
effect and outcome of well-defined treatment modalities.
SUMMARY
REFERENCES
Berliner, L., & Ernst, E. (1984). Group work with preadolescent sexual assault victim s. In 1.
R. Stuart & J. G. Greer (Eds .), Victims of sexual aggression: Treatment of children, women
and men (pp . 105-124). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Berliner, L., & Wheeler, J. R. (1988). Treating the effects of child sexual abuse on children.
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 2, 415-434 .
Blythe, B. J. (1983). A critique of outcome evaluation in child abuse treatment. ChildWelfare,
62, 325-335.
Cohen, J. A., & Mannarino, A. P. (1988). Psychological symptoms in sexually abused girls.
Child Abuse and Neglect, 12, 571-577.
Cohn, A. H. (1979). An evaluation of three demonstration child abu se and neglect treat-
ment programs. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 18, 283-291.
Conte, J., & Schuerman, J. R. (1987). The effects of sexual abuse on children: A multidi-
mensional view. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 2, 380-390 .
Damon, L., & Waterman, J. (1986). Parallel group treatment of children and their mothers.
In K. MacFarlane & J. Waterman (Eds .), Sexual abuse of young children: Evaluation and
treatment (pp . 244-298) . New York: Guilford Press.
Einbender, A. J., & Friedrich, W. N . (1989). The psychological functioning and behavior of
sexually abused girls . Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 155-157 .
Finkelhor, D. (1979). Sexually victimized children. New York: free Press .
Finkelhor, D., & Browne, A. (1985). The traumatic impact of child sexual abu se: A concep-
tualization. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 55, 530-541.
Frazier, D., & Levine , E. (1983). Reattachment therapy: Intervention with the very young
abused child. Psychotherapy: Theory, research and practice, 20, 90-100.
Friedrich, W. N ., Urquiza, A. J., & Beilke, R. (1986). Behavior problems in sexually abu sed
young children. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 11, 47-57.
Furniss-Tillman, T. (1983). Family proce ss in the treatment of intrafamilial child sexual
abuse. Journal of Family Therapy, 5, 263-278 .
Cabinet, L. (1983a). Child abuse treatment failures reveal need for redefinition of the
problem. Child Abuse and Neglect , 7, 395-402 .
Cabinet, L. (1983b). Shared parenting: A new paradigm for the treatment of child abuse.
Child Abuse and Neglect, 7, 403-411.
Giarretto, H . (1982). A comprehensive sexual abuse treatment program. Child Abuse and
Neglect, 6, 263-279 .
Green, A. H ., Power, E., Stonebrook, B., & Gaine s, R. (1981). Factors associated with
succe ssful and un successful intervention with child abuse in families . Child Abuse and
Neglect, 5, 45-52.
Heide, J., & Richardson, M. T. (1987). Maltreated children's developmental scores : Treat-
ment versus nontreatment. Child Abuse and Neglect , 11, 29-34 .
Huebner, E. S. (1984). A group treatment approach for abused middle school students.
Techniques, 1, 139-143.
Isaacs, C. D. (1982). Treatment of child abu se: A review of the behavioral interventions.
Journal of Applied Behavioral Analysis, 15, 273-294.
TREATING THE ABUSED CHILD 267
Kendall, P., & Braswell, L. (1985). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for impulsive children. New
York: Guilford Press .
Lubell, D., & Soon g, W. (1982). Group therapy with sexually abu sed adolescents. Canadian
Journal of Psychiatry, 27, 311-315 .
Lutzker, J. R., & Rice, J. M. (1984). Project 12-Ways: Measuring outcome of a large in-home
service for treatment and prevention of child abuse and neglect. Child Abuse and
Neglect, 8, 519-524 .
Mannarino, A. P., Christy, M., Durlak, J. A., & Magnussen, M. G. (1982). Evaluation of
social competence training in the schools. Journal of School Psychology, 20, 11-19.
Mannarino, A. P., & Cohen, J. A. (1986). A clinical-demographic study of sexually abused
children. Child Abuse and Neglect , 10, 17-28.
Mask, E., Johnson, C.; & Kovitz , K. (1983). A comparison of the mother-child interactions
of physically abused and non-abused children during play and task situations. Journal
of Clinical Child Psychology, 12, 337-346 .
Meichenbaum, D. (1977). Cognitive-behavior modification: An integrative approach. New York:
Plenum Press.
Michelson, L. , Sugai, D. P., Wood, R. P., & Kazdin , A. E. (1983). Social skillsassessment and
training with children: An empirically based handbook. New York: Plenum Press.
Mitchum, N. T. (1987). Developmental play therapy: A treatment approach for child vic-
tims of sexual molestation. Journal of Counseling and Development, 65, 320-321.
Myer, M. H . (1985). A new look at mothers of incest victims . Journal of Social Work and
Human Sexuality, 3, 47-58.
Myers, P. A., Brandner, A., & Templin, K. (1985). Developmental milestones in abused
children and their improvement with a family-oriented approach to the treatment of
child abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect, 9, 245-250.
Pelcovitz, D., Kaplan, S., Samit , C.; Krieger, R. , & Cornelius, D. (1984). Adolescent abuse:
Family structure and implications for treatment. Journal of the American Academy of
Child Psychiatry, 23, 85-90.
Porter, F. S., Bleck, L. c., & Sgroi, S. M. (1982). Treatment of the sexually abused child . In
S. M. Sgroi (Ed.), Handbook of clinical intervention in child sexual abuse (pp . 113-148).
Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Reid, J. B., Taplin, P. S., & Loeber, R. (1981). A social interactional approach to the treat-
ment of abu sive families. In R. B. Stua rt (Ed.), Violent behavior: Social learning approaches
to prediction , management, and treatment (pp . 83-101) . New York: Brunner/Maze!.
Sgroi, S. M. (1982). Family treatment of child sexual abuse. Journal of Social Work and Human
Sexuality, 1, 109-128 .
Shelton, P. R. (1982). Separation and treatment of child-abusing families . Family Therapy, 9,
53-60.
Smith, J. E., Rachman, S. J., & Yule, B. (1984). Non-accidental injury to children-III.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 22, 367-383.
Spivack, G., Platt, J. J., & Shure, M. B. (1976). Theproblem-solving approach to adjustment. San
Francisco : Jossey-Bass.
Sturkie, K. (1983). Structured group treatment for sexually abused children. Health and
Social Work, 8, 299-308 .
Trowell, J., & Castle, R. L. (1981). Treating abused children. ChildAbuseand Neglect, 5,187-
192.
Tufts New England Medical Center, Division of Child Psychiatry (1984). Sexuallyexploited
children: Service and research project. Final report for the Office of Juvenile Justice and
Delinquency Prevention. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice.
268 ANTHONY P. MANNARINO and JUDITH A. COHEN
Walker, C. E., Bonner, B. L., & Kaufman, K. L. (1988). The physically and sexually abused
child: Evaluation and treatment. New York: Pergamon Press .
Williams, G. J. (1980). Management and treatment of parental abu sive and neglect of
children. In G. J. Williams & J. Money (Eds .), Traumatic abuse and neglect of children at
home (pp . 483-487). Baltimore : Johns Hopkins University Press .
Wolfe, D., & Mosk, M. (1983). Behavioral comparisons of children from abu sive and
distressed families . Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 702-708 .
Zimmerman, M. L., Wolbert, W. A., Burgess, A. w., & Hartman, C. R. (1987). Art and
group work: Interventions for multiple victim s of child molestation (Part II). Archivesof
Psychiatric Nursing, 1, 40-46.
CHAPTER 11
INTRODUCTION
Scientific, empirically ba sed approache s to the treatment of any problem
depend upon the adequacy of theoretical models concerning the cause of
the problem. As discussed in other chapters, for many years there was a
paucity of sou n d empirical data that could be used either to account for
the presence of child abusive patterns or to gu ide the de velopment of
effective interventions for child abus ers. Prior to the mid-1970 s, mo st
models of child abuse were unifactorial in nature and attempted to
predict the occurrence of child maltreatment in familie s from single
etiological causes, such as parent psychopathology or sociological disad-
van tage (see reviews by Belsky, 1980; Burgess, 1979; Parke & Collmer,
1975). Treatment interventions based on such models also tended to be
unifactorial, usually emphasizing treatment of a parent's postulated un-
derlying psychiatric disorder or the alleviation of socioeconomic distress .
However, most of these therapeutic interventions were unevaluated or
investigated only with uncontrolled and anecdotal outcome reports.
Over the past 10 years, theories of child abuse have become more
ecological and multifactorial, stressing the functional interplay between
a variety of child characteristics; parent characteristics, cognitive-behav-
ioral skills, and coping strategies; and environmental influences on the
269
270 JEFFREY A. KELLY
family (Burgess, 1979; Burgess & Richardson, 1984; Kelly, 1983; Parke &
Collmer, 1975; Wolfe, 1985). The emergence of these more complex so-
cial-interactional conceptual models of child abuse has given rise to new
approaches to the treatment of child abusive parents. Because outcomes
of therapy for child maltreating parents have been studied scientifically
for only a short period of time, this field is still in its earliest stages, and
many questions remain uninvestigated. However, sufficient information
has already been gained to guide the development of clinical research
and intervention for child abusive parents. Thus, this chapter will briefly
discuss conceptual and practical issues related to treatment for child
abusers, review empirical research on therapy outcomes for this popula-
tion, and consider topics important for further treatment research.
ventory (ECBI) (Eyberg & Ross, 1978) and were evaluated on global
measures of family functioning by social service caseworkers unin-
volved in the parent-training program. In addition, all families in the
study were observed in their homes during free (unstructured) interac-
tion periods, during tasks in which the parent taught the child a new
puzzle, and during tasks in which the parent was asked to elicit the
child's compliance in picking up toys. All observed interactions were
coded for the frequency of parent positive reinforcement techniques,
appropriate commands and prompts, and appropriate punishment.
Following these baseline assessments, parents in the experimental
condition attended a series of group sessions that provided instruction
in child development and management, behavioral principles applied to
parenting (such as positive reinforcement, time out, shaping, and ap-
propriate punishment), problem-solving of child-management difficul-
ties, and relaxation and self-management skills. In addition to the week-
ly groups, each family received individual in-home child-management
training tailored to specific problems of that family. At the conclusion of
the experimental group's intervention, all families were reassessed on
the same set of measures that had been used during baseline. Significant
improvement was found for experimental group parents relative to the
control group parents on skill behaviors during the observed parent-
child interaction tasks, and changes were well maintained at a 10-week
followup of five of the parents. Differential change was not found be-
tween the child-management training and control groups for ECBI
scores or caseworker ratings, perhaps because of the relatively small
sample sizes in the study. Based on a I-year follow-up inspection of
welfare department records, none of the families that received treatment
was suspected of further abuse.
The Wolfe et al. (1981) project, like most others reported in the
literature on child-management training for abusive parents, taught par-
enting skills based largely on operant principles. Brunk, Henggeler, and
Whelan (1987) compared the relative efficacy of operant-behavioral
child-management training with a multisystemic family therapy inter-
vention approach in a sample of 33 families with a history of child abuse
or neglect Parents in the child-management training intervention at-
tended an 8-session series of groups, modeled after the group treatment
used by Wolfe et al. (1981), which taught general behavioral parenting
skills and skills for handling specific child problems experienced by each
family. In-the-home training was not conducted with individual fami-
lies. Subjects in the multisystemic therapy condition received eight ses-
sions of family therapy, conducted with individual family units rather
than in groups; the intervention was based on family restructuring prin-
276 JEFFREY A. KELLY
ciples (d. Haley, 1976; Minuchin, 1974) rather than behavioral training.
Before and after intervention, all parents were assessed using symptom
and child behavior problem checklists, family environment and social
system self-report measures, and treatment satisfaction questionnaires.
In addition, observations were made of parent-child interactions during
a 10-minute, in-home talk in which the parent was asked to teach his or
her child block designs of increasing difficulty level. These interactions
were rated for verbal and nonverbal measures of parental control style
using the Schaffer and Crook (1979) coding system.
Brunk et al. (1987) found that the child-management training and
the multisystemic family therapy produced significant and comparable
reductions in symptoms of parents' emotional distress, reduced overall
family stress, and reduced severity of identified problems. The multi-
systemic family therapy intervention produced greater improvement
than did the child-management training on observational measures of
effectiveness during the parent-child interaction task. Parents who re-
ceived this treatment showed increased effectiveness in child control
skills and were more appropriately responsive to child behavior. Also,
some collateral improvements in child behavior were observed. How-
ever, these results must be viewed as preliminary because child-man-
agement training was conducted in groups, whereas the family therapy
intervention was provided to individual family units, no follow-up was
conducted, and a no-treatment control was not employed. Nonetheless,
the results of Brunk et al. (1987) suggest that attention to factors beyond
child-management skill alone may produce additional improvement in
family functioning.
RESEARCH CRITIQUE
Research on the effects of treatment for child abusive parents is
difficult for a variety of reasons. Acts of parental violence typically occur
in private. Although the physical consequences of abuse-child inju-
ry-can be detected in some cases, the vast majority of instances of
parental violence are neither detected nor directly observable. Parent
reports of violent behavior are susceptible to bias, inaccuracy, and distor-
tion. Lack of candor is especially possible when a parent is under inves-
tigation or is involved in judicial processes related to child maltreatment.
For these and other reasons, research on treatment outcome with
abusive parents has relied on "probe" assessments of skill or behavioral
competence in situations which are presumed to have a functional rela-
tionship to abuse. As we have seen, the most common paradigm em-
ployed in recent studies involves "sampling" parent and child behavior
in naturalistic or staged interaction tasks, either in the home or in a clinic
setting. To the extent that these parent-child interaction tasks approxi-
mate the real situations that give rise to family conflict and abuse, they
constitute a valid assessment mode. To the extent that improvements in
parent skill observed in these interactions following treatment then gen-
eralize to in vivo (and unobserved) interactions in the home that could
actually trigger violence, the impact of therapy is also substantiated.
It now seems clear that training parents in child-management skills
does produce positive change in parent-child interactional style during
observational assessments and that change maintains over time when
TREATING THE CHILD ABUSER 281
the same assessments are repeated. How adequately the effects of this
training generalize outside formal interaction assessment observation
tasks is largely unknown because data collection has relied primarily on
performance in these tasks . Documentation that no further known
abuse was reported to authorities over a follow-up period is a positive
but, at best, imprecise indicator of intervention outcome because indi-
vidual acts of maltreatment are unlikely to be reported to authorities.
Increased attention to multimodal outcome assessment is needed in the
child abuser treatment literature. Confidence that the effects of child-
management training for abusers lessen violence and improve family
functioning would be increased if changes in parent-child interaction
skill were systematically corroborated with (1) ongoing self-monitoring
of behavior and child-related problems made by the parent and perhaps
also by the child; (2) evaluations made by significant others who regular-
ly see family members such as teachers or relatives; (3) physical, emo-
tional , and behavioral characteristics of the child; (4) measures sensitive
to family stress and functioning; or (5) parent performance during
novel, challenging parent-child interaction tasks different from those
repeatedly practiced in training. Establishing change across several such
measures following treatment could serve to validate more efficiently
and corroborate the clinical impact of an intervention.
The issue of generality versus specificity in parent training focus
may also playa role in clinical outcomes when treating child abusers.
Some abusive parents appear to exhibit generalized skill deficiencies
when interacting with their children in everyday, routine situations
(Burgess, 1979; Burgess & Conger, 1978). However, there may also be
specific and idiosyncratic child-problem situations that carry a high
probability for violence or inappropriate handling within a given family.
For example, Wolfe, Kelly, and Drabman (1981) described a case in which
spankings and beatings were administered by a parent when her chil-
dren "dawdled" excessively in the morning and at bedtime. Treatment
in the case entailed an analysis of this particular child-management
problem and the development of a parent-training intervention specifi-
cally tailored to it. The impact of child-management approaches would
appear to be greatest when intervention is relevant to those specific
conflict areas, child problems, and parent skill deficits known to affect
the family being treated (Kelly, 1983). On the other hand, an overly
narrow focus in child-management training may equip a parent to han-
dle only a few isolated problem situations, but fail to grasp underlying
principles needed to appropriately deal with other problems that were
not specifically covered in training.
282 JEFFREY A. KELLY
SUMMARY
Significant strides have recently been made in the development of
treatment approaches for child abusive parents. A number of outcome
studies now demonstrate the utility of child-management, parent-train-
ing, and other family intervention approaches in altering parents' re-
284 JEFFREY A. KELLY
REFERENCES
Alter-Reid, K., Gibbs, M. S., Lachnm eyer, J. R., Sigal, J., & Massoth , N. A. (1986). Sexual
abuse of children: A review of the empirical finding s. Clinical PsychologyReview, 6, 249-
266.
Ammerman, R. T., Van Hasselt, V. B., & Hersen, M. (1988). Maltreatment of handicapped
children: A critical review. Journal of Fam ily Violence, 3, 53-72.
Azar, S. T., Robinson , D. R., Hekinans, E., & Twentyman, G. T. (1984). Unrealistic expecta-
tion s and problems-solving ability in maltreating and compari son mothers. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 687-691.
Azrin, N. H., Holz, W. C,; & Hake, D. F. (1963). Fixed ratio punishment. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6, 141-148.
Barone, V. J., Greene, B. F., & Lutzker, J. R. (1986). Home safety with families being treated
for child abuse and neglect. Behavior Modification, 10, 93-114.
Bell, G. (1973). Parents who abuse their children. Canadian Psychiatric Association Journal, 18,
223-228.
Belsky, J. (1980). Child maltreatment: An ecological integration. American Psychologist, 35,
320-335 .
Bousha , D. M., & Twentyman, C. T. (1984). Mother-child interactional style in abuse,
neglect, and control groups: Naturalistic observations in the home. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 93, 106-114.
Brown e, A. , & Finkelhor, D. (1986). Impact of child sexual abu se: A review of the research.
Psychological Bulletin, 99, 66-77.
TREATING THE CHILD ABUSER 285
Brunk , M., Henggeler, S. W., & Whelan, J. P. (1987). Comparison of multisystemic therapy
and parent training in the brief treatment of child abuse and neglect. Journal of Consult-
ing and Clinical Psychology, 55, 171-178.
Burgess, R. L. (1979). Child abuse: A social interactional analysis. In B. B. Lahey & A. E.
Kazdin (Eds .), Advances in clinical child psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 142-172). New York:
Plenum Press.
Burges s, R. L., & Conger, R. (1978). Family interactions in abusive, neglectful, and normal
families. Child Development, 49, 1163-1173.
Burgess, R. L., & Richardson, R. A. (1984). Coercive interpersonal contingencies as deter-
minants of child abuse: Implications for treatment and prevention. In R. F. Dangel &
R. A. Polster (Eds.), Behavioral parent training: Issues in research and practice (pp . 239-
259). New York: Guilford Press .
Crimmins, D. B., Bradlyn, A. S., St. Lawrence, J. S., & Kelly, J. A. (1984). In-clinic training
to improve the parent-child interaction skills of a neglectful mother. Child Abuse and
Neglect, 8, 533-539.
Dawson, B., de Armas, A., McGrath, M. L., & Kelly, J. A. (1986). Cognitive problem-
solving training to improve the child-care judgment of child neglectful parents. Journal
of Family Violence, 1, 209-221.
Denicola, J., & Sandler, J. (1980). Training abusive parents in child management and self-
control skills. Behavior Therapy, 11, 263-270.
Disbrow, M. A., Doerr, H ., & Caulfield, C. (1977). Measuring the components of parents'
potential for child abuse and neglect. Child Abuse and Neglect, 1, 279-296.
D'Zurilla, T. J. , & Goldfried, M. R. (1971). Problem solving and behavior modification.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 78, 107-126.
Eyberg , S. M., & Ross, A. W. (1978). Assessment of child behavior problems: The valida-
tion of a new inventory. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 7, 113-116.
Finkelhor, D., & Hotaling, G. T. (1984). Sexual abuse in the national incidence study of
child abuse and neglect: An appraisal. Child Abuse Neglect, 8, 23-33.
Frodi, A. , & Lamb, M. E. (1980). Child abusers' responses to infant smiles and cries . Child
Development, 51, 238-241.
Garbarino, J. (1976). A preliminary study of some ecological correlates of child abuse: The
impact of socioeconomic stress on mothers. Child Development, 47, 178-185.
Garbarino, J., & Sherman, D. (1980). High risk neighborhoods and high risk families: The
human ecology of child maltreatment. Child Development, 51, 188-198.
Gelles, R. J. (1978). Violence toward children in the United States. American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 48, 580-592.
Giovannoni, J., & Billingsley, A. (1970). Child neglect among the poor: A study of parental
inadequacy in families of three ethnic groups. Child Welfare, 49, 196-204.
Haley, J. (1976). Problem solving therapy. San Francisco: [ossey-Bass .
Helfer, R. E. (1973). The etiology of child abuse. Pediatrics, 51, 777.
Kelly, J. A. (1983). Treating child-abusive families: Intervention based on skills-training principles.
New York: Plenum Press.
Kempe , C. H. (1973). A practical approach to protection of the abused child and rehabilita-
tion of the abusing parent. Pediatrics, 51, 804-812.
Kempe, R. S., & Kempe, C. H. (1978). Childabuse. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Kirkham, M. A., Schinke, S. P., Schilling II, R. F., Meltzer, N. J., & Norelius, K. L. (1986).
Cognitive-behavioral skills, social supports, and child abuse potential among mothers
of handicapped children. Journal of Family Violence, 1, 235-245.
Kolko, D. J. (1987). Treatment of child sexual abuse: Programs, progress, and prospects.
Journal of Family Violence, 2, 303-318.
286 JEFFREY A. KELLY
Lahey, B. B., Conger, R. D., Atkeson, B. M., & Treiber, F. A. (1984). Parenting behavior
and emotional status of physically abusive mothers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 52, 1062-1071.
Lutzker, J. R., & Rice, J. M. (1984). Project 12-ways: Measuring outcome of a large-scale in-
home service for the treatment and prevention of child abuse and neglect. ChildAbuse
and Neglect 8, 519-524.
Lutzker, J. R., & Rice, J. M. (1987). Using recidivism data to evaluate project 12-ways: An
ecobehavioral approach to the treatment and prevention of child abuse and neglect.
Journal of Family Violence, 2, 283-290.
Lutzker, J. R., & Rice, J. M. (in press) . Project 12-ways: Measuring outcome of a large in-
home service program for the treatment and prevention of child abuse and neglect .
Child Abuse and Neglect.
Lutzker, J. R., Frame, R. E., & Rice, J. M. (1982). Project 12-ways: An ecobehav ioral
approach to the treatment and prevention of child abuse and neglect. Education and
Treatment of Children.B, 141-155 .
Lutzker, J. R., Wesch, D., & Rice, J. M. (1984). A review of project 12-ways: An eco-
behavioral approach to the treatment and prevention of child abuse and neglect .
Advances in Behavior Research and Therapy, 6, 63-73 .
Mash, E. J., Johnston, C.; & Kovitz, K. (1983). A comparison of the mother-child interac-
tions of physically abused and non-abused children during play and task situations.
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 12, 337-346.
Mastria , E. 0 ., Mastria, M. A., & Harkins, J. C. (1979). Treatment of child abuse by
behavioral intervention: A case report. Child Welfare, 58, 253-261.
Mayhall, P. D., & Norgard, K. E. (1983). Childabuse and neglect: Sharing responsibility. New
York: Wiley.
Minuchin, S. (1974). Families and family therapy. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Mrazek, P. B. (1983). Sexual abuse of children. In Lahey, B. B. & Kazdin, A. E. (Eds.),
Advances in clinical child psychology (Vo!. 6, pp . 199-215). New York: Plenum Press .
Parke, R., & Collmer, M. (1975). Child abuse : An interdisciplinary analysis. In M.
Hetherington (Ed.), Review of childdevelopment research (Vol, 5, pp. 509-590). Chicago:
University of Chicago Press .
Patterson, G. R. (1977). A performance theory for coercive family interaction. In R. Cairns
(Ed.), Social interactions:Methods, analysis, and illustrations. Society for Research in Child
Development Monographs, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL.
Patterson, G. R., Ray, R., Shaw, D., & Cobb, T. (1969). A manual forcoding familyinteractions.
New York: Microfiche Publications.
Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. B., Jones, R. R., & Conger, R. E. (1975). A social learningapproach to
family intervention (Vo!. 1). Eugene, OR: Castalia .
Reid, J. B., Taplin, P. S., & Lorber, R. (1981). A social interactional approach to the treat -
ment of abusive families. In R. B. Stuart (Ed.), Violent behavior: Social learningapproaches
to prediction, management, and treatment (pp. 83-101). New York: Brunner/Maze!.
Reynolds, G. S. (1975). A primer of operant conditioning (rev. ed .). Glenview, IL: Scott,
Foresman.
Rohrbeck, C. A., & Twentyman, C. T. (1986). Multimodal assessment of impulsiveness in
abus ing, neglecting, and nonmaItreating mothers and their preschool children. Jour-
nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 231-236.
Sandler, J., Van Dercar, C; & Milhoan , M. (1978). Training child abusers in the use of
positive reinforcement practices . Behaviour Research and Therapy, 16, 169-175.
Schaffer, H. R., & Crook, C. K. (1979). Maternal control techniques in a directed play
situation. Child Development, 50, 989-996 .
TREATING THE CHILD ABUSER 287
Scott, W. O. N ., Baer, G., Chri stoff, K A., & Kelly, J. A. (1984). The use of skills training
pro cedures in th e treatment of a child abu sive par ent: A sys tematic case study. Journal
of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 15, 329-336.
Shure, M. B., & Spivak, G. (1972). Mean s-end th inking, adjus tment, and social class
amo ng elementary school-aged children . Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
38, 348-353.
Terting er, D. A., Gre ene, B. F., & Lutzker, J. R. (1984). Home safety: Development and
validation of one component of an ecobehavioral treatment program for abused and
negl ected children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 17, 159-174.
Wahler, R. G. (1980). The ins ular mother: Her problem in parent child treatment. Journalof
Applied Behavior Analysis, 13, 207-21 9.
Wahler, R. G., Leske, G. , & Rogers, E. S. (1979). The insular family: A deviance su ppo rt
sys tem for oppositional children . In L. A. Hamerlynch (Ed .), Behavioral systems for the
developmentally disabled: I. School and family environments. New York: Brunner /Maze!.
Wolfe, D. A. (1985). Child-abusive parents: An empirical review and anal ysis. Psychological
Bulletin, 97, 462-482.
Wolfe, D. A. , Kelly, J. A., & Drabman, R. S. (1981). "Beat the buz zer": A method for
tra ining an abusive mother to decrease recurrent child conflict s. Journal of Clinical Child
Psychology, 10, 114-116.
Wolfe, D. A., Fairbank, J. A., Kelly, J. A. , & Bradlyn, A. S. (1983). Child abu sive parents:
Physiological responses to stre ssful and nonstre ssful behavior in children. Behavioral
Assessment,S, 363-371.
Wolfe, D. A., & Sandler, J. (1981). Training abu sive parents in effective child management.
Behavior Modification, S, 320-335.
Wolfe, D. A., Sandler, J., & Kaufman, K. (1981). A competen cy-based parent training
pro gram for child abu sers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 633-640.
Wolfe, D. A., St. Lawrence, J. S., Graves, K., Brehon y, K , Bradlyn , A. S., & Kelly, J. A.
(1982). Intensive behavioral parent train ing for a child abu sive mother. Behavior Thera-
py, 13, 438-451.
PART V
CONCLUSIONS
CHAPTER 12
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
JAMES GARBARINO
INTRODUCTION
In this chapter we will consider future directions in understanding
factors contributing to child abuse and neglect. What does the future
hold for this important field? We may examine five such directions:
expanding definitions, increasing polarization of family experiences,
proliferating linkage between child maltreatment and other dimensions
of developmental risk, intensifying debate over the nature of communi-
ty responsibility for children, and growing importance of psychological
maltreatment as an integrating concept in the study of child abuse and
neglect.
1. Expanding definitions . Child maltreatment is a social judgment.
Thus, we create rather than discover categories of abuse and
neglect.
2. Polarization of family experiences. Socioeconomic polarization, cou-
pled with geographic segregation, predicts increasing concentra-
tion of risk for child maltreatment among low resource, high-
stress families, who are increasingly estranged from affluent and
socially connected families.
3. Proliferating linkage. As the definition of child abuse and neglect
JAMES GARBARINO • Erikson Institute for Advanced Study in Child Development, Chi-
cago, Illinois 60610.
291
292 JAMES GARBARINO
EXPANDING DEFINITIONS
Child maltreatment is not a natural fact; it is a social judgment. In
the future we must attend more to this basic principle. We do not "dis-
cover" child abuse and neglect; rather, we "create" it. Instead of being
some set of objective categories of action (as is sometimes implied by
standard definitions of "acts that harm the child"), child maltreatment is
the product of child advocacy in raising the minimal standard of care for
children. It is a social judgment that particular patterns of behavior are
sufficiently inappropriate and dangerous as to warrant community ac-
tion. Some parental treatment of children is judged to be inappropriate
but not dangerous (e.g ., letting children watch violently explicit televi-
sion or permissively indulging children). Other potential treatment is
dangerous but not thought inappropriate (e.g., playing football, or cir-
cumcision). To call something child maltreatment means that it meets
both criteria (e.g., beating a child with board, taking sexually explicit
pictures of the child, tying the child to a bed).
The key to this process is its historical dimension. It moves forward
through a series of negotiated settlements between professional exper-
tise and citizen values. We have seen this process at work actively in the
last 25 years, as we see clearly in the case of vehicular neglect.
What proportion of injuries to children as occupants of automobiles
were the result of child neglect in 1959? Virtually none: We did not have
a minimal standard of care for children in automobiles then. We do now,
and most injuries are now neglect related (because most injuries-and
FUTURE DIRECTIONS 293
PROLIFERATING LINKAGE
Over the last twenty years child maltreatment has emerged as a
central theme in efforts to understand the origins and consequences of
FUTURE DIRECTIONS 295
PSYCHOLOGICAL MALTREATMENT
As the study of children at-risk matures, I believe it will turn in-
creasingly to the concept of psychological maltreatment as its unifying
theme. If we can set minimal standards of care that address directly
emotional and intellectual development, identity, and self-esteem, we
as a society will have arrived at a mature conception of the social dimen-
sion of normality. Armed with this conception, we will be able to formu-
late better policy and practice for preventing developmental risk.
A national survey revealed that roughly three quarters of American
adults believe that repeated yelling or swearing at a child leads to long-
term emotional problems for the child much of the time (National Com-
mittee for Prevention of Child Abuse, 1987). This is the cornerstone for
community action to prevent one form of psychological maltreatment.
The future could provide increasing specificity in research and program-
ming around the component concepts of psychological maltreatment:
rejection, terrorization, ignoring, isolation, and corruption (Brassard,
Germain, & Hart, 1987; Garbarino, Guttmann, & Seeley, 1986). Such a
development would advance our understanding of developmental risk
and child maltreatment.
SUMMARY
Child maltreatment has emerged as a core issue for those of us
concerned with the quality of life for children. Virtually nonexistent as a
topic of study by students of child development until the 1970s, it has
come to center stage. The future promises to continue this trend, with
growing attention to issues of social context (defin itions, polarizations of
family experience and concentration of risk , and community responsibil-
ity) and the psychological processes linking maltreatment to develop-
ment.
REFERENCES
Brassard , M., Germain, R., & Hart, S. (Eds.). (1986). Psychological maltreatment of children
and youth. New York: Pergamon Press.
298 JAMES GARBARINO
Garbarino, J. (1988). The future as if it really mattered. Longmont, CO: Bookmakers Guild .
Garbarino, J., Guttman, E., & Seeley, J. (1986). The psychologically battered child: Strategies for
identification, assessment, and intervention. San Francisco: Iosey-Bass.
National Committee for Prevention of Child Abuse (1987). Public attitudes andactions regard-
ing child abuse and its prevention. Chicago, IL: Louis Harris Public Opinion Poll.
Olds, D., Henderson, C.; Chamberlin, R., & Tatelbaum, R. (1986). Preventing child abuse
and neglect: A randomized trial of nurse home visitation. Pediatrics, 78, 65-78 .
Straus, M., & Gelles, R. (1986). Societal change and change in family violence from 1975to
1985 as revealed by two national surveys. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 48, 465-
479.
AUTHOR INDEX
Abbott, D.A ., 235, 247 Anderson, c.. 180, 195 Barnett, PoAo, 180, 195
Aber, J.L. , 110, 112, 129, Anderson, E.S., 159, 165 Barone, N ., 29, 50
140, 141 Andreas, T., 25, 53 Barone, VoJ., 8, 17, 239,
Achenbach, T.M. , 95, 105 Aragona, J.A., 130, 131, 244, 279, 284
Acuff, D.5., 25, 53 140, 159, 164, 238, 248 Basham, RoB o, 157, 160, 166
Adams, S.L. , 235, 247 Arend, R., 121, 140 Baskiewicz, A., 211, 218
Adams, w., 29, 50 Arkowtiz, H. , 208, 220 Bassetti, M., 86, 105
Adel son, E., 152, 167 Asp , E., 57, 80, 161, 167 Bauman, MoK., 211, 217
Adler, R, 193, 195 Atkeson, B.M., 190, 196, Baumrind, D., 174, 195
Adleson, L., 230, 244 270,286 Beardslee, w.s.. 181, 194,
Adrian, c.. 182, 185, 196 Atteberry-Bennett, J., 64, 195
Ainsworth, M.D.5., 12, 78,79 Becerra, I.M., 64, 80
16, 113-115, 140, 151, Augoustinos, M., 93, 105 Becerra, R.M o, 25, 51, 111,
164, 205, 217 Aurand, j.c.. 212, 220 142
Alderson, L.R , 230, 244 Azar, S.T., 8, 19,233,234, Beckman, P., 212, 218
Allen, J.P., 129, 140 244, 245, 270, 284 Beezley, P., 214, 219
Allen, R., 211, 212, 221 Azrin, N.H ., 272, 284 Beidel ., o.c.. 179, 198
Altemeier, W.A ., 153, 154, Beilke, R, 251, 257, 266
158, 164, 170, 203, 220 Baer, D.M ., 113, 140 Bell, Go, 270, 284
Alter-Reid, K., 283, 284 Baer, G., 277, 282, 287 Bell, x.. 8, 18
Alvy, K.T., 64, 78, 100, 105 Baharal, R., 130, 133, 140 Bell, RQ o, 171, 195
American Association for Baker, L., 177, 178, 195, 196 Belsky, J., 4, 6, 13, 17, 91,
Protecting Children, 28- Bakwin, H ., 230, 244 104, 105, 109, 113, 115,
31,38,42,48-51,199,217 Balla, Do, 194, 196 118, 136, 137, 139, 140,
American Humane Asso- Ballen, Eo, 243, 246 143, 146, 151, 152, 154,
ciation, 7, 17, 28, 38, 39, Bandura, s ., 113, 140, 163,165,202,217,269,284
51 233,244 Bemporad, J. 181, 195
Amish, P.L., 89, 90, 108 Bank, L., 127, 144 Benedict, M ol., 211, 221
Ammerman, RT., 3, 5, 6, Barahal, RMo, 159, 165 Berk, RA., 60, 82
9, 11-14, 17, 97, 105, Barbieri , M.K., 71, 79 Berliner, L., 71, 73, 79,
199, 202, 207, 211-217, Barmayer, c .n.. 230, 244 260, 263, 265, 266
220, 283, 284 Barnes, K.To, 233, 234, 244 Besharov, o .j. 4, 17, 60,
Andereck, N.D., 41, 52 Barnett, D., 111, 118, 141 62, 64, 65, 79, 80
299
300 AUTHOR INDEX
Best, CL., 93, 94, 96, 108 Bromet, E.J., 157, 165 Caspi, A., 151, 155, 162,
Bickett, AD., 239, 245, Bronfenbrenner, U., 33, 166
247 51, 151, 161, 162, 165 Cassisi, J.E., 5, 9, 17, 199,
Biglan, A., 154, 168 Brooks, B., 43, 44, 46, 53 207, 216, 217
Bijou, s.w. 113, 140 Brown, C, 211, 218 Castle, R.L. , 251, 267
Billings, AG., 154, 165 Brown, R.H ., 63, 79 Catalano, R., 162, 170
Billingsley, A, 271, 285 Browne, A., 76, 77, 79, Caulfield, C, 128, 142,
Birrell, J., 213, 217 264, 266, 283, 284 270, 271, 285
Birrell, R., 213, 217 Browne, D.H., 212, 217, Ceresnie, 5., 6, 12, 19,
Bittner,S., 235, 244 234,245 159, 170, 214, 215, 220
Blackford, KA , 176, 195 Brunk, M., 275, 276, 285 Cerny, J.A., 179, 197
Bleck, L.C, 259, 262, 267 Brunnquell, D., 10, 12, 17, Chalmers, M.A., 86, 87,
Blehar, M.C, 114, 115, 154, 156, 158, 165, 166, 93, 94, 96, 107
140, 151, 164, 205, 217 204, 206, 216, 218 Chamberlin, R., 297, 298
Block, J., 155, 165 Bueno, G., 239, 247 Chandler, M., 151, 163,
Block, J.H., 155, 165 Bulkley, J., 58, 70, 79, 80 169
Blumberg, M.L., 135, 141 Burgdorff, K, 134, 141 Chapman, M., 188, 197
Blythe, B.J., 250, 251, 266 Burge, D., 182, 185, 196 Charnov, E.L., 115, 143
Boardman, H .E., 230, 244 Burger, A.M., 136, 143 Chesney, B.H., 211, 220
Bohmery S.; 156, 168 Burgess, A.W., 262-264, Child Protection and
Bonner, B.L., 15, 19, 250, 268 Treatment Act (Public
252, 255, 256, 268 Burgess, R.L., 95, 13, 17, Law 93-247), 66-68, 79
Boriskin, J.A., 4, 6, 12, 18, 105, 110, 113, 130, 131, Child Protection Report,
203,218 134, 137, 139, 141, 146, 231,245
Bourne, R., 56, 57, 79, 159, 165, 170, 202, 208, Child Sexual Abuse and
234, 239, 243, 247 213, 217, 219, 269-271 , Pornography Act, 69, 79
Bousha, D.M. , 8, 17, 128, 281, 285 Children's Defense Fund,
141, 208, 217, 272, 284 Burk, J.P., 182, 196 57,79
Bowlby, J.B., 113, 141 Burke, AE., 179, 197 Chisolm, B.A, 59, 62, 63, 79
Bradley, R.H ., 161, 165 Burkhart, B.R., 46, 51 Christensen, A., 189, 195
Bradlyn, AS., 270, 273, Bush, B.A., 23 Christoff, K.A., 277, 282,
274, 285, 287 Buttenweiser, E., 86, 87, 287
Brande~ A., 253, 267 93, 94, 96, 107 Christoffel, KK, 32, 51
Brassard, J., 160, 165 Christy, M., 255, 267
Brassard, M.R., 4, 17,87, Caffey, J., 230, 245 Cicchetti, D., 93, 105, 110-
94, 105, 106, 154, 168, Caldwell, B.M ., 161, 165 112, 116, 118, 124, 128-
297 Campbell, D.T., 99, 101, 130, 132, 137, 140-143,
Braswell, L., 255, 256, 267 103,105 145, 151, 153, 154, 156,
Braunling-McMorrow, D., Campbell, R.V., 15, 18, 163, 165, 168, 205, 211,
240, 246 239, 240, 243, 245, 246 217
Braunwald. KG., 112, Cantwell, D.P., 178, 195 Clarfield, 5., 227, 245
116, 118, 124, 141, 145 Caplan, G., 225-228, 245 Clark Foundation, 57, 79
Brehony, K , 273, 287 Carlsmith, J.M. , 57, 58, 61, Clark, D.C, 185, 196
Breitenbucher, M., 11, 17, 83,98,108 Clark, L.A ., 155, 170
160, 161, 163, 166, 201, Carlson, V., 112, 116, 118, Clark, M.e., 157, 160, 168
212,218 124, 141, 142, 145 Clasen, R.W., 29, 52
Bretherton, I., 115, 141, Carroll, J.C, 163, 165 Cobb, T., 273, 286
151, 165 Carson, B.A., 8, 19 Cochran, M., 160, 165
Breznitz, Z., 188, 195 Carstens, CC, 229-231, Cohen, J.A., 249, 251, 257,
Brody, G., 185, 186, 196 245 261,266
AUTHOR INDEX 301
Cohn, A.H., 61, 79, 99, Crook, c.x.. 276, 286 Dowdney, L., 136, 142
105, 232, 245, 253, 266 Cross, A.H ., 212, 218 Downey, G ., 151, 155, 166
Cohn, J.P., 187, 195 Cross, c.e.. 151, 162, 167 Drabman, R.5 ., 276, 281,
Colbus, D. , 8, 18 Crouter, A.e., 34, 51, 161, 287
Colletta, N .,D. , 154, 157, 165 Draper, P., 202, 208, 217
160, 166 Cumming, E., 226, 245 Drapier, P., 13, 17
Collins, D ., 127, 145 Cumming, J., 226, 245 Droegemueller, W., 4, 5,
Collmer, e. w., 4, 5, 19, Cummings, E.M ., 157, 7, 18, 55, 63, 81, 109,
89, 107, 109, 137, 144, 166, 188, 197 135, 143, 199, 219, 230,
192, 197, 200, 201, 219 Curtis, G ., 135, 142 246
Collmer, M ., 269, 270, 282, Cytryn, L., 183, 198 Drotar, D ., 211, 218
286 Dublin, c.c.. 135, 145
Commonwealth v. Adams, D'Zurilla, T.J., 278, 285 Dubow, E.P., 155, 166
73,79 Dachman, R.S ., 8, 18, 239, Dubowitz, H. , 23, 26, 27,
Conerly, S., 87, 105, 199, 245,246 51
200,219 Damon, L., 199, 200, 219, Dumas, J.E., 127, 142, 145,
Conger, R.D ., 95, 105, 260, 263, 266 194, 198
121, 130, 131, 141, 159, Daniels, }.H ., 153, 169 Duncan, D .P., 203, 213,
165, 170, 190, 196, 270, Dansky, L., 185, 196 219
271, 272, 281, 285, 286 Daro, D ., 32, 51, 99, 105 Durfee, M., 199, 200, 219
Conger, R.E., 272, 286 Davenport, Y.B., 183, 198 DUrlak , J.A ., 255, 267
Connecticut v. [arzbek, 73, Davidson, H .A. , 59, 64-
79 66,79,80 Easterbrooks, M.A. , 157,
Connell, D.B ., 116, 118, Davis, G .E., 102, 107 168
124, 144, 158, 169 Davis, H ., 194, 196 Eatman, R., 70, 80
Conte, J.R., 241, 245, 251, Dawson, B., 278, 279, 285 Eddelman, J., 240, 246
266 de Armas, A., 278, 279, Edelsohn, G ., 71, 82
Cook, T.D ., 99, 103, 105 285 Edwards, B., 238, 248
Cooper, S.P., 178, 195 DeConey, J.J., 62, 80 Egeland, B., 8-12, 17, 89,
Cornelius, D ., 251, 267 deLissovoy, Y., 6, 17, 89, 93, 105, 106, 110-113,
Comely, P.}., 157, 165 105, 200, 207, 218 115, 116, 124, 134, 136,
Corson, J., 65, 81 Denicola, J., 276, 282, 285 137, 142, 145, 154-161,
Costa, J.J., 60, 79 Diamond, L.J., 211, 214, 163, 165-167, 169,201,
Costello, A., 179, 198 216, 218, 219 204, 206, 212, 214, 216,
Coster, W., 112, 142 Dickens, B.M ., 61, 62, 66, 218
Cotterell, J.L. , 157, 166 80 Egolf , B., 88-90, 97, 106,
Coulter, M.L., 71, 82 Dietrich, KN ., 6, 12, 19, 107
Cowen, E.L. , 227, 245 158, 159, 166, 170, 214, Einbender, A.J., 5, 18, 199,
Cox, W.R., 230, 244 215,220 218, 257, 266
Coy v. Iowa 59, 75, 79 DiLalla , D .L. , 159, 166 Elder, G.H., [r., 151, 155,
Coyne, j.c.. 181, 195 Disbrow, M.A. , 128, 142, 162, 166, 169
Craig, M .E., 68, 81 270, 271, 285 Eldredge, R., 125, 126, 143
Crichton, L., 154, 165 Dishion, T.J., 127, 143, 144 Elmer, E., 4, 8, 17, 89, 91,
Crimmins, D .B., 274, 285 Doerr, H., 128, 142, 270, 93, 106, 110, 112, 114,
Crittenden, P.M ., 116, 118, 271,285 138, 142, 203, 218, 230,
124, 141, 153, 156, 158- Donovan, w., 204, 205, 245
160, 166, 206, 213, 217 218 Emde, R.N., 211, 218
Crnic, KA., 157, 160, 166 Dooley, D ., 162, 170 Emery, R., 177, 196
Crockenberg, S., 137, 142, Doran, L.D ., 128, 129, 144 Engfer, A., 151, 153, 157,
157, 163, 166 Dorr, D ., 227, 245 158, 161, 166, 167
302 AUTHOR INDEX
Eng lert, Y., 233, 245 Frazier, D., 253, 255, 266 George, c.. 125, 126, 142,
Epinosa, M.P., 156, 168 Freeman, M.L. , 25, 53 144, 156, 167
Erickson, M., 9, 10, 19, French, R. de S., 98, 108 Germain, RB., 4, 17, 87,
159, 166 Freud, A. , 58, 80, 98, 106 106, 297
Erickson, M.P. , 155, 167 Friedrich, WN., 4-6, 12, Ger sten , M., 112, 142
Ern st , E., 260, 263, 266 18, 199, 203, 218, 251, Giarretto, H., 261, 262,
Eron , L.D ., 155, 166 257,266 266
Estes, D., 115, 143 Fritz, G.S., 43-46, 51 Gibbens, T.E.N ., 135, 142
Estroff, T.W, 154, 159, 167 Frodi, A.M ., 12, 18, 118, Gibbs, M.S., 283, 284
Everson, M., 71, 82 129, 133, 142, 158, 167, Gil, D.G., 28, 39, 51, 86,
Eyberg, S.M. , 130, 131, 204, 205, 207, 218, 270, 106, 161, 162, 167, 213,
140, 159, 164, 275, 285 285 219
Fromuth, M.E ., 43-46, 51 Gilbreath, B., 154, 168
Factor, D.C. , 171 Furni ss-Tillman, T., 261, Gillespie, j.r, 194, 196
Fagot, B.l. , 132, 142 266 Gilliam , G., 24, 51, 134,
Fairbank, J.A., 270, 287 142, 156, 160, 161, 167
Falsey, S., 154, 164 Gabinet, L., 251, 266 Giovannoni, J.M., 25, 51,
Fantuzzo, J.W, 11, 15, 17, Gaboury, M.T., 57, 80 64, 80, 111, 142, 270, 285
234,245 Gaensbauer, T.J., 112, 116, Gladston, 135, 142
Fauber, R., 185, 186, 196 124, 142, 143, 156, 158, Glasgow, RE., 189, 195
Feiring, c.. 155, 169 167, 206, 218 Globe Newspaper Co . v.
Feldman, R., 156, 159, Gaines, R., 3, 18, 154, 159, Superior Court, 74
160, 169 167, 170, 201, 213, 218, Goins, c.. 15, 17
FeIner, R.D. , 194, 196 220, 251, 266 Goldberg, W.A ., 157, 168
Felton, D.K., 12, 18 Gallagh er, J.J., 212, 218 Goldfried, M.R, 278, 285
Ferrier, P.E., 236, 245 Gammon, G.D ., 184, 187, Gold stein, J., 58, SO, 98,
Feshbach, N .D ., 57, 80 198 106
Feshbach, S., 57, 80 Gan eles, D., 154, 168 Gold stein, M.J., 178, 196
Field, T.M., 154, 167, 170, Garbarin o, J., 5, 7, 12, 18, Gold ston , S. E., 100, 106
176, 197 24, 26, 33, 34, 51, 57, SO, Gold wyn, R., 153, 154, 169
Finkelhor, D., 27, 35, 43- 87, 91, 96, 106, 124, 135, Goodman, G.S., 72, 80
46, 51, 61, 77, 79, 80, 142, 154, 156, 160-163, Gordon , A.H., 116, 143
86, 88, 91, 106, 261, 264, 167, 170, 201, 218, 240, Gordon, D., 182, 185, 196
266, 271, 283-285 245, 270, 285, 291, 293, Gordon, M., 243, 246
Fischhoff, J., 6, 12, 19, 297,298 Gotlib, l. H ., 180, 181, 183,
159, 170, 214, 215, 220 Gardner, W.P., 115, 143 195, 196
Fisher, S.H. , 230, 245 Garmezy, N., 96, 106, 172, Gottfried , A.E. , 161, 168
Fleeson, J., 139, 145, 151, 196 Gottfried, A. W., 161, 168
170 Gath, A. , 211, 218 Gou ld, M., 154, 159, 167
Flicker, B.D ., 64, 80 Gavranidou, M., 157, 158, Graham, M.H ., 73, 80
Follingstad, D.R., 68, 81 161, 166 Grandjean, P., 57, 80
Fontana, V.J., 60, 62, 80, Gebhard, P.H ., 28, 43, 52 Grave s, K., 273, 287
203,218 Geca s, V., 161, 167 Gray, J., 91, 106
Forehand, R., 185, 186, Geffner, R , 88, 97, 106 Green, A.H., 3, 8, 18, 130,
196 Gelles , RJ., 4, 5, 18, 25, 143, 154, 159, 167, 170,
Forrest, D., 128, 129, 144 28, 29, 37, 39-42, 48-51, 191, 196, 201, 213, 218,
Fox, N.A. , 211, 218 53, 56, 80, 86, 88, 89, 91, 220, 251, 266
Fraiberg, S., 152, 158, 167 104, 106, 108, 134, 135, Green, B.V., 8, 17
Frame, R.E., 239, 246, 279, 156, 162, 170, 201, 212, Greenberg, M.T., 157, 160,
280, 282, 286 219, 234, 247, 283, 285 166
AUTHOR INDEX 303
Greene, B.F., 239, 241, HerrenkohI, E.C , 86, 88- Hyman, LA., 94, 107
244, 247, 279, 284, 287 90, 93, 95, 97, 106, 107,
Greenspan, 5.I., 152, 161, 128, 130-132, 143, 213, Iannotti, R.I., 190, 197
168 219 In re Freiberger, 74, 81
Gregg , G.5 ., 4, 8, 17, 203, Herrenkohl , R.C , 85, 86, Institute of Judicial Ad-
218 88-90, 93, 95, 97, 106, ministration /American
Griffin , n .c.. 207, 220 107, 128, 130-132, 143, Bar Association , 57, 61,
Gro ve, F., 121, 140 213,219 53,81
Gruber, K.J., 42, 51 Herrera, C , 154, 159, 167 Iowa Department of Social
Guttman , E., 26, 51, 87, Hersen , M., 3, 5, 6, 9, 12, Service s, 213, 219
96, 106, 297, 298 13, 17, 199, 207, 211- Irvin, N ., 211, 218
Gwartney-Gibbs , P.A., 217, 220, 283, 284 Isaacs, CD., 250, 251, 266
156, 168 Hershorn, M., 97, 107 Isabella, R., 115, 143
Hetherington, E.M ., 177,
Ha gan, R., 132, 142 194, 196 Jacobson, R.S., 128, 131-
Hagenhoff, C, 233, 245 Higgin s, J.E., 41, 53 133, 145
Hake, D.F., 272, 284 Hightower, A.D., 15, 17 [acobvitz, D., 89, 106, 110,
Hala sz, M.M ., 239, 245, Hiroto, D., 182, 185, 196 134, 136, 137, 142
247 Hodson, D. , 235, 246 Jaenicke , C , 182, 185, 196
Haley, J., 276, 285 Hoffman-Plotkin, D., 8, Jaffe, P.J., 186, 194, 198
Hall, O .K., 128, 130, 131, 18, 130, 132, 143, 156, Jameson, J.C , 116, 143
144, 209, 219 168 Janes, CL. , 184, 198
Hall , J.C, 207, 220 Hollenback, B.E., 190, 197 Jason, J., 41, 52
Hammen, C , 182, 185, Holz, W.C, 272, 284 [audes, P.K., 211, 214, 216,
196 Hop s, H ., 154, 168 218,219
Hammer, M. , 156, 159, Horowitz, R.M., 59, 64- Jen sen, P.5 ., 194, 196
160, 169 66, 80 John , K., 184, 187, 198
Hammond, M., 189, 198 Horwitz, B., 182, 196 John son, B., 138, 143, 213,
Hampton, R.L., 29, 37, 51 Hotaling, G.T., 35, 51, 86, 219
Harkin s, J.C , 272, 286 88, 91, 97, 106, 107, 271, Johnson, C , 252, 254, 267
Harpe r, L., 171, 195 285 Johnson , CF. , 29, 52
Harrold , M., 240, 243, 246 Hou se of Representatives Johnson , S.M ., 189, 195
Hart , S.N ., 4, 17, 87, 106, Report No . 685, 67, 81 Johnston , C, 131, 144,
154, 168, 297 Hov ell, M.F., 233, 245 159, 169, 270, 286
Hartman, C , 262-264, 268 Howes, C , 125, 126, 143, Joint Commission on Men-
Hartup, W.w., 177, 197 156, 168 tal Health of Children,
Haugaard, J.J., 27, 43-46, Hubinont, P.O ., 233, 245 226,246
52, 71, 72, 76, 81 Hu ebner, E.5 ., 253, 266 Jones, D., 97, 107
Hawkins, W.E., 203, 213, Huesmann, L.R., 155, 166 Jones, R.J., 42, 51
219 Hughes, H ., 88, 97, 106 Jones, R.R., 272, 286
Hazan, C , 152, 168 Hughs, J., 240, 246 [urecic , L., 15, 17
Heffron, W.M., 214, 219 Hunt, R.D ., 61, 82 Justice, B., 97, 107
Heide, J., 253, 266 Hunter, R., 134, 136, 137, Justice, R., 97, 107
Hekinans, E., 270, 284 143
Helfer, R.E., 282, 285 Hunter, W.M ., 71, 81, 82 Kadushin, A., 89, 90, 107,
Helgeson, V.S., 72, 80 Hurley, J., 154, 169 207,219
Henderson , C, 297, 298 Hutchings, B., 176, 196 Kahn, J., 181, 195
Henggeler, S.W., 275, 276, Hyatt, A., 157, 169 Kain , E.L., 162, 169
285 Hyde, J.N ., 153, 169 Kalichrnan, S.E., 68, 81
Hermalin, J., 243, 246 Hyd e, T.5 ., 179, 197 Kalmu ss, D., 156, 168
304 AUTHOR INDEX
Kamarck, T., 243, 246 Klein, D.N., 185, 196 Lewis , D.O., 194, 196
Kandel, E., 176, 196 Klein , M. , 203, 219 Lewis, M., 112, 124, 143,
Kaplan, M.G ., 158, 166 Klerman, G.L., 181, 187, 155, 169
Kaplan, 5.J., 154, 168, 251, 195, 198 Liddle, C , 136, 137, 144,
267 Knapp, R.E., 230, 247 153, 169
Kashani, J.H ., 182, 196 Knop, J., 176, 196 Lieberman, A., 121, 143,
Katz, M.H ., 161, 169 Knudsen, D.O., 29, 32, 52 152, 161, 168
Kaufman, J., 5, 18, 90, Knutsen, J.F., 136, 143 Light , R., 39, 52
107, 110, 128-130, 132, Kobak, R.R, 152, 168 Lightcap, J.L., 213, 219
136, 137, 139, 143, 153, Kohn, M.L. , 161, 168 Liker, J.K., 151, 162, 167
156, 168 Kolko, D.J., 240, 246, 271, Limber, 5., 72, 76- 78, 82
Kaufman, K.L., 15, 19, 283,285 Lipton, R, 158, 169
238, 248, 250, 252, 255, Korbin , J.E., 24, 52 Litz, J., 240, 246
256, 268, 274, 275, 282, Koss, M.P ., 43, 44, 46, 53 Liu, K., 32, 51
287 Kotelchuck, M., 153, 156, Loeber, R., 12, 18, 113,
Kazdin, A.E ., 8, 18, 255, 161, 168, 169 127, 128, 130, 144, 145,
267 Koverola, C, 238, 248 210, 216, 220, 252, 254,
Keller, M.B ., 181, 195 Kovitz, K., 131, 144, 159, 267, 270, 286
Kelley, M. , 211, 221 169, 252, 254, 267, 270, Long , F., 57, 80
Kelly, D.P., 71, 81 286 Long, N. , 185, 186, 196
Kelly, J.A. , 10, 14, 18, 269- Kramer, 5., 24, 52 Long, 5., 199, 200, 219
271, 274, 276-279, 281, Kreling, B., 227, 245 Lourie, R.5 ., 135, 145, 152,
282, 285, 287 Krieger, R, 251, 267 161, 168
Kempe, CH., 4, 5, 7, 18, Kuczynski, L., 128, 130, Lowe, A., 233, 245
55, 63, 81, 109, 135, 143, 131, 145, 159, 170, 188, Lubell, D., 260, 267
199, 219, 230, 246, 270, 197, 209, 220 Lubetsky, M.J., 13, 17,
285 Kumka, 161, 169 212, 214-217
Kemp e, R., 93, 106, 270, Kurland, J.A., 213, 219 Lucht, C, 231, 246
285 Lucht, CL. , 66, 81
Kendall, P., 255, 256, 267 Lachnmeyer, J.R., 283, 284 Lusk, R , 94, 107
Kennell, J., 203, 211, 218, LaGreca, 209, 220 Lutzk er, J.R , 8, 15, 17-19,
219 Lahey, B.B., 190, 194, 196, 200, 219, 225, 231, 239-
Kentucky v. Stincer, 75, 81 197, 270, 286 243, 244-247, 251, 266,
Kercher, G.A. , 43, 44, 46, Lamb, M.E., 112, 115, 116, 279, 280, 282, 284, 286,
52 124, 143, 158, 167, 204, 287
Ketcham, O.W., 59, 80 205, 218, 270, 285 Lutzker, 5.Z., 240, 246
Kidd , K.K., 180, 197 Lambert, R., 211, 219 Lynch, M.A. , 93, 107, 203,
Kievans, F., 243, 246 Lane, T.W., 102, 107 212, 219, 229, 247
Kilpatrick, D.G ., 71, 81 Last , J., 24, 52 Lyons-Ruth, K.I., 116, 118,
Kilstrom, J., 134, 136, 137, Lauderdale, M. , 34, 53 124, 144, 158, 169
143 Leach, C, 178, 195
Kinard, E.M. , 10, 18, 128, Leavitt, L., 204, 205, 218 Maccoby, E.E., 173, 174,
129, 143 Lee, CM., 183, 196 187, 198
King, M.P., 71, 81 Leiberman, P.H., 57, 61, MacFarlane, K., 199, 200,
Kinsey, A.C, 28, 43, 52 83 219
Kirkegaard-Sorensen, L., Leiderman, P.H ., 98, 108 MacMurray, B.K., 8, 19
176, 196 Lennon, M.C, 211, 221 Magnussen, M.G., 255, 267
Kirkham, M.A ., 283, 285 Leske, G., 282, 287 Main, M., 125, 126, 142,
Klaus, M. , 203, 211, 218, Levine, E., 253, 255, 266 144, 153, 154, 156, 167,
219 Levitt , M.J., 157, 160, 168 169
AUTHOR INDEX 305
Malkin , CM., 112, 116, Megson, D.A ., 8, 18, 239, Myers, P.A., 253, 267
124, 143 246
Maney, A. , 66, 68, 81 Mehrn, J.G., 136, 143 Nagi, S., 86, 107
Manion, C, 238, 248 Meichenbaum, D., 255, Nalepka, C , 29, 38, 52
Mannarino, A.P., 249, 251, 267 National Center for State
255, 257, 261, 266, 267 Melendez, L., 243, 246 Courts, 66, 82
Margolis, E.T., 185, 196 Melnick, B., 154, 169 National Center on Child
Marks, S., 41, 52 Melton, G.B., 55-59, 61, Abuse and Neglect, 24,
Marneffe, C, 233, 245 64-66, 68, 71, 72, 76-78, 28, 29, 32, 34-37, 39,
Marrs, S.R., 241, 248 81,82 48-50,52
Martin, B., 177, 194, 196 Meltzer, N .J., 283, 285 National Committee for
Martin CA., 214, 219 Meredith, W.H. , 235, 247 Prevention of Child
Martin, CE., 28, 43, 52 Merikangas, K.R., 184, Abuse, 69, 70, 82, 297,
Martin, H .P., 93, 95, 107, 187, 198 298
130, 133, 140, 159, 165, Messmer, M.C, 239, 247 Navarre, E.L., 94, 107
214, 219 Michelson, L., 255, 267 Neff , C, 204, 218
Martin, J., 89, 90, 107 Midonick, M.L. , 59, 61, Nelles, W.B., 179, 197
Martin, J.A., 173, 174, 187, 62,82 Nelson, B.J., 66, 82, 236,
198, 207, 219 Milhoan, M., 272, 273, 286 247
Marvell, T.B., 59, 80 Miller, S.H ., 41, 52 New York Social Services
Mash, E.J., 131, 144, 159, Miller-Perrin, CL., 241, Law Sec. 413, McKin-
169, 270, 286 248 ney, 64, 82
Mask, E., 252, 254, 267 Milner, J.S., 91, 107, 209, Newberger, E.H ., 29, 37,
Mason, R , 41, 53 219 51, 52, 56, 57, 79, 153,
Massoth, N.A., 283, 284 Minuchin, S., 177, 196, 169, 234, 235, 239, 243,
Masten, A.5 ., 172, 196 276,286 244,247
Mastria, E.O., 272, 286 Missing Children Assis - Newman, M.R ., 225, 231,
Mastria, M.A., 272, 286 tance Act of 1984, 68, 82 239, 240-243, 245, 246
Maurer, A., 201, 219 Mitchel, L., 32, 51, 52 Nezworski, T., 115, 140,
Mausner, J.5., 24, 52 Mitchum, N .T., 259, 267 152, 165
Maximus, Inc ., 32, 41, 52 Mnookin, RH., 57, 82 Nguyen, T.V., 162, 166
Mayfield, A., 183, 198 Moen, P., 33, 51, 162, 169 Norelius, K.L., 283, 285
Mayhall, P.O ., 271, 286 Moore, CK. , 214, 219 Norgard, K.E., 271, 286
McCleer, S.V., 89,107 Moos, RH., 154, 165 Nover, R , 152, 161, 168
McCombs, A., 185, 186, Morgan, S.R., 8, 12, 19,
196 213, 215, 216, 219 O'Brien, S., 239, 245
McConaughy, S.H., 95, Morris, J.L., 29, 52 O'Connor, S., 153, 154,
105 Morse, H.A., 138, 143, 158, 164, 170, 203, 220
McCord, J., 8, 19 213,219 O'Leary, D., 156, 169
McGonigle, J.J., 13, 17, Mortimer, J.T., 161, 169 O'Toole , R, 29, 38, 52
215-217 Moser, J., 8, 18 Oates, R.K., 93-95, 107,
McGrath, M.L., 278, 279, Moses, J.T., 240, 246 108, 128, 129, 144, 199,
285 Mosk , M.D., 130, 146, 219
McGuffog, C , 155, 169 252, 253, 268 Ohio v. Roberts, 73, 82
McKnew, D.H ., 183, 198 Mrazek, D., 96, 107, 136, Oldershaw, L., 128, 130,
McNeil , L.E., 94, 105 142 131, 144, 209, 219
McShane, M., 43, 44, 46, Mrazek, P. B., 96, 107, ous, D., 296, 298
52 271,286 Olsen, K., 153, 165
McWilliams, S.A., 227, 245 Myer, M.H., 261, 267 Olson, D.H ., 177, 196
Mednick, S.A., 176, 196 Myers, J.E.B., 64, 82 Olweus, D., 127, 144
306 AUTHOR INDEX
Orvaschel, H., 180, 181, Porter, F.S., 259, 262, 267 Risin , L.I., 43, 44, 46, 53
184, 197 Potter, L., 132, 142 Rizley, R , 93, 105, 1l1 ,
Otto, R.K., 55, 71, 81 Power, E., 3, 18, 201, 218, 129, 137, 141, 145, 151,
251,266 163, 165
Paisley, P.O. , 235, 247 Poythress, N.G., 56, 57, Roberts, J., 93, 107, 203, 219
Papatola, K., 89, 106, 1l0, 61, 66, 72, 78, 82 Robertson , E., 176, 197
134, 136, 137, 142 Pratt, M., 227, 245 Robins on, D.R, 270, 284
Pardek, J.A., 234, 247 Prince v. Massachusetts, Robinson, M.G. , 152, 161,
Pardek, J.T., 234, 247 56,82 168
Parental Kidnapping Pre- Prinz, R.J., 193, 198 Robinson, N.M ., 157, 160,
vention Act of 1980, 68, Provence, S., 158, 169 166
82 Prusoff, B.A., 184, 187, Roe v. Wade, 56, 82
Parke, RD., 4, 5, 19, 89, 198 Rogers, B., 127, 145
107, 109, 137, 144, 192, Rogers, c.u.. 71, 82
197, 200, 201, 219, 269, Quinton, D., 136, 137, Rogers, E.S., 282, 287
270, 282, 286 142, 144, 145, 153, 169 Rohner, E.e., 155, 169,
Patterson, G.R, 1l3, 127, 212,220
144, 209, 210, 216, 220, Rachrnan , S.J., 250, 251, Rohner, RP., 155, 169,
272, 273, 286 267 212,220
Paulsen, M., 231, 247 Radda Barnen, 24, 53 Rohrbeck, c:«, 89, 90,
Paulsen, M.G., 64, 82 Radke-Yarrow, M., 157, 108, 270, 286
Paykel, E.S., 187, 198 166, 183, 188, 197, 198 Rosario , M., 156, 159, 160,
Peacock, A., 128, 129, 144 Ragozin, A.S. , 157, 160, 169
Pelcovitz, D., 154, 168, 166 Rosen, c.. 241, 245
251,267 Raphael, B., 193, 195 Rosenbaum, A., 88, 97,
Pelton , L.H ., 29, 32, 34, Rappaport, J., 226-228, 106, 107, 156, 169
53, 91, 107, 111, 144 247 Rosenberg , A.H ., 60, 82
Pensk y, E., 136, 137, 139, Ray, R., 273, 286 Rosenberg, D., 11, 17, 160,
140 Reed, RB. , 153, 169 161, 163, 166, 201, 212,
Perri, M.G. , 8, 19 Reichler, RJ., 179, 197 218
Perry, M.A. , 128, 129, 144 Reid, J.B., 12, 18, 113, 127, Rosenberg, M.s., 14, 19,
Perry, P., 214, 219 128, 130, 144, 145, 210, 61, 82, 101, 107, 214,
Peters , S.D ., 43, 46, 47, 53 216, 220, 252, 254, 267, 220, 237, 241, 242, 247
Petrila, J., 56, 57, 61, 66, 270, 272, 286 Rosenberg, R, 176, 196
72,78,82 Reid, j.c.. 182, 196 Rosenfield-Schlichter,
Phillips, S., 189, 195 Reidy, T.J., 130, 131, 145 M.D ., 239, 247
Phipps-Yonas, S., 175, 197 Reppucci , N.D., 14, 19, Rosenthal, J.A., 31, 33, 53
Pianta, s.c.. 157, 169 27, 43-46, 52, 64, 71, 72, Rosman, B.L., 177, 196
Pierce v. Society of Sisters, 76, 79, 61, 101, 107, 214, Ross, A.W., 275, 285
56,82 220, 237, 241, 242, 247 Rovine , M., 1l5, 1l8, 140
Pines, A.M., 42, 53 Reynolds, G.S., 272, 286 Rugg , D., 233, 245
Piotrkowski, c.s.. 161, 169 Rice, J.M., 15, 19, 200, Runyan, D.K., 71, 81, 82
Platt, J.J., 255, 267 219, 239, 246, 251, 266, Rusch , RG ., 207, 220
Plotkin, s.c.. 8, 19 279, 280, 282, 286 Russell, A., 28, 30, 32, 38,
Podorefsky, D., 194, 195 Richardson, M.T., 253, 266 39,53
Poertner, J., 238, 247 Richardson, RA., 134, Russell, c.s.. 177, 196
Pokracki, D., 227, 245 141, 270, 285 Russell, D.E.H ., 28, 43-
Polansky, N., 26, 53, 86, Ricks, M.H ., 152, 169 45, 47-49, 53, 86, 107
87, 93, 94, 96, 107 Rigler, D., 4, 5, 19, 89, 90, Rutter, M., 1l0, 136, 137,
Pollock, c.. 135, 145 108, 191, 192, 197, 201, 142, 144, 145, 153, 169,
Pomeroy, W.B., 28, 43, 52 220 172, 176, 178, 197
AUTHOR INDEX 307
Sack, WH., 41, 53 Seiffer, R , 180, 197 Snell, L., 194, 196
Salzinger, S., 154, 156, Seloes, J., 135, 142 Snyder, J., 234, 239, 243,
159, 160, 168, 169 Sen ate Report No . 123 247
Sam ero ff, A.J., 151, 163, Sera fica, E C , 211, 217 Solnit, AJ., 58, 80, 98, 106
169, 180, 197 Sgro i, S.M ., 259, 261, 262, Solomo n, C R, 212, 221
Sarnit, C, 251, 267 267 Soong, W., 260, 267
Sandgrund, A., 3, 18, 159, Shaffe~ D., 154, 159, 167 Soumenkoff, G., 233, 245
170, 201, 213, 218, 220 Shaffer, H.R .,211, 220 Spearly, J.L., 34, 53
Sandler, H.M ., 153, 154, Shanok, S., 194, 196 Spinetta, J.J., 4, 5, 19, 89,
158, 164 Shapiro, V., 152, 167 90, 108, 154, 170, 191,
Sandler, J., 238, 248, 272- Shaver, P., 152, 168 192, 197, 201, 220
276, 282, 285-287 Shaw, D., 273, 286 Spivak, G., 255, 267, 278,
Sands, K., 206, 218 Shelton, P.R , 251, 267 287
Sands, S.K , 156, 158, 167 Shermack, R, 241, 245 Sprenkle, D.H ., 177, 196
Santosky v. Kramer, 56, 82 Sherman, D., 160, 162, Sroufe, L.A. , , 8- 10, 17,
Sap erstein, L., 241, 245 167, 270, 285 89, 93, 105, 110-113,
Sarber, RE., 239, 247 Sherman, L.W , 60, 82, 115, 116, 121, 124, 137,
Sceery, A., 152, 168 154, 168 139, 140-142, 145, 151,
Schachtel, D., 15, 17 Sherman, T., 188, 195 155, 159, 166, 167, 170,
Schaeffer, S., 112, 124, 143 Sherrod, K.B., 153, 154, 205,220
Schaffer, H.R , 276, 286 158, 164, 170, 203, 220 St. Lawrence, J.S., 273,
Sch akel, J.A. , 94, 95, 107 Sherry, D., 204, 218 274, 285, 287
Schau ghency, E.A., 194, Shilan sky, M., 211, 221 St. Pierre, J., 208, 221
197 Sholomska s, D., 184, 187, Stahl, J., 116, 118, 124,
Schell enbach, CJ., 134, 198 144, 158, 169
135, 142, 159, 165 Shure, M.B., 255, 267, Stamler, J., 32, 51
Schillin g II , R.E , 283, 285 278, 287 Starr, RH., Jr., 4, 6, 7, 10,
Schindler, E , 208, 220 Siegal, M., 161, 162, 170 12-14, 19, 23, 53, 87, 91,
Schinke, S.P., 283, 285 Sigal, J., 283, 284 96, 108, 158-160, 166,
Schneewind, KA , 151, Silbert, M.H., 42, 53 170, 200, 202, 213- 215,
153, 167 Silver, H .K., 4, 5, 7, 18, 220
Schn eid er, C, 91, 108 55, 63, 81, 199, 219, 230, Steele, B.E , 4, 5, 7, 18, 55,
Schneider-Rosen, K, 112, 246 63, 81, 89, 108, 109, 134,
116, 124, 142, 145 Silver, L.B., 135, 145 143, 145, 156, 170, 199,
Schore, E.L., 178, 197 Silverman , EN., 4, 5, 7, 214, 219, 220, 230, 246
Schuerman, J.R , 251, 266 18, 55, 63, 81, 109, 135, Steinberg, L., 162, 170
Schulsinger, E , 176, 196 143, 199, 219, 230, 246, Steinmetz, S.K, 37, 39,
Schultz, L., 112, 116, 124, 247 41, 42, 53, 86, 88, 89, 91,
143 Silverman, W.K , 179, 197 108, 134, 145, 156, 162,
Schultz, WJ ., 229, 231, 247 Sisson, L.A ., 212, 220 170, 234, 247
Schwartz, B.A , 175, 197 Skeen, P., 235, 246 Stem, L., 203, 219
Scott , KG., 176, 197 Sku se, D., 136, 142 Stevens, J.H ., 160, 170
Scott, R.A., 212, 220 Slobogin, C , 56, 57, 61, Stillwell, S.L., 241, 247
Scott , W.O .N ., 277, 282, 66, 72, 78, 82 Stockard, J., 156, 168
287 Slotkin, J., 185, 186, 196 Stocking, S.H. , & Associ-
Seb es, J., 134, 142 Smetan a, J., 129, 133, 142 ates, 162, 167
Sedney, M.A. , 43, 44, 46, Smets, A.C , 177, 197 Stoll, K , 43-46, 51
53 Smith, C , 98, 108 Ston e, N .W., 211, 220
Seech, M., 88, 106 Smith , J.E., 250, 251, 267 Stonebr ook, B., 251, 266
Seeley, J.W., 26, 51, 87, 96, Smith , R, 194, 196 Storer, D., 178, 195
106, 297, 298 Snedecor, S.T., 230, 247 Stovall, A. , 15, 17
308 AUTHOR INDEX
Straker, G., 128, 131-133, Tufts New England Medi- VVaterman, J., 94, 107, 130,
145 cal Center, 257, 267 133, 140, 159, 165, 199,
Straus, M.A, 28, 37, 39- Turbett, P., 29, 38, 52 200, 219, 260, 263, 266
42, 48, 49, 51, 53, 86, Turner, S.M., 179, 198 VVaters, E., 112, 114, 115,
88, 89, 91, 108, 134, 145, Twentyman, C.T., 8, 11, 140, 141, 146, 151, 164,
156, 162, 170, 212, 220, 17-19, 89, 90, 108, 128, 205, 217, 220
234, 247, 293, 298 130, 132, 141, 143, 156, VVaters, G., 128, 130, 131,
Stringer, S.A ., 209, 220 168, 208, 217, 233, 234, 144
Strock, B.D. , 184, 198 244, 245, 270, 272, 284, VVatson, D., 155, 170
Sturkie, K., 260, 263, 267 286 VVatson-Perczel, M., 15,
Sudia, C.E ., 96, 108 Tyler, c.w., [r., 41, 52 18,239,246
Sugal, D .P., 255, 267 VVebb, M.E., 8, 18, 239,
Sugarman, D., 97, 107 246
United States v. Iron Shell ,
Sussman, E.J., 128, 145, VVebe~ R.A., 157, 160, 168
74,83
159, 170, 190, 197 VVebster-Stratton, c.. 189,
Urquiza, A.J., 251, 257,
Sylvester, C.E ., 179, 197 198
266
VVeeks, D.G ., 184, 198
Urzi , T., 59, 60, 82
Talbot, N .E., 162, 170 VVeinraub, M., 157, 160,
Taplin, P.S., 113, 128, 130, 170
Valentine, D.P., 25, 53
145, 210, 22, 252, 254, VVeissman, M.M ., 180,
Van Dercar, c., 272, 273,
267, 270, 286 184, 186, 187, 197, 198
286
Tatelbaum, R, 297, 298 VVells, E.A ., 128, 129, 144
Van Hasselt, V.B., 5, 6, 9,
Taylor, D., 115, 140 wells, S., 66, 68, 81
12, 13, 17, 199, 207,
Templin, K., 253, 267 211-217, 220, 283, 284 VVelsh, R.J., 214, 219
Terrell, D., 227, 245 VVerner, E.E., 172, 198
Vaughn, B., 204, 214, 218
Tertinger, D.A., 239, 247, VVesch, D., 239, 246, 279,
Vietze, P.M ., 153, 154,
279,287 158, 164, 170, 203, 220 286
Thibaut, J., 71, 82 Von Eye, A., 115, 143
VVest, M.O ., 193, 198, 211,
Thompson, R,118, 142 Vondra, J., 7, 18, 149, 154, 219
Thompson, R.A ., 56, 58, Whatley, M.H ., 236, 237,
162, 163, 165, 167, 170
64, 65, 82, 115, 118, 142, 243,247
143 Wheeler, J.R, 265, 266
Toedter, L., 89, 95, 97, 106 VVagne~ N .N ., 43-46, 51 VVhelan, J.P., 275, 276, 285
Tong, L., 94, 108 VVahler, R.G ., 127, 142, VVhitcomb, D., 70, 72, 74,
Tonge, w.t.. 178, 195 145, 194, 198, 282, 287 78,83
Tooman, P., 29, 50 VVaisbren, S.E., 211, 221 VVhite, K.M. , 234, 239,
Toro, P.A, 93, 108 VVald, M.S ., 57, 58, 61, 83, 243,247
Trainor, C.M ., 28, 30, 32, 98,108 White, R, 211, 221
38,39,53 VValker, A, 135, 142 VVhitnig, L., 27, 53
Traylor, J., 194, 196 VValker, C.E ., 15, 19, 250, VVhittaker, J.K., 160, 170
Treiber, EA., 190, 196, 252, 255, 256, 268 Wieder, S., 152, 161, 168
270,286 VValker, L., 71, 82 Williams. D.P., 26, 53, 86,
Trickett, P.K., 128, 130, wsn. S., 114, 115, 140, 87, 93, 94, 96, 107
131, 145, 159, 170, 190, 151, 164, 205, 217 VViIIiams, G.J., 252, 268
197, 209, 220 VValsh-AIIis, G., 184, 197 Wilson, A ., 227, 245
Tronick, E.Z ., 154, 170, VValters, c .c.. 209, 219 VViIson, H .B., 230, 247
187, 195 VVarner, v.. 184, 187, 198 Wilson, S.K., 186, 194, 198
Trowell, J., 251, 267 VVarren, D.A , 212, 221 Wimberley, RC., 91, 107
Trudell, B., 236, 237, 243, VVasserman, G.A ., 211, VVisconsin v. Yoder, 56, 83
247 212,221 VVittig, B., 113, 114, 140
AUTHOR INDEX 309
Wolbert , W.A., 262-264, WurtelIe, S.K., 241, 248 Zak, L., 186, 194, 198
268 Wyatt, G.E., 43-48, 53 Zax, M., 180, 197
Wolf, B.M ., 157, 160, 170 Zigler, E., 5, 18, 90, 107,
Wolfe, D.A. , 4, 5, 10, 11, Xenak is, S.N., 194, 196 110, 136, 137, 139, 143,
13, 19, 89, 91, 93, 94, 96, 153, 162, 168, 170
100, 104, 108, 130, 146, Yanushefski, A.M ., 95, 106 Zimmerman, M.L., 262-
171, 173, 175, 183, 186, Ye, W., 184, 197 264,268
189, 192-194, 198, 199, Yllo, K., 86, 88, 91, 106 Zirpoli, T.]., 211, 221
201, 202, 207, 208, 221, Yoder, N .M., 211, 217 zen. D., 116, 118, 124,
238, 248, 252, 254, 268, Youngblade, L.M ., 109, 144, 158, 169
270, 273-276, 281, 282, 110, 113, 132, 134, 137, Zuravin, S.J., 33, 53, 243,
287 139, 141, 142, 146 248
Wolfe, V.V., 93, 94, 96, 108 Yule, B., 250, 251, 267 Zweier, D., 6, 12, 19, 159,
Wood, R.P., 255, 267 170, 214, 215, 220
Worland, J., 184, 198 Zahn-Waxler, C; 157, 166,
Wulff, L., 211, 221 183, 190, 197, 198
SUBJECT INDEX
311
312 SUBJECT INDEX